Introduction to Statistics in Psychology [6 ed.] 1292000740, 9781292000749

Statistics can be difficult, but this revised 3rd edition of Introduction to Statistics in Psychology makes it much easi

267 74 21MB

English Pages 712 [745] Year 2014

Report DMCA / Copyright

DOWNLOAD FILE

Polecaj historie

Introduction to Statistics in Psychology [6 ed.]
 1292000740, 9781292000749

Table of contents :
Cover
Contents
Guided tour
Introduction
Acknowledgements
1 Why statistics?
Overview
1.1 Introduction
1.2 Research on learning statistics
1.3 What makes learning statistics difficult?
1.4 Positive about statistics
1.5 What statistics doesn’t do
1.6 Easing the way
1.7 What do I need to know to be an effective user of statistics?
1.8 A few words about SPSS
Key points
PART 1 Descriptive statistics
2 Some basics: Variability and measurement
Overview
2.1 Introduction
2.2 Variables and measurement
2.3 Major types of measurement
Key points
Computer analysis
3 Describing variables: Tables and diagrams
Overview
3.1 Introduction
3.2 Choosing tables and diagrams
3.3 Errors to avoid
Key points
Computer analysis
4 Describing variables numerically: Averages, variation and spread
Overview
4.1 Introduction
4.2 Typical scores: mean, median and mode
4.3 Comparison of mean, median and mode
4.4 The spread of scores: variability
Key points
Computer analysis
5 Shapes of distributions of scores
Overview
5.1 Introduction
5.2 Histograms and frequency curves
5.3 The normal curve
5.4 Distorted curves
5.5 Other frequency curves
Key points
Computer analysis
6 Standard deviation and z-scores: The standard unit of measurement in statistics
Overview
6.1 Introduction
6.2 Theoretical background
6.3 Measuring the number of standard deviations – the z -score
6.4 A use of z -scores
6.5 The standard normal distribution
6.6 An important feature of z-scores
Key points
Computer analysis
7 Relationships between two or more variables: Diagrams and tables
Overview
7.1 Introduction
7.2 The principles of diagrammatic and tabular presentation
7.3 Type A: both variables numerical scores
7.4 Type B: both variables nominal categories
7.5 Type C: one variable nominal categories, the other numerical scores
Key points
Computer analysis
8 Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho
Overview
8.1 Introduction
8.2 Principles of the correlation coefficient
8.3 Some rules to check out
8.4 Coefficient of determination
8.5 Significance testing
8.6 Spearman’s rho – another correlation coefficient
8.7 An example from the literature
Key points
Computer analysis
9 Regression: Prediction with precision
Overview
9.1 Introduction
9.2 Theoretical background and regression equations
9.3 Standard error: how accurate are the predicted score and the regression equations?
Key points
Compu ter analysis
PART 2 Significance testing
10 Samples and populations: Generalising and inferring
Overview
10.1 Introduction
10.2 Theoretical considerations
10.3 The characteristics of random samples
10.4 Confidence intervals
Key points
Computer analysis
11 Statistical significance for the correlation coefficient: A practical introduction to statistical inference
Overview
11.1 Introduction
11.2 Theoretical considerations
11.3 Back to the real world: the null hypothesis
11.4 Pearson’s correlation coefficient again
11.5 The Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient
Key points
Computer analysis
12 Standard error: The standard deviation of the means of samples
Overview
12.1 Introduction
12.2 Theoretical considerations
12.3 Estimated standard deviation and standard error
Key points
Computer analysis
13 The t-test: Comparing two samples of correlated/related/paired scores
Overview
13.1 Introduction
13.2 Dependent and independent variables
13.3 Some basic revision
13.4 Theoretical considerations underlying the computer analysis
13.5 Cautionary note
Key points
Computer analysis
14 The t-test: Comparing two samples of unrelated/uncorrelated scores
Overview
14.1 Introduction
14.2 Theoretical considerations
14.3 Standard deviation and standard error
14.4 Cautionary note
Key points
Computer analysis
15 Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data
Overview
15.1 Introduction
15.2 Theoretical issues
15.3 Partitioning chi-square
15.4 Important warnings
15.5 Alternatives to chi-square
15.6 Chi-square and known populations
15.7 Chi-square for related samples – the McNemar test
15.8 Example from the literature
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
16 Probability
Overview
16.1 Introduction
16.2 The principles of probability
16.3 Implications
Key points
17 Reporting significance levels succinctly
Overview
17.1 Introduction
17.2 Shortened forms
17.3 Examples from the published literature
Key points
Computer analysis
18 One-tailed versus two-tailedsignificance testing
Overview
18.1 Introduction
18.2 Theoretical considerations
18.3 Further requirements
Key points
Computer analysis
19 Ranking tests: Nonparametric statistics
Overview
19.1 Introduction
19.2 Theoretical considerations
19.3 Nonparametric statistical tests
19.4 Three or more groups of scores
Key points
Computer analysis
PART 3 Introduction to analysis of variance
20 The variance ratio test: The F-ratio to compare two variances
Overview
20.1 Introduction
20.2 Theoretical issues and an application
Key points
Computer analysis
21 Analysis of variance (ANOVA): Introduction to the one-way unrelated or uncorrelated ANOVA
Overview
21.1 Introduction
21.2 Some revision and some new material
21.3 Theoretical considerations
21.4 Degrees of freedom
21.5 The analysis of variance summary table
Key points
Computer analysis
22 Analysis of variance for correlated scores or repeated measures
Overview
22.1 Introduction
22.2 Theoretical considerations underlying the computer analysis
22.3 Examples
Key points
Computer analysis
23 Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores: Two studies for the price of one?
Overview
23.1 Introduction
23.2 Theoretical considerations
23.3 Steps in the analysis
23.4 More on interactions
23.5 Three or more independent variables
Key points
Computer analysis
24 Multiple comparisons in ANOVA: Just where do the differences lie?
Overview
24.1 Introduction
24.2 Methods
24.3 Planned versus a posteriori (post hoc) comparisons
24.4 The Scheffé test for one-way ANOVA
24.5 Multiple comparisons for multifactorial ANOVA
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
25 Mixed-design ANOVA: Related and unrelated variables together
Overview
25.1 Introduction
25.2 Mixed designs and repeated measures
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
26 Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA): Controlling for additional variables
Overview
26.1 Introduction
26.2 Analysis of covariance
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
27 Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)
Overview
27.1 Introduction
27.2 MANOVA’s two stages
27.3 Doing MANOVA
27.4 Reporting your findings
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
28 Discriminant (function) analysis – especially in MANOVA
Overview
28.1 Introduction
28.2 Doing the discriminant function analysis
28.3 Reporting your findings
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
29 Statistics and the analysis of experiments
Overview
29.1 Introduction
29.2 The Patent Stats Pack
29.3 Checklist
29.4 Special cases
Key points
PART 4 More advanced correlational statistics
30 Partial correlation: Spurious correlation, third or confounding variables, suppressor variables
Overview
30.1 Introduction
30.2 Theoretical considerations
30.3 Doing partial correlation
30.4 Interpretation
30.5 Multiple control variables
30.6 Suppressor variables
30.7 An example from the research literature
30.8 An example from a student’s work
Key points
Computer analysis
31 Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data
Overview
31.1 Introduction
31.2 A bit of history
31.3 Concepts in factor analysis
31.4 Decisions, decisions, decisions
31.5 Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis
31.6 An example of factor analysis from the literature
31.7 Reporting the results
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
32 Multiple regression and multiple correlation
Overview
32.1 Introduction
32.2 Theoretical considerations
32.3 Stepwise multiple regression example
32.4 Reporting the results
32.5 An example from the published literature
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
33 Path analysis
Overview
33.1 Introduction
33.2 Theoretical considerations
33.3 An example from published research
33.4 Reporting the results
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
34 The analysis of a questionnaire/survey project
Overview
34.1 Introduction
34.2 The research project
34.3 The research hypothesis
34.4 Initial variable classification
34.5 Further coding of data
34.6 Data cleaning
34.7 Data analysis
Key points
Part 5 Assorted advanced techniques
35 The size of effects in statistical analysis: Do my findings matter?
Overview
35.1 Introduction
35.2 Statistical significance
35.3 Method and statistical efficiency
35.4 Size of the effect in studies
35.5 An approximation for nonparametric tests
35.6 Analysis of variance (ANOVA)
Key points
36 Meta-analysis: Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research
Overview
36.1 Introduction
36.2 The Pearson correlation coefficient as the effect size
36.3 Other measures of effect size
36.4 Effects of different characteristics of studies
36.5 First steps in meta-analysis
36.6 Illustrative example
36.7 Comparing a study with a previous study
36.8 Reporting the results
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
37 Reliability in scales and measurement: Consistency and agreement
Overview
37.1 Introduction
37.2 Item-analysis using item–total correlation
37.3 Split-half reliability
37.4 Alpha reliability
37.5 Agreement among raters
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
38 Confidence intervals
Overview
38.1 Introduction
38.2 The relationship between significance and confidence intervals
38.3 Regression
38.4 Other confidence intervals
Key points
Computer analysis
39 The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables
Overview
39.1 Introduction
39.2 Statistical approaches to finding moderator effects
39.3 The hierarchical multiple regression approach to identifying moderator effects (or interactions)
39.4 The ANOVA approach to identifying moderator effects (i.e. interactions)
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
40 Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right
Overview
40.1 Introduction
40.2 Types of statistical power analysis and their limitations
40.3 Doing power analysis
40.4 Calculating power
40.5 Reporting the results
Key points
Computer analysis
PART 6 Advanced qualitative or nominal techniques
41 Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables
Overview
41.1 Introduction
41.2 A two-variable example
41.3 A three-variable example
41.4 Reporting the results
Key points
Computer analysis
Recommended further reading
42 Multinomial logistic regression: Distinguishing between several different categories or groups
Overview
42.1 Introduction
42.2 Dummy variables
42.3 What can multinomial logistic regression do?
42.4 Worked example
42.5 Accuracy of the prediction
42.6 How good are the predictors?
42.7 The prediction
42.8 Interpreting the results
42.9 Reporting the results
Key points
Computer analysis
43 Binomial logistic regression
Overview
43.1 Introduction
43.2 Typical example
43.3 Applying the logistic regression procedure
43.4 The regression formula
43.5 Reporting the results
Key points
Computer analysis
Appendix A Testing for excessively skewed distributions
Appendix B1 Large-sample formulae for the nonparametric tests
Appendix B2 Nonparametric tests for three or more groups
Appendix C Extended table of significance for the Pearson correlation coefficient
Appendix D Table of significance for the Spearman correlation coefficient
Appendix E Extended table of significance for the t-test
Appendix F Table of significancefor chi-square
Appendix G Extended table of significance for the sign test
Appendix H Table of significance for the Wilcoxon matched pairs test
Appendix I Table of significance for the Mann–Whitney U-test
Appendix J Table of significance values for the F-distribution
Appendix K Table of significant values of t when making multiple t-tests
GLOSSARY
References
Index

Citation preview

Sixth edition

➜ SPSS steps are covered for each test,

illustrated by full-colour screenshots. ➜ Fully updated with reference to the latest

edition of SPSS Statistics version 22. ➜ Covers the statistics that SPSS doesn’t reach.

Dr Mike Cox, Newcastle University

Key aspects of the statistical analysis process are highlighted in these features: ➜ Key concepts boxes explain important terms

in more detail and Focus on sections explore more complex topics in greater depth. ➜ Explaining statistics present and separate out the

mathematical principles underlying the analyses.

Introduction to

Key features of this new edition:

‘An excellent text written by two experienced educators. Its well-crafted, informal style make it an ideal text for introducing the world of statistics  and SPSS to psychology undergraduates’

Statistics in Psychology

Introduction to Statistics in Psychology, sixth edition is a comprehensive, modern student guide to understanding and using statistics in psychological research. This edition has been significantly revised to incorporate the essential SPSS steps required for carrying out statistical analysis.

Introduction to

Statistics in Psychology

S ed ixt iti h on

with SPSS

The text is structured in short, accessible chapters and it suits statistics courses at all levels. Examples of real psychological studies employing the techniques are provided throughout, making this an essential guide to choosing tests, interpreting findings and writing up research.

Dennis Howitt and Duncan Cramer are with Loughborough University.

Cover image: © Getty Images

CVR_HOWI0749_06_SE_CVR.indd 1

Howitt & Cramer

This book is supported by a companion website featuring a range of resources to help students in their studies. Self-test questions, additional datasets and handy quick guides to carrying out tests can all be found at www.pearsoned.co.uk/howitt

Dennis Howitt & Duncan Cramer

www.pearson-books.com

22/11/2013 10:03

Introduction to Statistics in Psychology

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 1

11/18/13 10:18 AM

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 2

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Introduction to Statistics in Psychology Sixth Edition

Dennis Howitt  Loughborough University Duncan Cramer  Loughborough University

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 3

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Pearson Education Limited Edinburgh Gate Harlow CM20 2JE United Kingdom Tel: +44 (0)1279 623623 Web: www.pearson.com/uk First published 1997 (print) Second edition published 2000 (print) Revised second edition 2003 (print) Third edition 2005 (print) Fourth edition 2008 (print) Fifth edition 2011 (print) Sixth edition published 2014 (print and electronic) © Prentice Hall Europe 1997 (print) © Pearson Education Limited 2000, 2011 (print) © Pearson Education Limited 2014 (print and electronic) The rights of Dennis Howitt and Duncan Cramer to be identified as authors of this work have been asserted by them in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without either the prior written permission of the publisher or a licence permitting restricted copying in the United Kingdom issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency Ltd, Saffron House, 6–10 Kirby Street, London EC1N 8TS. The ePublication is protected by copyright and must not be copied, reproduced, transferred, distributed, leased, licensed or publicly performed or used in any way except as specifically permitted in writing by the publishers, as allowed under the terms and conditions under which it was purchased, or as strictly permitted by applicable copyright law. Any unauthorised distribution or use of this text may be a direct infringement of the author’s and the publishers’ rights and those responsible may be liable in law accordingly. All trademarks used herein are the property of their respective owners. The use of any trademark in this text does not vest in the author or publisher any trademark ownership rights in such trademarks, nor does the use of such trademarks imply any affiliation with or endorsement of this book by such owners. ISBN: 978-1-292-00074-9 (print) 978-1-292-00076-3 (PDF) 978-1-292-00075-6 (eText) British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data A catalogue record for the print edition is available from the British Library Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Howitt, Dennis. Introduction to statistics in psychology / Dennis Howitt, Loughborough University, Duncan Cramer, Loughborough University. -- 6th Edition. pages cm ISBN 978-1-292-00074-9 1. Psychometrics.  I. Cramer, Duncan, 1948-  II. Title. BF39.H74 2013 150.1’5195--dc23 2013037101 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 18 17 16 15 14 Cover image © Getty Images Print edition typeset in 9.5/12pt Sabon by 35 Print edition printed in Great Britain by Butler, Tanner and Dennis Ltd NOTE THAT ANY PAGE CROSS REFERENCES REFER TO THE PRINT EDITION

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 4

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Brief contents Contents Guided tour Introduction Acknowledgements

1 Why statistics?

Part 1 Descriptive statistics

2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

Some basics: Variability and measurement Describing variables: Tables and diagrams Describing variables numerically: Averages, variation and spread Shapes of distributions of scores Standard deviation and z-scores: The standard unit of measurement in statistics Relationships between two or more variables: Diagrams and tables Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho Regression: Prediction with precision

vii xx xxv xxvii 1 17 19 29 44 58 71 86 98 120

Part 2 Significance testing

133



135



10 Samples and populations: Generalising and inferring 11 Statistical significance for the correlation coefficient: A practical introduction to statistical inference 12 Standard error: The standard deviation of the means of samples 13 The t-test: Comparing two samples of correlated/related/paired scores 14 The t-test: Comparing two samples of unrelated/uncorrelated scores 15 Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data 16 Probability 17 Reporting significance levels succinctly 18 One-tailed versus two-tailed significance testing 19 Ranking tests: Nonparametric statistics

143 157 165 179 196 218 224 232 238

Part 3 Introduction to analysis of variance

253



255



20 The variance ratio test: The F-ratio to compare two variances 21 Analysis of variance (ANOVA): Introduction to the one-way unrelated or uncorrelated ANOVA 22 Analysis of variance for correlated scores or repeated measures 23 Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores: Two studies for the price of one? 24 Multiple comparisons in ANOVA: Just where do the differences lie? 25 Mixed-design ANOVA: Related and unrelated variables together 26 Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA): Controlling for additional variables

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 5

264 282 298 326 337 354

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Brief contents

vi



27 Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) 28 Discriminant (function) analysis – especially in MANOVA 29 Statistics and the analysis of experiments

Part 4 More advanced correlational statistics

30 Partial correlation: Spurious correlation, third or confounding variables, suppressor variables 31 Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data 32 Multiple regression and multiple correlation 33 Path analysis 34 The analysis of a questionnaire/survey project

370 386 401 409 411 423 444 460 476

Part 5 Assorted advanced techniques

485



487



35 The size of effects in statistical analysis: Do my findings matter? 36 Meta-analysis: Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research 37 Reliability in scales and measurement: Consistency and agreement 38 Confidence intervals 39 The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables 40 Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right

495 515 529 540 562

Part 6 Advanced qualitative or nominal techniques

587



589



41 Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables 42 Multinomial logistic regression: Distinguishing between several different categories or groups 43 Binomial logistic regression

614 632

Appendices 649 Glossary 685 References 693 Index 699

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 6

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Contents Guided tour xx Introduction xxv Acknowledgements xxvii



1 Why statistics?

1

Overview 1 1.1 Introduction 2 1.2 Research on learning statistics 4 1.3 What makes learning statistics difficult? 5 1.4 Positive about statistics 7 1.5 What statistics doesn’t do 10 1.6 Easing the way 12 1.7 What do I need to know to be an effective user of statistics? 13 1.8 A few words about SPSS 15 Key points 16

Part 1 Descriptive statistics

17



19

2 Some basics: Variability and measurement

Overview 19 2.1 Introduction 20 2.2 Variables and measurement 21 2.3 Major types of measurement 22 Key points 26 Computer analysis 27



3 Describing variables: Tables and diagrams

29

Overview 29 3.1 Introduction 30 3.2 Choosing tables and diagrams 31 3.3 Errors to avoid 39 Key points 40 Computer analysis 40

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 7

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Contents

viii



4 Describing variables numerically: Averages, variation and spread

44

Overview 44 4.1 Introduction 45 4.2 Typical scores: mean, median and mode 46 4.3 Comparison of mean, median and mode 50 4.4 The spread of scores: variability 50 Key points 55 Computer analysis 56



5 Shapes of distributions of scores

58

Overview 58 5.1 Introduction 59 5.2 Histograms and frequency curves 59 5.3 The normal curve 60 5.4 Distorted curves 62 5.5 Other frequency curves 64 Key points 68 Computer analysis 69



6 Standard deviation and z-scores: The standard unit of measurement in statistics

71

Overview 71 6.1 Introduction 72 6.2 Theoretical background 72 6.3 Measuring the number of standard deviations – the z-score 76 6.4 A use of z-scores 77 6.5 The standard normal distribution 78 6.6 An important feature of z-scores 82 Key points 83 Computer analysis 84



7 Relationships between two or more variables: Diagrams and tables

86

Overview 86 7.1 Introduction 87 7.2 The principles of diagrammatic and tabular presentation 88 7.3 Type A: both variables numerical scores 89 7.4 Type B: both variables nominal categories 91 7.5 Type C: one variable nominal categories, the other numerical scores 93 Key points 95 Computer analysis 96



8 Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho

98

Overview 98 8.1 Introduction 99 8.2 Principles of the correlation coefficient 100

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 8

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Contents ix





8.3

Some rules to check out

106



8.4

Coefficient of determination

108



8.5

Significance testing

109



8.6

Spearman’s rho – another correlation coefficient

109



8.7

An example from the literature

113

Key points 115 Computer analysis 116



9 Regression: Prediction with precision

120

Overview 120

9.1 Introduction

121



9.2

Theoretical background and regression equations

124



9.3

Standard error: how accurate are the predicted score and the regression equations?

128

Key points 130 Computer analysis 131

Part 2 Significance testing

10 Samples and populations: Generalising and inferring

133 135

Overview 135

10.1 Introduction

136



10.2

Theoretical considerations

136



10.3

The characteristics of random samples

138



10.4

Confidence intervals

140

Key points 140 Computer analysis 141



11 Statistical significance for the correlation coefficient: A practical introduction to statistical inference

143

Overview 143

11.1 Introduction

144



11.2

Theoretical considerations

144



11.3

Back to the real world: the null hypothesis

146



11.4

Pearson’s correlation coefficient again

148



11.5

The Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient

152

Key points 154 Computer analysis 155

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 9

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Contents

x



12 Standard error: The standard deviation of the means of samples

157

Overview 157

12.1 Introduction

158



12.2

Theoretical considerations

158



12.3

Estimated standard deviation and standard error

159

Key points 162 Computer analysis 163



13 The t-test: Comparing two samples of correlated/related/paired scores

165

Overview 165

13.1 Introduction

166



13.2

Dependent and independent variables

168



13.3

Some basic revision

168



13.4

Theoretical considerations underlying the computer analysis

169



13.5

Cautionary note

174

Key points 176 Computer analysis 177



14 The t-test: Comparing two samples of unrelated/uncorrelated scores

179

Overview 179

14.1 Introduction

180



14.2

Theoretical considerations

181



14.3

Standard deviation and standard error

186



14.4

Cautionary note

192

Key points 193 Computer analysis 194



15 Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data

196

Overview 196

15.1 Introduction

197



15.2

Theoretical issues

198



15.3

Partitioning chi-square

204



15.4

Important warnings

205



15.5

Alternatives to chi-square

205



15.6

Chi-square and known populations

210



15.7

Chi-square for related samples – the McNemar test

212



15.8

Example from the literature

212

Key points 214 Computer analysis 215

Recommended further reading 217

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 10

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Contents xi





16 Probability

218

Overview 218 16.1 Introduction 219 16.2 The principles of probability 219 16.3 Implications 221 Key points 223



17 Reporting significance levels succinctly

224

Overview 224 17.1 Introduction 225 17.2 Shortened forms 225 17.3 Examples from the published literature 226 Key points 230 Computer analysis 231



18 One-tailed versus two-tailed significance testing

232

Overview 232 18.1 Introduction 233 18.2 Theoretical considerations 233 18.3 Further requirements 235 Key points 237 Computer analysis 237



19 Ranking tests: Nonparametric statistics

238

Overview 238 19.1 Introduction 239 19.2 Theoretical considerations 239 19.3 Nonparametric statistical tests 241 19.4 Three or more groups of scores 249 Key points 250 Computer analysis 250 Recommended further reading 252

Part 3 Introduction to analysis of variance

20 The variance ratio test: The F-ratio to compare two variances

253 255

Overview 255 20.1 Introduction 256 20.2 Theoretical issues and an application 257 Key points 261 Computer analysis 262

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 11

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Contents

xii



21 Analysis of variance (ANOVA): Introduction to the one-way unrelated or uncorrelated ANOVA

264

Overview 264

21.1 Introduction

265



21.2

Some revision and some new material

265



21.3

Theoretical considerations

266



21.4

Degrees of freedom

270



21.5

The analysis of variance summary table

276

Key points 279 Computer analysis 280



22 Analysis of variance for correlated scores or repeated measures

282

Overview 282

22.1 Introduction

283



22.2

285



22.3 Examples

Theoretical considerations underlying the computer analysis

286

Key points 295 Computer analysis 296



23 Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores: Two studies for the price of one?

298

Overview 298

23.1 Introduction

299



23.2

Theoretical considerations

300



23.3

Steps in the analysis

302



23.4

More on interactions

315



23.5

Three or more independent variables

318

Key points 322 Computer analysis 323



24 Multiple comparisons in ANOVA: Just where do the differences lie?

326

Overview 326

24.1 Introduction

327



24.2 Methods

328



24.3

Planned versus a posteriori (post hoc) comparisons

329



24.4

The Scheffé test for one-way ANOVA

330



24.5

Multiple comparisons for multifactorial ANOVA

332

Key points 334 Computer analysis 335 Recommended further reading 336

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 12

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Contents xiii





25 Mixed-design ANOVA: Related and unrelated variables together

337

Overview 337

25.1 Introduction

338



25.2

338

Mixed designs and repeated measures

Key points 351 Computer analysis 351 Recommended further reading 353



26 Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA): Controlling for additional variables

354

Overview 354

26.1 Introduction

355



26.2

356

Analysis of covariance

Key points 366 Computer analysis 367 Recommended further reading 369



27 Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

370

Overview 370

27.1 Introduction

371



27.2

MANOVA’s two stages

374



27.3

Doing MANOVA

376



27.4

Reporting your findings

381

Key points 382 Computer analysis 383 Recommended further reading 385



28 Discriminant (function) analysis – especially in MANOVA

386

Overview 386 28.1 Introduction 387 28.2 Doing the discriminant function analysis 389 28.3 Reporting your findings 396 Key points 397 Computer analysis 398 Recommended further reading 400



29 Statistics and the analysis of experiments

401

Overview 401

29.1 Introduction

402



29.2

402



29.3 Checklist

403



29.4

407

The Patent Stats Pack Special cases

Key points 408

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 13

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Contents

xiv

Part 4 More advanced correlational statistics

30 Partial correlation: Spurious correlation, third or confounding variables, suppressor variables

409 411

Overview 411

30.1 Introduction

412



30.2

Theoretical considerations

413



30.3

Doing partial correlation

415



30.4 Interpretation

416



30.5

Multiple control variables

417



30.6

Suppressor variables

417



30.7

An example from the research literature

418



30.8

An example from a student’s work

419

Key points 420 Computer analysis 421



31 Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data

423

Overview 423

31.1 Introduction

424



31.2

A bit of history

425



31.3

Concepts in factor analysis

427



31.4

Decisions, decisions, decisions

429



31.5

Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis

434



31.6

An example of factor analysis from the literature

436



31.7

Reporting the results

439

Key points 440 Computer analysis 441 Recommended further reading 443



32 Multiple regression and multiple correlation

444

Overview 444

32.1 Introduction

445



32.2

Theoretical considerations

445



32.3

Stepwise multiple regression example

451



32.4

Reporting the results

454



32.5

An example from the published literature

454

Key points 456 Computer analysis 457 Recommended further reading 459

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 14

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Contents xv





33 Path analysis

460

Overview 460 33.1 Introduction 461 33.2 Theoretical considerations 461 33.3 An example from published research 468 33.4 Reporting the results 471 Key points 473 Computer analysis 473 Recommended further reading 475



34 The analysis of a questionnaire/survey project

476

Overview 476 34.1 Introduction 477 34.2 The research project 477 34.3 The research hypothesis 479 34.4 Initial variable classification 480 34.5 Further coding of data 481 34.6 Data cleaning 482 34.7 Data analysis 482 Key points 484

Part 5 Assorted advanced techniques

35 The size of effects in statistical analysis: Do my findings matter?

485 487

Overview 487 35.1 Introduction 488 35.2 Statistical significance 488 35.3 Method and statistical efficiency 489 35.4 Size of the effect in studies 490 35.5 An approximation for nonparametric tests 492 35.6 Analysis of variance (ANOVA) 492 Key points 494



36 Meta-analysis: Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research

495

Overview 495 36.1 Introduction 496 36.2 The Pearson correlation coefficient as the effect size 498 36.3 Other measures of effect size 498 36.4 Effects of different characteristics of studies 499 36.5 First steps in meta-analysis 500

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 15

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Contents

xvi

36.6 Illustrative example 506 36.7 Comparing a study with a previous study 510 36.8 Reporting the results 510 Key points 512 Computer analysis 512 Recommended further reading 514



37 Reliability in scales and measurement: Consistency and agreement

515

Overview 515 37.1 Introduction 516 37.2 Item-analysis using item–total correlation 517 37.3 Split-half reliability 518 37.4 Alpha reliability 519 37.5 Agreement among raters 522 Key points 526 Computer analysis 527 Recommended further reading 528



38 Confidence intervals

529

Overview 529 38.1 Introduction 530 38.2 The relationship between significance and confidence intervals 533 38.3 Regression 536 38.4 Other confidence intervals 537 Key points 538 Computer analysis 539



39 The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables

540

Overview 540 39.1 Introduction 541 39.2 Statistical approaches to finding moderator effects 545 39.3 The hierarchical multiple regression approach to identifying moderator effects (or interactions) 545 39.4 The ANOVA approach to identifying moderator effects (i.e. interactions) 555 Key points 559 Computer analysis 560 Recommended further reading 561



40 Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right

562

Overview 562 40.1 Introduction 563 40.2 Types of statistical power analysis and their limitations 573 40.3 Doing power analysis 575 40.4 Calculating power 577

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 16

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Contents xvii



40.5 Reporting the results 581 Key points 582 Computer analysis 583

Part 6 Advanced qualitative or nominal techniques

41 Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

587 589

Overview 589 41.1 Introduction 590 41.2 A two-variable example 592 41.3 A three-variable example 599 41.4 Reporting the results 610 Key points 611 Computer analysis 612 Recommended further reading 613



42 Multinomial logistic regression: Distinguishing between several different categories or groups

614

Overview 614 42.1 Introduction 615 42.2 Dummy variables 617 42.3 What can multinomial logistic regression do? 618 42.4 Worked example 620 42.5 Accuracy of the prediction 621 42.6 How good are the predictors? 622 42.7 The prediction 625 42.8 Interpreting the results 627 42.9 Reporting the results 628 Key points 629 Computer analysis 630



43 Binomial logistic regression

632

Overview 632 43.1 Introduction 633 43.2 Typical example 637 43.3 Applying the logistic regression procedure 640 43.4 The regression formula 644 43.5 Reporting the results 645 Key points 646 Computer analysis 647

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 17

11/18/13 10:18 AM

xviii

Contents

Appendices Appendix A Testing for excessively skewed distributions 649 Appendix B1 Large-sample formulae for the nonparametric tests 652 Appendix B2 Nonparametric tests for three or more groups 654 Appendix C Extended table of significance for the Pearson correlation coefficient 660 Appendix D Table of significance for the Spearman correlation coefficient 663 Appendix E Extended table of significance for the t-test 666 Appendix F Table of significance for chi-square 669 Appendix G Extended table of significance for the sign test 670 Appendix H Table of significance for the Wilcoxon matched pairs test 673 Appendix I Table of significance for the Mann–Whitney U-test 676 Appendix J Appendix K

Table of significance values for the F-distribution 679 Table of significant values of t when making multiple t-tests 682

Glossary 685 References 693 Index 699

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 18

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Companion Website For open-access student resources specifically written to complement this textbook and support your learning, please visit www.pearsoned.co.uk/howitt

ON THE WEBSITE

Lecturer Resources For password-protected online resources tailored to support the use of this textbook in teaching, please visit www.pearsoned.co.uk/howitt

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 19

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Guided tour

CHAPTER 8 Correlation CoeffiCients: Pearson Correlation and sPearman’s rho

106

ChApter 4

Describing variables numerically Averages, variation and spread

We would write something like: ‘It was found that musical ability was inversely related to mathematical ability. The Pearson correlation coefficient was −0.90 which is statistically significant at the 5% level with a sample size of 10.’ The information in the final sentence will not be informative to you until you have studied Chapters 10 and 11. If we were to heed the advice of the 2010 Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (APA) we could write: ‘Musical ability was significantly inversely related to mathematical ability, r(8) = −.90, p < .05’. The number in brackets after r is the sample size minus 2. This number is called the degrees of freedom and is explained in Section 21.4. Statistical significance is usually reported as a proportion rather than a percentage. Computer packages like SPSS Statistics give the exact significance level. We should report this as a figure as it is more informative.

Box 8.1

Key concepts

Covariance Overview ●

● ●

● ● ●







Scores can be described or summarised numerically – for example the average of a sample of scores can be given. there are several measures of central tendency – the most typical or most likely score. the mean score is simply the average score assessed by the total of the scores divided by the number of scores. the mode is the numerical value of the most frequently occurring score. the median is the score in the middle if the scores are ordered from smallest to largest. the spread of scores can be expressed as the range (which is the difference between the largest and the smallest score).

Many of the basic concepts taught in introductory stat­ istics are relevant even at the advanced level. The concept of covariance is one of these. As we have seen, covariance is basically the average of the deviation from the mean for the variable X multiplied by the deviation of the variable Y. In other words, it is the top part of the Pearson correla­ tion formula. The correlation coefficient is simply the ratio of the covariance over the largest value that the covariance could take for a particular pair of variables. In other words, it is a standardised measure of covariance. But the term covariance crops up throughout this book in a number of different contexts. It is involved in ANOVA (especially the analysis of covariance) and regression, for example – lots of places, some of them unexpected.

Variance (an indicator of variability around the average) indicates the spread of scores in the data. Unlike the range, variance takes into account all of the scores. It is a ubiquitous statistical concept. Nominal data can only be described in terms of the numbers of cases falling in each cat­ egory. the mode is the only measure of central tendency that can be applied to nominal (category) data. Outliers are unusually large or small values in your data which are very atypical of your data. they can create the impression of trends in your analysis which are not really present. Identifying such outliers and dealing with them effectively can have an important impact on the quality of your research.

preparation revise the meaning of nominal (category) data and numerical score data.

Variance–covariance matrix for three variables

Table 8.3

Variable X

8.3

One phrase that might cause some consternation is that of the ‘variance–covariance’ matrix for a number of variables. This is simply a table (matrix) which includes the variances of each variable in the diagonal and their covariances off the diagonal. This is illustrated for vari­ ables X, Y and Z in Table 8.3. The diagonal contains the variances but the other numbers are the covariances – each of these is presented twice because the covariance of X with Z is the same as the covariance of Z with Z. Similar matrices are produced for correlation coeffi­ cients. However, in this case the diagonal consist of 1.00s (the correlation of a variable with itself is always 1) and the off­diagonals have the correlation coefficients of each variable with the other different variables.

Variable Y

Variable Z

Variable X

2.400

1.533

1.244

Variable Y

1.533

4.933

3.733

Variable Z

1.244

3.733

5.156

Some rules to check out ●

You should make sure that a straight line is the best fit to the scattergram points. If the best­fitting line is a curve such as in Figure 8.7 then you should not use the Pearson correlation coefficient. The reason for this is that the Pearson correlation

Clear overview

Key concepts

Introduce the chapter to give students a feel for the topics covered.

Offer guidance on the important concepts and issues discussed in the text.

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 20

11/18/13 10:18 AM

Guided tour xxi



CHAPTER 11 StatiStical Significance for the correlation coefficient

148

Box 11.1

Focus on

Focus on

Do correlations differ? Notice that throughout this chapter we are comparing a particular correlation coefficient obtained from our data with the correlation coefficient that we would expect to obtain if there were no relationship between the two variables at all. In other words, we are calculating the likelihood of obtaining the correlation coefficient based on our sample of data if, in fact, the correlation between these two variables in the population from which the sample was taken is actually 0.00. However, there are circumstances in which the researcher might wish to assess whether two correlations obtained in their research are significantly different from each other. Imagine, for example, that the researcher is investigating the relationship between satisfaction with one’s marriage and the length of time that individuals have been married. The researcher notes that the correlation between satisfaction and length

11.4

of marriage is 0.25 for male participants but 0.53 for female participants. There is clearly a difference here, but is it a statistically significant one? So essentially the researcher needs to know whether a correlation of 0.53 is significantly different from a correlation of 0.25 (the researcher has probably already tested the significance of each of these correlations separately using the sorts of methods described in this chapter but, of course, this does not answer the question of whether the two correlation coefficients differ from each other). It is a relatively simple matter to do this calculation. It has to be done by hand, unfortunately. The procedure for doing this is described in Section 36.7 Comparing a study with a previous study. In this section you will read about how to assess whether two correlation coefficients are significantly different from each other.

Explore particular concepts in more detail.

Pearson’s correlation coefficient again If you only ever use computer programs for your statistical analyses then you will not need what is in the section. Computer programs such as SPSS give exact significance levels for your computations and so there is no need to know about other methods of working out the significance level of a correlation coefficient. However, from time to time this may not be enough. For example, imagine that you are reviewing the research literature and find that one study reports a correlation of 0.66 between two variables but fails to give the significance level, then what do you do? This sort of situation does happen and not every research paper is exemplary in its statistical analysis. Or you simply wish to check that there is not a typographical error for the given significance level then what do you do? There are other circumstances in which you cannot rely on using the computer. So this section we will explain how significance levels may be obtained from tables so long as you know the size of the correlation coefficient and the sample size (or degrees of freedom) involved. The null hypothesis for research involving the correlation coefficient is that there is no relationship between the two variables. In other words, the null hypothesis implies that the correlation coefficient between two variables is 0.00 in the population (defined by the null hypothesis). So what if, in a sample of 10 pairs of scores, the correlation is 0.94 as for the data in Table 11.3? Is it likely that such a correlation would occur in a sample if it actually came from a population where the true correlation is zero? We are back to our basic problem of how likely it is that a correlation of 0.94 would occur if there really is no correlation in the population. We need to plot the distribution of correlations in random samples of 10 pairs drawn from this population. Unfortunately we do not have the population of scores, only a sample of scores. However, statisticians can use the variability of this sample of scores to estimate the variability in the population. Then the likely distribution of correlations

12.3 ESTIMATED STANDARD DEVIATION AND STANDARD ERROR

161

Explaining statistics

Explaining statistics 12.1

How the estimated standard error works Table 12.3

Steps in calculating the standard error X2 (squared scores)

X (scores) 5

25

7

49

3

9

6

36

4

16

5

25

∑ X = 30

∑ X 2 = 160

Take students through a statistical test with a detailed step-by-step explanation.

Table 12.3 is a sample of six scores taken at random from the population: 5, 7, 3, 6, 4, 5.

Step 1

Using this information we can estimate the standard error of samples of size 6 taken from the same population. Taking our six scores (X), we need to produce Table 12.3, where N = 6.

Step 2

Substitute these values in the standard error formula:

∑ X2 − (estimated) standard error =

(∑ X )

N −1 N

2

30 02 6 6−1

160 −

N =

= =

6

160 − =

900 6

5 2.449

160 − 150 10 5 5 = 2.449 2.449 2 1.414 = = 0 .5 58 2.449 2.449

Interpreting the results Roughly speaking, this suggests that on average sample means differ from the population mean by 0.58.

Reporting the results Standard error is not routinely reported although sometimes it is seen. It is no more informative than the standard deviation which is more likely to be included in reports. Many psychologists report the variance or standard deviation instead since this is just as informative descriptive statistics as the standard error.

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 21

11/18/13 10:18 AM

xxii

Guided tour

366

CHAPTER 26 AnAlysis of covAriAnce (AncovA): controlling for AdditionAl vAriAbles

Research examples ANCOVA cumming and co-workers (2012) studied the effect of physically maturing early in adolescence on the physical activity of girls. research has suggested that girls reduce their amounts of physical activity during adolescence and the health-related issues that this entails are obvious. is there a role for early maturation in this? the study compared early and late maturing adolescent girls with an average age of 12.7 years. the dependent variables were health-related matters such as physical activity behaviour, physical self-concept, and health-related quality of life. in each case it was expected that early maturing girls would score lower. the analysis employed several AncovA analyses compared early and late maturing girls on these variables. chronological age was included as the covariate since obviously maturation and age correlate together. Although the size of the differences tended to be small to moderate, the AncovAs repeatedly showed that early maturing girls scored lower on the health-related variables. it is noteworthy that early maturing girls rated themselves lower in terms of body attractiveness. this may have a bearing on their lower levels of involvement in physical activity.

Research examples Demonstrate how the statistical tests have been used in real research.

estevis, basso and combs (2012) investigated the effect of practice on the Wechsler Adult intelligence scale– iv. the participants were given the test at the start of the study and again a few months later. for some it was three months later and for the others it was six months later. they used various subscales from the test including verbal comprehension, Working Memory, Perceptual reasoning and Processing speed as well as the full scale iQ. they analysed the data using an AncovA design in which test versus retest and the various subscales were the related factors and three months versus six months was the independent factor. gender was entered as the covariate. bonferroni adjustment was employed to deal with the repeated significance testing problem. the interval between testing and retesting did not have a significant effect. Wright and Hardie (2012) write that the previous research on the relationship between handedness and anxiety fails to indicate a clear conclusion. one reason for expecting a relationship between anxiety and handedness is that the right-hand side hemisphere of the brain is involved in negative emotional states and inhibition. Anxiety is often classified as being situational in nature or alternatively as a personality trait of the individual. the researchers found that left-handed people have statistically significantly higher scores on state anxiety which supports the idea of the role of the right hemisphere. no trait anxiety differences were found but trait and state anxiety were significantly correlated. so AncovA was employed with trait anxiety as the control variable because of this correlation. the handedness relationship to state anxiety remained even in this analysis. the authors suggest that left-handers are more reactive personalities and so respond with state anxiety to the new situation that they were experiencing in the research laboratory as part of the research.

Key points ●

relying on AncovA to deal with the problems due to employing non-randomised allocation to the cells of the AnovA ignores the basic reason for doing randomised experiments in the first place – that the researcher does not know what unknown factors influence the outcome of the research. random allocation to conditions is the only practical and sound way of fully controlling for variables not included in the design.



it is not wise to use AncovA to try to correct for the sloppiness of your original design or procedures. Although, especially when using computers, you can include many covariates, it is best to be careful when planning your research to reduce the need for this. in randomised experiments, probably the control of the pre-test measure is the only circumstance requiring AncovA. of course, there are circumstances in which pre-tests are undesirable, especially as they risk sensitising participants as to the purpose of the study or otherwise influencing the post-test measures.

176

CHAPTER 13 The t-TesT: Comparing Two samples of CorrelaTed/relaTed/paired sCores

Key points ●

The related or correlated t-test is merely a special case of the one-way analysis of variance for related samples (Chapter 22). although it is frequently used in psychological research it tells us nothing more than the equivalent analysis of variance would do. since the analysis of variance is generally a more flexible statistic, allowing any number of groups of scores to be compared, it might be your preferred statistic. however, the common occurrence of the t-test in psychological research means that you need to have some idea about what it is.



The related t-test assumes that the distribution of the difference scores is not markedly skewed. if it is then the test may be unacceptably inaccurate. appendix a explains how to test for skewness.



if you compare many pairs of samples with each other in the same study using the t-test, you should consult Chapter 24 to find out about appropriate significance levels. There are better ways of making multiple comparisons, as they are called, but with appropriate adjustment to the critical values for significance, multiple t-tests can be justified.



if you find that your related t-test is not significant, it could be that your two samples of scores are not correlated, thus not meeting the assumptions of the related t-test.



significance Table 13.1 applies whenever we have estimated the standard error from the characteristics of a sample. however, if we had actually known the population standard deviation and consequently the standard error was the actual standard error and not an estimate, we should not use the t-distribution table. in these rare (virtually unknown) circumstances, the distribution of the t-score formula is that for the z-scores.



although the correlated t-test can be used to compare any pairs of scores, it does not always make sense to do so. for example, you could use the correlated t-test to compare the weights and heights of people to see if the weight mean and the height mean differ. Unfortunately, it is a rather stupid thing to do since the numerical values involved relate to radically different things which are not comparable with each other. it is the comparison which is nonsensical in this case. The statistical test is not to blame. on the other hand, one could compare a sample of people’s weights at different points in time quite meaningfully.

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 22

Key points Each chapter concludes with a set of the key points to provide a useful reminder when revising a topic

11/18/13 10:19 AM

Guided tour xxiii



Computer analysis

Computer analysis

Computer analysis MANOVA using SPSS

Figure 27.2

383

Step-by-step advice and instruction on analysing data using SPSS Statistics is provided at the end of each chapter.

spss statistics steps for manoVa

interpreting and reporting the output

384



a number of different multivariate tests are given in the multivariate tests output. pillai’s trace is as good as any for most purposes. For the tests for Between subjects effects output you only need to concentrate on the row for Group in this example.



you could write: ‘manoVa showed that teamwork training was effective in improving sporting behaviours, pillai’s F(6, 76) = 4.12, p < .01.’

CHAPTER 27 Multivariate analysis of variance (Manova)

SPSS screenshots The guidance on how to use SPSS for each statistical test is accompanied by screenshots, so the processes can be easily followed. SCREEnSHoT 27.1

the data

SCREEnSHoT 27.2

select the test

SCREEnSHoT 27.3

select the variables

SCREEnSHoT 27.4

select options

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 23

11/18/13 10:19 AM

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 24

11/18/13 10:19 AM

Introduction

Our hope is that this sixth edition of Introduction to Statistics in Psychology will con­ tribute even more to the student learning experience. A number of changes have been made to this end. In particular, a new introductory chapter has been incorporated which discusses the importance of statistics and why some students find it difficult. One thing has not changed which sets this book apart from others aimed at students: it continues to provide an accessible introduction to the wide range of statistics that are employed by professional researchers. Students using earlier editions of the book will by now often be well into teaching and research careers of their own. We hope that these further enhance­ ments may encourage them to keep Introduction to Statistics in Psychology permanently on their desks while they instruct their students how to do statistics properly. We have considered very carefully the need for instruction into how to compute stat­ istics using SPSS and other computer programs. Our approach in this book is to provide the basic steps needed for the computation but we have added a number of screenshots to help the reader with the analysis. Students of today are very familiar with computers and many do not need overly detailed instructions. Too much detailed step-by-step instruc­ tion tends to inhibit exploration of the program – trying things out simply to see what happens and using one’s intelligence and a bit of knowledge to work out what things mean. Students can become fixated on the individual steps and fail to learn the complete picture of doing statistics using SPSS or other computer programs. In the end, learning to use a computer program is quicker if the user takes some responsibility for their learn­ ing. Much of our daily use of computers in general is on a trial and error basis (we don’t need step-by-step instructions to use Facebook or eBay) so why should this be different for statistics programs? How many of us read instructions for the iPhone in detail before trying things out? Of course, there is nothing unusual about tying statistics textbooks to computer packages such as SPSS Statistics. Indeed, our Introduction to SPSS Statistics in Psychology is a good example of this approach. It provides just about the speediest and most thorough introduction to doing psychological statistics on SPSS. Unfortunately, SPSS is not the complete answer to the statistical needs of psychologists. It simply does not do everything that students (and professionals for that matter) need to know about. Some of these things are very simple and easily computed by hand if instructions are provided. Other things do require computer programs other than SPSS when procedures are not available on SPSS. We think that ideally psychologists should know the statistics which their discipline needs and not simply those that SPSS provides. SPSS is very good at what it does but there are times when additional help is needed. This is why we introduce students to other programs which will be helpful to them when necessary. One of the most important features of SPSS Statistics is that it is virtually universally available to students for little or no cost thanks to site licensing agreements. Unfortunately, this is not true of other commercial statistics software. For that reason we have suggested and recommended programs which are essentially free for the user. The Web has a surprisingly large amount of such software to carry out a wide range of

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 25

11/18/13 10:19 AM

xxvi

Introduction

statistical routines. A few minutes using Google or some other search engine will often be bountifully productive. Some of these programs are there to be downloaded but others, applets, are instantly available for calculations. We have added at the end of each chapter, advice on the use of software. This does not mean that we have abandoned responsibility for teaching how statistics works in favour of explaining how to press keys on a computer keyboard. Although we think it best that statistics are computed using statistics programs because the risk of simple calculation errors is reduced, it seems to us that knowing how to go about doing the calculations that computer programs will do for you leads to an understanding of statistics which relying on computers alone does not. So we have included in this edition sections entitled ‘Explaining statistics’ which are based on hand calculation methods which should help students understand better what the computer program does (more or less) when it is used to do that calculation. Statistical techniques, after all, are little more than the mathematical steps involved in their calculation. Of course, they may be ignored where this level of knowledge is not required. The basic concept of the book remains the same – a modular statistics package that is accessible throughout to a wide ability range of students. We have attempted to achieve this while being as rigorous as possible where rigour is crucial. Ultimately this is a book for students, though its emphasis on statistics in practice means that it should be valuable to anyone seeking to familiarise themselves with the vast majority of common statistical techniques employed in modern psychology and related disciplines. Not all chapters will be useful to everyone but the book, taken as a whole, provides a sound basis for learning the statistics which professional psychologists use. In this sense, it eases the transition from being a student to being a professional.

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 26

11/18/13 10:19 AM

Acknowledgements



■ Authors’ acknowledgements We could not have produced this book without the skills and hard work of a number of individuals. Indeed, over the years, many people have contributed in a variety of ways which have helped to make the book what it is. Their contribution is highly valued by us but we would like to mention by name some of those who have been involved in this new edition. In no particular order they are: Ros Woodward, who was the copy editor for this edition. Her ability to turn the text design brief into the final layout is remarkable. At the same time, she spots so many typos and other problems in the manuscript that we are convinced that she has super­ human powers. Kevin Ancient supplied the text design without which the book would be far less read­ able and attractive to look at. Sue Gard was the proof reader this time. This is a really difficult job for this book as you can imagine. Not only have the words got to be checked but the numbers too. She did a fabulous job of correcting the proofs and checking that we had no gone astray. Kim Stringer prepared the index. This is a really important job for the user and you will find it all the easier to navigate the book thanks to Kim. Nicola Woowat designed the cover which probably made you want to pick the book up in the first place. It looks good on your bookshelf thanks to her. Kerrie Morton and Kay Holman were the production controllers. They do all the liaison work with typesetters and printers and keep things on schedule. Mary Lince was the project editor. She is therefore a super-efficient master/mistress of the intricacies of publishing who we hold in awe. There is nothing that she can’t do. Janey Webb was the acquisitions editor. Her job is to make our lives unbearable in the nicest possible way. We constantly make changes to improve the manuscript to ensure that her voracious appetite for the best possible manuscripts is satisfied no matter how temporarily. She is a constant strength. Finally, Jane Lawes has been the editorial assistant on this and other of our books for the past few years. She is leaving to go to pastures new and so this is a timely moment to wish her well in the future. We are grateful for everything that she has contributed and forgive her for getting us to do things that we didn’t want to do! Thanks to everyone for everything, especially their patience with us. Dennis Howitt and Duncan Cramer

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 27

11/18/13 10:19 AM

xxviii



Acknowledgements

■ Publisher’s acknowledgements We are grateful to the following for permission to reproduce copyright material:

Figures Figure 33.10 from The relation of formal education to ethnic prejudice: its reliability, validity and explanation, European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, pp. 41–56, Figure 1, p. 52 (Wagner, U. and Zick, A. 1995). Copyright © 1995 by John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Reproduced with permission of John Wiley & Sons Ltd; Figures 40.6, 40.7, 40.8 from G*Power.

Screenshots Screenshots 36.1, 36.2, 36.3, 36.4, 36.5, 36.6 from The Meta-Analysis Calculator, http://www.lyonsmorris.com/lyons/metaAnalysis/index.cfm, reproduced with permission from Larry C. Lyons; Screenshots 40.1, 40.2, 40.3 from G*Power. SPSS screen images are reprinted courtesy of International Business Machines Corpora­ tion, © International Business Machines Corporation. SPSS Inc. was acquired by IBM in October, 2009. IBM, the IBM logo, ibm.com, and SPSS are trademarks or registered trademarks of International Business Machines Corporation, registered in many jurisdic­ tions worldwide. Other product and service names might be trademarks of IBM or other companies. A current list of IBM trademarks is available on the Web at ‘IBM Copyright and trademark information’ at www.ibm.com/legal/copytrade.shtml.

Tables Tables Appendix I.1, Appendix I.2 adapted from Fundamentals of Behavioural Statistics, McGraw-Hill (Runyon, R.P. and Haber, A. 1989) Table I, The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc; Significance Table 19.3 adapted and extended from Table I of R.P. Runyon and A. Haber (1989), Fundamentals of Behavioural Statistics, McGraw-Hill, The McGrawHill Companies, Inc; Table 31.11 adapted from Motivational and informational func­ tions and consquences of children’s attention to peers’ work, Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(3), pp. 347–60 (Butler, R. 1995), published by APA; Table 32.3 adapted from Relationship of gender, self-esteem, social class and racial identity to depression in blacks, Journal of Black Psychology, 20(2), pp. 157–74 (Munford, M.B. 1994), Copyright © 1994, Association of Black Psychologists. Reprinted by Permission of Sage Publications; Table 33.2 from The relation of formal education to ethnic prejudice: its reliability, validity and explanation, European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, pp. 41–56 (Wagner, U. and Zick, A. 1995), John Wiley & Sons.

Text Extract on page 437 after Motivational and informational functions and consequences of children’s attention to peers’ work, Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(3), pp. 347–60, p. 350 (Butler, R. 1995), published by APA; Extract on page 471 from The relation of formal education to ethnic prejudice: its reliability, validity and explanation, European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, pp. 53–4 (Wagner, U. and Zick, A. 1995), John Wiley & Sons. In some instances we have been unable to trace the owners of copyright material, and we would appreciate any information that would enable us to do so.

A01_HOWI0749_06_SE_FM.indd 28

11/18/13 10:19 AM

Chapter 1

Why statistics?

Overview ●

Students do not regard statistics positively, research shows. More importantly, evidence suggests that a poor attitude towards statistics leads to poor learning. Student culture tends to reinforce what is bad in the learning environment for statistics.



A student’s experience within the school environment especially determines their attitudes to mathematics which in its turn impacts on their expectations concerning learning statistics.



There is a mistaken belief among students that statistics is not central to professional work in psychology and other related careers. Why study something which is unnecessary for psychological work? The truth is quite different. Professional psychologists do use research based on quantitative methods and statistics in their work. Furthermore they are frequently expected to do relevant psychological research as part of their work as psychologists. Many other professions employ statistics routinely and so a good working knowledge of statistics puts psychology students at an advantage in the employment market.



Learning statistics can be made hard simply because psychologists often employ old and outmoded statistical ideas. Some of these ideas are not only unhelpful but also unworkable. This can only contribute to the fog of confusion surrounding statistics experienced by many students. Textbook writers are frequently guilty of perpetuating these counterproductive ideas.



Too much emphasis is placed on significance testing. This encourages students to overlook other major contributions of statistics to dealing with the problems inherent in research. It is important to understand the extensive nature and variety of statistics in psychology.



The mathematical skills needed to develop a good working knowledge of statistics are few in number and well within the capabilities of most students. Even where these have been forgotten, they can be quickly learnt by a motivated student.

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 1

11/13/13 5:11 PM

2

CHAPTER 1  Why statistics?

1.1 Introduction For many psychology students the formula is simple: statistics = punishment. Statistics is ‘sadistics’. Students often find a less palatable subject than statistics unimaginable. The majority would steer well clear of statistics given the choice. All in all, this amounts to a very unpromising learning environment. We usually do best when studying things that we are interested in and want to study. A modern training in psychology inevitably includes statistics – the very thing that students want to avoid. It is not surprising, then, that statistics is a problem area for many students. No two learners are alike, of course, and there is a minority of students who are much more positive towards learning stat­ istics. And we should not forget the poor soul whose job it is to teach statistics to such reluctant students. At best this would appear to be a challenge, at worst an impossibility. Student ratings of statistics modules can bring tears to the eyes of all but the most class­ room weary and hardened of lecturers. All round, what could be more unsatisfactory? Why not just abandon the enterprise and leave statistics out of psychology degrees? What could be more simple? There are many good reasons why this cannot and will not happen. Statistics fills an important and central role in psychology and much psycho­ logical research is unthinkable without statistics. Wait a minute – statistics may be essen­ tial to many kinds of psychological research but surely there are many psychologists who help people immeasurably but who never do research? In the past this may have been the case but no longer. Most modern psychology careers are fundamentally tied to research in some way. Once this might have meant that psychologists working in fields such as education and mental health merely had to keep up with the relevant published research of others – i.e. the idea of evidence-based practice. Nowadays it is a much more difficult and complex situation. The majority of working psychologists are expected to do research as an aspect of their employment. That is, modern psychologists are practitioner-researchers. As an example, many psychologists working for the forensic prison services contribute much of the research to their particular field of work. Not for a long time has research been purely what academic psychologists do and it is increasingly what every psychologist does. This is also true for many of the other professions that psychology graduates may enter. We are living in an information-based society and research provides a great deal of that information in the modern world. The bottom line of all of this is that basic statistical skills as well as research skills are generally advantageous in the employment market.



■ Students and statistics Unlike most other disciplines, statistics (along with mathematics) is generally negatively evaluated in our culture. The average person in the street probably has an attitude to statistics without knowing anything much about what the discipline involves. That atti­ tude is unlikely to be that statistics is an important, valuable and central part of modern life. Instead, many will groan at the very mention of the word. Hackneyed old phrases such as ‘you can prove anything with statistics’ and ‘lies, damned lies and statistics’ will be trotted out to dismiss its achievements. Of course, misleading with statistics is pos­ sible but it is not the objective of most statisticians. A few minor adjustments to a graph can lead to a grossly misleading impression at a stroke. A modest growth or decline in a graph may be dramatically changed to seem miraculous or calamitous. But such an important part of modern life as statistics deserves greater respect than this. The word statistics comes from the Latin for State (as in nation). Statistics originally was the information collected by the State to help governments in their decision-making.

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 2

11/13/13 5:11 PM

1.1  Introduction 3

The government’s appetite for such figures is prodigious and all of us are affected by them in some way. Pay, pensions and taxes are all partly determined by statistical data as well as where schools and colleges are built. And, of course, we are all part of stat­ istics. Few modern professions do not use statistics in some way. Big supermarkets use it, small charities use it, the health services use it – you name it and they probably use statistics-based research. Without some statistical knowledge, doing and understanding research is very difficult and a precarious occupation. Nevertheless, on a personal level, students study psychology to study psychology – not to study statistics. Superficially it is possible to study psychology without statistics. Get deeper into psychology and some knowledge of statistics becomes increasingly necessary. This is not to deny the growing interest in qualitative research which does not involve statistics almost by definition. Much valuable research is done by qualitative researchers (Howitt, 2013). But this does not mean that quantitative statistical methods have released their grip on psychological research to any significant extent. Both qualita­ tive and quantitative research seem to be prospering in psychology. Statistics and psy­ chology are seemingly forever intertwined. OK, we are not serious that statistics is taught just to punish students – no matter that sometimes it may feel that way. You might try an alternative view of statistics – that it is a sort of cuddly friend which will help you in all sorts of ways. We are serious here. Criticisms of the dominance of statistics in psy­ chology are common, of course. As much as anyone else, we are as against the mindless application of statistics in psychology for its own sake. Psychology may seem obsessed with a few limited statistical topics such as significance testing but this is to overlook the myriad of more far-reaching positive benefits to be gained from the proper application of modern statistical ideas. Statistics provides a means of finding order in otherwise vast sets of confusing data. Some of this variety of use is illustrated in Figure 1.1.

Figure 1.1

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 3

Some things that statistics can do for the researcher

11/13/13 5:11 PM

4

1.2

CHAPTER 1  Why statistics?

Research on learning statistics Not surprising given the culturally negative view of statistics, the research on psychology students and statistics makes generally depressing reading. The response of student cul­ tures to statistics can just about be summed up with the words trepidation and anxiety. For example, Gordon (2004) surveyed a large number of Australian students about their experience of statistics on psychology courses. Three-quarters said that they would not study statistics but for the fact that it was compulsory. Predominantly they saw it as boring and difficult. These unwilling students felt that statistics was not necessary to psychology or to being a psychologist. They approach statistics as if it were merely a few mechanical procedures that one applies without needing to understand why. One student put it this way to Gordon (1995): I have a very pragmatic approach to university, I give them what they want  .  .  .  I really do like knowledge for knowledge’s sake, but my main motivation is to pass the course. Although some students try to master the methods and concepts of statistics, they may have difficulty in understanding the importance of statistics. Those who saw statistics as being more personally meaningful in their studies would say things like ‘It would prob­ ably be useful in whatever job I do’ (Gordon, 1995). As might be expected, these more positively orientated students performed a little better in their statistics tests and exam­ inations than the more negative group. The latter were not generally less able students since they did just as well as any other students in their other psychology courses. But not seeing the point of statistics did have a negative impact on their studies. Figure 1.2 provides a broad classification of students in terms of how they see the relevance of statistics and their personal assessment of the discipline.

Figure 1.2

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 4

The responses of students to statistics according to Gordon (1995)

11/13/13 5:11 PM

1.3  What makes learning statistics difficult? 5

1.3

What makes learning statistics difficult? University staff commonly recognise that teaching statistics involves dealing with prob­ lems such as anxieties, beliefs and negative attitudes concerning the subject (Schau, 2003). Indeed, these background issues may be the most important things in the learning process and consequently have a bearing on statistics teaching. University can be an experience full of emotion, and emotion affects learning. This is perhaps more true for a topic such as statistics. Real tears are shed. One student told Gordon (1995), ‘I was drowning in statistics’ – words which are both emotive and extreme, of course. Being at university and studying statistics follows a long period of personal development through schooling (and for some at work). This background provides the individual with ways of perceiving their own personal learning processes and their education more generally. What they think they know about themselves (e.g. ‘I’m no good at maths’ or ‘I’m an arty sort of person’) impacts on their response to statistics. Personal histories, personal experi­ ences, personal needs and personal goals are reflected in their strategies for coping with statistics (Gordon, 2004). In other words, students bring to learning statistics baggage which may seriously interfere with its learning. Inevitably, high on the list of background factors is one’s per­ sonal experience of mathematics. There is a strong belief that a high level of mathemat­ ical ability is crucial to the learning of statistics. This is reinforced by those universities which require good mathematical qualifications for admission to psychology degrees. Some students may (incorrectly) assume that statistics is beyond their mathematical ability. With so many other demands on their time at university, instead of getting down to studying statistics they may adopt avoidance tactics such as skipping lectures. Furthermore, every statistics class has its own culture in which students influence each other in terms of attitudes to learning statistics. A class dominated by students antagon­ istic to statistics is not a good learning environment, for example. The problem is that many chosen responses to statistics such as acting silly, talking in statistics lectures or plagiarising the work of other students just do not help. However, the importance of mathematical ability in using statistics effectively is questioned by many, including our­ selves, as we shall see.



■ But I’ve always struggled with maths  .  .  . Research strongly indicates that three factors – anxiety, attitudes and ability (see Figure 1.3) are involved in learning statistics and other somewhat unpopular activities such as learning second languages (Lalonde and Gardner, 1993). A negative attitude towards statistics is associated with poorer performances in statistics to some extent but the other factors are at least equally important. Anxiety plays its part primarily through a specific form of anxiety known as mathematics (math) anxiety. This is more important than trait

Figure 1.3

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 5

The formula for doing well in statistics based on research findings

11/13/13 5:11 PM

6

CHAPTER 1  Why statistics?

or general anxiety such as where someone has a generally anxious personality in all sorts of situations. Mathematics anxiety is common among psychology students. Those with higher levels of mathematics anxiety tend to do worst in statistics. To be sure, mathemat­ ical ability is associated with better test and examination results, but not to a major extent. Poor mathematical ability has its influence largely because it is associated with increased levels of mathematical anxiety. It is because poorer maths ability leads to increased levels of mathematical anxiety that mathematical anxiety leads to poor learn­ ing strategies. But is statistics particularly mathematical and, if it is, then does it need to be beyond a few basics? Along with others, we would argue that the level of mathematical ability needed to cope with the mathematical part of statistics is not great – fairly minimal in fact. We can safely lay aside the issue of the mathematical ability required to carry out statistical calculations as there are many computer programs such as SPSS and numerous applets on the Web which will do the calculation for you. Indeed, there is not a lot of sense in doing statistical calculations by hand as this invites errors to creep in. Computer programs, so long as you enter the data properly and tell them to do the right thing, will do the calculation without error. However, we do not believe that it is possible to learn statistics without using a little bit of mathematics. Equally, it is not necessary to go into all of the mathematical detail behind a statistical technique in order to understand the reasons why the technique was developed and how it can be used. You will find statist­ ical textbooks for psychologists which fall at these extremes. The idea of statistics with­ out maths or statistics without tears, even, cannot provide the necessary understanding in our view because some of the language of statistics is mathematical in nature. At the same time, books that rejoice in the mathematical intricacies of statistical techniques will lose many of their readers who simply do not have mathematical skills at this level. Best-selling statistics textbooks which appear to be student friendly and full of jokes will sometimes go into the most arcane detail about statistical techniques that are way beyond most of us. This seems to us just as unhelpful as not including any mathematics at all. Just what mathematical knowledge does one need to get a working insight into statistics? By and large if you understand the concepts of addition, subtraction, multi­ plication and division then you have the basics. You may get the answers wrong – the question is, do you understand what you are doing? What might you need beyond this? Little more than the following we would say: ●

You need to understand the concept of squaring (that is multiplying a number by itself).



You need to understand the concept of square root (the square root of a number is that number which when multiplied by itself gives the original number).



It is good too if you understand negative numbers – such as that when multiplying two negative numbers you get a positive number but when you multiply a positive number by a negative number then the result is a negative number. A few minutes trying out positive and negative calculations on a calculator is a good way to refresh yourself of these basics.



It is preferable if you understand the underlying principles or ‘rules’ governing math­ ematical formulae as these are used in statistical formulae but if you don’t, your computer does.

Not much else is necessary – if you know what a logarithm is then you are in the ultraadvanced class. So we think that the amount of mathematics needed to make a good statistics student and a skilled user of statistical techniques in research is fairly minimal. Anything that has been forgotten or never learnt will be quickly picked up by a motiv­ ated student. Not all lecturers will share this opinion but the overwhelming majority

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 6

11/13/13 5:11 PM

1.4  Positive about statistics 7

know that students can struggle with statistics and try to provide teaching which serves the needs of all students taking the psychology programme and not the maths-able elite. If more research evidence is needed, using a formal measure known as the Survey of Attitudes Toward Statistics, Zimprich (2012) was able to show that these attitudes towards statistics are made up of four components: ●

Affect  How positive or negative a student is about statistics (e.g. ‘I will like statistics’).



Cognitive competence  A student’s beliefs about their ability and competence to do statistics (e.g. ‘I will make a lot of maths errors in statistics’).



Value  Attitudes concerning the relevance and usefulness of statistics (e.g. ‘I use statistics in my everyday life’).



Difficulty  The student’s views about how difficult or easy statistics is (e.g. ‘Statistics is a complicated subject’).

All of these components were interrelated, as one might expect. When these attitudes were correlated with actual performance in statistics it was clear that attitudes were much more important than actual maths ability in students’ performances in statistics. In other words, how a student feels about statistics has a far more tangible effect on their performance on statistical tests and examinations than their mathematical ability. Irrespective of how mathematical statistics is or isn’t, it has to be acknowledged that statistics is a unique and distinctive way of thinking (Ben-Zvi & Garfield, 2004; Ruggeri, Dempster & Hanna, 2011). It is much like mathematics in employing a distinctive language and concepts. Nevertheless it is wrong to think that this statistical language and these concepts have much in common with mathematics. This means that statistics will always be a somewhat ‘different’ subject irrespective of the curriculum involved. Crucially, statistics is about the use of quantitative research skills in the attempt to answer real research problems. Without being skilful in quantitative research methods, statistics can only partially be understood – and might seem pointless as a consequence. Although research skills take a lot of time and effort to learn, they are very little to do with mathematics – they are primarily about thinking logically. Statistics interfaces with this understanding of research methods in a way which is not simply remembering and then regurgitating a few statistical formulae and ideas when required to do so.

1.4

Positive about statistics So how does one go about having a more positive attitude towards statistics? The answer lies in having an appreciation of what statistics does prior to being exposed to the nittygritty or detail taught in the stats lecture room. Take, for example, what is probably the best known statistical research – the national census. We discuss this in Chapter 2. This census, basically, is a questionnaire about all sorts of things of interest to the government and its decision-making, though probably less interesting to the rest of us. The head of every household is required to complete this detailed questionnaire for a particular day usually once every ten years. In the UK this has been going on for over 200 years. It is hard not to think, when the census envelope arrives, ‘what a waste of time’ and then ‘what a waste of money’. This is possibly because we are all aware that researchers use samples. If research always was so comprehensive as to include everyone then little research would ever get done because of the time and expense involved. This is obvious, but only from the hindsight that comes with living in modern times – people had to invent sampling to replace censuses. And this in statistics had its origins in the work of William Gossett.

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 7

11/13/13 5:11 PM

8

CHAPTER 1  Why statistics?

One of the most famous statistical techniques to impact psychology is the t-test (see Chapters 13 and 14) or the Student t-test as it is also known. Student was the pen name of William Gosset who had studied chemistry and mathematics at university. He was employed by the Guinness Brewery in Dublin as a ‘bright young thing’ in the 1890s. Even then, the firm believed in bringing new ideas to the company, thus keeping it abreast with developments. One issue relevant was that of quality control. There are obvious practical problems if every bottle or barrel of beer had to be tested, for example, in order to see if the alcoholic strength was constant throughout all batches. What Gosset did was to work out mathematically a way of estimating the extent that one is likely to be wrong (risks being wrong) if one took samples rather than tested the entire output. By how much are you likely to be wrong (or in error) if you simply took a sam­ ple, say, of ten bottles of beer? Of course, you will never know from a sample exactly what the error will be but Gosset was able to estimate what the likely extent of error will be. Put into a formula, this is the idea of standard error which plagues many stu­ dents on introductory statistics courses. By developing this, Gosset had laid the system­ atic basis for doing research on samples rather than on everything. Think about it: if it had not been for Gosset’s innovation then you would spend your lifetime carrying out your first research study simply because you need to test everyone or everything (the population). So rather than considering William Gosset as some sort of alien, it would be best to regard him as one of the statistical cuddly friends we mentioned earlier!



■ Is it statistically significant? The point of Gosset’s revolutionary ideas is probably easy to see when explained in this way. But instead students are introduced to what to them are rather complex formulae and the question ‘Are your findings statistically significant?’ The question ‘Is it signi­ ficant?’ is one of the fixations of psychologists – the question probably sounds like a mantra to students when they first begin to study psychology. So intrusive is the question that for most students, statistics in psychology is about knowing what test of statistical significance to apply in what setting. But this is only a small part of statistics, which provides a whole range of tools to help researchers (and students) address the practical problems of research. Research data can be very simple but also very complex. Statistics helps sort out the complexity and uncertainty involved in understanding your data. Testing for statistical significance merely means assessing whether the trend in your data could have been obtained by choosing a random sample if, in reality, there was no trend in general. That is, how likely is it that the trend could simply be the result of a fortuitous selection of a sample in which there appears to be a trend? (A trend might be, say, athletes scoring more highly on a measure of personal ambition than non-athletes or a relationship between a measure of ability to speak foreign languages and a measure of sociability.)



■ What sample size do I need? Testing for significance needs to be put into context. Really you want to know if there is any support for the ideas underlying your research question and the extent to which the trends in your data are big, little or non-existent. So if we put on our thinking head, and not our ‘Is it significant?’ head, we would ask rather more sophisticated questions. One would be whether if there really is a trend in our data, i.e. have we got a sample size big enough to show statistical significance for that trend? Statistics can help us with that question by helping us to decide the minimum sample size to show that trend to be statistically significant if there is a trend of a given size in reality rather than just in our

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 8

11/13/13 5:11 PM

1.4  Positive about statistics 9

data. There would be something perverse about planning research which involved a sample size so small that our findings could never be statistically significant. But that is done all of the time simply because researchers (especially students) do not address the question of minimum sample size properly. Often the advice is given to those asking what sample size to use is that they should use as big a sample size as possible. What does this mean? Possibly it means the largest sample size that you have the resources to col­ lect. But the availability of resources is hardly a satisfactory basis on which to formulate research – that would be a bit like going shopping with the objective of spending money for its own sake rather than to buy something that is necessary. For socially important research, funding may be fairly readily available such as in the case of a cure for cancer. Does this mean that all resources should be put into a particular research project? Not really, as this might well be a complete waste of money when the research question could be addressed satisfactorily with a fairly small sample size. Research takes a lot of time, effort and organisation. So naturally many students will ask the perfectly reasonable question ‘What sample size do I need?’, but frequently they will fail to get a satisfactory answer. This is partly because too many psychologists regard ‘statistical significance’ as the be all and end all of research. The question that the student is asking is actually far more sophisticated than the answers they receive. The conse­ quence of telling a student that they should get the biggest sample they can or that they should have a minimum sample of 50 or 100 or whatever is bewilderment on the part of the student, who realises but can’t explain why these answers are inadequate. Statistics is about sophisticated decision-making concerning what can be said on the basis of the research but also about whether to proceed further with a particular line of inquiry. Statistical significance has a part to play in this decision-making but it does not mean that research findings are significant in any other respect – they may be uninteresting, they may not be of any practical significance, and they may not address any theoretically important issues yet they are deemed statistically significant. It is far better if students understand that there are many issues that a researcher needs to address in their work way beyond statistical significance – while accepting that statistical significance is import­ ant in its own way. Many chapters in this book (such as Chapters 11 and 18) discuss statistical significance but the important question of sample size is addressed only in Chapter 40.



■ Is there a trend in my data? What the student really wants to know is the optimum sample size if there is truly a trend in the data (rather than one that is the consequence of the vicissitudes of sampling). Just taking the largest sample possible may result in a sample that is far too small or far too large. Both of these are unsatisfactory. A too-small sample might mean that your data do not reach statistical significance even where there is really in fact a trend in the real world. This research would be a waste of money and other resources as it cannot answer the question asked satisfactorily. A too-large sample might mean that very small and uninteresting trends in the data are statistically significant. Even where there is a substantial trend in the data, the too-large sample will nevertheless waste time and other resources because the question asked can be satisfactorily answered with a rather smaller sample. Imagine a big medical trial. This is likely to be expensive and every extra person in the research sample costs a great deal of money. This may be money wasted unneces­ sarily. For this reason, organisations that finance medical research expect the researcher to be able to say just what sample size is big enough to reach statistical significance if there is a trend in reality but not so big that a small, uninteresting trend is detected. What makes for an interesting trend is one which is sufficiently large that it has economic, commercial or some other form of potential. The size of the interesting trend depends on

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 9

11/13/13 5:11 PM

10

CHAPTER 1  Why statistics?

what is being considered. A pill which prevents cancer in 10% of people would be of more interest than a pill which prevents flatulence in 10% of people, for example. So if a researcher designs a study which has a sample size too low to establish a statistically significant trend then this would be more worrisome in the case of the cancer cure than in the case of the flatulence cure. Without the appropriate statistics (such as those in Chapter 40) then the researcher would struggle to know what to do about sample size. This is not the place to give a full overview of the role of statistics in psychological research. It is important, though, to stress that statistics can help you do many things in relation to research. This is hardly surprising since statisticians seek to address many of the problems which researchers face in their quantitative research. Statistical significance is only a small part of this. Now this book is just about as comprehensive as under­ standable statistics texts get but not everything that statistics can do is represented. Nevertheless, you will find a great deal which goes far beyond the issue of statistical significance. Take, for example, factor analysis (Chapter 31). This is not at all about statistical significance but a way of finding or identifying the basic dimensions in your data. So, for example, many famous theories of personality and theories of intelligence have emerged out of factor analysis – for instance, that of Hans Eysenck (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1976) which suggests that extraversion, neuroticism and psychoticism are the major underlying dimensions or components of personality on which people differ. There is no way that a researcher can simply look at their data which can be enormously complex and decide what its underlying structure is. It is not possible to identify extra­ version, neuroticism and psychoticism simply by looking at the data from a 50-item questionnaire that has been completed by 2000 participants. But statisticians (and psy­ chologists with a strong interest in statistics) have developed methods of doing just that and computers make this as simple as it can be. Statistics also has a very important role in model building. This sounds complicated but it isn’t too difficult. A model is simply a proposed set of relationships between vari­ ables. So, for instance, the relationships shown in Figure 1.3 between various character­ istics of students studying statistics and their achievement in tests and examinations is a sort of model. Just how well does the model fit the data is a question that statistics can help address – there may be other characteristics of the student that need to be consid­ ered in addition to those in Figure 1.3 in order to account fully for how well students do in statistics. The researcher may propose models but, equally, statistical techniques help identify potential models.

1.5

What statistics doesn’t do Years of experience teaching statistics means, of course, that we are the statistics doctors whom students having problems with analysing their data come to – or even get sent to. These encounters vary widely. Some students simply do not have a clue about statistics and cannot relate what they learnt in statistics lectures with their own research. Other students appear to want help but really they are seeking confirmation that their ideas for their analysis are correct or that they have understood their data correctly. Yet others have designed their research so badly that either it is difficult to analyse at all or it is difficult to analyse using the statistics that the student knows at this point. You should not blame your lack of statistical knowledge when your research does not allow you to answer the question that you set about addressing in the research plan. It is essential to think carefully about what your research design achieves prior to collect­ ing data. You need to ask yourself early on just how you will answer your research questions using the data you are collecting. The less clear you are about your research

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 10

11/13/13 5:11 PM

1.5  What statistics doesn’t do 11

questions then the more difficult this is to do. And your lack of statistical knowledge will rarely be the problem. It is surprising the number of students who stumble so early on in the research pro­ cess. Deadlines for research proposal submissions can result in the writing of a research plan which is not as good or clear as it should be. You should be in a position to plan your analysis in advance of collecting your data. Just how will you go about doing your analysis? This implies that you could insert more or less random numbers, etc. into your analysis and go on to perform the analysis based on these before you collect your actual data. What tables would you need? What statistical techniques would be employed? Such questions ought to be thought about very early in the planning of one’s research. This is a hard thing to do as a beginner but if you cannot detail your analysis early on then why do you expect to be hit by a wave of insight after you have collected your data? So sometimes students do not have a clear grasp of the research that they are proposing to do. This can be because they are trying to achieve too much with one study but often it is because they have not devoted enough time and effort to their proposal. It is difficult for any of us to be clear about our ideas without putting the work in. This is not simply a matter of reading more. You should talk to anyone prepared to listen. There is no quicker way of recognising problems with your research proposals than finding yourself unable to explain clearly to someone else just what you intend to do or how the data you collect will help answer the research question. The point is that you should not blame statistics for problems which are due to poor understanding and planning of one’s own research. In research, few of us are trailblazers who generate ideas and methods which have no bearing on what has gone before. What this means is that there usually is a wealth of research into a particular topic already. Read this research and you will find answers to many of the questions that you will have to ask yourself. Just how is it possible to meas­ ure ‘love’, religious beliefs, preferences and so forth? The likelihood is that others have thought long and hard about this. Why not pay attention to what they have to say? Ask yourself just what is an appropriate research design to address research questions like mine? Similarly, what statistical techniques did other researchers use to analyse their data? Surely the work of others must provide clues to how you might analyse your data? This is not to suggest that you slavishly follow what other people have done but that you learn from them and possibly improve on their work. All of this requires that you read the work of other researchers in copious amounts. This can be hard, and it can be tedi­ ous. And when we say read we mean try to understand each aspect of what the researcher did and why they did it. Don’t gloss over the hard bits as these may tell you what you need to know. In the end, thorough reading of research in the field that you are interested in will provide you with many of the answers you need. Simply concocting a research proposal on the back of an envelope without doing the necessary spade work is far more difficult and risky than building your ideas on the basis of what others have done. Statistics is just one aspect of the decision-making process which underlies research in psychology. It should not dominate a researcher’s thinking exclusively. It is not even the most important part of research. But without it your decision-making may not be optimal. Some of the things which statistics can help you with are:

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 11



Is the trend that I have just found in my data big or small?



Does this line of research show potential for further development?



Are the measures that I am using sufficiently reliable and valid to detect a trend that I am interested in?



Is it possible to amalgamate a number of variables into a single, more readily under­ stood one?



Can I eliminate competing explanations of my findings so as to give more credence to my hypothesis?

11/13/13 5:11 PM

12

1.6

CHAPTER 1  Why statistics?



How best can I present my data graphically in order to visually present my findings to an audience at a conference?



Can I combine the findings of different studies so as to have a good idea of the typical findings of past research?

Easing the way Is there an easy way of learning statistics? Yes and no is the answer – we are psycho­ logists after all. It clearly would take a lot of effort to become a statistician developing statistical knowledge and theory. But a psychologist wishing to use statistics effectively only needs a working knowledge of statistics, which is a very different thing from stat­ istical expertise. That is, using statistics correctly and effectively in our work, but no more than that, is a realistic target for most of us. The hard work has been done by many statisticians over the years but we do not need to know all of the details of how they developed their technique. We simply need to know enough to be able to use the technique effectively. This is not cheating in any way. You don’t need to know all of the intricacies of a car’s mechanics to be able to drive it and nor do you need to know the intricacies of the electronics of your iPad in order to find it useful. It is much the same with statistics – you need to work out what statistics are appropriate to your problem and apply them properly. Perhaps this is a slight understatement, but the basic principle is that you are a user of statistics and limited knowledge will get you a long way. One of the problems in learning statistics is that the advice of those around you can be misleading or unhelpful. This is not because of anything malicious on their part but simply because there is a great deal of false or incomplete knowledge around. Many psychologists learnt most of what they know about statistics when they were students. This may have been state-of-the-art then (though we suspect not) and has not been brought up to date since by some of them. Examples of old ideas which are no longer regarded as adequate are the following:

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 12



Many statistical tests require that your data are normally distributed  This means that your data should follow a bell-shaped distribution curve (known as the normal curve). The problem is that this assumption was built into developing the statistical technique by its inventor. Even though this assumption may not be met, the test still does an adequate job. Few psychologists know the extent to which assumptions may be violated without materially altering the value of the test. Furthermore, many of the statistical techniques used by psychologists were invented long before high-speed computers came along. Their inventors had to rely on theoretical mathematical distributions such as the t-distribution, the z-distribution, the F-distribution and the chi-square distribution. They then had to develop statistical formulae which corresponded with these distributions. But this can be avoided using something known as bootstrapping (see Chapter 19). In bootstrapping, one takes random samples from a scaled-up version of the data. The only trick is that in order to do this your sample is in effect made huge by repeating or replicating your data numerous times. Bootstrapping does not require that your data are bell-shaped or follow any particular distribution. Hence there are virtually no circum­ stances in which many statistical techniques (e.g. the t-test) cannot be used. SPSS, the most familiar statistical package used by psychologists, has included bootstrapping for quite some time yet it is not commonly used by psychologists or mentioned in student textbooks.



There are three types of scores – ordinal (rankable), interval and ratio  These can be differentiated conceptually (see Chapter 2) but rarely if ever can a psychologist say in

11/13/13 5:11 PM

1.7  What do I need to know to be an effective user of statistics? 13

which category their data belong. Students struggle to differentiate the three and, not surprisingly, they fail but see the failing as being their inadequacy rather than the futility of the task. This old-fashioned conceptualisation still has a strong hold on the statistical thinking of psychologists and is practically ubiquitous in statistics text­ books. However, for nearly every purpose these three different types of data can be analysed using the same statistics. That is, there is little point in trying to distinguish the three types as there is no practical consequence from doing so. ●

If your data do not meet the assumptions that the data are normally distributed, then you need a distribution free (or nonparametric test)  There are a number of problems with this. One is that nonparametric tests are not as versatile and effective as the parametric tests which assume the data is bell-shaped in distribution overall. That is, there may be no substitute to use when your data do not meet the parametric assump­ tions. The second problem is that it is not necessarily true that a nonparametric test works better than a parametric test when the latter’s requirements are not met. The nonparametric technique is built on its own assumptions. Thirdly, as explained above, there are now ways of getting around the problems of the bell-shaped distribution such as the boot-strapping methods, for example. What is confusing, in addition, is that if one reads psychological research journals the statistics employed are nearly all parametric in nature and little attention is paid to whether or not the data are norm­ ally distributed. Indeed, tests of normality of the distribution are seldom employed. We explain how this can be done in Chapter 5, however.

These are just examples and they will become clearer when you read the appropriate section of this book. There are other problems of the reverse nature. Some psychologists fail to apply the same level of caution that is applied in the examples above in circum­ stances where they should. A good example of this is the analysis of variance (especially Chapter 23). In this, things called main effects and interactions are often identified. But great care is needed because the technique gives priority to finding main effects and looks for interactions secondarily. What this means is that interactions may be subsumed by main effects when a little common sense would show that the main effects are really interactions. Again, this will become more understandable when you read the relevant chapters of this book on analysis of variance. The point is that the statistical environ­ ment in which many students learn their statistics is an intrinsically confusing one. There is a good chance that you will be exposed to mixed messages about statistics. This is made more difficult by fact that in research there may be numerous (appropriate) ways of analysing the same data. So the student may find it difficult to know which statistical test to apply but not realise that more than one may be appropriate. Some of these tech­ niques superficially seem so different that the student has problems believing that they could all be correct. But they are. So when we explain that the t-test and the correlation coefficient yield fundamentally the same answer when they can be applied to the same data, we are giving an example of this problem (see Chapter 36).

1.7

What do I need to know to be an effective user of statistics? So what do you really need to know in order to be an effective user of statistics? The essential things have nothing to do with mathematics: they are to do with basic concepts in research. If you can apply these key ideas to your data then the statistical analysis becomes relatively easy. Any statistical procedure has limits to where and when it can be applied. These limitations are often largely to do with the nature of the research design or the data. There are statistical techniques which are used for related designs and statistical techniques

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 13

11/13/13 5:11 PM

14

CHAPTER 1  Why statistics?

which are used for nominal data. So the appropriate statistical analysis depends on your recognising what the features of your research design are – what sort of research design you have. The things that you need to know are probably covered by the following list: ●

The difference between a score and a category variable. Overwhelmingly psycholo­ gists use score variables. ●

Score variables  are ones which imply a quantity of something. An IQ of 120 implies something quantitatively different from an IQ of 80. Most psychological tests give quantitative scores.



Category variables  (categorical variables or nominal variables) are ones where the categories have no quantitative implications. For example, male versus female is a category variable which we would refer to as gender. Similarly, Manchester United Football Supporter, Liverpool Football Supporter and Chelsea Football Supporter is also a category variable which we might refer to as football team supporter. This sort of data usually consists of the frequency (total number) of people (or things) which fall into each category. So the data might be 50 Manchester supporters, 23 Liverpool supporters and 70 Chelsea supporters, for example.

It is important to classify each of your variables as scores or category (categorical) variables. This allows you to decide the possible statistical techniques. Some statistical techniques work only for scores, some work only for category variables, and others use both. (Very occasionally, a category variable may be treated as a score variable but for now that is too sophisticated – it is explained in Chapter 42.) ●

Almost without exception, score variables in psychology simply indicate increasing quantities of something. Although many psychologists anguish over whether their variables are on what they call a ratio or equal interval scale, it is almost always impossible to say things like ‘Jean is twice as intelligent as John’ which implies a ratio scale. (This is discussed more in Chapter 2.) Statistically, these issues do not matter. As we pointed out earlier, the problem is that these varieties of scales can confuse stu­ dents if they are taught them. It is a total conundrum which will only perplex students and is not necessary in the first place. The most important thing to remember, though, is that for virtually every psychological variable imaginable it is impossible to make comments that imply that one person is twice as, three times as, half as, etc. intelligent, sociable, withdrawn or whatever as another person. So simply do not make such claims and you won’t go far wrong.



The difference between a related and an unrelated research design. Related designs tend to be more efficient in terms of data but are less common in psychology. In a related design, people are measured twice (or more) using a particular measure or alternative versions of the same measure. So studies where people are measured twice or more at different points in time are related designs. There is one slight complica­ tion which may be ignored most of the time. When groups are matched by having pairs of people who are similar on a measure or measures this is also a related design. In unrelated designs, each person is measured just once on each variable and no matching is attempted. Some designs are mixed related and unrelated designs (e.g. see Chapter 25). If you get this wrong then your analysis of your research design may not be as efficient as it could be. This is a key matter of psychological methodology and does not involve statistics as such but your understanding of research designs.

So, there we are, statistics is almost certain to be a challenge for most students but it should be less of a challenge than it usually appears to be. If we take the analogy that learning statistics has a lot in common with learning a foreign language then a few things become clearer. We do not expect to learn a foreign language well in just a few lessons.

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 14

11/13/13 5:11 PM

1.8  A few words about SPSS 15

However, we do expect that we can do some basic communication very quickly. We also may think that we recognise some of the words in the foreign language which is statistics but we should be careful as their meaning may not be the same as in our everyday lan­ guage. We will not learn a foreign language unless we practise it when possible – so do not be shy about talking about your statistical analyses to other people. When we know something of a foreign language then we can understand a lot more than we can actually speak. In statistics, we can understand the elements of new techniques even though they are very advanced. This may be enough for most psychologists in most circumstances.

1.8

A few words about SPSS The news is generally very good here. The calculation of statistics with computer pro­ grams such as SPSS is usually very easy. There are hardly any students who do not have at least a basic working knowledge of computers in some form. The Windows system of drop-down menus, etc. is almost second nature to modern university students. They are well used to exploring computer programs without recourse to detailed instruction manuals. That is the point of Windows: it is quick and generally intuitive. If you get it wrong then no harm is done and you can quickly try alternatives to see what button pressing works and what does not. For this reason, we could encourage everyone to adopt an exploratory approach to computing statistics using programs such as SPSS. A few minutes of this sort of exploratory behaviour will almost certainly rapidly result in a good level of competence. Although we have provided step-by-step instructions for many statistical procedures along with a number of screenshots, we have kept these down to a reasonable and manageable number. In this way we hope that the reader can begin to see the wood for the trees rapidly. Very detailed step-by-step manuals are readily available – we have published one ourselves – and can be helpful. But following someone’s step-by-step instructions is no substitute for trying to understanding how SPSS works and what its output means yourself. At some stage the tightrope walker needs to abandon the safety net. The teaching of statistics is often teaching to use SPSS, which is a fabulous statistical analysis package but is far from perfect. It is virtually universally available in universities and elsewhere, which is a major advantage. The disadvantage of SPSS is that it does not do everything that researchers need in their day-to-day work. This is not simply that it omits some important statistical techniques but also that some quite basic procedures need to be calculated by hand as the program cannot handle these either. Since this book is about learning a practical working knowledge of statistics, the statistical technique is the primary thing. Whether the statistics can be computed on SPSS is a secondary con­ sideration for us, unlike many other books. Many can be calculated using SPSS but some cannot. In some cases, we use alternative, readily available software. However, to be able to compute statistics satisfactorily is only one element of being competent in the use of statistics. To repeat ourselves, we would always recommend the use of computer programs for this wherever possible. Statistical computations can be very long-winded and repetitive with a high risk of simple but crucial errors. These risks are reduced using software as the computation will be correct so long as the data are entered correctly. But since statistical concepts are almost mathematical in nature then understanding something of the underlying mathematics of the techniques that you are using cannot be a bad thing. We have already explained that this sort of understanding does not demand much in terms of mathematical knowledge. This does mean facing up to one’s statistics demons, though these probably will not be too scary if you only just try.

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 15

11/13/13 5:11 PM

CHAPTER 1  Why statistics?

16

Key points ●

Statistics is a difficult topic for most students but an essential part of psychological research.



The difficulties in learning statistics are more to do with attitudes towards the subject and beliefs about one’s own mathematical abilities than actual ability levels. So a basic understanding of the positive contribution that statistics makes to psychological research is helpful as is a realistically low expectation of the mathematical demands that learning statistics imposes.



A sound working knowledge of statistics involves a basic understanding of the workings of the statistical technique in question together with the computational skills needed to execute this technique. Ignoring the first of these components will not help you to become competent in statistics.

M01_HOWI0749_06_SE_C01.indd 16

11/13/13 5:11 PM

PART 1

Descriptive statistics

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 17

11/13/13 5:11 PM

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 18

11/13/13 5:11 PM

Chapter 2

Some basics Variability and measurement

Overview ●

Statistics are used to describe our data but also assess what reliance we can place on information based on samples.



A variable is any concept that we can measure and that varies between individuals or cases.



Variables should be identified as nominal (also known as category, categorical and qualitative) variables or score (also known as numerical) variables.



Formal measurement theory holds that there are more types of variable – nominal, ordinal, interval and ratio. These are generally unimportant in the actual practice of doing statistical analyses. It is difficult to distinguish ordinal, interval and ratio measurement in practice in psychology.



Nominal variables consist of named categories whereas score variables are measured in the form of a numerical scale which indicates the quantity of the variable.

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 19

11/13/13 5:11 PM

20

CHAPTER 2  Some basics: Variability and measurement

2.1 Introduction Imagine a world in which everything is the same: people are identical in all respects. They wear identical clothes; they eat the same meals; they are all the same height from birth; they all go to the same school with identical teachers, identical lessons and identical facilities; they all go on holiday in the same month; they all do the same job; they all live in identical houses; and the sun shines every day. They do not have sex as we know it since there are no sexes so everyone self-reproduces at the age of 30; their gardens have the same plants and the soil is exactly the same no matter whose garden; they all die on their 75th birthdays and are all buried in the same wooden boxes in identical plots of land. They are all equally clever and they all have identical personalities. Their genetic make-up never varies. Mathematically speaking all of these characteristics are constants. If this world seems less than realistic then have we got news for you – you need statistics! Only in a world of standardisation would you not need statistics – in a richly varying world statistics is essential. If nothing varies, then everything that is to be known about people could be guessed from information obtained from a single person. No problems would arise in generalising since what is true of Sandra Green is true of everyone else – they’re all called Sandra Green after all. Fortunately, the world is not like that. Variability is an essential characteristic of life and the social world in which we exist. The sheer quantity of variability has to be tamed when trying to make statements about the real world. Statistics is largely about making sense of variability. Statistical techniques perform three main functions: 1. They provide ways of summarising the information that we collect from a multitude of sources. Statistics is partly about tabulating your research information or data as clearly and effectively as possible. As such, it merely describes the information collected. This is achieved using tables and diagrams to summarise data, and simple formulae which turn fairly complex data into simple indexes that describe numer­ ically the main features of the data. This branch of statistics is called descriptive statistics for very obvious reasons – it describes the information you collect as accurately and succinctly as possible. The first few chapters of this book are largely devoted to descriptive statistics. 2. Another branch of statistics is far less familiar to most of us: inferential statistics. This branch of statistics is really about economy of effort in research. There was a time when in order to find out about people, for example, everyone in the country would be contacted in order to collect information. This is done today when the government conducts a census of everyone in order to find out about the population of the country at a particular time. This is an enormous and time-consuming operation that cannot be conducted very often. But most of us are familiar with using relatively small samples in order to approximate the information that one would get by studying everybody. This is common in public-opinion surveying where the answers of a sample of 1000 or so people may be used, say, to predict the outcome of a national election. Even though samples can sometimes be misleading, nevertheless it is the principle of sampling that is important. Inferential statistics is about the confidence with which we can generalise from a sample to the entire population. 3. The amount of data that a researcher can collect is potentially massive. Some stat­ istical techniques enable the researcher to clarify trends in vast quantities of data using a number of powerful methods. Data simplification, data exploration and data reduction are among the names given to the process. Whatever the name, the objective is the same – to make sense of large amounts of data that otherwise would be much

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 20

11/13/13 5:11 PM

2.2  Variables and measurement 21



Photo 2.1



People vary in very obvious ways but they also vary in terms of their psychological characteristics. Just what would a small sample of people such as this tell us about the bigger crowd? (Photo: Dennis Howitt)

too confusing. These data exploration techniques are mainly dealt with in the later chapters of this book.

2.2

Variables and measurement The concept of a variable is basic but vitally important in statistics. It is also as easy as pie. A variable is anything that varies and can be measured. These measurements need not correspond very well with everyday notions of measurement such as weight, distance and temperature. So the gender of a person is a variable since it can be measured as either male or female – and gender varies among people. Similarly, eye colour is a variable because a set of people will include some with brown eyes, some with blue eyes and some with green eyes. Thus measurement can merely involve categorisation. Clinical psychologists might use different diagnostic categories such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and anxiety in research. These diagnostic categories constitute a variable since they are different mental and emotional problems to which people can be allocated. Such categorisation techniques are an important type of measurement in statistics. Another type of measurement in statistics is more directly akin to everyday concepts of measurement in which numerical values are provided. These numerical values are

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 21

11/13/13 5:11 PM

22

CHAPTER 2  Some basics: Variability and measurement

assigned to variables such as weight, length, distance, temperature and the like – for example, 10 kilometres or 30 degrees. These numerical values are called scores. In psychological research many variables are measured and quantified in much the same way. Good examples are the many tests and scales used to assess intelligence, personality, attitudes and mental abilities. In most of these, people are assigned a number (or score) in order to describe, for example, how neurotic or how extraverted an individual is. Psychologists will speak of a person having an IQ of 112 or 93, for example, or they will say an individual has a low score of 6 on a measure of psychoticism. Usually these numbers are used as if they corresponded exactly to other forms of measurement such as weight or length. For these, we can make statements such as that a person has a weight of 60 kilograms or is 1.3 metres tall.

2.3

Major types of measurement Traditionally, statistics textbooks for psychologists emphasise different types of meas­ urement – usually using the phrase scales of measurement. However, for virtually all practical purposes there are only two different types of measurement in statistics. These have already been discussed, but to stress the point: 1. Score/numerical measurement This is the assignment of a numerical value to a measurement. This includes most physical and psychological measures. In psychological jargon, these numerical measurements are called scores. We could record the IQ scores of five people as in Table 2.1. Each of the numerical values in the table indicates the named individual’s score on the variable IQ. It is a simple point, but note that the numbers contain information that someone with an IQ of 150 has a higher intelligence than someone with an IQ of 80. In other words, the numbers quantify the variable. 2. Nominal/category measurement This is deciding to which category of a variable a particular case belongs. It is also appropriate to refer to it as a qualitative measure. So, if we were measuring a person’s job or occupation, we would have to decide whether or not he or she was a lorry driver, a professor of sociology, a debt collector and so forth. This is called nominal measurement since usually the categories are described in words and, especially, given names. Thus the category ‘lorry driver’ is a name or verbal description of what sort of case should be placed in that category. Notice that there are no numbers involved in the process of categorisation as such. A person is either a lorry driver or not. However, you need to be warned of a possible



Table 2.1

IQ scores of five named individuals Individual

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 22

IQ score

Stan

80

Mavis

130

Sanjit

150

Sharon

145

Peter

105

11/13/13 5:11 PM

2.3  Major types of measurement 23



Table 2.2

Frequencies of different occupations Occupational category

Number or frequency in set

Lorry drivers

27

Sociology professors

10

Debt collectors

15

Other occupations

48

confusion that can occur. If you have 100 people whose occupations are known, you might wish to count how many are lorry drivers, how many are professors of sociology, and so forth. These counts could be entered into a data table like Table 2.2. Notice that the numbers this time correspond to a count of the frequency or number of cases falling into each of the four occupational categories. They are not scores, but frequencies. The numbers do not correspond to a single measurement but are the aggregate of many separate (nominal) measurements. There is more about the concept of frequency in Box 2.1. Make a habit of mentally labelling variables as numerical scores or nominal cat­ egories. Doing so is a big step forward in thinking statistically. This is all you really need to know about types of measurement. However, you should be aware that others use more complex systems. Read the following section to learn more about scales of measurement.



■ Formal measurement theory Many psychologists speak of four different scales of measurement. Conceptually they are distinct. Nevertheless, for most practical situations in psychologists’ use of statistics the nominal category versus numerical scores distinction discussed above is sufficient. The four ‘theoretical’ scales of measurement are as follows. The scales numbered 2, 3 and 4 are different types of numerical scores. 1. Nominal categorisation  This is the placing of cases into named categories – nominal clearly refers to names. It is exactly the same as our nominal measurement or cat­ egorisation process. 2. Ordinal (or rank) measurement The assumption here is that the values of the numerical scores tell us little else other than which is the smallest, the next smallest and so forth up to the largest. In other words, we can place the scores in order (hence ordinal) from the smallest to the largest. It is sometimes called rank measurement since we can assign ranks to the first, second, third, fourth, fifth, etc. in order from the smallest to the largest numerical value. These ranks have the numerical value 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, etc. You will see examples of this later in the book, especially in Chapters 8 and 19. However, few psychologists collect data directly as ranks. 3. Interval or equal-interval measurement  The basic idea here is that in some cases the intervals between numbers on a numerical scale are equal in size. Thus, if we mea­ sure distance on a scale of centimetres then the distance between 0 and 1 centimetre on our scale is exactly the same as the difference between 4 and 5 centimetres or between 11 and 12 centimetres on that scale. This is obvious for some standard physical measurements such as temperature. 4. Ratio measurement  This is exactly the same as interval scale measurement with one important proviso. A ratio scale of measurement has an absolute zero point that is

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 23

11/13/13 5:11 PM

24

CHAPTER 2  Some basics: Variability and measurement

Box 2.1

Key concepts

Frequency The concept of frequency tends to be taken a little for granted in statistics textbooks although it can cause some confusion in practice. A frequency is simply a count of how often a particular something occurs in your data. So counting the number of people with red hair in your sample gives you the frequency of red-haired people. Quite obviously, therefore, frequency and frequent are not the same – a frequency of 1 cannot usually be described as frequent. In some disciplines, frequency is defined as how often something occurs in a given period of time, such as in the frequency of sound waves. However, in psychology, this usage is not so common and frequency simply means the number of times something occurs in your data. You will find the word count used instead of frequency especially in statistical analysis computer program output. Frequency is the main statistical procedure which can be used with nominal category data. The analysis of nominal category data is largely in terms of counting the frequency of occurrence of each of the categories of nominal category variables. This is straightforward enough. Things risk getting confused when frequencies are used in relation to score data. So, as we have seen, we can count the frequency of any sort of characteristic in our data such as the frequency of children with dyslexia in a school class. But, equally, we can count the frequency of participants in a research study with an IQ of 140. That is, dyslexia and 140 are both categories (different values) in

our data and so their frequencies can be counted. Dyslexia may have a frequency of 15 and the IQ of 140 may have a frequency of 23 or whatever. It is in the idea that the IQ of 140 has the frequency of 23 that the confusion may emerge. Surely 140 and 23 are both numbers just as 15 is a number? Indeed they are all numbers, but 140 is a score on the variable IQ and 23 is its frequency. What this boils down to is as follows: ●

Frequency refers to the number of times that a par­ ticular category (or value) of a variable appears in the data. It is irrelevant whether these categories are given a name (e.g. dyslexia) or a number (e.g. 140).



Scores refer to the amount or extent or quantity of a variable. So a number can be a frequency or a score. Consequently, it is important to carefully distinguish between the two since both are numbers.

There is another potential confusion in relation to scores. Sometimes, a researcher will count how often a participant does something and use this as a score. So, for example, a researcher might be interested in people’s abilities to write text messages. A measure of skill at texting might be the number of errors that a person makes while texting for one minute. In this case, each person’s frequency of making errors is being used as a score on the variable ‘texting errors’, for example.

measured as 0. Most physical measurements such as distance and weight have zero points that are absolute. Thus zero on a tape measure is the smallest distance one can have – there is no distance between two coincident points. With this sort of scale of measurement, it is possible to work out ratios between measures. So, for example, a town that is 20 kilometres away is twice as far away as a town that is only 10 kilometres away. A building that is 15 metres high is half the height of a building that is 30 metres high. (Not all physical measures have a zero that is absolute zero – this applies particularly to several measures of temperature. Temperatures measured in degrees Celsius or Fahrenheit have points that are labelled as zero. However, these zero points do not correspond to the lowest possible temperature you can have. It is then meaningless to say, for example, that it is twice as hot if the temperature is 20 degrees Celsius than if it were 10 degrees Celsius.) These different scales of measurement are illustrated in Figure 2.1 which includes additional examples. Nominal or category measurement is to be found in a distinct, blue

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 24

11/13/13 5:11 PM

2.3  Major types of measurement 25

Figure 2.1

The different scales of measurement and their characteristics

box because it is very different from the other three types of measurement. Nominal or category measurement is about categorisation and involves qualities not quantification. The types of measurement in the green sections are similar to each other as they involve quantities. In practice, it is hard to separate them in terms of their applicability to psychological data. Thus it is far from easy to apply the last three types of measurement in psychology with certainty. Put another way, it is usually very difficult to distinguish between ordinal, interval and ratio scales of measurement. Most psychological scores do not have any directly observable physical basis which makes it impossible to decide whether they consist of equal intervals or have an absolute zero. It is noteworthy that the most convincing examples of these three different types of measurement come from the physical world, such as temperature, length and weight – it is virtually impossible to think of examples from psychology itself. For many years this problem caused great controversy and confusion among psycho­ logists. For the most part, much current usage of statistics in psychology ignores the distinctions between the three different types of numerical scores. This has the support of many statisticians. On the other hand, some psychologists prefer to emphasise that some data are best regarded as rankable and lack the qualities which are characteristic of interval/ratio data (see Figure 2.2). They are more likely to use the statistical techniques to be found in Chapter 19 and the ranking correlation coefficient (Chapter 8) than others. In other words, for precisely the same data, different psychologists will adopt different statistical techniques. Usually this will make little difference to the outcomes of their statistical analyses – the results. In general, it will cause you few, if any, problems if you ignore the three subdivisions of numerical score measurement in your practical use of statistics. The exceptions to this are discussed in Chapters 8 and 19. Since psychologists rarely if ever collect data in the form of ranks, Chapters 3 to 7 are unaffected by such considerations.

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 25

11/13/13 5:11 PM

CHAPTER 2  Some basics: Variability and measurement

26

Figure 2.2

The two practical types of scales of measurement

Key points ●

Always ask yourself what sort of measurement it is you are considering – is it a numerical score on a variable or is it putting individuals into categories?



Never assume that a number is necessarily a numerical score. Without checking, it could be a frequency of observations in a named category.



Clarity of thinking is a virtue in statistics – you will rarely be expected to demonstrate great creativity in your statistical work. Understanding precisely the meaning of terms is an advantage in statistics.

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 26

11/13/13 5:11 PM

2.3  Major types of measurement 27

Computer analysis Some basics of data entry using SPSS Nominal (category/categorical) data are usually analysed differently from data based on scores (including ordinal, interval and ratio data) in statistics. Generally nominal data are entered in the form of an arbitrary numerical code (e.g. 1 = females, 2 = males) which stands for verbal descriptions and, of course, scores are entered as numbers too. However, it is easy to label each of the categories of a nominal variable. These can be displayed on the data spreadsheet. You may be required by the computer program to indicate what kind of data (score, nominal category) each variable is. SPSS Statistics is not entirely consistent about this. (SPSS is an IBM company acquired in October 2009.) Overwhelmingly, psychological data is collected in the form of scores. In SPSS, data is entered into a spreadsheet known as ‘Data View’. Figure 2.3 shows this. The variables are set up using ‘Variable View’. You can switch between the two using the tab at the bottom of the screen.

Figure 2.3

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 27

Entering score and nominal data into SPSS

11/13/13 5:11 PM

28

CHAPTER 2  Some basics: Variability and measurement

Screenshot 2.1

Part of the Variable View screen

Screenshot 2.2

Part of Data View screen

Screenshot 2.3

Insert values

Screenshot 2.4

Input value and label

Screenshot 2.5

M02_HOWI0749_06_SE_C02.indd 28

Value and label inserted

11/13/13 5:11 PM

Chapter 3

Describing variables Tables and diagrams

Overview ●

Tables and diagrams are important aspects of descriptive statistics (the description of the major features of the data). Examining data in this sort of detail is a vital stage of any statistical analysis and should never be omitted. At most, a very small number of important tables and diagrams will be included in your report as they consume a lot of space.



This chapter describes how to create and present tables and diagrams for individual variables.



Statistical tables and diagrams should effectively communicate information about your data. Beware of complexity.



The type of data (nominal versus score) largely determines what an appropriate table and diagram will be.



If the data are nominal, then simple frequency tables, bar charts or pie charts are most appropriate. The frequencies are simply the numbers of cases in each of the separate categories.



If the data are scores, then frequency tables or histograms are appropriate. However, to keep the presentation uncluttered and to help clarify trends, it is often best to put the data into bands (or ranges) of adjacent scores.

Preparation Remind yourself what a variable is from Chapter 2. Similarly, if you are still not sure of the nominal (categorisation) form of measurement and the use of numerical scores in measurement then revise these too.

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 29

11/18/13 2:16 PM

30

CHAPTER 3  Describing variables: Tables and diagrams

3.1 Introduction You probably know a lot more about statistics than you think. Statistical tables and diagrams are fairly common in newspapers and magazines and on television; children become familiar with statistical tables and diagrams at school. Skill in constructing tables and diagrams is essential because researchers collect large amounts of data from numerous people (see Box 3.1). If we asked 100 people their age, gender, marital status (divorced, married, single, etc.), their number of children and their occupation this would yield 500 separate pieces of information. Although this is small fry compared with much research, it is not very helpful to present these 500 measurements in your research report. Such unprocessed information is called raw data. Statistical analysis has to be more than describing the raw ingredients. It requires the data to be structured in ways that effectively communicate the major trends or characteristics of your data. If you fail to structure your data, you may as well just give the reader copies of your questionnaires or observation schedules to interpret themselves. There are very few rules regarding how to produce tables and diagrams in statistics so long as they are clear to the reader and concise; they need to communicate quickly the important trends in the data. There is absolutely no point in using tables and dia­ grams that do not ease the task of communication. Probably the best way of deciding whether your tables and diagrams do their job well is to ask other people to decipher what they mean. Tables which are unclear to other people are generally useless. Of course, if you don’t understand your table or diagram then it is unlikely that other people can. Descriptive statistics are, by and large, relatively simple visual and numerical tech­ niques for describing your data’s major features. Researchers may produce descriptive statistics in order to communicate the major characteristics of their data to others, but in the first instance they are used by researchers themselves in order to understand the distribution of participants’ responses in the research. Never regard descriptive statistical analysis as an unnecessary or trivial stage in research. It is probably more informative

Box 3.1

Focus on

Multiple responses One of the easiest mistakes to make in research is to allow participants in your research to give more than one answer to a single question. So, for example, if you ask people to name their favourite television programme and allow each person more than one answer, you will find that the data can be very tricky to analyse thoroughly. Take our word for it for now: statistics in general does not handle multiple responses very well. Certainly it is possible to draw up tables and diagrams, but some of the more advanced statistical procedures become difficult to apply. You will sometimes read comments to the effect that the totals in a table exceed the number of participants in the research. This is usually because the researcher has allowed multiple responses to a

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 30

single variable. So only allow the participants in your research to give one piece of data for each variable you are measuring to avoid digging a pit for yourself. If you plan your data analysis in detail before you collect your data, you should be able to anticipate any difficulties. It is possible to do something about data which allow multiple responses. This is to use dummy coding, which is discussed later in Chapter 42. Essentially what one does is to take every possible response as a separate new variable and code each person’s data for the presence or absence of each of these new variables. Of course, if there are a lot of different responses then this involves creating a lot of new variables.

11/18/13 2:16 PM

3.2  Choosing tables and diagrams 31

Box 3.2

Key concepts

Descriptive statistics The basic concept of descriptive statistics is very clear. Descriptive statistics are the various techniques which help us get a picture of what is happening in our data. They include tables which give averages, frequencies and the like and diagrams which represent very much the same things but in a more graphic, pictorial form. Descriptive statistics can involve the examination of one variable on its own or the relationships between two or more vari­ ables. Many aspects of descriptive statistics are very famil­ iar to us all even before we study statistics. We were all taught at least some of them at school. One consequence of this is that we tend not to see them as playing the vital part that they should in our research. This is a mistake as descriptive statistical techniques contain what is essential to understanding our data – they provide a window through which we can begin to appreciate what is going on in our data. They are the bedrock on which other stat­ istical techniques are built. To be sure, there are more demanding techniques to learn about in statistics than tables and diagrams. This book and others are full of seemingly complex and, sometimes, difficult new things to learn and so the danger is that we neglect descriptive stat­ istics in favour of these. Indeed, there are some popular statistics textbooks which almost entirely overlook how to construct good tables and diagrams. But it is mainly through the effective use of descriptive statistics that we

can see the trends, patterns, quirks, bumps and irregular­ ities in our data. Keep sight of what descriptive statistics say about your data as this is the key to data analysis. They are an important part of understanding the ‘fancier’ stuff that comes later. Qualitative researchers in psychology spend considerable amounts of time and a great deal of effort in familiarising themselves with their data. So why should quantitative researchers not do the same? Try not to think of tables and diagrams as merely something to adorn your practical reports and dissertations. Often, at best these will contain only a fraction of the descriptive statistics that you have produced during the process of data analysis. Descriptive statistics are best seen as a tool in the analysis process rather than merely parts of the final product – your research report. Use descriptive techniques to explore your data thoroughly, knowing that you may need to modify your initial attempts in the light of experience. Data analysis is a sort of trial-and-error process of finding out what works for you and for your data. Statistics programs allow you to generate numerous tables and diagrams, some of which are useful and illuminating, although others verge on the useless. The not-so-good stuff is easily deleted from your computer. Be prepared to devote quite some time to this stage of your analysis. It will pay dividends in the long run and bring you close to the data from your study early on.

than any other aspect of data analysis. Box 3.2 explains the crucial role of descriptive statistics in research further. The distinction between nominal (category) data and numerical scores discussed in the previous chapter is important in terms of the appropriate tables and diagrams to use. Some only work for nominal data and some only work for score data.

3.2

Choosing tables and diagrams So long as you are able to decide whether your data are either numerical scores or nominal (category) data, there are few other choices to be made since the available tables and diagrams are essentially dependent upon this distinction. Figure 3.1 gives some of the key steps when considering tables and diagrams.

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 31

11/18/13 2:16 PM

32

CHAPTER 3  Describing variables: Tables and diagrams

Figure 3.1



Conceptual steps for understanding tables and diagrams

■ Tables and diagrams for nominal (category) data One of the main characteristics of tables and diagrams for nominal (category) data is that they have to show the frequencies of cases in each category used. While there may be as many categories as you wish, it is not the function of statistical analysis to com­ municate all of the data’s detail; the task is to identify the major trends or features. For example, imagine you are researching the public’s attitudes towards private health care. If you ask participants in your research their occupations then you might find that they mention tens if not hundreds of different job titles – newsagents, homemakers, company executives and so forth. Simply counting the frequencies with which different job titles are mentioned results in a vast number of categories. You need to think of relevant and meaningful ways of reducing this vast number into a smaller number of much broader categories that might reveal important trends. For example, since the research is about a health issue you might wish to form a category made up of those involved in health work – some might be dentists, some nurses, some doctors, some paramedics and so forth. Instead of keeping these as different categories, they might be combined into a category ‘health worker’. There are no hard-and-fast rules about combining to form broader categories. It depends on the purpose of your research and the detail of the data as much as anything. The following might be useful rules of thumb:

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 32



Keep your number of categories low, especially when you have only small numbers of participants in your research.



Try to make your ‘combined’ categories meaningful and sensible in the light of the purposes of your research. It would be nonsense, for example, to categorise jobs by the letter of the alphabet with which they start – nurses, nuns, nursery teachers and national footballers. All of these have jobs beginning with the same letter, but it is very difficult to see any other common thread which allows them to be combined meaningfully.

11/18/13 2:16 PM

3.2  Choosing tables and diagrams 33



Table 3.1



 ccupational status of participants in the research expressed as frequencies and O percentage frequencies

Occupation Nuns Nursery teachers

Frequency

Percentage frequency

17

21.25

3

3.75

Television presenters

23

28.75

Students

20

25.00

Other

17

21.25

In terms of drawing tables, all we do is to list the categories we have chosen and give the frequency of cases that fall into each of the categories (Table 3.1). The frequencies are presented in two ways in this table – simple frequencies and percentage frequencies. A percentage frequency is the frequency expressed as a percentage of the total of the frequencies (or total number of cases, usually). Notice also that one of the categories is called ‘other’. This consists of those cases which do not fit into any of the main categories. It is, in other words, a ‘rag bag’ category or miscellany. Generally it is best to have a small number of cases in the ‘other’ category.

Explaining statistics 3.1

How percentage frequencies work Many readers will not need this, but if you are a little rusty with simple maths, it might be helpful. Throughout this book you will find sections headed ‘Explaining statistics’. Although most of the statistics discussed in this book may be calculated using SPSS or other computer programs, not everyone is satisfied by simply pressing a few computer keys. They like to know a bit more about how the statistical analysis is carried out. Some may prefer simply to go to the instructions for doing the analysis on the computer and ignore the following. However, others will learn better by knowing something about what is involved in the calculation that the computer does. We will show you how to do the calculation by hand – not because we think that this is the best way to do the calculation, because it is not. By working through the calculation, you should get some idea though of the mechanics of the statistical technique and understand some things which a computer analysis alone will not clarify. We are not suggesting that the computer does things exactly this way but that this will approximate what the computer does. The percentage frequency for a particular category, say for students, is the frequency in that category expressed as a percentage of the total frequencies in the data table.

Step 1

What is the category frequency? For students in Table 3.1: category frequency[students] = 20

Step 2

Add up all of the frequencies in Table 3.1: total frequencies = nuns + nursery teachers + TV presenters + students + other = 17 + 3 + 23 + 20 + 17 = 80

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 33



11/18/13 2:16 PM

34

CHAPTER 3 DeSCrIBIng varIaBleS: TaBleS anD DIagramS

Step 3

percentage frequency [students] =

category frequenccyy [students] × 100 totall ffrequencies

20 × 100 2000 0 = = = 25% 80 80

One advantage of using computers is that they enable experimentation with different schemes of categorising data in order to decide which is best for your purposes. In this case, you would use initially narrow categories for coding your data. Then you can tell the computer which of these to combine into broader categories. This process is gener­ ally termed recoding and simply means putting a category into a new category or putting several categories into a new combined category. Recode is a procedure in SPSS. Sometimes it is preferable to turn frequency tables into diagrams. Good diagrams are quickly understood and add variety to the presentation. The main types of diagram for nominal (category) data are pie diagrams and bar charts. A pie diagram is a very familiar form of presentation – it simply expresses each category as a slice of a pie which repre­ sents all cases (see Figure 3.2). Notice that the number of slices is small – a multitude of slices can be confusing. Each slice is clearly marked with its category name, and the percentage frequency in each category also appears.

FiguRE 3.2

a simple pie diagram

Explaining statistics 3.2

how pie diagrams work There is nothing difficult in constructing a pie diagram. Our recommendation is that you turn each of your frequencies into a percentage frequency. Since there are 360 degrees in a circle, if you multiply each percentage frequency by 3.6 you will obtain the angle (in degrees) of the slice of the pie which you need to mark out. In order to create the diagram, you will require a protractor to measure the angles. However, computer graph packages are standard at any university or college and do an impressive job – SPSS included. In Table 3.1, 25.00% of cases were students. In order to turn this into the correct angle for the slice of the pie, you simply need to multiply 25.00 by 3.6 to give an angle of 90 degrees.

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 34

11/18/13 2:16 PM

3.2  Choosing tables and diagrams 35

Figure 3.3

A poor pie diagram

Figure 3.3 shows a bad example of a pie diagram for purposes of comparison. There are several problems with this pie diagram: ●

There are too many small slices identified by different shading patterns and the legend takes time to decode.



It is not too easily seen what each slice concerns, and the relative sizes of the slices are difficult to judge. We have the size of the slices around the figure and a separate legend or key to identify the components to help cope with the overcrowding problem. In other words, too many categories have resulted in a diagram which is far from easy to read – a cardinal sin in any statistical diagram.

A simple frequency table might be more effective in this case. Another very familiar form of statistical diagram for nominal (category) data is the bar chart. Again these charts are very common in the media. Basically they are diagrams in which bars represent the size of each category. An example is shown in Figure 3.4. The relative lengths (or heights) of the bars quickly reveal the main trends in the data. With a bar chart, there is very little to remember other than that the bars have a standard space separating them. The spaces indicate that the categories are not in a numerical order; they are frequencies of categories, not scores. It is hard to go wrong with a bar chart (that is not a challenge!) so long as you remem­ ber the following: ●

The heights of the bars represent frequencies (number of cases) in a category.



Each bar should be clearly labelled as to the category it represents.

Figure 3.4

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 35

Bar chart showing occupational categories in Table 3.1

11/18/13 2:16 PM

36

CHAPTER 3  Describing variables: Tables and diagrams



Too many bars make bar charts hard to follow.



Avoid having many empty or near-empty categories which represent very few cases. Generally, the information about substantial categories is the most important. (Small categories can be combined together as an ‘other’ category.)



Nevertheless, if important categories have very few entries then this needs recording. So, for example, a researcher who is particularly interested in opportunities for women surveys people in top management and finds very few women employed in such jobs. It is important to draw attention to this in the bar chart of males and females in top management. Once again, there are no hard-and-fast rules to guide you – common sense will take you a long way.



Make sure that the vertical axis (the heights of the bars) is clearly marked as being frequencies or percentage frequencies.



The bars should be of equal width.

In newspapers and on television you are likely to come across a variant of the bar chart called the pictogram. In this, the bars of the bar chart are replaced by varying sized drawings of something eye-catching to do with your categories. Thus, pictures of men or women of varying heights, for example, replace the bars. Pictograms are rarely used in professional presentations. The main reason is that pictures of things get wider as well as taller as they increase in size. This can misrepresent the relative sizes of the categories, given that readers easily forget that it is only the height of the picture that counts.



■ Tables and diagrams for numerical score data One crucial consideration when deciding what tables and diagrams to use for score data is the number of separate scores recorded for the variable in question. This can vary markedly. So, for example, age in the general population can range from newly born to over 100 years of age. If we merely recorded ages to the nearest whole year then a table or diagram may have entries for 100 different ages. Such a table or diagram would look horrendous. If we recorded age to the nearest month, then we could multiply this num­ ber of ages by 12! Such scores can be grouped into bands or ranges of scores to allow effective tabulation (Table 3.2). This sort of grouping into bands involves the recoding procedure when using SPSS. Many psychological variables have a much smaller range of numerical values. So, for example, it is fairly common to use questions which pre-specify just a few response alternatives. The so-called Likert-type questionnaire item is a good case in point. Typically this looks something like this:

Table 3.2

Ages expressed as age bands Age range 0–9 years

19

10–19 years

33

20–29 years

17

30–39 years

22

40–49 years

17

50 years and over

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 36

Frequency

3

11/18/13 2:16 PM

3.2  Choosing tables and diagrams 37

Statistics is my favourite university subject: Strongly agree  Agree  Neither agree nor disagree  Disagree  Strongly disagree Participants completing this questionnaire circle the response option which best fits their personal opinion. It is conventional in this type of research to code these different response alternatives on a five-point scale from one to five. Thus strongly agree might be coded 1, neither agree nor disagree 3, and strongly disagree 5. This scale therefore has only five possible values. Because of this small number of possible answers, a table based on this question will be relatively simple. Indeed, if students are not too keen on statistics, you may well find that they select only the disagree and strongly disagree categories. Tabulating such data is quite straightforward: you can simply report the numbers or frequencies of replies for each of the different categories or scores as in Table 3.3. A histogram might be the best form of statistical diagram to represent these data. At first sight, histo­ grams look very much like bar charts but without gaps between the bars. This is because the histogram does not represent distinct unrelated categories but different points on a numerical measurement scale. So a histogram of the above data might look like Figure 3.5. But what if your data have numerous different possible values of the variable in question? One common difficulty for most psychological research is that the number of respondents tends to be small. The large number of possible different scores on the variable is therefore shared among very few respondents. Tables and diagrams should present major features of your data in a simple and easily assimilated form. So, some­ times you will have to use bands of scores rather than individual score values, just as you did for Table 3.2. So, if we asked 100 people their ages we could categorise their replies into bands such as 0–9 years, 10–19 years, 30–39 years, 40–49 years and a final category of those 50 years and over. By using bands we reduce the risk of empty parts of the table

Table 3.3

Distribution of students’ attitudes towards statistics

Response category

Value

Frequency

Strongly agree

1

17

Agree

2

14

Neither agree nor disagree

3

6

Disagree

4

2

Strongly disagree

5

1

Figure 3.5

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 37

Histogram of students’ attitudes towards statistics

11/18/13 2:16 PM

38

CHAPTER 3  Describing variables: Tables and diagrams

Figure 3.6

Use of bands of scores to enable simple presentation

and allow any trends to become clear (Figure 3.6). This does not mean that you have to use these bands for additional statistical analyses – the point is that tables and diagrams need to show things clearly and if this needs the use of bands or ranges of scores then so be it. How one chooses the bands to use is an important question. The answer is a bit of luck and judgement, and a lot of trial and error. It is very time-consuming to rejig the ranges of the bands when one is analysing the data by hand. One big advantage of com­ puters is that they will recode your scores into bands repeatedly until you have tables which seem to do the job as well as possible. The criterion is still whether the table com­ municates information effectively. The one rule is that the bands ought to be of the same size – that is cover, for example, equal ranges of scores. Generally this is easy except at the upper and lower ends of the distribution. Perhaps you wish to use ‘over 70’ as your upper range. This, in modern practice, can be done as a bar of the same width as the others, but must be very carefully marked. (Strictly speaking, the width of the band should represent the range of scores involved and the height reduced in the light of this. However, this is rarely done in modern psychological statistics.) One might redefine the bands of scores and generate another histogram based on identical data but a different set of bands (Figure 3.7). It requires some thought to decide which of the diagrams is best for a particular pur­ pose. There are no hard-and-fast rules for this either.

Figure 3.7

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 38

Histogram showing ‘collapsed’ categories

11/18/13 2:16 PM

3.3  Errors to avoid 39

3.3

Errors to avoid There are a couple of mistakes that you can make in drawing up tables and diagrams: ●

Do not forget to head the table or diagram with a succinct description of what it concerns. You will notice that we have done our best throughout this chapter to supply each table and diagram with a clear title.



Label everything on the table or diagram as clearly as possible. What this means is that you have to mark your bar charts and histograms in a way that tells the reader what each bar means. Then you must indicate what the height of the bar refers to – probably either frequency or percentage frequency.

Note that this chapter has concentrated on describing a single variable as clearly as pos­ sible. In Chapter 8, methods of making tables and diagrams showing the relationships between two or more variables are described.

Research examples Using graphs and tables The extent of the use of tables and diagrams varies markedly in psychology. Some subfields use diagrams to a greater extent than other fields. While it is impossible usually to incorporate every diagram used in data analysis in the final report, diagrams can be very persuasive. So they should be considered for inclusion when they tell an interesting ‘story’. Carr, Whiteford, Groves, McGorry and Shepherd (2012) used the second Australian National Survey of High Impact Psychosis in order to identify its policy implications. Using bar charts, they show that financial matters, social isolation/loneliness, and lack of employment were the main challenges foreseen by sufferers of psychosis in the years to come. Rothbard and Wilk (2011) examined how a person’s mood at the start of the workday primes how they see events at work later in the day in relation to the worker’s job performance in a call centre. Graphical methods were used to show such things as the variation in mood at the start of day over time. Start of day mood affected the call centre employees’ perceptions of how the customer was feeling emotionally during the telephone conversation and the employees’ response to the calls. Skinner (e.g. 1948) developed operant conditioning which had a big influence on behaviourist psychology. He had a strong preference for the use of graphical methods rather than statistics in his work on animal conditioning. His research findings were usually presented in graph form and he had little time for the sort of inferential statistics which dominates modern psychological research. Smith-Bell, Burhans and Schreurs (2012) explored animal models of post-traumatic stress disorder. Such models assume that fear conditioning can result in responses to innocuous cues the same as to the traumatic event. The researchers employed classical conditioning methods. Their data was analysed to a substantial extent using graphs. Data from research using rabbits suggested that 25% exhibited a conditioned specific reflex modification similar to the response to innocuous cues that is characteristic of post-traumatic stress disorder. Spini, Elcheroth and Figini (2009) analysed the content of social psychology journals to establish how extensively the concept of time was involved. The contents of the articles were read and the articles coded for different aspects of the coverage of time. Using tables to present the frequencies, etc. involved, the researchers found that most research studies do not include time- or age-related explanatory variables.

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 39

11/18/13 2:16 PM

CHAPTER 3  Describing variables: Tables and diagrams

40

Key points ●

Try to make your tables and diagrams useful. It is not usually their purpose to record the data as you collected it in your research. Of course you can list your data in the appendix of projects that you carry out, but this is not useful as a way of illustrating trends. It is part of a researcher’s job to make the data accessible to the reader in a structured form that is easily understood by the reader.



Especially when using computers, it is very easy to generate useless tables and diagrams. This is usually because computer analysis encourages you not to examine your raw data in any detail. This implies that you should always regard your first analyses as tentative and merely a step towards something better.



If a table is not clear to you, it is unlikely to be any clearer to anyone else.



Check each table and diagram for clear and full labelling of each part. Especially, check that frequencies are clearly marked as such.



Check that there is a clear, helpful title to each table and diagram.

Computer analysis Tables and diagrams using SPSS



Figure 3.8

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 40

SPSS Statistics steps for producing tables and diagrams to describe a nominal category variable

11/18/13 2:16 PM

Computer analysis 41



Interpreting and reporting output ●

Tables and similar diagrams are primarily part of the initial analysis of your data and can help you to identify significant features of the data – such as unusual distributions of variables and so forth. It would be usual to generate many more charts and tables than you include in your report.



One therefore has to be selective about what charts and tables one includes in one’s report. They are space consuming and often can be summarised in a few words – and so might not need to be included. Charts and tables included in your report should be very clear, fully labelled and as informative as possible.

Screenshot 3.1

Enter nominal variables into the data editor

Screenshot 3.2

Enter data as nominal

Screenshot 3.4

Important output

Screenshot 3.3

Select Descriptive Statistics

See Computer Analysis in Chapter 4 for the analysis of score data.

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 41

11/18/13 2:16 PM

CHAPTER 3  Describing variables: Tables and diagrams

42

Figure 3.9

SPSS Statistics steps for recoding values

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

Tables and similar diagrams are primarily part of the initial analysis of your data and can help you to identify significant features of the data – such as unusual distributions of variables and so forth. It would be usual to generate many more charts and tables than you include in your report.



One therefore has to be selective about what charts and tables one includes in one’s report. They are space consuming and often can be summarised in a few words – and so might not need to be included. Charts and tables included in your report should be very clear, fully labelled and as informative as possible.

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 42

11/18/13 2:16 PM

Computer analysis 43



Screenshot 3.5

Select Recode

Screenshot 3.6

Name new variable

Screenshot 3.7

Select new values

Screenshot 3.8

New values in Data View

M03_HOWI0749_06_SE_C03.indd 43

11/18/13 2:16 PM

Chapter 4

Describing variables numerically Averages, variation and spread

Overview ●

● ●

● ● ●







Scores can be described or summarised numerically – for example the average of a sample of scores can be given. There are several measures of central tendency – the most typical or most likely score. The mean score is simply the average score assessed by the total of the scores divided by the number of scores. The mode is the numerical value of the most frequently occurring score. The median is the score in the middle if the scores are ordered from smallest to largest. The spread of scores can be expressed as the range (which is the difference between the largest and the smallest score). Variance (an indicator of variability around the average) indicates the spread of scores in the data. Unlike the range, variance takes into account all of the scores. It is a ubiquitous statistical concept. Nominal data can only be described in terms of the numbers of cases falling in each cat­ egory. The mode is the only measure of central tendency that can be applied to nominal (category) data. Outliers are unusually large or small values in your data which are very atypical of your data. They can create the impression of trends in your analysis which are not really present. Identifying such outliers and dealing with them effectively can have an important impact on the quality of your research.

Preparation Revise the meaning of nominal (category) data and numerical score data.

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 44

11/18/13 1:52 PM

4.1  Introduction 45

4.1 Introduction Tables and diagrams take up a lot of space. It can be more efficient to use numerical indexes to describe the distributions of variables. For this reason, you will find relatively few pie charts and the like in published research. One numerical index is familiar to everyone – the numerical average (or arithmetic mean). Large amounts of data can be described or summarised adequately using just a few numerical indexes. What are the major features of data that we might attempt to summarise in this way? Look at the two different sets of scores in Table 4.1. The major differences between these two sets of data are: ●

The sets of scores differ substantially in terms of their typical value – in one case the scores are relatively large (variable B); in the other case the scores are much smaller (variable A).



The sets of scores differ in their spread or variability – one set (variable B) seems to have more spread or a greater variability than the other.



If we plot these two sets of scores as histograms then we also find that the shapes of the distributions differ markedly. Variable A is much steeper and less spread out than variable B.

Each of these different features of a set of scores can be described using various indexes. They do not generally apply to nominal (category) variables. Figure 4.1 describes some of the key steps you need to consider when describing your data numerically.

Figure 4.1

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 45

Conceptual steps for understanding how to describe your variables numerically

11/18/13 1:52 PM

46

CHAPTER 4  Describing variables numerically: Averages, variation and spread

Table 4.1

Two different sets of scores Variable A scores

Variable B scores

2

27

2

29

3

35

3

40

3

41

4

42

4

45

4

45

4

49

4

49

5

49

5 5

4.2

Typical scores: mean, median and mode Researchers sometimes speak about the central tendency of a set of scores. By this they are raising the issue of what are the most typical and likely scores in the distribution of measurements. We could speak of the average score, but that can mislead us into think­ ing that the arithmetic mean is the average score when it is just one of several possible averages. There are three main measures of the typical scores used in statistics: the arith­ metic mean, the mode and the median. These are quite distinct concepts but generally simple enough in themselves.



■ The arithmetic mean The arithmetic mean is calculated by summing all of the scores in a distribution and dividing by the number of scores. This is the everyday concept of average. In statistical notation we can express this mean as follows: Bmean =

∑ X[scores]

N[number of scores]

As this is the first statistical formula we have presented, you should take very careful note of what each symbol means: X is the statistical symbol for a score ∑ is the summation or sigma sign ∑X means add up all of the scores X N is the number of scores B is the statistical symbol for the arithmetic mean of a set of scores We have added a few comments in small square brackets [just like this]. Although mathematicians may not like them very much, you might find they help you to interpret a formula a little

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 46

11/18/13 1:52 PM

4.2  Typical scores: mean, median and mode 47

more quickly. Calculating the average of a set of scores such as 7, 5, 4, 7, 7 and 5 is more quickly done than explained. In statistical notation, a score is usually given the sym­ bol X and subscripts identify the different numbers. So X1 = 7, X2 = 5, X3 = 4, X4 = 7, X5 = 7 and X6 = 5 for this set of six scores. You will find that this sort of use of subscripts is common in journal articles so it is useful to be familiar with it. The formula for the mean follows together with the calculation for our six scores: B mean =

∑ X[scores]

N[number of scores]

X1 + X 2 + X3 + X 4 + X 5 + X6 N 7 + 5 + 4 + 7 + 7 + 5 35 = = = 5.83 6 6

=



■ The median The median is the middle score of a set if the scores are organised from the smallest to the largest. Thus the set of scores 7, 5, 4, 7, 7, 5, 3, 4, 6, 8, 5 become 3, 4, 4, 5, 5, 5, 6, 7, 7, 7, 8 when put in order from the smallest to the largest. Since there are 11 scores and the median is the middle score from the smallest to the largest, the median has to be the sixth score, i.e. 5. With odd numbers of scores all of which are different, the median is easily calculated since there is a single score that corresponds to the middle score in the set of scores. However, if there is an even number of all different scores in the set then the mid-point will not be a single score but two scores. So if you have 12 different scores placed in order from smallest to largest, the median will be somewhere between the sixth and seventh score from smallest. There is no such score, of course, by definition – the 6.5th score just does not exist. What we could do in these circumstances is to take the average of the sixth and seventh scores to give us an estimate of the median. For the distribution of 40 scores shown in Table 4.2, the middle score from the small­ est is somewhere between the 20th and 21st scores. Thus the median is somewhere

Table 4.2

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 47

Frequency distribution of 40 scores Score

Frequency ( f )

1

1

2

2

3

4

4

6

5

7

6

8

7

5

8

3

9

2

10

1

11

0

12

1

11/18/13 1:52 PM

48

CHAPTER 4  Describing variables numerically: Averages, variation and spread

between score 5 (the 20th score) and score 6 (the 21st score). One could give the average of these two as the median score – that is, the median is 5.5. For most purposes this is good enough. You may find that computer programs give different values from this. The computer program is making adjustments since there may be several identical scores near the median, but you need only a fraction of them to reach your mid-point score. So, in the above example the 21st score comes in score category 6 although there are actually eight scores in that category. So in order to get that extra score we need take only one-eighth of score category 6. One-eighth equals 0.125 so the estimated median equals 5.125. To be frank, it is difficult to think of many circumstances in which this level of precision about the value of the median is required in psychological statistics. If you follow our advice to use a computer program to do your calculations wherever possible you will always have a precise, adjusted value for the median.



■ The mode The mode is the most frequently occurring category of score. It is merely the most com­ mon score or most frequent category of scores. In other words, you can apply the mode to any category of data and not just scores. In the above example where the scores were 7, 5, 4, 7, 7, 5 we could represent the scores in terms of their frequencies of occurrence (Table 4.3). Frequencies are often represented as f in statistics. It is very easy to see in this example that the most frequently occurring score is 7 with a frequency of 3. So the mode of this distribution is 7. If we take the slightly different set of scores 7, 5, 4, 7, 7, 5, 3, 4, 6, 8, 5, the frequency distribution of these scores is shown in Table 4.4. Here there is no single mode since scores 5 and 7 jointly have the highest frequency of 3. This sort of distribution is called

Table 4.3

Frequencies of scores Score

Table 4.4

4

1

5

2

6

0

7

3

A bimodal frequency distribution Score

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 48

Frequency ( f )

Frequency ( f )

3

1

4

2

5

3

6

1

7

3

8

1

11/18/13 1:52 PM

4.2  Typical scores: mean, median and mode 49

bimodal and the two modes are 5 and 7. The general term multimodal implies that a frequency distribution has several modes. The mode is the only measure in this chapter that applies to nominal (category/categorical) data as well as numerical score data.

Box 4.1

Key concepts

Outliers and identifying them statistically Outliers, potentially, put your analysis at risk of erroneous conclusions. This is because they are scores which are so atypical of your data in general that they distort any trend in the data because they are unusually large or small. In other words, outliers are a few cases which are out of step with the rest of the data and can mislead the researcher. Outliers may be the result of a wide range of different factors. One does not have to identify what is causing such big or small values, but it is important to eliminate them because they can be so misleading. Routinely, good researchers examine their data for possible outliers simply by inspecting tables of frequencies or scatterplots, for example. This is normally sufficient but does involve an element of judgement which you may not be comfortable with. There are more objective ways of identifying outliers which reduce this subjective element. Essentially, what is done is to define precise limits beyond which a score is suspected of being an outlier. One simple way of doing this is based on the interquartile range which is not affected by outliers since it is based on the middle 50% of scores put in order of their size (p. 47). To calculate the interquartile range, essentially the scores on a variable are arranged from smallest to largest and the 25% of smallest scores and the 25% of largest scores ignored. This leaves the 50% of scores in the middle of the original distribution. The difference between the largest and the smallest score in this middle 50% is the interquartile range. Outliers, which by definition are unusually large or small scores, cannot affect the interquartile range since they will be in the top or bottom 25% of scores and thus eliminated in calculating the interquartile range. Imagine that we had the following scores for the IQs (Intelligence Quotients) from a sample of 12 individuals: 120, 115, 65, 140, 122, 142, 125, 135, 122, 136, 144, 118 Common sense would suggest that the score of 65 is uncharacteristic of the sample’s IQs in general so we would probably identify it as a potential outlier anyway. To calculate the interquartile range we would first rearrange the scores from smallest to largest (or get a computer to do all of the work for us). This gives us:

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 49

65, 115, 118, 120, 122, 122, 125, 135, 136, 140, 142, 144 Since there are 12 scores, to calculate the interquartile range we delete the three (i.e. 25%) lowest scores and also delete the three (i.e. 25%) highest scores. The three lowest scores are 65, 115 and 118 and the three highest scores are 140, 142 and 144. With the extreme quarters deleted, we have the following six scores which are the middle 50% of scores: 120, 122, 122, 125, 135, 136 The interquartile range is the largest of these scores minus the smallest. Thus the interquartile range is 136 − 120 = 16 in this case. This interquartile range is multiplied by 1.5 which gives us 1.5 × 16 = 24. Outliers among the low scores are defined as any score which is smaller than the smallest score in the interquartile range −24 = 120 − 24 = 96. Outliers among the high scores are defined as any score which is bigger than the largest score in the interquartile range +24 = 136 + 24 = 160. In other words, scores which are not between 96 and 160 are outliers in this example. The IQ of 65 is thus regarded as an outlier. On the assumption that the scores are normally distributed, then less than 1% of scores would be defined as outliers. This method identifies the moderate outliers. Extreme outliers are identified in much the same way, but the interquartile range is multiplied by 3 (rather than 1.5). This gives us 3 × 16 = 48. Extreme outliers among the low scores are scores which are smaller than 120 − 48 = 72. Extreme outliers among the high scores are scores larger than 136 + 48 = 184. Thus the participant who has an IQ of 65 would be identified as an extreme outlier. In normally distributed scores, extreme outliers will occur only about once in half a million scores. It would be usual practice to delete outliers from your data. You might also wish to compare the outcome of the analysis with the complete data and with outliers excluded. However, it is important to mention what you have done in any report about your research.

11/18/13 1:52 PM

50

4.3

CHAPTER 4  Describing variables numerically: Averages, variation and spread

Comparison of mean, median and mode Usually the mean, median and mode will give different values of the central tendency when applied to the same set of scores. It is only when a distribution is perfectly sym­ metrical and the distribution peaks in the middle that they coincide completely. Regard big differences between the mean, median and mode as a sign that your distribution of scores is rather asymmetrical or lopsided. Distributions of scores do not have to be perfectly symmetrical for statistical analysis, but symmetry tends to make some calculations a little more accurate. It is difficult to say how much lack of symmetry there can be without it becoming a serious problem. There is more about this later, especially in Chapter 19 and Appendix A which make some suggestions about how to test for asymmetry. This is done relatively rarely in our experience.

4.4

The spread of scores: variability The concept of variability is essential in statistics. Variability is a non-technical term and is related to (but is not identical with) the statistical term variance. Variance is nothing more or less than a mathematical formula that serves as a useful indicator of variability. But it is not the only way of assessing variability. Table 4.5 gives a set of ages of 12 university students and can be used to illustrate some different ways of measuring variability in our data. These 12 students vary in age from 18 to 33 years. In other words, the range covers a 15-year period. The interval from youngest to oldest (or tallest to shortest, or fattest to thinnest) is called the range – a useful statistical concept. As a statistical concept, correctly range is always expressed as a single number such as 20 centimetres and not as an interval, say, from 15 to 25 centimetres. One trouble with range is that it can be heavily influenced by extreme cases. Thus the 33-year-old student in the Table 4.5 is having a big influence on the range of ages of the students. This is because he or she is much older than most of the students. For this reason, the interquartile range has advantages. To calculate the interquartile range, we split the age distribution into quarters and take the range of the middle two quarters (or middle 50%), ignoring the extreme quarters. Since we have 12 students, we delete the three youngest (the three 18-year-olds) and the three oldest (aged 33, 23 and 21). This leaves us with the middle two quarters (the middle 50%) which includes five 19-year-olds and one 20-year-old. The range of this middle two quarters, or the interquartile range, is one year (from 19 years to 20 years). The interquartile range is sometimes a better indicator of the variability of, say, age than the full range because extreme ages are excluded. Useful as the range is, a lot of information is ignored. It does not take into account all of the scores in the set, merely the extreme ones. For this reason, measures of spread or variability have been developed which include the extent to which each of the scores in the set differs from the mean score of the set.

Table 4.5

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 50

The ages of a sample of 12 students

18 years

21 years

23 years

18 years

19 years

19 years

19 years

33 years

18 years

19 years

19 years

20 years

11/18/13 1:52 PM

4.4  The spread of scores: variability 51

One such measure is the mean deviation. To calculate this we have to work out the mean of the set of scores and then how much each score in the set differs from that mean. These deviations are then added up, ignoring the negative signs, to give the total of deviations from the mean. Finally, we can divide by the number of scores to give the average or mean deviation from the mean of the set of scores. If we take the ages of the students listed above, we find that the total of the ages is 18 + 21 + 23 + 18 + 19 + 19 + 19 + 33 + 18 + 19 + 19 + 20 = 246. Divide this total by 12 and we get the average age in the set to be 20.5 years. Now if we subtract 20.5 years from each of the student’s ages we get the figures in Table 4.6. The average amount of deviation from the mean (ignoring the sign) is known as the mean deviation (for the above deviations this would give a value of 2.6 years). Although frequently mentioned in statistical textbooks, it has no practical applications in psycho­ logical statistics and is best forgotten. However, there is a very closely related concept, variance, that is much more useful and has widespread and extensive applications. Variance is calculated in an almost identical way to mean deviation but for one thing. When we draw up a table to calculate the variance, we square each deviation from the mean before summing the total of these squared deviations as shown in Table 4.7. Table 4.6

Table 4.7

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 51

Deviations from the mean Score – mean

Deviation from mean

18 – 20.5 21 – 20.5 23 – 20.5 18 – 20.5 19 – 20.5 19 – 20.5 19 – 20.5 33 – 20.5 18 – 20.5 19 – 20.5 19 – 20.5 20 – 20.5

−2.5 0.5 2.5 −2.5 −1.5 −1.5 −1.5 12.5 −2.5 −1.5 −1.5 −0.5

Squared deviations from the mean

Score – mean

Deviation from mean

Square of deviation from mean

18 – 20.5 21 – 20.5 23 – 20.5 18 – 20.5 19 – 20.5 19 – 20.5 19 – 20.5 33 – 20.5 18 – 20.5 19 – 20.5 19 – 20.5 20 – 20.5

−2.5 0.5 2.5 −2.5 −1.5 −1.5 −1.5 12.5 −2.5 −1.5 −1.5 −0.5 Total = 0

6.25 0.25 6.25 6.25 2.25 2.25 2.25 156.25 6.25 2.25 2.25 0.25 Total = 193

11/18/13 1:52 PM

CHAPTER 4  Describing variables numerically: Averages, variation and spread

52

Box 4.2

Focus on

Using negative (–) values Although psychologists rarely collect data that involve negative signs, some statistical techniques can generate them. Negative values occur in statistical analyses because working out differences between scores is common. The mean is often taken away from scores, for example, or one score is subtracted from another. Generally speaking, negative values are not a problem since either the com­ puter or the calculator will do them for you. A positive value is one which is bigger than zero. Often the + sign is omitted as it is taken for granted. A negative value (or minus value or – value) is a num­ ber which is smaller than (less than) zero. The negative sign is never omitted. A value of −20 is a smaller number than −3 (whereas a value of +20 is a bigger number than +3). Negative values should cause few problems in terms of calculations – the calculator or computer has no difficul­ ties with them. With a calculator you will need to enter that a number is a negative. A key labelled +/− is often used to do this. On a computer, the number must be entered with a – sign. Probably, the following are the only things you need to know to be able to understand negative numbers in statistics: ●

If a negative number is multiplied by another nega­ tive number the outcome is a positive number. So

−2 × −3 = +6. This is also the case when a number is squared – squaring is when a number is multiplied by itself. Thus −32 = +9. You need this information to understand how the standard deviation and variance formulae work, for example. ●

Psychologists often speak of negative correlations and negative regression weights. This needs care because the negative in this case indicates that there is a reverse relationship between two sets of scores. That is, for example, the more intelligent a person is, the less time will they take to complete a crossword puzzle.



If you have got negative values for your scores, it is often advantageous to add a number of sufficient size to make all of the scores positive. This normally makes absolutely no difference to the outcome of your statist­ ical analysis. For example, the variance and standard deviation of −2, −5 and −6 are exactly the same if we add 6 to each of them. That is, calculate the variance and standard deviation of +4, +1 and 0 and you will find them to be identical to those for −2, −5 and −6. It is important that the same number is added to all of your scores. Doing this is helpful since many of us experience anxiety about negative values and prefer it if they are not there.

The total of the squared deviations from the mean is 193. If we divide this by the number of scores (12), it gives us the value of the variance, which equals 16.08 in this case. Expressing the concept as a formula: variance =

∑ (X − B ) 2 N

This formula defines what variance is – it is the defining formula. This is the most precise definition of variance there is although the problem is that it does not correspond precisely to more everyday or common-sense ideas. The calculation of variance above corresponds to this formula. However, in statistics there are often quicker ways of doing calculations. These quicker methods involve computational formulae as described in Box 4.3 though these are largely outmoded in these days of high-speed computers. We include them for the reason that aspects of some basic computational formula are to be found in other contexts such as the analysis of variance summary table.

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 52

11/18/13 1:52 PM

4.4 the SpreAD OF SCOreS: VArIAbIlIty

Box 4.3

53

Focus on

Computational formulae in statistics Before there were computers, psychologists would com­ pute statistical formula by hand. This is time consuming and risks errors so we do not recommend that you do your computations by hand. One way of easing the com­ putational chore in the past was to use what are known as computational formulae. These are probably little used now with the advent of computer statistical packages. They occasionally pop­up in a slightly disguised form in some statistical techniques – especially the analysis of variance (Chapters 21 to 27). You may never need to use these computational formulae in calculations but it is important that you understand that they may be conceptually important in terms of your understanding of statistics. In the light of all of this one computational formula is worth mentioning here – the formula for computing variance:

variance[computationall fformula] =

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

∑ = sigma or the summation sign, i.e. add up all the things which follow ∑X2 = the sum of the square of each of the scores 2 (∑X) = sum all the scores and square that total N = the number of scores This formula saves a lot of subtraction steps and so is quicker. If you understand the formula then fine but, if not, the important thing is simply to remember that there are quick formulae for doing calculations which are now outmoded but which appear in the explanation of some statistics. The calculation for the scores is as follows:

(∑ X )

2

N N 246 2 5236 − 12 12 60 516 5236 − 12 = 12

variance[computationall fformula] =

2

N

N

Take care with elements of this formula:

=

X = the general symbol for each member of a set of scores



∑ X2 −

5236 − 5043 193 = = 16.0 08 12 12

■ Interpreting the results Variance is difficult to interpret in isolation from other information about the data since it is dependent on the measurement in question. Measures which are based on a wide numerical scale for the scores will tend to have higher variance than measures based on a narrow scale. Thus if the range of scores is only 10 then the variance is likely to be less than if the range of scores is 100. Frequently variance is treated comparatively – that is, variances of different groups of people are compared (see Chapter 20).



■ reporting the results Usually variance is routinely reported in tables which summarise a variable or a number of variables along with other statistics such as the mean and range. This is shown in Table 4.8. Table 4.8

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 53

Illustrating the table for descriptive statistics

Variable

N

Mean

Variance

Range

Age

12

20.50 years

16.08

15 years

11/18/13 1:52 PM

54

CHAPTer 4 DeSCrIbINg VArIAbleS NUmerICAlly: AVerAgeS, VArIAtION AND SpreAD

Standard deviation (see Chapter 6) is a concept which computationally is very closely related to variance. Indeed, many textbooks deal with them at the same time. Unfortunately, this tends in our view to confuse two very distinct concepts and adds nothing to clarity.

Box 4.4

Key concepts

Variance estimate There is a concept called the variance estimate (or esti­ mated variance) which is closely related to variance. The difference is that the variance estimate is your best guess as to the variance of a population of scores if you only have the data from a small set of scores from that popula­ tion on which to base your estimate. The variance estimate is described in detail in Chapter 12. It involves a slight amendment to the variance formula in that instead of dividing by N one divides by N − 1. The formula for the estimated variance is: estimated variance =

∑ (X − B)2 N −1

Although not strictly speaking correct, some psycholo­ gists, textbooks and computer programs such as SPSS choose to use this formula in all practical circumstances. Despite this, variance and estimated variance are not quite

the same thing. However, since virtually all statistical analyses in psychology are based on samples and we normally wish to generalise from these samples then the estimated variance is likely to be used in most if not all practical situations. Hence it is reasonable to use the estimated variance as the general formula for variance. The drawback to this is that if we are merely describing the data, this practice is theoretically imprecise. Probably it would be best to simply refer to this as the estimated variance as this is what it is – it is calling it variance that is wrong. If we calculate the estimated variance using the data in Table 4.5, we need to divide 193 by 11 instead of the 12 that we did earlier. 193 divided by 11 is 17.545 or 17.55. This is the value that we get in the section showing the SPSS steps. This shows that SPSS calculates the estimated variance rather than the variance.

Research examples Averages, variation and spread It is difficult to imagine quantitative research studies in psychology which do NOT give details of averages and variation in some form. Typically very little space is devoted to this and highly stylised and structured ways of presenting such information are used. So you could open virtually any psychology journal describing an empirical study and you are almost certain to find them reported. Although variance is the basic measure of variation it is not so often reported. Modern psychologists seem to prefer to use standard deviation (SD) instead (standard deviation is the square root of variance). However, variance, standard deviation and standard error can be used virtually interchangeably as they are closely related and any researcher worth their salt knows the relationship between the three. Here are just a few examples. Cetinkalp (2012) provides some basic information on those taking part in his study of achievement goals in sport in the following way: ‘participants comprised 208 adolescent athletes of whom 120 were female (M ± SD = 16.33 ± 0.47) and 88 male (M ± SD = 16.38 ± 0.49) with a mean of age of 16.35 ± 0.48 years. participants, who took part in handball and volleyball competition at a regional level in Adana, turkey, reported that their sport experience was 4.00 ± 2.41 years, and they trained for 3.59 ± 1.75 days per week.’ (pp. 474–5)

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 54

11/18/13 1:52 PM

Key points 55



Kenyon and her colleagues (2012) tested whether people with bulimia nervosa or other unspecified eating disorder were less able to infer the feelings, beliefs and knowledge of other people than people who did not have psychological disorders. As part of the study they measured various characteristics of the participants in the three groups such as their age, body mass index, IQ and so on. They presented the mean scores with the standard deviation in brackets for each of the three groups. So the mean age of the 48 people in their study with bulimia nervosa was 28.0 years with a standard deviation of 7.7 years. The mean age of the 34 people with other unspecified eating disorders was 27.6 years with a standard deviation of 6.9 years. Meeten and Davey (2012) manipulated five moods by showing participants one of five films. The five moods were sad, happy, anxious, angry and neutral. Participants rated how they felt in these five conditions in terms of four scales measuring sadness, happiness, anxiety and anger. The mean scores with their standard devia­ tions in brackets were presented in a table with the five conditions represented by five columns and the four moods by four rows. In another table, they presented the mean score, standard deviation and minimum and maximum score for participants in these five groups separately and combined together for three measures of anxiety, depression and worry. Otgaar, Horselenberg, van Kampen and Lalleman (2012) reported the characteristics of the participants of their study of correct and incorrect reports of being touched as: ‘Eighty 4/5-year-olds (40 girls; mean age 4.66 years (56 months), SD = 0.53 (6.36 months) and 80 9/10-year-olds (36 girls; mean age 9.50 (114 months), SD = 0.64 (7.68 months)) obtained parental consent for their participation. These children were recruited from different primary schools in the Netherlands.’ (p. 643) Van Schaik and Ling (2012) write of their study: ‘One hundred and fourteen undergraduate psychology students (91 female, 23 male), with a mean age of 22.66 years (SD = 6.03) took part in the experiment. There were 30 participants in the condition of low artifact complexity/low task complexity, 29 in the low/high condition, 28 in the high/low condition, and 27 in the high/high condition. All participants had used the Web. Mean experience using the Web was 9.68 years (SD = 3.03), mean time per week spent using the Web was 17.25 hr (SD = 16.73) and mean frequency of Web use per week was 14.76 (SD = 9.87).’ (p. 209)

Key points ●

Because they are routine ways of summarising the typical score and the spread of a set of scores, it is important always to report the following information for each of your variables: ●

mean, median and mode



range and variance



number of scores in the set of scores.



The above does not apply to nominal categories. For these, the frequency of cases in each category exhausts the main possibilities.



It is worth trying to memorise the definitional and computational formulae for variance. You will be surprised how often these formulae appear in statistics.



When using a computer, look carefully for variables that have zero variance. They can cause problems and generally ought to be omitted from your analyses. Normally the computer will not compute the calculations you ask for in these circumstances. The difficulty is that if all the scores of a variable are the same, it is impossible to calculate many statistical formulae. It is not surprising that a computer won’t calculate variance if there is no variance in the data!

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 55

11/18/13 1:52 PM

CHAPTER 4  Describing variables numerically: Averages, variation and spread

56

Computer analysis Descriptive statistics using SPSS

Figure 4.2

SPSS Statistics steps for descriptive statistics when dealing with scores

Interpreting and reporting the output ●



In the example calculated, we can see that the mean, median and mode are relatively similar. The variance is 17.55 to two decimal places. These are the basic facts. It is difficult to say much more without having additional variables for comparison. One could write ‘The ages of the sample had a mean of 20.50 years with a median of 19.00 and a mode of 19. These are fairly close and perhaps indicate that the distribution is fairly symmetrical. The estimated variance was large at 17.55 reflecting the large value of the range (15).’

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 56

11/18/13 1:52 PM

Computer analysis 57



Screenshot 4.1

Variable View

Screenshot 4.2

Data View

Screenshot 4.3

Select procedure

Screenshot 4.4

Select variables

Screenshot 4.5

Select statistics

Screenshot 4.6

Important output

M04_HOWI0749_06_SE_C04.indd 57

11/18/13 1:52 PM

Chapter 5

Shapes of distributions of scores

Overview ●

The shape of the distribution of scores is a major consideration in statistical analysis. It simply refers to the characteristics of the frequency distribution (i.e. histogram) of the scores.



The normal distribution is an ideal because it forms part of the theoretical basis of many statistical techniques. It is best remembered as a bell-shaped frequency diagram.



The normal distribution is a symmetrical distribution. That is, it can be folded perfectly on itself at the mean. Such symmetry is another ‘ideal’ in many statistical analyses. Non-symmetrical distributions are known as skewed distributions.



Kurtosis indicates how steep or flat a curve is compared with the normal (bell-shaped) curve.



Cumulative frequencies are ones which include all of the lower values on an accumulating basis. So the highest score will always have a cumulative frequency of 100% since it includes all of the smaller scores.



Percentiles are the numerical values of the score that cut off the lowest 10%, 30%, 95% or what have you of the distribution of scores.

Preparation Be clear about numerical scores and how they can be classified into ranges of scores (Chapter 3).

M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 58

11/13/13 5:13 PM

5.2  Histograms and frequency curves 59

5.1 Introduction The final important characteristic of sets of scores is the particular shape of their distribution. It is useful for a researcher to be able to describe this shape succinctly. Obviously it is possible to find virtually any shape of distribution amongst the multitude of variables that could be measured. So, intuitively, it seems unrealistic to seek to describe just a few different shapes. But there are some advantages in doing so, as we shall see. The key steps when planning to discuss the shapes of data distributions are given in Figure 5.1.

Figure 5.1

5.2

Conceptual steps for understanding shapes of distributions

Histograms and frequency curves Most of us have very little difficulty in understanding histograms; we know that they are plots of the frequency of scores (the vertical dimension) against a numerical scale (the horizontal dimension). Figure 5.2 is an example of a histogram based on a relatively small set of scores. This histogram has quite severe steps from bar to bar. In other words, it is quite angular and not a smooth shape at all. Part of the reason for this is that the horizontal numerical scale moves along in discrete steps, so resulting in this pattern. Things would be different if we measured on a continuous scale on which every possible score could be represented to the smallest fraction. For example, we might decide to measure people’s heights in centimetres to the nearest whole centimetre. But we know that heights do not really conform to this set of discrete steps or points; people who measure 120 centimetres actually differ in height by up to a centimetre from each other. Height can be measured in fractions of centimetres, not just whole centimetres. In other

M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 59

11/13/13 5:13 PM

60

CHAPTER 5  Shapes of distributions of scores : Shapes of distributions of scores 

Figure 5.2

Histogram showing steep steps

Figure 5.3

A smooth curve based on small blocks

words height is a continuous measurement with infinitesimally small steps between measures so long as we use sufficiently precise measuring instruments. So a histogram of heights measured in centimetre units is at best an approximation to reality. Within each of the blocks of the histogram is a possible multitude of smaller steps. For this reason, it is conventional when drawing frequency curves for theoretical purposes to smooth out the blocks to form a continuous curve. In essence, this is like taking much finer and more precise measurements and redrawing the histogram. Instead of doing this literally we approximate it by drawing a smooth curve through imaginary sets of extremely small steps. When this is done our histogram is ‘miraculously’ turned into a continuous unstepped curve (Figure 5.3). A frequency curve can, of course, be of virtually any shape but one shape in particular is of concern in psychological statistics – the normal curve.

5.3

The normal curve The normal curve describes a particular shape of the frequency curve. Although this shape is defined by a formula and so can be described mathematically, for most purposes it is sufficient to regard it as a symmetrical bell-shape (Figure 5.4).

M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 60

11/13/13 5:13 PM

5.3  The normal curve 61

Figure 5.4

A normal (bell-shaped) frequency curve

It is called the ‘normal’ curve because it was once believed that distributions in the natural world corresponded to this shape. Even though it turns out that the perfect normal curve is not universal, it is important because many distributions are more or less this shape – at least sufficiently so for most practical purposes. The crucial reason for the use of the normal curve in statistics is that theoreticians developed many statistical techniques on the assumption that the distributions of scores had this particular bell-shape. It so happens that these assumptions which are useful in the development of statistical techniques have relatively little bearing on their day-to-day application. That is, the statistical techniques developed on the assumption of normality generally work well even when they are applied to data which is only roughly bell-shaped. In run-of-the-mill psychological statistics, the question of whether a distribution is normal or bell-shaped is not that important since often substantial violations of normality in our data make little difference to the value of the statistical test. Exceptions to this will be mentioned as appropriate in later chapters. Don’t forget that for the perfectly symmetrical, bell-shaped (normal) curve the values of the mean, median and mode are identical. Disparities between the three are indications that you have an asymmetrical curve.

Box 5.1

Focus on

How normal are my curves? One thing which may trouble you is the question of how precisely your data need fit this normal or bell-shaped ideal. Is it possible to depart much from the ideal without causing problems? The short answer is that usually a lot of deviation is possible without affecting things too much. So, in the present context, you should not worry too much if the mean, median and mode do differ somewhat; for practical purposes, you can disregard deviations from the ideal distribution, especially when dealing with about 30 or more scores. Unfortunately, all of this involves a degree of subjective judgement since there are no useful ways of assessing what is an acceptable amount of deviation from

M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 61

the ideal when faced with the small amounts of data that student projects often involve. If you wish you can use statistics which do not involve the normal curve (Chapter 19). These are known as nonparametric or distribution-free methods. Furthermore, it is possible to use bootstrapping methods with many statistical techniques which do not make the assumption of normality. That is, there are alternative versions of many statistical tests for which the issue of normality is not applicable. You can use these bootstrapping methods on SPSS which makes them very easy. As yet, bootstrapping is not familiar to most psychologists despite the fact it deals with such an important issue.

11/13/13 5:13 PM

62

5.4

CHAPTER 5  Shapes of distributions of scores : Shapes of distributions of scores 

Distorted curves The main concepts which deal with distortions in the normal curve are skewness and kurtosis.



■ Skewness It is always worth examining the shape of your frequency distributions. Gross skewness is the exception to our rule of thumb that non-normality of data has little influence on statistical analyses. By skewness we mean the extent to which your frequency curve is lopsided rather than symmetrical. A mid-point of a frequency curve may be skewed either to the left or to the right of the range of scores (Figures 5.5 and 5.6). There are special terms for left-handed and right-handed skew: ●



M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 62

Negative skew: ●

more scores are to the left of the mode than to the right



the mean and median are smaller than the mode.

Positive skew: ●

more scores are to the right of the mode than to the left



the mean and median are bigger than the mode.

Figure 5.5

Negative skew

Figure 5.6

Positive skew

11/13/13 5:13 PM

5.4  Distorted curves 63

There is also an index of the amount of skew shown in your set of scores. Looking at the frequency curve for the variable in question will give you a good idea of whether there is skewness. With computer analyses the ease of obtaining the index of skewness makes using complex formulae methods unnecessary. The index of skewness is positive for a positive skew and negative for a negative skew. Appendix A explains how to test for skewness in your data.



■ Kurtosis (or steepness/shallowness) Some symmetrical curves may look rather like the normal bell-shaped curve except that they are excessively steep or excessively flat compared to the mathematically defined normal bell-shaped curve (Figures 5.7 and 5.8). Kurtosis is the term used to identify the degree of steepness or shallowness of a distribution. There are technical words for different types of curve: ●

a steep curve is called leptokurtic



a normal curve is called mesokurtic



a flat curve is called platykurtic.

These are terms beloved of statistics book writers. However, since the terms mean nothing more than steep, middling and flat there is probably good reason to avoid these Greek words in favour of clear descriptions in everyday English.

M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 63

Figure 5.7

A shallow curve

Figure 5.8

A steep curve

11/13/13 5:13 PM

64

CHAPTER 5  Shapes of distributions of scores : Shapes of distributions of scores 

It is possible to obtain indexes of the amount of shallowness or steepness of your distribution compared with the mathematically defined normal distribution. These are easily obtained as part of a computer analysis such as when using SPSS. For most purposes, an inspection of the frequency curve of your data will give you a good idea. Knowing what the index means should help you cope with computer output; quite simply: ●

a positive value of kurtosis means that the curve is steep



a zero value of kurtosis means that the curve is middling – just like the normal curve



a negative value of kurtosis means that the curve is flat.

Steepness and shallowness have little or no bearing on the statistical techniques you use to analyse your data, quite unlike skewness.

5.5

Other frequency curves ■ Bimodal and multimodal frequency distributions Of course, there is no rule that says that frequency curves have to peak in the middle and tail off to the left and right. As we have already explained, it is perfectly possible to have a frequency distribution with twin peaks (or even multiple peaks). Such twin-peaked distributions are called bimodal since they have two modes – most frequently occurring scores. Such a frequency curve might look like Figure 5.9.



■ Cumulative frequency curves There are any number of different ways of presenting a single set of data. Take, for example, the 50 scores in Table 5.1 for a measure of extraversion obtained from airline pilots. One way of tabulating these extraversion scores is simply to count the number of pilots scoring at each value of extraversion from 1 to 5. This could be presented in several forms, for example Tables 5.2 and 5.3 and Figure 5.10. Exactly the same distribution of scores could be represented using a cumulative frequency distribution. A simple frequency distribution merely indicates the number of people who achieved any particular score. A cumulative frequency distribution gives the number scoring, say, one, two or less, three or less, four or less, and five or less. In other

Figure 5.9

M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 64

A bimodal frequency histogram

11/13/13 5:13 PM

5.5  Other frequency curves 65







Extraversion scores of 50 airline pilots

Table 5.1 3

5

5

4

4

5

5

3

5

2

1

2

5

3

2

1

2

3

3

3

4

2

5

5

4

2

4

5

1

5

5

3

3

4

1

4

2

5

1

2

3

2

5

4

2

1

2

3

4

1

Table 5.2

Table 5.3

Frequency table based on data in Table 5.1 Number scoring 1

7

Number scoring 2

11

Number scoring 3

10

Number scoring 4

9

Number scoring 5

13

Alternative layout for data in Table 5.1 Number of pilots scoring

1

2

3

4

5

7

11

10

9

13

Figure 5.10

Histogram of Table 5.1

words, the frequencies accumulate. Examples of cumulative frequency distributions are given in Tables 5.4 and 5.5 and Figure 5.11. Cumulative frequencies can be given also as cumulative percentage frequencies in which the frequencies are expressed as percentages and these percentages accumulated. This is shown in Table 5.4. There is nothing difficult about cumulative frequencies. However, you must label such tables and diagrams clearly – simply by using the word cumulative wherever appropriate – or they can be very misleading.

M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 65

11/13/13 5:13 PM

66

CHAPTER 5  Shapes of distributions of scores : Shapes of distributions of scores 

Table 5.4

Cumulative frequency distribution of pilots’ extraversion scores from Table 5.1

Score range

Cumulative frequency

1



Cumulative percentage frequency

7

14%

2 or less

18

36%

3 or less

28

56%

4 or less

37

74%

5 or less

50

100%

Table 5.5



Alternative style of cumulative frequency distribution of pilots’ extraversion scores from Table 5.1 Number of pilots scoring

1

2 or less

3 or less

4 or less

5 or less

7

18

28

37

50

Figure 5.11



Cumulative histogram of the frequencies of pilots’ extraversion scores from Table 5.1

■ Percentiles Percentiles are merely a form of cumulative frequency distribution, but instead of being expressed in terms of accumulating scores from lowest to highest, the categorisation is in terms of whole numbers of percentages of people. In other words, the percentile is the score which a given percentage of scores equals or is less than. You do not necessarily have to report every percentage point and units of 10 might suffice for some purposes. Such a distribution would look something like Table 5.6. The table shows that 10% of scores are equal to 7 or less and 80% of scores are equal to 61 or less. Note that the 50th percentile corresponds to the median score (but not necessarily the mean or mode). Percentiles are commonly used in standardisation tables of psychological tests and measures. That is, tables which present information on the distribution of test scores based on large samples of people. For these it is often very useful to be able to describe a person’s

M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 66

11/13/13 5:13 PM

5.5  Other frequency curves 67

Table 5.6

Example of percentiles Percentile

Score

10th

 7

20th

 9

30th

14

40th

20

50th

39

60th

45

70th

50

80th

61

90th

70

100th

78

standing compared with the set of individuals on which the test or measure was initially researched. Thus if a particular person’s neuroticism score is described as being at the 90th percentile it means that they are more neurotic than about 90% of people. In other words, percentiles are a quick method of expressing a person’s score relative to those of others. Not using percentiles can result in rather clumsy and convoluted explanations. In order to calculate the percentiles for any data, it is first necessary to produce a table of cumulative percentage frequencies. This table is then examined to find the score which cuts off, for example, the bottom 10%, the bottom 20%, the bottom 30%, etc. of scores. It should be obvious that calculating percentiles in this way is actually easier if there are a large number of scores so that the cut-off points can be found precisely.

Research examples Kurtosis, skew, etc. Brasel and Gips (2011) were interested in people’s use of what the researchers term the media landscape, which includes television and the Internet. Just what happens when people use either of these media? Using a laboratory-based design, individuals were studied when they ‘multitasked’ (i.e. used a computer and television simultaneously). One of the findings was the strongly skewed nature of people’s gaze at the screen. People gazed longer at the computer than the television. Nevertheless the conclusion was that the distribution of gaze is strongly skewed – short duration gazes of only a few seconds dominate. One of the intriguing findings was that people were very poor at estimating the extent of their gaze-switching behaviour compared with the objective reality as measured by the researchers. Kenyon and her colleagues (2012) tested whether people with bulimia nervosa or other unspecified eating disorder were less able to infer the feelings, beliefs and knowledge of other people than people who did not have psychological disorders. As part of the study they assessed how depressed, anxious and stressed the three groups were. Because these three variables were not normally distributed and could not be transformed to be so, they carried out nonparametric tests to determine whether there were any differences between the three groups (see Chapter 19 for a discussion of nonparametric tests).



M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 67

11/13/13 5:13 PM

CHAPTER 5  Shapes of distributions of scores : Shapes of distributions of scores 

68

Linley and his colleagues (2009) investigated the relationship between various measures of psychological well-being. Before carrying out their main statistical analyses, they examined the skewness and kurtosis of their nine measures together with their standard errors which they present in a table. They also inspected the normality of these distributions by looking at a histogram of their scores. According to both these methods their scores were normally distributed. Peters and Durding (1978) were interested in the relationship between laterality (right versus left-handedness) and the differences between performance on a simple tapping task for the left and right hand. Of course, obvious preference for the use of one hand to perform tasks will tend to emphasise that laterality has a biased distribution (many people are right-handed, some are left-handed, and a few have no clear preference). However, handedness in task performances not allowing such a preference is different and some have regarded it as a continuous variable. The tapping task involved in this study had children tapping with the index finger as fast as possible over a series of timed trials using the different index fingers. Laterality preference was assessed by having the child show the researcher how to do things like hammering in a nail, combing hair and brushing teeth. The hand chosen was recorded as the preferred hand. An index of laterality was calculated for a range of this sort of task. There was a linear relationship between the left/right speed of finger tapping and the child’s laterality as measured by the preference test for activities. Furthermore, the distribution of the tapping task differences was symmetrical about the mean and it was unimodal rather than, say, bimodal which would indicate discontinuities in handedness. This was not at all the case for the preference task. However, the distribution for finger tapping differences was more peaked than the normal distribution, indicating a degree of kurtosis which was significant. Overall, the research provided some support for the idea that laterality in performance is a continuous variable. Shafran and her colleagues (2006) were interested in determining whether being asked to have higher general personal standards such as working very hard would result in more dysfunctional eating than those who were asked to have lower general personal standards such as taking it easy at work. Some of the measures used to assess dysfunctional eating such as trying to restrict the intake of food and feeling regret after eating were significantly positively skewed. Consequently, non-parametric tests were used to test for differences on these variables. Wickham, Morris and Fritz (2000) addressed the question of the distinctiveness of faces. One conventional assumption is that there are many relatively typical faces but rather few which are distinctive. This would indicate a highly skewed distribution in terms of facial distinctiveness. The researchers went about testing this using three separate but related studies which used different ways of estimating distinctiveness. For example, traditional ratings of distinctiveness produced normal distributions but ratings which emphasised the amount of deviation from the typical face were very skewed. In their first study, however, they used traditional ratings of distinctiveness of faces. They used the distance on a physical scale such that 0 equalled extremely typical and 9 would be extremely distinctive. The mean rating was found to be 3.7 cm with a skewness of 0.25 and kurtosis of −0.91. A bar chart for these data looks relatively flat and there is a long tail towards the distinctiveness end of the continuum.

Key points ●

The most important concept in this chapter is that of the normal curve or normal distribution. It is worth extra effort to memorise the idea that the normal curve is a bell-shaped symmetrical curve.



Be a little wary if you find that your scores on a variable are very skewed since this can lose precision in certain statistical analyses.

M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 68

11/13/13 5:13 PM

  Computer analysis 69



Computer analysis SPSS and frequencies using SPSS

Figure 5.12

SPSS Statistics steps for frequency tables and histograms

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

The frequency table and histogram should be studied to identify their most characteristic features. Since tables and histograms are basically descriptive methods then their features may simply be reported and little or nothing by way of interpretation may be necessary.



Although frequency tables and histograms may be presented in your report, be careful to ensure that what appears is clear and effective. Too many tables and histograms can be distracting if not confusing. Perhaps you should find ways of reducing their number without changing effectiveness. Make sure that any that you use are properly labelled and mentioned in the text. In our experience, SPSS tables and histograms can always be improved by careful reflection and using the chart editor etc. It is easy to create a bad impression by including tables and diagrams which add nothing or even confuse the reader.

M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 69

11/13/13 5:13 PM

CHAPTER 5  Shapes of distributions of scores : Shapes of distributions of scores 

70

Screenshot 5.1

Part of the data

Screenshot 5.2

Select frequencies



Move variables to the Variable(s) box

Screenshot 5.4

The frequency output table

Selecting histograms with Legacy Dialogs

Screenshot 5.6

The histogram output





Screenshot 5.3



Screenshot 5.5

M05_HOWI0749_06_SE_C05.indd 70

11/13/13 5:13 PM

Chapter 6

Standard deviation and z-scores The standard unit of measurement in statistics

Overview ●

Standard deviation computationally is the square root of variance (Chapter 4).



Conceptually, standard deviation is distance along a frequency distribution of scores.



Because the normal (bell-shaped) curve is a standard shape, it is possible to give the distribution as percentages of cases which lie between any two points on the frequency distribution. Tables are available to do this relatively simply.



It is common to express scores as z-scores. A z-score for a particular score is simply the number of standard deviations that the score lies from the mean of the distribution. (A negative sign is used to indicate that the score lies below the mean.) z-scores are also referred to as standardised scores or standard scores.

Preparation Make sure you know the meaning of variables, scores, ∑ and scales of measurement – especially nominal, interval and ratio (Chapter 2).

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 71

11/18/13 1:54 PM

72

CHAPTER 6  Standard deviation and z-scores: The standard unit of measurement in statistics

6.1 Introduction Measurement ideally uses standard or universal units. It would be really stupid if, when we ask people how far it is to the nearest railway station, one person says 347 cow’s lengths, another says 150 poodle jumps and a third person says three times the distance between my doctor’s house and my dentist’s home. If you ask us how hot it was on midsummer’s day you would be pretty annoyed if one of us said 27 degrees Howitt and the other said 530 degrees Cramer. We measure in standard units such as centimetres, degrees Celsius, kilograms and so forth. The advantages of doing so are obvious: standard units of measurement allow us to communicate easily, precisely and effectively with other people. It is much the same in statistics but there is a difficulty. Statistics is applied to data of all sorts and in all sorts of disciplines. Some variables are measured in physical ways such as metres and kilograms. Others use more abstract units of measurement such as scores on an intelligence test or a personality inventory. Statistics is used universally in research so it needs a universal measuring system. Although it would be nice if statisticians had a standard unit of measurement, it is not intuitively obvious what this should be.

6.2

Theoretical background Imagine a 30 centimetre rule – it will be marked in 1 centimetre units from 0 centimetres to 30 centimetres (Figure 6.1). The standard unit of measurement here is the centimetre. But you could have a different sort of rule in which instead of the scale being from 0 to 30 centimetres, the mid-point of the scale is 0 and the scale is marked as −15, −14, −13, .  .  .  , −1, 0, +1, .  .  .  , +13, +14, +15 centimetres. This rule is in essence marked in deviation units (Figure 6.2). The two rules use the same unit of measurement (the centimetre) but the deviation rule is marked with 0 in the middle, not at the left-hand side. In other words, the midpoint of the scale is marked as 0 deviation (from the mid-point). The standard deviation

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 72

Figure 6.1

A 30 centimetre rule

Figure 6.2

A 30 centimetre rule using deviation units

11/18/13 1:54 PM

6.2  Theoretical background 73

is similar to this rule in so far as it is based on distances or deviations from the average or mid-point. As it is the standard unit of measurement in statistics, it is a pity that statisticians chose to call it standard deviation rather than ‘standard statistical unit’. The latter phrase would better describe what it is. In contrast to a lot of measurements such as metres and kilograms, the standard deviation corresponds to no single standard of measurement that can be defined in absolute terms against a physical entity locked in a vault somewhere. The standard deviation of a set of scores is measured relative to all of the scores in that set. Put as simply as possible, a standard deviation is the ‘average’ amount by which scores differ from the mean or average score. Now this is an odd idea – basing your standard measure on a set of scores rather than on an absolute standard. Nevertheless, it is an important concept to grasp. Don’t jump ahead at this stage – there are a couple of twists in the logic yet. Perhaps you are imagining that if the scores were 4, 6, 3 and 7 then the mean is 20 divided by 4 (the number of scores), or 5. Each of the four scores deviates from the average by a certain amount – for example, 7 deviates from the mean of 5 by just 2. The sum of the deviations of our four scores from the mean of 5 is 1 + 1 + 2 + 2 which equals 6. Surely, then, the standard deviation is 6 divided by 4, which equals 1.5? But this is not how statisticians work out the average deviation for their standard unit. Such an approach might seem logical, but it turns out to be not very useful in practice. Instead standard deviation uses a different type of average which most mortals would not even recognise as an average. The big difference is that standard deviation is calculated as the average squared deviation. What this implies is that instead of taking our four deviation scores (1 + 1 + 2 + 2) we square each of them (12 + 12 + 22 + 22) which gives 1 + 1 + 4 + 4 = 10. If we divide this total deviation of 10 by the number of scores (4), this gives a value of 2.5. However, this is still not quite the end of the story since we then have to calculate the square root of this peculiar average deviation from the mean. Thus we take the 2.5 and work out its square root – that is, 1.58. In words, the standard deviation is the square root of the average squared deviation from the mean. And that really is it – honest. It is a pity that one of the most important concepts in statistics is less than intuitively obvious, but there we are. To summarise: ●

The standard deviation is the standard unit of measurement in statistics.



The standard deviation is simply the ‘average’ amount that the scores on a variable deviate (or differ) from the mean of the set of scores. In essence, the standard deviation is the average deviation from the mean. Think of it like this since most of us will have little difficulty grasping it in these terms. Its peculiarities can be safely ignored for most purposes.



Although the standard deviation is an average, it is not the sort of average which most of us are used to. However, it is of greater use in statistical applications than any other way of calculating the average deviation from the mean.

The standard deviation gives greater numerical emphasis to scores which depart by larger amounts from the mean. The reason is that it involves squared deviations from the mean which give disproportionately more emphasis to larger deviations. It should be stressed that the standard deviation is not a unit-free measure. If we measured a set of people’s heights in centimetres, the standard deviation of their heights would also be a certain number of centimetres. If we measured 50 people’s intelligences using an intelligence test, the standard deviation would be a certain number of IQ points. It might help you to remember this, although most people would say or write things like ‘the standard deviation of height was 4.5’ without mentioning the units of measurement. Figure 6.3 gives the key steps in relation to using standard deviation.

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 73

11/18/13 1:54 PM

74

CHAPTER 6 STandard devIaTIOn and z-SCOreS: The STandard unIT Of MeaSureMenT In STaTISTICS

FiguRE 6.3

Conceptual steps for understanding standard deviation

Explaining statistics 6.1

how standard deviation works The defining formula for standard deviation is as follows: standard deviation =

∑ (X − B)2 N

or the computationally quicker formula is:

standard deviation =

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

N

2

N

Table 6.1 lists the ages of nine students (N = number of scores = 9) and shows steps in calculating the standard deviation. Substituting these values in the standard deviation formula:

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 74

11/18/13 1:54 PM

6.2 TheOreTICal BaCkgrOund

Table 6.1

75

Steps in the calculation of the standard deviation Scores ( X ) (age in years)

Scores squared ( X 2)

20

400

25

625

standard deviation =

19

361

35

1225

19

361

17

289

15

225

30

900

27

729

∑ X = 207

∑ X = 5115

∑X

2

2

(∑ X ) − N

N

2

=

(207) 2 9 9

5115 −

=

5115 15 − 4761 9

=

354 = 39.333 = 6.2 27 9

(You may have spotted that the standard deviation is simply the square root of the variance.)

Interpreting the results Like variance, standard deviation is difficult to interpret without other information about the data. Standard deviation is just a sort of average deviation from the mean. Its size will depend on the scale of the measurement in question. The bigger the units of the scale, the bigger the standard deviation is likely to be.

reporting the results Usually standard deviation is routinely reported in tables which summarise a variable or a number of variables along with other statistics such as the mean and range. This is shown in Table 6.2.

Table 6.2

Illustrating the table for descriptive statistics

Variable

N

Mean

Range

Standard deviation

age

9

23.00 years

20.00 years

6.27 years

The standard deviation is important in statistics for many reasons. The most important is that the size of the standard deviation is an indicator of how much variability there is in the scores for a particular variable. The bigger the standard deviation the more spread there is in the scores. However, this is merely to use standard deviation as a substitute for its close relative variance.

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 75

11/18/13 1:54 PM

CHAPTER 6 STandard devIaTIOn and z-SCOreS: The STandard unIT Of MeaSureMenT In STaTISTICS

76

Box 6.1

Key concepts

estimated standard deviation In this chapter the standard deviation is discussed as a descriptive statistic; that is, it is used like the mean and median, for example, to characterise important features of a set of scores. Be careful to distinguish this from the estimated standard deviation which is discussed in Chapter 12. Estimated standard deviation is your best guess as to the standard deviation of a population of scores based on information known about only a small subset or sample of scores from that population. Estimated standard deviation involves a modification to the standard deviation formula so that the estimate is better – the formula is modified to read N − 1 instead of just N. The formula for the estimated standard deviation is:

6.3

estimated standard deviation =

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

N −1

2

N

If you wish, this formula could be used in all of your calculations of standard deviation. Some textbooks and some computer programs give you calculations based on the above formula in all circumstances. Since virtually all statistical analyses in psychology are based on samples and we normally wish to generalise from these samples to all cases then there is good justification for this practice. The downside is that if we are describing the data rather than generalising from them then the formula is theoretically a little imprecise. If we did this calculation we would obtain a value of 6.65. This is the value that SPSS calls the standard deviation though this is a bit of a misnomer.

Measuring the number of standard deviations – the z-score Given that one of the aims of statisticians is to make life as simple as possible for themselves, they try to use the minimum number of concepts possible. Expressing standard statistical units in terms of standard deviations is just one step towards trying to express many measures in a consistent way. Another way of achieving consistency is to express all scores in terms of a number of standard deviations. That is, we can abandon the original units of measurements almost entirely if all scores are re-expressed as a number of standard deviations. It is a bit like calculating all weights in terms of kilograms or all distances in terms of metres. So, for example, since there are 2.2 pounds in a kilogram, something that weighs 10 pounds converts to 4.5 kilograms. We simply divide the number of pounds weight by the number of pounds in a kilogram in order to express our weight in pounds in terms of our standard unit of weight, the kilogram. It is very much like this in statistics. If we know that the size of the standard deviation is, say, 7, we know that a score which is 21 above the mean score is 21 ÷ 7 or three standard deviations above the mean. A score which is 14 below the mean is 14/7 or two standard deviations below the mean. So, once the size of the standard deviation is known, all scores can be re-expressed in terms of the number of standard deviations they are from the mean. One big advantage of this is that, unlike other standard units of measurement such as distance and weight, the number of standard deviations will apply no matter what the variable being measured is. Thus it is equally applicable if we are measuring time, anxiety, depression, height or any other variable. So the number of standard deviations is a universal scale of measurement. But note the stress on the number of standard deviations. Despite sounding a bit space-age and ultra-modern, the z-score is nothing other than the number of standard deviations a particular score lies above or below the mean of the set of scores – precisely the concept just discussed. So in order to work out the z-score

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 76

11/18/13 1:54 PM

6.4 a uSe Of z-SCOreS

77

for a particular score (X) on a variable we also need to know the mean of the set of scores on that variable and the value of the standard deviation of that set of scores. Sometimes it is referred to as the standard score since it allows all scores to be expressed in a standard form.

Explaining statistics 6.2

how z-scores work To convert the age of a 32-year-old to a z-score, given that the mean of the set of ages is 40 years and the standard deviation of age is 6 years, just apply the following formula: z -score =

X− B SD

where X stands for a particular score, B is the mean of the set of scores and SD stands for standard deviation. The z-score of any age (e.g. 32) can be obtained as follows: z-score[of a 32-year-old] =

32 − 40 −8 = = − 1 .3 33 6 6

The value of −1.33 means that: ●

a 32-year-old is 1.33 standard deviations from the mean age of 40 for this set of age scores



the minus sign simply means that the 32-year-old is younger (lower) than the mean age for the set of age scores. A plus sign (or no sign) would mean that the person is older (higher) than the mean age of 40 years.

Interpreting the results There is little to be added about interpreting the z-score since it is defined by the formula as the number of standard deviations a score is from the mean score. Generally speaking, the larger the z-score (either positive or negative) the more atypical a score is of the typical score in the data. A z-score of about 2 or more is fairly rare.

reporting the results As z-scores are scores they can be presented as you would any other score using tables or diagrams. Usually there is no point in reporting the mean of a set of z-scores since this will be 0.00 if calculated for all of the cases.

6.4

A use of z-scores z-scores, at first sight, deter a lot of students. They are an odd, abstract idea which needs a little time to master. In addition, they seem to achieve very little for students who do their statistics using computer programs. There is some truth in this, but it overlooks the fact that standardised scores (which are very like z-scores) appear in many of the more advanced statistical techniques to be found later in this book. So if you master z-scores now, this will make your learning at later stages much easier. So z-scores are merely scores expressed in terms of the number of standard statistical units of measurement (standard deviations) they are from the mean of the set of scores.

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 77

11/18/13 1:54 PM

78

CHAPTER 6  Standard deviation and z-scores: The standard unit of measurement in statistics

One big advantage of using these standard units of measurement is that variables measured in terms of many different units of measurement can be compared with each other and even combined. A good example of this comes from a student project (Szostak, 1995). The researcher was interested in the amount of anxiety that child tennis players exhibited and its effect on their performance (serving faults) in competitive situations as compared with practice. One consideration was the amount of commitment that parents demonstrated to their children’s tennis. Rather than base this simply on the extent to which parents claimed to be involved, she asked parents the amount of money they spent on their child’s tennis, the amount of time they spent on their child’s tennis and so forth: 1. How much money do you spend per week on your child’s tennis coaching? 2. How much money do you spend per year on your child’s tennis equipment? 3. How much money do you spend per year on your child’s tennis clothing? 4. How many miles per week on average do you spend travelling to tennis events? 5. How many hours per week on average do you spend watching your child play tennis? 6. How many LTA tournaments does your child participate in per year? This is quite straightforward information to collect, but it causes difficulties in analysing the data. The student wanted to combine these six different measures of commitment to give an overall commitment score for each parent. However, the six items are based on radically different units of measurement – time, money and so forth. Her solution was simply to turn each parent’s score on each of the questionnaire items into a z-score. This involves only the labour of working out the mean and standard deviation of the answers to each questionnaire and then turning each score into a z-score. These six z-scores are then added (including the + or − signs) to give a total score on the amount of commitment by each parent, which could be a positive or negative value since z-scores can be + or −. This was an excellent strategy since this measure of parental commitment was the best predictor of a child performing poorly in competitive situations; the more parental commitment the worse the child does in real matches compared with practice. There are plenty of other uses of the standard deviation in statistics, as we shall see.

6.5

The standard normal distribution There is a remaining important use of standard deviation. Although it should now be obvious that there are some advantages in converting scores into standard units of measurement, you might get the impression that, in the end, the scores themselves on a variable contain information which the z-score does not fully capture. In particular, if one looks at a distribution of the original scores, it is possible to have a good idea of how a particular individual scores relative to other people. So, for example, if you know the distribution of weights in a set of people, it should be possible to say something about the weight of a particular person relative to other people. A histogram giving the weights of 38 children in a school class allows us to compare a child with a weight of, say, 42 kilograms with the rest of the class (Figure 6.4). We can see that a child of 42 kilograms is in the top four of the distribution – that is, in about the top 10% of the weight distribution. Counting the frequencies in the histogram tells us the percentage of the part of the distribution the child falls in. We can also work out that 34 out of 38 (about 90%) of the class are lighter than this particular child. Surely this cannot be done if we work with standard deviations? In fact it is relatively straightforward to do so since there are ready-made tables to tell us precisely how a

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 78

11/18/13 1:54 PM

6.5 The STandard nOrMal dISTrIBuTIOn

FiguRE 6.4

79

distribution of weights in a set of children

particular score (expressed as a z-score or number of standard deviations from the mean) compares with other scores. This is achieved by using a commonly available table which gives the frequency curve of z-scores assuming that this distribution is bellshaped or normal. This table is known as either the standard normal distribution or the z-distribution. To be frank, some versions of the table are rather complicated, but we have opted for the simplest and most generally useful possible. Many statistical tables are known as tables of significance for reasons which will become more apparent later on. Significance Table 6.1 gives the percentage number of scores which will be higher than a score with a given z-score. Basically this means that the table gives the proportion of the frequency distribution of z-scores which lie in the shaded portions in the example shown in Figure 6.5. The table assumes that the distribution of scores is normal or bell-shaped. The table usually works sufficiently well even if the distribution departs somewhat from the normal shape. Of course, since the area of the entire curve is 100% then it is quite easy to work out other characteristics of the curve. So if you know, for example, that 15.87% of scores will be above 1 standard deviation above the mean, it is a quick calculation to say that 100% − 15.87% = 84.13% will be below 1 standard deviation above the mean.

Explaining statistics 6.3

how the table of the standard normal distribution works Significance Table 6.1 is easy to use. Imagine that you have the IQs of a set of 250 people. The mean (B) of these IQs is 100 and you calculate that the standard deviation (SD) is 15. You could use this information to calculate the z-score of Darren Jones who scored 90 on the test: z -score =

X − B 90 − 100 = SD 15 =

−10 = −0.67 67 = −0.7 (to 1 decciimal place) 15

Taking a z-score of −0.7, Significance Table 6.1 tells us that 75.80% of people in the set would have IQs equal to or greater than Darren’s. In other words, he is not particularly intelligent. If the z-score of Natalie Smith is +2.0 then this would mean that only 2.28% of scores are equal to or higher than Natalie’s – she’s very bright. Of course, you could use the table to calculate the proportion of people with lower IQs than Darren and Natalie. Since the total amount of scores is 100%, we can calculate that there are 100% − 75.80% = 24.20% of people with IQs equal to or smaller than his. For Natalie, there are 100% − 2.28% = 97.72% of scores equal to or lower than hers.



M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 79

11/18/13 1:54 PM

80

CHAPTER 6  Standard deviation and z-scores: The standard unit of measurement in statistics

The standard normal z-distribution: this gives the percentage of z-scores which are higher than the tabled values

Significance Table 6.1

z-score

Percentage of scores higher than this particular z-score

−4.00

99.997%

−3.00 −2.90

z-score

Percentage of scores higher than this particular z-score

0.00

50.00%

99.87%

+0.10

46.02%

99.81%

+0.20

42.07%

−2.80

99.74%

+0.30

38.21%

−2.70

99.65%

+0.40

34.46%

−2.60

99.53%

+0.50

30.85%

−2.50

99.38%

+0.60

27.43%

−2.40

99.18%

+0.70

24.20%

−2.30

98.93%

+0.80

21.19%

−2.20

98.61%

+0.90

18.41%

−2.10

98.21%

+1.00

15.87%

−2.00

97.72%

+1.10

13.57%

−1.96

97.50%

+1.20

11.51%

+1.30

9.68%

+1.40

8.08%

+1.50

6.68%

+1.60

5.48%

z-scores above this point are in the extreme 5% of scores in either direction from the mean (i.e. the extreme 2.5% below the mean) −1.90

97.13%

−1.80

96.41%

−1.70

95.54%

−1.64

95.00%

z-scores above this point are in the extreme 5% below the mean

z-scores below this point are in the extreme 5% above the mean +1.64

5.00%

+1.70

4.46%

+1.80

3.59%

+1.90

2.87%

−1.60

94.52%

−1.50

93.32%

−1.40

91.92%

−1.30

90.32%

−1.20

88.49%

+1.96

2.50%

−1.10

86.43%

+2.00

2.28%

−1.00

84.13%

+2.10

1.79%

−0.90

81.59%

+2.20

1.39%

−0.80

78.81%

+2.30

1.07%

−0.70

75.80%

+2.40

0.82%

−0.60

72.57%

+2.50

0.62%

−0.50

69.15%

+2.60

0.47%

−0.40

65.54%

+2.70

0.35%

−0.30

61.79%

+2.80

0.26%

−0.20

57.93%

+2.90

0.19%

−0.10

53.98%

+3.00

0.13%

0.00

50.00%

+4.00

0.0003%

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 80

z-scores below this point are in the extreme 5% of scores in either direction from the mean (i.e. the extreme 2.5% above the mean)

11/18/13 1:54 PM

6.5  The standard normal distribution 81

Figure 6.5

The part of the z-distribution which is listed in Significance Table 6.1

■ More about Significance Table 6.1



Significance Table 6.1 is just about as simple as we could make it. It is not quite the same as similar tables in other books:

Box 6.2



We have given negative as well as positive values of z-scores.



We have only given z-scores in intervals of 0.1 with a few exceptions.



We have given percentages – many other versions of the table give proportions out of 1. In order to convert the values in Significance Table 6.1 into proportions, simply divide the percentage by 100 and delete the % sign.



We have introduced a number of ‘cut-off points’ or zones into the table. These basically isolate extreme parts of the distribution of z-scores and identify those z-scores which come into the extreme 5% of the distribution. If you like, these are the exceptionally high and exceptionally low z-scores. The importance of this might not be obvious right now but will be clearer later on. The extreme zones are described as ‘significant’. We have indicated the extreme 5% in either direction (that is, the extreme 2.5% above and below the mean) as well as the extreme 5% in a particular direction.

Focus on

Negative signs One thing which can cause confusion is when psychologists talk about plus two standard deviations or minus one standard deviation. The first thing to say is that a standard deviation can never itself have a negative value – a standard deviation is positive. The reason why psychologists talk about minus standard deviations is because they are saying how many standard deviations a score is below the

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 81

mean. Thus a plus indicates that a score is so many standard deviations above the mean and a minus means that the score is so many standard deviations below the mean. Really, what they should be saying is that a score has a z-score of +2 or a z-score of −1 since this is where the pluses and minuses come from and nobody would get confused.

11/18/13 1:54 PM

82

CHAPTER 6  Standard deviation and z-scores: The standard unit of measurement in statistics

6.6 An important feature of z-scores By using z-scores the researcher is able to say an enormous amount about a distribution of scores extremely succinctly. If we present the following information: ●

the mean of a distribution



the standard deviation of the distribution



that the distribution is roughly bell-shaped or normal

then we can use this information to make very clear statements about the relative position of any score on the variable in question. In other words, rather than present an entire frequency distribution, these three pieces of information are virtually all that is required. Indeed, the third assumption is rarely mentioned since in most applications it makes very little difference.

Research examples Standard deviation and z-scores Contador, Fernández-Calvo, Cacho, Ramos and López-Rolón (2010) used z-scores to define the level of memory scores which they describe as impaired: ‘To find the proportion of the subjects whose performance fell outside of the normal range, scores were converted to z-scores. Patients were considered to be impaired if their z-scores were lower than −1.5. Considering that −2 SD are also often used as a cut off for such purposes, we computed additionally the proportion of patients whose z-scores fell lower than −2 SD.’ (p. 255) Di Filippo, de Luca, Judica, Spinelli and Zoccolotti (2006) were interested in the lexicality (readability) of words in relation to word length in a sample of dyslexic Italian children and a sample of age-matched con­ trols. They analysed their data twice: once using the raw scores on reaction times to the words and again using z-score transformations. The raw reaction time data demonstrated that reaction times to non-words were bigger than for real words and bigger for long words than for short words in dyslexics than proficient readers. But things changed when the data had been transformed into z-scores. The lexicality effect dis­ appeared although the length of word effect remained. The researchers put this down to what they call the ‘overadditivity’ effect in the raw data. The authors explain this in the following way: ‘However, overall performance changes can directly influence the size of the interaction (so-called overadditivity effect  .  .  .  ) when response time is considered, one can expect that the effect due to any experimental manipulation will be smaller for a subject with relatively fast responses than a subject with slower responses. As a consequence, a “spurious” interaction may be produced.’ (p. 142). Faust and colleagues (1999) proposed transformations (z-scores) to control for this overadditivity effect. Green, Rohling, Lees-Hayley and Allen (2001) studied the performance of patients who had been given a battery of neuropsychological tests. The researchers also included measurement of the effort put into the testing by

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 82

11/18/13 1:54 PM

  Key points 83



the patients. The context of the assessment was compensation claims for the patient’s disabilities. There were a total of 43 neuropsychological test scores. The researchers obtained the z-score values for each of these tests from standardisation data for the tests. This allowed the scores of each patient to be summed and averaged to give an Overall Test Battery mean. That is to say, average z-scores were obtained on the basis of normative data rather than by reference to the means and standard deviations for the sample involved in the research. The variable measuring effort correlated with the overall test battery mean quite substantially. The evidence suggested that sub-optimum effort reduced the overall score by several times the amount that moderate or severe brain injury did. If only patients making a good effort on the effort variable were included, then patients with severe brain injuries and neurological diseases performed substantially worse than the patients presumed not to have neurological problems. These data support the need for the assessment of effort as part of neuropsychological testing as without it, the expected relationship between brain injury and neurological disease may be reversed. Tremont and Alosco (2011) investigated the correlates of lack of awareness of their condition in Alzheimer’s sufferers. Such lack of insight into one’s condition is known as anosognosia. It is common in Alzheimer’s disease but its role in cognitive performance has not been extensively researched. The participants were 65 Alzheimer’s sufferers who took part in an extensive neuropsychological evaluation using a range of different measures. About half were aware and about half were unaware of their condition. This classification was done using the ratings of a clinical interview which also included a family member as informant. In order to compare their cognitive functioning based on a wide variety of measures, the researchers chose to convert each measure to a z-score by subtracting the sample mean from each individual’s score and dividing by the standard deviation of that measure. The z-scores for each individual could be added up and averaged. This gave a measure of cognitive performance based on each measure contributing equally. Although there were no significant differences between the aware and non-aware groups in terms of age, gender, education level, the unaware group did significantly worse on cognitive tasks which involved learning. Despite this, generally, the groups performed similarly on cognitive tasks.

Key points ●

Do not despair if you have problems in understanding standard deviation; it is one of the most abstract ideas in statistics, but so fundamental that it cannot be avoided. It can take some time to absorb completely.



Remember that the standard deviation is a sort of average deviation from the mean and you will not go far wrong.



Remember that using z-scores is simply a way of putting variables on a standard unit of measurement irrespective of special characteristics of that variable. Standardised values are common in the more advanced statistical techniques so it is good to master them at an early stage.



Remember that virtually any numerical score variable can be summarised using the standard deviation and that virtually any measurement can be expressed as a z-score. The main exception to its use is measurements which are in nominal categories like occupation or eye colour. Certainly if a score is interval or ratio in nature, standard deviation and z-scores are appropriate.

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 83

11/18/13 1:54 PM

CHAPTER 6  Standard deviation and z-scores: The standard unit of measurement in statistics

84

Computer analysis Standard deviation and z-scores using SPSS

Figure 6.6

SPSS Statistics steps for standard deviation and z-scores

How to interpret and report your data ●

The standard deviation of just one variable is readily mentioned in the text of your report: ‘The standard deviation of age was 6.65 years (n = 9).’ However, if you have a lot of variables, a table giving basic descriptive statistics for several variables may be more effective. Remember that SPSS gives the estimated standard deviation so the value here is the one we calculated in Box 6.1 – the estimated standard deviation.



It is not usual to report standard scores as this would be somewhat like reporting the raw scores for each individual. However, you need to understand standard scores as these can be meaningfully added, etc. because they have been standardised to be on the same scale of measurement.

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 84

11/18/13 1:54 PM

  Computer analysis 85



Screenshot 6.1

Enter the data

Screenshot 6.2

Select Descriptive Statistics

Screenshot 6.3

Move variable to Variable(s) box

Screenshot 6.4

Part of the output

Screenshot 6.5

Select Save standardized values

Screenshot 6.6

z-scores appear in Data View

M06_HOWI0749_06_SE_C06.indd 85

11/18/13 1:54 PM

Chapter 7

Relationships between two or more variables Diagrams and tables

Overview ●

Most research in psychology involves the relationships between two or more variables.



Relationships between two score variables may be represented pictorially as a scattergram (or scatterplot). Alternatively, a crosstabulation table with the scores broken down into ranges (or bands) is sometimes effective.



If both variables are nominal (category) then compound bar charts of various sorts may be used or, alternatively, crosstabulation tables.



If there is one score variable and one nominal (category) variable then often tables of means of the score variable tabulated against the nominal (category) variable will be adequate. It is possible, alternatively, to employ a compound histogram.

Preparation You should be aware of the meaning of variables, scores and the different scales of measurement, especially the difference between nominal (category) measurement and numerical scores.

M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 86

11/13/13 5:13 PM

7.1  Introduction 87

7.1 Introduction Although it is fundamental and vitally important to be able to describe the characteristics of each variable in your research both diagrammatically and numerically, interrelationships between variables are more characteristic of research in psychology and the social sciences. Public opinion polling is the most common use of single-variable statistics that most of us come across. Opinion pollsters ask a whole series of questions about political leaders and voting intentions which are generally reported separately. However, researchers often report relationships between two variables. So, for example, if one asks whether the voting intentions of men and women differ, it is really to enquire whether there is a relationship between the variable ‘gender’ and the variable ‘voting intention’. (Or another way of putting this is to ask whether there is a difference between men and women in terms of their voting intention.) Similarly, if one asks whether the popularity of the President of the USA changed over time, this really implies that there may be a relationship between the variable ‘time’ and the variable ‘popularity of the President’. Questions such as these are so familiar to us that we regard them almost as common sense. Consequently, we should not have any great difficulty in understanding the concept of interrelationships among variables. Interrelationships between variables form the bedrock of virtually all psychological research. It is rare in psychology to have research questions which require data from only one variable at a time. Much of psychology concerns explanations of why things happen – what causes what – which clearly is about relationships between variables. This chapter describes some of the main graphical and tabular methods for presenting interrelationships between variables. Diagrams and tables often overlap in function, as will become apparent in the following discussion. Often they are simply alternative ways of doing much the same thing. Importantly, graphs and tables are not simply ways of smartening up a report or dissertation. Their function in statistical analysis is much deeper than this and they are at the heart of the analytic work of the researcher. Graphs and tables should be the mainstay of a good statistical analysis, not the end product. Their role is crucial from the start of the analysis as part of the familiarisation process with one’s data which leads to understanding of what is going on in the data. So looking at charts which first of all give the distributions of each of the variables in your study is the initial stage. This can lead you to identify problems such as very skewed distributions for a variable or bunching and clustering around particular data points. Then you can move onto the graphs and tables which allow you to understand the relationships between two variables. This may well be your first indication that your expectations are being confirmed by your data. But it may show that the relationships that you are expecting are more complex than you imagined or that there is a possibility that there are outliers which spuriously appear to create a relationship between your variables but there is no relationship for the bulk of the data. One has to enter this phase with an open mind since it involves getting to understand your data and becoming familiar with its characteristics. This is why you do research. These procedures may seem very basic compared with the riches of more advanced statistics but they are basic because they are the base from which your analysis is built. Computers allow you to produce charts and tables very quickly, which makes it easy to look at the detail of your data. A good statistician may get as much from this aspect of their analysis than from the more fancy statistical techniques to be found later in this book. Figure 7.1 gives the key steps to consider when describing relationships between two variables in diagram and table form.

M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 87

11/13/13 5:13 PM

88

CHAPTER 7  Relationships between two or more variables: Diagrams and tables

Figure 7.1

7.2

Conceptual steps for showing relationships between two variables

The principles of diagrammatic and tabular presentation Choosing appropriate techniques to show relationships between two variables requires an understanding of the difference between nominal category data and numerical score data. If we are considering the interrelationships between two variables (X and Y) then the types of variable involved are as shown in Table 7.1. Once you have decided to which category your pair of variables belongs, it is easy to suggest appropriate descriptive statistics. We have classified different situations as type A, type B and type C. Thus type B has both variables measured on the nominal category scale of measurement.



Table 7.1

Types of relationships based on nominal categories and numerical scores Variable X = numerical scores

M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 88

Variable X = nominal categories

Variable Y = numerical scores

type A

type C

Variable Y = nominal categories

type C

type B

11/13/13 5:13 PM

7.3  Type A: both variables numerical scores 89

7.3

Type A: both variables numerical scores Where both variables take the form of numerical scores, generally the best form of graphical presentation is the scattergram or scatterplot. This is a sort of graph in which the values on one variable are plotted against the values on the other variable. The most familiar form of graph is one that plots a variable against time. These are very familiar from newspapers, especially the financial sections (see Figure 7.2). Time is no different, statistically speaking, from a wide range of other numerical scores. Figure 7.3 is an example of a scattergram from a psychological study. You will see that the essential features remain the same. In Figure 7.3, the point marked with an

M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 89

Figure 7.2

The dramatic fall in share price in the Timeshare Office Company

Figure 7.3

A scattergram showing the relationship between two variables

11/13/13 5:13 PM

90

CHAPTER 7  Relationships between two or more variables: Diagrams and tables

arrow represents a case (person) whose score on the X-variable is 8 and whose score on the Y-variable is 120. It is sometimes possible to see that the points of a scattergram fall more or less on a straight line. This line through the points of a scattergram is called the regression line. It is the best fitting straight line to the data points. Figure 7.3 includes the regression line for the points of the scattergram. One complication you sometimes come across is where several points on the scattergram overlap completely. In these circumstances you may well see a number next to a point which corresponds to the number of overlapping points at that position on the scattergram. In line with general mathematical notation, the horizontal axis or horizontal dimension is described as the X-axis and the vertical axis or vertical dimension is called the Y-axis. It is helpful if you remember to label one set of scores the X scores since these belong on the horizontal axis, and the other set of scores the Y scores because these belong on the vertical axis (Figure 7.4). In Figure 7.4, overlapping points are marked not with a number but with lines around the point on the scattergram. These are called ‘sunflowers’ – the number of ‘petals’ is the number of cases overlapping at the same point. So if there are two ‘petals’ then there are two people with the same pattern of scores on the two variables. If there are three ‘petals’ then three people have exactly the same pattern of scores on the two variables. Another way of indicating overlaps is simply to put the number of overlaps next to the scattergram point. Apart from cumbersomely listing all of your pairs of scores, it is often difficult to think of a succinct way of presenting data from pairs of numerical scores in tabular form. The main possibility is to categorise each of your score variables into ‘bands’ of scores and express the data in terms of frequencies of occurrence in these bands; a table like Table 7.2 might be appropriate. Just to remind you, on SPSS and other statistics programs it is possible to recode ranges of scores into bands.

Figure 7.4

M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 90

A scattergram with the X- and Y-axes labelled and overlapping points illustrated

11/13/13 5:13 PM

7.4  Type B: both variables nominal categories 91



Use of bands of scores to tabulate the relationship between two numerical score variables

Table 7.2 Variable X

Variable Y 1–5

6–10

11–15

16–20

21–25

0–9

15

7

6

3

4

10–19

7

12

3

5

4

20–29

4

9

19

8

4

30–39

1

3

2

22

3

40–49

3

2

3

19

25

Such tables are known as ‘crosstabulation’ or ‘contingency’ tables. In Table 7.2 there does seem to be a relationship between variable X and variable Y. People with low scores on variable X also tend to get low scores on variable Y. High scorers on variable X also tend to score highly on variable Y. However, the trend in the table is less easily discerned than in the equivalent scattergram.

7.4

Type B: both variables nominal categories Where both variables are in nominal categories, it is necessary to report the frequencies in all of the possible groupings of the variables. If you have more than a few nominal categories, the tables or diagrams can be too big and cumbersome. Take the imaginary data shown in Table 7.3 on the relationship between a person’s gender and whether they have been hospitalised at any time in their life for a psychiatric reason. These data are ideal for certain sorts of tables and diagrams because there are



M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 91

Gender and whether previously hospitalised for a set of 89 people

Table 7.3

Person

Gender

Previously hospitalised

1

male

yes

2

male

no

3

male

no

4

male

yes

5

male

no

.  .  .

.  .  .

.  .  .

85

female

yes

86

female

yes

87

female

no

88

female

no

89

female

yes

11/13/13 5:13 PM

92

CHAPTER 7  Relationships between two or more variables: Diagrams and tables

Table 7.4





Crosstabulation table of gender against hospitalisation Male

Female

Previously hospitalised

f = 20

f = 25

Not previously hospitalised

f = 30

f = 14

Table 7.5

Crosstabulation table with all frequencies expressed as a percentage of the total  number of frequencies Male

Female

Previously hospitalised

22.5%

28.1%

Not previously hospitalised

33.7%

15.7%

Table 7.6

Crosstabulation table with hospitalisation expressed as a percentage of the male and  female frequencies taken separately Male

Female

Previously hospitalised

40.0%

64.1%

Not previously hospitalised

60.0%

35.9%

few categories of each variable. Thus a suitable table for summarising these data might look like Table 7.4 – it is called a contingency or crosstabulation table. The numbers (frequencies) in each category are instantly obvious from this table. You might prefer to express the table in percentages rather than frequencies, but some thought needs to go into the choice of percentages. For example, you could express the frequencies as percentages of the total of males and females (Table 7.5). You probably think that Table 7.5 is not much of an improvement in clarity. An alternative is to express the frequencies as percentages of males and percentages of females (Table 7.6). By presenting the percentages based on males and females separ­ ately, it is easier to see the trend for females to have had a previous psychiatric history relatively more frequently than males. The same data can be expressed as a compound bar chart. In a compound bar chart information is given about the subcategories based on a pair of variables. Figure 7.5 shows one example in which the proportions are expressed as percentages of the males and females separately.

M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 92

11/13/13 5:13 PM

7.5  Type C: one variable nominal categories, the other numerical scores 93

Figure 7.5

Compound percentage bar chart showing gender trends in previous hospitalisation

Figure 7.6

How not to do a compound bar chart

The golden rule for such data is to ensure that the number of categories is man­ ageable. In particular, avoid having too many empty or near-empty categories. The compound bar chart shown in Figure 7.6 is a particularly bad example and is not to be copied. This chart fails any reasonable clarity test and is too complex to decipher quickly. Your chart should be a model of clarity if you are to impress others with your thoughtful approach to statistical analysis.





7.5

Type C: one variable nominal categories, the other numerical scores This final type of situation offers a wide variety of ways of presenting the relationships between variables. We have examined the compound bar chart so it is not surprising to find that there is also a compound histogram. To be effective, a compound histogram needs to consist of:

M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 93



a small number of categories for the nominal category variable



a few ranges for the numerical scores.

11/13/13 5:13 PM

94

CHAPTER 7  Relationships between two or more variables: Diagrams and tables

Figure 7.7

A compound histogram

So, for example, if we wish to plot the relationship between managers’ anxiety scores and whether they are managers in a high-tech or a low-tech industry, we might create a compound histogram like Figure 7.7 in which there are only two values of the nominal variable (high-tech and low-tech) and four bands of anxiety score (low anxiety, medium anxiety, high anxiety and very high anxiety). An alternative way of presenting such data is to use a crosstabulation table as in Table 7.7. Instead, however, it is almost as easy to draw up a table (Table 7.8) which gives the mean, median, mode, etc. for the anxiety scores of the two different groups.



Table 7.7

Crosstabulation table of anxiety against type of industry Frequency of anxiety score



M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 94

0–3

4–7

8–11

12–15

Low-tech industry

7

18

3

1

High-tech industry

17

7

0

0

Table 7.8

Comparison of the statistical characteristics of anxiety in two different types  of industry Mean

Median

Mode

Interquartile range

Variance

High-tech industry

3.5

3.9

3

2.3–4.2

2.2

Low-tech industry

5.3

4.7

6

3.9–6.3

3.2

11/13/13 5:13 PM

Key points 95



Research examples Crosstabulation and charts Arden and Plomin (2006) drew a compound histogram to show how the standard deviation of intelligence scores differed between boys and girls at the ages of 2, 3, 4, 7, 9 and 10. Deary and his colleagues (1991) looked at the relation between intelligence and deciding which of two vertical lines was the longer. They used three groups of different people. They presented the relation between intellig­ence and the time to do the task as a correlation and as a scattergram for the three groups combined. The correlation was negative with lower intelligence scores associated with longer inspection times. Jenkins, Conley, Rienecke Hoste, Meyer and Blissett (2012) used three bar charts to show the differences in means in eating disorder pathology, general pathology and quality of life between five groups. These five groups differed in whether they over-ate and had lost control of their over-eating. Meeten and Davey (2012) manipulated five moods by showing participants one of five films. The five moods were sad, happy, anxious, angry and neutral. Participants rated how they felt in these five conditions in terms of four scales of sadness, happiness, anxiety and anger. The mean scores and their standard deviations were presented in a crosstabulation with the five conditions represented by five columns and the four moods by four rows. They used a compound histogram to show the mean number of instances of exaggerating negative consequences using one of two rules in the five mood conditions. Sierra, Livianos and Rojo (2005) employed a bar chart to show the differences in means on eight subscale scores of a measure of quality of life between patients with bipolar depression and a sample from the general population. Wickett and his colleagues (1994) wanted to know whether there was a correlation between intelligence and brain size as measured by magnetic resonance imaging. They found a positive correlation of .395 which they showed as a scatterplot. Greater brain size was positively associated with higher intelligence scores.

Key points ●

Never assume that your tables and diagrams are good enough at the first attempt. They could probably be improved with a little care and adjustment.



Do not forget that tables and diagrams are there to present clearly the major trends in your data (or lack of them). There is not much point in having tables and diagrams that do not clarify your data.



Your tables and diagrams are not means of tabulating your unprocessed data. If you need to present your data in full then most of the methods to be found in this chapter will not help you much.



Labelling tables and diagrams clearly and succinctly is an important part of the task – without clear titling and labelling you are probably wasting your time.

M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 95

11/13/13 5:13 PM

CHAPTER 7  Relationships between two or more variables: Diagrams and tables

96

Computer analysis Crosstabulation and compound bar charts using SPSS

Figure 7.8

SPSS Statistics steps for contingency (crosstabulation) tables and compound charts

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

You should have a good idea of what you want your tables and charts or diagrams to tell you. If you find the chart difficult to understand then you cannot expect anyone reading your report to understand it any better. You might wish to start again. Basically in order to interpret the chart or table you are looking for evidence for a relationship between the two variables.



Always think carefully about whether to present tables and diagrams in reports. They may be very important to the researcher when they are analysing their data but less important in the light of this analysis in terms of their inclusion in the report. If you do choose to include a table or diagram, always refer to it in the main text of your report – never leave it to the reader to interpret what it indicates. As always, make sure that the labelling, etc. of the chart is as good as you can make it.

M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 96

11/13/13 5:13 PM

Computer analysis 97





Screenshot 7.2



Select Custom Tables for a contingency table

Screenshot 7.1

Enter the data



Move the variables for a contingency table

Screenshot 7.4

The contingency table output

Define Clustered Bar Chart dialog box

Screenshot 7.6

The clustered bar chart

Screenshot 7.3

Screenshot 7.5

M07_HOWI0749_06_SE_C07.indd 97

11/13/13 5:13 PM

Chapter 8

Correlation coefficients Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho

Overview ●















Correlation coefficients are numerical indexes of the relationship between two variables. They are the bedrock of much statistical analysis. The correlation coefficient may be positive or negative depending on whether both sets of scores increase together (positive correlation) or whether one set increases as the other decreases (negative correlation). The numerical size of the correlation coefficient ranges from 0 (no relationship) to 1 (a perfect relationship). Intermediary values indicate different amounts of spread around the bestfitting straight line through the points (i.e. the spread around the regression line). If the points on the scattergram do not cluster closely to the regression line then the correlation is poor. The Pearson correlation is primarily used for score variables (though it can be used where one or both variables are nominal variables with just two categories). Spearman’s correlation is used when the scores are ranked from smallest to largest. Apart from this, conceptually it is the same as Pearson’s correlation coefficient. Great care should be taken to inspect the scattergram between the two variables in question in order to make sure that the best-fitting line is a straight line rather than a curve. Small numbers of very extreme scores can substantially mask the true trend in the data – these are called outliers. The chapter explains what to do about them. The statistical significance of correlation coefficients is dealt with in Chapter 11.

Preparation Revise variance (Chapter 4) and the use of the scattergram to show the relationship between two variables (Chapter 7).

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 98

11/18/13 2:08 PM

8.1  Introduction 99

8.1 Introduction Although the scattergram is an important statistical tool for showing relationships between two variables, it is space consuming. For many purposes, it is more convenient to have the main features of the scattergram expressed as a single numerical index – the correlation coefficient. This is merely a number index which summarises some, but not all, of the key features of a scattergram. The commonest correlation coefficient is the Pearson correlation, also known more grandly and obscurely as the Pearson product– moment correlation coefficient. It includes two major pieces of information: ●

The closeness of the fit of the points of a scattergram to the best-fitting straight line through those points.



Information about whether the slope of the scattergram is positive or negative.

It therefore omits other information such as the scales of measurement of the two variables and specific information about individuals. The correlation coefficient thus neatly summarises a great deal of information about a scattergram. It is especially useful when you have several variables which would

Figure 8.1

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 99

Conceptual steps for understanding the correlation coefficient

11/18/13 2:08 PM

100

CHAPTER 8  Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho

involve drawing numerous scattergrams, one for each pair of variables. It most certainly does not replace the scattergram entirely but merely helps you to present your findings rather more concisely than other methods. Indeed, we recommend that you draw a scattergram for every correlation coefficient you calculate even if that scattergram is not intended for inclusion in your report. Although the correlation coefficient is a basic descriptive statistic, it is elaborated in a number of sophisticated forms such as partial correlation, multiple correlation and factor analysis, which form the more advanced statistics to be found later in this book. Correlation is of paramount importance in many forms of research, especially survey, questionnaire and similar kinds of research. Figure 8.1 gives the key steps to consider when using the correlation coefficient.

8.2

Principles of the correlation coefficient The correlation coefficient basically takes the following form: r[correlation coefficient]  = +1.00 or 0.00 or −1.00 or 0.30 or −0.72, etc. So a correlation coefficient consists of two parts: ●

A positive or negative sign (although for positive values the sign is frequently omitted).



Any numerical value in the range of 0.00 to 1.00.

The + or − sign tells us something important about the slope of the correlation line (i.e. the best-fitting straight line through the points on the scattergram). A positive value means that the slope is from the bottom left to the top right of the scattergram (Figure 8.2).

Figure 8.2

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 100

Positive correlation between two variables

11/18/13 2:08 PM

8.2  Principles of the correlation coefficient 101

Figure 8.3

Negative correlation between two variables

On the other hand, if the sign is negative (−) then the slope of the straight line goes from upper left to lower right on the scattergram (Figure 8.3). The numerical value of the correlation coefficient (0.50, 0.42, etc.) is an index of how close the points on the scattergram fit the best-fitting straight line. A value of 1.00 means that the points of the scattergram all lie exactly on the best-fitting straight line (Figure 8.4), unless that line is perfectly vertical or perfectly horizontal, in which case it means that there is no variation in the scores on one of the variables and so no correlation can be calculated. A value of 0.00 means that the points of the scattergram are randomly scattered around the straight line. It is purely a matter of luck if any of them actually touch the straight line (Figure 8.5). In this case, the best-fitting straight line for the scattergram could be virtually any line you arbitrarily decide to draw through the points. Con­ ventionally it is drawn as a horizontal line, but any other angle of slope would do just as well since there is no discernible trend in the relationship between the two variables on the scattergram.

Figure 8.4

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 101

Perfect correlation between two variables

11/18/13 2:08 PM

102

CHAPTER 8  Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho

Near-zero correlation between two variables

Figure 8.5

A value of 0.50 would mean that although the points on the scattergram are generally close to the best-fitting straight line, there is considerable spread of these points around that straight line. The correlation coefficient is merely an index of the amount of variance of the scattergram points from the straight line. However, it is calculated in such a way that the maximum variance around that straight line results in a correlation of zero. In other words, the closer the relationship between the two variables, the higher is the correlation coefficient, up to a maximum value of 1.00. To summarise, the components of the correlation coefficient are the sign (+ or −), which indicates the direction of the slope, and a numerical value which indicates how much variation there is around the best-fitting straight line through the points (i.e. the higher the numerical value the closer the fit).



■ Covariance The actual computation of the correlation coefficient involves little more than an elabora­ tion of the formula for variance: variance =

∑ (X − B)2 N

where X = scores on variable X B = mean score on variable X N = number of scores ∑ = sum of what follows If you wished (you will see why in a moment), the formula for variance could be re-expressed as: variance =

∑ (X − B)(X − B) N

All we have done is to expand the formula so as not to use the square sign. (A square is simply a number multiplied by itself.)

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 102

11/18/13 2:08 PM

8.2  Principles of the correlation coefficient 103

In the formula for the correlation coefficient we use something called the covariance. This is almost exactly the same as the formula for variance, but instead of multiplying scores by themselves we multiply the score on one variable (X) by the score on the second variable (Y) having subtracted the relevant mean: covariance[of variable X with variable Y] =

∑ (X − B)(Y − C ) N

where X = scores on variable X B = mean score on variable X Y = scores on variable Y C = mean score on variable Y N = number of pairs of scores ∑ = sum of what follows We get a large positive value of covariance if there is a strong positive relationship between the two variables, and a big negative value if there is a strong negative relationship between the two variables. If there is no relationship between the variables then the covariance is zero. Notice that, unlike variance, the covariance can take positive or negative values. However, the size of the covariance is affected by the size of the variances of the two separate variables involved. The larger the variances, the larger is the covariance, potentially. Obviously this would make comparisons difficult. So the covariance is adjusted by dividing by the square root of the product of the variances of the two separate variables. (Because N, the number of pairs of scores, in the variance and covariance formulae can be cancelled out in the correlation formula, the usual formula includes no division by the number of scores.) Once this adjustment is made to the covariance formula, we have the formula for the correlation coefficient: r[correlation coef1 cient] =

∑ (X − B)(Y − C ) ∑ (X − B)2 ∑ (Y − C )2

The lower part of the formula gives the largest possible value of the covariance of the two variables – that is, the theoretical covariance if the two variables lay perfectly on the straight line through the scattergram. Dividing the covariance by the maximum value it could take (if there were no spread of points away from the straight line through the scattergram) ensures that the correlation coefficient can never be greater than 1.00. The covariance formula also contains the necessary sign to indicate the slope of the relationship. A slightly quicker computational formula which does not involve the calculation of the mean scores directly is as follows, though we will not illustrate it here as we assume that you will prefer to do your calculations on a computer:

r[correlation coefficient] =

X Y ∑ XY − ∑ N∑   ∑ X2 −  

(∑ X ) N

2

  ∑Y2 −  

(∑ Y ) N

2

   

The resemblance of parts of this formula to the computational formula for variance should be fairly obvious. This is not surprising as the correlation coefficient is a measure of the lack of variation around a straight line through the scattergram.

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 103

11/18/13 2:08 PM

104

CHAPTER 8  Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho

Explaining statistics 8.1

How the Pearson correlation works Our data for this calculation come from scores on the relationship between mathematical ability and musical ability for a group of 10 children (Table 8.1). It is always sound practice to draw the scattergram for any correlation coefficient you are calculating. For these data, the scattergram will be like Figure 8.6. Notice that the slope of the scattergram is negative, as one could have deduced from the tendency for those who score highly on mathematical ability to have low scores on musical ability. You can also see not only that a straight line is a pretty good way of describing the trends in the points on the scattergram but that the points fit the straight line reasonably well. Thus we should expect a fairly high negative correlation from the correlation coefficient.

Table 8.1

Scores on musical and mathematical ability for 10 children

Individual

Music score

Mathematics score

Jessica

2

8

Joshua

6

3 9

Tyler

4

Daniel

5

7

Emily

7

2

Brittany

7

3

Samantha

2

9

Alexis

3

8

Ryan

5

6

Nicola

4

7

Figure 8.6

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 104

Scattergram for Table 8.1

11/18/13 2:08 PM

8.2 PrInCIPleS Of the COrrelatIOn COeffICIent

105

Step 1

Set the scores out in a table (Table 8.2) and follow the calculations as shown. Here N is the number of pairs of scores, i.e. 10.

Step 2

Substitute the appropriate values from Table 8.2 in the formula:

r[correlation coefef1 ef1 cient] = =

∑ (X − B)(Y − C ) ∑ (X − B)2 ∑ (Y − C )2 −39 30.5 × 61.6

−39 = 43.35 = − 0 .9 0

Interpreting the results So the value obtained for the correlation coefficient equals −0.90. This value is in line with what we suggested about the scattergram which serves as a rough check on our calculation. There is a very substantial negative relationship between mathematical and musical ability. In other words, the good mathematicians tended to be the poor musicians and vice versa. It is not claimed that they are good at music because they are poor at mathematics but merely that there is an inverse association between the two.

reporting the results When reporting a correlation coefficient it is usual to report its statistical significance. The meaning of statistical significance is explained in Chapters 10 and 11. However, the important point for now is to remember that statistical significance is invariably reported with the value of the correlation coefficient.

Table 8.2

essential steps in the calculation of the correlation coefficient

X score (music)

Y score (maths)

X−T

(X − T)2

Y−U

(Y − U)2

(X − T) (Y − U)

2

8

2 − 4.5 = −2.5

6.25

8 − 6.2 = 1.8

3.24

−2.5 × 1.8 = −4.5

6

3

6 − 4.5 = 1.5

2.25

3 − 6.2 = −3.2

10.24

1.5 × −3.2 = −4.8

4

9

4 − 4.5 = −0.5

0.25

9 − 6.2 = 2.8

7.84

−0.5 × 2.8 = −1.4

5

7

5 − 4.5 = 0.5

0.25

7 − 6.2 = 0.8

0.64

7

2

7 − 4.5 =

2.5

6.25

2 − 6.2 = − 4.2

17.64

2.5 × −4.2 = −10.5

7

3

7 − 4.5 =

2.5

6.25

3 − 6.2 = −3.2

10.24

2.5 × −3.2 = −8.0

2

9

2 − 4.5 = −2.5

6.25

9 − 6.2 = 2.8

7.84

−2.5 × 2.8 = −7.0

3

8

3 − 4.5 = −1.5

2.25

8 − 6.2 = 1.8

3.24

−1.5 × 1.8 = −2.7

5

6

5 − 4.5 = 0.5

0.25

6 − 6.2 = −0.2

0.04

0.5 × −0.2 = −0.1

4 − 4.5 = −0.5

0.25

7 − 6.2 = 0.8

0.64

4

7

∑ X = 45

∑Y = 62

Mean = T = 4.5

mean = U = 6.2

∑(X − T ) = 31.50 2

0.5 × 0.8 =

0.4

−0.5 × 0.8 = −0.4

∑(Y − U ) = −61.6 2

∑(X − T ) (Y − U ) = −39



M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 105

11/18/13 2:08 PM

CHAPTER 8  Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho

106

We would write something like: ‘It was found that musical ability was inversely related to mathematical ability. The Pearson correlation coefficient was −0.90 which is statistically significant at the 5% level with a sample size of 10.’ The information in the final sentence will not be informative to you until you have studied Chapters 10 and 11. If we were to heed the advice of the 2010 Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (APA) we could write: ‘Musical ability was significantly inversely related to mathematical ability, r(8) = −.90, p < .05’. The number in brackets after r is the sample size minus 2. This number is called the degrees of freedom and is explained in Section 21.4. Statistical significance is usually reported as a proportion rather than a percentage. Computer packages like SPSS Statistics give the exact significance level. We should report this as a figure as it is more informative.

Box 8.1

Key concepts

Covariance Many of the basic concepts taught in introductory stat­ istics are relevant even at the advanced level. The concept of covariance is one of these. As we have seen, covariance is basically the average of the deviation from the mean for the variable X multiplied by the deviation of the variable Y. In other words, it is the top part of the Pearson correlation formula. The correlation coefficient is simply the ratio of the covariance over the largest value that the covariance could take for a particular pair of variables. In other words, it is a standardised measure of covariance. But the term covariance crops up throughout this book in a number of different contexts. It is involved in ANOVA (especially the analysis of covariance) and regression, for example – lots of places, some of them unexpected.

Variance–covariance matrix for three variables

Table 8.3

Variable X

8.3

One phrase that might cause some consternation is that of the ‘variance–covariance’ matrix for a number of variables. This is simply a table (matrix) which includes the variances of each variable in the diagonal and their covariances off the diagonal. This is illustrated for vari­ ables X, Y and Z in Table 8.3. The diagonal contains the variances but the other numbers are the covari­ances – each of these is presented twice because the covariance of X with Z is the same as the covariance of Z with Z. Similar matrices are produced for correlation coefficients. However, in this case the diagonal consist of 1.00s (the correlation of a variable with itself is always 1) and the off-diagonals have the correlation coefficients of each variable with the other different variables.

Variable Y

Variable Z 1.244

Variable X

2.400

1.533

Variable Y

1.533

4.933

3.733

Variable Z

1.244

3.733

5.156

Some rules to check out ●

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 106

You should make sure that a straight line is the best fit to the scattergram points. If the best-fitting line is a curve such as in Figure 8.7 then you should not use the Pearson correlation coefficient. The reason for this is that the Pearson correlation

11/18/13 2:08 PM

8.3  Some rules to check out 107

assumes a straight line which is a gross distortion if you have a curved (curvilinear) relationship. ● Make

sure that your scattergram does not contain a few extreme cases which are unduly influencing the correlation coefficient (Figure 8.8). In this diagram you can see that the points at the top left of the scattergram are responsible for the apparent negative correlation between the two variables. Your eyes probably suggest that for virtually all the points on the scattergram there is no relationship at all. You could in these circumstances eliminate the ‘outliers’ (i.e. extreme, highly influential points) and recalculate the correlation coefficient based on the remaining, more typical group of scores. If the correlation remains significant with the same sign as before then your interpretation of your data is likely to remain broadly unchanged. However, there needs to be good reason for deleting the ‘outliers’; this should not be done simply because the data as they stand do not support your ideas. It may be that something unusual had happened – perhaps an outlier arose from the responses of a slightly deaf person who could not hear the researcher’s instructions, for example.

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 107

Figure 8.7

A curved relationship between two variables

Figure 8.8

Influence of outliers on a correlation

11/18/13 2:08 PM

108

CHAPTER 8  Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho

Box 8.2

Key concepts

Correlation and causality It is typically argued that a correlation does not prove causality. Just because two variables are related to each other is not sufficient reason to say anything other than that they are related. Statistical analysis is basically incapable of showing that one variable influenced the other variable directly one way or the other. Questions such as whether one variable affected the other are addressed primarily through the nature of your research design and not through the statistical analysis as such. Conventionally psychologists

have turned to laboratory experiments in which variables could be systematically manipulated by the researcher in order to be able to enhance their confidence in making causal interpretations about relationships between variables. Be careful when you read phrases such as ‘the effect of Variable A on Variable B’ in psychological writings. This can be misleading as it does not always mean causal effect. Instead, sometimes it merely means that there is a relationship, causal or otherwise.

8.4 Coefficient of determination The correlation coefficient is an index of how much variance two variables have in common. However, you need to square the correlation coefficient in order to know precisely how much variance is shared. The squared correlation coefficient is also known as the coefficient of determination. The proportion of variance shared by two variables whose correlation coefficient is 0.5 equals 0.52 or 0.25. This is a proportion out of 1 so as a percentage it is 0.25 × 100% = 25%. A correlation coefficient of 0.8 means that 0.82 × 100% or 64% of the variance is shared. A correlation coefficient of 1.00 means that 1.002 × 100% = 100% of the variance is shared. Since the coefficient of determination is based on squaring the correlation coefficient, it should be obvious that the amount of variance shared by the two variables declines increasingly rapidly as the correlation coefficient gets smaller (Table 8.4).

Table 8.4

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 108

Variance shared by two variables Correlation coefficient

Variance the two variables share

1.00

100%

0.90

81%

0.80

64%

0.70

49%

0.60

36%

0.50

25%

0.40

16%

0.30

9%

0.20

4%

0.10

1%

0.00

0%

11/18/13 2:08 PM

8.6  Spearman’s rho – another correlation coefficient 109

8.5

Significance testing Some readers who have previously studied statistics a little will be familiar with the notion of significance testing and might be wondering why this has not been dealt with for the correlation coefficient. The answer is that we will be dealing with it, but not until Chapter 11. In the present chapter we are presenting the correlation coefficient as a descriptive statistic which numerically summarises a scattergram between two variables. For those who wish to understand significance testing for the correlation coefficient, simply skip Chapter 9 on regression for now and proceed to Chapters 10 and 11.

8.6 Spearman’s rho – another correlation coefficient Spearman’s rho is often written as rs. We have not used this symbol in the following discussion although it is common in textbooks. The Pearson correlation coefficient is the dominant correlation index in psychological statistics. There is another called Spearman’s rho which is not very different – practically identical, in truth. Instead of taking the scores directly from your data, the scores on a variable are ranked from smallest to largest. That is, the smallest score on variable X is given rank 1, the second smallest score on variable X is given rank 2, and so forth. The smallest score on variable Y is given rank 1, the second smallest score on variable Y is given rank 2, etc. Then Spearman’s rho is calculated like the Pearson correlation coefficient between the two sets of ranks as if the ranks were scores. A special procedure is used to deal with tied ranks. Sometimes certain scores on a variable are identical. There might be two or three people who scored 7 on variable X, for example. This situation is described as tied scores or tied ranks. The question is what to do about them. The conventional answer in psychological statistics is to pretend first of all that the tied scores can be separated by fractional amounts. Then we allocate the appropriate ranks to these ‘separated’ scores but give each of the tied scores the average rank that they would have received if they could have been separated (Table 8.5). The two scores of 5 are each given the rank 2.5 because if they were slightly different they would have been given ranks 2 and 3, respectively. But they cannot be separated and so we average the ranks as follows: 2+3 = 2 .5 2 This average of the two ranks corresponds to what was entered into Table 8.5. There are three scores of 9 which would have been allocated the ranks 7, 8 and 9 if the scores had been slightly different from each other. These three ranks are averaged to give an average rank of 8 which is entered as the rank for each of the three tied scores in Table 8.5.

Ranking of a set of scores when tied (equal) scores are involved

Table 8.5

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 109

Scores

4

5

5

6

7

8

9

9

9

10

Ranks

1

2.5

2.5

4

5

6

8

8

8

10

11/18/13 2:08 PM

110

CHAPTER 8  Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho

There is a special computational formula (see Explaining statistics 8.3 later in this chapter) which can be used and is quicker than applying the conventional Pearson correlation formula to data in the form of ranks. It is nothing other than a special case of the Pearson correlation formula. Most statistics textbooks provide this formula for routine use. Unfortunately this formula is accurate only when you have absolutely no tied ranks at all – otherwise it gives a slightly wrong answer. As tied ranks are common in psychological research it is dubious whether there is anything to be gained in using the special Spearman’s rho computational formula as opposed to the Pearson correlation coefficient applied to the ranks. You may wonder why we have bothered to turn the scores into ranks before calculating the correlation coefficient. The reason is that ranks are commonly used in psychological statistics when the distributions of scores on a variable are markedly unsymmetrical and do not approximate (even poorly) a normal distribution. In the past it was quite fashionable to use rankings of scores instead of the scores themselves, but we would suggest that you avoid ranking if possible. Use ranks only when your data seem extremely distorted from a normal distribution. We realise that others may argue differently. The reasons for this are explained in Chapter 19.

Explaining statistics 8.2

How Spearman’s rho works We could apply the Spearman rho correlation to the data on the relationship between mathematical ability and musical ability for a group of 10 children which we used previously. But we must rank the two sets of scores before applying the normal Pearson correlation formula since there are tied ranks (see Table 8.6). In our calculation, N is the number of pairs of ranks, i.e. 10. For this calculation we have called the maths score the X score and the music score the Y score (the reverse of Explaining statistics 8.1). This makes no difference to the calculation of the correlation coefficient.

Table 8.6 Person

Steps in the calculation of Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient Maths score

Music score

Maths rank

Maths rank squared

Music rank

Music rank squared

Maths rank × music rank

X score

Y score

Xr

X r2

Yr

Y r2

Xr × Yr

1

8

2

7.5

56.25

1.5

2.25

11.25

2

3

6

2.5

6.25

8

64.00

20.00

3

9

4

9.5

90.25

4.5

20.25

42.75

4

7

5

5.5

30.25

6.5

42.25

35.75

5

2

7

1

1.00

9.5

90.25

9.50

6

3

7

2.5

6.25

9.5

90.25

23.75

7

9

2

9.5

90.25

1.5

2.25

14.25

8

8

3

7.5

56.25

3

9.00

22.50

9

6

5

4

16.00

6.5

42.25

26.00

10

7

4

5.5

30.25

4.5

20.25

24.75

∑ Xr = 55

∑ X = 383

∑Yr = 55

∑Y = 383

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 110

2 r

2 r

∑ XrYr = 230.50

11/18/13 2:08 PM

8.6 SPearman’S rhO – anOther COrrelatIOn COeffICIent

111

We then substitute the totals in the computational formula for the Pearson correlation coefficient, although now we call it Spearman’s rho:

r[correlation coefef1 ef1 cient] =

=

= = =

∑ X rYr −   ∑ X2 − r 

(∑ X ) r

N

2

∑ X rYr N

  ∑Y 2 − r 

(∑ Y ) r

N

2

  

 55 × 55  230.5 −    10   552   552   383 − 10   383 − 10     230.5 − 302.5 (383 − 302.5)(383 − 302.5) −72.00 (80.5)(8 80 0.5) −72.00 8 0 .5

= − 0 .8 89

Interpreting the results So, Spearman’s rho gives a substantial negative correlation just as we would expect from these data. You can interpret the Spearman correlation coefficient more or less in the same way as the Pearson correlation coefficient so long as you remember that it is calculated using ranks. It so happens in this case that the Spearman coefficient gives virtually the same numerical value as Pearson’s applied to the same data. This is fortuitous. Usually there is a discrepancy between the two.

reporting the results Just as with the Pearson correlation (Explaining statistics 8.1), when reporting the Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient it is normal to report the statistical significance of the coefficient. The meaning of statistical significance is explained in Chapters 10 and 11 and especially Explaining statistics 11.2. Proceed to these chapters for a discussion of statistical significance. The most important thing for the time being is to remember that statistical significance is almost invariably reported with the value of the correlation coefficient. We would write up the results something like: ‘It was found that musical ability was inversely related to mathematical ability. The value of Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient was −0.89 which is statistically significant at the 5% level with a sample size of 10.’ The last sentence will not mean much until Chapters 10 and 11 have been studied. Alternatively, following the recommendations of the APA (2010) Publication Manual we could write it as ‘Musical ability was significantly inversely related to mathematical ability, rs(8) = −.89, p < .05’.

We referred earlier to a special computational formula which could be used to calculate Spearman’s rho when there are no ties. There seems little point in learning this formula, since a lack of tied ranks is not characteristic of psychological data. You may as well simply use the method of Explaining statistics 8.2 irrespective of whether there are ties or not. For those who want to save a little time when there are no tied ranks, the procedure of Explaining statistics 8.3 may be used.

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 111

11/18/13 2:08 PM

112

CHAPTer 8 COrrelatIOn COeffICIentS: PearSOn COrrelatIOn and SPearman’S rhO

Explaining statistics 8.3

how Spearman’s rho with no tied ranks works The formula used in this computation applies only when there are no tied scores. If there are any, the formula becomes increasingly inaccurate and the procedure of Explaining statistics 8.2 should be applied. However, some psychologists use the formula whether or not there are tied ranks, despite the inaccuracy problem. For illustrative purposes we will use the same data on maths ability and musical ability despite there being ties, as listed in Table 8.7. Once again, N = 10.

r[Spearman’s rho] = 1 −

6∑ D 2

N (N 2 − 1)

= 1−

6 × 305 10(10 2 − 1)

= 1−

1830 10 × (100 − 1)

= 1−

1830 10 × 99

= 1−

1830 990

= 1 − 1.848 848 = − 0.848 = − 0.85 85 to 2 decimal places

Table 8.7

Steps in the calculation of Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient using the speedy formula

Person

Maths score Xscore

Music score Yscore

Maths rank Xr

Music rank Yr

Maths rank – music rank D (difference)

1

8

2

7.5

1.5

2

3

6

2.5

8

−5.5

30.25

3

9

4

9.5

4.5

5

25.00

4

7

5

5.5

6.5

−10

5

2

7

1

9.5

−8.5

72.25

6

3

7

2.5

9.5

−7.0

49.00

6.0

Square of previous column D2 36.00

1.00

7

9

2

9.5

1.5

8.0

64.00

8

8

3

7.5

3

4.5

20.25

9

6

5

4

6.5

−2.5

6.25

10

7

4

5.5

4.5

1.0

1.00 ∑ D2 = 305

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 112

11/18/13 2:08 PM

8.7  An example from the literature 113

Interpreting the results It should be noted that this value of Spearman’s rho is a little different from its correct value (−0.89) as we calculated it in Explaining statistics 8.2. The reason for this difference is the inaccuracy of the speedy formula when there are tied scores. Although the difference is not major, you are strongly recommended not to incorporate this error. Otherwise the interpretation of the negative correlation is the same as we have previously discussed.

Reporting the results As with the Pearson correlation (Explaining statistics 8.1), when reporting the Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient we would report the statistical significance of the coefficient. The meaning of statistical significance is explained in Chapters 10 and 11 and especially Explaining statistics 11.2. Ignore Chapter 9 and proceed directly to these chapters for an explanation. However, the important point for now is to remember that statistical significance is invariably reported with the value of the correlation coefficient. We would write up the results something along the lines of the following: ‘It was found that musical ability was inversely related to mathematical ability. The value of Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient was −0.85 which is statist­ ically significant at the 5% level with a sample size of 10.’ The last sentence will not mean much until Chapters 10 and 11 have been studied.

8.7

An example from the literature Pearson correlation coefficients are extremely common in published research. They can be found in a variety of contexts so choosing a typical example is virtually meaningless. The correlation coefficient is sometimes used as an indicator of the validity of a psychological test. So it might be used to indicate the relationship between a test of intelligence and children’s performance in school. The test is a valid predictor of school performance if there is a substantial correlation between the test score and school performance. The correlation coefficient is also very useful as an indicator of the reliability of a psychological test. This might mean the extent to which people’s scores on the test are consistent over time. You can use the correlation coefficient to indicate whether those who perform well now on the test also performed well a year ago. For example, Gillis (1980) in the manual accompanying the Child Anxiety Scale indicates that he retested 127 US schoolchildren in the first to third grades immediately after the initial testing. The reliability coefficients (test–retest reliability) or the correlation coefficients between the two testings were: Grade 1 = 0.82 Grade 2 = 0.85 Grade 3 = 0.92 A sample of children retested after a week had a retest reliability coefficient of 0.81. It is clear from this that the reliability of the measure is good. This means that the children scoring the most highly one week also tend to get the highest scores the next week. It does not mean that the scores are identical from week to week – only that the relative scores are the same. Practically all reliability and validity coefficients used in psychological testing are variants on much the same theme and are rarely much more complex than the correlation coefficient itself.

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 113

11/18/13 2:08 PM

114

CHAPTER 8  Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho

Research examples Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho Blom, van Middendorp and Geenen (2012) propose that embitterment is the consequence of childhood attachment problems such as anxious attachment. Embitterment involves the overall feeling of being invalidated by others such as having persistent feelings that one has been let down, or one is a loser, or that one needs revenge but is helpless to do so. Attachment was measured using the Attachment Styles Questionnaire which measures 1) fearful attachment, 2) preoccupied attachment, 3) dismissive attach­ ment and 4) secure attachment. Embitterment was measured using the Bern Embitterment Inventory. Some of the subscales of the embitterment inventory had very skewed distributions which led the researchers to choose Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient to assess associations. Embitterment correlated .39 with fearful attachment and .44 with preoccupied attachment. These two scales are the ones measuring anxious attachment. Carlson, Vazire and Oltmanns (2011) investigated narcissistic personalities, asking such questions as whether such individuals understand the negative aspects of their personalities and reputations. Various measures of narcissism were used including clinical ones. Their meta-perceptions of others concerning themselves were also measured. The research suggested that narcissistic individuals did have a degree of self-insight into how others see them. However, using Pearson correlation coefficients, it was shown that individuals scoring higher on narcissism also saw themselves more positively on such traits as being funny (r = .25), extravert (r = .43) and intelligence (r = .31). Casarett and his colleagues (2010) were interested in doctors’ use of metaphors and analogies in their consultations with patients with advanced cancer. Using Spearman rho correlations, they found that there was a significant positive correlation between doctors’ use of metaphors and analogies and patients’ rating of how good the doctors’ communications were. The more doctors used analogies and metaphors, the more highly patients rated their communications to them. Teissedre and Chabrol (2004) examined depression in 299 French women using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scales. They completed the measure at two to three days following giving birth and four to six weeks after giving birth. They decided to use the Spearman Rho correlation coefficient because of the nonnormality of the distribution of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. The Spearman Rank Correlation or Rho was fairly high at 0.61 which was significant at the 0.0001 level. Kenyon and her colleagues (2012) tested whether people with bulimia nervosa or other unspecified eating disorder were less able to infer the feelings, beliefs and knowledge of other people than people who did not have psychological disorders. As part of the study they assessed how depressed, anxious and stressed the three groups were and two tests which measured how well they evaluated the feelings of others. They were interested in whether the scores on these two tests were related to their score on an Eating Disorder Examination as well as other clinical variables in the two eating disordered groups. As the clinical variables were not normally distributed and could not be transformed to be so, they carried out Spearman rho correlations. They reported that no significant correlations were found for three of the variables, including the Eating Disorder Examination score. They presented the correlations for these three variables. Lounsbury and his colleagues (2003) were interested in whether five personality factors and work drive would predict the grades students obtained on a course once intelligence had been taken into account. Initially they presented the correlations between these eight variables in a table. Which correlations were statistically significant was indicated by one asterisk for the .05 level and two asterisks for the .01 level. In the Results section, they reported the correlations and the significance level for the four variables that were significantly related to grades. So, for example, general intelligence had a .01 significant correlation with grades.

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 114

11/18/13 2:08 PM

Key points 115



Warren, Holland, Billings and Parker (2012) explored whether stress would moderate the positive relationship of talking about being too fat to body dissatisfaction and drive for thinness in 121 female students. To show the relationships between the main variables of their study, they initially presented the Pearson correlations between these four variables in a table together with age and body mass index. The .01 significance level of the correlations was indicated by two asterisks. They described the direction of the main significant correlations in the Results. So ‘fat talk and perceived stress were both significantly positively correlated with body dissatisfaction and drive for thinness’ (p. 360).

Key points Most of the major points have been covered already. But they bear repetition: ●

Check the scattergram for your correlation coefficient for signs of a nonlinear relationship – if you find one you should not be using the Pearson correlation coefficient. In these circumstances you should use coefficient eta (η) which is designed for curvilinear relationships. However, eta is a relatively obscure statistic. It is mentioned again in Chapter 35.



Check the scattergram for outliers which may spuriously be producing a correlation when overwhelmingly the scattergram says that there is a poor relationship.



Examine the scattergram to see whether there is a positive or negative slope to the scatter and form a general impression of whether the correlation is good (the points fit the straight line well) or poor (the points are very widely scattered around the straight line). Obviously you will become more skilled at this with experience, but it is useful as a rough computational check among other things.



Before concluding your analysis, look at Chapter 10 to decide whether or not to generalise from your set of data.

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 115

11/18/13 2:08 PM

CHAPTER 8  Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho

116

Computer analysis The correlation coefficient using SPSS

Figure 8.9

SPSS Statistics steps for correlation coefficient

Interpreting and reporting output ●

Interpretation of the output is complicated by the fact that SPSS intercorrelates each of the variables with itself and with the other variables. The correlation of a variable with itself is always 1. No significance level is given for this. The table also includes the correlation between the variables with the other variables twice. So you have the correlation of Variable X with Variable Y and the correlation of Variable Y with Variable X. These are, of course, the same. The output gives the correlation (−.900), the statistical significance (.000) and the sample size (10).



It would be good to report the significance level as being less than 0.001 and something known as the degrees of freedom which for the correlation coefficient is N − 2 or 8 in this case. Significance is discussed in Chapter 11 and degrees of freedom in Chapter 21.



In a report, we could write ‘There is a significant negative correlation between musical ability and mathematical ability, r(8) = .90, p ≤ 0.001. Children with more musical ability have lower mathematical ability.’

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 116

11/18/13 2:08 PM

Computer analysis 117



Screenshot 8.1

The data

Screenshot 8.2

Select the test

Screenshot 8.3

Select Pearson correlation

Screenshot 8.4

Selecting additional statistics



Output table giving means and standard deviations

Screenshot 8.6

Output table giving correlations



Screenshot 8.5

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 117

11/18/13 2:08 PM

CHAPTER 8  Correlation coefficients: Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho

118

Computer analysis The scattergram using SPSS

Figure 8.10

SPSS Statistics steps for scattergrams

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

Interpreting a scatterplot is important. In particular, the researcher should look to make sure that a straight line is the best description of the pattern of points. Also, the researcher should look for outliers which are data points which are radically out of line with most of the data. Both of these mean that the Pearson correlation coefficient should not be used.



By all means include the scattergram in your report especially if it reveals something of importance about your data. Make sure that it is properly labelled. Too many scattergrams can make your report too cumbersome so be selective in terms of the ones that you use. Comment on the linearity of the data points and the presence of outliers, if any.

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 118

11/18/13 2:08 PM

Computer analysis 119



Screenshot 8.7

Select the analysis

Screenshot 8.8

Define variable properties

Screenshot 8.9

Select and move the variables

Screenshot 8.10

Fit the straight line

Screenshot 8.11

Adjust marker size if needed

Screenshot 8.12

The scatterplot

M08_HOWI0749_06_SE_C08.indd 119

11/18/13 2:08 PM

Chapter 9

Regression Prediction with precision

Overview ●













Regression basically identifies the regression line (the best-fitting straight line) for a scatterplot between two variables. (By this token, the correlation coefficient can be seen as an index of the spread of the data points around this regression line.) It uses a variable X (which is the horizontal axis of the scatterplot) and a variable Y (which is the vertical axis of the scatterplot). Sometimes (somewhat misleadingly) the X variable is known as the independent variable and the Y variable is known as the dependent variable. Alternatively, the X variable may be called the predictor variable and the Y variable the criterion variable. To describe the regression line, one needs the slope of the line and the point at which it touches the vertical axis (the intercept). Using this information, it is possible to estimate the most likely score on the variable Y for any given score on variable X. Sometimes this is referred to as making predictions. Standard error is a term used to describe the variability of any statistical estimate including those of the regression calculation. So there is a standard error of the slope, a standard error of the intercept and so forth. Standard error is analogous to standard deviation and indicates the likely spread of any of the estimates. Regression is the foundation of many of the more advanced techniques described later in this book. So the better you understand the concept at this stage, the easier will be your later work.

Preparation You should have a working knowledge of the scattergram, of the relationship between two variables (Chapter 7) and understand the correlation coefficient (Chapter 8).

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 120

11/18/13 1:53 PM

9.1  Introduction 121

9.1 Introduction Regression, like the correlation coefficient, numerically describes important features of a scattergram relating two variables. However, it does it in a different way from the correlation coefficient. Among its important uses is that it allows the researcher to make predictions (for example, when choosing the best applicant for a job on the basis of an aptitude or ability test). Assume that research has shown that a simple test of manual dexterity is capable of distinguishing between the better and not-so-good assembly workers in a precision components factory. Manual dexterity is a predictor variable and job performance the criterion variable. So it should be possible to predict which applicants are likely to be the more productive employees from scores on this easily administered test of manual dexterity. Using the test might be a lot cheaper than employing people who do not make the grade in the factory. Imaginary data for such a study are shown in Table 9.1. The scattergram (Figure 9.1) shows imaginary data on the relationship between scores on the manual dexterity test and the number of units per hour the employee produces in the components factory. Notice that we have made scores on the manual dexterity test the horizontal dimension (X-axis) and the number of units produced per hour the vertical dimension (Y-axis). In regression in order to keep the number of formulae to the minimum, the horizontal dimension (X-axis) should always be used to represent the variable from which the prediction is being made, and the vertical dimension (Y-axis) should always represent what is being predicted. It requires a different formula to predict the X values from the Y values and this is not commonly available. Furthermore, statistical packages such as SPSS require that you enter the predictor and criterion variables in a standard way. It is clear from the scattergram that the number of units produced by workers is fairly closely related to scores on the manual dexterity test. If we draw a straight line as best we can through the points on the scattergram, this line could be used as a basis for making predictions about the most likely score on work productivity from the aptitude test score of manual dexterity. This line through the points on a scattergram



M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 121

Table 9.1

Manual dexterity and number of units produced per hour Manual dexterity score

Number of units produced per hour

56

17

19

6

78

23

92

22

16

9

23

10

29

13

60

20

50

16

35

19

11/18/13 1:53 PM

122

CHAPTER 9  Regression: Prediction with precision

Figure 9.1

Scattergram of the relationship between manual dexterity and productivity

Figure 9.2

Using a regression line to make approximate predictions

is called the regression line. In order to predict the likeliest number of units per hour corresponding to a score of 70 on the manual dexterity test, we simply draw a right angle from the score 70 on the horizontal axis (manual dexterity test score) to the regression line, and then a right angle from the vertical axis to meet this point. In this way we can find the productivity score which best corresponds to a particular manual dexterity score (Figure 9.2). Estimating from this scattergram and regression line, it appears that the best prediction from a manual dexterity score of 70 is a productivity rate of about 19 or 20. There is only one major problem with this procedure – the prediction depends on the particular line drawn through the points on the scattergram. You might draw a somewhat different line from the one we did. Subjective factors such as these are not desirable in statistical analyses and it would be better to have a method which was not affected in

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 122

11/18/13 1:53 PM

9.1  Introduction 123

this way. So mathematical ways of determining what the regression line should be have been developed. Fortunately, the computations involved are generally straightforward and SPSS and other computer programs do all of the hard work for you. Regression is a component of many of the more advanced statistical techniques which we describe later in this book and a good understanding of the basics will make your more advanced work easier. See Box 9.1 for a discussion of the General Linear Model which underlies a great deal of the statistical analyses used by psychologists. Figure 9.3 describes the key steps when using regression.

Box 9.1

Key concepts

The General Linear Model GLM, the General Linear Model, is the basis of many of the statistical techniques discussed in this book. It is quite simple – it simply refers to the assumption that the effects of variables on other variables are additive. In other words, an increase of 1 unit on variable A is associated with an increase of x on variable B. This is basically assumed to be the case irrespective of where the increase of 1 unit is on variable A (i.e. at the top, middle or bottom of the distribution, etc.). The basis of the General Linear Model is the formula that you can see in Explaining statistics 9.1 which is used to predict values on one variable from values on another. The formula only needs slight modification to give the relationship between one set of data Y and another set of data X:

Figure 9.3

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 123

Ydata set = aconstant + (bregression weight × Xscores) + eerror All that we have done is to add in e for error. That is, there is not a perfect relationship between the Y data and the X data. The imperfection is the result of error in the measurements. The General Linear Model is actually more general than this basic formula implies. The reason is that there may be several Y variables (as in multivariate ANOVA – Chapter 27), several X variables (as in multiple regression – Chapter 32), several intercept values for each X variable and several regression coefficients also for each X variable. But the basic regression equation is the simplest version of the General Linear Model.

Conceptual steps for understanding regression

11/18/13 1:53 PM

124

9.2

CHAPTER 9  Regression: Prediction with precision

Theoretical background and regression equations The line through a set of points on a scattergram is called the regression line. In order to establish an objective criterion, the regression line is chosen which gives the closest fit to the points on the scattergram. In other words, the procedure ensures that there is a minimum sum of distances of the regression line to the points in the scattergram. In theory, then, one could keep trying different possible regression lines until one is found which has the minimum deviation of the points from it. The sum of the deviations (∑d) of the scattergram points from the regression line should be minimal. Actually, the precise criterion is the sum of the squared deviations. This is known as the least squares solution. But it would be really tedious work drawing different regression lines then calculating the sum of the squared deviations for each of these in order to decide which regression line has the smallest sum of squared deviations. Fortunately things are not done like that and trial and error is not involved at all. The formulae for regression do all of that work for you as with SPSS and other computer programs. In order to specify the regression line for any scattergram, you quantify two things: 1. The point at which the regression line cuts the vertical axis at X = 0 – this is a number of units of measurement from the zero point of the vertical axis. It can take a positive or negative value, denoting whether the vertical axis is cut above or below its zero point. It is normally denoted in regression as point a or the intercept. 2. The slope of the regression line or, in other words, the gradient of the best-fitting line through the points on the scattergram. Just as with the correlation coefficient, this slope may be positive in the sense that it goes up from bottom left to top right or it can be negative in that it goes downwards from top left to bottom right. The slope is normally denoted by the letter b. The intercept and slope are both shown in Figure 9.4. To work out the slope, we have drawn a horizontal dashed line from X = 30 to X = 50 (length 20) and a vertical dashed line up to the regression line (length about 4 up the Y-axis). The slope b is the

Figure 9.4

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 124

Slope b and intercept a of a regression line

11/18/13 1:53 PM

9.2  Theoretical background and regression equations 125

increase (+) or decrease (−) of the units produced (in this case +4) divided by the increase in the manual dexterity score (in this case 20), i.e. +0.2. The slope is simply the number of units that the regression line moves up the vertical axis for each unit it moves along the horizontal axis. In other words, you mark a single step along the horizontal axis and work out how much increase this represents on the vertical axis. So, for example, if you read that the slope of a scattergram is 2.00, this means that for every increase of 1.00 on the horizontal axis (X-axis) there is an increase of 2.00 on the vertical axis (Y-axis). If there is a slope of −0.5 then this means that for every increase of 1 on the horizontal axis (X-axis) there is a decrease of 0.5 on the vertical axis (Y-axis). In our example, for every increase of 1 in the manual dexterity score, there is an increase of 0.2 (more accurately, 0.21) in the job performance measure (units produced per hour). We have estimated this value from the scattergram – it may not be exactly the answer that we would have obtained had we used mathematically more precise methods or a computer program. This increase defines the slope. (Note that you do not work with angles, merely distances on the vertical and horizontal axes.) Fortunately, the application of two relatively simple formulae (see Explaining statistics 9.1) provides all the information we need to calculate the slope and the intercept. A third formula is used to make our predictions from the horizontal axis to the vertical axis. The major differences between correlation and regression are as follows: ●

Regression retains the original units of measurement so direct comparisons between regression analyses based on different variables are difficult. Correlation coefficients can readily be compared as they are essentially on a standardised measurement scale and free of the original units of measurement.



The correlation coefficient does not specify the slope of a scattergram. Correlation indicates the amount of spread or variability of the points around the regression line in the scattergram.

In other words, correlation and regression have somewhat different functions despite their close similarities.

Box 9.2

Focus on

Regression lines One of the things which can cause difficulty when using regression is the question of what variable should go on the horizontal axis and what variable should go on the vertical axis. Get them the wrong way around and your calculation will be incorrect. There are, in reality, always two regression lines between two variables: that from which variable A is predicted from variable B, and that from which variable B is predicted from variable A. They almost always have different slopes. But you probably will never come across these two different formulae. The reason is that life is made simpler if we always have the

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 125

predictor on the horizontal axis and the criterion to be predicted on the vertical axis. You need to be careful what you are trying to predict and from what and make sure that you put your predictor on the horizontal axis. If you are using regression weights to calculate actual scores on the dependent variable then it is sensible to produce a scattergram for your data. From this you should be able to estimate what the correct answer should be. If this is very different from what your calculation says, then one possibility is that you have got the axes the wrong way round.

11/18/13 1:53 PM

126

CHAPTER 9 RegReSSIOn: PRedICtIOn wIth PReCISIOn

Explaining statistics 9.1

how regression works To facilitate comparison, we will take the data used in the computation of the correlation coefficient (Chapter 8). The data concern the relationship between mathematical and musical ability for a group of 10 individuals. The 10 scores need to be set out in a table like Table 9.2 and the various intermediate calculations carried out. However, it is important with regression to make the X scores the predictor variable; the Y scores are the criterion variable. N is the number of pairs of scores, i.e. 10. (Strictly speaking the Y 2 and ∑Y 2 calculations are not necessary for regression but are included here because they highlight the similarities between the correlation and regression calculations.) The slope b of the regression line is given by the following formula:

b=

 X Y ∑ XY −  ∑ N∑ 

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

2

N

Thus, substituting the values from the table in the above formula:

b[slope]

 62 × 45  240 −    10  = 2 (62) 446 − 10 2790 10 = 3844 446 − 10 240 −

=

240 − 279 446 − 384.4

=

−39 61.6

= − 0 .6 63

Table 9.2

Important steps in calculating the regression equation

Person

Maths score X score

Music score Y score

X2

Y2

XY

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

8 3 9 7 2 3 9 8 6 7 ∑ X = 62

2 6 4 5 7 7 2 3 5 4 ∑ Y = 45

64 9 81 49 4 9 81 64 36 49 ∑ X 2 = 446

4 36 16 25 49 49 4 9 25 16 ∑ Y 2 = 233

16 18 36 35 14 21 18 24 30 28

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 126

∑ XY = 240

11/18/13 1:53 PM

9.2 theORetICaL BaCkgROUnd and RegReSSIOn eqUatIOnS

127

This tells us that the slope of the regression line is negative – it moves downwards from top left to bottom right. Furthermore, for every unit one moves along the horizontal axis, the regression line moves 0.63 units down the vertical axis since in this case it is a negative slope. We can now substitute in the following formula to get the cut-off point or intercept a of the regression line on the vertical axis: a [intercept on vertical axis] =

∑ Y − b∑ X N

=

45 − ( − 0.6 63 × 62) 10

=

45 − ( − 39.06) 10

=

84.06 10

= 8 .4 41 This value for a is the point on the vertical axis (musical ability) cut by the regression line. If one wishes to predict the most likely score on the vertical axis from a particular score on the horizontal axis, one simply substitutes the appropriate values in the following formula: Y[predicted scone] = a [intercept] + (b[slope] × X[known score]) Thus if we wished to predict musical ability for a score of 8 on mathematical ability, given that we know the slope b is −0.63 and the intercept is 8.41, we simply substitute these values in the formula: Y[predicted scone] = a [intercept] + (b[slope] × X[known score]) = 8.41 + (−0.63 × 8) = 8.41 + (−5.04) = 3.37 This is the best prediction – it does not mean that people with a score of 8 on mathematical ability inevitably get a score of 3.37 on musical ability. It is just our most intelligent estimate.

Interpreting the results The proper interpretation of the regression equations depends on the scattergram between the two variables showing a more or less linear (i.e. straight line) trend. If it does not show this, then the interpretation of the regression calculations for the slope and intercept will be misleading since the method assumes a straight line. Curvilinear relationships (see Chapter 8) are difficult to handle mathematically. If the scattergram reveals a linear relationship, then the interpretation of the regression equations is simple as the formulae merely describe the scattergram mathematically.

Reporting the results This regression analysis could be reported as follows: ‘Because of the negative correlation between mathematical and musical abilities, it was possible to carry out a regression analysis to predict musical ability from mathematical ability. The slope of the regression of mathematical ability on musical ability b is −0.63 and the intercept a is 8.41.’

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 127

11/18/13 1:53 PM

CHAPTER 9  Regression: Prediction with precision

128

Box 9.3

Focus on

Problems interpreting regression The use of regression in prediction is a fraught issue not because of the statistical methods but because of the characteristics of the data used. In particular, note that our predictions about job performance are based on data from the people already in the job. So, for example, those with the best manual dexterity might have developed these skills on the job rather than having them when they were





9.3

interviewed. Thus it may not be that manual dexterity determines job performance but that they are both influenced by other (unknown) factors. Similarly, if we found that age was a negative predictor of how quickly people get promoted in a banking corporation, this may simply reflect a bias against older people in the profession rather than greater ability of younger people.

Standard error: how accurate are the predicted score and the regression equations? You may prefer to leave studying the following material until you have had the opportunity to study Chapter 12. The accuracy of the predicted score on the criterion is dependent on the closeness of the scattergram points to the regression line; if there is a strong correlation between the variables there is little error in the prediction. Examining the scattergram between two variables will give you an idea of the variability around the regression line and hence the precision of the estimated or predicted scores. Statisticians prefer to calculate what they call the standard error to indicate how certain one can be about aspects of regression such as the prediction of the intercept or cut-off points, and the slope. A standard error is much the same as the standard deviation except it applies to the means of samples rather than individual scores. So the standard error of something is the average deviation of sample means from the mean of the sample means. Don’t worry too much if you don’t quite understand the concept

Box 9.4

Key concepts

Standard error Standard error is discussed again in later chapters. Superficially, it may appear to be quite different from the ideas in this chapter. However, remember that whenever we use any characteristic of a sample as the basis for estimating the characteristic of a population, we are likely to be wrong to some extent. The standard error is merely

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 128

the average amount by which the characteristics of samples from the population differ from the characteristic of the whole population. In other words, the standard error is very much like the standard deviation but applied to sample means and not to scores.

11/18/13 1:53 PM

9.3  Standard error: how accurate are the predicted score and the regression equations? 129

yet, since we come back to it in (Chapters 11 and 12). Just regard standard error of an estimate as the average amount by which an estimate is likely to be wrong. As you might expect, since this is statistics, the average is calculated in an unexpected way, as it was for the standard deviation, which is little different. Although the formulae for calculating the standard errors of the various aspects of the regression line are readily available, they add considerably to the computational labour involved in regression, so we recommend that you use a computer to relieve you of this computational chore. The main standard errors involved in regression are: ●

the one for your predicted (or estimated) value on the criterion (this is known as the standard error of the estimate of y)



the one for the slope of the regression line b



the one for the intercept on the vertical axis a.

Don’t forget that the formulae for calculating these standard errors merely give you the average amount by which your estimate is wrong. It might be more useful to estimate the likely range within which the true value of the prediction, slope or intercept is likely to fall. In other words, to be able to say that, for example, the predicted score on the criterion variable is likely to be between 2.7 and 3.3. In statistics, this likely range of the true value is known as the confidence interval. Actually there are several confidence intervals depending on how confident you wish to be that you have included the true value – the interval is obviously going to be wider if you wish to be very confident rather than just confident. In statistics one would routinely use the 95% confidence interval. This 95% confidence interval indicates the range of values within which the true value will fall 95% of the time. That is, we are likely to be wrong only 5% of the time. The following is a rule of thumb which is accurate enough for your purposes for now. Multiply the standard error by 2. This gives you the amount which you need to add and subtract from the estimated value to cut off the middle 95% of the possible values – that is the 95% confidence interval. In other words, if the estimated value of the criterion (Y-variable) is 6.00 and the standard error of this estimate is 0.26, then the 95% confid­ ence interval is 6.00 ± (2 × 0.26) which is 6.00 ± 0.52. This gives us a 95% confidence interval of 5.48 to 6.52. Thus it is almost certain that the person’s score will actually fall in the range of 5.48 to 6.52 although the most likely value is 6.00. Exactly the same applies to the other aspects of regression. If the slope is 2.00 with a standard error of 0.10, then the 95% confidence interval is 2.00 ± (2 × 0.10), which gives a confidence interval of 1.80 to 2.20. The use of confidence intervals is not as common as it ought to be despite the fact that it gives us a realistic assessment of the precision of our estimates. The above calculations of confidence intervals are approximate if you have fewer than about 30 pairs of scores. If you have between 16 and 29 pairs of scores the calculation will be more accurate if you multiply by 2.1 rather than 2.0. If you have between 12 and 15 pairs of scores then multiplying by 2.2 would improve the accuracy of the calculation. With fewer than 12 pairs the method gets a little more inaccurate. When you have become more knowledgeable about statistics, you could obtain precise confidence intervals by multiplying your standard error by the appropriate value of t from Significance Table 13.1. The appropriate value is in the row headed ‘Degrees of freedom’, corresponding to your number of pairs of scores minus 2 under the column for the 5% significance level (i.e. if you have 10 pairs of scores then you would multiply by 2.31).

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 129

11/18/13 1:53 PM

130

CHAPTER 9  Regression: Prediction with precision

Research examples Simple regression Examples of the use of simple regression in the modern psychological research literature are not common. One likely reason is the general ease of adding in more predictor variables into a study than one. So regard our discussion of simple regression as primarily preparing you to understand multiple regression. Of course, anytime that you use Pearson correlation then it would be appropriate to include the statistics from the equivalent simple regression. Ang and Huan (2006) tested whether depression mediated the relation between academic stress and thoughts of killing oneself (suicidal ideation) in adolescents. As a first step, they carried out simple regressions of academic stress with depression and suicidal ideation. Both depression and suicidal ideation were positively related to academic stress. Greater academic stress was associated with greater depression and suicidal ideation. Fayed, Klassen, Dix, Klaassen and Sung (2011) were interested in the sorts of factors which predict optimism in the parents of children who are suffering from cancer. They obtained a sample of such parents whose children were actively undergoing treatment. Their measure of optimism was the Life Orientation Test and they included another 26 predictor variables based on stress process theory expectations. They included a number of measures of positive intrapsychic traits which they found to be more predictive of optimism than factors to do with the child’s cancer such as the prognosis. They chose to analyse each of their predictors of optimism separately in order to find the predictors which explained substantial amounts of variation. On the basis of their choices made in this way, the initial simple regressions were followed up with multiple regression analysis. Gallagher and his colleagues (2013) investigated the relation between patients’ weight and a number of other variables such as their confidence in exercising and following a cholesterol-lowering diet. Before carrying out a multiple regression, they conducted simple regressions. They found a number of significant regressions such as greater weight being associated with less confidence in exercising and with following a cholesterol-lowering diet. Norman and his colleagues (2013) wanted to know whether early childhood trauma was associated with increased blood pressure in older adults. As part of their analysis, they carried out a simple regression between these two variables which was found to be statistically significant.

Key points ●

Drawing the scattergram will invariably illuminate the trends in your data and strongly hint at the broad features of the regression calculations. It will also provide a visual check on your computations.



These regression procedures assume that the best-fitting regression line is a straight line. If it looks as if the regression line ought to be curved or curvilinear, do not apply these numerical methods. Of course, even if a relationship is curvilinear you could use the curved-line scattergram to make graphically based predictions.



It may be that you have more than one predictor variable that you wish to use – if so, look at (Chapter 32) on multiple regression.

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 130

11/18/13 1:53 PM

Computer analysis 131



Computer analysis Simple regression using SPSS

Figure 9.5

SPSS Statistics steps for performing simple regression

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

The most important part of the regression output is the B weight and its sign as this tells you about the direction of the relationship in the scattergram. The significance level is also important, of course. You can largely ignore the row for the constant as this generally is not involved in the interpretation. Remember that there is always a direction to the prediction and that one variable will be the predictor variable and the other the predicted variable.



One way of reporting this regression analysis would be: ‘Because of the negative correlation between mathematical and musical abilities, it was possible to carry out a regression analysis to predict musical ability from mathematical ability. The slope of the regression of mathematical ability on musical ability b is −0.63 and the intercept a is 8.41. It is statistically significant at less than the 0.001 level.’

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 131

11/18/13 1:53 PM

132

CHAPTER 9  Regression: Prediction with precision

Screenshot 9.1

The data

Screenshot 9.2

Select the test

Screenshot 9.3

Move variables into box for analysis

Screenshot 9.4

Select options

Screenshot 9.5

The SPSS output

M09_HOWI0749_06_SE_C09.indd 132

11/18/13 1:53 PM

PART 2

Significance testing

M10_HOWI0749_06_SE_C10.indd 133

11/13/13 5:15 PM

M10_HOWI0749_06_SE_C10.indd 134

11/13/13 5:15 PM

Chapter 10

Samples and populations Generalising and inferring

Overview ●

Samples are characteristic of all modern research. Their use requires inferential statistical techniques in the analysis of data.



A population in statistics is all of the scores on a particular variable and a sample is a smaller set of these scores.



Random samples are systematically drawn samples in which each score in the population has an equal likelihood of being selected.



Random samples tend to be like the population from which they are drawn in terms of characteristics such as the mean and variability of scores.



Standard error is a measure of the variation in the means of samples drawn from a population. It is essentially the standard deviation of the sample means.

Preparation This chapter introduces some important new ideas. They can be understood by anyone with a general familiarity with Chapters 2–9.

M10_HOWI0749_06_SE_C10.indd 135

11/13/13 5:15 PM

136

CHAPTER 10  Samples and populations: Generalising and inferring

10.1 Introduction Most research in psychology relies on just a small sample of data from which general statements are made. The terms sample and population are familiar to most of us, although the fine detail may be a little obscure. So far we have mainly discussed sets of data. This was deliberate since everything that we have discussed in previous chapters is applicable to either samples or populations. The next stage is to understand how we can use a sample of scores to make general statements or draw general conclusions that apply well beyond that sample. This is a branch of statistics called inferential statistics because it is about drawing inferences about all scores in the population from just a sample of those scores.

10.2 Theoretical considerations We need to be careful when defining our terms. A sample is fairly obvious – it is just a small number of scores selected from the entirety of scores. A population is the entire set of scores. In other words, a sample is a small set, or a subset, taken from the full set or population of scores. You need to notice some special features of the terminology we have used. We have mentioned a population of scores and a sample of scores. In other words, population and sample refer to scores on a variable. We do not deal with a population of people or even a sample of people as such in statistics. So, in statistical terms, all of the people living in Scotland do not constitute a population. Similarly, all of the people working in clothing factories in France or all of the goats on the Isle of Capri are not statistical populations. They may be populations for geographers or for everyday purposes, but they are not statistical populations. A statistical population is merely all of the scores on a particular variable. This notion can take a little getting used to. However, there is another feature of statistical populations that can cause confusion. In some cases, all of the scores are potentially obtainable, for example the ages of students entering psychology degree courses in a particular year. However, often the population of scores is infinite and otherwise impossible to specify. An example of this might be the amount of time people take to react to an auditory signal in a laboratory. The number of possible measures of reaction time in these circumstances is bounded only by time and resources. No one could actually find out the population of scores other than by taking measurement after measurement – and then there is always another measurement to be taken. The notion of population in statistics is much more of a conceptual tool than something objective. Normally a psychologist will only have a few scores (his or her sample) and no direct knowledge of what all the scores or the population of scores are. Thus the sample is usually known about in detail in research whereas the population generally is unknown. But the real question is, what can we possibly say about the population based on our knowledge of this limited entity, the sample? The answer is quite a lot. The use of sampling in public opinion polls, for example, is so familiar that we should need little convincing of the value of samples. Samples may only approximate the characteristics of the population but generally we accept that they are sufficient to base decisions on.

M10_HOWI0749_06_SE_C10.indd 136

11/13/13 5:15 PM

10.2  Theoretical considerations 137

If we know nothing about the population other than the characteristics of a sample drawn from that population of scores, our best guess or inference about the characteristics of the population is the characteristics of the sample from that population. It does not necessarily have to be particularly precise since an informed guess has to be better than nothing. So, in general, if we know nothing else, our best guess as to the mean of the population is the mean of the sample, our best guess as to the mode of the population is the mode of the sample, and our best guess as to the variance of the population is the variance of the sample. It is a case of beggars not being able to be choosers. In statistical inference, it is generally assumed that samples are drawn at random from the population. Such samples are called random samples from the population. The concept of randomness is sometimes misunderstood. Randomness is not the same as arbitrariness, informality, haphazardness or any other term that suggests a casual approach to drawing samples. A random sample of scores from a population entails selecting scores in such a way that each score in the population has an equal chance of being selected. In other words, a random sample favours the selection of no particular scores in the population. Although it is not difficult to draw a random sample, it does require a systematic approach. Any old sample you choose because you like the look of it is not a random sample. There are a number of ways of drawing a random sample. Here are just a couple: ●

Put the information about each member of the population on a slip of paper, put all of the slips into a hat, close your eyes, give the slips a long stir with your hand and finally bring one slip out of the hat. This slip is the first member of the sample; repeat the process to get the second, third and subsequent members of the sample. Technically the slip of paper should be returned to the container after being selected so it may be selected again. However, this is not done, largely because with a large population it would make little difference to the outcome.



Number each member of the population. Then press the appropriate randomisation button on your scientific calculator to generate a random number. If it is not one of the numbers in your population, ignore it and press the button again. The member of the population corresponding to this number becomes a member of the sample. Computer programs are also available for generating random numbers or use an Internet site for random numbers to do the same thing.

Low-tech researchers might use the random number tables found in many statistics textbooks. Essentially what you do is choose a random starting point in the table (closing your eyes and using a pin is recommended) and then choose numbers using a predetermined formula. For example, you could take the first three numbers after the pin, then a gap of seven numbers and then the three numbers following this, then a gap of seven numbers and then the three numbers following this, etc. Do not laugh at these procedures – they are all valid and convenient ways of choosing random samples. However, they are a little labour intensive given that there are available computer programs and applets on the Internet which will generate a random sequence of numbers for you. These are clearly preferable but less intuitive than the above approaches. Figure 10.1 gives the key steps in significance testing.

M10_HOWI0749_06_SE_C10.indd 137

11/13/13 5:15 PM

CHAPTER 10  Samples and populations: Generalising and inferring

138

Figure 10.1

Conceptual steps for understanding significance testing

10.3 The characteristics of random samples In Table 10.1 there is a population of 100 scores – the mode is 2, the median is 6.00 and the mean is 5.52. Have a go at drawing random samples of, say, five scores from this population. Repeat the process until you have a lot of sets (or samples) of scores. For each sample calculate any of the statistics just mentioned – the mean is a particularly useful statistic. We have drawn 40 samples from this population at random using a random sampling procedure from a computer program. The means of each of the 40 samples are shown in Table 10.2. It is noticeable that these means vary quite considerably. However, if we plot them graphically we find that sample means that are close to the population mean of 5.52 are relatively common. The average of the sample means is 5.20 which is close to the population mean. The minimum sample mean is 2.00 and the maximum is 8.80; these contrast with minimum and maximum values of 0 and 12 in the population. Sample means that are very different from this population mean become increasingly uncommon the further away they are from the population mean.

Table 10.1 7

A population of 100 scores 5

11

3

4

3

5

8

9

1

9

4

0

2

2

2

9

11

7

12

4

8

2

9

7

0

8

0

8

10

10

7

4

6

6

2

2

1

12

2

2

5

6

7

10

6

6

2

1

9

3

4

2

4

9

7

5

1

6

4

5

7

12

2

8

8

3

4

6

5

9

2

6

0

7

7

5

9

10

8

6

1

7

12

3

5

2

7

2

7

2

2

8

11

4

5

8

6

4

6

M10_HOWI0749_06_SE_C10.indd 138

11/13/13 5:15 PM

10.3  The characteristics of random samples 139

Table 10.2 2.20

Means of 40 samples each of five scores taken at random from the population in Table 10.1 5.60

4.80

5.00

8.40

6.80

4.60

6.60

4.00

3.00

5.00

5.60

8.80

5.60

4.60

6.80

3.00

8.20

8.20

3.80

5.40

6.00

4.80

5.20

3.20

5.20

3.00

5.00

5.40

4.80

6.00

7.40

5.00

2.00

3.60

4.60

5.60

4.60

4.40

6.00

We could calculate the standard deviation of these 40 sample means by entering each mean into the (quick) standard deviation formula:

standard deviation =

∑X

2

(∑ X ) − N

2

N

This gives us a standard deviation of sample means of 1.60. The standard deviation of sample means has a technical name, although the basic concept differs only in that it deals with means of samples and not scores. The special term is standard error. So, in general, it would seem that sample means are a pretty good estimate of population means although not absolutely necessarily so. All of this was based on samples of size 5. Table 10.3 shows the results of exactly the same exercise with samples of size 20. Much the same trends appear with these larger samples but for the following: ●

The spread of the sample means is reduced somewhat and they appear to cluster closer to the population mean. The minimum value is 4.25 and the maximum value is 6.85. The overall mean of these samples is 5.33, close to the population mean of 5.52.



The standard deviation of these means (i.e. the standard error) of larger samples is smaller. For Table 10.3 the standard deviation is 0.60.



The distribution of sample means is a steeper curve than for the smaller samples.

The conclusion to be drawn from all of this is that the larger sample size produces better estimates of the mean of the population. For statistics, this verges on common sense. Great emphasis is placed on the extreme samples in a distribution. We have seen that samples from the above population differ from the population mean by varying amounts, that the majority of samples are close to that mean, and that the bigger the sample the closer to the population mean the sample means are likely to be. There is a neat trick in statistics by which we try to define which sample means are very unlikely

Table 10.3

Means of 40 samples each of size 20 taken at random from the population in Table 10.1

4.50

5.70

5.90

5.15

4.25

5.25

5.60

5.00

5.35

5.90

6.85

5.55

5.30

5.60

5.70

4.55

6.35

6.30

4.40

5.25

4.65

5.30

4.80

5.65

4.85

5.35

5.70

4.35

5.25

5.10

6.45

5.05

5.50

6.15

5.65

5.05

5.15

5.10

4.65

4.95

M10_HOWI0749_06_SE_C10.indd 139

11/13/13 5:15 PM

140

CHAPTER 10  Samples and populations: Generalising and inferring

to occur through random sampling. It is true that in theory just about any sample mean is possible in random sampling, but those very different from the population mean are relatively rare. In statistics, the extreme 5% of these samples are of special interest. Statisticians identify the extreme 2.5% of means on each side of the population mean for special consideration. Two 2.5%s make 5%. These extreme samples come in the zone of relative rarity and are termed significant. Significance in statistics really means that we have a sample with characteristics very different from those of the population from which it was drawn. Significance at the 5% level of confidence means falling into the 5% of samples which are most different from the population. These extremes are, as we have seen, dependent on the size of sample being used.

10.4 Confidence intervals There is another idea that is fundamental to some branches of statistics – confidence interval of the mean. In public opinion surveys you often read of the margin of error being a certain percentage. The margin of error is simply the amount for, say, voting intention which defines the middle 95% most likely sample means. This is expressed relative to the obtained sample mean. So when public opinion pollsters say that the margin of error is a certain percentage they are telling us the cut-off points from the obtained percentage which would include the middle 95% of sample means. The con­ fidence interval in more general statistics is the range of means that cuts off the extreme 5% of sample means. So the 95% confidence interval merely gives the range of sample means which occupies the middle 95% of the distribution of sample means. The con­ fidence interval will be larger for smaller samples, all other things being equal. Finally, a little more jargon. The correct term for characteristics of samples such as their means, standard deviations, ranges and so forth is statistics. The same characteristics of populations are called parameters. In other words, you use the statistics from samples to estimate or infer the parameters of the population from which the sample came.

Key points ●

The material in this chapter is not immediately applicable to research. Regard it as a conceptual basis for the understanding of inferential statistics.



You need to be a little patient since the implications of this chapter will not be appreciated until later.

M10_HOWI0749_06_SE_C10.indd 140

11/13/13 5:15 PM

  Computer analysis 141



Computer analysis Selecting a random sample using SPSS

Figure 10.2

M10_HOWI0749_06_SE_C10.indd 141

SPSS Statistics steps for selecting a random sample of cases

11/13/13 5:15 PM

142

CHAPTER 10  Samples and populations: Generalising and inferring

Screenshot 10.1

Screenshot 10.2

M10_HOWI0749_06_SE_C10.indd 142

Part of the data

Select the procedure

Screenshot 10.3

Select the variable

Screenshot 10.4

Select the sample characteristics

Screenshot 10.5

Part of the selected sample

11/13/13 5:15 PM

Chapter 11

Statistical significance for the correlation coefficient A practical introduction to statistical inference

Overview ●

It is generally essential to report the statistical significance of the correlation coefficient and many other statistical techniques.



Statistical significance is little other than an indication that your statistical findings are unlikely to be the result of chance factors.



It can be shown that samples drawn randomly from a population generally tend to have similar characteristics to those of the population. However, there are some samples which tend to be unlike the population.



The null hypothesis always states that there is no relation between two variables. Significance testing always seeks to assess the validity of the null hypothesis.



If our data sample is in the middle 95% of samples if the null hypothesis is true, we say that our sample is not statistically significant at the 5% level and prefer the null hypothesis.



However, if our data sample is in the extreme 5% of samples if the null hypothesis is true, our sample does not seem to support the null hypothesis. In this case, we tend to prefer the alternative hypothesis and reject the null hypothesis. We also say that our findings are statistically significant.

Preparation You must be familiar with correlation coefficients (Chapter 8) and populations and samples (Chapter 10).

M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 143

11/13/13 5:15 PM

CHAPTER 11  Statistical significance for the correlation coefficient

144

11.1 Introduction Researchers have correlated two variables for a sample of 20 people. They obtained a correlation coefficient of 0.56. The problem is that they wish to generalise beyond this sample and make statements about the trends in the data which apply more widely. However, their analyses are based on just a small sample which might not be characteristic of the trends in the population.

11.2 Theoretical considerations We can all sympathise with these researchers. The reason why they are concerned is straightforward. Imagine that Table 11.1 contains the population of pairs of scores. Overall, the correlation between the two variables in this population is 0.00. That is, there is absolutely no relationship between variable X and variable Y in the population. What happens, though, if we draw many samples of, say, eight pairs of scores at random from this population and calculate the correlation coefficients for each sample?

Table 11.1

An imaginary population of 60 pairs of scores with zero correlation between the pairs

Pair

Variable

Pair

X

Y

01

14

12

04

3

07

5

10 13

Variable

Pair

X

Y

02

5

11

13

05

14

12

08

17

15

5

11

3

16

7

14

14

16

13

8

17

Variable X

Y

03

12

5

9

06

10

14

17

09

4

8

3

12

19

12

9

15

12

13

15

11

18

15

7

19

12

17

20

11

14

21

5

13

22

12

11

23

11

9

24

15

14

25

5

12

26

15

9

27

12

13

28

6

13

29

14

7

30

18

13

31

12

1

32

19

12

33

12

19

34

11

14

35

12

17

36

13

9

37

14

12

38

15

5

39

18

13

40

17

11

41

3

12

42

16

9

43

16

12

44

11

9

45

18

2

46

12

14

47

12

14

48

15

11

49

16

12

50

12

14

51

8

14

52

5

11

53

7

8

54

16

8

55

13

13

56

12

15

57

18

2

58

3

1

59

7

8

60

11

6

M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 144

11/13/13 5:15 PM

11.2  Theoretical considerations 145

Some of the correlation coefficients are indeed more-or-less zero, but a few are sub­ stantially different from zero, as we can see from Table 11.2. Plotted on a histogram, the distribution of these correlation coefficients looks like Figure 11.1. It is more or less a normal distribution with a mean correlation of zero and most of the correlations being close to that zero point. However, some of the correlation coefficients are substantially different from 0.00. This shows that even where there is zero relationship between the





Table 11.2 −0.56

Two hundred correlation coefficients obtained by repeatedly random sampling eight pairs of scores from Table 11.1 −0.30

0.36

0.54

−0.27

0.05

−0.33

−0.19

0.54

0.18

−0.54

0.11

0.25

−0.15

−0.57

−0.31

−0.24

0.17

−0.69

−0.19

−0.53

0.68

−0.22

−0.22

−0.26

−0.42

0.08

−0.30

−0.41

0.29

−0.45

−0.09

−0.06

−0.30

−0.72

−0.53

0.04

−0.66

0.65

−0.53

−0.39

−0.21

0.07

−0.80

−0.68

0.08

0.13

0.76

−0.04

0.18

−0.36

−0.19

0.29

0.24

0.38

−0.55

−0.40

0.50

−0.09

−0.30

−0.30

−0.56

0.68

−0.14

0.35

−0.28

0.56

−0.38

−0.16

0.15

−0.29

−0.23

−0.42

−0.27

0.01

0.43

0.01

−0.33

−0.20

0.49

−0.26

−0.41

−0.09

0.00

0.54

0.17

0.34

0.52

−0.11

0.67

−0.26

−0.16

−0.70

0.00

−0.17

0.40

0.03

−0.02

0.35

−0.01

−0.23

0.03

0.30

−0.52

−0.05

−0.26

−0.32

−0.37

−0.51

0.18

−0.20

−0.17

−0.43

−0.39

0.37

0.23

−0.10

0.32

0.02

0.52

−0.18

0.38

0.45

−0.50

−0.58

0.28

−0.34

−0.28

0.24

0.53

−0.17

−0.02

−0.34

−0.23

−0.54

0.25

−0.71

0.72

0.03

−0.13

−0.08

−0.30

−0.06

−0.10

−0.65

0.27

−0.04

0.32

−0.52

−0.42

−0.04

0.59

−0.29

−0.31

0.48

−0.48

0.02

−0.30

0.81

−0.23

0.10

−0.12

−0.51

−0.19

0.08

0.18

−0.27

−0.67

−0.69

0.50

0.15

−0.54

−0.15

0.05

0.01

0.52

0.19

0.19

0.07

0.27

0.34

−0.44

−0.11

−0.21

−0.02

−0.07

0.17

−0.30

−0.06

−0.49

0.57

−0.10

−0.23

0.01

−0.09

−0.27

0.22

−0.28

0.43

−0.34

Figure 11.1

M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 145

Distribution of correlation coefficients presented in Table 11.2

11/13/13 5:16 PM

146

CHAPTER 11  Statistical significance for the correlation coefficient

two variables in the population, random samples can appear to have correlations which depart from 0.00. So you will find in the table correlations as large as 0.8 which most researchers would be delighted to obtain in their research – though we know that this correlation is really due to chance and that there is no real correlation in the population. Just about anything is possible in samples although only certain things are likely. Consequently, we try to stipulate which are the likely correlations in samples of a given size and which are the unlikely ones (if the population correlation is zero). Actually all we say is that correlations in the middle 95% of the distribution of samples are likely if the population correlation is zero. Correlations in the extreme 5% (usually the extreme 2.5% in each direction) are unlikely in these circumstances. These are arbitrary cut-off points, but they are conventional in statistics and have long antecedents. It is also not an unreasonable cut-off for most purposes to suggest that if a sample has only a 1 in 20 chance of occurring then it is unlikely to represent the population value. If a correlation is in the extreme 5% of the distribution of samples from a population where the correlation is zero, it is deemed statistically significant. We should be sitting up and taking notice if this happens. In other words, statistical significance merely signals the statistically unusual or unlikely. In the above example, by examining Table 11.2 we find that the correlations 0.81, 0.76, 0.72, 0.68 and 0.68 and −0.80, −0.72, −0.71, −0.70 and −0.69 are in the extreme 5% of correlations away from zero. This extreme 5% is made up of the extreme 2.5% positive correlations and the extreme 2.5% negative correlations. Therefore, a correlation of between 0.68 and 1.00 or −0.69 and −1.00 is in the extreme 5% of correlations. This is the range which we describe as statistically significant. Statistical significance simply means that our sample falls in the relatively extreme part of the distribution of samples obtained if the null hypothesis (see the next section) of no relationship between the two variables is true. These ranges of significant correlations mentioned above only apply to samples of size eight. A different size of sample from the same population results in a different spread of correlations obtained from repeated sampling. The spread is bigger if the samples are smaller and less if the samples are larger. In other words, there is more variation in the distribution of samples with small sample sizes than with larger ones. On the face of things, all of this is merely a theoretical meandering of little value. We know that the population correlation is zero – actually we made it zero. A major difficulty is that we are normally unaware of the population correlation since our information is based solely on a sample which may or may not represent the population very well. However, it is not quite the futile exercise it appears. Some information provided by a sample can be used to infer or estimate the characteristics of the population. For one thing, information about the variability or variance of the scores in the sample is used to estimate the variability of scores in the population.

11.3 Back to the real world: the null hypothesis There is another vitally important concept in statistics – the hypothesis. Hypotheses in psychological statistics are usually presented as antithetical pairs – the null hypothesis and its corresponding alternative hypothesis: ●

M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 146

The null hypothesis is in essence a statement that there is no relationship between two variables. The following are all examples of null hypotheses: ●

There is no relationship between brain size and intelligence.



There is no relationship between gender and income.

11/13/13 5:16 PM

11.3  Back to the real world: the null hypothesis 147





There is no relationship between baldness and virility.



There is no relationship between children’s self-esteem and that of their parent of the same sex.



There is no relationship between ageing and memory loss.



There is no relationship between the amount of carrots eaten and ability to see in the dark.

The alternative hypothesis simply states that there is a relationship between two variables. In its simplest forms the alternative hypothesis says only this: ●

There is a relationship between the number of years of education people have and their income.



There is a relationship between people’s gender and how much they talk about their emotional problems.



There is a relationship between people’s mental instability and their artistic creativity.



There is a relationship between abuse in childhood and later psychological problems.



There is a relationship between birth order and social dominance.



There is a relationship between the degree of similarity of couples and their sexual attraction for each other.

So the difference between null and alternative hypotheses is merely the word ‘no’. Of course, sometimes psychologists dress their hypotheses up in fancier language than this but the basic principle is unchanged. (Actually there is a complication – directional hypotheses – but these are dealt with in Chapter 18.) The statistical reason for using the null hypothesis and alternative hypothesis is that they clarify the populations in statistical analyses. In statistics, inferences are based on the characteristics of the population as defined by the null hypothesis. Invariably the populations defined by the null hypothesis are ones in which there is no relation between a pair of variables. Thus, the population defined by the null hypothesis is one where the correlation between the two variables under consideration is 0.00. The characteristics of a sample can be used to assess whether it is likely that the correlation for the sample comes from a population in which the correlation is zero. So the basic trick is to use certain of the characteristics of a sample together with the notion of the null hypothesis to define the characteristics of a population. Other characteristics of the sample are then used to estimate the likelihood that this sample comes from this particular population. To repeat and summarise: ●

The null hypothesis is used to define a population in which there is no relationship between two variables.



Other characteristics, especially the variability of this population, are estimated or inferred from the known sample.

It is then possible to decide whether or not it is likely that the sample comes from this population defined by the null hypothesis. If it is unlikely that the sample comes from the null hypothesis-based population, the possibility that the null hypothesis is true is rejected. Instead the view that the alternative hypothesis is true is accepted. That is, the alternative hypothesis that there really is a relationship is preferred. This is the same thing as saying that we can safely generalise from our sample.

M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 147

11/13/13 5:16 PM

148

CHAPTER 11  Statistical significance for the correlation coefficient

Box 11.1

Focus on

Do correlations differ? Notice that throughout this chapter we are comparing a particular correlation coefficient obtained from our data with the correlation coefficient that we would expect to obtain if there were no relationship between the two variables at all. In other words, we are calculating the likelihood of obtaining the correlation coefficient based on our sample of data if, in fact, the correlation between these two variables in the population from which the sample was taken is actually 0.00. However, there are circumstances in which the researcher might wish to assess whether two correlations obtained in their research are significantly different from each other. Imagine, for example, that the researcher is investigating the relationship between satisfaction with one’s marriage and the length of time that individuals have been married. The researcher notes that the correlation between satisfaction and length

of marriage is 0.25 for male participants but 0.53 for female participants. There is clearly a difference here, but is it a statistically significant one? So essentially the researcher needs to know whether a correlation of 0.53 is significantly different from a correlation of 0.25 (the researcher has probably already tested the significance of each of these correlations separately using the sorts of methods described in this chapter but, of course, this does not answer the question of whether the two correlation coefficients differ from each other). It is a relatively simple matter to do this calculation. It has to be done by hand, unfortunately. The procedure for doing this is described in Section 36.7 Comparing a study with a previous study. In this section you will read about how to assess whether two correlation coefficients are significantly different from each other.

11.4 Pearson’s correlation coefficient again If you only ever use computer programs for your statistical analyses then you will not need what is in the section. Computer programs such as SPSS give exact significance levels for your computations and so there is no need to know about other methods of working out the significance level of a correlation coefficient. However, from time to time this may not be enough. For example, imagine that you are reviewing the research literature and find that one study reports a correlation of 0.66 between two variables but fails to give the significance level, then what do you do? This sort of situation does happen and not every research paper is exemplary in its statistical analysis. Or you simply wish to check that there is not a typographical error for the given significance level then what do you do? There are other circumstances in which you cannot rely on using the computer. So this section we will explain how significance levels may be obtained from tables so long as you know the size of the correlation coefficient and the sample size (or degrees of freedom) involved. The null hypothesis for research involving the correlation coefficient is that there is no relationship between the two variables. In other words, the null hypothesis implies that the correlation coefficient between two variables is 0.00 in the population (defined by the null hypothesis). So what if, in a sample of 10 pairs of scores, the correlation is 0.94 as for the data in Table 11.3? Is it likely that such a correlation would occur in a sample if it actually came from a population where the true correlation is zero? We are back to our basic problem of how likely it is that a correlation of 0.94 would occur if there really is no correlation in the population. We need to plot the distribution of correlations in random samples of 10 pairs drawn from this population. Unfortunately we do not have the population of scores, only a sample of scores. However, statisticians can use the variability of this sample of scores to estimate the variability in the population. Then the likely distribution of correlations

M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 148

11/13/13 5:16 PM

11.4  Pearson’s correlation coefficient again 149

A sample of 10 pairs of scores

Table 11.3

Pair number

X score

Y score

1

5

4

2

2

1

3

7

8

4

5

6

5

0

2

6

1

0 3

7

4

8

2

2

9

8

9

10

6

7

in repeated samples of a given size drawn from the population with this amount of variability can be calculated. Mere mortals like us use tables provided by statisticians. Although SPSS and other computer programs will tell you the statistical significance of your analysis, tables are available which, for any given size of sample, tell you the minimum size of a correlation coefficient which cuts the middle 95% of correlations from the extreme 5% of correlations (assuming the null hypothesis is true in the population). These cut-off points are usually called critical values: ●

If the sample’s correlation is in the middle 95% of correlations then we accept the null hypothesis that there is no relationship between the two variables. By accept, we mean that in the absence of any other information or considerations, the null hypothesis is a more plausible explanation of the data than the hypothesis.



However, if the correlation in the sample is in the extreme 5% of correlations then the alternative hypothesis is accepted (that there is a relationship between the two variables).

Significance Table 11.1 reveals that for a sample size of 10, a correlation has to be between −0.63 and −1.00 or between 0.63 and 1.00 to be sufficiently large as to be in the extreme 5% of correlations which support the alternative hypothesis. Correlations closer to 0.00 than these come in the middle 95% which are held to support the null hypothesis. Figure 11.2 gives the key steps in testing statistical significance.

Explaining statistics 11.1

Statistical significance of a Pearson correlation coefficient Given that you know the value of the Pearson correlation coefficient, whether or not this is significant or not may be found from Significance Table 11.1. You need either the sample size or the degrees of freedom to do this. The degrees of freedom (df ) for a correlation coefficient is the sample size minus 2 so it is easy to convert degrees of freedom to sample sizes simply by adding 2 to the degrees of freedom. In the example in Chapter 8 (Explaining Statistics 8.1), the correlation between mathematical scores and musical scores was found to be −0.90 with a sample size of 10. If this



M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 149

11/13/13 5:16 PM

150

CHAPTER 11  Statistical significance for the correlation coefficient

Significance Table 11.1

5% significance values of the Pearson correlation coefficient (two-tailed test). An extended and conventional version of this table is given in Appendix C

Sample size

Significant at 5% level Accept hypothesis

5

−0.88

to

−1.00

or

+0.88

to

+1.00

6

−0.81

to

−1.00

or

+0.81

to

+1.00

7

−0.75

to

−1.00

or

+0.75

to

+1.00

8

−0.71

to

−1.00

or

+0.71

to

+1.00

9

−0.67

to

−1.00

or

+0.67

to

+1.00

10

−0.63

to

−1.00

or

+0.63

to

+1.00

11

−0.60

to

−1.00

or

+0.60

to

+1.00

12

−0.58

to

−1.00

or

+0.58

to

+1.00

13

−0.55

to

−1.00

or

+0.55

to

+1.00

14

−0.53

to

−1.00

or

+0.53

to

+1.00

15

−0.51

to

−1.00

or

+0.51

to

+1.00

16

−0.50

to

−1.00

or

+0.50

To

+1.00

17

−0.48

to

−1.00

or

+0.48

to

+1.00

18

−0.47

to

−1.00

or

+0.47

to

+1.00

19

−0.46

to

−1.00

or

+0.46

to

+1.00

20

−0.44

to

−1.00

or

+0.44

to

+1.00

25

−0.40

to

−1.00

or

+0.40

to

+1.00

30

−0.36

to

−1.00

or

+0.36

to

+1.00

40

−0.31

to

−1.00

or

+0.31

to

+1.00

50

−0.28

to

−1.00

or

+0.28

to

+1.00

60

−0.25

to

−1.00

or

+0.25

to

+1.00

100

−0.20

to

−1.00

or

+0.20

to

+1.00

Your value must be in the listed ranges for your sample size to be significant at the 5% level (i.e. to accept the hypothesis). If your required sample size is not listed, then take the nearest smaller sample size. Alternatively, extrapolate from listed values.

correlation is in the range of correlations listed as being in the extreme 5% of correlations for this sample size, the correlation is described as being statistically significant at the 5% level of significance.

Interpreting the results In this case, since our obtained value of the correlation coefficient is in the significant range of the correlation coefficient (−0.63 to −1.00 and 0.63 to 1.00), we reject the null hypothesis in favour of the alternative hypothesis that there is a relationship between mathematical and musical scores.

Reporting the results In our report of the study we would conclude by writing something to the following effect: ‘There is a negative correlation of −0.90 between mathematical and musical scores which is statistically significant at the 5% level with a sample size of 10.’ Alternatively, following the recommendations of the APA (2010) Publication Manual we could say something like ‘Mathematical scores were significantly negatively correlated with musical scores, r(8) = −.90, p < .05.’

M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 150

11/13/13 5:16 PM

11.4  Pearson’s correlation coefficient again 151

Figure 11.2

Box 11.2

Conceptual steps for understanding statistical significance testing

Key concepts

Type I and II errors ●

The terms Type I error and Type II error frequently appear in statistics textbooks although they are relatively uncommon in reports and other publications. They refer to the risk that no matter what decision you make in research based on your statistical analysis there is always a chance that you have made the wrong decision. There are two types of wrong decision – one involves deciding that there is a trend when there is in reality no trend; the other involves deciding that there is not a trend when in reality there is.



A Type I error is deciding that the null hypothesis is false when it is actually true.



A Type II error is deciding that the null hypothesis is true when it is actually false. Powerful statistical tests are those in which there is less chance of a Type II error.

Figure 11.3 shows the process by which correct decisions are made and the processes by which Type I errors and Type II errors are made. Of course, these are not errors which it is easy to do anything about since the researcher simply does not know what is truly the case in general (i.e. in the population) since they only have information from the sample of data that they have collected. So these are rather abstract concepts rather than concrete situations. You may have also noticed that if the researcher does something to minimise the risk of a Type I error then the risk of a Type II error increases. So to avoid a Type I error then the researcher could set a more stringent level of significance than the 5% level – say the 1% level – but this would reduce the risk of a Type I error at the cost of increasing the risk of a Type II error. The main issue in succeeding chapters is significance testing and the



M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 151

11/13/13 5:16 PM

152

CHAPTER 11  Statistical significance for the correlation coefficient

Type I error. However, Chapter 40 discusses statistical power which is greatly related to the matter of the Type II error. Unfortunately, the terms are not particularly useful in the everyday application of statistics where it is hard

Figure 11.3

enough making a decision let alone worrying about the chance that you have made the wrong decision. Given that statistics deals with probabilities and not certainties, it is important to remember that there is always a chance that any decision you make is wrong in statistical analysis.

Type I and Type II errors

11.5 The Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient If you followed our advice in Chapter 8 to use the standard Pearson correlation co­ efficient formula on the ranked scores instead of using the Spearman rho formula (Explaining statistics 8.3) then the significance of your calculation should be assessed using Significance Table 11.1. This is the approach used by standard computer packages such as SPSS Statistics. However, assuming that you used the Spearman rho formula in Explaining statistics 8.2, especially if there are tied scores, then Significance Table 11.2 should be used. In Chapter 8 (Explaining statistics 8.2) we calculated Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient between mathematical score and musical score. The correlation was found to be −0.89 with a sample size of 10. Significance Table 11.2 reveals that in order to be significant at the 5% level with a sample size of 10, correlations have to be in the range 0.65 to 1.00 or −0.65 to −1.00.

M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 152

11/13/13 5:16 PM

11.5  The Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient 153

Significance Table 11.2

5% significance values of the Spearman correlation coefficient (two-tailed test). An extended and conventional version of this table is given in Appendix D

Sample size

Significant at 5% level Accept hypothesis −1.00

5

or

+1.00

6

−0.89

to

−1.00

or

+0.89

to

+1.00

7

−0.79

to

−1.00

or

+0.79

to

+1.00

8

−0.74

to

−1.00

or

+0.74

to

+1.00

9

−0.68

to

−1.00

or

+0.68

to

+1.00

10

−0.65

to

−1.00

or

+0.65

to

+1.00

11

−0.62

to

−1.00

or

+0.62

to

+1.00

12

−0.59

to

−1.00

or

+0.59

to

+1.00

13

−0.57

to

−1.00

or

+0.57

to

+1.00

14

−0.55

to

−1.00

or

+0.55

to

+1.00

15

−0.52

to

−1.00

or

+0.52

to

+1.00

16

−0.51

to

−1.00

or

+0.51

to

+1.00

17

−0.49

to

−1.00

or

+0.49

to

+1.00

18

−0.48

to

−1.00

or

+0.48

to

+1.00

19

−0.46

to

−1.00

or

+0.46

to

+1.00

20

−0.45

to

−1.00

or

+0.45

to

+1.00

25

−0.40

to

−1.00

or

+0.40

to

+1.00

30

−0.36

to

−1.00

or

+0.36

to

+1.00

40

−0.31

to

−1.00

or

+0.31

to

+1.00

50

−0.28

to

−1.00

or

+0.28

to

+1.00

60

−0.26

to

−1.00

or

+0.26

to

+1.00

100

−0.20

to

−1.00

or

+0.20

to

+1.00

Your value must be in the listed ranges for your sample size to be significant at the 5% level (i.e. to accept the hypothesis). If your required sample size is not listed, then take the nearest smaller sample size. Alternatively, extrapolate from listed values.



■ Interpreting the results Since our obtained value of the Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient is in the range of significant correlations we accept the alternative hypothesis that mathematical and musical scores are (inversely) related and reject the null hypothesis.



■ Reporting the results We can report a significant correlation: ‘There is a negative correlation of −0.89 between mathematical and musical scores which is statistically significant at the 5% level with a sample size of 10.’ Alternatively, following the APA (2010) Publication Manual recommendations we could write something like ‘Mathematical scores were significantly negatively correlated with musical scores, rs(8) = −.89, p < .05’.

M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 153

11/13/13 5:16 PM

154

CHAPTER 11  Statistical significance for the correlation coefficient

Research examples Significance of Pearson correlation and Spearman’s rho Rohmer and Louvet (2012), in their analysis of stereotyping of people with disability, report some correlations as follows: ‘To examine the relationships between the implicit and the explicit measures, separate scores were computed on competence and warmth at both the implicit and the explicit level, by subtracting the scores given to targets with disability from those given to targets without disability. Results indicated that there were no significant correlations for both competence (r = .08, p = .46) and warmth (r = .05, p = .65).’ (p.738) Gannon and Barrowcliffe (2012) in their study of firesetters make the comment: ‘Overall scores on the Fire Setting Scale and the Fire Proclivity Scale were not significantly related to impression management scores across the whole sample (r = −.12 and −.01, respectively). However, when these correlations were computed for firesetters and nonfiresetters separately, scores on the Fire Setting Scale were significantly negatively related to impression management scores for the firesetters (r = −.64; p = .01).’ (p.9) Vallat-Azouvi, Pradat-Diehl and Azouvi (2012) report on the Working Memory Scale which measures aspects of working memory including short-term storage, executive control and attention. As part of this, they investigated the validity of this scale by reference to the Cognitive Failure Questionnaire (CFQ) and the Rating Scale of Attentional Behaviour (RSAB). They write: ‘Concurrent validity was assessed by computing Spearman rank order correlation coefficients between the total score of the scale on the one hand and the CFQ and the RSAB on the other hand  .  .  .  Both correlations were significant (rho = .90, p < .0001 with the CFQ, and rho = .81, p < .0001 with the RSAB).’ (pp.642–3)

Key points ●

There is nothing complex in the calculation of statistical significance for the correlation coefficients. However, statistical tables normally do not include every sample size. When a particular sample size is missing you can simply use the nearest (lower) tabulated value. Alternatively you could extrapolate from the nearest tabulated value above and the nearest tabulated value below your actual sample size.



It is a bad mistake to report a correlation without indicating whether it is statistically significant.



Chapter 17 explains how to report your significance levels in a more succinct form. Try to employ this sort of style as it eases the writing of research reports and looks professional.



Beware that some statistical textbooks provide significance tables which are distributed by degrees of freedom rather than sample size. For any given sample size, the degrees of freedom are two less. Thus, for a sample size of 10, the degrees of freedom are 10 − 2, or 8.

M11_HOWI0749_06_SE_C11.indd 154

11/13/13 5:16 PM

  Computer analysis 155



Computer analysis The correlation coefficient using SPSS

Figure 11.4

SPSS Statistics steps for the significance of the correlation coefficient

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

Interpretation of the output is complicated by the fact that SPSS intercorrelates each of the variables with itself and with the other variables. The correlation of a variable with itself is always 1. No significance level is given for this. The table also includes the correlation between the variables with the other variables twice. So you have the correlation of Variable X with Variable Y AND the correlation of Variable Y with Variable X. These are, of course, the same. The output gives the correlation (−.900), the statistical significance (.000) and the sample size (10).



It would be good to report the significance level as being less than 0.001 and something known as the degrees of freedom which for the correlation coefficient is N-2 or 8 in this case. Significance is discussed in Chapter 11 and degrees of freedom in Chapter 21.



In a report, we could write ‘There is a significant negative correlation between musical ability and mathematical ability, r(8) = .90, p 0.05.’ The material in the last part of the second sentence simply gives the statistic used (the t-test), the degrees of free­ dom (7), its value (−1.98), and the level of significance which is more than that required for the 5% level (p > 0.05). Chapter 17 explains this in greater detail.

Warning  The distribution of the difference scores should not be markedly skewed if the t-test is to be used. Appendix A explains how to test for significant skewness. If the distribution of difference scores is markedly skewed, you might wish to consider the use of the Wilcoxon matched pairs test (Explaining statistics 19.2).

13.5 Cautionary note Many psychologists act as if they believe that it is the design of the research which determines whether you should use a related test. Related designs are those, after all, in which people serve in both research conditions. It is assumed that there is a correlation between subjects’ scores in the two conditions. What if there is no correlation between the two samples of scores? The standard error becomes relatively large compared to the num­ ber of degrees of freedom so your research is less likely to be statistically significant (especially if the samples are small). So while trying to control for unwanted sources of error, if there is no correlation between the scores in the two conditions of the study, the researcher may simply reduce the likelihood of achieving statistical significance. The reason is that the researcher may have obtained non-significant findings simply because a) they have reduced the error degrees of freedom, which therefore b) increases the error estimate, thus c) reducing the significance level perhaps to nonsignificance. Some computer programs print out the correlation between the two variables as part of the correlated t-test output. If this correlation is not significant then you might be wise to think again about your test of significance. This situation is particularly likely to occur where you are using a matching procedure (as opposed to having the same people in

M13_HOWI0749_06_SE_C13.indd 174

11/13/13 5:17 PM

13.5  Cautionary note 175

both conditions). Unless your matching variables actually do correlate with the dependent variable, the matching can have no effect on reducing the error variance. In the previous calculation, we found no significant change in eye contact in older babies compared with younger ones. It is worth examining the correlation between the two sets of scores to see if the assumption of correlation is fulfilled. The correlation is 0.42 but we need a correlation of 0.71 or greater to be statistically significant. In other words, the correlated scores do not really correlate – certainly not significantly. Even applying the uncorrelated version of the t-test described in the next chapter makes no difference. It still leaves the difference between the two age samples non-significant. We are not suggesting that if a related t-test fails to achieve significance you should replace it by an unrelated t-test, merely that you risk ignoring trends in your data which may be important. The most practical implication is that matching variables should relate to the dependent variable, otherwise there is no point in matching in the first place.

Research examples Correlated/related/paired t-test Drees and Mack (2012) argue that mental toughness is critical for achieving athletic success and that it comes with experiences. The researchers wanted to know, among other things, whether the mental toughness ability of high school wrestlers change over time (the competitive season). Participants in the study completed MeBTough (the Mental, Emotional and Bodily Toughness Inventory). A related/correlated/paired t-test was used to examine the change in mental toughness over the sporting season. No significant change was found. Jafari, Zamani, Farajzadegan, Bahrami, Emami and Loghmani (2013) examined whether spiritual therapy improved the quality of life of women undergoing radiation therapy for breast cancer. In a randomised control experiment, quality of life was assessed psychometrically. Using the related/correlated/paired t-test it was found that for the treated group there was an improvement in quality of life scores from the start of the treatment until after six weeks of the intervention. This was not the case for the control group. Wilkes, Cordier, Bundy, Docking and Munro (2011) studied children with ADHD (attention deficit hyperactivity disorder), who can be deficient in social skills. The study examined the effectiveness of a new intervention. This, in part, involved play sessions incorporating feedback and peer modelling. This was intended to enhance play and social skills in children with ADHD and their playmates. The design was a matched samples one of the sort to which the paired t-test can be applied. The pre- and post-measures were the Test of Playfulness. A related samples t-test (they call it the dependent samples t-test) was used to test for improvement. Separate related t-tests showed that both the ADHD and the paired controls improve over the period of the intervention.

M13_HOWI0749_06_SE_C13.indd 175

11/13/13 5:17 PM

176

CHAPTER 13  The t-test: Comparing two samples of correlated/related/paired scores

Key points ●

The related or correlated t-test is merely a special case of the one-way analysis of variance for related samples (Chapter 22). Although it is frequently used in psychological research it tells us nothing more than the equi­ valent analysis of variance would do. Since the analysis of variance is generally a more flexible statistic, allow­ ing any number of groups of scores to be compared, it might be your preferred statistic. However, the common occurrence of the t-test in psychological research means that you need to have some idea about what it is.



The related t-test assumes that the distribution of the difference scores is not markedly skewed. If it is then the test may be unacceptably inaccurate. Appendix A explains how to test for skewness.



If you compare many pairs of samples with each other in the same study using the t-test, you should consult Chapter 24 to find out about appropriate significance levels. There are better ways of making multiple comparisons, as they are called, but with appropriate adjustment to the critical values for significance, multiple t-tests can be justified.



If you find that your related t-test is not significant, it could be that your two samples of scores are not correlated, thus not meeting the assumptions of the related t-test.



Significance Table 13.1 applies whenever we have estimated the standard error from the characteristics of a sample. However, if we had actually known the population standard deviation and consequently the standard error was the actual standard error and not an estimate, we should not use the t-distribution table. In these rare (virtually unknown) circumstances, the distribution of the t-score formula is that for the z-scores.



Although the correlated t-test can be used to compare any pairs of scores, it does not always make sense to do so. For example, you could use the correlated t-test to compare the weights and heights of people to see if the weight mean and the height mean differ. Unfortunately, it is a rather stupid thing to do since the numerical values involved relate to radically different things which are not comparable with each other. It is the comparison which is nonsensical in this case. The statistical test is not to blame. On the other hand, one could compare a sample of people’s weights at different points in time quite meaningfully.

M13_HOWI0749_06_SE_C13.indd 176

11/13/13 5:17 PM

Computer analysis 177



Computer analysis The related/correlated t-test using SPSS

Figure 13.2

SPSS Statistics steps for calculating the related/correlated t test

Interpretation and write-up ●



In the example calculated, although the mean score at nine months is higher than the mean score at six months, the difference is not statistically significant at the 5% level so there is no reliable increase in scores with age. One could write, therefore: ‘Eye contact was slightly higher at nine months (M = 6.75) than at six months (M = 5.25). However, the difference did not support the hypothesis that eye contact differs in six-month and nine-month-old babies since the obtained value of a t of −1.98 is not statistically significant at the 5% level.’

M13_HOWI0749_06_SE_C13.indd 177

11/13/13 5:17 PM

178

CHAPTER 13  The t-test: Comparing two samples of correlated/related/paired scores

Screenshot 13.1

The data

Screenshot 13.3

Select variables

Screenshot 13.5

Table for related t-test

M13_HOWI0749_06_SE_C13.indd 178

Screenshot 13.2

Screenshot 13.4

Select the test

Basic tables

11/13/13 5:17 PM

Chapter 14

The t-test Comparing two samples of unrelated/ uncorrelated scores

Overview ●

The unrelated t-test is used to compare the mean scores of two different samples on a single variable. So it is used with score data.



It tells you whether the two means are statistically significant or not: that is, whether to accept the alternative hypothesis or the null hypothesis that there is, or is not, a difference between the two means.



The unrelated t-test combines the variation in the two sets of scores to estimate standard error. This leads to a rather clumsy calculation which superficially is very daunting. The calculation is easily done using SPSS or another computer program.



The t-value is simply the number of standard errors that the two means are apart by.



The statistical significance of this t-value may be obtained from tables though it is probably preferable to use computer output which usually gives statistical significance levels exactly.

Preparation This chapter will be easier if you have mastered the related t-test of Chapter 13. Revise dependent and independent variables from that chapter.

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 179

11/13/13 5:17 PM

180

CHAPTER 14  The t-test: Comparing two samples of unrelated/uncorrelated scores

14.1 Introduction The t-test described in this chapter has various names. The unrelated t-test, the uncorrelated scores t-test and the independent samples t-test are the most common variants. It is also known as the Student t-test after its inventor who used the pen-name Student. Often researchers compare two groups of scores from two separate groups of individuals to assess whether the average score of one group is higher than that of the other group. The possible research topics involved in such comparisons are limitless: ●

One might wish to compare an experimental group with a control group. For example, do volunteer women who are randomly assigned to a sexually abstinent condition have more erotic dreams than those in the sexually active control group? The independent variable is sexual activity (which has two levels – sexually abstinent and sexually active) and the dependent variable is the number of erotic dreams in a month (see Table 14.1). The independent variable differentiates the two groups being compared. In the present example, this is the amount of sexual activity (sexually abstinent versus sexually active). The dependent variable is the variable which might be influenced by the independent variable. These variables correspond to the scores given in the main body of the table (i.e. number of erotic dreams).



A group of experienced managers may be compared with a group of inexperienced managers in terms of the amount of time which they take to make complex decisions. The independent variable is experience in management (which has two levels – experienced versus inexperienced) and the dependent variable is decision-making time (Table 14.2).



A researcher might compare the amount of bullying in two schools, one with a strict and punitive policy and the other with a policy of counselling on discipline infringements. A sample of children from each school is interviewed and the number of times they have been bullied in the previous school year obtained. The independent variable is policy on discipline (which has two levels – strict versus counselling); and the

Table 14.1

Table 14.2

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 180

Number of erotic dreams per month in experimental and control groups Experimental group Sexually abstinent

Control group Sexually active

17

10

14

12

16

7

Decision time (seconds) in experienced and inexperienced managers Experienced managers

Inexperienced managers

24

167

32

133

27

74

11/13/13 5:17 PM

14.2  Theoretical considerations 181

Table 14.3

Number of times bullied in a year in schools with different discipline policies Strict policy

Counselling

8

12

5

1

2

3

dependent variable is the number of times a child has been bullied in the previous school year (see Table 14.3). The basic requirements for the unrelated/uncorrelated scores t-test are straightforward enough – two groups of scores coming from two distinct groups of people. The scores should be roughly similar in terms of the shapes of their distributions. Ideally both distri­ butions should be bell-shaped and symmetrical. However, there can be marked deviance from this ideal and the test will remain sufficiently accurate. The t-test is the name of a statistical technique which examines whether the two groups of scores have significantly different means – in other words, how likely is it that there could be a difference between the two groups as big as the one obtained if there is no difference in reality in the population?

14.2 Theoretical considerations The basic theoretical assumption underlying the use of the t-test involves the chara­cter­ istics of the null hypothesis. We explained null hypotheses in Chapter 11. The following explanation uses the same format for null hypotheses as we used in that chapter. Figure 14.1 gives the steps in carrying out a t-test.

Figure 14.1

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 181

Conceptual steps for understanding the t-test

11/13/13 5:17 PM

182

CHAPTER 14  The t-test: Comparing two samples of unrelated/uncorrelated scores

Null hypotheses are statements that there is no relationship between two variables. The two variables in question at the moment are the independent and dependent vari­ ables. This is another way of saying that there is no difference between the means of the two groups (i.e. columns) of scores. The simplest null hypotheses for the above three studies are: ●

There is no relationship between sexual activity and the number of erotic dreams that women have.



Managerial experience is not related to speed of complex decision-making.



The disciplinary style of a school is not related to the amount of bullying.

The alternative hypotheses to these null hypotheses can be obtained by simply deleting no or not from each of the above. Notice that the above way of writing the null hypothesis is relatively streamlined compared with what you often read in books and journals. So do not be surprised if you come across null hypotheses expressed in much more clumsy language such as: ●

Women who abstain from sex will have the same number of erotic dreams as women who are sexually active.



Erotic dreams do not occur at different frequencies in sexually active and sexually inactive women.

These two statements tend to obscure the fact that null hypotheses are fundamentally similar irrespective of the type of research under consideration. The erotic dreams experiment will be used to illustrate the theoretical issues. There are two different samples of scores defined by the independent variable – one for the sexually abstinent group and the other for the sexually active group. The scores in Table 14.4 are the numbers of sexual dreams that each woman in the study has in a seven-day period. We can see that, on average, the sexually active women have fewer erotic dreams. Does this reflect a generalisable (significant) difference? The data might be as in Table 14.4. Apart from suggesting that Wendy’s fantasy life is wonderful, the table indicates that sexual abstinence leads to an increase in erotic dreams. The null hypothesis suggests that the scores in the two samples come from the same population since it claims that there is no relationship between the independent and dependent variables. That is, for all intents and purposes, the two samples can be



Table 14.4

Participant

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 182

Possible data from the sexual activity and erotic dreams experiment (dreams per seven days) Sexually abstinent

Participant

Sexually active

Lindsay

6

Janice

2

Claudine

7

Jennifer

5

Sharon

7

Joanne

4

Natalie

8

Anne-Marie

5

Sarah

9

Helen

6

Wendy

10

Amanda

6

Ruth

8

Sophie

5

Angela

9

11/13/13 5:17 PM

14.2  Theoretical considerations 183

Table 14.5

Imaginary population of scores for erotic dreams study Experimental group Sexually abstinent

Sexually active Control group

8

3

6

6

7

6

8

4

6

7

7

7

7

7

4

9

5

9

6

8

5

8

9

7

2

7

10

5

4

6

2

7

6

7

3

5

10

8

6

5

9

6

7

7

7

4

8

6

5

7

construed as coming from a single population of scores; there is no difference between them due to the independent variable. Any differences between samples drawn from this null-hypothesis-defined population are due to chance factors rather than a true rela­ tionship between the independent and dependent variables. Table 14.5 is an imaginary population of scores from this null-hypothesis-defined population on the dependent variable ‘number of erotic dreams’. The table also indicates whether the score is that of a sexually abstinent woman or a sexually active one. If the two columns of scores are examined carefully, there are no differences between the two sets of scores. In other words, they have the same average scores. Statistically, all of the scores in Table 14.5 can be regarded as coming from the same population. There is no relationship between sexual activity and the number of erotic dreams. Given that the two samples (sexually abstinent and sexually active) come from the same population of scores on erotic dreams, in general we would expect no difference between pairs of samples drawn at random from this single population. Of course, samp­ ling always introduces a chance element so some pairs of samples would be different, but mostly the differences will cluster around zero. Overall, numerous pairs of samples will yield an average difference of zero. We are assuming that we consistently subtract the sexually active mean from the sexually abstinent mean (or vice versa – it does not matter so long as we always do the same thing) so that positive and negative differences cancel each other out. Since in this case we know the population of scores under the null hypothesis, we could pick out samples of 10 scores at random from the population to represent the sexually abstinent sample and, say, nine scores from the population to represent the sexually active sample. (Obviously the sample sizes will vary and they do not have to be equal.) Any convenient randomisation procedure could be used to select the samples (e.g. computer generated, random number tables or numbers drawn from a hat). The two samples selected at random, together with their respective means, are listed in Table 14.6.

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 183

11/13/13 5:17 PM

184

CHAPTER 14  The t-test: Comparing two samples of unrelated/uncorrelated scores





Table 14.6

Random samples of scores from population in Table 14.5 to represent experimental and control conditions Experimental group Sexually abstinent

Control group Sexually active

4

5

5

5

10

10

7

9

7

7

5

7

7

8

9

6

9

2 8

T1 = 7

T2 = 6.7

Examining Table 14.6, we can clearly see that there is a difference between the two sample means. This difference is 7.0 − 6.7 = 0.3. This difference between the two sample means has been obtained despite the fact that we know that there is no relationship between the independent variable and the dependent variable in the null-hypothesisdefined population. This is the nature of the random sampling process. We can repeat this experiment by drawing more pairs of samples of these sizes from the null-hypothesis-defined population. This is shown for 40 new pairs of variables in Table 14.7. Many of the differences between the pairs of means in Table 14.7 are very close to zero. This is just as we would expect since the independent and dependent variables are not related. Nevertheless, the means of some pairs of samples are somewhat different. In Table 14.7, 95% of the differences between the two means come in the range 0.922 to −1.400. (Given the small number of samples we have used, it is not surprising that this range is not symmetrical. If we had taken large numbers of samples, we would have expected more symmetry. Furthermore, had we used normally distributed scores, the symmetry may have been better.) The middle 95% of the distribution of differences between pairs of sample means are held clearly to support the null hypothesis. The extreme 5% beyond this middle range are held more likely to support the alternative hypothesis. The standard deviation of the 40 ‘difference’ scores gives the standard error of the differences. Don’t forget we are dealing with sample means so the term standard error is the correct one. The value of the standard error is 0.63. This is the ‘average’ amount by which the differences between sample means is likely to deviate from the population mean difference of zero.

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 184

11/13/13 5:17 PM

14.2  Theoretical considerations 185

Table 14.7

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 185

Forty pairs of random samples from the population in Table 14.5

Experimental group Sexually abstinent N = 10

Control group Sexually active N=9

Difference (column 1 – column 2)

6.100

6.444

−0.344

6.300

5.444

0.856

6.000

6.556

−0.556

6.400

6.778

−0.378

6.600

6.111

0.489

5.700

6.111

−0.411

6.700

6.111

0.589

6.300

5.667

0.633

6.400

6.667

−0.267

5.900

5.778

0.122

6.400

6.556

−0.156

6.360

6.444

−0.084

6.400

6.778

−0.378

6.200

6.222

−0.022

5.600

5.889

−0.289

6.100

6.222

−0.122

6.800

6.667

0.133

6.100

6.222

−0.122

6.900

6.000

0.900

7.200

5.889

1.311

5.800

7.333

−1.533

6.700

6.889

−0.189

6.200

6.000

0.200

6.500

6.444

0.056

5.900

6.444

−0.544

6.000

6.333

−0.333

6.300

6.778

−0.478

6.100

5.778

0.322

6.000

6.000

0.000

6.000

6.667

−0.667

6.556

6.778

−0.222

6.700

5.778

0.922

5.600

7.000

−1.400

6.600

6.222

0.378

5.600

6.667

−1.067

5.900

7.222

−1.322

6.000

6.667

−0.667

7.000

6.556

0.444

6.400

6.556

−0.156

6.900

6.222

0.678

11/13/13 5:17 PM

186

CHAPTER 14  The t-test: Comparing two samples of unrelated/uncorrelated scores

14.3 Standard deviation and standard error The trouble with all of the above is that it is abstract theory. Normally, we know nothing for certain about the populations from which our samples come. Fortunately, quite a lot can be inferred about the population given the null hypothesis and information from the samples: ●

Since the null hypothesis states that there is no relationship between the independent and dependent variables in the population, it follows that there should be no systematic difference between the scores in the pair of samples. That is, the average difference between the two means should be zero over many pairs of samples.



We can use the scores in a sample to estimate the standard deviation of the scores in the population. However, if we use our usual standard deviation formula the estimate tends to be somewhat too low. Consequently we have to modify our standard deviation formula (Chapter 6) when estimating the standard deviation of the population. The change is minimal – the N in the bottom half of the formula is changed to N − 1:

∑X

estimated standard deviation = ●

2

(∑ X ) −

N −1

2

N

The net effect of this adjustment is to increase the estimated standard deviation in the population – the amount of adjustment is greatest if we are working with small sample sizes for which subtracting 1 is a big adjustment.

But this only gives us the estimated standard deviation of the scores in the population. We really need to know about the standard deviation (i.e. standard error) of sample means taken from that population. Remember, there is a simple formula which converts the estimated standard deviation of the population to the estimated standard error of sample means drawn from that population: we simply divide the estimated standard deviation by the square root of the sample size. It so happens that the computationally most useful way of working out the standard error is as follows:

∑ X2 − standard error =

(∑ X )

N −1

2

N

N

Still we have not finished because this is the estimated standard error of sample means; we want the estimated standard error of differences between pairs of sample means. It makes intuitive sense that the standard error of differences between pairs of sample means is likely to be the sum of the standard errors of the two samples. After all, the standard error is merely the average amount by which a sample mean differs from the population mean of zero. So the standard error of the differences between pairs of sample means drawn from a population should be the two separate standard errors combined. Well, that is virtually the procedure. However, the two different standard errors (SE) are added together in a funny sort of way:

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 186

11/13/13 5:17 PM

14.3  Standard deviation and standard error 187

SE[ of differences between sample means ] =

(SE12 + S E22 )

Finally, because the sample sizes used to estimate the two individual standard errors are not always the same, it is necessary to adjust the equation to account for this, otherwise you end up with the wrong answer. The computational formula for the estimated standard error of differences between pairs of sample means is as follows: Standard error of differences between pairs of sample means

=

  ∑ X12 −  

(∑ X )

2

+ ∑X

1

2 2

N1 N1 + N 2 − 2

(∑ X ) −

2

2

N2

  1   1   N + N  1 2

Although this looks appallingly complicated, the basic idea is fairly simple. It looks complex because of the adjustment for different sample sizes. Now we simply use the t-test formula. The average difference between the pairs of sample means is zero assuming the null hypothesis to be true. The t formula is: t=

sample 1 mean − sample 2 mean − 0 standard error of differences between sample means

t=

differences between the two sample means − 0 standard error of differences between sample meanss

or

Since in the above formula the population mean of difference between pairs of sample means is always zero, we can omit it: t=

sample 1 mean − sample 2 mean standard error of differences between sample means

The formula expressed in full looks even more complicated: t=

X1 − X 2   ∑ X12 −  

(∑ X ) 1

2

+ ∑X

2 2

N1 N1 + N 2 − 2

(∑ X ) − 2

N2

2

  1   1   N + N  1 2

So t is the number of standard errors by which the difference between our two sample means differs from the population mean of zero. The distribution of t is rather like the distribution of z if you have a large sample – thus it approximates very closely the normal distribution. However, with smaller sample sizes the curve of t becomes increasingly flat and more spread out than the normal curve. Consequently we need different t-distributions for different sample sizes. Significance Table 14.1 gives values for the t-distributions. Notice that the distribution is dependent on the degrees of freedom which for this t-test is the total number of scores in the two samples combined minus 2.

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 187

11/13/13 5:17 PM

CHAPTER 14 The t-TeST: COmparIng TwO SampleS Of unrelaTed/unCOrrelaTed SCOreS

188

5% significance values of unrelated t (two-tailed test). appendix e gives a fuller and conventional version of this table

Significance Table 14.1

Degrees of freedom (always N − 2 for unrelated t-test)

Significant at 5% level Accept hypothesis

3

±3.18 or more extreme

4

±2.78 or more extreme

5

±2.57 or more extreme

6

±2.45 or more extreme

7

±2.37 or more extreme

8

±2.31 or more extreme

9

±2.26 or more extreme

10

±2.23 or more extreme

11

±2.20 or more extreme

12

±2.18 or more extreme

13

±2.16 or more extreme

14

±2.15 or more extreme

15

±2.13 or more extreme

18

±2.10 or more extreme

20

±2.09 or more extreme

25

±2.06 or more extreme

30

±2.04 or more extreme

40

±2.02 or more extreme

60

±2.00 or more extreme

100

±1.98 or more extreme



±1.96 or more extreme

your value must be in the listed ranges for your degrees of freedom to be significant at the 5% level (i.e. to accept the hypothesis). If your required degrees of freedom are not listed, then take the nearest smaller listed values. refer to appendix e if you need a precise value of t. ‘more extreme’ means that, for example, values in the ranges of +3.18 to infinity or −3.18 to (minus) infinity are statistically significant with 3 degrees of freedom.

Explaining statistics 14.1

how the unrelated t-test works The calculation of the unrelated t-test uses the following formula: t=

B1 − B 2   X2 − ∑ 1  

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 188

(∑ X ) 1

2

+ ∑ X 22 −

N1 N1 + N 2 − 2

(∑ X ) 2

N2

2

   1 1  +     N 1 N 2 

11/13/13 5:17 PM

14.3 STandard devIaTIOn and STandard errOr

Table 14.8

189

emotionality scores in two-parent and lone-parent families Two-parent family X1

Lone-parent family X2

12

6

18

9

14

4

10

13

19

14

8

9

15

8

11

12

10

11

13

9

15 16

Horrific, isn’t it? Probably the worst formula that you are likely to come across in psychological statistics. However, it contains little that is new. It is probably best to break the formula down into its component calculations and take things step by step. However, if you prefer to try to work directly with the above formula do not let us stand in your way. The data are from an imaginary study involving the emotionality of children from lone-parent and two-parent families. The independent variable is family type which has two levels – the lone-parent type and the two-parent type. The dependent variable is emotionality on a standard psychological measure – the higher the score on this test, the more emotional is the child. The data are listed in Table 14.8. A key thing to note is that we have called the scores for the two-parent family condition X1 and those for the loneparent family condition X2.

Step 1

Extend the data table by adding columns of squared scores and column totals as in Table 14.9. The sample size for X1 = N1 = 12; the sample size for X2 = N2 = 10. ∑X1 = sum of scores for two-parent family sample ∑X12 = sum of squared scores for two-parent family sample ∑X2 = sum of scores for lone-parent family sample ∑X22 = sum of squared scores for lone-parent family sample

Step 2

Do each of the following calculations. Calculation of A: A = B1 − B 2 =

∑ X1 − ∑ X 2 N1

N2

161 95 = − 12 10 = 13.417 − 9.500 = 3.917 917

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 189



11/13/13 5:17 PM

190

CHAPTER 14 The t-TeST: COmparIng TwO SampleS Of unrelaTed/unCOrrelaTed SCOreS

Table 14.8 extended to include steps in the calculation

Table 14.9

Two-parent family X1

Square previous column X 21

Lone-parent family X2

Square previous column X 22

12

144

6

36

18

324

9

81

14

196

4

16

10

100

13

169

19

361

14

196

8

64

9

81

15

225

8

64

11

121

12

144

10

100

11

121

13

169

9

81

15

225 ∑ X2 = 95

∑ X 22 = 989

16

256

∑ X1 = 161

∑ X = 2285 2 1

Calculation of B:

B=

(∑ X )

∑ X12 −

= 2285 −

2

1

N1 1612 25 921 = 2285 − 12 12 = 2285 − 21 160.0833 = 124.9167

Calculation of C:

C=

∑ X 22 −

= 989 −

(∑ X )

2

2

N2 2

95 9025 = 989 − 10 10 = 989 − 902.5 =8 86 6 .5

Calculation of D: D = N1 + N2 − 2 = 12 + 10 − 2 = 20

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 190

11/13/13 5:17 PM

14.3 STandard devIaTIOn and STandard errOr

191

Calculation of E: E=

1 1 1 1 + = + N 1 N 2 12 10

= 0.0833 + 0.1000 = 0.1833 Calculation of F:  B + C F=  ×E  D   124.9167 + 86.5000  =  × 0.1833 20   2 211 11.4167  =  × 0.1833 20   = 10.57083 × 0.1833 = 1.938 Calculation of G: G=

F = 1.938 = 1.392

Calculation of t: t=

Step 3

A 3.917 = = 2 .8 81 G 1.392

t is the t-score or the number of standard errors our sample data are away from the population mean of zero. We can use Significance Table 14.1 to check the statistical significance of our value of 2.81 by checking against the row for degrees of freedom (i.e. N1 + N2 − 2 = 20 degrees of freedom). This table tells us that our value of t is in the extreme 5% of the distribution because it is larger than 2.09; so we reject the null hypothesis that family structure is unrelated to emotionality. Our study showed that emotionality is significantly greater in the two-parent family structure as opposed to the lone-parent family structure.

Interpreting the results Remember to check carefully the mean scores for both groups in order to know which of the two groups has the higher scores on the dependent variable. In our example, this shows that the greater emotionality was found in the children from the two-parent families. The significant value of the t-test means that we are reasonably safe to conclude that the two groups do differ in terms of their emotionality.

reporting the results The statistical analysis could be reported in the following style: ‘It was found that emotionality was significantly higher (t = 2.81, df = 20, p < 0.05) in the two-parent families (B = 13.42) than in the lone-parent families (B = 9.50).’ The material in the final brackets simply reports the significance test used (the t-test), its value (2.81), the degrees of freedom (df = 20) and that the value of t is statistically significant (p < 0.05). Chapter 17 explains the approach to reporting the outcomes of statistical analyses in greater detail. Alternatively, following the recommendations of the APA (2010) Publication Manual, we could write the results as follows: ‘It was found that emotionality was significantly higher, t(20) = 2.81, p < 0.05, in the two-parent families (M = 13.42) than in the lone-parent families (M = 9.50).’

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 191

11/13/13 5:17 PM

192

CHAPTER 14  The t-test: Comparing two samples of unrelated/uncorrelated scores

Box 14.1

Focus on

Avoiding rounding errors When doing calculations of any sort by hand, there is a risk of inaccuracy if you use too few numbers after the decimal point. These inaccuracies are known as rounding errors. So you risk getting a somewhat different answer from that calculated by the computer. Generally speaking, you need to work to at least three decimal places on your calculator though the actual calculated figures given by the calculator

are best and easiest to use. Because of limitations of space and for clarity, the calculations reported in this book have been given to a small number of decimal places – usually three decimal places. When you report the results of the calculation, however, round the figure to no more than two decimal places. Remember to be consistent in the number of decimal places you present in your results.

14.4 Cautionary note You should not use the t-test if your samples are markedly skewed, especially if they are skewed in opposite directions. Appendix A explains how to test for skewness. You might consider using the Mann–Whitney U-test in these circumstances (Explaining statistics 19.3).

Research examples Unrelated/uncorrelated/independent sample t-test Centinkalp (2012) was interested in achievement goals in adolescent athletes. Participants were on average just over 16 years of age. The researcher compared male and female athletes on a range of achievement-related measures using unrelated t-tests. None of the comparisons was significant with the exception of the mastery avoidance scale which was significantly higher for the female athletes than for the male athletes. Mutsvunguma and Gwandure (2011) compared the psychological well-being of two groups of South African bank employees – those who handled cash versus those who did not. The measures included a Burnout Inventory and a Life Satisfaction scale. Each of these dependent variables was analysed separately using independent samples t-tests. The findings indicated that the two groups differed significantly in terms of the measures of stress and burnout used. Passmore and Rehman (2012) studied the way in which driving development could be enhanced using a coaching-based paradigm as opposed to an instruction-based approach. Participants were learning to drive large goods vehicles. Their methodology was basically a randomised controlled trial (experiment) though they did supplement this with semi-structured interviews and qualitative analysis which are not reported here. Participants were randomly allocated to one of the two learning conditions so there were different participants in each group. The first group was taught by instructors who were trained in coaching skills which involved a

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 192

11/13/13 5:17 PM

  Key points 193



mixture of coaching and instruction. The second group of participants was taught by driving instructors using exclusively an instruction-based approach much like a driving instructor at a driving school. The coaching approach sought to teach a wider variety of skills and abilities. For example, vehicle control ranges from the basic manual handling of the vehicle through driving in traffic to goals for life and skills for living. The data were analysed with independent samples t-tests using a variety of dependent variables. For example, the coaching group spent fewer hours in total in learning to drive (M = 21.43) whereas the control group spent 30.12 hours on average (t = 4.014, p < 0.01, p = 0.0005, one-tailed). Schulenberg and Yutrzenka (2001) researched whether a conventionally administered and a computerised version of the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) produced equivalent results. Their concern was that the then aversion to computers might affect responses to a computerised version of the scale. Although overall their research was a little more complicated than this, one of their primary analyses was to compare the results of those who received the conventional version of the Beck scale first with those who received the computerised version of the Beck scale first using an Independent Samples t-test. The results showed that the two versions were equivalent as with fairly substantial overall samples of 180, the t-test was not significant at the 5% level. Skipper and Douglas (2012) compared the effects of praise delivered using personal terms such as ‘you are clever’ with praise using process terms such as ‘you worked hard’ with an objective outcome control condition where only factual information was given. The different conditions were presented in the form of written scenarios. The child participants then read scenarios where they failed a task. Receiving personal praise resulted in the most negative responses in these circumstances. The researchers used the unrelated t-test to compare the children in the person condition with those in the process and control conditions in combination. This confirmed that the negative response was associated with the personal condition compared with the other two.

Key points ●

The t-test is commonly used in psychological research, so it is important that you have an idea of what it does. However, it is only a special case of the analysis of variance (Chapter 21) which is a much more flexible statistic. Given the analysis of variance’s ability to handle any number of samples, you might prefer to use it instead of the t-test in most circumstances. To complicate matters, some use the t-test in the analysis of variance.



The t-test assumes that the variances of the two samples are similar so that they can be combined to yield an overall estimate. However, if the variances of the two samples are significantly different from each other, you should not use this version of the t-test. The way to see if two variances are dissimilar is to use the variance ratio test described in Chapter 20.



If you wish to use the t-test but find that you fall foul of this F-ratio requirement, there is a version of the t-test which does not assume equal variances. The best way of doing such t-tests is to use a computer package which applies both tests to the same data. Unfortunately, the calculation for the degrees of freedom is a little complex (you can have decimals involved in the values) and it goes a little beyond reasonable hand calculations. The calculation details are provided in Blalock (1972).

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 193

11/13/13 5:17 PM

CHAPTER 14  The t-test: Comparing two samples of unrelated/uncorrelated scores

194

Computer analysis The unrelated t-test using SPSS

Figure 14.2

SPSS Statistics steps for the unrelated t-test

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

The means tell you which group has the highest scores. Normally the highest scores mean a higher amount of the variable in question. In this example, emotionality was found to be higher for the children of two-parent families. The significance level should be noted as this tells you whether the difference is likely to be the product of chance.



Reporting the results can following this pattern: ‘It was found that emotionality was significantly higher, t = 2.81, df = 20, p < .05 in the two-parent families (M = 13.42) than in the lone-parent families (M = 9.50).’

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 194

11/13/13 5:17 PM

  Computer analysis 195



Screenshot 14.1

The data

Screenshot 14.5

Main output

M14_HOWI0749_06_SE_C14.indd 195

Screenshot 14.2

Select the test

Screenshot 14.3

Select variables for the analysis

Screenshot 14.4

Define the two groups of scores

11/13/13 5:17 PM

Chapter 15

Chi-square Differences between samples of frequency data

Overview ● ● ●









Chi-square is used with nominal (category) data in the form of frequency counts. A minimum of two categories is involved. It tests whether the frequency counts in the various nominal categories could be expected by chance or whether there is a relationship. Chi-square is relatively uncommon in psychological research because psychological research usually uses score rather than category measures. However, in some circumstances its use is necessary. One-sample chi-square compares the frequencies obtained in each category with a known expected frequency distribution. Two-sample chi-square uses a crosstabulation or frequency table for two variables. This gives the frequencies in the various possible combinations of categories of these two variables. The disparity between the actual frequencies in the data and what the frequencies would be if the null hypothesis were true is at the heart of the calculation. The bigger the disparity, the bigger the value of chi-square and the more one’s findings are statistically significant. When the chi-square table has more than four cells (i.e. combinations of categories), interpretation becomes difficult. It is possible to subdivide a big table into a number of smaller chi-squares in order to facilitate interpretation. This is known as partitioning. Sometimes data may violate the mathematical foundations of chi-square too much. In these circumstances, the data may have to be modified to meet the mathematical requirements, or an alternative measure such as the Fisher exact test may be employed.

Preparation You should be familiar with crosstabulation and contingency tables (Chapter 7) and samples and populations (Chapter 10).

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 196

11/18/13 2:10 PM

15.1  Introduction 197

15.1 Introduction Often, chi-square is written as χ2. However, we have avoided Greek letters as far as possible. If a researcher has several samples of data which involve frequencies rather than scores, a statistical test designed for frequency data must be used. The following are some examples of research of this sort: ●

Male and female schoolchildren are compared in terms of wanting to be psychologists when they leave school (Table 15.1).



The sexual orientations of a sample of religious men are compared with those of a non-religious sample (Table 15.2).



Choosing to play with either a black or a white doll in black and white children (Table 15.3).

In each of these examples, both variables consist of a relatively small number of categories. In other words, schematically each study approximates to the form shown in Table 15.4 in which the independent variable is the sample and the dependent variable consists of one of several categories. The precise number of samples may vary from study to study and the number of categories of the dependent variable can be two or more. As a rule of thumb, it is better to have just a few samples and a few categories, since large tables can be difficult to interpret and generally require large numbers of participants or cases to be workable.

Table 15.1

Relationship between gender and wanting to be a psychologist

Intention

Male

Female

Wants to be a psychologist

f = 17

f = 98

Does not want to be a psychologist

f = 67

f = 35

Table 15.2

Relationship between sexual orientation and religion

Orientation

Religious

Non-religious

Heterosexual

57

105

Gay

13

27

8

17

Bisexual

Table 15.3 Choice

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 197

Relationship between doll choice and ethnicity Black child

White child

Mixed-parentage

Black doll

19

17

5

White doll

16

18

9

11/18/13 2:10 PM

198

CHAPTER 15  Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data

Table 15.4

Stylised table for chi-square

Category

Sample 1

Sample 2

Sample 3

Category 1

27

21

5

Category 2

19

20

19

Category 3

9

17

65

The ‘cells’ of Table 15.4 (called a crosstabulation or contingency table) contain the frequencies of individuals in that particular sample and that particular category. So the ‘cell’ that corresponds to sample 2 and category 3 contains the frequency 17. This means that in your data there are 17 cases in sample 2 which also fit category 3. In other words, a cell is the intersection of a row and a column. The statistical question is whether the distribution of frequencies in the different samples is so varied that it is unlikely that these all come from the same population. As ever, this population is the one defined by the null hypothesis (which suggests that there is no relationship between the independent and dependent variables).

15.2 Theoretical issues Imagine a research study in which children are asked to choose between two television programmes, one violent and the other non-violent. Some of the children have been in trouble at school for fighting and the others have not been in trouble. The researcher wants to know if there is a relationship between the violence of the preferred television programme and having been in trouble for fighting at school. The data might look something like Table 15.5. We can see from Table 15.5 that the fighters (sample 1) are more likely to prefer the violent programme and the non-fighters (sample 2) are more likely to prefer the nonviolent programme. The frequencies obtained in the research are known as the observed frequencies. This merely refers to the fact that we obtain them from our empirical observations (that is, the data). Assume that both of the samples come from the same population of data in which there is no relationship between the dependent and independent variables. This implies that any differences between the samples are merely due to the chance fluctuations of sampling. A useful index of how much the samples differ from each other is based on how different each sample is from the population distribution defined by the null hypo­ thesis. As ever, since we do not know the population directly in most research, we have to estimate its characteristics from the characteristics of samples.

Table 15.5

Relationship between preferred TV programme and fighting

Preference

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 198

Sample 1 Fighters

Sample 2 Non-fighters

Violent TV preferred

40

15

Non-violent TV preferred

30

70

11/18/13 2:10 PM

15.2  Theoretical issues 199



Table 15.6



Relationship between preferred TV programme and fighting including the marginal frequencies (column and row frequencies)

Preference

Sample 1 Fighters

Sample 2 Non-fighters

Row frequencies

Violent TV preferred

40

15

55

Non-violent TV preferred

30

70

100

Column frequencies

70

85

Overall frequency = 155

With the chi-square test, we simply add together the frequencies for whatever number of samples we have. These sums are then used as an estimate of the distribution of the different categories in the population. Since differences between the samples under the null hypothesis are solely due to chance factors, by combining samples the best possible estimate of the characteristics of the population is obtained. In other words, we simply add together the characteristics of two or more samples to give us an estimate of the population distribution of the categories. The first stage of doing this is illustrated in Table 15.6. So in the null-hypothesis-defined population, we would expect 55 out of every 155 to prefer the violent programme and 100 out of 155 to prefer the non-violent programme. But we obtained 40 out of 70 preferring the violent programme in sample 1, and 15 out of 85 preferring the violent programme in sample 2. How do these figures match the expectations from the population defined by the null hypothesis? We need to calculate the expected frequencies of the cells in Table 15.6. This calculation is based on the assumption that the null hypothesis population frequencies are our best information as to the relative proportions preferring the violent and non-violent programmes if there truly was no difference between the samples. Sample 1 contains 70 children. If the null hypothesis is true then we would expect 55 out of every 155 of these to prefer the violent programme. Thus our expected frequency of those preferring the violent programme in sample 1 is: 70 ×

55 = 70 × 0.355 = 24.84 155

Remember that these figures have been rounded for presentation and give a slightly different answer from that generated by a calculator. Similarly, since we expect under the null hypothesis 100 out of every 155 to prefer the non-violent programme, then our expected frequency of those preferring the non-violent programme in sample 1, out of the 70 children in that sample, is: 70 ×

100 = 70 × 0.645 = 45.16 155

Notice that the sum of the expected frequencies for sample 1 is the same as the number of children in that sample (24.84 + 45.16 = 70). We can apply the same logic to sample 2 which contains 85 children. We expect that 55 out of every 155 will prefer the violent programme and 100 out of every 155 will prefer the non-violent programme. The expected frequency preferring the violent programme in sample 2 is: 85 ×

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 199

55 = 85 × 0.355 = 30.18 155

11/18/13 2:10 PM

200

CHAPTER 15  Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data

Table 15.7

Contingency table including both observed and expected frequencies

Preference

Sample 1 Fighters

Sample 2 Non-fighters

Row frequencies

Violent TV preferred

observed frequency = 4 expected frequency = 24.84

observed frequency = 15 expected frequency = 30.16

55

Non-violent TV preferred

observed frequency = 30 expected frequency = 45.16

observed frequency = 70 expected frequency = 54.84

100

Column frequencies (i.e. sum of observed frequencies in column)

70

85

Overall frequencies = 155

The expected frequency preferring the non-violent programme in sample 2 is: 85 ×

100 = 85 × 0.645 = 54.83 155

We can enter these expected frequencies (population frequencies under the null hypo­ thesis) into our table of frequencies (Table 15.7). The chi-square statistic is based on the differences between the observed and the expected frequencies. It should be fairly obvious that the greater the disparity between the observed frequencies and the population frequencies under the null hypothesis, the less likely is the null hypothesis to be true. Thus if the samples are very different from each other, the differences between the observed and expected frequencies will be large. Chi-square involves calculating the overall disparity between the observed and expected frequencies over all the cells in the table. To be precise, the chi-square formula involves the squared deviations over the expected frequencies, but this is merely a slight diversion to make our formula fit a convenient statistical distribution which is called chi-square. The calculated value of chi-square is then compared with a table of critical values of chi-square (Significance Table 15.1) in order to estimate the probability of obtaining our pattern of frequencies by chance (if the null hypothesis of no differences between the samples was true). This table is organised according to degrees of freedom, which is always (number of columns of data − 1) × (number of rows of data − 1). This would be (2 − 1) × (2 − 1) or 1 for Table 15.7. Figure 15.1 gives the key steps when carrying out a chi-square test.

Box 15.1

Focus on

Yates’s correction Yates’s correction is a slightly outmoded statistical pro­ cedure when the expected frequencies in chi-square are small. This is intended to make such data fit the theoret­ ical chi-square distribution a little better. In essence, all you do is subtract 0.5 from each (observed frequency – expected frequency) in the chi-square formula prior to

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 200

squaring that difference. With large expected frequencies, this has virtually no effect. With small tables, it obviously reduces the size of chi-square and therefore its statistical significance. We have opted for not using it in our calculations. Really it is a matter of personal choice as far as convention goes.

11/18/13 2:10 PM

15.2  Theoretical issues 201

5% and 1% significance values of chi-square (two-tailed test). Appendix F gives a fuller and conventional version of this table

Significance Table 15.1

Degrees of freedom

Significant at 5% level Accept hypothesis

Significant at 1% level Accept hypothesis

1

3.8 or more

6.7 or more

2

6.0 or more

9.2 or more

3

7.8 or more

11.3 or more

4

9.5 or more

13.3 or more

5

11.1 or more

15.1 or more

6

12.6 or more

16.8 or more

7

14.1 or more

18.5 or more

8

15.5 or more

20.1 or more

9

16.9 or more

21.7 or more

10

18.3 or more

23.2 or more

11

19.7 or more

24.7 or more

12

21.0 or more

26.2 or more

Your value must be in the listed ranges for your degrees of freedom to be significant at the 5% level (column 2) or the 1% level (column 3) (i.e. to accept the hypothesis). Should you require more precise values than those listed below, these are to be found in the table in Appendix F.

Figure 15.1

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 201

Conceptual steps for understanding the chi-square test

11/18/13 2:10 PM

202

CHAPTER 15 ChI-Square: DIFFerenCeS BetWeen SampleS OF FrequenCY Data

Explaining statistics 15.1

how chi-square works The calculation of chi-square involves several relatively simple but repetitive calculations. For each cell in the chi-square table you calculate the following: (observed d ffrequencyy − eexpected frequency) 2 expected ffre requency The only complication is that this small calculation is repeated for each of the cells in your crosstabulation or contingency table. The formula in full becomes: chi-square =



(O − E) 2 E

where O = observed frequency and E = expected frequency. The following is an imaginary piece of research in which teenage boys and girls were asked to name their favourite type of television programme from a list of three: (1) soap operas, (2) crime dramas and (3) neither of these. The researcher suspects that gender may be related to programme preference (Table 15.8). We next need to calculate the expected frequencies for each of the cells in Table 15.8. One easy way of doing this is to multiply the row total and the column total for each particular cell and divide by the total number of observations (i.e. total frequencies). This is shown in Table 15.9.

Table 15.8

relationship between favourite type of tV programme and gender of respondent

Respondents

Soap opera

Crime drama

Neither

Totals

observed = 19

row 1 = 60

observed = 27

observed = 14

Females

observed = 17

observed = 33

observed = 9

row 2 = 59

Total

Column 1 = 44

Column 2 = 47

Column 3 = 28

Total = 119

males

Table 15.9

Calculation of expected frequencies by multiplying appropriate row and column totals and then dividing by overall total

Respondents males

Females

Total

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 202

Soap opera

Crime drama

Neither

observed = 27

observed = 14

observed = 19

expected = 60 × 44/119 = 22.185

expected = 60 × 47/119 = 23.698

expected = 60 × 28/119 = 14.118

observed = 17

observed = 33

observed = 9

expected = 59 × 44/119 = 21.815

expected = 59 × 47/119 = 23.303

expected = 59 × 28/119 = 13.882

Column 1 = 44

Column 2 = 47

Column 3 = 28

Total row 1 = 60

row 2 = 59

Total = 119

11/18/13 2:10 PM

15.2 theOretICal ISSueS

203

We then simply substitute the above values in the chi-square formula: chi-square =



(O − E 2 ) E

(27 − 22.185 . ) 2 (14 − 23.698 . 98) 2 (19 − 14.118) 2 + + 22.185 23.698 14.118

=

+

(17 − 21.185) 2 (33 − 23.303) 2 (9 − 1 13 3.882) 2 + + 21.81 15 5 23.303 13.882

=

4.8152 −9.6982 4.882 2 −4.8152 9.697 2 −4.882 2 + + + + + 22 2 2.185 23.698 14.118 21.815 23.303 13.882

=

23.184 94.05 51 1 23.834 23.184 94.032 23.834 + + + + + 22.185 23.698 14.118 21.815 23.303 13.882

= 1.045 + 3.969 + 1.688 + 1.063 + 4.035 + 1.717 = 13.52 The degrees of freedom are (the number of columns − 1) × (the number of rows − 1) = (3 − 1) × (2 − 1) = 2 degrees of freedom. We then check the table of the critical values of chi-square (Significance Table 15.1) in order to assess whether or not our samples differ among each other so much that they are unlikely to be produced by the population defined by the null hypothesis. The value must equal or exceed the tabulated value to be significant at the listed level of significance. Some tables will give you more degrees of freedom, but you will be hard pressed to do a sensible chi-square that exceeds 12 degrees of freedom.

Interpreting the results Our value of chi-square is well in excess of the minimum value of 6.0 needed to be significant at the 5% level for 2 degrees of freedom, so we reject the hypothesis that the samples came from the population defined by the null hypothesis. Thus we accept the hypothesis that there is a relationship between television programme preferences and gender. Only if you have a 2 × 2 chi-square is it possible to interpret the significance level of the chi-square directly in terms of the trends revealed in the data table. As we will see in Section 15.3, if we have a bigger chi-square than this (say 3 × 2 or 3 × 3) then a significant value of chi-square merely indicates that the samples are dissimilar to each other overall without stipulating which samples are different from each other. Because the sample sizes generally differ in contingency tables, it is helpful to convert the frequencies in each cell to percentages of the relevant sample size at this stage. It is important, though, never to actually calculate chi-square itself on these percentages as you will obtain the wrong significance level if you do. It seems from Table 15.10 that males prefer soap operas more often than females do, females have a preference for crime drama, and males are more likely than females to say that they prefer another type of programme. Unfortunately, as things stand we are not able to say which of these trends are statistically significant unless we partition the chi-square as described in Section 15.3.

reporting the results The results could be written up as follows: ‘The value of chi-square was 13.52 which was significant at the 5% level with 2 degrees of freedom. Thus there is a gender difference in favourite type of TV programme. Compared with

Table 15.10

Observed percentages in each sample based on the observed frequencies in table 15.8

Respondents

Soap opera

Crime drama

Neither

males

45.0%

23.3%

31.7%

Females

28.8%

55.9%

15.3%



M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 203

11/18/13 2:10 PM

204

CHAPTER 15  Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data

females, males were more likely to choose soap operas and less likely to choose crime dramas as their favourite programmes and more likely to prefer neither of these.’ However, as this table is bigger than a 2 × 2 table, it is advisable to partition the chi-square as discussed in Section 15.3 in order to say which of these trends are statistically significant. Alternatively, following the recommendations of the APA (2010) Publication Manual we could write: ‘There was a significant gender difference in favourite type of TV programme, χ2(2, N = 119) = 13.52, p < 0.05. Compared with females, males were more likely to choose soap operas and less likely to choose crime dramas as their favourite programmes and more likely to prefer neither of these.’ Chapter 17 explains how to report statistical significance in the shorter, professional way used in this version.

15.3 Partitioning chi-square There is no problem when the chi-square contingency table is just two columns and two rows. The chi-square in these circumstances tells you that your two samples are different from each other. Examine your contingency table to see just what the difference is. But if you have, say, a 2 × 3 chi-square (e.g. you have two samples and three categories) then there is some uncertainty as to what a significant chi-square means – does it mean that all three samples are different from each other, that sample 1 and sample 2 are different, that sample 1 and sample 3 are different, or that sample 2 and sample 3 are different? In the television programmes example, although we obtained a significant overall chisquare, there is some doubt as to why we obtained this. The major differences between the genders are between the soap opera and crime drama conditions rather than between the soap opera and the ‘other’ conditions. It is a perfectly respectable statistical procedure to break your large chi-square into a number of 2 × 2 chi-square tests to assess precisely where the significant differences lie. Thus in the TV programmes study you could generate three separate chi-squares from the 2 × 3 contingency table. These are illustrated in Table 15.11. These three separate chi-squares each have just one degree of freedom (because they are 2 × 2 tables). If you calculate chi-square for each of these tables you hopefully should be able to decide precisely where the differences are between samples and conditions. The only difficulty is the significance levels you use. Because you are doing three separate chi-squares, the normal significance level of 5% still operates, but it is divided between the three chi-squares you have carried out. In other words, we share the 5% between three to give us the 1.667% level for each – any of the three chi-squares would have to be significant at this level to be reported as being significant at the 5% level. Significance Table 15.2 gives the adjusted values of chi-square required to be significant at the 5% level (two-tailed test). Thus if you have three comparisons to make, the

Significance Table 15.2

Chi-square 5% two-tailed significance values for 1–10 unplanned comparisons

Degree of freedom

1

Number of comparisons being made 1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

3.84

5.02

5.73

6.24

6.64

6.96

7.24

7.48

7.69

7.88

To use this table, simply look under the column for the number of separate comparisons you are making using chi-square. Your values of chi-square must equal or exceed the listed value to be significant at the 5% level with a two-tailed test.

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 204

11/18/13 2:10 PM

15.5  Alternatives to chi-square 205

Table 15.11

Three partitioned sub-tables from the 2 × 3 contingency table (Table 15.8)

Soap opera versus crime drama Respondents

Soap opera

Crime drama

Totals

27

14

row 1 = 41

Males

17

33

row 2 = 50

Column 1 = 44

Column 2 = 47

Total = 91

Soap opera

Neither

Totals

Females Totals Soap opera versus neither Respondents Males

27

19

row 1 = 46

Females

17

 9

row 2 = 26

Column 1 = 44

Column 3 = 28

Total = 72

Crime drama

Neither

Totals

Totals Crime drama versus neither Respondents Males

14

19

row 1 = 33

Females

33

 9

row 2 = 42

Column 2 = 47

Column 3 = 28

Total = 75

Totals

minimum value of chi-square that is significant is 5.73. The degrees of freedom for these comparisons will always be 1 as they are always based on 2 × 2 contingency tables.

15.4 Important warnings Chi-square is rather less user friendly than is warranted by its popularity among psychologists. The following are warning signs not to use chi-square or to take very great care: ●

If the expected frequencies in any cell fall lower than 5 then chi-square becomes rather inaccurate. Some authors suggest that no more than one-fifth of values should be below 5, but this is a more generous criterion. Some computers automatically print an alternative to chi-square if this assumption is breached.



Never do chi-square on percentages or anything other than frequencies.



Always check that your total of frequencies is equal to the number of participants in your research. Chi-square should not be applied where participants in the research are contributing more than one frequency each to the total of frequencies.

15.5 Alternatives to chi-square The situation is only salvageable if your chi-square violates the expected cell frequencies rule – none should fall below 5. Even then you cannot always save the day. The alternatives are as follows:

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 205

11/18/13 2:10 PM

206

CHAPTER 15  Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data

Table 15.12





A 3 × 3 contingency table

Sample

Category 1

Category 2

Category 3

Sample 1

10

6

14

Sample 2

3

12

4

Sample 3

4

2

5

If you have a 2 × 2 or a 2 × 3 chi-square table then you can use the Fisher exact probability test which is not sensitive to small expected frequencies (see Explaining statistics 15.2 below). Apart from omitting very small samples or categories, sometimes you can save the day by combining samples and/or categories in order to avoid the small expected frequencies problem; by combining in this way, you should increase the expected frequencies somewhat. So, for example, take the data set out in Table 15.12. It should be apparent that by combining two samples and/or two categories you are likely to increase the expected frequencies in the resulting chi-square table.

But you cannot simply combine categories or samples at a whim – the samples or categories have to be combined meaningfully. So, if the research was on the relationship between the type of degree that students take and their hobbies, you might have the following categories and samples: category 1 – socialising category 2 – dancing category 3 – stamp collecting sample 1 – English literature students sample 2 – media studies students sample 3 – physics students Looking at these, it would seem reasonable to combine categories 1 and 2 and samples 1 and 2 since they seem to reflect rather similar things. No other combinations would seem appropriate. For example, it is hard to justify combining dancing and stamp collecting.

Explaining statistics 15.2

How the Fisher exact probability test works The Fisher exact probability test is not usually presented in introductory statistics books. We will only give the calculation of a 2 × 2 Fisher exact probability test although there is a version for 2 × 3 tables. The reason for its inclusion is that much student work for practicals and projects has very small sample sizes. As a consequence, the assumptions of the chi-square test are frequently broken. The Fisher exact probability test is not subject to the same limitations as the chi-square and can be used when chi-square cannot. It is different from chi-square in that it calculates the exact probability rather than a critical value. Apart from that, a significant result is interpreted much as the equivalent chi-square would be, so we will not explain it further.

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 206

11/18/13 2:10 PM

15.5 alternatIVeS tO ChI-Square

207

A number followed by ! is called a factorial. So 5! = 5 × 4 × 3 × 2 × 1 = 120. And 9! = 9 × 8 × 7 × 6 × 5 × 4 × 3 × 2 × 1 = 362 880. Easy enough but it can lead to rather big numbers which make the calculation awkward to handle. Table 15.13 lists factorials up to 15. The Fisher exact probability test is applied to a 2 × 2 contingency table by extending the table to include the marginal row and column totals of frequencies as well as the overall total (see Table 15.14). The formula for the exact probability is as follows: exact probability =

W ! X !Y ! Z ! N ! a!b!c!d !

Imagine you have collected data on a small group of exceptionally gifted children. You find that some have ‘photographic’ memories and others do not. You wish to know if there is a relationship between gender of subject and having a photographic memory (Table 15.15).

Table 15.13

Factorials of numbers from 0 to 15 Number

Factorial

0

Table 15.14

1

1

1

2

2

3

6

4

24

5

120

6

720

7

5 040

8

40 320

9

362 880

10

3 628 800

11

39 916 800

12

479 001 600

13

6 227 020 800

14

87 178 291 200

15

1 307 674 368 000

Symbols for the Fisher exact probability Column 1

Column 2

Row totals

row 1

a

b

W (= a + b)

row 2

c

d

X (= c + d)

Y (= a + c)

Z (= b + d)

Overall total = N

Column totals



M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 207

11/18/13 2:10 PM

CHAPTER 15  Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data

208

Table 15.15

Steps in calculating the Fisher exact probability

Respondents

Photographic memory

No photographic memory

Row totals

a=2

b=7

W (= a + b) = 9

Males Females Column totals

c=4

d=1

X (= c + d) = 5

Y (= a + c) = 6

Z (= b + d) = 8

Overall total = 14

Substituting in the formula gives: exact probability =

9 ! 5! 6 !8! 14 ! 2 ! 7 ! 4 !1!

The values of each of these factorials can be obtained from Table 15.13: exact probability =

362 880 × 120 × 720 × 40 320 87 178 291 20 00 × 2 × 5040 × 24 × 1

Unfortunately you will need a scientific calculator to do this calculation. The alternative is to cancel wherever possible numbers in the upper part of the formula with those in the lower part: exact probability =

9 ×8× 7 ×6 × 5× 4 ×3× 2 ×1× 5× 4 ×3× 2 ×1×6 × 5× 4 ×3× 2 ×1×8× 7 ×6 × 5× 4 ×3× 2 ×1 14 × 13 × 12 × 11 × 10 × 9 × 8 × 7 × 6 × 5 × 4 × 3 × 2 × 1 × 2 × 1 × 7 × 6 × 5 × 4 × 3 × 2 × 1 × 4 × 3 × 2 × 1 × 1

=

5×4×3×6×5×8 14 440 = = 0.060 14 × 13 × 12 × 11 × 10 × 1 240 240

The value of 0.06 is the probability of getting exactly two males in the photographic memory condition. This then is not the end of the calculation. The calculation also ought to take into account the more extreme outcomes which are relevant to the hypothesis. Basically the Fisher exact probability calculation works out (for any pattern of column and row totals) the probability of getting the obtained data or data more extreme than our obtained data. Table 15.16 gives all of the possible versions of the table if the marginal totals in Table 15.15 are retained along with the probability of each pattern calculated using the above formula. Notice that some patterns are not possible with our marginal totals, for example, one with 0 in the top left-hand cell. If 0 goes in that cell then we could only make the figures fit the marginal totals by using negative values. And that would be meaningless as one cannot have a negative case in a contingency table. The calculation of significance levels is as follows: ●

One-tailed significance is simply the sum of the probability of the actual outcome (0.060) plus the probability of any possible outcome which has a more extreme outcome in the predicted direction. That is, in the present example, 0.060 + 0.003 = 0.063. The precise number of values to be added will depend on a number of factors and will sometimes vary from our example according to circumstances.



Two-tailed significance is not accurately estimated by doubling the one-tailed probability for the simple reason that the distribution is not symmetrical. Instead, two-tailed significance is calculated by adding to the one-tailed prob­ ability (0.063) any probabilities at the other end of the distribution from the obtained distribution which are smaller than the probability of the obtained distribution. In our example, the probability of the distribution of data in Table 15.15 is 0.060. At the other end of the distribution, only one probability is equal or less than 0.060 – the final table has a probability of 0.028. We therefore add this 0.028 to the one-tailed probability of 0.063 to give a two-tailed probability of 0.091.

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 208

11/18/13 2:10 PM

15.5  Alternatives to chi-square 209

Table 15.16

All of the possible patterns of data keeping the marginal row and column totals unchanged from Table 15.15

1

8

5

0

p = 0.003* 2

7

4

1

p = 0.060* * The sum of the probabilities of these two tables is the one-tailed significance level. 3

6

3

1

p = 0.280 4

5

2

3

p = 0.420 5

4

1

4

p = 0.210 6

3

0

5

p = 0.028** ** The two-tailed probability level is the one-tailed probability calculated above plus the probability of this table. That is, the two-tailed probability is 0.063 + 0.028 = 0.091.

Our two-tailed probability value of 0.091 is not statistically significant at the conventional 5% level (neither would the one-tailed test if that were appropriate).

Interpreting the results The two-tailed significance level is 0.09 which is not statistically significant at the 0.05 (or 5%) level. Thus we cannot reject the null hypothesis that the incidence of photographic memory is related to gender. It would be useful to convert the frequencies in Table 15.15 into percentages of the relevant sample size when interpreting these data as we have different numbers of males and females. Such a table would show that 80% of the females had photographic memories but only 22% of the males. Despite this, with such a small amount of data, the trend is not statistically significant.

Reporting the results The following would be an appropriate description: ‘Although photographic memory was nearly four times more common in females than in males, this proved not to be statistically significant using the Fisher exact probability test. The exact probability was 0.09 which is not significant at the 0.05 level. Thus we must reject the hypothesis that photographic memory is related to gender.’ Alternatively, following the recommendations of the APA (2010) Publication Manual we could write something like: ‘Photographic memory was nearly four times more common in females than in males. However, the difference was not statistically significant, Fisher, p = 0.091. Thus we must reject the hypothesis that photographic memory is related to gender.’ This style of presenting statistical significance is explained in detail in Chapter 17.

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 209

11/18/13 2:10 PM

210

CHAPTER 15  Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data

Table 15.17

Stages in the calculation of a 2 × 3 Fisher exact probability test

Row 1 Row 2 Column totals



Column 1

Column 2

Column 3

Row totals

a

b

c

W (= a + b + c)

d

e

f

X (= d + e + f )

K (= a + d)

L (= b + e)

M (= c + f )

Overall total = N

■ Fisher exact probability test for 2 × 3 tables This is calculated in a very similar way as for the Fisher 2 × 2 test, the difference being simply the increased numbers of cells (Table 15.17). The formula for the 2 × 3 exact probability of the obtained outcome is as follows: exact probability =

W !X !K !L!M ! N ! a!b!c!d !e! f !

The calculation needs to be extended to cover all of the more extreme outcomes just as with the 2 × 2 version. Nevertheless, this is a very cumbersome calculation and best avoided by hand if possible.

15.6 Chi-square and known populations Sometimes, but rarely, in research we know the distribution in the population. If the population distribution of frequencies is known then it is possible to employ the singlesample chi-square. Usually the population frequencies are known as relative frequencies or percentages. So, for example, if you wished to know the likelihood of getting a sample of 40 university psychology students in which there are 30 female and 10 male students if you know that the population of psychology students is 90% female and 10% male, you simply use the latter proportions to calculate the expected frequencies of females and males in a sample of 40. If the sample were to reflect the population then 90% of the 40 should be female and 10% male. So the expected frequencies are 40 × 90 ÷ 100 for females and 40 × 10 ÷ 100 for males = 36 females and 4 males. These are then entered into the chi-square formula, but note that there are only two cells. The degrees of freedom for the one-sample chi-square is the number of cells minus 1 (i.e. 2 − 1 = 1).

Explaining statistics 15.3

How the one-sample chi-square works The research question is whether a sample of 80 babies of a certain age in foster care show the same level of smiling to their carer as a population of babies of the same age assessed on a developmental test. On this developmental test, 50% of babies at this age show clear evidence of the smiling response, 40% clearly show no evidence, and for 10% it is impossible to make a judgement. This is the population from which the foster babies are considered to be a sample. It

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 210

11/18/13 2:10 PM

15.6 ChI-Square anD KnOWn pOpulatIOnS

211

Data for a one-sample chi-square

Table 15.18

Clear smilers

Clear non-smilers

Impossible to classify

Observed frequency

35

40

5

expected frequency

40

32

8

is found that 35 clearly showed evidence of smiling, 40 showed no clear evidence of smiling and the remaining 5 were impossible to classify (Table 15.18). We can use the population distribution to work out the expected frequency in the sample of 80 if this sample precisely matched the population. Thus 50% of the 80 (= 40) should be clear smilers, 40% of the 80 (= 32) should be clear non-smilers, and 10% of the 80 (= 8) should be impossible to classify. Table 15.18 gives the expected frequencies (i.e. population based) and observed frequencies (i.e. sample based). These observed and expected frequencies are entered into the usual chi-square formula. The only difference is that the degrees of freedom are not quite the same – they are the number of conditions minus 1 (i.e. 3 − 1 = 2 in the above example): chi-square =



(O − E) 2 E

=

(35 − 40) 2 (40 − 32) 2 (5 − 8) 2 + + 40 32 8

=

(−5) 2 82 (−3) 2 + + 40 32 8

=

25 64 9 + + 40 32 8

= 0.625 + 2.0 000 + 1.125 = 3.7 75 But from Significance Table 15.1 we can see that this value of chi-square is far below the critical value of 6.0 required to be significant at the 5% level. Thus the sample of foster babies is not significantly different from the population of babies in terms of their smiling response.

Interpreting the results A significant value of the one-sample chi-square means that the distribution over the various categories departs markedly from that of the known population. That is, the sample is significantly different from the population and is unlikely to come from that population. In our example, however, the sample does not differ significantly from the population. This shows that smiling behaviour in our sample of babies is no different from that of the population of babies. For the one-sample chi-square, it is sufficient to compare the observed frequencies with the expected frequencies (which are the population values). In our example, there seems to be little difference between the sample and the population values.

reporting the results The following would summarise the findings of this study effectively: ‘It was possible to compare smiling behaviour in babies in foster care with population values of known smiling behaviour on a standard developmental test. A onesample chi-square test yielded a chi-square value of 3.75 which was not statistically significant with two degrees of freedom. Thus it can be concluded that the fostered babies were no different in terms of smiling behaviour from the general population of babies of this age.’ Alternatively, following the recommendations of the APA (2010) Publication Manual we could write: ‘It was found that smiling behaviour in babies in foster care was not different from population figures obtained from a standard developmental test, χ2(2, N = 80) = 3.75, pns. Thus it can be concluded that the fostered babies were no different in terms of smiling behaviour from the general population of babies of this age.’ This style of reporting statistical significance is discussed in greater detail in Chapter 17.

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 211

11/18/13 2:10 PM

212

CHAPTER 15  Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data

15.7 Chi-square for related samples – the McNemar test It is possible to use chi-square to compare related samples of frequencies. Essentially, this involves arranging the data in such a way that the chi-square contingency table only includes two categories: those that change from the first to the second occasion. For example, data are collected on whether or not teenage students wish to go to university; following a careers talk favouring university education the same informants are asked again whether they wish to go to university. The data can be tabulated as in Table 15.19. We can see from this table that although some students did not change their minds as a consequence of the talk (30 wanted to go to university before the talk and did not change their minds, 32 did not want to go to university before the talk and did not change their minds), some students did change. Fifty changed their minds and wanted to go to university following the talk and 10 changed their minds and did not want to go to university after the talk. The McNemar test simply uses the data on those who changed; non-changers are ignored. The logic of the test is that if the talk did not actually affect the teenagers, just as many would change their minds in one direction after the talk as change their minds in the other direction. That is, 50% should change towards wanting to go to university and 50% should change against wanting to go to university, if the talk had no effect. We simply create a new table (Table 15.20) which only includes changers and calculate chisquare on the basis that the null hypothesis of no effect would suggest that 50% of the changers should change in each direction. The calculation is now exactly like that for the one-sample chi-square. This gives us a chi-square value of 25.35 with one degree of freedom (since there are two conditions). This is very significant when checked against the critical values in Significance Table 15.1. Thus there appears to be more change towards wanting to go to university following the careers talk than change towards not wanting to go to university.

Table 15.19

Illustrative data for the McNemar test Before talk ‘yes’

Before talk ‘no’

After talk ‘yes’

30

50

After talk ‘no’

10

32

Table 15.20

Table of those who changed in a positive or negative direction based on Table 15.19 Positive changers

Negative changers

Observed frequency

50

10

Expected frequency

30

30

15.8 Example from the literature In a study of the selection of prison officers, Crighton and Towl (1994) found the relationship shown in Table 15.21 between the ethnicity of the candidate and whether or not they were selected during the recruitment process.

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 212

11/18/13 2:10 PM

15.8  Example from the literature 213

Table 15.21

Relationship between ethnicity and selection Selected

Not selected

1

3

17

45

Ethnic minority Ethnic majority

Chi-square = 0.43; p = ns

The interpretation of this table is that there is no significant relationship (p = ns) between selection and ethnicity. In other words, the table does not provide evidence of a selection bias in favour of white applicants, for example. While this is not an unreasonable conclusion based on the data if we ignore the small numbers of ethnic minority applicants, the statistical analysis itself is not appropriate. In particular, if you calculate the expected frequencies for the four cells you will find that 50% of the expected frequencies are less than 5, and thus a rule has been violated. The Fisher exact probability test would be better for these data.

Research examples Chi-square and Fisher’s exact probability test Remember that these tests require nominal category variables. Hughes and Trafimow (2012) were interested in the extent to which attributions of intentionality are influenced by motive and character of the person doing the act. Their prediction was that character can influence intentionality judgements. Participants read a scenario concerning a doctor who was described positively, negatively or neutrally. His motive was a positive one – he was seeking a cure for cancer but that his attempts had the side effect of increasing the risk of viral infections. In effect, the design involved two levels of consequence (good versus bad) and three levels of character (positive, negative or neutral). After reading the scenario, participants had to decide whether the doctor had deliberately caused the viral infection. The researchers used chi-square to examine the pattern of intentionality attributions but do not report the findings of the overall chi-square. Because of the precise predictions they had made, the researchers partitioned the chi-square in order to see whether their specific predictions were supported. As predicted, the positive target person with the good side effect produced high levels of intentionality but the researchers found no significant differences for the good side effects or bad side effects for the negatively described version. Huisman and her colleagues (2010) investigated whether psychiatric diagnosis, gender and status as in- or out-patient were more likely to kill themselves using a particular method. Initially they examined the relation between suicide method and each of these three variables separately using chi-square. Six categories of suicide methods were used. Each of these chi-squares was significant. So, for the chi-square for gender and suicide method, significantly more male patients (41%) hanged themselves than female patients (26%). Significantly more female patients (27%) poisoned themselves than male patients (12%). Kogan (2004) examined factors that predicted disclosure in women who had unwanted sexual experiences in their childhood or adolescence. The dependent variables were the timing of disclosure and the person disclosed to. Timing of disclosure consisted of the three categories of immediate, delayed and non-disclosure. The



M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 213

11/18/13 2:10 PM

214

CHAPTER 15  Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data

categories of the sorts of person disclosed to were adult, peers only and non-disclosure. Predictors of these two dependent variables included age at which the experience first occurred. The person disclosed to was then recategorised by the researcher into four groups – whether the person knew the other person, whether they were family and so on. Initially, chi-square tests were carried out between each of the dependent variables and each of the predictor variables. There were a number of significant findings. For example, whether the experience was with a family member was significantly related to both dependent variables. Women who had the experience with a family member were less likely to disclose or disclose immediately and less likely to disclose to peers only. Kois, Pearson, Chauhan, Goni and Saraydarian (2013) wished to explore whether the research on competency to stand trial among male inpatients extended to female inpatients. They used chi-square to look for significant relationships between findings of incompetence and other variables. They found significant associations, for example, between incompetence (versus competence) and the nominal category variables of active psychotic symptoms, diagnosis of a psychotic disorder, noncompliance with medication, and non-felony charges and competency. Matthews and co-workers (2012) studied theory of mind in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). ASD typically involves restricted repetitive behaviour patterns and impairments in interpersonal communication and social interaction. Deficits in theory of mind have been held to characterise autistic children though some researchers point out that such deficits are not unique to such children and that it is not universal in autistic children. In the study, the ability to infer the mental states of others (theory of mind) was compared for youngsters with early-onset autism and those with regressive autism with ‘normally’ developing youngsters. Using the Fisher exact test, the children were allocated to pass or non-pass groups on different measures of theory of mind deficits and the various groups compared with each other. The results showed among other things that high levels of theory of mind scores (a non-verbal appearance reality task) were more common in the normally developing group compared with the early-onset group. This task involved children to identify objects which superficially looked different from what they were. For example, the object might be a candle which looked like a crayon. McKiernan and his colleagues (2010) were interested in determining the effectiveness of a cognitivebehavioural group intervention for patients with early breast cancer compared with a control group which received an educational programme. As part of their assessment of these two groups, they asked patients whether or not they used any health services other than their doctor and whether they had attended all specialist oncology appointments during the six-month follow-up period. They used chi-square to analyse the results for these two questions separately. There were no significant differences between the two groups on these two questions.

Key points ●

Avoid as far as possible designing research with a multiplicity of categories and samples for chi-square. Large chi-squares with many cells are often difficult to interpret without numerous sub-analyses.



Always make sure that your chi-square is carried out on frequencies and that each participant contributes only one to the total frequencies.



Check for expected frequencies under 5; if you have any then take one of the escape routes described if possible.

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 214

11/18/13 2:10 PM

Computer analysis 215



Computer analysis Chi-square using SPSS

Figure 15.2

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 215

SPSS Statistics for chi-square

11/18/13 2:10 PM

CHAPTER 15  Chi-square: Differences between samples of frequency data

216

Interpreting and reporting the output There are two alternative ways of describing these results for the data in Table 15.8 and the output shown in Screenshot 15.5. To the inexperienced eye they may seem very different but they amount to the same thing: ●

We could describe the results in the following way: ‘There was a significant difference between the observed and expected frequency of teenage boys and girls in their preference for the three types of television programme, χ2(2) = 13.51, p = .001.’



Alternatively, and just as accurate: ‘There was a significant association between gender and preference for different types of television programme, χ2(2) = 13.51, p = .001.’



In addition, we need to report the direction of the results. One way of doing this is to state that: ‘Girls were more likely than boys to prefer crime programmes and less likely to prefer soap operas or both programmes.’



With greater than 2 × 2 tables as in this case, it is most probably worthwhile presenting a table of the frequencies.

Screenshot 15.1

Part of the data for two variables

Screenshot 15.3

Select two-way chi-square

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 216

Screenshot 15.2

Select one-way chi-square



Select expected frequencies and unstandardized residuals

Screenshot 15.4



11/18/13 2:10 PM

Recommended further reading 217



Screenshot 15.5

A two-way chi-square output

Recommended further reading Maxwell, A.E. (1961). Analysing qualitative data. London: Methuen.

M15_HOWI0749_06_SE_C15.indd 217

11/18/13 2:10 PM

Chapter 16

Probability

Overview ●

Although probability theory is at the heart of statistics, in practice the researcher needs to know relatively little of this.



The addition rule basically suggests that the probability of, say, any of three categories occurring is the sum of the three individual probabilities for those categories.



The multiplication rule suggests that the probability of different events occurring in a particular sequence is the product of the individual probabilities.

Preparation General familiarity with previous chapters.

M16_HOWI0749_06_SE_C16.indd 218

11/13/13 5:18 PM

16.2  The principles of probability 219

16.1 Introduction From time to time, researchers need to be able to calculate the probabilities associated with certain patterns of events. One of us remembers being a student in a class that carried out an experiment based on newspaper reports of a Russian study in which people appeared to be able to recognise colours through their fingertips. So we designed an experiment in which a blindfolded person felt different colours in random order. Most of us did not do very well but some in the class seemed excellent. The media somehow heard about the study and a particularly good identifier in our experiment quickly took part in a live TV demonstration of her skills. She was appallingly bad at the task this time. The reason why she was bad on television was that she had no special skills in the first place. It had been merely a matter of chance that she had done well in the laboratory. On the television programme, chance was not on her side and she turned out to be as bad as the rest of us. Actually, this reflects a commonly referred to phenomenon called regression to the mean. Choose a person (or group) because of their especially high (or, alternatively, especially low) scores and they will tend to score closer to the mean on the next administration of the test or measurement. This is because the test or measure is to a degree unreliable and by choosing exceptional scores you have to an extent capitalised on chance factors. With a completely unreliable test or measure, the reversion towards the mean will be dramatic. In our colour experiment the student did badly on TV because she had been selected totally on the basis of a criterion that was fundamentally unreliable – that is, completely at random. Similar problems occur in any investigation of individual paranormal or psychic powers. For example, a spiritual medium who addresses a crowd of 500 people is doing nothing spectacular if in Britain she claims to be speaking to a dead relative of someone and that relative is Mary or Martha or Margaret. The chances of someone in the 500 having such a relative are very high.

16.2 The principles of probability When any of us use a test of significance we are utilising probability theory. This is because most statistical tests are based on it. Our working knowledge of probability in most branches of psychology does not have to be very great for us to function well. We have been using probability in previous chapters on significance testing when we talked about the 5% level of significance, the 1% level of significance and the 95% confidence intervals. Basically what we meant by a 5% level of significance is that a particular event (or outcome) would occur on five occasions out of 100. Although we have adopted the percentage system of reporting probabilities in this book, statisticians would normally not write of a 5% probability. Instead they would express it as being out of a single event rather than 100 events. Thus: ●

0.05 (or just .05) is an alternative way of writing 5%



0.10 (or .10) is an alternative way of writing 10%



1.00 is an alternative way of writing 100%.

The difficulty for some of us with this alternative, more formal, way of writing probability is that it leaves everything in decimals, which does not appeal to the less

M16_HOWI0749_06_SE_C16.indd 219

11/13/13 5:18 PM

220

CHAPTER 16  Probability

mathematically skilled. However, you should be aware of the alternative notation since it appears in many research reports. Furthermore, much computer output can give probabilities to several decimal places which can be confusing. For example, what does a probability of 0.000  01 mean? The answer is one chance in 100  000 or a 0.001%   1 probability  × 100 = 0.001% . 1000 000   There are two rules of probability with which psychologists ought to be familiar. They are the addition rule and the multiplication rule. ●

The addition rule is quite straightforward. It merely states that for a number of mutually exclusive outcomes the sum of their probabilities adds up to 1.00. So if you have a set of 150 people of whom 100 are women and 50 are men, the probability of picking a woman at random is 100 ÷ 150 or 0.667. The probability of picking a man at random is 50/150 or 0.333. However, the probability of picking either a man or a woman at random is 0.667 + 0.333 or 1.00. In other words, it is certain that you will pick either a man or a woman. The assumption is that the categories or outcomes are mutually exclusive, meaning that a person cannot be in both the man and woman categories. Being a man excludes that person from also being a woman. In statistical probability theory, one of the two possible outcomes is usually denoted p and the other is denoted q, so p + q = 1.00. Outcomes that are not mutually exclusive include, for example, the categories man and young since a person could be a man and young.



The multiplication rule is about a set of events. It can be illustrated by our set of 150 men and women, in which 100 are women and 50 are men. Again the assumption is that the categories or outcomes are mutually exclusive. We could ask how likely it is that the first five people that we pick at random will all be women, given that the probability of choosing a woman on a single occasion is 0.667. The answer is that we multiply the probability associated with the first person being a woman by the probability that the second person will be a woman by the probability that the third person will be a woman by the probability that the fourth person will be a woman by the probability that the fifth person will be a woman: Probability of all five being women = p × p × p × p × p = 0.667 × 0.667 × 0.667 × 0.667 × 0.667 = 0.13 Therefore there is a 13% probability (0.13) that we will choose a sample of five women at random. That is not a particularly rare outcome. However, picking a sample of all men from our set of men and women is much rarer: Probability of all five being men = p × p × p × p × p = 0.333 × 0.333 × 0.333 × 0.333 × 0.333 = 0.004 Therefore there is a 0.4% probability (0.004) of choosing all men. The multiplication rule as stated here assumes that once a person is selected for inclusion in the sample, he or she is replaced in the population and possibly selected again. This is called random sampling with replacement. However, normally we do not do this in psychological research, though if the population is big then not replacing the individual back into the population has negligible influence on the outcome. Virtually all statistical analyses assume replacement, but it does not matter that people are usually not selected more than once for a study in psychological research.

M16_HOWI0749_06_SE_C16.indd 220

11/13/13 5:18 PM

16.3  Implications 221

16.3 Implications Such theoretical considerations concerning probability theory have a number of implications for research. They ought to be carefully noted. ●

Repeated significance testing within the same study  It is tempting to carry out several statistical tests on data. Usually we find that a portion of these tests are statistically significant at the 5% level whereas a number are not. Indeed, even if there were absolutely no trends in the population, we would expect, by chance, 5% of our comparisons to be significant at the 5% level. This is the meaning of statistical significance, after all. The more statistical comparisons we make on our data the more significant findings we would expect. If we did 20 comparisons we would expect one significant finding even if there are no trends in the population. In order to cope with this, the correct procedure is to make the statistical significance more stringent the more tests of significance we do. So, if we did two tests then our significance level per test should be 5%/2 or 2.5%; if we did four comparisons our significance level would be 5%/4 or 1.25% significance per test. In other words, we simply divide the 5% significance level by the number of tests we are doing. Although this is the proper thing to do, few psychological reports actually do it. However, the consequence of not doing this is to find more significant findings than you should.



Significance testing across different studies An application of the multiplication rule in assessing the value of replicating research shows the dramatic increase in significance that this can achieve. Replication means the essential repeating of a study at a later date and possibly in radically different circumstances such as other locations. Imagine that the significance level achieved in the original study is 5% (p = 0.05). If one finds the same significance level in the replication, the probability of two studies producing this level of significance by chance is p × p or 0.05 × 0.05 = 0.0025 or 0.25%. This considerably enhances our confidence that the findings of the research are not the result of chance factors but reflect significant trends.

Explaining statistics 16.1

The addition rule A psychologist wishes to calculate the chance expectations of marks on a multiple choice test of general knowledge. Since a person could get some answers correct simply by sticking a pin into the answer paper, there has to be a minimum score below which the individual is doing no better than chance. If each question has four response options then one would expect that by chance a person could get one in four or one-quarter of the answers correct. That is intuitively obvious. But what if some questions have three possible answers and others have four? This is not quite so obvious, but we simply apply the law of addition and add together the probabilities of being correct for all of the questions on the paper. This entails adding together probabilities of 0.33 and 0.25 since there are three or four possible answers. So if there are 10 questions with three possible answers and five questions with four possible answers, the number of answers correct by chance is (10 × 0.33) + (5 × 0.25) = 3.3 + 1.25 = 4.55.

M16_HOWI0749_06_SE_C16.indd 221

11/13/13 5:18 PM

222

CHAPTER 16  Probability

Explaining statistics 16.2

The multiplication rule A psychologist studies a pair of male twins who have been brought up separately and who have never met. The psychologist is surprised to find that the twins are alike on seven out of ten different characteristics, as presented below. The probability of their characteristics occurring in the general population is given in brackets: 1. They both marry women younger than themselves (0.9). 2. They both marry brunettes (0.7). 3. They both drive (0.7). 4. They both swim (0.6). 5. They have both spent time in hospital (0.8). 6. They both take foreign holidays (0.5). 7. They both part their hair on the left (0.9). However, they are different in the following ways: 8. One attends church (0.4) and the other does not. 9. One has a doctorate (0.03) and the other does not. 10. One smokes (0.3) and the other does not. The similarities between the two men are impressive if it is exceptional for two randomly selected men to be similar on each of the items. As stated above, the probabilities in brackets are the proportions of men in the general population demonstrating these characteristics. For many of the characteristics it seems quite likely that they will be similar. So two men taken at random from the general population are most likely to marry a younger woman. Since the probability of marrying a younger woman is 0.9, the probability of any two men marrying younger women is 0.9 × 0.9 = 0.81. The probability of two men taken at random both being drivers is 0.7 × 0.7 = 0.49. In fact the ten characteristics listed above are shared by randomly selected pairs of men with the following probabilities: 1. 0.9 × 0.9 = 0.81 2. 0.7 × 0.7 = 0.49 3. 0.7 × 0.7 = 0.49 4. 0.6 × 0.6 = 0.36 5. 0.8 × 0.8 = 0.64 6. 0.5 × 0.5 = 0.25 7. 0.9 × 0.9 = 0.81 8. 0.4 × 0.4 = 0.16 9. 0.03 × 0.03 = 0.0009 10. 0.3 × 0.3 = 0.09 The sum of these probabilities is 4.10. Clearly the twins are more alike than we might expect on the basis of chance. However, it might be that we would get a different answer if instead of taking the general population of men, we took men of the same age as the twins.

M16_HOWI0749_06_SE_C16.indd 222

11/13/13 5:18 PM

Key points 223



Key points ●

Although probability theory is of crucial importance for mathematical statisticians, psychologists generally rely on an intuitive approach to the topic. This may be laziness on their part, but we have kept the coverage of probability to a minimum given the scope of this book. It can also be very deterring to anyone not too mathematically inclined. If you need to know more, especially if you need to estimate precisely the likelihood of a particular pattern or sequence of events occurring, we suggest that you consult books such as Kerlinger (1986) for more complete accounts of mathematical probability theory.



However, it is important to avoid basic mistakes such as repeated significance testing on the same data without adjusting your significance levels to allow for the multitude of tests. This is not necessary for tests designed for multiple testing such as those for the analysis of variance, some of which we discuss later (Chapter 24), as the adjustment is built in.

M16_HOWI0749_06_SE_C16.indd 223

11/13/13 5:18 PM

Chapter 17

Reporting significance levels succinctly

Overview ●

A glance at reports in psychology journals suggests that relatively little space is devoted to reporting the outcomes of statistical analysis.



Usually, authors report their findings using very succinct methods which occupy very little space.



Normally the test statistic, the sample size (or degrees of freedom), significance level and whether it is a one-tailed test are reported.



It is recommended that students adopt this succinct style of reporting for their research as it will result in a more professional-looking product.



The American Psychological Association (APA) has produced guidelines for how to report statistical findings in its journals. These are also recommended for British psychological journals.



In this chapter we will discuss the wide variety of styles which have appeared in the research literature over the years as well as the up-to-date APA version.

Preparation You need to know about testing significance, from Chapter 12 onwards.

M17_HOWI0749_06_SE_C17.indd 224

11/18/13 12:03 PM

17.2  Shortened forms 225

17.1 Introduction So far, the reporting of statistical significance in this book has been a relatively clumsy and long-winded affair. In contrast, a glance at any psychology journal will suggest that precious little space is devoted to reporting significance. Detailed expositions of the statistical significance of your analyses have no place in professional reports. Researchers can make life much simpler for themselves by adopting a standard style for reporting statistical significance. Clarity is one great benefit; another is the loss of wordiness. The one slight problem is that the standard style for reporting statistical significance not only varies from context to context but has a tendency to change over time. For these reasons, we will look at a variety of ways of reporting statistical significance but provide detail for that recommended by the American Psychological Association (APA). This is used for APA published journals as well as those published by the British Psychological Society (BPS). You should check locally what the preferred style is for your university since this may differ from the APA method. Although approaches to reporting statistical significance do vary to some extent, generally this will cause you few difficulties. At a minimum, the following should be mentioned when reporting statistical significance: ●

The statistical distribution used (F, chi-square, r, z, t, etc.).



The degrees of freedom (df). Alternatively, for some statistical techniques you may report the sample size (N).



The value of the calculation (e.g. the value of your z-score or your chi-square).



The probability or significance level. Sometimes ‘not significant’, ‘not sig.’ or ‘ns’ is used.



If you have a one-tailed hypothesis then this should be also mentioned. Otherwise a two-tailed hypothesis is assumed. You can also state that you are using a two-tailed test. This is most useful when you have several analyses and some are one-tailed and others are two-tailed.

This list may not be complete as some systems encourage the user to include things like confidence intervals (Chapter 38) and effect size (Chapter 35). However, the basics are generally very clear.

17.2 Shortened forms In research reports, comments such as the following are to be found: ●



There was a trend for drunks to slur their words more than sober people (t = 2.88, df = 97, significance = 1%).



The null hypothesis that drunks do not slur their words more than sober people was rejected (t = 2.88, degrees of freedom = 97, p = 0.01).



The analysis supported the hypothesis since drunks tended to slur their words the most often (t(97) = 2.88, p = 0.01, two-tailed test).



M17_HOWI0749_06_SE_C17.indd 225

The hypothesis that drunks slur their words was supported (t = 2.88, degrees of freedom = 97, p < 0.01).

The hypothesis that drunks slur their words was accepted (t(97) = 2.88, p < 0.005, 1-tail).

11/18/13 12:03 PM

226

CHAPTER 17  Reporting significance levels succinctly

All of the above say more or less the same thing. We have used a variety of styles just to prepare you for the inconsistency that can be found. The symbol t indicates that the t-test was used. The symbol < indicates that your probability level is smaller than the given value. Thus p < 0.01 could mean that the probability is, say, 0.008 or 0.005. That is, the test is statistically significant at better than the reported level of 0.01. Sometimes, the degrees of freedom are put in brackets after the symbol for the statistical test used, as in t(97) = 2.88. In all of the above examples, the hypothesis was supported and the null hypothesis rejected. The following are examples of what might be written if the hypothesis was not supported by your data: ●

The hypothesis that drunks slur their words was rejected (t = 0.56, degrees of freedom = 97, p > 0.05).



Drunks and sober people did not differ in their average rates of slurring their speech (t = 0.56, df = 97, not significant).



The hypothesis that drunks slur their words was rejected in favour of the null hypothesis (t = 0.56, df = 97, p > 0.05, not significant).



The hypothesis that drunks slur their words was rejected, t(97) = 0.13, ns, 1-tail.

All four statements mean much the same. The symbol > means that your probability is greater than the listed value. It is usually in a context which indicates that your calculation is not statistically significant at the stated level. Notice throughout this chapter that the reported significance levels are not standardised on the 5% level of significance. It is possible, especially with computers, to obtain much more exact values of probability than the critical values used in tables of signi­ ficance. The use of these exact values is encouraged by the APA’s system for reporting statistical findings. The underlying idea is that more information is communicated by giving the exact significance. One possible objection to this is that it gives a false sense of precision to the statistical findings. Statistical significance can become a holy grail in statistics, supporting the view ‘the smaller the probability the better’. We have seen the variability that is possible in randomly selected data so we should be very cautious about assuming that a significance level of .003 is really better in some sense than a significance level of .006. Although significance is important, the size of the trend in your data is even more crucial. A significant result with a strong trend is the ideal which is not obtained simply by exploring the minutiae of probability. One thing causes a lot of confusion in the significance levels given by computers. Sometimes values like p < 0.0000 are listed. All that this means is that the probability level for the statistical test is less than 0.0001. In other words, the significance level might be, say, 0.000 03 or 0.000 000 4. These are very significant findings, statistically speaking. We would recommend that you report them slightly differently in your writings. Values such as 0.0001 or 0.001 are clearer to some readers. So consider changing your final 0 in the string of zeros to 1.

17.3 Examples from the published literature

■ Example 1 .  .  .  a post hoc comparison was carried out between means of the adult molesters’ and adult control groups’ ratings using Student’s t-test. A significant (t(49) = 2.96, p < 0.001) difference was found between the two groups. (Johnston & Johnston, 1986, p. 643)

M17_HOWI0749_06_SE_C17.indd 226

11/18/13 12:03 PM

17.3  Examples from the published literature 227

The above excerpt is fairly typical of the ways in which psychologists summarise the results of their research. To the practised eye, it is not too difficult to decipher. However, some difficulties can be caused to the novice statistician. A little patience at first will help a lot. The extract contains the following major pieces of information: ●

The statistical test used was the t-test. The authors mention it by name, but it is also identified by the t mentioned in the brackets. However, the phrase ‘Student’s t-test’ might be confusing. Student was the pen-name of a researcher at the Guinness Brewery who invented the t-test. It is quite redundant nowadays – the name Student, not Guinness!



The degrees of freedom are the (49) contained in the brackets. If you check the original paper you will find that the combined sample size is 51. It should be obvious, then, that this is an unrelated or uncorrelated t-test since the degrees of freedom are clearly N − 2 in this case.



The value of the t-test is 2.96.



The difference between the two groups is statistically significant at the 0.001 or 0.1% level of probability. This is shown by p < 0.001 in the above excerpt.



‘Post hoc’ merely means that the researchers decided to do the test after the data had been collected. They had not planned it prior to collecting the data.



No mention is made of whether this is a one-tailed or a two-tailed test so we would assume that it is a two-tailed significance level.

Obviously, there are a variety of ways of writing up the findings of any analysis. The following is a different way of saying much the same thing: .  .  .  a post hoc comparison between the means of the adult molesters’ and adult control groups’ ratings was significant (t = 2.96, df = 49, p < 0.001).



■ Example 2 The relationship of gender of perpetrators and victims was examined. Perpetrators of female victims were more often male (13 900 of 24 947, 55.8%) while perpetrators of males were more often female (10 977 of 21 373, 51.4%, χ2(1) = 235.18, p < 0.001). (Rosenthal, 1988, p. 267) The interpretation of this is as follows: ●

M17_HOWI0749_06_SE_C17.indd 227

The chi-square test was used. χ is the Greek symbol for chi, so chi-square can be written as χ2.



The value of chi-square is 235.18.



It is statistically very significant as the probability level is less than 0.001 or less than 0.1%.



Chi-square is usually regarded as a directionless test. That is, the significance level reported is for a two-tailed test unless stated otherwise.



Although the significance level in this study seems impressive, just look at the sample sizes involved – over 46 000 children in total. The actual trends are relatively small –55.8% versus 51.4%. This is a good example of when not to get excited about statistically significant findings.

11/18/13 12:03 PM

228

CHAPTER 17  Reporting significance levels succinctly

An alternative way of saying much the same thing is: Female victims were offended against by males in 55.8% of cases (N = 24 947). For male victims, 51.4% of offenders were female (N = 21 373). Thus victims were more likely to be offended against by a member of the opposite sex (chi-square = 235.18, degrees of freedom = 1, p < 0.1%).



■ Example 3 A 2 × 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) with anger and sex of target as factors was conducted on the BP2 (after anger manipulation) scores. This analysis yielded a significant effect for anger, F(1, 116) = 43.76, p < 0.004, with angered subjects revealing a larger increase in arousal (M = 6.01) than the non-angered subjects (M = 0.01). (Donnerstein, 1980, p. 273) This should be readily deciphered as: ●

A two-way analysis of variance with two different levels of each independent variable. One of the independent variables is anger (angered and non-angered conditions are the categories). The other independent variable is the gender of the target of aggression. Something called BP2 (whatever that may be – it turns out to be blood pressure) is the dependent variable. (We cover two-way analysis of variance in Chapter 23.)



The mean BP2 score for the angered condition was 6.01 and the mean for the nonangered condition was 0.01. The author is using M as the symbol of the sample mean.



The test of significance is the F-ratio test. We know this because this is an analysis of variance but also because the statistic is stated to be F.



The value of F, the variance ratio, equals 43.76.



There are 1 and 116 degrees of freedom for the F-ratio for the main effect of the variable anger. The 1 degree of freedom is because there are two different levels of the variable anger (df = c − 1 or the number of groups of data minus one). The 116 degrees of freedom means that there must have been 120 participants in the experiment. The degrees of freedom for a main effect is N – number of cells = 120 − (2 × 2) = 120 − 4 = 116. All of this is clarified in Chapter 22.



The difference between the angered and non-angered conditions is statistically significant at the 0.4% level.

A slightly different style of describing these findings is: Blood pressure following the anger manipulation was included as the dependent variable on a 2 × 2 analysis of variance. The two independent variables were gender of the target of aggression and anger. There was a significant main effect for anger (F = 43.76, df = 1, 116, p < 0.4%). The greater mean increase in blood pressure was for the angered group (6.01) compared to the non-angered group (0.01).

M17_HOWI0749_06_SE_C17.indd 228

11/18/13 12:03 PM

17.3  Examples from the published literature 229

Box 17.1

Focus on

APA (2010) Publication Manual recommended practice of reporting statistical significance The APA produces a Publication Manual which sets out the ways in which manuscripts should be typed to be considered for publication in the journals that they publish. The latest publication manual is the sixth edition and was published in 2010. The recommendations of this guide are also used by the BPS for manuscripts to be submitted to the journals that they publish. The recommendations for reporting statistics seem to be relatively straightforward. Some of the main ones are listed below: ●

Decimals should generally be reported to no more two decimal places.



One exception to this are probability values which may be reported to three decimal places.



Probability values of less than .001 should be reported as < .001.







Leading zeroes (i.e. zeroes before the decimal point) should not be used for numbers which cannot be more than 1.00 such as correlation coefficients. For example, correlations should not be reported with a leading zero such as 0.671 but as .671. It is preferable to report the exact significance level to three decimal places as given by statistics software such as SPSS Statistics. For example, it is more informative to report p = .343 than p > .05 or p ns. Means (M), standard deviations (SD) and confidence intervals (95% CI) when reported within sentences should be appropriately abbreviated and reported within round brackets. The value of the lower limit should be followed by a comma and the value of the upper limit. Both values should be placed within square brackets. For example, we could write ‘Post-test depression was lower in the treated (M = 3.52, SD = 1.09,

M17_HOWI0749_06_SE_C17.indd 229

95% CI [3.42, 3.62]) than in the untreated group (M = 5.39, SD = 2.13, 95% CI [5.20, 5.68]).’ Confidence intervals are discussed in Chapter 38. ●

Details of the results of the inferential test are placed after a comma and are not bracketed. Bracketing is reserved for the degrees of freedom. The appropriate letter of the statistical test is given first (e.g. t, χ2) followed by the degrees of freedom in brackets, an = sign, the value of the statistical test to two decimal places, a comma, p, an = sign and the probability level to three decimal places or a < sign and .001.



Effect sizes should be reported if these are readily available and you are familiar with them. They are discussed in Chapter 36.

It is always a good idea to use a published journal article as a model or guide to your use of the style. Any paper published by the APA or the BPS in their journals should be suitable. Examples of the APA method from its journals include: 1 ‘Participants indicated that selfless behaviors are driven more by the internal force, the moral conscience (M = 4.57, SD = 1.41), t(185) = 5.47, p < .001.’ (Critcher & Dunning, 2013, p. 34) 2 ‘Asian Americans who heard a positive stereotype about their group evaluated their partner more negatively (M = 3.57, SD = 1.27) than Asian Americans who did not hear a positive stereotype (M = 2.69, SD = 0.60), t(39) = 2.70, p = .01, d = 0.89.’ (Siy & Cheryan, 2013, p. 90) The two both report the results of a t-test. The main difference is that the second example includes a measure of the effect size ‘d’. Effect size is discussed in Chapter 35.

11/18/13 12:03 PM

CHAPTER 17  Reporting significance levels succinctly

230

Research examples Reporting significance succinctly In the first example, a measure of effect size (d) is given in addition to other basic information (see Chapter 35). Mitsumatsu (2013) reported part of the statistical analysis of his study concerning the perception of causality as follows: ‘In the dual-cause condition, the mean screen locations were 2.0 (1.1) cm right and 0.3 (0.9) cm above when rating the finger; when rating the object, touch locations were 2.0 (0.7) cm right and 0.5 (0.7) cm above. The mean time between space bar release and screen touch was 471 ms (SD = 95) in the single-cause condition and 467 ms (SD = 73) and 454 ms (SD = 108) in blocks of rating the finger and object in the dual-cause conditions, respectively. t-tests showed that no mean finger touch time was significantly different from the time when the effect object started moving, t(9) = 0.9, p > .3, d = 0.30, t(9) = 1.3, p > .2, d = 0.43, t(9) = 1.3, p > .2, d = 0.42, respectively. The mean finger touch times did not differ significantly by condition, F(2, 18) = 0.45, p > .6, d = 0.05.’ (p. 104) Rowe (2012) wrote of her statistical analysis: ‘Child PPVT [Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test] scores varied widely at each age. At child age 30 months, the mean normed score was 96.2 (SD = 15.2), compared to 106.2 (SD = 17.4) at 42 months and 110.4 (SD = 18.2) at 54 months. PPVT scores at each age were positively related to one another (rs = .65 − .84, p < .001). At child ages 30 and 54 months, 2 children did not complete the PPVT and the sample size is 48 for each of those ages. At child age 42 months, all 50 children completed the PPVT.’ (p. 1767)

Key points ●

Remember that the important pieces of information to report are: ●

the symbol for the statistic (t, T, r, etc.)



the value of the statistic for your analysis – two decimal places are enough



an indication of the degrees of freedom or the sample size involved (df = .  .  .  , N = .  .  .  )



the probability or significance level



whether a one-tailed test was used.



Sometimes you will see symbols for statistical techniques that you have never heard of. Do not panic since it is usually possible to work out the sense of what is going on. Certainly if you have details of the sort described in this chapter, you know that a test of significance is involved.



Using the approaches described in this chapter creates a good impression and ensures that you include pertinent information. However, standardise on one of the variants in your report. Eventually if you submit papers to a journal for consideration, you should check out that journal’s method of reporting significance.



Some statistical tests are regarded as being directionless. That is, their use always implies a two-tailed test. This is true of chi-square and the analysis of variance. These tests can only be one-tailed if the degrees of freedom equal one. Otherwise, the test is two-tailed. Even when the degrees of freedom equal one, only use a one-tailed test if you are satisfied that you have reached the basic requirements of one-tailed testing (see Chapter 18).

M17_HOWI0749_06_SE_C17.indd 230

11/18/13 12:03 PM

Computer analysis 231



Computer analysis Statistical significance on SPSS Modern statistical packages almost invariably provide precise significance levels in their output. However, traditionally the 0.05 (5%) and 0.01 (1%) levels of significance were the main ones reported. SPSS allows you the choice of the two approaches for some statistics such as correlation coefficients. There are advantages to reporting these critical values of 5% and 1% in that just the two levels as these provide a relatively simple choice: either accept or reject the hypothesis. Exact levels of significance can cause some confusion when they are given as, say, the 0.000 level of significance. Although this is correct it is easily misunderstood. It means that the significance level is less than 0.0005 rounded to three decimal places. It does not mean that there is no probability that the hypothesis is false. The exact levels of significance can have their uses when trying to calculate the correct levels of significance of multiple statistical tests (e.g. Chapter 24).

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

The basic criterion for statistical significance usually adopted is 5% or 0.05. Whether or not you use exact significances or critical values you use this as the cut-off point between significance and non-significance.



You may use critical values or exact significance in your report but do not use them interchangeably within the report. That is, use exact values consistently if you choose to report these.

M17_HOWI0749_06_SE_C17.indd 231

11/18/13 12:03 PM

Chapter 18

One-tailed versus twotailed significance testing

Overview ●

Hypotheses which do not or cannot stipulate the direction of the relationship between variables are called non-directional. So far we have only dealt with non-directional tests of hypotheses. These are also known as two-tailed tests.



Some hypotheses stipulate the direction of the relationship between the variables – either a positive relation or a negative relation. These are known as directional hypotheses. They are also known as one-tailed tests.



Directional tests for any given data result in more significant findings than non-directional tests when applied to the same data. This is provided that the trend is in the direction stipulated.



However, there are considerable restrictions on when directional tests are allowable. Without very carefully planning, it is wise to deal with one’s data as if it were nondirectional. Most student research is likely to fail to meet the requirements of one-tailed testing.

Preparation Revise the null hypothesis and alternative hypothesis (Chapter 11) and significance testing.

M18_HOWI0749_06_SE_C18.indd 232

11/18/13 12:11 PM

18.2  Theoretical considerations 233

18.1 Introduction Sometimes researchers are so confident about the likely outcome of their research that they make pretty strong predictions about the relationship between their independent and dependent variables. So, for example, rather than say that the independent variable age is correlated with verbal ability, the researcher predicts that the independent variable age is positively correlated with the dependent variable verbal ability. In other words, it is predicted that the older participants in the research will have better verbal skills. Equally the researcher might predict a negative relationship between the independent and dependent variables. It is conventional in psychological statistics to treat such directional predictions differently from non-directional predictions. Normally psychologists speak of a directional prediction being one-tailed whereas a non-directional prediction is two-tailed. The crucial point is that if you have a directional prediction (one-tailed test) the critical values of the significance test become slightly different. In order to carry out a one-tailed test you need to be satisfied that you have met the criteria for one-tailed testing. These, as we will see, are rather stringent. In our experience, many one-tailed hypotheses put forward by students are little more than hunches and certainly not based on the required strong past research or strong theory. In these circumstances it is unwise and wrong to carry out one-tailed testing. It would be best to regard the alternative hypothesis as non-directional and choose two-tailed significance testing exactly as we have done so far in this book. One-tailed testing is a contentious issue and you may be confronted with different points of view; some authorities reject it although it is fairly commonplace if not frequent in psychological research.

18.2 Theoretical considerations If we take a directional alternative hypothesis (such as that intelligence correlates positively with level of education) then it is necessary to revise our understanding of the null hypothesis somewhat. (The same is true if the directional alternative hypothesis suggests a negative relationship between the two variables.) In the case of the positively worded alternative hypothesis, the null hypothesis is: Intelligence does not correlate positively with level of education. Our previous style of null hypothesis would have left out the word positively. There are two different circumstances which support the null hypothesis that intelligence does not correlate positively with level of education: If intelligence does not correlate at all with level of education, or If intelligence correlates negatively with level of education. That is, it is only research which shows a positive correlation between intelligence and education which supports the directional hypothesis – if we found an extreme negative correlation between intelligence and education this would lead to the rejection of the alternative hypothesis just as would zero or near-zero relationships. Because, in a sense, the dice is loaded against the directional alternative hypothesis, it is conventional to

M18_HOWI0749_06_SE_C18.indd 233

11/18/13 12:11 PM

234

CHAPTER 18  One-tailed versus two-tailed significance testing

Figure 18.1

Areas of statistical significance for two-tailed and one-tailed tests

argue that we should not use the extremes of the sampling distribution in both directions for our test of significance for the directional hypothesis. Instead we should take the extreme samples in the positive direction (if it is positively worded) or the extreme samples in the negative direction (if it is negatively worded). In other words, our extreme 5% of samples which we define as significant should all be from one side of the sampling distribution, not 2.5% on each side as we would have done previously (see Figure 18.1). Because the 5% of extreme samples, which are defined as significant, are all on the same side of the distribution, you need a smaller value of your significance test to be in that extreme 5%. Part of the attraction of directional or one-tailed significance tests of this sort is that basically you can get the same level of significance with a smaller sample or smaller trend than would be required for a two-tailed test. Essentially the probability level can be halved – what would be significant at the 5% level with a two-tailed test is significant at the 2.5% level with a one-tailed test, for example. There is a big proviso to this. If you predicted a positive relationship but found what would normally be a significant negative relationship, with a one-tailed test you ought to ignore that negative relationship – it merely supports the null hypothesis. The temptation is, however, to ignore your original directional alternative hypothesis and pretend that you had not predicted the direction. Given that significant results are at a premium in psychology and are much more likely to get published, it is not surprising that psychologists seeking to publish their research might be tempted to ‘adjust’ their hypotheses slightly. It is noteworthy that the research literature contains very few tests of significance of directional hypotheses which are rejected when the trend in the data is strongly (and significantly with a two-tailed test) in the opposite direction to that predicted. The only

M18_HOWI0749_06_SE_C18.indd 234

11/18/13 12:11 PM

18.3  Further requirements 235

Figure 18.2

Conceptual steps for understanding one-tailed and two-tailed significance testing

example we know of was written by one of us. Figure 18.2 gives the key steps to consider in understanding one- and two-tailed significance testing.

18.3 Further requirements There are a number of other rules which are supposed to be followed if one is to use a directional hypothesis, including the following: ●

The prediction is based on strong and well-researched theory, and not on a whim or intuition.



The prediction is based on previous similar research demonstrating consistent trends in the predicted direction.



One should make the above predictions in advance of any information about the trends in the data about which the prediction is to be made. That is, for example, you do not look at your scattergrams and then predict the direction of the correlation between your variables. That would be manifestly cheating but a ‘good’ way otherwise of getting significant results with a one-tailed test when they would not quite reach significance with a two-tailed test.

There is another practical problem in the use of directional hypotheses. That is, if you have more than two groups of scores it is often very difficult to decide what the

M18_HOWI0749_06_SE_C18.indd 235

11/18/13 12:11 PM

236

CHAPTER 18  One-tailed versus two-tailed significance testing

predicting trends between the groups would be. For this reason, many statistical techniques are commonly regarded as directionless when you have more than two groups of scores or subjects. This applies to techniques such as chi-square, the analysis of variance and other related tests. Although this is clearly a controversial area, you will probably find that as a student you rarely if ever have sufficient justification for employing a one-tailed test. As you might have gathered, most of these criteria for selecting a one-tailed test are to a degree subjective which makes the use of one-tailed tests less objective than might be expected. We would recommend that you choose a two-tailed or directionless test unless there is a pressing and convincing reason to do otherwise. Otherwise the danger of loading things in favour of significant results is too great. In addition to two-tailed critical values, the significance tables in the appendices give the one-tailed values where these are appropriate.

Research examples One-tailed and two-tailed significance testing The use of one-tailed significance testing seems to be relatively uncommon in modern psychology research publications. Whether this is a good thing depends to some extent on one’s point of view. It possibly indicates a diminished interest in testing highly specific hypotheses based on theory and past research in favour of a more exploratory approach to data analysis. With the latter, it is not really appropriate to make predictions about the direction of trends. Meeten and Davey (2012) manipulated one of five moods by showing participants one of five films reflecting those moods. The five conditions were sad, happy, anxious, angry and neutral. Participants rated how they felt immediately after seeing the film and at the end of the study in terms of the four moods of sadness, happiness, anxiety and anger. Because each of the five conditions were expected to produce a particular mood immediately after seeing the film, one-tailed unrelated t-tests were carried out to compare each of the five conditions on each of the four moods. The five conditions were found to produce the expected mood. In order to see whether the induced mood was still present at the end of the study they carried out two-tailed related t-tests as they did not predict any particular results. There was only a significant change for anger, which was less at the end of the study. Hoicka and Akhtar (2012) report in their study of early humour in children that ‘Mann–Whitney U-tests revealed no effects of children’s age or gender for whether children produced each humour type (all p > .281).’ (p. 589) No indication is given of whether one- or two-tailed tests of significance were used but the default option is two-tailed tests. Only if the testing is indicated to be one-tailed do we assume that it is.

M18_HOWI0749_06_SE_C18.indd 236

11/18/13 12:11 PM

Computer analysis 237



Key points ●

Routinely make your alternative hypotheses two-tailed or directionless. This is especially the case when the implications of your research are of practical or policy significance. However, this may not be ideal if you are testing theoretical predictions when the direction of the hypothesis might be important. Nevertheless, it is a moot point whether you should take advantage of the ‘less stringent’ significance requirements of a onetailed test.



If you believe that the well-established theoretical or empirical basis for predicting the direction of the outcomes is strong enough, then still be a little cautious about employing one-tailed tests. In particular, do not formulate your hypothesis after collecting or viewing your data.



You cannot be faulted for using two-tailed tests since they are less likely to show significant relationships. Thus they are described as being statistically more conservative. Student research often does not arise out of previous research or theory. Often the research is initiated before earlier research and theory have been reviewed. In these circumstances one-tailed tests are not warranted.

Computer analysis One- and two-tailed statistical significance using SPSS If there are good grounds for predicting the direction of the relationship between two variables, it is conventional to use a one-tailed rather than a two-tailed significance level. SPSS provides a one-tailed significance level for correlations and a 2 × 2 chi-square. It does not do this for t-tests and analysis of variance with two groups. To obtain the one-tailed level for these tests, the two-tailed significance level needs to be divided by 2.

M18_HOWI0749_06_SE_C18.indd 237

11/18/13 12:11 PM

Chapter 19

Ranking tests Nonparametric statistics

Overview ●

There are many statistical techniques which are not based on the notion of the normal curve.



Some data violate the assumption of normality which underlies many of the statistical tests in this book. However, violations rarely have much impact on the outcome of a statistical analysis.



Nonparametric and distribution-free statistics are often helpful where one’s data violate the assumptions of other tests too much.



For each of the tests discussed in the earlier chapters of this book, a nonparametric or distribution-free alternative is available.



Unfortunately, in many cases there is no satisfactory alternative to the parametric tests.

Preparation Be aware of the t-tests for related and unrelated samples. Revise ranking (Chapter 8).

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 238

11/18/13 2:48 PM

19.2  Theoretical considerations 239

19.1 Introduction From time to time, any researcher will be faced with the distinction between parametric and nonparametric significance tests. The difference is quite straightforward. Many statistical techniques require that the details are known or estimates can be made of the characteristics of the population. These are known as parametric tests (a parameter is a characteristic of a population). Almost invariably, as we have seen, the population is the population defined by the null hypothesis. Generally speaking, the numerical scores we used had to roughly approximate to the normal (bell-shaped) distribution in order for our decisions to be precise. The reason for this is that the statistician’s theoretical assumptions, when developing the test, included the normal distribution of the data. It is widely accepted that the assumption of a bell-shaped or normal distribution of scores is a very broad criterion and that the distribution of scores on a variable would have to be very lopsided (skewed) in order for the outcomes to be seriously out of line. Appendix A explains how to test for such skewness. But what if assumptions such as that of symmetry are so badly violated that the use of the test seems somewhat unacceptable? One traditional alternative approach is called nonparametric testing because it makes few or no assumptions about the distribution in the population. Many nonparametric tests of significance are based on rankings given to the original numerical scores – it is unusual for researchers to collect their data in the form of ranks in the first place. Conventionally these tests for ranks were regarded as relatively easy computations for students – this is part of their appeal. However, in the age of computers this is hardly a compelling reason for their use. There are problems with their use as follows: ●

They become disproportionately cumbersome with increasing amounts of data. This is not a problem for the computer.



They also suffer from the difficulty that many psychological data are gathered using rather restricted ranges of scores. This often results in the same values appearing several times in a set of data. The tests based on ranks also become cumbersome with increased tied scores and, consequently, somewhat inaccurate. The extent of this is generally unknown.



Worst of all, the variety and flexibility of these nonparametric statistical techniques are nowhere as great as for parametric statistics. For this reason it is generally best to err towards using parametric statistics in our opinion. Certainly current research practice seems to increasingly disfavour nonparametric statistics. You will find them only relatively rarely in the modern research literature.

Figure 19.1 gives some of the key steps in deciding to use non-parametric statistics.

19.2 Theoretical considerations Ranking merely involves the ordering of a set of scores from the smallest to the largest. The smallest score is given the rank 1, the second smallest score is given the rank 2, the 50th smallest score is given the rank 50 and so on. Since many nonparametric statistical techniques use ranks, the question is raised why this is so. The answer is very much the same as the reason for using the normal distribution as the basis for parametric statistics – it provides a standard distribution of scores with standard characteristics. It is much the same for the tests based on ranks. Although

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 239

11/18/13 2:48 PM

240

CHAPTER 19  Ranking tests: Nonparametric statistics

Figure 19.1

Conceptual steps for understanding nonparametric statistics

there are incalculable varieties in samples of data, for any given number of scores the ranks are always the same. So the ranks of 10 scores which represent the IQs of the 10 greatest geniuses of all time are exactly the same as the ranks for the scores on introversion of the 10 members of the local stamp collectors’ club: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9 and 10. Since all sets of 10 scores use exactly the same set of ranks, this considerably eases the statistician’s calculations of the distribution of the ranks under the null hypothesis that there is no relationship between pairs of variables. Instead of an infinite variety of 10 scores, there is just this one set of 10 ranks on which to do one’s calculations. Only sample size makes a difference to the ranks, not the precise numerical values of the scores themselves. The other advantage, of course, is that it uses ideas which can be seen as fairly commonsensical and more intuitive.

Box 19.1

Key concepts

Bootstrapping Probably the history of statistics in psychology would have been somewhat different if computers had been available earlier. Many of the statistical techniques which modern psychologists routinely compute using powerful computer programs are actually quite elderly and, in some ways, creak a little when used in this digital age. In particular, the various standard statistical distributions such as the t-distribution and the F-distribution which are used in statistics were particularly important because they enabled calculations to be carried out relatively easily, long before electronic calculators, let alone computers,

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 240

had been invented. But could things have been different if computers had been available to the early statisticians? Bootstrapping refers to a number of techniques which do not assume a particular shape or distribution to the population. We have seen that, for example, the t-test assumes that the data are normally distributed. Instead bootstrapping simply assumes that a sample or samples of scores represents what is going on in the population. In other words, the population simply has the characteristics of the sample(s). So if the distribution of scores in a sample is 6, 8, 9, 9, 11, 12, 13 then it is assumed that the population is exactly the

11/18/13 2:48 PM

19.3  Nonparametric statistical tests 241

same. Wait a minute, you might be saying, just how does this help? We cannot work out a sampling distribution of samples of seven scores if we only have seven scores. There is only one sample of seven possible from seven scores. This is quite right. The ‘trick’ in bootstrapping is to take these seven scores and reproduce them, say, a thousand times, so that instead of seven scores we have 7000 scores. There is nothing in this bootstrapped ‘population’ that was not in the sample – everything is merely reproduced many times. Now it is possible to work out a sampling distribu­ tion of samples of seven taken from this bootstrapped population. Actually, literally hundreds if not thousands of samples are drawn and the distribution, say, of their means can be plotted. So it is possible to work out the likelihood of getting a particular sample mean given this bootstrapped sampling distribution. This is number crunching with a vengeance but the sort of work that computers are excellent at doing. The good news is that it is no longer necessary to do the bootstrapping calculations yourself since SPSS

Statistics does bootstrapping as part of some of the stat­ istical routines that it carries out – for example, the t-test. Where it is available, bootstrapping only requires the minimum effort of selecting the bootstrapping option. Bootstrapping is capable of calculating things which are not easily calculated by traditional means – for example, it can work out the standard deviation of the median and any number of other statistics. From the point of view of the present chapter on nonparametric statistics, bootstrapping does not make assumptions about the data of the sort that parametric statistical tests do. Consequently, it can be seen as a powerful alternative to parametric testing but based on the same routines as the parametric test. The main problem though is that bootstrapping has not entered psychological statistics to any great extent and so many psychologists may be unfamiliar with the concept. But the fact that it overcomes some of the problems associated with the use of statistics in psychology should be a reason for its more widespread acceptance.

19.3 Nonparametric statistical tests There is an extensive battery of nonparametric tests, although many are interchangeable with each other or rather obscure with very limited applications. In this chapter we will consider only a small number of tests which you may come across during your university courses or general reading. We have discussed chi-square (for frequencies) and Spearman’s rho (for correlations) elsewhere in this book. The nonparametric tests discussed in this chapter are usually applicable in very much the same experimental designs as the parametric tests we have discussed elsewhere (see Table 19.1).



■ Tests for related samples Two nonparametric tests are common in the literature – the sign test (which is not based on ranks) and the Wilcoxon matched pairs test (which is based on ranks). Because they would apply to data for the related t-test, we will use the data for Explaining statistics 13.1 to illustrate the application of both of these tests.

Table 19.1

Similar parametric and nonparametric tests Parametric test

Nonparametric equivalent

Related t-test

Wilcoxon matched pairs test Sign test

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 241

Unrelated t-test

Mann–Whitney U-test

One-way ANOVA

Kruskal–Wallis test (Appendix B2)

Related ANOVA

Friedman test (Appendix B2)

11/18/13 2:48 PM

242

CHAPTER 19  Ranking tests: Nonparametric statistics

Explaining statistics 19.1

How the sign test works The sign test is like the related t-test in that it takes the differences between the two related samples of scores. However, instead of considering the size of the difference, the sign test merely uses the sign of the difference. In other words, it loses a lot of the information inherent in the size of the difference.

Step 1

Delete from the analysis any case which has identical scores for both variables. They are ignored in the sign test. Take the second group of scores away from the first group (Table 19.2). Remember to include the sign of the difference (+ or −).

Steps in the calculation of the sign test

Table 19.2 Subject

Six months X1

Nine months X2

Difference D = X1 − X2

Baby Clara

3

7

−4

Baby Martin

5

6

−1

Baby Sally

5

3

+2

Baby Angie

4

8

−4

Baby Trevor

3

5

−2

Baby Sam

7

9

−2

Baby Bobby

8

7

+1

Baby Sid

7

9

−2

Step 2

Count the number of scores which are positively signed and then count the number of scores which are negatively signed. (Don’t forget that zero differences are ignored in the sign test.)

Step 3

Take whichever is the smaller number – the number of positive signs or the number of negative signs.

Step 4

Look up the significance of this smaller number in Significance Table 19.1. You need to find the row which contains the sum of the positive and negative signs (i.e. ignoring zero differences). Your value has to be in the tabulated range to be statistically significant.

In our example, there are 6 negative and 2 positive signs; 2 is the smaller number. The sum of positive and negative signs is 8. Significance Table 19.1 gives the significant values of the smaller number of signs as 0 only. Therefore our value is not statistically unusual and we accept the null hypothesis. It would be a good approximation to use the one-sample chi-square formula (Explaining statistics 15.3), given that you would expect equal numbers of positive and negative differences under the null hypothesis that ‘the two samples do not differ’. That is, the distributions of the sign test and the McNemar test (Section 15.7) for the significance of changes are the same.

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 242

11/18/13 2:48 PM

19.3  Nonparametric statistical tests 243

Significance Table 19.1

5% significance values for the sign test giving values of T (the smaller of the sums of signs) (two-tailed test). An extended table is given in Appendix G Number of pairs of scores (ignoring any tied pairs)

Significant at 5% level Accept hypothesis

6–8

0 only

9–11

0 to 1

12–14

0 to 2

15–16

0 to 3

17–19

0 to 4

20–22

0 to 5

23–24

0 to 6

25

0 to 7

26–28

0 to 8

29–30

0 to 9

31–33

0 to 10

34–35

0 to 11

36–38

0 to 12

39–40

0 to 13

41–42

0 to 14

43–45

0 to 15

46–47

0 to 16

48–49

0 to 17

50

0 to 18

Your value must be in the listed ranges for your sample size to be significant at the 5% level (i.e. to accept the hypothesis).

Interpreting the results The mean scores for eye contact at six months and nine months need to be checked in order to know what the trend is in the data. Although eye contact was greater at nine months, the sign test is not significant which means that we should accept the null hypothesis of no differences in eye contact at the two ages.

Reporting the results The following could be written: ‘Eye contact was higher at nine months (B = 6.75) than at six months (B = 5.25). However, this difference was insufficient to cause us to reject the null hypothesis that the amount of eye contact is the same at six months and nine months of age (sign test, n = 8, pns).’ Alternatively, following the APA (2010) Publication Manual recommendations, we could rewrite these results as follows: ‘Eye contact was higher at nine months (M = 6.75) than at six months (M = 5.25). However, this difference was insufficient to cause us to reject the null hypothesis that the amount of eye contact is the same at six months and nine months of age (sign test (n = 8), p ns).’

The calculation steps for the Wilcoxon matched pairs (or signed ranks) test are similar. However, this test retains a little more information from the original scores by ranking the differences.

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 243

11/18/13 2:48 PM

244

CHAPTER 19  Ranking tests: Nonparametric statistics

Explaining statistics 19.2

How the Wilcoxon matched pairs test works The test is also known as the Wilcoxon signed ranks test. It is similar to the sign test except that when we have obtained the difference score we rank-order the differences ignoring the sign of the difference.

Step 1

The difference scores are calculated and then ranked ignoring the sign of the difference (Table 19.3). Notice that where there are tied values of the differences, we have allocated the average of the ranks which would be given if it were possible to separate the scores. Thus the two difference scores which equal 1 are both given the rank 1.5 since if the scores did differ minutely one would be given the rank 1 and the other the rank 2. Take care: zero differences are ignored and are not ranked.

Steps in the calculation of the Wilcoxon matched pairs test

Table 19.3 Subject

Six months X1

Nine months X2

Difference D = X 1 − X2

Rank of difference ignoring sign during ranking

Baby Clara

3

7

−4

7.5−

Baby Martin

5

6

−1

1.5−

Baby Sally

5

3

2

4.5+

Baby Angie

4

8

−4

7.5−

Baby Trevor

3

5

−2

4.5−

Baby Sam

7

9

−2

4.5−

Baby Bobby

8

7

1

1.5+

Baby Sid

7

9

−2

4.5−

Step 2

The ranks of the differences can now have the sign of the difference reattached.

Step 3

The sum of the positive ranks is calculated = 4.5 + 1.5 = 6. The sum of the negative ranks is calculated = 7.5 + 1.5 + 7.5 + 4.5 + 4.5 + 4.5 = 30.

Step 4

We then decide which is the smaller of the two sums of ranks – in this case it is 6. This is normally designated T.

Step 5

We then find the significance values of T (the smaller of the two sums of ranks) from Significance Table 19.2. This is structured in terms of the number of pairs of scores used in the calculation, which is 8 in the present case. The critical value for a two-tailed test at the 5% level is 4 or less. Our value is 6 which is not statistically significant.

If your sample size is larger than Significance Table 19.2 deals with, Appendix B1 explains how to test for significance.

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 244

11/18/13 2:48 PM

19.3  Nonparametric statistical tests 245

Significance Table 19.2

5% significance values for the Wilcoxon matched pairs test (two-tailed test). An extended and conventional significance table is given in Appendix H Number of pairs of scores (ignoring any tied pairs)

Significant at 5% level Accept hypothesis

6

0 only

7

0 to 2

8

0 to 4

9

0 to 6

10

0 to 8

11

0 to 11

12

0 to 14

13

0 to 17

14

0 to 21

15

0 to 25

16

0 to 30

17

0 to 35

18

0 to 40

19

0 to 46

20

0 to 52

21

0 to 59

22

0 to 66

23

0 to 74

24

0 to 81

25

0 to 90

Your value must be in the listed ranges for your sample size to be significant at the 5% level (i.e. to accept the hypothesis).

Interpreting the results As always, it is important to examine the means of the two sets of scores in order to know what the trend in the data is. Although the amount of eye contact at nine months was greater than at six months, the Wilcoxon matched pairs test failed to reach statistical significance so it is not possible to reject the null hypothesis of no differences in eye contact at the two ages.

Reporting the results The following gives a reasonably concise account of our findings: ‘Eye contact was slightly higher at nine months (B = 6.75) than at six months (B = 5.25). However, this difference did not reach statistical significance so it was not possible to reject the null hypothesis that eye contact does not change between these ages (T = 6, n = 8, p > 0.05, ns).’ Alternatively, following the APA (2010) Publication Manual recommendations we could rewrite the results as follows: ‘Eye contact was slightly higher at nine months (M = 6.75) than at six months (M = 5.25). However, this difference did not reach statistical significance so it was not possible to reject the null hypothesis that eye contact does not change between these ages, T(n = 8) = 6, p > .05.’

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 245

11/18/13 2:48 PM

246

CHAPTER 19 RaNkiNg teStS: NONpaRametRiC StatiStiCS

Generally speaking, it is difficult to suggest circumstances in which the sign test is to be preferred over the Wilcoxon matched pairs test. The latter uses more of the information contained within the data and so is more likely to detect significant differences where they exist. The sign test can be applied in virtually any circumstance in which the expected population distribution under the null hypothesis is 50% of one outcome and 50% of another. In other words, the table of significance of the sign test can be used to check for departures from this 50/50 expectation.

■ tests for unrelated samples



The major nonparametric test for differences between two groups of unrelated or uncorrelated scores is the Mann–Whitney U-test.

Explaining statistics 19.3

how the mann–Whitney U-test works The most common nonparametric statistic for unrelated samples of scores is the Mann–Whitney U-test. This is used for similar research designs as the unrelated or uncorrelated scores t-test (Chapter 14). In other words, it can be used whenever you have two groups of scores which are independent of each other (i.e. they are usually based on different samples of people). We will use the identical data upon which we demonstrated the calculation of the unrelated/uncorrelated scores t-test (Chapter 14).

Step 1

Rank all of the scores from the smallest to the largest (Table 19.4). Scores which are equal are allocated the average of ranks that they would be given if there were tiny differences between the scores. Be careful! All of your scores are ranked irrespective of the group they are in. To avoid confusion, use the first column for the larger group of scores. If both groups are equal in size then either can be entered in the first column. Group size N1 = 12 for the two-parent families and N2 = 10 for the lone-parent families.

Step 2

Sum the ranks for the larger group of scores. This is R1. (If the groups are equal in size then either can be selected.)

Step 3

The sum of ranks (R1) of Group 1 (174.5) (the larger group) and its sample size N1 (N1 = 12) together with the sample size N2 of Group 2 (N2 = 10) are entered into the following formula which gives you the value of the statistic U: U = (N 1 × N 2 ) + = (12 × 10) +

N 1 × (N 1 × 1) − R1 2

12 × (12 + 1) − 174.5 2

= 120 +

12 × 13 − 174.5 2

= 120 +

156 − 174.5 2

= 120 + 78 − 174.5 = 198 − 174.5 = 23.5

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 246

11/18/13 2:48 PM

19.3  Nonparametric statistical tests 247

Steps in the calculation of the Mann–Whitney U-test

Table 19.4

Two-parent families (X1)

Rankings

(This column is for the larger group)

Lone-parent families (X2)

Rankings

(This column is for the smaller group)

12

12.5

6

2

18

21

9

6

14

16.5

4

1

10

8.5

13

14.5

14

16.5

19

22

8

3.5

9

6

15

18.5

8

3.5

11

10.5

12

12.5

10

8.5

11

10.5

13

14.5

9

6

15

18.5

16

20 ∑R1 = 174.5 (Note that this is the sum of ranks for the larger group)

Step 4

Check the significance of your value of U by consulting Significance Table 19.3. In order to use this table, you need to find your value of N1 in the column headings and your value of N2 in the row headings. (However, since the table is symmetrical it does not matter if you use the rows instead of the columns and vice versa.) The table gives the two ranges of values of U which are significant. Your value must be in either of these two ranges to be statistically significant. (Appendix B1 explains what to do if your sample size exceeds the largest value in the table.)

The table tells us that for sample sizes of 12 and 10, the ranges are 0 to 29 or 91 to 120. Our value of 23.5 there­ fore is significant at the 5% level. In other words, we reject the null hypothesis that the independent variable is unrelated to the dependent variable in favour of the view that family structure has an influence on scores of the dependent variable.

Interpreting the results The means of the two groups of scores must be examined to know which of the two groups has the higher scores on the dependent variable. In our example, greater emotionality was found in the children from the two-parent families. The significant value of the Mann–Whitney U-test suggests that we are reasonably safe to conclude that the two groups do differ in terms of their emotionality.

Reporting the results The statistical analysis could be reported in the following APA (2010) Publication Manual style: ‘It was found that emotionality was significantly higher, U(n = 22) = 23.5, p < .05, in the two-parent families (M = 13.42) than in the loneparent families (M = 9.50).’



M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 247

11/18/13 2:48 PM

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 248

0–27

93–120

80–100

71–90

61–75

0–20

0–19

0–14

0–17

67–84

0–13

57–70

0–16

62–78

0–12

58–72

49–60

53–65

0–14

53–66

46–55

0–11

0–13

0–9

0–11

49–60

0–8

42–50

0–10

44–54

0–7

40–48

34–40

38–45

0–8

36–42

30–35

0–6

0–6

0–5

0–5

31–36

0–3

27–30

106–140

0–34

81–105

0–24

76–98

0–22

71–91

0–20

66–84

0–18

61–77

0–16

56–70

0–14

51–63

0–12

46–56

0–10

41–49

0–8

36–42

0–6

30–35

0–5

7

119–160

0–41

91–120

0–29

86–112

0–26

80–104

0–24

74–96

0–22

69–88

0–19

63–80

0–17

57–72

0–15

51–64

0–13

46–56

0–10

40–48

0–8

34–40

0–6

8

130–180

0–48

101–135

0–34

95–126

0–31

89–117

0–28

82–108

0–26

76–99

0–23

70–90

0–20

64–81

0–17

57–72

0–15

51–63

0–12

44–54

0–10

38–45

0–7

9

145–200

0–55

111–150

0–39

104–140

0–36

97–130

0–33

91–120

0–29

84–110

0–26

77–100

0–23

70–90

0–20

63–80

0–17

56–17

0–14

49–60

0–11

42–50

0–8

10

158–220

0–62

121–165

0–44

114–154

0–40

106–143

0–37

99–132

0–33

91–121

0–30

84–110

0–26

76–99

0–23

69–88

0–19

61–77

0–16

53–66

0–13

46–55

0–9

11

171–240

0–69

131–180

0–49

123–168

0–45

115–156

0–41

107–144

0–37

99–132

0–33

91–120

0–29

82–108

0–26

74–96

0–22

66–84

0–18

58–72

0–14

49–60

0–11

12

184–260

0–76

141–195

0–54

132–182

0–50

124–169

0–45

115–156

0–41

106–143

0–37

97–130

0–33

89–117

0–28

80–104

0–24

71–91

0–20

62–78

0–16

53–65

0–12

13

197–280

0–83

151–210

0–59

141–196

0–55

132–182

0–50

123–168

0–45

114–154

0–40

104–140

0–36

95–126

0–31

86–112

0–26

76–98

0–22

67–84

0–17

57–70

0–13

14

210–300

0–90

161–225

0–64

151–210

0–59

141–195

0–54

131–180

0–49

121–165

0–44

111–150

0–39

101–135

0–34

91–120

0–29

81–105

0–24

71–90

0–19

61–75

0–14

15

273–400

0–127

210–300

0–90

197–280

0–83

184–260

0–76

171–240

0–69

158–220

0–62

145–200

0–55

130–180

0–48

119–160

0–41

106–140

0–34

93–120

0–27

80–100

0–20

20

Source: Adapted and extended from Table I of R.P. Runyon and A. Haber (1989). Fundamentals of Behavioral Statistics. New York: McGraw-Hill. With the kind permission of the publisher. Your value must be in the listed ranges for your sample sizes to be significant at the 5% level (i.e. to accept the hypothesis).

20

15

14

13

12

11

10

9

8

7

6

0–3

27–30

0–2

23–25

5

6

5

Sample size for larger group

5% significance values for the Mann–Whitney U-test (two-tailed test)

Sample size for smaller group

Significance Table 19.3

248 CHAPTER 19  Ranking tests: Nonparametric statistics

11/18/13 2:48 PM

19.4  Three or more groups of scores 249

19.4 Three or more groups of scores The Kruskal–Wallis test and the Friedman test are essentially extensions of the Mann– Whitney U-test and the Wilcoxon matched pairs test, respectively. Appendix B2 gives information on how to calculate these nonparametric statistics.

Research examples Ranking tests Blackmore and her colleagues (2006) were interested in whether a number of factors such as how long the pregnancy had lasted were associated with developing post-pregnancy bipolar depression in women who had such depression. They compared the length of pregnancy in pregnancies which had resulted in depression with those which had not resulted in depression using a Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank test and found no significant difference. Casarett and his colleagues (2010) studied the use of metaphors and analogies in their consultations with patients with advanced cancer. Using a sign test, they found that significantly more conversations contained metaphors than analogies. With the Wilcoxon sign-rank test, they found that the number of metaphors used in conversations was also significantly higher than the number of analogies. Hannaford and his colleagues (1996) evaluated an educational package which was designed to help general practitioners identify patients with depression. A Wilcoxon matched-pairs test showed that doctors missed significantly fewer cases of depression after receiving the package than before they had received it. Kenyon and her colleagues (2012) tested whether people with bulimia nervosa or other unspecified eating disorders were less able to infer the feelings, beliefs and knowledge of other people than people who did not have psychological disorders. As part of the study they assessed how depressed, anxious and stressed the three groups were (the two eating disorder groups and the control group). Because these three variables were not normally distributed thus violating an assumption of parametric statistics and could not be transformed to be so, the researchers carried out a Kruskal–Wallis test to determine if there was a statistical difference between the three groups. If there was a significant difference, they used a Mann–Whitney test to deter­ mine which groups differed from each other. They found that the two eating disorder groups did not differ from each other on these measures but were significantly more depressed, anxious and stressed than the healthy group. Shafran and her colleagues (2006) were interested in determining whether being asked to have higher general personal standards such as working very hard would result in more dysfunctional eating than those who were asked to have lower general personal standards such as taking it easy at work. Some of the measures used to assess dysfunctional eating such as trying to restrict the intake of food and feeling regret after eating were significantly positively skewed. On these measures Mann–Whitney tests were used to test for differences between these two groups before and after manipulating personal standards. After manipulation, those in the higher personal condition reported significantly more attempts to restrict their overall food intake and reported significantly more regret after eating.

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 249

11/18/13 2:48 PM

250

CHAPTER 19  Ranking tests: Nonparametric statistics

Key points ● ●

● ● ● ●

Often you will not require nonparametric tests of significance of the sort described in this chapter. The t-test will usually fit the task better. Only when you have marked symmetry problems in your data will you require the nonparametric tests. But even then remember that a version of the unrelated t-test is available to cope with some aspects of the problem (Chapter 14). The computations for the nonparametric tests may appear simpler. A big disadvantage is that when the sample sizes get large the problems in ranking escalate disproportionately. Some professional psychologists tend to advocate nonparametric techniques for entirely outmoded reasons. There is no guarantee that the nonparametric test will always do the job better when the assumptions of parametric tests are violated. There are large sample formulae for the nonparametric tests reported here for when your sample sizes are too big for the printed tables of significance. However, by the time this point is reached the computation is getting clumsy and can be better handled by a computer; also the advantages of the nonparametric tests are very reduced.

Computer analysis Two-group ranking tests using SPSS

Figure 19.2

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 250

SPSS Statistics steps for the sign, Wilcoxon and Mann–Whitney nonparametric tests

11/18/13 2:48 PM

Computer analysis 251



Interpreting and reporting the output







We could report the Wilcoxon results for the data in Table 19.2 as follows: ‘There was no significant difference in the amount of eye-contact by babies between 6 and 9 months, Wilcoxon, z(n = 8) = −1.71, two-tailed p = .088.’



We could report the Mann-Whitney results of the data in Table 19.4 as follows: ‘The Mann–Whitney U test found that the emotionality scores of children from two-parent families were significantly higher than those of children in lone-parent families, U(n1 = 10, n2 = 12) = 23.5, two-tailed p = 0.016.’

Screenshot 19.1



Select ranking tests for two related groups





Select ranking tests for two unrelated groups



Screenshot 19.3

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 251



Select the two related groups and tests



Select the variables for the unrelated groups

Screenshot 19.2

Screenshot 19.4

11/18/13 2:48 PM

252

CHAPTER 19  Ranking tests: Nonparametric statistics

Screenshot 19.5

Wilcoxon output

Screenshot 19.6

Mann–Whitney output

Recommended further reading Mariscuilo, L.A., & McSweeney, M. (1977). Nonparametric and distribution-free methods for the social sciences. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. Siegel, S., & Castellan, N.J. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.

M19_HOWI0749_06_SE_C19.indd 252

11/18/13 2:48 PM

PART 3

Introduction to analysis of variance

M20_HOWI0749_06_SE_C20.indd 253

11/18/13 12:10 PM

M20_HOWI0749_06_SE_C20.indd 254

11/18/13 12:10 PM

Chapter 20

The variance ratio test The F-ratio to compare two variances

Overview ●

The variance ratio test (the F-ratio test) assesses whether the variances of two different samples are significantly different from each other.



That is, it tests whether the spread of scores for the two samples is significantly different. This is not dependent on the value of the means of each sample.



However, it is more commonly used as part of other statistical techniques especially the analysis of variance. So understanding the F-ratio is an important step towards understanding the analysis of variance.



It is also used to test one of the underlying assumptions of the unrelated t-test since this assumes that the variances of the two sets of scores are more or less equal (i.e. not significantly different).

Preparation Make sure that you understand variance and the variance estimate (Chapters 4 and 12). Familiarity with the t-test will help with some applications (Chapters 13 and 14).

M20_HOWI0749_06_SE_C20.indd 255

11/18/13 12:10 PM

256

CHAPTER 20  The variance ratio test: the f-ratio to compare two variances

20.1 Introduction In a number of circumstances in research it is important to compare the variances of two samples of scores. The conventions of psychological research stress the comparison of two or more sample means with each other. This is to overlook other important effects which may occur in a study. For instance, it is perfectly possible to find that despite the means of two groups of scores being identical, their variances are radically different. Take the following simple experiment in which men and women are shown advertisements for women’s underwear (tights), set out in Table 20.1. The dependent variable is the readers’ degree of liking for the product rated on a scale from 1 to 7 (on which 1 means that they strongly disliked the advertisement and 7 means that they strongly liked the advertisement). The big difference between the two groups is not in terms of their means – the men’s mean is 4.4 whereas the women’s mean is 4.3. This is a small and unimportant difference. What is more noticeable is that the women seem to be split into two camps. The women’s scores tend to be large or small with little in the centre. There is more variance in the women’s scores. Just how does one test to see whether the difference in variance is significant? There are other circumstances in which we compare variances: ●

For the unrelated t-test, it is conventional to make sure that the two samples do not differ significantly in terms of their variances – if they do then it is better to opt for an ‘unpooled’ t-test (see Chapter 14) which is easily computed using SPSS. This is different from and in addition to testing for the significance of the difference between the sample means.



Another major application is the analysis of variance in which variance estimates are compared (see Chapter 21). SPSS uses a slight variation on this which makes little difference for the analysis of variance.

The statistical test to use in all these circumstances is called the F-ratio test or the variance ratio test. There is not a great deal that is new as it is dependent on the variance estimate, which we have already discussed several times. Table 20.1

Data comparing men and women on ratings of tights Men

Women

5

1

4

6

4

7

3

2

5

6

4

7

3

5

6

7

5

2

5

6 1 2

M20_HOWI0749_06_SE_C20.indd 256

11/18/13 12:10 PM

20.2  Theoretical issues and an application 257

20.2 Theoretical issues and an application The variance ratio simply compares two variances in order to test whether they come from the same population. In other words, are the differences between the variances simply the result of chance sampling fluctuations? Of course, since we are comparing samples from a population we need the variance estimate formula. In the simpler applications of the variance ratio test (F-ratio), the variance estimate involves using the sample size minus one (N − 1) as the denominator (lower part) of the variance estimate formula. (This does not apply in quite the same way in the more advanced case of the analysis of variance, as we will see in Chapter 21 onwards.) The variance ratio formula is as follows:

F=

larger variance estimate mate smaller variance estim

There is a table of the F-distribution (Significance Table 20.1) which is organised according to the degrees of freedom of the two variance estimates. Unlike the t-test, the F-ratio is a one-tailed test. It determines whether the numerator, the top part of the formula, is larger than the denominator, the lower part. The variance ratio cannot be smaller than one. The 5% or .05 applies to the upper or right-hand tail of the distribution. The larger the F-ratio is, the more likely it is that the larger variance estimate is significantly larger than the lower variance estimate. Figure 20.1 shows the key steps in the variance test.

Significance Table 20.1

5% significance values of the F-distribution for testing differences in variance estimates between two samples (one-tailed test). Additional values are given in Significance Table 21.1

Degrees of freedom for smaller variance estimate (denominator) 5

Degrees of freedom for larger variance estimate (numerator) 5

7

10

20

50



5.1 or more

4.9

4.7

4.6

4.4

4.4

6

4.4

4.1

4.1

3.9

3.8

3.7

7

4.0

3.8

3.6

3.4

3.3

3.2

8

3.7

3.5

3.3

3.2

3.0

2.9

10

3.3

3.1

3.0

2.8

2.6

2.5

12

3.1

2.9

2.8

2.5

2.4

2.3

15

2.9

2.7

2.6

2.3

2.2

2.1

20

2.7

2.5

2.4

2.1

2.0

1.8

30

2.5

2.3

2.2

1.9

1.8

1.6 1.4

50

2.4

2.2

2.0

1.8

1.6

100

2.3

2.1

1.9

1.7

1.5

1.3



2.2

2.0

1.8

1.6

1.4

1.0

Your value has to equal or be larger than the tabulated value to be significant at the 5% level.

M20_HOWI0749_06_SE_C20.indd 257

11/18/13 12:10 PM

258

CHAPTER 20  The variance ratio test: the f-ratio to compare two variances

Figure 20.1

Conceptual steps for understanding the variance ratio test

Explaining statistics 20.1

How the variance ratio (F-ratio) works It is not possible to calculate the variance ratio directly on SPSS so here are the steps in the calculation. It is possible to use SPSS to calculate the variance estimates involved, as we show in the Computer Analysis. Imagine a very simple piece of clinical research which involves the administration of electroconvulsive therapy (ECT). There are two experimental conditions: in one case the electric current is passed through the left hemisphere of the brain and in the other case it is passed through the right hemisphere of the brain. The dependent variable is scores on a test of emotional stability following treatment. Patients were assigned to one or other group at random. The scores following treatment were as listed in Table 20.2. Quite clearly there is no difference in terms of the mean scores on emotional stability. Looking at the data, though, it looks as if ECT to the right hemisphere tends to push people to the extremes whereas ECT to the left hemisphere leaves a more compact distribution.

Table 20.2

Emotional stability scores from a study of ECT to different hemispheres of the brain

M20_HOWI0749_06_SE_C20.indd 258

Left hemisphere

Right hemisphere

20

36

14

28

18

4

22

18

13

2

15

22

9

1

Mean = 15.9

Mean = 15.9

11/18/13 12:10 PM

20.2 TheOReTICAl ISSueS AnD An APPlICATIOn

259

To calculate the variance ratio, the variance estimates of the two separate samples (left and right hemispheres) have to be calculated using the usual variance estimate formula. The following is the computational formula version of this:

estimated variance =

Step 1

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

N −1

2

N

Calculate the variance of the first group of scores (i.e. the left hemisphere group), as in Table 20.3. The sample size (number of scores) is N1 = 7. Substituting in the formula:

variance estimate[ group group 1] = =

∑X

2 1

(∑ X ) −

2

1

N1 N −1

=

1112 12 321 1879 − 7 = 7 7 −1 6

1879 9−

1879 − 1760.143 118.857 = 6 6

= 19.81 (degrees of freedom = N 1 − 1 = 6)

Table 20.3

Step 2

Step 1 in the calculation of the variance estimate X1 = left hemisphere

X 21

20

400

14

196

18

324

22

484

13

169

15

225

9

81

∑ X1 = 111

∑ X = 1879 2 1

The variance estimate of the right hemisphere group is calculated using the standard computational formula as in Table 20.4. The sample size N2 = 7.

Table 20.4

Step 2 in the calculation of the variance estimate X1 = right hemisphere

X12

36

1296

28

784

4

16

18

324

2

4

22

484

1 ∑ X2 = 111

M20_HOWI0749_06_SE_C20.indd 259

1 ∑ X = 2909 2 2

11/18/13 12:10 PM

260

CHAPTER 20 The vARIAnCe RATIO TeST: The F-RATIO TO COMPARe TwO vARIAnCeS

Substituting in the formula:

variance estimate[ group group 1] = =

∑X

2 2

(∑ X ) −

2

2

N2 N2 − 1

=

1112 12 321 2909 − 7 = 7 7 −1 6

2909 09 −

2909 − 1760.143 1148.85 57 7 = 6 6

= 191.48 (degrees of freedom = N 2 − 1 = 6)

Step 3

The larger variance estimate is divided by the smaller: F= =

larger variance estimate smaller variance estima mate 191.48 19.81

= 9.67 67 (df larger variance estimaatte = 6, df smaller variance estimate = 6)

Step 4

We need to check whether or not a difference between the two variance estimates as large as this ratio implies would be likely if the samples came from the same population of scores. Significance Table 20.1 contains the critical values for the F-ratio. To use the table you find the intersection of the column for the degrees of freedom for the larger variance estimate and the degrees of freedom for the smaller variance estimate. Notice that the degrees of freedom we want are not listed for the numerator, so we take the next smaller listed value. Thus the table tells us we need a value of 4.4 at a minimum to be significant at the 5% level with a one-tailed test. Our calculated value of F is substantially in excess of the critical value. Thus we conclude that it is very unlikely that the two samples come from the same population of scores. We accept the hypothesis that the two sample variances are significantly different from each other.

Interpreting the results The interpretation of the F-ratio test is simply a matter of examining the two variance estimates to see which is the largest value. If the F-ratio is statistically significant then the larger of the variance estimates is significantly larger than the smaller one.

Reporting the results The results could be written up according to the APA (2010) Publication Manual recommendations as follows: ‘Despite there being no difference between the mean scores on emotionality following ECT to left and right brain hemispheres, the variance of emotionality was significantly higher for ECT to the right hemisphere, F(6, 6) = 9.67, p < 0.05. This suggests that ECT to the right hemisphere increases emotionality in some people but decreases it in others.’

M20_HOWI0749_06_SE_C20.indd 260

11/18/13 12:10 PM

Key points 261



Research examples Comparing variances Arden and Plomin (2006) were interested in determining whether greater variance in intelligence in males and females were found in early childhood. They compared the variance of intelligence in boys and girls at the ages of 2, 3, 4, 7, 9 and 10 and found greater variance in boys compared to girls at every age apart from at 2. In this analysis, they used Levene’s test of homogeneity of variance rather than the F-ratio which would have been an alternative test. Ruscio and Roche (2012) addressed the question of the extent to which the parametric assumptions of statistical tests in terms of equality of variances (and normality) are met by researchers. The past evidence is that normality assumptions are frequently violated but sample variance inequality has received little attention. Ruscio and Roche took 455 studies published in top psychology journals and noted the variances of the different groups in each study on the dependent variable. It is an assumption that the variances of groups used in statistics such as ANOVA and the regular version of the t-test should be equal – that is, not differ significantly. It was found that the variances of groups in a study often varied significantly using the F-ratio test and similar procedures. Vista and Care (2011) point to the scarcity of research on gender differences in intelligence in non-Western countries and evidence from Southeast Asia is uncommon. They administered a non-verbal intelligence test (the Naglieri Non-verbal Ability Test) to a national sample of 2700 public schoolchildren in the Philippines in three different age groups. Studying mean scores from the research showed very little by way of gender differences. The trend is non-existent or, at most, very trivial. However, this was not at all the case when variance ratio tests (F-ratios) were calculated. There was evidence of greater variability of scores for males compared to females in the upper half of the distribution of scores and the reverse trend of greater variability of scores for females compared to males in the lower part of the distribution of scores. Although the research provides little evidence of gender differences in intelligence, it raises important questions about the distribution of intelligence between the genders in this context.

Key points ●

Psychologists often fail to explore for differences in variances in their data. It is good practice to routinely examine your data for them where they might be meaningful.



The F-ratio is a necessary adjunct to applying the unrelated t-test correctly. Make sure that you check that the variances are indeed similar before using the t-test.



Be very careful when you use the F-ratio in the analysis of variance (Chapter 21 onwards). The F-ratio in the analysis of variance is not quite the same. In this you do not always divide the larger variance estimate by the smaller variance estimate.

M20_HOWI0749_06_SE_C20.indd 261

11/18/13 12:10 PM

CHAPTER 20  The variance ratio test: the f-ratio to compare two variances

262

Computer analysis The F-ratio test using SPSS

Figure 20.2

SPSS Statistics steps for computing variance

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

Decide which of the variance estimates is the larger. If the F-ratio is statistically significant then this variance is significantly larger than the smaller one.



In APA recommended style you could write: ‘Despite there being no difference between the mean scores on emotionality following ECT to left and right brain hemispheres, the variance of emotionality was significantly higher for ECT to the right hemisphere, F(6, 6) = 9.67, p < 0.05. This suggests that ECT to the right hemisphere increases emotionality in some people but decreases it in others.’

M20_HOWI0749_06_SE_C20.indd 262

11/18/13 12:10 PM

Computer analysis 263



Screenshot 20.1

The data

Screenshot 20.2

Select the test

Screenshot 20.3

Move variables

Screenshot 20.4

Select options

Screenshot 20.5

M20_HOWI0749_06_SE_C20.indd 263

The important output

11/18/13 12:10 PM

Chapter 21

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) Introduction to the one-way unrelated or uncorrelated ANOVA

Overview ●

The one-way analysis of variance compares the variation in the means of a minimum of two groups but is most commonly used when there are three or more mean scores to compare.



This chapter concentrates on the case where the samples of scores are unrelated – that is, there is no relation between the samples and they consist of different people.



The scores are the dependent variable, the groups are the independent variable.



In essence, the ANOVA estimates the variance in the population due to the cell means (between variance) and the variance in the population due to random (or error) processes (within variance). These are compared using the F-ratio test.



Error is variation which is not under the researcher’s control.



A significant finding for the analysis of variance means that overall some of the means differ from each other.

Preparation It is pointless to start this chapter without a clear understanding of how to calculate the basic variance estimate formula and the computational formula for variance estimate (Chapter 4). A working knowledge of the variance ratio test (F-ratio test) is also essential (Chapter 20).

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 264

11/18/13 12:12 PM

21.2  Some revision and some new material 265

21.1 Introduction Up to this point we have discussed research designs comparing the means of just two groups of scores. The analysis of variance (ANOVA) can do this but in addition can extend the comparison to three or more groups of scores. Analysis of variance takes many forms but is primarily used to analyse the results of experiments. Nevertheless, the simpler forms of ANOVA are routinely used in surveys and similar types of research. This chapter describes the one-way analysis of variance. This can be used whenever we wish to compare two or more groups in terms of their mean scores on a dependent variable. The scores must be independent (uncorrelated or unrelated). In other words, each respondent contributes just one score to the statistical analysis. Stylistically, Table 21.1 is the sort of research design for which the (uncorrelated or unrelated) oneway analysis of variance is appropriate.

Table 21.1

Stylised table of data for unrelated analysis of variance

Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

Group 4

9

3

1

27

14

1

4

24

11

5

2

25

12

5

31

The scores are those on the dependent variable. The groups are the independent variable. There are very few limitations on the research designs to which this is applicable: ●

It is possible to have any number of groups with the minimum being two.



The groups consist of independent samples of scores. For example the groups could be: ●

men versus women



an experimental versus one control group



four experimental groups and one control group



three different occupational types – managers, office personnel and production workers.



The scores (the dependent variable) can be for virtually any variable. The main thing is that they are numerical scores suitable for calculating the mean and variance.



It is not necessary to have equal numbers of scores in each group. With other forms of analysis of variance, not having equal numbers can cause complications.

21.2 Some revision and some new material You should be familiar with most of the following. Remember the formula for variance: variance[ de 1 nitional formula ] =

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 265

∑ (X − B )

2

N

11/18/13 12:12 PM

266

CHAPTER 21  Analysis of variance (ANOVA): Introduction to the one-way unrelated or uncorrelated ANOVA

If you wish to estimate the variance of a population from the variation in a sample from that population, you use the variance estimate formula which is: variance estimate[ de 1 nitional formula ] =

∑ (X − B )

2

N −1

(By dividing by N − 1 we get an unbiased estimate of the population variance from the sample data.) It is useful if you memorise the fact that the top part of the formula, i.e. ∑(X − B)2 is called the sum of squares. It is the sum of the squared deviations from the mean. The phrase ‘sum of squares’ occurs repeatedly in all forms of the analysis of variance so cannot be avoided. The bottom part of the variance formula (N) or variance estimate formula (N − 1) is called the degrees of freedom. In the analysis of variance it is a little complex in that its calculation can vary. Nevertheless, memorising that the phrase ‘degrees of freedom’ refers to the bottom part of the variance formulae is a useful start. We can rewrite this formula as a computational formula:

variance estimate[ computational formula ] =

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

N −1

2

N

21.3 Theoretical considerations The analysis of variance involves very few new ideas. However, some basic concepts are used in a relatively novel way. Unfortunately, most textbooks confuse readers by presenting the analysis of variance rather obscurely. In particular, they use a variant of the computational formula for the calculation of the variance estimate, which makes fol­ lowing the logic of what is happening very difficult. This is a pity since the analysis of variance is relatively simple in many respects. The main problem is the number of steps which have to be coped with. All measurement assumes that a score is made up of two components: ●

the ‘true’ value of the measurement



an ‘error’ component.

In other words, the score that is obtained through measurement consists of a True Score plus an Error component. This is illustrated in Figure 21.1. The obtained score com­pon­ ent can take any value and so can the error component but they add up to the measured score. Error can take a positive or negative value. Most psychological measurements tend to have a large error component compared with the true component. Error results from all sorts of factors – tiredness, distraction,

Figure 21.1

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 266

The components of a measured score in ANOVA

11/18/13 12:12 PM

21.3  Theoretical considerations 267

unclear instructions and so forth. Normally we cannot say precisely to what extent these factors influence our scores. It is further assumed that the ‘true’ and ‘error’ components add together to give the obtained scores (i.e. the data). So, for example, an obtained score of 15 might be made up of: 15[obtained score] = 12[true] + 3[error] or an obtained score of 20 might be made up as follows: 20 = 24 + (−4) We have no certain knowledge about anything other than the obtained scores. The true and error scores cannot be known directly. However, in some circumstances we can infer them through intelligent guesswork. It is not difficult to understand how this is done in ANOVA. So in the analysis of variance, each score is separated into the two components – true scores and error scores. This is easier than it sounds. Look at the data of some fictitious research in Table 21.2. It is a study of the effects of two different hormones and an inert (placebo) control on depression scores in men. Tables 21.3 and 21.4 give the best estimates possible of the ‘true’ scores and ‘error’ scores in Table 21.2. Try to work out the simple ‘tricks’ we have employed. All we did to produce these two new tables was the following: ●

In order to obtain a table of ‘true’ scores we have simply substituted the column mean for each group for the individual scores, the assumption being that the obtained scores deviate from the ‘true’ score because of the influence of varying amounts of error in the measurement. In statistical theory, error is assumed to be randomly distributed. Thus we have replaced all of the scores for Group 1 by the mean of 9.667. The column mean is simply the best estimate of what the ‘true’ score would be for the group if we could get rid of the ‘error’ component. As all of the scores are the same, there is absolutely no error component in any of the conditions of Table 21.3. The

Stylised table of data for unrelated analysis of variance with means

Table 21.2

Group 1 Hormone 1

Group 2 Hormone 2

Group 3 Placebo control

9

4

3

12

2

6

8

5

3

Mean = 9.667

Mean = 3.667

Mean = 4.000 Overall mean = 5.778

Table 21.3

‘True’ scores based on the data in Table 21.2

Group 1 Hormone 1

Group 2 Hormone 2

Group 3 Placebo control

9.667

3.667

4.000

9.667

3.667

4.000

9.667

3.667

4.000

Mean = 9.667

Mean = 3.667

Mean = 4.000 Overall mean = 5.778

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 267

11/18/13 12:12 PM

268

CHAPTER 21  Analysis of variance (ANOVA): Introduction to the one-way unrelated or uncorrelated ANOVA

Table 21.4

’Error’ scores based on the data in Table 21.2

Group 1 Hormone 1

Group 2 Hormone 2

Group 3 Placebo control

−0.667

0.333

−1.000

2.333

−1.667

2.000

−1.667

1.333

−1.000

Mean = 0.000

Mean = 0.000

Mean = 0.000 Overall mean = 0.000

assumption in this is that the variability within a column is due to error so the average score in a column is our best estimate of the ‘true’ score for that column. Notice that the column means are unchanged by this. ●

We have obtained the table of ‘error’ scores (Table 21.4) simply by subtracting the scores in the ‘true’ scores table (Table 21.3) away from the corresponding score in the original scores table (Table 21.2). What is not a ‘true’ score is an ‘error’ score by definition. Notice that the error scores show a mixture of positive and negative values, and that the sum of the error scores in each column (and the entire table for that matter) is zero. This is always the case with error scores and so constitutes an import­ ant check on your calculations should you wish to try out ANOVA for yourself. An alternative way of obtaining the error scores is to take the column (or group) mean away from each score in the original data table. This, of course, will give you exactly the same values for the error component.

So what do we do now that we have the ‘true’ scores and ‘error’ scores? The two derived sets of scores – the ‘true’ and the ‘error’ scores – are used separately to estimate the variance of the population of scores from which they are samples. (That is, the calculated variance estimate for the ‘true’ scores is an estimate of the ‘true’ variation in the population, and the calculated variance estimate of the ‘error’ scores is an estimate of the ‘error’ variation in the population.) Remember, the null hypothesis for this research would suggest that differences between the three groups are due to error rather than real differences related to the influence of the independent variable. The null hypothesis suggests that both the ‘true’ and ‘error’ variance estimates are similar since they are both the result of error. If the null hypothesis is correct, the variance estimate derived from the ‘true’ scores should be no different from the variance estimate derived from the ‘error’ scores. After all, under the null hypothesis the variation in the ‘true’ scores is due to error anyway. If the alternative hypothesis is correct, then there should be rather more variation in the ‘true’ scores than is typical in the ‘error’ scores. We calculate the variance estimate of the ‘true’ scores and then calculate the variance estimate for the ‘error’ scores. See Chapter 20 for a discussion of variance estimates. Next the two variance estimates are examined to see whether they are significantly different using the F-ratio test (the variance ratio test). This involves the following calculation: F=

variance estimate[of true scores] variance estimate[of error scores]

(The error variance is always at the bottom in the analysis of variance. This is different from the F-ratio test described in the previous chapter. This is because we want to know if the variance estimate of the true scores is bigger than the variance estimate of the ‘error’ scores. We are not simply comparing the variances of two conditions.)

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 268

11/18/13 12:12 PM

21.3  Theoretical considerations 269

Significance Table 21.1

5% significance values of the F-ratio for unrelated ANOVA. Additional values are given in Significance Table 20.1

Degrees of freedom for error or within cells mean square (or variance estimate) 1

Degrees of freedom for true or between-treatment mean square (or variance estimate) 1

161 or more

2

3

4

5



200

216

225

230

254

2

18.5

19.0

19.2

19.3

19.3

19.5

3

10.1

9.6

9.3

9.1

9.0

8.5

4

7.7

6.9

6.6

6.4

6.3

5.6

5

6.6

5.8

5.4

5.2

5.1

4.4

6

6.0

5.1

4.8

4.5

4.4

3.7

7

5.6

4.7

4.4

4.1

4.0

3.2

8

5.3

4.5

4.1

3.8

3.7

2.9

9

5.1

4.3

3.9

3.6

3.5

2.7

10

5.0

4.1

3.7

3.5

3.3

2.5

13

4.7

3.8

3.4

3.2

3.0

2.2

15

4.5

3.7

3.3

3.1

2.9

2.1

20

4.4

3.5

3.1

2.9

2.7

1.8

30

4.2

3.3

2.9

2.7

2.5

1.6

60

4.0

3.2

2.8

2.5

2.4

1.4



3.8

3.0

2.6

2.4

2.2

1.0

Your value has to be equal or be larger than the tabulated value for an effect to be significant at the 5% level.

It is then a fairly straightforward matter to use Significance Table 21.1 for the F-distribution to decide whether or not these two variance estimates are significantly different from each other. We just need to be careful to use the appropriate numbers of degrees of freedom. The F-ratio calculation was demonstrated in Chapter 20. If the variance estimates are similar then the variance in ‘true’ scores is little different from the variance in the ‘error’ scores; since the estimated ‘true’ variance is much the same as the ‘error’ variance in this case, both can be regarded as ‘error’. On the other hand, if the F-ratio is significant it means that the variation due to the ‘true’ scores is much greater than that due to ‘error’; the ‘true’ scores represent reliable differences between groups rather than chance factors. As mentioned in Chapter 20, the F-ratio, unlike the t-test, is a one-tailed test. It simply determines whether the true variance estimate is bigger than the error variance estimate. The F-ratio cannot be smaller than zero. In other words, it is always positive. The 5% or .05 probability only applies to the upper or right-hand tail of the distribution. The larger the F-ratio is, the more likely it is to be statistically significant. And that is just about it for the one-way analysis of variance. There is just one remaining issue: the degrees of freedom. If one were to work out the variance estimate of the original data in our study we would use the formula as given above:

variance estimate[original data] =

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

N −1

2

N

where N − 1 is the number of degrees of freedom.

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 269

11/18/13 12:12 PM

270

CHAPTER 21  Analysis of variance (ANOVA): Introduction to the one-way unrelated or uncorrelated ANOVA

Figure 21.2

Conceptual steps for understanding the one-way analysis of variance

However, the calculation of the number of degrees of freedom varies in the analysis of variance (it is not always N − 1). With the ‘true’ and ‘error’ scores the degrees of freedom are a little more complex although easily calculated using formulae. But the idea of degrees of freedom can be understood at a more fundamental level with a little work as shown in the next section. Figure 21.2 gives the key steps for an unrelated analysis of variance.

21.4 Degrees of freedom This section gives a detailed explanation of degrees of freedom. You may find it easier to return to this section when you are a little more familiar with ANOVA. Degrees of freedom refer to the distinct items of information contained in your data. By information we mean something which is new and not already known. For example, if we asked you what is the combined age of your two best friends and then asked you the age of the younger of the two, you would be crazy to accept a bet that we could tell you the age of your older best friend, the reason being that if you told us that the combined ages of your best friends was 37 years and that the younger was 16 years, any fool could work out that the older best friend must be 21 years. The age of your older best friend is contained within the first two pieces of information. The age of your older friend is redundant because you already know it from your previous information. It is much the same sort of idea with degrees of freedom – which might be better termed the quantity of distinct information.

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 270

11/18/13 12:12 PM

21.4  Degrees of freedom 271

Table 21.5

‘True’ scores based on the data in Table 21.2

Group 1 Hormone 1

Group 2 Hormone 2

Group 3 Placebo control

9.667

3.667

4.000

9.667

3.667

4.000

9.667

3.667

4.000

Mean = 9.667

Mean = 3.667

Mean = 4.000 Overall mean = 5.778

Table 21.6

Insertion of arbitrary values in the first column Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

10.000





10.000





10.000





Mean = 10.000 Overall mean = 5.778

Table 21.5 repeats the table of the ‘true’ scores that we calculated earlier as Table 21.3. The question is how many items of truly new information the table contains. You have to bear in mind that what we are looking at is the variance estimate of the scores which is basically their variation around the overall mean of 5.778. Don’t forget that the overall mean of 5.778 is our best estimate of the population mean under the null hypothesis that the groups do not differ. Just how many of the scores in this table are we able to alter and still obtain this same overall mean of 5.778? For this table, we simply start rubbing out the scores one by one and putting in any value we like. So if we start with the first person in group 1 we can arbitrarily set their score to 10.000 (or any other score you can think of). But, once we have done so, each score in group 1 has to be changed to 10.000 because the columns of the ‘true’ score table have to have identical entries. Thus the first column has to look like the column in Table 21.6 (the dashes represent parts of the table we have not dealt with yet). The scores in italics are ones which are not free to vary. We have been free to vary just one score so far. We can now move on to the group 2 column. Here we can arbitrarily put in a score of 3.000 to replace the first entry. Once we do this then the remaining two scores in the column have to be the same because this is the nature of ‘true’ tables – all the scores in a column have to be identical (Table 21.7). Thus so far we have managed to vary only two scores independently. We can now move on to group 3. We could start by entering, say, 5.000 to replace the first score, but there is a problem. The overall mean has to end up as 5.778 and the number 5.000 will not allow this to happen given that all of the scores in group 3 would have to be 5.000. There is only one number which can be put in the group 3 column which will give an overall mean of 5.778, that is 4.333 (Table 21.8). We have not increased the number of scores we were free to vary by changing group 3 – we have changed the scores but we had no freedom other than to put one particular

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 271

11/18/13 12:12 PM

272

CHAPTER 21  Analysis of variance (ANOVA): Introduction to the one-way unrelated or uncorrelated ANOVA

Table 21.7

Insertion of arbitrary values in the second column Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

10.000

3.000



10.000

3.000



10.000

3.000



Mean = 10.000

Mean = 3.000 Overall mean = 5.778



Table 21.8

Forced insertion of a particular value in the third column because of the requirement that the overall mean is 5.778 Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

10.000

3.000

4.333

10.000

3.000

4.333

10.000

3.000

4.333

Mean = 10.000

Mean = 3.000

Mean = 4.333 Overall mean = 5.778

Table 21.9

‘Error’ scores based on the data in Table 21.2 Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

−0.667

0.333

−1.000

2.333

−1.667

2.000

−1.667

1.333

−1.000

Mean = 0.000

Mean = 0.000

Mean = 0.000 Overall mean = 0.000

score in their place. Thus we have varied only two scores in the ‘true’ scores table – notice that this is one less than the number of groups we have. We speak of the ‘true’ scores having two degrees of freedom. It is a similar process with the error table. The requirements this time are (a) that the column averages equal zero and (b) that the overall average equals zero. This is because they are error scores which must produce these characteristics – if they do not they cannot be error scores. Just how many of the scores can we vary this time and keep within these limitations? (We have ‘adjusted’ the column means to ignore a tiny amount of rounding error.) The answer is six scores (Table 21.9). The first two scores in each group can be varied to any values you like. However, having done this the value of the third score has to be fixed in order that the column mean equals zero. Since there are three equal-size groups then there are six degrees of freedom for the error table in this case.

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 272

11/18/13 12:12 PM

21.4  Degrees of freedom 273

Just in case you are wondering, for the original data table the degrees of freedom correspond to the number of scores minus one. This is because there are no individual column constraints – the only constraint is that the overall mean has to be 5.778. The lack of column constraints means that the first eight scores could be given any value you like and only the final score is fixed by the requirement that the overall mean is 5.778. In other words, the variance estimate for the original data table uses N − 1 as the denominator – thus the formula is the usual variance estimate formula for a sample of scores. Also note that the degrees of freedom for the ‘error’ and ‘true’ scores tables add up to N − 1.

■ Quick formulae for degrees of freedom



Anyone who has difficulty with the above explanation of degrees of freedom should take heart. Few of us would bother to work out the degrees of freedom from first principles. It is much easier to use simple formulae. For the one-way analysis of variance using unrelated samples, the degrees of freedom are as follows: N = number of scores in the table degrees of freedom[original data] = N − 1 degrees of freedom[‘true’ scores] = number of columns − 1 degrees of freedom[‘error’ scores] = N − number of columns This is not cheating – most textbooks ignore the meaning of degrees of freedom and merely give the formulae anyway.

Explaining statistics 21.1

How the unrelated/uncorrelated one-way analysis of variance works Step-by-step, the following is the calculation of the analysis of variance.

Step 1

Draw up your data table using the format shown in Table 21.10. The degrees of freedom for this table are the number of scores minus one = 9 − 1 = 8.

Table 21.10

Data table for an unrelated analysis of variance

Group 1 Hormone 1

Group 2 Hormone 2

Group 3 Placebo control

9

4

3

12

2

6

8

5

3

Mean = 9.667

Mean = 3.667

Mean = 4.000 Overall mean = 5.778



M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 273

11/18/13 12:12 PM

274

CHAPTER 21 ANAlySIS Of VArIANCe (ANOVA): INtrOduCtION tO the ONe-wAy uNrelAted Or uNCOrrelAted ANOVA

Although this is not absolutely necessary you can calculate the variance estimate of your data table as a computational check – the sum of squares for the data table should equal the total of the sums of squares for the separate components. Thus, adding together the true and error sums of squares should give the total sum of squares for the data table. Similarly, the data degrees of freedom should equal the total of the true and error degrees of freedom. We will use the computational formula:

variance estimate[original data] =

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

2

N

df

∑X2 means square each of the scores and then sum these individual calculations:

∑X2 = 92 + 42 + 32 + 122 + 22 + 62 + 82 + 52 + 32 = 81 + 16 + 9 + 144 + 4 + 36 + 64 + 25 + 9 = 388 2

(∑X) means add up all of the scores and then square the total:

(∑ X )

2

= (9 + 4 + 3 + 12 + 2 + 6 + 8 + 5 + 3)2 = (52)2 = 2704

The number of scores N equals 9. The degrees of freedom (df ) equal N − 1 = 9 − 1 = 8. Substituting in the formula:

variance estimate[original data] = =

Step 2

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

df

N

2

=

2704 9 8

388 88 −

388 − 300.444 87.556 = = 10.944 8 8

Draw up Table 21.11 of ‘true’ scores by replacing the scores in each column by the column mean.

∑X2 = 9.6672 + 3.6672 + 4.0002 + 9.6672 + 3.6672 + 4.0002 + 9.6672 + 3.6672 + 4.0002 = 93.451 + 13.447 + 16.000 + 93.451 + 13.447 + 16.000 + 93.451 + 13.447 + 16.000 = 368.694

(∑ X )

2

= (9.667 + 3.667 + 4.000 + 9.667 + 3.667 + 4.000 + 9.667 + 3.667 + 4.000)2 = (52.000)2 = 2704

Table 21.11

‘true’ scores based on the data in table 21.10 Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

9.667

3.667

4.000

9.667

3.667

4.000

9.667

3.667

4.000

Mean = 9.667

Mean = 3.667

Mean = 4.000 Overall mean = 5.778

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 274

11/18/13 12:12 PM

21.4 degreeS Of freedOm

275

The number of scores N equals 9. The degrees of freedom (df ) are given by: degrees of freedom[true scores] = number of columns − 1 = 3 − 1 = 2 We can now substitute in the formula:

variance estimate[true scores] =

=

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

2

N

df 368..694 − 2

2704 9

368.964 − 300.444 . = 2 =

Step 3

68.250 = 34.125 2

Draw up the table of the ‘error’ scores (Table 21.12) by subtracting the ‘true’ scores table from the original data table (Table 21.10). Remember all you have to do is to take the corresponding scores in the two tables when doing this subtraction. The alternative is to take the appropriate column mean away from each score in your data table.

variance estimate[error] =

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

2

N

df

∑X2 = (−0.667)2 + 0.3332 + (−1.000)2 + 2.3332 + (−1.667)2 + 2.0002 + (−1.667)2 + 1.3332 + (−1.000)2 = 0.445 + 0.111 + 1.000 + 5.443 + 2.779 + 4.000 + 2.779 + 1.777 + 1.000 = 19.334

(∑ X )

2

= [(−0.667) + 0.333 + (−1.000) + 2.333 + (−1.667) + 2.000 + (−1.667) + 1.333 + (−1.000)] =0

The number of scores N equals 9. The degrees of freedom (df ) equal N minus the number of columns, i.e. 9 − 3 = 6. We can now substitute in the above formula:

Table 21.12

‘error’ scores based on the data in table 21.10 Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

−0.667

0.333

−1.000

2.333

−1.667

2.000

−1.667

1.333

−1.000

Mean = 0.000

Mean = 0.000

Mean = 0.000 Overall mean = 0.000



M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 275

11/18/13 12:12 PM

CHAPTER 21 ANAlySIS Of VArIANCe (ANOVA): INtrOduCtION tO the ONe-wAy uNrelAted Or uNCOrrelAted ANOVA

276

variance estimate[error] =

Step 4

∑X

2

(∑ X ) − N

df

2

19.334 − =

6

0 9

= 3.222

We can now work out the F-ratio by dividing the variance estimate[true scores] by the variance estimate[error scores]: F − ratio = =

variance estimate[true scores] variance eesstimate[error scores] 34.125 3.222

= 10.6 (deg (degre reeess of freedom = 2 for true and 6 for error) From Significance Table 21.1, we need a value of F of 5.1 or more to be significant at the 5% level of significance. Since our value of 10.6 is substantially larger than this, we can reject the null hypothesis and accept the hypothesis that the groups are significantly different from each other at the 5% level of significance.

21.5

The analysis of variance summary table The analysis of variance calculation can get very complicated with complex experimental designs. In preparation for this, it is useful to get into the habit of recording your analysis in an analysis of variance summary table. This systematically records major aspects of the calculation. Table 21.13 is appropriate for this. Notice that the sums of squares for ‘true’ and ‘error’ added together are the same as the sum of squares of the original data (allowing for rounding errors). Don’t forget that the sum of squares is simply the upper part of the variance estimate formula. Similarly the degrees of freedom of ‘true’ and ‘error’ scores added together give the degrees of freedom for the original data. The degrees of freedom are the lower part of the variance estimate formula. In Table 21.13, we have used the terminology from our explanation. This is not quite standard in discussions regarding the analysis of variance. It is more usual to see the analysis of variance summary table in the form of Table 21.14 which uses slightly different terms. Tables 21.13 and 21.14 are equivalent except for the terminology and the style of reporting significance levels:

Table 21.13 Source of variation

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 276

Analysis of variance summary table for unrelated ANOVAs Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Variance estimate

F-ratio

Significance

10.6

5%

‘true’ scores

68.222

2

34.111

‘error’ scores

19.334

6

3.222

Original data

87.556

8

10.944

11/18/13 12:12 PM

21.5  The analysis of variance summary table 277

Table 21.14

Analysis of variance summary table for unrelated ANOVAs using alternative terminology

Source of variation

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square

F-ratio

10.6*

Between groups

68.222

2

34.111

Within groups

19.334

6

3.222

Total

87.556

8

10.944

* Significant at 5% level.





‘Mean square’ is analysis of variance terminology for variance estimate. Unfortunately the name ‘mean square’ loses track of the fact that it is an estimate and suggests that it is something new.



‘Between’ is another way of describing the variation due to the ‘true’ scores. The idea is that the variation of the ‘true’ scores is essentially the differences between the groups or experimental conditions. Sometimes these are called the ‘treatments’.



‘Within’ is just another way of describing the ‘error’ variation. It is called ‘within’ since the calculation of ‘error’ is based on the variation within a group or experimental condition.



Total is virtually self-explanatory – it is the variation of the original scores which combine ‘true’ and ‘error’ components.

■ Interpreting the results The most important step in interpreting your data is simple. You need a table of the means for each of the conditions such as Table 21.10. It is obvious from this table that two of the cell means are fairly similar whereas the mean of Group 1 is relatively high. This would suggest to an experienced researcher that if the one-way analysis of variance is statistically significant, then a multiple comparisons test (Chapter 24) is needed in order to test for significant differences between pairs of group means.



■ Reporting the results The results of this analysis could be written up following the APA (2010) Publication Manual recommendations as: ‘The data were analysed using an unrelated one-way analysis of variance. It was found that there was a significant effect of the independent variable drug treatment on the dependent variable depression, F(2, 6) = 10.59, p < 0.05. The mean for the hormone 1 group (M = 9.67) appears to indicate greater depression scores than for the hormone 2 group (M = 3.67) and the placebo control (M = 4.00).’ Of course, you can use Appendix J to test for significance at other levels. In order to test whether the mean for group 1 is significantly greater than for the other two groups, it is necessary to apply a multiple comparisons test such as the Scheffé test (Chapter 24) if the differences had not been predicted. The outcome of this should also be reported. We have given intermediary calculations for the F-ratio; these are not usually reported but may be helpful for calculation purposes.

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 277

11/18/13 12:12 PM

278

CHAPTER 21  Analysis of variance (ANOVA): Introduction to the one-way unrelated or uncorrelated ANOVA

Research examples Unrelated one-way ANOVA Carolan and Power (2011) asked about the sorts of emotion experienced by those diagnosed with bipolar disorders. One relevant theoretical model (SPAARS) suggests that mania involves mainly the emotions of happiness and anger in combination. In contrast, depression (including bipolar) involves predominantly the emotions of sadness and disgust. The structured clinical interview was used to confirm the clinical group (mania or depression) to which the person belonged. The participants’ mood states were measured using different psychological measures: 1) the Beck Depression Inventory, 2) the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory and 3) a Mania Scale and a Basic Emotions Scale. One-way unrelated ANOVAs compared the bipolar, unipolar and control groups in terms of the emotions primarily experienced. The analysis clearly showed that for the bipolar condition (mania) the emotions of happiness, anger and fear tended to be significantly higher. However, in depressed states the most elevated emotions are fear, sadness, disgust and anger. The findings tended to support the SPAARS model well. Edenfield, Adams and Briihl (2012) studied adult relationship attachment style in post-secondary level students. Their research focus was on the relationship maintenance strategies employed by those manifesting the different attachment styles (e.g. secure, fearful, dismissive). Measurement instruments were 1) for relationship style the Experiences in Close Relationships Inventory and 2) for relationship maintenance strategies the Relationship Maintenance Questionnaire. The participants were sorted into the different relationship attachment style groups. These groups were then used in an unrelated one-way ANOVA in order to examine the characteristic use of each of the relationship maintenance tactics as measured by the Relationship Maintenance Questionnaire. The avoidance relationship maintenance style was characteristic of the fearful and dismissive relationship style. This style was associated with fewer assurances, less positivity and less openness. Frank and his colleagues (2012) tested whether intolerance for uncertainty is significantly higher in women with the eating disorders of 1) bulimia or 2) anorexia nervosa than ‘healthy’ women. They found a significant effect with a one-way analysis of variance. To determine which of the three groups differed significantly from one another the Tukey post hoc test was employed. Intolerance of uncertainty was significantly higher in women with bulimia or anorexia nervosa than in the healthy women. Jenkins, Conley, Rienecke Hoste, Meyer and Blissett (2012) investigated whether eating disorder pathology, general psychopathology and quality of life varied in five groups of female students who differed in whether they over-ate and had lost control of their over-eating. They used a one-way ANOVA to show that there was a significant effect for the groups. They then used Tukey B post hoc tests to see which groups differed from each other. Groups which had lost control of their over-eating showed significantly greater eating disorder pathology, greater general psychopathology and lower quality of life than groups which had not lost control. MacCabe and co-workers (2012) addressed whether neurocognitive impairment is a central characteristic deficit in schizophrenics. There are, however, schizophrenic patients in the superior intelligence range. MacCabe et al. studied schizophrenics with a pre-illness IQ of 115 or greater in order to assess their neuropsychological profile. Thirty-four patients meeting the DSM-1V diagnostic requirements for schizophrenia were used in the study. Their mean pre-illness IQ estimate was 120 IQ points. They were divided into two groups – those whose IQ had declined by 10 IQ points from their pre-illness estimate and those whose IQ had not declined in this way. The IQs of these were compared with a group of matched healthy controls and another sample of typical schizophrenia patients. These various groups were compared using the one-way unrelated ANOVA plus Bonferroni adjusted comparisons (see Chapter 24). It was not possible to distinguish schizophrenia patients whose IQs were in the superior range statistically from the matched healthy controls on any of the neurocognitive tests. Furthermore, their relative performances on the various subtests (e.g. picture completion, letter-number sequencing, and forward and backward memory of digits) were indistinguishable from typical

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 278

11/18/13 12:12 PM

Key pOINtS

279

schizophrenia patients. In other words, intellectually superior schizophrenia patients are not characterised at all by gross neuropsychological deficits. meeten and davey (2012) researched the question of whether manipulating mood by showing participants one of five films influenced their emotions. the five mood conditions were sad, happy, anxious, angry and neutral. these five conditions were rated on the four mood scales of sadness, happiness, anxiety and anger. there was a significant one-way ANOVA effect for each mood rating. planned t-tests showed that sadness was highest in the sad condition, happiness in the happy condition, anxiety in the anxious condition and anger in the angry condition. Sierra, livianos and rojo (2005) researched the question whether the eight subscale scores of a measure of quality of life in patients with bipolar depression differed according to four categories of marital status and eight categories of employment status. None of the one-way ANOVAs for either of these variables were statistically significant showing that these quality of life indices did not differ according to marital or employment status. tyson, wilson, brailsford and law (2010) looked at the association between physical activity and anxiety and depression in a student sample. they broke down physical activity into three groups of low, medium and high physical activity and used a one-way analysis to determine whether anxiety and depression differed significantly between the three groups. they found a significant difference for both dependent variables – anxiety and depression. they used post hoc tests to determine which groups differed significantly. they found that the lowest level of anxiety and depression was shown by the high physical activity group and the highest level of anxiety and depression was shown by the low physical activity group.

Key points ●

the t-test is simply a special case of one-way ANOVA, so these tests can be used interchangeably when you have two groups of scores. they give identical significance levels. the square of the two-tailed t-value equals the one-tailed F-value (e.g. 1.962 = 3.8416) and the square root of the one-tailed F-value equals the two-tailed t value (e.g. 3.8 8416 = 1.96.



do not be too deterred by some of the strange terminology used in the analysis of variance. words like treatments and levels of treatment merely reveal the agricultural origins of these statistical procedures; be warned that it gets worse. levels of treatment simply refers to the number of different conditions for each independent variable. thus if the independent variable has three different values it is said to have three different levels of the treatment.



the analysis of variance with just two conditions or sets of scores is relatively easy to interpret. you merely have to examine the difference between the means of the two conditions. It is not so easy where you have three or more groups. your analysis may not be complete until you have employed a multiple comparisons procedure as in Chapter 24. which multiple comparisons test you use may be limited by whether your ANOVA is significant or not.



when the F-ratio is statistically significant for a one-way analysis of variance with more than two groups, you need to determine which groups differ significantly from each other. If you had good grounds for predicting which groups differed, you could use an unrelated t-test to see if the difference was significant (see Chapter 14). If you did not have a sound basis for predicting which groups differed, you would use a multiple comparison test such as the Scheffé test (Chapter 24.)

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 279

11/18/13 12:12 PM

CHAPTER 21  Analysis of variance (ANOVA): Introduction to the one-way unrelated or uncorrelated ANOVA

280

Computer analysis One-way analysis of variance using SPSS

Figure 21.3

SPSS Statistics steps for one-way analysis of variance

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

Start with the table of means for each of the conditions of study. Ask yourself what the pattern of different means implies. In this case, one of the means seems to be very different from the other two. The implication of this is that a multiple comparison test such as explained in Chapter 24 would be helpful.



An APA (2010) style write-up for this analysis might be: ‘Using a one-way analysis of variance, it was found that there was a significant effect of the independent variable drug treatment on the dependent variable depression, F(2, 6) = 10.59, p < 0.05. The mean for the hormone 1 group (M = 9.67) appears to indicate greater depression scores than for the hormone 2 group (M = 3.67) and the placebo control (M = 4.00).’

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 280

11/18/13 12:12 PM

Computer analysis 281



Screenshot 21.1

The data

Screenshot 21.2

Select the test

Screenshot 21.3

Select variables

Screenshot 21.4

Select options

Screenshot 21.5

The output

M21_HOWI0749_06_SE_C21.indd 281

11/18/13 12:12 PM

Chapter 22

Analysis of variance for correlated scores or repeated measures Overview ●

The related analysis of variance is used to compare two or more related samples of means: for example, when the same group of participants is assessed three times on a measure. That is, measurement takes place under a number of conditions.



The scores are the dependent variable, the different occasions on which the measure is taken constitute the independent variable.



Because individuals are measured more than once, it is possible to estimate the impact of the characteristics of the individual on the scores. This allows a separate assessment of the variation in the data due to these individual differences. Effectively this variation can be removed from the data.



The amount of error variance is lower in related designs since the variation due to individual differences is removed. What remains of the error is known as the ‘residual’ or residual error. The value of the residual is compared to the variation due to the condition using the F-ratio.



A significant value of the F-ratio shows that the means in the conditions differ from each other overall. It does not tell you that all the means differ overall or that different pairs of means differ from each other. These differences are tested for separately using a multiple comparisons procedure.

Preparation You need a good understanding of the unrelated/uncorrelated analysis of variance (Chapter 21). In addition, the difference between correlated/related samples and unrelated/uncorrelated samples (or repeated measures) should be revised.

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 282

11/18/13 1:40 PM

22.1  Introduction 283

22.1 Introduction The analysis of variance covered in this chapter is also called the related, related scores, related samples, repeated measures and matched analysis of variance. Correlated or related research designs are held to be efficient forms of planning research. Generally these designs involve the same group of participants being assessed in two or more research conditions. The assumption is that by doing so, many of the differences between people are ‘allowed for’ by having each person ‘serve as their own control’ – that is, appear in all of the research conditions. The different sets of scores in the related or correlated analysis of variance are essentially different treatment conditions. We can describe them as either different levels of the treatment or different experimental conditions (Table 22.1). The numerical scores are scores on the dependent variable. They can be any measures for which it is possible to calculate their means and variances meaningfully, in other words basically numerical scores. The treatments are the levels of the independent variable. There are very few limitations to the use of this research design: ●

It is possible to have any number of treatments with two being the minimum.



The groups consist of related or correlated sets of scores. For example: ●

Children’s IQs assessed at the age of 5 years, then again at 8 years and finally at 10 years (Table 22.2).



Two experimental conditions versus two control conditions so long as the same subjects are in each of the conditions. The research is a study of reaction time to recognising words. The two experimental conditions are very emotive words (fourletter words) and moderately emotive words (mild swear words). The two control

Table 22.1 Case Case 1 (John)

Treatment 1

Treatment 2

Treatment 3

Treatment 4

9

14

6

18

Case 2 (Heather)

7

12

9

15

Case 3 (Jane)

5

11

6

17

Case 4 (Tracy)

10

17

12

24

Case 5 (Paul)

8

15

7

19

Table 22.2

Research design of IQ assessed sequentially over time

Child

Age 5 years

Age 8 years

Age 10 years

John

120

125

130

93

90

100

130

140

110

Paula Sharon, etc.

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 283

Stylised research design for the analysis of variance

11/18/13 1:40 PM

284

CHAPTER 22  Analysis of variance for correlated scores or repeated measures





Table 22.3

Reaction time in seconds comparing two experimental conditions with two control conditions

Subject

Four-letter words

Mild swear words

Neutral words

Nonsense syllables

Darren

0.3

0.5

0.2

0.2

Lisa, etc.

0.4

0.3

0.3

0.4

Table 22.4

Weight in pounds before and after dieting

Dieter

Before diet

After diet

Ben

130

120

Claudine, etc.

153

141

Table 22.5

Stylised ANOVA design using matched samples

Matched set

Treatment 1

Treatment 2

Treatment 3

Treatment 4

Matched set 1

9

14

6

18

Matched set 2

7

12

9

15

Matched set 3

5

11

6

17

Matched set 4

10

17

12

24

Matched set 5

8

15

7

19

conditions are using neutral words and using nonsense syllables; the dependent variable is reaction time (Table 22.3). ●

A group of weight-watchers’ weights before and after dieting. The dependent variable is their weight in pounds (Table 22.4).



It is necessary to have equal numbers of scores in each group since this is a related subjects or repeated measures design. Obviously in the above examples we have used small numbers of cases.

The related/correlated analysis of variance can also be applied when you have matched sets of people (Table 22.5). By this we mean that although there are different people in each of the treatment conditions, they are actually very similar. Each set is as alike as possible on specified variables such as age or intelligence. One member of each matched set is assigned at random to each of the treatment conditions. The variables forming the basis of the matching are believed or known to be correlated with the dependent variable. There is no point in matching if they are not. The purpose of matching is to reduce the amount of ‘error’ variation.

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 284

11/18/13 1:40 PM

22.2  Theoretical considerations underlying the computer analysis 285

One advantage of using matched sets of people in experiments rather than the same person in several different treatment conditions is their lack of awareness of the other treatment conditions. That is, they only respond in one version of the experimental design and so cannot be affected by their experience of the other conditions. Matching can be done on any variables you wish but it can get cumbersome if there are too many variables on which to match. So, for example, if you believed that age and sex were related to the dependent variable, you could control for these variables by using matched sets which contained people of the same sex and a very similar age. In this way variation due to sex and age is equally spread between the different treatments or conditions. Thus, matched set 1 might consist of four people matched in that they are all females in the age range 21–25 years. Each one of these is randomly assigned to one of the four treatment conditions. Matched set 2 might consist of four males in the age range 16–20 years. Once again, one of each of these four people is randomly assigned to one of the four treatment conditions.

22.2 Theoretical considerations underlying the computer analysis It is a very small step from the uncorrelated to the correlated analysis of variance. All that is different in the correlated ANOVA is that the error scores are reduced (or adjusted) by removing from them the contribution made by individual differences. The basic idea is shown in Figure 22.1. By an individual difference we mean the tendency of a particular person to score generally high or generally low irrespective of the research treatment or condition they are being tested in. So, for example, bright people will tend to score higher on tests involving intellectual skills no matter what the test is. Less bright people may tend to score relatively poorly no matter what the intellectual test is. In uncorrelated research designs there is no way of knowing the contribution of individual differences. In effect, the individual differences have to be lumped together with the rest of the variance which we have called error. But repeated/related/correlated designs allow us to subdivide the error variance into two sorts: a) that which is explained (as individual differences) and b) that which remains unexplained (or residual error variance). So far we have discussed error variance as if it were purely the result of chance factors, but error variance is to some extent explicable in theory – the problem is that we do not know what causes it. If we can get an estimate of the contribution of an individual’s particular characteristics to their scores in our research we should be able to revise the error scores so that they no longer contain any contribution from the individual differences of that participant. (Remember that individual differences are those characteristics of individuals which tend to encourage them to score generally high or generally low on the dependent variable.) Figure 22.2 shows the key steps in related ANOVA.

Figure 22.1

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 285

How scores are broken up in related ANOVA

11/18/13 1:40 PM

286

CHAPTER 22  Analysis of variance for correlated scores or repeated measures

Figure 22.2

Conceptual steps for related ANOVA

22.3 Examples Once we have measured the same participant twice (or more) then it is possible to estimate the individual difference. Take the data from two individuals given in Table 22.6. Looking at these data, we can see the participants’ memory ability for both words and numbers. It is clear that Ann Jones tends to do better on these memory tasks irrespective of the precise nature of the task; John Smith generally does worse no matter what the task. Although both of them seem to do better on memory for numbers, this does not alter the tendency for Ann Jones to generally do best overall. This is not measurement error but a general characteristic of Ann Jones. On average, Ann Jones tends to score six points above John Smith or three points above the overall mean of 15.5 and John Smith tends to score three points below the overall mean of 15.5. In other words, we can give a numerical value to their individual difference relative to the overall mean.

Table 22.6 Subject

Individual differences for two people Memory for words

Memory for numbers

Row mean

Ann Jones

17

20

18.5

John Smith

11

14

12.5 Overall mean = 15.5

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 286

11/18/13 1:40 PM

22.3  Examples 287

Table 22.7

Pain relief scores from a drugs experiment

Participant

Aspirin

Product X

Placebo

Row mean

7

8

6

7.000

Mavis Fletcher

5

10

3

6.000

Bob Polansky

6

6

4

5.333

Ann Harrison

9

9

2

6.667

Bob Robertson

Bert Entwistle

3

7

5

5.000

Column mean

6.000

8.000

4.000

Overall mean = 6.000

Table 22.8 Participant

Amount of adjustment of Table 22.7 for individual differences Overall mean

Row mean

Adjustment needed to error scores to allow for individual differences (overall mean – row mean)

Bob Robertson

6.000

7.000

−1.000

Mavis Fletcher

6.000

6.000

0.000

Bob Polansky

6.000

5.333

0.667

Ann Harrison

6.000

6.667

−0.667

Bert Entwistle

6.000

5.000

1.000

A physiological psychologist is researching the effects of different pain-relieving drugs on the amount of relief from pain that people experience in a controlled trial. In one condition people are given aspirin, in another condition they are given the trial drug product X, and in the third condition (the control condition) they are given a dummy tablet which contains no active ingredient (this is known as a placebo). The amount of relief from pain experienced in these conditions is rated by each of the participants. The higher the score, the more pain relief. Just to be absolutely clear, participant 1 (Bob Robertson) gets a relief from pain score of 7 when given one aspirin, 8 when given product X and 6 when given the inactive placebo tablet (Table 22.7). It is obvious that Bob Robertson tends to get the most relief from pain (the row mean for Bob is the highest there is) because of the tablets whereas Bert Entwistle tends to get the least relief from pain (his row mean is the lowest there is).

Explaining statistics 22.1

How correlated samples analysis of variance works The end point of our calculations is the analysis of variance summary table (Table 22.9). Hopefully by the time we reach the end of our explanation you will understand all of the entries in this table.

Step 1

To begin, you need to tabulate your data. We will use the fictitious relief from pain experiment described above. This is given in Table 22.10.



M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 287

11/18/13 1:40 PM

288

CHAPTER 22  Analysis of variance for correlated scores or repeated measures

Table 22.9

Analysis of variance summary table

Source of variation

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square (or variance estimate)

F-ratio

Probability (sig.)

Between treatments

40.00

2

20.00

5.10

5% (i.e. drugs)

8.67

4

2.17

31.33

8

3.92

80.00

14

Between people (i.e. individual differences) Error (i.e. residual) Total

Table 22.10 Participant

Pain relief scores from a drugs experiment Aspirin

Product X

Placebo

Row mean

Bob Robertson

7

8

6

7.000

Mavis Fletcher

5

10

3

6.000

Bob Polansky

6

6

4

5.333

Ann Harrison

9

9

2

6.667

Bert Entwistle

3

7

5

5.000

Column mean

6.000

8.000

4.000

Overall mean = 6.000

If you wish, you may calculate the variance estimate of this table using the standard variance estimate formula. As this is generally only a check on your calculations, it is unnecessary for our present purposes since it contains nothing new. If you do the calculation then you should find that the sum of squares is 80 and the degrees of freedom 14 which would give a variance estimate value of 5.71 (i.e. 80 divided by 14). The first two pieces of information are entered into the analysis of variance summary table.

Step 2

We then produce a table of the ‘true’ scores. Remember that ‘true’ scores are usually called the ‘between’ or ‘between groups’ scores in analysis of variance. To do this, we simply substitute the column mean for each of the individual scores in that column so leaving no variation within the column – the only variation is between the columns. The results are given in Table 22.11.

Table 22.11

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 288

‘True’ scores (obtained by replacing each score in a column by its column mean)

Participant

Aspirin

Product X

Placebo

Row mean

Bob Robertson

6.000

8.000

4.000

6.000

Mavis Fletcher

6.000

8.000

4.000

6.000

Bob Polansky

6.000

8.000

4.000

6.000

Ann Harrison

6.000

8.000

4.000

6.000

Bert Entwistle

6.000

8.000

4.000

6.000

Column mean

6.000

8.000

4.000

Overall mean = 6.000

11/18/13 1:40 PM

22.3 exAmpleS

289

The estimated variance of these data can be calculated using the standard computational formula:

estimated variance[ true true/b /between /bet ween scor scores es ] =

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

2

N

df

∑X2 = 6.0002 + 8.0002 + 4.0002 + 6.0002 + 8.0002 + 4.0002 +6.0002 + 8.0002 + 4.0002 +6.0002 + 8.0002 + 4.0002 + 6.0002 + 8.0002 + 4.0002

= 36.000 + 64.000 + 16.000 + 36.000 + 64.000 + 16.000 + 36.000 + 64.000 + 16.000 +36.000 + 64.000 + 16.000 + 36.000 + 64.000 + 16.000 = 580

(∑ X )

2

= (6.000 + 8.000 + 4.000 + 6.000 + 8.000 + 4.000 + 6.000 + 8.000 + 4.000 + 6.000 + 8.000 + 4.000 + 6.000 + 8.000 + 4.000)2 = (90)2 = 8100

The number of scores N equals 15. The degrees of freedom (df ) equals the number of columns of data minus 1 (3 − 1 = 2). Substituting in the formula:

estimated variance[ true true/b /between /bet ween scor scores es ] =

= =

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

2

N

df 8100 15 2

580 −

580 − 540 40 = 2 2 = 20.0

Step 3

The error table is now calculated as an intermediate stage. As ever, this is done by subtracting the true/ between scores from the scores in the original data table (see Table 22.12). Alternatively, we subtract the column mean from each of the scores in the data table.

Table 22.12 Participant

‘error’ scores (original data table minus true/between scores) Aspirin

Product X

Placebo

Row mean

Bob robertson

1.000

0.000

2.000

1.000

mavis fletcher

−1.000

2.000

−1.000

0.000

Bob polansky

0.000

−2.000

0.000

−0.667

Ann harrison

3.000

1.000

−2.000

0.667

Bert entwistle

−3.000

−1.000

1.000

−1.000

Column mean

0.000

0.000

0.000

Overall mean = 0.000



M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 289

11/18/13 1:40 PM

290

CHAPTER 22 AnAlYSIS Of vArIAnCe fOr COrrelAted SCOreS Or repeAted meASureS

This is essentially our table of ‘error’ scores, but since the row means vary (Bert Entwistle’s is −1.000 but Mavis Fletcher’s is 0.000) then we still have to remove the effects of the individual differences. This we do simply by taking away the row mean from each of the error scores in the row. That is, we take 1.000 away from Bob Robertson’s error scores, 0.000 from Mavis Fletcher’s, −0.667 from Bob Polansky’s, 0.667 from Ann Harrison’s and −1.000 from Bert Entwistle’s. (Don’t forget that subtracting a negative number is like adding a positive number.) This gives us a revised table of error scores without any individual differences. It is usually called the residual scores table in analysis of variance, but it is just a more refined set of error scores (Table 22.13).

Table 22.13

Participant

‘residual (error)’ scores (obtained by subtracting individual differences or row means from table 22.12) Aspirin

Product X

Placebo

Row mean

Bob robertson

0.000

mavis fletcher

−1.000

−1.000

1.000

0.000

2.000

−1.000

0.000

Bob polansky

0.667

Ann harrison

2.333

−1.333

0.667

0.000

0.333

−2.667

0.000

Bert entwistle

−2.000

0.000

2.000

0.000

Column mean

0.000

0.000

0.000

Overall mean = 0.000

Notice that both the column and row means now equal zero. This is because not only have the ‘true’ or between scores been removed from the table but the individual differences are now gone. We need to check out the degrees of freedom associated with this table. There are more constraints now because the row totals have to equal zero. Thus in the aspirin column we can adjust four scores, but the fifth score is fixed by the requirement that the mean equals zero. In the product X condition we can again vary four scores. However, once we have made these changes, we cannot vary any of the scores in the placebo condition because the row means have to equal zero. In other words, there is a total of eight degrees of freedom in the residual error scores. The formula for the degrees of freedom is quite straightforward: degrees of freedom[residual error scores] = (number of columns of error scores − 1) × (number of rows of error scores − 1) The variance estimate of this residual error can be calculated using the standard formula:

variance estimate[residual error scores] =

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

2

N

df

∑X2 = 0.0002 + (−1.000)2 + 1.0002 + (−1.000)2 + 2.0002 + (−1.000)2 + 0.6672

+ (−1.333)2 + 0.6672 + 2.3332 + 0.3332 + (−2.667)2 + (−2.000)2 + 0.0002 + 2.0002

= 0.000 + 1.000 + 1.000 + 1.000 + 4.000 + 1.000 + 0.445 + 1.777 + 0.445 + 5.443 + 0.111 + 7.113 + 4.000 + 0.000 + 4.000 = 31.334

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 290

11/18/13 1:40 PM

22.3 exAmpleS

(∑ X )

2

291

= [0.000 + (−1.000) + 1.000 + (−1.000) + 2.000 + (−1.000) + 0.667 + (−1.333) + 0.667 + 2.333 + 0.333 + (−2.667) + (−2.000) + 0.000 + 2.000]2 =0

The number of scores N equals 15 as before. The degrees of freedom are given by: degree of freedom = (number of columns − 1) × (number of rows − 1) = (3 − 1) × (5 − 1) =2×4=8 Substituting in the formula:

variance estimate[ resi residua duall erro errorr sco score ress ] =

= =

Step 4

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

2

N

df 31.334 −

0 15

8 31.334 = 3 .9 92 8

This is not absolutely necessary, but the conventional approach to correlated/repeated measures analysis of variance calculates the variance estimate of the individual differences. This is usually described as the between-people variance estimate or ‘blocks’ variance estimate. (The word ‘blocks’ originates from the days when the analysis of variance was confined to agricultural research. Different amounts of fertiliser would be put on a single area of land and the fertility of these different ‘blocks’ assessed. The analysis of variance contains many terms referring to its agricultural origins such as split plots, randomised plots, levels of treatment and so forth.) If you wish to calculate the between-people (or individual differences) variance estimate, you need to draw up Table 22.14, which consists of the individual differences component in each score (this is obtained by the difference between the row means and the overall mean in the original data). In other words, it is a table of the amount of adjustment required to everyone’s scores in order to remove the effect of their individual characteristics.

Table 22.14

Participant

Between-people (individual difference) scores (obtained by taking the difference between the row means and overall mean in the original data) Aspirin

Product X

Placebo

Row mean

Bob robertson

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

mavis fletcher

0.000

0.000

0.000

0.000

Bob polansky

−0.667

−0.667

−0.667

−0.667

Ann harrison

0.667

0.667

0.667

0.667

Bert entwistle

−1.000

−1.000

−1.000

−1.000

Column mean

0.000

0.000

0.000

Overall mean = 0.000



M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 291

11/18/13 1:40 PM

292

CHAPTER 22  Analysis of variance for correlated scores or repeated measures

We calculate the variance estimate of this using the usual variance estimate formula for the analysis of variance. The degrees of freedom are constrained by the fact that the column means have to equal zero and that all the scores in the row are the same. In the end, this means that the degrees of freedom for this table are the number of rows minus one. We have five rows so therefore the number of degrees of freedom is four. The sum of squares for Table 22.14 is 8.67 and the degrees of freedom are 4, therefore the variance estimate is 8.67 ÷ 4 = 2.17. These values can be entered in the analysis of variance summary table. (Strictly speaking, this is another unnecessary stage in the calculation, but it does provide a check on the accuracy of your calculations.)

Step 5

We can enter the calculations into an analysis of variance summary table. It might be more conventional to see an analysis of variance summary table written in the form shown in Table 22.15. Some calculations are unnecessary and we have omitted them.

Table 22.15

Analysis of variance summary table

Source of variation

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square (or variance estimate)

F-ratio

Between treatments (i.e. drugs)

40.00

2

20.00

5.10*

8.67

4

2.17



Between people (i.e. individual differences) Error (i.e. residual)

31.33

8

3.92



Total

80.00

14





* Significant at 5% level.

Notice that the total sum of squares (80.00) is the same as the sum of the individual components of this total (40.00 + 8.67 + 31.33) and this applies also to the degrees of freedom. This can provide a useful check on the accuracy of your calculations.

Interpreting the results The most important part of the analysis is the F-ratio. This is the between-groups variance estimate divided by the error (residual) variance estimate. In other words, it is 20.00/3.92 = 5.10. The statistical significance of this value can be assessed by the use of Significance Table 22.1. With two degrees of freedom for between treatments and eight for the error, a minimum F-ratio of 4.5 is needed to be statistically significant. Thus the obtained F-ratio of 5.10 is significant at the 5% level. The significant probability value of 5% tells us that the variance in the between-groups scores is substantially greater than the error (residual) variance. Thus the null hypothesis that the drugs have no effect on the amount of relief from pain is rejected and the hypothesis that the drugs treatments have an effect at the 5% level of significance is accepted. What you do not know as a result of this analysis is which of the particular groups or conditions differ from each other. The F-ratio is just an overall test. Further analyses using multiple comparisons tests are necessary to say just where the significant differences lie (see Chapter 24).

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 292

11/18/13 1:40 PM

22.3  Examples 293

5% significance values of the F-ratio for related ANOVA (one-tailed test). Additional values are to be found in Significance Table 20.1

Significance Table 22.1

Degrees of freedom for residual or residual error mean square (or variance estimate)

Degrees of freedom for between-treatments mean square (or variance estimate) 1

2

3

4

5



1

161 or more

200

216

225

230

254

2

18.5

19.0

19.2

19.3

19.3

19.5

3

10.1

9.6

9.3

9.1

9.0

8.5

4

7.7

6.9

6.6

6.4

6.3

5.6

5

6.6

5.8

5.4

5.2

5.1

4.4

6

6.0

5.1

4.8

4.5

4.4

3.7

7

5.6

4.7

4.4

4.1

4.0

3.2

8

5.3

4.5

4.1

3.8

3.7

2.9

9

5.1

4.3

3.9

3.6

3.5

2.7

10

5.0

4.1

3.7

3.5

3.3

2.5

13

4.7

3.8

3.4

3.2

3.0

2.2

15

4.5

3.7

3.3

3.1

2.9

2.1

20

4.4

3.5

3.1

2.9

2.7

1.8

30

4.2

3.3

2.9

2.7

2.5

1.6

60

4.0

3.2

2.8

2.5

2.4

1.4



3.8

3.0

2.6

2.4

2.2

1.0

Your value has to equal or be larger than the tabulated value for an effect to be significant at the 5% level.

The use of SPSS and other computer programs make very sophisticated statistical analyses to be computed which would have been very difficult without them. One of these which is applicable here is the test of sphericity. This simply tests whether certain assumptions about your data are met. If they are, then the test of significance is slightly different and, for the same data, more likely to be statistically significant. This is discussed further in the Computer Analysis section at the end of this chapter.

Reporting the results There are a number of ways of reporting this output. ‘One-way repeated measures analysis of variance was used to compare the treatment means. A significant treatment effect was found for the three conditions, F(2, 8) = 5.10, p < .05. The Aspirin mean was 6.00, the Product X mean 8.00, and the Placebo mean was 4.00.’ The results of Bonferroni related t-tests could be added. These are discussed in Chapter 24.

The related/correlated scores analysis of variance is different in that we make adjustments for these tendencies for individuals to typically score generally high or generally low or generally in the middle. We simply subtract each person’s row mean from the table’s overall mean of 6.000 to find the amount of adjustment needed to each person’s score in order to ‘eliminate’ individual differences from the scores. Thus for Bob Robertson we need to add −1 (i.e. 6.000 − 7.000) to each of his scores in order to overcome the tendency of his scores to be 1.000 higher than the overall mean (i.e. average

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 293

11/18/13 1:41 PM

294

CHAPTER 22  Analysis of variance for correlated scores or repeated measures

score in the table). Do not forget that adding −1 is the same as subtracting 1. Table 22.8 shows the amount of adjustment needed to everyone’s scores in order to eliminate individual differences. Apart from the adjustment for individual differences, the rest of the analysis of variance is much as in Chapter 21.

Research examples Correlated/related ANOVA Chan and Singhal (2013) investigated the effect of seeing positive, negative, neutral and no words while driving in a simulator. All participants were run in all four conditions counterbalanced in a Latin square design. In the ideal case, this involves participants taking part in each condition and that the orders of the conditions varying in a way in which all possible orders are employed equally. A number of one-way repeated measures ANOVAs were carried out. For example, ANOVA was used to analyse differences in mean driving speed between the four conditions. A significant main effect was found. Planned contrasts were used to determine which means differed significantly. These showed 1) that the no word condition had a significantly higher mean speed than the neutral or negative word conditions and 2) the positive word condition had a significantly higher mean speed than the neutral word condition. Dumont and Louw (2009) analysed the impact of the work of Henri Tajfel (1919–1982) on social psychology. They suggest that his work formed the infrastructure to European social psychology over a long period of time. They collected data on the citations to his work in five prominent psychology journals. Six time periods starting with 1972–1976 and ending with 1997–2002 formed the conditions for a related samples one-way analysis of variance. This showed that the percentages of articles published in these journals varied significantly over the six time periods. Furthermore, multi-comparisons employing Bonferroni correction showed that the percentage for each time period was significantly greater than that of the preceding one. Hunter and his colleagues (2011) manipulated mood by showing the same participants pictures that were designed to elicit happy, neutral or sad feelings. To check whether these pictures evoked these feelings, participants rated each picture on a 7-point bipolar sad–happy scale. A one-way repeated measures ANOVA was carried out which found a significant effect. Related t-tests were used to show that the happy pictures made participants feel significantly happier than the neutral pictures, which made them feel significantly happier than the sad pictures. Kam and his colleagues (2012), in trying to understand health professionals’ intentions to refer cancer patients for psychosocial support services, asked them how likely they were to refer patients to three support services. A one-way repeated measures ANOVA found a significant effect. Although it was reported that ‘that referral intentions for complementary therapies were significantly lower than allied professionals and the Cancer Helpline (Wilks’ lambda = 50.28, F(2, 58) = 574.96, p < 0.001, multivariate partial eta squared = 50.72)’, no results were presented for multiple comparison tests. McKiernan and his colleagues (2010) were interested in determining the effectiveness of a cognitivebehavioural group intervention for patients with early breast cancer. This intervention was known as the Time to Adjust Programme. The researchers had four measures of how well the patients were doing which were

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 294

11/18/13 1:41 PM

Key points 295



assessed before the patients received treatment, immediately after treatment had ended and at six-month follow-up. To test their hypothesis that patients in this group would show a significant improvement over time on each of these four dependent variables, four repeated measures ANOVAs were used. Each of the four ANOVAs were statistically significant. Looking at the means for each measure at the three measurement times showed that they had improved. Perlman (2011) points out that research suggests that teachers often use teaching styles which undermine the motivation of students. Using what is known as self-determination theory teachers’ behaviours have been changed to be more motivationally supportive. The purpose of Perlman’s research was to assess the influence of using a Sport Education approach as opposed to a skill-drill game approach on the teaching behaviour of pre-service physical education teachers. An observation protocol was used to code teacher–student interaction episodes employing 15 different categories. Furthermore, the teachers were given a breakdown of their use of autonomy supportive, controlling and neutral comments. The Learning Climate Questionnaire and the Sport Motivation Scale were completed by the students which provided scores on their perceptions of autonomy–support and individual motivation. The data were collected on a repeated basis over time from this group of participants including the questionnaire data. Related analysis of variance was used to assess the data. The use of the Sports Education Approach resulted in higher levels of autonomy supportive interactions on the part of the teachers. Stasiewicz, Schlauch, Bradizza, Bole and Coffey (2013) suggest that pretreatment consumption of alcohol offers a challenge to the view that treatment for alcoholism is largely responsible for alcohol consumption changes. More needs to be known about pre-treatment change processes which follow the decision to request help but precede the treatment itself. The researchers studied the pre-treatment behaviours in a group of participants volunteering for a cognitive-behavioural treatment for alcohol dependence. Several pre-treatment intervals were created such as the period from the first phone call to baseline assessment, the period between baseline assessment to first treatment. Days abstinent from drinking in these periods was the dependent variable. The data were analysed using a related ANOVA because each participant was measured in each time period. The data analysis revealed that there were significant reductions in the days drinking and the number of drinks in the pretreatment period – especially between the telephone call seeking help and the baseline assessment. It is noteworthy that those who changed rapidly in this period tended to be those still abstinent at 90-day follow-up.

Key points ●

Working out this analysis of variance by hand is quite time-consuming and extremely repetitive. Computers will save most people time.



Do not be deterred by some of the strange terminology used in the analysis of variance. Words like blocks, split-plots and levels of treatment have their origins in agricultural research, as does ANOVA.



The analysis of variance in cases in which you have just two conditions or sets of scores is relatively easy to interpret. It is not so easy where you have three or more groups; then your analysis is not complete until you have employed a multiple comparisons procedure as in Chapter 24.

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 295

11/18/13 1:41 PM

CHAPTER 22  Analysis of variance for correlated scores or repeated measures

296

Computer analysis The correlated ANOVA using SPSS

Figure 22.3

SPSS Statistics steps for a repeated measures analysis of variance

Interpreting and reporting the results ●

In this example, assuming sphericity, the exact significance level for F is .037, which means that the analysis is significant at the 5% (.05 probability). If sphericity cannot be assumed then one of the tests of significance below would have to be used (e.g. Greenhouse-Geisser). Since we have three groups, it is appropriate to compare each group using the related t-test adjusted for the number of comparisons (three in this case). In this case, this would mean that the significance obtained has to be smaller than .05/3 = .0167 in order to be reported statistically significant at the .05 level. None of them is statistically significant.



We could describe the results of this analysis in the following way: ‘A one-way repeated measures analysis of variance showed a significant treatment effect for the three conditions, F(2, 8) = 5.10, p = .037, partial η2 = .56. The Aspirin mean was 6.00, the Product X mean 8.00, and the Placebo mean was 4.00. None of the three treatments differed from one another with related t-tests when a Bonferroni adjustment was made for the number of comparisons.’

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 296

11/18/13 1:41 PM

Computer analysis 297



Screenshot 22.1

Data input

Screenshot 22.2

Select the test

Screenshot 22.3

Define the number of groups

Screenshot 22.4

Select variables

Screenshot 22.5

Most important part of the output

M22_HOWI0749_06_SE_C22.indd 297

11/18/13 1:41 PM

Chapter 23

Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/ uncorrelated scores Two studies for the price of one?

Overview ● ● ● ●





The two-way analysis of variance involves two independent variables and a single dependent variable which is the score. It then has the potential to indicate the extent to which the two independent variables may combine to influence scores on the dependent variable. The main effects are the influence of the independent variables acting separately, the interaction is the influence of the independent variables acting in combination. Much of the two-way analysis of variance proceeds like two separate one-way analyses. However, there is the interaction which is really a measure of the multiplicative (rather than additive) influence of the two independent variables acting in combination. Two-way analysis of variance requires great care in its interpretation. It is not possible to adopt a purely mechanical approach. Interpretation is required. The problem is that the main effects are estimated before the interaction effects. Sometimes interaction effects become subsumed as main effects. Care is needed to examine the graph of the interaction to identify this possibility. The two-way analysis of variance can be extended to any number of independent variables though the process rapidly becomes very cumbersome with each additional independent variable.

Preparation Chapter 21 on the one-way analysis of variance contains material essential to the full understanding of this chapter.

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 298

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.1  Introduction 299

23.1 Introduction Often researchers wish to assess the influence of more than a single independent variable at a time in experiments. The one-way analysis of variance deals with a single inde­ pendent variable which can have two or more levels. However, analysis of variance copes with several independent variables in a research design. These are known as multi­ factorial ANOVAs. The number of ‘ways’ is the number of independent variables. Thus a two-way analysis of variance allows two independent variables to be included, threeway analysis of variance allows three independent variables and five-way analysis of variance means that there are five independent variables. There is only one dependent variable no matter how many ‘ways’ in each analysis of variance. If you have two or more dependent variables, each of these will entail a separate analysis of variance. Although things can get very complicated conceptually, two-way analysis of variance is relatively straightforward and introduces just one major new concept – interaction. In this chapter we will be concentrating on examples in which all of the scores are independent (uncorrelated). Each person therefore contributes just one score to the analysis. In other words, it is an uncorrelated design. Generally speaking, the ‘multivariate’ analysis of variance is best suited to experi­ mental research in which it is possible to allocate participants at random into the various conditions. Although this does not apply to the one-way analysis of variance, there are problems in using two-way and multi-way analyses of variance in survey and other non-experimental research. The difficulty is that ideally you should have equal numbers of scores in each cell otherwise the calculation involves estimates. It is hard to do these calculations by hand though easy on the computer as no extra effort is involved. However, the ideal still would be equal numbers in each cell. A typical research design for a two-way analysis of variance is the effect of the independent variables alcohol and sleep deprivation on the dependent variable of people’s comprehension of complex video material expressed in terms of the number of mistakes made on a test of understanding of the video material. The research design and data might look like that shown in Table 23.1. In a sense, one could regard this experiment conceptually as two separate experiments, one studying the effects of sleep deprivation and the other studying the effects of alcohol. The effects of each of the two independent variables are called the main effects. Additionally, the analysis normally looks for interactions which are basically findings that cannot be explained on the basis of the distinctive effects of alcohol level and

Table 23.1

Data for typical two-way analysis of variance: number of mistakes on video test Sleep deprivation

Alcohol

No alcohol

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 299

4 hours

12 hours

24 hours

16

18

22

12

16

24

17

25

32

11

13

12

9

8

14

12

11

12

11/18/13 2:49 PM

300

CHAPTER 23  Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores

sleep deprivation acting separately. For example, it could be that people do especially badly if they have been deprived of a lot of sleep and have been given alcohol. They do more badly than the additive effects of alcohol and sleep deprivation would predict. Interactions are about the effects of specific combinations of variables. If we look carefully at Table 23.1, it is possible to see that the scores in the Alcohol–24 hours cell seem to be rather higher on average than the scores in any of the other cells. Similarly, the scores in the No alcohol–4 hours cell seem to be rather smaller, typically, than the other cells. This is an example of what we mean by an interaction – an outcome which does not seem to be the consequence of the two main variables acting separately. We will return to the concept of interaction later. In the analysis of variance, we sometimes talk of the levels of a treatment – this is simply the number of values that any independent variable can take. In the above example, the alcohol variable has two different values – that is, there are two levels of the treatment or variable alcohol. There are three levels of the treatment or variable sleep deprivation. Sometimes, a two-way ANOVA is identified in terms of the numbers of levels of treatment for each of the independent variables. So a 2 × 3 ANOVA has two different levels of the first variable and three for the second variable. This corresponds to the above example.

23.2 Theoretical considerations Much of the two-way analysis of variance is easy if it is remembered that it largely involves two separate ‘one-way’ analyses of variance as if there were two separate experiments. Imagine an experiment in which one group of subjects is given iron supplements in their diet to see if iron has any effect on their depression levels. In the belief that women have a greater need for iron than men, the researchers included gender as their other independent variable. The data are given in Table 23.2. Figure 23.1 gives the key steps in a two-way ANOVA.

Table 23.2

Males

Females

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 300

Data table for study of dietary supplements Iron supplement

No iron supplement

3

9

7

5

4

6

6

8

Cell mean = 5.00

Cell mean = 7.00

11

19

7

16

10

18

8

15

Cell mean = 9.00

Cell mean = 17.00

Row mean = 13.00

Column mean = 7.00

Column mean = 12.00

Overall mean = 9.50

Row mean = 6.00

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.2  Theoretical considerations 301

Figure 23.1

Conceptual steps for understanding the two-way ANOVA

Table 23.2 represents a 2 × 2 ANOVA. Comparing the four condition means (cell means), the depression scores for females not receiving the supplement seem rather higher than those of any other groups. In other words, it would appear that the lack of the iron supplement has more effect on women. Certain gender and iron supplement conditions in combination have a great effect on depression scores. This suggests an interaction. That is, particular cells in the analysis have much higher or lower scores than can be explained simply in terms of the gender trends or dietary supplement trends acting separately. The assumption in the two-way analysis of variance is that the variation in Table 23.2 comes from four sources: ●

‘error’



the main effect of gender



the main effect of iron supplement



the interaction of gender and iron supplement.

The first three components above are dealt with exactly as they were in the one-way unrelated analysis of variance. The slight difference is that instead of calculating the variance estimate for one independent variable we now calculate two variance estimates – one for each independent variable. However, the term main effect should not cause any confusion. It is merely the effect of an independent variable acting alone as it would if the two-way design were turned into two separate one-way designs. The only difference is that the error term may be smaller than in a one-way ANOVA as part of the error may now be accounted for by the other independent variable and the interaction of the two independent variables. A smaller error term makes it more likely to obtain significant effects. The interaction consists of any variation in the scores which is left after we have taken away the ‘error’ and main effects for the gender and iron supplements sub-experiments. That is, priority is given to finding main effects at the expense of inter­actions. This is important and can lead to incorrectly interpreted analyses if it is not appreciated.

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 301

11/18/13 2:49 PM

302

CHAPTER 23  Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores

23.3 Steps in the analysis

■ Step 1 To produce an ‘error’ table we simply take our original data and subtract the cell mean from every score in the cell. Thus, for instance, we need to subtract 5.00 from each score in the cell for males receiving the iron supplement and 17.00 from each cell for the females not receiving the iron supplement, etc. In the present example the ‘error’ table is as in Table 23.3.

Table 23.3

‘Error’ scores for study of dietary supplements Iron supplement

Males

No iron supplement

3 − 5 = −2

9−7=2

7−5=2

5 − 7 = –2

4 − 5 = −1

6 − 7 = –1

6−5=1

8−7=1

Cell mean = 0.00

Cell mean = 0.00

11 − 9 = 2

19 − 17 = 2

Females

7 − 9 = −2

Row mean = 0.00

16 − 17 = –1

10 − 9 = 1

18 − 17 = 1

8 − 9 = −1

15 − 17 = –2

Cell mean = 0.00

Cell mean = 0.00

Row mean = 0.00

(i) Column mean = 0.00

Column mean = 0.00

Overall mean = 0.00

We calculate the ‘error’ variance estimate for this in the usual way. The formula, as ever, is:

variance estimate[ error ] =

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

2

N

df

The degrees of freedom (df), analogously to the one-way analysis of variance, is the number of scores minus the number of conditions or cells. This leaves 12 degrees of freedom (16 scores minus 4 conditions or cells).



■ Step 2 To produce a table of the main effects for the iron supplement treatment, simply substitute the column means from the original data for each of the scores in the columns. The iron supplement mean was 7.00 so each iron supplement score is changed to 7.00, thus eliminating any other source of variation. Similarly, the no-iron supplement mean was 12.00 so each score is changed to 12.00 (see Table 23.4). The variance estimate of the above scores can be calculated using the usual variance estimate formula. The degrees of freedom are calculated in the familiar way – the number of columns minus one (i.e. df = 1).

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 302

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.3  Steps in the analysis 303

Diet main effect scores for study of dietary supplements

Table 23.4

Iron supplement

No iron supplement

7.00

12.00

7.00

12.00

7.00

12.00

7.00

12.00

7.00

12.00

7.00

12.00

7.00

12.00

7.00

12.00

Row mean = 9.50

Column mean = 7.00

Column mean = 12.00

Overall mean = 9.50

Row mean = 9.50

■ Step 3



To produce a table of the main effect of gender, remember that the independent variable gender is tabulated as the rows (not the columns). In other words, we substitute the row mean for the males and the row mean for the females for the respective scores (Table 23.5).

Gender main effect scores for study of dietary supplements

Table 23.5

6.00

6.00

6.00

6.00

6.00

6.00

6.00

6.00

Row mean = 6.00

13.00

13.00

13.00

13.00

13.00

13.00

13.00

13.00

Row mean = 13.00

Males Females

The variance estimate of the above scores can be calculated with the usual variance estimate formula. Even the degrees of freedom are calculated in the usual way. However, as the table is on its side compared to our usual method, the degrees of freedom are the number of rows minus one in this case (2 − 1 or 1 degree of freedom). The calculation of the main effects (variance estimates) for gender and the iron supplement follows exactly the same procedures as in the one-way analysis of variance.



■ Step 4 The remaining stage is to calculate the interaction. This is simply anything which is left over after we have eliminated ‘error’ and the main effects. So for any score, the interaction score is found by taking the score in your data and subtracting the ‘error’ score and the gender score and the iron supplement score. Table 23.6 is our data table less the ‘error’ variance, in other words a table which replaces each score by its cell mean. It is obvious that the row means for the males and females are not the same. The row mean for males is 6.00 and the row mean for females is 13.00. To get rid of the gender effect, we can subtract 6.00 from each male score and 13.00 from each female score in the previous table. The results of this simple subtraction are found in Table 23.7.

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 303

11/18/13 2:49 PM

304

CHAPTER 23  Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores

Table 23.6

Males

Females

Table 23.7

Males

Females

Data table with ‘error’ removed Iron supplement

No iron supplement

5.00

7.00

5.00

7.00

5.00

7.00

5.00

7.00

9.00

17.00

9.00

17.00

9.00

17.00

9.00

17.00

Row mean = 13.00

Column mean = 7.00

Column mean = 12.00

Overall mean = 9.50

Row mean = 6.00

Data table with ‘error’ and gender removed Iron supplement

No iron supplement

−1.00

1.00

−1.00

1.00

−1.00

1.00

−1.00

1.00

−4.00

4.00

−4.00

4.00

−4.00

4.00

−4.00

4.00

Row mean = 0.00

Column mean = −2.50

Column mean = 2.50

Overall mean = 0.00

Row mean = 0.00

You can see that the male and female main effect has been taken into account since now both row means are zero. That is, there remains no variation due to gender. But you can see that there remains variation due to iron treatment. Those getting the supplement now score −2.50 on average and those not getting the iron treatment score +2.50. To remove the variation due to the iron treatment, subtract −2.50 from the iron supplement column and 2.50 from the non-iron supplement column (Table 23.8). Do not forget that subtracting a negative number is like adding a positive number. Looking at Table 23.8, although the column and row means are zero throughout, the scores in the cells are not. This shows that there still remains a certain amount of variation in the scores even after ‘error’ and the two main effects have been taken away. That is, there is an interaction, which may or may not be significant. We have to check this using the F-ratio test. What the interaction table implies is that women without the iron supplement and men with the iron supplement are getting the higher scores on the dependent variable. We can calculate the variance estimate for the interaction by using the usual formula. Degrees of freedom need to be considered. The degrees of freedom for the above table of the interaction are limited by:

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 304

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.3  Steps in the analysis 305

Table 23.8

Interaction table, i.e. data table with ‘error’, gender and iron supplement all removed Iron supplement

No iron supplement

1.5

−1.5

1.5

−1.5

1.5

−1.5

Males

Females

1.5

−1.5

−1.5

1.5

−1.5

1.5

−1.5

1.5

Row mean = 0.00

−1.5

1.5

Row mean = 0.00

Column mean = 0.00

Column mean = 0.00

Overall mean = 0.00



all scores in the cells having to be equal (i.e. no ‘error’ variance)



all marginal means (i.e. row and column means) having to equal zero.

In other words, there can be only one degree of freedom in this case. There is a general formula for the degrees of freedom of the interaction: degrees of freedom[interaction] = (number of rows − 1) × (number of columns − 1) Since there are two rows and two columns in this case, the degrees of freedom are: (2 − 1) × (2 − 1) = 1 × 1 = 1

■ Step 5



All of the stages in the calculation are entered into an analysis of variance summary table (Table 23.9).

Table 23.9

Analysis of variance summary table

Source of variation

Sums of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square

F-ratio

Gender

196.00

1

196.00

58.96*

Iron supplement

100.00

1

100.00

30.00*

36.00

1

36.00

10.81*

Main effects

Interaction Gender with iron supplement ‘Error’ Total (data)

40.00

12

3.33



372.00

15





* Significant at the 5% level.

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 305

11/18/13 2:49 PM

306

CHAPTER 23  Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores

Notice that there are several F-ratios because you need to know whether there is a significant effect of gender, a significant effect of the iron supplement and a significant interaction of the gender and iron supplement variables. In each case, you divide the appropriate mean square by the ‘error’ mean square. If you wish to check your understanding of the processes involved, see if you can obtain the above table by going through the individual calculations. The significant interaction indicates that some of the cells or conditions are getting exceptionally high or low scores which cannot be accounted for on the basis of the two main effects acting independently of each other. In this case, it would appear that females getting the iron supplement and males not getting the iron supplement are actually getting higher scores than the gender or supplement acting separately and independently of each other would produce. In order to interpret an interaction, you have to remember that the effects of the independent variables are separately removed from the table (i.e. the main effects are removed first). It is only after this has been done that the interaction is calculated. In other words, ANOVA gives priority to main effects, and sometimes it can confuse interactions for main effects. Table 23.10 presents data from the present experiment in which the cell means have been altered to emphasise the lack of main effects. In this example, it is absolutely clear that all the variation in the cell means is to do with the female/no-supplement condition. All the other three cell means are identical at 5.00. Quite clearly the males and females in the iron supplement condition have exactly the same average score. Similarly, males in the iron supplement and no-supplement conditions are obtaining identical means. In other words, there seem to be no main effects at all. The females in the no-supplement condition are the only group getting exceptionally high scores. This would suggest that there is an interaction but no main effects. However, if you do the analysis of variance on these data you will find that there are two main effects and an interaction! The reason for this is that the main effects are estimated before the interaction, so the exceptionally high row mean for females and the exceptionally high column mean for the no-supplement condition will lead to the interaction being mistaken for main effects as your ANOVA summary table might show significant main effects. So you need to examine your data with great care as you carry out your analysis of variance, otherwise you will observe main effects which are an artefact of the method and ignore interactions which are actually there! The analysis of variance may be tricky to execute, but it can be even trickier for the novice to interpret properly – to be frank, many professional psychologists are unaware of the problems. It is yet another example of the importance of close examination of the data alongside the statistical analysis itself.

Table 23.10

Males Females

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 306

Alternative data table showing different trends Iron supplement

No iron supplement

Cell mean = 5.00

Cell mean = 5.00

Row mean = 5.00

Cell mean = 5.00

Cell mean = 17.00

Row mean = 11.00

Column mean = 5.00

Column mean = 11.00

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.3  Steps in the analysis 307

Explaining statistics 23.1

How two-way unrelated analysis of variance works Without a safety net we will attempt to analyse the sleep and alcohol experiment mentioned earlier. It is described as a 2 × 3 analysis of variance because one independent variable has two values and the other has three values (Table 23.11).

Data for sleep deprivation experiment: number of mistakes on video test

Table 23.11

Sleep deprivation 4 hours

12 hours

16

18

22

12

16

24 32

Alcohol

No alcohol

Step 1

24 hours

17

25

11

13

12

9

8

14

12

11

12

(total variance estimate)  We enter the row and column means as well as the means of each of the six cells (Table 23.12).

Table 23.12

Data for sleep deprivation experiment with the addition of cell, column and row means Sleep deprivation

Alcohol

No alcohol

4 hours

12 hours

24 hours

16

18

22

12

16

24

17

25

32

Cell mean = 15.000

Cell mean = 19.667

Cell mean = 26.000

11

13

12

9

8

14

Row mean = 20.222

12

11

12

Cell mean = 10.667

Cell mean = 10.667

Cell mean = 12.667

Row mean = 11.333

Column mean = 12.833

Column mean = 15.167

Column mean = 19.333

Overall mean = 15.777



M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 307

11/18/13 2:49 PM

308

CHAPTER 23 TwO-way analySIS Of varIanCe fOr unrelaTed/unCOrrelaTed SCOreS

variance estimate[ data data ] =

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

2

N

df

∑X2 = 162 + 182 + 222 + 122 + 162 + 242 + 172 + 252 + 322 + 112 + 132 + 122 + 92 + 82 + 142 + 122 + 112 + 122

= 256 + 324 + 484 + 144 + 256 + 576 + 289 + 625 + 1024 + 121 + 169 + 144 + 81 + 64 + 196 + 144 + 121 + 144 = 5162 2

(∑X)

= (16 + 18 + 22 + 12 + 16 + 24 + 17 + 25 + 32 + 11 + 13 + 12 + 9 + 8 + 14 + 12 + 11 + 12)2 = (284)2 = 80 656

The number of scores N equals 18. The degrees of freedom (df ) equal the number of scores minus one, i.e. 17. Substituting in the formula:

variance estimate[ data data ] =

∑ X2 −

(∑ X ) N

df

2

=

80 656 18 1 8 17

5162 −

=

5162 − 4480.889 17

=

681.111 = 40.065 17

The sum of squares here (i.e. 681.111) is called the total sum of squares in the ANOVA summary table. (Strictly speaking, this calculation is unnecessary in that its only function is a computational check on your other calculations.)

Step 2

(‘error’ variance estimate) scores (Table 23.13).

Subtract the cell mean from each of the scores in a cell to obtain the ‘error’

‘error’ scores

Table 22.13

Sleep deprivation 4 hours alcohol

no alcohol

12 hours

24 hours

1.000

−1.667

−4.000

−3.000

−3.667

−2.000

2.000

5.333

6.000

0.333

2.333

−0.667

−1.667

−2.667

1.333

1.333

0.333

−0.667

row mean = 0.000

Column mean = 0.000

Column mean = 0.000

Overall mean = 0.000

Column mean = 0.000

row mean = 0.000

Apart from rounding errors, the cell means, the row means, the column means and the overall mean are all zero – just as required of an ‘error’ table.

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 308

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.3 STepS In The analySIS

309

We calculate the ‘error’ variance estimate using the usual variance estimate formula:

variance estimate[ data data ] =

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

2

N

df

∑X2 = 1.0002 + (−1.667)2 + (−4.000)2 + (−3.000)2 + (−3.667)2 + (−2.000)2 + 2.0002 + 5.3332 + 6.0002 + 0.3332 + 2.3332 + (−0.667)2 + (−1.667)2 + (−2.667)2 + 1.3332 + 1.3332 + 0.3332 + (−0.667)2

= 1.000 + 2.779 + 16.000 + 9.000 + 13.447 + 4.000 + 4.000 + 28.444 + 36.000 + 0.111 + 5.443 + 0.445 + 2.779 + 7.113 + 1.777 + 1.777 + 0.111 + 0.445 = 134.668 2

(∑X)

= [1.000 + (−1.667) + (−4.000) + (−3.000) + (−3.667) + (−2.000) + 2.000 + 5.333 + 6.000 + 0.333 + 2.333 + (−0.667) + (−1.667) + (−2.667) + 1.333 + 1.333 + 0.333 + (−0.667)]2 =0

(Notice that this latter calculation is unnecessary as it will always equal 0 for ‘error’ scores.) The number of scores N equals 18. The degrees of freedom (df ) equal the number of scores minus the number of cells, i.e. 18 − 6 = 12. We can now substitute these values in the formula:

variance estimate[‘error’ scores ] =

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

2

N

df 134 1 34.668 −

=

0 18

12

134.668 = 12 = 11.222

Step 3

(sleep deprivation variance estimate) We now derive our table containing the scores in the three sleep deprivation conditions (combining over alcohol and non-alcohol conditions) simply by replacing each score in the column by the column mean (Table 23.14).

variance estimate[‘sleep ‘sleep depri deprivat vation’ ion’ scor scores es ] =

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

2

N

df

∑X2 = 12.8332 + 15.1672 + 19.3332 + 12.8332 + 15.1672 + 19.3332 + 12.8332

+ 15.1672 + 19.3332 + 12.8332 + 15.1672 + 19.3332 + 12.8332 + 15.1672 + 19.3332 + 12.8332 + 15.1672 + 19.3332

= 164.686 + 230.038 + 373.765 + 164.686 + 230.038 + 373.765 + 164.686 + 230.038 + 373.765 + 164.686 + 230.038 + 373.765 + 164.686 + 230.038 + 373.765 + 164.686 + 230.038 + 373.765 = 4610.934



M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 309

11/18/13 2:49 PM

310

CHAPTER 23 TwO-way analySIS Of varIanCe fOr unrelaTed/unCOrrelaTed SCOreS

Scores due to sleep deprivation

Table 23.14

Sleep deprivation 4 hours

12 hours

24 hours

12.833

15.167

19.333

12.833

15.167

19.333

12.833

15.167

19.333

12.833

15.167

19.333

12.833

15.167

19.333

12.833

15.167

19.333

Column mean = 12.833

Column mean = 15.167

Column mean = 19.333

2

(∑X)

= (12.833 + 15.167 + 19.333 + 12.833 + 15.167 + 19.333 + 12.833 + 15.167 + 19.333 + 12.833 + 15.167 + 19.333 + 12.833 + 15.167 + 19.333 + 12.833 + 15.167 + 19.333)2 = 2842 = 80 656

The number of scores N equals 18. The degrees of freedom (df ) equal the number of columns minus one, i.e. 3 − 1 = 2. We can now substitute these values in the formula:

variance estimate[‘sleep ‘sleep depri deprivat vation’ ion’ scor scores es ] =

Step 4

∑X

2

(∑ X ) − N

df

2

=

4610.934 −

80 656 18

2

=

4610.934 − 4480.889 2

=

130.045 = 65.023 2

(alcohol variance estimate) The main effect for alcohol (or the table containing scores for the alcohol and no-alcohol comparison) is obtained by replacing each of the scores in the original data table by the row mean for alcohol or the row mean for no-alcohol as appropriate. In this way the sleep deprivation variable is ignored (Table 23.15).

Table 23.15

Scores due to alcohol effect alone

alcohol

no alcohol

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 310

20.222

20.222

20.222

20.222

20.222

20.222

20.222

20.222

20.222

11.333

11.333

11.333

11.333

11.333

11.333

11.333

11.333

11.333

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.3 STepS In The analySIS

311

The variance estimate of these 18 scores gives us the variance estimate for the independent variable alcohol. We calculate:

variance estimate[‘alc ‘alcohol ohol’’ sco score ress ] =

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

2

N

df

∑X2 = 20.2222 + 20.2222 + 20.2222 + 20.2222 + 20.2222 + 20.2222 + 20.2222 + 20.2222 + 20.2222 + 11.3332 + 11.3332 + 11.3332 + 11.3332 + 11.3332 + 11.3332 + 11.3332 + 11.3332 + 11.3332

= 408.929 + 408.929 + 408.929 + 408.929 + 408.929 + 408.929 + 408.929 + 408.929 + 408.929 + 128.437 + 128.437 + 128.437 + 128.437 + 128.437 + 128.437 + 128.437 + 128.437 + 128.437 = 4836.294 2

(∑X)

= (20.222 + 20.222 + 20.222 + 20.222 + 20.222 + 20.222 + 20.222 + 20.222 + 20.222 +11.333 + 11.333 + 11.333 + 11.333 + 11.333 + 11.333 + 11.333 + 11.333 +11.333)2 = (284)2 = 80 656

The number of scores N equals 18. The degrees of freedom (df ) equal the number of rows minus one, i.e. 2 − 1 = 1. We can now substitute these values in the formula:

variance estimate[‘alcohol’ scores ] =

Step 5

∑X

2

(∑ X ) −

2

N

df

=

4836.294 −

80 656 18

1

=

4836.294 − 4480.889 1

=

355.405 = 355.405 1

(interaction variance estimate) The final stage is to calculate the interaction. This is obtained by getting rid of ‘error’, getting rid of the effect of sleep deprivation and then getting rid of the effect of alcohol: ●

Remove ‘error’ by simply replacing our data scores by the cell mean (Table 23.16).



Remove the effect of the alcohol versus no-alcohol treatment. This is done simply by subtracting the row mean (20.222) from each of the alcohol scores and the row mean (11.333) from each of the noalcohol scores (Table 23.17).



Remove the effect of sleep deprivation by subtracting the column mean for each sleep deprivation condition from the scores in the previous table. In other words, subtract –2.944, –0.611 or 3.556 as appropriate. (Do not forget that subtracting a negative number is like adding the absolute value of that number.) This leaves us with the interaction (Table 23.18).

The variance estimate from the interaction is computed using the usual formula:

variance estimate[‘inte ‘intera ract ction’ ion’ scor scores es ] =

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

2

N

df



M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 311

11/18/13 2:49 PM

CHAPTER 23  Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores

312

Table 23.16

Data minus ‘error’ (each data score replaced by its cell mean) Sleep deprivation

Alcohol

No alcohol

Table 23.17

4 hours

12 hours

24 hours

15.000

19.667

26.000

15.000

19.667

26.000

15.000

19.667

26.000

10.667

10.667

12.667

10.667

10.667

12.667

10.667

10.667

12.667

Row mean = 11.333

Column mean = 12.833

Column mean = 15.167

Column mean = 19.333

Overall mean = 15.777

Row mean = 20.222

Data minus ‘error’ and alcohol effect (row mean subtracted from each score in Table 23.16) Sleep deprivation 4 hours

Alcohol

No alcohol



Table 23.18

12 hours

24 hours

−5.222

−0.555

5.778

−5.222

−0.555

5.778

−5.222

−0.555

5.778

−0.666

−0.666

1.334

Row mean = 0.000

−0.666

−0.666

1.334

−0.666

−0.666

1.334

Row mean = 0.000

Column mean = −2.944

Column mean = −0.611

Column mean = 3.556

Overall mean = 0.000

Interaction table: data minus ‘error’, alcohol and sleep deprivation (column mean subtracted from each score in Table 23.17) Sleep deprivation

Alcohol

No alcohol

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 312

4 hours

12 hours

24 hours

−2.278

0.056

2.222

−2.278

0.056

2.222

−2.278

0.056

2.222

2.278

−0.056

−2.222

Row mean = 0.000

2.278

−0.056

−2.222

2.278

−0.056

−2.222

Row mean = 0.000

Column mean = 0.000

Column mean = 0.000

Column mean = 0.000

Overall mean = 0.000

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.3 STepS In The analySIS

313

∑X2 = (−2.278)2 + 0.0562 + 2.2222 + (−2.278)2 + 0.0562 + 2.2222 + (−2.278)2

+ 0.0562 + 2.2222 + 2.2782 + (−0.056)2 + (−2.222)2 + 2.2782 + (−0.056)2 + (−2.222)2 + 2.2782 + (−0.056)2 + (−2.222)2

= 5.189 + 0.003 + 4.937 + 5.189 + 0.003 + 4.937 + 5.189 + 0.003 + 4.937 + 5.189 + 0.003 + 4.937 + 5.189 + 0.003 + 4.937 + 5.189 + 0.003 + 4.937 = 60.774 2

(∑X)

= [(−2.278) + 0.056 + 2.222 + (−2.278) + 0.056 + 2.222 + (−2.278) + 0.056 + 2.222 + 2.278 + (−0.056) + (−2.222) + 2.278 + (−0.056) + (−2.222) + 2.278 + (−0.056) + (−2.222)]2 =0

(This latter calculation is an unnecessary calculation as it will always equal 0.) The number of scores N equals 18. The degrees of freedom (df ) are given by the following formula: df = (number of rows − 1) × (number of columns − 1) = (2 − 1) × (3 − 1) =1×2 =2 We can now substitute the above values in the formula:

variance estimate[‘inte ‘intera ract ction’ ion’ scor scores es ] =

∑ X2 −

(∑ X )

2

N

df

= =

Step 6

60.774 −

0 18

2 60.774 − 0 = 30.387 2

Table 23.19 is the analysis of variance summary table. The F-ratios are always the mean square of either one of the main effects or the interaction divided by the variance estimate (mean square) due to ‘error’.

Table 23.19

analysis of variance summary table

Source of variation

Sums of square

Degrees of freedom

Mean square

F-ratio

Sleep deprivation

130.045

2

65.023

5.79a

alcohol interaction

355.405

1

355.405

31.67a

Sleep deprivation with alcohol

60.774

2

30.387

2.71

‘error’

134.668

12

11.222



b

17





Main effects

Total (data)

681.111

a

Significant at 5% level.

b

This form of calculation has introduced some rounding errors.



M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 313

11/18/13 2:49 PM

CHAPTER 23  Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores

314

Significance Table 23.1

5% significance values of the F-ratio for unrelated ANOVA. Additional values are to be found in Significance Table 20.1

Degrees of freedom for error or mean square (or variance estimate) 1

Degrees of freedom for between-treatments mean square (or variance estimate) 1 161 or more

2

3

4

5



200

216

225

230

254

2

18.5

19.0

19.2

19.3

19.3

3

10.1

9.6

9.3

9.1

9.0

19.5 8.5

4

7.7

6.9

6.6

6.4

6.3

5.6

5

6.6

5.8

5.4

5.2

5.1

4.4

6

6.0

5.1

4.8

4.5

4.4

3.7

7

5.6

4.7

4.4

4.1

4.0

3.2

8

5.3

4.5

4.1

3.8

3.7

2.9

9

5.1

4.3

3.9

3.6

3.5

2.7

10

5.0

4.1

3.7

3.5

3.3

2.5

13

4.7

3.8

3.4

3.2

3.0

2.2

15

4.5

3.7

3.3

3.1

2.9

2.1

20

4.4

3.5

3.1

2.9

2.7

1.8

30

4.2

3.3

2.9

2.7

2.5

1.6

60

4.0

3.2

2.8

2.5

2.4

1.4



3.8

3.0

2.6

2.4

2.2

1.0

Your value has to equal or be larger than the tabulated value for an effect to be significant at the 5% level for a two-tailed test (i.e. to accept the hypothesis).

The significance of each F-ratio is checked against Significance Table 23.1. Care must be taken to use the appropriate degrees of freedom. The error in this case is 12, which means that alcohol (with one degree of freedom) must have an F-ratio of 4.8 or more to be significant at the 5% level. Sleep deprivation and the interaction need to have a value of 3.9 or more to be significant at the 5% level. Thus the interaction is not significant, but sleep deprivation is.

Interpreting the results At first glance, the interpretation of the analysis of variance summary table and thus the results of the analysis appears to be quite straightforward in this case: ●

Alcohol has a significant influence on the number of mistakes in the understanding of the video.



The amount of sleep deprivation has a significant influence on the number of mistakes in the understanding of the video.



There is apparently no significant interaction – that is, the differences between the conditions are fully accounted for by alcohol and sleep deprivation acting independently.

But this only tells us that there are significant differences; we have to check the column and row means in order to say precisely which condition produces the greatest number of mistakes. In other words, the analysis of variance summary table has to be interpreted in the light of the original data table with the column, row and cell means all entered.

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 314

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.4  More on interactions 315

Carefully checking the data suggests that the above interpretation is rather too simplistic. It seems that sleep deprivation actually has little effect unless the person has been taking alcohol. The high cell means are associated with alcohol and sleep deprivation. In these circumstances, there is some doubt that the main effects explanation is good enough.

Reporting the results We would conclude, in these circumstances, ‘Although, in the ANOVA, only the main effects were significant, there is reason to think that the main effects are actually the results of the interaction between the main effects. Careful examination of the cell means suggests that especially high scores are associated with taking alcohol and undergoing higher amounts of sleep deprivation. In contrast, those in the no-alcohol condition were affected only to a much smaller extent by having high amounts of sleep deprivation.’ This is tricky for a student to write up since it requires a rather subtle interpretation of the data which might exceed the statistical skills of the readers of their work.

23.4 More on interactions A conventional way of illustrating interactions is through the use of graphs such as those in Figures 23.2 and 23.3. These graphs deal with the sleep and alcohol study just analysed. Notice that the means are given for each of the cells of the two-way ANOVA. Thus the vertical axis is a numerical scale commensurate with the scale of the dependent variable; the horizontal axis simply records the different levels of one of the independent variables. In order to indicate the different levels of the second independent variable, the different cell means for each level are joined together by a distinctively different line. The main point to remember is that main effects are assumed to be effects which can be added directly to the scores in the columns or rows for that level of the main effect

Figure 23.2

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 315

ANOVA graph illustrating possible interactions

11/18/13 2:49 PM

316

CHAPTER 23  Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores

Figure 23.3

ANOVA graph illustrating lack of interactions

Figure 23.4

ANOVA graph illustrating an alternative form of interaction

and that the effect is assumed to be common and equal in all of the cells involved. This implies that: ●

if there is no interaction, then the lines through the points should move more or less parallel to each other



if there is an interaction, then the lines through the points will not be parallel; they may touch, move together or move apart.

Figure 23.3 illustrates the sort of pattern we might expect if there is no interaction between the independent variables. Figure 23.4 shows that it is possible for an interaction to involve the crossing of the lines through the points. Crucially, the pattern illustrated in Figure 23.5 demonstrates the circumstances in which the risk of confusing main effects for the interaction is minimal. This is because, although the two lines are definitely not parallel, the evidence for main effects is not strong but there is evidence of an interaction. Thus there seems to be no sleep deprivation main effect since the means of the no alcohol and alcohol groups combined vertically are more or less the same. Thus there is no main effect of sleep deprivation in

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 316

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.4  More on interactions 317

Figure 23.5

ANOVA graph illustrating interaction when it cannot be mistaken for main effects

Figure 23.5 because of this similarity. In much the same way, if the three means for the no-alcohol condition are averaged and the three means for the alcohol condition are averaged, these two overall means are very similar. In other words, if the means of the combined conditions are the same, this implies, by definition, there is no interaction. In any of the other circumstances such as in Figures 23.2–23.4, combining the means vertically and combining the means horizontally produces combined means which differ. So be comforted if you obtain the pattern shown in Figure 23.5 in your research; there is no element of judgement involved in its interpretation. In the end, simple statistics usually tell you more about your data than many of the more complex statistics. If, when doing ANOVA, you look at graphs like these then you should be able to work out what is happening in your study. The F-ratios and the like simply confirm whether or not what you see is significant.

■ Interpreting the results



Remember that the interpretation of any data should be based first of all on an examination of cell means and variances (or standard deviations) as in Table 23.20. The tests of significance merely confirm whether or not your interpretations can be generalised. It would appear from Table 23.20 that the cell means for the no-alcohol condition are relatively unaffected by the amount of sleep deprivation. However, in the alcohol conditions increasing levels of sleep deprivation produce a greater number of mistakes. There also appears to be a tendency for there to be more mistakes when the participants have taken alcohol than when they have not.

Table 23.20

Table of means for the two-way ANOVA Sleep deprivation 4 hours

12 hours

24 hours

Alcohol

15.000

19.667

26.000

Row mean = 20.222

No alcohol

10.667

10.667

12.667

Row mean = 11.333

Column mean = 12.833

Column mean = 15.167

Column mean = 19.333

Overall mean = 15.777

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 317

11/18/13 2:49 PM

318



CHAPTER 23  Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores

■ Reporting the results The results of this analysis may be written up according to the APA (2010) Publication Manual’s recommendation as follows: ‘A two-way ANOVA was carried out on the data. The two main effects of sleep deprivation, F(2, 12) = 31.67, p < 0.05, and alcohol, F(1, 12) = 5.84, p < 0.05, were statistically significant. The number of errors related to the number of hours of sleep deprivation. Four hours of sleep deprivation resulted in an average of 12.83 errors, 12 hours of sleep deprivation resulted in an average of 15.17 errors, and 24 hours of sleep deprivation resulted in 19.33 errors on average. Consuming alcohol before the test resulted on average in 20.22 errors and the no-alcohol condition resulted in substantially fewer errors (M = 15.78). The interaction between sleep deprivation was not significant despite the tendency of the scores in the alcohol condition with 24 hours of sleep deprivation to be much higher than those in the other conditions, F(2, 12) = 2.71, p ns. Inspection of the graph (Figure 23.2) suggests that there is an interaction since the alcohol and no-alcohol lines are not parallel. It would appear that the interaction is being hidden by the main effects in the ANOVA.’ The significant F-ratio for the main effect of sleep deprivation needs to be explored further by the use of multiple comparisons tests (Chapter 24). Because there are only two alcohol conditions, this is unnecessary for independent variables having only two levels: there is no doubt where the differences lie in circumstances where there are only two values of an independent variable. Given the implications of the graph for the question of an interaction, it would be sensible to carry out multiple comparisons comparing all of the six cell means of the 2 × 3 ANOVA with each other (Chapter 24).

23.5 Three or more independent variables The two-way ANOVA can be extended to include three or more independent variables although you are always restricted to analysing a single dependent variable. Despite this, it should be noted that the complexity of experimental research is constrained by a number of factors including the following: ●

Having a lot of different conditions in an experiment may involve a lot of research and planning time. Preparing complex sets of instructions for participants in the different experimental conditions, randomly assigning individuals to these groups and many other methodological considerations usually limit our level of ambition in research designs. In non-psychological disciplines, the logistics of experiments are different since the units may not be people but, for example, seedlings in pots containing one of several different composts, with different amounts of fertiliser, and one of several different growing temperatures. These are far less time-consuming.



Interpreting ANOVA is more skilful than many researchers realise. Care is needed to interpret even a two-way analysis properly because main effects are prioritised in the calculation, which results in main effects being credited with variation which is really due to interaction.

Since theoretically but not practically there is no limit to the number of independent variables possible in the analysis of variance, the potential for complexity is enormous. However, caution is recommended when planning research. The problems of interpretation get somewhat more difficult the more independent variables there are. The complexity is largely the result of the number of possible interactions. Although there is just one interaction with a two-way analysis of variance, there are four with a three-way analysis

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 318

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.5  Three or more independent variables 319

of variance. The numbers accelerate rapidly with greater numbers of independent variables. As far as possible, we would recommend any psychologist to be wary of going far beyond a two-way analysis of variance without very careful planning and without some experience with these less complex designs. It is possible to disregard the interactions and simply to analyse the different variables in the experiment as if they were several one-way experiments carried out at the same time. The interpretations would be simpler by doing this. However, this is rarely if ever done in psychological research and it is conventional always to consider interactions. Imagine the following three-way or three-factor analysis of variance. The three independent variables are: ●

age – coded as either young or old



gender – coded as either male or female



noise – the research takes place in either a noisy or a quiet environment.

So this is a three-way ANOVA with a total of eight different conditions (2 ages × 2 gender × 2 different noise levels). The dependent variable is the number of errors on a numerical memory test in the different conditions. The main features of this research are presented in Table 23.21. The sheer number of comparisons possible between sections of the data causes problems. These comparisons are: ●

The main effect of gender  that is, comparing males and females irrespective of age or noise.



The main effect of age  that is, comparing young and old irrespective of gender or noise.



The main effect of noise  that is, comparing noisy and quiet conditions irrespective of age or gender.



The interaction of age and gender  that is, comparing age and gender groups ignoring the noise conditions. This would look like Table 23.22.

Table 23.21

A stylised three-way analysis of variance study Noisy conditions Young

Quiet conditions Old

Young

Old

Males Females

Table 23.22

The interaction of age and gender Young

Old

Males Females

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 319

11/18/13 2:49 PM

320

CHAPTER 23  Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores



The interaction of age and noise  that is, comparing age and noise groups ignoring gender. This is shown in Table 23.23.



The interaction of noise and gender  that is, comparing the noise and gender groups ignoring age. This is shown in Table 23.24.



There is a fourth interaction  the interaction of noise and gender and age which is represented by Table 23.25. Notice that the cell means of each of the conditions are involved in this.

Although Table 23.25 looks like the format of the original data table (Table 23.21), the scores in the cells will be very different because all of the other sources of variation will have been removed. The steps in calculating this three-way analysis of variance follow the pattern demonstrated earlier in this chapter but with extra layers of complexity: 1. The error term is calculated in the usual way by subtracting the cell mean from each score in a particular cell. The variance estimate of this table can then be calculated.

Table 23.23

The interaction of age and noise Noisy conditions

Young

Table 23.24

Quiet conditions Old

Young

Old

The interaction of noise and gender Noisy conditions

Quiet conditions

Males Females

Table 23.25

The interaction of noise, gender and age Noisy conditions Young

Quiet conditions Old

Young

Old

Males Females

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 320

11/18/13 2:49 PM

23.5  Three or more independent variables 321

2. The main effect of gender is calculated by substituting the male mean for each of the male scores and the female mean for each of the female scores. The variance estimate of this table can then be calculated. 3. The age main effect is calculated by substituting the mean score of the young people for each of their scores and substituting the mean score of the old people for each of their scores. The variance estimate of this table can then be calculated. 4. The noise main effect is obtained by substituting the mean score in the noisy conditions for each score in the noisy conditions and substituting the mean score in the quiet conditions for each score in the quiet conditions. The variance estimate of this table can then be calculated. 5. The interaction of age and gender is arrived at by taking the table of scores with the error removed and then removing the age and gender difference simply by taking away the column mean and then the row mean. This is the same procedure as we applied to get the interaction in the two-way analysis of variance. The variance estimate of this table can then be calculated. 6. We arrive at the interaction of age and noise by drawing up a similar table and then taking away the appropriate age and noise means in turn. The variance estimate of this table can then be calculated. 7. We arrive at the interaction of noise and gender by drawing up a similar table and then taking away the appropriate noise and gender means in turn. The variance estimate of this table can then be calculated. 8. The three-way interaction (age × noise × gender) is obtained by first of all drawing up our table of the age × noise × gender conditions. We then take away the main effects by subtracting the appropriate age, noise and gender means from this table. But we also have to take away the two-way interactions of age × noise, age × gender and noise × gender by subtracting the appropriate means from the above table. Whatever is left is the three-way interaction. The variance estimate of this final table can then be calculated.

Research examples Two-way unrelated analysis of variance Curseu, Schruijer and Boros (2012) explain that groups in which a minority dissent from the dominant view are complex situations in which the dissent might lead to greater complexity of thinking by the majority but also the rejection and relationship conflict which may ensue also has its influence. Groups need to deal with this. The research involved a design in which some groups experienced minority dissent whereas others did not and some groups retained all members and others lost the dissenting member or a random other member where there was no dissent. These conditions were manipulated by the researchers. Using two-way analysis of variance, it was found that groups with dissent where the deviant left the group tended to have the highest complexity of cognitions about the topic under discussion. It may be that the absence of the dissenting member reduced the need to deal with the ill feelings and upset that their presence would have



M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 321

11/18/13 2:49 PM

322

CHAPTER 23  Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores

caused. The group then might be better placed to think about the nature of the disagreement in a positive and stimulating way. Harinck and Van Kleef (2012) argue that emotion is an important component of conflict resolution. Anger can lead to the other party conceding. The researchers make a case that anger is effective when the matter is one concerning conflicts of interest but not so when conflicts of values are involved. The research design was a 2 × 2 factorial design with one unrelated independent variable being the conflict issue (interest versus values) and the other unrelated independent variable was emotion (anger versus neutral). Psychology students participated for course credit. The experimental manipulation was achieved through the use of scenarios containing different information pertinent to the various experimental conditions. The goal of the negotiation was a pay rise. This could be for self-interest reasons or for reasons of fairness (i.e. the value reason). People perceive anger as unfair in value conflicts to a greater extent than if the conflict is one of interest. That is, there was a significant interaction. Wyrick and Bond (2011) were interested in the influence of the mode of administration of a questionnaire on the amount of disclosure. They used the POSIT (Problem Oriented Screening Instrument for Teenagers) Instrument in either pencil and paper form or in a web-based administration method. They used as one independent variable age (middle versus high school students) and the two modes of administration as the other to give a 2 × 2 ANOVA design. One dependent variable was the number of items omitted by the respondents and another was the perceived risk involved in answering the questions. There was no evidence that risk was related to the experimental manipulation. Contrary to expectations, the students were more likely to skip items on the web than in the pencil and paper version.

Key points ●

Only when you have a 2 × 2 unrelated analysis of variance is the interpretation of the data relatively straightforward. For 2 × 3 or larger analyses of variance, you need to read Chapter 24 as well.



Although at heart simple enough, the two-way analysis of variance is cumbersome to calculate by hand and is probably best done on a computer if you have anything other than small amounts of data.



Analysis of variance always requires a degree of careful interpretation of the findings and cannot always be interpreted in a hard-and-fast way. This is a little disconcerting given its apparent mathematical sophistication.



Before calculating the analysis of variance proper, spend a good deal of effort trying to make sense of the pattern of column, row and cell means in your data table. This should alert you to the major trends in your data. You can use your interpretation in combination with the analysis of variance summary table to obtain as refined an interpretation of your data as possible.

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 322

11/18/13 2:49 PM

Computer analysis 323



Computer analysis Two-way analysis of variance using SPSS

Figure 23.6

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 323

SPSS Statistics steps for two-way analysis of variance

11/18/13 2:49 PM

CHAPTER 23  Two-way analysis of variance for unrelated/uncorrelated scores

324

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

Make sure that you examine the cell means in particular together with the row and column means in order to understand what is going on in the data. Such an inspection would appear to suggest that the cell means for the no-alcohol condition are not related to the amount of sleep deprivation. Where alcohol is consumed, sleep deprivation leads to greater numbers of errors. More mistakes occur, apparently, when alcohol has been taken.



Following APA (2010) style, one might write: ‘A two-way ANOVA revealed that the main effects for sleep deprivation, F(2, 12) = 31.67, p < 0.05, and alcohol, F(1, 12) = 5.84, p < 0.05, were statistically significant. The more sleep deprivation the greater the number of errors. Four hours of sleep deprivation gave an average of 12.83 errors, 12 hours of sleep deprivation 15.17 errors, and 24 hours of sleep deprivation 19.33 errors. Consuming alcohol led to an average of 20.22 errors compared to a mean of 15.78 for the no alcohol condition. There was not a significant interaction though scores in the alcohol condition with 24 hours of sleep deprivation were much higher than those in the other conditions, F(2, 12) = 2.71, p ns. Inspection of the graph (Figure 23.2) suggests that there is an interaction since the alcohol and no-alcohol lines are not parallel. It would appear that the interaction is being disguised by the main effects in the ANOVA.’

Screenshot 23.1

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 324

The data

Screenshot 23.2

Select the test

Screenshot 23.3

Select the plot

11/18/13 2:49 PM

Computer analysis 325



Screenshot 23.4

Screenshot 23.5

M23_HOWI0749_06_SE_C23.indd 325

Select options

The output

11/18/13 2:49 PM

Chapter 24

Multiple comparisons in ANOVA Just where do the differences lie?

Overview ●

Generally speaking, analyses of variance are relatively easy to interpret if the independent variables all have just two different values.



Interpretation becomes difficult with greater numbers of values of the independent variables.



This is because the analysis does not stipulate which means are significantly different from each other. If there are only two values of each independent variable, then statistical significance means that those two values are significantly different.



Multiple comparison tests are available to indicate just where the differences lie.



These multiple comparison tests have built-in adjustment for the numbers of comparisons being made. Hence they are generally to be preferred over multiple comparisons using the t-test.



It is very difficult to know which multiple comparison tests are the most appropriate for any particular data or purpose. Consequently, it is reasonable advice that several different tests should be used. The only problem that arises is when the different tests yield different conclusions.



Some multiple comparison tests may be applied whether or not the ANOVA itself is statist­ ically significant.

Preparation You will need a working knowledge of Chapters 20, 21 and 22 on the analysis of variance. Chapter 15 introduces the problem of multiple comparisons in the context of partitioning chisquare tables.

M24_HOWI0749_06_SE_C24.indd 326

11/18/13 2:50 PM

24.1  Introduction 327

24.1 Introduction When in research there are more than two levels of an independent variable it is not always obvious where the differences between conditions lie. There is no problem when you have only two groups of scores to compare in a one-way or a 2 × 2 ANOVA. However, if there are three or more different levels of any independent variable the interpretation problems multiply. Take, for example, Table 24.1 of means for a one-way analysis of variance. Although the analysis of variance for the data which are summarised in this table may well be statistically significant, there remains a very obvious problem. Groups 1 and 2 have virtually identical means and it is group 3 which has the exceptionally large scores. Quite simply we would be tempted to assume that groups 1 and 2 do not differ signific­ antly and that any differences are due to group 3. Our eyes are telling us that only parts of the data are contributing to the significant ANOVA. Although the above example is very clear, it becomes a little more fraught if the data are less clear-cut than this (Table 24.2). In this case, it may well be that all three groups differ from each other. Just by looking at the means we cannot know for certain since they may just reflect sampling differences.

Table 24.1

Sample means in a one-way ANOVA Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

5.6

5.7

12.9

Mean

Box 24.1

Focus on

Does it matter that the F-ratio is not significant? Traditionally, the advice to users of statistics was that unless the ANOVA itself is statistically significant, no further analyses should be carried out. That is, a significant ANOVA is a prerequisite for multiple comparison testing. Perhaps this was sound advice before the sophisticated modern multiple range tests were developed though it is not now. However, this is a fairly controversial topic which makes straightforward advice difficult. Some multiple range tests are deemed by some authorities to be permissible in circumstances where ANOVA was not significant. With post hoc testing, depending on which multiple comparison

M24_HOWI0749_06_SE_C24.indd 327

test is being contemplated, you do not need a significant ANOVA first. Of course, if the ANOVA is statistically significant then any multiple comparison test is appropriate. Among a number of multiple comparison tests which can be applied irrespective of overall significance are the Neuman–Keuls test and Duncan’s new multiple range test. If one is operating within the strictures of a priori (planned) specific comparisons, then concerns which apply to the post hoc test simply do not apply, as explained elsewhere.

11/18/13 2:50 PM

328

CHAPTER 24  Multiple comparisons in ANOVA : Just where do the differences lie?

Table 24.2

Sample means in another one-way ANOVA

Mean

Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

5.6

7.3

12.9

Obviously it is essential to test the significance of the differences between the means for all three possible pairs of sample means from the three groups. These are: group 1 with group 2 group 1 with group 3 group 2 with group 3 If there had been four groups then the pairs of comparisons would be: group 1 with group 2 group 1 with group 3 group 1 with group 4 group 2 with group 3 group 2 with group 4 group 3 with group 4 This is getting to be a lot of comparisons! It is worthwhile asking yourself whether you need all of the comparisons.

24.2 Methods There are a number of different procedures which you could employ to deal with the problem of just where the differences exist. One traditional approach involves comparing each of the pairs of groups using a t-test (or you could use one-way analysis of variance for two groups). So for the four-group experiment there would be six separate t-tests to calculate (group 1 with group 2, group 1 with group 3, etc.). The problem with this procedure (which is not so bad really) is the number of separate comparisons being made. The more comparisons you make between pairs of means the more likely is a significant difference merely due to chance (always the risk in inferential statistics). Similar procedures apply to the multifactorial (two-way, etc.) analysis of variance. You can compare different levels of any of the main effect pairs simply by comparing their means using a t-test or the equivalent. However, the multiple comparison difficulty remains unless you make an adjustment. To cope with this problem a relatively simple procedure, the Bonferroni method, is used. It assumes that the significance level should be shared between the number of comparisons made. So, if you are making four comparisons (i.e. conducting four separate t-tests) then the appropriate significance level for the individual tests is as follows:

M24_HOWI0749_06_SE_C24.indd 328

11/18/13 2:50 PM

24.3  Planned versus a posteriori (post hoc) comparisons 329

signi 1cance level for each test =

overall signi 1ccance level number of comparisons

5% 4 = 1.25% =

In other words, a comparison actually needs to be significant at the 1.25% level according to the significance tables before we accept that it is significant at the equivalent of the 5% level. This essentially compensates for our generosity in doing many comparisons and reduces the risk of inadvertently capitalising on chance differences. (We adopted this procedure for chi-square in Chapter 15.) Although this is the proper thing to do, we have often seen examples of analyses which fail to make this adjustment. Some researchers tend to stick with the regular 5% level per comparison no matter how many they are doing, although sometimes they point out the dangers of multiple comparisons without making an appropriate adjustment. So long as you adjust your critical values to allow for the number of comparisons made, there is nothing much wrong with using multiple t-tests. Indeed, this procedure, properly applied, is a slightly ‘conservative’ one in that it errs in favour of the null hypothesis. However, there are better procedures for making multiple comparisons, which are especially convenient when using a computer. These include such procedures as the Scheffé test and the Duncan multiple range test. The advantage of these is that they report directly significance levels which are adjusted for the numbers of comparisons being made. Appendix K contains a table of t-values for use when there are a number of comparisons being made (i.e. multiple comparisons). Say you wished to test the statistical significance of the differences between pairs of groups in a three-group one-way analysis of variance. This gives three different comparisons between the pairs. The significant t-test values for this are found under the column for three comparisons.

24.3 Planned versus a posteriori (post hoc) comparisons In the fantasy world of statisticians, there is a belief that researchers are meticulous in planning the last detail of their statistical analysis in advance of doing research. As such an ideal researcher, one would have planned in advance precisely what pairs of cells or conditions in the research are to be compared and unnecessary ones are excluded. These choices are based on the hypotheses and other considerations. In other words, they are planned comparisons. More usual, in our experience, is that the details of the statistical analysis are decided upon after the data have been collected. Psychological theory is rarely so powerful that we can predict from it the precise pattern of outcomes we expect. Comparisons decided upon after the data have been collected and tabulated are called a posteriori or post hoc comparisons. Since properly planned comparisons are not the norm in psychological research, for simplicity we will just consider the more casual situation in which comparisons are made as the data are inspected. (Basically, if your number of planned comparisons is smaller than your number of experimental conditions, then they can be tested by the multiple t-test without adjusting the critical values.) There are a number of tests which deal with the situation in which multiple comparisons are being made. These include Dunnett’s test, Duncan’s test and others. The Scheffé test will serve as a model of the sorts of things achieved by many of these

M24_HOWI0749_06_SE_C24.indd 329

11/18/13 2:50 PM

330

CHAPTER 24  Multiple comparisons in ANOVA : Just where do the differences lie?

tests and is probably as good as any other similar test for general application. Some other tests are not quite so stringent in ensuring that the appropriate level of significance is achieved.

24.4 The Scheffé test for one-way ANOVA Although this can be computed by hand without too much difficulty, the computer output of the Scheffé test is particularly useful as it gives subsets of the groups (or conditions) in your experiment which do not differ significantly from each other. For example, take a look at the following:

Scheffea Subset for alpha = .05

Condition

1

a

3

4.00

1

5.60

2

7.00

Sig.

0.424

Uses Harmonic Mean Sample Size = 3.000.

This indicates that groups 1, 2 and 3 are not significantly different from each other since they all belong in the same group. The right-hand column indicates that all three conditions are in the same subset – it also gives the means involved. If you had significant differences between all three groups then you would have three subsets (subset 1, subset 2 and subset 3) each of which contained just one group. If groups 1 and 3 did not differ from each other but they both differed from group 2 then you would obtain something like the following:

Scheffea Subset for alpha = .05

Condition 1 3

4.00

1

5.60

2 Sig. a

M24_HOWI0749_06_SE_C24.indd 330

2

7.00 0.975

1.000

Uses Harmonic Mean Sample Size = 3.000.

11/18/13 2:50 PM

24.4  The Scheffé test for one-way ANOVA 331

Explaining statistics 24.1

How the Scheffé test works The calculation of the Scheffé test is straightforward once you have carried out an analysis of variance and have the summary table. The test tells you whether two group means in an ANOVA differ significantly from each other. Obviously the calculation has to be repeated for every pair of groups you wish to compare, but no adjustments are necessary for the number of pairs of groups being compared. The following worked example is based on the data in Explaining statistics 21.1. Table 24.3 reminds us about the data and Table 24.4 is the analysis of variance summary table for that calculation.

Step 1

The formula used is based on the F-distribution. It involves the two group means in question, the sample sizes in the relevant conditions and the error (within) mean square. All of these are to be found in Tables 24.3 and 24.4.

Data table for an unrelated analysis of variance

Table 24.3

Group 1 Hormone 1

Group 2 Hormone 2

Group 3 Placebo control

9

4

3

12

2

6

8

5

3

Mean = 9.667

Mean = 3.667

Mean = 4.000

N1 = 3

N2 = 3

N3 = 3 Overall mean = 5.778

Table 24.4

Analysis of variance summary table

Source of variation

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square (variance estimate)

F-ratio

10.59*

Between groups

68.222

2

34.111

Error (within-groups)

19.334

6

3.222

Total

87.556

8

* Significant at the 5% level.



M24_HOWI0749_06_SE_C24.indd 331

11/18/13 2:50 PM

CHAPTER 24 MultIple COMpArISONS IN ANOVA : JuSt where dO the dIffereNCeS lIe?

332

F=

=

(mean of group1 − mean of group 2 ) 2 N + N2 error mean sqaarre × 1 N1 × N 2 (9.667 − 3.667) 2 6 .0 000 2 = 3+3 6 3.222 × 3.222 × 3×3 9 =

36.000 3.222 × 0.667

=

36.000 = 16.75 2.149

Step 2

The significance of this F-ratio depends on the degrees of freedom. The degrees of freedom for the columns in Significance Table 23.1 are the number of groups being compared minus one (i.e. 3 − 1 = 2). The degrees of freedom for the error term (i.e. number of scores – number of groups = 9 − 3 = 6) corresponds to the rows in Significance Table 23.1. The critical value of the F-ratio for these degrees of freedom needs to be adjusted by number of groups minus one. This critical value is about 5.143 which multiplied by 2 is 10.29. This indicates that the difference between the mean of group 1 and that of group 2 is significant at the 5% level with the Scheffé test since the critical F-value is only 10.29.

Step 3

In essence, step 3 repeats steps 1 and 2 but compares group 1 with group 3 and group 2 with group 3. This gives us: F when comparing group 1 with group 3 = 14.94 (significant at 5% level) F when comparing group 2 with group 3 = 0.16 (not significant)

Interpreting the results The use of a multiple comparison test is necessary whenever there are more than two groups to compare in ANOVA. If there are only two groups then any further test is superfluous.

reporting the results The results of this analysis may be written up according to the APA (2010) Publication Manual’s recommendations as follows: ‘The main effect was significant, F(2, 6) = 10.59, p < 0.05. Consequently, the Scheffé test was used to compare pairs of group means in order to assess where the differences lie. Group 1 (M = 9.67) was significantly higher than group 2 (M = 3.67) and group 3 (M = 4.00), p < .05, but the means of groups 2 and 3 did not differ from each other. Thus hormone 1 was associated with higher levels of depression than either hormone 2 or the placebo control which did not differ from each other.’

24.5

Multiple comparisons for multifactorial ANOVA If your experimental design is multifactorial (that is, with two or more independent variables), multiple comparisons are tackled much as for the two-way ANOVA using exactly the same methods (including the adjusted multiple t-test procedure or the Scheffé test). Of course, you would only need such a test if any of the independent variables

M24_HOWI0749_06_SE_C24.indd 332

11/18/13 2:50 PM

24.5  Multiple comparisons for multifactorial ANOVA 333

(factors) have three or more different levels. Otherwise, the significance of the comparisons in the ANOVA is obvious from the ANOVA summary table since there are only two groups of scores to compare with each other (except for interactions). If you have an independent variable with three or more different levels then multiple comparisons are important to tell you precisely where the significant differences lie. It would be possible to carry out multiple comparisons between every cell mean in the ANOVA, but generally this would not be helpful. All one would do is to produce a table analogous to a one-way ANOVA by making the data in each cell of the multifactorial ANOVA into a column of the one-way ANOVA. The difficulty is that this multiplicity of comparisons would be practically uninterpretable since each cell consists of several sources of variation – the various main effects, for example. It is much more useful and viable to employ multiple comparisons to compare the means of the several different levels of the independent variable(s). If there are four different levels of the independent variable, then one would essentially set out the table like a one-way ANOVA with four different levels of the independent variable. It is then possible to test the significance of the differences among the four means using, for example, the Scheffé test. Figure 24.1 shows the key steps for multiple comparison tests.

Figure 24.1

M24_HOWI0749_06_SE_C24.indd 333

Conceptual steps for understanding multiple comparison testing

11/18/13 2:50 PM

334

CHAPTER 24  Multiple comparisons in ANOVA : Just where do the differences lie?

Research examples Multiple comparison tests Ivancevich (1976) conducted a field experiment in which sales personnel were assigned to various goal setting groups. One was a participative goal-setting situation, another was an assigned goal group, and a third group served as a comparison group. Various measures of performance and satisfaction were collected at various data collection points which included a before training baseline, then 6 months, 9 months and 12 months after training. ANOVA was used together with the Duncan’s multiple range test to examine where the significant differences were to be found between the experimental and control conditions. The results suggested that for up to nine months both the participative and assigned goal setting groups had higher performance and satisfaction levels. At 12 months, this advantage no longer applied. Touliatos and Lindholm (1981) compared the ratings on the Behavior Problem Checklist for parents and teachers. Some of the children rated were in counselling and others were not in counselling. Using ANOVA, it was found that the youngsters in counselling were more likely to exhibit deviant behaviour. The independent variables for the ANOVA were counselling versus not in counselling and ratings by mothers versus fathers versus teachers. The researchers wanted to know just where in their data the differences lay. So they used Duncan’s Multiple Range Test which showed that more behavioural problems were seen by parents than by the children’s teachers. Yildirim (2008) investigated the relationship between occupational burnout and the availability of various sources of social support among school counsellors in Turkey. The analysis included other sociodemographic variables. There was a significant negative relationship between burnout and sources of social support. However, burnout was not related to age, gender or marital status in this study. Some of the subdimensions of burnout were related to some of these variables. The Scheffé Test was employed to make finer comparisons between the conditions of the ANOVA. For example, it was found that counsellors with only up to three years of experience had higher levels of depersonalisation of burnout than those with more experience in this sort of counselling.

Key points ●

If you have more than two sets of scores in the analysis of variance (or any other test for that matter), it is important to employ one of the procedures for multiple comparisons.



Even simple procedures such as multiple t-tests are better than nothing, especially if the proper adjustment is made for the number of t-tests being carried out and you adjust the critical values accordingly.



Modern computer packages, especially SPSS Statistics, have a range of multiple comparison tests. It is a fine art to know which is the most appropriate for your particular circumstances. Usually it is expedient to compare the results from several tests; often they will give much the same results, especially where the trends in the data are clear.

M24_HOWI0749_06_SE_C24.indd 334

11/18/13 2:50 PM

Computer analysis 335



Computer analysis Multiple comparison tests using SPSS

Figure 24.2

SPSS Statistics steps for multiple comparison tests

Interpreting and reporting the output ●



Screenshot 24.5 is one way in which SPSS gives multiple comparisons. The table gives all of the possible comparisons between the conditions of the study. It is a little repetitive so you will find similar comparisons included twice. The significance column tells you which means are significantly different from the others. Following APA (2010) guidelines, we might write: ‘The main effect was significant, F(2, 6) = 10.59, p < 0.05. Consequently, the Scheffé test was used to compare pairs of group means. The mean for Hormone 1 (M = 9.67) was significantly higher than Hormone 2 (M = 3.67) and the placebo group (M = 4.00) but no other groups differed significantly.’

M24_HOWI0749_06_SE_C24.indd 335

11/18/13 2:50 PM

336

CHAPTER 24  Multiple comparisons in ANOVA : Just where do the differences lie?

Screenshot 24.1

The data

Screenshot 24.2

Select the test

Screenshot 24.3

Move variables for analysis

Screenshot 24.4

Select multiple comparison tests

Screenshot 24.5

Output for the Scheffé test

Recommended further reading Howell, D. (2013). Statistical methods for psychology (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Duxbury Press.

M24_HOWI0749_06_SE_C24.indd 336

11/18/13 2:50 PM

Chapter 25

Mixed-design ANOVA Related and unrelated variables together

Overview ●

The analysis of variance has procedures for dealing with a variety of research designs.



Mixed designs refer to the situation in which there is a mixture of related and unrelated independent variables.



There is just a single dependent variable.



Mixed designs are complicated by the fact that there is more than one error term. That is, there are different error terms for the unrelated variables and the related variables.

Preparation Chapters 21 to 23 are essential as this chapter utilises many of the ideas from different types of ANOVA.

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 337

11/13/13 5:23 PM

338

CHAPTER 25  Mixed-design ANOVA: Related and unrelated variables together

25.1 Introduction This chapter deals with a useful variant of the analysis of variance: the mixed design. Although we are moving into quite advanced areas of statistics, the key to most statistical analysis lies more in the interpretation of simple statistics such as cell means. Avoid letting the complex calculations employed blind you to the major purpose of your analysis – understanding what your data say. The mixed-design analysis of variance is similar to the two-way ANOVA described in Chapter 23. The big difference is that one of the independent variables is related and one is unrelated – hence the term mixed design. Thus it is used when participants take part in all of the conditions of one independent variable but in just one condition of the other variable. A good example of this type of design is when a pre-test has been given on the dependent variable before the different experimental treatments and a post-test given afterwards. So all participants are measured on both the pre-test and post-test, making pre-test/post-test a related measure. Of course, the unrelated independent variable involves the important experimental manipulation. This design may be extended to involve more than one independent variable and more than one related variable.

Box 25.1

Focus on

Equal cell sizes? Before the introduction of computers, it was conventional in many of the variants of the analysis of variance to ensure that all conditions or cells had the same number of scores. The reason for this was that the hand calculations are simpler if this is the case. When carrying out laboratory studies, equal cell sizes are relatively easy to achieve even if it involves randomly discarding scores from some cells. However, it is possible to do any analysis of variance with unequal numbers of scores in each condition or cell. The calculations tend to be cumbersome and so it is best to use a computer package such as SPSS Statistics to reduce the computational load. The exception to this is the one-way analysis of variance described in Chapter 21 which can be calculated with

no adjustments for unequal sample size. Of course, with the related one-way analysis of variance it is not possible to have different numbers of participants in different conditions of the experiment since participants have to take part in all conditions. One issue remains, though, and that is whether it is better to have equal cell sizes no matter whether a computer package is being used or not. The answer to the question is that it is always better to have equal cell sizes for the simple reason that if data are not there then the computer package has to employ estimates. While the bias caused by this is probably minimal in most cases, anyone employing really complex ANOVA designs would be well advised to try to ensure that equal sample sizes are used.

25.2 Mixed designs and repeated measures Repeated measures designs have the same subjects (or matched groups of subjects) measured in all conditions just as in the repeated measures one-way analysis of variance except that there are two or more independent variables. The repeated measures design is intended to increase the precision of research by measuring the error variance (residual variance) in a way which excludes the individual differences component. The individual

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 338

11/13/13 5:23 PM

25.2  Mixed designs and repeated measures 339

difference component is obtained from the general tendency of individual participants to score relatively high or relatively low irrespective of the experimental condition. The trend for each individual can simply be deducted from the error scores to leave (residual) error. Fully repeated measures designs can be analysed, but they are beyond the scope of this book (see Howell, 2013, for calculation methods). Some independent variables do not allow for repeated measures – gender, for example, is not a repeated measure since a person cannot change their gender during the course of an experiment. Only where matching of groups on the basis of gender has been carried out is it possible to have gender as a repeated measure.

Box 25.2

Key concepts

Fixed and random effects The issue of fixed versus random effects is a typical ana­ lysis of variance misnomer. It really means fixed or random choice of the different levels of an independent variable. The implication is that you can select the levels of a treatment (independent variable) either by a systematic decision or by choosing the levels by some random procedure. Most psychological research assumes a fixed effects model, and it is hard to find instances of the use of random effects. A fixed effect is where you as the researcher choose or decide or fix what the different values of the independent variable are going to be. In some cases you have no choice at all – a variable such as gender gives you no discretion since it has just two different values (male and female). Usually we just operate as if we have the choice of the different treatments for each independent variable. We simply decide that the experimental group is going to be deprived of sleep for five hours and the control group not deprived of sleep at all.

But there are many different possible amounts of sleep deprivation – no hours, one hour, two hours, three hours, four hours and so forth. Instead of just selecting the number of hours of sleep deprivation on the basis of a particular whim, practicality or any other similar basis, it is possible to choose the amounts of sleep deprivation at random. We could draw the amount out of a hat containing the possible levels. In circumstances like these we would be using a random effects model. Because we have selected the hours of sleep deprivation at random, it could be said that our ability to generalise from our experiment to the effects of sleep deprivation in general is enhanced. We have simply chosen an unbiased way of selecting the amount of sleep deprivation after all. Since the random effects model rarely corresponds to practice in psychological research it is not dealt with further in this book. Psychologists’ research is more likely to be the result of agonising about time, money and other practical constraints on the choices available.

Much more common in psychology are mixed designs in which the repeated measure is on just some of the independent variables. Mixed designs are two- or more-way analyses of variance in which participants are measured in more than one experimental condition but not every experimental condition. (This means that for at least one of the independent variables in a mixed design, scores on different participants will be found in the different levels of this independent variable.) Usually you will have to check through the experimental design carefully in order to decide whether a researcher has used a mixed design, although many will stipulate the type of design. One common mixed design is the pre-test/post-test design. Participants are measured on the dependent variable before and after the experimental treatment. This is clearly a related design since the same people are measured twice on the same dependent variable. However, since the experimental and control groups consist of different people, this

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 339

11/13/13 5:23 PM

340

CHAPTER 25  Mixed-design ANOVA: Related and unrelated variables together

Table 25.1

Stylised version of the mixed ANOVA design

Unrelated variable

Related variable Pre-test

Post-test

Experimental condition Control condition

comparison is unrelated. Hence this form of the pre-test/post-test design is a mixed design. This sort of design is illustrated in Table 25.1. Imagine that the dependent variable is self-esteem measured in children before and after the experimental manipulation. The experimental manipulation involves praising half of the children (the experimental group) for good behaviour but telling the other half (the control group) nothing. Obviously this type of design allows the researcher to test whether the two groups are similar prior to the experimental manipulation by comparing the experimental and control groups on the pre-test measure. The hypothesis that praise affects self-esteem suggests that the post-test measure should be different for the two groups. (Notice that the hypothesis predicts an interaction effect in which the related and unrelated independent variables interact to yield rather different scores for the experimental group and the control group on the post-test.) In virtually all respects, the computation of the mixed design is like that for the two-way (unrelated) ANOVA described in Chapter 23. Both main effects and the interaction are calculated in identical fashion. The error is treated differently though as shown in Tables 25.13 and 25.14 (Explaining statistics 25.1). Although the total error is calculated by subtracting the cell mean from each of the data scores to leave the error score (as in Chapter 23), in the mixed design this error is then subdivided into two component parts: (a) the individual differences component and (b) the (residual) error component: ●

the error due to individual differences is calculated and then used as the error term for the unrelated independent variable (this error term is often called ‘subjects within groups’)



the (residual) error term is used as the error term when examining the effects of the related independent variable (this error term is often called ‘B × subjects within groups’).

Note the slight amendments made to the tables such as Table 25.3 (Explaining statistics 25.1) compared to those given in Chapter 23; columns headed ‘subject’ and ‘subject mean’ have been added. If there is variation in the subject mean column it shows that there is still an individual differences component in the scores in the main body of the table. Careful examination of a) the column means and row means, b) cell means, c) subject means and d) the individual scores in the cells will hint strongly whether there remains any variation due to a) the main effects, b) interaction, c) individual differences and d) (residual) error). Table 25.2 shows a typical ANOVA summary table for the mixed design. The main effects are A and B and there is an interaction AB. However, there are rows such as that for Subjects within groups and B × subjects within groups. These are used as error terms in the mixed design ANOVA – that is, more than one error term is used (Figure 25.1). Figure 25.2 shows the key steps in a mixed ANOVA.

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 340

11/13/13 5:23 PM

25.2  Mixed designs and repeated measures 341

Table 25.2

Analysis of variance summary table for the mixed design

Source of variation

Sums of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square

F-ratio

Between subjects A (Unrelated variable) Subjects within groups Within subjects B (Related variable) AB (Interaction) B × subjects within groups

Figure 25.1

The components of the measured score in the mixed design

Figure 25.2

Conceptual steps for understanding the mixed ANOVA

If you feel confident with the two-way unrelated ANOVA described in Chapter 23, we suggest that you need to concentrate on steps 2 and 7 overleaf as these tell you how to calculate the error terms. The other steps should be familiar.

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 341

11/13/13 5:23 PM

342

CHAPTER 25  Mixed-design ANOVA: Related and unrelated variables together

Explaining statistics 25.1

How the mixed-design two-way unrelated analysis of variance works The variance estimate for the data in Table 25.3 for N − 1 degrees of freedom is 76.89 ÷ 11 = 6.99. N is the number of scores.

Example of a mixed ANOVA design

Table 25.3

Control

Experimental

Subject

Pre-test measure

Post-test measure

Subject mean

S1

6

5

5.500

S2

4

6

5.000

S3

5

7

6.000 Mean = 5.500

Mean = 5.000

Mean = 6.000

S4

7

10

8.500

S5

5

11

8.000

S6

5

12

8.500

Mean = 5.667

Mean = 11.000

Mean = 8.333

Mean = 5.333

Mean = 8.500

Overall mean = 6.917

Just to remind you, 6.99 is the variance estimate (or mean square) based on the 12 scores in Table 25.3. To avoid repe­ titious calculations with which you should now be familiar, we have given only the final stages of the calculation of the various variance estimates. This is to allow you to work through our example and check your calculations. In the mixed-design ANOVA the following steps are then calculated.

Step 1

(between-subjects scores)  Between-subjects scores are the data but with the pre-test/post-test difference eliminated. In other words, each subject’s scores in the pre-test and post-test conditions are replaced by the corresponding subject mean. Thus the column means for the pre-test and post-test have the (residual) error removed since the remaining variation within the cells is due to individual differences. However, there still remains variation within the table due to individual differences as well as the main effects and interaction. (To be absolutely clear, the first entry of 5.500 for both the pre-test and post-test measure is obtained by averaging that first person’s scores of 5 and 6 in Table 25.4.) The variance estimate for the between-subjects scores is 25.40/5 = 5.08 (df = number of subjects − 1, i.e. 6 − 1 = 5).

Step 2

(subjects within groups scores, i.e. individual difference component)  If we take away the cell mean from the scores in Table 25.4, we are left with the individual difference component for each subject for each score. Thus, S2’s scores are on average –0.500 below the row mean. Table 25.5 gives the individual difference component of every score in the original data. The variance estimate for the subjects within groups scores is 1.32/4 = 0.33 (the df is the number of subjects – number of rows of data, i.e. 6 − 2 = 4).

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 342

11/13/13 5:23 PM

25.2  Mixed designs and repeated measures 343

Table of between-subjects scores, i.e. with (residual) error removed

Table 25.4

Control

Experimental

Subject

Pre-test

Post-test

Subject mean

S1

5.500

5.500

5.500

S2

5.000

5.000

5.000

S3

6.000

6.000

6.000

Mean = 5.500

Mean = 5.500

Mean = 5.500

S4

8.500

8.500

8.500

S5

8.000

8.000

8.000

S6

8.500

8.500

8.500

Mean = 8.333

Mean = 8.333

Mean = 8.333

Mean = 6.917

Mean = 6.917

Overall mean = 6.917

Subjects within groups scores, i.e. error due to individual differences removed

Table 25.5

Control

Subject

Pre-test

Post-test

Subject mean

S1

5.500 − 5.500 =   0.000

0.000

0.000

S2

5.000 − 5.500 = −0.500

−0.500

−0.500

S3

6.000 − 5.500 =   0.500

0.500

0.500

S4

8.500 − 8.333 =   0.167

0.167

0.167

S5

−0.333

−0.333

−0.333

S6

0.167

0.167

Mean = 0.000 Experimental

0.167 Mean = 0.000

Mean = 0.000

Mean = 0.000

Overall mean = 0.000

You will see that these individual difference scores seem rather like error scores – they add to zero for each cell. Indeed they are error scores – the individual differences component of error. The variance estimate of the individual differences is used as the error variance estimate for calculating the significance of the control/experimental comparison (i.e. the unrelated independent variable).

Step 3

(experimental/control scores: main effect)  The best estimate of the effects of the experimental versus the control condition involves simply replacing each score for the control group with the control group mean (5.500) and each score for the experimental group by the experimental group mean (8.333). This is shown in Table 25.6. The variance estimate for the experimental/control main effect is 24.08/1 = 24.08 (the df is the number of rows of data −1, i.e. 2 − 1 = 1). The statistical significance of the main effect of the experimental versus control manipulation independent variable involves the variance estimate for the main effects scores in Table 25.6 and the variance estimate for the individual differences error scores in Table 25.5. By dividing the former by the latter



M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 343

11/13/13 5:23 PM

344

CHAPTER 25  Mixed-design ANOVA: Related and unrelated variables together

Main effect (experimental/control comparison)

Table 25.6

Control

Subject

Pre-test

Post-test

Subject mean

S1

5.500

5.500

5.500

S2

5.500

5.500

5.500

S3

5.500

5.500

5.500 Mean = 5.500

Experimental

S4

8.333

8.333

8.333

S5

8.333

8.333

8.333

S6

8.333

8.333

8.333 Mean = 8.333

Mean = 6.917

Mean = 6.917

Overall mean = 6.917

variance estimate, we obtain the F-ratio for testing the effects of the experimental versus control conditions. If this is significant then there is an overall difference between the control and experimental group scores.

Step 4

(within-subjects scores)  Subtract the between-subjects scores (Table 25.4) from the data table (Table 25.3) and you are left the within-subjects scores. In other words, the scores in Table 25.7 are what is left when the effects of the experimental/control comparison and the individual difference component of the scores are removed. Notice that the subject means in Table 25.7 are all zero as are the row means. This indicates that there are no individual differences or differences due to the experimental/control comparison remaining in Table 25.7. The variance estimate for this table is 51.54/6 = 8.59 (df is the number of scores minus the number of subjects = 12 − 6 = 6).

Step 5

(within-subjects independent variable main effect: pre-test/post-test scores)  This is the main effect of the repeated measure. It is obtained simply by substituting the appropriate column average from the data table (Table 25.3) for each of the scores (Table 25.8).

Table 25.7

Within-subjects scores (i.e. the scores with individual differences and control/experimental differences eliminated) Subject

Control

Pre-test

Post-test

Subject mean

S1

0.5

−0.5

0.000

S2

−1.0

1.0

0.000

S3

−1.0

1.0

0.000 Mean = 0.000

Experimental

S4

−1.5

1.5

0.000

S5

−3.0

3.0

0.000

S6

−3.5

3.5

0.000 Mean = 0.000

Mean = −1.583

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 344

Mean = 1.583

Overall mean = 0.000

11/13/13 5:23 PM

25.2  Mixed designs and repeated measures 345

The main effects of the pre-test/post-test comparison

Table 25.8

Control

Subject

Pre-test

Post-test

Subject mean

S1

5.333

8.500

6.917

S2

5.333

8.500

6.917

S3

5.333

8.500

6.917

S4

5.333

8.500

6.917

S5

5.333

8.500

6.917

S6

5.333

8.500

Mean = 6.917 Experimental

6.917 Mean = 6.917

Mean = 5.333

Mean = 8.500

Overall mean = 6.917

The variance estimate for the pre-test/post-test main effect is 30.09/1 = 30.09 (the df is the number of columns of data −1, i.e. 2 − 1 = 1).

Step 6

(the interaction of experimental/control with pre-test/post-test)  The calculation of the interaction is much as for the two-way unrelated ANOVA (Chapter 23): ●

We can eliminate error by making every score in the data table the same as the cell mean (Table 25.9).



We can eliminate the effect of the control versus experimental treatment by simply taking the corres­ ponding row means away from all of the scores in Table 25.9 (Table 25.10).



Note that Table 25.10 still contains variation between its pre-test and post-test columns. We eliminate this by subtracting the corresponding column mean from each of the scores in the pre-test and posttest columns (Table 25.11).

Table 25.11 contains the scores for the interaction. The variance estimate for the interaction is 14.08/1 = 14.08 (the df is the number of rows of data −1 × the number of columns of data −1 (i.e. (2 − 1) × (2 − 1) = 1 × 1 = 1)).

Table 25.9

Control

Removing (total) error from the data table Subject

Pre-test

Post-test

Subject mean

S1

5.000

6.000

5.500

S2

5.000

6.000

5.500

S3

5.000

6.000

5.500 Mean = 5.500

Experimental

S4

5.667

11.000

8.333

S5

5.667

11.000

8.333

S6

5.667

11.000

8.333

Mean = 5.333

Mean = 8.500

Overall mean = 6.917

Mean = 8.333



M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 345

11/13/13 5:23 PM

CHAPTER 25  Mixed-design ANOVA: Related and unrelated variables together

346

Removing experimental/control main effect (total error removed in previous step)

Table 25.10

Control

Subject

Pre-test

Post-test

Subject mean

S1

5.000 − 5.500 = −0.500

0.500

0.000

S2

−0.500

0.500

0.000

S3

−0.500

0.500

0.000

S4

5.667 − 8.333 = −2.666

2.667

0.000

S5

−2.666

2.667

0.000

S6

−2.666

2.667

0.000

Mean = 1.583

Overall mean = 0.000

Mean = 0.000 Experimental

Mean = 0.000 Mean = −1.583



Removing pre-test/post-test differences (error and experimental/control main effect already removed in previous two steps)

Table 25.11



Control

Subject

Pre-test

Post-test

Subject mean

S1

−0.500 − (−.583) = 1.083

−1.083

0.000

S2

1.083

−1.083

0.000

S3

1.083

−1.083

0.000

S4

2.666 − (−1.583) = −1.083

1.083

0.000

S5

−1.083

1.083

0.000

S6

−1.083

1.083

0.000

Mean = 0.00

Overall mean = 0.00

Mean = 0.000 Experimental

Mean = 0.000 Mean = 0.00

Step 7

(pre-test/post-test × subjects within groups)  Earlier we explained that pre-test/post-test × subjects within groups is an error term which is in essence the (residual) error that we calculated in Chapter 22. It is actually quite easy to calculate the (residual) error simply by: ●

drawing up a total error table by subtracting the cell means from each score in the data table (Table 25.8) as we did for the two-way unrelated ANOVA in Chapter 23 and then



taking away from these (total) error scores the corresponding (residual) error in Table 25.10. In other words, (Residual) error = (Total) error – Individual difference error

Most statistical textbooks present a rather more abstract computational approach to this which obscures what is really happening. However, to facilitate comparisons with other textbooks, if required, we will present the calculation using essentially the computational method. The calculation of this error term involves taking the data (Table 25.3) and then a) subtracting the interaction score (Table 25.11), b) subtracting the individual differences score (Table 25.4) and c) adding the between-subjects score (Table 25.8). Notice that the scores in Table 25.12 are just as we would expect of

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 346

11/13/13 5:23 PM

25.2 Mixed desigNs ANd RepeAted MeAsuRes

347

the pre-test/post-test × subjects within groups scores (i.e. (residual) error)

Table 25.12

Subject

Pre-test

Post-test

Subject mean

s1

6 − 5.000 − 5.500 + 5.500 = 1.000

5 − 6.000 − 5.500 + 5.500 = −1.000

0.000

s2

4 − 5.000 − 5.000 + 5.500 = −0.500

6 − 6.000 − 5.000 + 5.500 = 0.500

0.000

s3

5 − 5.000 − 6.000 + 5.500 = −0.500

7 − 6.000 − 6.000 + 5.500 = 0.500

0.000

s4

7 − 5.667 − 8.500 + 8.333 = 1.167

10 − 11.000 − 8.500 + 8.333 = 1.167

0.000

s5

5 − 5.667 − 8.000 + 8.333 = −0.334

11 − 11.000 − 8.500 + 8.333 = 0.333

0.000

s6

5 − 5.667 − 8.500 + 8.333 = −0.834

12 − 11.000 − 8.500 + 8.333 = 0.833

0.000

Mean = 0.000

Mean = 0.000

Overall mean = 0.000

Control

Mean = 0.000

Mean = 0.000

error scores – the cells all add up to zero. It is (residual) error since there is no variation left in the subject mean column. The variance estimate for the pre-test/post-test × subjects within groups (or residual error) is 7.37/4 = 1.84 (the df is (number of subjects – number of rows) × (number of columns − 1) = (6 − 2) × (2 − 1) = 4 × 1 = 4). This (residual) error term is used in assessing the significance of the pre-test/post-test comparison as well as the interaction. The various calculations in steps 1–7 can be made into an analysis of variance summary table. Table 25.13 is a summary table using the basic concepts we have included in this book; Table 25.14 is the same except that it uses the conventional way of presenting mixed designs in statistics textbooks. You might be wondering about the reasons for the two error terms. The (residual) error is merely that with no individual differences remaining, and in Chapter 22 we examined how removing individual differences helps to control

Table 25.13

Analysis of variance summary table (using basic concepts)

Source of variation

Sums of squares

Degrees of freedom

Variance estimate

F-ratio

24.08

1

24.08

24.08 = 72.97 a 0.333

1.32

4

0.33

Main effect (related variable)

30.09

1

30.09

30.09 = 16.35 a 1 .8 84

interaction (related × unrelated variables)

14.08

1

14.08

14.08 = 7.6 65 a 1 .8 84

7.37

4

1.84

Unrelated Main effect (unrelated variable) individual differences error Related

(Residual) error a

significant at the 5% level.



M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 347

11/13/13 5:23 PM

CHAPTER 25 Mixed-desigN ANOVA: RelAted ANd uNRelAted VARiAbles tOgetheR

348

Table 25.14

Analysis of variance summary table (with layout in the conventional form)

Source of variation

Sums of squares

Degrees of freedom

Variance estimate

F-ratio

24.08

1

24.08

24.08 = 72.97 a 0.333

1.32

4

0.33

b (time)

30.09

1

30.09

Ab

14.08

1

14.08

7.37

4

1.84

Between subjects A (praise) subjects within groups Within subjects

b × subjects within groups

30.09 = 16.35 a 1 .8 84 14.08 = 7.6 65 a 1 .8 84

a

significant at the 5% level. the above which is conceptually correct is based on calculations subject to compounded rounding errors. so the figures do not correspond exactly to those in table 25.16 for example.

error variation in related designs. Not surprisingly, it is used for the main effect and interaction which include related components. However, since the individual differences error contains only that source of variation, it makes a good error term for the unrelated scores comparison. After all, by getting rid of ‘true’ error variation the design allows a ‘refined’ error term for the unrelated comparison. Perhaps we ought to explain why rather unusual names are used conventionally for the error terms in mixed ANOVAs. The reason is that the individual differences component of the scores cannot be estimated totally independently of the interaction between the main variables since they are both dependent on pre-test/post-test differences. Consequently, the estimate of individual differences cannot be totally divorced from the interaction. It follows that both error terms ought to be labelled in ways which indicate this fact. On balance, then, you would be wise to keep to the conventional terminology.

interpreting the results The interpretation of the mixed-design two-way ANOVA is virtually identical to the interpretation of any two-way ANOVA design such as the unrelated two-way ANOVA in Chapter 24. It is the calculation of the error terms which is different and this does not alter the interpretation although obviously may affect the significance level. Remember that the interpretation of any data should be based first of all on an examination of cell means and variances (or standard deviations) such as those to be found in Table 25.15. It is the pattern that you find in these which tells you just what the data say. The tests of significance merely confirm whether or not your interpretations may be generalised. An examination of Table 25.15 suggests that it is the experimental group at the post-test which has by far the highest mean score. There seems to be little difference between the other cells. This seems to suggest that there is an interaction between the two independent variables. The ANOVA summary table confirms this.

Table 25.15

table of means for mixed ANOVA design Pre-test measure

Post-test measure

Control

Cell mean = 5.000

Cell mean = 6.000

Row mean = 5.500

experimental

Cell mean = 5.667

Cell mean = 11.000

Row mean = 8.333

Column mean = 5.333

Column mean = 8.500

Overall mean = 6.917

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 348

11/13/13 5:23 PM

25.2  Mixed designs and repeated measures 349

Reporting the results These results may be written up according to the APA (2010) Publication Manual’s recommendations as follows: ‘A mixed-design analysis of variance with praise as the unrelated independent variable and pre-test versus post-test as the related independent variable was carried out on the dependent variable self-esteem. The independent variable praise had a significant effect on self-esteem, F(1, 4) = 72.32, p < 0.05. The scores in the control group (M = 5.50) were significantly lower than those in the experimental group which was given praise (M = 8.33). Similarly, scores at the post-test were significantly higher in the post-test (M = 8.50) than in the pre-test (M = 5.33), F(1, 4) = 16.40, p < 0.05. However, the hypothesis suggests that there is an interaction between the two independent variables such that the post-test measures of the experimental group given praise score more highly on the dependent variable than the other cells. There was a significant interaction, F(1, 4) = 7.68, p < 0.05. Furthermore, it would seem that it is the experimental groups following the praise manipulation which had the highest self-esteem scores. Table 25.15 shows the cell means for the four conditions of the experiment. It would appear that the variation between the cells is the result of the interaction effect and that the main effects are slight in comparison.’

Box 25.3

Focus on

A simpler alternative The sort of mixed design dealt with in this chapter requires a significant interaction for the experimental hypothesis to be supported. However, it has the drawback that the main effect of the pre-test/post-test comparison may well be affected by this interaction. (Remember that ANOVA takes out main effects first and interactions can be confused for these unless you keep your eye firmly on the descriptive output for the means, etc.) Furthermore, the unrelated comparison can also be affected in the same way. A simpler analysis of these same data, although not



so thorough as the mixed design ANOVA, would be a t-test comparing the differences between the pre-test and post-test scores for the experimental and control groups. In other words, you have two groups of scores based on the change from pre-test scores. So you can compare the amount of change in your experimental group compared to the amount of change in the control group using an unrelated t-test. Of course, if you had three groups then you could use one-way ANOVA to much the same effect.

■ The ‘risks’ in related subjects designs The advantage of related designs is that the error component of the data can be reduced by the individual differences component. Similarly, in matched-subject designs the matching variables, if they are carefully selected because they correlate with the dependent variable, reduce the amount of error in the scores. However, there is a trade-off between reducing the error term and the reduction in degrees of freedom involved (Glantz & Slinker, 1990) since the degrees of freedom in an unrelated ANOVA error term are higher than for the related ANOVA error term. If one’s matching variables are poorly related to the dependent variable or if the individual differences component of error is very small, there may be no advantage in using the related or matched ANOVA. Indeed, there can be a reduction in the power of the related ANOVA to reject your null hypothesis. This is a complex matter. The most practical advice is: ●

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 349

Do not employ matching unless you know that there is a strong relationship between the matching variables and the dependent variable (for example, it is only worthwhile

11/13/13 5:23 PM

350

CHAPTER 25  Mixed-design ANOVA: Related and unrelated variables together

matching subjects by their gender if you know that there is a gender difference in scores on the dependent variable). ●

Do whatever you can to reduce the error variance by standardising your methods and using highly reliable measures of the dependent variable.

Research examples Mixed-design ANOVA Blankenship, Wegener and Murray (2012) pointed out that much of the research on persuasion deals with the attitude of interest directly. There are circumstances where indirect methods could work better. They suggest that tackling persuasion through the indirect method of changing values might be more effective than directly dealing with attitudes. By dealing with values directly, confidence in the value might be undermined and this may lead to attitude change. Undermining the attitude might lead to resistance. In research related to these ideas, Blankenship et al. used psychology students as participants. Two independent variables were created: a) the target of the persuasive attack, which was either on pertinent values or a policy attack on the issue of affirmative action, and b) the time which was either a pre-attack measure vs. post-attack measures. In other words, their attitudes to affirmative action were measured both before and after the persuasive communication. The type of persuasive communication was randomly assigned but the pre-test and post-test measure was a correlated variable since all participants provided both measures. So the appropriate ANOVA was a mixeddesign. The study showed that attitudes towards affirmative action changed more when equality was attacked as a value than when affirmative action as a policy was attacked directly using the same arguments. As this was a 2 × 2 design there was no need for multiple comparison testing. Fitneva, Lam and Dunfield (2013) were interested in children’s strategies for information gathering. The sources of information may be asking other people for the information but they can involve direct experience. What is not known from previous research is the extent to which children understand when it is better to ask and when it is better to find out. The researchers set up a situation in which the children were asked questions about ‘moozle’ figures. They could seek the answer by looking at the figure or by asking an adult who was ‘the moozle expert’. The questions asked could be about physical properties (such as hair colour) or invisible properties (such as whether the moozle spoke French). The analysis was basically a repeated measures analysis of variance. The age of the children was one independent variable (4-year-olds versus 6-year-olds) and the related measures independent variable was visible versus invisible aspects of the moozle. The dependent variable was the number of times that the child chose to look at the moozle. It was found that children were significantly more likely to look at the moozle for information in the visible condition. There was an interaction showing the stronger tendency for the older children to look for visible properties and ask the expert for invisible properties. Signal, van den Berg, Mulrine and Gander (2012) discuss the transitory state following waking from sleep. This is a period of poor functioning, confusion and low levels of arousal. This occurs despite the opportunity for recovery that might be expected to follow sleep. It is of particular concern where a worker performs a critical task immediately after being woken up (e.g. when called out to an emergency at night). During such periods, performance at tasks can be inferior to before going to sleep. The study investigated the extent and course of sleep inertia. Participants were awakened after a short nap of 20 minutes, 40 minutes or 60 minutes. This was a simulation study taking place in a controlled setting of the laboratory. There was a no nap control condition. Dependent measures included a short test battery including a Sleepiness Scale and a Working Memory Task repeated several times after waking. The statistical analysis employed the mixed-model analyses of variance using time post-nap (a repeated measure), duration of nap and order of completing protocols as the independent variables. There was no effect of sleep inertia on the Sleepiness Scale. Nevertheless, the Working Memory task showed impairment in the form of slower reaction time, fewer correct responses and increased omissions due to sleep inertia.

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 350

11/13/13 5:23 PM

Computer analysis 351



Key points ●

Research designs which require complex statistics such as the above ANOVAs are difficult and cumbersome to implement. Use them only after careful deliberation about what it is you really need from your research.



Avoid the temptation to include basic demographic variables such as age and gender routinely as independent variables in the analysis of variance. If they are key factors then they should be included, otherwise they can merely lead to complex interactions which may be hard to interpret and not profitable when you have done so.

Computer analysis Mixed design ANOVA using SPSS

Figure 25.3

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 351

SPSS Statistics steps for a mixed ANOVA

11/13/13 5:23 PM

CHAPTER 25  Mixed-design ANOVA: Related and unrelated variables together

352

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

The post-test mean for the experimental condition is higher than the other means in the Descriptive Statistics output suggesting an interaction. This is confirmed in the Tests of Within Subjects Contrasts table. Both the main effect of order and the interaction between order and condition are statistically significant. It is important that Box’s Test of Equality of Covariance Matrices and Levene’s Test of Equality of Error Variances are non-significant.



In line with APA (2010) conventions, the results could be written as follows: ‘The mixed-design analysis of variance with praise as the unrelated independent variable and pre-test versus post-test as the related independent variable showed significant effects on the dependent variable selfesteem. Praise had a significant effect on self-esteem, F (1, 4) = 72.32, p < 0.05. The scores in the control group (M = 5.50) were significantly lower than those in the experimental group which was given praise (M = 8.33). Similarly, scores at the post-test were significantly higher in the post-test (M = 8.50) than in the pre-test (M = 5.33), F (1, 4) = 16.40, p < 0.05’.

Screenshot 25.1

The data

Screenshot 25.2

Steps in the analysis

Screenshot 25.3

Defining the repeated measures

Screenshot 25.4

Selecting the variables

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 352

11/13/13 5:23 PM

Recommended further reading 353



Screenshot 25.5

Select options

Screenshot 25.6

Key output

Recommended further reading Glantz, S.A., & Slinker, B.K. (1990). Primer of applied regression and analysis of variance. New York: McGraw-Hill.

M25_HOWI0749_06_SE_C25.indd 353

11/13/13 5:23 PM

Chapter 26

Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) Controlling for additional variables

Overview ●

The analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) involves procedures by which it is possible to control for additional variables which may be influencing the apparent trends in the data.



Analysis of covariance designs often include a pre-test measure of the dependent variable. The analysis adjusts for these pre-test differences. Very approximately speaking, it adjusts or controls the data so that the pre-test scores are equal. This is especially useful when participants cannot be randomly allocated to different conditions of the design.



Remember that in properly randomised experimental designs, extraneous influences are controlled partly by this process of randomly assigning participants to conditions. Of course, this may not always have the desired outcome which is why some researchers will use a pre-test to check that the participants are similar on the dependent variable prior to actually running the experiment. If the pre-test data suggest that the participants are not equated on the dependent variable then ANCOVA may be employed to help correct this.

Preparation Chapters 21 to 23 are essential as this chapter utilises many of the ideas from different types of ANOVA.

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 354

11/18/13 3:01 PM

26.1  Introduction 355

26.1 Introduction Another useful variant of the analysis of variance is the analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). This adds extra complexity but is especially valuable when there is reason to believe that the randomisation process cannot be relied on to have equated participants in the various conditions (cells) prior to the experimental manipulation. Of course, in non-randomised studies using analysis of variance (ANOVA) this is especially likely to be the case. The analysis of covariance described in this chapter is basically an elaboration of the unrelated analysis of variance (Chapter 23). The crucial difference is that an additional variable known as the covariate is measured as well as the dependent variable and independent variable(s). This covariate is a variable which correlates potentially with the dependent variable. That is, the researcher suspects that the covariate is an uncontrolled source of variation which is affecting the outcome of the study. The participants in the various conditions of the experiment may be different in terms of a covariate, for example. Thus not all differences between the experimental conditions are due to the influence of the independent variable (experimental manipulation) on the dependent variable if the covariate is having an influence. In the analysis of covariance the scores on the dependent variable are adjusted so that they are equated on the covariate. Although the procedures do not actually use the adjusted scores, the cell means for the adjusted scores are obtained as part of an additional stage in the statistical analysis. In experiments random assignment of participants to different conditions of the experiment is used so that any pre-existing differences between participants are randomly distributed – hopefully. However, randomisation does not fully guarantee that participants are similar in all conditions for every study. Randomisation avoids systematic biases, but it cannot ensure that there are no differences between participants in the different conditions prior to the experimental manipulation which affect their scores on the dependent variable. Furthermore, non-experimental studies cannot employ randomisa­ tion properly. In one important application of analysis of covariance, pre-test measures can be thought of as covariates of the post-test measure and thus handled using the analysis of covariance as an alternative to the mixed design described in the previous chapter. Figure 26.1 shows the key steps in ANCOVA.

Figure 26.1

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 355

Conceptual steps for understanding ANCOVA

11/18/13 3:01 PM

356

CHAPTER 26  Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA): Controlling for additional variables

26.2 Analysis of covariance The analysis of covariance is very much like the analysis of variance. The big difference is that it allows you to take account of any variable(s) which might correlate with the dependent variable (apart, of course, from any independent variables in your analysis of variance design). In other words, it is possible to adjust the analysis of variance for differences between your groups that might affect the outcome. For example, you might find that social class correlates with your dependent variable, and that social class differs for the groups in your analysis of variance. Using analysis of covariance you can effectively ‘adjust’ the scores on your dependent variable for these social class differences. This is, in essence, to equate all of the groups so that their mean social class is the same. Although it is possible to calculate analysis of covariance by hand, we would recommend the use of a computer package since you are likely to want to equate for several variables, not just one. Furthermore, you should check to see that your covariate does, in fact, correlate with the dependent variable otherwise your analysis becomes less sensitive, not more so. In any form of analysis of variance, there is a balance between the gains from additional controls on variance and the loss of degrees of freedom as a consequence of doing so. Controlling for covariates which do not correlate with the dependent variable effectively reduces the degrees of freedom but does nothing to remove these sources of variance – because they simply do not correlate with the dependent variable. Reducing degrees of freedom reduces the likelihood of statistical significance all other things being equal. Table 26.1 gives data that could be analysed using the analysis of covariance. The study is of the effects of different types of treatment on the dependent variable depression. For each participant, a pre-test measure of depression taken prior to therapy is also given. Notice that the pre-test scores of group 3, the no-treatment control group, tend to be larger on this pre-measure. Therefore, it could be that the apparent effects of therapy are to do with pre-existing differences between the three groups. Analysis of covariance could be used to allow for these pre-existing differences.

Example of analysis of covariance data

Table 26.1

Group 1 Psychotherapy

Group 2 Anti-depressant

Group 3 No-treatment control

Independent variable Depression

Covariate Pre-test

Independent variable Depression

Covariate Pre-test

Independent variable Depression

Covariate Pre-test

27

38

30

40

40

60

15

32

27

34

29

52

2

35

24

32

35

57

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 356

11/18/13 3:01 PM

26.2 ANAlysIs Of COVArIANCe

357

Explaining statistics 26.1

how the one-way analysis of covariance works The data are found in Table 26.1. The analysis of covariance involves a number of steps which remove the influence of the covariate on the dependent variable prior to calculating the analysis of variance on these adjusted scores. It is unnecessary to calculate the adjusted scores directly and adjusted sums of squares are used instead. The one-way analysis of covariance involves three major steps: ●

Calculating a one-way ANOVA on the dependent variable (depression) using exactly the same methods as found in Explaining statistics 21.1.



Calculating a one-way ANOVA on the covariate (in this case the pre-test scores) again using exactly the same methods as found in Explaining statistics 21.1.



Calculating a variation on the one-way ANOVA which involves the regression of the covariate on the dependent variable. In essence this is the covariation which is subtracted from the variation in the scores on the dependent variable to adjust them for the effect of the covariate.

The above steps are then used to calculate the analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). Finally, in order to judge what the data say after the influence has been removed, we also need a table of the adjusted cell means for the dependent variable, i.e. what is left when the covariate is removed from the dependent variable.

Step 1

(one-way unrelated ANOVA on the dependent variable) For clarity we have given the data on the dependent variable in Table 26.2. Consult Explaining statistics 21.1 for fuller details of calculating the one-way ANOVAs. 1. Calculate the sum of the squared scores by squaring each score on the dependent variable and adding to give the total: ∑X2 = 272 + 152 + 222 + 302 + 272 + 242 + 402 + 292 + 352 = 7309 2. Sum the scores to give: G = 27 + 15 + 22 + 30 + 27 + 24 + 40 + 29 + 35 = 249 3. Calculate the total number of scores on the dependent variable, N = 9. 4. Calculate the correction factor using the following formula: G2 249 2 = = 6889.000 N 9

Table 26.2

scores on the dependent variable Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

27

30

40

15

27

29

22

24

35



M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 357

11/18/13 3:01 PM

CHAPTer 26 ANAlysIs Of COVArIANCe (ANCOVA): CONtrOllINg fOr AddItIONAl VArIAbles

358

Analysis of variance summary table for scores on the dependent variable

Table 26.3

Source of variation

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square (variance estimate)

F-ratio

268.667

2

134.333

5.33a

25.222

between groups[dependent]

a

error[dependent]

151.333

6

total[dependent]

420.000

8

significant at the 5% level.

5. Obtain the total sum of squares for the dependent variable by taking the sum of the squared scores minus the correction factor. This is 7309 − 6889.000 = 420.000. This is entered into the ANOVA summary table for the dependent variable (Table 26.3). 6. Enter the degrees of freedom for the total sum of squares for the dependent variable. This is always N − 1 or the number of scores − 1 = 9 − 1 = 8. 7. The sum of squares between groups (SS[between]) can be calculated as follows using the correction factor calculated above, the totals of each column and the number of scores in each column (e.g. N1). SS[between] = =

T 12 T 22 T 23 G 2 + + − N1 N 2 N 3 N 64 2 812 104 2 + + − 6889.000 3 3 3

= 268.667 This value of the between-groups sum of squares for the dependent variable is entered into the ANOVA summary table (Table 26.3). 8. Enter the degrees of freedom for the between-groups sum of squares = columns − 1 = c − 1 = 3 − 1 = 2. 9. Calculate the error (i.e. error or within) sum of squares (SS[error]) by subtracting the between-groups sum of squares from the total sum of squares: SS[error] = SS[total] − SS[between] = 420.000 − 268.667 = 151.333 10. The degrees of freedom for error are the number of scores minus the number of columns = N − c = 9 − 3 = 6.

Step 2

(unrelated ANOVA on the covariate) Again we can create a table of the covariate scores (Table 26.4) and carry out an unrelated ANOVA in exactly the same way as above for the dependent variable. 1. Calculate the sum of the squared scores by squaring each score on the covariate and adding to give the total: ∑X2 = 382 + 322 + 352 + 402 + 342 + 322 + 602 + 522 + 572 = 17 026 2. Sum the scores to give: G = 38 + 32 + 35 + 40 + 34 + 32 + 60 + 52 + 57 = 380

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 358

11/18/13 3:01 PM

26.2 ANAlysIs Of COVArIANCe

359

scores on the covariate

Table 26.4

Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

38

40

60

32

34

52

35

32

57

3. Calculate the total number of scores for the covariate, N = 9. 4. Calculate the correction factor using the following formula: G2 380 2 = = 16 044.444 N 9 5. Obtain the sum of squared scores for the covariate by taking the sum of the squared scores minus the correction factor. This is 17 026 − 16 044.444 = 981.556. This is entered into the ANOVA summary table for the covariate (Table 26.5). 6. Enter the degrees of freedom for the total sum of squares for the dependent variable. This is always N − 1 or the number of scores − 1 = 9 − 1 = 8. 7. The sum of squares between groups (SS[between]) can be calculated as follows using the correction factor which has already been calculated, the totals of each column and the number of scores in each column for the covariate (e.g. N1): SS[between] =

T 12 T 22 T 23 G 2 1052 1032 169 69 2 + + − = + + − 16 044.444 = 896.223 N1 N 2 N 3 N 3 3 3

This value of the between-groups sum of squares for the covariate is entered into the ANOVA summary table (Table 26.5). 8. Also, enter the degrees of freedom for the between-groups sum of squares for the covariate = columns − 1 = c − 1 = 3 − 1 = 2. 9. Calculate the error (i.e. error or within) sum of squares (SS[error]) by subtracting the between-groups sum of squares from the total sum of squares: SS[error] = SS[total] − SS[between] = 981.556 − 896.223 = 85.333

Table 26.5

Analysis of variance summary table for scores on the covariate

Source of variation

between groups[covariate]

a

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square (variance estimate)

F-ratio

896.223

2

448.112

31.51a

14.222

error[covariate]

85.333

6

total[covariate]

981.556

8

significant at the 0.1% level.



M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 359

11/18/13 3:01 PM

360

CHAPTER 26  Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA): Controlling for additional variables

The degrees of freedom for error are the number of scores minus the number of columns = N − c = 9 − 3 = 6.

Step 3

(calculating the covariation summary table)  This is very similar to the calculation of the unrelated ANOVA but is based on the cross-products of the dependent variable and covariate scores (Table 26.6). Basically it involves multiplying each dependent variable score by the equivalent covariate score. In this way it is similar to the calculation of the Pearson correlation coefficient which involves the calculation of the covariance. Table 26.6 can be used to calculate a summary table for the cross-products (Table 26.7). The calculation is analogous to that for ANOVA in steps 1 and 2 above. The only substantial difference is that it involves calculation of the cross-products of X × Y instead of X2. 1. Calculate the overall (or grand) total of the X scores: GX = 27 + 15 + 22 + 30 + 27 + 24 + 40 + 29 + 35 = 249 2. Calculate the overall (or grand) total of the Y scores: GY = 38 + 32 + 35 + 40 + 34 + 32 + 60 + 52 + 57 = 380 3. Calculate the number of scores for the dependent variable, N = 9.

Table 26.6

Data and cross-products table Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

X Dependent

Y Covariate

X×Y

X Dependent

Y Covariate

X×Y

X Dependent

Y Covariate

X×Y

27

38

1026

30

40

1200

40

60

2400

15

32

480

27

34

918

29

52

1508

22

35

770

24

32

768

35

57

1995

∑ X = 64

∑Y = 105

∑ XY = 2276

∑ X = 81

∑Y = 106

∑ XY = 2886

∑ X = 104

∑Y = 169

∑ XY = 5903

∑ X ∑Y = 64 × 105 = 6720

∑ X ∑Y = 81 × 106 = 8586

∑ X ∑Y = 104 × 169 = 17  576

N1 = 3

N2 = 3

N3 = 3

Grand total of all X scores = ∑ X = GX = 64 + 81 + 104 = 249 Grand total of all Y scores = ∑Y = GY = 105 + 106 + 169 = 380

Table 26.7

Summary table for the covariation

Source of variation

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Between groups[covariation]

447.334

2

Error[covariation]

104.333

5

Total[covariation]

551.667

8

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 360

Mean square (variance estimate)

F-ratio

11/18/13 3:01 PM

26.2 ANAlysIs Of COVArIANCe

361

4. Calculate the correction factor by substituting the already calculated values: Correction factor =

GX × GY 249 × 380 94 620 = = = 10 513.333 333 N 9 9

5. Calculate the number of scores for each group (N1, N2, N3). In our example these are all 3 as the group sizes are equal, but this does not have to be so. 6. Total degrees of freedom for the data table = the number of scores − 1 = 9 − 1 = 8. 7. Multiply each X score by the equivalent Y score to give the cross-products and sum these crossproducts to give ∑XY which is the sum of cross-products:

∑XY = (27 × 38) + (15 × 32) + (22 × 35) + (30 × 40) + (27 × 34) + (24 × 32) + (40 × 60) + (29 × 52) + (35 × 57)

= 1026 + 480 + 770 + 1200 + 918 + 768 + 2400 + 1508 + 1995 = 11 065 8. Obtain the total sum of covariation by subtracting the correction factor from the sum of cross-products: Total sum of covariation =

∑ XY −

GX × GY N

= 11 065 − 10 513..333 = 551.667 9. These values of the total sum of covariation (551.667) and the degrees of freedom (8) can be entered into Table 26.7 (the summary table for covariation). 10. Sum the scores on the dependent variable and independent variables separately for each of the groups separately as in Table 26.6. This gives us ∑X1, ∑X2, ∑X3, ∑Y1, ∑Y2, ∑Y3, since we have three groups in our instance. 11. The sum of the covariation between groups is calculated as follows: Sum of covariation between groups =

∑ X1 ∑ Y1 + ∑ X 2 ∑ Y2 + ∑ X3 ∑ Y3 N1

N2

N3



GXGY N

=

64 × 105 81 × 106 104 × 169 + + − 10 513.333 3 3 3

=

6720 8586 17 576 + + − 10 513.333 3 3 3

= 2240.000 + 2862.000 + 5858.667 − 10 513.333 = 447.334 12. The degrees of freedom for the covariation between groups is the number of groups − 1 = 3 − 1 = 2. 13. These values of the sum of covariation between groups and degrees of freedom between groups can be entered in Table 26.7. 14. The sum of the covariation of error can be obtained now by subtracting the sum of the betweengroups covariation from the total covariation: Sum of the covariation of error = Total of covariation – Covariation between groups = 551.667 − 447.334 = 104.333

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 361



11/18/13 3:01 PM

CHAPTer 26 ANAlysIs Of COVArIANCe (ANCOVA): CONtrOllINg fOr AddItIONAl VArIAbles

362

15. This value of the covariation for error can now be entered into Table 26.7. 16. The degrees of freedom for error are calculated in a way which removes one degree of freedom for the covariation. This is simply the total number of scores – the number of groups − 1 = 9 − 3 − 1 = 5. This can be entered in Table 26.7. The above calculation steps for covariation are only superficially different from those for the analysis of variance in steps 1 and 2. They are actually different only so far as variance and covariance differ (pp. 102–103).

Step 4

(calculating the ANCOVA summary table, i.e. the dependent table with the covariate partialled out) This is achieved by taking away the variation in the scores due to the covariate from the variation in the dependent variable. Once we have the three summary tables (dependent variable, covariate and crossproducts) then it is a fairly simple matter to calculate the adjusted dependent variable sums of squares and enter them into Table 26.8, the summary table for a one-way ANCOVA. The formulae are: SSError[adjusted] = SSError[dependent] −

SSTotal [adjusted] = SSTotal [dependent] −

(Error[covariation] ) 2 SSError[covariate] (Total [covariation]) 2 SSError[covariate]

Be very careful to distinguish between the covariation and the covariate. These calculations are as follows: SS Error[adjusted] = SSError[dependent] −

(Error[covariation]) 2 SS Error[covariate]

= 151.333 −

104.3332 85.333

= 151.333 −

10 885.375 85.333

= 151.333 − 127.563 = 23.77

Table 26.8

ANCOVA summary table

Source of variation

a

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square (variance estimate)

between[adjusted]

86.175

2

43.088

error[adjusted]

23.770

5

4.754

total[adjusted]

109.945

8

F-ratio

43.088 = 9.06 a 4.7754

significant at the 5% level.

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 362

11/18/13 3:01 PM

26.2 ANAlysIs Of COVArIANCe

SSTotal [adjusted] = SSTotal [dependent] −

363

(Total [covariation]) 2 SSError[covariate]

= 420.000 −

551.667 2 981.556

= 420.000 −

304 336.479 981.556

= 420.000 − 310.055 = 109.945 Enter these values into the ANCOVA summary table (Table 26.8) and the between sum of squares obtained by subtracting the error sum of squares from the total sum of squares. Note that the degrees of freedom for the error term in the ANCOVA summary table are listed as 5. This is because we have constrained the degrees of freedom by partialling out the covariate. The formula for the degrees of freedom for the adjusted error is number of scores – number of groups − 1 = 9 − 3 − 1 = 5.

Step 5

The F-ratio in the ANCOVA summary table is calculated in the usual way. It is the between mean square divided by the error mean square. This is 9.06. The significance of this is obtained from Significance Table 23.1 for 2 and 5 degrees of freedom (or Appendix J if other levels of significance are required). We look under the column for 2 degrees of freedom and the row for 5 degrees of freedom. This indicates that our F-ratio is above the minimum value for statistical significance and is therefore statistically significant.

Step 6

(adjusting group means) No analysis of variance can be properly interpreted without reference to the means of the data table. This is not simple with ANCOVA as the means in the data are the means unadjusted for the covariate. Consequently it is necessary to adjust the means to indicate what the mean would be when the effect of the covariate is removed. The formula for this is as follows: Adjusted group mean = Unadjusted group mean (Error[ covariance ]) × (Group mean[ covariate ] − Grand mean[covariate]) − SSError[ covariate ] The unadjusted group means are merely the means of the scores on the dependent variable for each of the three groups in our example. These can be calculated from Table 26.2. The three group means are: group 1 = 21.333, group 2 = 27.000 and group 3 = 34.667. The group means for the covariate can be calculated from Table 26.4. They are group 1 = 35.000, group 2 = 35.333 and group 3 = 56.333. The grand mean of the covariate is simply the mean of all of the scores on the covariate in Table 26.4 which equals 42.222 for our example. The sums of squares for error have already been calculated. The sum of squares for error for the crossproducts is 104.333 and is found in Table 26.7. The sum of squares for error for the covariate is 85.333 and is found in Table 26.5. We can now substitute all of these values into the formula and enter these values into Table 26.9. Group 1: Adjusted mean = 30.27 obtained as follows  104.333  21.333 −  × (35.000 − 42.222)  = 21.33 33 3 − [1.237 × (−7.222) ] 84 . 333   = 21.333 − (−8.934) = 30.267



M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 363

11/18/13 3:01 PM

364

CHAPTer 26 ANAlysIs Of COVArIANCe (ANCOVA): CONtrOllINg fOr AddItIONAl VArIAbles

Table 26.9

unadjusted and adjusted means for depression

Means

Group 1 Psychotherapy

Group 2 Antidepressants

Group 3 Control

unadjusted

21.33

27.00

34.67

Adjusted

30.27

35.52

17.21

Group 2: Adjusted mean = 35.52 obtained as follows  104.333  27.000 −  × (35.333 − 42.222)  = 27.00 00 0 − [1.237 × (−6.889) ] 84 . 333   = 27.000 − (−8.522) = 35.522 Group 3: Adjusted mean = 17.21 obtained as follows  104.333  34.667 −  × (56.333 − 42.222)  = 34.66 67 7 − (1.237 × 14.111)  84.333  = 34.667 − 17.455 = 17.21 Notice how the adjusted means in Table 26.9 show a completely different pattern from the unadjusted means in this case.

Step 7

The simplest way of testing which of the adjusted means are different from the others is to use the Fisher protected LSD (least significant difference) test (Huitema, 1980). It is convenient since the component parts have largely been calculated by now. This test gives us an F-ratio with always one degree of freedom for the comparison and N − the number of groups − 1 = 9 − 3 − 1 = 5 in our example for the error. Because we have three groups, there are three possible comparisons between pairs of groups. We will show the calculation in full for the comparison between groups 1 and 2: F=

(Adjusted group1 mean − Adjusted group 2 mean) 2  1 1   (Covar Covaria iate group1 mean mean − Covar Covariate Co varia iate te group group 2 mean) 2   Meeaan square error adjusted ×   + +   Sum of squares of error for the covariate     N 1 N 2   where: Adjusted group1 mean is found in Table 26.9. Adjusted group2 mean is found in Table 26.9. Mean square error adjusted is found in Table 26.8. Covariate group1 mean is found by consulting Table 26.4 and dividing the sum of covariate scores for group 1 by the number of scores for group 1 = ∑Y/N = 105/3 = 35.000. Covariate group2 mean is found in exactly the same way. Consult Table 26.4 and divide the sum of covariate scores for group 2 by the number of scores = 106 ÷ 3 = 35.333.

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 364

11/18/13 3:01 PM

26.2 ANAlysIs Of COVArIANCe

365

Sum of squares of error for the covariate is found in Table 26.5. F=

=

=

(30.27 . − 35..52) 2   1 1  (35.000 . − 35.333) 2  4.754   +  +  85.333   3 3   5 .2 5 2  (−0.33 33 3) 2  4..754 ((0.333 + 0.333 .333) +  85.333   27.563  0.111  4.754  0.666 + 85.333  

=

27.563 4.754(0.666 + 0.001)

=

27.563 27.563 = = 8.692 4.754(0.667) 3 .1 7

This value of the F-ratio with 1 and 5 degrees of freedom is statistically significant at the 5% level. So the adjusted means of group 1 and group 2 are significantly different from each other. We also carried out the comparisons between group 1 and group 3 (the obtained F-ratio of 5.98 was not significant at the 5% level) and group 2 and group 3 (the obtained F-ratio 12.09 was statistically significant at the 5% level).

Interpreting the results The analysis of covariance makes it clear that the post-test measures of depression differ overall once the pre-test differences are controlled. However, by considering the means of the adjusted levels of depression it seems clear that the depression scores of the control group were actually lower than those of either of the treatment groups. In other words, once pre-test levels of depression are adjusted for, then the obvious interpretation is that depression is actually being increased by the treatment rather than being reduced relative to the control group. The multiple comparisons test indicates that the significant differences are between the anti-depressant group and the control group and the psychotherapy group and the control group. The two treatment groups did not differ significantly from each other.

reporting the results This analysis may be written up according to the APA (2010) Publication Manual’s recommendations as follows: ‘An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was applied to the three groups (psychotherapy, anti-depressant and no-treatment control) in order to see whether the different treatments had an effect on post-test levels of depression controlling for pre-test depression. There was found to be a significant effect of the type of treatment, F(2, 5)= 9.06, p < .05. The unadjusted means indicated that depression was higher in the control group (M = 34.67) than with psychotherapy (M = 21.33) or with anti-depressant treatment (M = 35.52). However, this seems to be the result of the influence of the covariate (pre-therapy levels of depression as measured at the pre-test) since the adjusted means for the groups indicate that the least depression is found in the untreated control group (M = 17.21), compared with the psychotherapy group (M = 30.27) and the anti-depressant group (M = 35.52). Thus, the two treatment conditions increased depression relative to the control group. This was confirmed in a comparison of the adjusted means using the Fisher protected LSD test. The analysis indicated that group 1 (psychotherapy) and group 2 (anti-depressant) differed significantly, F(1, 5) = 8.69, p < .05. Group 2 (anti-depressant) and group 3 (control condition) differed significantly, F(1, 5) = 12.09, p < .05. Group 1 (psychotherapy) and group 3 (control) did not differ significantly, F(1, 5) = 5.98, p ns.’

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 365

11/18/13 3:01 PM

366

CHAPTER 26  Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA): Controlling for additional variables

Research examples ANCOVA Cumming and co-workers (2012) studied the effect of physically maturing early in adolescence on the physical activity of girls. Research has suggested that girls reduce their amounts of physical activity during adolescence and the health-related issues that this entails are obvious. Is there a role for early maturation in this? The study compared early and late maturing adolescent girls with an average age of 12.7 years. The dependent variables were health-related matters such as physical activity behaviour, physical self-concept, and health-related quality of life. In each case it was expected that early maturing girls would score lower. The analysis employed several ANCOVA analyses compared early and late maturing girls on these variables. Chronological age was included as the covariate since obviously maturation and age correlate together. Although the size of the differences tended to be small to moderate, the ANCOVAs repeatedly showed that early maturing girls scored lower on the health-related variables. It is noteworthy that early maturing girls rated themselves lower in terms of body attractiveness. This may have a bearing on their lower levels of involvement in physical activity. Estevis, Basso and Combs (2012) investigated the effect of practice on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale– IV. The participants were given the test at the start of the study and again a few months later. For some it was three months later and for the others it was six months later. They used various subscales from the test including Verbal Comprehension, Working Memory, Perceptual Reasoning and Processing Speed as well as the Full Scale IQ. They analysed the data using an ANCOVA design in which test versus retest and the various subscales were the related factors and three months versus six months was the independent factor. Gender was entered as the covariate. Bonferroni adjustment was employed to deal with the repeated significance testing problem. The interval between testing and retesting did not have a significant effect. Wright and Hardie (2012) write that the previous research on the relationship between handedness and anxiety fails to indicate a clear conclusion. One reason for expecting a relationship between anxiety and handedness is that the right-hand side hemisphere of the brain is involved in negative emotional states and inhibition. Anxiety is often classified as being situational in nature or alternatively as a personality trait of the individual. The researchers found that left-handed people have statistically significantly higher scores on state anxiety which supports the idea of the role of the right hemisphere. No trait anxiety differences were found but trait and state anxiety were significantly correlated. So ANCOVA was employed with trait anxiety as the control variable because of this correlation. The handedness relationship to state anxiety remained even in this ana­ lysis. The authors suggest that left-handers are more reactive personalities and so respond with state anxiety to the new situation that they were experiencing in the research laboratory as part of the research.

Key points ●

Relying on ANCOVA to deal with the problems due to employing non-randomised allocation to the cells of the ANOVA ignores the basic reason for doing randomised experiments in the first place – that the researcher does not know what unknown factors influence the outcome of the research. Random allocation to conditions is the only practical and sound way of fully controlling for variables not included in the design.



It is not wise to use ANCOVA to try to correct for the sloppiness of your original design or procedures. Although, especially when using computers, you can include many covariates, it is best to be careful when planning your research to reduce the need for this. In randomised experiments, probably the control of the pre-test measure is the only circumstance requiring ANCOVA. Of course, there are circumstances in which pre-tests are undesirable, especially as they risk sensitising participants as to the purpose of the study or otherwise influencing the post-test measures.

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 366

11/18/13 3:01 PM

Computer analysis 367



Computer analysis Analysis of covariance using SPSS

Figure 26.2

SPSS Statistics steps for ANCOVA

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

Firstly check that the covariate is related to the dependent variable. If not, then do not use ANCOVA. Also the relation between the covariate and the dependent variable should be similar across the conditions of the independent variable. This assumption is known as homogeneity of regression lines. Otherwise the effect of controlling will be different for each condition.



Using APA (2010) style, one might write: ‘An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) in which the effect of treatment on post-treatment depression was examined controlling for pre-treatment depression. The treatment effect was significant, F(2, 5) = 9.06, p < .05. The Fisher protected LSD test showed the adjusted post-treatment mean for the anti-depressant group (M = 35.52) was significantly higher than that for psychotherapy group (M = 30.27) and the no-treatment control group (M = 17.21).’

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 367

11/18/13 3:01 PM

368

CHAPTER 26  Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA): Controlling for additional variables

Screenshot 26.1

The data

Screenshot 26.2

Select the test

Screenshot 26.3

Select variables

Screenshot 26.4

Select model

Screenshot 26.6

Important output – basic descriptives



Important output – descriptives after adjusting for pre-test

Screenshot 26.5

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 368

Select more options

Screenshot 26.7



11/18/13 3:01 PM

Recommended further reading 369



Screenshot 26.8

The ANCOVA summary table

Screenshot 26.9

LSD pairwise comparisons

Recommended further reading Cramer, D. (2003). Advanced quantitative data analysis (Chapter 11). Buckingham: Open University Press. Glantz, S.A., & Slinker, B.K. (1990). Primer of applied regression and analysis of variance. New York: McGraw-Hill.

M26_HOWI0749_06_SE_C26.indd 369

11/18/13 3:01 PM

Chapter 27

Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

Overview ●

The multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) is very much like the analysis of variance (ANOVA). The big difference is that it uses several different dependent variables at the same time. These dependent variables are all score variables. The independent variable is a category variable (or variables). That is, MANOVA is very much like ANOVA except for the dependent variables.



There are versions of MANOVA which are equivalent to the various ANOVA designs covered in Chapters 21 to 23. Thus it is possible to have one-way MANOVAs, two-way and more (factorial) MANOVAs, and MANCOVAs in which the effects of one or more covariates can be removed from the data. Related designs are possible but beyond the scope of this book, as is MANCOVA.



Essentially MANOVA combines the dependent variables to see whether the different groups (conditions) differ in terms of their ‘means’ on this combined set of dependent variables.



A MANOVA summary table is produced which includes a multivariate test of significance. Commonly these include Pillai’s trace, Wilks’ lambda, Hotelling’s trace and Roy’s largest root. Computer software such as SPSS Statistics gives all of these making the output look a little complex.



Consideration has to be given to what is to be gained by using MANOVA. For example, where the dependent variables are highly correlated and have a single underlying dimension, the scores on the dependent variables could be totalled and used as the score variable (i.e. the dependent variable) in ANOVA instead. This may yield a slightly more powerful test but only in these circumstances where the dependent variables correlate substantially.



If MANOVA is significant, then this indicates that the groups in the study differ in terms of a combination(s) of the dependent variables. This leaves the researcher to examine the

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 370

11/13/13 5:23 PM

27.1  Introduction 371

data in more detail by doing ANOVAs on the individual dependent variables or, much better, discriminant function analysis which will allow you to better know just how the dependent variables have been combined for the MANOVA. This is dealt with in Chapter 28. ●

If MANOVA fails to reach statistical significance, then no further analyses are needed or are appropriate. The hypothesis that the groups are distinguishable on the basis of the set of dependent variables has been rejected because of this lack of significance.



If you are planning to use MANOVA before collecting your data, problems may be avoided by making sure that each of your groups (or cells) have the same number of participants. If you do this then violating the assumptions of MANOVA is less of a problem.

Preparation Revise Chapters 20 to 23 on analysis of variance (ANOVA). MANOVA adds little to this in terms of conceptual difficulty and so cannot be adequately carried out without understanding ANOVA which is also part of the MANOVA procedures.

27.1 Introduction ANOVA looks for differences in group means on a single dependent variable. The dependent variable is always a score variable. MANOVA is essentially similar but examines the influence of the group participants on a set of several dependent variables simultaneously. Again, each dependent variable is a score variable. As a very simple example, the research question may be whether a new drug, Therazine supplement, affects motor skills in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (see Table 27.1). Thus, at random, some patients are given Therazine, others are given a placebo (inactive) pill and others

Data table for a study of effects of Therazine on motor skills

Table 27.1

Group (independent variable) Therazine condition

a b

Placebo condition

RTa

Sp

8b

5

7

7

7

7

6

5

9

8

5

9

7

5

8

8

Hd

W

RT

No treatment condition

Sp

Hd

W

RT

Sp

Hd

W

1

3

2

2

4

3

5

4

4

5

3

3

1

2

3

6

7

2

1

2

3

5

2

6

2

5

6

1

1

4

6

2

RT = reaction time, Sp = clarity of speech, Hd = steadiness of hand and W = writing speed. Scores are from four cases in each column. The scores are for the four dependent variables.

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 371

11/13/13 5:23 PM

372

CHAPTER 27  Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

are given nothing at all. Now there are many different motor skills that the researcher might wish to assess in this study – for example, reaction time, clarity of speech, steadiness of the hand and writing speed. All of these motor skills seem related conceptually, at least, to the research question and it would seem somewhat short-sighted simply to select one. MANOVA allows the researcher to include a number of variables which may be affected by the drug treatment. Better and clearer outcomes will be achieved in your analysis if you avoid the trap of throwing variables into the MANOVA simply because you have these data available. Carefully selecting the dependent variables because they have a strong conceptual or theoretical bearing on the research question will yield dividends. For example, as the Alzheimer research is about motor skills then adding in variables about social class or social networking to the list of dependent variables would add nothing to the MANOVA analysis. Thus, MANOVA is simply an extension of the analysis of variance to cover circumstances where there are multiple dependent variables measured in the form of scores. In the analysis of variance (Chapters 21 to 26) we have seen that it is possible to analyse research designs with: ●

Just one independent variable. This is known as a one-way analysis of variance. The independent variable is that which forms the different groups. (See Table 21.1 for an example.)



Two or more independent variables. It would be possible to extend our Alzheimer study to include more than one independent variable. So the next step might be to have a second independent variable. We previously referred to this design as a twoway ANOVA design. If we added a third grouping variable (independent variable) then this would be termed a three-way ANOVA design, and so forth. These two-way, three-way and so forth designs are sometimes referred to as factorial designs, of course.



Any of the above designs with additional covariates controlled for. So, for example, age of participants might be added as a covariate in the above designs. This is known as the analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) (Chapter 26).

MANOVA can deal with all three of the above types of design and more.

Box 27.1

Focus on

Hotelling’s two sample t 2 You may have a very simple study with just two groups (e.g. experimental and control conditions) yet have several dependent variables which relate to your hypothesis. Such designs are usually analysed using the t-test (Chapter 14). There is a multivariate version of the t-test for research designs in which there are just two groups of participants but where there are several dependent variables. This is

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 372

known as Hotelling’s two sample t 2. (If you want to do this analysis, just remember it is the same as MANOVA which reduces effectively to Hotelling’s two sample t 2 if you just have two groups in your study. So simply follow the MANOVA procedures.) This is, of course, much the same as for the t-test and ANOVA.

11/13/13 5:23 PM

27.1  Introduction 373

There are two obvious questions to ask about MANOVA at this stage: ●

Just why would one wish to analyse several different dependent variables at the same time rather than do a number of separate ANOVAs?



Just how does one combine several dependent variables?

The answers to these questions are not simple or straightforward but are important things to understand: ●

Why not do several ANOVAs?  The answer to this question partly lies in the common comment in statistics that the more tests of significance one carries out then the more likely that significant findings emerge by chance. These do not represent real differences and, consequently, are not meaningful. So the more ANOVAs one does on one’s data the more likely that a statistically significant finding will emerge. The consequence of multiple testing of this sort has been dealt with elsewhere when dealing with multiple comparisons (Chapter 25). But multiple testing of this sort can create other difficulties which do not at first appear to be statistical in nature. The purpose of research is not primarily to obtain significant findings but to provide an account or narrative or theoretical explanation which links together the findings of the researcher. Thus if the findings are not reliable then one may be trying to explain chance findings thinking that they are meaningful findings which represent something which is happening in the real world. One obvious solution may strike you. Why not apply the Bonferroni adjustment (Section 24.2) to the significance levels of the ANOVAs carried out on each dependent variable? That is, adjust the probability levels to take into account the number of comparisons made. This is sensible thinking but not entirely satisfactory in this case because multiple significance testing is the biggest problem when the dependent variables correlate with each other poorly (or not at all). Where the dependent variables correlate highly then the risk of a spurious significant ANOVA is not so great. More technically, there is a risk of Type I errors (accepting the hypothesis when it is in fact false) which increases when the dependent variables do not correlate with each other beyond a minimal level. So the use of MANOVA can be thought of as a way of replacing several ANOVAs with one blanket test on a set of dependent variables. It thus protects against Type I errors.



How to combine dependent variables?  At first sight, the answer to this question seems self-evident if the scores are positively correlated – just add up the scores for each participant to give a total score. In this way, one has generated a single dependent variable which can be entered into a regular ANOVA and there would be no reason to bother with MANOVA. There are circumstances in which this would be a good way to proceed. However, the drawback is that by doing something like this you risk losing some of the information contained in your data. If this is not clear then imagine you ask your participants six questions, the answers to which are scored on a five-point Likert scale from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Then you give a score from 1 to 5 for each of the different points on the rating scales. Finally you add up each individual’s scores to give a total score. Usually, information is lost from the data by doing so. So if someone scores 17 on the scale you simply do not know from that total what answers they gave. There are many possible ways of scoring 17 on the six questions. The total score does represent something, but it has lost some of the detail of the original replies. Hence, ANOVA carried out on the total scores also loses information from the original data.

This is not always a problem. It is a problem when more than one dimension underlies scores on the various dependent variables. If the correlations between our dependent

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 373

11/13/13 5:23 PM

374

CHAPTER 27  Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

variables show some high correlations but also some low correlations then it is likely that more than one dimension underlies our scores. However, if the variables are all highly intercorrelated and constitute a single underlying dimension, totalling the scores and then subjecting the resultant total scores to ANOVA may be extremely effective. It also has the advantage that there is no loss of degrees of freedom in the analysis – loss of degrees of freedom can be a problem in MANOVA, but this is dependent on the total picture of the analysis and there is no simple way of balancing the different advantages and disadvantages of the different approaches. On a sort of loss–gains analysis, if your dependent variables are highly correlated then more is lost than gained through the use of MANOVA. MANOVA is somewhat more abstract than ANOVA so perhaps best avoided if there is not a clear gain. It would also be legitimate to use just one dependent variable if it is highly intercorrelated with the other dependent variables. However, since psychological measures tend to be unreliable, one cannot generally expect extremely high intercorrelations between variables. Further­ more, there is no advantage of this over the summation approach of adding up the dependent variables to get a total score if the variables correlate highly.

27.2 MANOVA’s two stages Actually MANOVA is a two-stage process. These stages are usually separate in the computer programs most of us do our statistical analyses with nowadays. SPSS Statistics does have a method for doing MANOVA in the GLM procedures, but that only does half the job. In addition, you probably will need to carry out a discriminant function analysis which is a different SPSS Statistics procedure. Let us look at these two stages in turn.



■ Stage 1: MANOVA In ANOVA the researcher wants to know whether the different groups defined by the independent variable(s) are associated with different mean scores on the dependent variable. This is generally discussed in terms of the sums of squares associated with the different group means compared with the estimated sums of squares due to error variance. The ratio between the sums of squares due to the different groups of participants and the sum of squares due to error provides the basis of the statistical significance testing using the F-ratio or something similar. We have illustrated the calculation of this from basics in previous chapters on ANOVA. This is a somewhat tedious and unnecessary process given that the work is better done by computers. Much the same process is involved in MANOVA except that we have several dependent variables to examine at the same time. So the question is whether the various groups are different in terms of the means that they have on several dependent variables. Once again, these differences in means are turned into sums of squares. But there is a big problem in doing this for a MANOVA design. It is not merely that there are several dependent variables, but also the several dependent variables may well be correlated with each other – that is, they measure, in part, the same thing. The analysis needs to make allowance for the extent to which the dependent variables are correlated. If it did not do so then the analysis would be claiming the same variance several times over. The extent of this depends on the size of the correlations between variables and the number of variables which correlate. Once the sums of squares associated with the different groups in the research design have been calculated, then multivariate tests of significance are computed and a significance level(s) provided. If the analysis is significant, then this

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 374

11/13/13 5:23 PM

27.2  MANOVA’s two stages 375

shows that the groups of participants differ in terms of their scores over the set of dependent variables combined. It does not tell us which dependent variables are responsible for the differences. That is the job of the second stage. Things are more complicated than this, of course. Life is never simple halfway through a statistics textbook. Like all tests of significance, MANOVA was subject to a set of assumptions by the person who developed the procedures. Parts of the computer output for MANOVA simply tell the user whether these assumptions have been met.



■ Stage 2: The relative importance of each dependent variable From the MANOVA procedure, we know whether the groups in our research are different overall on the several dependent variables combined. That is the basic test of the hypothesis. Of course, if the multivariate test of significance in MANOVA is not significant, this basically is the end of the story. The researcher has drawn a blank in terms of his or her hypothesis and the null hypothesis is preferred over the alternative hypothesis. Even if we get a significant result from the multivariate test of significance, we remain at something of a loss as to what our analysis means since this tells us nothing as such about which groups vary and on what variables. We really need to understand something more about the pattern of variables on which the groups differ – that is, what combinations of variables tend to produce differences in group means? A less than perfect but intuitively reasonable approach to this is to do a number of ANOVAs – one for each dependent variable. Hold on a minute, you may be thinking, didn’t we decide at the start of the chapter that it was not a good idea to do this? The problem was the multitude of tests of significance being employed and this was part of the reason for opting for MANOVA in the first place. But MANOVA gives us protection from Type I errors (accepting the hypothesis when it is in fact false) so we do not need to worry. If the MANOVA is not significant then the analysis is protected from the risk of Type I error simply because no further analyses are carried out on the individual dependent variables. If the MANOVA is statistically significant, then this supposedly ‘protects’ the analysis from Type I errors and indicates that it is legitimate to do ANOVAs on each of the various dependent variables. In other words, a significant MANOVA puts a cap on the risk of finding a significant result by chance – that is, the Type I error. Unfortunately, this is just not adequate for a number of reasons. The main one is that often there is one variable which is affected by the independent variable and the rest of the dependent variables are not affected. In these circumstances, the significant MANOVA protects the affected dependent variable from Type I errors, but the other variables are not protected. So one of the ANOVAs would be protected but the rest not. Quite what will happen depends on the details of the data and analysis. Some textbooks still recommend doing this second stage analysis but there is an alternative approach, so you may choose that instead (unless your local statistical expert advises otherwise, in which case it would be politic to follow their advice). Another problem with it is that even if you test each dependent variable separately, in the end you do not quite know what was affected by the independent variable(s). Although you could name the various significant dependent variables, this does not tell you what it is about the dependent variable which is affected. That is, what do the dependent variables have in common which produces the differences between the groups of participants? Ideally, the problem of finding which dependent variables are influential on the findings is addressed through the use of discriminant function analysis (see Box 27.2 and Chapter 28). In this chapter, we will simply describe the MANOVA procedure followed up by ANOVAs.

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 375

11/13/13 5:23 PM

376

CHAPTER 27  Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

27.3 Doing MANOVA If you have mastered the basics of ANOVA then you may regard MANOVA, in its essence, as just a small step further. Ignoring discriminant function analysis for now, the major problem in implementing MANOVA lies in seeing the wood for the trees in terms of the computer output. But by this stage, this is probably a familiar difficulty which you can deal with since you are used to SPSS and other computer output. The reason that MANOVA is essentially easy is that the only new thing that you really need to know is that there are things known as multivariate tests. These are analogous to the F-ratios (or Levene’s test which is used by SPSS Statistics) which we are familiar with from ANOVA. Actually there are several multivariate tests which, despite being differently calculated, do much the same sort of thing – tell you if your group ‘means’ are different on the set of dependent variables as a whole. These multivariate tests include Pillai’s trace, Wilks’ lambda, Hotelling’s trace and Roy’s largest root. These are the ones that SPSS Statistics calculates for you. You don’t have to choose between them – the computer computes them all for you. Figure 27.1 gives the key steps in understanding a MANOVA analysis. Let’s look at the research summarised in Table 27.2. This is basically a one-way MANOVA design in which we have a single independent variable – the group – but several dependent variables. So apart from having several dependent variables, this is much the same as the design in Chapter 21 for one-way ANOVA. The study investigates the efficiency of team-building sessions with a sports psychologist, team-building sessions with a sports coach or no team building. Participants were randomly assigned to these three different conditions. Gender is regarded as a second independent variable. There are equal numbers of male and female participants. If you can, it is best to have equal group sizes for MANOVA as it helps you to avoid problems (see later). Three dependent measures were used: 1) the difference between the liking ratings for the participant’s favourite and least favourite team member which is believed to be a measure of team cohesion, 2) the number of voluntary gym sessions the player attends and 3) the number of games each player plays in a season.

Figure 27.1

Conceptual steps for understanding MANOVA

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 376

11/13/13 5:23 PM

27.3  Doing MANOVA 377

Table 27.2

Data for the MANOVA analysis Group (independent variable)

Team building with sports psychologist Dependent variables

Team building with sports coach Dependent variables

No team building controls Dependent variables

Likea

Gym

Game

Like

Gym

Game

Like

Gym

Game

9b

12

14

4

6

15

9

6

10

5

9

14

5

4

12

1

2

5

8

11

12

4

9

15

6

10

12

4

6

5

3

8

8

2

5

6

9

12

3

4

9

9

3

6

7

9

11

14

5

3

8

4

7

8

6

13

14

2

8

12

1

6

13

6

11

18

6

9

11

4

9

12

8

11

22

4

7

15

3

8

15

8

13

22

4

8

28

3

2

14

9

15

18

5

7

10

2

8

11 10

7

12

18

4

9

9

6

9

8

10

13

5

18

18

3

8

13

6

11

22

7

12

24

6

14

22

Like = difference between ratings of most and least liked team members, Gym = number of gym sessions voluntarily attended and Game = number of games played. b The scores are the scores on the three dependent variables. a

The three dependent variables correlate at the levels indicated in Table 27.3. As can be seen, all three measures intercorrelate positively, but there is some considerable variation in the size of the correlations. This suggests that more than one dimension underlies these variables. The variables cannot convincingly be totalled in this case given the wide range in the size of the correlations. So a MANOVA analysis seems appropriate in this case.

Table 27.3

Correlations between the three dependent variables

Difference between ratings of most/least liked team members Number of gym sessions voluntarily attended Number of games played

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 377

Difference between ratings of most/least liked team members

Number of gym sessions voluntarily attended

Number of games played



.60

.30



.51 –

11/13/13 5:23 PM

378

CHAPTER 27  Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

Table 27.4

Result of multivariate tests

Effect

Value

F

Hypothesis df

Error df

Sig.

Intercept Pillai’s trace

.94

184.71

3.00

37.00

.00

Groups Pillai’s trace

.49

4.11

6.00

76.00

.00

The MANOVA analysis of the data produces primarily a MANOVA summary table which is similar to the ANOVA summary table in Chapter 21. There are even values of the F-ratio much as in ANOVA. However, this is based on different calculations from ANOVA since it is applied to the multivariate test (e.g. Pillai’s trace, Wilks’ lambda, Hotelling’s trace and Roy’s largest root). Pillai’s trace is probably the one to rely on because it is more robust and less affected by the data not meeting its requirements. To keep the tables as simple as possible, we have confined our analysis to Pillai’s trace only. In MANOVA you do not calculate the sums of squares but the value of Pillai’s trace (and possibly the others). The MANOVA summary table (Table 27.4) gives the results of this analysis. Apart from that, you will find much the same statistics as for analysis of variance. So it should be self-evident from Table 27.4 that we have a significant effect of group (type of team building). Generally speaking, Pillai’s trace gives much the same outcome as Wilks’ lambda, Hotelling’s trace and Roy’s largest root. It does not much matter which you choose – and, of course, you can use all four if you so wish though this will clutter your report to no advantage. If you do not get any significant findings then this is the end of MANOVA – you do not go any further since your hypothesis has been rejected. Of course, if you have significant findings, then you need to know what they indicate. There are several steps in order to do this.



■ Step 1 The simplest interpretation would be to conclude that there are differences on the composite of the three dependent variables related to the independent variables (groups). This may in any research study be sufficient to confirm the hypothesis. In our particular example, there are differences in the ‘means’ of the composite of the three dependent variables due to the independent variable group (condition).



■ Step 2 In order to have a better understanding of more precisely what is going on in the data, you need the corresponding univariate ANOVAs to the MANOVA. SPSS Statistics gives you these as part of the basic output from MANOVA. An example is given in Table 27.5. As you can see, there is an ANOVA for each dependent variable. It may look confusing at first, but taking one dependent variable at a time a basic understanding of one-way ANOVA will suffice. What seems clear from Table 27.5 is that two of the three dependent variables show virtually identical significant patterns. That is, the summary table shows that the two main effects for 1) difference between ratings of most and least liked team members and 2) number of gym sessions voluntarily attended are significant. The third dependent variable, number of games played, does not reach significance. This would suggest that the significant MANOVA is largely the result of the first two variables rather than the third variable. But it should be noted that you may obtain a significant

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 378

11/13/13 5:23 PM

27.3  Doing MANOVA 379

Table 27.5

Part of a table of the individual ANOVAs for the three dependent variables

Dependent variable

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square

F-ratio

Significance

Number of games played

98.14

2

49.07

1.67

0.20

Difference between ratings of most liked and least liked team members

97.19

2

48.60

15.71

0.00

Number of gym sessions voluntarily attended

122.33

2

61.16

6.86

0.03

MANOVA yet none of the ANOVAs is statistically significant. This means exactly what it says, but you also need to realise that a linear combination of the dependent variables is related to group membership despite the fact that individually the dependent variables may fail to be related to group membership. Using these methods, you do not know much about that linear combination of variables. Discriminant function analysis would help you with this (see Chapter 28 and Box 27.2).

Box 27.2

Focus on

Discriminant function analysis and MANOVA In the present chapter, we have concentrated on the very basics of MANOVA. A significant MANOVA means that the groups defined by your independent variable are different in terms of the composite of the dependent variables you have used. The next question is just what aspects of the dependent variable(s) are responsible for the significant MANOVA. The approach used in the present chapter is to do a number of ANOVA analyses for the different dependent variables. This tells you if the means for your groups are different for any of the dependent variables. The trouble with this is that one is left somewhat unclear about the nature of the underlying combination variable derived from the several dependent variables. An improvement in understanding can be achieved using discriminant function analysis (which is covered in detail in Chapter 28). This analysis helps you to understand how your dependent variables were combined to give the significant MANOVA. These combinations of variables are known as discriminant functions. In other

words, the analysis creates artificial variables which it derives from one or more of the original dependent variables. This is usually done on a computer program such as SPSS Statistics as the calculations are tedious to do by hand – and you would be ill-advised to spend time doing so with the attendant risk of computational errors. There is one centroid (which is a sort of mean score) for each group of participants on each of the discriminant functions. Discriminant functions are obviously abstractions from the original dependent variables and, as such, they cannot be expected to be as clear initially as the variables that you included in the set of dependent variables. Interpretation is involved and using your intelligence, insight, and other thinking skills may come unexpected to those who wish to believe that statistics is a purely mechanical process. There can be several discriminant functions based on a set of dependent variables as already indicated. If there is just one dependent variable, as in ANOVA, then there is



M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 379

11/13/13 5:23 PM

380

CHAPTER 27  Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

just one discriminant function which is the same as that single dependent variable. With two dependent variables there can be two discriminant functions and so forth. The number of discriminant functions is the smaller number of the number of dependent variables or one less the number of groups. Each discriminant function that emerges in an analysis is unrelated to the other discriminant functions that emerge. That is, discriminant functions do not correlate with each other. The term discriminant function seems odd at first, but it means just what it says on the label. It is a mathematical equation (function) which discriminates things. What does it discriminate? Well, it is the mathematical function of the dependent variables which best discriminate between the different groups (i.e. levels of the independent variable). Basically the calculation (computer) works out the pattern of weights to give to each of the dependent variables in order to produce the maximum discrimination between the various groups on the discriminant function. Of course, there are many different possible discriminant functions since it is basically a pattern of weights to apply to the different dependent variables, but only one function will give the greatest degree of discrimination between the



different groups. In other words, discriminant function analysis produces a new measure (function) which maxi­ mises the difference between the groupings of participants on that measure (function). As indicated, there may be several discriminant functions. The first discriminant function essentially emerges from the original data whereas the second discriminant function is calculated on the data after the first dis­ criminant function has been taken into account. The third discriminant function is calculated from the data after the first and second discriminant functions have been removed. There is a conceptual problem when we move from MANOVA to discriminant function analysis. In MANOVA we tend to speak of the scores as being the dependent variable and the variable on which the groups differ is the independent variable. Well, discriminant function ana­ lysis, like the various forms of regression, works the other way round. In this case, the score variables become the independent variables and the dependent variable is the variable on which the different groups are categorised. Yes, this is confusing, but if you concentrate on the nature of the variable in question (category variable or score) then Chapter 28 should be straightforward.

■ Step 3 A table of estimated marginal means is helpful at this stage. SPSS Statistics generates separate tables for each of the main effects and each interaction. In the present case, we have reproduced only the estimated marginal means for the significant main effect (the team building variable). This can be seen in Table 27.6. It is clear from this that scores on each of the first two dependent variables are lowest for the control, second highest

Table 27.6

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 380

Estimated marginal means for groups on each dependent variable Difference between ratings of most and least liked team members

Number of gym sessions voluntarily attended

Number of games played

Teamwork training by sports psychologist

7.29

11.21

14.93

Teamwork training by coach

4.43

8.36

13.86

Control – no teamwork training

3.79

7.14

11.29

11/13/13 5:23 PM

27.4  Reporting your findings 381

Table 27.7

The Box’s M test for covariance homogeneity (equality) Box’s M

18.70

F

1.38

df1

12

df2

7371

Significance

0.17

for team building by a coach, and highest for team building by the psychologist. It is not immediately obvious which of the three dependent variables best discriminates the three conditions. Remember that this is a down-to-basics account of MANOVA. We do not pretend that it offers the most sophisticated approach. You might wish, especially, to check whether your data actually meet the requirements of MANOVA in terms of the characteristics of the data. One quite important thing is the Box’s test of equality of the covariance matrix. We don’t need to know too much about this test, but we do need to know what to do if the test is statistically significant. The Box’s test is illustrated in Table 27.7. If it yields a significant value (as it does in our case), this means that the covariances are not similar, which violates one of the assumptions on which MANOVA was built. This can affect the probability levels obtained in the MANOVA. However, this is crucial only if the MANOVA significance levels just reach the 0.05 level of significance. If your MANOVA findings are very significant then there is not a great problem. You should not worry if the different cells (groupings) of your MANOVA have equal sample sizes as violating the requirements of the MANOVA makes no practical difference to the significance level in this case. If you have very different sample sizes and your findings are close to the boundary between statistical significance and statistical non-significance, then you should worry more – one solution is to equate the sample sizes by randomly dropping cases from cells as necessary. But this could have as much effect on your findings as violating the equal covariances principle anyway. So bear this in mind when designing your MANOVA.

27.4 Reporting your findings If your MANOVA was not significant, you could write the following, after the APA (2010) Publication Manual’s recommendations: ‘MANOVA was used to test the hypothesis that team work training had an effect on sporting behaviours, but the null hypothesis was supported, Pillai’s F(6, 76) = 1.08, p ns.’ However, since the findings were significant, you could write: ‘MANOVA showed that teamwork training was effective in improving sporting behaviours, Pillai’s F(6, 76) = 4.12, p < .01. The individual dependent variables were subject to ANOVAs in order to assess whether the three dependent variables showed the same trend. For the measure of the difference between favourite and least favourite team member measure it was found that the psychologist teamwork sessions (M = 7.29) were superior to the coach team work sessions (M = 4.43) and the control condition (M = 3.79), F(2, 39) = 15.71, p < .01. The mean number of gym sessions attended was higher for the psychologist (M = 11.21) than the coach (M = 8.36) and control (M = 7.14), F(2, 39) = 6.86, p < .05.’

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 381

11/13/13 5:23 PM

382

CHAPTER 27  Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

Research examples MANOVA Guzman and Kingston (2012) studied sport dropout. At one point in time, variables believed to be predictors of sport dropout were measured and whether the individual had persisted with the sport or dropped out was assessed after 19 months. The participants were 857 young athletes with a mean age of around 15 years. Part of the study involved a MANOVA analysis. The design was dropout or persistence × male or female × age (three categories). The several dependent variables analysed at the same time were psychological need satisfaction from sport, intention to practise sport, perceived conflict between sport and study, and the self-determination index. Drop-out was related to these dependent variables in MANOVA as was age. There were no interaction effects. Lowe and Ang (2012) were interested in the experience of test anxiety (fear of evaluation) in elementary students in the USA and Singapore. Culture and gender were the independent variables making this a 2 × 2 design. MANOVA was used for the statistical analysis because several dependent variables were employed – physiological hyperarousal, social concerns, task-irrelevant behaviour and worry. The MANOVA (and additional regular ANOVAs) showed that Singapore males had more test anxiety than US males whereas the US females scored more highly than the Singapore females on the overall test anxiety scale and the physiological hyper­ arousal subscale. Singapore males had higher anxiety on the Worry subscale. Casidy (2012) chose to examine differences in the personality of consumers which were related to the variables of a) fashion consciousness – which is the individual’s involvement in fashionable dressing and so forth and b) prestige sensitivity – preference for the high-priced, higher-quality, designer clothes. The data were collected from undergraduate students using self-completion questionnaires. She included items from what she calls the big five scales used to measure consumer personality in the literature. Using cluster analysis, she found four clusters of highly related items in the responses of the students to these items. These clusters she identified as ‘openness to experience, extraversion, agreeableness and consciousness’. The data were analysed using MANOVA. The independent variables in this study were each of the personality clusters. The multiple dependent variables were fashion consciousness and prestige sensitivity. There were personality differences in terms of the prestige sensitivity/fashion consciousness dependent variable.

Key points ●

MANOVA basically deals with a very simple problem – the risk of falsely accepting a hypothesis because you have carried out multiple tests of significance.



Try to avoid an unfocused approach to MANOVA. It is not a particularly useful technique for sorting out what to do with numerous dependent variables that you have measured merely because you could.



MANOVA is not appropriate if all of your dependent variables are highly intercorrelated. It may be better in these circumstances to combine the dependent variables to give a total score which is then analysed using ANOVA, for example.



A complete MANOVA would preferably involve a discriminant function analysis. This is described in Chapter 28.

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 382

11/13/13 5:23 PM

Computer analysis 383



Computer analysis MANOVA using SPSS

Figure 27.2

SPSS Statistics steps for MANOVA

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

A number of different multivariate tests are given in the Multivariate Tests output. Pillai’s trace is as good as any for most purposes. For the Tests for Between Subjects Effects output you only need to concentrate on the row for Group in this example.



You could write: ‘MANOVA showed that teamwork training was effective in improving sporting behaviours, Pillai’s F(6, 76) = 4.12, p < .01.’

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 383

11/13/13 5:23 PM

384

CHAPTER 27  Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

Screenshot 27.1

The data

Screenshot 27.2

Select the test

Screenshot 27.3

Select the variables

Screenshot 27.4

Select options

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 384

11/13/13 5:23 PM

Recommended further reading 385



Screenshot 27.5

The important output

Recommended further reading Diekhoff, G. (1992). Statistics for the social and behavioral sciences (Chapter 15). Dubuque, IL: Wm. C. Brown. Hair, J.F., Jr, Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.L., & Black, W.C. (2009). Multivariate data analysis (7th ed., Chapter 6). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall. Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed., Chapter 7). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

M27_HOWI0749_06_SE_C27.indd 385

11/13/13 5:23 PM

Chapter 28

Discriminant (function) analysis – especially in MANOVA Overview ●

Discriminant function analysis uses a set of score variables to assess the extent to which they are associated with the different groups in a study. In other words, it asks the question of whether it is possible to discriminate between groupings of participants (conditions) on the basis of a set of independent variables.



It is similar to logistic regression (Chapters 42 and 43) in terms of what it does, though it cannot use category variables as predictor variables and is based on more restrictive assumptions.

● The main use of discriminant function analysis is following a significant MANOVA (Chapter 27).

It serves as a way of understanding what combinations of a set of variables best differentiate the different groups (conditions) in a study. ●

A discriminant function is a variable derived from a set of variables which maximises the differences between the groups on that set of variables. It computes a set of weights which are applied to each variable in the set of score variables. More than one discriminant function may emerge.



It is important to avoid extremely highly correlated variables since this creates high collinearity which distorts the analysis and puts the meaning of the findings in doubt. If two variables highly correlate then one could be omitted from the analysis (it contains no different information from the other variable with which it correlates highly). To check, the omitted variable could then be used instead of that variable and the analysis repeated and the two outcomes compared.



Discriminant function analysis can classify participants into groups on the basis of their ‘scores’ on the discriminant functions. This classification can then be compared with the group that the participants actually belong to.

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 386

11/13/13 5:24 PM

28.1  Introduction 387



Discriminant function analysis has concepts which are new – especially those of centroids and canonical correlation. But a centroid is nothing more than the mean of a group on a discriminant function (which is just a special sort of variable) and canonical correlation is just a correlation coefficient but between one set of variables and another set of variables.

Preparation Read Chapter 27 on MANOVA, but understanding something about regression (Chapters 9 and 32) and especially logistic regression (Chapters 42 and 43) will be beneficial.

28.1 Introduction Discriminant function analysis once did the job for which logistic regression is now the preferred technique. For most purposes, logistic regression is better than discriminant function analysis since its underlying basic assumptions are less demanding (restricting). Both techniques tell the researcher whether different groups of participants (categories of the dependent variable) can be accurately classified on the basis of a number of other variables (the independent variables) in combination. For discriminant function analysis, these other variables must be score variables (logistic regression can handle nominal or category variables in addition). The main (perhaps only) reason why discriminant function analysis is included in this textbook is its role in relation to MANOVA (see Chapter 27). In many ways, discriminant function analysis and MANOVA are built on the same basic mathematical calculations. Consequently, it is not surprising that when MANOVA cannot answer a particular question, discriminant function analysis is used to fill in the information gap. Apart from that, we would not recommend its use. Its role in relation to MANOVA is to indicate the combination of variables which best discriminate between the different groups of participants. In order to do this, a researcher must examine what the discriminant functions which significantly discriminate between the groups actually represent. This is done by seeing which variables correlate best with the discriminant function. In this regard, it is a little like factor analysis (Chapter 31). Table 28.1 illustrates a study for which discriminant function analysis is appropriate. It deals with three drug conditions (including one no-treatment control). Much the same data were previously discussed in Chapter 27 on MANOVA, though notice that we have reversed the labelling of the independent variable and the dependent variables. There are four independent variables (reaction time, clarity of speech, steadiness of hand and writing speed). The dependent variable is the drug condition. The research question is basically what pattern or combination of the independent variables best classifies individuals in terms of the group to which they belong. That is, can we predict group membership accurately on the basis of the scores we have on the independent variables? Some authorities describe discriminant function analysis as the reverse of MANOVA. This is a reasonable description, especially since the independent and dependent variables are reversed between MANOVA and discriminant function analysis. In MANOVA, the categories of the independent variable become the dependent variable in discriminant function analysis. The dependent variables (the score variables) in MANOVA become the

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 387

11/13/13 5:24 PM

CHAPTER 28  Discriminant (function) analysis – especially in MANOVA

388

Table 28.1

Data table for a study of effects of Therazine on motor skills Group (dependent variable)

Therazine condition Independent variables

a b

Placebo condition Independent variables

RTa

Sp

8b

5

7

7

1

3

7

7

6

5

4

5

9

8

5

9

7

2

7

5

8

8

2

5

Hd

W

RT

Sp

Hd

No-treatment condition Independent variables W

RT

Sp

Hd

W

2

2

4

3

5

4

3

3

1

2

3

6

1

2

3

5

2

6

6

1

1

4

6

2

RT = Reaction time, Sp = clarity of speech, Hd = steadiness of hand and W = writing speed. Scores are from four cases in each column. The scores are for the four independent variables.

independent variables in discriminant function analysis. There is a strong relationship between ANOVA and multiple regression – indeed many calculations of ANOVA actually use regression techniques. The strongest indication of that is the use of the term intercept (from regression) in some ANOVA analyses. There is also a very strong relationship between MANOVA and discriminant function analysis, as we have indicated. Of course, what is really confusing is the use of the terms independent and dependent variables, which should not be taken to indicate that one thing causes the other. Predictor and criterion variables are another way of saying the same thing. No matter, in discriminant function analysis the independent variables are the score variables whereas the dependent variable consists of the different groups of participants. So essentially in discriminant function analysis we are trying to predict which group of participants individuals belong to on the basis of a number of predictor variables. Another way of saying exactly the same thing is to suggest that discriminant function analysis seeks to find whether the different groups of participants are different in terms of their means on the independent variables. This is often expressed in terms of the means of each group on the discriminant functions. These means are called centroids. Though this sounds like a radically new concept, it merely indicates the group mean on a discriminant function. One thing is vital to understand. A discriminant function is basically a way of totalling or combining the scores on the independent variables. Instead of adding the scores on variables A, B, C and D as follows: A + B + C + D, etc. in discriminant function analysis, each score variable is given a different weight (w) so that the formula for the discriminant function is: w1A + w2B + w3C + w4D, etc. This is little different from the formula for multiple regression (Chapter 32), though we have omitted a constant from the above for the purposes of clarity. Of course, there are any number of different sets of weights that can be applied. However, in discriminant function analysis the discriminant function used is the one which best discriminates (differentiates) the various groups of participants. Only one discriminant function can meet

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 388

11/13/13 5:24 PM

28.2  Doing the discriminant function analysis 389

this criterion. One could think of the discriminant function as simply a variable based on a combination of other variables, just as factors in factor analysis are variables (see Chapter 31). Just so long as you remember that this combination variable is one that maximises differences between the groups (conditions) in the study then you cannot go far wrong. It is a little more complicated than that since there can be several discriminant functions calculated for any set of data. The first discriminant function maximises the differences between the groups of participants (on that discriminant function). In other words, the discriminant function is the weighted combination of the predictor variables that maximises the difference between the groups of participants (i.e. conditions of the study). Thus it is the function (weighted combination of variables) that best discrim­ inates the groups in the study. There may remain important variation in the data after this has been done. So a second discriminant function may sometimes be calculated based on the original data minus variation due to the first discriminant function. The process can continue to produce further discriminant functions depending on the number of groups to differentiate and the number of predictor variables (score variables). The discriminant functions are unrelated to each other – that is to say, they are independent of each other or orthogonal. Basically this means that discriminant functions from an analysis do not correlate. Actually, discriminant function analysis does not handle more than one dependent variable at a time. This is no great problem as the main effects from the MANOVA can be dealt with one at a time (the main effects of MANOVA are independent of each other). It is important, so as not to be flustered when you come across new terminology, to know that discriminant function analysis works largely using canonical correlations. These are similar to multiple correlations (see Chapter 32) which are the correlation of several variables with one other variable. Canonical correlation is the correlation of a set of several variables with another set of several variables. In discriminant function analysis there are several independent variables and also several dependent variables since there are usually several different groups. Don’t worry. We have read claims that canonical analysis has the dubious distinction of being the hardest multivariate concept to understand. Actually, apart from knowing that there is such a thing as canonical correlations, there is not a great deal more that you need to know about the computer output for discriminant function analysis that you probably don’t know already from other parts of this book. There is a limit to the number of discriminant functions that can be produced for any set of data. The number of groups being discriminated minus 1 is one criterion and the number of (score) variables in the analysis is the other criterion. Whichever is the smaller of the two is the maximum number of discriminant functions.

28.2 Doing the discriminant function analysis Table 28.2 gives the discriminant function analysis version of the data that we used in Chapter 27 to illustrate the steps in a MANOVA analysis. Chapter 27 left the MANOVA analysis incomplete since it lacked a discriminant function analysis, which adds to our ability to understand what is happening in our data. In the following discussion we concentrate solely on using discriminant function analysis to identify group membership in terms of team-building procedures. We know from MANOVA (Chapter 27) that we have a significant effect of the teamwork condition on the scores on the set of three dependent variables. In the MANOVA chapter, the fact that we had found a significant

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 389

11/13/13 5:24 PM

CHAPTER 28  Discriminant (function) analysis – especially in MANOVA

390

Table 28.2

Data for the discriminant function analysis Group (dependent variable)

Team building with sports psychologist Independent variables

Team building with sports coach Independent variables

No team building controls Independent variables

Likea

Gym

Game

Like

Gym

Game

Like

Gym

Game

9b

12

14

4

6

15

9

6

10

5

9

14

5

4

12

1

2

5

8

11

12

4

9

15

6

10

12

4

6

5

3

8

8

2

5

6

9

12

3

4

9

9

3

6

7

9

11

14

5

3

8

4

7

8

6

13

14

2

8

12

1

6

13

6

11

18

6

9

11

4

9

12

8

11

22

4

7

15

3

8

15

8

13

22

4

8

28

3

2

14

9

15

18

5

7

10

2

8

11 10

7

12

18

4

9

9

6

9

8

10

13

5

18

18

3

8

13

6

11

22

7

12

24

6

14

22

Like = difference between ratings of most and least liked team members, Gym = number of gym sessions voluntarily attended and Game = number of games played. b The scores are the scores on the three independent variables. a

MANOVA freed us to do several ANOVAs to see which of the dependent variables (score variables) were influenced by the particular group to which participants belonged. We found that teamwork training did not seem to have an influence on the number of games played, but it did have an influence on the other two dependent variables. In other words, we already know quite a bit from the MANOVA. This should be remembered now that we move on to the discriminant function analysis. And don’t forget that when we say independent variable in discriminant function analysis we would call it a dependent variable in MANOVA and vice versa. So the group variable is labelled the dependent variable in Table 28.2 and the independent variables are the scores. Figure 28.1 gives the key steps in discriminant function analysis.



■ Step 1 Before we start a discriminant function analysis, there is one important thing to repeat. Highly correlated score variables should be avoided. This is because of the problem of collinearity (discussed in Chapter 32). Basically you need to check the correlations between all of the score variables to make sure that this is not the case. Just compute a correlation matrix between all of your score variables to see whether there are any highly correlated items. In discriminant function analysis, the score variables are the independent or predictor variables. We are thinking of correlations of the order of maybe 0.7 and

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 390

11/13/13 5:24 PM

28.2  Doing the discriminant function analysis 391

Figure 28.1

Conceptual steps for understanding discriminant function analysis

above. The question, then, is what you can do about this situation. The higher the correlation the more the two variables are assessing more or less the same thing in terms of the variation in scores. So it would be reasonable to do the analysis leaving out one of the two highly correlated variables and any others you find. There is no harm in this since the two variables are virtually the same and so leaving one of them out is no real handicap. Of course, if you wish, you could reinstate those variables and leave out the other variables which you had previously included. Almost certainly, you will find virtually no difference in terms of the two analyses except for the names of the variables involved.



■ Step 2 Having run your data through SPSS Statistics or some other program which does discriminant function analysis, the next thing to do is to look for the eigenvalues in the output. Note that for our example, the maximum number of discriminant functions possible is the number of groups – 1 which is 3 – 1 which is 2 discriminant functions. (The number of variables is the other formula but this is bigger than 2 in this case, so it is ignored.) The first discriminant function is the most discriminating between the groups and the later discriminant functions are the most discriminating after the earlier discriminant functions have been removed from the data. Table 28.3 gives the characteristic eigenvalue information that is provided. For discriminant function 1, we can see that the eigenvalue is 0.87. This is indicative of the amount of variation in the data which is

Table 28.3 The eigenvalues and variance explained by each discriminant function Function

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 391

Eigenvalue

% of variance

Canonical correlation

Discriminant function 1

0.87

97

0.68

Discriminant function 2

0.03

3

0.16

11/13/13 5:24 PM

392

CHAPTER 28  Discriminant (function) analysis – especially in MANOVA

accounted for by the first discriminant function. Looking at the column for % of variance, this corresponds to 97% of the total variance. This is not the total of the variance in the data but the total of the variance explained by the discriminant functions. It is, in other words, the reliable variance explained and excludes error variance. Thus in this example the total of the two eigenvalues is 0.87 + 0.03 = 0.90. Thus the proportion of variance explained = 0.87 ÷ 0.90 which equals 0.97; expressed as a percentage this becomes the figure of 97% of the total variance explained in the penultimate column in Table 28.3. Don’t be misled into thinking that the research has identified a weighted set of variables (the discriminant function) which explains 97% of the variation in the data. That would be phenomenal. What it means is that the researcher has found a discriminant function which explains the reliable variance (i.e. with error variance ignored) which is much less impressive. Also notice that a cumulative % of variance explained figure is given. Since there is a maximum of two discriminant functions possible where one has three groups, then it is hardly surprising that those two discriminant functions account for all of the variation that could be accounted for by the discriminant functions. It is all the reliable variance that can be explained. The final column in Table 28.3 gives the canonical correlation for the first dis­ criminant function as being 0.68. If you interpret this much as you would do any correlation, it is clear that it is quite a substantial correlation and indicative of a strong relationship between the predictor (score) variables and the groups of participants. In contrast, the second discriminant function not only explains very little of the variance but the canonical correlation is fairly low at 0.16. Again, if this were an ordinary correlation coefficient we would regard the value as fairly low. This is exactly the same for the canonical correlation. So we are left with the impression that there is one substan­tial discriminant function and a rather unsubstantial second discriminant function for these data.



■ Step 3 A crucial part of the analysis is the information on Wilks’ lambda (Table 28.4). The table can be a little confusing at first. What the analysis basically does is to indicate whether or not your discriminant function analysis is statistically significant. It does so by first of all giving the significance of all of your discriminant functions together. So where you see in the first column 1 through 2 this includes all of the discriminant functions in the analysis. For our example, the maximum is 2, so that row reads 1 through 2. If we had four groupings then this would read 1 through 3 because 3 is the maximum number of discriminant functions with four groupings (see above). The next row in our example reads just 2. This is the test for the second discriminant function alone. So, as we can see, discriminant functions 1 and 2 together are very significant at 0.00, but discriminant

Table 28.4 The values of Wilks’ lambda Test of function(s)

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 392

Wilks’ lambda

Chi-square

df

Sig.

Discriminant functions 1 through 2

0.52

24.69

6

0.00

Discriminant function 2

0.97

1.01

2

0.61

11/13/13 5:24 PM

28.2  Doing the discriminant function analysis 393

function 2 on its own is not significant. Of course, this means that the discriminant functions do not individually have to be statistically significant in order that you have a significant discriminant function analysis overall. The table for Wilks’ lambda becomes more complex with increasing numbers of discriminant functions. But, despite this, the key is the first row in the table since if that is not significant then you need proceed no further in examining the output. (You probably would not have done the discriminant function analysis anyway since the MANOVA that you probably have computed previously would have indicated a lack of significance in your data already.) If the Wilks’ lambda is statistically significant then this indicates that the means (centroids) of the different groups on the discriminant function(s) are statistically different. The value of lambda can range from 0 to 1. It is the amount of variation in the discriminant function which cannot be accounted for by the different groups (con­ ditions). The value of lambda will increase as you go down the column for lambda since the discriminant functions lower down the list have more variance than cannot be explained by differences between the groups in the analysis. We know that already, since they are the discriminant functions which are poorest at differentiating the groups in the analysis.



■ Step 4 The main point of doing the discriminant function analysis following MANOVA is to understand which of your predictor (independent) variables are associated with the discriminant functions that have been calculated. In other words, just what weights are given to the predictor variables in calculating each of the discriminant functions. Just knowing the weights as such is not very helpful since the weights depend on the exact scale and range of scores on each variable. These may be different for each of your predictor variables, so it is better to have a standardised version of the weights (coefficients) as this then allows meaningful comparison. These standardised weights can be seen in Table 28.5. The big weights given to the first discriminant function are for ‘difference between ratings of most and least liked team members’ (0.85). The number of gym sessions voluntarily attended has a smaller relative weight (0.30). It is also notable that ‘number of games played’ has a near zero weight (−0.04). Thus, the first discriminant function is most clearly identified with the variable concerning the most and least favourite team member, though there is also a component of the discriminant function which is associated with the voluntary attendance at gym session. The second discrimin­ ant function, which we already have seen is in itself not statistically significant, has its major weighting solely for number of games played.

Table 28.5 Standardised coefficients for the different discriminant functions Group (condition)

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 393

Function Discriminant function 1

Discriminant function 2

Difference between ratings of most and least liked team members

0.85

−0.31

Number of gym sessions voluntarily attended

0.30

0.05

Number of games played

0.04

1.00

11/13/13 5:24 PM

394

CHAPTER 28  Discriminant (function) analysis – especially in MANOVA

The structure matrix of the correlation of the predictors and the standardised Table 28.6 discriminant functions Group (condition)

Discriminant function 1

Discriminant function 2

Difference between ratings of most and least liked team member

0.97

0.09

Number of gym sessions voluntarily attended

0.64

0.38

Number of games played

0.27

0.96

Alternatively, we could look at the correlations between the predictor variables and the standardised discriminant functions (the structure matrix). Although the values of the correlations in Table 28.6 naturally differ from the weights shown in Table 28.5, the direction of the results is similar. The variable that is most highly correlated with the first standardised discriminant function in Table 28.6 is the difference between the most and least liked team members (0.97). This variable also has the largest weight (0.85) on the first standardised discriminant function in Table 28.5. The variable that is most highly correlated with the second discriminant function in Table 28.6 is the number of games played (0.96). This variable also has the greatest weight (1.00) on the second discriminant function in Table 28.5. So the picture seems to be that the first discriminant function consists largely of ‘difference between ratings of most and least liked team members’ with a smaller con­ tribution from ‘number of gym sessions voluntarily attended’. The second discriminant function is the ‘number of games played’, though it is fairly clear by now that this function is unimportant relative to the first discriminant function and non-significant statistically. Although this interpretation makes sense and fits the statistical analysis, it has to be said that the discriminant function analysis does not shed a great deal of light on the combination of predictor variables which best discriminate between the groups. It is not like, say, exploratory factor analysis (Chapter 31) which can unveil patterns which are meaningful and informative. This is partly because that is not really the job of discriminant function analysis. More light may be shed if you have more variables than in this case. However, if you have many variables (such as where you have administered a lengthy questionnaire) then it would be wise to subject this questionnaire to factor analysis initially rather than throw all of the variables into a discriminant function analysis.



■ Step 5 Since the discriminant scores are variables of a special sort, it is useful to examine the mean ‘scores’ for each discriminant function for each group of participants. These are shown in Table 28.7. Remember that discriminant functions are just variables so each participant can be scored on each discriminant function. So it is possible to find the average score for each group on the discriminant functions. This basically tells us which

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 394

11/13/13 5:24 PM

28.2  Doing the discriminant function analysis 395

Table 28.7

Means (centroids) for the groups (conditions) on each discriminant function

Group

Discriminant function 1

Discriminant function 2

1.24

−0.04

Team psychologist Team coach

−0.42

0.21

Control

−0.83

−0.17

groups are high and low on each discriminant function. So in terms of the first dis­ criminant function, the teamwork talk by the sports psychologist generates the highest mean (remember that the discriminant function is largely about the ‘difference between the most and least favourite team member’). The other two groups are more similar to each other on this discriminant function. The second discriminant function (number of games played was the most associated variable) seems to suggest that the teamwork talk by the team coach produced higher scores. However, this second discriminant function is to be discounted because of its lack of significance.



■ Step 6 Finally, we need to ask to what extent the discriminant functions can be used to accurately classify participants in terms of the group that they were in. This is done by comparing the predicted group based on the discriminant functions with the actual group membership. Examining Table 28.8, it can be seen that the accuracy of the prediction depends on which group one is considering. The discriminant functions accurately classified 78.6% of the 14 participants who underwent the team-building sessions with the psychologist, but only 28.6% of the 14 participants who had team-building sessions with the coach were correctly classified. For the control group, accuracy was 57.1% since 8 out of the 14 members of the control group were accurately classified by the discriminant functions. Fifty per cent of those allocated to the coach teamwork con­ dition were actually misclassified as being in the control condition.

Table 28.8

Accuracy of the classification based on the discriminant functions

Actual group membership

Predicted group membership Psychologist teamwork

Psychologist teamwork

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 395

Coach teamwork

Controls

11 (78.6%)

2 (14.3%)

1 (7.1%)

Coach teamwork

3 (21.4%)

4 (28.6%)

7 (50.0%)

Controls

4 (28.6%)

2 (14.3%)

8 (57.1%)

11/13/13 5:24 PM

396



CHAPTER 28  Discriminant (function) analysis – especially in MANOVA

■ Step 7 There is an alternative way of doing the discriminant function analysis – using a stepwise process. Stepwise processes are discussed in Chapter 32. In SPSS Statistics, this involves pressing one additional button. Stepwise would have advantages in terms of simplicity of the output. This is because it chooses the biggest discriminant functions and does not include any discriminant function which is not statistically significant. Thus in the above tables, only one discriminant would be mentioned because only the first discriminant function is significant. This is a considerable saving of effort, of course. Unfortunately, and this may be sufficient reason for you not to use stepwise, this is not the same model as the original MANOVA employed. In that MANOVA, essentially all discriminant functions were used as the basis of the calculation (though it would not be apparent that this was what was happening) – that is, the significant MANOVA is based on all of the discriminant functions. So there is no reason why this should change for the discrimin­ ant function analysis. But by using stepwise you are probably violating the MANOVA model. Some textbooks, nevertheless, advise the use of stepwise discriminant function analysis. In truth, it probably makes very little difference to the way you understand your analysis.

28.3 Reporting your findings One way of summarising the results of this analysis according to the APA (2010) Publication Manual’s recommendations is as follows: ‘A direct discriminant analysis was carried out using the three predictors of the difference between the most and least liked team member, the number of gym sessions voluntarily attended and the number of games played to determine which of these variables best discriminate between teams built with a sports psychologist, teams built with a coach and teams built with neither of these (the control condition). Two discriminant functions were calculated, explaining about 97% and 3% of the variance, respectively. Wilks’ lambda was significant for the combined functions, X2(6, N = 42) = 24.69, p < .001 but was not significant when the first function was removed, X2(2, N = 42) = 1.01, p = .605. The first discriminant function maximally differentiated the psychologist’s teamwork training from the other two groups and correlated most highly with the difference between the most and least liked members (.96) and the number of gym sessions attended (.64). The second discriminant function maximally distinguished the coach’s team from the other two groups and loaded most strongly with the number of games played (.96). About 80% of the cases were correctly classified compared with 33% expected by chance. About 79% of the psychologist’s team members were correctly identified with 14% misclassified as the coach’s team members. Fifty-seven per cent of the control team members were correctly identified with 29% misclassified as the psychologist’s team members. Twenty-nine per cent of the coach’s team members were correctly identified with 50% misclassified as the control team members.’

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 396

11/13/13 5:24 PM

Key points 397



Research examples Discriminant function analysis Although discriminant function analysis is included in this book largely to help with the interpretation of MANOVA, it can be used in its own right as in the examples below. However, we would recommend using logistic regression in these circumstances. Gannon and Barrowcliffe (2012) used a group of both university students and community participants. The participants were asked to indicate confidentially whether they have ever been involved in firesetting. At intervals they were also asked to complete a new Fire Setting Scale and Fire Proclivity Scale. Eleven per cent admitted firesetting. Using discriminant function analysis an attempt was made to see whether the firesetters could be effectively discriminated from the non-firesetters using the two scales. Just one subscale from the Fire Propensity Scale known as the propensity behavioural index significantly discriminated between the two groups of participants. The overall hit rate was 91% but only 72% of the firesetters were correctly classified. Gray, LaPlante and Shaffer (2012), using records of actual Internet gambling, were able to study a group of gamblers who had triggered an irresponsible gambling alert with a matched group of controls who had had the same amount of exposure to gambling on the Internet but did not trigger concerns. Discriminant function analysis was used to differentiate the two groups. It was found that indices reflecting the intensity of the gambling activity best differentiated the two groups. These indices included the total number of bets made, the number of Euros per bet and the number of bets per betting day especially for live sports betting. Ridenour, McCoy and Dean (1996) investigated the possibility of malingering by patients under neuropsychological assessment. One possible reason for the malingering was the involvement of an insurance claim. Some participants were asked to fake symptoms on the Neuropsychological Symptom Inventory whereas other reported honestly. The items of the Inventory were used in a discriminant function analysis in an attempt to see whether the two groups could be differentiated on the basis of their replies. Overall, participants were correctly classified according to their group membership. There were just over 2% false positives.

Key points ●

Although discriminant function analysis is a general technique to assess the accuracy with which different groups can be classified on the basis of a set of score variables, it is not the best technique for doing so. It is important in relation to MANOVA since discriminant function analysis and MANOVA are based on very similar assumptions and mathematics.



Check out logistic regression (Chapters 42 and 43) if you simply want to know which variables accurately classify groups of participants. Discriminant function analysis has drawbacks compared to logistic regression.



Only where you have a significant MANOVA do you need to consider using discriminant function analysis.

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 397

11/13/13 5:24 PM

CHAPTER 28  Discriminant (function) analysis – especially in MANOVA

398

Computer analysis Discriminant function analysis using SPSS

Figure 28.2 SPSS Statistics steps for a discriminant function analysis

Interpreting and reporting the output ● The main task of interpreting the data is to identify how many significant discriminant functions there are

from the Wilks’ Lambda output table. In this case there are two significant discriminant functions. The Structure Matrix then tells you how each of the variables loads on each of the discriminant functions. ●

A detailed approach to reporting these findings is given in Section 28.3 in this chapter. This draws on additional output tables. Refer to this section to find help on how to report your findings.

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 398

11/13/13 5:24 PM

Computer analysis 399



Screenshot 28.1 Select the test

Screenshot 28.2 Select variables

Screenshot 28.3 Select options



The Structure Matrix Table giving correlations of each variable with the functions

Screenshot 28.5 Screenshot 28.4 The Wilks’ lambda output

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 399

11/13/13 5:24 PM

400

CHAPTER 28  Discriminant (function) analysis – especially in MANOVA

Recommended further reading Cramer, D. (2003). Advanced quantitative data analysis (Chapter 13). Buckingham: Open University Press. Diekhoff, G. (1992). Statistics for the social and behavioral sciences (Chapter 14). Dubuque, IL: Wm. C. Brown. Hair, J.F., Jr, Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.L., & Black, W.C. (2009). Multivariate data analysis (7th ed., Chapter 5). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall. Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed., Chapter 9). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

M28_HOWI0749_06_SE_C28.indd 400

11/13/13 5:24 PM

Chapter 29

Statistics and the analysis of experiments

Overview ●

Leaving consideration of the statistical analysis for your study until the last minute is not a good idea. Making it integral with the planning of your research is the ideal but many find it difficult to give it that priority.



It is a good discipline to sketch out the statistical analysis of your data at a minimum as early as possible. It may help you discover analytic problems while something can be done to correct them.



Choosing an appropriate statistical analysis depends on a clear statement of what you want the analysis to achieve (e.g. the hypotheses or relationships to be tested), clearly identifying what variables are nominal (category) variables and what are score variables, and whether you are looking for correlations or for differences in mean scores.

● Researchers need to understand that they may need to be creative in their approach to the

analysis. It is essential to feel free to manipulate the data to create new variables or develop composite measures based on several items.

Preparation Make sure that you understand hypotheses (Chapter 11) and nominal category data versus numerical score data (Chapter 2).

M29_HOWI0749_06_SE_C29.indd 401

11/13/13 5:24 PM

402

CHAPTER 29  Statistics and the analysis of experiments

29.1 Introduction Feeling jaded and listless? Don’t know what stats to use to analyse your study? Make money from home. Try Professor Warburton’s Patent Stats Pack. All the professional tricks revealed. Guaranteed not to fail. Gives hope where there is no hope. Professor Warburton’s Stats Pack troubleshoots the troubleshooters. Since the death of Professor Warburton in 1975, through thrombosis of the wallet, his Patent Stats Pack had been feared lost. Libraries on three continents were searched. Miraculously it was discovered after many years in Australia in a trunk under the bed of a dingo farmer. Auctioned recently at Sotheby’s to an unknown buyer – reputedly a German antiquarian – it broke all records. Controversy broke out when scholars claimed that Professor Warburton was a fraud and never held an academic appointment in his life. To date, it has not been possible to refute this claim. These are vile slurs against Professor Warburton whom many regard as the founder of the postmodernist statistics movement and the first person to deconstruct statistics. We have exclusive rights to the Patent Stats Pack so judge for yourself.

29.2 The Patent Stats Pack

■ Principle 1 Practically nothing needs to be known about statistical calculations and theory to choose appropriate procedures to analyse your data. The characteristics of your research are the main considerations – not knowledge of statistics books.



■ Principle 2 Ideally you should not undertake research without being able to sketch out the likely features of your tables and diagrams.



■ Principle 3 You can make a silk purse out of a sow’s ear. First catch your silk pig.  .   .   .   A common mistake is thinking that the data as they are collected are the data as they will be analysed. Sometimes, especially when the statistical analysis has not been planned prior to collecting data, you may have to make your data fit the available statistical techniques. Always remember that you may need to alter the format of your data in some way in order to make them suitable for statistical analysis or a particular analysis. These changes include: ●

adding scores from several variables to get a single overall or composite variable

● separating

a variable into several different components (especially where you have collected data as frequencies in nominal categories and have allowed multiple answers).

M29_HOWI0749_06_SE_C29.indd 402

11/13/13 5:24 PM

29.3  Checklist 403

Box 29.1

Focus on

Where to get advice One should be wary about from where to get statistical advice. A little knowledge can be a dangerous thing and this applies to statistical advice as much as anything. Sometimes a sort of blind panic sets in whereby a student, for example, feels that they cannot cope with the demands of a quantitative analysis of their data and so becomes reliant on anyone who will listen and appears to know a little more than they do. It is easy to impress by bandying about statistical terminology and alluding to the inherent problems of various statistical techniques. None of this is particularly helpful to the person with a pressing need to get on with analysing their data. What we are trying

to say, hopefully subtly, is that in our experience students’ difficulties with statistical analysis are made worse by being given wrong or impractical advice given the circumstances. Worse still, sometimes this third party advice is communicated with such conviction that the hapless student is torn between this advice and what they know about statistics in general already. Consequently, because they lack confidence in their statistical ability, this contradictory advice pushes them into a tail-spin from which it is difficult to recover. Whatever, a clear head and sufficient time are needed to sort out your statistical analysis.

29.3 Checklist Years of experience providing statistical advice suggests that rarely is the statistical analysis the basic problem – far more important is the inadequate conceptualisation which underlies the research design. Statistics is of some help – but limited – even where a research design was inadequate in some way. It is far better to get your research design clear before you start. Profound knowledge of statistical techniques is probably not the most important skill of the researcher. Instead, apparently simple skills such as being able to understand how one’s research aims can be met using your chosen research questions are more important. Just what is there in the research design which allows one to answer the research question? If you can’t answer this question satisfactorily then there is likely to be some sort of conceptual muddle which is hindering your process. If you knew virtually nothing about statistics, how could you use your data to answer your research question? For example, you might answer this question by saying draw a scattergram between this variable and that variable or the average score in one group should be higher than the average score in another group. That is, just what would you look for in your data to answer your research question? Of course, prevention is better than cure in statistical analysis. Sometimes it is easy to see the root cause of the conceptual muddle which has resulted in someone seeking statistical advice. It is hard to be clear about concepts, and the more concepts involved in a study then the greater the capacity for muddle. It is important to be able to write your ideas down, but it is equally or even more important to be able to talk about your ideas to other people. By talking about your plans to your research supervisor, colleagues and friends then you are actively engaging with the all-important building blocks of your study. It may be embarrassing to do so, sometimes, but this might encourage a re-think if it proves problematic to communicate your ideas clearly to others. Some non-statistical steps which are important for a good statistical analysis are given in Figure 29.1.

M29_HOWI0749_06_SE_C29.indd 403

11/13/13 5:24 PM

404

CHAPTER 29  Statistics and the analysis of experiments

Figure 29.1

Important things in order to get your statistical analysis right

M29_HOWI0749_06_SE_C29.indd 404

11/13/13 5:24 PM

29.3  Checklist 405

So what can be done where the statistical analysis does not seem to flow from your research design? The following are some of the major considerations which will help you choose an appropriate statistical analysis for your data. 1. Write down your hypothesis. Probably the best way of doing this is to simply fill in the blanks in the following: ‘My hypothesis is that there is a relationship between variable 1 ___ and variable 2 ___’ Do not write in the names of more than two variables. There is nothing to stop you having several hypotheses. Write down as many hypotheses as seems appropriate – but only two variable names per hypothesis. Treat each hypothesis as a separate statistical analysis at least for now. If you cannot name the two variables you see as correlated then it is possible that you wish only to compare a single sample with a population. In this case check out the single-sample chi-square (Chapter 15) or the single-sample t-test (Chapter 13). 2. If you cannot meet the requirements of 1 above then you are possibly confused about the purpose of the research. Go no further until you have sorted this out – do not blame statistics for your conceptual muddle. Writing out your hypotheses until they are clear may sound like a chore, but it is an important part of statistical analysis. Your first attempts may be hopelessly inadequate but they can be improved upon. You need to start from somewhere. 3. Classify each of the variables in your hypothesis into either of the following categories: a) numerical score variables b) nominal (category) variables – and count the number of categories. 4. Based on 3, decide which of the following statements is true of your hypothesis: a) I have two numerical score variables. (Yes/No) (if yes then go to 5) b) I have two nominal category variables. (Yes/No) (if yes then go to 6) c) I have one nominal category variable and one numerical score variable. (Yes/No) (if yes then go to 7) 5. If you answered yes to 4(a) above (i.e. you have two numerical score variables) then your statistical analysis involves the correlation coefficient. This might include Pearson correlation, Spearman correlation or regression. Turn to Chapter 34 on the analysis of questionnaire research for ideas of what is possible. 6. If you answered yes to 4(b), implying that you have two nominal category variables, then your statistical analysis has to be based on contingency tables using chi-square or closely related tests. The range available to you is as follows: a) chi-square b) Fisher exact probability test for 2 × 2 or 2 × 3 contingency tables, especially if the samples are small or expected frequencies low c) the McNemar test if you are studying change in the same sample of people d) at the more advanced level, logistic regression (Chapters 42 and 43) and log-­ linear analysis (Chapter 41) may be appropriate where you have many nominal variables.

M29_HOWI0749_06_SE_C29.indd 405

11/13/13 5:24 PM

406

CHAPTER 29  Statistics and the analysis of experiments

Table 29.1 Food preferences of a sample of 50 teenagers Food type

Frequency

Vegetarian

19

Fast food

28

Italian

9

Curry

8

The only problem you are likely to experience with such tests is if you have allowed the participants in your research to give more than one answer to a question. If you have, then the solution is to turn each category into a separate variable and code each individual according to whether or not they are in that category. This is referred to as dummy coding and is covered in detail later in this book (Chapter 42). So, for example, in a frequency table such as Table 29.1 it is pretty obvious that multiple responses have been allowed since the total of the frequencies is in excess of the sample size of 50. This table could be turned into four new tables: ●

Table 1: The number of vegetarians (19) versus the number of non-vegetarians (31)



Table 2: The number of fast food preferrers (28) versus the non-fast food preferrers (22)



Table 3: Italian preferrers (9) versus Italian non-preferrers (41)



Table 4: Curry preferrers (8) versus non-curry preferrers (42).

7. If you answered yes to 4(c) then the nominal (category) variable is called the independent variable and the numerical score variable is called the dependent variable. The number of categories for the independent variable partly determines the statist­ ical tests you can apply: a) If you have two categories for the independent (nominal category) variable then: ● the ●

t-test is a suitable statistic (Chapters 13 and 14)

the one-way analysis of variance is suitable (Chapters 21 and 22).

The choice between the two is purely arbitrary as they give equivalent results. Remember to check whether your two sets of scores are independent or correlated/ related. If your scores on the dependent variable are correlated then it is appro­ priate to use the related or correlated versions of the t-test (Chapter 13) and the analysis of variance (Chapter 22). b) If you have three or more categories for the independent (nominal category) variable then your choice is limited to the one-way analysis of variance. Again, if your dependent variable features correlated or related scores, then the related or correlated one-way analysis of variance can be used (Chapters 20 and 21).

M29_HOWI0749_06_SE_C29.indd 406

11/13/13 5:24 PM

29.4  Special cases 407

Box 29.2

Focus on

Problematic data If it becomes clear that the basic assumptions of parame­ tric tests are violated by your data (which for all practical purposes means that the distribution of scores is very skewed), then you might wish to employ a nonparametric equivalent (Chapter 19 and Appendix B2). However, you may wish to look at bootstrapping procedures (see Box 19.1) which are not as reliant on symmetrical and normally distributed data as conventionally many of the parametric tests are. Bootstrapping makes no more

assumptions about the nature of the data than can be seen from your data. The big advantage of bootstrapping procedures is that they can be applied to many conventional parametric techniques. Bootstrapping is a welcome recent feature in SPSS Statistics. The use of a computer package is essential for bootstrapping procedures which involve vast numbers of samples drawn randomly from your data set (which is multiplied numerous times in order to get a large sample).

Sometimes you may decide that you have two or more independent variables for each dependent variable. Here you are getting into the complexities of the analysis of variance and you need to consult Chapters 23 and 25 for advice.

29.4 Special cases

■ Multiple items to measure the same variable Sometimes instead of measuring a variable with a single question or with a single technique, that variable is measured in several ways. Most likely is that a questionnaire has been used which contains several questions pertaining to the same thing. In these circumstances, you will probably want to combine these questions to give a single numerical score on that variable. The techniques used to do this include the use of standard scores and factor analysis (which are described in Chapters 6 and 31). Generally by combining these different indicators of a major variable together to give a single score you improve the reliability and validity of your research. The combined scores can be used as a single variable and analysed with t-tests or analyses of variance, for example.



■ Assessing change over time The simplest way of studying change over time is to calculate the difference between the first testing and the second testing. This is precisely what a repeated measures t-test, for example, does. However, these difference scores can themselves be used in whatever way you wish. In particular, it would be possible to compare difference scores from two or more different samples in order to assess if the amount of change over time depended on gender or any other independent variable. In other words, it is unnecessary to have a complex analysis of variance design which includes time as one independent variable and gender as the other.

M29_HOWI0749_06_SE_C29.indd 407

11/13/13 5:24 PM

CHAPTER 29  Statistics and the analysis of experiments

408

Key points ● Nobody

ever learnt to play a musical instrument simply by reading a book and never practising. It takes time to become confident in choosing appropriate statistical analyses.



Simple statistical analyses are not automatically inferior to complex ones.

● Table

29.2 should help you choose an appropriate statistical procedure for your experimental data. It is designed to deal only with studies in which you are comparing the means of two or more groups of scores. It is not intended to deal with correlations between variables.

Table 29.2 An aid to selecting appropriate statistical analyses for different experimental designs

a

Type of data

One sample compared with known population

Two independent samples

Two related samples

Two or more independent samples

Two or more related samples

Two or more independent variables

Nominal (category) data

One-sample chi-square

Chi-square

McNemar test

Log-linear

Not in this booka

Chi-square

Numerical score data

One-sample t-test

Unrelated t-test, Unrelated one-way ANOVA

Related t-test, related one-way ANOVA

Unrelated ANOVA

Related ANOVA

Two-way, etc. ANOVA

Numerical score data which violate assumptions of parametric tests

Not in this booka

Mann– Whitney U-test

Wilcoxon matched pairs test

Kruskal– Wallis (Appendix B2)

Friedman (Appendix B2)

Not in this booka

These are fairly specific nonparametric tests which are rarely used.

M29_HOWI0749_06_SE_C29.indd 408

11/13/13 5:24 PM

PART 4

More advanced correlational statistics

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 409

11/18/13 3:05 PM

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 410

11/18/13 3:05 PM

Chapter 30

Partial correlation Spurious correlation, third or confounding variables, suppressor variables

Overview ●

Partial correlation is used to statistically adjust a correlation between two variables to take into account the possible influence of a third (or confounding) variable or variables. These are sometimes known as control variables.



That is, partial correlation deals with the third-variable problem in which additional vari­ ables may be the cause of spurious relationships or hide (suppress) the relationship between two variables.



If one control variable is used then we have a first-order partial correlation. If two control variables are used then the result is a second-order partial correlation. And so forth.



A zero-order correlation is the original unmodified correlation between two variables.



Partial correlation may be helpful in trying to assess the possibility that a relationship is a causal relationship though it cannot supply definitive proof.

Preparation Revise the Pearson correlation coefficient (Chapter 8) if necessary. Make sure you know what is meant by a causal relationship.

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 411

11/18/13 3:05 PM

412

CHAPTER 30  Partial correlation

30.1 Introduction The partial correlation coefficient is particularly useful when trying to make causal statements from field research. It is not so useful in experimental research where different methods are used to establish causal relationships. Look at the following research outlines taking as critical a viewpoint as possible: ●

Project 1  Researchers examine the published suicide rates in different geographical locations in the country. They find that there is a significant relationship between unemployment rates in these areas and suicide rates. They conclude that unemploy­ ment causes suicide.



Project 2  Researchers examine the relationship between shoe size and liking football matches. They find a relationship between the two but claim that it would be nonsense to suggest that liking football makes your feet grow bigger.

Although both of these pieces of research are superficially similar, the researchers draw rather different conclusions. In the first case it is suggested that unemployment causes suicide whereas in the second case the researchers are reluctant to claim that liking foot­ ball makes your feet grow bigger. The researchers in both cases may be correct in their interpretation of the correlations, but should we take their interpretations at face value? The short answer is no, since correlations do not demonstrate causality in themselves. In both cases, it is possible that the relationships obtained are spurious (or artificial) ones which occur because of the influence of other variables which the researcher may not have considered. So, for example, the relationship between shoe size and liking football might be due to gender – men tend to have bigger feet than women and tend to like football more than women do. So the relationship between shoe size and liking football is merely a consequence of gender differences. The relationship between unemployment and suicide, similarly, could also be due to the influence of a third variable. In this case, the variable might be social class. If we found, for example, that being from a lower social class was associated with a greater likelihood of unemployment and with being more prone to suicide, this would suggest that the relationship between unemployment and suicide was due to social class differences, not because unemployment leads directly to suicide. Partial correlation is a statistically precise way of calculating what the relationship between two variables would be if one could take away the influence of one (or more) additional variables. Sometimes this is referred to as controlling for a third variable or partialling out a third variable. In essence it revises the value of your correlation coefficient to take into account third variables.

Box 30.1

Focus on

Causality This is intended as a timely reminder of things discussed in depth earlier in this book. Partial correlation can never confirm that a causal relationship exists between two variables. The reason is that partialling out a third, fourth or fifth variable does not rule out the possibility that there is an additional variable which has not been considered

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 412

which is the cause of the correlation. However, partial correlation may be useful in examining the validity of claims about specified variables which might be causing the relationship. Considerations of causality are a minor aspect of partial correlation.

11/18/13 3:05 PM

30.2  Theoretical considerations 413

30.2 Theoretical considerations Partial correlation can be applied to your own data if you have the necessary correla­ tions available. However, partial correlation can also be applied to published research without necessarily obtaining the original data itself – so long as the appropriate correla­ tion coefficients are available. All it requires is that the values of the correlations between your two main variables and the possible third variable are known. It is not uncommon to have the necessary tables of correlations published in books and journal articles, although the raw data (original scores) are rarely included in published research. A table of correlations between several variables is known as a correlation matrix. Table 30.1 is an example featuring the following three variables: numerical intelligence test score (which we have labelled X in the table), verbal intelligence test score (which we have labelled Y in the table) and age (which we have labelled C in the table) in a sample of 30 teenagers. Notice that the diagonal from top left to bottom right consists of 1.00 repeated three times. This is because the correlation of numerical score with itself, verbal score with itself and age with itself will always be a perfect relationship (r = 1.00) – it has to be since you are correlating exactly the same numbers together. Also notice that the matrix is sym­ metrical around the diagonal. This is fairly obvious since the correlation of the numerical score with the verbal score has to be the same as the correlation of the verbal score with the numerical score. More often than not a researcher would report just half of Table 30.1, so the correlations would look like a triangle. It doesn’t matter which triangle you choose, although it is usual to display the lower left triangle as we read from left to right. Remember that we have used the letters X, Y and C for the different columns and rows of the matrix. The C column and C row are the column and row, respectively, for the control variable (age in this case). Not only is partial correlation an important statistical tool in its own right, it also forms the basis of other techniques such as multiple regression (Chapter 32).

Table 30.1

A correlation matrix involving three variables Variable X Numerical score

Box 30.2

Variable Y Verbal score

Variable C Age in years

Variable X Numerical score

1.00

0.97

0.80

Variable Y Verbal score

0.97

1.00

0.85

Variable C Age In years

0.80

0.85

1.00

Key concepts

Mediator and moderator variables There is a crucial conceptual distinction in research which has a bearing on our discussion of partialling or control­ ling for third variables. This is the difference between

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 413

moderator and mediator variables. This is not a statistical issue, as such, but a key issue in relation to research design and methodology. A knowledge of statistics, however, is



11/18/13 3:05 PM

CHAPTER 30  Partial correlation

414

helpful in understanding the distinction and putting it into effect. Put crudely, a mediator variable is a variable which explains the relationship between two other variables (usually best expressed as the independent and dependent variable). For example, imagine that there is a correlation between annual income (independent variable) and happi­ ness (dependent variable) such that richer people are happier. Although this relationship would be interesting, it is somewhat unsatisfactory from a psychological and theoretical point of view since we do not know the psy­ chological processes which create the relationship. So we might imagine another variable, extensiveness of social network, which might be influenced by annual income and might lead to greater happiness. We know from previous research that a supportive social network contributes to happiness. Now the reason why income may be associated with happiness may be because having more money allows one to socialise more and that the more one socialises the more likely it is that one forms an extensive social net­ work. The variable, extensiveness of social network, can be described as a mediator variable since it mediates the relationship between income and happiness. The way that we have described this implies a causal relationship. That is, basically, higher income (independ­ ent variable) influences social networking (the mediator variable) which then influences happiness (the dependent variable) (see Figure 30.1). This is only established in ran­ domised studies as is any causal relationship. That is, in order to really establish a causal relationship the researcher would have to randomly allocate participants to the richer and poorer conditions and study the effects of this on both the mediator variable (social networking) and the dependent variable (happiness). Without randomisation, the causal interpretation is much more tentative. For instance, it is perfectly possible that people with extensive social networks have higher incomes as a consequence of their ability to network rather than vice versa.



Figure 30.1



M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 414

Possible circumstances for partial correlation mediated by a third variable

A moderator variable is something quite different. It is a variable which reveals that the relationship between the independent and dependent variable is not consistent throughout the data. Imagine that, once again, the researcher is investigating the relationship between income (independent variable) and happiness (the dependent variable). However, this time the researcher is interested in whether the gen­ ders differ in terms of the size of the relationship. Imagine that for men the correlation between income and happi­ ness is 0.6 but that for women the correlation is very small only 0.0. This implies quite different conclusions for men and for women. In one case there is quite a substantial correlation and in the other case no correlation. In other words, gender moderates the relationship between income and happiness. Quite clearly, how we understand the rela­ tionship between income and happiness would be different for men and women. A moderator variable does not explain the relationship, of course. We would have to consider further the explanation of why the relationship is different in women and men. It could be, for example, that women’s social networks are more influenced by having children and so mixing with other women with children than men’s social networks. Perhaps men’s social networks are more affected by having the money to go to the pub, the golf club or the yacht club, for instance. This, of course, is to begin to ask why gender moderates the relationship between income and happiness – notice that we are hinting at possible mediating variables. Moderator variables, in themselves, are not directly about establishing causal rela­ tionships so randomisation is not an issue for the research design. Chapter 39 covers moderator variables in detail. Quite clearly, the techniques that we have described in this chapter are ways of studying moderator and mediator variables. But there are other techniques described in this book which can also contribute. For example, interactions in ANOVA (Chapter 23) can be regarded as evidence of moderator effects as explained in Chapter 39 on moder­ ator variables. The same is true for significant log-linear interactions (Chapter 41). The appropriate statistics really depend on whether you have score or category variables or both. However, it is the research design which influences whether or not a variable is conceived as a moderator or mediator variable. For example, if a variable cannot be influenced by the independent variable, then it can only be conceived as a moderator variable. For example, income (independent variable) cannot affect a person’s gender so gender cannot be a mediator variable between income and happiness. It can, however, be a moderator variable in the relationship between income and happiness.

11/18/13 3:05 PM

30.3 dOInG PARTIAl CORRelATIOn

30.3

415

Doing partial correlation In order to understand partial correlation better, it is useful to understand the gist of how the calculation is done. Once you have the correlation coefficients involved in the partial correlation, the rest of the calculation is fairly quick. Computer programs for the partial correlation will normally calculate the correlations for you. Explaining Statistics 30.1 works out the relationship between verbal and numerical scores in the Table 30.1 con­ trolling for age (rXY.C).

Explaining statistics 30.1

how the partial correlation coefficient works The calculation is based on the correlations found in Table 30.1. The formula is as follows: rXY.C =

rXY − (rXC × rYC ) 2 2 1 − r XC 1 − r YC

where rXY.C = correlation of verbal and numerical scores with age controlled as denoted by C rXY = correlation of numerical and verbal scores (= 0.97) rXC = correlation of numerical scores and age (the control variable) (= 0.80) rYC = correlation of verbal scores and age (the control variable) (= 0.85). Using the values taken from the correlation matrix in Table 30.1 we find that rXY.C = = =

0.97 97 − (0.80 × 0.8 85) 1 − 0.80 2 1 − 0.852 0.97 97 − (0.68) 1 − 0.64 1 − 0.72 . 0 .2 9 0.36 0.28 .

=

0 .2 9 0 .2 9 = = 0 .9 91 0 .6 × 0 .5 3 0 .3 32 2

Thus controlling for age has hardly changed the correlation coefficient – it decreases only very slightly from 0.97 to 0.91.

Interpreting the results A section on interpretation follows. However, when interpreting a partial correlation you need to consider what the unpartialled correlation is. This is the baseline against which the partial correlation is understood. Although usually we would look to see if partialling reduces the size of the correlation, it can increase it.

Reporting the results The following is one way of reporting this analysis: ‘Since age was a correlate of both verbal and numerical ability, it was decided to investigate the effect of controlling for age on the correlation. After partialling, the correlation of .97 declined slightly to .91. However, this change is very small and so age had little or no effect on the correlation between verbal and numerical abilities.’

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 415

11/18/13 3:05 PM

416

CHAPTER 30  Partial correlation

30.4 Interpretation What does the result of Explaining statistics 30.1 mean? The original correlation between numerical and verbal scores of 0.97 is reduced to 0.91 when we control for age. This is a very small amount of change and we can say that controlling for age has no real influence on the original correlation coefficient. The following is the original pattern of relationships between the three variables: the partial correlation essentially removes all the variation between verbal scores and age and also between numerical scores and age. This is rather like making these correlations zero. But, in this case, when we make these correlations zero we still find that there is a very substantial correlation between verbal and numerical scores:

This is an important lesson since it suggests that controlling for a third variable does not always affect the correlation, despite the fact that in this case the control variable age had quite substantial relationships with both verbal and numerical ability scores. This should be a warning that simply showing that two variables are both correlated with a third variable does not in itself establish that the third variable is responsible for the main correlation.

Despite this, often the partial correlation coefficient substantially changes the size of the correlation coefficient. Of course, it is important to know that a third variable does not change the correlation value. In contrast, the example in Section 30.7 and Explaining statistics 30.3 shows a major change following partialling.

Explaining statistics 30.2

How the statistical significance of the partial correlation works The calculation of statistical significance for the partial correlation can be carried out simply using tables of the signifi­ cance of the Pearson correlation coefficient such as Significance Table 11.1 or the table in Appendix C. However, in order to do this you will need to adjust the sample size by subtracting three. Thus if the sample size is 10 for the Pearson correlation, it is 10 − 3 = 7 for the partial correlation coefficient with one variable controlled. So in our example in Table 30.1,

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 416

11/18/13 3:05 PM

30.6  Suppressor variables 417

which was based on a sample of 30 teenagers, we obtain the 5% significant level from the table in Appendix C by finding the 5% value for a sample size of 30 − 3 = 27. The minimum value for statistical significance at the 5% level is 0.367 (two-tailed).

Interpreting the results The statistical significance of the partial correlation coefficient is much the same as for the Pearson correlation coeffi­ cient on which it is based. A statistically significant finding means that the partial correlation coefficient is unlikely to have been drawn from a population in which the partial correlation is zero.

Reporting the results The statistical significance of the partial correlation may be reported in exactly the same way as for any correlation coefficient. The degrees of freedom are different since they have to be adjusted for the number of control variables. If the sample size for the correlation is 10, then subtract three to give seven degrees of freedom if just one variable is being controlled for. In other words, subtract the total number of variables including the two original variables plus all of the control variables. So if there were four control variables in this example, the degrees of freedom become 10 − 2 − 4 = 4.

30.5 Multiple control variables It may have struck you that there might be several variables that a researcher might wish to control for at the same time. For example, a researcher might wish to control for age and social class at the same time, or even age, social class and gender. This can be done relatively easily on a computer but is rather cumbersome to do by hand. There are a number of terms that are used which are relatively simple if you know what they mean: ●

Zero-order correlation – the correlation between your main variables (e.g. rXY).



First-order partial correlation – the correlation between your main variables con­ trolling for just one variable (e.g. rXY.C).



Second-order partial correlation – the correlation between your main variables controlling for two variables at the same time (the symbol for this might be rXY.CD).

Not surprisingly, we can extend this quite considerably; for example, a fifth-order partial correlation involves five control variables at the same time (e.g. rXY.CDEFG). The principles remain the same no matter what order of partial correlation you are examining.

30.6 Suppressor variables Sometimes you might find that you actually obtain a low correlation between two vari­ ables which you had expected to correlate quite substantially. In some instances this is because a third variable actually has the effect of reducing or suppressing the correlation between the two main variables. Partial correlation is useful in removing the inhibitory effect of this third variable. In other words, it can sometimes happen that controlling the influence of a third variable results in a larger correlation. Indeed, it is possible to find

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 417

11/18/13 3:05 PM

418

CHAPTER 30  Partial correlation

Figure 30.2

Conceptual steps for understanding partial correlation

that an initially negative correlation becomes a positive correlation when the influence of a third variable is controlled. Figure 30.2 outlines the key steps in partial correlation.

30.7 An example from the research literature Baron and Straus (1989) took the officially reported crime rates for rapes from most US states and compared these with the circulation figures for soft-core pornography in these areas. The correlation between rape rates and the amounts of pornography over these states was 0.53. (If this confuses you, the correlations are calculated ‘pretending’ that each state is like a person in calculating the correlation coefficient.) The temptation is to interpret this correlation as suggesting that pornography leads to rape. Several authors have done so. However, Howitt and Cumberbatch (1990) took issue with this. They pointed out that the proportions of divorced men in these areas also correlated substantially with both pornography circulation rates and rape rates. The data are listed in Table 30.2. It might be the case that rather than pornography causing rape, the apparent relation­ ship between these two variables is merely due to the fact that divorced men are more likely to engage in these ‘alternative sexual activities’. It is a simple matter to control for this third variable, as set out in Explaining statistics 30.3.

Table 30.2

Correlation between rape, pornography and divorce Variable X Rape rates

Variable X: Rape rates Variable Y: Pornography circulation Variable C: Proportion of divorced men

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 418

1.00

Variable Y Pornography circulation

Variable C Proportion of divorced men

0.53

0.67

1.00

0.59 1.00

11/18/13 3:05 PM

30.8 An eXAMPle FROM A STudenT’S WORK

419

Explaining statistics 30.3

Another example of how the partial correlation works The formula is: rXY.C =

rXY − (rXC × rYC ) 2 2 1 − r XC 1 − r YC

where rXY.C = correlation of rape rates with pornography controlling for proportion of divorced men rXY = correlation of rape and pornography (= 0.53) rXC = correlation of rape and proportion of divorced men (= 0.67) rYC = correlation of pornography and proportion of divorced men (= 0.59). Using the values taken from the correlation matrix in Table 30.2 we find that: rXY.C =

.53 − (.67 × .59) 1 − .67 2 1 − .59 .59 2

= .22

In this case, the correlation when the third variable is taken into account has changed substantially to become much nearer zero. It would be reasonable to suggest that the partial correlation coefficient indicates that there is no causal relationship between pornography and rape – quite a dramatic change in interpretation from the claim that pornography causes rape. The argument is not necessarily that the proportion of divorced men directly causes rape and the purchase of pornography. However, since it is an unlikely hypothesis that rape and pornography cause divorce then the fact that partialling out divorce reduces greatly the correlation between rape and pornography means that our faith in the original ‘causal’ link is reduced.

30.8 An example from a student’s work It is becoming increasingly common to teach children with special educational needs in classrooms along with other children rather than in special schools. Butler (1995a) measured the number of characteristics a sample of 14 teachers possessed which have been held to be of special importance in the effective teaching of special needs children. These qualities would include ‘empathy towards special needs children’, ‘attitude towards integrating special needs children’ and about ten others. In order to assess the quality of the learning experience, the student researcher time­ sampled children’s task­centred behaviour – the number of time periods during which the child was concentrating on the task in hand rather than, say, wandering around the classroom causing a nuisance. The researcher rated one special needs child and one ‘normal’ child from each teacher’s class. She found that there was a very high correlation of 0.96 between the number of qualities that a teacher possessed and the amount of time that the special needs children spent ‘on task’ (df = 12, p < 0.01). Interestingly, the correlation of the measure of teacher qualities with the behaviour of normal children in the class was only 0.23. The student used partial correlation to remove the task­orientated behaviour of the ‘normal’ children in order to control for the extent to which teacher qualities had a beneficial effect on ordinary teaching. This made absolutely no difference to the correlation between the number of qualities the teacher possessed and the amount of time special needs children spent on educational tasks. In other words, the student could be confident that she had identified qualities of teachers which were especially beneficial to special needs children.

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 419

11/18/13 3:05 PM

420

CHAPTER 30  Partial correlation

In terms of the research design there might be some worries, as the student was well aware. In particular, in an ideal research design there would be a second observer rating the behaviour of the children in order to check the consistency of the ratings among different observers.

Research examples Partial correlation Gotwals, Stoeber, Dunn and Stoll (2012) argue that sport perfectionist research has not established whether or not perfectionism is adaptive or maladaptive. They distinguish between perfectionist striving and perfectionist concerns. It is clear that perfectionist concerns are maladaptive but not so for perfectionist strivings. They systematically reviewed 31 studies which contained 201 correlations of perfectionism. When normal correlations are considered the evidence was slightly in favour of the view that perfectionist strivings lead to adaptive characteristics in sport rather than maladaptive ones. However, the results of partial correlation analysis added a great deal of clarity. The researchers correlated perfectionist strivings with adaptive/maladaptive measures but controlled for perfectionist concerns. This materially altered the interpretation since perfectionist strivings were overwhelmingly associated with adaptive characteristics. That is, perfectionist strivings are a good thing especially when the negative aspect of perfectionist concerns is eliminated from the strivings measure. Nair, Collins and Napolitano (2012) point out that in women smoking can sometimes be regarded as a maladaptive means of weight control. Indeed, they perceive benefits in smoking such as weight control, enhanced mood and anxiety, even though physical activity has much the same influence. The researchers used what they call a cue reactivity paradigm which involved looking at one’s own body in a mirror and verbal accompaniments to increase body concerns. Smoking was measured using indices such as the women’s urge to smoke and the latency until their first smoke after the exposure sessions using the mirror, etc. They could then engage in intense physical activity. Partial correlations controlling for body mass index, nicotine dependency, withdrawal and depressive symptoms showed that the amount of time engaging in intense physical activity was associated with a lower self-reported urge to smoke. The time to the first puff did not show this relationship. Potter, Hartman and Ward (2009) point out that there is a role of depression and anxiety in the memory complaints of older adults. Their study explored the influence of perceived stress, life events and activity level on memory complaints made by older women in a healthy population. Fifty-four women completed self-report questionnaires dealing with these key variables. The General Frequency of Forgetting Scale was used to measure memory complaints and various reasonably well-established scales to measure the other variables. It was shown using partial correlation that high levels of perceived stress was correlated with more memory complaints after controlling for the influence of depression and anxiety. However, recent life events and activity level were not involved in memory complaints. The authors regard perceived stress as a psychological variable which affects the person’s assessment of their cognitive abilities.

Key points ●

If you are doing a field rather than a laboratory project, check your research hypotheses. If they appear to suggest that one variable causes another then consider using partial correlation. It can potentially enhance one’s confidence about making causal interpretations if a significant correlation remains after partialling. However, caution should still be applied since there always remains a risk that an additional variable suppresses the relationship between your two main variables. ● Do not forget that even after partialling out third variables, any causal interpretation of the correlation coefficient remaining has to be tentative. No correlation coefficient (including partial correlation coefficients) can establish causality in itself. You establish causality largely through your research design, not the statistics you apply.

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 420

11/18/13 3:05 PM

Computer analysis 421



● Do

not overlook the possibility that you may need to control more than one variable.

● Do

not assume that partial correlation has no role except in seeking causal relationships. Sometimes, for example, the researcher might wish to control for male–female influences on a correlation without wishing to establish causality. Partial correlation will reveal the strength of a non-causal relationship having controlled for a third variable. Causality is something the researcher considers; it is not something built into a correlation coefficient as such.

● Do

not forget to test the statistical significance of the partial correlation – as shown above, it is very easy.

Computer analysis Partial correlation using SPSS

Figure 30.3

SPSS Statistics steps for partial correlation

Interpreting and reporting the output ● Usually

you will wish to compare the partial correlation with the original (zero order) correlation. The output table contains the original correlations at the top and the correlations with age partialled out towards the bottom of the table.



We could write: ‘Because age was correlated with both verbal and numerical ability, age was controlled in this relationship using partial correlation. The correlation of .92 declined to 0.78 on partialling. The partial correlation was not significant at the 5% level.’

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 421

11/18/13 3:05 PM

422

CHAPTER 30  Partial correlation

Screenshot 30.1

The data

Screenshot 30.2

Select the test

Screenshot 30.3

Select variables for analysis

Screenshot 30.4

Select options

Screenshot 30.5

The important output

M30_HOWI0749_06_SE_C30.indd 422

11/18/13 3:05 PM

Chapter 31

Factor analysis Simplifying complex data

Overview ●

Factor analysis is used largely when the researcher has substantial numbers of variables seemingly measuring similar things. The question is just what pattern underlies this complex pattern of intercorrelations. It has proven particularly useful with questionnaires. ● It examines the pattern of correlations between the variables and calculates new variables (factors) which account for the correlations. In other words, it reduces data involving a number of variables down to a smaller number of factors which encompass the original variables. ● Factors are simply variables. The correlations of factors with the original variables are known as factor loadings, although they are merely correlation coefficients. Hence they range from –1.0 through 0.0 to +1.0. It is usual to identify the nature of each factor by examining the original variables which correlate highly with it. Normally each factor is identified by a meaningful name. ● Because the process is one of reducing the original variables down to the smallest number of factors, it is important not to have too many factors. The scree plot may be used to identify those factors which are likely to be significantly different from a chance factor. ● Factors are mathematically defined to have the maximum sum of squared factor loadings at every stage. They may be more easily interpreted if they are rotated. This procedure maximises the numbers of large factor loadings and small factor loadings while minimising the number of moderate factor loadings, making interpretation easier as a consequence. ● Factor scores provide a way of treating factors like any other variable. They are similar to standard or z-scores in that they have symmetrical numbers of positive and negative values and their mean is 0.00. They can be used to compare groups in terms of their mean factor scores.

Preparation Review variance (Chapter 6), correlation coefficient (Chapter 8) and correlation matrix (Chapter 30).

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 423

11/13/13 5:25 PM

424

CHAPTER 31  Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data

31.1 Introduction Researchers frequently collect large amounts of data. Sometimes, speculatively, they add extra questions to a survey without any pressing reason. With data on so many variables, it becomes difficult to make sense of the complexity of the data. With questionnaires, one naturally seeks patterns in the correlations between questions. However, the sheer number of interrelationships makes this hard. Take the following brief questionnaire:

Item 1: It is possible to bend spoons by rubbing them. Agree strongly Agree

Neither Disagree Disagree strongly

Item 2: I have had ‘out of body’ experiences. Agree strongly Agree

Neither Disagree Disagree strongly

Item 3: Satanism is a true religion. Agree strongly Agree

Neither Disagree Disagree strongly

Item 4: Tarot cards reveal coming events. Agree strongly Agree

Neither Disagree Disagree strongly

Item 5: Speaking in tongues is a peak religious experience. Agree strongly Agree

Neither Disagree Disagree strongly

Item 6: The world was saved by visiting space beings. Agree strongly Agree

Neither Disagree Disagree strongly

Item 7: Most people are reincarnated. Agree strongly Agree

Neither Disagree Disagree strongly

Item 8: Astrology is a science, not an art. Agree strongly Agree

Neither Disagree Disagree strongly

Item 9: Animals have souls. Agree strongly Agree

Neither Disagree Disagree strongly

Item 10: Talking to plants helps them to grow. Agree strongly Agree

Neither Disagree Disagree strongly

Agree strongly could be scored as 1, agree scored as 2, neither as 3, disagree as 4 and disagree strongly as 5. This turns the words into numerical scores. Correlating the answers to each of these 10 questions with each of the others for 300 respondents generates a large correlation matrix (a table of all possible correlations between all of the possible pairs of questions). Ten questions will produce 10 × 10 or 100 correlations. Although the correlation matrix is symmetrical about the diagonal from top left to bottom right, there remain 45 different correlations to examine. Such a matrix might be much like the one in Table 31.1.

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 424

11/13/13 5:25 PM

31.2  A bit of history 425

Table 31.1

Correlation matrix of 10 items Item 1

Item 2

Item 3

Item 4

Item 5

Item 6

Item 7

Item 8

Item 9

Item 10

Item 1

1.00

0.50

0.72

0.30

0.32

0.20

0.70

0.30

0.30

0.10

Item 2

0.50

1.00

0.40

0.51

0.60

0.14

0.17

0.55

0.23

0.55

Item 3

0.72

0.40

1.00

0.55

0.64

0.23

0.12

0.17

0.22

0.67

Item 4

0.30

0.51

0.55

1.00

0.84

0.69

0.47

0.44

0.56

0.35

Item 5

0.32

0.60

0.64

0.84

1.00

0.14

0.77

0.65

0.48

0.34

Item 6

0.20

0.14

0.23

0.69

0.14

1.00

0.58

0.72

0.33

0.17

Item 7

0.70

0.17

0.12

0.47

0.77

0.58

1.00

0.64

0.43

0.76

Item 8

0.30

0.55

0.17

0.44

0.65

0.72

0.64

1.00

0.27

0.43

Item 9

0.30

0.23

0.22

0.56

0.48

0.33

0.43

0.27

1.00

0.12

Item 10

0.10

0.55

0.67

0.35

0.34

0.17

0.76

0.43

0.12

1.00

It is not easy to make complete sense of this; the quantity of information makes over­ all interpretation difficult. Quite simply, large matrices are too much for our brains to comprehend. This is where factor analysis can be beneficial. It is a technique which helps you overcome the complexity of correlation matrices. In essence, it takes a matrix of correlations and generates a much smaller set of ‘supervariables’ which characterise the main trends in the correlation matrix. These supervariables or factors are generally much easier to understand than the original matrix.

31.2 A bit of history Factor analysis is not a new technique – it dates back to shortly after the First World War. It is an invention largely of psychologists, originally to serve a very specific purpose in the field of mental testing. There are numerous psychological tests of different sorts of intellectual ability. The original purpose of factor analysis was to detect which sorts of mental skills tend to go together and which are distinct abilities. It has proven more generally useful and is used in the development of psychological tests and questionnaires. Personality, attitude, intelligence and aptitude tests are often based on it since it helps select which items from the tests and measures to retain. By using factors, it is possible to obtain ‘purer’ measures of psychological variables than is possible by arbitrarily and subjective deciding what can be combined with what else in order to measure a construct that the researcher wants to measure. Not surprisingly, then, some theorists have used it extensively. The personality theories of researchers Raymond Cattell and Hans Eysenck (Cramer, 1992) were heavily dependent on factor analysis. The development of highspeed electronic computers has made the technique relatively routine since no longer does it require months of hand calculations.

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 425

11/13/13 5:25 PM

426

CHAPTER 31  Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data

Box 31.1

Focus on

Data issues in factor analysis One crucial question is what sample size is appropriate for a factor analysis. There is no simple answer to this. Well, that is not quite true – often it is suggested that for every variable in the analysis there should be many more partici­ pants. You will read suggestions such as have ten times as many participants as variables in a factor analysis to get reliable outcomes. As the advice we have seen ranges from just 2 participants per variable in the analysis to 20 part­ icipants per variable, you have quite a lot of discretion! The main alternative approach has been to stipulate a minimum number of participants, though, once again, as these recommendations vary from 100 to 1000 partici­ pants you might well feel a little confused. Furthermore, students rarely have the opportunity to collect data from this sort of number of participants. Does this mean that they should never carry out a factor analysis? It is not just a difficult issue for students. Professional researchers may have problems in getting samples of this sort of size. This is not merely a matter of sloth on their part. It can be notoriously difficult to obtain particular sorts of sample. For example, how much effort would be involved in getting a sample of 100 serial killers in the United Kingdom? Again, should researchers ignore factor analysis as an analytic technique in these circumstances? There are no alternatives to factor analysis which could be effectively used to do much the same job as factor analysis. The advice we would offer is simple – but there is a more complex version that you should consider. The sim­ ple advice is to have as big a sample size as possible – the more variables, the bigger the sample size should be. Your work is almost certain to be acceptable to most researchers if it is based on 300 or more participants. The smaller your sample size is below this, the more your work is likely to be criticised by someone. So what should one do in circumstances where this conventional criterion cannot be met? One thing to remember is that none of the sample size criteria has a real empirical justification and little in psychology is ever accepted simply on the basis of a single study. So there are circumstances in which one might be justified carrying out a factor analysis on a smaller sample size – for example, the more exploratory your study is the more you are likely to be simply trying to find interesting aspects of your data for future further exploration. Of course, one should acknowledge the limitations of your factor analysis

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 426

because of the small sample size. But we would also sug­ gest that you consider the following: ●

It is bad practice to simply throw a bunch of variables into a factor analysis. The axiom ‘junk in, junk out’ applies here. Be selective about which variables you put into a factor analysis. Probably you could do a factor analysis on most of your data in the sense that a com­ puter will perform a calculation. But what is the point of this? It is better to confine yourself to variables which you feel are likely to measure a particular con­ cept which you think important as well. As soon as you begin to be selective, the smaller the number of varia­ bles you put into the factor analysis and the less will small sample size be a problem.



If you have a small sample size, then be especially vigilant when you carry out your basic examination of your data using descriptive statistics. For example, variables that have little variability; variables that produce the same response from the vast majority of participants because, for example, they are rarely agreed with; variables for which many of your participants fail to give an answer; and variables that participants have difficulty understand­ ing may be omitted from the factor analysis. In other words, get rid of variables which are in some way prob­ lematic as they contribute junk (error) to your data. You would be well advised to do this anyway for any data.



The bigger the typical correlation there is between your variables, the more likely it is that your factor analysis will be reliable (stable across studies) so the more acceptable would be a smaller sample size. Similarly, the bigger the communality estimates, the smaller the sample size can be. Communality estimates are dis­ cussed later in this chapter.



The more variables you have for each factor you extract, the more stable your analysis is likely to be. In other words, if you have only one or two factors rather than 10 fac­ tors then the more reliable your factor analysis will be.

The minimum sample size issue tends to be most strongly expressed in relation to factor analysis. Other statistical techniques tend not to be subject to the same stringency. It is important to bear in mind the sample size issue since you need to be able to estimate roughly the confidence you can place in your analysis.

11/13/13 5:25 PM

31.3  Concepts in factor analysis 427

31.3 Concepts in factor analysis In order to understand factor analysis, it is useful to start with a simple and highly sty­ lised correlation matrix such as the one in Table 31.2. You can probably detect that there are two distinct clusters of variables. Variables A, C and E all tend to correlate with each other pretty well. Similarly, variables B, D and F all tend to correlate with each other. Notice that the members of the first cluster (A, C, E) do not correlate well with members of the second cluster (B, D, F) – they would not be very distinct clusters if they did. In order to make the clusters more meaningful, we need to decide what variables contribut­ ing to the first cluster (A, C, E) have in common; next we need to explore the similarities of the variables in the second cluster (B, D, F). Calling the variables by arbitrary letters does not help us very much. But what if we add a little detail by identifying the variables more clearly and relabelling the matrix of correlations as in Table 31.3? Interpretation of the clusters is now possible. Drawing the clusters from the table we find: First cluster variable A = skill at batting variable C = skill at throwing darts variable E = skill at juggling Second cluster variable B = skill at doing crosswords variable D = skill at doing the word game Scrabble variable F = skill at spelling

Table 31.2

Stylised correlation matrix between variables A to F Variable A

Variable B

Variable C

Variable D

Variable E

Variable F

Variable A

1.00

0.00

0.91

−0.05

0.96

0.10

Variable B

0.00

1.00

0.08

0.88

0.02

0.80

Variable C

0.91

0.08

1.00

−0.01

0.90

0.29

Variable D

−0.05

0.88

−0.01

1.00

−0.08

0.79

Variable E

0.96

0.02

0.90

−0.08

1.00

0.11

Variable F

0.10

0.80

0.29

0.79

0.11

1.00

Table 31.3

Stylised correlation matrix with variable names added Batting

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 427

Crosswords

Darts

Scrabble

0.00

0.91

−0.05

Batting

1.00

Crosswords

0.00

1.00

0.08

Darts

0.91

0.08

1.00

Scrabble

−0.05

0.88

−0.01

Juggling

0.96

0.02

0.90

Spelling

0.10

0.80

0.29

0.79

Juggling

Spelling

0.96

0.10

0.88

0.02

0.80

−0.01

0.90

0.29

1.00

−0.08

0.79

−0.08

1.00

0.11

0.11

1.00

11/13/13 5:25 PM

428

CHAPTER 31  Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data

Once this ‘fleshing out of the bones’ has been done, the meaning of each cluster is some­ what more apparent. The first cluster seems to involve a general skill at hand–eye coordination; the second cluster seems to involve verbal skill. This sort of interpretation is easy enough in clear-cut cases like this and with small correlation matrices. Life and statistics, however, are rarely that simple. Remember that in Chapter 30 on partial correlation we found that a zero correlation between two variables may become a large positive or negative correlation when we take away the influence of a third variable or a suppressor variable which is hiding the true relationship between two main variables. Similar sorts of things can happen in factor analysis. Factor analysis enables us to handle such complexities which would be next to impossible by just inspecting a correlation matrix. Factor analysis is a mathematical procedure which reduces a correlation matrix containing many variables into a much smaller number of factors or supervariables. A supervariable cannot be measured directly and its nature has to be inferred from the relationships of the original variables with the abstract supervariable. However, in identifying the clusters above we have begun to grasp the idea of supervariables. The abilities which made up cluster 2 were made meaningful by suggesting that they had verbal skill in common. The output from a factor analysis based on the correlation matrix presented above might look rather like the one in Table 31.4. What does this table mean? There are two things to understand: 1. Factor 1 and factor 2 are like the clusters of variables we have seen above. They are really variables, but we are calling them supervariables because they take a large number of other variables into account. Ideally there should only be a small number of factors to consider. 2. The numbers under the columns for factor 1 and factor 2 are called factor loadings. Really they are nothing other than correlation coefficients recycled with a different name. So the variable ‘skill at batting’ correlates 0.98 with the supervariable which is factor 1. ‘Skill at batting’ does not correlate at all well with the supervariable which is factor 2 (the correlation is nearly zero at −0.01). Factor loadings follow all of the rules for correlation coefficients so they vary from −1.00 through 0.00 to +1.00. We interpret the meaning of factor 1 in much the same way as we interpreted the clusters above. We find the variables which correlate best with the supervariable or factor in question by looking at the factor loadings for each of the factors in turn. Usually you will hear phrases like ‘batting, darts and juggling load highly on factor 1’. All this means is that they correlate highly with the supervariable, factor 1. Since we find that batting, darts and juggling all correlate well with factor 1, they must define the

Table 31.4

Factor loading matrix

Variable

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 428

Factor 1

Factor 2

Skill at batting

0.98

−0.01

Skill at crosswords

0.01

0.93

Skill at darts

0.94

0.10

Skill at Scrabble

−0.07

0.94

Skill at juggling

0.97

−0.01

Skill at spelling

0.15

0.86

11/13/13 5:25 PM

31.4  Decisions, decisions, decisions 429

factor. We try to see what batting, darts and juggling have in common – once again we would suggest that hand–eye coordination is the common element. We might call the factor hand–eye coordination. Obviously there is a subjective element in this since not everyone would interpret the factors identically. In order to interpret the meaning of a factor we need to decide which items are the most useful in identifying what the factor is about. Although every variable may have something to contribute, those with the highest loadings on a factor probably have the most to contribute to its interpretation. So where does one draw the line between useful factor loadings and not so useful? Generally speaking, you will not go far wrong if you take factor loadings with an absolute value of 0.50 and above as being important in assessing the meaning of the factor. Now this is a rule of thumb and with a very big sample size then smaller factor loadings may be taken into account. With a very small sample size, then the critical size of the loading might be increased to 0.60. Generally speaking, this is not a vital issue. When you have identified the highly loading items on the factor, write them out as a group on a piece of paper. Then peruse these items over and over again until you are able to suggest what these items seem to have in common or what it is they represent. There are no rules for doing this and, of course, different researchers may well come up with different interpretations of exactly the same list of items. This is not a problem anymore than it is whenever we try to label any sort of concept.

31.4 Decisions, decisions, decisions This entire section can be ignored by the faint-hearted who are not about to carry out a factor analysis. Now that you have an idea of how to interpret a factor loading matrix derived from a factor analysis, it is time to add a few extra complexities. As already mentioned, factor analysis is more subjective and judgemental than most statistical techniques you have studied so far. This is not solely because of the subjectivity of interpreting the meaning of factors. There are many variants of factor analysis. By and large these are easily coped with as computers do most of the hard work. However, there are five issues that should be raised as they underlie the choices to be made.



■ Rotated or unrotated factors? The most basic sort of factor analysis is the principal components method. It is a mathematically based technique which has the following characteristics: ●

The factors are extracted in order of magnitude from the largest to smallest in terms of the amount of variance explained by the factor. Since factors are variables they will have a certain amount of variance associated with them.



Each of the factors explains the maximum amount of variance that it possibly can.

The amount of variance ‘explained’ by a factor is related to something called the eigenvalue. This is easy to calculate since it is merely the sum of the squared factor loadings of a particular factor. Thus the eigenvalue of a factor for which the factor loadings are 0.86, 0.00, 0.93, 0.00, 0.91 and 0.00 is 0.862 + 0.002 + 0.932 + 0.002 + 0.912 + 0.002 which equals 2.4.

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 429

11/13/13 5:25 PM

430

CHAPTER 31  Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data

But maximising each successive eigenvalue or amount of variance is a purely math­ ematical choice which may not offer the best factors for the purposes of understanding the conceptual underlying structure of a correlation matrix. For this reason, a number of different criteria have been suggested to determine the ‘best’ factors. Usually these involve maximising the number of high factor loadings on a factor and minimising the number of low loadings (much as in our stylised example). This is not a simple process because a factor analysis generates several factors – adjustments to one factor can adversely affect the satisfactoriness of the other factors. This process is called rotation because in pre-computer days it involved rotating (or twisting) the axes on a series of scattergrams until a satisfactory or ‘simple’ (i.e. easily interpreted) factor structure was obtained. Nowadays we do not use graphs to obtain this simple structure since procedures such as varimax do this for us. Principal components are the unadjusted factors which explain the greatest amounts of variance but are not always particularly easy to interpret. These are quite abstract ideas and you may still feel a little confused as to which to use. Experimentation by statisticians suggests that the rotated factors tend to reveal underlying structures a little better than unrotated ones. We would recommend that you use rotated factors until you find a good reason not to.



■ Orthogonal or oblique rotation? Routinely researchers will use orthogonal rotations rather than oblique rotations. The difference is not too difficult to grasp if you remember that factors are in essence vari­ ables, albeit supervariables: ●

Orthogonal rotation simply means that none of the factors or supervariables is actu­ ally allowed to correlate with each other. This mathematical requirement is built into the computational procedures.

● Oblique

rotation means that the factors or supervariables are allowed to correlate with each other (although they can end up uncorrelated) if this helps to simplify the interpretation of the factors. Computer procedures such as promax and oblimin pro­ duce correlated or oblique factors.

There is something known as second-order factor analysis which can be done if you have correlated factors. Since the oblique factors are supervariables which correlate with each other, it is possible to produce a correlation matrix of the correlations between factors. This matrix can then be factor analysed to produce new factors. Since second-order factors are ‘factors of factors’ they are very general indeed. You cannot get second-order factors from uncorrelated factors since the correlation matrix would contain only zeros. Some of the controversy among factor analysts is related to the use of such second-order factors.



■ How many factors? We may have misled you into thinking that factor analysis reduces the number of vari­ ables that you have to consider. It can, but not automatically so, because in fact without some intervention on your part you could have as many factors as variables you started off with. This would not be very useful as it means that your factor matrix is as complex as your correlation matrix. Furthermore, it is difficult to interpret all of the factors since the later ones tend to be junk and consist of nothing other than error variance. You need to limit the number of factors to those which are ‘statistically significant’. There are no commonly available and universally accepted tests of the significance of a

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 430

11/13/13 5:25 PM

31.4  Decisions, decisions, decisions 431

factor. However, one commonly accepted procedure is to ignore any factor for which the eigenvalue is less than 1.00. The reason for this is that a factor with an eigenvalue of less than 1.00 is not receiving its ‘fair share’ of variance by chance. What this means is that a factor with an eigenvalue under 1.00 cannot possibly be statistically significant – although this does not mean that those with an eigenvalue greater than 1.00 are actually statistically significant. For most purposes it is a good enough criterion although skilled statisticians might have other views. Another procedure is the scree test. This is simply a graph of the amount of variance explained by successive factors in the factor analysis. The point at which the curve flattens out indicates the start of the non-significant factors. Getting the number of factors right matters most of all when one is going to rotate the factors to a simpler structure. If you have too many factors the variance tends to be shared very thinly.



■ Communality Although up to this point we have said that the diagonal of a correlation matrix from top left to bottom right will consists of ones, an exception is usually made in factor analysis. The reason for this is quite simple if you compare the two correlation matrices in Tables 31.5 and 31.6. You will notice that matrix 1 contains substantially higher correlation coefficients than matrix 2. Consequently the ones in the diagonal of matrix 2 contribute a dispro­ portionately large amount of variance to the matrix compared to the equivalent ones in matrix 1 (where the rest of the correlations are quite large anyway). The factors obtained from matrix 2 would largely be devoted to variance coming from the diagonal. In other words, the factors would have to correspond more or less to variables A, B and C. Hardly a satisfactory simplification of the correlation matrix. Since most psychological data tend to produce low correlations, we need to do something about the problem. The difficulty is obviously greater when the intercorrelations between the variables tend to

Table 31.5

Variable A

Correlation matrix 1 Variable A

Variable B

Variable C

1.00

0.50

0.40

Variable B

0.50

1.00

0.70

Variable C

0.40

0.70

1.00

Variable B

Variable C

Table 31.6

Correlation matrix 2 Variable A

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 431

Variable A

1.00

0.12

0.20

Variable B

0.12

1.00

0.30

Variable C

0.20

0.30

1.00

11/13/13 5:25 PM

432

CHAPTER 31  Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data

be small than where the intercorrelations tend to be large. This is simply because the value in the diagonal is disproportionately larger than the correlations. The solution usually adopted is to substitute different values in the diagonal of the correlation matrix in place of the ones seen above. These replacement values are called the communalities. Theoretically, a variable can be thought of as being made of three different types of variance: 1. Specific variance  Variance which can only be measured by that variable and is spe­ cific to that variable. 2. Common variance Variance which a particular variable has in common with other variables. 3. Error variance  Just completely random variance which is not systematically related to any other source of variance. A correlation of any variable with itself is exceptional in that it consists of all of these types of variance (that is why the correlation of a variable with itself is 1.00), whereas a correlation between two different variables consists only of variance that is common to the two variables (common variance). Communality is in essence the correlation that a variable would have with itself based solely on common variance. Of course, this is a curious abstract concept. Obviously it is not possible to know the value of this correlation directly since variables do not come ready broken down into the three different types of variance. All that we can do is esti­ mate the communality as best we can. The highest correlation that a variable has with any other variable in a correlation matrix is used as the communality. This is shown in Table 31.7. So if we want to know the communality of variable A we look to see what its highest correlation with anything else is (in this case it is the 0.50 correlation with variable B). Similarly we estimate the communality of variable B as 0.70 since this is its highest cor­ relation with any other variable in the matrix. Likewise the communality of variable C is also 0.70 since this is its highest correlation in the matrix with another variable. We then substitute these communalities in the diagonal of the matrix as shown in Table 31.8.

Table 31.7

Variable A

Variable B

Variable C

1.00

0.50

0.40

Variable B

0.50

1.00

0.70

Variable C

0.40

0.70

1.00

Variable A

Table 31.8

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 432

Correlation matrix 1 (communality italicised in each column)

Correlation matrix 1 but using communality estimates in the diagonal Variable A

Variable B

Variable C

Variable A

0.50

0.50

0.40

Variable B

0.50

0.70

0.70

Variable C

0.40

0.70

0.70

11/13/13 5:25 PM

31.4  Decisions, decisions, decisions 433

Table 31.9 Part of a factor loading matrix Factor 1

Factor 2

Variable A

0.50

0.70

Variable B

0.40

0.30

These first estimates can be a little rough and ready. Normally in factor analysis, following an initial stab using methods like this, better approximations are made by using the ‘significant’ factor loading matrix in order to ‘reconstruct’ the correlation matrix. For any pair of variables, the computer multiplies their two loadings on each factor, then sums the total. Thus if part of the factor loading matrix was as shown in Table 31.9, the correlation between variables A and B is (0.50 × 0.40) + (0.70 × 0.30) = 0.20 + 0.21 = 0.41. This is not normally the correlation between variables A and B found in the original data but one based on the previously estimated communality and the significant factors. However, following such a procedure for the entire correlation matrix does provide a slightly different value for each communality compared with our original estimate. These new communality estimates can be used as part of the factor analysis. The whole process can be repeated over and over again until the best possible estimate is achieved. This is usually referred to as a process of iteration – successive approximations to give the best estimate. Actually, as a beginner to factor analysis you should not worry too much about most of these things for the simple reason that you could adopt an off-the-peg package for factor analysis which, while not satisfying every researcher, will do the job pretty well until you get a little experience and greater sophistication.



■ Factor scores We often carry out a factor analysis to determine whether we can group a larger number of variables such as questionnaire items into a smaller set of ‘supervariables’ or factors. For example, we may have made up 10 questions to measure the way in which people express anxiety and a further 10 questions to assess how they exhibit depression. Suppose that the results of our factor analysis show that all or almost all of the 10 ques­ tions on anxiety load most highly on one of these factors and all or almost all of the 10 questions on depression load most highly on the other factor. This result would suggest that rather than analyse each of the 20 questions separately we could combine the answers to the 10 questions on anxiety to form one measure of anxiety and combine the answers to the 10 questions on depression to form a measure of depression. In other words, rather than have 20 different measures to analyse, we now have two measures. This greatly simplifies our analysis. The most common way of combining variables which are measured on the same scale is simply to add together the numbers which represent that scale. This is sometimes referred to as a summative scale. For example, if respondents only had to answer ‘Yes’ or ‘No’ to each of our 20 questions, then we could assign an answer which indicated the presence of either anxiety or depression a higher number than an answer which reflected the absence of either anxiety or depression. We could assign the number 2 to show the presence of either anxiety or depression and the number 1 to show the absence of either

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 433

11/13/13 5:25 PM

434

CHAPTER 31  Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data

anxiety or depression. Alternatively, we could assign the number 1 to indicate the pres­ ence of either anxiety or depression and the number 0 to the absence of either. We would then add together the numbers for the anxiety items to form a total or overall anxiety score and do the same for the depression items. If we had assigned the number 2 to indicate the presence of either anxiety or depression, then the total score for these two variables would vary between a minimum score of 10 and a maximum score of 20. Alternatively, if we had assigned the number 1 to reflect the presence of either anxiety or depression, then the total score for these two variables would vary between a minimum score of 0 and a maximum score of 10. Another way of assigning numbers to each of the variables or items that go to make up a factor is to use the factor score for each factor. There are various ways of producing factor scores and this is generally done with the computer program which carries out the factor analysis. A factor score may be based on all the items in the factor analysis. The items which load or correlate most highly on a factor are generally weighted the most heavily. So, for example, anxiety items which load or correlate most highly with the anxiety factor will make a larger contribution to the factor score for that factor. Factor scores may be positive or negative but will have a mean of zero. The main advantage of factor scores is that they are more closely related to the results of the factor analysis. In other words, scores represent these factors more accurately. Their disadvantage is that the results of a factor analysis of the same variables are likely to vary according to the method used and from sample to sample so that the way that the factor scores are derived is likely to vary. Unless we have access to the data, we will not know how the factor scores were calculated. One key thing to remember about factor scores is that they allow you to use the factors as if they were like any other variable. So they can be correlated with other variables, for example, or they might be used as the dependent variable in ANOVA.

31.5 Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis So far, we have presented factor analysis as a means of simplifying complex data matrices. In other words, factor analysis is being used to explore the structure (and, as a consequence, the meaning) of the data. This is clearly a very useful analytical tool. Of course, the danger is that the structure obtained through these essentially mathematical procedures is assumed to be the basis for a definitive interpretation of the data. This is problematic because of the inherent variability of most psychological measurements which suggest that the factors obtained in exploratory factor analysis may themselves be subject to variability. As a consequence, it has become increasingly common to question the extent to which exploratory factor analysis can be relied upon. One development from this is the notion of confirmatory factor analysis. Put as simply as possible, confirmatory factor analysis is a means of confirming that the factor structure obtained in exploratory factor analysis is robust and not merely the consequence of the whims of random variability in one’s data. Obviously it would be silly to take the data and re-do the factor analysis. That could only serve to check for computational errors. However, one could obtain a new set of data using more or less the same measures as in the original study. Then it is possible to factor analyse these data to test the extent to which the characteristics of the original factor analysis are reproduced in the fresh factor analysis of fresh data. In this way, it may be possible to confirm the original analysis. Box 31.2 contains more information about confirmatory factor analysis. Figure 31.1 gives the key steps in exploratory factor analysis.

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 434

11/13/13 5:25 PM

31.5  Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis 435

Figure 31.1

Box 31.2

Conceptual steps for exploratory factor analysis

Key concepts

Confirmatory versus exploratory factor analysis Most of this chapter discusses factor analysis as a means of exploring data. Probably this process is best regarded as a way of throwing up hypotheses about the nature of relationships between variables than definitive evidence that the underlying structure of the data is that indicated by the factors. There are a number of reasons why one should be careful about exploratory factor analyses such as the ones described in this chapter. One reason is that sometimes we have to interpret the factors on the basis of very limited information. Another reason is that the results of a factor analysis are somewhat dependent on the choice of method of factor analysis adopted. So when some authorities write of factor analysis as being a good hypothesis-generating tool rather than a good hypothesisconfirming tool, the reasons for caution become obvious as well as the reasons for the great popularity of factor analysis. It is probably going too far to describe explora­ tory factor analysis as ‘shotgun empiricism’ or ‘empiricism gone mad’. Anyone who has carried out an exploratory factor analysis will realise that identifying the nature of a factor is a somewhat creative act – and often based on relatively little information.

So why confirmatory factor analysis? The reasons are not to do with the inadequacies of the factor analysis methods described in this chapter. Factor analysis is gener­ ally regarded as a very powerful analytic technique. The problem lies more with the way in which it is employed rather than its computational procedures. Ideally, in research, knowledge and understanding should be built on previous research. Out of this previous research, ‘models’ or sets of variables are built up which effectively account for observed data. Frequently factor analysis is used simply to explore the data and to suggest the underlying nature of the rela­ tionships between variables. As a consequence, there is no model or hypothesis to test. It is at the stage at which there is a clear model or hypothesis that analyses can be used to properly test that model or hypothesis. So the reason why factor analysis cannot be used for model and hypothesis testing is that there is nothing to be tested. If there was a model or hypothesis available, then factor analysis could be used to test that model or hypothesis. This is a tradi­ tional approach which uses principal axes factor analysis. The researcher would include ‘indicator variables’ in the data to be factor analysed. These indicator variables would



M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 435

11/13/13 5:25 PM

436

CHAPTER 31  Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data

have predicted relationships with the factors. For example, if a factor is proposed to be ‘feminist attitudes’ an appro­ priate indicator variable for this might be gender as it might be a reasonable supposition that females would be more inclined towards feminist views. Gender would load heavily on the factor if the factor and its relationship with the indicator variable was as expected by the researcher. The modern approach is to use some sort of structural equation modelling procedure such as employed by the computer software LISREL, though there are others. The researcher must begin with a hypothesis about the rela­ tionships between variables and factors as well as which (if any) factors are interrelated with each other. The hypothesis is based on a reserve of theoretical and empir­ ical resources which have been built up from previous investigations in that research field. Typically the researcher will have an idea of how many different factors are required to account for the data which ultimately consist of a correlation matrix of relationships between variables.

The researcher will have hypotheses about what variables will correlate with which factors or which factors will correlate with each other. Of course, a number of different models will always be potentially viable for any given set of data. Hence the researcher will have more than a single model to compare. Models are specified by the research by fixing (or freeing) certain specific characteristics of the model. This could be the number of factors or the size of the correla­ tion between factors or any other aspect deemed appropriate. These various models are compared for their adequacy by assessing how well the different models may fit the data. The best-fitting model is, of course, the preferred model – though if there is any competition then the simplest (most parsimonious) model will be selected. Of course, there may be a better model that the researcher has not formu­ lated or tested. The fit of the models to the data is assessed by a number of statistics including the chi-square/degrees of freedom or a number of alternative statistics.

31.6 An example of factor analysis from the literature Butler (1995b) points out that children at school spend a lot of time looking at the work of their classmates. Although the evidence for this is clear, the reasons for their doing so are not researched. She decided to explore children’s motives for looking at the work of other children and proposed a four component model of the reasons they gave. Some children could be concerned mainly about learning to do the task and developing their skills and mastery of a particular type of task; other children might be more concerned with the quality of the product of their work. Furthermore, a child’s motivation might be to evaluate themselves (self-evaluation); on the other hand, their primary motivation might be in terms of evaluating the product of their work on the task. In other words, Butler proposed two dichotomies which might lead to a fourfold categorisation of moti­ vations for looking at other children’s work (Table 31.10). Based on this sort of reasoning, the researcher developed a questionnaire consisting of 32 items, ‘Why I looked at other children’s work’. Raters allocated a number of items to each of the above categories and the best eight items in each category were chosen for this questionnaire.

Table 31.10

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 436

Butler’s model of reasons to look at the work of others Product improvement

Self-improvement

Performance oriented

Doing better than others with little effort

Comparing task skills with those of others

Mastery oriented

Wanting to learn and improve

Checking whether own work needs improving

11/13/13 5:25 PM

31.6  An example of factor analysis from the literature 437

An example of a question from this questionnaire is: I wanted to see if my work is better or worse than others. The children’s answers had been coded from 1 to 5 according to their extent of agreement with the statements. Each child was given a page of empty circles on which they drew many pictures using these circles as far as possible. When this had been completed, they answered the ‘Why I looked at other children’s work’ questionnaire. The researcher’s task was then to establish whether her questionnaire actually consisted of the four independent ‘reasons’ for looking at the work of other children during the activity. An obvious approach to this questionnaire is to correlate the scores of the sample of children on the various items on the questionnaire. This produced a 32 × 32 correlation matrix which could be factor analysed to see whether the four categories of motives for looking at other children’s work actually emerged: Principal-components analysis1 with oblique rotation2 yielded five factors with eigen­ values greater than 1.03 which accounted for 62% of the variance4  .  .  .  Three factors corresponded to the mastery-oriented product improvement (MPI), performanceoriented product improvement (PPI), and performance-oriented self-evaluation (PSE) categories, but some items loaded high on more than one factor5. Items expected a priori to load on a mastery-oriented self-evaluation (MSE) category formed two factors. One (MSE) conformed to the original conceptualization, and the other (checking procedure [CP]) reflected concern with clarifying task demands and instructions. (Butler, 1995b, p. 350, superscripts added) The meaning of the superscripted passages is as follows: 1. Principal components analysis was the type of factor analysis employed – it means that communalities were not used. Otherwise the term ‘principal axes’ is used where communalities have been estimated. 2. Oblique rotation means that the factors may well correlate with each other. That is, if one correlates the factor loadings on each factor with the factor loadings on each of the other factors, a correlation matrix would be produced in which the correlations may differ from zero. Orthogonal rotation would have produced a correlation matrix of the factors in which the correlation coefficients are all zero. 3. This means that there are five factors which are potentially statistically significant – the minimum value of a potentially significant eigenvalue is 1.0 although this is only a minimum value and no guarantee of statistical significance. 4. These five factors explain 62% of the variance, apparently. That is, the sum of the squared factor loadings on these five factors is 62% of the squared correlation coefficients in the 32 × 32 correlation matrix. Doing this is problematic for oblique rotation as the factors are correlated which means that the variance of a factor is not specific to that factor. 5. In factor analysis, some items may load on more than one factor – this implies that they are measuring aspects of more than one factor. Table 31.11 gives an adapted version of the factor analysis table in which some items have been omitted for simplicity’s sake in the presentation. You will notice that many factor loadings are missing. This is because the researcher has chosen not to report low factor loadings on each factor. This has the advantage of simplifying the factor loading matrix by emphasising the stronger relationships. The disadvantage is that the reporting

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 437

11/13/13 5:25 PM

438

CHAPTER 31  Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data

Table 31.11

Butler’s factor loading matrix

Item: I wanted to see  .  .  .

Performanceoriented self-evaluation

Masteryoriented product improvement

Checking procedures

Performanceoriented product improvement

Mastery oriented selfevaluation

Who had the most ideas

0.61





–0.37



Whose work was best

0.74









If others had better ideas than me

0.68









Whether there were ideas I hadn’t thought of



0.68





0.34

Ideas which would help me develop my own ideas



0.68







If I’d understood what to do





0.85





Whether my drawings were appropriate





0.86





If I was working at the appropriate speed









0.63

How I was progressing on this new task

– 







0.70

I didn’t want to hand in poor work







0.67



I didn’t want my page to be emptier than others’







0.74



Factor loadings with absolute values less than 0.30 are not reported. Source: Table adapted from Butler (1995b).

of the analysis is incomplete and it is impossible for readers of the report to explore the data further. (If the original 32 × 32 correlation matrix had been included then it would be possible to reproduce the factor analysis and carry out variants on the original analysis.) The researcher has inserted titles for the factors in the matrix. Do not forget that these titles are arbitrary and are the researcher’s interpretation. Consequently, you may wish to consider the extent to which her titles are adequate. The way to do this is to examine the set of questions which load highly on each of the factors to see whether a radically different interpretation is possible. Having done this you may feel that Butler’s inter­ pretations are reasonable. Butler’s difficulty is that she has five factors when her model would predict only four. While this means that she is to a degree wrong, her model is substantially correct because the four factors she predicted appear to be present in the factor analysis. The problem is that some of the questionnaire items do not appear to measure what she suggested they should measure. Some researchers might be tempted to re-do the factor analysis with just four factors. The reason for this is that the proper number of factors to extract in factor analysis is not clear-cut. Because Butler used a minimal cut-off point for significant factors (eigen­ values of 1.0 and above), she may have included more factors than she needed. It would strengthen Butler’s argument if such a re-analysis found that four factors reproduced Butler’s model better. However, we should stress that factor analysis does not lead to hard-and-fast solutions and that Butler would be better confirming her claims by the analysis of a fresh study using the questionnaire.

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 438

11/13/13 5:25 PM

31.7  Reporting the results 439

31.7 Reporting the results There is no standard way of reporting the results of a factor analysis which will suffice irrespective of circumstances. However, it is essential to report the type of factor analysis, the type of rotation, how the number of factors was determined, and the rela­ tive importance of the factors in terms of variance explained or eigenvalues. Although the original author’s description is given above, the following is another way of writing much the same: A principal components factor analysis was conducted on the correlation matrix of the 36 items on the ‘Why I looked at other children’s work’ questionnaire. Five factors were extracted which accounted for 62% of the variance overall. Three of these factors corresponded to components of the proposed model. Oblique rotation of the factors was employed which yielded the factor structure given in Table 31.11. One factor was identified as mastery-oriented product improvement (MPI), another was performance-oriented product improvement (PPI) and a third was performanceoriented self-evaluation (PSE). These are as the model predicted. The fourth category predicted by the model (mastery-oriented self-evaluation (MSE)) was also identified but some of the items expected to load on this actually formed the fifth factor checking procedures. Notice that some aspects of this description would be fairly general to any factor analysis, but there are other aspects which are idiosyncratic in nature and due to the distinctive characteristics and purposes of this particular study. Ideally, you should study reports of factor analyses which are similar to yours (coming from the same area of research) for more precise examples of how your work could be reported.

Research examples Factor analysis Gibbs and Powell (2012) studied the beliefs of teachers in the efficacy of the teaching skills in dealing with children’s classroom behaviour as well as the question of whether the use of exclusion as a sanction was associated with this. Over 200 primary and nursery school teachers in the UK completed questionnaires assessing their efficacy beliefs. They used principal components factor analysis on the efficacy belief items together with a scree test to estimate the proper number of factors and were guided too by previous research findings. Promax rotation to simple structure was also applied. Three factors accounted for the teachers individual efficiency beliefs. These were labelled a) classroom management, b) children’s engagement and c) instructional strategies. For individual efficacy beliefs, none of the factors was associated with school exclusions. However, an analysis of collective efficacy beliefs showed some evidence of an association with exclusion. Motes, Hubbard, Courtney and Rypma (2008) discuss the research on spatial memory for moving targets. This seems to suggest that this ability is frequently based on the implied direction of momentum of the target and implied gravity. Implied gravity is illustrated by the fact that after viewing a drawing of a flowerpot on a table and then viewing a flowerpot without support then the position of the flowerpot is often judged to be



M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 439

11/13/13 5:25 PM

CHAPTER 31  Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data

440

lower than it actually it is – i.e. a shift in the direction of gravity. Similar effects are created by downwardly or horizontally moving targets. They set up a situation in which participants viewed targets moving horizontally in a left–right direction and then, finally disappearing. Alternatively, as a control, they were briefly shown a stationary target. Both targets disappeared. The participants in the research were then asked to show the point at which the target disappeared. The vertical (gravity) error was measured and could be negative or positive according to whether it was in the direction of gravity. The horizontal (momentum) error was measured and could be negative or positive depending on whether the error was in the direction of momentum or not. The misplacements in the location identified were subjected to a principal components factor analysis in which rows were the participants and the columns were the horizontal and vertical displacement for each target activity. Overall, the analysis indicated that two underlying dimensions account for this variability. That is, the expected implied gravity and implied direction of momentum. Pechey and Halligan (2011) studied anomalous experiences such as hearing voices when there was nobody around. They occur in psychiatric conditions and in non-patients also. The researchers studied the distribution and relationships of self-reported anomalous experiences in a sample of 1000 UK non-clinical participants. Nearly half of the sample of the general population reported that anomalous experiences occurred sometimes or often. In order to know whether there were common underlying factors to delusional beliefs, the researchers carried out exploratory factor analysis. As an indication of the stability of the factor structure they analysed two halves of the sample separately. Principal components factor analysis was carried out. The Kaiser test which counts factors with an eigenvalue of 1.00 or more suggested two factors but a scree test indicated just one factor. So a single component solution was adopted which accounted for about a third or more of the variance explained. The experiences which loaded most highly on this single factor included 1) seen or sensed a ghost, 2) sensed when a friend or family member was in trouble, 3) seen things which other people cannot, and 4) felt that familiar objects appeared different even though you knew they hadn’t changed. These had factor loadings of about .6 or greater.

Key points ● Do

not be afraid to try out factor analysis on your data. It is not difficult to do if you are familiar with using simpler techniques on a computer.

● Do

not panic when faced with output from a factor analysis. It can be very lengthy and confusing because it contains things that mere mortals simply do not want to know. Usually the crucial aspects of the factor analysis are to be found towards the end of the output. If in doubt, do not hesitate to contact your local expert – computer output is not always user friendly.

● Take

the factor analysis slowly – it takes a while to build your skills sufficiently to be totally confident.

● Do

not forget that interpreting the factors can be fairly subjective – you might not always see things as other people do and it might not be you who is wrong.



Factor analysis can be applied only to correlations calculated using the Pearson correlation formula.

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 440

11/13/13 5:25 PM

Computer analysis 441



Computer analysis Exploratory factor analysis using SPSS

Figure 31.2

SPSS Statistics steps for exploratory factor analysis

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

Factor analysis produces a lot of output on SPSS and we can only present a small amount. It is important to make sure that you obtain the ‘right’ number of factors which you do using the Scree Plot. Where the curve flattens then the factors are not significant. The interpretation of the factors is based on an examination of which variables correlate with the factor (what do these have in common?) and to a lesser extent those which do not correlate with the factor.

● You might write: ‘The variables were subjected to a principal components analysis and rotated using the

Varimax method. Two factors met the requirements of the Scree Test and these seemed to be a factor on which sensory motor skills loaded highly and another factor on which verbal skills loaded highly.’

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 441

11/13/13 5:25 PM

442

CHAPTER 31  Factor analysis: Simplifying complex data

Screenshot 31.1 The data

Screenshot 31.2

Screenshot 31.3

Screenshot 31.4 The factor loading output

Select variables for inclusion

Select the test

Screenshot 31.5 The scree plot

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 442

11/13/13 5:25 PM

Recommended further reading 443



Recommended further reading Bryman, A., & Cramer, D. (2011). Quantitative data analysis with IBM SPSS 17, 18 and 19: A guide for social scientists (Chapter 11). London: Routledge. Child, D. (1970). The essentials of factor analysis. London: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Kline, P. (1994). An easy guide to factor analysis. London: Routledge. Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed., Chapter 13). New York: Allyn & Bacon.

M31_HOWI0749_06_SE_C31.indd 443

11/13/13 5:25 PM

Chapter 32

Multiple regression and multiple correlation

Overview ●

So far we have studied regression and correlation in which just two variables are used – variable X and variable Y. We can consider variable X the independent or predictor variable and variable Y the dependent or criterion variable.

● The terms independent and dependent variable do not imply a causal relationship between

the two variables. ● Multiple

regression and correlation are extensions of the two variable regressions to include several different X variables (X1, X2, X3,  .  .  .). Only one Y variable is involved. If we wish to relate how well a student does in an examination, we may wish to correlate examination performance with intelligence. This would be simple or bivariate correlation (or regression). If we add in an additional predictor variable – amount of preparation – then we would expect higher correlations with examination performance.



Multiple regression and correlation basically indicate the best predictor of the Y variable, then the next best predictor (correlate) and so forth. They indicate how much weight to give to each predictor to yield the best prediction or correlation.

● There

are many versions of multiple regression which are appropriate in different circumstances and which work in slightly different ways.



Usually there are two versions of multiple regression. One works with the original scores and yields unstandardised regression or b-weights. Another version works with the scores turned into z-scores. This yields standardised regression or beta ( β ) weights which are essentially correlation coefficients. The advantage of beta weights is that they are standardised values and so independent of the variance of the original variables. This means that they can be compared directly.

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 444

11/13/13 5:25 PM

32.2  Theoretical considerations 445

Preparation Revise Chapter 9 on simple regression and the standard error in relation to regression. You should also be aware of standard scores from Chapter 6 and the coefficient of determination for the correlation coefficient in Chapter 8. Optimal understanding of this chapter is aided if you have insight into the basic concepts of partial correlation and zero-order correlation described in Chapter 30.

32.1 Introduction Traditionally, psychologists have assumed that the primary purpose of research is to isolate the influence of one variable on another. So researchers might examine whether paternal absence from the family during childhood leads to poor mathematical skills in children. The fundamental difficulty with this is that other variables which might influence a child’s mathematical skills are ignored. In real life, away from the psychology laboratory, variables do not act independently of each other. An alternative approach is to explore the complex pattern of variables which may relate to mathematical skills. Numerous factors may be involved in mathematical ability including maternal educational level, the quality of mathematical teaching at school, the child’s general level of intel­ligence or IQ, whether or not the child went to nursery school, the gender of the child and so forth. We rarely know all the factors which might be related to important variables such as mathe­ matical skills before we begin research; so we will tend to include some variables in our studies which turn out to be poor predictors of the criterion. Multiple regression quite simply helps us choose empirically the most effective set of predictors for any criterion. Multiple regression can be carried out with scores or standardised scores (z-scores). Standardised multiple regression has the advantage of making the regression values directly analogous to correlation coefficients. The consequence of this is that it is easy to make dir­ ect comparisons between the influence of different variables. In unstandardised multiple regres­ sion the variables are left in their original form. Standardised and unstand­ardised multiple regression are usually done simultaneously by computer programs including SPSS.

32.2 Theoretical considerations The techniques described in this chapter concern linear multiple regression which assumes that the relationships between variables fall approximately on a straight line. Multiple regression is an extension of simple (or bivariate) regression (Chapter 9). In simple regression, a single dependent variable (or criterion variable) is related to a single independent variable (or predictor variable). For example, marital satisfaction may be regressed against the degree to which the partners have similar personalities. In other words, can marital satisfaction be predicted from the degree of personality similarity between partners? In multiple regression, on the other hand, the criterion is regressed against several potential predictors. For example, to what extent is marital satisfaction related to various factors such as socio-economic status of both partners, similarity in

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 445

11/13/13 5:25 PM

446

CHAPTER 32  Multiple regression and multiple correlation

socio-economic status, religious affiliation, similarity in religious affiliation, duration of courtship, age of partners at marriage and so on? Of course, personality similarity might be included in the list of predictors studied. Multiple regression serves two main functions: 1. To determine the minimum number of predictors needed to predict a criterion. Some of the predictors which are significantly related to the criterion may also be correlated with each other and so may not all be necessary to predict the criterion. Say, for example, that the two predictors of attraction to one’s spouse and commitment to one’s marriage both correlate highly with each other and that both these variables were positively related to the criterion of marital satisfaction (although marital commitment is more strongly related to marital satisfaction than is attraction to the spouse). If most of the variation between marital satisfaction and attraction to the spouse was also shared with marital commitment, then marital commitment alone may be sufficient to predict marital satisfaction. Another example of this would be the industrial psychologist who wished to use psychological tests to select the best appli­ cants for a job. Obviously a lot of time and money could be saved if redundant or very overlapping tests could be weeded out, leaving just a minimum number of tests which predict worker quality. 2. To explore whether certain predictors remain significantly related to the criterion when other variables are controlled or held constant. For example, marital commit­ ment might be partly a function of religious belief so that those who are more reli­ gious may be more satisfied with their marriage. We may be interested in determining whether marital commitment is still significantly related to marital satisfaction when strength of religious belief is controlled. When trying to understand multiple regression, it is useful to remember the main fea­ tures of simple regression. These are listed below as a quick summary of what you need to know already so that you can study this chapter effectively. Generally speaking, mul­ tiple regression as dealt with in this chapter is a relatively straightforward extension of simple regression:

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 446



Simple regression can be represented by the scatterplot in Figure 32.1 in which values of the criterion are arranged along the vertical axis and values of the predictor are arranged along the horizontal axis. For example, marital satisfaction may be the cri­ terion and personality similarity the predictor. Each point on the scatterplot indicates the position of the criterion and predictor scores for a particular individual in the sample. The relationship between the criterion and the predictor is shown by the slope of the straight line through the points on the scattergram. This best-fitting straight line is the one which minimises the sum of the (squared) distances between the points and their position on the line. This slope is known as the regression line or the line of best fit and the slope of this line is given by the regression coefficient.



The intercept constant is the point at which the regression line intersects or cuts the vertical axis, in other words, the value on the vertical axis when the value on the horizontal axis is zero. Confusingly, in multiple regression this is sometimes referred to as the coefficient of the intercept. It is a constant and so is not variable.



To determine the predicted score of the criterion from a particular score of the predic­ tor, we draw a line parallel to the vertical axis from the score on the horizontal axis to the regression line. From here we draw a second line parallel to the horizontal axis to the vertical axis, which gives us the predicted score of the criterion. More precisely, we can use the regression weights to make our prediction. In this, we simply multiply the regression weight by the score that we are interested in on the independent vari­ able and add the regression weight (i.e. cut point) for the intercept. This gives us our predicted score.

11/13/13 5:25 PM

32.2  Theoretical considerations 447

Figure 32.1 A simple scatterplot



Unless there is a perfect relationship between the predictor and the criterion, the predicted score of the criterion will usually differ from the actual score for a particular case.

● Unlike

the correlation coefficient, regression is dependent on the variability of the units of measurement involved. This makes regressions on different samples and dif­ ferent variables very difficult to compare. However, we can standardise the scores on the predictor and the criterion variables. By expressing them as standard scores (i.e. z-scores), each variable will have a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1. Further­ more, the intercept or intercept constant will always be 0 in these circumstances.



■ Regression equations Simple regression is usually expressed in terms of the following regression equation as we have already mentioned in the brief notes on simple regression: Y = a + bX predicted score on regression coefficient criterion variable intercept constant × predictor score



In other words, to predict a particular criterion score, we multiply the particular score of the predictor by the regression coefficient and add to it the intercept constant. Note that the values of the intercept constant and the regression coefficient remain the same for the equation, so the equation can be seen as describing the relationship between the criterion and the predictor. When the scores of the criterion and the predictor are standardised to z-scores, the regres­ sion coefficient is the same as Pearson’s correlation coefficient and ranges from +1.00 through 0.00 to −1.00. Regression weights standardised in this way are known as beta weights. In multiple regression, the regression equation is the same except that there are several predictors and each predictor has its own (partial) regression coefficient (Figure 32.2): Y = a + b1X1 + b2X2 + b3X3 +  .  .  . A partial regression coefficient expresses the relationship between a particular predictor and the criterion controlling for, or partialling out, the relationship between that pre­ dictor and all the other predictors in the equation. This ensures that each predictor variable provides an independent contribution to the prediction.

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 447

11/13/13 5:25 PM

448

CHAPTER 32  Multiple regression and multiple correlation

Figure 32.2

Simple regression and multiple regression formula

The relationship between the criterion and the predictors is often described in terms of the percentage of variance of the criterion that is explained or accounted for by the predictors. (This is much like the coefficient of determination for the correlation coeffi­ cient.) One way of illustrating what the partial regression coefficient means is through a Venn diagram (Figure 32.3) involving the criterion Y and the two predictors X1 and X2. Each of the circles signifies the amount of variance of one of the three variables. The area shaded in Figure 32.3a is common only to X1 and Y, and represents the variance of Y that it shares with variable X1. The shaded area in Figure 32.3b is shared only by X2 and

Figure 32.3

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 448

Venn diagrams illustrating partial regression coefficients

11/13/13 5:25 PM

32.2  Theoretical considerations 449

Y, and signifies the amount of variance of Y that it shares with variable X2. Often a phrase such as ‘the amount of variance explained by variable X’ is used instead of ‘the amount of variance shared by variable X’. Both terms signify the amount of overlapping variance.

Box 32.1

Focus on

Standardised or unstandardised regression weights Regression can involve the raw scores or standard scores. Computers will usually print out both sorts. ●

Regression involving ‘standard scores’ gives regression coefficients (weights) which can more readily be com­ pared in terms of their size since they range between +1.0 and −1.0 like simple correlation coefficients (i.e. Pearson correlation). In other words, the predictor variables are comparable irrespective of the units of measurement on which they were originally based. This is just like any other standard scores (Chapter 6). The regression weights for this are usually called beta (β ).





Regression involving ‘non-standardised scores’ or raw scores is about the ‘nuts and bolts’ of prediction. The unstandardised regression coefficient (weight) can take, theoretically, any positive or negative value. Like our account of simple regression, it provides predicted numerical values for the criterion variable based on an individual’s scores on the various predictor variables. However, the size of the regression coefficient (weight) is no indication of the importance of the unstandard­ ised predictor since the size is dependent on the units of measurement involved. The unstandardised regression weight is usually given the symbol b.

■ Selection Since multiple regression is particularly useful with a large number of predictors, such an analysis potentially would involve many regression equations. That is to say, one might stipulate a wide variety of different ‘models’ to examine in the multiple regression. Obviously the complexity of the analysis could be awesome. In practice, however, a researcher does not need to consider every possible regression equation when carrying out multiple regression. This involves deciding the broad analysis strategy for the multi­ ple regression and stipulating this as part of the analysis and when running multiple regression on a computer package. A number of different approaches have been sug­ gested for selecting and testing predictors. These approaches include hierarchical (or blockwise) selection and stepwise selection. Hierarchical selection enters predictors into the regression equation on some practical or theoretical consideration. Stepwise selection employs statistical criteria to choose the smallest set of predictors which best predict the variation in the criterion. In contrast to these methods, entering all predictors into the regression equation is known as standard or simultaneous multiple regression. Finally, setwise regression compares all possible sets of predictors such as all predictors singly, in pairs, in trios and so on until the best set of predictors is identified. ●

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 449

Hierarchical selection  Predictors are entered singly or in blocks according to some practical or theoretical rationale. For example, potentially confounding variables such as socio-demographic factors may be statistically controlled by entering them first into the regression equation. Alternatively, similar variables may be grouped (or ‘blocked’) together and entered as a block, such as a block of personality variables, a block of attitude variables and so on. The computer tells us the net influence of each block in turn.

11/13/13 5:25 PM

450

CHAPTER 32  Multiple regression and multiple correlation ●

Stepwise selection The predictor with the highest zero-order correlation is entered first into the regression equation if it explains a significant proportion of the variance of the criterion. The second predictor to be considered for entry is that which has the highest partial correlation with the criterion. If it explains a significant proportion of the variance of the criterion, it is entered into the equation. At this point, the predictor which was entered first is examined to see if it still explains a significant proportion of the variance of the criterion. If it no longer does so, it is dropped from the equation. The analysis continues with the predictor which has the next highest partial correla­ tion with the criterion. The process stops when no more predictors are entered into or removed from the equation.

Box 32.2 gives an overview of some of the possibilities for multiple regression analyses. Figure 32.4 shows the key steps in a multiple regression.

Figure 32.4

Conceptual steps for a multiple regression

Box 32.2

Focus on

Different approaches to multiple regression Among the choices of methods for multiple regression are the following: ● Single-stage

entry of all predictors and all predictors are employed whether or not they are likely to be good predictors (i.e. irrespective of their potential predictive power).



Blocks: There are circumstances in which the researcher does not wish to enter all of the variables at the same time. Instead, it is possible to enter the predictors in

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 450

sets, one set at a time. These are sets specified by the researcher and are usually called blocks. There can be any number of variables in a block from a minimum of one. There are a number of advantages to this. Putting variables into blocks allows the variables in the block to be analysed together, either before or after other variables. One might put variables into blocks because they are similar in some way. For instance, they may be a particular type of variable (e.g. health variables, edu­ cation variables, social class variables could all form

11/13/13 5:25 PM

32.3  Stepwise multiple regression example 451

separate blocks). Another use is to ‘control’ for certain variables first – that is, age and social class may be entered as the first block. This is often done as a way of controlling for the influence of demographic variables. If the first block included demographic variables such as gender, age and social class, this is the equivalent of partialling them out of the analysis (see Chapter 30). Once this is done, one can compare the outcome of this block with what happens when other predictors are introduced. This is known as hierarchical multiple regression. ●

Finding best predictors: The analysis may proceed on a stepwise basis by finding the best predictors in a set of predictors and eliminating the poor predictors. This is particularly appropriate where the main objective of the researcher is to predict with the highest possible accuracy – rather than to find explanatory models of influences on the dependent variable.



Reverse (backwards) elimination of predictors: In this the first model is initially employed. That is, the model in our earlier example is calculated. All of the predictor variables are included. Having done that, the worst predictor is dropped. Usually this is the least significant predictor. Essentially the model is recalculated on the basis of the remaining predictors. Then the remaining worst predictor is dropped and again the model recal­ culated. The researcher is looking to see whether drop­ ping a variable or variables actually substantially worsens the model. This is not simply a matter of the goodnessof-fit of the model to the data; some models may be

better at predicting one value of the dependent variable rather than the other. If one is trying to avoid letting men out of prison early if they are likely to re-offend, the model which maximises the number of recidivists (re-offenders) correctly identified may be preferred over the model which misclassifies recidivists as likely to be non-recidivists. This is obviously a complex judgement based on a wide variety of considerations. ● There

are models which mix blocks and stepwise approaches.

● In

this chapter, we largely deal with individual pre­ dictors acting alone or their combined effects on the dependent variable. Following the General Linear Model (see Box 9.1), this essentially means that the analysis adds a standard amount to the prediction of the dependent variable for each increment in each of the predictor (independent) variables. In other words, the effects of the independent variables are additive. It is, however, possible to deal with interactions between predictors in multiple regression just as we do in analy­ sis of variance (ANOVA). This is discussed in detail in Chapter 39 which deals with moderator variables.

Except for the simple case where the maximum possible accuracy of prediction is required and all variables may be entered en masse, the choice of approach is a matter of judge­ ment that partly comes with experience and practice. It does no harm to try out a variety of approaches on one’s data, especially if one is inexperienced with the techniques. Of course, one has to be able to justify the final choice of model.

32.3 Stepwise multiple regression example Since we will need to use standard multiple regression to carry out path analysis in the next chapter, we will illustrate stepwise multiple regression in the present chapter. Our example asks whether a person’s educational achievement (the criterion variable) can be predicted from their intellectual ability, their motivation to do well in school and their parents’ interest in their education (the predictor variables). The minimum information we need to carry out a multiple regression is the number of people in the sample and the correlations between all the variables, though you would normally work with the actual scores when carrying out a multiple regression. It has been suggested that with stepwise regression it is desirable to have 40 times more cases than predictors. Since we have three predictors, we will say that we have a sample of 120 cases. (However, much reported research fails to follow this rule of thumb.) In order to interpret the results of multiple regression it is usually necessary to have more information than this, but for our pur­ poses the fictitious correlation matrix presented in Table 32.1 should be sufficient.

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 451

11/13/13 5:25 PM

452

CHAPTER 32  Multiple regression and multiple correlation

The calculation of multiple regression with more than two predictors is complicated and so will not be shown. However, the basic results of a stepwise multiple regression analysis are given in Table 32.2. What this simple example shows is that only two of the three ‘predictors’ actually explain a significant percentage of variance in educational achievement. That they are significant is assessed using a t-test. The values of t are given in Table 32.2 along with their two-tailed significance levels. A significance level of 0.05 or less is regarded as statistically significant. The two significant predictor variables are intellectual ability and school motivation. The first variable to be considered for entry into the regression equation is the one with the highest zero-order correlation with educational achievement. This variable is intellectual ability. The proportion of variance in educational achievement explained or predicted by intellectual ability is the square of its correlation with educational achievement which is 0.49 (0.72 = 0.49). The next predictor to be considered for entry into the regression equa­ tion is the variable which has the highest partial correlation with the criterion (after the variance due to the first predictor variable has been removed). These partial correlations have not been presented; however, school motivation is the predictor variable with the highest partial correlation with the criterion variable educational achievement. The two predictors together explain 0.52 of the variance of educational achievement. The figure of the total proportion of variance explained is arrived at by squaring the overall R (the multiple correlation) which is 0.722 or 0.52. The multiple correlation is likely to be bigger the smaller the sample and for more predictors. Consequently, this figure is usually adjusted for the size of the sample and the number of predictors, which reduces it in size somewhat. Finally, the partial regression or beta coefficients for the regression equation containing the two predictors are also shown in Table 32.2 and are 0.65 for intellectual ability and 0.16 for school motivation. There is also a constant (usually denoted as a) which is −0.17 in this instance. The constant is the equivalent to the cut-point described in Chapter 9. We can write this regression equation as follows: Educational achievement = a + (0.83 × intellectual ability) + (0.17 × school motivation) According to our fictitious example, intellectual ability is more important than school motivation in predicting educational achievement.

Table 32.1

Correlation matrix for a criterion (educational achievement) and three predictors Educational achievement

Intellectual ability

Intellectual ability

0.70

School motivation

0.37

0.32

Parental interest

0.13

0.11

Table 32.2

School motivation

0.34

Some regression results – significant predictors only r

b

Beta β

t

Significance

Intellectual ability

0.70

0.83

0.65

9.56

0.001

School motivation

0.37

0.17

0.16

2.42

0.02

Predictor variables

Constant = −0.17, R2 = 0.52, Adjusted R2 = 0.51, R = 0.72

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 452

11/13/13 5:25 PM

32.3  Stepwise multiple regression example 453

Box 32.3

Key concepts

Multicollinearity There is a concept, multicollinearity, which needs consid­ eration when planning a multiple regression analysis. This merely refers to a situation in which several of the predic­ tor variables correlate with each other very highly. This results in difficulties because small sampling fluctuations may result in a particular variable appearing to be a powerful predictor while other variables may appear to be relatively weak predictors. So variables A and B, both of which predict the criterion, may correlate with each other at, say, 0.9. However, because variable A, say, has a minutely better correlation with the criterion it is selected first by the computer. Variable B then appears to be a far less good predictor. When the intercorrelations of your

Box 32.4

predictor variables are very high, perhaps above 0.8 or so, then the dangers of multicollinearity are also high. In terms of research design, it is a well-known phenomenon that if you measure several different variables using the same type of method then there is a tendency for the vari­ ables to intercorrelate simply because of that fact. So, if all of your measures are based on self-completion question­ naires or on ratings by observers then you may find strong intercorrelations simply because of this. Quite clearly, care should be exercised to ensure that your predictor measures do not intercorrelate highly. If multicollinearity is appar­ ent then be very careful about claiming that one of the predictors is far better than another.

Focus on

Prediction in multiple regression Prediction in regression is often not prediction at all. This can cause some confusion. In everyday language, predic­ tion is indicating what will happen in the future on the basis of some sign in the present. Researchers, however, often use regression analysis with no intention of predict­ ing future events. Instead, they collect data on the relation between a set of variables (let’s call them X1, X2 and X3) and another variable (called Y). They think that the X variables may be correlated with Y. The data on all of these variables are available to the researcher. The analysis proceeds essentially by calculating the overall correlation of the several X variables with the Y variable. The overall correlation of a set of variables with another single vari­ able is called multiple correlation. If there is a multiple correlation between the variables then this means that we

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 453

can use the value of this correlation together with other information to estimate the value of the Y variable from a pattern of X variables. Since the multiple correlation is rarely a perfect correlation, then our estimate of Y is bound to be a little inaccurate. Explained this way, we have not used the concept of prediction. If we know the multiple correlation between variables based on a particu­ lar sample of participants, we can use the size of the cor­ relation to estimate the value of Y for other individuals based on knowing their pattern of scores on the X vari­ ables. That is the task of multiple regression. Prediction in multiple regression, then, is really estimating the unknown value of Y for an individual who was not part of the original research sample from that individual’s known pattern of scores on the X variables.

11/13/13 5:25 PM

454

CHAPTER 32  Multiple regression and multiple correlation

32.4 Reporting the results Multiple regression can be performed in a variety of ways for a variety of purposes. Consequently, there is no standard way of presenting results from a multiple regression analysis. However, there are some things which are best routinely mentioned. In particu­ lar, the reader needs to know the variables on which the analysis was conducted, the particular form of the multiple regression used, regression weights and the main pattern of predictors. Other information may be added as appropriate. By all means consult journal articles in your field of study for other indications as to style. We would say the following when reporting the simple example in Section 32.3: A stepwise multiple regression was carried out in order to investigate the best pattern of variables for predicting educational achievement. Intellectual ability was selected for entry into the analysis first and explained 49% of the variance in educational achievement. School motivation was entered second and together with intellectual ability explained 52% of the variance in educational achievement. Greater educa­ tional attainment was associated with greater intellectual ability and school motiva­ tion. A third variable, parental interest, was not included in the analysis as it was not a significant, independent predictor of educational achievement.

32.5 An example from the published literature Munford (1994) examined the predictors of depression in African-Americans. The research involved her administering the following measures: 1. The Beck Depression Inventory. 2. The Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale. 3. The Hollingshead two-factor index of social position – this is a measure of the occu­ pational social class and educational standards (i.e. a measure of social class). 4. The gender (self-reported sex) of the individual. 5. The Racial Identity Attitude Scale which measures several different stages in the development of racial identity: a) Pre-encounter: the stage before black people become exposed to racism. It is the stage at which they accept the definitions of themselves imposed by the white racist community b) Encounter: the stage where identity is challenged by direct experiences of racism c) Immersion: the individual is learning to value his or her own race and culture d) Internalisation: the individual has achieved a mature and secure sense of his or her own race and identity. As one might expect, Munford was interested in the relationship between depression as measured by the Beck Depression Inventory (the criterion variable) and the remaining variables (the predictor variables). She computed a correlation matrix between all of the variables, but as this involved 28 different correlation coefficients it is obvious that she needed a means of simplifying its complexity. She subjected her correlation matrix to a stepwise regression which yielded the outcome shown in Table 32.3.

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 454

11/13/13 5:25 PM

32.5  An example from the published literature 455

Table 32.3

Summary of stepwise multiple regression: self-esteem, gender, social class and racial identity attitudes as predictors of depression R2 increments

R2 (adjusted) total

Beta F

Self-esteem

0.37

0.37

134.10

Pre-encounter

0.02

0.39

8.97

Predictor

Encounter

0.01

0.41

4.71

Gender

0.01

0.42

4.77

Source: Adapted from Munford, 1994.

As you can see, many of the predictors are not included in the table, indicating that they were not significant independent predictors of depression (thus social class and internalisation, for example, are excluded). Self-esteem is the best predictor of depression – those with the higher self-esteem tended to have lower depression scores. One cannot tell this directly from the table as it presents squared values which would have lost any negative signs. We have to assess the direction of the relationship from the sign of the regression coefficient. This sign is negative. Although pre-encounter, encounter and gender all contribute something to the predic­ tion, the increment in the amount of variation explained is quite small for each of them. Thus R2 for pre-encounter is only 0.02 which means (expressed as a percentage) that the increase in variation explained is only 2% (i.e. 0.02 × 100%). Beta F in essence reports F-ratios (Chapter 21) for each of the predictor variables. All of those presented are statistically significant since otherwise the variable in question would not correlate significantly with depression.

Research examples Multiple regression Ang and Huan (2006) tested whether depression mediated the relation between academic stress and thoughts of killing oneself (suicidal ideation) in adolescents. Academic stress was significantly correlated with both depression and suicidal ideation. To determine whether depression mediated the relation between academic stress and suicidal ideation, they regressed suicidal ideation on both depression and academic stress. The standardised partial regression coefficient between academic stress and suicidal ideation was smaller than the correlation between them but was still significant which suggested that depression was a partial rather than a complete mediator of the relation between them. Childs and Klimoski (1986) carried out a standard multiple regression to determine whether a biographical data inventory given to students on real-estate courses would predict their career success two years later. Career success was measured in terms of a composite index of earnings, job prestige and career identification. Twenty-four per cent of the variance in career success was explained by the five factors of the biographical data inventory. These factors were educational achievement, social orientation, interpersonal confidence, economic stability and work ethic orientation.

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 455

11/13/13 5:25 PM

456

CHAPTER 32  Multiple regression and multiple correlation

Lounsbury and his colleagues (2003) conducted a hierarchical multiple regression to determine whether five personality factors and work drive would predict the grades students obtained on a course once intelligence had been taken into account. In the first analysis they present, intelligence was entered in the first step of the regression, the five personality variables were entered in the second step, and work drive was entered in the third step. Intelligence accounted for a significant 16% of the variance in course grades. The five personality variables accounted for a significant additional 7% of the variance and work drive a significant further 4%. As they found work drive to explain a significant percentage of the variance in course grades, they checked to see whether the five personality variables would explain a significant amount of the variance if they were entered after work drive which they did not. When work drive was entered in the second step, it explained a significant 8% of the variance with the five personality variables explaining a non-significant further 3%. Nicholas and his colleagues (2009) were interested in which pain variables were related to depression in patients with chronic pain once age, gender and pain duration had been controlled. After entering these three variables in the first step of the regression to control for them, they carried out a forward entry multiple regression in which variables were selected in terms of their statistical significance. The first three variables of age, gender and pain duration explained a significant 5% of the variance in depression. The first variable with the highest statistical significance which was statistically significant was catastrophising which is a tendency for patients to despair about their pain. This variable explained a significant further 39% of the variance in depression. There were four other variables which explained further significant amounts of variance and there were three which did not.

Key points ●

Multiple regression is only practicable in most cases using a computer since the computations are numerous.

● Normally

one does not have to compute the correlation matrix independently between variables. The computer program usually does this on the raw scores. There may be a facility for entering correlation matrices which might be useful once in a while when you are reanalysing someone else’s correlation matrix.



Choose hierarchical selection for your multiple regression if you are trying to test theoretical predictions or if you have some other rationale. One advantage of this is that you can first of all control for any social or demographic variables (gender, social class, etc.) which might influence your results. Then you can choose your remaining predictors in any order which you think best meets your needs.



Choose stepwise selection methods in circumstances in which you simply wish to choose the best and smallest set of predictors. This would be ideal in circumstances in which you wish to dispense with timeconsuming (and expensive) psychological tests, say in an industrial setting involving personnel selection. The main considerations here are entirely practical.

● Avoid

construing the results of multiple regression in cause and effect terms.

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 456

11/13/13 5:25 PM

Computer analysis 457



Computer analysis Stepwise multiple regression using SPSS

Figure 32.5

SPSS Statistics steps for stepwise multiple regression

Interpreting and reporting the output ● The

most important part of the output is the Coefficients table. This has produced a two predictor model involving Ability and Motivation as the important predictors. The B weights are both positive and so indicate positive relationships. Both are statistically significant. The Beta weights are standardised versions of the B weights.



You might write: ‘The data were subjected to a stepwise multiple regression analysis in order to ascertain what were the best predictors of school achievement. A two variable model was indicated in which Ability was found to have a B weight of .83 and motivation a B weight of .17. Intellectual ability was entered first and explained 49 per cent of the variance in educational achievement, F(1, 118) = 113.76, p = .001. School motivation was entered second and explained a further 2 per cent, F(1, 117) = 5.85, p = .017. Greater educational attainment was associated with greater intellectual ability and school motivation.’

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 457

11/13/13 5:25 PM

458

CHAPTER 32  Multiple regression and multiple correlation

Screenshot 32.1 The data

Screenshot 32.2

Select the test

Screenshot 32.3

Screenshot 32.4

Select options

Select variables

Screenshot 32.5 The most important output

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 458

11/13/13 5:25 PM

Recommended further reading 459



Recommended further reading Cramer, D. (2003). Advanced quantitative data analysis (Chapters 5 and 6). Buckingham: Open University Press. Glantz, S.A., & Slinker, B.K. (1990). Primer of applied regression and analysis of variance. New York: McGraw-Hill. Pedhazur, E.J. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction (2nd ed., Chapter 6). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed., Chapter 5). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

M32_HOWI0749_06_SE_C32.indd 459

11/13/13 5:25 PM

Chapter 33

Path analysis

Overview ●

Path analysis is based on multiple regression, but its conceptualisation of the predictors (independent variables) is more complex.

● The

primary objective of path analysis is to indicate likely relationships between the independent variables as predictors of the dependent variable.

● There

are numerous possible relationships among the predictor variables. Variable X1 may affect variable X2, or variable X2 may affect variable X1, or they may both affect each other (a bidirectional relationship).

● The relationships between variables in path analysis are present as path coefficients. These

are essentially correlation coefficients based on the beta weights (standardised regression coefficients) calculated in multiple regression. ●

Path analysis is about trying to establish a causal model of how predictor variables are combined to affect the level of the dependent variable.

Preparation Path analysis requires that you understand the basic principles of multiple regression (Chapter 32).

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 460

11/13/13 5:25 PM

33.2  Theoretical considerations 461

33.1 Introduction As modern psychology has increasingly drawn from real issues and non-laboratory research methods, the problems of establishing what variables affect what other variables have changed. The methodological sophistication of laboratory experiments in which causal linkages are determined by random assignment of individuals to an experimental and control group has been supplemented by a strong wish to understand people better in their natural environment. Causal modelling is merely a generic name for attempts to explore the patterns of interrelationships between variables in order to suggest how some variables might be causally influencing others. Of course, some suggestions might be rather better than others; some theoretical links might not fare well against actual empirical data. In path analysis, it is possible to estimate how well a particular suggested pattern of influences fits the known data. The better the model or causal pattern is supported by the actual data then the more likely we are to believe that the model is a useful theoretical development. There is no suggestion intended that path analysis will always provide indisputable evidence strongly favouring one particular causal model over a number of other pos­ sibilities. It is not a question of showing that one model is the best model. Path analysis simply seeks to describe a particular path which explains the relationships among the variables well and precisely; the researcher may have overlooked other variables when planning the study or analysing it and it is feasible that these variables, if they had been included, would radically change our understanding of what is happening in the data. Thought is part of the process just as much as statistics, so, as an example, we can exclude some causal pathways on logical grounds. For example, a causal influence has to precede changes in the variable of interest. If it does not, it cannot be a cause. So changes in a causal influence need to precede changes in the variable being explained (the dependent variable). Thus, childhood experiences might possibly influence our adult behaviour and so it is reasonable to include childhood experiences as influences on adult behaviour. But the reverse pattern is not viable. Our childhood experiences cannot possibly be caused by things that happen to us in our adult years; the temporal sequence is wrong. In other words, some causal models are not convincing simply because they are not logically feasible whereas other models may be possible by logical criteria of this sort.

33.2 Theoretical considerations Path analysis involves specifying the assumed causal relationships among several vari­ ables. Take, for example, the variables: ●

marital satisfaction



the love between a couple and



remaining married.

A reasonable assumption which might lead to a causal model is that couples who love one another are more likely to be satisfied with their marriage and consequently are more likely to stay together. Such a pattern of influences (or causal model) can be drawn as a path diagram such as the one in Figure 33.1. This is little more than a flow diagram indicating the direction of influence of one variable on another. In this particular model (and it clearly is just one of several possibilities), variables to the left (marital love) are thought to influence variables towards the right (marital satisfaction and remaining

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 461

11/13/13 5:25 PM

462

CHAPTER 33  Path analysis

Figure 33.1

Possible path from marital love to remaining married

married). Right-facing arrows between variables indicate the causal direction. So the model is quite simply that marital love causes marital satisfaction which in turn is responsible for remaining married. Of course, the temptation is simply to correlate scores on the three variables in this model. Suppose that we find that they all intercorrelate – then what? Well this might appear to be evidence in support of the suggested model, but it would also support many other models based on these three variables. The main point is that relationships between variables do not, in themselves, establish that marital love really causes marital satisfaction. Just taking two variables at a time results in four possible causal relationships: ●

As suggested by our model, marital love may increase marital satisfaction.



The opposite effect may occur with marital satisfaction heightening marital love.



Both variables may affect each other, marital love bringing about marital satisfaction and marital satisfaction enhancing marital love. This kind of relationship is variously known as a two-way, bidirectional, bilateral, reciprocal or non-recursive relationship.



The relationship may not really exist but may appear to exist because both variables are affected by some further confounding factor(s). For example, both marital love and marital satisfaction may be weaker in emotionally unstable people and stronger in emotionally stable people. This creates the impression that marital love and marital satisfaction are related when they are not, because emotionally unstable people are lower in both marital love and marital satisfaction while emotionally stable people are higher in both. This fourth sort of relationship is known as a spurious relationship.

In path analysis, a distinction is often made between exogenous and endogenous variables: ●

An exogenous variable is one for which assumed causes have not been measured or tested as part of the model. In other words, it refers to those variables which do not have arrows pointing to them in a path diagram.

● An

endogenous variable is one for which one or more possible causes have been measured and have been posited in the causal model. In other words, endogenous variable have arrows pointing to them in the path diagram.

So, in the above model, marital love is an exogenous variable while marital satisfaction and remaining married are endogenous variables. There will be some variation in endogenous variables which is unaccounted for or unexplained by causal variables in the model. This unexplained variance in an endogen­ ous variable is indicated by vertical arrows pointing towards that variable as shown in the path diagram in Figure 33.2. For example, the variance in marital satisfaction not explained by marital love is represented by the vertical arrow from e2. Similarly, the variance in remaining married unaccounted for by marital satisfaction is depicted by the vertical arrow from e3. The e stands for error – the term used to describe unexplained variance. The word residual is sometimes used instead to refer to the variance that remains to be explained and the phrase disturbance term is also applied, in path analysis, to exactly the same concept. It is important to realise that e refers to the influence of unknown factors rather than random error. In other words, the variance e may eventually be explained by a more complex model.

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 462

11/13/13 5:25 PM

33.2  Theoretical considerations 463



Influence of endogenous variables on relationship between marital love and Figure 33.2 remaining married

Figure 33.3 Direct effect between marital love and remaining married

In this model, marital love is assumed to have an indirect effect on remaining married through its effect on marital satisfaction. However, marital love may also have a direct effect on remaining married as shown in the path diagram of Figure 33.3.



■ Path coefficients The values of the direct effects are expressed as path coefficients. They are usually the standardised beta coefficients taken from the sort of multiple regression analysis which was introduced in the previous chapter. In other words, they can essentially be understood as analogous to correlation coefficients. The values of the paths reflecting error (or residual) variance are known as error or residual path coefficients. We will use the following symbols: ●

p1 for the path coefficient for the direct effect of marital love on marital satisfaction



p2 for the direct effect of marital love on remaining married



p3 for the direct effect of marital satisfaction on remaining married



p4 for the path reflecting the error variance of marital satisfaction



p5 for the path reflecting error variance for remaining married.

These are illustrated in Figure 33.4.

Figure 33.4

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 463

Path coefficients

11/13/13 5:25 PM

464

CHAPTER 33  Path analysis

To calculate these path coefficients we need to calculate the following two regression equations, which are essentially the same as for the multiple regression discussed in the previous chapter: marital satisfaction = a + p1 marital love remaining married = b + p2 marital love + p3 marital satisfaction (Actually, in practice, a and b will always be zero and so may be ignored. The symbols a and b are intercept coefficients for the two regression equations. Intercept coefficients are the points at which the regression lines cut the vertical axis. They are identified with different symbols in our example simply because they refer to different regression equations with different variables. However, they will always take a value of 0.00 if we are using standardised multiple regression as we do in path analysis. We can, therefore, omit or ignore them for present purposes.) Suppose that the correlation between marital love and marital satisfaction is 0.50, between marital love and remaining married 0.40 and between marital satisfaction and remaining married 0.70 for a sample of 100 couples. These are correlations which have been made up for the purposes of this example. We have carried out our multiple regression using this correlation matrix. This is possible, for example, with SPSS Statistics though one has to use syntax commands. Normally the researcher will have the raw data available so the regression analysis will be based on this. So the path coefficients about to be discussed are based on this analysis of the correlation matrix. The path coefficients are the standardised beta coefficients for these two equations which are: marital satisfaction = a + 0.50 marital love remaining married = b + 0.07 marital love + 0.67 marital satisfaction In other words, the path coefficient for p1 is 0.50, for p2, 0.07 and for p3, 0.67 as shown in Figure 33.5. Since there is only one predictor variable in the first regression, the standardised beta coefficient of 0.50 is the same as the zero-order correlation of 0.50 between marital love (the predictor variable) and marital satisfaction (the criterion variable). (If there are several predictors then partial regression coefficients would be involved.) Note that the path coefficient between marital love and remaining married is virtually zero (0.07) and statistically not significant. This means that marital love does not directly affect remaining married. The path coefficient (0.67) between marital satisfaction and remaining married differs little from the correlation (0.70) between them. This indicates that the relationship between marital satisfaction and remaining married is not due to the spurious effect of marital love. To determine an indirect effect (such as that between marital love and remaining married which is mediated by marital satisfaction), the path coefficient between marital love and marital satisfaction (0.50) is multiplied by the path coefficient between marital satisfaction and remaining married (0.67). This gives an indirect effect of 0.335 (0.50 × 0.67 = 0.335). To calculate the total effect of marital love on remaining married, we add

Figure 33.5 Actual values of path coefficients inserted

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 464

11/13/13 5:25 PM

33.2  Theoretical considerations 465

the direct effect of marital love on remaining married (0.07) to its indirect effect (0.335) which gives a sum of 0.405. The total effect of one variable on another should be, within rounding error, the same as the zero-order correlation between the two variables. As we can see, the total effect of marital love on remaining married is 0.405, which is very close to the value of the zero-order correlation of 0.40. In other words, path analysis breaks down or decomposes the correlations between the endogenous and exogenous variables into their component parts, making it easier to understand or work out what might be happening. So, for example, the correlation between marital love and remaining married is decomposed into (a) the indirect effect of marital love on remaining married and (b) the direct effects of marital love on marital satisfaction and of marital satisfaction on remaining married. Doing this shows us that although the correlation between marital love and remaining married is moderately strong (0.40), this relationship is largely mediated indirectly through marital satisfaction. It will always be far easier to see this by drawing up a path diagram than in the computer output. The correlation between marital satisfaction and remaining married can also be decomposed into the direct effect we have already calculated (0.67) and a spurious component due to the effect of marital love on both marital satisfaction and remaining married. This spurious component is the product of the direct effect of marital love on marital satisfaction (0.50) and of marital love on remaining married (0.07) which gives 0.035 (0.50 × 0.07 = 0.035). This is clearly a small value. We can reconstitute the correlation between marital satisfaction and remaining married by summing the direct effect (0.67) and the spurious component (0.035) which gives a total of 0.67 + 0.035 = 0.705. This value is very similar to the original correlation of 0.70. To calculate the proportion of variance not explained in an endogenous variable we subtract the adjusted multiple R-squared value for that variable from 1. The adjusted multiple R-squared value is 0.24 for marital satisfaction and 0.48 for remaining married. So 0.76 (1 − 0.24 = 0.76) or 76% of the variance in marital satisfaction is not explained, and 0.52 (1 − 0.48) or 52% of the variance in remaining married is not explained. In path analysis, it is a basic assumption that the variables representing error are unrelated to any other variables in the model (otherwise it would not be error). Consequently, the error path coefficient is the correlation between the error and the endogenous variable which can be obtained by taking the square root of the proportion of unexplained variance in the endogenous variable. In other words, the residual path coefficient is 0.87 (= 0.87) for marital satisfaction and 0.72 (= 0.72) for remaining married (Figure 33.6). Where there is a relationship between two variables whose nature is not known or specified, this relationship is depicted in a path diagram by a curved double-headed arrow. Suppose, for example, the two exogenous variables of similarity in personality and similarity in physical attractiveness, which were assumed to influence marital satisfaction, were known to be related, but this relationship was thought not to be causal. This relationship would be shown in a path diagram as in Figure 33.7.

Figure 33.6 Residual path coefficients

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 465

11/13/13 5:25 PM

466

CHAPTER 33  Path analysis

Figure 33.7 An unspecified relationship

The correlation between these two exogenous variables is not used in calculating the effect of these two variables on marital satisfaction and remaining married. Figure 33.8 shows the key steps in a path analysis.



■ Generalisation To determine whether our path analysis is generalisable from the sample to the population, we calculate how well our model reflects the original correlation matrix between the variables in that model using the large sample chi-square test. This will not be described here other than to make these two points:

Figure 33.8



If this chi-square test is statistically significant, then this means that the model does not fit the data.



Other things being equal, the larger the sample, the more likely it is that the chi-square test is statistically significant and the model is to be rejected.

Conceptual steps for understanding a path analysis

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 466

11/13/13 5:25 PM

33.2  Theoretical considerations 467

Table 33.1

Original and recomposed correlations

Pairs of variables

Original correlations

Recomposed correlations

Marital love and marital satisfaction

0.50

0.500

Marital love and remaining married

0.40

0.405

Marital satisfaction and remaining married

0.70

0.705

In terms of our model in Figure 33.6, we can see that the recomposed correlations for the model are very similar to the original correlations between the three variables as shown in Table 33.1. This is not always true, as explained in Box 33.1.

Box 33.1

Key concepts

Identification Although in Table 33.1 we give an example where the correlations between the variables and the recomposed correlations based on path analysis are very similar, not all models which emerge in path analysis demonstrate this feature. It is always true when the model is just-identified. Identification is an important concept in path analysis. There are three types of identification: ●



Just-identified This means that all the variables in the path analysis model put forward by the researcher are connected by unidirectional paths (single-headed arrows). Actually, even with the arrows entirely reversed in direction this would still be the case. Since the standardised beta coefficients are essentially correlation coefficients, this entirely reversed model would fit our data just as well as our preferred model. In other words, the recomposed correlations for this reversed just-identified model are just the same as for the forward model. The reconstituted correlations for any just-identified model are similar to the original correlations. Consequently it is not possible to use the match between the model and the data as support for the validity of the model. Under-identified In this, there are assumed to be one or more bidirectional pathways (double-headed arrows between variables) in the model. For example, the relationship between marital love and marital satisfaction

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 467

may be thought of as being reciprocal, both variables having an influence on each other. Since it is impossible to provide an estimate of the influence of marital love on marital satisfaction which is entirely independent of the influence of marital satisfaction on marital love, it is not possible to say what the unique estimate for these pathways would be. Consequently, we would need to modify our model to avoid this. That is, we need to respecify it as a just-identified or an over-identified model in order to deal with this problem. ●

Over-identified In an over-identified model, it is assumed that some pairs of variables do not relate. Using our example, an over-identified model assumes that there is no relationship between two pairs of variables. For instance, take the following model which postulates that marital love does not lead directly to remaining married: marital love → marital satisfaction → remaining married This is over-identified because a third possible pathway between marital love and remaining married has not been suggested (that is, the direct pathway from marital love to remaining married). Thus there are more variables (three) than pathways (two).

11/13/13 5:26 PM

468

CHAPTER 33  Path analysis

33.3 An example from published research Path analysis can be as simple or as complex as the researchers’ theories about the interrelationships between variables in their research. Increasing the numbers of variables under consideration rapidly accelerates the complexity in the path diagram. Not only does the analysis look more daunting if many variables are involved, but the path diagram becomes harder to draw. In this section we will discuss a path analysis by Wagner and Zick (1995) of the causes of blatant ethnic prejudice as a typical example of path analysis in psychology. It is fairly well known and established that there is a relationship between people’s level of formal education and their expressions of prejudice: the more prejudiced tend to have the least formal education. This suggests that there is something about education which leads to less prejudice, but what is the mechanism involved? Does education act directly to reduce prejudice or does it do so indirectly through some mediating variable (Figure 33.9)? Thus there are two possible paths: (1) the direct path from formal education to blatant prejudice and (2) the indirect path which involves a mediating variable(s). As we have indicated, the apparent complexity of this path diagram can be increased if several mediating variables are used rather than just one. Furthermore, if several direct variables are used instead of formal education alone, the diagram will become increasingly complex. Wagner and Zick (1995) collected information in a number of European countries on several potential mediating variables linking formal education and blatant prejudice: ●

Individual (relative) deprivation  The feeling of an individual that he or she is economically deprived compared with other people.



Group (relative) deprivation  The feeling that one’s social group (e.g. ethnic group) has fared badly economically compared with the rest of society.



Perceived incongruency The incompatibility between an ethnic group’s values and those dominant in society.



Political conservatism The individual’s position on the political left-wing to rightwing dimension.



National pride Pride in being a member of the national group (e.g. French or German).



Contact with foreign people The numbers of foreign people living in one’s neighbourhood.

Although this list of mediating variables far from exhausts the possibilities, it does identify a number of variables which are related to blatant ethnic prejudice according to a number of empirical studies.



M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 468

Figure 33.9

Path diagram of the direct and indirect influence of formal education on blatant prejudice

11/13/13 5:26 PM

33.3  An example from published research 469

In addition, the researchers had other measures which they could have included in the path diagram (e.g. gender and age) but omitted because the researchers did not consider them relevant to their immediate task. However, they were used by the researchers as control variables, as we shall see. There was another variable, social strata, which was a measure of social class. This was included in the path diagram by the researchers as social class was actually affected by a person’s level of education. There is no mystery about the path diagram; it is merely one of several path diagrams which the researchers could have studied. Most of the possibilities were ignored and the researchers concentrated on why those with the most formal education tend to express the least blatant prejudice. Drawing the diagram is a paper-and-pencil task based on elaborating the simple path diagram in Figure 33.9. Wagner and Zick’s path diagram is shown in Figure 33.10. It includes both direct and indirect (mediated) relationships. Arrows pointing more or less towards the right are the only ones included as these indicate possible causal directions. Having drawn the elaborated diagram, the researchers inserted the values of the relationships between the variables (i.e. path coefficients which are in essence correlation coefficients) next to the appropriate arrows. These path coefficients were obtained, of course, using multiple regression. The researchers omitted arrows (pathways) when the path coefficient did not reach statistical significance. However, because the sample was big (N = 3788), very small values were significant at the 5% level. A correlation of 0.04 is statistically significant, but its coefficient of determination or amount of variation shared by the two variables is 0.042 or 0.0016 or 0.16%. The square of e = 0.83 in Figure 33.10 indicates how much variation in blatant prejudice is unexplained by the path diagram. The path coefficients themselves are to be found in Table 33.2. As you can see, this contains a lot of information. These are the main considerations that you need to bear in mind when considering this table: ●

A zero-order correlation is merely the Pearson correlation coefficient as described in Chapter 8. First-order, second-order, etc. correlations are partial correlations as described in Chapter 30.



The upper triangle of the matrix in the table is merely a correlation matrix involving the range of measures in the path diagram with age and gender added.



Correlation coefficients are not used in the path analysis but are used in a multiple regression to obtain the beta weights.

Figure 33.10 Significant path on blatant prejudice (from Wagner and Zick, 1995, Fig 1, p. 52)

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 469

11/13/13 5:26 PM

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 470

0.01a −0.06*a

−0.11* 0.09*a −0.21*a

−0.01

−0.13*a

0.00a

−0.22*a

0.08*a

National pride

Contact

Blatant prejudice

0.02

Source: Table reproduced from Wagner and Zick (1995) © John Wiley and Sons Limited. Reproduced with permission.

* p ≤ 0.01; †p ≤ 0.05.

0.20*

b

0.18*b

−0.07*b

Partial correlation with the effects of age, gender, education and social strata partialled out unless otherwise indicated: Pearson correlation coefficient.

0.25*a

0.01b

0.01

b

0.08*b

0.09*

0.01

−0.09*

0.13*

−0.05*

Beta-coefficient from a simultaneous regression (i.e. the enter method on SPSS Statistics).

0.17*a

0.06b

0.04

b

−0.03b

0.11*b

0.12*

0.01

−0.02

−0.08*

0.13* 0.02

0.06*

Conservatism

−0.02

Incongruency

a

0.03†a

−0.08*b

−0.09* b

−0.07*b

−0.02b

0.28*b

−0.17*

−0.20* 0.30*

−0.14*

−0.13*

0.04

0.03†



Group deprivation

0.07*

Individual deprivation

b

0.01

a

a

a

0.15*

0.14*a

−0.06†a

0.02a

0.12*a

Conservatism a

0.00a

−0.07*a

−0.02a

0.04†a

Incongruency

−0.14*a

−0.10*a

0.01a

0.01a

Group deprivation

0.27*

−0.18*a

0.29*

a

−0.07†a

0.03a

0.05*a

Individual deprivation

0.01

0.07*

Social strata

−0.08*a a

−0.28*a

Education

−0.02

−0.01

−0.28*

−0.02 −0.07*

Social strata

Education

Gender

a

−0.02

Age

Zero-order correlations between variables (upper part) and results of a path analysis (lower part)

Gender

Age

Table 33.2

0.06*a

−0.03b

0.23*

0.04†

0.03†

−0.07*

−0.02

−0.15*

−0.01

0.18*

National pride

−0.13*a

−0.09*

−0.10*

−0.01

0.01

−0.08*

0.34†

0.17*

−0.14*

−0.24*

Contact

−0.21

0.18*

0.24*

0.32*

0.25*

0.12*

−0.14*

−0.34*

0.04*

0.23*

Blatant prejudice

470 CHAPTER 33  Path analysis

11/13/13 5:26 PM

33.4  Reporting the results 471 ● Wagner

and Zick (1995) carried out a simultaneous multiple regression on the correlation matrix in order to predict blatant prejudice from age, gender, formal education, social strata and the mediating variables (individual deprivation, group deprivation, etc.). Standard or simultaneous multiple regression is called the enter method on SPSS Statistics. It simply means that all of the predictor variables are included in the analysis at the same time rather than being entered in stepwise order, for example, such as where the variable explaining the most variance is dealt with as a priority.



The beta weights from this multiple regression are indicated by a letter b in the lower half of the matrix in Table 33.2.

● The

coefficients marked c in the lower half of the matrix in Table 33.2 are partial correlations which take away the effects of age, gender, education and social strata from the relationships between the pairs of variables. That is, one needs to insert in the indirect pathways the correlations having removed the influence of age, gender, education and social strata. In other words, the coefficients marked c are the partial correlation coefficients controlling for age, gender, education and social strata simultaneously, they are fourth-order correlation coefficients. Although this procedure is perfectly adequate, it is more conventional to use hierarchical multiple regression to achieve much the same end. This would involve having the four control variables as the first block in a hierarchical multiple regression. This essentially controls for these variables in the later blocks of the analysis.

33.4 Reporting the results Path analysis is a difficult procedure to apply and few students would carry out such analysis at undergraduate level. Even at the postgraduate level, novices to path analysis would probably be wise to seek some experienced support. Part of the difficulty in writing a simple way of reporting the results of a path analysis is that the reasons for this particular analysis can be complex and dependent on elaboration of previous theory. Nevertheless, readers may find it helpful to read Wagner and Zick’s description of the results of their path analysis: The path analysis shows that the predictors of ethnic prejudice mentioned above are determined by formal education, even though some of the direct paths from education are relatively weak. However, for individual and group relative deprivation, and for political conservatism, social strata mediates part of the determination by formal education. The influence of mediating variables means that the covariation of formal education and ethnic prejudice can be partially explained especially by variations in social strata, group deprivation, incongruency, conservatism and acceptance of contact with foreigners. In addition to this, the path analysis indicates a strong direct path from education to blatant prejudice which cannot be explained by the mediation variables measured. A chi-square analysis shows that a restricted model without the assumption of a direct path from education to prejudice is significantly worse than the full model presented (chi-square = 84.02, df = 1). Thus, the path analysis demonstrates that part of the educational differences in ethnic outgroup rejection can be accounted for by the mediating psychological variables, even though a substantial proportion of the covariance of respondents’ education and outgroup rejection remained unexplained. (Wagner & Zick, 1995, pp. 53–4)

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 471

11/13/13 5:26 PM

472

CHAPTER 33  Path analysis

Major points which might clarify the Wagner and Zick quotation include: ●

Education influences variables which influence blatant prejudice. Often the influences are very weak. Most studies would use far smaller sample sizes so the tiny coefficients sometimes obtained in the study would be dismissed as not significant.

● The

chi-square tests whether the indirect paths model is significantly improved by adding in the direct path from formal education to blatant prejudice. The results of the analysis suggest that the direct plus indirect effects model is superior to the indirect effects alone model.

Research examples Path analysis Kuhnle, Hofer and Kilian (2012) describe how a number of studies have shown the importance of self-control to achieving positive outcomes in life especially in terms of learning and academic performance. They theorised that school students who manifest the highest levels of self-control 1) would be more effective at balancing their academic and leisure time satisfactorily and 2) would protect their studying from the negative influence of distractions. Nearly 700 schoolchildren with an average age of 13 completed a questionnaire measuring 1) self-control, 2) subjective life balance and 3) flow while studying as well as school grades. The same questionnaire was completed on two occasions – once at the beginning of the school year and again at its end. The analysis employed structural equation modelling. Self-control was important in predicting school grades, life balance and flow. (Flow is the experience of concentration on the task unaffected by things like other tasks to be done or negative emotions – the student can isolate themselves from distractions like phone calls and talking with other people.) The researchers argue that self-control helps young people to be prepared and coordinated in various areas of life including school. Lamoureux, Palmieri, Jackson and Hobfoll (2012) explored a model in which child sexual abuse as a consequence of 1) its effect on resiliency resources (self-esteem and self-efficacy) and 2) psychological distress affects adulthood interpersonal functioning and sexual risk. A sample of nearly 700 inner-city women were interviewed twice (the interviews were six months apart). It was found that childhood sexual abuse influenced interpersonal problems via its effect on psychological distress. In contrast, child sexual abuse affected HIV/ sexual risk via its effect on resiliency resources. Maguire-Jack, Gromoske and Berger (2012) used data from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing national representative sample study of 3870 children in the USA. They wanted to know whether smacking children at 1 and 3 years of age leads to lower cognitive skills and worse behaviour problems at the ages of 3 and 5 years. Various correlates which did not change over time were controlled for. Path analysis showed that smacking at age 1 led to higher levels of behavioural problems in the form of externalising behaviour at the age of 5 years. The path was largely mediated through ongoing smacking at age 3. No association was found between early smacking at the age of 1 year and cognitive skills at the age of 3 and 5 years.

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 472

11/13/13 5:26 PM

Computer analysis 473



Key points ●

Path analysis requires a degree of mastery of statistical concepts which many students will not achieve during their degree course. Anyone who is convinced that it is appropriate for their research will need to consult supplementary sources and any local expert who might be available.

● The

complexity of path analysis should not be allowed to interfere with one’s critical faculties. A path analysis cannot be any better than the quality of the data which go into it.



Path analysis involves exploring data in ways which seem alien to those who feel that statistics should be a hard-and-fast discipline in which there is only one right way of doing things. It is an example of a statistical technique which is an exploratory tool rather than a fixed solution to a fixed problem.

Computer analysis Hierarchical or ‘Enter’ multiple regression using SPSS

Figure 33.11 SPSS Statistics steps for the hierarchical or ‘Enter’ regression procedure

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 473

11/13/13 5:26 PM

474

CHAPTER 33  Path analysis

Interpreting and reporting the output ● SPSS

produces a great deal of statistics. For a simple path analysis involving three variables, the correlations between these three variables need to be noted. For a mediator relation to be present, the correlations with the mediator should be significant. The standardised regression coefficient between the predictor variable and the criterion variable controlling for the mediating variable needs to be examined. If this standardised regression is substantially different from the correlation between the predictor and the criterion variable, it suggests there is a mediating effect.

● According

to the American Psychological Association (2010) Publication Manual, one way of reporting the results of the analysis illustrated is as follows: ‘As the relation between intellectual ability and educational achievement, r(118) = .70, 2-tailed p < .001, was little affected when school motivation was controlled, B = .65, t(117) = 9.56, 2-tailed p < .001, school motivation was not considered to mediate the relation between intellectual ability and educational achievement. Greater educational attainment was associated with greater intellectual ability.’

Screenshot 33.1

Part of the data

Screenshot 33.3 Select variables

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 474

Screenshot 33.2 Select the text

Screenshot 33.4 Select statistics

11/13/13 5:26 PM

Recommended further reading 475



Screenshot 33.5

Key output

Recommended further reading Bryman, A., & Cramer, D. (2011). Quantitative data analysis with IBM SPSS 17, 18& 19: A guide for social scientists (Chapter 10). London: Routledge. Cramer, D. (2003). Advanced quantitative data analysis (Chapter 7). Buckingham: Open University Press. Pedhazur, E.J. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction (2nd ed., Chapter 15). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

M33_HOWI0749_06_SE_C33.indd 475

11/13/13 5:26 PM

Chapter 34

The analysis of a questionnaire/ survey project Overview ●

One of the hardest things facing newcomers to research is the transition between the contents of statistics textbooks and the process of data collection and analysis. This chapter seeks to clarify how to develop an appropriate statistical analysis in circumstances in which planning has been less than perfect. Ideally, the data analysis should be planned in advance at the same time as the data collection is planned, but this is probably asking too much of student researchers in general. They have a number of research skills to bring together and little or no experience of doing so. ● A researcher needs clear understanding of what they are trying to achieve in their research. Hence it is important to clarify the broad research question and any hypotheses that derive from this. Hypotheses are merely statements of relationships that one wishes to explore. ● The data need to be mapped to identify the characteristics of variables. This is a basic requirement but easily forgotten in the complexity and confusion of planning research. Identifying which of your variables are score variables and which are nominal (category) variables is not merely important but essential. Many of the choices in the statistical ana­ lysis depend on this. Nominal (category) variables with just two categories can be treated as scores by giving the values 1 and 2 to the two values. ● Data may need recoding to make the analysis acceptable. This is generally quickly done by computer programs such as SPSS, ● Ineffective variables should be removed through a process of data cleaning. For example, variables with too little variance can contribute little. ● Data analysis consists of presenting descriptive statistics on the major variables and finding the extent of relationships among variables. This may be more or less complex.

Preparation Review correlation (Chapter 8) and regression (Chapter 9).

M34_HOWI0749_06_SE_C34.indd 476

11/13/13 5:26 PM

34.2  The research project 477

34.1 Introduction How does one go about analysing questionnaire and/or survey projects? This style of research is adopted for a lot of student research projects. The key feature and a potential difficulty is the number of variables involved; this type of research tempts researchers to write lengthy questionnaires with numerous items. It takes little effort to write a question, even less to write a poor question. Still less time is required to answer the questions if they are in a closed-ended format in which just one option is circled. An exploratory or pilot study will not always identify faulty questions and, more often than not, the pressures on students’ time are such that pilot studies are rudimentary and based on very few individuals. So inadequacies are often built in from the very beginning. Some students, wanting to get closer to the experiences of the participants in their research, choose to ask the questions themselves rather than have a self-completion question­ naire. This form of open questioning can have many advantages in terms of the quality of data collected in some studies. However, the basic problem remains very much the same – too much data. The difference is that with the open-ended interview approach the data have to be coded in a form suitable for statistical analysis unless the plan is to carry out a quali­ tative analysis if the data are suitably in depth or ‘rich’ (Howitt, 2013). Any sort of coding process presents the researcher with its own problems – largely what coding categories to use which is rarely self-evident and whether the categories used are easily and reliably used by the coders. Profound disagreements between the coders suggest either that the categories are inadequate in some way or that they need to be very much more carefully defined. Of course, there are student projects which utilise ready-made questionnaires purchased from a supplier of psychological tests and measures, downloaded from the Web, or found in books and journal articles. Although there are numerous questions involved, these are reduced to a single ‘score’ or measurement (or sometimes a small number of sub-scores) in the standardisation processes employed by the original researchers. So the groundwork of turning the questionnaire into a small number of ‘scores’ or a single score has already been done by its writers and will not be elaborated upon here. Using established measurement methods may be helpful though sometimes such questionnaires may need adapting to be of maximum use by the researcher. For example, the questions may be too American for use in Britain without modification. But modifying the questions means that the value of the original standardisation process is reduced.

34.2 The research project Sarah Freeman is a bright young psychology student who has partied for most of her time at university. When it is time to plan a research project she has little background knowledge of psychological research and theory. Stuck for a final-year project, she designs a piece of research based on her main interest in life – thinking about sex. Her project explores the hypothesis that a religious upbringing leads to sexual inhibitions. Naturally, her supervisor is reluctant to let Sarah loose on the public at large and so insists that the research is carried out on a consenting sample of fellow students. Pressured by deadlines for coursework essays, she hastily prepares a questionnaire which she pushes under bedroom doors in the Elisha Briggs Hall of Residence. Participants in the research are requested to return the completed questionnaires to her via the student mail system as soon as possible. Her questionnaire is a simple enough affair. Sarah’s questions – with spelling corrected – are as follows.

M34_HOWI0749_06_SE_C34.indd 477

11/13/13 5:26 PM

478

CHAPTER 34  The analysis of a questionnaire/survey project

1. My gender is Male Female 2. My degree course is ______________________ 3. I am ___ years of age 4. My religion is _____________________________ 5. I would rate my religious faith as: Very strong

Strong

Neither

Weak Very weak

Neither

Disagree

Strongly disagree

Neither

Disagree

Strongly disagree

6. I attend a place of worship ___ per year 7. My faith in God is important to me Strongly agree Agree 8. I am a virgin Agree Disagree 9. I am sexually promiscuous Strongly agree Agree

10. I fantasise about sex with several partners at the same time Strongly agree Agree

Neither

Disagree

Strongly disagree

11. I feel guilty after sex with more than three people at the same time Strongly agree Agree

Neither

Disagree

Strongly disagree

Neither

Disagree

Strongly disagree

Neither

Disagree

Strongly disagree

12. Oral sex is an abomination Strongly agree Agree 13. Sadomasochism is appealing to me Strongly agree Agree 14. I like sex Once a week

Twice a week Every day Every morning and evening All the time

15. Pornography Is disgusting

Is a stimulant

Is best home-made

Suddenly Sarah sees the light of day – just a few months before she finishes at university and is launched onto the job market. Paying back her student loan is on the horizon. Despite being due for submission, her project is in a diabolical mess. Suddenly there is no more partying for her – she has become a serious-minded student (well, sort of) and she is determined to resurrect her flagging and ailing attempts at research. No longer does she burn the candle at one end – she now burns it at both ends trying to make sense of statistics and research methods books. Pretty dry stuff it all is. If only she had spent some time on statistics in her misspent youth she would not have been in this hole. Can she get out of the mess? The short answer is no. The longer answer is that she could improve things considerably with a well-thought-out analysis of her data. Research has to be carefully planned

M34_HOWI0749_06_SE_C34.indd 478

11/13/13 5:26 PM

34.3  The research hypothesis 479

to be at its most effective. She needed to consider her hypotheses, methods and statistical analysis in advance of even collecting the data. Sarah is now paying for the error of her ways. One positive aspect of all this is that Sarah can at least show that she is aware of the major issues and problems with her sort of research.

34.3 The research hypothesis Although statistics is not particularly concerned about the details of the hypotheses underlying research, a clear statement of the purposes of the research often transforms the analysis of haphazardly planned research. To repeat ourselves, of course, it is by far the best to plan meticulously before doing your research. However, this does not always happen in research – even in research by professionals. Simply stated, Sarah’s research aims to discover whether there is a relationship between religious upbringing and sexual inhibitions. The trouble with this is that it is unclear quite what is meant by a religious upbringing – does it matter which sort of religion or how intensely it is part of family life? Furthermore, it is unclear what she means by sexual inhibitions – for example, not carrying out certain activities might be the result of inhibitions, but it may also be that the person does not find them arousing at all. So something needs to be done to sort out the tangles that Sarah built into her study. Given the limited range of questions which Sarah included in her questionnaire, we might suggest to her that she has several measures of religiousness: ● The

first is what religion they claim to be. The range is wide and includes Roman Catholic, Protestant, Muslim and a variety of other religions. Is it possible for Sarah to make suggestions as to which religions are most likely to encourage sexual repression? Perhaps she thinks that Roman Catholicism and Islam are the religions most likely to inculcate sexual inhibitions? If so, she could formulate a hypothesis which relates aspects of the type of religion to sexual inhibition.



There is a question about actual attendance at church. It could be that involvement in the religious community is a key variable in the influence of religion on sexual inhibitions. This might be specified as a hypothesis. Religious belief and church attendance can be differentiated in this way. Different hypotheses might be generated for the two different types of measure.



There are two questions which involve the importance of religious beliefs in the lives of the respondents. Again, a hypothesis might specify importance of religious beliefs as the important element in the possible relationship.

In terms of her measures of sexual activity, there are some very obvious things to point out. The first is that it is very difficult to relate any of the sex questions to sexual inhibition as such. Some of the questions deal with frequency of sexual activities, some deal with sexual fantasy and others deal with somewhat ‘unusual’ sexual practices. Probably Sarah is stuck with a fatal flaw in her research – that is, she failed to operationalise her concept properly; she may not have turned her idea of sexual inhibitions into a measure of that thing. It may or may not be that her measures do reflect sexual inhibitions how can she argue that they are? This is really a matter of the validity of her measures for her purposes. At the level of the superficial validity of the questions we may have our doubts. Clearly Sarah might have done better to include some questions which ask about sexual inhibitions. In the circumstances, it might be appropriate for Sarah to reformulate her hypothesis to suggest that religious upbringing influences sexual behaviours and sexual fantasy. At least this might make more sense in terms of her questionnaire. Unfortunately there is a downside to this – sexual inhibition seemed to be a psychologically interesting

M34_HOWI0749_06_SE_C34.indd 479

11/13/13 5:26 PM

480

CHAPTER 34  The analysis of a questionnaire/survey project

concept in this context. Notice that the difficulties should have been spotted very early on. Had she written her hypotheses down when she was planning her research, Sarah or someone else might have spotted that she was missing something vital.

34.4 Initial variable classification It is useful for novice researchers to classify the variables that they have collected into category variables and numerical scores: ●

You should remember that psychologists frequently turn answers on a verbal scale into numerical scores. So questions 5, 7 and 9–14 of Sarah’s questionnaire all have fixed answer alternatives. Although they do not involve numbers, it is conventional in psychological research to impose a numerical scale of 1 to 5 onto these answer categories. The reason for this is that the categories have verbal labels which imply increasing quantities of something. Scaling from 1 to 5 is arbitrary but has been shown to work pretty well in practice in a great deal of research. It is so commonplace as to be routine.



Some variables which appear at first to be just nominal categories can be turned into numerical scores simply and easily. The classic example of this in research is the variable gender which consists of just two categories: male and female. Innumerable research reports code the gender variable numerically as 1 = male and 2 = female. The logic is obvious, the numerical codings implying different quantities of the variable femaleness (or maleness). However, such variables can legitimately be treated in either way. This may help with the data analysis.

So, with these points in mind, we can classify each of our variables as ‘category’ or ‘numerical score’ or ‘other’ – meaning anything we are uncertain about (as in Table 34.1).

Table 34.1

Sarah’s 15 questions classified as category or score variables

Nominal or category variables

Numerical score variables

Question 1: Gendera

Question 1: Gendera

Other

Question 2: Degree course Question 3: Age Question 4: Religion Question 5: Faith Question 6: Attend Question 7: God Question 8: Virgin Question 9: Promiscuous Question 10: Fantasise Question 11: Guilty Question 12: Oral Question 13: Sadomasochism Question 14: Like sex Question 15: Pornography a

M34_HOWI0749_06_SE_C34.indd 480

Means that the variable may be placed in more than one column.

11/13/13 5:26 PM

34.5  Further coding of data 481

This is quite promising in terms of statistical analysis as 12 out of the 15 variables can be classified as numerical scores, so allowing some of the more powerful correlational statistical techniques to be used if required. This still leaves three variables classified as categories. These are the degree course the student is taking, their religion and their views on pornography. These are probably quite varied in terms of their answers anyway. So, Sarah may find that there may be 20 or more different degree courses included in the list of replies with only a few students in each of these 20 or more categories. Similarly, the religion question could generate a multiplicity of different replies. As they stand, these three variables are of little use in statistical analysis – they need to be recoded in some way. The problem is that with many categories for some of the variables, the size of any tables, etc. based on them can be enormous and, con­ sequently, unwieldy. A focused and compact analysis usually works best in statistics.

34.5 Further coding of data It is difficult to know why Sarah included the degree course question – it does not seem to have much to do with the issues at hand – so one approach is to discreetly ignore it. This is not uncommon in psychological research though it is not to be encouraged. Probably a better approach is to recode the answers in a simple but appropriate way. One thing which could be done is to recode them as science or arts degree courses. In other words, the degree course could be coded as 1 if it is science and 2 if it is arts. If this is done then the variable could be classified as a numerical score much as the gender variable could be. The religion question is more of a problem. Given that the answers will include Catholics, Mormons, Baptists and many more, the temptation might be to classify the variable simply as religion given versus no religion given. However, this may not serve Sarah’s purposes too well since it may be that the key thing is whether the religion is sexually controlling or not. One approach that Sarah could take is to obtain the services of people who are knowledgeable about various religions. They could be asked to rate the religions in terms of their degree of sexual control over their members. This could be done on a short scale such as: Very sexually controlling  Sexually controlling  Not sexually controlling This would transform the religion variable into a numerical scale if ratings were applied from 0 to 2, for example. Those not mentioning a religion might be deemed to be in the ‘not sexually controlling’ category. Obviously Sarah should report the degree of agreement between the raters of the religion (i.e. the inter-rater reliability). How to do this is discussed in Chapter 37. Of course, Sarah might decide to categorise the religions in a category form: 1. None 2. Catholic 3. Protestant 4. Muslim 5. Other. Unfortunately, this classification retains the nominal category characteristics of the original data although reducing the numbers of categories quite substantially. Another

M34_HOWI0749_06_SE_C34.indd 481

11/13/13 5:26 PM

482

CHAPTER 34  The analysis of a questionnaire/survey project

approach that is discussed elsewhere is to turn each of the categories of religion into dummy variables. This means creating a new variable for religious or not, another new variable for Catholic or not, another new variable for Protestant or not, and so forth. The problem with this is that it generates many additional potential comparisons which are both untidy and increases the problem of Type 1 errors due to multiple comparisons. Of course, Sarah can’t remember what a Type 1 error is. The question about pornography seems to be a natural nominal category variable given the response alternatives. Perhaps it is best to treat it as such although it could be recoded in such a way that the ‘is a stimulant’ and ‘is best home-made’ answers are classified together as being pro-pornography while the ‘is disgusting’ answer is given a different score. There are no hard-and-fast rules about these decisions and at some stage you have to come to terms with the fact that some choices seem almost arbitrary. Nevertheless, you should try to base your decisions on reasoned rational argument as far as possible. The project needs to be coherent after all.

34.6 Data cleaning There is little point in retaining variables in your research which contain little or no variance. It is particularly important with analyses of questionnaire-type materials to systematically exclude useless variables since they can create misleading impressions at a later stage. The important steps are as follows: 1. Draw up or print out frequency counts of the values of each variable you have. This can be done as frequency tables or histograms/bar charts. It should become obvious to you if virtually every participant in the research gives much the same answer to a question. Consider deleting such non-discriminating questions. If you retain such questions, then your analysis is on shaky grounds because it is putting a lot of reliance on the answers of just a few people. 2. In the case of variables which have a multiplicity of different values, you might consider recoding these variables into a small number of ranges. This might apply in the case of the age question in Sarah’s research. But there is no point in doing this unless the statistical analysis benefits in some way by doing so. For example, using a small number of ranges may help show peculiarities in the distributions of age. 3. Where you find empty or virtually empty response categories then consider com­bining categories. Some categories may contain just a few cases. These are probably useless for your overall analysis.

34.7 Data analysis

■ A relatively simple approach If Sarah follows our advice, all or virtually all of the variables will be coded as numerical scores. Any variables not coded in this way will have to be analysed by statistics suitable for category data – this might be the chi-square but more likely they will be treated as

M34_HOWI0749_06_SE_C34.indd 482

11/13/13 5:26 PM

34.7  Data analysis 483

different values of the independent variable for the analysis of variance or a similar test. We would recommend, as far as possible within the requirements of your hypotheses, that all variables are transformed into numerical scores. Accepting this, it would be a relatively simple matter to calculate the correlations between all of the variables in Sarah’s list and each other. The trouble with this is that it results in a rather large correlation matrix of 15 × 15 correlation coefficients – in other words a table of 225 coefficients. Although the table will be symmetrical around the diagonal of the matrix, this still leaves over 100 different correlations. It is not the purpose of statistical analysis to pour complexity on your analysis; statistics are there to simplify as far as is possible. In Sarah’s research, the sex questions are quite numerous. She has eight different questions about sexual matters. Obviously it would be satisfactory if there were some way of combining these different answers in order that a single measure of ‘sexual inhibition’ could be developed. One simple thing that might be done is simply to add the scores on the questions together. This would require the following: 1. That the different questions are scored in the same direction. Looking at Sarah’s questionnaire we see that, for example, the question ‘I like sex’ if scored numerically from left to right would give a bigger score to those who liked sex most often. However, the answers to the question on sadomasochism if scored numerically from left to right would give a lower score to those who liked sadomasochistic sex. It is necessary to recode her answers in such a way that they are consistent. In this case, all the answers which are more sexual could be rescored as necessary to make the high scores pro-sex. 2. That the standard deviations of scores on questions to be added together are similar, otherwise the questions with the biggest standard deviations will swamp the others. If they differ radically, then it is best to convert each score on a variable to a standard score and then add up answers to several questions (Chapter 6). There is a case for Sarah to adopt the more sophisticated approach as this would suggest that she has learnt something in her time at university. A similar sort of thing could be done with the three religious questions, although it might be equally appropriate, given their relatively small number, to treat them as three separate variables. In order to test her hypotheses, Sarah could correlate the sex and religion variables together. A significant relationship in the predicted direction would support Sarah’s hypotheses. (It would be equally appropriate to apply t-tests or analyses of variance with religion as the independent variable and sex questions as the dependent variables.) The advantage of using correlations is that it is then possible to control for (or partial out) obvious background variables which might influence the relationships found. In this study gender and age are of particular interest since both of them might relate to our main variables of interest. Partial correlation could be used to remove the influence of gender and age from the correlation between religion and sexual inhibition.



■ A more complex approach Given the number of questions Sarah has included on her questionnaire, it is arguable that she ought to consider using factor analysis on the sex questions to explore the pattern of interrelations between the variables. She may well find that the answers to the sex questions tend to cluster together to form small groups of questions which tend to measure separate aspects of sex. For example, questions which deal with unusual sexual practices might be grouped together. Factor analysis would identify the important clusters or factors. In addition, factor analysis will usually give factor scores which are weighted scores for each individual on

M34_HOWI0749_06_SE_C34.indd 483

11/13/13 5:26 PM

484

CHAPTER 34  The analysis of a questionnaire/survey project

each factor separately. These are expressed on the same scale and so are comparable. In other words, they have already been expressed in terms of standard scores. It is then possible to relate scores on the religion variable(s) with scores on each of the factors just as before. Partialling out gender and age might also be appropriate.



■ An alternative complex approach One could also employ multiple regression (Chapter 32). Probably the best approach is to use religion as the dependent (criterion) variable(s) and the separate sex variables as the independent (predictor) variables. In this way, it is possible to find out which of the sex variables contribute to the prediction of the religious experiences of the participants in childhood. Sarah may find that only certain of the questions are particularly and independently related to religion. Actually, Sarah could control for age and gender by forcing them into the regression early in the analysis.

Key points ● Although

statistics can help structure poor data, it is impossible to remedy all faults through statistics. Research design and planning are always vital.



Statistics is useful in simplifying complex data into a small number of variables. Unfortunately, for most practical purposes it is impossible to do this without resorting to computer analysis. This is because of the sheer number of variables to be analysed.



Do not let your partying outstrip your studying.

M34_HOWI0749_06_SE_C34.indd 484

11/13/13 5:26 PM

Part 5

Assorted advanced techniques

M35_HOWI0749_06_SE_C35.indd 485

11/13/13 5:26 PM

M35_HOWI0749_06_SE_C35.indd 486

11/13/13 5:26 PM

Chapter 35

The size of effects in statistical analysis Do my findings matter?

Overview ● Statistical

significance is not the key attribute of a successful statistical analysis. Significance is merely a matter of whether the trend in the sample is likely if there is not a trend in the population.



More important is the size of the relationship or difference obtained. This is not always easily assessed on the basis of tests of significance.



One standardised way of indicating the strength of a relationship is simply to turn the statistic into a correlation coefficient. This is easily done for chi-square, the t-test, nonparametric tests and the analysis of variance using the simple formulae presented in this chapter. There are other ways of doing this including Cohen’s d which is discussed in later chapters.

Preparation Significance testing (Chapter 11) and the correlation coefficient (Chapter 8) are the basic ideas. Since this chapter contrasts with much of current practice in the use of statistics by academic psychologists, a degree course at the University of Real Life might help.

M35_HOWI0749_06_SE_C35.indd 487

11/13/13 5:26 PM

488

CHAPTER 35  The size of effects in statistical analysis: Do my findings matter?

35.1 Introduction One of the most neglected questions in statistical analysis is that of whether or not the researcher’s findings are of any real substance. Obviously part of the answer depends very much on the particular research question being asked. One needs to address issues such as: ●

Is this a theoretically important issue?



Is this an issue of social relevance?



Will this research actually help people?

These are not statistical matters. Statistics can help quantify the strength of the relationships established in the research. Very few research publications seriously discuss this issue with respect to the research they describe.

35.2 Statistical significance Students sometimes get confused as to the meaning of significance in statistics. Perhaps it is a pity that the word significance was ever used in this context since all that it actually means is that it is reasonable to generalise from your sample data to the population. That is to say, significance merely gives you an estimate of the extent to which you can be confident that your findings are not simply artefacts of your particular sample or samples. It has absolutely nothing to do with whether or not there are really substantial trends in your data. Researchers tend to keep a little quiet about the substance of their findings, preferring merely to report the statistical significance. It is common – but bad – practice to dwell on statistical significance, but this is encouraged by the fact that publication of one’s research in psychology depends to some extent on obtaining statistical significance. Increasingly, however, journals are requiring the inclusion of effect size statistics in the articles they select for publication. But the bottom line is that the size of any effect (trend) that you find in your research is important in its own right. The size of the samples being used has a profound effect on the statistical significance of one’s research. A correlation of 0.81 is needed to be statistically significant at the 5% level with a sample size of 6. However, with a much larger sample size (say, 100), a much smaller correlation of 0.20 is statistically significant at the 5% level. In other words, with a large enough sample size quite small relationships can be statistically significant. This is discussed extensively in Chapter 40 on statistical power analysis. We have already seen that the squared correlation coefficient basically gives us the proportion of the total variance shared by two variables. Sometimes r2 is referred to as the coefficient of determination. With a correlation of r = 1.00 the value of r2 is still 1.00 (i.e. the total amount of variance). That means that all of the variation in one of the variables is predictable from the other variable. In other words, 100% of the variation on one variable is determinable from the variation in the other variable. Expressed graphically, it would mean that all of the points on a scattergram would fit perfectly on a straight line. If, however, the correlation between two variables is 0.2 then this means that r2 equals 0.04. That is to say, the two variables have only 4% of their variance in common. This is not very much at all despite the fact that such a small correlation may well be statistically significant given a large enough sample size. The scatterplot of such a small correlation has points which tend to scatter quite a lot from the best-fitting

M35_HOWI0749_06_SE_C35.indd 488

11/13/13 5:26 PM

35.3  Method and statistical efficiency 489

straight line between the points – in other words, there is a lot of error variance compared to the strength of the relationship between the two variables.

35.3 Method and statistical efficiency Before going any further, we should emphasise that the quality of your research methods is an important factor determining the strength of the relationships found in your research. Sloppy research methods or poor measurements are to be avoided at all costs. Anything which introduces measurement error into your research design will reduce the apparent trends in the research. So, for example, a laboratory experimenter must take scrupulous care in standardising her or his procedures as far as possible. Sloppy methods may lead to disappointment because they introduce error. This is clearly demonstrated if we consider a researcher trying to assess the relationship between children’s ages and their heights in a sample of pre-school children. An excellent method for doing this would be to obtain each child’s birth certificate so that their date of birth will give their precise age and to take the child down to the local clinic to have the child’s height precisely measured by the clinic nurse who is experienced at doing this. In these circumstances, there is probably very little we can do further to maximise our chances of assessing the true relationship between age and height in children. A much sloppier way of doing this research on the relation between children’s ages and heights might be as follows. The researcher asks the child’s nursery teacher to estimate the child’s height and tells them to guess if they complain that they do not know. The children’s ages are measured by asking the children themselves. It is pretty obvious that these measures of age and height are a little rough and ready. Using these approximate measures we would expect rather poor correlations between age and height – especially compared with the previous, very precise method. In other words, the precision of our measurement procedures has an important influence on the relationships we obtain. The difference between the two studies is that the second researcher is using very unreliable measures of height and age compared with the very reliable measures of the first researcher. There are a number of ways of measuring reliability in psychology including inter-rater reliability which is essentially the correlation between a set of measurements taken by person A with those taken by person B. So, for example, we would expect that the birth certificate method of measuring age would produce high correlations between the calculations of two different people, and that asking the children themselves would not produce very reliable measures compared with the answer we would get from the same children even the next day. If you can calculate the reliability of your measurements, it is possible to adjust the correlation between two measures for the unreliability of each of the measures. This essentially inflates the reliability coefficients upwards towards 1.00. In other words, you get the correlation between age and height assuming that the measures were totally reliable. The formula for doing this is: rx∞y∞ =

rxy rxx ryy

The symbol rx y is the coefficient of attenuation. It is merely the correlation between variables x and y if these variables were perfectly reliable. The symbols rxx and ryy are the separate reliability coefficients of the variables x and y. Often in research we do not have estimates of the reliability of our measures so the procedure is not universally applicable. ∞ ∞

M35_HOWI0749_06_SE_C35.indd 489

11/13/13 5:26 PM

490

CHAPTER 35  The size of effects in statistical analysis: Do my findings matter?

35.4 Size of the effect in studies Although it is relatively easy to see the size of the relationships in correlation research, it is not quite so obvious in relation to experiments which have been analysed using t-tests, chi-square or a nonparametric test such as the Wilcoxon matched pairs. One of the approaches to this is to find ways of turning each of these statistics into a correlation coefficient. Generally this is computationally easy. The resulting correlation coefficient makes it very easy to assess the size of your relationships as it can be interpreted like any other correlation coefficient.



■ Chi-square It is easy to turn a 2 × 2 chi-square into a sort of correlation coefficient by substituting the appropriate values in the following formula: rphi =

chi-square N

rphi is simply a Pearson correlation coefficient for frequency scores. In fact, it is merely a special name for the Pearson correlation coefficient formula used in these circumstances. Interpret it more or less like any other correlation coefficient. It is always positive because chi-square itself can only have positive values. Remember that N in the above formula refers to the number of subjects and not to the degrees of freedom. If your chi-square is bigger than a 2 × 2 table, you can calculate the contingency coefficient instead. The formula for this is: contingency coef 1cient =

chi-square chi-square + N

As above, N in this case is the sample size, not the number of degrees of freedom. It is possible to interpret the contingency coefficient very approximately as if it were a Pearson correlation coefficient. But avoid making precise parallels between the two.



■ The t-test Essentially what is done here is to turn the independent variable into numerical values. That is to say, if the research design has, say, an experimental and a control group we code one group with the value 1 and the other group with the value 2. Take the research design in Table 35.1, for example, which compares men and women in terms of level of job ambition (the dependent variable). Of course, normally we would analyse the difference between the means in terms of the t-test or something similar. However, we can correlate the scores on the dependent variable (job ambition) if we code the independent variable as 1 for a man and 2 for a woman (Table 35.2). The two sets of scores can then be correlated using a Pearson correlation. This should be a simple calculation for you using SPSS. However, if you have already worked out your t-values for the t-test you can use the following formula to enable you to calculate the correlation quicker: rbis =

M35_HOWI0749_06_SE_C35.indd 490

t2 t 2 + df

11/13/13 5:26 PM

35.4  Size of the effect in studies 491

Table 35.1

Scores of men and women on a dependent variable Men

Women

5

2

4

1

9

3

6

2

4

1

7

6

5

2

1

2

4



Table 35.2

Arranging the data in Table 35.1 so that gender can be correlated with the dependent variable Score on dependent variable ( job ambition)

Score on independent variable gender (men coded as 1, women coded as 2)

5

1

4

1

9

1

6

1

4

1

7

1

5

1

1

1

4

1

2

2

1

2

3

2

2

2

1

2

6

2

2

2

2

2

where t is the value of the t-statistic and df equals the degrees of freedom for the t-test. Do not worry too much about rbis since it is merely the Pearson correlation coefficient when one variable (e.g. gender) has just one of two values.

M35_HOWI0749_06_SE_C35.indd 491

11/13/13 5:26 PM

492

CHAPTER 35  The size of effects in statistical analysis: Do my findings matter?

35.5 An approximation for nonparametric tests We have to approximate to obtain a correlation coefficient for nonparametric tests such as the Mann–Whitney U-test. One possible procedure is to work out the statistic (e.g. Mann–Whitney U-test), check its probability value (significance level) and then look up what the value of the t-test would be for that same significance level and sample size. For example, if we get a value of the Mann–Whitney U of 211 which we find to be significant at the 5% level (two-tailed test) on a sample of 16 subjects, we could look up in the t-table the value of t which would be significant at the 5% level (two-tailed test) on a sample of 16 subjects (i.e. the degrees of freedom = 14). This value of t is 2.15 which could be substituted in the formula: rbis =

t2 t 2 + df

35.6 Analysis of variance (ANOVA) It is possible to compute from analysis of variance data a correlation measure called eta. This is analogous to a correlation coefficient but describes a curvilinear rather than the linear relationship which the Pearson correlation coefficient does. It is of particular use in the analysis of variance since it is sometimes difficult to know which of the independent variables explains the most variance. The probability value of an F-ratio in itself does not enable us to judge which of the independent variables accounts for the largest amount of the variance of the dependent variable. Table 35.3 is a summary table from an analysis of variance considering the influence of intel­ ligence and social class on a dependent variable. It is difficult to know from the table whether intelligence or social class explains more of the variance as the degrees of freedom differ. In order to calculate the value of eta for any of the variables all we need to do is substitute in the following formula:

eta =

treatment df × F -ratio (treatment df × F -ratio) + within df

So, for example, if we take intelligence then we substitute the values from Table 35.3 in the formula:

Table 35.3 Source of variance

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square

F-ratio

Significance

Intelligence

1600

2

800

8.9

1%

Social class

2400

3

800

8.9

1%

Interaction

720

6

120

1.3

ns

9720

108

90

Within (error)

M35_HOWI0749_06_SE_C35.indd 492

Analysis of variance summary table

11/13/13 5:26 PM

35.6  Analysis of variance (ANOVA) 493

Table 35.4

Analysis of variance summary table with values of eta added

Source of variance

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square

F-ratio

Significance

Eta

Intelligence

1600

2

800

8.9

1%

0.38

Social class

2400

3

800

8.9

1%

0.44

720

6

120

1.3

ns

0.26

9720

108

90

Interaction Within (error)

eta =

2 × 8.9 = (2 × 8.9) + 108 =

17.8 17.8 + 108 17.8 = 125.8

0.1415 = 0.38

If we do a similar calculation for the two other sources of variation, we can extend our summary table to include eta (Table 35.4). What this extra information tells us is that social class accounts for more variation in the dependent variable than does either intelligence or the interaction. Eta is calculated by some statistics packages such as SPSS Statistics.

Research examples Effect sizes Effect sizes can be reported using a number of statistics. What is appropriate depends partly on the statistical design involved. So eta is used for ANOVA whereas Cohen’s d or the correlation coefficient can be used for the t-test. Gervais, Vescio and Allen (2012) in their study of people’s interchangeability as sex objects (fungibility) report the effect size for one of their ANOVAs as follows: ‘A main effect of body type, F(1, 65) = 5.47, p = .02, η2p = .08, revealed that ideal targets (M = 13.51, SD = 7.39) were more fungible than average targets (M = 12.79, SD = 7.06). This effect, however, was modified by the presence of the hypothesised interaction between body type and target gender, F(1, 65) = 6.11, p = .02, η2p = .10, indicating that the tendency for ideal targets to be perceived as more fungible than average targets was moderated by target gender.’ (p. 507) [The symbol η2p represents eta which is used to indicate effect size in ANOVA.] Lautamo, Laakso, Aro, Ahonen and Törmäkangas (2011), in an investigation of children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI), used Cohen’s d as their measure of effect size: ‘The results revealed significant differences between the two groups of 3.1 to 6.5-year-old children (with and without SLI). In the first analysis of differences in play performance (conducted with 38 items) independent samples t-tests confirmed that the means differed significantly (t(108) = 5.80, p < 0.01), and the effect size was large (Cohen’s d = 1.11).’ (p. 227)



M35_HOWI0749_06_SE_C35.indd 493

11/13/13 5:26 PM

494

CHAPTER 35  The size of effects in statistical analysis: Do my findings matter?

Levine, Asada and Carpenter (2009) were interested in the effect sizes reported in the literature involving meta analyses (see Chapter 36). They took a sample of 51 published meta-analyses which involved over 3600 separate studies. Levine et al. wanted to know what the correlation between effect sizes found in the analyses and the sample sizes involved. In approximately 80% of meta-analyses there was a negative correlation between effect size and sample size. In other words, the larger the effect size then the smaller the sample size was likely to be. For the researchers, the best interpretation of this involves a publication bias against non-significant results. That is, studies which do not reach statistical significance are systematically excluded from publication because the journals reject them or because the researchers do not attempt to publish them. The broader conclusion is that effect sizes reported in meta-analyses are likely to be overestimates of those found by the researchers doing research in an area.

Key points ●

Do not expect the things in this chapter to feature regularly in other researchers’ reports. They tend to get ignored despite their importance.



Do be aware of the need to assess the degree of explanatory power obtained in your research as part of your interpretation of the value of your findings. All too frequently psychologists seek statistical significance and forget that their findings may be trivial in terms of the amount of variance explained.



Do try to design your research in such a way that the error and unreliability are minimised as far as possible.

M35_HOWI0749_06_SE_C35.indd 494

11/13/13 5:26 PM

Chapter 36

Meta-analysis Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research

Overview ● A

review of the findings of previous research is a typical component of any research report. However, this is a very difficult thing to do adequately given the availability of large numbers of research studies which may have been carried out on the topic.



Meta-analysis provides a way of handling the complexity of the multiple research studies available on many topics and to make systematic statistical summaries.



It consists of methods of assessing the size of relationships between variables or differences between sample means. The finding of each study is converted into a standard measure of effect such as a Pearson correlation coefficient or Cohen’s d. We concentrate on the correlation coefficient because of its familiarity. It is easy to convert the Pearson correlation to Cohen’s d and vice versa.

● Effect ●

sizes from several studies may be combined to give an overall effect size.

Furthermore, studies may be coded in a number of ways such as the type of study, the number of participants and even the geographic location of the study. The relationship between these variables and effect size can be calculated. The findings may suggest that, for example, laboratory studies reveal greater effects than field studies.

Preparation Review effect size (Chapter 35). In particular, make sure that you understand the difference between statistical significance and effect size.

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 495

11/18/13 3:06 PM

496

CHAPTER 36  Meta-analysis: Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research

36.1 Introduction Meta-analysis is a general term to describe statistical techniques which allow a researcher to statistically analyse the pattern of findings from a variety of published and unpublished studies into a particular research question. Most statistical analyses investigate the data from a single research study. However, when we review the research literature we frequently find a number of studies researching similar hypotheses and similar variables. Such studies can vary enormously in terms of the method they employ (for example, field studies versus laboratory studies) or the populations they sample (for example, students versus the general population). Sometimes a number of studies may find positive evid­ ence in favour of the hypothesis whereas others support the reverse trend. The main objectives of meta-analysis are as follows: ●

To assess the strength of relationships over a range of studies and, if possible, to combine these into a single overall indicator of the relationship.

● To

assess the influence of various characteristics of pertinent studies (the type of sample, the type of method, etc.) on the strength of the relationships found in the studies.

Meta-analysis is a highly organised literature review process compared with the normal literature reviews found in journal articles and the like. There is another very structured review process known as the systematic review. This employs rigorous database search and article summarising methods but its primary objective, however, is not statistical. Sometimes the systematic review and meta-analysis are combined but this is not necessarily so. The objective is to structure literature reviews such that they are freed from the influence of the whims of the researcher. Systematic reviews are discussed in Howitt and Cramer (2014a). Meta-analysis involves some new concepts. Although relatively rare in student work, a meta-analysis is a feasible proposition where time and resources are available for a thorough literature search. A crucial feature of any research study is the process of reviewing the available empirical literature on a particular topic. To date, meta-analysis has not routinely been applied to these reviews although some elements of it would be easy to incorporate. Usually a meta-analysis is carried out as an independent exercise because of a number of difficulties in its use: ●

Because meta-analysis is a study of studies, it is necessary to obtain copies of relevant reports and publications dealing with the statistical analysis of the relationship in question. Sometimes these may have to be obtained, say, from other libraries (or from the researchers themselves if the study has been recently published). Sometimes publications will be untraceable. The process of obtaining research reports costs time. Since there may be a bias towards the publishing of significant research findings, ideally a meta-analysis should also include unpublished research findings. These can be even more difficult to identify and obtain.



The meta-analyst needs to be familiar with computerised database searches. Unless a variety of databases are searched using a variety of appropriate keywords, important research studies may be overlooked. Published articles and books may be sources of additional studies which have not been found using the databases.

● There

is inconsistency in the reporting of research findings. Sometimes important pieces of information are missing. A meta-analysis can be done with minimal information – sample size and significance level are all that are required. If effect size were routinely reported for every research study there would be no problem and, increasingly,

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 496

11/18/13 3:06 PM

36.1  Introduction 497

journals are requiring this information. Nevertheless, meta-analysts may have to use a range of formulae to transpose published findings into measures of effect size. This is clearly important when a review includes studies from the past when the contents of journal articles were subject to fewer formal requirements than today. ●

There is a good deal of non-computer work involved in meta-analysis. Meta-analysis is not available on any of the standard statistical packages. However, much of the work is computationally easy with just a few simple hand calculations. Computers can be useful in later stages of the analysis, but they are far from essential.



Meta-analysis involves defining the variables and types of study of interest with some precision. This requires some understanding of the field of study which is difficult to achieve within the timescale of student projects.

It is to be hoped that readers will not be too deterred by the above comments. After all, they simply imply diligence, planning, hard work and understanding of the chosen field of research. These are reasonable targets for any researcher whether or not using meta-analysis. Criticisms of meta-analysis usually apply equally to conventional reviews of the empirical studies. So, for example, problems of retrieval of studies, biases favouring the publication of statistically significant findings in research publications, glossing over details in particular studies and similar issues are common to both meta-analytic and other attempts to synthesise the literature. This chapter provides a practical introduction to meta-analysis which should be sufficient to guide students through the major stages involved. It does not pretend to be an exhaustive coverage. It should provide a foundation for a meta-analysis and to studying its techniques further. Figure 36.1 gives the key steps to consider in understanding meta-analysis.

Figure 36.1

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 497

Conceptual steps for understanding meta-analysis

11/18/13 3:06 PM

498

CHAPTER 36  Meta-analysis: Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research

36.2 The Pearson correlation coefficient as the effect size Effect size is the central concept in meta-analysis. It means exactly what it says – the size of the effect of one variable on a second. In other words, the effect size indicates the amount of relationship between one variable and another variable in a standardised way. This should not be confused with a causal effect, though in controlled experiments it may actually mean causal effect. Ultimately, and largely for practical reasons, the most convenient measure of effect size is the Pearson correlation coefficient between the two variables (say the independent variable and the dependent variable). In Chapter 35, the correlation coefficient was used as a measure of effect size (i.e. the strength of the relationship between the two variables). Effect size is not the same as statistical significance, which is about generalising from a sample to a population. The larger the correlation coefficient between two variables, the larger the effect of one variable on the other. Chapter 35 also showed just how easy it is to convert a number of different statistical tests such as the t-test and chi-square into a Pearson correlation coefficient. It is the ease of such conversions that ensures the Pearson correlation coefficient’s practical utility as a measure of effect size. Irrespective of the nature of the statistical analysis reported by the researcher in the primary report, it is highly likely that a correlation coefficient can be obtained from this information. The minimum information required, remember, is merely the significance level and sample size. Do not assume that effect size is useful only when comparing independent variables and dependent variables in experimental studies. Meta-analysis can be used in virtually any type of study. Also, remember that the techniques can be useful when combining the results of just two studies.

36.3 Other measures of effect size There are other, perhaps more common, measures of effect size. Cohen’s d is probably the most common measure of effect size reported. Its major disadvantage is that it can be more difficult to calculate from the statistical analyses usually presented in reports of psychological research. Cohen’s d is the difference between the mean of one group of participants and the mean of the other group adjusted by dividing by the standard deviation of the scores. In other words, it is the difference between the two groups standardised by dividing by the standard deviation. Just as we can turn any score into a z-score by dividing the score by the standard deviation, we can generate a standardised effect score by dividing the unstandardised effect size (for example, the difference between the experimental and control group) by the size of the standard deviation of the scores. Expressed as a formula, Cohen’s d is usually given as: Cohen’s d =

mean of Group A − mean of Group B standard deviations of both groups of scores pooled together

The standard deviation is obtained by subtracting the experimental group scores from the experimental group’s mean and subtracting the control group scores from the control group’s mean. These difference scores are then pooled (combined) as a first step in computing their standard deviation. Actually, this is not Cohen’s formulation since he simply recommended using the standard deviation of one of the populations on the assumption that both populations should have the same standard deviation. It is possible to find on the Web a number of programs and applets which will calculate Cohen’s d for you.

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 498

11/18/13 3:06 PM

36.4  Effects of different characteristics of studies 499

G*Power (discussed in Chapter 40) can do this as part of power analysis. (See the Computer Analysis section at the end of this chapter.) Although there is a similarity, it is not true to say that Cohen’s d is the same as the t-test despite overlaps in their calculation. In the t-test, the division is by the standard error of the difference between the sample means; in Cohen’s d, the division is by the standard deviation of the pooled groups of scores. (In essence the two standard deviations are combined arithmetically.) Although Cohen’s d is commonly used in meta-analysis, it is not quite as flexible in use as the Pearson correlation coefficient. Most important is the fact that it is much easier to estimate the Pearson correlation coefficient from the minimal information that researchers sometimes supply. We saw in Chapter 35 how we can calculate a correlation coefficient from a range of tests of significance. This is not so easy with Cohen’s d. Furthermore, the conversion of a correlation coefficient to Cohen’s d is easy using the table we provide later in this chapter. Consequently, it is probably best to work with correlation coefficients and then convert them to Cohen’s d should this seem appropriate.

36.4 Effects of different characteristics of studies Modern meta-analyses are not simply about determining the effect size over a range of studies. They also try to estimate what characteristics of studies may be responsible for large effect sizes and what characteristics of studies may be responsible for smaller effect sizes. It is usual to select a range of possible study variables which may be related to effect size. These may include: ●

the gender of the participants



the size of the study



the quality of the study as rated by a panel of psychologists or from the prestige of the journal in which the study was published



whether the study involved behavioural rather than attitudinal measures



the sex of the researcher



whether the study was a laboratory experiment or field study

● any

other variable that the meta-analyst judges to be pertinent and which can be assessed from the primary published reports of studies or by other means such as ratings by experts.

This list is not the ideal or complete list. The study variables you choose may be very different from the above list which should not simply be routinely applied without further consideration. The selection of study variables is a subjective matter in the sense that it depends on knowledge, skill and a degree of insight. These are much the same characteristics that are required by any researcher. Note that the information the meta-analyst wants may not be given in the available research reports – for example, the researcher may not have analysed data for males and females separately. The basic procedures for investigating the influence of study variables on effect size are very simple. So if a meta-analyst wished to study effect size in studies involving female participants compared with those involving male participants, the following effect sizes could be calculated: ●

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 499

The overall (combined) effect size for relevant studies irrespective of the gender of the participants.

11/18/13 3:06 PM

500

CHAPTER 36  Meta-analysis: Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research

Table 36.1

Meta-analysis table for different age and gender combinations

Males

Young

Older

r = 0.32

r = 0.13

r = 0.45

r = 0.03

r = 0.35 Females

r = 0.22

r = −0.04

r = 0.12

r = 0.05

r = 0.15

r = 0.15 r = 0.11



The overall (combined) effect size for female participant studies.



The overall (combined) effect size for male participant studies.

It may well be that the overall effect size is more or less the same for both males and females. However, the male and female effect sizes could be very different. Such a simple analysis may be insufficient for the analyst’s particular purposes. For example, the analysis would be a little more complicated if the analyst wished to compare the effect sizes for young males, young females, older males and older females. This would involve the calculation of effect sizes for the four different age/gender combinations (see Table 36.1). Not all meta-analyses investigate the influence of study characteristics. Carry out such an analysis only if it is relevant to your purposes and meaningful in terms of the range of types of study found in the relevant literature search.

36.5 First steps in meta-analysis

■ Step 1: Define the variables of interest to you Decide precisely which two variables you are investigating in your meta-analysis. (Other pairs of variables can also be considered and treated in the same way in parallel.) This is in essence deciding the nature of the research hypothesis to be tested.



■ Step 2: Plan your database search Plan your search for relevant studies involving your chosen variables. This search should involve a computer search of the relevant databases. Perusing studies referred to in relevant research publications may generate additions to your list of relevant studies. Of course, you may wish to omit certain types of study because they are not relevant or do not meet other criteria. It is important to do this using stipulated criteria rather than on whims. If possible, seek out unpublished studies.

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 500

11/18/13 3:06 PM

36.5  First steps in meta-analysis 501

Table 36.2 Equivalent effect sizes expressed as Cohen’s d and Pearson correlation coefficient Pearson r

Cohen’s d

Pearson r

Cohen’s d

Pearson r

Cohen’s d

Pearson r

Cohen’s d

Pearson r

Cohen’s d

0.00

0.00

0.20

0.41

0.40

0.87

0.60

1.50

0.80

2.67 2.76

0.01

0.02

0.21

0.43

0.41

0.90

0.61

1.54

0.81

0.02

0.04

0.22

0.45

0.42

0.93

0.62

1.58

0.82

2.87

0.03

0.06

0.23

0.47

0.43

0.95

0.63

1.62

0.83

2.98

0.04

0.08

0.24

0.49

0.44

0.98

0.64

1.67

0.84

3.10

0.05

0.10

0.25

0.52

0.45

1.01

0.65

1.71

0.85

3.23

0.06

0.12

0.26

0.54

0.46

1.04

0.66

1.76

0.86

3.37

0.07

0.14

0.27

0.56

0.47

1.06

0.67

1.81

0.87

3.53

0.08

0.16

0.28

0.58

0.48

1.09

0.68

1.85

0.88

3.71

0.09

0.18

0.29

0.61

0.49

1.12

0.69

1.91

0.89

3.90

0.10

0.20

0.30

0.63

0.50

1.15

0.70

1.96

0.90

4.13

0.11

0.22

0.31

0.65

0.51

1.19

0.71

2.02

0.91

4.39

0.12

0.24

0.32

0.68

0.52

1.22

0.72

2.08

0.92

4.69

0.13

0.26

0.33

0.70

0.53

1.25

0.73

2.14

0.93

5.06

0.14

0.28

0.34

0.72

0.54

1.28

0.74

2.20

0.94

5.51

0.15

0.30

0.35

0.75

0.55

1.32

0.75

2.27

0.95

6.08

0.16

0.32

0.36

0.77

0.56

1.35

0.76

2.34

0.96

6.86 7.98

0.17

0.35

0.37

0.80

0.57

1.39

0.77

2.31

0.97

0.18

0.37

0.38

0.82

0.58

1.42

0.78

2.49

0.98

9.85

0.19

0.39

0.39

0.85

0.59

1.46

0.79

2.58

0.99

14.04



■ Step 3: Obtain research reports Obtain copies of research reports containing the statistical analyses of the relevant studies. These may be available in your local university or college library, but sometimes they have to be ordered from other libraries. The authors of recently published studies may be contacted by mail or email to obtain copies of reports. Databases usually contain an adequate address for the senior author and, nowadays, the records on databases frequently include an email address for the corresponding authors. Remember that at the very minimum, you need a significance level and sample size to calculate an effect size. Sometimes a previous meta-analytic study may supply you with details of otherwise unobtainable studies. It may be possible to use the effect sizes reported in this earlier meta-analysis. Cohen’s d is easily converted to r (and vice versa) by using Table 36.2. This table also serves as a ready reference to compare effect sizes expressed as r with those given as Cohen’s d.



■ Step 4: Calculating effect sizes for each study A standard measure of effect size should be calculated for each of the relationships between the variables for each study reviewed. Our chosen measure of effect size is the Pearson correlation coefficient or r. Some studies may report this value or some other measure of effect size, but usually they do not. Where effect sizes are not reported they need to be calculated by the meta-analyst.

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 501

11/18/13 3:06 PM

502

CHAPTER 36 Meta-analysIs: COMbInIng and explOrIng statIstICal FIndIngs FrOM prevIOus researCh

Table 36.3

Converting various tests of significance to a correlation coefficient

Statistic

Formula for converting to Pearson correlation

t-test

t2 t +d df

rbis = Chi-square r= Cohen’s d

2

Chi-square N

Convert to r using table 36.2

nonparametric test

r=

pearson correlation coefficient and variants

z N

no conversion necessary

Most common tests of significance and when only significance level and sample size given

r=

z N

Notes

Can be used for a related or unrelated t-test Only use this formula for a 2 × 2 chi-square useful if no source of data from a study is available other than another meta-analysis alternatively convert to parametric equivalent and substitute this value in formula (see section 35.5) these are already the value of the effect size Convert the significance level to z using table 36.4. then divide by the square root of the sample size involved

It is usually possible to use the test of significance reported in the original analysis to calculate the effect size r. Table 36.3 gives this conversion for common tests of significance. We have already seen some of these in Chapter 35. However, sometimes this information is missing from the primary source. This is less of a problem with modern research publications but it is nevertheless useful to know what to do when even minimum information is missing. If you know the sample size and the significance level, then the following formula can be used to approximate the effect size irrespective of the particular test of significance involved. The significance levels should be converted to their one-tailed equivalents if they are given as two-tailed probabilities because the absolute value of z refers to one tail of the standard normal distribution. The sign of the z-score needs to be noted. r=

z N

The value of z for the significance level is obtained by consulting Table 36.4. So, if the significance level for a particular study is 0.7% (i.e. the probability is 0.007), then the value of z obtained from Table 36.4 is 2.44. Assuming that the one-tailed significance level is based on 40 participants, the effect size is: r=

2.44 40

=

2.44 = 0.39 6.325

This is a good approximation given the limited information required. The formula has obvious advantages for use with uncommon tests of significance or those for which a conversion formula to r is not available. It can also be used to convert nonparametric significance levels to effect sizes. Of course, significance levels are not always reported very precisely, which may cause problems especially when the findings are not significant at the 5% level. Just what is the effect size for this? Some authors report it as an effect

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 502

11/18/13 3:06 PM

36.5  First steps in meta-analysis 503

Table 36.4

z-distribution for converting one-tailed probability levels to z-scores

p

z

p

z

p

z

p

z

p

z

0.000  01

4.265

0.19

0.878

0.40

0.253

0.61

−0.279

0.82

−0.915

0.00  01

3.719

0.20

0.842

0.41

0.228

0.62

−0.306

0.83

−0.954

0.001

3.090

0.21

0.806

0.42

0.202

0.63

−0.332

0.84

−0.995

0.01

2.326

0.22

0.772

0.43

0.176

0.64

−0.359

0.85

−1.036

0.02

2.054

0.23

0.739

0.44

0.151

0.65

−0.385

0.86

−1.080

0.03

1.881

0.24

0.706

0.45

0.126

0.66

−0.413

0.87

−1.126

0.04

1.751

0.25

0.675

0.46

0.100

0.67

−0.440

0.88

−1.175

0.05

1.645

0.26

0.643

0.47

0.075

0.68

−0.468

0.89

−1.227

0.06

1.555

0.27

0.613

0.48

0.050

0.69

−0.496

0.90

−1.282

0.07

1.476

0.28

0.583

0.49

0.025

0.70

−0.524

0.91

−1.341

0.08

1.405

0.29

0.553

0.50

0.000

0.71

−0.553

0.92

−1.405

0.09

1.341

0.30

0.524

0.51

−0.025

0.72

−0.583

0.93

−1.476

0.10

1.282

0.31

0.496

0.52

−0.050

0.73

−0.613

0.94

−1.555

0.11

1.227

0.32

0.468

0.53

−0.075

0.74

−0.643

0.95

−1.645

0.12

1.175

0.33

0.440

0.54

−0.100

0.75

−0.675

0.96

−1.751

0.13

1.126

0.34

0.413

0.55

−0.126

0.76

−0.706

0.97

−1.881

0.14

1.080

0.35

0.385

0.56

−0.151

0.77

−0.739

0.98

−2.054

0.99

−2.326

0.15

1.036

0.36

0.359

0.57

−0.176

0.78

−0.772

0.16

0.995

0.37

0.332

0.58

−0.202

0.79

−0.806

0.17

0.954

0.38

0.306

0.59

−0.228

0.80

−0.842

0.18

0.915

0.39

0.279

0.60

−0.253

0.81

−0.878

Find the appropriate significance or probability level p value from the table, the required z-score is adjacent to the right. Reverse this process if you wish to convert your z-score back to a significance or probability level. Remember that a probability needs to be multiplied by 100% to get the percentage probability.

size of zero, though clearly this is not likely to be the case. Others take it as the 50% or 0.5 level of significance. In these circumstances, it would be better to estimate the effect size from the formulae in Table 36.3 if at all possible. At the end of this step, you should have values or estimated values of the effect size for each of the studies you are using in your meta-analysis. If you are unable to give an effect size because of incomplete information in the original report of a study or because the report was unobtainable, it will have to be omitted. This omission should be mentioned in your report of your meta-analysis.



■ Step 5: Combining effect sizes over a number of studies One aim of meta-analysis is to combine the findings of several studies (or a selected subset of studies such as those involving female participants) into a single composite effect size. The obvious way of doing this is to average the effect sizes. However, the simple numerical average of the effect sizes can give a distorted value, particularly when some of the values of the correlation coefficients are large. Instead, we average the effect sizes by converting each r into a z-score (zr) for the correlation coefficient using Table 36.5.

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 503

11/18/13 3:06 PM

CHAPTER 36  Meta-analysis: Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research

504

Table 36.5 Extended table of Fisher’s zr transformation of the correlation coefficient r

zr

r

zr

r

zr

r

zr

r

zr

r

zr

r

zr

0.01

0.10

0.41

0.436

0.801

1.101

0.841

1.225

0.881

1.380

0.921

1.596

0.961

1.959

0.02

0.020

0.42

0.448

0.802

1.104

0.842

1.228

0.882

1.385

0.922

1.602

0.962

1.972

0.03

0.030

0.43

0.460

0.803

1.107

0.843

1.231

0.883

1.389

0.923

1.609

0.963

1.986

0.04

0.040

0.44

0.472

0.804

1.110

0.844

1.235

0.884

1.394

0.924

1.616

0.964

2.000

0.05

0.050

0.45

0.485

0.805

1.113

0.845

1.238

0.885

1.398

0.925

1.623

0.965

2.014

0.06

0.060

0.46

0.497

0.806

1.116

0.846

1.242

0.886

1.403

0.926

1.630

0.966

2.029

0.07

0.070

0.47

0.510

0.807

1.118

0.847

1.245

0.887

1.408

0.927

1.637

0.967

2.044

0.08

0.080

0.48

0.523

0.808

1.121

0.848

1.249

0.888

1.412

0.928

1.644

0.968

2.060

0.09

0.090

0.49

0.536

0.809

1.124

0.849

1.253

0.889

1.417

0.929

1.651

0.969

2.076

0.10

0.100

0.50

0.549

0.810

1.127

0.850

1.256

0.890

1.422

0.930

1.658

0.970

2.092

0.11

0.110

0.51

0.563

0.811

1.130

0.851

1.260

0.891

1.427

0.931

1.666

0.971

2.110

0.12

0.121

0.52

0.576

0.812

1.133

0.852

1.263

0.892

1.432

0.932

1.673

0.972

2.127

0.13

0.131

0.53

0.590

0.813

1.136

0.853

1.267

0.893

1.437

0.933

1.681

0.973

2.146

0.14

0.141

0.54

0.604

0.814

1.139

0.854

1.271

0.894

1.442

0.934

1.689

0.974

2.165

0.15

0.151

0.55

0.618

0.815

1.142

0.855

1.274

0.895

1.447

0.935

1.697

0.975

2.185

0.16

0.161

0.56

0.633

0.816

1.145

0.856

1.278

0.896

1.452

0.936

1.705

0.976

2.205

0.17

0.172

0.57

0.648

0.817

1.148

0.857

1.282

0.897

1.457

0.937

1.713

0.977

2.227

0.18

0.182

0.58

0.663

0.818

1.151

0.858

1.286

0.898

1.462

0.938

1.721

0.978

2.249

0.19

0.192

0.59

0.678

0.819

1.154

0.859

1.290

0.899

1.467

0.939

1.730

0.979

2.273

0.20

0.203

0.60

0.693

0.820

1.157

0.860

1.293

0.900

1.472

0.940

1.738

0.980

2.298

0.21

0.213

0.61

0.709

0.821

1.160

0.861

1.297

0.901

1.478

0.941

1.747

0.981

2.323

0.22

0.224

0.62

0.725

0.822

1.163

0.862

1.301

0.902

1.483

0.942

1.756

0.982

2.351

0.23

0.234

0.63

0.741

0.823

1.166

0.863

1.305

0.903

1.488

0.943

1.764

0.983

2.380

0.24

0.245

0.64

0.758

0.824

1.169

0.864

1.309

0.904

1.494

0.944

1.774

0.984

2.410

0.25

0.255

0.65

0.775

0.825

1.172

0.865

1.313

0.905

1.499

0.945

1.783

0.985

2.443

0.26

0.266

0.66

0.793

0.826

1.175

0.866

1.317

0.906

1.505

0.946

1.792

0.986

2.477

0.27

0.277

0.67

0.811

0.827

1.179

0.867

1.321

0.907

1.510

0.947

1.802

0.987

2.515

0.28

0.288

0.68

0.829

0.828

1.182

0.868

1.325

0.908

1.516

0.948

1.812

0.988

2.555

0.29

0.299

0.69

0.848

0.829

1.185

0.869

1.329

0.909

1.522

0.949

1.822

0.989

2.599

0.30

0.310

0.70

0.867

0.830

1.188

0.870

1.333

0.910

1.528

0.950

1.832

0.990

2.647 2.700

0.31

0.321

0.71

0.887

0.831

1.191

0.871

1.337

0.911

1.533

0.951

1.842

0.991

0.32

0.332

0.72

0.908

0.832

1.195

0.872

1.341

0.912

1.539

0.952

1.853

0.992

2.759

0.33

0.343

0.73

0.929

0.833

1.198

0.873

1.346

0.913

1.545

0.953

1.863

0.993

2.826

0.34

0.354

0.74

0.951

0.834

1.201

0.874

1.350

0.914

1.551

0.954

1.875

0.994

2.903

0.35

0.365

0.75

0.973

0.835

1.204

0.875

1.354

0.915

1.557

0.955

1.886

0.995

2.995

0.36

0.377

0.76

0.996

0.836

1.208

0.876

1.358

0.916

1.564

0.956

1.897

0.996

3.106

0.37

0.388

0.77

1.020

0.837

1.211

0.877

1.363

0.917

1.570

0.957

1.909

0.997

3.250

0.38

0.400

0.78

1.045

0.838

1.214

0.878

1.367

0.918

1.576

0.958

1.921

0.998

3.453

0.999

3.800

0.39

0.412

0.79

1.071

0.839

1.218

0.879

1.371

0.919

1.583

0.959

1.933

0.40

0.424

0.80

1.098

0.840

1.221

0.880

1.376

0.920

1.589

0.960

1.946

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 504

11/18/13 3:06 PM

36.5  First steps in meta-analysis 505

This table is of the correlation coefficient expressed as a normal distribution. It is different from the z-distribution so take care. You need the purple columns to find your value of the correlation coefficient r and the required value of zr is to the right of this in the blue column. The several values of zr are then summed and averaged by dividing by the number of values. This average can then be turned back into the combined effect size by using Table 36.5 in the reverse mode. (That is, you look for your value of the combined zr in the right-hand side (blue) of the pairs of columns and find the value of r to the left of this.) Thus if we wish to calculate the average effect size from three studies with the following effect sizes: study A: r = 0.3 study B: r = 0.7 study C: r = 0.5 we convert each to their zr by using Table 36.5. These values are 0.310, 0.867 and 0.549, respectively. The numerical average of these is (0.310 + 0.867 + 0.549)/3 = 0.575. But this is the average zr. We can then reconvert this value to an overall effect size by using Table 36.5 in reverse. The effect size r for the three studies combined is therefore 0.52. It is possible that a particular study has findings in the reverse direction from those of the majority. In this case, its effect size is given a negative value. Thus the overall effect size will be reduced.



■ Step 6: The significance of the combined studies The significance level of the combined studies can also be assessed. Once again, the simple numerical average of the probability levels is misleading. Intuitively we may appre­ ciate that this simple average makes no allowance for the greatly increased effective sample size obtained by combining studies. There are numerous different ways of combining significance levels from a range of studies to give an overall significance level, each having different advantages or disadvantages. The simplest and one of the most satisfactory methods is to convert each significance level into a z-score using Table 36.4. Rather than divide by the number of z-scores to obtain the average, the sum of the z-scores is divided by the square root of the number of z-scores: I=

∑z N

Thus if the significance levels from a set of studies are 0.08, 0.15 and 0.02, each of these is converted to a z-score using Table 36.4. This gives us z-scores of 1.405, 1.036 and 2.054, respectively. These z-scores are summed and divided by the square root of the number of z-scores: I=

1.405 + 1.036 + 2.054 3

=

4.495 = 2.595 1.732

This average z is converted back into a significance level using Table 36.4. In this case, this gives a combined significance level of 0.001 (or 1.0%). Note that if the findings of a study are in the reverse direction from those of the majority, the corresponding z-score is given a negative sign. Once again, this tends to reduce the overall significance level.

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 505

11/18/13 3:06 PM

506



CHAPTER 36  Meta-analysis: Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research

■ Step 7: Comparing effect sizes from studies with different characteristics Finally, what if one wished to compare effect sizes between studies with different characteristics? For example, what if one wanted to know whether studies involving female participants differed from those involving male participants in terms of their effect size? The easiest way of doing this is to turn your data into a table like Table 36.6. In this table, the effect sizes for the male and female studies are listed in separate columns. It is then a relatively simple matter to compare these two sets of effect-size ‘scores’ using the Mann–Whitney U-test (Explaining statistics 19.3) or the t-test (Explaining statistics 14.1). This is an approximate procedure in the eyes of some experts since all studies are considered equal although they may differ in terms of the sample size. Despite criticisms of such an approach, it uses familiar statistics and may well be sufficiently powerful for most purposes. There is a significant difference between these two groups as assessed by either the Mann–Whitney U-test or the unrelated t-test. Thus the effect sizes are greater in studies which involved female participants than in studies involving male participants. If you choose the t-test, it might be advantageous to convert your effect sizes to zr values since this will reduce the undue influence of extreme values a little.

Table 36.6

Illustrating the comparison of effect sizes for different study characteristics

Effect sizes of studies of males

Effect sizes of studies of females

0.27

0.41

0.15

0.52

0.22

0.43

0.29

0.47

Mean = 0.23

Mean = 0.45

36.6 Illustrative example There is evidence that men’s physiological responses to sexually explicit pictures may differentiate sex offenders from non-offenders and non-sex offenders. Physiological response in these studies is assessed by plethysmographs which measure either changes in the volume of the penis or changes in the circumference of the penis. The latter measure is generally not well regarded. The data reported are fictitious but help to illustrate the processes involved in meta-analysis.



■ Step 1: Define the variables of interest to you In this case, the researchers wished to review the available studies which might indicate whether physiological responses to sexual images could be used to differentiate sex offenders from other men. Consequently the independent variable was sex offender versus non-offender or non-sex offender and the dependent variable was measured by scores on a plethysmograph assessment of the men’s response to erotic pictures.

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 506

11/18/13 3:06 PM

36.6  Illustrative example 507



■ Step 2: Plan your database search The researchers searched the psychological abstract database (PsycINFO – this database is discussed in Chapter 5 of Howitt & Cramer, 2014a) and also the medical science database using the keywords plethysmograph, sex offender, rapist, paedophile and molester. Additionally, as the field is relatively small, the researchers chose to write to one hundred researchers in the field requesting relevant research reports, either published or unpublished.



■ Step 3: Obtain research reports The researchers found nine studies from their database search to be obtained from their own or other university libraries. These are listed in column 1 of Table 36.7 but they also received two additional unpublished studies from their request to key researchers. Table 36.7 also includes information relevant to calculating the effect size gleaned from these reports and information about possible study variables.



■ Step 4: Calculating an effect size for each study Table 36.7 lists the information obtained from each study relevant to calculating the effect size. The formula (or table) used is mentioned and the final column provides effect sizes expressed as r for each of the studies. Edwards’s study, however, is so lacking in the statistical detail provided that it has been deleted from this meta-analysis.



■ Step 5: Combining effect sizes over a number of studies The meta-analyst combined the effect sizes for all of the studies by converting each effect r into a zr, averaging these and finally converting back to an effect size. This involves turning each effect size correlation into a Fisher zr using Table 36.5. The effect sizes in order are 0.24, 0.54, 0.52, 0.37, 0.19, 0.34, 0.49, 0.34, 0.22, and 0.50 according to Table 36.7. Remember that the final study has been discarded from the analysis. The average of the corresponding zr is: average z r 0.245 + 0.604 + 0.576 + 0.388 + 0.192 + 0.354 + 0.536 + 0.354 + 0.224 + 0.549 10 4.022 = = 0.4022 10

=

This value of zr according to Table 36.5 corresponds to an average of the effect sizes of 0.38 (this is obtained by looking for the average zr of 0.4022 in the body of Table 36.5 and reading off the value of r which corresponds to this value of the averaged zr). This process could be repeated to obtain, say, the overall effect size of the volume measure and the circumference measure separately.



■ Step 6: The significance of the combined studies The overall significance of the combined studies is obtained by turning each significance level into the corresponding z-score using Table 36.4. The various z-scores are then

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 507

11/18/13 3:06 PM

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 508

0.025

0.10

0.01 0.025

0.075

gives effect size as r = 0.54

F given as significant at 0.01 with 20 cases

t-value given as 2.31 with 34 cases (i.e. df = 32)

t-test reported as 1.45, df = 54

Mann–Whitney U significant at 1% level based on 47 cases

related t = 2.53, df = 10, this was a matched design in which prisoners served as own control

t = 1.67 with total N = 23

Cohen’s d given as 0.45

F reported as significant at 0.03 with df = 1, 54 (i.e. N = 56)

t-value not reported. Findings not significant at 5% level with sample size = 14

grey (1998)

black (1983)

White (1995)

Jones (1966)

Williams (1987)

parton (unpublished)

Carter (unpublished)

elliot (1999)

smith (1989)

edwards (1953)

>0.05 or set at p = 0.50

0.03

0.001

0.01

?

0.05

chi-square value given as 4.06 with 1 degree of freedom based on N of 73 cases

brown (1976)

Significance

Effect-size information

circumference

volume

circumference

circumference

volume

circumference

circumference

volume

circumference

volume

circumference

Plethysmograph measure

Illustrative summary of studies and the conversion of statistics to effect sizes

Study (fictitious)

Table 36.7

prisoners

non-prisoners

prisoners

prisoners

prisoners

non-prisoners

non-prisoners

non-prisoners

prisoners

prisoners

prisoners

Control group

N

z

t2 t2 + d df

t2 t +d df 2

t2 t +d df 2

t2 t +d df 2

N use of formula too crude because of uncertainty about exact significance and no other statistics (alternatively r could be set at 0.00)

r=

z

convert to r with table 36.2

rbis =

(this formula works with related t-tests)

rbis =

r=

rbis =

rbis =

N

study ignored

0.50

0.22

0.34

0.49

0.34

0.19

0.37

0.52

r=

0.54

0.24

z

chi-square N

Effect size r

none needed

r=

Effect-size formula

508 CHAPTER 36 Meta-analysIs: COMbInIng and explOrIng statIstICal FIndIngs FrOM prevIOus researCh

11/18/13 3:06 PM

36.6  Illustrative example 509

summed and divided by the square root of the number (N) of significance levels employed. Note that for two studies the significance level is not reported or is not precise enough. Thus the calculation is based on just nine studies. The formula for z is: z=

∑z N

This gives: =

1.645 + 2.326 + 1.960 + 1.281 + 2.326 + 1.960 + 1.440 + 3.090 + 1.880 9

17.908 = = 5.97 3 Remember that this is the value of z which has to be converted back to a significance level using Table 36.4. Thus the combined significance level is 0.000 01 or 0.001%.



■ Step 7: Comparing effect sizes from studies with different characteristics Because there is some question whether the circumference measure is as good as the volume measure, the overall effect sizes were calculated for the circumference measure studies and the volume measure studies separately. This yielded the data in Table 36.8. Comparing these overall effect sizes, it would seem that there are some grounds for thinking that circumference studies produce the smallest effect size, implying that they are inferior at identifying sex offenders from other men. This comparison is significant at only the 0.067 level with a Mann–Whitney test but significant at 0.04 with the unrelated t-test. Using zr instead of the effect size made no substantial difference to the outcome. This seems reasonably strong evidence that the volume measure tends to produce greater effects than the circumference measures. A similar analysis comparing the effect of having a prisoner versus a non-prisoner control group showed no significant difference in terms of effect size using the same tests of significance.

Table 36.8 Effect size data for volume measures and circumference penile measures compared Effect sizes of studies involving volume measure

Effect sizes of studies involving circumference measure

0.54

0.24

0.37

0.52

0.49

0.19

0.50

0.34 0.34 0.22

Mean = 0.48

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 509

Mean = 0.31

11/18/13 3:06 PM

510

CHAPTER 36  Meta-analysis: Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research

36.7 Comparing a study with a previous study Meta-analysis is useful when you are replicating another researcher’s study as it provides a method of combining the results of the two studies. Furthermore, you can test to see if your effect size is significantly different from that found in the previous research. The formula involves converting each effect size to zr using Table 36.5 and then subtracting one from the other and making other calculations involving N (the sample sizes) as in the following formula: z=

r1 − r2 1 1 1 + N1 − 3 N 2 − 3

Thus if the effect sizes under consideration are 0.43 (with N = 25) and 0.62 (with N = 47) then these are first converted to zr using Table 36.5. This gives us values of 0.460 and 0.725. The calculation is then: z=

=

= =

0.460 − 0.725 1 1 + 25 − 3 47 − 3 −0.265 1 1 + 22 44 −0.265 0455 + 0.0227 0.0 −0.265

0.0682 −0.265 = 0.261 = −1.015 This value of z (not zr) is turned into a significance level by using Table 36.4. This gives a probability value of 0.15 (or 15%) which is not statistically significant. Our conclusion in this case would be that the effect sizes of the two studies are similar and certainly not significantly different from each other. We could go on to report the effect size of the combined studies and the combined significance levels using the methods described above. Of course, this formula can be used to compare any two correlation coefficients with each other to see whether they are significantly different.

36.8 Reporting the results Meta-analytic studies are almost always substantial research studies in their own right. Consequently, many of the requirements of reporting a meta-analytic study are the very same requirements that one would require when writing a substantial report such as a journal article. You may find the detailed account of writing psychological reports in the authors’ companion volume (Howitt & Cramer, 2014a) invaluable in reporting a

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 510

11/18/13 3:06 PM

36.8  Reporting the results 511

meta-analysis as a consequence. Because there may be details of a large number of studies to tabulate, then special care may be required in generating the tables using, say, Excel or Word. SPSS Statistics would not be particularly helpful in this regard. Since any meta-analysis needs to make reference to previous relevant meta-analytic studies, often there is a model already available for one to consult to get an idea of the sort of style to adopt. None of this should be a deterrent to using meta-analytic techniques as part of the literature review, say, for any study you are writing up. As we have seen, many of the calculations are relatively simple and straightforward by hand. It is perfectly feasible to, say, add in effect sizes for the findings of relevant previous research as you report them. Not only would this be good practice but it would also change the emphasis from stat­ istical significance to that of effect size.

Research examples Meta-analysis Freund and Kasten (2012) explain that we have perceptions of our cognitive abilities which are involved in the self-concept because they relate our abilities to the abilities of other people. This process of self- and otherevaluation may be regarded as a continuous feature of our lives and is employed in formal settings too (e.g. the careers guidance setting where psychometric measurements are available). The researchers performed a meta-analysis based on 41 published studies of the relationship between self-estimated and psychometrically assessed cognitive abilities. This involved 41 published studies and a total of 154 effect sizes obtained from them. The overall relationship between the self-estimated and psychometric cognitive abilities was a correlation of 0.33. Amongst other things, the analysis also showed that the relationship was greater when mathematical abilities were the focus as opposed to more global cognitive abilities. Sedlmeier and co-workers (2012) carried out a meta-analysis of the psychological effects of meditation. Their main focus was on nonclinical groups of people using meditation, i.e. psychologically healthy adults. But there were problems since a big proportion (75%) of the studies they identified were excluded for reasons such as psychological measures were not used or that the study did not involve nonclinical samples. So the study itself was based on the remaining 163 studies which met the study criteria. The average effect size was r = .28. Taking the 125 studies which were published in reviewed journals the average effect size remained much the same at r = .27. The effects of medication were large for emotionality and relationship problems with smaller effects for measures of attention and smaller still for cognitive variables. The details of the findings varied for different approaches (transcendental meditation, mindfulness meditation, etc.). The authors tried a number of possible mediating variables such as length of time doing meditation and age but little of sufficient clarity to draw conclusions emerged from this. Taylor, Rastle and Davis (2013) point out that reading in many language systems depends on both knowledge of the word (e.g. sew) and a knowledge of how to generate sounds from spellings such as when pseudowords are read (e.g. gew). The neural basis for these skills has been discussed by researchers but Taylor et al. propose that such skills depend on a) the degree of engagement of a brain region brought about by the stimulus word and b) the amount of effort involved in processing that stimulus. Predictions from this were assessed with a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of reading. Among other things, the meta-analysis of the studies revealed that real words compared with pseudowords led to the activation of the left anterior fusiform gyrus of the brain. Pseudowords compared to words generated activity in a more anterior part of the left fusiform gyrus and the occipotemporal cortex.

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 511

11/18/13 3:06 PM

512

CHAPTER 36  Meta-analysis: Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research

Key points ● This account of meta-analysis should convince you of the importance of reporting effect sizes for all studies

you carry out. The most useful effect size formula is simply the Pearson’s correlation coefficient between two variables. ●

When carrying out a literature review, it is a positive advantage to report the effect sizes for all of the important studies. This is more important than reporting statistical significance alone.

● Experience

will show that the difference between significant and non-significant findings can be very small indeed when their effect sizes are compared. Consequently, you need to consider near-significant results carefully when evaluating the research literature.

Computer analysis Some meta-analysis software The basic calculations for meta-analysis are essentially straightforward and well within the capabilities of anyone prepared to give this chapter careful study. Although some of the calculations can benefit from computer assistance, the common statistical computer packages will only be of occasional help with a metaanalysis. SPSS Statistics does not deal with meta-analysis. Generally speaking, this program provides no particular help in relation to meta-analysis. There are a number of commercial software options available to help with meta-analysis though these may or may not be available to you at your university or college, for example. Of more immediate practical help may be the following free meta-analytic software: ●

Meta-Analyst is documented and available for download at http://tuftscaes.org/meta_analyst/



Meta-Stat – A tool for the meta-analysis of research studies by Lawrence M. Rudner, Gene V. Glass, David L. Evartt and Patrick J. Emery. This is documented and can be downloaded at http://echo.edres.org:8080/ meta/metastat.htm



Statistics software for meta-analysis by Ralf Schwarzer. This is documented and can be downloaded at http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/health/meta_e.htm



The meta-analysis calculator. This can be used as an applet at http://www.lyonsmorris.com/lyons/ metaAnalysis/index.cfm



The MIX program for meta-analysis which uses Microsoft’s Excel spreadsheet. Details and downloads are available at http://www.mix-for-meta-analysis.info/about/index.html

Of course, a search of the Internet will find others. Chapter 52 of Howitt and Cramer (2014a) gives detailed steps for using the meta-analysis calculator.

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 512

11/18/13 3:06 PM

Computer analysis 513



Screenshot 36.1 Select test

Screenshot 36.2 Enter value of statistic

Screenshot 36.3 Enter number of cases



Enter if effect was in predicted direction

Screenshot 36.5

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 513

Screenshot 36.4

Enter number of cases if there is another group

Screenshot 36.6 Output

11/18/13 3:06 PM

514

CHAPTER 36  Meta-analysis: Combining and exploring statistical findings from previous research

Recommended further reading Howitt, D., & Cramer, D. (2014). Introduction to research methods in psychology (Chapter 5). Harlow: Pearson. Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage (especially Chapters 1–4).

M36_HOWI0749_06_SE_C36.indd 514

11/18/13 3:06 PM

Chapter 37

Reliability in scales and measurement Consistency and agreement

Overview ●

Reliability as discussed in this chapter is about the consistency of a psychological scale or similar measurements. That is, are all components of the scale measuring similar things?



One of the conventional ways of achieving internal consistency is to ensure that all items correlate with the sum of the items on the scale. This is known as item analysis. A typical method is item–whole or, more clearly, item–total analysis. Any item which does not correlate significantly with the total (of all of the items) is deleted because it is not measuring the same thing as the total score.



Split-half reliability is little more than the correlation between the total of one half of the items and the total of the other half of the items. If the two halves are measuring the same thing then they should correlate highly. Sometimes the sum of the odd-numbered items is correlated with the sum of the even-numbered items.



Alpha reliability is the average of every possible split-half reliability that could be calculated on a scale. This overcomes the influence of the particular selection of items chosen for each half can have on split-half reliability.



Kappa is a measurement of the agreement between raters or observers. That is, it assesses inter-rater or inter-observer agreement.

Preparation The concept of correlation (Chapter 8) is an essential prerequisite to understanding the assessment of reliability. Chapter 22 on the correlated scores analysis of variance and Chapter 31 on factor analysis may also help with particular sections of this chapter.

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 515

11/18/13 12:04 PM

516

CHAPTER 37  Reliability in scales and measurement: Consistency and agreement

37.1 Introduction An important role for statistics is in assessing the adequacy of psychological scales and measures. Usually in psychology, but not always, measures consist of several different components added together to give a total score on that measure. Thus many attitude and personality tests consist of a large number of questionnaire items which are combined to give a total score on some dimension of attitude or personality. Although the analysis of such scales using factor analysis (Chapter 31) is an important and necessary part of modern psychological test and measure construction, factor analysis is not the only approach to understanding the structure of a test or measure. In many circumstances, a researcher may be concerned simply to obtain a fairly general measure of a particular psychological variable. In these circumstances, relatively simple checks on the structure of the measure may suffice. So, for example, a questionnaire designed to measure ‘love’ for one’s partner might consist of several different questions. The researcher needs to know the extent to which the items measure much the same thing. Generally speaking, if the items measure aspects of love then we would expect that they would intercorrelate with each other to a modest level at least. However, since it is the overall or total score on the measure of love which matters then for a good scale we would expect:

Figure 37.1



that scores on each item correlate with the total score (this is item–total or item– whole correlation)



that a score based on half of the items of the scale would correlate with scores based on the remainder of the scale (this is called split-half reliability which can be elaborated into Cronbach’s coefficient alpha).

Conceptual steps for understanding reliability

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 516

11/18/13 12:04 PM

37.2  Item-analysis using item–total correlation 517

The procedures described in this chapter are about the internal consistency of psychological measures. Internal consistency is the extent to which all of the items constituting a measure are measuring much the same thing. If they are measuring similar things, each item should correlate with the other items in the measure. Although this is referred to as reliability, it is a very different matter from reliability across two different points in time, for example. Figure 37.1 gives the key steps in understanding reliability.

37.2 Item-analysis using item–total correlation Look at Table 37.1. It contains scores on four different items for ten different participants. There is also a total score given in the total column consisting of the scores on each of item 1, item 2, item 3 and item 4. So the second participant has a total score of 2 + 1 + 1 + 2 = 6. The correlations between the scores of the ten participants for item 1 and the total score can be calculated with the Pearson correlation formula (Explaining statistics 8.1) or using a computer package, of course. The value of the correlation is 0.74 which suggests that item 1 is a fairly good measure of what the total score on the measure is measuring. The other items may be treated in the same way in order to see whether this is true of all of them. Generally speaking, we would be happy with this scale given the relatively high item– total or item–whole correlation. Notice that when an item is excluded from the total score, its correlation with this adjusted total score is reduced. Thus, in Table 37.2 the correlation of item 1 with the total score (based on summing items 2, 3 and 4) is 0.49 as opposed to a correlation of 0.74 when all items are included. This more refined analysis does nothing to revise our opinion of the scale. Generally speaking, the items which seem to be the poorest are items 1 and 4 which have the lowest item–total correlations with the item removed from the total. Of course, four-item scales are unusual in psychological research. Normally we have many items. If we had a lot more items, we might be inclined to try to shorten the scale a little, perhaps to make it more appealing to participants. The technique for doing this is simple. Delete the low-correlating items and re-do the analysis based on the shortened

Table 37.1

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 517

Data from ten cases from a four-item questionnaire

Person

Item 1

Item 2

Item 3

Item 4

Total score

1

1

3

5

6

15

2

2

1

1

2

6

3

1

1

1

1

4

4

5

2

4

2

13

5

6

4

3

2

15

6

5

4

5

6

20

7

4

5

3

2

14

8

2

1

2

1

6

9

1

2

1

1

5

10

1

1

2

2

6

11/18/13 12:04 PM

518

CHAPTER 37  Reliability in scales and measurement: Consistency and agreement

Table 37.2

Table 37.3

Correlations of items with the total score on the scale Correlation with total score

Correlation with total score excluding item in question

Item 1

0.74

0.49

Item 2

0.84

0.71

Item 3

0.91

0.84

Item 4

0.76

0.55

Correlations of shortened-scale items with the total score on that scale Correlation with total score

Correlation with total score excluding item in question

0.77

0.56

Item 3

0.94

0.87

Item 4

0.90

0.73

Item 2

scale. Although our example is a short scale, if we wanted to reduce its length then we would probably wish to delete item 1 since it has the lowest correlation with the total score. Table 37.3 gives the outcome of shortening the scale in this way. You will see that compared with the correlations in Table 37.2, the shortened scale has increased item– total correlations. In this sense, a better scale has been achieved by shortening it. The difficulty is that we can carry on deleting items and improving the internal consistency of the items but this may result in a shorter scale than we want. Usually it is best to exclude only the poorest of items. By doing so we leave a scale which covers a wide range of the aspects of the thing being measured. The appropriate scale length involves a degree of judgement. A standard statistical package such as SPSS Statistics reduces the work in calculating item–total (item–whole) correlations of various sorts and makes shortening the number of items in the scale easy. The results of this analysis can be written up as follows: ‘An item–whole analysis was carried out on the items on the scale. As can be seen from Table 37.2, each item had a satisfactory correlation with the total score on all of the items combined. After the item– whole correlations had been recalculated with the item removed from the total score, there was a decline in the item–whole correlations. However, the relationships remained substantial and it was decided not to shorten the scale given that it consists of just four items.’

37.3 Split-half reliability A computationally less demanding way of assessing the internal structure of a questionnaire is split-half reliability. Remember that internal reliability refers to the extent to which all of the items in a questionnaire (or similar measure) are assessing much the same thing. Split-half reliability simply involves computing scores based on half of the

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 518

11/18/13 12:04 PM

37.4 alpha RelIabIlIty

519

items and scores based on the other half of the items. The correlation between the scores for these two halves is the split-half reliability (more or less, but read on). There are no rules for deciding which of the items should be in which half. There are common practices, however. Odd–even reliability is based on taking the odd-numbered items (1, 3, 5, etc.) as one set and the even-numbered items (2, 4, 6, etc.) as the other set. Alternatively, the first half of the items could be correlated with the second half. But there would be nothing against selecting the halves at random.

Explaining statistics 37.1

how the split-half reliability works Taking the data in Table 37.1, we could sum items 1 and 2 for the total of the first half and sum items 3 and 4 for the total of the second half. The correlation between the two halves is 0.477. There is a further step. The difficulty is that we are correlating a scale half the length of our original scale with another scale half the length of our scale. Because of this, the reliability will be lower than for the full length scale. Fortunately, it is quite easy to compute the reliability of a full scale from the reliability of half of the scale using the following formula: full scale reliability =

n × known reliability 1 + [ (n − 1) × known reliability]

where n is the ratio by which the number of items is to be increased or decreased. Since we know the reliability of the half scale (rhh) is 0.477, the full scale reliability is: full scale reliability =

2 × 0.4771 0.954 = = 0 .6 62 1 + 0.477 1.477 477

Thus the value of the split-half reliability is 0.65 when corrected to the full scale length. Standard computer statistics packages such as SPSS Statistics can do most of the hard work for you.

Reporting the results The results of this analysis may be written up as follows: ‘The split-half reliability of the scale was found to be 0.65. This is a somewhat low value but given the exploratory nature of this research, the scale was nevertheless employed.’ (As a rule of thumb, a value of about 0.7 or above would generally be seen as adequate evidence of reliability for general use.)

37.4

Alpha reliability There is a problem with split-half reliability – its value will depend on which items are selected for each half. The odd–even reliability will not be the same as that found by comparing the first half of the items with the second half, for example. There is an obvious solution: calculate every possible split-half reliability having every possible combination of items in each half and then simply take the average of these. The average of all possible split-half reliabilities from a scale is known as coefficient alpha. We will calculate this from first principles and an alternative approach based on the analysis of variance.

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 519

11/18/13 12:04 PM

520

CHAPTER 37  Reliability in scales and measurement: Consistency and agreement

Table 37.4 contains all of the possible ways of splitting four items into two halves. There are only three different ways of doing this with our short scale: ●

The total of items 1 and 2 compared with the total of items 3 and 4.



The total of items 1 and 3 compared with the total of items 2 and 4.



The total of items 1 and 4 compared with the total of items 2 and 3.

The reliability coefficients for these three different possibilities are to be found in Table 37.5. The average of the split-half coefficients corrected (adjusted) for length is coefficient alpha. So the average of 0.642 + 0.844 + 0.946 (or coefficient alpha) is 0.81. It is generally accepted that a coefficient alpha of 0.7 or above is satisfactory for psychological research. This calculation may be feasible with a short scale of four items and a sample of ten individuals, but what, say, if the scale consisted of 100 items? The number of ways of sorting these 100 items into two separate sets of 50 is huge. Obviously the conceptually correct approach given so far would take too much computation time. The alternative hand-computation method is not quite so cumbersome but still time-consuming. Basically it involves carrying out one-way analysis of variance for correlated scores on the data on each of the items. Thus the data would look like Table 37.6. Following through the procedure described in Explaining statistics 22.1 would lead to the ANOVA

Table 37.4 Person

Scores for all possible split-halves from four items Split-half version 1

Split-half version 2

Items 1 + 2

Items 3 + 4

Items 1 + 3

Items 2 + 4

Items 1 + 4

Items 2 + 3

1

4

11

6

9

7

8

2

3

3

3

3

4

2

3

2

2

2

2

2

2

4

7

6

9

4

7

6

5

10

5

9

6

8

7

6

9

11

10

10

11

9

7

9

5

7

7

6

8

8

3

3

4

2

3

3

9

3

2

2

3

2

3

10

2

4

3

3

3

3

Table 37.5

Correlations between split-halves and with corrections for shortened length Pearson correlation

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 520

Split-half version 3

Corrected for scale length

Items 1 + 2 with items 3 + 4

0.477

0.642

Items 1 + 3 with items 2 + 4

0.730

0.844

Items 1 + 4 with items 2 + 3

0.898

0.946

11/18/13 12:04 PM

37.4  Alpha reliability 521

Table 37.6

Data on four-item questionnaire for ten cases arranged as for correlated one-way ANOVA

Person

Item 1

Item 2

Item 3

Item 4

1

1

3

5

6

2

2

1

1

2

3

1

1

1

1

4

5

2

4

2

5

6

4

3

2

6

5

4

5

6

7

4

5

3

2

8

2

1

2

1

9

1

2

1

1

1

1

2

2

2.8

2.4

2.7

2.5

10 Cell mean

Table 37.7

ANOVA summary table on four-item questionnaire data

Source of variation

Degrees of freedom

Mean square (or variance estimate)

F-ratio

Significance

1.00

3

not needed

not needed

not needed

Between people (i.e. individual differences)

70.60

9

7.84

Error (i.e. residual)

40.00

27

1.48

Between treatments (i.e. between items)

Sum of squares

summary table presented in Table 37.7. Values from this table are then substituted in the following computational formula for coefficient alpha: coef 1cient alpha = =

between-people variance − error v ariance between-people variance 7.84 − 1.48 6.36 = 0.81 = 7.84 7.84

This would be generally accepted as evidence of a satisfactory level of internal consistency since coefficients alpha above 0.7 are regarded as sufficient. The results of this analysis may be written up as follows: ‘Coefficient alpha was calculated for the scale and found to be 0.81 which is generally accepted to be satisfactory.’ It should be fairly obvious that the hand calculation of coefficient alpha even with this ANOVA method has little to recommend it. It might be useful to anyone who has access to a computer program for the correlated ANOVA but not to one which computes coefficient alpha directly. It need hardly be said that the use of a computer package such as SPSS Statistics which includes coefficient alpha is highly recommended.

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 521

11/18/13 12:04 PM

522

CHAPTER 37  Reliability in scales and measurement: Consistency and agreement

37.5 Agreement among raters Not all research involves psychological scales. Some research involves ratings by a pair of judges or even a panel of judges or assessors. Sometimes rating is used because it is felt that self-completion questionnaires might be inappropriate. Let us take the concept of dangerousness, i.e. the risk posed to members of the public by the release of sex offenders or psychiatric hospital patients. One might be very unhappy about using selfcompletion questionnaires in these circumstances. It might be considered preferable to have expert clinical psychologists, forensic psychologists and psychiatrists interview the sex offenders or patients to assess the dangerousness of these people on release into the community. Let us assume that we have one clinical psychologist, one forensic psychologist and one psychiatrist who are used in a study of 12 sex offenders. Having interviewed each offender, read all case notes and obtained any further information they required, each of the three professionals rates each offender on a three-point dangerousness index: ●

a rating of 1 means that there is no risk to the public



a rating of 2 means that there is a moderate risk to the public



a rating of 3 means that there is a high risk to the public.

Their ratings of the 12 offenders are shown in Table 37.8. Table 37.9 shows the Pearson correlations between the ratings of the three profes­ sionals. The figures seem to suggest a very high level of relationship between the forensic psychologist’s and the psychiatrist’s ratings. A correlation of 0.83 is, after all, a very strong relationship. The difficulty with this only becomes apparent when we examine Table 37.10 which gives agreements between the forensic psychologist and the psychi­ atrist. This is constructed by tabulating the forensic psychologist’s ratings against those of the psychiatrist. The frequencies in the diagonal represent agreements, all other frequencies represent a degree of disagreement.

Table 37.8

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 522

The data from the three professionals for each of the 12 sex offenders Clinical psychologist

Forensic psychologist

Psychiatrist

Offender 1

2

3

3

Offender 2

3

3

3

Offender 3

3

3

3

Offender 4

1

1

1

Offender 5

2

1

2

Offender 6

3

3

3

Offender 7

1

2

3

Offender 8

1

3

3

Offender 9

2

2

3

Offender 10

3

3

3

Offender 11

3

3

3

Offender 12

2

3

3

11/18/13 12:04 PM

37.5  Agreement among raters 523

Correlations between the ratings of various professions

Table 37.9



Forensic psychologist

Psychiatrist

Clinical psychologist

0.55

0.44

Forensic psychologist



0.83

Table 37.10

Agreements and disagreements between the forensic psychologist and the psychiatrist on ratings of sex offenders



Forensic psychologist’s ratings

Psychiatrist’s ratings 1

2

3

1

1

1

0

2

0

0

2

3

0

0

8

At first sight it still might appear that there is strong agreement between the two sets of ratings. A total of 9 out of the 12 ratings suggest perfect agreement. So what is the problem? A closer examination of Table 37.10 suggests that virtually all of the agreement occurs when the two experts rate the sex offender as a high risk to the public (rating 3). For the other two ratings they agree only one time out of four. This is a much lower level of agreement. Of course, if the experts rated all of the offenders as a high risk to the public then the agreement would be perfect – although they would not appear to be discriminating between levels of risk. If it were decided to release only sex offenders rated as a low risk to the public, only one sex offender would be released on the basis of the combined ratings of the psychiatrist and forensic psychologist. In other words, correlation coefficients are not very helpful when the exact agreement of raters is required. The index of agreement between raters needs to have the following characteristics: ●

It provides an index of the extent of overlap of ratings.



It should be sensitive to the problem that agreement is rather meaningless if both raters are using only one rating and do not vary their ratings.

Kappa is a useful index of agreement between a pair of raters since it is responsive to both of these things. The kappa coefficient is calculated from the following formula: kappa =

total frequency of agreement − expected total frequency of agreement by chance number of th hings rated − expected total frequency of agreement by chance

Kappa can take negative values if the raters agree at less than chance level. It is zero if there is no agreement greater or lesser than chance. Coefficients approaching +1.00 indicate very good agreement between the raters.

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 523

11/18/13 12:04 PM

524

CHAPTER 37 RelIabIlIty In SCaleS and meaSuRement: COnSIStenCy and agReement

Explaining statistics 37.2

how kappa coefficient works The above data on the ratings of the forensic psychologist and the psychiatrist will be used to calculate kappa for their ratings.

Step 1

Draw up a crosstabulation table of the data for the two raters and insert the marginal totals (i.e. the sum of frequencies for each row, the sum of frequencies for each column and the overall sum). This is shown in Table 37.11.

agreements and disagreements between the forensic psychologist and the psychiatrist on ratings of sex offenders with marginal totals added

Table 37.11

Forensic psychologist’s ratings

Psychiatrist’s ratings

Marginal totals

1

2

3

1

1

1

0

2

0

0

2

2

3

0

0

8

8

1

1

10

total = 12

marginal totals

2

Step 2

Calculate the frequencies of agreement. These are the frequencies in the diagonal of Table 37.11. They have been given in bold. So the frequency of agreements is 1 + 0 + 8 = 9.

Step 3

Calculate the expected frequency of agreement by firstly calculating the following for each of the diagonals: expected frequency =

column totall × rrow total total

Thus the expected frequency of agreement for ratings of 3 is the product of the column total of 10 and the row total of 8 divided by the overall total of 12. This is 80 ÷ 12 or 6.667. Table 37.12 contains the results of these calculations.

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 524

11/18/13 12:04 PM

37.5 agReement amOng RateRS

expected frequencies for agreement

Table 37.12

Forensic psychologist’s ratings

Psychiatrist’s ratings

1 1

2

3 2

0.167

2 6.667

3 marginal totals

Marginal totals

0.167

2

Step 4

525

1

1

10

8 total = 12

The expected total frequency of agreement by chance is therefore 0.167 + 0.167 + 6.667 = 7.001.

Step 5

We can then substitute the values in the formula: kappa = =

totall ffrequency of agreementt − eexpected totaall frequency of agreement by chance number of thi hings rated − expected totall ffrequency of agreemeen nt by chance 9 − 7.001 1.999 = = 0 .4 40 12 − 7.001 4.999

Interpreting the results Notice that although the actual agreement seems high at 9 of the 12 ratings, coefficient kappa implies fairly low agreement. This reflects the relative lack of variability in the expert’s ratings and the tendency for both to rate the offenders as 3 rather than any other value. Consequently, we can appreciate that coefficient kappa is superior to the simple proportion of agreement in assessing the reliability of ratings.

Reporting the results The results of this analysis can be written up as follows: ‘Coefficient kappa was calculated on the relationship between the forensic psychologist’s and the psychiatrist’s ratings of dangerousness. Despite there being a high level of agreement overall, it was found that kappa was only 0.40, suggesting that much of the apparent agreement was in fact due to both professionals using the highest dangerousness rating much of the time.’

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 525

11/18/13 12:04 PM

526

CHAPTER 37  Reliability in scales and measurement: Consistency and agreement

Research examples Reliability using Cronbach’s alpha and kappa Helvik, Engedal, Skancke and Selbæk (2011) carried out a psychometric study of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale for the medically hospitalised elderly. Few studies had been carried out using this scale employing clinical samples of elderly. The participants in the research were 484 elderly patients between 65 and 101 years of age. The Coefficient Alpha for the entire scale was 0.78 and for the depression subscale of the test it was 0.71. Coefficient Alphas at this level are generally considered to indicate satisfactory internal consistency by researchers. Ingravallo and co-workers (2008) discuss impairment of job performance due to narcolepsy and indicate that there is a lack of accepted criteria for its assessment. Narcolepsy is a chronic neurological condition in which the brain cannot maintain daytime–night-time sleep cycles properly and sleepiness can occur frequently in circumstances not conducive to employment. In Italy there are benefits available but in order for the sufferer to receive them their case has to go through a medical commission. Fifteen narcolepsy claimees were assessed by four different commissions in simulated assessments. The different commissions were unaware of the decision making of the other commissions in the study. Inter-observer reliability using kappa ranged from 0.10 to 0.35 for decisions concerning disability benefits. The raw agreement levels for the pairs of medical commissions ranged from 20.0% to 53.4%. The lack of agreed criteria for identifying narcolepsy is an obvious problem. Laaksonen, Lindors, Knekt and Aalberg (2012) report work on an interview-based scale concerning suitability for psychotherapy which was intended to assess suitability for long- and short-term therapy. The scale was used with 326 psychiatric outpatients to obtain baseline measures. The usefulness of the Suitability for Psychotherapy Scale to assess changes in symptoms at a one year follow-up was also measured. Coefficient kappa was used to measure the extent of agreement between interviewers and a reference decision. In general, the agreement level was in the range of fair to good. Mostly the kappa coefficients ranged from .41 to .62 between interviewers and between interviewers and the reference. Vassari and Crosby (2008) were concerned about the internal consistency reliability of the well-established UCLA Loneliness Scale (Revised). This scale has been widely used and it is associated with several distressing or negative psychological states. The authors were interested in knowing the reliability of this measure over a wide range of studies using the Loneliness Scale. Eighty studies were found which reported Cronbach coefficient alpha reliability coefficients. They used a variety of meta-analysis known as Reliability Generalisation to do this. The mean internal consistency reliability coefficient across all of the samples in the studies was 0.87 indicating a good level of internal consistency. However, the variability of alpha was quite considerable over studies and ranged from 0.53 to 0.95. Further analysis suggested that coefficient alphas varied according to 1) type of article, 2) where the report was published and 3) the standard deviations involved.

Key points ●

Although the methods employed in calculating internal reliability are straightforward, great care is needed to differentiate between internal reliability as assessed by the methods described in this chapter and measures of external reliability which are very different. External reliability includes the correlation between scores on a measure at two different points in time (i.e. test–retest reliability).



The difference between a correlation between scores and agreement between scores is very important. Remember that there can be a strong correlation between two variables with absolutely no match in the scores.

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 526

11/18/13 12:04 PM

Computer analysis 527



Computer analysis Cronbach’s alpha and kappa using SPSS

Figure 37.2

SPSS Statistics steps for Cronbach’s alpha internal reliability and kappa

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

It is generally accepted that a value of Alpha of about 0.7 approximately or larger indicates that a scale has satisfactory reliability. See main text of chapter for more on item analysis using item-total statistics.



We would write something like: ‘The Alpha reliability of the scale was .81 which indicates satisfactory internal reliability for the scale.’

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 527

11/18/13 12:04 PM

528

CHAPTER 37  Reliability in scales and measurement: Consistency and agreement

Screenshot 37.1

The data

Screenshot 37.2

Select the test

Screenshot 37.3

Select variables

Screenshot 37.4

Select options

Screenshot 37.5

The value of alpha output

Screenshot 37.6

Item–total statistics

Recommended further reading Tinsley, H.E.A., & Weiss, D.J. (1975). Interrater reliability and agreement of subjective judgments. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 22, 358–376.

M37_HOWI0749_06_SE_C37.indd 528

11/18/13 12:04 PM

Chapter 38

Confidence intervals

Overview ●

Confidence intervals are an alternative way of conceptualising inferential statistics that stresses the uncertainty of statistical data. In recent years, they have received some enthusiastic support.



A confidence interval is essentially a range (of means, differences between means, correlations, etc.) within which the population value (based on our sample data) is most likely to lie. That is, instead of estimating the population value as a single point value (such as the population mean equals 6.0), the confidence interval approach estimates that the population mean will lie between 4.5 and 7.5 based on the characteristics of the sample, for example.



The confidence interval is usually calculated as the 95% confidence interval. This is the interval between the largest and the smallest values which cut off the most extreme 2.5% of values in either direction. In other words, the 95% confidence interval covers the most central 95% of values.



The calculation of the confidence interval involves the calculation of the standard error. Since for any given sample size, tables of the t-distribution are available which indicate how many standard errors embrace the middle 95% of values, the 95% confidence interval is easily found.

Preparation Read the previous discussions of confidence intervals in Chapters 9 and 10. Revise the concepts of standard error and sampling distributions from Chapters 12 and 14.

M38_HOWI0749_06_SE_C38.indd 529

11/13/13 5:27 PM

530

CHAPTER 38  Confidence intervals

38.1 Introduction The concept of confidence intervals has been discussed briefly in earlier chapters. Although confidence intervals have been used in psychological statistics for many years, their greater use has been advocated strongly in recent years. More radically, it has been proposed that confidence intervals should replace statistical significance testing. Whatever the merits of the argument for this, both confidence intervals and significance testing based on point estimates are informative approaches to statistical analysis and likely to coexist for a good many years. This chapter provides some information on the com­ putation of confidence intervals for a variety of statistics already discussed. Despite the fact that any measure based on a sample has a confidence interval in theory, methods of calculating confidence intervals are not readily available for many statistical procedures. However, the availability of bootstrapping methods (Box 19.1) makes such calculations much easier because with bootstrapping there is no need to develop statistical theory giving the confidence intervals for a particular statistic. Instead, purely empirical methods can be used to calculate the confidence interval. Confidence intervals concern the estimates of population characteristics (parameters) based on a sample or samples taken from that population. The characteristics of samples tend to vary somewhat from the characteristics of the population from which they came – and from each other (Chapter 14). Consequently, estimates of the characteristics of a population based on a sample drawn from that population are unlikely to be exact. Nevertheless, they remain the best estimates we can have when ignorant of the exact details of the population. In previous chapters, we have used point estimates of population parameters based on sample statistics. A point estimate is merely a single figure estimate as opposed to a range. Thus if the mean of a sample is 5.3 then the point estimate of the mean of the population is 5.3. Since this point estimate is only our best guess from the char­ acteristics of the sample, usually it only approximates the true population mean at best. The alternative to point estimates, the confidence interval approach, acknowledges the approximate nature of the point estimates more directly. Confidence intervals give the range of values likely to include the population value. This range of likely values is called the confidence interval since it reflects the range of values likely to include the true population mean (if we only knew this). Thus, instead of saying that our estimate of the population mean is 5.3, we say that the population mean is likely or almost certain to be in the range 4.0–6.6. By expressing our inference or estimate in this way, we reinforce the notion of uncertainty as to the precise value. So a confidence interval is simply the range of values of a statistic such as the mean or correlation which is likely to contain the true population mean or correlation. The size of the confidence interval will depend on the variability of scores. The more variable the scores in a sample, the larger the confidence interval has to be for any level of confidence. There is an obvious problem with confidence intervals. We can never be absolutely cer­ tain how different a sample mean is from the mean of the population from which it was drawn (if we are basing our estimate on a sample). Consequently, the following strategy is adopted. We state the range of sample means that includes (usually) the most likely 95% of sample means drawn at random from the population. In other words, the 95% confidence interval is the range of values we are 95% certain includes the ‘true’ population mean. Chapter 14 explained how we take the characteristics of a sample to infer the most likely characteristics of the population from which that sample was taken. Furthermore, we can even calculate the distributions of samples taken from that inferred population. Remember that the standard error is the usual index of the amount of variability in sample means drawn at random from a population. Standard error is simply the stand­ ard deviation of sample means. The calculation of standard error is a crucial phase in estimating confidence intervals for all parametric tests.

M38_HOWI0749_06_SE_C38.indd 530

11/13/13 5:27 PM

38.1  Introduction 531

Normal distribution theory (Chapter 12) tells us that for large samples, 95% of sample means lie within plus or minus 1.96 standard deviations from the population mean. Thus if the standard error for samples has been calculated as 2.6, then 95% of sample means lie between −5.096 and +5.096 (1.96 × 2.6 = 5.096) of the mean of our sample (i.e. the estimate of the population mean). If the sample mean is 10.00 then the confidence interval is 10.00 ± 5.096. That is, the confidence interval is between 4.904 and 15.096. Since this covers 95% of the most likely sample means, it is known as the 95% confidence interval. In other words, the 95% confidence interval is 4.90 to 15.10. However, this is approx­ imate where the sample size is small. Confidence intervals can be set at other levels such as 99%. The more stringent 99% confidence interval involves multiplying the standard error by 2.576 = 2.576 × 2.6 = 6.698. The resulting 99% confidence interval would be 10.00 (the sample mean) ± 6.698, or 3.30 to 16.70. So the more confident we want to be, the larger the confidence interval is. We can use tables of the z-distribution to work out other confidence intervals, but the 95% and 99% are fairly conventional. However, with small samples the z-distribution does not work perfectly. It is more usual to use the distribution of t (which is identical to that of z for large samples). With small samples, the value of t corresponding to our chosen confidence interval would be obtained from Table 38.1. This is distributed by the degrees of freedom. Thus if the degrees of freedom for a particular sample were 25, then the value of t for 95% confidence is 2.06 (from Table 38.1). So the confidence interval would be 2.06 ± 2.6 on either side of the estimated population mean. That is, the 95% confidence interval would be 4.64 to 15.36. The degrees of freedom will vary according to the statistical estimate in question. Sometimes the concept confidence limits is used. Confidence limits are merely the extreme values of the confidence interval. In the above example, the 95% confidence limits are 4.64 and 15.36. While this introduction explains confidence intervals in principle, their calculation varies from this pattern for some statistics. Figure 38.1 gives the key steps to consider in understanding confidence intervals.

Figure 38.1

M38_HOWI0749_06_SE_C38.indd 531

Conceptual steps for understanding confidence intervals

11/13/13 5:27 PM

532

CHAPTER 38  Confidence intervals

Table of t-values for 95% and 99% confidence intervals

Table 38.1

Degrees of freedom

t for 95% confidence

t for 99% confidence

1

12.71

63.66

2

4.30

9.93

3

3.18

5.84

4

2.78

4.60

5

2.57

4.03

6

2.45

3.71

7

2.37

3.50

8

2.31

3.36

9

2.26

3.25

10

2.23

3.17

11

2.20

3.11

12

2.18

3.06

13

2.16

3.01

14

2.15

2.98 2.95

15

2.13

16

2.12

2.92

17

2.11

2.90

18

2.10

2.88

19

2.09

2.86

20

2.09

2.85

25

2.06

2.79

30

2.04

2.75

35

2.03

2.72

40

2.02

2.70

45

2.01

2.69

50

2.01

2.68

60

2.00

2.66

70

1.99

2.65

80

1.99

2.64

90

1.99

2.63

100

1.98

2.63



1.96

2.58

Note: If the required number of degrees of freedom is missing, take the nearest lower number.

M38_HOWI0749_06_SE_C38.indd 532

11/13/13 5:27 PM

38.2  The relationship between significance and confidence intervals 533

38.2 The relationship between significance and confidence intervals At first sight, statistical significance and confidence intervals appear dissimilar concepts. This is incorrect since they are both based on much the same inferential process. Remember that in significance testing we usually test the null hypothesis of no relation­ ship between two variables. This usually boils down to a zero (or near-zero) correlation or to a difference of zero (or near-zero) between sample means. If the confidence interval does not contain this zero value then the obtained sample mean is statistically significant at 100% minus the confidence level. So if the 95% confidence interval is 2.30 to 8.16 but the null hypothesis would predict the population value of the statistic to be 0.00, then the null hypothesis is rejected at the 5% level of significance. In other words, con­ fidence intervals contain enough information to judge statistical significance. However, statistical significance alone does not contain enough information to calculate confidence intervals.

Explaining statistics 38.1

How confidence intervals for a population mean based on a single sample works Step 1

Calculate the standard error of the scores in the sample. The stages in doing this are given in Explaining statistics 12.1. You will also need to calculate the mean of the sample and the degrees of freedom (i.e. sample size − 1).

Step 2

For the data in Table 12.1, the standard error is 0.58, the estimated population mean (the sample mean) is 5.00, and the degrees of freedom are 6 − 1 = 5 degrees of freedom.

Step 3

Decide what confidence level you require. We will use the 95% level. This is the minimum value of confidence in general use. If it was especially important that your confidence interval included the true population mean than you could use the 99% level or even the 99.9% level.

Step 4

Use Table 38.1 to find the value of t corresponding to the 95% confidence level. You need the row for the appropriate number of degrees of freedom (i.e. N − 1 = 5). This value of t is 2.57. Table 38.1 is merely a version of the table of the t-distribution that appears elsewhere in the book. It is included as some will find it initially less confusing to be able to look up the values directly.

Step 5

Calculate the confidence interval. It is the sample mean ± (t × the standard error). Therefore, the 95% confidence interval for the population mean is 5.00 ± (2.57 × 0.58). This gives us a 95% confidence interval of 3.51 to 6.49.

Reporting the results The results of this analysis may be written up as follows: ‘The 95% confidence interval for the population mean was 3.51 to 6.49. As this interval does not include 0.00 then the null hypothesis that the sample comes from a population with a mean of 0.00 can be rejected at the 5% level of significance.’

M38_HOWI0749_06_SE_C38.indd 533

11/13/13 5:27 PM

534

CHAPTER 38  Confidence intervals

Explaining statistics 38.2

How confidence intervals for the unrelated t-test work Step 1

As most of the major steps in calculating the confidence interval involve steps in the calculation of the unrelated t-test, use Explaining statistics 14.1 to calculate the necessary values.

Step 2

Make a note of the difference between the two sample means, the degrees of freedom (N + N − 2), and the standard error of the difference between two sample means. For the example in Explaining statistics 14.1 (Table 14.9), the difference between the sample means = 3.917, the degrees of freedom = 20 and the standard error = 1.392.

Step 3

Decide what level of confidence you require. This time we will use the 99% level of confidence.

Step 4

From Table 38.1, the t-value for 99% confidence with 20 degrees of freedom = 2.85.

Step 5

The confidence interval is obtained by taking the difference between the two sample means ± (t × the standard error). Thus the 99% confidence interval for the population of differences between sample means = 3.917 ± (2.85 × 1.392). Therefore the 99% confidence interval is 3.917 ± 3.97, which gives a 99% confidence interval of −0.05 to 7.89.

Reporting the results The results of this analysis can be written up as follows: ‘The 99% confidence interval for the difference in emotionality scores in two-parent and lone-parent families is −0.05 to 7.89. Since the null hypothesis holds that this difference is 0.00 then we can accept the null hypothesis at the 1% level of significance since the confidence interval includes the value 0.00. The hypothesis that emotionality is different in two-parent and lone-parent families is not supported at the 1% level of significance.’

Explaining statistics 38.3

How confidence intervals for the related t-test work Step 1

Follow the calculation of the related t-test as described in Explaining statistics 13.1. We will use these data to obtain the 95% confidence interval for the difference between the means.

Step 2

Make a note of the difference between the sample means, the degrees of freedom and the standard error for your data. Explaining statistics 13.1 yields a value of the difference between the sample means of −1.50, a standard error of the difference of 0.756 with 7 degrees of freedom.

Step 3

Decide what level of confidence you require. This time we are using the 95% level of confidence.

M38_HOWI0749_06_SE_C38.indd 534

11/13/13 5:27 PM

38.2 the relatIOnShIp between SIgnIfICanCe and COnfIdenCe IntervalS

535

Step 4

From Table 38.1, the t­value for 95% confidence with 7 degrees of freedom = 2.37.

Step 5

The confidence interval is obtained by taking the difference between the two sample means ± the (t − value × the standard error); i.e. −1.50 ± (2.37 × 0.756) = −1.10 ± 1.79 = −3.29 to 0.29

Step 6

Thus the 95% confidence interval for the population of differences between sample means is −1.94 to 1.64.

reporting the results The results of this analysis can be written up as follows: ‘The 95% confidence interval for the difference in eye contact at six months and nine months was −3.29 to 0.29. According to the null hypothesis, this difference should be 0.00. Consequently, as this value is included in the 95% confidence interval then the null hypothesis is supported and the alternative hypothesis that eye contact is related to age is rejected.’

Explaining statistics 38.4

how confidence intervals for the pearson correlation coefficient work Step 1

The calculation of the Pearson correlation coefficient is described in Explaining statistics 8.1. Work through these steps for your data or compute the value of r using a computer.

Step 2

Make a note of the value of the correlation coefficient and the sample size. For the data in Table 8.1, the value of the correlation coefficient is −0.90 and the sample size is 10. We do not require the degrees of freedom for calculating the confidence interval for a Pearson correlation coefficient.

Step 3

To calculate the confidence interval, it is necessary to convert the correlation coefficient to its zr using Table 36.5. Note that zr is the Fisher normalised correlation coefficient. This table gives a value of zr for a correlation of −0.90 as −1.472. The negative sign is added because the correlation is negative.

Step 4

The standard deviation of zr is obtained using the formula: standard deviation of z =

1 N −3

Given that in our example the sample size N is 10, the standard deviation according to this formula is: standard deviation of z =

1 10 − 3

=

1 7

=

1 = 0.378 2.646



M38_HOWI0749_06_SE_C38.indd 535

11/13/13 5:27 PM

536

CHAPTER 38  Confidence intervals

This standard deviation is distributed as for z so that the 95% confidence interval is 1.96× the standard deviation. Thus the 95% confidence interval of zr is the value of zr for the correlation coefficient ±1.96 × 0.378. That is, in our example, −1.472 ± 0.741. Therefore the 95% confidence interval for zr is −0.731 to −2.213.

Step 5

The above is the confidence interval for zr rather than for the original correlation coefficient. We can use Table 36.5 to convert this zr back to the range of correlation coefficients. Thus the 95% confidence interval for the correlation coefficient is −0.62 to −0.97.

Interpreting the results You will notice that this confidence interval is not symmetrical around the sample correlation of −0.90. The correlation coefficient is not a linear variable so it cannot be added and divided as if it were. Hence the transformation to zr which has linear characteristics.

Reporting the results The results of this analysis can be written up as follows: ‘The 95% confidence interval for the Pearson correlation between musical and mathematical ability was −0.62 to −0.97. The null hypothesis suggests that this relationship will be 0.00. Since the value under the null hypothesis was not included in the confidence interval, the null hypothesis of no relationship between musical and mathematical ability was rejected in favour of the alternative hypothesis that there is a negative correlation between mathematical and musical ability.’

38.3 Regression There are several confidence intervals for even a simple regression analysis since regres­ sion involves several estimates of population parameters – the slope of the regression line, the cut-point for the vertical axis and the predicted score from scores on the X variable.

Explaining statistics 38.5

How confidence intervals for a predicted score work Step 1

Carry out the simple regression analysis according to Explaining statistics 9.1. This will give the slope and the intercept (cut-point) of the regression line. These can be used to calculate the most likely value of variable Y from a particular value of variable X. For a value of X = 8, the best prediction of Y is 3.37 for the data in Table 9.2.

Step 2

Calculate the Pearson correlation between variable X and variable Y in Table 9.2 using Explaining statistics 8.1. This gives r as −0.90.

M38_HOWI0749_06_SE_C38.indd 536

11/13/13 5:27 PM

38.4 Other COnfIdenCe IntervalS

537

Step 3

Calculate the standard deviation of the Y variable scores using Explaining statistics 6.1. The standard deviation of the Y scores is 1.75.

Step 4

Using the information calculated in the previous three steps, the standard error of the estimate of Y from a particular value of X is given by the following formula:

standard error of estimate off Y = SD of of Y ×

N (1 − r 2 ) N−2

= 1 .7 5 ×

10(1 − 0.90 2 ) 10 − 2

= 1 .7 5 ×

10(1 − 0.81) 8

= 1 .7 75 × 0.2375 = 1.75 × 0.4873 = 0.853

Step 5

This standard error can be converted to the confidence interval by multiplying the value of the standard error by the appropriate value of t. The degrees of freedom for this are N − 2. Table 38.1 indicates that the t­value for N − 2 or 3 degrees of freedom is 3.18 for the 95% confidence interval. This gives us a value of the confidence interval around the predicted Y score of 3.37 of ±0.85 × 3.18 = ±2.70. Thus we can be 95% sure that the population value of Y predicted from X is within the range of 0.67 to 6.07.

Interpreting the results Of course, the confidence interval will vary numerically according to which X score is being used to predict Y. The size of the interval between the upper and lower confidence limits though does not vary. This is because the standard error is an average for all estimated Y scores.

reporting the results The results of this analysis can be written up as follows: ‘The 95% confidence interval for predicting musical ability from maths score was 0.89 to 5.85 for a point­prediction of 3.37.’

38.4 Other confidence intervals In theory, any statistic (i.e. characteristic of a sample) will have a sampling distribution and, hence, a confidence interval. In practice, however, these can be obscure or unavail­ able though bootstrap statistics may make their estimation possible.

M38_HOWI0749_06_SE_C38.indd 537

11/13/13 5:27 PM

538

CHAPTER 38  Confidence intervals

Research examples Confidence intervals Confidence intervals are available for a substantial number of statistical methods. The basic principle is the same in all cases but expect to come across them in relation to statistics about which you know little. You should, nevertheless, be able to interpret them as a confidence interval is an indication of the spread of samples on that statistic. Ang and Huan (2006) tested whether depression mediated the relation between academic stress and thoughts of killing oneself (suicidal ideation) in adolescents. They carried out a simple regression of academic stress with depression and suicidal ideation and a multiple regression of academic stress and depression with suicidal ideation. They presented the 95% confidence intervals for the unstandardised regression coefficients. Hannaford and his colleagues (1996) evaluated an educational package which was designed to help general practitioners identify patients with depression. There was a 7% decrease in the number of cases of depression that were missed after receiving the educational package. The 95% confidence interval for this decrease varied from 2 to 12%. Huisman and her colleagues (2010) were interested in whether psychiatric diagnosis, gender and status as in- or out-patient were more likely to kill themselves using a particular method. They used multinomial logistic regression to determine which of these variables were related to suicide method when examined together. The dependent variables were the four categories of 1) self-poisoning, 2) jumping before a train, 3) jumping from a high place and 4) all other methods apart from hanging, which as the most common method was chosen to be the reference category. They reported the odds ratio of being in these categories together with the 95% confidence interval for the odds ratio. So, for example, the odds ratio for jumping before a train compared to hanging for patients with bipolar disorders was 5.53 with a 95% confidence interval of 1.23 to 24.82.

Key points ●

Confidence intervals for many statistical estimates are not easily obtained. Do not expect to find unusual confidence intervals explained in other than relatively difficult sources.



Standard statistical packages routinely calculate standard errors from which confidence intervals are relatively easy to derive.

M38_HOWI0749_06_SE_C38.indd 538

11/13/13 5:27 PM

Computer analysis 539



Computer analysis Examples of SPSS output containing confidence intervals SPSS includes confidence intervals in much of its output. This is routinely done and so no special computer steps are needed generally. The following screenshots give a few examples of confidence intervals in SPSS output.

Screenshot 38.1

Simple regression

Screenshot 38.2

Related t-test

Screenshot 38.3

Unrelated t-test

Screenshot 38.4

One-way ANOVA

Screenshot 38.5

Multiple comparison tests

Screenshot 38.6

One-way ANCOVA

M38_HOWI0749_06_SE_C38.indd 539

11/13/13 5:27 PM

Chapter 39

The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables Overview ● A

moderator effect is where the size of the relationship between one variable and another variable is different for the different values of a third variable. The moderating effect is also known as an interaction effect.



Where all of the variables are score variables then it is recommended that hierarchical multiple regression is used to identify interactions which indicate moderator effects. This method makes full use of the information contained in the scores.



The interaction term is created by multiplying the two predictors together. It is recommended that the means of the two predictors should be made to be zero by the predictors being centred or standardised. One reason for this is to reduce the size of the correlations between the predictors and the interaction.



The two predictors are entered in the first step (block 1) of the hierarchical multiple regression and the interaction in the second step (block 2). There is a moderator effect if the interaction explains a significant proportion of the variance in the criterion.

● To

interpret the interaction, values of the criterion are predicted for widely separated values of the two predictors such as their mean and one standard deviation above and below their mean.



Whether these regression coefficients differ significantly from zero can be determined but not whether the slopes differ significantly from each other.

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 540

11/13/13 5:27 PM

39.1  Introduction 541



Moderator effects of categorical variables on a continuous or score variable can be tested with the analysis of variance (ANOVA). If an interaction is found between the independent and moderator variables then this indicates a moderator effect, but it remains important to determine which group means differ significantly from each other and the direction of these differences.

Preparation You should have a working knowledge of z-scores (Chapter 6), simple regression (Chapter 9), two-way analysis of variance (Chapter 23) and multiple regression (Chapter 32).

39.1 Introduction There are various circumstances in which a relationship between two variables is in some way affected by a third variable. Two types of third variables are mediator vari­ ables and moderator variables. We discussed mediating variables in Chapters 30 and 33, but it is worthwhile reminding ourselves of what a mediator variable is. Take a look at Figure 39.1. It indicates that there is a relationship between the level of stress experi­ enced by an individual and how depressed they feel. The more stress, the more depres­ sion. A mediating variable is a third variable which is responsible for the relationship between the main variables – stress and depression in this case. One reason why stress might lead to depression is that stress reduces one’s available time to engage in close social relationships with friends and family and that it is the absence of close relation­ ships which leads to depression shown in Figure 39.2. In other words, stress in our

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 541

Figure 39.1

Stress and depression

Figure 39.2

Social relationships as the variable moderating between stress and depression

11/13/13 5:27 PM

542

CHAPTER 39  The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables

example does not directly lead to depression but it causes changes in a third variable (social relationships) which then affects depression. In this case, social relationships would be a mediator variable for the relationship between stress and depression. This chapter deals with another type of third variable – moderator variables. These are conceptually quite distinct from mediator variables, though a variable which is a moderator variable may also be a mediating variable in another context. How do you know whether a variable is a moderator variable? Quite simply, if the main relationship you are interested in is different for different levels of the third variable then this third variable is having a moderating effect – and so it is a moderator variable. A simple exam­ ple of a moderator might be gender if it were found to be the case, say, that there is no relationship between stress and depression in women but a strong relationship between the two in men. Gender is having a moderating effect on the relationship between stress and depression. Male and female are different levels of the variable gender. So we would say that gender is a moderator variable in this case. Another possible moderator variable for the relationship between stress and de­ pression might be the variable social support – this refers to the extent to which an individual has family and friends which provide them with a warm, supportive social environment. Figure 39.3 illustrates the interrelationships between stress and depression and social support. Stress, depression and social support, we shall assume, have each been measured using a psychological scale so each variable consists of scores. But just what is the nature of the relationships involved? There are several possible options: ●

stress leads directly to depression



depression leads to stress



both of these are true



stress leads the individual to be more isolated (lack social support) and this lack of social support leads to depression



depression leads the individual to be more isolated (lack social support) which makes them susceptible to stress.

Figure 39.3

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 542

Stress, social support and depression

11/13/13 5:27 PM

39.1  Introduction 543

It is very difficult to decide which of the first three might be the case. However, the last two options are examples of mediating variables – that is, the reason why stress leads to depression is because stress affects social support which then leads to depression. Or a similar argument might apply in which depression affects social support which then leaves the individual susceptible to stress. Partial correlation (Chapter 30) and other statistical techniques (Chapter 33) can help you decide whether social support is mediat­ ing the relationship between stress and depression. However, remember that moderator variables are very different from mediating vari­ ables though they are easily confused semantically unless one is very careful. One would say that social support is a moderating variable if the extent of the relationship between stress and depression is not the same for people with excellent social support networks, people with moderate social support networks and people whose social support net­ works are poor – they have few friends and family who they can turn to in time of dif­ ficulty. This is illustrated in Figure 39.4. As you can see, for that group of individuals who have poor social support there is a strong relationship between stress and depres­ sion. If social support is moderate or excellent, then there is little or no relationship between stress and depression. In other words, then, the relationship between stress and depression depends on the level of social support (excellent, moderate or poor) experi­ enced by participants in the study. In this example, individuals who lack social support seem to be vulnerable to depression when under stress. Those who have moderate or excellent social support networks seem not to be vulnerable to depression when they are stressed. That is, social support has a sort of cushioning effect preventing stress leading to depression. This is perfectly sensible since social support is associated with helping with problems and preventing difficulties from getting worse. So social support as a moderating variable in the relationship between stress and depression would seem to make good psychological sense. Another way of visualising this situation is in terms of Table 39.1. This table indi­ cates that where there is a high level of stress but poor social support then the mean of the depression score is very high. In all other cells the level of depression is much the same. In other words, only where social support is poor do high levels of stress lead to high levels of depression. Of course, the outcome could be more complex than this. Nevertheless, this is the basic situation which leads to the suggestion that there is a moderator variable, social support, different levels of which lead to different relation­ ships between stress and depression. In this example, there is no relationship between stress and depression except in circumstances in which social support is poor.

Figure 39.4

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 543

Social support as a moderator variable in the relationship between stress and depression

11/13/13 5:27 PM

544

CHAPTER 39  The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables

Table 39.1

Mean depression scores for groups formed on the basis of level of social support and level of stress Poor social support

Moderate social support

Excellent social support

Low level of stress

6.20

5.60

5.65

Medium level of stress

6.10

4.60

5.30

12.25

5.60

6.25

High level of stress

Of course, there may well be other potential moderator variables which could be included in the analysis – we simply need to work out what they may be, measure them and then establish that they do play this sort of role. But this does rely on the researcher having bright ideas about likely moderator variables. We may also look for moderator variables (interactions) in circumstances where we expect variable A to be related to variable B but nevertheless find that in reality the relationship between the two variables is weak. It is appropriate in these circumstances to think of the sorts of reasons why we would expect a stronger relation than the one we found. We have mentioned simple instances of this, but gender, age group, occupational group and so forth might be con­ sidered. What is a possibility really depends on what is being studied – and perhaps some insight on the part of the researcher. Figure 39.5 gives the key steps to consider in under­ standing moderator variables.

Figure 39.5

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 544

Conceptual steps for understanding moderator variables

11/13/13 5:27 PM

39.3  The hierarchical multiple regression approach 545

39.2 Statistical approaches to finding moderator effects You may have spotted something – that is, surely what is being referred to as a moderator variable here is part of what we called an interaction in analysis of variance (ANOVA). This is absolutely correct. It also suggests one way of examining one’s data to see if there is a moderator variable – that is, simply carry out a two-way analysis of variance on the data of a sort which resulted in Table 39.1. If there is a significant interaction then there is a moderator effect. Chapter 23 discusses a two-way independent samples ANOVA design which corresponds to Table 39.1. However, it should be noted that if the stress and social support categories are based on score data, then there is information in the data which is being lost by simply classifying the scores into high, medium and low categories. (That is to say, for example, that although the people in the high category would have dif­ ferent scores, this information about order is lost when they have been classified into the high social support category.) This is bad form in statistical analysis. However, if the study had involved variables measured in terms of nominal categories rather than scores, then the ANOVA approach is the accepted approach. Since the data which psychologists collect are usually in the form of scores rather than nominal categories, then a different form of analysis would be preferred in most cases. The alternative method – the one used where the independent variable (e.g. stress) and the moderator variable (social support) are score variables – is based on hierarchical multiple regression which is presented in Chapter 32. In other words, both the ANOVA approach and the hierarchical multiple regression approaches are very substantially the same analyses discussed in other chapters. The big difference is that the way in which we are conceptualising the analysis is somewhat different. To summarise: ●

If all of your variables are score variables then the best way to look for moderator effects is to use the hierarchical multiple regression approach.



If your predictor and moderator variables are measured using a nominal (i.e. category or categorical) classification scheme but your dependent variable is a score, then you can use the ANOVA approach.

However, you might wish to take note of the following: ● Sometimes

researchers use the ANOVA approach where all of their variables are scores. They merely categorise the moderator and the independent variables into high, medium and low categories. You may find this approach intuitively more appealing.



You might be wondering what you can do if all of your variables are nominal categ­ ory ones. Well you can’t apply the two approaches described in this data for the obvious reason that the dependent variable is also a nominal category. You might wish to check out Chapter 41 on log-linear analysis since this can help you deal with these circumstances.

We will discuss the hierarchical multiple regression approach first and then go on to the ANOVA method.



The hierarchical multiple regression approach to identifying moderator effects (or interactions)

39.3

The multiple regression approach to identifying moderator variables involves many of the ideas that were discussed in Chapter 32 on multiple regression and multiple correlation

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 545

11/13/13 5:27 PM

546

CHAPTER 39  The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables

and, to a lesser extent, Chapter 33 on path analysis. If you have read those chapters then there should be few nasty surprises in what follows. However, there are new things in this section, particularly a) the use of standardisation of variables and b) the introduction of a new predictor variable – the interaction. The interaction is where the moderator effect is found. So standardisation and interaction are given particular attention in the follow­ ing discussion. We did not standardise variables for the multiple regressions described in Chapters 32 and 33, so why do we need to now? Standardisation usually means in statistics turning scores into z-scores and this applies in this case. That is, for each of our variables every score is turned into a z-score (using the methods described in Chapter 6, though we will describe the process again in this chapter). There is a technical reason for this standardisa­ tion which boils down to the fact that if this is not done, then the chances of detecting a moderator effect where one exists are reduced. A more detailed explanation depends on understanding how the regression calculation is actually done, so read the following explana­ tion at your peril. In multiple regression where there are two or more predictors, the regres­ sion weights or regression coefficients are calculated setting the value of the other predictor variable at 0. For the raw scores, depending on what is being measured, the value of 0 could be anywhere in the distribution – i.e. it could represent a very big, a medium or a very low score. So it makes sense to involve the more typical scores in the middle of the distribution. So if we have ensured that a score of 0 is equivalent to the middle value of the distribution of scores by using z-scores where the middle of the distribution is 0, then we have ensured that the value of the ‘other’ variable is set at the mid-point of the distribution. The other new thing in this chapter is the use of the interaction term in multiple regres­ sion. Although interactions have been discussed in Chapter 23 in relation to the analysis of variance (ANOVA), we have not previously discussed them in relation to multiple regression. It has to be said that there is a far closer relationship between ANOVA and multiple regression than appears on the surface. And if you understood interaction in terms of ANOVA then this should help you with it in relation to multiple regression. Essentially, the interaction term is created as a new computed variable simply by multiplying the score on one variable (stress) and the score on the moderator variable (social support). The interaction is really a new variable and is treated as such in multiple regression. In order to understand why we standardise in this context, it is informative to com­ pare the correlations between the three predictor variables (independent, moderator and interaction) in their unstandardised and standardised forms. Table 39.2 gives the correla­ tions between the three raw variables involved in the multiple regression and then, separ­ ately, between the three standardised versions of the same variables for our data. It can be seen that the correlations between the two predictor variables and the interaction of the two predictor variables are larger for the raw scores than for the standardised versions of the same variables and their interaction. The correlation between stress and social support for the raw scores is −0.26 and exactly the same for the standardised scores. This is not surprising since these correlations are based on exactly the same data

Table 39.2

Correlations between the variables and their interaction in raw scores and in standardised form Raw scores Social support

Stress Interaction

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 546

Standardised scores Stress

−0.26 0.29

Social support

Stress

−0.26 0.83

−0.00

0.02

11/13/13 5:27 PM

39.3  The hierarchical multiple regression approach 547

apart from the fact that they have been standardised in one case. What is more interest­ ing is that the correlations between the interaction and the predictor variable change from the raw scores to the standardised scores. The correlations with the interactions are much lower for the standardised scores than for the raw scores. This means that the problem of multicollinearity has been virtually eliminated by using standardised scores rather than the original unstandardised raw scores. The correlations of social support and stress with the interaction are 0.29 and 0.83 for the raw data, but in the standard­ ised scores these correlations decline to −0.00 and −0.02 – essentially zero correlations in both cases. The reduction in multicollinearity means that the interaction of the two predictor variables (which indicates a moderator effect) is more likely to be identified. In hierarchical multiple regression, as with any form of regression, the basic task is to assess the extent to which a set of predictor variables (independent variables if you prefer) is related to the criterion (or dependent variable). In our example, stress and social support would be independent or predictor variables and depression would be the dependent or criterion variable. Although social support is believed to be a moderator variable in this research, it is also a predictor variable for the purposes of the hierarch­ ical multiple regression. The interaction, as we have seen, is obtained quite simply by multiplying the scores for the two independent variables (stress and social support). An interaction would normally be indicated by the term stress × social support or whatever is appropriate. The interaction term is essentially treated as an additional predictor variable – which is precisely what it is. However, in the multiple regression the interac­ tion is dealt with after the effects of the independent and moderator variables acting independently have been taken into account. The hierarchical multiple regression procedure is essentially as illustrated in Figure 39.6. The basic principles of the hierarchical multiple regression process are as follows: ●

The independent or predictor variables are entered in blocks.



The first block is used in the analysis first and the second, third, etc. in strict order following that. In our example, there are only two blocks.



There has to be a minimum of one independent variable in each block.

In other words, there are priorities in hierarchical multiple regression which are deter­ mined by the order of blocks of variables. Each block may have just one independent variable in it, but it may have more according to the researcher’s purpose. As Figure 39.6 indicates, the first block includes both the stress and the social support variables. The second block involves the interaction of the two variables – that is, the interaction term or, in other words, a predictor variable which is created from the multiplication of the stress and social support scores. By multiplying stress and social support together we get a new variable which is normally referred to as the interaction of stress with social sup­ port. If the interaction is statistically significant in the multiple regression analysis then we have a moderator effect; if not then there is no moderator effect. To reiterate, in our example, the first block comprises both the stress and the social support variables. This stage of the analysis seeks to find out what influence stress and

Figure 39.6

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 547

Structure of a hierarchical multiple regression to test for interactions (moderator variables)

11/13/13 5:27 PM

548

CHAPTER 39  The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables

social support, acting separately, have on the dependent variable – i.e. depression. By analysing these in the first block, their influence on the interaction term is taken into account just as the main effects are taken into account first in ANOVA. The second block is the interaction of stress and social support. This is calculated by multiplying each individual’s score on the stress variable by their score on the social support variable. In our example, we have just one potential moderator variable, but we could have two or three if we so wished. The problem with multiple moderators is that there are multiple interactions in this case since the number of potential interactions increases dispropor­ tionately the more potential moderator variables we have. This is reminiscent of what happens in ANOVA when you have too many independent variables. In a phrase, the result is information overload. So be parsimonious in terms of the number of moderator variables you include in your analysis. If the hierarchical multiple regression does produce a significant interaction then this is indicative of a moderator effect. Unfortunately, it does not tell us just what the mod­ erator effect is. To see what the form of the interaction is, it is necessary to carry out further analyses. It is suggested by some statisticians that this is done by predicting the scores on the dependent or criterion variable (i.e. depression) for low, medium and high scores on the independent variable and the moderator variable using the unstandardised regression coefficients (Aiken & West, 1991). The advantage of this method is that it takes into account particular scores when determining the significance of the interaction term (i.e. moderator effect) and does not bundle together participants into somewhat arbitrarily defined groups. Multiple regression assumes that the relationship between the criterion and the inter­ action can be represented by a straight line although nonlinear relations can sometimes be tested if an appropriate transformation method is available for turning the nonlinear relationships into linear ones (Aiken & West, 1991). However, this is beyond the scope of this chapter and you should consult Aiken and West if you need more information.

Box 39.1

Key concepts

Interaction in multiple regression Interaction can be seen as a multiplicative effect in mul­ tiple regression. That is, different levels of the predictor variables have an effect on the scores which is greater than can be understood in terms of the individual effects of the predictor variables. This is much as we described interac­ tions in ANOVA in Chapter 23. The individual predictor variables have an additive effect on the dependent variable – that is, each predictor variable has a certain influence on the dependent variable and their combined influence is simply the sum of their separate influences. Of course, it is possible that the relationship between two variables is not a simple linear (and therefore additive) one. So sometimes, but rarely, you will find other relationships explored – the square of the scores on one variable in relation to the scores on another variable, for example. However, it is possible that the influence of the pre­ dictor variable is not simply additive (or even based on a

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 548

squared or quadrupled relationship) but multiplicative instead. That is, the effects of the predictor variables are multiplied together and not simply added together. As a consequence, when we seek to understand the influence of the predictor variables on the dependent variable in mul­ tiple regression, we look for the additive effects and also the multiplicative or interaction effects. That is why, quite simply, to get the interaction in multiple regression we multiply the scores on the independent variables together. Of course, the interaction is partly predictable from the independent variables which went to make up the inter­ action, but not entirely so. So if the simple effects of the independent variables are removed first, then we have the ‘pure’ multiplicative effect. This is precisely what happens in the calculations – the main effects of the variables act­ ing individually are removed which leaves a ‘pure’ interac­ tion effect.

11/13/13 5:27 PM

39.3  The hierarchical multiple regression approach 549

Explaining statistics 39.1

Identifying moderator variables using the hierarchical multiple regression approach The amount of data needed to study moderator variables is large. So instead of presenting the data in a table we have provided an SPSS Statistics file of the data on the website for this book. The steps in hierarchical multiple regression for moderator variables are summarised in Figure 39.7.



Step 1

Figure 39.7

Conceptual steps for understanding the use of hierarchical multiple regression to identify moderator effects

When using hierarchical multiple regression to identify moderator variables, the usual practice is to standardise each of the variables (the independent variable, the dependent variable and the moderator variable). The interaction is based on the standardised independent and moderator variables. Multi­ collinearity problems are likely to occur if one uses the raw scores to calculate the interaction term and these are likely to reduce the statistical significance of the interaction and risk making it non-significant. In general, it is less likely that a moderator effect will be detected if there are multicollinearity problems. In the approach to moderating variables used in this chapter, standard scores are used in order to eliminate collinearity influences. However, there are two possible methods for dealing with collinearity:



M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 549

11/13/13 5:27 PM

550

CHAPTer 39 The InfluenCe Of MOderaTOr VarIableS On relaTIOnShIpS beTWeen TWO VarIableS

1. Instead of using the raw scores, the scores are ‘centred’ to make 0 the mean value of the variables. This can be done simply by taking the mean score on the variable away from each score. This needs to be carried out for both the moderator and the independent variables, but it is not necessary for the dependent variable. The formula for centring is: centred score = individual score − mean score 2. The alternative to this uses standardised scores (i.e. z­scores). This ensures that the mean score on each variable is 0.00, just as the previous method. This is regarded as the preferred approach (e.g. Aiken & West, 1991). It is the method we describe in this chapter. Essentially the standard scores approach adds an extra stage to the calculation in that each centred score is divided by the standard deviation of the scores. This gives us the standardised score or z­score. We calculated z­scores in Chapter 6 if you need to refresh yourself on these. Thus to standardise scores on each variable we simply apply the following formula to obtain the standardised values: z-score −

individual score − mean score standard deviiaation of scores

Scores standardised in this way will always have a mean of 0.00 and a standard deviation of 1.00 (this is always true of z­scores as explained in Chapter 6). The criterion or dependent variable should also be standardised if the z­score method is employed rather than the centring approach (e.g. Aiken & West, 1991). Although it is easy to turn a score into a z­score by a hand calculation, there are many such calculations to be done so a computer package is essential. Turning scores into standard scores is easy with a computer program like SPSS Statistics – it merely requires ticking a box in the ‘Descriptive’ analysis routine. The new variable based on z­scores will appear as a new column in the data with a slightly different variable name.

Step 2

It is equally easy to calculate the interaction variable by multiplying each z­score for the moderator variable by the corresponding z­score for the independent variable. On SPSS Statistics the ‘Compute’ procedure will do this for you, of course. Once again, the outcome of these calculations will be shown as a new variable on the Data View spreadsheet of SPSS. You will need to give this new variable a meaning­ ful name. Otherwise it is easy to get confused by the eventual computer output.

Step 3

So, now we have three standardised variables – the independent variable, the moderator variable and the dependent variable – plus the interaction term which is essentially a variable created by multiplying the first two variables together. The next step is to carry out a hierarchical multiple regression on these vari­ ables. The structure of this analysis is summarised in Figure 39.6. In hierarchical multiple regression different sets of variables are entered into the analysis in blocks. The variables in block 1 will be dealt with together and before variables in block 2. There is a minimum of one variable in each block. The point of this is that the interaction term needs to be analysed after the two independent variables have been dealt with since the interaction is essentially what is left over after the effects of the two independ­ ent variables have been ‘removed’. Given the general advice that a large sample size is needed when looking for moderator effects it is probably wise to use a computer package to do this calculation too.

Step 4

Table 39.3 summarises the outcome of running a hierarchical multiple regression analysis on the data. From this table you can obtain values for the intercept, the regression weights for each variable and their statistical significance. Since we are mainly interested in moderator effects, the significance level of the interaction term in Table 39.3 is most important since it tells us whether or not we have a significant moderator effect. However, the table also has the values of the regression weights needed for us to identify just what the nature of the moderator effect is. It is clear that all of the regression weights are statistically significant in this example.

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 550

11/13/13 5:27 PM

39.3  The hierarchical multiple regression approach 551

Table 39.3 Regression summary table B (regression weight)

t

Sig.

−0.05

0.69

0.49

Intercept (constant) Stress (standardised)

0.21

2.86

0.01

Social support (standardised)

−0.21

2.87

0.01

Interaction

−0.19

2.86

0.01

The dependent variable is depression.

The most important thing in Table 39.3 is the statistical significance of the interaction. The interaction is what indicates the presence or not of a moderator effect. If the interaction is not statistically significant, then there is no moderator effect – i.e. social support is not a moderator variable for these data. However, if there is a significant interaction then you do have a moderator effect. Unfortunately, this does not tell us precisely what the nature of this moderator effect is. (This is analogous to the situation in ANOVA where a significant ANOVA does not tell you just where the differences between the cell means lie.) So there is another step that needs to be carried out.

Step 5

The problem at this point is that the output from the hierarchical multiple regression merely gives us regression weights and their significance levels. What it does not tell us is just what parts of the data show markedly different trends from the other parts of the data differentiated by different levels of the mod­ erator variable. The solution adopted is to choose a high score, a medium score and a low score on both the independent variable (stress) and the moderator variable (social support). This gives us nine possible combinations of high, medium and low stress and high, medium and low social support. So in other words, some fairly arbitrary values for the high, medium and low scores are chosen. The high, medium and low scores are defined simply as the score one standard deviation above the mean, a score at the mean and a score one standard deviation below the mean. Of course, expressed as standard scores (z-scores) these are +1, 0 and −1, respectively. Don’t forget that scores on the independent, dependent and moderator variables have been turned into z-scores at an earlier stage. So the score corresponding to a high score is already +1, a medium score is already 0 and a low score is already −1. What happens next is the regression weights shown in Table 39.3 are used to predict the most likely score on the dependent variable, depression, for each of the nine combinations of high, medium and low stress scores with high, medium and low social support scores. We will look at the formula for calculating the estimated depression scores in the next paragraph. However, it is important to understand that these nine predicted depression scores are examined to find out just where the interaction effect is. That is, one is looking for just where exceptionally large or small predicted scores are to be found. Having found these, then one has identified the location of the moderator effect – that is, what combination of high, medium or low scores on stress and high, medium and low predicted scores on social support are associ­ ated with these exceptionally high or low predicted scores on depression? In order to predict the depression score from the independent variable (X), the moderator variable (M) and the interaction (XM), we simply apply the following formula (which is an extension of what we saw in Chapter 32): F = a + b1X + b2M + b3XM



M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 551

11/13/13 5:27 PM

552

CHAPTER 39  The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables

That is, we multiply the relevant X, M and XM scores by the relevant regression weight from Table 39.3 plus the constant or intercept and this gives us the best prediction of the depression score based on our predictors. The following lists the elements of the above formula for clarity: F = the predicted score on the dependent variable a = the intercept (cut-point) for the regression line – it is a constant for any particular analysis so is the same in every case b1 = the regression weight for the predictor (independent variable) X = the score (z-score) on the predictor variable (i.e. +1, 0 or −1) b2 = the regression weight for the moderator variable M = the score (z-score) on the moderator variable (i.e. +1, 0 or −1) b3 = the regression weight for the interaction XM = the interaction of the independent and moderator variables – this is not a z-score though it is the product of the two z-scores So, in order to work out the predicted value of the dependent variable for each of the nine combinations of high, medium and low scores for the two variables, we calculate the above equation nine times which gives nine estimated scores on the dependent variable (depression) for each of the possible combinations of the high, medium and low scores for the independent variable (stress) and the moderator variable (social support). Well, that is what we do in theory, but there is a problem using the above formula. The problem basic­ ally is that we do not know precisely what the interaction term means. We do not know which scores it is made up from. For example, for a particular interaction value – say 2.00 – there are many different values of the moderator and the independent variable which multiplied together would give a value of 2.00. So it could be, for example, 1 on the moderator variable and 2 on the independent variable – but equally it could be 2 on the moderator variable and 1 on the independent variable. Both of these give a value of 2.00. Fortunately, it is possible to rewrite the equation so that it does not involve the use of the interaction term. The formula for regression given above can be rearranged (by anyone clever enough) to yield the following version of that original formula: a + (b1 + b3M)X + b2M This formula is the one which is actually used in the calculation as you can see in Table 39.4. The nine calculations are illustrated in the nine cells of Table 39.4. As you can see: ●

The prediction formula is the same in each cell, of course.



The constant or intercept a is the same throughout for this particular analysis (it is −0.05).



The various regression weights are the same throughout.

● Only

M (the value of the moderator variable) and X (the value of the independent variable) vary in the formulae. They will be +1, 0 or −1 according to the particular cell in question. The scores are entered as appropriate depending on the row and column of the cell in question. The value that goes into the calculation can be found at the top of the relevant row and the top of the relevant column.

The predicted mean depression scores can be plotted on a graph (Figure 39.8) in order to illustrate the predicted score’s relationship to different levels of stress and social support. It is quite obvious that the slopes in Figure 39.8 are very different. The blue slope for low social support is quite steep whereas the red slope for high social support is quite flat. It is clear that the chart indicates that for individuals with high levels of social support, the level of stress made no difference to the level of depression. On the other

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 552

11/13/13 5:27 PM

39.3  The hierarchical multiple regression approach 553



Table 39.4

Illustrating the three levels of the predictor and moderator variable and the calculation of the expected mean on the dependent variable

High score on predictor variable (X) (i.e. score at +1 standard deviation)

a + (b1 + b3M)X + b2M = −0.05 + (0.21 + −0.19 × 1) × 1 + −0.21 × 1 = −.14

a + (b1 + b3M)X + b2M = −0.05 + (0.21 + −0.19 × 0) × 1 + −0.21 × 0 = −.26

a + (b1 + b3M)X + b2M = −0.05 + (0.21 + −0.19 × −1) × 1 + −0.21 × −1 = −.66

Medium score on predictor variable (X) (i.e. score at mean)

a + (b1 + b3M)X + b2M = −0.05 + (0.21 + −0.19 × 1) × 0 + −0.21 × 1 = −.16

a + (b1 + b3M)X + b2M = −0.05 + (0.21 + −0.19 × 0) × 0 + −0.21 × 0 = −.05

a + (b1 + b3Z)X + b2M = −0.05 + 0.21 + −0.19 × −1 × 0 + −0.21 × −1 = −.26

Low score on predictor variable (X) (i.e. score at –1 standard deviation)

a + (b1 + b3M)X + b2M = −0.05 + (0.21 + −0.19 × 1) × −1 + −0.21 × 1 = −.18

a + (b1 + b3M)X + b2M = −0.05 + (0.21 + −0.19 × 0) × −1 + −0.21 × 0 = −.16

a + (b1 + b3M)X + b2M = −0.05 + (0.21 + −0.19 × −1) × −1 + −0.21 × −1 = −.14

Figure 39.8 Plot of predicted depression scores based on output from hierarchical multiple regression

hand, for those who have low social support, it is clear that as stress levels increase then so does depres­ sion. The lines are straight lines because they represent a linear (straight line) relationship between stress and depression. Although it is possible to calculate a numerical value for each slope, unfortunately there is no statistical test to establish whether the slopes of these lines differ significantly. In order to carry the analysis further then we adopt the procedure described in Step 6. However, it is important to point out that what our eyes see in Figure 39.8 should convince us that the interaction or moderator effect is largely to do with low levels of social support.



M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 553

11/13/13 5:27 PM

554

CHAPTer 39 The InfluenCe Of MOderaTOr VarIableS On relaTIOnShIpS beTWeen TWO VarIableS

Step 6

Although one might expect to be able to test statistically whether the slopes in Figure 39.8 differ from each other, actually there are no available statistical techniques to differentiate these slope coefficients (e.g. Cohen, Cohen, West & Aiken, 2003). Hence we did not calculate these coefficients as there is no point in doing so. One solution to this problem involves dividing the sample into two approximately equal sized groups in terms of the moderator variable. So there is a high (above the mean) and low (below the mean) group on the moderator variable. Basically the idea is to see whether the correlations between the independent variable and the dependent variable are different for the high and low groups. In Section 36.7 we discuss how to test for significant differences between correlations. So it is, first of all, simply a matter of dividing your data into two groups on the basis of being high or low on the moderator variable (social support). This can be done on SPSS Statistics by using the Recode procedure to divide the social support scores into two groups. The correlation between the independent variable and the dependent variable is then calculated for the high group followed by the low group. Finally, the formula for the significance of the difference between two correlation coefficients can be applied. This is not available on SPSS Statistics though applets for doing the calculation are available on the web. However, it is not too complicated to calculate the significance of the difference between two correla­ tion coefficients. The test is a variant of the z­test and it is also discussed in Chapter 36. Although the usual advice is to divide the sample to give equal sized groups, you might wish to modify this if you think that the moderator effect occurs towards the higher end or the lower end of the moderator variable. In this case, you might wish to adjust the split point. Using the mid­point of the social support variable resulting in the following Pearson correlations between stress and depression: ●

for the high social support group the correlation is 0.002 (sig. = .989, N = 84)



for the low social support group the correlation is 0.375 (sig. = 0.001, N = 96).

These two correlations have to be transformed into standardised (z) correlations using Table 36.5. The formula for the test of the difference between the two correlation coefficients is as follows: z=

z r1 − z r2 1 1 + N1 − 3 N 2 − 3

In this equation, zr1 and zr2 are the two standardised correlation coefficients obtained by using Table 36.5. The standardised value of r = 0.002 is 0.000 and for r = 0.375 the standardised value is 0.400. These can be substituted in the formula along with the relevant sample sizes (N1 = 84, N2 = 96): z=

=

= = =

0.000 − 0.400 1 1 + 84 − 3 96 − 3 −0.400 1 1 + 81 93 −0.400 0.012 012 + 0.011 −0.400 0.023 −0.400 0.152

= − 2 .6 63

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 554

11/13/13 5:27 PM

39.4  The ANOVA approach to identifying moderator effects (i.e. interactions) 555

Interpreting the results z must equal ±1.96 or more to be statistically significant at the 0.05 level with a two-tailed test of significance (1.65 or more for the one-tailed test of significance). In other words, this analysis confirms that there is a significant difference between the correlations for high scorers on social support and low scorers on social support. The low social support group shows a strong correlation between stress and depression whereas there is a virtually zero correlation for the group high on social support. One disadvantage of this method is that it involves dividing the sample into two smaller samples which means that the correlations and the difference between them are less likely to be statistically significant. Of course, if you wanted a quick assessment of your data in terms of possible moderator effects, the approach taken in this step would give you a good indication of any moderator effects though it is not as powerful as going through the full process including the hierarchical multiple regression.

Reporting the results One way of reporting the multiple regression results is as follows: ‘Baron and Kenny (1986) have suggested that a moderator effect is most appropriately tested with multiple regression. Such an effect is indicated if the interaction of the two predictor variables explains a significant increment in the variance of the criterion variable while the two predic­ tor variables are controlled. Aiken and West (1991) recommended that the criterion and the two predictor variables be standardised. Following these recommendations, a significant proportion of the variance in depression was accounted for by the interaction of stress and social support after the individual variables comprising the interaction were con­ trolled, R2 change = .04, p < .01. To interpret the significant interaction three separate unstandardised regression lines were plotted between standardised stress, standardised social support and the standardised level of depression at the mean and at one standard deviation above and below the mean of standardised stress and standardised social support. The relation between stress and depression was strongest at low levels of social support.’



The ANOVA approach to identifying moderator effects (i.e. interactions)

39.4

The ANOVA approach is used where the independent variable and the moderator vari­ able are in the form of nominal categories. Sometimes it is used to analyse data which have been collected in the form of scores. In this case, the scores have to be divided into three separate groups indicating high, medium and low scores for both the independent variable and the moderator variable. It is best to use three groups of scores since non­ linear relationships can be identified whereas they cannot with only two groups. This grouping system can be seen in Table 39.1 which is simply a table of mean scores on depression for high, medium and low scoring groups of the independent and moderator variables. Generally speaking, it is best not to do this since information is lost from the data by doing so. On the other hand, the ANOVA approach does have some advantages in terms of being clearer and less complex.

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 555

11/13/13 5:27 PM

556

CHAPTER 39  The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables

Explaining statistics 39.2

Identifying moderator effects using the ANOVA approach for nominal independent and moderator variables Step 1

This calculation is based on the example already discussed in the previous section. However, the essential features of the analysis of this study can be seen in Table 39.1. Unless one or more of your predictor variables is qualitative in nature – that is, a nominal/category variable – then you need to categorise the scores on your variable as being in the high, medium and low categories in terms of their size. Although you could use just two categories – such as high and low scores – this is inadvisable if you have a sub­ stantial sample size though you may need to try it if not. It is possible to use the Recode procedure on SPSS Statistics to categorise a score variable into groups.

Step 2

The next step is to run the ANOVA calculation. We have a 3 × 3 ANOVA design of the sort described in Chapter 23. Chapter 3 includes an explanation of the procedure and instructions on how to carry out the analysis by hand. However, we will not repeat these instructions for this particular example. Instead, we will present the results of a computer analysis as outlined in Figure 39.9 since testing for moderator effects tends to involve substantial sample sizes.

Figure 39.9 Plot of means of depression scores for the stress and social support groups

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 556

11/13/13 5:27 PM

39.4  The ANOVA approach to identifying moderator effects (i.e. interactions) 557

Step 3

The mean scores for each of the cells of the ANOVA analysis can be found in Table 39.1. However, it is generally easier to interpret the meaning of the mean scores in a significant interaction by plotting them in a graph where the dependent variable is represented by the vertical axis: one of the predictors is indi­ cated by relatively widely separated points on the horizontal axis and the other predictor is shown by different types of lines. This kind of graph is shown in Figure 39.9 where the horizontal axis represents the three levels of stress and the separate lines represent the three levels of social support. Figure 39.9 plots the mean depression scores of the nine groups formed on the basis of the three levels of stress and the three levels of social support. What seems clear from this plot is that there is one group – high stress and poor social support – which has a particularly high mean score on depression. The other eight groups, although their means do vary a little, have similar means. To anyone familiar with ANOVA, this pattern is very suggestive of a strong interaction between stress and social support. Differences in stress level alone and differences in social support level alone do not have much bearing on depression – in general, the depression is more or less the same for each of the stress and each of the social support groups. The exception, as we have seen, is the one group with high stress but poor social support. In other words, Figure 39.9 demonstrates a clear moderator effect.

Step 4

You also need to check out the analysis of variance (ANOVA) summary table based on this analysis (Table 39.5). The significance levels for the variables stress and social support and the interaction between them are all statistically significant. However, it is the interaction which is the most important in terms of assessing whether or not there is a moderator effect. A significant interaction effect indicates the pres­ ence of a moderator effect. You will see that the row for the interaction of stress and social support is statistically significant at the 0.00 level which indicates strongly the presence of a moderator effect. In this case, this is clear and unproblematic. However, note that the main effects for stress and social support are also both statistically significant. On the face of things, this seems to suggest that stress and social support, acting separately, each have an effect. This is a case where one should be somewhat cautious in the light of what Figure 39.9 suggests about the group means in general – that most of the means are about the same with the one exception. It is important to remember that ANOVA adopts a particular model for analysing data in which main effects such as stress and social support take precedence in the analysis to any interactions. So what is happening here is that some of the variation due to the interaction is being misleadingly allocated to the main effects. Despite this, in this particular case there is no doubt that there is a significant moderator effect which is what you need to know. A problem would arise if the main effects had been significant and the interaction non-significant – the plot of means as in Figure 39.9 is clearly the key to identifying the risk of assuming erroneously that there is no interaction and, hence, no moderator effect. The way in which ANOVA favours main effects was explained in Chapter 23.



Table 39.5

ANOVA summary table giving significance levels for the effects of stress and social support on depression

Source of variance

Sum of squares

Degrees of freedom

Mean square

F-ratio

Sig.

0.00

Stress

248.74

2

124.37

13.67

Social support

294.54

2

147.27

16.19

0.00

Interaction

270.06

4

67.51

7.42

0.00

1555.65

171

9.10

Error



M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 557

11/13/13 5:27 PM

558

CHAPTER 39  The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables

Table 39.6

Illustrating significant differences if there are main effects Poor social support

Moderate social support

Excellent social support

Low level of stress

6.20

5.60

5.65

Medium level of stress

6.10

4.60

5.30

12.25

5.60

6.25

High level of stress

Step 5

One relatively simple way of checking whether there truly are main effects is to compare appropriate pairs of cells in the ANOVA table. Remember that a main effect should apply to all pairs of cells in Table 39.1. So if there is a main effect of stress, then the group with excellent social support should have significantly different depression in the low stress condition from the medium stress condition and so forth. That is, the main effect of stress should apply at each different level of social support. Table 39.6 illustrates this. The vertical arrows indicate the cells which should be different from each other if there is a main effect of stress. The horizontal arrows indicate the cells which should be different from each other if there is a main effect of social support. Of course, this is the perfect scenario and, of course, in reality things will not be so perfect. One quick and simple way of checking is to run a post hoc multiple comparison test such as the Scheffé test on all of the cells. To do this, you need to turn the ANOVA into a one-way ANOVA with, in this case, nine separate cells. On a computer, one could simply add another column indicating which of the nine groups each score of the dependent variable (depression) belonged to. That is, a code of 1 to 9 is added to the data to indicate which of the nine groups each score is from. When this analysis is carried out on this data, the outcome is simple. None of the cell means differs from each other except for the high level of stress with poor social support. The mean of this cell is significantly higher than all other means in the table, just as we would expect from the plots in Figure 39.9. In other words, there are no main effects – just the interaction demonstrating that social support is, indeed, a moderator variable. This is exactly what one would expect from the pattern of means. Of course, this is, in part, a matter of judgement about the data, but ANOVA analyses can need interpretation if misleading conclusions are to be avoided.

Reporting the results One way of reporting the ANOVA results is as follows: ‘ANOVA was used to seek for moderator effects in the data. A moderator effect is indicated by a significant interaction in the ANOVA. The 3 × 3 ANOVA on the data indicated main effects on depression for stress, F(2, 171) = 13.67, p < .001, η2 = .14, and social support, F(2, 171) = 16.19, p < .001, η2 = .16, were statistically significant. However, more importantly in this context, it was found that the interaction of stress and social support was also statistically significant, F(4, 171) = 67.51, p < .001, η2 = .15. This interaction effect indicates that social support moderates the relationship between stress and depression. In order to identify more precisely the nature of the moderator effect, multiple comparison tests were made between the means of the nine groups. It became clear that the relationship between stress and depression was strong only for participants who lacked social support.’

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 558

11/13/13 5:27 PM

Key points 559



Research examples Moderator variables Ang, Chong, Chye and Huan (2012) examined whether adolescents’ perceptions of parents’ knowledge of their online activities would moderate the positive relation between loneliness and problematic Internet use. They found that the positive relation between loneliness and problematic Internet use was stronger in adolescents who thought that their parents did not know than in those who thought they did know. Sprung, Sliter and Jex (2012) examined spirituality as a moderator of the relation between being aggressive at work and various outcomes. Spirituality was partly defined as finding meaning in one’s life. They found that spirituality moderated the relation between physical aggression and workplace stress. The positive relation between physical aggression and workplace stress was greater in those with higher spirituality than those with lower spirituality which was contrary to what they had hypothesised. Wang and Huguley (2012) looked at whether parental racial socialisation practices moderated the relation between racial discrimination and various educational outcomes among African-American adolescents. Parental racial socialisation practices were measured with four questions which asked parents to indicate how often they had talked or engaged in activities with children that promoted feelings of racial knowledge, pride and connection. They found that the negative relation between teacher discrimination and grade point average was moderated by parental racial socialisation practices. This relation was more negative in children whose parents engaged less often in racial socialisation practices than those who engaged more often. Warren, Holland, Billings and Parker (2012) determined whether stress would moderate the positive relationship of talk about being too fat to body dissatisfaction and drive for thinness. They found that stress did moderate these relationships. Contrary to what they had predicted these positive relationships were stronger in those with less stress than those with more stress. Ziegler and Britta Diehl (2012) investigated whether job ambivalence moderates the positive relation between job satisfaction and job performance. Job ambivalence was defined as having positive and negative feelings about one’s job. They found that job ambivalence moderated the relation between job satisfaction and job performance. The positive relation between job satisfaction and job performance was stronger in managers who were less rather than more ambivalent about their job.

Key points ●

The most appropriate way of determining whether there is a moderating or interaction effect between two continuous (score) variables is a hierarchical multiple regression.



This analysis involves the standardisation of the measures into z-scores which overcomes some technical problems raised by using raw data.

● Another

name for moderator effect is interaction, and the assessment of moderator effects is based on the identification of interactions through either multiple regression or ANOVA.



It is not possible to do the calculations in their entirety just using a standard computer package such as SPSS Statistics. There is a certain amount of hand calculations to do or doing computations on SPSS Statistics using the Compute procedure, for example.



To interpret the interaction from a multiple regression, it is recommended that the slope or regression coefficient of the criterion on one of the predictors is calculated for three widely separated values of the other predictor such as its mean and one standard deviation above and below the mean.

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 559

11/13/13 5:27 PM

560

CHAPTER 39  The influence of moderator variables on relationships between two variables

Computer analysis Regression moderator analysis using SPSS

Figure 39.10

SPSS Statistics steps for a moderator analysis with hierarchical multiple regression

Screenshot 39.1 Part of the data

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 560

Screenshot 39.2

Select regression

11/13/13 5:27 PM

Recommended further reading 561



Screenshot 39.3 Entering variables

Screenshot 39.5

Screenshot 39.4

Select statistics

Model summary table output

Recommended further reading Aiken, L.S., & West, S.G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S.G., & Aiken, L.S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

M39_HOWI0749_06_SE_C39.indd 561

11/13/13 5:27 PM

Chapter 40

Statistical power analysis Getting the sample size right

Overview ● The

main purpose of statistical power analysis is to guide the planning of research. In particular, it seeks to optimise the sample size(s) used such that it is neither so small that significant results are impossible nor so large that time and other resources are used unnecessarily.



Statistical power is the likelihood that the research study will detect an effect (i.e. trend, correlation or difference) in the sample(s) selected when one exists in reality (in the statistical population).

● The

probability of deciding that there is an effect when in reality there is none is known as the Type I error and it is usually given the symbol alpha (α). Statistical significance testing gives α low probability figure – usually 0.05 – to keep the risk of Type I errors to a reasonable minimum.

● The

probability of failing to detect an effect when one exists is known as the Type II error. It is usually designated as beta (β).



Statistical power is, therefore, simply 1 − β. Usually a figure of 0.80 is regarded as very satisfactory.



Statistical power is interrelated with three things: (a) the standardised effect size (such as Cohen’s d or the correlation coefficient), (b) the alpha (α) or significance level and (c) the sample(s) size involved in the study. The larger that any of these values is, the more power there is in the study. It is possible, though a little complex, to calculate statistical power if the other three things are known or can be estimated rationally.

● Furthermore,

the researcher can calculate the required sample size(s) based on the required or estimated statistical power, the expected effect size and the significance level required. These calculations are best carried out using programs available on the web for ease.

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 562

11/18/13 10:17 AM

40.1  Introduction 563



Statistical power calculations carried out before the main study is conducted are regarded as valuable. Questions have been asked, however, about using statistical power calculations after the data have been analysed. That is, statistical power analysis is uncontroversial in terms of planning a study but its use is more controversial as part of the analysis of the data.

● Conceptually,

statistical power analysis is quite sophisticated and relies on a mature understanding of decision-making in research and the applicability of research findings. It requires the researcher to shed some faith in the significance testing model in favour of understanding decision-making in research and its application.

Preparation This is a relatively advanced technique which can only be built on a thorough understanding of the use of statistics in research. You need a thorough understanding of the statistical tests that you will use in your research together with knowledge of sampling distributions and sampling error.

40.1 Introduction One of the commonest questions asked by students planning research is ‘What sample size do I need? Would 25 be enough?’ Such questions are probably motivated by a number of factors: ●

Keeping the amount of work on data collection to a minimum since time is at a premium and collecting data is not always a speedy matter. Often there seems to be small reward for spending a great deal of time collecting data compared to, say, spending that time reviewing the research literature or working on the report. ● Giving themselves a reasonable chance of obtaining statistical significance which always feels like the preferred option when it comes to writing up a research report or submitting a dissertation. This even applies to professional research where it is well known and documented that it is easier to publish research based on statistically significant findings than non-significant ones. Now these are perfectly understandable reasons for asking the sample size question. However, unlike many topics in statistics, this is a question unlikely to provoke a completely satisfactory answer from psychologists. Amongst the likely answers are: ●

Get as big a sample as you can. ● You can probably get away with 50 (or some other number) participants. ● It is impossible to say – depends on too many things. Each of these is inadequate in its own way. Taking them in turn we can consider why this is the case: ●

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 563

Get as big a sample as you can  The suggestion that a researcher should get the largest possible sample is wasteful at best. Although student research generally has mainly time costs, research in general is a surprisingly costly activity. With some types of study, the financial cost of each additional participant may run to several hundred

11/18/13 10:17 AM

564

CHAPTER 40  Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right

pounds or dollars. The researcher’s time is expensive, in the first place, but there may well be substantial additional costs in terms of things like travel, transcription, equipment usage and so forth. Such expenditure is justifiable if the money is being well-spent, but what if it is not? Imagine that you were the chair of a research committee allocating research funds to eager researchers: you would have many responsibilities. You would need to be satisfied that the research you fund is feasible and of potential value, that the research design, etc. is optimal and so forth. Furthermore, it is also important that the research is not unnecessarily expensive. Hence you would want a reasoned explanation for the researcher’s chosen sample size so as not to waste money. Clearly, the sample size should be as big as necessary to answer the research question effectively. This optimum is dependent on various factors including the size of the effect in the study. Ordinary statistics such as the correlation coefficient and the t-test are indicative of the size of the effect – we are discussing the size of the correlation coefficient and the value of the t-test here not their statistical significance. But it is also dependent on the extent to which we are prepared to risk Type I and Type II errors (Chapter 11). A Type I error is accepting the hypothesis when it is, in fact, false and a Type II error is rejecting the hypothesis when it is, in fact, true. These are dealt with in Figure 40.1. The concept of power (as in the title of this chapter) simply refers to the probability of not making a Type II error – if there is actually a trend or difference in reality. There is another reason why aiming for the largest sample size possible is regarded as unsatisfactory. This is because with very large sample sizes, the slightest relationship or trend in the data is likely to be statistically significant. Now if statistical significance is a researcher’s sole criterion of importance then this means that extremely unimpressive trends (effects) in the data will be elevated to a level of importance which they do not warrant. For example, although a correlation of 0.70 is needed to be significant at the 5% level with a sample of 10, it only takes a correlation of .20 to be significant with 100 participants and a correlation of .06 to be significant with 1000 participants. In other words, a very small relationship in the data may achieve the status of statistical significance and all that entails if the sample size is sufficiently large. Of course, a good researcher will modify their interpretation of their analysis in the light of such considerations. It is probably worth mentioning at this stage that there is a view among statisticians that the null hypothesis is unlikely to be exactly true, so a study with an extremely

Figure 40.1 Type I and Type II errors

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 564

11/18/13 10:17 AM

40.1  Introduction 565

large sample size is very likely to produce a statistically significant trend. Obviously, such statistically significant but relatively minuscule trends are unlikely to be of much real interest to researchers. ●

You can probably get away with 50 (or some other number) participants What about the second suggestion that there is a sample size which is likely to ‘do the trick’? This has some merit in that it implies that there is a sample size likely to detect ‘statistically significant effects’ where they exist and that it does not demand that the researcher samples beyond what is necessary. However, just where has the proposed sample size come from? If it is based on considerable experience in the particular area of research in question then it is probably of some value as it is based on inside information about what sample size ‘works’ in a particular field of research. For example, a student who is carrying out a research project in a field of research in which their supervisor is expert might well get useful advice on sample size from them. Similarly, if a student is carrying out research which is very similar to that already published then there may be a case for considering using a similar sample size. This approach may seem a little rough and ready but, failing anything else, it is informative. The trouble is that it is only worth considering if it is based on relevant experience. The central problem is that the optimum sample size which is just big enough to meet the requirements of a) being big enough to potentially produce statistical significant outcomes and b) not being unnecessarily large depends on quite sophisticated statist­ ical ideas which do not readily lend themselves to ‘plucking’ numbers out of the air. Of course, such suggestions about sample size may be based on rather different considerations – the idea that a certain sample size demonstrates that the student or researcher has put in sufficient effort to achieve satisfactory outcomes. This is an irrational emotional approach which is, therefore, difficult to justify in this context.



It is impossible to say – depends on too many things  We can turn now to the final suggestion that optimum sample size depends on too many factors, many of which are unknown to the researcher, and so cannot be estimated. This runs counter to everything that you will learn about in this chapter. While the general idea that the estimation of appropriate sample size is not as common among psychologists as it perhaps ought to be, it is not too difficult to estimate this despite the estimate involving some of the sort of intelligent guesswork (i.e. inference and estimation) for which statistics is infamous.

One reason why you need to know about statistical power analysis is that it is increasingly expected in terms of professional level research. For one thing, journals are increasingly demanding that researchers include power as part of the statistical analyses submitted for publication. For another, as we have seen, those funding research are also increasingly likely to ask for estimates of the optimum sample size based on power calculations for reasons of economy and the viability of a study. There are other reasons too. If a researcher is carrying out research into the effectiveness of a particular form of psychotherapy using a control group, this means that some people participating in the research will not receive the treatment because they have been allocated to the control group. So using a sample size which is unnecessarily big will mean that if the treatment is shown to be effective then the excess of people in the control group will not get treatment. As a consequence, they may suffer a distressing condition for much longer than perhaps is necessary. In other words, research which goes on beyond what is necessary can be counterproductive. Statistical power is simply the likelihood that a study will detect a trend (or effect) in the data in circumstances in which, in reality, there is a trend. The concept of power is reviewed in Box 40.1. Remember that research deals with samples so reality, in this case, refers to the actual trend in the population which can be regarded as the baseline of truth or reality. Of course, this is largely an abstract concept since the researcher only knows about their sample(s) of data, not what is actually happening in the population. So we

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 565

11/18/13 10:17 AM

566

CHAPTER 40  Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right



Figure 40.2

The possible correct and incorrect (errors) decisions that a researcher can make based on their data

are talking estimation and inference once again. There are two basic risks in research which are taught to students very early on in their statistics courses. The most familiar is the idea that the sample(s) of data collected for the study will show a trend or a relationship when in reality there is no trend or relationship in the population from which the data were collected. This is known as a Type I error and is illustrated in Figure 40.2. Significance testing tries to minimise the risk of the Type I error by imposing the .05 or .01 significance criteria which refer to the level of risk of a Type I error that the researcher is prepared to take. Type I error is involved in power analysis because power depends partly on the significance level you choose for your study. The other risk is that of making a Type II error. This is failing to find a trend or relationship in the study when in reality there is a trend or relationship. The Type II error is also illustrated in Figure 40.2. This is a somewhat formal account which makes reference to H0 which is the null hypothesis and H1 which is the alternative hypothesis. You can regard the null hypo­ thesis and alternative hypothesis in the way that they are discussed in experimental design, but they are simply the situation in which there is no trend at all in the data and the situation where there is a trend in the data. You implicitly consider Type I error everytime you carry out a significance test. On the other hand, Type II error is likely to be much less familiar as its importance is often neglected. Indeed, this chapter is probably the first occasion when understanding it is of crucial importance. The concept of statist­ ical power is essentially the opposite of that of the Type II error. Thus statistical power is the probability of not making Type II error if there is a trend or relationship in the data. If the probability of making a Type II error is 0.15 then the power of the analysis (or the probability of not making a Type II error) is 1.00 − 0.15 = 0.85. There is one important point that needs to be stressed. Power is calculated on the basis that the hypothesis (H1) is true – in other words it only concerns the circumstances in which it is assumed that there is a relationship or trend in the population. So statistical power is the likelihood of detecting a trend or relationship in circumstances in which there is in reality a trend or a relationship exists. If you think about it, much the same applies for the Type I error – the probabilities are in terms of the likelihood of making an error if in reality the null hypothesis is true.

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 566

11/18/13 10:17 AM

40.1  Introduction 567

Box 40.1

Key concepts

Statistical power? The concept of statistical power is not quite what it seems. It is very much a conceptual matter which can be appreciated only if the basic concepts of statistical testing are understood. If you have reached this chapter then prob­ ably you have mastered at least some of the essential basic ideas. So be warned that statistical power is not a common-sense notion in itself. Nor is it possible to suggest that the more power there is the better. Research is essentially about finding trends in whatever the researcher’s field of interest is. It is traditional in psychology to conduct research by measuring the size of a correlation between two variables or the size of a difference between group means, for example. Then the researcher calculates the likelihood that a trend of this particular size could be obtained as a result of sampling fluctuations. If this is unlikely, the researcher will declare that their results are statistically significant. Usually a relatively arbitrary probability level of .05 or .01 is used to assess statistical sig­ nificance. Sometimes this is expressed as the 5% level of significance or the 1% level of significance. This is little other than the likelihood that the researcher has selected a sample(s) which appears to show a trend or relationship which does not represent the true situation – that is, there is no correlation or difference in the population from which the sample(s) was selected though there does appear to be one in the sample being studied. The level of significance (.05, .01) simply represents the extreme unchar­ acteristic samples which will be found due to sampling fluctuations despite the reality that there is no correlation or difference. Certain things might be mentioned in respect of significance testing: ●

Discussion of the concept of Type I error is fairly commonplace in analyses of research data though this is usually in terms of statistical significance. A Type I error is where the researcher accepts that there is a trend (correlation or difference) based on what can be seen in their data though this is an erroneous decision. The .05 and .01 levels of significance are probabilities that the researcher may have made a Type I error – if there is in reality no correlation or difference. If the researcher chooses the .01 level of significance then this means that there is less chance of making a Type I error than if they had chosen the .05 level of significance. In

this sense, the .01 level of significance can be seen as more stringent than the .05 level of significance. But it is only one part of the picture despite statistical significance frequently being used as if it were the gold standard in research. ● Every

psychology student will know that statistical significance is related to such things as sample size (the bigger the sample size the more likely a trend in the data is to be statistically significant – all other things being equal) and the size of the correlation or difference (the bigger the trend in the data the more likely one is to obtain statistical significance – all other things being equal).

All of this is likely to be very familiar. Nevertheless, it is not what power is about. Statistical power is more about a part of decision-making in research which is commonly taught but tends to be overlooked in the quest to achieve the status of statistical significance. So although psychologists should know about the concept of the Type II error, it is probably not actively considered when making decisions based on their research. The Type II error is the likelihood that a researcher has collected data which suggest that there is not a trend when in reality there is a trend. Sampling error is responsible for Type II errors just as it is for Type I errors. However, it is the sampling distribution of the population in which there is a real trend, i.e. not of the hypothetical population distribution of the null hypothesis of no trends. So a Type II error is where the sample(s) on which a research study was based do not seem to show the trend which actually exists in reality. Now both Type I and Type II errors are bad news in research for very different but feasible reasons. The concentration on Type I errors is unfortunate, but stat­ istics is a complex discipline and inevitably things will get simplified if their importance is not understood. Research is largely about establishing that there are trends and relationships in whatever is being studied rather than showing that there are no trends and relationships. Rarely do researchers set out to establish that there is no trend or relationship in their data. Quite the reverse – they are usually keen to show them in their data. If you make a Type II error you are essentially claiming that there is no relationship when there is one. Given that this is



M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 567

11/18/13 10:17 AM

568

CHAPTER 40  Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right

simply not what researchers want (no matter how objective and dispassionate some claim to be), then greater clarity about the implications of Type II errors seems to be essential. Statistical power is essentially the opposite side of the coin to the Type II error. Statistical power concerns the abil­ ity of a research study to detect a relationship when there is indeed, in reality, a relationship. A Type II error is, in contrast, the likelihood of failing to detect a relationship where one exists in reality. So statistical power is really the extent to which the researcher is likely not to be making a Type II error – but remember that the phrase ‘if the hypo­ thesis that there is a relationship in reality’ always needs to be appended to the definitions of both power and Type II errors. So statistical power = 1 − the probability of making a Type II error. If the probability of making a Type II error in a particular study is .20, the power of the study to detect a real trend or difference is 1.00 − 0.20 = 0.80. Remember that 1.00 in probability theory (Chapter 16) refers to a single event or instance. So in this case the 1.00 refers to a single instance of a researcher’s decision to decide either that there is a trend or that there is not a trend in the data. The 0.80, therefore, is the probability that this decision has been in favour of concluding that there is a trend or difference when one actually exists in reality outside of the researcher’s study.

The Type II error and power both depend on the distribution of samples taken at random from the population in which there is a trend or difference. Some of these samples will depart quite markedly from what is happening in the population from which the samples were taken. Any of these samples which are in the range of the non-statistically significant samples according to the null hypothesis of zero differences or correlations would be erroneously identified as coming from the population where the null hypothesis is true. The amount of overlap between the two sampling distributions will obviously affect the size of the Type II error and the statistical power. This can be seen in Figure 40.3. This boils down to the following. Statistical power reflects the risk that the researcher will fail to show the relationship or difference which was the real purpose of the research. Imagine that the researcher was searching for a cure for cancer – accepting the null hypothesis erroneously might lead to the abandonment of this line of research which might have led to a cure for cancer. This would be an extremely serious consequence – some might say much more serious than mistakenly thinking that one had found a potential cure for cancer when, in truth, your treatment did not work. These are clearly complex arguments which are far more socially important than dry discussions of Type I and Type II errors.

Statistical power is affected by other aspects of the research, most of which should be very familiar to you by now. You might find it easier to think about the factors which will reduce the likelihood of making a Type II error and consequently increase the power of the analysis. These factors can all be seen in Figure 40.3 which lists things which will affect statistical power (and the risk of making a Type II error). Most of these you could probably guess were involved anyway:

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 568



The bigger the sample size then the greater the power of your study (and the less likely it is that a Type II error will be made), all other things being equal. This makes intuitive sense since a study with bigger samples is more likely to detect trends or relationships where they exist than one using smaller samples. One reason is that the bigger the sample then the smaller the sampling error (or spread of sample means taken from the population).



The bigger the significance level (i.e. alpha or α) you choose for your test of significance, the greater the power of your study (and once again the less likely it is that you will make a Type II error) all other things being equal. Now alpha is the significance level that you choose when assessing the statistical significance of the trend or relationship in your data. If you select an alpha of .05 then this is bigger than an alpha of .01. Thus an analysis using the .05 level of significance has more power than one using the .01 level of significance – all other things being equal. It is important, though, to remember that the significance level does not have to be .05 and can vary depending on a range of circumstances associated with the research, although it is a sound fallback choice. Also remember that the significance level is the probability of identifying a trend or relationship in the data when there is no trend or relationship in reality due to sampling fluctuations (i.e. the risk of a Type I error). It is fairly obvious that where

11/18/13 10:17 AM

40.1  Introduction 569

Figure 40.3 The components of statistical power calculations

the researcher accepts a greater risk of making a Type I error (finding a trend where there isn’t one) then the risk of Type II error will be lower as a consequence. But we will return to this shortly. ●

The bigger the size of the effect in a study (i.e. the stronger the relationship or the greater the trend or the bigger the difference between the mean scores for each group) then the greater the power your study has (and the lower the risk of making a Type II error) – all other things being equal. If you consider that the size of the effect in a study is indicated by the size of the correlation or the size of the t-statistic, for example, then once again this should not surprise you. A correlation of .6 is more likely to be statistically significant than one of .3, for example, for a given sample size. This means that the researcher is more likely to accept that there is a relationship or trend in the data. Since power is the likelihood of identifying a trend or relationship in a study when one exists in reality, it is not unexpected that sample size relates to power. It is usual in power calculations to use a standardised measure of effect size most commonly Cohen’s d, but sometimes a correlation coefficient is used as this is standardised too. See Box 40.2 for a more detailed discussion of measures of effect size.

Other things influence power, in particular the variability in the data: ●

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 569

The greater the variability in the data, however, the lower the power in the study. This is because increased variability reduces the chance that your findings will be statistic­ ally significant. Remember that power is the likelihood of detecting a relationship or trend in your study when there is one in reality. Thus if the possibility of significant findings is reduced because of higher variability in the data then the power is reduced because the trend or relationship will not be detected even though in reality there is one. However, this is not necessarily an important feature of power calculations since the standardised measures of effect size (e.g. the Pearson correlation coefficient and Cohen’s d) which are used in power analysis take this into account in their calculation. Nevertheless, the variability in the data is something which the researcher can often do something about – anything that can be done to reduce this variability increases the power of the analysis. For example, the researcher can standardise their methods of conducting the research and also use well-constructed tests and other measures as both these things will reduce variability and consequently increase power. That is, reduce, if they can, any unwanted source of variability in the study.

11/18/13 10:17 AM

570

CHAPTER 40  Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right

All of these aspects intertwine in a study to produce the power of your analysis. Furthermore, the analysis is different for different statistical procedures (tests of signi­ ficance), which means that the calculation of statistical power can be a little complex. There are two main ways to deal with this and make statistical power accessible to researchers: (a) produce tables for every test of significance which results in numerous tables to consult and (b) use computer programs in which the researcher enters key aspects of their study (sample size, effect size and the significance level [i.e. alpha level] involved) and the calculation is left to the computer. Of course, alternative (c) is to do the calculations yourself by hand though this is not a particularly helpful option. Figure 40.4 is important. It shows the (theoretical) distribution of samples taken from the population. The curve on the left, in red, is the distribution of differences between samplemeans (of a given size) if the null hypothesis that there is no trend in the data is correct. It is therefore the sampling distribution according to the null hypothesis: ●

Notice that the mean or midpoint of this curve is 0.0 as you would expect if the null hypothesis is true since there should be no difference between the samples except that due to sampling error.



The pink area is the portion of this curve selected to be the significance level for testing the hypothesis. The pink area is bigger for a .05 significance test than for a .01 significance test. That is, the vertical green line will be further to the left for .05 significance than for .01 significance.

The curve with the dotted blue line is not so familiar. It is the distribution of differences between sample means if the hypothesis is true – that is, if in reality there is a trend in the data. The mean of the dotted blue curve is about 2.3; that is, the effect of being in one group compared to the other. If this is standardised then it is an effect size.

Figure 40.4 Power, significance and Type I errors based on G*Power output

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 570

11/18/13 10:17 AM

40.1  Introduction 571

There are a number of things that may be obvious from Figure 40.4: ●

The part of the blue dotted curve which is shaded in blue to the left of the pink shaded area indicates the extent of Type II error in this particular study. The power of this particular study is indicated by the remainder of the blue dotted curve. This includes the area of statistical significance for the red curve.

● If

the size of the effect of the study is increased (i.e. by mentally moving the blue dotted curve to the right) the power would increase as there would be less of the blue dotted curve to the left of the pink alpha area. Move the curve to the left and the power would decrease.



If variability in the study were to be reduced, then the curves would have less spread and power would increase as a consequence.



If the significance level changes then the pink alpha area will be bigger or smaller. It is smaller if the significance level is .01 which will have the effect of increasing the Type II error and so decreasing the power of the analysis. It is larger if the significance level is .05 which means that the Type II error will be smaller and the power greater, as a consequence.

What cannot be seen from Figure 40.4 is the influence of sample size on power. However, if you remember from Chapter 10 that the sampling distribution for larger samples is smaller than for smaller samples then it can be understood why sample size influences power by reducing the spread of the sampling distribution, all other things being equal.

Box 40.2

Key concepts

Effect size An effect size is the extent of the trend demonstrated by a study. Effect is a slightly odd word in the context of most research since it implies the influence or impact of one variable on another variable. However, effect size is really about the relationship between two variables rather than anything to do with cause and effect. It refers essentially to two things: ●

The size of the relationship (i.e. correlation) between two variables  The most familiar measure of this is the Pearson correlation coefficient which can be used as a measure of the effect size. In Chapter 36 the Pearson correlation coefficient is used as the measure of effect size for meta-analysis as we are very familiar with it. The Pearson correlation coefficient is a standardised measure of the relationship between two variables. That is, one can meaningfully compare correlation coefficients even when they are taken from different studies. The bigger the correlation between two vari­ ables the bigger the effect size.



The difference between the mean scores of different groups of scores  For example, this could be the difference between the mean of the experimental and control group. Although such differences do indicate the actual effect, it is not used as a measure of effect size because it is dependent on the variation in the data. In other words, differences in themselves are not standardised measures.

If the difference between two means is to be used as a measure of effect size then that difference needs to be standardised so that differences in means may be compared from study to study. You should be familiar with some forms of standardisation by now. They basically involve adjusting by the variability in the data. The commonest way of doing this is to use Cohen’s d as a measure of effect size. Cohen’s d is simply the difference between the two mean scores (B1 − B2) divided by the standard deviation of the population:



M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 571

11/18/13 10:17 AM

572

CHAPTER 40  Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right

Cohen’s d =

B−B standard deviation of population

Cohen originally suggested dividing by the standard deviation based on one or other group of scores. The assumption was that the two standard deviations should be equal. But, of course, they are likely to be different to some extent in practice. Consequently, it is more usual to pool (combine together) the two standard deviations when calculating Cohen’s d, much as it is when calculating the value of t for a t-test. But this actually leads to a situation in which there are various formulae for combining the two standard deviations since there is more than one way of doing this. The commonest formula for Cohen’s d which involves pooling standard deviations is as follows: B1 − B 2 (n1 − 1)s12 + (n1 − 1)s22 n1 + n2 The two standard deviations are listed as s1 and s2 in the above and their respective sample sizes are given as n1 and n2.

Should you wish to calculate the effects size for your study then there a number of effect size calculators available free on the Web which involve entering some information based on your data following which the effect size is given. Simply type the words ‘effect size calculator’ into your favourite search engine and make your selection from the sites that this generates. G*Power, which is used in this chapter to calculate power, will also calculate the appropriate measure of effect size for the particular test of significance that you are using. The information required is readily available from the output of the t-test on programs such as SPSS Statistics. Alternatively, you could calculate the effect size as a correlation coefficient (using SPSS Statistics or any other statistics program) – or by hand using the procedures described in Chapter 36). The dependent variable of the study is one variable in the calculation and the other variable is the group to which the participant in question belongs which is coded 1 for the control group and coded 2 for the experimental group, etc. If you want the effect size as a value of Cohen’s d then Table 40.1 could be used to convert the resulting

Table 40.1 Equivalent effect sizes expressed as Cohen’s d and Pearson correlation coefficient Pearson r

Cohen’s d

Pearson r

Cohen’s d

Pearson r

0.00

0.00

0.01

0.02

Cohen’s d

Pearson r

Cohen’s d

Pearson r

Cohen’s d

0.20

0.41

0.21

0.43

0.40

0.87

0.60

1.50

0.80

2.67

0.41

0.90

0.61

1.54

0.81

2.76

0.02

0.04

0.22

0.45

0.42

0.93

0.62

1.58

0.82

2.87

0.03

0.06

0.23

0.47

0.43

0.95

0.63

1.62

0.83

2.98

0.04

0.08

0.24

0.49

0.44

0.98

0.64

1.67

0.84

3.10

0.05

0.10

0.25

0.52

0.45

1.01

0.65

1.71

0.85

3.23

0.06

0.12

0.26

0.54

0.46

1.04

0.66

1.76

0.86

3.37

0.07

0.14

0.27

0.56

0.47

1.06

0.67

1.81

0.87

3.53

0.08

0.16

0.28

0.58

0.48

1.09

0.68

1.85

0.88

3.71

0.09

0.18

0.29

0.61

0.49

1.12

0.69

1.91

0.89

3.90

0.10

0.20

0.30

0.63

0.50

1.15

0.70

1.96

0.90

4.13

0.11

0.22

0.31

0.65

0.51

1.19

0.71

2.02

0.91

4.39

0.12

0.24

0.32

0.68

0.52

1.22

0.72

2.08

0.92

4.69 5.06

0.13

0.26

0.33

0.70

0.53

1.25

0.73

2.14

0.93

0.14

0.28

0.34

0.72

0.54

1.28

0.74

2.20

0.94

5.51

0.15

0.30

0.35

0.75

0.55

1.32

0.75

2.27

0.95

6.08

0.16

0.32

0.36

0.77

0.56

1.35

0.76

2.34

0.96

6.86

0.17

0.35

0.37

0.80

0.57

1.39

0.77

2.31

0.97

7.98

0.18

0.37

0.38

0.82

0.58

1.42

0.78

2.49

0.98

9.85

0.19

0.39

0.39

0.85

0.59

1.46

0.79

2.58

0.99

14.04

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 572

11/18/13 10:17 AM

40.2  Types of statistical power analysis and their limitations 573

correlation coefficient to the equivalent value of Cohen’s d as explained in the next paragraph. There is a close relationship between Cohen’s d and the Pearson correlation coefficient. This is to be seen in Table 40.1. This allows a value of Cohen’s d effect size to be converted to a Pearson correlation coefficient and vice versa. In Table 40.1 the purple shaded columns contain values of the Pearson correlation coefficient (Pearson r) and the blue shaded columns contain the values of Cohen’s d. If you wish to find the equivalent Cohen’s d for a Pearson r of 0.40, simply find the 0.40 in the purple columns and look to its right in the blue column. You will find that a Pearson r of 0.40 corresponds to a Cohen’s d of 0.87. If you have a Cohen’s d of 2.20 then you look at

the purple column to the left of this where you will find that the corresponding Pearson r value is 0.74. If you have values which go to more than two decimal places then you will need to round down to two decimal places before you use the table. If the precise value is not in the table then use the nearest value instead. Such a conversion from Cohen’s d to a correlation coefficient is useful if you are not very familiar with Cohen’s d. In these circumstances the Pearson r will almost certainly be more meaningful to you. There are other measures of effect size when comparing two groups (Glass’s Δ and Hedge’s g) though Cohen’s d tends to be the most commonly used. There are also other measures of effect size to deal with analysis of variance (ANOVA) designs.

40.2 Types of statistical power analysis and their limitations There are a number of ways in which statistical power is used in research. These divide into a) the prospective (a priori) use which is part of the planning of a research study and b) the retrospective (post hoc) use where statistical power is calculated as part of the analysis of the data. These are illustrated in Figure 40.5. It should be quickly pointed out that opinion is divided on the value of different aspects of power analysis. Most

Figure 40.5

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 573

Uses of power analysis

11/18/13 10:17 AM

574

CHAPTER 40  Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right

statisticians acknowledge that the prospective uses of statistical power are of value when planning substantial research projects. On the other hand, some question the value of retrospective power analyses. Some of the arguments are quite vehement. The argument for the prospective use of power analysis as part of the planning of a research study has largely been made earlier in this chapter. However, one argument might be particularly difficult for those trained in psychological research. Psychologists tend to stress the importance of finding statistically significant differences or relationships, often forgetting other considerations. In brief, it is good to get statistically significant results. Certainly, there is a view that significant research findings are easier to get published. Statistical power analysis puts a very different gloss on the research and basically argues that only relationships or differences of a certain magnitude are important and worthy of consideration and that sample size, in particular, should be geared to making it likely that significant trends will be found in the data where they exist in reality. In other words, for a particular area of research or a particular research study, what is the size of effect which the researcher is warranted in searching for? And, given this, what is the size of sample(s) which can detect an effect of this magnitude? If a researcher’s sole criterion for effective research is that statistically significant findings are obtained then very large sample sizes should normally do the trick. What emerges, though, is likely to be of trivial or no value. There is an argument in the statistical liter­ ature that null hypotheses are never (or hardly ever) true – take a large enough sample and a statistically significant difference or correlation will be found even if it involves sampling thousands of cases. Unfortunately, this is a fairly accurate caricature of some research in psychology though it is essentially mindless. Of course, statistics textbooks often inadvertently and unintentionally reinforce the view that statistical significance is a holy grail in research. Planning research should be a much more thoughtful process than this implies. Another prospective use of power analysis is where a pilot study has been carried out on a relatively modest number of participants in order to make sure that the procedures of the study work well enough to encourage the researcher to consider a later larger-scale study. It is likely to be possible to use the basic information from this pilot study concerning effect size to allow an intelligent approach to deciding an optimal sample size for the subsequent large-scale study. This is a thoughtful intelligent approach to planning research which should be encouraged. The argument about the retrospective use of statistical power analysis is, as mentioned, somewhat more controversial and acrimoniously presented. It boils down to the question of what value such retrospective power analyses are to the researcher. This retrospective power analysis is known as observed power and is calculated as part of some of the statistical routines on SPSS Statistics, for example. Quite obviously, if your study produces statistically significant findings then the study had sufficient statistical power for the particular sample size used – otherwise you would not have obtained statistically significant results. It might also be of some interest to quantify the power of your study when considering the effect size. For example, you might find that your study had a very large power value. This suggests that you may have used a far too large sample size given the effect size. In other words, any future studies could involve a smaller sample size which brings consequent economies to the research. Power analysis could tell you the size of sample that future similar studies require. This, though, is rather like the case of using pilot studies discussed above and amounts to good practice for much the same reasons. Controversy arises when retrospective power analysis has been used to make a rather different sort of argument. Some researchers have argued that if a study fails to obtain significant findings, a power analysis can be used to help decide what is going on in the data as a sort of data analysis method. Imagine that the power analysis suggests that the study involves adequate statistical power yet the findings are not statistically significant.

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 574

11/18/13 10:17 AM

40.3  Doing power analysis 575

Basic statistics courses tell us that in these circumstances where we have not obtained statistical significance we reject the hypothesis and accept the null hypothesis of no difference or no relationship. Now retrospective power analysis (observed power) is sometimes used to suggest that where a) there is no evidence in support of the hypothesis and, consequently, b) the null hypothesis should be accepted then if the observed power is low then this suggests that the evidence in support of the null hypothesis should be regarded as weak. That is, the argument goes that the study was incapable of rejecting the null hypothesis because of its low power. The problem with this argument is that there is a direct relationship between the observed power and the probability (significance) level found using a test of significance. In other words, discussing observed power is a longwinded way of saying things which the significance level already indicates. One further criticism of using observed power (erroneously) in this way is that there are acceptable methods of testing the strength of the null hypothesis. For example, it is possible to test whether two group means are statistically equivalent rather than simply ‘not significantly different’ as in the case of conventional significance testing (Hoenig & Heisey, 2001). The controversy, in summary, is about whether observed power statistics add anything to the interpretation of non-significant research findings. Nevertheless, psychology journals are increasingly likely to require the reporting of observed power statistics. The message from this seems to be clear. Power analysis as part of the planning process for a research study is generally regarded favourably by both researchers and statisti­ cians as an important tool. It helps ensure clarity about what is an adequate effect size for a particular study but also encourages consideration of what would be an adequate sample size. In contrast, however, retrospective power analysis should be used with great caution because it is only of very limited value and is not a tool for data analysis as such. Indeed, the retrospective use of power analysis is most acceptable in circumstances where it potentially contributes to the planning of further research studies – but that is the case for prospective power analysis.

40.3 Doing power analysis It must be understood that the following are interdependent: ●

statistical power



sample size



effect size



statistical significance.

If three of these four things are known for a particular test of significance then it is possible to work out the fourth. Usually it is statistical power or sample size which is calculated from the other three using power analysis programs. Although it is feasible to do these calculations without the aid of a computer, there is little point in spending time on this when one has better things to do with one’s time. Type the words power analysis calculator into your preferred Internet search engine and any number of resources for doing particular aspects of power analysis will be listed. Mostly they will do what you need though some have a more specialised function than others. You can download G*Power from the web, which is our preferred program, but there are others available. The authors of this have kindly allowed us to include it on the book’s website so you should have access to it permanently. Some other programs are in the form of applets on web pages so you do the calculation on screen but the program is not downloaded on your computer.

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 575

11/18/13 10:17 AM

576

CHAPTER 40  Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right

You will probably have noticed a stumbling block. What is the value for statistical power, effect size and statistical significance if I want to use statistical power analysis to calculate the appropriate sample size? Where can I find these? The answer is, in general, that you can’t find them but you will have to rationally decide what the appropriate values for each of these are. Let us go through the different aspects of the power calculation in turn: ● The

level of statistical significance is traditionally set at .05 in psychology. This is often described as an arbitrary value and it is. There is no logic in choosing it except that it stipulates a pretty low value of probability that the researcher will choose to accept the hypothesis erroneously when the null hypothesis of no trend is in reality true. That is, there is only a 1 in 20 chance (5%) of accepting the hypothesis when it is, in fact, false in reality. But is this value always an adequate criterion? What if the research was about a cure for hay fever and a decision whether or not to spend a very large amount of money on research and development rested on the outcome of the study? In these circumstances, would it not be wiser to adopt a more stringent significance level (e.g. .01) in place of .05? The answer is probably yes. On the other hand, if the planned research is more exploratory and in a field where there is little previous research, then maybe a less stringent significance level of .10 might be adopted. For example, if the study was being carried out on a shoestring for purposes with no such immediate consequence as the spending of huge sums of money, then surely the risk of prematurely abandoning research on this topic (because the study fails to obtain significant results) might be more serious than the consequences of reaching by chance the erroneous conclusion that the hypothesis was true. Quite clearly, these are not really statistical decisions but ones, nevertheless, of some import­ ance to the researcher. Of course, it is possible to explore the effect on sample size of the various possible levels of significance to see if it makes any practical difference to your research. (It should be added that it is possible to calculate significance against not the usual zero effect model of the null hypothesis but against a low level size of effect which is of no practical interest to the researcher – that is, a size of effect which, although tangible, is so small that it is not worthwhile when making decisions based on the outcome of the research. For example, if it is known that an inexpensive drug such as aspirin has a particular size of effect then this effect might be set as the baseline against which to evaluate a new much more expensive drug. Such procedures are discussed in Murphy and Myors, 2004.)



The required level of the effect size must be estimated for your proposed study. This can be based on one of several sources of information: a) If there have been similar studies using the measures that you are planning to use in your study, then the effect sizes from these previous studies may be used. Obviously the more similar the other study(ies) to yours the better this estimate is likely to be. b) Alternatively, a more general approach could be used. For example, Lipsey and Wilson (1993) collated effect sizes from a range of different sizes of study. They found that treatment programmes for juvenile delinquents in terms of future delinquency, worksite anti-smoking programmes in terms of rates of quitting smoking and small versus large school class sizes in terms of measures of achievement typic­ ally had small effect sizes of Cohen’s d values of .20 or less. On the other hand, behaviour therapy compared to placebo controls on various outcome measures and enrichment programmes for gifted children in terms of cognitive, creativity and affective outcomes had effect sizes of .5 or so – that is medium size effect sizes. Finally, psychotherapy in terms of various outcomes and positive reinforcement in

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 576

11/18/13 10:17 AM

40.4  Calculating power 577

the classroom had effect sizes of .85 or more – that is, large effect sizes. Cohen (1988) stipulated a small effect as a Cohen’s d of .20, a medium effect as a Cohen’s d of .50 and a large effect as a Cohen’s d of .80. So if a particular type of research is known to generally produce a particular effect size then this could be used. c) One could simply look at the consequences of using Cohen’s three levels of effect size in terms of sample size. It may be that each of them indicates a sample size which is feasible in terms of the proposed research. Otherwise, the conservative approach would be to take a Cohen’s d of .20 (the smallest of his effect sizes) as the basis for the sample size calculation. ●

The level of power required needs to be at a minimum .50 – otherwise the study is likely not to reject the null hypothesis. There is, of course, very little point in designing a study which is more than likely to support the null hypothesis. It is conventional – and no more than that – to regard a power of .80 or greater as adequate. This means that if there is truly a trend or difference in the data that it has an 80% chance of being identified by the researcher using a particular significance level and sample size(s). There is no reason why a higher level of power cannot be chosen, if this is considered appropriate by the researcher.

40.4 Calculating power Since one cannot use SPSS Statistics to carry out most aspects of statistical power ana­ lysis we will use G*Power, which is a free-to-download and flexible program that carries out a variety of statistical power analysis calculations. Of course, SPSS Statistics output does contain relevant information to be entered into these additional programs. The SPSS company does have a power analysis program SamplePower® 2.0, but this is not generally available at universities, etc. in the way SPSS Statistics is and it is quite expensive to purchase. So it is just as well to turn to other software which is available in some variety. Many of the programs have to be purchased and so it makes sense to opt for the free resources available on the Web. G*Power is a serious competitor for commercially available software and it is well-regarded. It is also flexible in terms of the number of different research designs that it can deal with. For this reason, we have adopted G*Power as our primary resource for this chapter. Figure 40.6 illustrates the active interface of G*Power, but expect slight variations according to circumstances. G*Power does a wide variety of power analyses for a variety of statistical tests which are organised into ‘Test families’ such as those based on the t-test and those based on the F-distribution. The term ‘Test family’ can be seen immediately under the big white box in the screenshot (Figure 40.6). Select the Test family you require from the drop-down list which appears when you hover your mouse cursor over this box. Then select the ‘Statistical test’ you require from the drop-down menu. Finally select the ‘Type of power analysis’ again from a drop-down menu. Since these drop-down menus each offer a variety of options, it is worthwhile checking out what is available by trying out a number of these options. So, as you can see in Figure 40.6, the following have been selected but, of course, the choices made depend on the design of the study:

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 577



Test family: t-test



Statistical test: Means: difference between two independent means (two-groups)



Type of power analysis: A priori: Compute required sample size – given α, power and effect size.

11/18/13 10:17 AM

578

CHAPTER 40  Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right

Figure 40.6 A screenshot of the G*Power interface

These particular selections indicate the following: The analysis corresponds to an unrelated t-test comparing the difference between two means. The power analysis is intended for planning a new study (i.e. A priori) and it is required that the optimum sample size is computed based on the significance level (α), required level of power and the effect size. This would be a reasonable standard option for statistical power analysis when comparing, say, an experimental group with a control group – or for comparing the means of any two groups, for that matter. It is obvious that you then need to enter (overwrite) the Input Parameters in the white boxes. We will return to the question of just what you need to enter into each of the input boxes a little later, but we need to concentrate on the box labelled Effect size first of all as this is the most complex. There are various different measures of effect size to deal with different research designs. However, once you have selected the Test family, Statistical test and Type of power analysis, G*Power indicates what measure of Effect size is to be used. If you have relevant data, Effect size can be calculated from this. G*Power will calculate the relevant type of Effect size for you if you ask it to do so. However, this calculation is not based on the raw data but on things like the sample means and standard deviations (depending on the research design that you are dealing with). Consequently, this information needs to be calculated by you before you can enter it into G*Power. Figure 40.7 shows the side-menu or drawer revealed on screen when you click on the ‘Determine=>’ button next to ‘Effect size d’. In this screenshot, the information has

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 578

11/18/13 10:17 AM

40.4  Calculating power 579

Figure 40.7 Effect size calculator side-menu on G*Power3

already been entered into the appropriate boxes. Notice, however, that you need to select different options according to whether your sample sizes are equal or different. Although the calculation of these figures could be carried out by hand, it is probably more convenient to obtain them using SPSS Statistics or some other statistical analysis program. For example, to calculate the effect size for two independent groups using Cohen’s d, you need to enter the means and standard deviations for the two samples into G*Power. These are part of the output of SPSS Statistics for the unrelated t-test. The means and standard deviations (SD) have been entered using SPSS Statistics t-test output. You probably will wish to click on the ‘Calculate and transfer to the main window’ button as it saves you copying it yourself into the ‘Input Parameters’. Of course, it is more likely that you have no data from which to calculate Effect size unless you have conducted a pilot study. Consequently, you might be well advised to examine previous research studies to see if a typical effect size can be identified. Metaanalyses are particularly useful in this regard. Failing that, you may wish to use the ‘standard’ high, medium and low effect sizes which have been recommended by Cohen (1988) and others. When you hover your mouse cursor over the ‘Effect size’ box in G*Power these standard sizes will appear on screen. So that is one important Input Parameter dealt with. The following are suggestions as to what goes into the input boxes seen in Figure 40.6. The calculation is based on the study we used to illustrate the unrelated t-test in Chapter 14. If the researcher has no strong basis for predicting the direction of the outcome of the research, a two-tailed test is selected. One could select a one-tailed test if this were appropriate. ●

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 579

The effect size has been entered as a Cohen’s d of 1.2645161 in Figure 40.6. This is a calculated value based on the data in the study that we are using. If no such calculation is possible then you could enter other values according to whether you expect a small (0.2), medium (0.5) or large effect (0.8). Alternatively, if this were possible, the effect size could be based on data such as when a pilot study has been carried out or the typical effect size for similar research.

11/18/13 10:17 AM

580

CHAPTER 40  Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right ● The

significance level (‘α err prob’) is the conventional significance level of .05 though, of course, another value could be selected if there were reasons to be more or less stringent about avoiding rejecting the null hypothesis.

● The

required power has been set at .80 which is a realistic but nevertheless high requirement and difficult to exceed in practice in psychological research. Consequently it is generally accepted as a reasonable level to choose. Of course it could be lower but not below .50 as explained earlier.



The ‘Allocation ratio’ is simply the ratio of the two sample sizes. If you want these to be equal then the allocation ratio is 1. But, say, you wanted one group bigger than the other then you would have to juggle with this ratio. Probably there is little point in doing so for most research though sometimes researchers prefer to have small control groups relative to the experimental group.

Once your Input Parameters have been inserted in the relevant boxes, then press the ‘Calculate’ button. The interface will change to something like you see in the screenshot at Figure 40.8. The interface also includes a graphical representation of the power analysis. We discussed a similar graphical representation earlier (Figure 40.4) so refer to this discussion if you need clarification (p. 570). Remember that Figure 40.8 refers to the t-test analysis reported in Explaining statistics 14.1 in Chapter 14. The most important thing is that it suggests that a sample size of 22 could generate the .80 level of power that we stipulated. That is, 11 in each group. The ‘Actual power’ simply is the consequence of turning the sample sizes into

Figure 40.8 The G*Power interface showing the output parameters and graph

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 580

11/18/13 10:17 AM

40.5  Reporting the results 581

whole numbers whereas the calculation would produce decimals. So the Actual power is the power based on a total sample size of 22 rather than the Input Parameters that we entered, which can result in fractions for the sample sizes. One of the main reasons why the required sample size is quite low at 22 (11 in each group) is because the effect size (Cohen’s d = 1.26) is so large. It corresponds to a correlation of .53 between the independent and dependent variable. Now if this really were a pilot study then the implications are obviously very clear – however, this is data made up for the purpose of demonstrating the unrelated t-test so we should not get too excited. Pretending that it was a real pilot study then the researcher could be excused for feeling delighted. The effect is a very strong one requiring a sample size of only 22 in total to detect it with a power requirement of .80. This is about as good as it gets. Even if we increased the required power to .95 and made the significance level more stringent at .01, then G*Power tells us that a total sample size of only 50 is needed. That is to say, with just about the most demanding criteria for a power analysis, the required sample size is relatively small in this case. Try for yourself the effects of a low effect size of, say, .2 on the required sample size. You will find that the required sample size is massive to obtain the statistical significance at the required power. Power analysis encourages more careful thought in planning research and how it requires the researcher to evaluate what sorts of research outcomes have practical implications for decision-making following the completion of the proposed study. This is a very different sort of approach from that of relying solely on significance testing as the holy grail of research. Obviously, power analysis forces the researcher into considering the bigger picture of research, especially when important decisions about social interventions, therapy and so forth are contingent on the outcome of the research.

40.5 Reporting the results As might be expected, reporting the outcomes of a power analysis depends on what sort of analysis it is. If you simply wish to indicate the power of a statistical analysis that you have carried out, you could add immediately after you have reported statistical significance something like: ‘The observed power of this analysis was 0.7.’ Especially if the power was low, you might wish to comment on the size of the observed power. On the other hand, if you have carried out a power analysis in advance of the study, then the appropriate place to discuss this is where you discuss your samples in the Methods section of your report. The following might serve as a template for what you write in these circumstances: Power analysis was used to estimate the appropriate sample size(s) for the study. An examination of the research literature suggests that the typical effect size in similar studies is of the order of Cohen’s d = 0.5. For example, Smith and Lawson (2007) found an effect size of 0.47 in their study and Brown (2010) reports an effect size of 0.63. While Edwinston (2002) does not give an effect size, it can be calculated as 0.53 from their reported statistical analysis. It was decided to adopt the power level of 0.80 following what is considered satisfactory by authorities in the field (e.g. Cohen, 1988). The conventional 0.05 significance level was adopted as it was not considered that Type I errors were an important consideration, especially given that previous research had consistently detected significant trends in this sort of research (e.g. Green, 2006; Kirkham, 2002). A one-tailed significance test was used given the clear evidence from previous research that the experimental condition produces higher means than the control condition. Based on these parameters, the optimal sample size was 102 (51 in each group). A total of 55 participants were, in fact, run in the experimental condition and 53 in the control condition.

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 581

11/18/13 10:17 AM

582

CHAPTER 40  Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right

Research examples Statistical power Schimmack (2012) takes note of the evidence that despite numerous warnings about the statistical power of research in psychology, the typical statistical power of studies has not improved as a consequence. What has changed, however, over the years is the number of separate studies reported in a single paper. One possible implication of this is that multiple studies of modest statistical power result in a high probability of non-significant findings because the power of an analysis decreases the more significance tests that are applied. The statistically unacceptable but common practices employed by some researchers may be partially responsible. For example, HARKing (hypothesising after the results are known) can be involved in the problem. Because research is very expensive and because non-significant findings are difficult or impossible to publish, researchers design very complex studies which are capable of testing multiple hypotheses. There is a good chance that one of these hypotheses will appear to be supported but only because Type 1 error increases but is ignored. Thus something publishable may come out of the research even though it has little value otherwise. Schimmack provides information on the total number of participants required in multiple study articles to achieve 80% statistical power and to produce significant results in all of the studies. The study design involved in this exercise was a simple between-subjects experiment. These details do not matter so much as the fact that for a small effect size (Cohen’s d = .2) then for one study the total sample size would need to be 788, for five studies 6750 and for ten studies 15  820. One doesn’t need to know much about psychological research to realise that these numbers of participants would be remarkable. Simpson and Karageoghis (2006) carried out a study of the motivating effect and otherwise on athletic performance. Of significance here, they conducted a power analysis in order to decide a satisfactory sample size for their experimental group. Using alpha at .05, a two-tailed significance test, power set at .70, and an expected effect size in the moderate range, the appropriate sample size was calculated at 35. Woods, Rippeth, Conover, Carey, Parsons and Troster (2006) point out the possible problems of the statistical power of studies using novel neuropsychological interventions using clinical populations such as Parkinsonism sufferers. Such studies often have rather limited sample sizes. They examined the literature of the cognitive consequences of deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus. Using the findings of 30 different studies of this, they found that the studies only had adequate statistical power to detect real trends where the effect size was very large. However, for small, medium and large effect sizes there was too little power in the studies to detect real trends in the population. In other words, there was a significant risk of Type II errors.

Key points ●

Statistical power analysis is best used to inform the planning of research studies. It is less useful after data have been collected as part of the data analysis.

● Different

statistical tests require power calculations to be done differently. In particular, the measure of (standardised) effect size will differ.



Statistical power analysis includes assumptions and estimates which cannot be standardised for all circumstances as this would defeat its purpose. So reporting a power analysis may involve justifying the estimates and decisions that you have taken.

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 582

11/18/13 10:17 AM

Computer analysis 583



Computer analysis Power analysis with G*Power SPSS Statistics does compute some power values, but it is not very helpful for the bulk of the analyses outlined in this chapter. Exceptionally, this chapter contains a detailed illustration of using G*Power to calculate statistical power. Nevertheless, you may find the quick summary in Figure 40.9 a useful memory aid. Please check the following link for updates on G*Power and further documentation: http://www.psycho.uni-duesseldorf.de/ abteilungen/aap/gpower3/download-and-register. Although G*Power is free, its authors would appreciate that you cite one or both of the following papers in any published papers you produce using it in your research: ●

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149–1160.

● Faul,

F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.

Chapter 51 of Dennis Howitt and Duncan Cramer (2014), Introduction to SPSS Statistics in psychology: for version 22 and earlier, Harlow: Pearson, gives detailed step-by-step procedures for using G*Power 3.

Figure 40.9

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 583

Computer steps for power analysis using G*Power

11/18/13 10:17 AM

584

CHAPTER 40  Statistical power analysis: Getting the sample size right

Interpreting and reporting the output ● Although

this is often not done, ideally we need to carry out a power analysis to determine the size of the sample we need.

● The

results of this power analysis are reported in the Method section. We might write something like: ‘Statistical power analysis was used to estimate an appropriate sample size. Because of the lack of previous research in this field, it was decided to use Cohen’s high, medium and low effect sizes in order to explore the consequences of this on the appropriate sample size. It was decided that the significance (alpha) level would be kept high at 10% as would the power at 90%. It was felt to be much more important for this study to avoid Type II errors because of the risks of falsely accepting the null hypothesis.’

Screenshot 40.1

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 584

Opening window

11/18/13 10:17 AM

Screenshot 40.2

Select tests and values

Screenshot 40.3

Sample sizes output



M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 585

Computer analysis 585

11/18/13 10:17 AM

M40_HOWI0749_06_SE_C40.indd 586

11/18/13 10:17 AM

PART 6

Advanced qualitative or nominal techniques

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 587

11/18/13 3:10 PM

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 588

11/18/13 3:10 PM

Chapter 41

Log-linear methods The analysis of complex contingency tables

Overview ●

The analysis of nominal (category) data using chi-square is severely limited by the fact that a maximum of only two variables can be used in any one analysis.



Log-linear can be conceived as an extension of chi-square to cover greater numbers of variables.



Log-linear uses the likelihood ratio chi-square (rather than the Pearson chi-square we are familiar with from Chapter 15). This involves natural or Napierian logarithms.



The analysis essentially examines the adequacy of the various possible models. The simplest model merely involves the overall mean frequency – that is, the model does not involve any influence of the variables either acting individually or interactively in combination. The most complex models involve in addition the individual effects of the variables (main effects) as well as all levels of interactions between variables. If there are three variables, there would be three main effects, plus several two-way interactions plus one three-way interaction.



A saturated model is the most complex model and involves all of the possible components. As a consequence, the saturated model always explains the data completely, but at the price of not being the simplest model to fit the actual data. It is essentially a conceptual and computational device.

Preparation If you are hazy about contingency tables then look back to the discussion in Chapter 7. Also revise chi-square (Chapter 15) since it is involved in log-linear analyses. Log-linear shares concepts such as main effect and interaction with ANOVA which ought to be reviewed as general preparation (especially Chapter 23).

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 589

11/18/13 3:10 PM

590

CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

41.1 Introduction In essence, log-linear methods are used for the analysis of complex contingency (or crosstabulation) tables. Data in Chapter 15 which were analysed using chi-square could be subjected to log-linear procedures although with no particular benefits in that case. Log-linear goes further than this and comes into its own when dealing with three or more variables. Log-linear analysis identifies how the variables acting alone or in combination influence the frequencies in the cells of the contingency table. The frequencies can be regarded as if they are the dependent variable. Some basic ideas need to be mentioned: ●

Interactions  Like analysis of variance (ANOVA), log-linear analysis uses the concept of interactions between two or more variables. The concept is a little difficult to understand at first especially if you have not read our earlier discussions of interaction. It refers to the effects of the variables that cannot be explained by the effects of these variables acting separately. Interactions involve variables acting in combination. Much of this chapter is devoted to explaining the concept in more detail.



Models  A model in log-linear analysis is a statement (which can be expressed as a formula) which explains how the variables such as gender, age and social class result in the cell frequencies found in the contingency table. For example, one model might suggest that the pattern of frequencies in the contingency table is the result of the independent influences of the variable gender and the variable age. There are probably other contending models for all but the simplest cases. An alternative model for this example is that the data are the result of the influence of variable social class plus the influence of variable gender plus the combined influence of variable gender interact­ ing with the variable age. Table 41.1 gives the components of models for different numbers of variables in the contingency table. We will return to this later, but notice how the components include a constant (or the average frequency) plus the main effects of the variables plus interactive effects of the variables. Log-linear analysis helps a researcher to decide which of the possible models (i.e. which selection of the components in Table 41.1) is the best for the actual data. These different components will become clearer as we progress through this chapter. Model building can serve different purposes. Unless you have theoretical reasons for being interested in a particular model, then log-linear methods allow you to build up the model purely empirically.



Goodness-of-fit  This is simply the degree of match between the actual data and those values predicted on the basis of the model. Chi-square is a common measure of goodness-of-fit. Chi-square is zero if the observed data are exactly the same as the expected (or predicted) data. The bigger the value of chi-square, the greater the misfit between obtained and expected values. In Chapter 15, a significant value of chisquare caused us to reject the ‘model’ specified by the null hypothesis. A good-fitting model would have a chi-square value approximating zero whereas a badly fitting model would have a large chi-square value.



Pearson chi-square This is the version of chi-square used in Chapter 15 although common practice is simply to call it chi-square. The formula for the Pearson chisquare is: Pearson chi-square =



(observed − expected)2 expected

The Pearson chi-square is used in log-linear analysis but it is not essential.

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 590

11/18/13 3:10 PM

41.1  Introduction 591

Table 41.1

Possible model components for different sizes of contingency table

Component of model

1

2

3

4

5

yes

yes

yes

yes

yes

Main effects

A

A+B

A+B+C

A+B+C+D

A+B+C+D+E

Two-way interactions



A*B

A*B

A*B

A* B

A*C

A*C

A* C

B*C

A*D

A* D

B*C

A* E

B*D

B* C

C*D

B* D

Overall mean (equal frequencies)

B* E C* D C* E Three-way interactions

A*B*C

A*B*C

A* B* C

A*B*D

A* B* D

A*C*D

A* B* E

B*C*D

A* C* D A* C* E A* D* E B* C* D B* C* E B* D* E C* D* E

Four-way interactions

A*B*C*D

A* B* C* D A* B* C* E A* B* D* E B* C* D* E



Likelihood ratio chi-square  This is the more common formula when doing log-linear analysis: Likelihood ratio chi-square = 2 × ∑ observed frequency × ln of

observed frequency expected frequency

The term ln is a symbol for natural logarithm. Don’t worry if you know nothing about natural logarithms. Although tables of natural logarithms are available, it is easier to obtain them from a scientific calculator (or the calculator built into Windows, for instance). Observed frequency refers to the obtained data and expected frequency refers to the values expected according to the particular model being tested. ●

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 591

Differences between Pearson and likelihood ratio chi-square The formulae give slightly different values of chi-square for small sample sizes but converge as the sample sizes get large. Both formulae are often computed as measures of goodness-of-fit by computer programs for log-linear analysis. Nevertheless, it is best to concentrate on likelihood ratio chi-square in log-linear analysis because of its additive properties.

11/18/13 3:10 PM

592

CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

This means that different components of chi-square can be added together to give the combined effects of different components of the chosen model. The Pearson chisquare does not act additively so cannot be used in this way, hence its comparative unimportance in log-linear analysis.

41.2 A two-variable example The distinctive approach of log-linear analysis can take a little time to absorb. Its characteristic logic is probably best explained by re-analysing an example from Chapter 15. The study of favourite types of television programme of males and females (Explaining statistics 15.1) will be presented using the log-linear perspective. The data are given in Table 41.2, but they are exactly the same as the data in Table 15.8. The two variables were gender and favourite type of programme. In the (Pearson) chi-square analysis (Explaining statistics 15.1) there is a gender difference in favourite type of television programme. Another way of putting this is that there is an interaction between a person’s gender and their favourite type of television programme. (In Chapter 15, it was found that gender and favourite type of programme acting separately were insufficient to account for the data. The expected frequencies in that chapter are the frequencies expected on the basis of gender and programme effects having separate and unrelated effects. In Chapter 15, a significant value of chi-square meant that the distribution of cell frequencies could not be explained on the basis of this independent influence of gender and favourite programme type. The different genders had different preferences. This would be an interaction in terms of log-linear analysis.) A log-linear analysis of the data in Table 41.2 would examine possible underlying models (combinations of the variables) which might predict the obtained data. Theoretically, there are a number of possibilities according to log-linear analysis: ●

Equal frequencies model  This suggests that the observed cell frequencies are merely the total of cell frequencies divided equally between the cells. Since there are 119 observations in Table 41.2 and six cells then we would expect a frequency of 119 ÷ 6 = 19.833 in each cell. Obviously this model, even if it fits the data best, is virtually a non-model.



Main effects model This suggests that the observed cell frequencies are the consequence of the separate effects of the variables which add together to give their overall effect. Although this might seem an important possibility if you recall main effects for ANOVA, in log-linear analysis, main effects are often trivial. The object of log-linear analysis is to account for the pattern of observed frequencies in the data. In Table 41.2 note that there are slightly unequal numbers of males and females (60 males and 59 females) but, more importantly, the choices of the different programme types are unequal. That is, the different values of gender (male and female) and favourite television programme (soap opera, crime drama and neither) are not equally represented. For the main effect of gender, the inequality is small (60 males versus 59 females), but

Table 41.2 Data to be modelled using log-linear analysis

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 592

Soap opera

Crime drama

Neither

Males

observed = 27

observed = 14

observed = 19

Females

observed = 17

observed = 33

observed = 9

11/18/13 3:10 PM

41.2  A two-variable example 593

it is somewhat larger for the main effect of favourite television programme (44 choosing soap operas, 47 choosing crime dramas and 28 choosing neither). The main effects merely refer to these inequalities which may be uninteresting in terms of the primary purpose of the analysis. In our example, a researcher is likely not to be particularly interested in these main effects but much more interested if the interaction between gender and favourite programme type explains the data. In order for there to be no main effects, each of the categories of each of the variables would have to have the same frequency. This is rare in research. ●

The interaction(s) An interaction is the effect of the interrelationship between the variables. In the present example, because we have only two variables, there is just one interaction which could be termed gender × favourite TV programme interaction. You will see from Table 41.1 that had there been more variables there would be more interactions to investigate. The number of interactions escalates with increasing numbers of variables (much as it does for ANOVA). Interactions interest researchers because they indicate the associations or correlations between variables.

The interactions and main effects are combined in log-linear analysis in order to see what main effects and what interactions are necessary to best account for (fit with) the observed data. Log-linear analysis for this simple example involves the consideration of several different models.



■ Step 1: The equal frequencies model In a manner of speaking, this is the no-model model. It tests the idea that the cell fre­ quencies require no explanation since they are equally distributed. This is not the same as the null hypothesis predictions made in Chapter 15 since these predicted not equal frequencies but proportionate frequencies according to the marginal totals. The equal fre­ quencies model simply assumes that all of the cells of the contingency table have equal frequencies. Since we have a total frequency of 119 in the six cells of our analysis, the equal frequencies model predicts (expects) that there should be 119/6 or 19.833 in each cell as shown in Table 41.3. The likelihood ratio chi-square applied to this table is calculated in Table 41.4. Remember that the natural logarithms are obtained from a scientific calculator or one you find as a program on your computer. The use of natural logarithms is only important for understanding the basic calculation of log-linear. The fit of the equal frequencies model to the data is poor. The likelihood ratio chisquare is 19.418. This is the amount of misfit of that particular model to the data. (It is also the amount by which the main effects and the interactions can increase the fit of the best model to the data.)

Table 41.3

Contingency table for testing the equal frequencies model, i.e. the expected frequencies Soap opera

Males Females Total

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 593

Crime drama

Neither

observed = 27

observed = 14

observed = 19

expected = 19.833

expected = 19.833

expected = 19.833

observed = 17

observed = 33

observed = 9

expected = 19.833

expected = 19.833

expected = 19.833

Total

119

11/18/13 3:10 PM

594

CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

Table 41.4

Calculation of the fit of the equal frequencies model

Observed frequency

Expected frequency according to equal frequencies model

Observed + expected

Natural logarithm of observed + expected

Observed frequency × natural logarithm of observed + expected

27

19.833

1.361

0.308

8.329

14

19.833

0.857

−0.154

−2.620

19

19.833

0.706

−0.348

−4.876

17

19.833

1.664

0.509

16.802

33

19.833

0.958

−0.043

−0.815

9

19.933

0.454

0.857

−7.111 Total = 9.709

Likelihood ratio chi-square = 2 × total = 2 × 9.709 = 19.418

The differences between the values expected according to the model and what is actually found in the data are known as the residuals. The residuals can be used to assess the fit of the model to the data in addition to the likelihood ratio chi-squares. Often, residuals are standardised so that comparisons can be made easily between the different cells, in which case they are known as standardised or adjusted residuals. The smaller the residuals the better the fit of the model to the data.



■ Step 2: The saturated model The log-linear analysis of these data could be carried out in a number of ways since there are a variety of different models that could be tested. In general, we will concentrate on the procedures which would commonly be employed when using computer programs such as SPSS Statistics. Often these compute the saturated model for you. A saturated model is one which includes all of the possible components as shown in Table 41.1 which, consequently, accounts perfectly for the data. That is, the values predicted by the saturated model are exactly the same as the data. Any model based on all of the com­ ponents by definition accounts for the data perfectly. Since there is always a perfect correspondence or fit between the observed data and the predictions based on the likelihood ratio chi-square for the saturated model, this chi-square is always zero for the saturated model. Table 41.5 gives the data and the expected frequencies for the saturated model. Notice, as we have already indicated, that the observed and expected frequencies for any cell of the contingency table are identical for this model. We will not bother to do this calculation. It is worth noting that computer programs often routinely increase the observed values by 0.5. This is done to avoid undesirable divisions by zero in the calculation while making very little difference to the calculation otherwise.



■ Step 3: Preparing to test for the main effects components of the model The perfectly fitting model of the data (the saturated model) involves all possible components. It is not quite as impressive as the perfect fit suggests. We do not know what it

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 594

11/18/13 3:10 PM

41.2  A two-variable example 595

Table 41.5

Contingency table for testing the saturated model Soap opera

Males Females

Crime drama

Neither

observed = 27

observed = 14

observed = 19

expected = 27.000

expected = 14.000

expected = 19.000

observed = 17

observed = 33

observed = 9

expected = 17.000

expected = 33.000

expected = 9.000

Total

Total

119

is about our model which caused such a good fit. It could be the effects of gender, the effects of the type of programme, or the effects of the interaction of gender with type of programme, or any combination of these three possibilities. It could even mean that the equal frequencies model is correct if we had not already rejected that possibility. Further exploration is necessary to assess which of these components are contributing to the goodness-of-fit of the data to that predicted by the model. Any component of the model which does not increase the goodness-of-fit of the model to the data is superfluous since it does nothing to explain the data. (To anticipate a common practice in log-linear analysis, the corollary of this is also true: components are only retained if they decrease the fit of the model when they are removed.) (Usually in the initial stages of log-linear analyses using a computer, similar com­ ponents of the model are dealt with collectively. That is, the main effects of gender and favourite programme type are dealt with as if they were a unit of analysis. Had there been more than one interaction, these would also be dealt with collectively. At a later stage, it is usual to extend the analysis to deal with the combined components indivi­ dually. That is, the data are explored in more detail in order to assess what main effects are actually influencing the data.) To reiterate what we have already achieved we can say that we have examined two extremes of the model-building process: the saturated model and the equal frequencies model. We have established that the equal frequencies model is a poor fit to the data on this occasion (the saturated model is always a perfect fit). The misfit of the equal frequencies model to the data (likelihood ratio chi-square = 19.418) is the amount of improvement in fit achieved by the saturated model.



■ Step 4: TV programme type main effect Main effects are one level of components in the saturated model. Understanding their calculation is fairly simple. Let us take the main effect of programme type. In order to predict the frequencies in the data based solely on the effects of the different programme type we simply replace each cell by the average of the frequencies in cells referring to that programme type. This in effect means that for our example we combine the data frequencies for the males and females who prefer soap operas and average this total by the number of cells involved (i.e. two cells). Twenty-seven males and 17 females claim to prefer soap operas so the total is 44, which is divided between the two cells involved in this case. This gives us a predicted frequency on the basis of the main effects model for programme type of 22.00 in each of the soap opera cells. This is shown in Table 41.6. The predicted value for crime drama is 14 + 33 divided by 2 which equals 23.50. The predicted value for the neither category is 19 + 9 divided by 2 = 14.00. Again these can be seen in Table 41.6.

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 595

11/18/13 3:10 PM

596

CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

Table 41.6

Table of data and expected frequencies based solely on the main effect of programme type Soap opera

Males Females Total

Crime drama

Neither

observed = 27

observed = 14

observed = 19

expected = 22.000

expected = 23.500

expected = 14.000

observed = 17

observed = 33

observed = 9

expected = 22.000

expected = 23.500

expected = 14.000

44

47

28

Total

119

Now one can calculate the goodness-of-fit of this model simply by calculating the likelihood ratio chi-square for the data in Table 41.6. Just follow the model of the calculation in Table 41.4. The value of the likelihood ratio chi-square is 13.849. Compare this with the misfit based on the equal frequencies model (likelihood ratio chi-square = 19.418). It seems that there has been an improvement of 19.418 − 13.849 = 5.569 in the fit due to the programme main effect. (Remember that the bigger the likelihood ratio chi-square then the poorer the fit of the model to the data.) Because the likelihood ratio chi-square has additive properties, this difference of 5.569 is the contribution of the main effect of programme type.



■ Step 5: Gender main effect Because the frequencies of males and females in the data are nearly equal, there clearly is a minimal main effect due to the variable gender in this case. Nevertheless, this minimal value needs to be calculated. A similar procedure is adopted to calculate the main effects of gender. This time we need to sum the frequencies over the three different programme types for each gender separately and average this total frequency by the three programme types. Thus the sum of the observed frequencies for males in each of the three different programme type conditions is (27 + 14 + 19)/3 = 60/3 = 20. This gives a predicted value per male cell of 20. This is entered in Table 41.7. Similarly, the calculation for females is to sum the three observed frequencies and divide by the number of female cells. This is (17 + 33 + 9)/3 = 59/3 = 19.667. Again these values are entered in Table 41.7. The likelihood ratio chi-square for the main effect of gender in Table 41.7 is 19.405. Compared with the value of 19.418 for the equal frequencies model, there is virtually no change, indicating the smallness of the gender difference in row frequencies. The improvement in fit due to gender alone is only 0.013.

Table 41.7

Table of data and expected frequencies based on the main effect of gender type Soap opera

Males Females Total

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 596

Crime drama

Neither

observed = 27

observed = 14

observed = 19

expected = 20.000

expected = 20.000

expected = 20.000

observed = 17

observed = 33

observed = 9

expected = 19.667

expected = 19.667

expected = 19.667

Total 60 59 119

11/18/13 3:10 PM

41.2  A two-variable example 597



■ Step 6: The main effects of programme type plus gender This can now be obtained. It involves taking each cell in turn and working out the effect on the frequencies of the programme type and the gender concerned. This is done relative to the frequencies from the equal frequencies model (that is, 119/6 = 19.833 in every case). So, looking at Table 41.6, the expected frequency for soap operas is 22.000. This means that being a soap opera cell increases the frequency by 22.000 − 19.833 = 2.167 as shown in Table 41.8. It may sound banal, but in order to add in the effect of being a soap opera cell we have to add 2.167 to the expected frequencies under the equal frequencies model. Similarly, being a crime drama cell increases the frequency to 23.500 from our baseline equal frequencies expectation of 19.833. Being a crime drama cell increases the frequency by 23.500 − 19.833 = 3.667. In contrast, being in the neither category tends to decrease the frequencies in the cell compared with the equal frequencies expectation of 19.833. From Table 41.6 we can see that the expected frequencies in the neither column due to programme type are 14.000, which is below the equal frequencies expectation of 19.833 as shown in Table 41.8. Thus, being a neither cell changes frequencies by 14.000 − 19.833 = −5.833. That is, being neither decreases frequencies by −5.833. In order to adjust the equal frequencies expectations for the programme type main effect, we have to add 2.167 to the soap opera cells, add 3.667 to the crime drama cells and subtract 5.833 from (that is add −5.833 to) the neither cells. This can be seen in Table 41.8. We also need to make similar adjustments for the main effect of gender although these are much smaller. Compared with the equal frequencies value of 19.833, the male cells have an expected frequency of 20.000 which is an increase of 0.167. In order to adjust the equal frequencies baseline of 19.833 for a cell being male we therefore have to add 0.167. This can be seen in Table 41.8. For female cells, the expected frequency is 19.667, a reduction of 0.166. In short, we add −0.166 for a cell being female. This is also shown in Table 41.8. (Of course, the additions and subtractions for the males and females should be identical, which they are within the limits of calculation rounding.) At this point there is a big problem. That is, the values of the expected frequencies based on the main effects model give the wrong answers according to computer output. For that matter, it does not give the same expected frequencies as given in the equivalent Pearson chi-square calculation we did in Chapter 15. Actually, the computer prints our expected frequencies which are the same as those calculated in Chapter 15. The problem



Table 41.8

Table of expected (predicted) frequencies based on adding the main effects of programme type and gender to the equal frequencies expectation Soap opera

Males

Females

Crime drama

Neither

observed = 27

observed = 14

observed = 19

expected = −19.833 + 2.167 + 0.167 = 22.167a

expected = 19.833 + 3.667 + 0.167 = 23.667a

expected = 19.833 + −5.833 + 0.167 = 14.167a

observed = 17

observed = 33

observed = 9

expected = 19.833 + 2.167 + −0.166 = 21.834a

expected = 19.883 + 3.667 + −0.166 = 23.334a

expected = 19.883 + −5.833 + −0.166 = 13.834a

Total a

Total 60

59

119

These hand-calculated values are very approximate and do not correspond to the best values for reasons discussed in the text.

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 597

11/18/13 3:10 PM

598



CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

Table 41.9

Table of expected (predicted) frequencies based on adding the main effects of programme type and gender to the equal frequencies expectation as obtained by the iterative computer process Soap opera

Males Females

Crime drama

Neither

observed = 27

observed = 14

observed = 19

expected = 22.18

expected = 23.70

expected = 14.72

observed = 17

observed = 33

observed = 9

expected = 21.82

expected = 23.30

expected = 13.88

44

47

28

Total

Total 60 59 119

is that we are not actually doing what the computer is doing. Think back to the two-way analysis of variance. These calculations worked as long as you have equal numbers of scores in each of the cells. Once you have unequal numbers, then the calculations have to be done a different way (and best of all by computer). This is because you are not adding effects proportionately once you have different cell frequencies. In log-linear analysis, the problem arises because the marginal totals are usually unequal for each variable. This inequality means that simple linear additions and subtractions of main effects such as we have just done do not give the best estimates. That is in essence why a computer program is vital in log-linear analysis. Better estimates of expected frequencies are made using an iterative process. This means that an approximation is calculated but then refined by re-entering the approximation in recalculations. This is done repeatedly until a minimum criterion of change is found between calculations (i.e. between iterations). Computer programs allow you to decide on the size of this change and even the maximum number of iterations. Now that we have some idea of how the adjustments are made for the main effects, even though we must rely on the computer for a bit of finesse, we will use the computergenerated values to finish off our explanation. Table 41.9 contains the observed and expected values due to the influence of the main effects as calculated by the computer’s iterative process. The value of the likelihood ratio chi-square for the data in Table 41.9 is, according to the computer, 13.841 (which is significant at 0.001 with df = 2). At this point, we can obtain the value of the gender*programme type interaction. We now know the following: ● The

fit of the saturated model which includes main effects plus the interaction is 0.000.



The fit of the model based on the two main effects is 13.841.



The fit of the model based on the equal frequencies model is 19.418.

It becomes a simple matter of subtraction to work out the improvement in fit due to the different components. Thus: The increase in fit due to the two main effects = 19.418 − 13.841 = 5.577. The increase in fit due to the interaction = 13.841 − 0.000 = 13.841. These numerical values are likelihood ratio chi-squares. Only the interaction is statist­ ically significant out of these major components. The main effect of programme type taken on its own would be statistically significant as it includes fewer degrees of freedom and has nearly the same likelihood ratio chi-square value. This is of no real interest as it merely shows that different proportions of people were choosing the different programme

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 598

11/18/13 3:10 PM

41.3  A three-variable example 599

types as their favourites. In short, the interesting part of the model is the interaction which is statistically significant. Formally, this model is expressed simply as: constant (i.e. equal frequency cell mean) + programme main effect + A*B interaction As the interaction is fairly simple, it is readily interpreted with the help of Table 41.8. So we can conclude that in order to model the data effectively we need the two-variable interaction. This we did in essence in Chapter 15 when interpreting the Pearson chisquare analysis of those data. Remember that the interaction is based on the residuals in that table (i.e. the differences between the observed and expected frequencies). As can be clearly seen, males are less inclined to choose crime dramas than women but are more inclined to choose soap operas.

41.3 A three-variable example Interactions when the data have three or more variables become a little more difficult to understand. In any case, only when there are three or more variables does log-linear analysis achieve much more than the Pearson chi-square described in Chapter 15. Consequently it is important to study one of these more complex examples. Even though log-linear analysis usually requires the use of a computer using the iterative procedures, quite a lot can be achieved by trying to understand approximately what the computer is doing when it is calculating several second-order and higher-order interactions. Table 41.1 gives the possible model components of any log-linear analysis for one to five variables. It is very unlikely that anyone would wish to use log-linear analysis when they have just one variable, but it is useful to start from there just so that the patterns build up more clearly in the table. Computer programs can handle more variables than five, but we are constrained by space and, moreover, log-linear analyses of ten variables are both atypical of psychological research designs and call for a great deal of statistical sophistication – especially experience with simpler log-linear analyses. Basically, the more variables you have the more components there will be to the model. All models consist of main effects plus interactions. The variables involved in model-building are those which might possibly cause differences between the frequencies in the different cells. These variables have to be measurable by the researcher too for them to be in the analysis. Also note that the more variables in a model, the more complex the interactions. Our example involves three variables. If you look in the column for three variables in Table 41.1 you will find listed all of the possible components of the model for these data. In this column there are three main effects (one for each of the variables), three two-way interactions between all possible distinct pairs taken from the three variables and one three-way interaction. The analysis is much the same as for our earlier two-variable example, but there are more components to add in. In particular, the meaning of the interactions needs to be clarified as there are now four of them rather than just one. Remember that it is usual to take similar levels of the model together for the initial model fitting. Thus all the main effects are combined; all of the second-order interactions (two-variable interactions) together; all of the third-order (three-variable interactions) together and so forth. Only when this analysis is done is it usual to see precisely which combinations at which levels of the model are having an effect. Our example involves the relationship between gender, sexual abuse and physical abuse in a sample of psychiatric patients. The data are to be found in Table 41.10 which gives the three-way crosstabulation or contingency table for our example. The num­ bers in the cells are frequencies. Each of the variables has been coded as a dichotomy:

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 599

11/18/13 3:10 PM

600



CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

Table 41.10

A three-way contingency showing the relationship between gender, sexual abuse and physical abuse in a sample of psychiatric hospital patients

Variable B

Variable C

Variable A Gender

Margin totals

Sexual abuse

Physical abuse

Female

Male

Sexually abused

Physical abuse

45

55

100

No physical

40

60

100

Physical abuse

55

45

100

No physical

80

20

100

220

180

400

Not sexually abused Margin totals

a) female or male, b) sexually abused or not and c) physically abused or not. (Variables could have more than two categories, but this is not the case for these particular data.) The researchers are interested in explaining the frequencies in the table on the basis of the three variables – gender, sexual abuse and physical abuse – acting individually (main effects) or in combination (interactions). This would be described as a three-way con­ tingency table because it involves three variables. It is worthwhile remembering that the more variables and the more categories of each variable the greater the sample size needs to be in order to have sufficient frequencies in each cell. Two of the possible models are easily tested. They are the equal frequencies and the saturated models, which are the extreme possibilities in log-linear analyses. The equal frequencies model simply involves the first row of Table 41.10 and no other influences. The saturated model includes all sources of influence in the table for the column for three variables.



■ Step 1: The equal frequencies model The equal frequencies model is, in a sense, the worst-fit scenario for a model. It is what the data would look like if none of the variables in isolation or combination was needed to explain the data. The equal frequencies model merely describes what the data would look like if the frequencies were equally distributed through the cells of the table. As there are eight cells and a total of 400 observations, under the equal frequencies model it would be expected that each cell contains 400/8 = 50 cases. This model and the calculation of its fit with the observed data for which we are developing a model are shown in Table 41.11. Just a reminder – the likelihood ratio chi-square is zero if the model fits the data exactly and increasingly bigger with greater amounts of misfit between the data and the data as predicted by the model. The chi-square value for the equal frequencies model is an indication of how much the variables and their interaction have to explain. The value of 44.58 obtained for the likelihood ratio chi-square on the equal frequencies model indicates that the data are poorly explained by that model. That is, there is a lot of variation in the frequencies which remains to be explained by models other than the equal frequencies model. Notice that the equal frequencies model only contains the mean frequency which is one of the potential components of all models. The equal frequencies value is sometimes called the constant. If it is zero or nearly zero then the equal frequencies model fits the model well. Do not get too excited if the equal frequencies model fits badly

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 600

11/18/13 3:10 PM

41.3  A three-variable example 601

Table 41.11

The calculation of the likelihood ratio chi-square for the equal frequencies model

Observed frequency

Expected frequency according to the equal frequencies model

Observed + expected

Natural logarithm of observed + expected

Observed frequency × natural logarithm of observed + expected

45

50.0

0.90

−0.1054

−4.743

40

50.0

0.80

−0.2231

−8.924

55

50.0

1.10

0.0953

5.242

80

50.0

1.60

0.4700

37.600

55

50.0

1.10

0.0953

5.242

60

50.0

1.20

0.1823

10.938

45

50.0

0.90

−0.1054

−4.741

20

50.0

0.40

−0.9163

−18.326 Total = 22.290

Likelihood ratio chi-square = 2 × sum of final column = 2 × 22.290 = 44.580

since the variation in frequencies between the cells might be explained by the main effects. To reiterate, main effects in the log-linear analysis are often of very little interest to psychologists. Only rarely will real-life data have no main effects in log-linear analysis since main effects occur when the categories of a variable are unequally distributed. In our data in Table 41.10, the marginal totals for physical abuse and sexual abuse are the same since equal numbers had been abused as had not been abused. Nevertheless, there is a possible main effect for gender since there are more females in the study than males. Whether or not this gender difference is significant has yet to be tested. So whatever the final model we select, it has already been clearly established that there is plenty of variation in the cell means to be explained by the main effects acting independently and the two-way interactions of pairs of these variables plus the three-way interaction of all of the variables.



■ Step 2: The saturated model This model involves all of the possible variables acting separately and in combination. It includes all components given in Table 41.1 for a given number of variables. For a three-way contingency table the saturated model includes the mean frequency per cell (i.e. constant) plus the main effects plus the three two-variable interactions plus the three-variable interaction. Predictions based on the saturated model are exactly the same as the data themselves – they have to be since the saturated model includes every possible component of the model and so there is no other possible source of variation. It is hardly worth computing the saturated model as it has to be a perfect fit to the data thus giving a likelihood ratio chi-square of 0.000. A zero value like this indicates a perfect fit between the observed data and the expectations (predictions) based on the model. Remember that for the saturated model, most computer programs will automatically add 0.5 to the observed frequencies to avoid divisions by zero which are unhelpful mathematically. This addition of 0.5 to each of the frequencies is not always necessary so some computer programs will give you the choice of not using it. Its influence is so negligible that the analysis is hardly affected.

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 601

11/18/13 3:10 PM

602



CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

■ Step 3: Building up the main-effects model The process of building up a model in log-linear analysis is fairly straightforward once the basic principles are understood, as we have seen. The stumbling block is the calculation of expected frequencies when marginal frequencies are unequal. They are unequal most of the time in real data. In these circumstances, only an approximate explanation can be given of what the computer is doing. Fortunately, as we have already seen, we can go a long way using simple maths. Table 41.12 contains the expected frequencies based on different components of the model. Remember that the expected frequencies are those based on a particular model or component of the model. The first column contains the data (which are exactly the same as the predictions based on the saturated model already discussed). The fifth column gives the expected frequencies based on the equal frequencies model. This has already been discussed – the frequencies are merely the total frequencies averaged over the number of cells. The next three cells have the major heading ‘Main effects’, and there are separate columns for the main effect of gender, the main effect of sexual abuse and the main effect of physical abuse. The fourth column headed ‘All’ is for the added effect of these three main effects. How are these expected (predicted) values calculated? They are simply the averages of the appropriate cells. Thus for females, the four cells in Table 41.12 are 45, 40, 55 and 80, which totals 220. Thus if the cells in the female column reflect only the effects of being female then we would expect all four female cells to contain 220/4 = 55.00 cases. In Table 41.12, the expected frequencies under gender for the four female cells are all 55.00. Similarly for the four remaining cells in that column which all involve males, the total male frequency is 180 so we would expect 180/4 or 45.00 in each of the male cells. Exactly the same process is applied to the sexual abuse column. Two hundred of the cases were sexually abused in childhood whereas 200 were not. Thus we average the 200 sexually abused cases over the four cells in Table 41.12 which involve sexually abused individuals (i.e. 200/4 = 50.00). Then we average the 200 non-sexually abused individuals over the four cells containing non-sexually abused individuals (i.e. 200/4 = 50.00). Because there are equal numbers of sexually and non-sexually abused individuals, no main effect of sexual abuse is present and all of the values in the sexual abuse column are 50.00. Given that there are also 200 physically abused and 200 non-physically abused cases, it is not surprising to find that all of the expected frequencies are 50.00 in the physical abuse column too. The reasoning is exactly the same as for sexual abuse in the previous paragraph. The combined main effects column labelled ‘All’ is easily computed for our example. It is simply the combined individual effects of the three separate main effects. So it is the effect of gender plus sexual abuse plus physical abuse. Thus being female adds a frequency of five compared with the equal frequencies model figure of 50.00, being sexually abused adds zero and being physically abused adds zero. For example, for the first row which consists of 45 females who had been sexually abused and physically abused, we take the equal frequencies frequency of 50.00 and add 5 for being female, + 0 for being sexually abused and + 0 for being physically abused. This gives the expected figure of 55.00 under the all main effects column. To give another example, take the fifth row down where the data give a frequency of 55. This row refers to males who had been sexually abused and physically abused. Being male subtracts 5.00 from the equal frequency value, being sexually abused adds nothing and being physically abused also adds nothing. So our expected value is 50 − 5 + 0 + 0 = 45, the expected value for all of the main effects added together.

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 602

11/18/13 3:10 PM

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 603

male

20

no

no

yes

yes

no

no

no

yes

no

yes

no

yes

no

yes

Physical abuse

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

Equal frequencies model

* Between two or more variable names is one way of indicating interations.

male

male

male

55

45

female

80

60

female

55

yes

yes

female

female

45

Sexual abuse

Gender

Details of cell

45.00

45.00

45.00

45.00

55.00

55.00

55.00

55.00

Gender

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

Sexual

Main effects

The expected (or predicted) frequencies based on separate components of the model

40

Data

Table 41.12

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

Physical

45.00

45.00

45.00

45.00

55.00

55.00

55.00

55.00

All

32.50

32.50

57.50

57.50

67.50

67.50

42.50

42.50

Gender* Sexual

40.00

50.00

40.00

50.00

60.00

50.00

60.00

50.00

Gender* Physical

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

50.00

Sexual* Physical

Two-way interactions

27.77

37.23

52.23

62.77

72.33

62.77

47.77

37.23

All

41.3  A three-variable example 603

11/18/13 3:10 PM

604



CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

■ Step 4: The two-variable interactions The two-way interactions are not difficult to estimate either. The two-way interaction for gender*sexual abuse is obtained by combining the physical abuse categories. In our example, there are some who have been physically abused and some who have not among the females who had been sexually abused. Of these sexually abused females, 45 had been physically abused and 40 had not been physically abused. Combining these two frequencies and averaging them across the two relevant cells gives us: 45 + 40 85 = = 42.5 2 2 This is the value that you see under the gender*sexual abuse interaction for the first two rows. If you need another example, take the last two rows which have values in the data column of 45 and 20. These rows consist of the males who had not been sexually abused. One row is those who had been physically abused and the other those who had not been physically abused. The two-way interaction of gender*sexual abuse is obtained by adding together the two different physical abuse categories and entering the average of these into the last two rows. So the frequencies are 45 and 20 which equals 65, which divided between the two relevant cells gives us 32.5. This is the value that you see for the gender*sexual abuse interaction for the final two rows. What about the next interaction – gender*physical abuse? The calculation is basically the same. The only difficulty is that the rows corresponding to the cells we are interested in are physically further apart in the table. The gender*physical abuse interaction is obtained by combining the sexual abuse categories (i.e. the sexually abused and nonsexually abused). Let us take the females who had not been physically abused. These are the second and fourth rows. If we look at the observed values in the data these are frequencies of 40 and 80. The average of these is 60, and this is the value you find in the second and fourth rows of the gender*physical abuse interaction. The sexual abuse*physical abuse interaction is calculated in a similar way – this time we combine the male and female groups for each of the four sexual abuse*physical abuse combinations. Take the sexually and physically abused individuals. These are to be found in rows 1 and 5. The data (observed) values for these rows are 45 and 55. This averages at 50.00 – the value of the entry for this two-way interaction in the first and fifth rows. (Actually all of the rows for this particular column have the same value indicating a lack of a sexual abuse*physical abuse interaction.) The combined effects of the three two-way interactions cannot be seen directly from the table. This is because the values are based on an iterative process which involves several computational stages which are best done by the computer. The values in the last column of Table 41.12 are taken from SPSS Statistics computer output. Although we will not be showing this calculation here because of its complexity, we can show the essential logic although, as you will see, it gives slightly the wrong answers. All effects in log-linear analysis are additive so we should be able to combine the three two-way interactions in order to obtain the sum of the three two-way interactions. This is quite simple. Compared with the equal frequencies mean frequency of 50.00 for each cell, what is the effect of each interaction? Taking the first row, we can see that the gender*sexual abuse interaction changes the score by −7.50 (i.e. 42.5 − 50.00), the gender*physical abuse interaction changes the score by 0.00 (i.e. 50.00 − 50.00) and the sexual abuse*physical abuse interaction changes the score by 0.00 (50.00 − 50.00). Adding these separate effects to the equal frequencies mean frequency of 50.00 we get: 50.00 + (−7.50) + 0.00 + 0.00 = 42.50

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 604

11/18/13 3:10 PM

41.3  A three-variable example 605

This at first sight is the wrong answer since it is nowhere near the 37.23 obtained from the computer. What we have not allowed for is the fact that these interactions also include the effect of the main effects. The main effects for this row combined to give a prediction of 55.00 compared with the equal frequencies mean of 50.00. That is to say, the main effects are increasing the prediction for this row by 5.00. This would have to be taken away from the prediction based on the interaction to leave the pure effects of the two-way interactions. So our value 42.50 contains 5.00 due to the main effects; getting rid of the main effects gives us the prediction of 37.50 based on the two-way interactions. This is pretty close to the 37.23 predicted by the model but not sufficiently so. The unequal marginal totals necessitate the adjustments made automatically by the iterative computer program. Had our marginal totals been a lot more unequal then our fit to the computer’s value would have been much poorer. Simple methods are only suitable as ways of understanding the basics of the process. If you would like another example of how the entries are computed, look at the final row of Table 41.12. The predicted value based on all the two-way interactions is 27.77. How is that value achieved? Notice that the two-way gender*sexual abuse interaction prediction is 32.50, which is 17.50 less than that according to the equal frequencies model prediction of 50.00; the gender*physical abuse prediction is 40.00, which is 10.00 less and the sexual abuse*physical abuse prediction is 50.00, exactly the same. So to get the prediction based on the three two-way interactions together, the calculation is the equal frequencies mean (50.00) + (−17.50) + (−10.00) + 0.00 = 22.50, but then we need to take away the influence of all the main effects which involves adding 5.00 this time. Thus we end up with a prediction of 27.50. Again this is not precisely the computer predicted value but it is close enough for purposes of explanation. Remember, it is only close because the main effects are small or zero. What is the normal output of a computer program such as SPSS Statistics? The important point to remember is that it is usual to explore the model first of all as combined effects – the sum of the interactions, the sum of the main effects – rather than the individual components in the first analysis. For the data in Table 41.10 we obtained the information in Tables 41.13 and 41.14 from the computer by stipulating a saturated model. What do Tables 41.13 and 41.14 tell us? Remember that when we assessed the fit of the data based on the equal frequencies model we obtained a likelihood ratio chi-square value of 44.580. This large value indicates a large misfit of the model to the data. (The smaller the size of chi-square the better the fit.) Notice that this value of chi-square is exactly the same (within the errors of rounding) as the chi-square value in Table 41.13

Table 41.13

Tests of the increase in fit for the main effects and higher-order effects

Level of effects

Degrees offreedom

Likelihood ratio chi-square

Probability

three-way interaction

1

10.713

0.0011

2 (and above)

all the two-way interactions + the three-way interaction

4

40.573

0.0000

1 (and above)

all the main effects + the two-way interaction + the three-way interactiononly

7

44.579

0.0000

3

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 605

Types of effectinvolved

11/18/13 3:10 PM

606

CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

Table 41.14

Tests that the levels of effect are zero

Level of effects

Types of effect involved

Degrees of freedom

Likelihood ratio chi-square

Probability

1

all the main effects only

3

4.007

0.2607

2

all the two-way interactions only

3

29.860

0.0000

3

three-way interaction only

1

10.713

0.0011

for the contribution of the main effects, two-way interactions and three-way interactions. Thus 44.580 is the improvement in the fit of the model created by including the three different levels of effect together. If we take just the two-way and three-way interactions (omitting the main effects from the model), the improvement is a little less at 40.573 according to Table 41.13. Remember that the likelihood ratio chi-square is linear, so you can add and subtract values. Consequently, the improvement in fit due to the main effects is 44.579 − 40.573 = 4.006. Within the limits of rounding error, this is the same value as for the sum of all of the main effects in Table 41.14 (i.e. 4.007). If we take only the three-way interaction in Table 41.13 (i.e. omitting the two-way interaction and main effects from the model), we get a value of 10.713 for the amount of misfit. This is the value given in Table 41.14. Where does the value for the two-way interactions come from? We have just found that the value for the main effect is 4.006 and the value for the three-way interaction is 10.713. If we take these away from the chi-square of 44.580 we get 44.580 − 4.006 − 10.713 = 29.861 for the contribution of the two-way interactions to the fit (exactly as can be found in Table 41.14 within the limits of rounding error). It looks as if a good model for the data can exclude the main effects which are failing to contribute significantly to the goodness-of-fit even though the value of the likelihood ratio chi-square is 4.007. Thus a model based on the two-way and three-way interactions accounts for the data well.



■ Step 5: Which components account for the data? This analysis has demonstrated the substantial contributions of the two-way and threeway interactions to the model’s fit to the data. Since there is only one three-way interaction in this case, then there is no question what interaction is causing this three-way effect. There are three different two-way interactions for this model, not all of which may be contributing to the fit to the data. The way of checking for the relative influence of the different two-way interactions is to repeat the analysis but omitting one of the two-way interactions. This is easy to do on most computer programs. Doing this for the data in Table 41.10, we obtain the following: ●

Based solely on gender*sexual abuse: chi-square = 15.036, df = 4, p = 0.005.



Based solely on gender*physical abuse: chi-square = 36.525, df = 4, p = 0.000.



Based solely on sexual abuse*physical abuse: chi-square = 44.579, df = 4, p = 0.000.

Working backwards, compared with the value of 44.580 for the misfit between the data and the equal frequencies model, there is no improvement in the fit by adding in the sexual abuse*physical abuse interaction since the value of likelihood ratio chi-square

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 606

11/18/13 3:10 PM

41.3 A ThRee-VARIAbLe exAmPLe

607

does not change (significantly) from that value 44.580. This means that the sexual abuse*physical abuse interaction contributes nothing to the model fit and can be dropped from the model. Considering solely the gender*physical abuse interaction, there is a moderate improvement in fit. The maximum misfit of 44.580 as assessed by the likelihood ratio chi-square reduces to 36.526 when the gender*physical abuse interaction is included. This suggests that this interaction is quite important in the model and should be retained. Finally, using solely the gender*sexual abuse interaction, the likelihood ratio chi-square value declines to 15.036 from the maximum of 44.579, suggesting that the gender*sexual abuse interaction has a substantial influence and improves the fit of the model substantially. It should be remembered that there is a main effect for gender in all of the above twoway interactions except for the sexual abuse*physical abuse interaction where it is not present. (Check the marginal totals for the expected frequencies to see this.) In order to understand just how much change in fit is due to the two-way interaction, we need to adjust for the main effect of gender which we have already calculated as a likelihood ratio chi-square of 4.007. So to calculate the likelihood ratio chi-square of the gender*physical abuse interaction we have to take 36.525 from 44.580, which gives a value for the improvement in fit of 8.054. This value is the improvement in fit due to the gender main effect and the gender*physical abuse interaction. So for the improvement in fit due to the gender*physical abuse interaction only, we take 8.055 and subtract 4.007 to give a value of 4.048. This value is only roughly correct because of the unequal marginals involved, which means that a better approximation will be achieved through an iterative process. Table 41.15 gives the results of an analysis starting with the saturated model and gradually removing components. If a removed component is having an effect on the fit there will be a non-zero value for the chi-square change for that row which needs to be tested for significance. The saturated model is a perfect fit (i.e. chi-square = 0.000), but taking away the three-way interaction increases the misfit to 10.713. This change (10.713 − 0.000) is the influence of the three-way interaction on the degree of fit. Taking away the interaction of gender*sexual abuse gives a chi-square change of 25.812 which indicates that the gender*sexual abuse interaction is having a big effect on the fit of the model.

Box 41.1

Focus on

degrees of freedom Using the computer means that you never need to actually calculate the degrees of freedom. However, if you understand their calculation from chi-square in Chapter 15, then you should have few problems with their calculation for log-linear. When reading degrees of freedom in tables, they will often include extra degrees of freedom for lowerlevel interactions or main effects. Adjustments may have to be made. Here are a few examples: ●

Total degrees of freedom are always the number of cells − 1.

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 607



Degrees of freedom for the equal frequencies model = 1.



Degrees of freedom for a main effect =



total degrees of freedom number of different cate tegories of the main effect ff ffect

Degrees of freedom for the saturated model = 0.

Remember that the degrees of freedom for all of the main effects, for example, are not the same as the degrees of freedom for any of the main effects taken separately.

11/18/13 3:10 PM

608

CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

Table 41.15

The amounts of fit due to different components of the model Model

Likelihood ratio chi-square

Degrees of freedom

Prob.

Chi-square change

All two-way interactions + all main effects (i.e. minus three-way interaction)

10.713

1

0.001

10.713a

Previous row less gender*sexual abuse

36.525

2

0.000

25.812a

Previous row less sexual abuse*physical abuse

36.525

3

0.000

0.000

Previous row less gender*physical abuse

40.573

4

0.000

4.048

Previous row less sexual abuse

40.573

5

0.000

0.000

Previous row less gender

44.579

6

0.000

4.006

Previous row less physical abuse

44.579

7

0.000

0.000

Saturated

a

0.000



Change significant at the 5% level.

When we take away sexual abuse*physical abuse there is a 0.000 chi-square change. This indicates that this interaction is doing nothing to improve the fit of the model. Thus the sexual abuse*physical abuse interaction may be dropped from the model. Similarly, the row of Table 41.15 where the main effect of sexual abuse is dropped has a zero likelihood ratio chi-square, indicating that the main effect of sexual abuse can be dropped from the model. Also the final row where the main effect of physical abuse is dropped also shows no change, implying that this main effect can be dropped from the model. Actually, only two of the components are statistically significant at the 5% level so that the model could be built on these solely. Our model then becomes: mean frequency (i.e. equal frequencies mean) + gender*sexual abuse interaction + gender*sexual abuse*physical abuse interaction.



■ Step 6: More on the interpretation of log-linear analysis By this stage, it should be possible to attempt fitting a log-linear model. Of course, a little practice will be necessary with your chosen computer in order to familiarise yourself with its procedures. This is not too technical in practice with careful organisation and the creation of systematic tables to record the computer output. If these things are not done, the sheer quantity of frequently redundant computer output will cause confusion. Specifying the best-fitting model using likelihood ratio chi-squares is not a complete interpretation of the model. This is much as the value of Pearson chi-square in Chapter 15 is insufficient without careful examination of the data. An important concept in this respect is that of residuals. A residual is merely the difference between the data and the data predicted on the basis of the model. These can be expressed merely as the data value minus the modelled value. So residuals may take positive or negative values and there is one residual per cell. Not only this, since in a log-linear analysis you may be comparing one or more components of the model with the data then several sets of residuals will

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 608

11/18/13 3:10 PM

41.3  A three-variable example 609

have to be computed, and so you may be calculating different residuals for different components of the model or different models. Residuals can be standardised so that values are more easily compared one with another. The good news is twofold. There is no difficulty in calculating simple residuals, and computers generally do it for you anyway as part of calculating the model fit. If you look back to Table 41.12, you can easily calculate the residuals by subtracting any of the predicted model values from the actual data. The residuals for the saturated model are all zero, of course, indicating a perfect fit. The residuals for the equal frequencies model are −5.00, −10.00, 5.00, 30.00, 5.00, 10.00, −5.00 and −30.00; that is, the value of the frequency for that cell in the data −50.000 in each case. The other helpful thing when interpreting log-linear models is the estimated cell frequencies based on different components of the model. Remember that not only can you calculate these fairly directly but they are usually generated for you by the computer. The important thing about these estimated cell frequencies is that they tell you the trends in the data caused by, say, the interactions. For example, look at Table 41.12 and the column for the gender*sexual abuse interaction. You can see there that there are relatively few females who had been sexually abused and relatively more males who had been sexually abused in these data. It is best to compare these frequencies with the ones for the effects of the three main effects since the interaction figures actually include the effects of the main effects. Thus this comparison removes the main effects from the interaction. Figure 41.1 gives the key steps in log-linear analysis.

Figure 41.1

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 609

Conceptual steps for log-linear analysis

11/18/13 3:10 PM

610

CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

41.4 Reporting the results With something as complex as a log-linear analysis, you might expect that writing up the results of the analysis will be complex. Indeed it can be, and expect to write much more about your analysis than you would, for example, writing up the results of a correlation coefficient or a t-test. The purposes of log-linear analysis can be very varied, stretching from a fairly empirical examination of the data of the sort described earlier to testing the fit of a theoretical model to the actual data. Obviously there is no single sentence that can be usefully employed for describing the outcome of a log-linear analysis. Nevertheless certain things are very important. They are: ●

A table giving the data and the residuals for each of the models that you examine. Without this, the reader cannot assess precisely the form of the fit of the models to the data. Table 41.12 would be a useful format for doing this.



A table giving indications of the improvement in fit due to each component of the model. This will almost invariably be the likelihood ratio chi-square. Table 41.15 could be adapted to your particular data.

The text should discuss the final model which you have selected on the basis of your log-linear analysis. These could be expressed in terms of the components of the model which contribute significantly to the fit or, alternatively, as the lambda values mentioned in the panel. Earlier in this chapter we indicated the models for our two examples in a simple form.

Box 41.2

Focus on

Lambda and hierarchical models Lambda Often in log-linear analysis, the models are specified in terms of lambda (λ). This is simply the natural log of the influence of each of the different sorts of component of the cell frequencies. Thus a model may be built up from a succession of lambdas. These are given superscripts to denote what type of effect is involved: λA is the main effect of variable A and λA*B is the effect of the interaction of variables A and B. So an equation involving these and other components might be: Model = λ + λA + λB + λA*B This simply means that we add to the natural logarithm of the equal-cell mean or constant (λ), the natural logarithm of the main effects of the variable A (remember that this has positive and negative values), the natural

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 610

logarithm of the main effects of the variable B and the natural logarithm of the interaction of the variables A*B.

Hierarchical models Hierarchical models imply lower-order components and do not specify what these lower-order components are. Thus a hierarchical model may specify a four-variable interaction A*B*C*D. Any component involving A, B, C and D is assumed to be a component of that model. So the main effects A, B, C and D, the two-way interactions A*B, A*C, A*D, B*C, B*D and C*D, and the three-way interactions A*B*C, A*B*D, A*C*D and B*C*D are automatically specified as possible components in a hierarchical model. Notice that our examples employ a hierarchical approach.

11/18/13 3:10 PM

Key points 611



Research examples Log-linear methods Ahrens, Campbell, Ternier-Thames, Wasco and Sefl (2007) conducted qualitative interviews with over 100 female rape survivors. However, the researchers felt that a quantitative analysis would be helpful in this case and chose to use log-linear analysis to help them better understand what happens when victims decide to report the events to their informal social network rather than a formal social network for victims. They analysed the sort of support provider, the victim’s reasons for disclosure, social reactions to the disclosure and the impact of the disclosure on the survivor. Positive rather than negative reactions were commonest with help from informal support providers but negative reactions were commonest following help from formal support providers. However, this was not the case when the formal support providers initiated the support provision themselves. In this case, exclusively positive reactions were experienced by the victims. Bridges, Williamson, Thompson and Windsor (2001) used a variation of Milgram’s famous lost letter technique in which letters are deliberately lost in the street in order to see whether details of the addressee affect return rates. They compared an ‘emotive’ address (Advocates for Battered and Abused Lesbians) with non-emotive ones. Hierarchical log-linear analysis was used to analyse the variables of 1) returned versus not, 2) geographical location, 3) community size (city versus town) and 4) emotive versus non-emotive addressees. The findings showed complex relationships. Return rates were higher for Ohio than Florida/Alabama, higher for city than town and higher for the control addressees than the ‘emotive’ addressee. Tracey, Sherry, Bauer, Robins, Todaro and Briggs (1984) presented a random sample of university students with one of eight different descriptions of workshop programmes. The programs varied according to 1) whether the workshop dealt with exam or relationship skills, 2) whether the workshop was about skills enhancement or skills deficit reduction and 3) whether the orientation was towards self-change or changing the external environment for oneself (this the authors term ‘focus of effect’). Log-linear analysis was used in several ways. For example, information requests versus no requests were accounted for by a model which involved the interaction of gender*focus of effect.

Key points ●

It is recommended that before analysing your own data with log-linear, you reproduce our analyses in order to become familiar with the characteristics of your chosen computer program.



Confine yourself to small numbers of variables when first using log-linear analysis. Although computers may handle, say, ten variables, you may find it difficult without a lot of experience.



Log-linear analysis can include score variables if these are treated as frequencies.



Log-linear analysis is not as commonly used in psychological research as it is in other disciplines. The reason is the preference of psychologists for using score variables.

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 611

11/18/13 3:10 PM

CHAPTER 41  Log-linear methods: The analysis of complex contingency tables

612

Computer analysis Log-linear analysis using SPSS

Figure 41.2 SPSS Statistics steps for log-linear analysis

Interpreting and reporting the data ●

Log-linear analysis is not the most familiar statistical technique in psychology and understanding it needs care and effort. The main text for this chapter goes through the process of understanding log-linear output in some detail. It helps if you ignore most of the Step Summary table and concentrate on its final row as this gives the final model.



There is no standard way of presenting the result of a log-linear analysis but you could, for example, write something like ‘A log-linear analysis was carried out to determine the best model to fit the data. The interaction of physical abuse with gender and sexual abuse were the components which best accounted for the data. Their removal significantly affected the fit of the model to the data.’

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 612

11/18/13 3:10 PM

Recommended further reading 613



Screenshot 41.1

Part of the data

Screenshot 41.3

The main output

Screenshot 41.2 Select the test

Recommended further reading Agresti, A. (1996). An introduction to categorical data analysis (Chapters 1–4). New York: Wiley. Anderson, E.B. (1997). Introduction to the statistical analysis of categorical data (Chapters 2–4). Berlin: Springer.

M41_HOWI0749_06_SE_C41.indd 613

11/18/13 3:10 PM

Chapter 42

Multinomial logistic regression Distinguishing between several different categories or groups

Overview ●

Multinomial logistic regression is a form of multiple regression in which a number of predictors are used to predict values of a single nominal dependent or criterion variable.

● There may be any number of values (categories) of the dependent variable with a minimum

of 3. It can be used with just two categories but binomial logistic regression (Chapter 43) would be more appropriate in these circumstances. ●

It is used to assess the most likely group (category) to which a case belongs on the basis of a number of predictor variables. That is, the objective is to find the pattern of predictor variables that identify of which category an individual is most likely to be a member.

● Multinomial

logistic regression uses nominal or category variables as the criterion or dependent variable. The independent or predictor variables may be score variables or nominal (dichotomised) variables. In this chapter we concentrate on nominal variables as predictors.

● The

concept of dummy variable is crucial in multinomial logistic regression. A dummy variable is a way of dichotomising a nominal category variable with three or more different values. A new variable is computed for each category ( just one!) and participants coded as having that characteristic or not. The code for belonging to the category is normally 1 and the code for belonging to any of the other categories is normally 0.



Multinomial logistic regression produces B-weights and constants just as in the case of other forms of regression. However, the complication is that these are applied to the logit. This is the natural (or Napierian) logarithm of the odds ratio (a close relative of probability). This allows the computation of the likelihood that an individual is in a particular category of the dependent or criterion variable given his or her pattern on the predictor variables.

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 614

11/13/13 5:28 PM

42.1  Introduction 615

● A

classification table is produced which basically describes the accuracy of the predictors in placing participants correctly in the category or group to which they belong.

Preparation Make sure you are familiar with Chapter 15 on chi-square and Chapters 9 and 32 on regression.

42.1 Introduction A simple example should clarify the purpose of multinomial logistic regression. Professionals who work with sex offenders would find it helpful to identify the patterns of characteristics which differentiate between three types – rapists, incestuous child abusers and paedophiles. The key variable would be type of sex offence, and rapists, incestuous child abusers and paedophiles would be the three different values (categories) of this nominal (category) variable. In a regression, type of sex offender would be called the dependent variable or the criterion or the predicted variable. Just what is different between the three groups of offenders – that is, what differentiates the groups defined by the different values of the dependent variable? The researcher would collect a number of measures (variables) from each of the participants in the study in addition to their offence type. These measures are really predictor variables since the researcher wants to know whether it is possible to assess which sort of offender an individual is on the basis of information about aspects of their background. Such predictors are also known as independent variables in regression. Imagine the researcher has information on the following independent variables (predictor variables). They are all nominal/category variables in this example, but it is possible to use score variables or a mixture of score and nominal/category variables as predictors. The dependent variable has to be a nominal/category variable (see Box 42.1): ●

age of offender (younger versus older; i.e. 30 plus)



physically abused when a child



sexually abused when a child



depression (low depression versus high depression) measured on the DASS (Depression Anxiety Stress Scale)



offender spent a period of childhood in children’s homes



mother’s hostility as assessed by a family experiences scale (mother not hostile versus mother hostile)



father’s hostility as assessed by a family experiences scale (father not hostile versus father hostile).

These data could be analysed in a number of ways. One very obvious choice would be to carry out a succession of chi-square tests. The type of offender could be one of the

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 615

11/13/13 5:28 PM

616

CHAPTER 42  Multinomial logistic regression

Box 42.1

Focus on

Using score variables in logistic regression Although we concentrate on nominal or category vari­ ables as the independent or predictor variables in logistic regression in this chapter, this is because it is conceptually harder to deal with them than score variables as independent variables. So for pedagogic reasons, we have not considered score variables directly in this chapter. However, score variables can be used as the independent or predictor variables and can be mixed with nominal/category variables in logistic regression. Conceptually, you should have no difficulty going on to using score variables in this

way once you have mastered the material in this chapter and Chapter 43. You may have more difficulty running the analyses on SPSS Statistics since it uses somewhat idiosyncratic terminology to refer to the two types of variable and it is not even consistent between the binomial logistic regression and multinomial logistic regression. If you consult the companion Introduction to SPSS Statistics in Psychology: For version 22 and earlier (Howitt & Cramer, 2014b) what to do should be clear. Alternatively use the SPSS Statistics steps provided at the end of this chapter.

variables and any of the variables in the above list could be the predictor variable. An example of this is shown in Table 42.1. Examining the table, these data seem to suggest that if the offender had a hostile father then he is unlikely to be a rapist, more likely to be an incestuous offender, but most likely to be a paedophile. Similar analyses could be carried out for each of the predictor variables in the list. There is not a great deal wrong with this approach – it would readily identify the specific variables on which the three offender groups differ (and those on which they did not differ). One could also examine how the three offender groups differed from the others on any of the predictor variables. Since the analysis is based on chi-square, then partitioning would help to test which groups differ from the others (Chapter 15) in terms of any of the predictors. The obvious problem with the chi-square approach is that it handles a set of predictors one by one. This is fine if we only have one predictor, but we have several predictor variables. A method of handling all of the predictor variables at the same time would have obvious advantages. Predictor variables are often correlated and this overlap also needs to be taken into account (as it is with multiple regression – see Chapters 32 and 33). That is, ideally the pattern of variables that best predicts group membership should be identified. In many ways, multinomial logistic regression is the more general case of binomial logistic regression described in Chapter 43. The dependent variable in multinomial logistic

Table 42.1 An example of how the offender groups could be compared on the predictors

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 616

Rapists

Incestuous offender

Paedophile

Father hostile to offender as a child

30

50

40

Father not hostile to offender

40

30

10

11/13/13 5:28 PM

42.2  Dummy variables 617

regression can have one of several (not just two) nominal values. Nevertheless the two forms of logistic regression share many essential characteristics. For example, the dependent variable is membership of a category (e.g. group) in both cases. Like binomial logistic regression, multinomial logistic regression uses nominal (category) variables. However, not all of the sophisticated regression procedures which are available for binomial logistic regression can be used in multinomial logistic regression. Because of this, multinomial logistic regression is actually easier than binomial logistic regression. Nevertheless, there is a disadvantage for the more advanced user since there are few model-building options (no stepwise, no forward selection, no backward selection). This makes multinomial logistic regression simpler. Sometimes multinomial logistic regression is described as being rather like doing two or more binomial logistic regressions on the data. It could replace binomial logistic regression for the dichotomous category case – that is, when the dependent variable consists of just two categories.

42.2 Dummy variables A key to understanding multinomial logistic regression lies in the concept of dummy variables. In our example, there are three values of the dependent variable, category A, category B and category C. These three values could be converted into two dichotomous variables and these dichotomous variables are known as dummy variables: ●

Dummy variable 1  Category A versus categories B and C.



Dummy variable 2  Category B versus categories A and C.

Dummy variables are as simple as that. The two values of each dummy variable are normally coded 1 and 0. What about the comparison of category C with categories A and B? Well, no such dummy variable is used. The reason is simple. All of the information that distinguishes category C from categories A and B has already been provided by the first two dummy variables. The first dummy variable explains how to distinguish category C from cat­ egory A, and the second dummy variable explains how to distinguish category C from category B. The third dummy variable is not used because it would overlap completely with the variation explained by the first two dummy variables. This would cause something called multicollinearity, which means that some predictors intercorrelate highly with each other. So, in our example, only two of the dummy variables can be used. Multicollinearity should be avoided in any form of regression as it is the cause of a great deal of confusion in the interpretation of the findings. The choice of which dummy variable to omit in dummy coding is arbitrary. The outcome is the same in terms of prediction and classification whatever value is omitted. If you are struggling with dummy variables and collinearity consider the following. Imagine the variable gender which consists of just two values – male and female. Try to change gender into dummy variables. One dummy variable would be ‘male or not’ and the other dummy variable would be ‘female or not’. There would be a perfect negative correlation between these two dummy variables – they are simply different ways of measuring the same thing. So one dummy variable has to be dropped since it has already been accounted for by the other dummy variable. If there are more than two dummy variables then the same logic applies although the dropped dummy variable is accounted for by several dummy variables, not just one.

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 617

11/13/13 5:28 PM

618

CHAPTER 42  Multinomial logistic regression

42.3 What can multinomial logistic regression do? Multinomial logistic regression can help: ●

identify a small number of variables which effectively distinguish between groups or categories of the dependent variable



identify the other variables which are ineffective in terms of distinguishing between groups or categories of the dependent variable

● make

actual predictions of which group an individual will be a member (i.e. what category of the dependent variable) on the basis of their known values on the predictor variables.

What are we hoping to achieve with our multinomial logistic regression? The main things are: ● whether

our predictors actually predict the offence categories at better than the chance level

● the

constants and regression weights that need to be applied to the predictors to optimally allocate the offenders to the actual offending group



a classification table that indicates how accurately the classification is based on the predictors compared to the known category of offence



to identify the pattern of predictor variables which classifies the offenders into their offence category most accurately.

This list is more or less the same as would be applied to any form of regression. Some researchers would use a different technique (discriminant analysis or discriminant function analysis) to analyse our data (see Box 42.2). However, multinomial logistic regression does an arguably better job since it makes fewer (unattainable?) assumptions about the characteristics of the data. More often than not, there will be little difference between the two in terms of your findings. In those rare circumstances when substantially different outcomes emerge, the multinomial logistic regression is preferred because of its relative lack of restrictive assumptions about the data. In other words, there is no advantage in using discriminant function analysis but there are disadvantages. Figure 42.1 outlines the key steps in multinomial logistic regression.

Box 42.2

Key concepts

The difference between discriminant function analysis and logistic regression Discriminant function analysis is very similar in its application to multinomial logistic regression. There is no particular advantage of discriminant function analysis which is in some circumstances inferior to multinomial logistic regression. It could be used for the data in this chapter on different types of sex offenders. However, it is more

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 618

characteristically used when the independent variables are score variables. It would help us to find what the really important factors are in differentiating between the three groups of sex offenders. The dependent variable in discriminant function analysis consists of the various categories or groups which we want to differentiate.

11/13/13 5:28 PM

42.3  What can multinomial logistic regression do? 619

The discriminant function is a weighted combination of predictors which maximise the differentiation between the various groups which make up the dependent variable. So the formula for a discriminant function might be as follows: Discriminant (function) score = constant + b1x2 + b2x2 + b3x3 + b4x4 + b5x5 + b6x6 The statistic Wilks’ lambda indicates the contribution of each of the predictor variables to distinguishing the groups. A small value of lambda indicates the greater the power of the predictor variable to differentiate groups. The bs in the formula above are merely regression weights (just like in multiple regression) and x1, etc. are an individual’s scores on each of the predictor variables. As with multiple regressions, regression weights may be expressed in unstandardised or standardised form. When expressed in standardised form, the relative impact of the different predictors is more accurately indicated. In our example, there will be two discriminant functions because there are three groups to differentiate. The number of discriminant

Figure 42.1

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 619

functions is generally one less than the number of groups. However, if the number of predictors is less than the number of discriminant functions, the number of discriminant functions may be reduced. The centroid is the average score on the discriminant function of a person who is classified as belonging to one of the groups. If the analysis involves just two groups, there are two centroids. For a two-group discriminant function analysis there are two centroids. Cut-off points are provided which help the researcher identify to which group an individual belongs. This cut-off point lies halfway between the two centroids if both groups are equal in size. The cut-off point is weighted towards one of the centroids in the case of unequal group size. A classification table (in this context also known as a confusion matrix or prediction table) indicates how good the discrimination between the groups is in practice. Such a table gives the known distribution of groups compared to how the dis­ criminant function analysis categorises the individuals. Chapter 28 covers discriminant function analysis in relation to MANOVA.

Conceptual steps for understanding multinomial logistic regression

11/13/13 5:28 PM

CHAPTER 42  Multinomial logistic regression

620

42.4 Worked example The data used are shown in Table 42.2. To make the output realistic, these 20 cases have been entered ten times to give a total sample of 200 cases. This is strictly improper as a statistical technique, of course, but helpful for pedagogic reasons. It is not feasible to calculate multinomial logistic regression by hand. Inevitably a computer program has to be used. Consequently, the discussion refers to a computer analysis rather than to computational steps to be followed. Figure 42.2 reminds us of the basic task in multinomial logistic regression. The predictors are scores and/or nominal variables. The criterion being predicted is always a nominal variable but one with more than two categories or values. Since nominal variables have no underlying scale by definition, the several nominal categories are essentially re-coded individually as present or absent. In this way, each value is compared with all of the other values. It does not matter which comparison is left out and, of course, computer programs largely make the choices for you. Figure 42.3 takes the basic structure and applies it directly to our study of offenders in order to make things concrete. Remember that one dummy variable is not used in the analysis.

Table 42.2 The data for the multinomial logistic regression Age

DASS

Mother hostile

Father hostile

Children’s home

Physical abuse

Sexual abuse

Type of offence

1

younger

low

high

low

no

yes

no

rapist

2

younger

low

high

low

no

yes

yes

rapist

3

older

low

high

low

no

yes

yes

rapist

4

older

high

high

high

yes

no

no

incest

5

older

high

high

high

yes

yes

yes

rapist

6

younger

low

high

low

no

no

no

rapist

7

older

high

low

high

no

yes

yes

rapist

8

older

high

low

high

yes

no

no

incest

9

younger

low

low

high

yes

no

yes

incest

10

older

high

high

low

no

yes

yes

incest

11

older

high

low

low

yes

no

yes

incest

12

younger

high

low

high

no

yes

no

rapist

13

older

high

low

high

yes

no

yes

incest

14

older

high

high

low

yes

yes

yes

incest

15

older

low

high

high

no

yes

yes

incest

16

younger

high

high

low

yes

no

no

paedophile

17

older

high

low

high

yes

no

yes

paedophile

18

older

low

high

high

no

no

yes

paedophile

19

younger

high

low

high

yes

yes

yes

paedophile

20

older

low

low

high

yes

no

no

paedophile

etc.

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 620

11/13/13 5:28 PM

42.5  Accuracy of the prediction 621

Figure 42.2

Multinomial logistic regression

Figure 42.3 The structure of the example

Our list of independent or predictor variables actually only includes two-value variables (binary or dichotomous variables). We could use more complex nominal variables as predictors. However, they would have to be made into several dummy variables just as the dependent variable is turned into several dummy variables. Remember that the dependent variable in this case is the type of offence. There are three different types, categories or values of sex offender in the study. Hence there are two dummy variables listed (out of the maximum of three possible). Dummy variable 1 is rapist versus not (not implies the offender is an incestuous child abuser or a paedophile). Dummy variable 2 is incestuous child abuser versus not (not implies the offender is a rapist or a paedophile). The choice of which dummy variable to leave out of the analysis is purely arbitrary, makes no difference to the outcome and, typically, is automatically chosen by the computer program.

42.5 Accuracy of the prediction Once the analysis has been run through an appropriate computer program, a useful starting point is the classification table (that is, an accuracy assessment). Sometimes this is an option that you will have to select rather than something automatically produced by the program. The classification table is a crosstabulation (or contingency) table which compares the predicted allocation of the offenders to the three offender groups to which they are known to belong. Usually, such tables include percentage figures to indicate the degree of accuracy of the prediction. Classification tables make a lot of sense intuitively and help clarify what the analysis is achieving. Table 42.3 is such a classification table for our data. We have yet to look at the calculation steps that allow this table to be generated. This comes later.

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 621

11/13/13 5:28 PM

622

CHAPTER 42  Multinomial logistic regression

Table 42.3 Predicted versus actual offence category of offenders Observed

Predicted to be rapist offender

Predicted to be incestuous

Predicted to be a paedophile

Percentage correct for row

Actually a rapist

60

10

0

85.7%

Actually an incestuous offender

20

50

10

62.5%

Actually a paedophile

0

30

20

40.0%

Column percentage

40.0%

45.0%

15.0%

Overall percentage correct = 65%

For the rapists, the analysis is rather accurate. Indeed, the overwhelming majority of rapists have been correctly identified as being rapists. Hence the row percentage correctly classified for rapists is 85.7%. This calculation is simply the number of correctly identified rapists (60) expressed as a percentage of the number of rapists in total (70). So the accuracy for the prediction for rapists is 60 ÷ 70 × 100% = 0.857 × 100% = 85.7%. None of the rapists was predicted to be a paedophile though some were predicted to be incestuous offenders. The other two categories of offender could not be differentiated to the same level of accuracy. For the incestuous offenders, 62.5% were correctly identified as being incestuous offenders. The paedophiles were relatively poorly predicted – only 40% of paedophiles were correctly identified. Interestingly, the paedophiles are only ever wrongly classified as being incestuous offenders, they are never wrongly classified as rapists. So it would appear that the model (pattern of predictors of offence category) is reasonably successful at distinguishing the offender types. Nevertheless, the identification of incestuous offenders and paedophiles is not particularly good. Obviously, if we were to persist in our research then we would seek to include further predictor variables that were better at differentiating the incestuous and paedophile groups.

42.6 How good are the predictors? Calculating multinomial logistic regression using a computer program generates a variety of statistical analyses apart from the classification table discussed so far. We need to turn to other aspects of this multinomial logistic regression output in order to identify just how successful each predictor is and what predictors should be included in the model. The classification table gives no indication of this since it does not deal with the individual predictor variables. Is the prediction better than chance? At some point in the analysis, there should be a table or tables including output referring to, say, ‘Cox and Snell’ or ‘Nagelkerke’ or ‘McFadden’ or to several of these. These may be described as pseudo r-square statistics. They refer to the amount of variation in the dependent variable which is predicted by the predictor variables collectively. The maximum value of this, in theory, is 1.00 if the relationship is perfect; it will be 0.00 if there is no relationship. They are pseudo-statistics because they appear to be like r-square (which is the square of the multiple correlation between the independent and dependent variable – see p. 452) but they are only actu­ ally analogous to it. One simply cannot compute a Pearson correlation involving a nominal variable with more than two values (categories). The nearer the pseudo-statistic is to a perfect relationship of 1.00 the better the prediction (just as it would with a

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 622

11/13/13 5:28 PM

42.6  How good are the predictors? 623

Table 42.4 Pseudo r-square statistics Pseudo-statistic Cox and Snell



Table 42.5

Nagelkerke

0.617

McFadden

0.365

Model fitting information indicating whether the prediction actually changes significantly from the values if the predictors were not used −2 log likelihood statistica

Model components

a

0.546

Intercept (i.e. constant) only

407.957

Final model

248.734

Chi-square for change

Degrees of freedom

Significance

159.224

14

0.001

See Box 42.3 for a discussion of this statistic.

proper r-square). The value for ‘Cox and Snell’ is 0.546, the value for Nagelkerke is 0.617 and the value for McFadden is 0.365. So the relationship between the predictors and the criterion is moderate (see Table 42.4). We would interpret these values more or less as if they were analogous to a squared Pearson correlation coefficient. Another table will be found in the computer output to indicate how well the model improves fit over using no model at all (Table 42.5). This is also an indication of whether the set of predictors actually contributes to the classification process over and above what random allocation would achieve. This is known as the model fit (but really is whether the modelled predictions are different from purely random predictions). This involves a statistic called −2 log likelihood which is discussed in Box 42.3. Often the value for the intercept is given (remember this is a regression so there is a constant of some fixed value). Table 42.5 illustrates this aspect of the output. The chi-square value is calculated using the −2 log likelihood statistic. This amounts to a measure of the amount of change due to using the predictors versus not using the predictors. As can be seen, there is a significant change, so it is worthwhile using the model. (It is significant at the 0.001 level. That is, it is a change in predictive power which is significant at better than the 5% level or 0.05 level.) There is yet another statistic that is worth considering – the goodness-of-fit of the model to the data. The model is not merely intended to be better than no model at all but, ideally, it will fit or predict the actual data fairly precisely. A chi-square test can be performed comparing the fit of the predicted data to the actual data. In this case, of course, the ideal outcome is no significant difference between the actual data and those predicted from the model. This would indicate that it is pointless searching for additional predictors to fit the model – assuming that the sample is fairly large so sampling fluctuations may not be too much of a problem. In this example, the model makes predictions which are significantly different from the obtained classification of the offender. The incomplete match between the data and the predicted data is not surprising given the classification table (Table 42.3). This does not mean that the model is no good, merely that it could be better. Table 42.6 gives the goodness-of-fit statistics. Probably in psychology and the social sciences, it is unrealistic to expect any model to predict the actual data perfectly. Moderate levels of fit would be acceptable.

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 623

11/13/13 5:28 PM

624

CHAPTER 42  Multinomial logistic regression

Table 42.6 Goodness-of-fit of the actual offence category to the predicted offence category

Pearson goodness-of-fit statistic

Box 42.3

Chi-square

Degrees of freedom

Significance

228.010

22

0.001

Key concepts

Change in the −2 log likelihood Logistic regression uses a statistic called −2 log likelihood. This statistic is used to indicate a) how well both the model (the pattern of predictors) actually fits the obtained data, b) the change in fit of the model to data if a predictor is removed from the model and c) the extent to which using the model is an improvement on not using the model. These uses are different although the same statistic is used in assessing them. There is a similarity, however. All of them involve the closeness of fit between different versions of the classifi­ cation table. Earlier in studying statistics, we would have used chi-square in order to assess the significance of these discrepancies between one classification table and another. Actually that is more or less what we are doing when we use the −2 log likelihood statistic. This statistic is distributed like the chi-square statistic. Hence, you will find reference to chi-square values close to where the −2 log likelihood statistic is reported. The −2 is there because it ensures that the log likelihood is distributed according to the chi-square distribution. It is merely a pragmatic adjustment.

Just like chi-square, then, a 0 value of the −2 log likelihood is indicative that the two contingency tables involved fit each other perfectly. That is, the model fits the data perfectly, dropping a predictor makes no difference to the predictive power of the analysis, or the model is no different from a purely chance pattern. All of these are more similar than they might at first appear. Similarly, the bigger the value of the −2 log likelihood statistic, the more likely is there to be a significant difference between the versions of the contingency table. That is, the model is less than per­ fect in that it does not reproduce the data exactly (though it may be a fairly useful model); the variable which has been dropped from the model should not be dropped since it makes a useful contribution to understanding the data; or the model is better than a chance distribution – that is, makes a useful contribution to understanding the pattern of the data on the dependent variable. The statistic usually reported is the change in the −2 log likelihood. The calculation of the degrees of freedom is a little less straightforward than for chi-square. It is dependent on the change in the number of predictors associated with the change in the −2 log likelihood.

So which are the best predictors? It was clear from Table 42.4 that the predictors improve the accuracy of the classification. However, this is for all of the predictors. It does not tell us which predictors (components of the model) are actually responsible for this improvement. To address that issue, it is necessary to examine the outcomes of a number of likelihood ratio tests. Once again these use the −2 log likelihood calculation, but the strategy is different. There is a succession of such tests that examine the effect of removing one predictor from the model (set of potential predictors). The change in the −2 log likelihood statistic consequent on doing this is distributed like the chi-square distribution. Table 42.7 shows such a set of calculations for our data. Notice that in

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 624

11/13/13 5:28 PM

42.7  The prediction 625

Table 42.7 Likelihood ratio tests Predictor

Intercept (constant)

–2 log likelihood of reduced model; i.e. without the predictor to the left

Chi-square

Degrees of freedom

Significance

0.000

248.734

Age

267.272

18.538

2

DASS

249.454

0.721

2

0.697

Mother’s hostility

248.932

0.199

2

0.905

Father’s hostility

256.089

7.355

2

0.025

Children’s home

259.677

10.943

2

0.004

Physical abuse

287.304

38.571

2

0.000

Sexual abuse

263.914

15.181

2

0.001

general little changes (i.e. the chi-square values are small) in a number of cases – DASS anxiety and hostility of the mother. Removing these variables one at a time makes no difference of any importance in the model’s ability to predict. In other words, neither DASS anxiety nor hostility of the mother are useful predictors. Other predictors can be seen to be effective predictors simply because removing them individually makes a significant difference to the power of the model. That is, the model with any of these predictors taken away is a worse fit to the data than when the predictor is included (i.e. the full model). Although we have identified the good predictors, this is not the end of the story since we cannot say what each of the good predictors is good at predicting – remember that we have several (two in this example) dummy variables to predict. The predictors may be good for some of the dummy variables but not for others.

42.7 The prediction So how do we predict to which group an offender is likely to belong given his particular pattern on the predictor variables? This is very much the same question as asking which of the predictor variables have predictive power. It is done in exactly the same way that we would make the prediction in any sort of regression. That is we multiply each of the ‘scores’ by its regression weight, add up all of these products and, finally, add the intercept (i.e. constant) (see Chapter 32 for this sort of calculation). In logistic regression we are actually predicting category membership or, in other words, which value of the dependent or criterion variable the offender has. Is he a rapist, incestuous offender or paedophile? This is done mathematically by calculating something known as ‘the logit’ (see also Chapter 43 on binomial logistic regression). The logit is the natural logarithm of something known as the odds ratio. The odds ratio relates very closely and simply to the probability that an offender is in one category rather than the others. A key thing to note is that multinomial logistic regression, like multiple regression (Chapter 32), actually calculates a set of regression weights (B) which are applied to the logit. It also calculates a constant or cut-point as in any other form of regression.

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 625

11/13/13 5:28 PM

626

CHAPTER 42  Multinomial logistic regression

Table 42.8

Constants and regression weights for predictors used

Category

Predictor

B

Rapist – not

Intercept

−0.260

1.158

Age (younger)

−0.159

0.678 0.735

0.612

0.791

0.171

Age (older)

0

DASS (lower)

0.575

DASS (higher) Mother’s hostility (lower)

0

Father’s hostility (lower)

0.838

Father’s hostility (higher)

0

Children’s home (no) Physically abused (yes) Physically abused (no) Sexually abused (yes) Sexually abused (no)

Wald

Degrees of freedom

Sig.

0.050

1

0.822

0.055

1

0.814

0

0 −0.328

Mother’s hostility (higher)

Children’s home (yes)

Standard error

−1.576

0.863

0.943

0.815

3.739

0.713

830.866

0.000



0 0.150

abuser – not

Age (younger)

−1.970

0.542

13.187

DASS (higher) Mother’s hostility (lower) Mother’s hostility (higher)

0

Father’s hostility (lower)

1.486

Father’s hostility (higher)

0

Children’s home (yes)

0.479

Children’s home (no)

0

Physically abused (yes)

0.652

Physically abused (no)

0

Sexually abused (yes)

0.498

Sexually abused (no)

0

0.053*

1

0.000*

1 1

0.699

1

0.000*

0 0.562

0.024

0.505

0.01

0.615

5.836

0 −0.014

1

0 0.813

0.086

0.332

0

−0.314

DASS (lower)

1

0

Intercept

0

0.679

0

Incestuous child

Age (older)

1 0

0 −18.570

0.434

0

0 20.540

1

1

0.878

0 1

0.977

0 1

0.016*

0 0.704

0.463

1

0.496

0 0.582

1.255

0.498

1.003

1

0.263

0 1

0.317

0

* Wald test is significant at better than the 0.05 level.

Table 42.8 gives the regression values calculated for our data. There are a number of things to bear in mind: ●

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 626

The table is in two parts because there is more than one dependent variable to predict – that is, there are two dummy variables. If there were three dummy variables then this table would be in three parts and so forth.

11/13/13 5:28 PM

42.8  Interpreting the results 627 ● The

dichotomous variables are each given a regression weight (B) value for each value. The value coded 1 has a numerical value which may be positive or negative. The other value is given a regression weight of 0 every time. That is, by multiplying the numerical value by 0 we are always going to get 0. In other words, one of the values of a dichotomous predictor has no effect on the calculation.



There is a statistic called the Wald statistic in Table 42.8. This statistic is based on the ratio between the B-weight and the standard error. Thus for the first dummy variable it is 0.055. This is not statistically significant (p = 0.814). Sometimes the output will be a little misleading since if the standard error is 0.00 then it is not possible to calculate the Wald statistic as it is an infinitely large value. Any value divided by 0 is infinitely large. An infinitely large value is statistically significant, but its significance value cannot be calculated. The significance values of the Wald statistic indicate which of our predictors is statistically significant.

42.8 Interpreting the results It is fairly self-evident that the features which distinguish the three groups of offenders are as follows: ●

Rapists (as opposed to incestuous and paedophile offenders) are less likely to have been in a children’s home (B = −1.576, the minus sign meaning that the reverse of spending some time in a children’s home is true). This is significant at 0.053 which is just about significant. The rapists were also more likely to have been physically abused (B = 20.540 and the sign is positive). This is much more statistically significant and the best predictor of all. Finally, the rapists were less likely to have been sexually abused. There is no significance level reported for this because the standard error is 0.000 which makes the Wald statistic infinitely large. Hence a significance level cannot be calculated but really it is extremely statistically significant.



Incestuous abusers (as opposed to rapists and paedophile offenders) are more likely to be in the young group and to have a father low on hostility.

The findings are presented in Table 42.9. There were two dummy variables so there are two dimensions to the table. This table probably will help you to understand why only two dummy variables are needed to account for the differences between three groups.

Table 42.9

Differentiating characteristics of the three offender types

Children’s home: Not physically abused, but sexually abused Never in children’s home: Physically abused, but not sexually abused

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 627

Younger age group: Father not hostile

Older age group: Father hostile

incestuous abuser

paedophile rapist

11/13/13 5:28 PM

628

CHAPTER 42  Multinomial logistic regression

42.9 Reporting the results As with some other more advanced statistical procedures, there is no standard way of presenting the outcome of a multinomial logistic regression. One way of reporting the broad findings of the analysis would be as follows: A multinomial logistic regression was conducted using six dichotomous predictors to predict classification on the multinomial dependent variable offence type (paedophile, incestuous offender, rapist). The predictors were capable of identifying the offender group at better than the chance level. Two regression patterns were identified – one for rapists versus the other two groups, the second for incestuous offenders versus the other two groups). The pseudo-r 2 (Cox and Snell) was 0.55, indicating a moderate fit between the total model and data although the fit was less than perfect. Rapists were differentiated from the other two groups by not having spent time in a children’s home, being physically abused but not being sexually abused. Incestuous offenders were significantly differentiated from the other two groups by being in the younger age group and their father not being hostile to them as children. Rapists were correctly identified with a high degree of accuracy (85.7% correct). Incestuous offenders were less accurately identified (62.5% correct). Paedophiles were more likely to be wrongly classified (accuracy 40.0% correct) but as incestuous offenders rather than rapists. The regression weights are to be found in Table 42.8.

Research examples Multinomial logistic regression Griffin and Hesketh (2008) asked what factors predict post-retirement work intentions. They used multinomial logistic regression to predict membership of various groups – 1) not work, voluntary work, 2) part-time paid employment, and 3) voluntary work plus part-time work. The predictors included the participants’ evaluations of pre-retirement work, attitudes to retirement, demographics and so forth. Positive evaluations of preretirement work predicted both volunteer work and paid-work post retirement. The variables gender, health, and retirement satisfaction were associated with volunteer work and higher levels of education were predictive of paid work. Huisman and her colleagues (2010) were interested in whether patients with particular psychiatric diagnoses were more likely to kill themselves with a particular method. They found that psychiatric diagnosis, gender and the status of patients as in- or out-patient were significantly related to the method of suicide used. They used multinomial logistic regression to determine which of these variables were related to suicide method when examined together. The dependent variable was suicide method with the four categories of 1) self-poisoning, 2) jumping before a train, 3) jumping from a high place and 4) all other methods apart from hanging, which as the most common method was chosen to be the reference category. They reported a number of significant findings. For example, ‘compared to suicide by hanging, patients who poisoned themselves were more likely to have a substance-related disorder (OR = 4.13), to be in outpatient treatment (OR = 3.22) and less likely to be male (OR = 0.23)’ (p.96). OR is odds ratio. Kogan (2004) examined the factors that predicted disclosure in women who had unwanted sexual experiences in their childhood or adolescence. The dependent variables were the timing of disclosure and the person

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 628

11/13/13 5:28 PM

Key points 629



disclosed to. Timing of disclosure consisted of the three categories of 1) immediate, 2) delayed and 3) nondisclosure, with immediate disclosure being the reference category. Person disclosed to contained the three categories of adult, peers only and non-disclosure, with adult being the reference category. Multinomial logistic regressions were carried out on these two dependent variables separately. Predictors of these two dependent variables included age at which the experience first occurred which was then recategorised into four groups: whether the person knew the other person, whether they were family and so on. Various significant findings were reported. For example, ‘participants who knew their perpetrator were 3.1 times more likely to non-disclose and 3.7 times more likely to delay disclosure than to disclose within a month’ (p. 157). Lampropoulos, Schneider and Spengler (2009) wished to investigate the factors associated with different sorts of outcomes of counselling services provided as part of the training of counsellors at an American university. Drop-out rates, in particular, have been studied in relation to counselling but the research was too varied in outcome to allow its generalisation to student counselling services. Three types of outcome were included in the new study: 1) clients who failed to return for treatment after the initial intake appointment (intake dropouts); 2) clients who ended treatment later than this yet did not complete the counselling therapy treatment programme (i.e. therapy drop-outs), and 3) clients who completed their course of therapy (i.e. completers). These three groups made up the dependent variable for the multinomial logistic regression employed by the researchers. The predictors were based on information collected 1) before intake to the programme and 2) the termination report by the counsellor. The predictors of intake drop-out were lower age and lower income and the therapist’s initial assessment of how difficult it would be to work with the client. The predictors for therapy drop-out were the GAF (Global Assessment of Functioning) scores but no other predictor. The lower the functioning the more likely that the client would be in the therapy drop-out group. Niemeier, Marwitz, Lesher, Walker and Bushnik (2007) used a database on traumatic brain injury patients. They employed the Wisconsin Card Sort Test as their measure of executive function. One aspect of this was perseveration. They used the categories achieved on the Wisconsin test as the dependent variable of a multinomial logistic regression. The predictor variables were gender, minority status, level of education, prior substance abuse, cause of injury and length of coma. The longer the coma, being male and being from a minority group predicted severity of the perseveration symptom. Testa and her colleagues (2007) looked at various factors which might predict later sexual victimisation in women. The dependent variable consisted of four groups: 1) no victimisation, which was the reference group, 2) victimisation by someone they knew well (intimate partner), 3) victimisation by someone they did not know well (non-intimate perpetrator) and 4) victimisation by someone they knew well plus someone they did not know well. They carried out a multinomial logistic regression with 11 predictors such as how assertive they were in refusing sex. A number of these predictors distinguished these groups. For example, women who were victimised by someone they did not know well were more likely to be single, engage in heavy episodic drinking and have more consensual sexual partners.

Key points ● The

power of multinomial logistic regression to help identify differences among psychologically interesting – but different – groups of individuals means that it has far greater scope within psychological research than has yet been fully appreciated by researchers.

● The

unfamiliarity of some of the concepts should not be regarded as a deterrent. The key features of the analysis are accessible to any researcher no matter how statistically unskilled.

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 629

11/13/13 5:28 PM

630

CHAPTER 42  Multinomial logistic regression

Computer analysis

Figure 42.4 SPSS Statistics steps for binomial logistic regression

Interpreting and reporting the data ● The

parameter estimates table provides the regression data for predicting the different types of offending. Each of the predictors is presented twice and you can ignore the rows which have 0b towards the beginning under the B (regression weight) column. The significant predictors can be found from the Sig. column. Check the B weight for the direction of the relationship as you would with any form of regression.

● A

brief write-up might be: ‘Multinomial logistic regression showed that only some of the six predictors effectively predicted offence type (rapist, paedophile or incestuous abusers). The pseudo-r 2 (Cox and Snell) was 0.55 indicating a moderate fit between the model and the data. Rapists were different from the other types of offenders in that they had not having spent time in a children’s home and had been physically abused but not sexually abused. Incestuous offenders tended to be younger and their fathers were not hostile to them as a child.’

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 630

11/13/13 5:28 PM

Computer analysis 631



Screenshot 42.1 Part of the data

Screenshot 42.2 Select test

Screenshot 42.4 Select statistics

Screenshot 42.3 Select variables

Screenshot 42.5 Parameter estimates table output

M42_HOWI0749_06_SE_C42.indd 631

11/13/13 5:28 PM

Chapter 43

Binomial logistic regression

Overview ●

Binomial (or binary) logistic regression is a form of multiple regression which is applied when the dependent variable is dichotomous – that is, has only two different possible values. ● A set of predictors is identified which assesses the most likely of the two nominal categ­ ories a particular case falls into. ● The predictor variables may be any type of variable including scores. However, in this chap­ ter we concentrate on using dichotomous predictor variables. ● As in multiple regression, different ways of entering predictor variables are available. What is appropriate is determined partly by the purpose of the analysis. Blocks may be used in order to control for, or partial out, demographic variables for example. ● Classification tables compare the actual distribution on the dependent variable with that predicted on the basis on the independent variables. ● Like other forms of regression, logistic regression generates B-weights (or slope) and a constant. However, these are used to calculate something known as the logit rather than scores. The logit is the natural logarithm of odds for the category. The percentage predicted in each category of the dependent variable can be calculated from this and compared with the actual percentage. ● As in all multivariate forms of regression, the final regression calculation provides informa­ tion about the significant predictors among those being employed.

Preparation Look back at Chapter 9 on simple regression, Chapter 15 on chi-square and Chapter 32 on multiple regression. Chapter 42 on multinomial logistic regression may be helpful in consoli­ dating understanding of the material in this chapter.

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 632

11/13/13 5:29 PM

43.1  Introduction 633

43.1 Introduction Binomial (or binary) logistic regression may be used to: ●

determine a small group of variables which characterise the two different groups or categories of cases

● identify

which other variables are ineffective in differentiating these two groups or categories of cases

● make

actual predictions about which of the two groups a particular individual is likely to be a member given that individual’s pattern on the other variables.

A simple way to understand binomial logistic regression is to regard it as a variant of linear multiple regression (Chapter 32). Binomial logistic regression, however, uses a dependent variable which is nominal and consists of just two nominal categories. By employing a weighted pattern of predictor variables, binary logistic regression assesses a person’s most likely classification on this binary dependent variable. This prediction is expressed as a probability or using some related concept. Other examples of possible binomial dependent variables include: ●

success or failure in an exam



suffering schizophrenia or not



going to university or not.

If the dependent variable has three or more nominal categories, then multinomial logistic regression should be used (Chapter 42). In other words, if there are three or more groups or categories, multinomial logistic regression is the appropriate approach. Often, but not necessarily, the independent variables are also binary nominal category variables. So gender and age group could be used as the predictor variables to estimate whether a person will own a mobile phone or not, for example. Because the dependent variable is nominal data, regression weights are calculated which help calculate the probability that a particular individual will be in category A rather than category B of the dependent variable. More precisely: ●

The regression weights and constant are used to calculate the logit.



This in its turn is the natural logarithm of something called the odds.



Odds are not very different from probability and are turned into probabilities using a simple formula.

This is a little daunting at first, but it is not that difficult in practice – especially given that one rarely would need to calculate anything by hand! You may find it helpful to turn to Box 43.1 on simple logistic regression. Studying this will introduce you to most of the concepts in binomial logistic regression without too much confusing detail and complexity. Simple logistic regression would not normally be calculated since it achieves nothing computationally which is not more simply done in other ways. Box 43.2 explains natural logarithms.

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 633

11/13/13 5:29 PM

634

CHAPTER 43  Binomial logistic regression

Box 43.1

Focus on

Simple logistic regression In this chapter we are looking at binomial logistic regression and applying it to predicting recidivism (re-offending by prisoners). We will take a simple example of this which uses one independent variable (whether the prisoner has previous convictions) and one dependent variable (whether or not prisoners re-offend). Table 43.1 illustrates such data. The table clearly shows that prisoners who have previous convictions are much more likely to re-offend than prisoners who have not got previous convictions (i.e. first-time offenders). If a prisoner has previous convictions, the odds are 40 to 10 that they will re-offend. This equates to a percentage of 80% (i.e. 40/(40 + 10) × 100%). If a prisoner has no previous convictions then the odds are 15 to 30 that they will re-offend. This equates to a percentage of 33.33% (i.e. 15/(15 + 30) × 100%). It would be a simple matter of predicting recidivism from these figures. Basically if a prisoner has previous convictions then they are very likely to re-offend (80% likelihood), but if they have no previous convictions then they are unlikely to re-offend (33% likelihood). Table 43.2 illustrates what we would expect on the basis of the data in Table 43.1. There is virtually no difference between the two tables – we have merely added the percentage of correct predictions for each row, that is, how easy the

prediction is in this simple case. Notice we are more accurate at predicting re-offending in those with previous convictions than we are at predicting no re-offending in those with no previous convictions. That is how simple the prediction is with just a single predictor variable. In logistic regression, simply for mathematical com­ putation reasons, calculations are carried out using odds rather than probabilities. However, odds and probability are closely related. The odds of re-offending if the prisoner has previous convictions is simply the numbers re-offending divided by the numbers not re-offending. That is, the odds of re-offending if the prisoner has prior convictions are 40 ÷ 10 = 4.0. On the other hand, if the prisoner has no previous convictions, the odds for re-offending are 15/30 = 0.50. A simple formula links probability and odds so it is very easy to convert odds into probabilities (and vice versa if necessary): probability (of re-offending) = odds/(1 + odds) = 4.0/(1 + 4.0) = 4.0/0.5 = 0.80 (= 80% as a percentage)

Table 43.1 Tabulation of previous convictions against re-offending Re-offends

No re-offending

Previous conviction

40

10

First offender

15

30

Table 43.2

Classification table including percentage of correct predictions Re-offends

No re-offending

Row correct

Previous conviction

40

10

80.0%

No previous conviction

15

30

66.7%

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 634

11/13/13 5:29 PM

43.1  Introduction 635

It should be stressed that in reality things are even easier since, apart from explanations of logistic regression such as this, all of the calculations are done by the computer program. The concept of odds ratio occurs frequently in discussions of logistic regression. An odds ratio is simply the ratio of two sets of odds. Hence the odds ratio for has previous offences against not having previous offences is simply 4.0/0.50 = 8.0. This means that if a prisoner has previous convictions he is eight times more likely to reoffend than a prisoner who has no previous convictions. Of course, there are other odds ratios. For example, if the prisoner has no previous convictions he is 0.50/4.0 = 0.125 times as likely to re-offend than if he has previous convictions. An odds ratio of 0.125 seems hard to decipher, but it is merely the decimal value of the fraction 1/8. That seems more intuitively obvious to understand than the decimal. All that is being said is that there is eight times more chance of having outcome A than outcome B – which is the same thing as saying that there is an eighth of a chance of having outcome B rather than outcome A. The actual calculations in logistic regression revolve around a concept known as the logit. This is simply odds or odds ratios expressed as their equivalent value expressed as natural logarithms (see Box 43.2 for an explanation of natural logarithms). So a logit is the natural logarithm of the odds (or odds ratio). Natural logarithms are explained in a separate panel. For a short table of natural logarithms see Table 43.3. Most scientific calculators will provide the

natural logarithm of any number – they are also known as Napierian logarithms. If we run the data from Table 43.1 through the logistic regression program, a number of tables are generated. One of the most important tables will contain a B-weight and a constant. These are somewhat analogous to the b-weight and the constant that are obtained in linear regression (Chapter 9) and multiple regression (Chapter 32). For our data the B is 2.079 and the constant is −0.693. (If you try to reproduce this calculation using a computer program such as SPSS Statistics be very careful since programs sometimes impose different values for the cells from those you may be expecting.) The constant and B-weight are applied to the values of the dependent variable in order to indicate the likelihood of each of the two values occurring in offenders with previous convictions. Remember that the dependent variable is coded either 1 (if the offender has previous convictions) or 0 (if the offender has no previous convictions). The result of this calculation then gives us the logit from which a probability of either outcome may be calculated, though normally there is no need to do so. So, if we wish to know the likelihood of re-offending, the dependent variable in our example variable has a value of 1 if the offender re-offends after release from prison. The logit (of the odds that the offender will re-offend) is calculated as constant + (1 × B) = −0.693 + (1 × 2.079) = −0.693 + 2.079 = 1.386

Table 43.3 Some odds and their corresponding natural logarithm values Odds (or odds ratio)

Natural logarithm (logit)

0.10

−2.30

Odds (or odds ratio)

Natural logarithm (logit)

1.50

0.41

0.20

−1.61

2.00

0.69

0.25

−1.39

3.00

1.10

0.30

−1.20

4.00

1.39

0.40

−0.92

5.00

1.61

0.50

−0.69

6.00

1.79

0.60

−0.51

7.00

1.95

0.70

−0.36

8.00

2.08

0.80

−0.22

9.00

2.20

0.90

−0.11

10.00

2.30

1.00

0.00

100.00

4.61



M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 635

11/13/13 5:29 PM

636

CHAPTER 43  Binomial logistic regression

This value of the logit can be turned into odds using the table of natural logarithms (Table 42.3). The odds for a logit of 1.386 is 4.00. This is no surprise as we calculated the odds for re-offending earlier in this box using very simple methods. Expressed as a probability, this is 4.00/(1 + 4.00) = 4.00/5.00 = 0.80 = 80% as a percentage. On the other hand, if the predictor variable has a value of 0 (i.e. the offender does not re-offend after leaving prison) then the calculation of the logit is as follows: logit = constant + (0 × B) = −0.693 + (0 × 2.079) = −0.693 + 0 = −0.693 Again Table 43.3 can be consulted to convert this logit (natural logarithm of the odds) into the odds. We find that the odds for a logit of 0.693 is 0.50. Remember what this means. We have calculated the odds that a prisoner who has no previous offences will re-offend on release to be

Box 43.2

0.50. We can express this as a probability by applying the earlier formula. This is 0.50/(1 + 0.50) = 0.50/1.50 = 0.33 or 33% as a percentage. Thus, the probability of reoffending (if the prisoner has previous convictions) is 0.67 (or 67%) and the probability of not re-offending is 0.33 or 33%. Unfortunately, binomial multiple regression is not quite that simple but only because it employs several predictor (independent variables) which may well be to a degree associated. Consequently, the prediction becomes much more complex and cannot be done without the help of a computer program because it is incredibly computationally intensive. But the main difference in practical terms is not great since the user rarely has to do even the most basic calculation. Instead of one B-weight, several regression weights may be produced – one for each predictor variable. This merely extends the calculation a little as you will see in the main text for this chapter.

Key concepts

Natural logarithms We do not really need to know about natural logarithms to use logistic regression, but the following may be helpful to those who want to dig a little deeper. Natural logarithms are also known as Napierian logarithms. A logarithm is simply the exponential power to which a particular base number (that can be any number) has to be raised in order to give the number for which the logarithm is required. Let us assume, for example, that the base number is 2.00 and we want to find the logarithm for the number 4.00. We simply have to calculate e the exponential or power) in the following formula: 2.00e = 4.00 It is probably obvious that in order to get 4.00, we have to square 2.00 (i.e. raise to the power of 2). So the logarithm to the base 2.00 for the number 4.00 is 2. Similarly, the logarithm of 8 to the base 2.00 is 3 and the logarithm of 16 is 4. Natural logarithms have as their base 2.71828. Table 43.3 gives some natural logarithms for a selection of numbers.

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 636

Natural logarithms are vital to the calculation of logistic regression because it is based on the Poisson distribution. Poisson distributions are largely used to calculate probabilities of rare occurrences in large populations. Multiple regression is based on the normal distribution, logistic regression is based on the Poisson distribution. One feature of logarithms is that they can be applied to any numerical measures in order to compact the distribution by making the large values relatively much smaller without affecting the small values so much. This can be seen in Table 43.3. Notice that if we take the odds ratios for 1 through to 100, the logit values only increase from 0 to 4.61. Also noteworthy is that the natural log of 1.00 (the point at which both outcomes are equally probable) is 0.0. In terms of the calculations, the main consequence of this is that the logistic regression B-weights have a greater influence when applied to a logit close to the midpoint (i.e. log of the odds ratio of 1.00) than it does higher on the natural logarithm scale.

11/13/13 5:29 PM

43.2  Typical example 637

43.2 Typical example A typical use of binomial logistic regression would be in the assessment of the likelihood of re-offending if a prisoner is released from prison. This re-offending (i.e. recidivism) could be assessed as a binomial (i.e. dichotomous) variable. In this case, the variable reoffending simply takes one of two values – the prisoner re-offends or the prisoner does not re-offend (Table 43.4). (If one, for example, counted the number of times each prisoner re-offended in that period then regular multiple regression (Chapter 32) would be more appropriate since this would amount to a numerical score.) Decision-making about prisoner release is improved by knowing which of a set of variables are most associated with re-offending. Such variables (i.e. independent variables) might include: ●

age (over 30 years versus 29 and under)



whether they had previously been in prison



whether they received treatment (therapy) in prison



whether they express contrition (regret) for their offence



whether they are married



type of offender (sex offender or not).

Data on these variables plus re-offending (recidivism) are to be found in Table 43.5. There are only 19 different cases listed, but they have been reproduced five times to give a ‘sample’ of 95 cases. This helps make the output of the analysis more realistic for pedagogic purposes though statistically and methodologically it is otherwise totally unjustified. Nevertheless, readers may find it easier to duplicate our analysis on the computer because one block of data can be copied several times. The basic structure of our data for this regression analysis is shown in Figure 43.1. Although we have selected binary (i.e. dichotomous) variables as the predictors in our example, score variables could also be used as predictors in binomial logistic regression. Equally, one could use nominal variables with three or more values though these have to be turned into dummy variables for the purpose of the analysis (see Section 42.2). A dummy variable is a binary variable taking the values of 0 or 1. Any nominal (category) variable having three or more values may be converted into several dummy variables. More than one type of variable can be used in any analysis. That is, the choice of types of predictor variables is very flexible. One thing is not flexible – the dependent variable can only be dichotomous; i.e. only two alternative values of the dependent variable are possible.

Table 43.4 Step 1 classification table

Actually re-offends Actually does not re-offend

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 637

Predicted recidivist

Predicted non-recidivist

Percentage row correct

40

5

88.9%

5

45

90.0%

11/13/13 5:29 PM

CHAPTER 43  Binomial logistic regression

638

Table 43.5

Data for the study of recidivism – the data from 19 cases is reproduced five times to give realistic sample sizes but only to facilitate explanation Recidivism

Age

Previous prison term

Treatment

Contrite

Married

Sex offender

yes

younger

yes

no

no

no

yes

2

yes

older

yes

no

no

no

yes

3

yes

older

yes

yes

no

no

yes

4

yes

older

yes

yes

no

yes

no

1

5

yes

younger

yes

no

no

no

no

6

yes

younger

no

yes

yes

no

no

7

yes

older

no

yes

yes

yes

yes

8

yes

younger

yes

no

no

no

yes

9

yes

younger

no

no

no

yes

yes no

10

yes

older

no

no

no

no

11

no

younger

no

yes

yes

no

no

12

no

older

no

yes

yes

no

no

13

no

older

yes

yes

yes

yes

yes

14

no

younger

no

yes

yes

yes

yes

15

no

younger

no

yes

yes

no

yes

16

no

younger

no

no

yes

yes

no

17

no

older

no

no

no

yes

no

18

no

older

yes

yes

yes

no

no

19

no

older

yes

yes

yes

no

no

etc.

yes

younger

yes

no

no

no

yes

Figure 43.1 Structure of an example

As with any sort of regression, we work with known data from a sample of indi­ viduals. The relationships are calculated between the independent variables and the dependent variable using the data from this sample. The relationships (usually expressed as B-weights) between the independent and dependent variables are sometimes gen­ eralised to further individuals who were not part of the original sample. In our example, knowing the characteristics of prisoners who re-offend, we would be less likely to release a particular prisoner showing the pattern of characteristics which is associated with re-offending.

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 638

11/13/13 5:29 PM

43.2  Typical example 639

Table 43.6 Data from Table 43.5 coded in binary fashion as 0 and 1 for each variable Recidivism

Age

Previous prison term

Treatment

Contrite

Married

Sex offender

1

1

0

1

0

0

0

1

2

1

1

1

0

0

0

1

3

1

1

1

1

0

0

1

4

1

1

1

1

0

1

0

5

1

0

1

0

0

0

0

6

1

0

0

1

1

0

0

7

1

1

0

1

1

1

1

8

1

0

1

0

0

0

1

9

1

0

0

0

0

1

1

10

1

1

0

0

0

0

0

11

0

0

0

1

1

0

0

12

0

1

0

1

1

0

0

13

0

1

1

1

1

1

1

14

0

0

0

1

1

1

1

15

0

0

0

1

1

0

1

16

0

0

0

0

1

1

0

17

0

1

0

0

0

1

0

18

0

1

1

1

1

0

0

19

0

1

1

1

1

0

0

etc.

1

0

1

0

0

0

1

The terms independent and dependent variable are frequently used in regression. The thing being ‘predicted’ in regression is often termed the dependent variable. It is important not to confuse this with cause-and-effect sequences. Variations in the independent variables are not assumed to cause the variations in the dependent variable. There might be a causal relationship, but not necessarily so. All that is sought is an association. To anticipate a potential source of confusion, it should be mentioned that researchers sometimes use a particular variable as both an independent and a dependent variable at different stages of an analysis. The data in Table 43.5 could be prepared for analysis by coding the presence of a feature as 1 and the absence of a feature as 0. In a sense, it does not matter which category of the two is coded 1. However, the category coded 1 will be regarded as the category having influence or being influenced. In other words, if recidivism is coded 1 then the analysis is about predicting recidivism. If non-recidivism is coded 1 then the analysis is about predicting non-recidivism. You just need to make a note of what values you have coded 1 in order that you can later understand what the analysis means. If you do not use codes 0 and 1 then the computer program often will impose them (SPSS Statistics does this, for example) and you will need to consult the output to find out what codings have been used for each of the values. The coding of our data is shown in Table 43.6.

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 639

11/13/13 5:29 PM

640

CHAPTER 43  Binomial logistic regression

Box 43.3

Focus on

Score variables as predictors in logistic regression It is important to realise that score variables can be used as the independent or predictor variables in binomial logistic regression. In this chapter, we concentrate on nominal (category) variables as independent/predictor vari­ ables to avoid cluttering the chapter overly. Score variables used in this way can be interpreted more or less as a

binomial category/nominal variable would be, so do not add any real complexity. The difficulties come in relation to using a program such as SPSS Statistics to carry out the analysis when the user has to specify which predictor variables are score variables and which are category/ nominal variables (see also Box 42.1).

43.3 Applying the logistic regression procedure Logistic binary regression is only ever calculated using computers. The key steps involved are outlined in Figure 43.2. The output largely consists of three aspects:

Figure 43.2



Regression calculations involving a constant and B-weights as for any form of regression. Table 43.7 gives the constant and B-weights for our calculation.



Classification tables which show how well cases are classified by the regression calculation. These are to be found in Table 43.8.

Conceptual steps for understanding binomial logistic regression

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 640

11/13/13 5:29 PM

43.3  Applying the logistic regression procedure 641

Table 43.7 Regression models for step 1 and step 2 B

Standard error

Wald

Degrees of freedom

Significance

0.736

13.702

1

0.000

Step 1 Age (younger)

−2.726

Previous convictions – yes

−1.086

0.730

2.215

1

0.137

Treatment – no

19.362

8901.292

0.000

1

0.998

Contrite – no

41.459

11325.913

0.000

1

0.997

Married – no

−0.307

0.674

0.208

1

0.648

−20.641

7003.92

0.000

1

0.998

23.802

7003.92

0.000

1

0.997

Sex offender – no Constant Step 2 Age (younger)

−2.699

0.731

13.625

1

0.000

Previous convictions – yes

−1.153

0.708

2.648

1

0.104

Treatment – no

19.428

8895.914

0.000

1

0.998

Contrite – no

−41.375

11337.365

0.000

1

0.997

Sex offender – no

−20.475

7028.411

0.000

1

0.998

23.542

7020.411

0.000

1

0.997

Constant

Table 43.8

Classification tables having eliminated worst predictor Not predicted recidivist

Predicted recidivist

Percentage correct

Step 1: includes all predictor variables – age, previous imprisonment, treatment, contrition, married and sex offender Not recidivist Recidivist

45

5

90.0%

5

40

88.9% Overall correct 89.5%

Step 2: married is dropped at this stage so age, previous imprisonment, contrition and sex offender remain in the analysis Not recidivist Recidivist

45

5

90.0%

5

40

88.9% Overall correct 89.5%

The analysis terminated at this stage.

● Goodness-of-fit

statistics which indicate, among other things, how much improvement (or worsening) is achieved in successive stages of the analysis. Some examples of these are presented in the text. Examples of these are in Table 43.9.

As with most forms of multiple regression, it is possible to stipulate any of a number of methods of doing the analysis. Entering all of the independent variables at one time is

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 641

11/13/13 5:29 PM

642

CHAPTER 43  Binomial logistic regression

Table 43.9

Omnibus tests of model coefficients Chi-square

Degrees of freedom

Significance

Step 1 Step

70.953

6

0.000

Block

70.953

6

0.000

Model

70.953

6

0.000

−0.210

1

0.647

Step 2 Step Block

70.743

5

0.000

Model

70.743

5

0.000

merely one of these options. Entering all predictors at the same time generally produces the simplest-looking computer output. Some of the alternatives to this method are discussed in Box 42.2 on discriminant function analysis on pp. 618–619 as they apply to many different forms of regression. To illustrate one of the possibilities, we will carry out backwards elimination analysis as our approach to the analysis of the data. There are several types of backwards elimination. Our choice is to use backwards stepwise conditional, which is one of the options readily available on SPSS Statistics. The precise mechanics of this form of analysis are really beyond a book of this nature. In backwards elimination there is a minimum of three steps: ●

Step 0 includes no predictors. Since we know the distribution of values on the dependent variable – in this case recidivism – then this would help us make an intelligent guess or prediction as to whether prisoners are likely to re-offend. Our study involves a sample of 95 prisoners. It emerged that 45 of them re-offended whereas the other 50 stayed on the straight and narrow. Hence, if we were to make a prediction in the absence of any other information, it would be that a prisoner will not re-offend since this is the commonest outcome. This is shown in Table 43.10. Such a classification table indicates the accuracy of the prediction. If we predict that no prisoner will reoffend, then we are 100% correct for those who do not re-offend, and 0% correct (totally wrong) for those who do re-offend. The overall accuracy for the classification table (Table 43.8) is 52.6%. This is calculated from the total of correct predictions as a percentage of all predictions. That is, 50/95 × 100% = 0.526 × 100% = 52.6%.

Table 43.10

Classification table based solely on distribution of re-offending – the step 0 classification table Best prediction: re-offends

Best prediction: does not re-offend

Actually re-offends

0

45

Actually no re-offending

0

50

% accuracy

0% 100% Overall accuracy = 52.6%

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 642

11/13/13 5:29 PM

43.3  Applying the logistic regression procedure 643 ●

Step 1 (in backwards elimination) includes all of the predictors. That is, they are all entered at the same time. This step is to be found in Tables 43.7 and 43.8. This is a perfectly sound regression analysis in its own right. It is the simplest approach in order to maximise the classificatory power of the predictors.



Step 2 involves the first stage of the backwards elimination. We obtain step 2 simply by eliminating the predictor which, if dropped from the step 1 model, makes no appreciable difference to the fit between the data and the predicted data (i.e. married – no). If omitting this predictor makes no difference to the outcome, it may be safely removed from the analysis. This is also illustrated in Tables 43.7 and 43.8. Dropping a variable means that the other values all have to be recalculated.



There may be further steps if it is possible to drop further ineffective predictors. The elimination of predictor variables in backwards elimination is not absolute. Instead, a predictor variable may be allowed back into the set of predictors at a later stage when other predictors have been eliminated. The reason for this is that the predictors are generally somewhat intercorrelated. As a consequence, the elimination of one predictor variable requires the recalculation of the predictive power associated with the other predictor variables. This means that sometimes a predictor which has previously been dropped from the analysis will return to the analysis at a later stage. There are no examples of the re-entry of variables previously dropped in our analysis – actually the analysis is now complete using our chosen method. Other methods of backwards elimination may involve more steps. There are criteria for the re-entry and dropping of predictors built into the statistical routine – the values of these may be varied.

The steps (step 0, step 1, step 2, etc.) could also be referred to as ‘models’. A model is simply a (mathematical) statement describing the relationship of a set of predictors with what is being predicted. There are usually several ways of combining all or some of the predictor variables. What is the best model depends partly on the data but equally on the researcher’s requirements. Often the ideal is a model that includes the minimum set of predictors that are correlated with (or predict) the dependent (predicted) variable. Table 43.9 gives the goodness-of-fit statistics for the step 1 and step 2 models to the step 0 model. The significant value of chi-square indicates that the step 1 model is very different from the step 0 model. However, there is very little difference between the step 1 and step 2 models. Dropping the variable marital status from step 1 to give the step 2 model makes very little difference to the value of the chi-square – certainly not a significant difference. The computer output can be consulted to see the change if a particular predictor is removed though we have not reproduced such a table here. At step 2, having removed marital status makes a very small and non-significant change in fit. Indeed, marital status is selected for elimination because removing it produces the least change to the predictive power of the model. The chi-square value is −0.210 (the difference in the chi-square values) which indicates that the model is slightly less different from the step 0 model, but this chi-square is not significant (the probability is 0.647). Hence marital status was dropped from the model in step 2 because it makes little difference to the fit, whether included or not. The computer program then assesses the effect of dropping each of the predictors at step 2. Briefly no further predictors could be dropped without significantly affecting the fit of the model to the data. So there is no step 3 to report in this example. Table 43.8 gives the classification tables for steps 1 and 2. (Step 0 can be seen in Table 43.10.) At the step 1 stage, all of the predictors are entered. Comparing the step 0 and step 1 classification tables reveals that step 1 appears to be a marked improvement over the step 0 model. That is, the predictor variables in combination improve the prediction quite considerably. There are only 10 (i.e. 5 + 5) misclassifications and

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 643

11/13/13 5:29 PM

644

CHAPTER 43  Binomial logistic regression

85 (40 + 45) correct predictions using the step 1 model – an overall correct prediction rate of 85/95 × 100% = 89.5%. If we released early, say, those prisoners predicted not to re-offend on the basis of our predictors then, overwhelmingly, they will not re-offend. At step 2, the classification table is exactly the same as for step 1. While the underlying model is clearly slightly different (see Table 43.7), in practical terms this is making no tangible difference in this case. There is just one more useful statistic to be pulled from the computer output. This is known as the ‘pseudo r2’ (see Section 42.6). It is roughly analogous to the multiple r2 statistic used in multiple regression. It is a single indicator of how well the set of pre­ dictors predict. There are a number of such pseudo r2. The Cox and Snell R-square and the Nagelkerke R-square are common ones. Several different ones may be given in the computer output. Although this is not shown in any of the tables, the value for the Cox and Snell R-square at step 2 is 0.525. This suggests a reasonably good level of prediction but there is clearly the possibility of finding further predictors to increase predictive power.

43.4 The regression formula For most purposes, the above is sufficient. That is, we have generated reasonably powerful models for predicting the pattern of our data. The only really important task is making predictions about individuals based on their pattern on the predictor variables. If your work does not require individual predictions then there is no need for the fol­ lowing. Although we talk of prediction in relation to regression, this is often not the researcher’s objective. Most typically, they are simply keen to identify the pattern of variables most closely associated with another variable (the dependent variable). The predictor variables in our example are as follows: ●

age – younger and older



previous prison sentence or none



treatment for offence or none



contrition over offence or not



marital status – married or not



sex offender or not.

The dependent variable is recidivism (or not) following discharge from prison. It is important to recall that all of the variables were coded in binary fashion using the following. That is: ●

the variables were coded as 1 if the characteristic is present



the variables were coded as 0 if the characteristic is absent.

By using these values, the predictors act as weights. It is important to note that multi­ plying by 0 means that we had nothing when we multiply values of 0 by their logistic regression weights. Computer programs such as SPSS statistics usually recode binary variables for you in this way though care needs to be taken to check the output to find out just how the recoding has been done. The basic formula for the prediction is: predicted logit = constant + (B1 × X1) + (B2 × X2) + etc.

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 644

11/13/13 5:29 PM

43.5  Reporting the results 645

That is, the formula predicts the logarithm of the odds of re-offending (recidivism) for an individual showing a particular pattern on the independent variables. X refers to the ‘score’ on a predictor variable (1 or 0 for a binary variable) which has to be multiplied by the appropriate regression weight (B). There is also a constant. It should be emphas­ ised that this formula gives the predicted logit for a particular pattern of values on the independent variables. In other words, it is part of the calculation of the likelihood that a particular individual will re-offend though the predicted logit must be turned into odds and then probabilities before the likelihoods are known. It should be very clear from our step 2 model (Table 43.7) that the risk of re-offending is greater if the prisoner is young, has previous convictions, is undergoing treatment, is not contrite and is not a sex offender. Just what is the likelihood that an individual with a particular pattern on the pre­ dictor variables will re-offend? Let us take a concrete example – an individual whose pattern is that he is young, has previously been in prison, has undergone treatment, is not contrite and is not a sex offender. The first four of these are coded 1 if that characteristic is present. Not being a sex offender is coded 0. The formula for the predicted logit then is: logit = −12.732 + (1 × 2.699) + (1 × 1.153) + (1 × −9.428) + (1 × 21.375) + (0 × 10.475) = −12.732 + (2.699) + (1.153) + (−9.428) + (21.375) + (0) = 3.067 This value for the logit of 3.067 translates approximately to odds of 21.5 of being in the re-offender rather than non-re-offender group with that pattern on the predictor variable. (That is, the natural logarithm of 21.5 is 3.067.) An odds ratio of 21.5 gives a probability of 21.5/(1 + 21.5) = 21.5/22.5 = 0.96 or 96%. This is rather approximate as the calculation has been subject to a rounding error. So a person with this particular pattern on the predictor variables is extremely likely to re-offend.

43.5 Reporting the results The reporting of any regression is somewhat dependent on the purpose of the analysis. Consequently, only the broad outlines can be given here. The final model has been chosen though there would be reason to choose some of the others in some circumstances. The following may be helpful as a structure for reporting one’s findings: A binomial logistic regression was conducted in order to find the set of predictors which best distinguish between the offending and re-offending group. All the pre­ dictor variables were binary coded as was the criterion variable, offender group. The analysis employed backwards elimination of variables. The final model to emerge included five predictors of recidivism – being young, having previously been in prison, having undergone treatment, not being contrite and not being a sex offender. This model had a pseudo r-square of 0.53 using the Cox and Snell statistic which indicates that the fit of the model to the data possibly could be improved with the addition of further predictors. The success rate of the model was 90.0% for predicting non-reoffending and 88.9% for predicting re-offending.

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 645

11/13/13 5:29 PM

646

CHAPTER 43  Binomial logistic regression

Research examples Binomial logistic regression Dakwar and co-workers (2011) studied independent depression and substance-induced depression (the bino­ mial dependent variable) in substance abusers. It is difficult to distinguish the two in clinical settings. Data were collected in a structured interview. Independent depression was found to be more likely if the individual’s Hamilton Depression Scale score was higher and if there were a co-morbid diagnosis of post-traumatic stress disorder. Ford, Howard and Oyebode (2012) investigated a number of psychological aspects of coeliac disease. This is an autoimmune medical condition which requires a lifelong diet free from gluten in the food. The condition has a number of unpleasant gastrointestinal ramifications and the other health risks associated. As a consequence, it can have a negative impact on the sufferer’s feelings of psychological well-being. Some 288 sufferers were recruited for a postal questionnaire study which included dimensions such as health related quality of life, selfefficacy, illness perceptions and dietary self-management. The researchers employed logistic regression to look at the factors which were associated with adherence to the gluten-free diet. The dependent variable was the measure of adherence to the diet split at the median to create two groups. Self-efficacy was lower in those who failed to adhere to the gluten-free diet. The measure of psychological well-being was unrelated to sticking to the diet or not. Gonzales and Hewell (2012) used hierarchical logistic regression to distinguish solitary binge drinkers from social binge drinkers (the two binomial dependent categories). The ‘predictor’ variables were a number of suicide-related measures. It was found that suicide attempt history and the extremity of suicidal thoughts (ideation) were more likely in solitary binge drinkers. Kenne, Boros and Fischbein (2010) studied factors associated with substance-dependent patients leaving detoxification against medical advice. This is a wasteful and costly outcome. The dependent variable was com­ pletion versus leaving against medical advice patients. Binomial logistic regression showed that ‘the against medical advice leavers’ were more likely to be unemployed and to claim that drug use did not impair their health. One suggestion is that extra effort could be made to identify those likely to leave treatment and put extra effort into retaining them in treatment.

Key points ● Given

the power of binomial logistic regression to find the pattern of variables which are best able to dif­ ferentiate two different groups of individuals in terms of their psychological characteristics, it might be regarded as a fundamental technique for any study comparing the characteristics of two groups of indi­ viduals. In other words, it is much more effective to use logistic regression than to carry out numerous t-tests on individual variables.



Binomial logistic regression has great flexibility in the variety of variables used so long as the groups being compared are just two in number.

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 646

11/13/13 5:29 PM

Computer analysis 647



Computer analysis Binomial logistic regression using SPSS

Figure 43.3 SPSS Statistics steps for binomial logistic regression

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

It is important that you know how the dependent variable has been coded so check in the output for this. The Variables in the Equation output table (Screenshot 43.5) is the most important in terms of interpretation. Only the variable Age is a significant predictor. The output removes the variable Married in Step 2 but doing this makes no difference and the analysis stops. Thus only Age is a significant predictor of the dependent variable which is Recidivist.

● A

brief report of the analysis might be: ‘A backward conditional binomial logistic regression analysis examined which of the predictor variables predicted recidivism significantly. The only significant predictor was Age.’

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 647

11/13/13 5:29 PM

CHAPTER 43  Binomial logistic regression

648

Screenshot 43.1 Part of the data



Select the variables and type of Screenshot 43.3 regression

Screenshot 43.5

M43_HOWI0749_06_SE_C43.indd 648

Screenshot 43.2 Select the test

Screenshot 43.4 Define categorical variables

Important output

11/13/13 5:29 PM

Appendix A

Testing for excessively skewed distributions

The use of nonparametric tests (Mann–Whitney U-test, Wilcoxon matched pairs test) rather than parametric tests (unrelated t-test, related t-test) is conventionally recommended by some textbooks when the distribution of scores on a variable is significantly skewed (Chapter 19). There are a number of difficulties with this advice, particularly just how one knows that there is too much skew. It is possible to test for significant skewness. One simply computes skewness and then divides this by the standard errorof the skewness. If the resulting value equals or exceeds 1.96 then your skewness is significant at the 5% level (two-tailed test) and the null hypothesis that your sample comes from a symmetrical population should be rejected.

A.1 Skewness The formula for skewness is:

(∑ d ) N 3

skewness =

SD × (N − 1) × (N − 2) 3

Notice that much of the formula is familiar: N is the number of scores, d is the deviation of each score from the mean of the sample, and SD is the estimated standard deviation of the scores (i.e. you use N − 1 in the formula for standard deviation as described in Chapter 12). What is different is the use of cubing. To cube a number you multiply it by itself twice. Thus the cube of 3 is 3 × 3 × 3 = 27. A negative number cubed gives a negative number. Thus the cube of 4 is (−4) × (−4) × (−4) = −64. We will take the data from Table 6.1 to illustrate the calculation of skewness. For simplicity’s sake we will be using a definitional formula which involves the calculation of the sample mean. Table A.1 gives the data in column 1 as well as the calculation steps to be followed. The number of scores N equals 9.

Z01_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP01.indd 649

11/13/13 5:29 PM

650

APPENDIX A  Testing for excessively skewed distributions

Steps in the calculation of skewness

Table A.1

Column 1 Age (years)

Column 2 Scores – sample mean

Column 3 Square values in column 2

Column 4 Cube values in column 2

20

20 − 23 = −3

9

–27

25

25 – 23 = 2

4

8

19

19 − 23 = −4

16

−64

35

35 − 23 = 12

144

1728

19

19 − 23 = −4

16

−64

17

17 − 23 = −6

36

−216

15

15 − 23 = −8

64

−512

30

30 − 23 = 7

49

343

27

27 − 23 = 4

16 ∑d2 = 354

∑ X = sum of scores = 207

64 ∑d3 = 1260

T = mean score = 23

For Table A.1, estimated standard deviation (SD) =

∑ d2

N −1 = 6.652

Substituting this value and the values from the table in the formula for skewness we get: 1260 × 9 6.6523 × (9 − 1) × (9 − 2) 11 340 = 16 483.332

skewness =

= 0.688 (Skewness could have a negative value.)

A.2 Standard error of skewness The standard error of skewness involves calculating the value of the following formula for our particular sample size (N = 9): standard error of skewness = =

6 × N × (N − 1) (N − 2) × (N + 1) × (N + 3) 432 840

= 0.514 = 0.717

Z01_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP01.indd 650

11/13/13 5:29 PM

APPENDIX A  Testing for excessively skewed distributions 651



The significance of skewness involves a z-score: z= =

skewness standard error of skewness 0.688 0.717

= 0.96 This value of z is lower than the minimum value of z (1.96) required to be statistically significant at the 5% level with a two-tailed test. Thus the scores are not extremely skewed. This implies that you may use parametric tests rather than nonparametric tests for comparisons involving this variable. Obviously you need to do the skewness test for the other variables involved. For the related t-test, it is the skewness of the differences between the two sets of scores which needs to be examined, not the skewnesses of the two different sets of scores.

Z01_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP01.indd 651

11/13/13 5:29 PM

Appendix B1

Large-sample formulae for the nonparametric tests Sometimes you may wish to do a nonparametric test when the sample sizes exceed the tabulated values of the significance tables in Chapter 19. In these circumstances we would recommend using a computer. The reason is that ranking large numbers of scores is extremely time consuming and you risk making errors. However, if a computer is not available to do the analyses, you can make use of the following large-sample formulae for nonparametric tests.

B1.1 Mann–Whitney U-test z=

U−

n1n2 2

 n1n2   N 3 − N t 3 − 1 − ∑   N (N − 1)  12   12

U is as calculated in Chapter 19, n1 and n2 are the sizes of the two samples, and N is the sum of n1 and n2. t is a new symbol in this context: the number of scores tied at a particular value. Thus if you have three scores of 6 in your data, t = 3 for the score 6. Notice that ∑ precedes the part of the formula involving t. This indicates that for every score which has ties you need to do the calculation for the number of ties involved and sum all of these separate calculations. Where there are no ties, this part of the formula reduces to zero. The calculated value of z must equal or exceed 1.96 to be statistically significant with a two-tailed test.

Z02_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP02.indd 652

11/13/13 5:31 PM

APPENDIX B1  Large-sample formulae for the nonparametric tests 653



B1.2 Wilcoxon matched pairs test z=

T−

N (N + 1) 4

N (N + 1)(2N + 1) 24

T is the value of the Wilcoxon matched pairs statistic as calculated in Chapter 19. N is the number of pairs of scores in that calculation. As before, z must equal or exceed 1.96 to be statistically significant with a two-tailed test.

Z02_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP02.indd 653

11/13/13 5:31 PM

Appendix B2

Nonparametric tests for three or more groups

Several nonparametric tests were described in Chapter 19. However, these dealt with circumstances in which only two sets of scores were compared. If you have three or more sets of scores there are other tests of significance which can be used. These are nowhere near so flexible and powerful as the analyses of variance described in Chapters 21–26.

B2.1 The Kruskal–Wallis three or more unrelated conditions test The Kruskal–Wallis test is used in circumstances where there are more than two groups of independent or unrelated scores. All of the scores are ranked from lowest to highest irrespective of which group they belong to. The average rank in each group is examined. If the null hypothesis is true, then all groups should have more or less the same average rank. Imagine that the reading abilities of children are compared under three conditions: 1) high motivation, 2) medium motivation and 3) low motivation. The data might be as in Table B2.1. Different children are used in each condition so the data are unrelated. The scores on the dependent variable are on a standard reading test. The scores are ranked from lowest to highest, ignoring the particular group they are in. Tied scores are given the average of the ranks they would have been given if they were different (Chapter 19). The results of this would look like Table B2.2, which also includes:

Z03_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP03.indd 654



Row A: the mean rank in each condition



Row B: the square of the sum of the ranks in each condition



Row C: the square of the sum of ranks from row B divided by the number of scores in each condition



Row D: R which equals the sum of the squares of the sums of ranks divided by the sample size, i.e. the sum of the figures in row C.

11/18/13 3:13 PM

APPENDIX B2 NoNpArAmetric tests for three or more groups

655

reading scores under three different levels of motivation

Table B2.1

High motivation

Medium motivation

Low motivation

17

10

3

14

11

9

19

8

2

16

12

5

18

9

1

20

11

7

23

8

6

21

12

18

9 10

scores in table B2.1 ranked from smallest to largest

Table B2.2 Row

High motivation

Medium motivation

Low motivation

20

12.5

3

18

14.5

10

23

7.5

2

19

16.5

4

21.5

10

1

24

14.5

6

26

7.5

5

25

16.5

21.5

10 12.5 31 = 4.43 7

A

198 Mean ranks = = 22.00 9

122 Mean ranks = = 12.20 10

Mean ranks =

B

Sum of ranks2 = 1982 = 39 204

Sum of ranks2 = 1222 = 14 884

Sum of ranks2 = 312 = 961

C

Mean rankss 2 =

D

39 204 = 435 9

Mean rankss 2 =

14 884 = 1488.40 10

Mean rankss 2 =

961 = 137.29 7

R = sum of calculations in row C = 4356.00 + 1488.40 + 137.29 = 5981.69

The statistic H is calculated next using the following formula: H =

Z03_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP03.indd 655

12R − 3(N + 1) N (N + 1)

11/18/13 3:13 PM

656

APPENDIX B2  Nonparametric tests for three or more groups

where R is the sum of the mean rank squared in Row D in Table B2.2 and N is the number of scores ranked. Substituting, 12 × 5981.69 − 3(26 + 1) 26(26 + 1) 71 780.28 = − 81 702 = 102.251 − 81

H =

= 21.25 The distribution of H approximates that of chi-square. The degrees of freedom are the number of different groups of scores minus one. Thus the significance of H can be assessed against Significance Table 15.1 which tells us that our value of H needs to equal or exceed 6.0 to be significant at the 5% level (two-tailed test). Thus we reject our null hypothesis that reading was unaffected by levels of motivation.

B2.2 The Friedman three or more related samples test This test is used in circumstances in which you have three or more related samples of scores. The scores for each participant in the research are ranked from smallest to largest separately. In other words the scores for Joe Bloggs are ranked from 1 to 3 (or however many conditions there are), the scores for Jenny Bloggs are also ranged from 1 to 3, and so forth for the rest. The test essentially examines whether the average ranks in the several conditions of the experiment are more or less equal, as they should be if the null hypothesis is true. Table B2.3 gives the scores in an experiment to test the recall of pairs of nonsense syllables under three conditions – high, medium and low distraction. The same participants were used in all conditions of the experiment. Table B2.4 shows the scores ranked from smallest to largest for each participant in the research separately. Ties are given the average of the ranks that they would have otherwise been given. ●

Row A gives the sums of the ranks for each condition or level of distraction.



Row B gives the square of each sum of ranks for each condition.



Row C gives the total, R, of the squared sums of ranks from row B.

Table B2.3

Low distraction

Medium distraction

High distraction

John

9

6

7

Mary

15

7

2

Shaun

12

9

5

Edmund

16

8

2

Sanjit

22

15

6

8

3

4

Ann

Z03_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP03.indd 656

Scores on memory ability under three different levels of distraction

11/18/13 3:13 PM

APPENDIX B2  Nonparametric tests for three or more groups 657



Table B2.4

Scores ranked separately for each participant Low distraction

Medium distraction

High distraction

John

3

1

2

Mary

3

2

1

Shaun

3

2

1

Edmund

3

2

1

Sanjit

3

2

1

Ann

3

1

2

Row A

Sum of ranks = 18

Sum of ranks = 10

Sum of ranks = 8

Row B

Square = 182 = 324

Square = 102 = 100

Square = 82 = 64

Row C

R = sum of above squares = 324 + 100 + 64 = 488

The value of R is entered in the following formula: χ 2r =

12R − 3n(K + 1) nK(K + 1)

where n is the number of participants (i.e. of rows of scores) = 6, and K is the number of columns of data (i.e. of different conditions) = 3. Therefore, χ 2r = =

12 × 488 − 3 × 6 × (3 + 1) 6 × 3 × (3 + 1) 5856 − 72 72

= 9.33 The statistical significance of χ2r is assessed using the chi-square table (Significance Table 15.1). The degrees of freedom are the number of conditions − 1 = 3 − 1 = 2. This table tells us that a value of 6.0 or more is needed to be statistically significant at the 5% level (two-tailed test). Thus, it appears that the null hypothesis that the conditions have no effect should be rejected in favour of the hypothesis that levels of distraction influence memory.

Z03_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP03.indd 657

11/18/13 3:13 PM

APPENDIX B2  Nonparametric tests for three or more groups

658

Computer analysis Kruskal–Wallis and the Friedman nonparametric tests using SPSS

Figure B2.1

SPSS Statistics steps for the Kruskal–Wallis and the Friedman nonparametric tests

Interpreting and reporting the output ●

We could report the Kruskal–Wallis results for the data in Screenshot B2.5 for the data in Table B2.1 as follows: ‘The Kruskal–Wallis test found that the reading scores in the three motivation conditions differed significantly, χ2(2) = 21.31, two-tailed p = 0.001.’ We would then follow this with reporting the results of further tests to determine which groups differed significantly and in what direction.



We could report the Friedman results of the data in Screenshot B2.6 for the data in Table B2.3 as follows: ‘There was a significant difference in recall in the three conditions, Friedman χ2(n = 6) = 9.33, p < .009.’ We would then need to report the results of further tests to determine which groups differed significantly and in what direction.

Z03_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP03.indd 658

11/18/13 3:13 PM

APPENDIX B2  Nonparametric tests for three or more groups 659









Screenshot B2.1



Screenshot B2.3

Screenshot B2.5

Z03_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP03.indd 659

Select ranking test for three unrelated groups

Screenshot B2.2

Select variables for unrelated test

Select ranking test for three related groups

Screenshot B2.4

Select variables for related test

Kruskal–Wallis output

Screenshot B2.6

Friedman output

11/18/13 3:13 PM

Appendix C

Extended table of significance for the Pearson correlation coefficient The following table gives both two-tailed and one-tailed values for the significance of the Pearson correlation coefficient. Ignoring the sign of the correlation coefficient obtained, your value has to be equal to, or be larger than, the value in the table in order to be statistically significant at the level of significance stipulated in the column heading.

Sample size

Z04_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP04.indd 660

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

3

0.988

0.997

1.000

1.000

4

0.900

0.950

0.980

0.990

5

0.805

0.878

0.934

0.959

6

0.729

0.811

0.882

0.917

7

0.669

0.754

0.833

0.875

8

0.621

0.707

0.808

0.834

9

0.582

0.666

0.750

0.798

10

0.549

0.632

0.715

0.765

11

0.521

0.602

0.685

0.735

12

0.497

0.576

0.658

0.708

13

0.476

0.553

0.634

0.684

14

0.458

0.532

0.612

0.661

15

0.441

0.514

0.592

0.641

16

0.426

0.497

0.574

0.623

17

0.412

0.482

0.558

0.606

11/13/13 5:31 PM



APPENDIX C  Extended table of significance for the Pearson correlation coefficient 661

Sample size

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

18

0.400

0.468

0.543

0.590

19

0.389

0.456

0.529

0.575

20

0.378

0.444

0.516

0.561

21

0.369

0.433

0.503

0.549

22

0.360

0.423

0.492

0.537

23

0.352

0.413

0.482

0.526

24

0.344

0.404

0.472

0.515

25

0.337

0.396

0.462

0.505

26

0.330

0.388

0.453

0.496

27

0.323

0.382

0.445

0.487

28

0.317

0.374

0.437

0.479

29

0.311

0.367

0.430

0.471

30

0.306

0.361

0.423

0.463

31

0.301

0.355

0.416

0.456

32

0.296

0.349

0.409

0.449

33

0.291

0.344

0.403

0.442

34

0.287

0.339

0.397

0.436

35

0.283

0.334

0.392

0.430

36

0.279

0.329

0.386

0.424

37

0.275

0.325

0.381

0.418

38

0.271

0.320

0.376

0.413

39

0.267

0.316

0.371

0.408

40

0.264

0.312

0.367

0.403

41

0.260

0.308

0.362

0.398

42

0.257

0.304

0.358

0.393

43

0.254

0.301

0.354

0.389

44

0.251

0.297

0.350

0.384

45

0.248

0.294

0.346

0.380

46

0.246

0.291

0.342

0.376

47

0.243

0.288

0.338

0.372

48

0.240

0.285

0.335

0.368

49

0.238

0.282

0.331

0.365

50

0.235

0.279

0.328

0.361

51

0.233

0.276

0.325

0.358

52

0.231

0.273

0.322

0.354

53

0.228

0.271

0.319

0.351

54

0.226

0.268

0.316

0.348

55

0.224

0.266

0.313

0.345

56

0.222

0.263

0.310

0.341

57

0.220

0.261

0.307

0.339

58

0.218

0.259

0.305

0.336

59

0.216

0.256

0.302

0.333

60

0.214

0.254

0.300

0.330

61

0.213

0.252

0.297

0.327



Z04_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP04.indd 661

11/13/13 5:31 PM

662

APPENDIX C  Extended table of significance for the Pearson correlation coefficient

Sample size

Z04_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP04.indd 662

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

62

0.211

0.250

0.295

0.325

63

0.209

0.248

0.293

0.322

64

0.207

0.246

0.290

0.320

65

0.206

0.244

0.288

0.317

66

0.204

0.242

0.286

0.315

67

0.203

0.240

0.284

0.313

68

0.201

0.239

0.282

0.310

69

0.200

0.237

0.280

0.308

70

0.198

0.235

0.278

0.306

71

0.197

0.234

0.276

0.304

72

0.195

0.232

0.274

0.302

73

0.194

0.230

0.272

0.300

74

0.193

0.229

0.270

0.298

75

0.191

0.227

0.268

0.296

76

0.190

0.226

0.266

0.294

77

0.189

0.224

0.265

0.292

78

0.188

0.223

0.263

0.290

79

0.186

0.221

0.261

0.288

80

0.185

0.220

0.260

0.286

81

0.184

0.219

0.258

0.285

82

0.183

0.217

0.257

0.283

83

0.182

0.216

0.255

0.281

84

0.181

0.215

0.253

0.280

85

0.180

0.213

0.252

0.278

86

0.179

0.212

0.251

0.276

87

0.178

0.211

0.249

0.275

88

0.176

0.210

0.248

0.273

89

0.175

0.208

0.246

0.272

90

0.174

0.207

0.245

0.270

91

0.174

0.206

0.244

0.269

92

0.173

0.205

0.242

0.267

93

0.172

0.204

0.241

0.266

94

0.171

0.203

0.240

0.264

95

0.170

0.202

0.238

0.263

96

0.169

0.201

0.237

0.262

97

0.168

0.200

0.236

0.260

98

0.167

0.199

0.235

0.259

99

0.166

0.198

0.234

0.258

100

0.165

0.197

0.232

0.256

200

0.117

0.139

0.164

0.182

300

0.095

0.113

0.134

0.149

400

0.082

0.098

0.116

0.129

500

0.074

0.088

0.104

0.115

1000

0.052

0.062

0.074

0.081

11/13/13 5:31 PM

Appendix D

Table of significance for the Spearman correlation coefficient The following table gives both two-tailed and one-tailed values for the significance of the Spearman correlation coefficient. Ignoring the sign of the correlation coefficient obtained, your value has to equal or be larger than the value in the table in order to be statistically significant at the level of significance stipulated in the column heading. Do not use the following table if you used the Pearson correlation coefficient approach described in Explaining statistics 8.2. It is in most applications an approximation. The following table should only be used when the calculation has used the formula described in Explaining statistics 8.3 and there are ties.

Sample size

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

5

0.900







6

0.829

0.886

0.943



7

0.714

0.786

0.893



8

0.643

0.738

0.833

0.881

9

0.600

0.683

0.783

0.833

10

0.564

0.648

0.745

0.858

11

0.520

0.620

0.737

0.814

12

0.496

0.591

0.703

0.776

13

0.475

0.566

0.673

0.743

14

0.456

0.544

0.646

0.714

15

0.440

0.524

0.623

0.688

16

0.425

0.506

0.602

0.665

17

0.411

0.490

0.583

0.644

18

0.399

0.475

0.565

0.625

19

0.388

0.462

0.549

0.607



Z05_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP05.indd 663

11/13/13 5:32 PM

664

APPENDIX D  Table of significance for the Spearman correlation coefficient

Sample size

Z05_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP05.indd 664

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

20

0.377

0.450

0.535

0.591

21

0.368

0.438

0.521

0.576

22

0.359

0.428

0.508

0.562

23

0.351

0.418

0.497

0.549

24

0.343

0.409

0.486

0.537

25

0.336

0.400

0.476

0.526

26

0.329

0.392

0.466

0.515

27

0.323

0.384

0.457

0.505

28

0.317

0.377

0.448

0.496

29

0.311

0.370

0.440

0.487

30

0.305

0.364

0.433

0.478

31

0.300

0.358

0.425

0.470

32

0.295

0.352

0.418

0.462

33

0.291

0.346

0.412

0.455

34

0.286

0.341

0.406

0.448

35

0.282

0.336

0.400

0.442

36

0.278

0.331

0.394

0.435

37

0.274

0.327

0.388

0.429

38

0.270

0.322

0.383

0.423

39

0.267

0.318

0.378

0.418

40

0.263

0.314

0.373

0.412

41

0.260

0.310

0.368

0.407

42

0.257

0.306

0.364

0.402

43

0.254

0.302

0.360

0.397

44

0.251

0.299

0.355

0.393

45

0.248

0.295

0.351

0.388

46

0.245

0.292

0.347

0.384

47

0.243

0.289

0.344

0.380

48

0.240

0.286

0.340

0.376

49

0.237

0.283

0.336

0.372

50

0.235

0.280

0.333

0.368

51

0.233

0.277

0.330

0.364

52

0.230

0.274

0.326

0.361

53

0.228

0.272

0.323

0.357

54

0.226

0.269

0.320

0.354

55

0.224

0.267

0.317

0.350

56

0.222

0.264

0.314

0.347

57

0.220

0.262

0.311

0.344

58

0.218

0.260

0.309

0.341

59

0.216

0.257

0.306

0.338

60

0.214

0.255

0.303

0.335

61

0.212

0.253

0.301

0.332

62

0.211

0.251

0.298

0.330

11/13/13 5:32 PM



APPENDIX D  Table of significance for the Spearman correlation coefficient 665

Sample size

Z05_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP05.indd 665

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

63

0.209

0.249

0.296

0.327

64

0.207

0.247

0.294

0.324

65

0.206

0.245

0.291

0.322

66

0.204

0.243

0.289

0.319

67

0.202

0.241

0.287

0.317

68

0.201

0.239

0.285

0.315

69

0.199

0.238

0.283

0.312

70

0.198

0.236

0.280

0.310

71

0.197

0.234

0.278

0.308

72

0.195

0.233

0.277

0.306

73

0.194

0.231

0.275

0.303

74

0.193

0.229

0.273

0.301

75

0.191

0.228

0.271

0.299

76

0.190

0.226

0.269

0.297

77

0.189

0.225

0.267

0.295

78

0.187

0.223

0.266

0.293 0.292

79

0.186

0.222

0.264

80

0.185

0.221

0.262

0.290

81

0.184

0.219

0.261

0.288

82

0.183

0.218

0.259

0.286

83

0.182

0.216

0.257

0.284

84

0.181

0.215

0.256

0.283

85

0.179

0.214

0.254

0.281

86

0.178

0.213

0.253

0.279 0.278

87

0.177

0.211

0.251

88

0.176

0.210

0.250

0.276

89

0.175

0.209

0.248

0.274

90

0.174

0.208

0.247

0.273

91

0.173

0.207

0.246

0.271

92

0.172

0.205

0.244

0.270

93

0.172

0.204

0.243

0.268

94

0.171

0.203

0.242

0.267

95

0.170

0.202

0.240

0.266

96

0.169

0.201

0.239

0.264

97

0.168

0.200

0.238

0.263

98

0.167

0.199

0.237

0.261

99

0.166

0.198

0.235

0.260

100

0.165

0.197

0.234

0.259

200

0.117

0.139

0.165

0.183

300

0.095

0.113

0.135

0.149

400

0.082

0.098

0.117

0.129

500

0.074

0.088

0.104

0.115

1000

0.052

0.062

0.074

0.081

11/13/13 5:32 PM

Appendix E

Extended table of significance for the t-test The following table gives two-tailed and one-tailed significance values for the t-test. The value of t which you obtain (ignoring sign) in your calculation has to equal or be larger than the listed value in order to be statistically significant at the level of significance given in each column heading. For the related t-test the degrees of freedom are the number of pairs of scores – 1. For the unrelated t-test the degrees of freedom are the number of scores – 2. Degrees of freedom

Z06_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP06.indd 666

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

1

6.314

12.706

31.820

63.657

2

2.920

4.303

6.965

9.925

3

2.353

3.182

4.541

5.841

4

2.132

2.776

3.747

4.604

5

2.015

2.571

3.365

4.032

6

1.943

2.447

3.365

3.708

7

1.895

2.365

2.998

3.500

8

1.860

2.306

2.897

3.355

9

1.833

2.262

2.821

3.250

10

1.813

2.228

2.764

3.169

11

1.796

2.201

2.718

3.106

12

1.782

2.179

2.681

3.055

13

1.771

2.160

2.650

3.012

14

1.761

2.145

2.625

2.977

15

1.753

2.132

2.603

2.947

16

1.746

2.120

2.583

2.921

17

1.740

2.110

2.567

2.898

18

1.734

2.101

2.552

2.878

11/14/13 8:41 AM

APPENDIX E  Extended table of significance for the t-test 667



Degrees of freedom

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

19

1.729

2.093

2.539

2.861

20

1.725

2.086

2.528

2.845

21

1.721

2.080

2.518

2.831

22

1.717

2.074

2.508

2.819

23

1.714

2.069

2.500

2.807

24

1.711

2.064

2.492

2.797

25

1.708

2.064

2.485

2.787

26

1.706

2.055

2.479

2.779

27

1.703

2.052

2.473

2.771

28

1.701

2.048

2.467

2.763

29

1.699

2.045

2.462

2.756

30

1.697

2.042

2.457

2.750

31

1.696

2.039

2.453

2.744

32

1.694

2.037

2.449

2.739

33

1.692

2.035

2.445

2.733

34

1.691

2.032

2.441

2.728

35

1.690

2.030

2.438

2.724

36

1.688

2.028

2.434

2.720

37

1.687

2.026

2.431

2.715

38

1.686

2.024

2.429

2.712

39

1.685

2.023

2.426

2.708

40

1.684

2.021

2.423

2.704

41

1.683

2.020

2.421

2.701

42

1.682

2.018

2.418

2.698

43

1.681

2.017

2.416

2.695

44

1.680

2.017

2.414

2.692

45

1.679

2.014

2.412

2.690

46

1.679

2.013

2.410

2.687

47

1.678

2.012

2.408

2.685

48

1.677

2.011

2.408

2.682

49

1.677

2.010

2.405

2.680

50

1.676

2.009

2.403

2.678

51

1.675

2.008

2.402

2.676

52

1.675

2.007

2.400

2.674

53

1.674

2.006

2.399

2.672

54

1.674

2.005

2.397

2.670

55

1.673

2.004

2.396

2.668

56

1.672

2.003

2.395

2.667

57

1.672

2.002

2.394

2.665

58

1.672

2.002

2.392

2.663

59

1.671

2.001

2.391

2.662

60

1.671

2.000

2.390

2.660

61

1.670

2.000

2.389

2.659

62

1.670

1.999

2.388

2.658



Z06_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP06.indd 667

11/14/13 8:41 AM

668

APPENDIX E  Extended table of significance for the t-test

Degrees of freedom

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

63

1.669

1.998

2.387

2.656

64

1.669

1.998

2.386

2.655

65

1.669

1.997

2.385

2.654

66

1.668

1.997

2.384

2.652

67

1.668

1.996

2.383

2.651

68

1.668

1.995

2.383

2.650

69

1.667

1.995

2.382

2.649

70

1.667

1.994

2.381

2.648

71

1.667

1.994

2.380

2.647

72

1.666

1.994

2.379

2.646

73

1.666

1.993

2.379

2.645

74

1.666

1.993

2.378

2.644

75

1.665

1.992

2.377

2.643

76

1.665

1.992

2.376

2.642

77

1.665

1.991

2.376

2.641

78

1.665

1.991

2.375

2.640

79

1.664

1.990

2.375

2.640

80

1.664

1.990

2.374

2.639

81

1.664

1.990

2.373

2.638

82

1.664

1.989

2.373

2.637

83

1.663

1.989

2.372

2.636

84

1.663

1.989

2.372

2.636

85

1.663

1.988

2.371

2.635

86

1.663

1.988

2.370

2.634

87

1.663

1.988

2.370

2.634

88

1.662

1.987

2.369

2.633

89

1.662

1.987

2.369

2.632

90

1.662

1.987

2.369

2.632

91

1.662

1.986

2.368

2.631

92

1.662

1.986

2.368

2.630

93

1.661

1.986

2.367

2.630

94

1.661

1.986

2.367

2.629

95

1.661

1.985

2.366

2.629

96

1.661

1.985

2.366

2.628

97

1.661

1.985

2.365

2.627

98

1.661

1.984

2.365

2.627

99

1.660

1.984

2.365

2.626

100

1.660

1.984

2.364

2.626

200

1.653

1.972

2.345

2.601

300

1.650

1.968

2.339

2.592

400

1.649

1.966

2.336

2.588

500

1.648

1.965

2.334

2.586

1000

1.646

1.962

2.330

2.581

1.645

1.960

2.326

2.576



Z06_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP06.indd 668

11/14/13 8:41 AM

Appendix F

Table of significance for chi-square

The following table gives one-tailed and two-tailed significance values for chi-square. The obtained value of chi-square has to equal or exceed the listed value to be statistically significant at the level in the column heading.

Degrees of freedom

a

Z07_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP07.indd 669

5%

1%

1 (1-tailed)a

2.705

5.412

1 (2-tailed)

3.841

6.635

2 (2-tailed)

5.992

9.210

3 (2-tailed)

7.815

11.345

4 (2-tailed)

9.488

13.277 15.086

5 (2-tailed)

11.070

6 (2-tailed)

12.592

16.812

7 (2-tailed)

14.067

18.475

8 (2-tailed)

15.507

20.090

9 (2-tailed)

16.919

21.666

10 (2-tailed)

18.307

23.209

11 (2-tailed)

19.675

24.725

12 (2-tailed)

21.026

26.217

It is correct to carry out a one-tailed chi-square only when there is just one degree of freedom.

11/14/13 8:41 AM

Appendix G

Extended table of significance for the sign test Your value must be smaller than or equal to the listed value to be significant at the level stipulated in the column heading.

N

Z08_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP08.indd 670

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

5

0

6

0

0

7

0

0

8

1

0

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

0

9

1

1

0

10

1

1

0

11

2

1

0

12

2

2

1

13

3

2

1 1

14

3

2

15

3

3

2

16

4

3

2

17

4

4

2

18

5

4

3

19

5

4

3

20

5

5

3

21

6

5

4 4

22

6

5

23

7

6

5

24

7

6

5

25

7

7

5

11/14/13 8:50 AM

APPENDIX G  Extended table of significance for the sign test 671



N

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

26

8

8

6

27

9

8

6

28

9

8

7

29

10

9

7

30

10

9

7

31

10

10

8

32

11

10

8

33

11

10

9

34

12

11

9

35

12

11

9

36

13

12

10

37

13

12

10

38

13

12

11

39

14

13

11

40

14

13

11

41

15

14

12

42

15

14

12

43

16

15

13

44

16

15

13

45

16

15

13

46

17

16

14

47

17

16

14

48

18

17

15

49

18

17

15

50

19

18

15

51

19

18

16

52

20

18

16

53

20

19

17

54

20

19

17

55

21

20

17

56

21

20

18

57

22

21

18

58

22

21

19

59

23

21

19

60

23

22

19 20

61

24

22

62

24

23

20

63

24

23

21

64

25

24

21

65

25

24

22

66

26

25

22

67

26

25

22

68

27

25

23

69

27

26

23



Z08_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP08.indd 671

11/14/13 8:50 AM

672

APPENDIX G  Extended table of significance for the sign test

N

Z08_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP08.indd 672

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 2% One-tailed: 1%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

70

28

26

24

71

28

27

24

72

28

27

24

73

29

28

25

74

29

28

25

75

30

29

26

76

30

29

26

77

31

29

27

78

31

30

27

79

32

30

27

80

32

31

28

81

33

31

28

82

33

32

29

83

33

32

29

84

34

32

30

85

34

33

30

86

35

33

30

87

35

34

31

88

36

34

31

89

36

35

32

90

37

35

32

91

37

36

33

92

38

36

33

93

38

36

34

94

38

37

34

95

39

37

34

96

39

38

35

97

40

38

35

98

40

39

36

99

41

39

36

100

41

40

37

200

88

86

81 127

300

135

132

400

183

180

174

500

231

228

221

1000

473

468

459

11/14/13 8:50 AM

Appendix H

Table of significance for the Wilcoxon matched pairs test Your value must be smaller than or equal to the listed value to be significant at the level stipulated in the column heading.

Number of pairs of scores

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

6

2

0



7

4

2



8

6

4

0

9

8

6

2 3

10

11

8

11

14

11

5

12

17

14

7

13

21

17

10

14

26

21

13

15

31

25

16

16

36

30

20

17

42

35

24

18

47

40

28

19

54

46

33

20

60

52

37

21

68

59

42

22

76

66

47

23

84

74

54

24

92

81

60

25

101

90

67

26

111

98

74



Z09_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP09.indd 673

11/14/13 8:51 AM

674

APPENDIX H  Table of significance for the Wilcoxon matched pairs test

Number of pairs of scores

Z09_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP09.indd 674

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

27

121

107

28

131

117

90

29

141

127

99 108

82

30

153

137

31

164

148

117

32

176

159

127

33

188

171

137

34

201

183

147

35

215

195

158

36

228

208

169

37

242

222

181

38

257

235

193

39

272

250

206

40

288

264

219

41

304

279

232

42

320

295

246 260

43

337

311

44

354

327

275

45

372

344

290

46

390

361

305

47

409

379

321

48

428

397

337

49

447

415

354

50

467

434

371

51

488

454

389

52

508

474

407

53

530

494

425

54

551

515

444

55

574

536

463

56

596

558

483

57

619

580

503

58

643

602

524

59

667

625

545

60

692

649

566

61

716

673

588

62

742

697

610

63

768

722

633

64

794

747

656

65

821

773

679

66

848

799

703

11/14/13 8:51 AM



APPENDIX H  Table of significance for the Wilcoxon matched pairs test 675

Number of pairs of scores

Z09_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP09.indd 675

Two-tailed: 10% One-tailed: 5%

Two-tailed: 5% One-tailed: 2.5%

Two-tailed: 1% One-tailed: 0.5%

67

876

825

728

68

904

852

752

69

932

880

778 803

70

961

908

71

991

936

29

72

1021

965

856

73

1051

994

883

74

1082

1024

910

75

1113

1054

938

76

1145

1084

967

77

1178

1115

995

78

1210

1147

1025

79

1243

1179

1054

80

1277

1211

1084

81

1311

1244

1115

82

1346

1278

1146

83

1381

1311

1177

84

1416

1346

1209

85

1452

1380

1241

86

1488

1415

1274

87

1525

1451

1307

88

1563

1487

1340

89

1600

1523

1374

90

1639

1560

1409

91

1677

1598

1444

92

1717

1636

1479

93

1756

1674

1515

94

1796

1713

1551

95

1837

1752

1588

96

1878

1792

1625

97

1919

1832

1662

98

1961

1872

1700 1739

99

2004

1913

100

2047

1955

1778

200

8702

8444

7944

300

20101

19628

18710

400

36294

35565

34154

500

57308

56290

54318

1000

235222

232344

226772

11/14/13 8:51 AM

Appendix I

Table of significance for the Mann–Whitney U-test

5% significant values level for the Mann–Whitney Table I.1 U-statistic (one-tailed test) See Table I.1 opposite: Your value must be in the listed ranges for your sample sizes to be significant at the 5% level; i.e. to accept the hypothesis. In addition, you should have predicted which group would have the smaller sum of ranks.



1% significant values level for the Mann–Whitney U-statistic (two-tailed test)

Table I.2

See Table I.2 on page 678: Your value must be in the listed ranges for your sample sizes to be significant at the 1% level; i.e. to accept the hypothesis at the 1% level.

Z10_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP10.indd 676

11/14/13 8:51 AM

Z10_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP10.indd 677

0–18

67–90

0–25

88–120

57–75

20

75–100

59–78

50–65

15

0–15

13

0–16

55–72

47–60

61–82

0–13

12

54–70

50–66

43–55

14

0–12

36–45

11

0–9

9

0–11

38–48

32–40

46–60

0–8

8

39–50

42–54

34–42

29–35

10

0–12

0–6

7

0–7

101–140

0–32

77–105

0–23

72–98

0–21

67–91

0–19

63–84

0–17

58–77

0–16

53–70

0–14

43–56

0–10

38–49

0–8

34–42

0–5

29–36

6

25–30

0–5

29–35

0–4

25–30

5

5

113–160

0–39

87–120

0–28

81–112

0–26

76–104

0–24

70–96

0–21

65–88

0–19

60–80

0–17

48–63

0–15

49–64

0–13

43–56

0–11

38–48

0–8

32–40

0–6

6

126–180

0–47

96–135

0–33

90–126

0–31

84–117

0–28

78–108

0–26

72–99

0–23

66–90

0–20

54–72

0–18

54–72

0–15

48–63

0–13

42–54

0–10

36–45

0–8

7

138–200

0–54

106–150

0–39

99–140

0–36

93–130

0–33

86–120

0–30

79–110

0–27

73–100

0–24

60–81

0–21

60–80

0–18

53–70

0–15

46–60

0–12

39–50

0–9

8

151–220

0–62

115–165

0–44

108–154

0–41

101–143

0–37

94–132

0–34

87–121

0–31

79–110

0–27

66–90

0–24

65–88

0–20

58–77

0–17

50–66

0–14

43–55

0–11

9

163–240

0–69

125–180

0–50

109–168

0–46

109–156

0–42

102–144

0–38

94–132

0–34

86–120

0–31

72–99

0–27

70–96

0–23

63–84

0–19

55–72

0–16

47–60

0–12

10

Sample size for larger group

5% significant values level for the Mann–Whitney U-statistic (one-tailed test)

21–25

Sample size for smaller group

Table I.1

200–260

0–77

153–195

0–55

126–182

0–51

118–169

0–47

109–156

0–42

101–143

0–38

93–130

0–34

78–108

0–30

76–104

0–26

67–91

0–21

59–78

0–17

50–65

0–13

11

188–280

0–84

144–210

0–61

135–196

0–56

126–182

0–51

117–168

0–47

108–154

0–42

99–140

0–37

84–117

0–33

81–112

0–28

72–98

0–24

61–82

0–19

54–70

0–15

12

200–300

0–92

153–225

0–66

144–210

0–61

134–195

0–56

125–180

0–51

115–165

0–46

106–150

4–41

90–126

0–36

87–120

0–31

77–105

0–26

67–90

0–21

57–75

0–16

13

262–400

0–100

200–300

0–72

188–280

0–66

176–260

0–61

163–240

0–55

151–220

0–50

138–200

0–44

96–135

0–39

113–160

0–33

101–140

0–28

88–120

0–23

75–100

0–18

14

Source: The above table has been adapted from Table I of Fundamentals of Behavioral Statistics, The McGraw Hill Companies Inc. (Runyon, R.P. and Haber, A., 1989), with permission.



0–138

0–100

0–92

0–84

0–77

0–69

0–62

126–180

0–54

0–47

0–39

0–32

0–25

20

APPENDIX I  Table of significance for the Mann–Whitney U-test 677

11/14/13 8:51 AM

Z10_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP10.indd 678

102–120

0–13

20

87–100

78–90

67–75

68–78

58–65

0–8

0–7

13

15

63–72

54–60

0–7

0–6

12

71–82

59–66

50–55

63–70

0–5

11

14

0–4

49–54

42–45

54–60

0–3

9

46–50

44–48

38–40

10

0–2

8

116–140

0–18

89–105

0–12

83–98

0–11

78–91

0–10

72–84

0–9

67–77

0–7

61–70

0–6

56–63

0–5

50–56

0–4

45–49

0–3

0–1

39–42

0–2

39–42

0–1

34–36

6

29–30

7

0–1

34–35

0

29–30

5

25

34–35

6

130–160

0–24

100–120

0–16

94–112

0–15

87–104

0–13

81–96

0–12

75–88

0–10

69–80

0–9

63–72

0–7

57–64

0–6

50–56

0–4

44–48

0–3

38–40

0–1

7

144–180

0–30

111–135

0–20

104–126

0–18

97–117

0–17

90–108

0–15

83–99

0–13

77–90

0–11

70–81

0–9

63–72

0–7

56–63

0–6

49–54

0–4

42–45

0–2

8

158–200

0–36

121–150

0–24

114–140

0–22

106–130

0–20

99–120

0–18

92–110

0–16

84–100

0–13

77–90

0–11

69–80

0–9

61–70

0–7

54–60

0–5

46–50

0–3

9

172–220

0–42

132–165

0–29

124–154

0–26

116–143

0–24

108–132

0–21

90–111

0–18

92–110

0–16

81–99

0–13

75–88

0–11

67–77

0–9

59–66

0–6

50–55

0–4

10

186–240

0–48

143–180

0–33

134–168

0–30

125–156

0–27

117–144

0–24

108–132

0–21

99–120

0–18

90–108

0–16

81–96

0–13

72–84

0–10

63–72

0–7

54–60

0–5

11

Sample size for larger group

1% significant values level for the Mann–Whitney U-statistic (two-tailed test)

5

Sample size for smaller group

Table I.2

200–260

0–54

153–195

0–37

144–182

0–34

135–169

0–31

115–156

0–27

106–143

0–24

97–130

0–21

89–117

0–18

87–104

0–15

78–91

0–12

68–78

0–9

58–65

0–6

12

213–280

0–60

164–210

0–42

154–196

0–38

144–182

0–34

134–168

0–31

124–154

0–27

114–140

0–24

104–126

0–20

94–112

0–17

83–98

0–13

71–82

0–10

67–70

0–7

13

227–300

0–67

174–225

0–46

164–210

0–42

153–195

0–38

143–180

0–34

132–165

0–30

111–150

0–26

101–135

0–22

100–120

0–18

89–105

0–15

78–90

0–11

67–75

0–7

14

295–400

0–73

227–300

0–51

213–280

0–46

200–260

0–42

186–240

0–37

172–220

0–33

158–200

0–29

144–180

0–24

130–160

0–20

116–140

0–16

102–120

0–12

87–100

0–8

15

Source: The above table has been adapted from Table I of Fundamentals of Behavioral Statistics, The McGraw Hill Companies Inc. (Runyon, R.P. and Haber, A., 1989), with permission.



0–105

0–73

0–67

0–60

0–54

0–48

0–42

0–36

0–30

0–24

0–18

0–13

20

678 APPENDIX I  Table of significance for the Mann–Whitney U-test

11/14/13 8:51 AM

Appendix J

Table of significance values for the F-distribution J.1

5% significance levels for the F-distribution (one-tailed test) Your value has to equal or be larger than the tabled value to be significant at the 5% level for an effect to be significant.

Degrees of freedom for error or within-cells mean square (or variance estimate)

Degrees of freedom for between-treatments mean square (or variance estimate) 1

2

3

4

5



1

161.448

199.500

215.707

224.583

230.162

254.314

2

18.513

19.000

19.165

19.247

19.297

19.496

3

10.128

9.553

9.277

9.118

9.014

8.527

4

7.709

6.945

6.592

6.389

6.257

5.628

5

6.608

5.787

5.410

5.193

5.051

4.365 3.669

6

5.988

5.144

4.758

4.534

4.388

7

5.592

4.738

4.347

4.121

3.972

3.230

8

5.318

4.459

4.067

3.838

3.688

2.928

9

5.118

4.257

3.863

3.634

3.482

2.707

10

4.965

4.103

3.709

3.479

3.326

2.538

13

4.668

3.806

3.411

3.180

3.026

2.207

15

4.544

3.683

3.288

3.056

2.902

2.066

20

4.352

3.493

3.099

2.867

2.711

1.844

30

4.171

3.316

2.923

2.690

2.534

1.623

60

4.002

3.151

2.759

2.526

2.369

1.390



3.842

2.996

2.605

2.372

2.215

1.000

Z11_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP11.indd 679

11/14/13 8:51 AM

APPENDIX J  Table of significance values for the F-distribution

680

J.2

1% significant values of the F-distribution (one-tailed test) Your value has to equal or be larger than the tabled value to be significant at the 1% level for an effect to be significant.

Degrees of freedom for error or within-cells mean square (or variance estimate)

Degrees of freedom for between-treatments mean square (or variance estimate) 1

2

3

4

5



1

4052.180

4999.500

5403.350

5624.580

5763.650

6365.860

2

98.503

99.000

99.167

99.250

99.300

99.500

3

34.117

30.817

29.457

28.710

28.238

26.126

4

21.198

18.000

16.695

15.977

15.522

13.464

5

16.259

13.274

12.060

11.392

10.967

9.021

6

13.745

10.925

9.780

9.149

8.746

6.880

7

12.247

9.547

8.452

7.847

7.461

5.650

8

11.259

8.650

7.591

7.007

6.632

4.859

9

10.562

8.022

6.992

6.423

6.057

4.311

10

10.045

7.560

6.553

5.995

5.637

3.909

13

9.074

6.701

5.740

5.206

4.862

3.166

15

8.684

6.359

5.417

4.894

4.556

2.869 2.422

20

8.096

5.849

4.939

4.431

4.103

30

7.563

5.391

4.510

4.018

3.699

2.007

60

7.078

4.978

4.126

3.650

3.339

1.607



6.635

4.606

3.782

3.320

3.018

1.000

Z11_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP11.indd 680

11/14/13 8:51 AM

APPENDIX J  Table of significance values for the F-distribution 681







J.3

10% significance levels for the F-distribution for testing differences between two groups (one-tailed test) This table is only to be used for determining whether an F-value which is not significant at the 5% level for two groups is significant at the 10% level which is the equivalent to a one-tailed t-test. You should only do this if you have good grounds for predicting the direction of the difference between the two means. Your value has to equal or be larger than the tabled value to be significant at the one-tailed 5% level for the t-test.

Degrees of freedom for error or within-cells mean square (or variance estimate)

Degrees of freedom for between-treatments mean square (or variance estimate)

1

39.864

2

8.527

1

Z11_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP11.indd 681

3

5.539

4

4.545

5

4.061

6

3.776

7

3.590

8

3.458

9

3.361

10

3.285

13

3.137

15

3.074

20

2.975

30

2.881

60

2.792



2.706

11/14/13 8:51 AM

Appendix K

Table of significant values of t when making multiple t-tests The following table gives the 5% significance values for two-tailed t-tests when you are making up to ten unplanned comparisons. The number of comparisons you decide to make is up to you and does not have to be the maximum possible. This table can be used in any circumstances where you have multiple t-tests.

Degrees of freedom

Number of comparisons being made 1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

1

12.706

25.452

38.188

50.923

63.657

76.390

89.124

101.856

114.589

127.321

2

4.303

6.205

7.649

8.860

9.925

10.886

11.769

12.590

13.360

14.089

3

3.182

4.177

4.857

5.392

5.841

6.231

6.580

6.895

7.185

7.453

4

2.776

3.495

3.961

4.315

4.604

4.851

5.067

5.261

5.437

5.598

5

2.571

3.163

3.534

3.810

4.032

4.219

4.382

4.526

4.655

4.773

6

2.447

2.969

3.288

3.521

3.708

3.863

3.997

4.115

4.221

4.317

7

2.365

2.841

3.128

3.335

3.500

3.636

3.753

3.855

3.947

4.029

8

2.306

2.752

3.016

3.206

3.355

3.479

3.584

3.677

3.759

3.833 3.690

9

2.262

2.685

2.933

3.111

3.250

3.364

3.462

3.547

3.622

10

2.228

2.634

2.870

3.038

3.169

3.277

3.368

3.448

3.518

3.581

11

2.201

2.593

2.820

2.981

3.106

3.208

3.295

3.370

3.437

3.497 3.428

12

2.179

2.560

2.780

2.934

3.055

3.153

3.236

3.308

3.371

13

2.160

2.533

2.746

2.896

3.012

3.107

3.187

3.257

3.318

3.373

14

2.145

2.510

2.718

2.864

2.977

3.069

3.146

3.213

3.273

3.326

15

2.132

2.490

2.694

2.837

2.947

3.036

3.112

3.177

3.235

3.286

16

2.120

2.473

2.673

2.813

2.921

3.008

3.082

3.146

3.202

3.252

17

2.110

2.458

2.655

2.793

2.898

2.984

3.056

3.119

3.174

3.222

Z12_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP12.indd 682

11/14/13 8:51 AM

APPENDIX K  Table of significant values of t when making multiple t-tests 683



Degrees of freedom

18

Number of comparisons being made 1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

2.101

2.445

2.639

2.774

2.878

2.963

3.034

3.095

9

10

3.149

3.197

19

2.093

2.433

2.625

2.759

2.861

2.944

3.014

3.074

3.127

3.174

20

2.086

2.423

2.613

2.744

2.845

2.927

2.996

3.055

3.107

3.153 3.135

21

2.080

2.414

2.601

2.732

2.831

2.912

2.980

3.038

3.090

22

2.074

2.406

2.591

2.720

2.819

2.898

2.966

3.023

3.074

3.119

23

2.069

2.398

2.582

2.710

2.807

2.886

2.953

3.010

3.059

3.104

24

2.064

2.391

2.574

2.700

2.797

2.875

2.941

2.997

3.047

3.091

25

2.064

2.385

2.566

2.692

2.787

2.865

2.930

2.986

3.035

3.078

26

2.055

2.379

2.559

2.684

2.779

2.856

2.920

2.975

3.024

3.067

27

2.052

2.373

2.553

2.676

2.771

2.847

2.911

2.966

3.014

3.057

28

2.048

2.369

2.547

2.670

2.763

2.839

2.902

2.957

3.005

3.047

29

2.045

2.364

2.541

2.663

2.756

2.832

2.894

2.949

2.996

3.038

30

2.042

2.360

2.536

2.657

2.750

2.825

2.887

2.941

2.988

3.030

31

2.039

2.356

2.531

2.652

2.744

2.818

2.880

2.934

2.981

3.022

32

2.037

2.352

2.526

2.647

2.739

2.812

2.874

2.927

2.974

3.015

33

2.035

2.348

2.522

2.642

2.733

2.807

2.868

2.921

2.967

3.008

34

2.032

2.345

2.518

2.638

2.728

2.801

2.863

2.915

2.961

3.002

35

2.030

2.342

2.515

2.633

2.724

2.797

2.857

2.910

2.955

2.996

36

2.028

2.339

2.511

2.630

2.720

2.792

2.853

2.905

2.950

2.990

37

2.026

2.336

2.508

2.626

2.715

2.788

2.848

2.900

2.945

2.985

38

2.024

2.334

2.505

2.622

2.712

2.784

2.844

2.895

2.940

2.980

39

2.023

2.331

2.502

2.619

2.708

2.780

2.839

2.891

2.936

2.976

40

2.021

2.329

2.499

2.616

2.704

2.776

2.836

2.887

2.931

2.971

41

2.020

2.327

2.496

2.613

2.701

2.772

2.832

2.883

2.927

2.967

42

2.018

2.325

2.494

2.610

2.698

2.769

2.828

2.879

2.923

2.963

43

2.017

2.323

2.491

2.607

2.695

2.766

2.825

2.875

2.920

2.959

44

2.017

2.321

2.489

2.605

2.692

2.763

2.822

2.872

2.916

2.955

45

2.014

2.319

2.487

2.602

2.690

2.760

2.819

2.869

2.913

2.952

46

2.013

2.317

2.485

2.600

2.687

2.757

2.816

2.866

2.910

2.949

47

2.012

2.316

2.483

2.598

2.685

2.755

2.813

2.863

2.907

2.946

48

2.011

2.314

2.481

2.595

2.682

2.752

2.810

2.860

2.904

2.943

49

2.010

2.312

2.479

2.593

2.680

2.750

2.808

2.857

2.901

2.940

50

2.009

2.311

2.477

2.591

2.678

2.747

2.805

2.855

2.898

2.937

51

2.008

2.309

2.476

2.589

2.676

2.745

2.803

2.853

2.896

2.934

52

2.007

2.308

2.474

2.588

2.674

2.743

2.801

2.850

2.893

2.932

53

2.006

2.307

2.472

2.586

2.672

2.741

2.798

2.848

2.891

2.929

54

2.005

2.306

2.471

2.584

2.670

2.739

2.797

2.846

2.889

2.927

55

2.004

2.304

2.469

2.583

2.668

2.737

2.795

2.844

2.887

2.925

56

2.003

2.303

2.468

2.581

2.667

2.735

2.793

2.842

2.885

2.922

57

2.002

2.302

2.467

2.579

2.665

2.734

2.791

2.840

2.882

2.920

58

2.002

2.301

2.465

2.578

2.663

2.732

2.789

2.838

2.881

2.918

59

2.001

2.300

2.464

2.577

2.662

2.730

2.787

2.836

2.879

2.916



Z12_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP12.indd 683

11/14/13 8:51 AM

APPENDIX K  Table of significant values of t when making multiple t-tests

684

Degrees of freedom

Number of comparisons being made 1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

60

2.000

2.299

2.463

2.575

2.660

2.729

2.786

2.834

61

2.000

2.298

2.462

2.574

2.659

2.727

2.784

62

1.999

2.297

2.461

2.573

2.658

2.726

2.782

9

10

2.877

2.915

2.833

2.875

2.913

2.831

2.873

2.911

63

1.998

2.296

2.460

2.571

2.656

2.724

2.781

2.829

2.872

2.909

64

1.998

2.295

2.459

2.570

2.655

2.723

2.779

2.828

2.870

2.908

65

1.997

2.295

2.458

2.569

2.654

2.721

2.778

2.826

2.869

2.906

66

1.997

2.294

2.457

2.568

2.652

2.720

2.777

2.825

2.867

2.905

67

1.996

2.293

2.456

2.567

2.651

2.719

2.775

2.824

2.866

2.903

68

1.995

2.292

2.455

2.566

2.650

2.718

2.774

2.822

2.864

2.902

69

1.995

2.291

2.454

2.565

2.649

2.716

2.773

2.821

2.863

2.900

70

1.994

2.291

2.453

2.564

2.648

2.715

2.772

2.820

2.862

2.899

71

1.994

2.290

2.452

2.563

2.647

2.714

2.770

2.818

2.860

2.898

72

1.994

2.289

2.451

2.562

2.646

2.713

2.769

2.817

2.859

2.896

73

1.993

2.289

2.450

2.561

2.645

2.712

2.768

2.816

2.858

2.895

74

1.993

2.288

2.450

2.560

2.644

2.711

2.767

2.815

2.857

2.894

75

1.992

2.287

2.449

2.559

2.643

2.710

2.766

2.814

2.856

2.893

76

1.992

2.287

2.448

2.559

2.642

2.709

2.765

2.813

2.854

2.891

77

1.991

2.286

2.447

2.558

2.641

2.708

2.764

2.812

2.853

2.890

78

1.991

2.285

2.447

2.557

2.640

2.707

2.763

2.811

2.852

2.889 2.888

79

1.990

2.285

2.446

2.556

2.640

2.706

2.762

2.810

2.851

80

1.990

2.284

2.445

2.555

2.639

2.705

2.761

2.809

2.850

2.887

81

1.990

2.284

2.445

2.555

2.638

2.705

2.760

2.808

2.849

2.886

82

1.989

2.283

2.444

2.554

2.637

2.704

2.759

2.807

2.848

2.885

83

1.989

2.283

2.444

2.553

2.636

2.703

2.759

2.806

2.847

2.884

84

1.989

2.282

2.443

2.553

2.636

2.702

2.758

2.805

2.846

2.883

85

1.988

2.282

2.442

2.552

2.635

2.701

2.757

2.804

2.845

2.882

86

1.988

2.281

2.442

2.551

2.634

2.701

2.756

2.803

2.845

2.881

87

1.988

2.281

2.441

2.551

2.634

2.700

2.755

2.803

2.844

2.880

88

1.987

2.280

2.441

2.550

2.633

2.699

2.755

2.802

2.843

2.880

89

1.987

2.280

2.440

2.550

2.632

2.699

2.754

2.801

2.842

2.879 2.878

90

1.987

2.280

2.440

2.549

2.632

2.698

2.753

2.800

2.841

91

1.986

2.279

2.439

2.548

2.631

2.697

2.752

2.800

2.841

2.877

92

1.986

2.279

2.439

2.548

2.630

2.696

2.752

2.799

2.840

2.876

93

1.986

2.278

2.438

2.547

2.630

2.696

2.751

2.798

2.839

2.876

94

1.986

2.278

2.438

2.547

2.629

2.695

2.750

2.797

2.838

2.875 2.874

95

1.985

2.277

2.437

2.546

2.629

2.695

2.750

2.797

2.838

96

1.985

2.277

2.437

2.546

2.628

2.694

2.749

2.796

2.837

2.873

97

1.985

2.277

2.436

2.545

2.627

2.694

2.748

2.795

2.836

2.873 2.872

98

1.984

2.276

2.436

2.545

2.627

2.693

2.748

2.795

2.836

99

1.984

2.276

2.435

2.544

2.626

2.692

2.747

2.794

2.835

2.871

100

1.984

2.276

2.435

2.544

2.626

2.692

2.747

2.793

2.834

2.871



1.960

2.241

2.394

2.498

2.576

2.638

2.690

2.734

2.773

2.807

Z12_HOWI0749_06_SE_APP12.indd 684

11/14/13 8:51 AM

GLOSSARY

−2 log likelihood (ratio) test: Used in logistic regression, it is a form of chi-square test which compares the goodness of fit of two models where one model is a part of (i.e. nested or a subset of) the other model. The chi-square is the difference in the −2 log likelihood values for the two models. A priori test: A test of the difference between two groups of scores when this comparison has been planned ignorant of the actual data. This contrasts with a post hoc test which is carried out after the data have been collected and which has no particularly strong expectations about the outcome. Adjusted mean: A mean score when the influence of one or more covariates have been removed especially in analysis of covariance. Alpha level: The level of risk that the researcher is prepared to mistakenly accept the hypothesis on the basis of the available data. Typically this is set at a maximum of 5% or .05 and is, of course, otherwise referred to as the level of significance. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA): A variant of the analysis of variance (ANOVA) in which scores on the dependent variable are adjusted to take into account (control) a covariate(s). For example, differences between conditions of an experiment at pre-test can be controlled for. Analysis of variance (ANOVA): An extensive group of tests of significance which compare means on a dependent variable. There may be one or more independent (grouping) variables or factors. ANOVA is essential in the analysis of most laboratory experiments. Association: A relationship between two variables.

Between-groups design: Basically a design where different participants are allocated to different groups or conditions. Between-subjects design: see Between-groups design. Bimodal: A frequency distribution with two modes. Bivariate: Involving two variables as opposed to univariate which involves just one variable. Bivariate correlation: A correlation between two variables. Block: A subset of variables which will be analysed together in a sequence of blocks. Bonferroni adjustment: A method of adjusting significance levels for the fact that many statistical analyses have been carried out on the data. Bootstrapping: A method of creating sampling distributions from the basic sample which is reproduced numerous times to approximate the ‘population’. This allows repeated sampling and hence the calculation of sampling distributions for all sorts of statistics. Boxplot: A diagram indicating the distribution of scores on a variable. It gives the median in a box, the left and right hand sides of which are the lower and upper values of the interquartile range. Lines at each side of the box identify the largest and smallest scores. Box’s M: A statistical test which partly establishes whether the data meet the requirements for a MANOVA analysis. It examines the extent to which the covariances of the dependent variables are similar for each of the groups in the analysis. Ideally, then, Box’s M should not be significant. The test is used in MANOVA though its interpretation is complex.

Bar chart: A picture in which frequencies are represented by the height of a set of bars. It should be the areas of a set of bars but SPSS Statistics ignores this and settles for height. Bartlett’s test of sphericity: A test used in MANOVA of whether the correlations between the variables differ significantly from zero. Beta level: The risk that we are prepared to accept rejecting the null hypothesis when it is in fact true. Beta weight: The standardised regression weight in multiple regression. It corresponds to the correlation coefficient in simple regression.

Case: The basic unit of analysis on which data are collected such as individuals or organisations. Categorical variable: A nominal or category variable. Category variable: A variable which consists of categories rather than numerical scores. The categories have no particular quantitative order. However, usually on SPSS Statistics they will be coded as numbers. Cell: The intersection of one category of a variable with another category of one or more other variables. So if a variable has categories A, B and C and the other variable has categories X, Y and Z, then the cells are A with X, A

Z13_HOWI0749_06_SE_GLOS.indd 685

11/14/13 8:51 AM

686

GLOSSARY

with Y, A with Z, B with X, B with Y, etc. It is a term frequently used in ANOVA as well as with chi-square tables (i.e. crosstabulation and contingency tables). Chart: A graphical or pictorial representation of the characteristics of one’s data. Chart Editor window: In SPSS Statistics it is a Window which can be opened up to refine a chart. Chi-square distribution: A set of theoretical probability distributions which vary according to the degrees of freedom and which are used to determine the statistical significance of a chi-square test. Chi-square test, Pearson’s: A test of goodness-of-fit or association for frequency data. It compares the observed data with the estimated (or actual) population distribution (this is usually based on combining two or more samples). Cluster analysis: A variety of techniques which identify the patterns of variables or cases which tend to be similar to each other. No cluster analysis techniques are dealt with in this book as they are uncommon in psychology. Often factor analysis, which is in this book, does a similar job. Cochran’s Q test: A test of whether the frequencies of a dichotomous variable differ significantly for more than two related samples or groups. Coefficient of determination: The square of Pearson’s correlation coefficient. So a correlation of 0.4 has a coefficient of determination of 0.16. It is useful especially since it gives a numerically more accurate representation of the relative importance of different correlation coefficients than the correlation coefficients themselves do. Common variance: The variance that two or more variables share. Communality: The variance that a particular variable in an analysis shares with other variables. It is distinct from error variance and specific variance (which is confined to a particular variable). It mainly appears in factor analysis. Component matrix: A table showing the correlations between components and variables in factor analysis. Compute: In SPSS Statistics, this procedure allows the researcher to derive new variables from the original variables. For example, it would be possible to sum the scores for each participant on several variables. Condition: One of the groups in ANOVA or the t-test. Confidence interval: A more realistic way of presenting the outcomes of statistical analysis than, for example, the mean or the standard deviation would be. It gives the range within which 95% or 99% of the most common means, standard deviations, etc. would lie. Thus instead of saying that the mean is 6.7 we would say that the 95% confidence interval for the mean is 5.2 to 8.2. Confirmatory factor analysis: A test of whether a particular model or factor structure fits a set of data satisfactorily. Confounding variable: Any variable which clouds the interpretation of a correlation or any other statistical rela­ tionship. Once the effects of the confounding variable are removed, the remaining relationship presents a truer picture of what is going on in reality.

Z13_HOWI0749_06_SE_GLOS.indd 686

Contingency table: A frequency table giving the frequencies in all of the categories of two or more nominal (category) variables tabulated together. Correlation coefficient: An index which gives the extent and the direction of the linear association between two variables. Correlation matrix: A matrix of the correlations of pairs of variables. Count: The number of times (frequency) a particular observation (score or category, for example) occurs. Counterbalancing: If some participants take part in condition A of a study first, followed by condition B later, then to counterbalance any time or sequence effects other participants should take part in condition B first followed by condition A second. Covariance: The variance which two or more score variables have in common (i.e. share). It is basically calculated like variance but instead of squaring each score’s deviation from the mean the deviation of variable X from its mean is multiplied by the deviation of variable Y from its mean. Covariate: A variable which correlates with the variables that are the researcher’s main focus of interest. In the analysis of covariance it is the undesired influence of the covariate which is controlled for. Cox and Snell’s R2: The amount of variance in the criterion variable accounted for by the predictor variables. It is used in logistic regression. Cramer’s V: Also known as Cramer’s phi, this correlation coefficient is usually applied to a contingency or crosstabulation table greater than 2 rows × 2 columns. Critical value: Used when calculating statistical significance with statistical tables such as those in the back of this book. It is the minimum value of the statistical calculation which is statistically significant (i.e. which rejects the null hypothesis). Cronbach’s alpha: A measure of the extent to which cases respond in a similar or consistent way on all the variables that go to make up a scale. Data Editor window: The data spreadsheet in which data items are entered in SPSS Statistics. Data handling: The various techniques to deal with data from a study excluding its statistical analysis. It would include data entry into the spreadsheet, the search for errors in data entry, recoding variables into new values, computing new variables and so forth. Data View: The window in SPSS Statistics which allows you to see the data spreadsheet. Degrees of freedom: The number of components of the data that can vary while still yielding a given population value for characteristics such as mean scores. All other things being equal, the bigger the degrees of freedom the more likely it is that the research findings will be statistically significant. Dependent variable: A variable which potentially may be affected or predicted by other variables in the analysis. It is sometimes known as the criterion or outcome variable.

11/14/13 8:51 AM



Descriptive statistics: Indices which describe the major characteristics of variables or the relationships between variables. It includes measures of central tendency (mean, median and mode for example) and measures of spread (range, variance, etc.). Deviation: Usually the difference between a score and the mean of the set of scores. Dialog or Dialogue box: A rectangular picture in SPSS Statistics which allows the user to select various procedures. Dichotomous: A nominal (category) variable with just two categories. Gender (male/female) is an obvious example. Direct Oblimin: A rotation procedure for making factors in a factor analysis more meaningful or interpretable. Its essential characteristic is that the factors are not required to be uncorrelated (independent) of each other. Discriminant (function) analysis: A statistical technique for score variables which maximises the difference(s) between two or more groups of participants on a set of variables. It gen­ erates a set of ‘weights’ which are applied to these variables. Discriminant function: Found mainly in discriminant (function) analysis. A derived variable based on combining a set of variables in such a way that groups are as different as possible on the discriminant function. More than one discriminant function may emerge but each discriminant function is uncorrelated with the others. Discriminant score: An individual’s score on a discriminant function. Dummy coding: Used when analysing nominal (category) data to allow such variables to be used analogously to scores. Each category of the nominal (category) variable is made into a separate dummy variable. If the nominal (category) variable has three categories A, B and C then two new variables, say A versus not A and B versus not B are created. The categories may be coded with the value 1 and 0. It would not be used where a variable has only two different categories. Dummy variable: A variable created by dummy coding. Effect size: A measure of the strength of the relationship between two variables. Most commonly used in metaanalysis. The Pearson correlation coefficient is a very familiar measure of effect size. Also commonly used is Cohen’s d. The correlation coefficient is recommended as the most user-friendly measure of effect size as it is very familiar to most of us and easily understood. Eigenvalue: The variance accounted for by a factor. It is simply the sum of the squared factor loadings. The concept is also used for discriminant functions. Endogenous variable: Any variable in path analysis that can be explained on the basis of one or more variables in that analysis. Eta: A measure of association for non-linear (curved) relationships. Exact significance: The precise significance level at and beyond which a result is statistically significant. Exogenous variable: A variable in path analysis which is not accounted for by any other variable in that analysis.

Z13_HOWI0749_06_SE_GLOS.indd 687

GLOSSARY 687

Exploratory factor analysis: The common form of factor analysis which finds the major dimensions of a correlation matrix using weighted combinations of the variables in the study. It identifies combinations of variables which can be described as one or more superordinate variable or factor. Exponent or power: A number with an exponent or power superscript is multiplied by itself by that number of times. Thus 32 means 3 × 3 whereas 43 means 4 × 4 × 4. Extraction: The process of obtaining factors in factor analysis. F-ratio: The ratio of two variances. It can be used to test whether these two variances differ significantly using the F-distribution. It can be used on its own but is also part of the t-test and ANOVA. Factor matrix: A table showing the correlations between factors and the variables. Factor scores: Standardised scores for a factor. They provide a way of calculating an individual’s score on a factor which precisely reflects that factor. Factor, factor analysis: A variable derived by combining other variables in a weighted combination. A factor seeks to synthesise the variance shared by variables into a more general variable to which the variables relate. Factor, in analysis of variance: An independent or subject variable but is best regarded as a variable on which groups of participants are formed. The variances of these groups are then compared using ANOVA. A factor should consist of a nominal (category) variable with a small number of categories. Factorial ANOVA: An analysis of variance with two or more independent or subject variables. Family error rate: The probability or significance level for a finding when a family or number of tests or comparisons are being made on the same data. Fisher test: Tests of significance (or association) for 2 × 2 and 2 × 3 contingency tables. Frequency: The number of times a particular category occurs. Frequency distribution: A table or diagram giving the fre­ quencies of values of a variable. Friedman’s test: A nonparametric test for determining whether the mean ranks of three or more related samples or groups differ significantly. Goodness-of-fit index: A measure of the extent to which a particular model (or pattern of variables) designed to describe a set of data actually matches the data. Graph: A diagram for illustrating the values of one or more variables. Grouping variable: A variable which forms the groups or conditions which are to be compared. Harmonic mean: The number of scores, divided by the sum of the reciprocal (1/x) of each score. Help: A facility in software with a graphical interface such as SPSS Statistics which provides information about its features. Hierarchical agglomerative clustering: A form of cluster analysis, at each step of which a variable or cluster is

11/14/13 8:51 AM

688

GLOSSARY

paired with the most similar variable or cluster until one cluster remains. Hierarchical or sequential entry: A variant of regression in which the order in which the independent (predictor) variables are entered into the analysis is decided by the analyst rather than mathematical criteria. Hierarchical regression: see Hierarchical or sequential entry. Histogram: A chart which represents the frequency of particular scores or ranges of scores in terms of a set of bars. The height of the bar represents the frequency of this score or range of scores in the data. Homogeneity of regression slope: The similarity of the regression slope of the covariate on the criterion variable in the different groups of the predictor variable. Homogeneity of variance: The similarity of the variance of the scores in the groups of the predictor variable. Homoscedasticity: The similarity of the scatter or spread of the data points around the regression line of best fit in different parts of that line. Hypothesis: A statement expressing the expected or predicted relationship between two or more variables. Icicle plot: A graphical representation of the results of a cluster analysis in which xs are used to indicate which variables or clusters are paired at which stage. Identification: The extent to which the parameters of a structural equation model can be estimated from the original data. Independence: Events or variables being unrelated to each other. Independent groups design: A design in which different cases are assigned to different conditions or groups. Independent t-test: A parametric test for determining whether the means of two unrelated or independent groups differ significantly. Independent variable: A variable which may affect (predict) the values of another variable(s). It is used to form the groups in experimental designs. But it is also used in regression for the variables used to predict the dependent variable. Inferential statistics: Statistical techniques which help predict the population characteristics from the sample characteristics. Interaction: This describes outcomes in research which cannot be accounted for on the basis of the separate influences of two or more variables. So, for example, an interaction occurs when two variables have a significant influence when combined. Interaction graph: A graph showing the relationship of the means of two or more variables. Interquartile range: The range of the middle 50% of a distribution. By ignoring the extreme quarter in each direction from the mean, the interquartile range is less affected by extreme scores. Interval data: Data making up a scale in which the distance or interval between adjacent points is assumed to be the same or equal but where there is no meaningful zero point.

Z13_HOWI0749_06_SE_GLOS.indd 688

Just-identified model: A structural equation model in which the data are just sufficient to estimate its parameters. Kaiser or Kaiser–Guttman criterion: A statistical criterion in factor analysis for determining the number of factors or components for consideration and possible rotation in which factors or components with eigenvalues of one or less are ignored. Kendall’s tau (τ): An index of the linear association between two ordinal variables. A correlation coefficient for nonparametric data in other words. Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for two samples: A nonparametric test for determining whether the distributions of scores on an ordinal variable differ significantly for two unrelated samples. Kruskal–Wallis test: A nonparametric test for determining whether the mean ranked scores for three or more unrelated samples differ significantly. Kurtosis: The extent to which the shape of a bell-shaped curve is flatter or more elongated than a normal distribution. Latent variable: An unobserved variable that is measured by one or more manifest variables or indicators. Level: Used in analysis of variance to describe the different conditions of an independent variable (or factor). The term has its origins in agricultural research where levels of treatment would correspond to, say, different amounts of fertiliser being applied to crops. Levels of measurement: A four-fold hierarchical distinction proposed for measures comprising nominal, ordinal, equal interval and ratio. Levene’s test: An analysis of variance on absolute differences to determine whether the variances of two or more unrelated groups differ significantly. Likelihood ratio chi-square test: A form of chi-square which involves natural logarithms. It is primarily associated with log-linear analysis. Line graph: A diagram in which lines are used to indicate the frequency of a variable. Linear association or relationship: This occurs when there is a straight line relationship between two sets of scores. The scattergram for these data will be represented best by a straight line rather than a curved line. Linear model: A model which assumes a linear relationship between the variables. LISREL: The name of a particular software designed to carry out linear structural relationship analysis also known as structural equation modelling. Loading: An index of the size and direction of the association of a variable with a factor or discriminant function of which it is part. A loading is simply the correlation between a variable and the factor or discriminant function. Log likelihood: An index based on the difference between the frequencies for a category variable(s) and what is predicted on the basis of the predictors (i.e. the modelled data). The bigger the log likelihood the poorer the fit of the model to the data.

11/14/13 8:51 AM



Logarithm: The amount to which a given base number (e.g. 10) has to be multiplied by itself to obtain a particular number. So in the expression 32, 2 would be the logarithm for the base 3 which makes 9. Sometimes it is recommended that scores are converted to their logarithms if this results in the data fitting the requirements of the stat­ istical procedure better. Logistic or logit regression: A version of multiple regression in which the dependent, criterion or outcome variable takes the form of a nominal (category) variable. Any mixture of scores and nominal (category) variables can act as predictors. The procedure uses dummy variables extensively. Log-linear analysis: A statistical technique for nominal (category) data which is essentially an extension of chi-square where there are three or more independent variables. Main effect: The effect of an independent or predictor variable on a dependent or criterion variable. Manifest variable: A variable which directly reflects the measure used to assess it. Mann–Whitney test: A nonparametric test for seeing whether the number of times scores from one sample are ranked significantly higher than scores from another unrelated sample. Marginal totals: The marginal totals are the row and column total frequencies in crosstabulation and contingency tables. Matched-subjects design: A related design in which participants are matched in pairs on a covariate or where participants serve as their own control. In other words, a repeated or related measures design. Matrix: A rectangular array of rows and columns of data. Mauchly’s test: A test for determining whether the assumption that the variance–covariance matrix in a repeated measures analysis of variance is spherical or circular. Maximum likelihood method: A method for finding estimates of the population parameters of a model which are most likely to give rise to the pattern of observations in the sample data. McNemar test: A test for assessing whether there has been a significant change in the frequencies of two categories on two occasions in the same or similar cases. Mean: The everyday numerical average score. Thus the mean of 2 and 3 is 2.5. Mean square: A term for variance estimate used in analysis of variance. Measure of dispersion: A measure of the variation in the scores such as the variance, range, interquartile range and standard error. Median: The score which is halfway in the scores ordered from smallest to largest. Mediating variable: One which is responsible for the relationship between two other variables. Mixed ANOVA: An ANOVA in which at least one independent variable consists of related scores and at least one other variable consists of uncorrelated scores. Mixed design: see Mixed ANOVA. Mode: The most commonly occurring score or category.

Z13_HOWI0749_06_SE_GLOS.indd 689

GLOSSARY 689

Moderating or moderator effect: A relationship between two variables which differs according to a third variable. For example, the correlation between age and income may be moderated by a variable such as gender. In other words, the correlation for men and the correlation for women between age and income is different. Multicollinearity: When two or more independent or predictor variables are highly correlated. Multimodal: A frequency distribution having three or more modes. Multiple correlation or R: A form of correlation coefficient which correlates a single score (A) with two or more other scores (B + C) in combination. Used particularly in multiple regression to denote the correlation of a set of predictor variables with the dependent (or outcome) variable. Multiple regression: A parametric test to determine what pattern of two or more predictor (independent) variables is associated with scores on the dependent variable. It takes into account the associations (correlations) between the predictor variables. If desired, interactions between predictor variables may be included. Multivariate: Involving more than two variables. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA): A variant of ana­ lysis of variance in which there are two or more dependent variables combined. MANOVA identifies differences between groups in terms of the combined dependent variable. Nagelkerke’s R2: The amount of variance in the criterion variable accounted for by the predictor variables. Natural or Napierian logarithm: The logarithms calculated using 2.718 as the base number. Nested model: A model which is a simpler subset of another model and which can be derived from that model. Nonparametric test: A statistical test of significance which requires fewer assumptions about the distribution of values in a sample than a parametric test. Normal distribution: A mathematical distribution with very important characteristics. However, it is easier to regard it as a bell-shaped frequency curve. The tails of the curve should stretch to infinity in both directions but this, in the end, is of little practical importance. Numeric variables: Variables for which the data are collected in the form of scores which indicate quantity. Oblique factors: In factor analysis, oblique factors are ones which, during rotation, are allowed to correlate with each other. This may be more realistic than orthogonal rotations. One way of looking at this is to consider height and weight. These are distinct variables but they correlate to some degree. Oblique factors are distinct but they can correlate. Odds: Obtained by dividing the probability of something occurring by the probability of it not occurring. Odds ratio: The number by which the odds of something occurring must be multiplied for a one unit change in a predictor variable. One-tailed test: A version of significance testing in which a strong prediction is made as to the direction of the relationship.

11/14/13 8:51 AM

690

GLOSSARY

This should be theoretically and empirically well founded on previous research. The prediction should be made prior to examination of the data. Ordinal data: Numbers for which little can be said other than the numbers give the rank order of cases on the variable from smallest to largest. Orthogonal: Essentially means at right angles. Orthogonal factors: In factor analysis, orthogonal factors are factors which do not correlate with each other. Outcome variable: A word used especially in medical statistics to denote the dependent variable. It is also the criterion variable. It is the variable which is expected to vary with variation in the independent variable(s). Outlier: A score or data point which differs substantially from the other scores or data points. It is an extremely unusual or infrequent score or data point. Output window: The window of computer software which displays the results of an analysis. Over-identified model: A structural equation model in which the number of data points is greater than the number of parameters to be estimated, enabling the fit of the model to the data to be determined. Paired comparisons: The process of comparing each variable mean with every (or most) other variable mean in pairs. Parameter: A characteristic such as the mean or standard deviation which is based on the population of scores. In contrast, a statistic is a characteristic which is based on a sample of scores. Parametric: To do with the characteristics of the population. Parametric test: A statistical test which assumes that the scores used come from a population of scores which is normally distributed. Part or semi-partial correlation: The correlation between a criterion and a predictor when the predictor’s correlation with other predictors is partialled out. Partial correlation: The correlation between a criterion and a predictor when the criterion’s and the predictor’s correlation with other predictors have been partialled out. Participant: Someone who takes part in research. A more appropriate term than the archaic and misleading ‘subject’. PASW Statistics: The name for SPSS Statistics in 2008–9. Path diagram: A diagram in which the relationships (actual or hypothetical) between variables are presented. Pathway: A line in a path diagram depicting a relationship between two variables. Phi: A measure of association between two binomial or dichotomous variables. Pivot table: A table in SPSS Statistics which can be edited. Planned comparisons: Testing whether a difference between two groups is significant when there are strong grounds for expecting such a difference. Point-biserial correlation: A correlation between a score variable and a binomial (dichotomous) variable – i.e. one with two categories. Population: All of the scores from which a sample is taken. It is erroneous in statistics to think of the population as people since it is the population of scores on a variable.

Z13_HOWI0749_06_SE_GLOS.indd 690

Post hoc test: A test to see whether two groups differ significantly when the researcher has no strong grounds for predicting or expecting that they will. Essentially they are unplanned tests which were not stipulated prior to the collection of data. Power: In statistics the ability of a test to reject the null hypothesis when it is false. Principal component analysis: Primarily a form of factor analysis in which the variance of each variable is set at the maximum value of 1 as no adjustment has been made for communalities. Probably best reserved for instances in which the correlation matrix tends to have high values which is not common in psychological research. Probability distribution: The distribution of outcomes expected by chance. Promax: A method of oblique rotation in factor analysis. Quantitative research: Research which at the very least involves counting the frequency of categories in the main variable of interest. Quartimax: A method of orthogonal rotation in factor analysis. Randomisation: The assignment of cases to conditions using some method of assigning by chance. Range: The difference between the largest and smallest score of a variable. Ratio data: A measure for which it is possible to say that a score is a multiple of another score such as 20 being twice 10. Also there should be a zero point on the measure. This is a holy grail of statistical theory which psychologists will never find unless variables such as time and distance are considered. Recode: Giving a value, or set of values, another value such as recoding age into ranges of age. Regression coefficient: The weight which is applied to a predictor variable to give the value of the dependent variable. Related design: A design in which participants provide data in more than one condition of the experiment. This is where participants serve as their own controls. More rarely, if samples are matched on a pairwise basis to be as similar as possible on a matching variable then this also constitutes a related design if the matching variable correlates with the dependent variable. Related factorial design: A design in which there are two or more independent or predictor variables which have the same or similar cases in them. Reliability: Internal reliability is the extent to which items which make up a scale or measure are internally consistent. It is usually calculated either using a form of split-half reliability in which the score for half the items is correlated with the score for the other half of the items (with an adjustment for the shortened length of the scale) or using Cronbach’s alpha (which is the average of all possible split-half reliabilities). A distinct form of reliability is test– retest reliability which measures consistency over time. Repeated measures ANOVA: An analysis of variance which is based on one or more related factors having the same or similar cases in them.

11/14/13 8:51 AM



Repeated measures design: A design in which the groups of the independent variables have the same or similar cases in them. Residual: The difference between an observed and expected score. Residual sum of squares: The sum of squares that are left over after other sources of variance have been removed. Rotation: see Rotation of factors. Rotation of factors: This adjusts the factors (axes) of a factor analysis in order to make the factors more interpretable. To do so, the numbers of high and low factor loadings are maximised whereas the numbers of middle-sized factor loadings are made minimal. Originally it involved plotting the axes (factors) on graph paper and rotating them physically on the page, leaving the factor loadings in the same points on the graph paper. As a consequence, the factor loadings change since these have not moved but the axes have. Sample: A selection or subset of scores on a variable. Samples cannot be guaranteed to be representative of the population but if they are selected at random then there will be no systematic difference between the samples and the population. Sampling distribution: The theoretical distribution of a particular size of sample which would result if samples of that size were repeatedly taken from that population. Saturated model: A model (set of variables) which fully accounts for the data. It is a concept used in log-linear analysis. Scattergram: see Scatterplot. Scatterplot: A diagram or chart which shows the relationship between two score variables. It consists of a horizontal and a vertical scale which are used to plot the scores of each individual on both variables. Scheffé test: A post hoc test used in analysis of variance to test whether two group means differ significantly from each other. Score statistic: A measure of association in logistic regression. Scree test: A graph of the eigenvalues of successive factors in a factor analysis. It is used to help determine the ‘significant’ number of factors prior to rotation. The point at which the curve becomes flat and ‘straight’ determines the number of ‘significant’ factors. Select cases: The name of an SPSS Statistics procedure for selecting subsamples of cases based on one or more cri­ teria such as the gender of participants. Sign test: A nonparametric test which determines whether the number of positive and negative differences between the scores in two conditions with the same or similar cases differ significantly. Significance level: The probability level at and below which an outcome is assumed to be unlikely to be due to chance. Simple regression: A test for describing the size and direction of the association between a predictor variable and a criterion variable. Skew: A description given to a frequency distribution in which the scores tend to be in one tail of the distribution.

Z13_HOWI0749_06_SE_GLOS.indd 691

GLOSSARY 691

In other words, it is a lop-sided frequency distribution compared to a normal (bell-shaped) curve. Sort cases: The name of an SPSS Statistics procedure for ordering cases in the data file according to the values of one or more variables. Spearman’s correlation coefficient: A measure of the size and direction of the association between two variables rank ordered in size. Sphericity: Similarity of the correlations between the dependent variable in the different conditions. Split-half reliability: The correlation between the two halves of a scale adjusted for the number of variables in each scale. SPSS: A statistical computer package which in 2008–9 was renamed PASW Statistics. In 2010 it was renamed SPSS Statistics. Squared Euclidean distance: The sum of the squared differences between the scores on two variables for the sample. Standard deviation: Conceptually, the average amount by which the scores differ from the mean. Standard error: Conceptually, the average amount by which the means of samples differ from the mean of the population. Standard or direct entry: A form of multiple regression in which all of the predictor variables are entered into the analysis at the same time. Standardised coefficients or weights: The coefficients or weights of the predictors in an equation are expressed in terms of their standardised scores. Stepwise entry: A form of multiple regression in which variables are entered into the analysis one step at a time. In this way, the most predictive predictor is chosen first, then the second most predictive predictor is chosen second having dealt with the variance due to the first predictor, and so forth. Sum of squares: The total obtained by adding up the squared differences between each score and the mean of that set of scores. The ‘average’ of this is the variance. Syntax: Statements or commands for carrying out various procedures in computer software. Test–retest reliability: The correlation of a measure taken at one point in time with the same (or very similar) measure taken at a different point in time. Transformation: Ways of adjusting the data to meet the requirements for the data for a particular statistical technique. For example, the data could be changed by taking the square root of each score, turning each score into a logarithm, and so forth. Trial and error may be required to find an appropriate transformation. Two-tailed test: A test which assesses the statistical significance of a relationship or difference in either direction. Type I error: Accepting the hypothesis when it is actually false. Type II error: Rejecting the hypothesis when it is actually true. Under-identified model: A structural equation model in which there are not enough data points to estimate its parameters.

11/14/13 8:51 AM

692

GLOSSARY

Unique variance: Variance of a variable which is not shared with other variables in the analysis. Univariate: Involving one variable. Unplanned comparisons: Comparisons between groups which were not stipulated before the data were collected but after its collection. Unstandardised coefficients or weights: The coefficients or weights which are applied to scores (as opposed to standardised scores). Value label: The name or label given to the value of a variable such as ‘Female’ for ‘1’. Variable label: The name or label given to a variable. Variable name: The name of a variable. Variable View: The window in SPSS Statistics Data Editor which shows the names of variables and their specification. Variance: The mean of the sum of the squared difference between each score and the mean of the set of scores. It constitutes a measure of the variability or dispersion of scores on a quantitative variable. Variance ratio: The ratio between two variances, commonly referred to in ANOVA (analysis of variance). Variance–covariance matrix: A matrix containing the variance of the variables (in the diagonal) and the covariances between pairs of variables in the rest of the table.

Z13_HOWI0749_06_SE_GLOS.indd 692

Variance estimate: The variance of the population of scores calculated from the variance of a sample of scores from that population. Varimax: In factor analysis, a procedure for rotating the factors to simplify understanding of the factors which maintains the zero correlation between all of the factors. Wald statistic: The ratio of the beta coefficient to its standard error. Used in logistic regression. Weights: An adjustment made to reflect the size of a variable or sample. Wilcoxon signed-rank test: A nonparametric test for assessing whether the scores from two samples that come from the same or similar cases differ significantly. Wilks’ lambda: A measure, involving the ratio of the withingroups to the total sum of squares, used to determine if the means of variables differ significantly across groups. Within-subjects design: A correlated or repeated measures design. Yates’s continuity correction: An outmoded adjustment to a 2 × 2 chi-square test held to improve the fit of the test to the chi-square distribution. z-score: A score expressed as the number of standard deviations a score is from the mean of the set of scores.

11/14/13 8:51 AM

References

Ahrens, C., Campbell, R., Ternier-Thames, N., Wasco, S., & Sefl, T. (2007). Deciding whom to tell: Expectations and outcomes of rape survivors’ first disclosures. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 38–49. Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ang, R. P., Chong, W. H., Chye, S., & Huan, V. S. (2012). Loneliness and generalized problematic internet use: Parents’ perceived knowledge of adolescents’ online activities as a moderator. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 1342–1347. Ang, R. P., & Huan, V. S. (2006). Relationship between academic stress and suicidal ideation: Testing for depression as a mediator using multiple regression. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 37, 133–143. APA (2010). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Arden, R., & Plomin, R. (2006). Sex differences in variance of intelligence across childhood. Individual Differences and Personality, 41, 39–48. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator– mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Con­ceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. Baron, L., & Straus, M. (1989). Four theories of rape: A state-level analysis. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Ben-Zvi, D., & Garfield, J. (Eds.) (2004). The challenge of developing statistical literacy, reasoning, and thinking. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Bierie, D. M. (2013). Procedural justice and prison violence: Examining complaints among federal inmates (2000– 2007). Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 19(1), 15–29. Blackmore, E. R., Jones, I., Doshi, M., Haque, S., Holder, R., Brockington, I., & Craddock, N. (2006). Obstetric variables associated with bipolar affective puerperal psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 188, 32–36.

Z14_HOWI0749_06_SE_BIB.indd 693

Blankenship, K. L., Wegener, D. T., & Murray, R. A. (2012). Circumventing resistance: using values to indirectly change attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(4), 606–621. Blalock, H. M. (1972). Social statistics. New York: McGraw-Hill. Blom, D., van Middendorp, H., & Geenen, R. (2012). Anxious attachment may be a vulnerability factor for developing embitterment. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 85(4), 351–355. Brasel, A., & Gips, J. (2011). Media multitasking behavior: Concurrent television and computer usage. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(9), 527–534. Bridges, F. S., Williamson, C. B., Thompson, P. C., & Windsor, M. A. (2001). Lost letter technique: Returned responses to battered and abused women, men, and lesbians. North American Journal of Psychology, 3, 263–276. Butler, C. (1995a). Teachers’ qualities, resources and involvement of special needs children in mainstream classrooms. Unpublished thesis, Department of Social Sciences, Loughborough University. Butler, R. (1995b). Motivational and informational functions and consequences of children’s attention to peers’ work. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(3), 347–360. Carlson, E. N., Vazire, S., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2011). You probably think this paper’s about you: Narcissists’ perceptions of their personality and reputation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 185–201. Carolan, L. A., & Power, M. J. (2011). What basic emotions are experienced in bipolar disorder? Clinical Psychology& Psychotherapy, 18(5), 366–378. Carr, V. J., Whiteford, H., Groves, A., McGorry, P., & Shepherd, A. M. (2012). Policy and service development implications of the second Australian National Survey of High Impact Psychosis (SHIP). Australia and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 46(8), 708–718.

11/14/13 8:52 AM

694

References

Casarett, D., Pickard, A., Fishman, J. M. Alexander, S. C., Arnold, R. M., Pollak, K. I., & Tulsky, J. A. (2010). Can metaphors and analogies improve communication with seriously ill patients? Journal of Palliative Medicine, 13, 255–260. Casidy, R. (2012). Discovering consumer personality clusters in prestige sensitivity and fashion consciousness context. Journal of International Consumer Marketing, 24, 291–299. Cetinkalp, Z. K. (2012). Achievement goals and physical self-perceptions of adolescent athletes. Social Behaviour and Personality, 40(3), 473–480. Chan, M., & Singhal, A. (2013). The emotional side of cognitive distraction: Implications for road safety. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 50, 147–154. Childs, A., & Klimoski, R. J. (1986). Successfully predicting career success: An application of the biographical inventory. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 3–8. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Contador, I., Fernández-Calvo, B., Cacho, L. J., Ramos, F., & López-Rolón, A. (2010). Non-verbal memory tasks in early differential diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease and unipolar depression. Applied Neuropsychology, 17(4), 251–261. Cramer, D. (1992). Personality and psychotherapy. Milton Keynes: Open University Press. Crighton, D., & Towl, G. (1994). The selection and recruitment of prison officers. Forensic Update: A Newsletter for Forensic Psychologists, 39, 4–7. Critcher, C. R., & Dunning, D. (2013). Predicting persons’ versus a person’s goodness: Behavioral forecasts diverge for individuals versus populations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 28–44. Cumming, S. P., Sherar, L. B., Hunter Smart, J. E., Rodriques, A. M. M., Standage, M., Gillison, F. B., & Malina, R. M. (2012). Physical activity and physical self-concept in adolescence: A comparison of girls at the extremes of the biological maturation continuum. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 22(4), 746–757. Curseu, P. L., Schruijer, S. G. L., & Boros, S. (2012). Socially rejected while cognitively successful: The impact of minority dissent on groups’ cognitive complexity. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(4), 570–582. Dakwar, E., Nunes, E. V., Bisaga, A., Carpenter, K. C., Mariani, J. J., Sullivan, M. A., Raby, W. N., & Levin F. R. (2011). A comparison of independent depression and substance-induced depression in cannabis-, cocaine-, and opioid-dependent treatment seekers. American Journal on Addictions, 20(5), 441–446. Deary, I. J., Hunter, R., Langan, S. J., & Goodwin, G. M. (1991). Inspection time, psychometric intelligence and

Z14_HOWI0749_06_SE_BIB.indd 694

clinical estimates of cognitive ability in pre-senile Alzheimer’s disease and Korsakoff’s psychosis. Brain, 114, 2543–255. Di Filippo, G., de Luca, M., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Lexicality and stimulus length effects in Italian dyslexics: Role of overadditivity effect. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 141–149. Donnerstein, E. (1980). Aggressive erotica and violence against women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(2), 269–277. Drees, M. J., & Mack, M. G. (2012). An examination of mental toughness over the course of a competitive season. Journal of Sport Behavior, 35(4), 377–386. Dumont, K., & Louw, J. (2009). The recognition of Henri Tajfel’s work on intergroup relations. International Journal of Psychology, 44, 46–59. Edenfield, J. L., Adams, K. S., & Briihl, D. (2012). Relationship maintenance strategy use by romantic attachment style. North American Journal of Psychology, 14(1), 149–162. Estevis, E., Basso, M. R., & Combs, D. (2012). Effects of practice on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV across 3- and 6-month intervals. Clinical Neuropsychology, 26(2), 39–54. Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1976). Psychoticism as a dimension of personality. London: Hodder & Stoughton. Faust, M. E., Balota, D. A., Spieler, D. H., & Ferraro, F. R. (1999). Individual differences in information processing rate and amount: implications for group differences in response latency. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 777–799. Fayed, N., Klassen, A. F., Dix, D., Klaassen, R., & Sung, L. (2011). Exploring predictors of optimism among parents of children with cancer. Psycho-Oncology, 20(4), 411–418. Fitneva, S. A., Lam, N. H., & Dunfield, K. A. (2013). The development of children’s information gathering: To look or to ask? Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 533–542. Ford, S., Howard, R., & Oyebode, J. (2012). Psychosocial aspects of coeliac disease: A cross-sectional survey of a UK population. British Journal of Health Psychology, 17(4), 743–757. Frank, G. K. W., Roblek, T., Shott, M. E., Jappe, L. M., Rollin, M. D. H., Hagman, J. O., & Pryor, T. (2012). Heightened fear of uncertainty in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45, 227–232. Freund, P. A., & Kasten, N. (2012). How smart do you think you are? A meta-analysis on the validity of self-estimates of cognitive ability. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 296–321. Gallagher, P., Yancy, W. S. Jr, Jeffreys, A. S., Coffman, C. J., Weinberger, M., Bosworth, H. B., & Voils, C. I. (2013). Patient self-efficacy and spouse perception of spousal support are associated with lower patient weight: Baseline results from a spousal support behavioral intervention. Psychology, Health& Medicine, 18, 175–181.

11/14/13 8:52 AM



Gannon, T. A., & Barrowcliffe, E. (2012). Firesetting in the general population: The development and validation of the Fire Setting and Fire Proclivity Scales. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 17(1), 105–122. Gervais, S. J., Vescio, T. K., & Allen, J. (2012). When are people interchangeable sexual objects? The effect of gender and body type on sexual fungibility. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(4), 499–513. Gibbs, S., & Powell, B. (2012). Teacher efficacy and pupil behaviour: The structure of teachers’ individual and collective beliefs and their relationship with numbers of pupils excluded from school. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(4), 564–584. Gillis, J. S. (1980). Child Anxiety Scale Manual. Champaign, IL: Institute of Personality and Ability Testing. Glantz, S. A., & Slinker, B. K. (1990). Primer of applied regres­sion and analysis of variance. New York: McGraw-Hill. Gonzalez, V. M., & Hewell, V. M. (2012). Suicidal ideation and drinking to cope among college binge drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 37(8), 994–997. Gordon, S. (1995). A theoretical approach to understanding learners of statistics. Journal of Statistics Education [Online], 3(3) http://www.amstat.org/publications/jse/ v3n3/gordon.html Gordon, S. (2004). Understanding students’ experiences of statistics in a service course. Statistics Education Research Journal, 3(1), 40–59. Gotwals, J. K., Stoeber, J., Dunn, J. G. H., & Stoll, O. (2012). Are perfectionistic strivings in sport adaptive? A systematic review of confirmatory, contradictory, and mixed evidence. Canadian Psychology, 53(4), 263–279. Gray, H. M., LaPlante, D. A., & Shaffer, H. J. (2012). Behavioral characteristics of Internet gamblers who trigger corporate responsible gambling interventions. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 26(3), 527–535. Green, P., Rohling, M., Lees-Haley, P., & Allen, L. (2001). Effort has a greater effect on test scores than severe brain injury in compensation claimants. Brain Injury, 15(12), 1045–1060. Griffin, B., & Hesketh, B. (2008). Post-retirement work: The individual determinants of paid and volunteer work. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 81, 101–121. Guzman, J. F., & Kingston, K. (2012). Prospective study of sport dropout: a motivational analysis as a function of age and gender. European Journal of Sports Science, 12, 431–442. Hannaford, P. C., Thompson, C., & Simpson, M. (1996). Evaluation of an educational programme to improve the recognition of psychological illness by general practitioners. British Journal of General Practice, 46, 333–337. Harinck, F., & van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Be hard on the interests and soft on the values: Conflict issue moderates the effects of anger in negotiations. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(4), 741–752.

Z14_HOWI0749_06_SE_BIB.indd 695

References 695

Helvik, A.-S., Engedal, K., Skancke, R. H., & Selbæk, G. (2011). A psychometric evaluation of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale for the medically hospitalized elderly. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 65, 338–344. Hoenig, J. M., & Heisey, D. M. (2001). The abuse of power: The pervasive fallacy of power calculations for data analysis. The American Statistician, 55(1), 19–24. Hoicka, E., & Akhtar, N. (2012). Early humour production. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 586–603. Howell, D. (2013). Statistical methods for psychology (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Duxbury Press. Howitt, D. (2013). Introduction to qualitative methods in psychology (2nd ed.). Harlow: Pearson Education. Howitt, D., & Cramer, D. (2014a). Introduction to research methods in psychology. Harlow: Pearson. Howitt, D., & Cramer, D. (2014). Introduction to SPSS Statistics in Psychology: For version 22 and earlier (6th ed.). Harlow: Pearson Education. Howitt, D., & Cumberbatch, G. (1990), Pornography: Impacts and influences, London: Home Office Research and Planning Unit. Hughes, J. S., & Trafimow, D. (2012). Inferences about character and motive influence intentionality attributions about side effects. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51, 661–673. Huisman, A., van Houwelingen, C. A. J., & Kerkhof, A. J. F. M. (2010). Psychopathology and suicide method in mental health care. Journal of Affective Disorders, 121, 94–99. Huitema, B. E. (1980). The analysis of covariance and its alternatives. New York: Wiley. Hunter, P. G., Schellenberg, E. G., & Griffith, A. T. (2011). Misery loves company: Mood-congruent emotional responding to music. Emotion, 11, 1068–1072. Ingravallo, F., Vignatelli, L., Brini, M., Brugaletta, C., Franceschini, C., Lugaresi, F., Manca, M. C., Garbarino, S., Montagna, P., Cicognani, A., & Plazzi, G. (2008). Medico-legal assessment of disability in narcolepsy: an interobserver reliability study. Journal of Sleep Research, 17(1), 111–119. Ivancevich, J. M. (1976). Effects of goal setting on performance and job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61(5), 605–612. Jafari, N., Zamani, A., Farajzadegan, Z., Bahrami, F., Emami, H., & Loghmani, A. (2013). The effect of spiritual therapy for improving the quality of life of women with breast cancer: A randomized controlled trial. Psychology, Health and Medicine, 18(1), 56–69. Jenkins, P. E., Conley, C. S., Rienecke Hoste, R., Meyer, C., & Blissett, J. M. (2012). Perception of control during episodes of eating: Relationships with quality of life and eating psychopathology. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45, 115–119. Johnston, F. A., & Johnston, S. A. (1986). Differences between human figure drawings of child molesters and control groups. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42(4), 638–647.

11/14/13 8:52 AM

696

References

Kam, L. Y. K., Knott, V. E. Wilson, C., & Chambers, S. K. (2012). Using the theory of planned behavior to understand health professionals’ attitudes and intentions to refer cancer patients for psychosocial support. Psycho-Oncology, 21, 316–323. Kenne, D. R., Boros, A. P., & Fischbein, R. L. (2010). Characteristics of opiate users leaving detoxification treatment against medical advice. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 29, 283–294. Kenyon, M., Samarawickrema, N., DeJong, H., Van den Eynde, F., Startup, H., Lavender, A., Goodman-Smith, E., & Schmidt, U. (2012). Theory of mind in bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45, 377–384. Kerlinger, F. N. (1986). Foundations of behavioural research. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Kogan, S. M. (2004). Disclosing unwanted sexual experiences: Results from a national sample of adolescent women. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 147–165. Kois, L., Pearson, J., Chauhan, P., Goni, M., & Saraydarian, L. (2013). Competency to stand trial among female inpatients. Law and Human Behavior, in press. Kuhnle, C., Hofer, M., & Kilian, B. (2012). Self-control as predictor of school grades, life balance, and flow in adolescents. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(4), 533–548. Laaksonen, M. A., Lindfors, O., Knekt, P., & Aalberg, V. (2012). Suitability for Psychotherapy Scale (SPS) and its reliability, validity, and prediction. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(4), 351–375. Lalonde, R. N., & Gardner, R. C. (1993). Statistics as a second language? A model for predicting performance in psychology students. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 25(1), 108–125. Lamoureux, B. E., Palmieri, P. A., Jackson, A. P., & Hobfoll, S. E. (2012). Child sexual abuse and adulthoodinterpersonal outcomes: Examining pathways for intervention. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 4(6), 605–613. Lampropoulos, G. K., Schneider, M. K., & Spengler, P. M. (2009). Predictors of early termination in a university counseling training clinic. Journal of Counseling and Development, 87, 36–46. Lautamo, T., Laakso, M. L., Aro, T., Ahonen, T., & Törmäkangas, K. (2011). Validity of the play assessment for group settings: An evaluation of differential item functioning between children with specific language impairment and typically developing peers. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 58 (4), 222–230. Levine, T. R., Asada, K. J., & Carpenter, C. (2009). Sample size and effect size are negatively correlated in meta-analysis: Evidence and implications of a publication bias against non-significant findings. Communication Monographs, 76, 286–302. Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Wood, A. M., Osborne, G., & Hurling, R. (2009). Measuring happiness: The higher order factor structure of subjective and psychological

Z14_HOWI0749_06_SE_BIB.indd 696

well-being measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 878–884. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of psy­chological, educational, and behavioral treatment: Confirmation from meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 48(12), 1181–1209. Lounsbury, J. W., Sundstrom, E., Loveland, J. M., & Gibson, L. W. (2003). Intelligence, ‘big five’ personality traits, and work drive as predictors of course grade. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1231–1239. Lowe, P., & Ang, R. (2012). Cross-cultural examination of test anxiety among US and Singapore students on the Test Anxiety Scale for Elementary Students (TAS-E) Educational Psychology, 32(1), 107–126. MacCabe, J. H., Brebion, G., Reichenber, A., Ganguly, T., McKenna, P. J., Murray, P. J., & David, A. S. (2012). Superior intellectual ability in schizophrenia: Neuropsychological characteristics. Neuropsychology, 26(2), 181–190. Maguire-Jack, K., Gromoske, A. N., & Berger, L. M. (2012). Spanking and child development during the first five years of life. Child Development, 83(6), 1960–1977. Matthews, N. L., Goldberg, W. A., Lukowski, A. F., Osann, K., Abdullah, M. M., Ly, A. R., Thorsen, K., & Spence, M. A. (2012). Does theory of mind performance differ in children with early-onset and regressive autism? Developmental Science, 15(1), 25–34. McKiernan, A., Steggles, S., Guerin, S., & Carr, A. (2010). A controlled trial of group cognitive behavior therapy for Irish breast cancer patients. Journal of Psychosocial Oncology, 28, 143–156. Meeten, F., & Davey, G. C. L. (2012). Mood as input and perseverative worrying following the induction of discrete negative moods. Behavior Therapy, 43, 393–406. Mercer, S. H., Harpole, L. L., Mitchell, R. R., McLemore, C., & Hardy, C. (2012). The impact of probe variability on brief experimental analysis of reading skills. School Psychology Quarterly, 27, 223–235. Mitsumatsu, H. (2013). Stronger discounting of external cause by action in human adults: Evidence for an action-based hypothesis of visual collision perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 101–118. Motes, M. A., Hubbard, T. L., Courtney, J. R., & Rypma, B. (2008). A principal components analysis of dynamic spatial memory biases. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 34, 1076–1083. Munford, M. B. (1994). Relationship of gender, self-esteem, social class and racial identity to depression in blacks. Journal of Black Psychology, 20, 157–174. Murphy, K. R., & Myors, B. (2004), Statistical power analysis: A simple and general model for traditional and modern hypothesis tests (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

11/14/13 8:52 AM



Mutsvunguma, P., & Gwandure, C. (2011). The psychological well-being of employees who handle cash in a bank in inner city Johannesburg. Psychology, Health& Medicine, 16(4), 430–436. Nair, U. S., Collins, B. N., & Napolitano, M. A. (2012). Differential effects of a body image exposure session on smoking urge between physically active and sedentary female smokers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27(1), 322–327. Nicholas, M. K, Coulston, C. M., Asghari, A., & Malhi, G. S. (2009). Depressive symptoms in patients with chronic pain. Medical Journal of Australia, 190, S66–S70. Niemeier, J. P., Marwitz, J. H., Lesher, K., Walker, W., & Bushnik, T. (2007). Gender differences in executive functions following traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 17, 293–313. Norman, G. J., Hawkley, L., Ball, A., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2013). Perceived social isolation moderates the relationship between early childhood trauma and pulse pressure in older adults. International Journal of Psychophysiology, in press. Otgaar, R., Horselenberg, van Kampen, R., & Lalleman, K. (2012). Clothed and unclothed human figure drawings lead to more correct and incorrect reports of touch in children. Psychology, Crime & Law, 18(7), 641–653. Passmore, J., & Rehman, H. (2012). Coaching as a learning methodology – a mixed methods study in driver development using a randomised controlled trial and thematic analysis. International Coaching Psychology Review, 7(2), 166–184. Pechey, R., & Halligan, P. (2011). The prevalence of delusion-like beliefs relative to sociocultural beliefs in the general population. Psychopathology, 44(2), 106–115. Perlman, D. (2011). Examination of self-determination within the sport education model. Asia-Pacific Journal of Health, Sport and Physical Education, 2(1), 79–92. Peters, M., & Durding, B. M. (1978). Handedness measured by finger tapping: A continuous variable. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 32(4), 257–261. Potter, G. G., Hartman, M., & Ward, T. (2009). Perceived stress and everyday memory complaints among older adult women. Anxiety Stress and Coping, 2(4), 475–81. Ridenour, T. A., McCoy, K. D., & Dean, R. S. (1996). An exploratory stepwise discriminant function analysis of malingered and nondistorted responses to the neuro­ psychological symptom inventory. International Journal of Neuroscience, 87, 91–95. Rohmer, O., & Louvet, E. (2012). Implicit measures of the stereotype content associated with disability. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(4), 732–740. Rosenthal, J. A. (1988). Patterns of reported child abuse and neglect. Child Abuse and Neglect, 12, 263–271. Rothbard, N., & Wilk, S. L. (2011). Waking up on the right or wrong side of the bed: Start-of-workday mood, work events, employee affect, and performance. Academy of Management Journal, 54(5), 959–980.

Z14_HOWI0749_06_SE_BIB.indd 697

References 697

Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 1762–1774. Ruggeri, K., Dempster, M., & Hanna, D. (2011). The impact of misunderstanding the nature of statistics. Psychology Teaching Review, 17(1), 33–38. Ruscio, J., & Roche, B. (2012). Variance heterogeneity in published psychological research: A review and a new index. Methodology: European Journal of Research Methods for the Behavioral and Social Sciences, 8(1), 2, 1–11. Schau, C. (2003). Students’ attitudes: The ‘other’ important outcome in statistics education. http:// evaluationandstatistics.com/JSM2003.pdf. Accessed 24 May 2013. Schimmack, U. (2012). The ironic effect of significant results on the credibility of multiple-study articles. Psychological Methods, 17(4), 551–566. Schulenberg, S. E., & Yutrzenka, B. A. (2001). Equivalence of computerized and conventional versions of the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). Current Psychology, 20, 216–230. Sedlmeier, P., Eberth, J., Schwarz, M., Zimmermann, D., Haarig, F., Jaeger, S., & Kunze, S. (2012). The psychological effects of meditation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138(6), 1139–1171. Shafran, R., Lee, M., Payne, E., & Fairburn, C. G. (2006). The impact of manipulating personal standards on eating attitudes and behaviour. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 897–906. Sierra, P., Livianos, L., & Rojo, L. (2005). Quality of life for patients with bipolar disorder: relationship with clinical and demographic variables. Bipolar Disorders, 7, 159–165. Signal, T. L., van den Berg, M. J., Mulrine, H. M., & Gander, P. H. (2012). Duration of sleep inertia after napping during simulated night work and in extended operations. Chronobiology International, 29(6), 769–779. Simpson, S., & Karageorghis, C. I. (2006). The effects of synchronous music on 400-m sprint performance, Journal of Sports Sciences, 24, 1095–1102. Siy, J. O., & Cheryan, S. (2013). When compliments fail to flatter: American individualism and responses to positive stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 87–102. Skinner, B. F. (1948). ‘Superstition’ in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168–172. Skipper, Y., & Douglas, K. M. (2012). Is no praise good praise? Effects of positive feedback on children’s and university students’ responses to subsequent failures. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(2), 327–339. Smith-Bell, C. A., Burhans L. B., & Schreurs B. G. (2012). Predictors of susceptibility and resilience in an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behavioral Neuroscience, 126(6), 749–761.

11/14/13 8:52 AM

698

References

Spini, D., Elcheroth, G., & Figini, D. (2009). Is there space for time in social psychology publications? A content analysis across five journals. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 19(3), 165–240. Sprung, J. M., Sliter, M. T., & Jex, S. M. (2012). Spirituality as a moderator of the relationship between workplace aggression and employee outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 930–934. Stasiewicz, P. R., Schlauch, R. C., Bradizza, C. M., Bole, C. W., & Coffey, S. F. (2013). Pretreatment changes in drinking: Relationship to treatment outcomes. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, in press. Szostak, H. (1995). Competitive performance, anxiety and perceptions of parental pressure in young tennis players. Unpublished thesis, Department of Social Sciences, Loughborough University. Taylor, J. S., Rastle, K., & Davis, M. H. (2013). Can cognitive models explain brain activation during word and pseudoword reading? A meta-analysis of 36 neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin, in press. Teissedre, F., & Chabrol, H. (2004). Detecting women at risk for post-natal depression using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale at 2 to 3 days post-partum. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 49(1), 51–54. Testa, M., Van Zile-Tamsen, C., & Livingston, J. A. (2007). Prospective prediction of women’s sexual victimization by intimate and nonintimate male perpetrators. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 52–60. Touliatos, J., & Lindholm, B. W. (1981). Congruence of parents’ and teachers’ ratings of children’s behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 9(3), 347–354. Tracey, T. J., Sherry, P., Bauer, G. P., Robins, T. H., Todaro, L., & Briggs, S. (1984). Help seeking as a function of student characteristics and program description: A logit-loglinear analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 54–62. Tremont, G., & Alosco, M. L. (2011). Relationship between cognition and awareness of deficit in mild cognitive impairment. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 29, 299–306. Tyson, P., Wilson, K., Brailsford, R., & Law, K. (2010). Physical activity and mental health in a student population. Journal of Mental Health, 19, 492–499. Vallat-Azouvi, C., Pradat-Diehl, P., & Azouvi, P. (2012). The Working Memory Questionnaire: A scale to assess everyday life problems related to deficits of working memory in brain injured patients. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: An International Journal, 22(4), 634–649. van Schaik, P., & Ling, J. (2012). An experimental analysis of experiential and cognitive variables in web navigation. Human Computer Interaction, 27, 199–234.

Z14_HOWI0749_06_SE_BIB.indd 698

Vassari, M., & Crosby, J. W. (2008). A reliability generalization study of coefficient alpha for the UCLA Loneliness Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 90(6), 601–607. Vista, A., & Care, E. (2011). Gender differences in variance and means on the Naglieri Non-verbal Ability Test: Data from the Philippines. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(2), 292–308. Wagner, U., & Zick, A. (1995). The relation of formal education to ethnic prejudice: Its reliability, validity and explanation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 41–56. Wang, M-T., & Huguley, J. P. (2012). Parental racial socialization as a moderator of the effects of racial discrimination on educational success among African American adolescents. Child Development, 83, 1716–1731. Warren, C. S., Holland, S., Billings, H., & Parker, A. (2012). The relationships between fat talk, body dissatisfaction, and drive for thinness: Perceived stress as a moderator. Body Image, 9, 358–364. Wickett, J. C., Vernon, P. A., & Lee, D. H. (1994). In vivo brain size, head perimeter, and intelligence in a sample of healthy adult females. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 831–838. Wickham, L. H., Morris, P. E., & Fritz, C. O. (2000). Facial distinctiveness: Its measurement, distribution and influence on immediate and delayed recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 99–123. Wilkes, S., Cordier, R., Bundy, A., Docking, K., & Munro, N. (2011). A play-based intervention for children with ADHD: A pilot study. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 58(4), 231–240. Woods, S. P., Rippeth, J. D., Conover, E., Carey, C. L., Parsons, T. D., & Troster, A. I. (2006). Statistical power of studies examining the cognitive effects of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation in Parkinson’s disease. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 27–38. Wright, L., & Hardie, S. M. (2012). Are left-handers really more anxious? Laterality, 17(5), 629–642. Wyrick, D. L., & Bond, L. (2011). Reducing sensitive survey response bias in research on adolescents: a comparison of web-based and paper-and-pencil administration. American Journal of Health Promotion, 25(5), 349–352. Yildirim, h. (2008). Relationships between burnout, sources of social support and sociodemographic variables. Social Behavior and Personality, 36(5), 603–616. Ziegler, R. H., & Britta Diehl, M. (2012). Relationship between job satisfaction and job performance: Job ambivalence as a moderator. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42, 2019–2040. Zimprich, D. (2012). Attitudes toward statistics among Swiss psychology students. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 71(3), 149–155.

11/14/13 8:52 AM

Index

Note: Glossary page numbers appear in bold. –2 log likelihood 624, 685 a priori statistical power analysis 573 – 4, 575 a priori test 685 Aalberg, V. 526 Adams, K. S. 278 addition rule 220, 221 adjusted mean 685 advanced correlational statistics 409 – 84 analysis of questionnaire/survey project 476 – 84 factor analysis 423 – 43 multiple regression and multiple correlation 444 –59 partial correlation 411–22 path analysis 460–75 advanced qualitative or nominal techniques 587 – 648 analysis of complex contingency tables 589 – 613 binomial logistic regression 632 – 48 multinomial logistic regression 614 –31 advanced techniques 485 –585 confidence intervals 529 –39 effect size 487 – 94 meta-analysis 495 –514 moderator effects 540– 61 reliability in scales and measurement 515 –28 statistical power analysis 562 – 85 agreement between raters 522 –5 kappa coefficient calculation 523 –5 Agresti, A. 613 Ahonen, T. 493 Ahrens, C. 611 Aiken, L. S. 548, 550, 554, 555, 561 Akhtar, N. 236 Allen, J. 493 Allen, L. 82 –3 Alosco, M. L. 83 alpha level 568–9, 685 alpha reliability 519 –21 alternative hypothesis 146, 147 alternatives to chi-square 205 –10 analysis of complex contingency tables 589–613, 686 degrees of freedom 607

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 699

hierarchical models 610 key points 611 lambda 610 log-linear methods 590–2 reporting results 610 three-variable example 599 – 609 two-variable example 592 – 9 analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) 354–69, 372, 685 calculation: one-way analysis of covariance 357 – 66 computer analysis 367 – 9 key points 366 research examples 366 analysis of questionnaire/survey project 476 – 84 data analysis 482 – 4 data cleaning 482 data coding 481–2 initial variable classification 480–1 key points 484 research hypothesis 479 – 80 research project 477 – 9 analysis of variance (ANOVA) 13, 685 effect size 492 –3 moderator effects 545, 555 – 8 analysis of variance (ANOVA): correlated scores/repeated measures 282 – 97 calculation 287 – 93 computer analysis 296 –7 dependent variable 283 examples 286 – 94 key points 295 matched sets 284 –5 research examples 294 –5 theory 285 – 6 analysis of variance (ANOVA): mixed design 337 –53 calculation 342 – 9 cell sizes 338 computer analysis 351–3 fixed vs random effects 339 key points 351 mixed designs and repeated measures 338 –50 research examples 350 simpler alternative 349

11/14/13 8:52 AM

700

Index

analysis of variance (ANOVA): multiple comparisons 326 –36 calculation: Scheffé test 331–2 computer analysis 335 – 6 Duncan multiple range test 327, 329 F-ratio significance 327 key points 334 methods 328 – 9 multifactorial ANOVA 332 –3 Neuman–Keuls test 327 planned vs a posteriori (post hoc) comparisons 329 –30 research examples 334 Scheffé test 330–2 analysis of variance (ANOVA): one-way unrelated/ uncorrelated 264 – 81 calculation 273 – 6 computer analysis 280–1 degrees of freedom 266, 269 –73 key points 279 research examples 278 – 9 revision and new material 265 – 6 sum of squares 266 summary table 276 –7 theory 266 –70 variance 265 – 6 analysis of variance (ANOVA): two-way for unrelated/ uncorrelated scores 298 –325 calculation 307 –15 computer analysis 323 – 4 interactions 315 –18 key points 322 research examples 321–2 steps 302 –15 theory 300–1 three or more independent variables 318 –21 ANCOVA see analysis of covariance Anderson, E. B. 613 Anderson, R. E. 385, 400 Ang, R. P. 130, 382, 455, 538, 559 ANOVA see analysis of variance anxiety about statistics 5 – 6 applications of statistics 3 Arden, R. 95, 261 arithmetic mean 46 –7 calculation 47 Aro, T. 493 Asada, K. J. 494 assessing change over time 407 association 685 attitudes towards statistics 2 –3, 5, 7 averages, variation and spread 44 –57 calculation: numerical or arithmetic mean 47 calculation: variance using computational formula 53 computer analysis 56 –7 key points 55 mean, median and mode 46 –50 mean, median and mode comparison 50 numerical indexes 45 research examples 54 –5 variability 50–5 variance estimate 54 see also variables Azouvi, P. 154

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 700

backwards elimination analysis, logistic regression procedure 642 –3 Bahrami, F. 175 bands of scores 37 – 8 bar charts 34, 35–6, 685 compound 92 –3, 96 –7 computer analysis 96 –7 pictogram 36 Baron, L. 418 Baron, R. M. 555 Barrowcliffe, E. 154, 397 Bartlett’s test of sphericity 685 Basso, M. R. 366 Bauer, G. P. 611 Ben-Zvi, D. 7 Berger, L. M. 472 beta level 685 beta weight 685 between-groups design 685 between-subjects design 685 bidirectional relationships 462 Bierie, D. M. 162 bilateral relationships 462 Billings, H. 115, 559 bimodal 685 bimodal and multimodal frequency distributions 64 binomial logistic regression 632 – 48 computer analysis 647 – 8 example 637 – 9 key points 646 logistic regression procedure 640– 4 natural logarithms 636 odds ratio 635 regression formula 644 –5 reporting findings 645 research examples 646 simple logistic regression 634 – 6 uses 633 bivariate 685 bivariate correlation 685 Black, W. C. 385, 400 Blackmore, E. R. 249 Blalock, H. M. 193 Blankenship, K. L. 350 Blissett, J. M. 95, 278 block 685 Blom, D. 114 Bole, C. W. 295 Bond, L. 322 Bonferroni adjustment 328, 373, 685 bootstrapping 12, 240–1, 685 Boros, A. P. 646 Boros, S. 321–2 Box’s test of equality 381, 685 boxplot 685 Bradizza, C. M. 295 Brailsford, R. 279 Brasel, A. 67 Bridges, F. S. 611 Briggs, S. 611 Briihl, D. 278 Britta Diehl, M. 559 Brown 581

11/14/13 8:52 AM



Bryman, A. 443, 475 Buchner, A. 583 Bundy, A. 175 Burhans, L. B. 39 Bushnik, T. 629 Butler, C. 419 –20 Butler, R. 436 – 8 Cacho, L. J. 82 Campbell, R. 611 canonical correlations 389 Care, E. 261 Carey, C. L. 582 Carlson, E. N. 114 Carolan, L. A. 278 Carpenter, C. 494 Carr, V. J. 39 Casarett, D. 114, 249 case 685 Casidy, R. 382 Castellan, N. J. 252 categorical variable 685 categories/groups see multinomial logistic regression category variables (categorical variables, nominal variables) 14, 685 Cattell, Raymond 425 causality 412 cell 685 – 6 censuses 7, 20 centroids 388, 619 Cetinkalp, Z. K. 54, 192 Chabrol, H. 114 Chan, M. 294 change in –2 log likelihood 624 chart 686 Chart Editor window 686 Chauhan, P. 214 checklist 402 –7 chi-square 196–217, 241, 329, 686 alternatives 205 –10 calculation 202 – 4 calculation: Fisher exact probability test 206 –10 calculation: one sample 210–11 computer analysis 215 –17 crosstabulation/contingency table 198 effect size 490 Fisher exact probability test 206 –10 key points 214 and known populations 210 McNemar test 212 one sample 210–11 partitioning 204 –5 research examples 213 –14 table of significance 669 theory 198 –204 warnings 205 Yates’s correction 200 Child, D. 443 Childs, A. 455 Chong, W. H. 559 Chye, S. 559 cluster analysis 686 Cochran’s Q test 686

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 701

Index 701

coefficient alpha 519 –21 coefficient of attenuation 489 coefficient of determination 108, 488, 686 Coffey, S. F. 295 Cohen, J. 554, 561, 577, 579, 581 Cohen, P. 554, 561 Cohen’s d 498 – 9, 501, 571–3, 576 –7 Collins, B. N. 420 Combs, D. 366 common variance 686 communality 431–3, 686 iteration 433 comparison of studies 510 complex data see factor analysis component matrix 686 compound bar chart 92 –3 computer analysis 96 –7 compound histogram 93 – 4 Compute 686 condition 686 confidence intervals 129, 140, 529–39, 686 calculation: Pearson correlation coefficient 535 – 6 calculation: predicted score 536 –7 calculation: related t-test 534 –5 calculation: single sample 533 calculation: unrelated t-test 534 computer analysis 539 confidence limits 531 key points 538 other confidence intervals 537 parameters 140 point estimates 530 regression 536 –7 relationship between significance and confidence intervals 533 – 6 research examples 538 standard error 530–1 statistics 140 confidence limits 531 confirmatory factor analysis 686 confounding variable 686 Conley, C. S. 95, 278 Conover, E. 582 consistency and agreement see reliability in scales and measurement Contador, I. 82 contingency coefficient 490 contingency tables see analysis of complex contingency tables Cordier, R. 175 correlated scores designs 167 correlation and causality 108 correlation coefficients 98–119, 686 calculation: Pearson correlation coefficient 104 – 6 calculation: Spearman’s rho with no tied ranks 112 –13 calculation: Spearman’s rho with/without tied ranks 110–11 coefficient of determination 108 computer analysis 116 –19 covariance 102 – 6 example 113 key points 115 principles 100– 6

11/14/13 8:52 AM

702

Index

correlation coefficients (continued) research design issue 108 research examples 114 –15 rules 106 –7 significance testing 109 Spearman’s rho 109 –13 and t-test 13 see also statistical significance of correlation coefficient correlation matrix 413, 686 count 686 counterbalancing 167, 686 Courtney, J. R. 439 – 40 covariance 102–6, 686 covariate 686 Cox and Snell’s R2 622, 623, 644, 686 Cramer, D. 369, 400, 425, 443, 459, 475, 496, 507, 510, 512, 514, 583, 616 Cramer’s V 686 Crighton, D. 212 critical value 686 Cronbach’s alpha 686 computer analysis 527 – 8 research examples 526 Crosby, J. W. 526 crosstabulation (contingency) tables 91, 92, 94, 198 computer analysis 96 –7 research examples 95 Cumberbatch, G. 418 Cumming, S. P. 366 cumulative frequency curves 64 – 6 Curseu, P. L. 321–2 Dakwar, E. 646 data analysis 482 – 4 cleaning 482 coding 481–2 exploration techniques 20–1 handling 686 types see statistics see also factor analysis Data Editor window 686 DataView 686 Davey, G. C. L. 55, 95, 236, 279 Davis, M. H. 511 De Luca, M. 82 Dean, R. S. 397 Deary, I. J. 95 decisions in factor analysis 429 –34 communality 431–3 factor scores 433 – 4 number of factors 430–1 orthogonal or oblique rotation 430 rotated or unrotated factors 429 –30 degrees of freedom 266, 269–73, 607, 686 quick formulae 273 t-test 170, 171 Dempster, M. 7 dependent and independent variables 168 dependent variable 283, 686 descriptive statistics 17–132, 687 averages, variation and spread 44 –57 correlation coefficients 98 –119

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 702

regression 120–32 relationships between variables 86 – 97 shapes of distributions of scores 58 –70 standard deviation 71– 85 statistics 19 –28 tables and diagrams 29 – 43 deviation 687 Di Filippo, G. 82 diagrammatic and tabular presentation 88 diagrams and tables see relationships between variables Dialogue box 687 dichotomous 687 Diekhoff, G. 385, 400 differences between Pearson and likelihood ratio chi-square 591–2 direct entry 691 Direct Oblimin 687 discriminant function 687 discriminant function analysis 386–400, 618–19, 687 computer analysis 398 – 9 key points 397 MANOVA and 379 – 80, 387 – 9 reporting your findings 396 research examples 397 stepwise 396 using 389 – 96 discriminant score 687 distinguishing between categories/groups see multinomial logistic regression distorted curves 62 – 4 kurtosis (steepness/shallowness) 62, 63 – 4 skewness 62 –3 distributions of scores see shapes of distributions of scores disturbance term 462 Dix, D. 130 Docking, K. 175 Donnerstein, E. 228 Douglas, K. M. 193 Drees, M. J. 175 dummy coding 687 dummy variables 617, 687 Dumont, K. 294 Duncan multiple range test 327, 329 Dunfield, K. A. 350 Dunn, J. G. H. 420 Durding, B. M. 68 Edenfield, J. L. 278 Edwinston 581 effect size 487–94, 687 analysis of variance (ANOVA) 492 –3 approximation for nonparametric tests 492 chi-square 490 key points 494 meta-analysis 498, 501–5 method and statistical efficiency 421 Pearson correlation coefficient as 498 research examples 493 – 4 statistical power analysis 569, 571–3, 576 –7 statistical significance 488 – 9 in studies 490–1 t-test 490–1 effects of different characteristics of studies 499 –500

11/14/13 8:52 AM

Index 703



eigenvalues 429–30, 687 Elcheroth, G. 39 Emami, H. 175 Emery, Patrick J. 512 endogenous variable 462, 687 Engedal, K. 526 equal frequencies model 592, 593 – 4, 600–1 proportionate frequencies 593 equal-interval measurement 23, 25, 26 Erdfelder, E. 583 Estevis, E. 366 estimated standard deviation 76 estimated standard deviation and standard error, degrees of freedom 160 eta 492–3, 687 Evartt, David L. 512 exact significance 687 exogenous variable 462, 687 exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis 394, 434–6, 687 computer analysis 441–2 exponent 687 extraction 687 Eysenck, Hans J. 10, 425 Eysenck, S. B. G. 10 F-distribution table of significance values 679 – 81 F-ratio 374, 376, 378, 687 significance 327 see also variance ratio test factor 687 factor analysis 10, 389, 423–43, 687 computer analysis 441–2 concepts 427 – 9 data issues in 426 decisions 429 –34 exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis 434 – 6, 441–2 history 425 key points 440 literature example 436 – 8 reporting results 439 research examples 439 – 40 second-order 430 factor loadings 428 – 9 factor matrix 687 factor scores 687 factorial ANOVA 687 factorials 207 family error rate 687 Farajzadegan, Z. 175 Faul, F. 583 Fayed, N. 130 Fernández-Calvo, B. 82 Fidell, L. S. 385, 400, 443, 459 Figini, D. 39 findings 627 see also meta-analysis; statistical power analysis Fischbein, R. L. 646 Fisher exact probability test 206 – 9, 213 research examples 213 –14 Fisher’s z 504 Fisher test 687

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 703

Fitneva, S. A. 350 Ford, S. 646 Frank, G. K. W. 278 frequencies 23, 24, 687 computer analysis 69 –70 percentage 33 – 4 simple 33 frequency curves 64 –7 bimodal and multimodal frequency distributions 64 cumulative frequency curves 64 – 6 percentiles 66 –7 frequency data see chi-square frequency distribution 687 Freund, P. A. 511 Friedman test 249, 656–7, 687 computer analysis 658 – 9 Fritz, C. O. 68 G*Power 499, 575, 577 – 81, 583 –5 Gallagher, P. 130 Gander, P. H. 350 Gannon, T. A. 154, 397 Gardner, R. C. 5 Garfield, J. 7 Geenen, R. 114 General Linear Model (GLM) 123 generalising and inferring see samples and populations Gervais, S. J. 493 Gibbs, S. 439 Gillis, J. S. 113 Gips, J. 67 Glantz, S. A. 349, 353, 369, 459 Glass, Gene V. 512 Goni, M. 214 Gonzalez, V. M. 646 goodness-of-fit 590, 687 Gordon, S. 4, 5 Gosset, William 7 – 8 Gotwals, J. K. 420 graph 687 Gray, H. M. 397 Green, P. 82 –3, 581 Griffin, B. 628 Gromoske, A. N. 472 grouping variable 687 Groves, A. 39 Guzman, J. F. 382 Gwandure, C. 192 Hair, J. F., Jr 385, 400 Halligan, P. 440 Halpole, L. L. 162 Hanna, D. 7 Hannaford, P. C. 249, 538 Hardie, S. M. 366 Hardy, C. 162 Harinck, F. 322 harmonic mean 687 Hartman, M. 420 Heisey, D. M. 575 Help 687 Helvik, A.-S. 526 Hesketh, B. 628

11/14/13 8:52 AM

704

Index

Hewell, V. M. 646 hierarchical agglomerative clustering 687 – 8 hierarchical entry 688 hierarchical models 610 hierarchical multiple regression approach to identifying moderator effects 545 –55 computer analysis 473 –5 hierarchical regression 688 hierarchical selection 449 histograms 37–8, 688 compound 93 – 4 and frequency curves 59 – 60 Hobfoll, S. E. 472 Hoenig, J. M. 575 Hofer, M. 472 Hoicka, E. 236 Holland, S. 115, 559 homogeneity of regression slope 688 homogeneity of variance 688 homoscedasticity 688 Horselenberg 55 Hotelling’s trace 376, 378 Hotelling two sample t2 372 Howard, R. 646 Howell, D. 336, 339 Howitt, D. 3, 418, 477, 496, 507, 510, 512, 514, 583, 616 Huan, V. S. 130, 455, 538, 559 Hubbard, T. L. 439 – 40 Hughes, J. S. 213 Huguley, J. P. 559 Huisman, A. 213, 538, 628 Huitema, B. E. 364 Hunter, P. G. 294 hypothesis 688 icicle plot 688 identification 467, 688 independence 688 independent groups design 688 independent t-test 688 independent variable 688 inference see statistical significance of correlation coefficient inferential statistics 20, 136, 688 Ingravallo, F. 526 initial variable classification 480–1 interaction graph 688 interactions 590, 593, 688 moderator effects 546, 547 – 8 internal consistency of scales and measurements 517 interquartile range 49, 50, 688 inter-rater reliability 522 –5 interval data 688 interval measurement 23, 25, 26 interval scores 12 –13 Introduction to Research Methods in Psychology 510 Introduction to SPSS Statistics in Psychology xxv, 583, 616 item analysis using item–total correlation 517 –18 iteration 433 Ivancevich, J. M. 334 Jackson, A. P. 472 Jafari, N. 175

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 704

Jenkins, P. E. 95, 278 Jex, S. M. 559 Johnston, F. A. 226 Johnston, S. A. 226 Judica, A. 82 just-identified model 467, 688 Kaiser or Kaiser–Guttman criterion 688 Kam, L. Y. K. 294 kappa coefficient calculation 523 –5 computer analysis 527 – 8 research examples 526 Karageoghis, C. I. 582 Kasten, N. 511 Kendall’s tau 688 Kenne, D. R. 646 Kenny, D. A. 555 Kenyon, M. 55, 67, 114, 249 Kerlinger, F. N. 223 Kilian, B. 472 Kingston, K. 382 Kirkham 581 Klaassen, R. 130 Klassen, A. F. 130 Klimoski, R. J. 455 Kline, P. 443 Kneckt, P. 526 Kogan, S. M. 213 –14, 628 – 9 Kois, L. 214 Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for two samples 688 Kruskal–Wallis test 249, 654–6, 688 computer analysis 658 – 9 Kuhnle, C. 472 kurtosis (steepness/shallowness) 62, 63–4, 688 leptokurtic curve 63 mesokurtic curve 63 platykurtic curve 63 research examples 67 – 8 Laakso, M. L. 493 Laaksonen, M. A. 526 Lalleman, K. 55 Lalonde, R. N. 5 Lam, N. H. 350 lambda 610 Lamoureux, B. E. 472 Lampropoulos, G. K. 629 Lang, A.-G. 583 LaPlante, D. A. 397 large-sample formulae for nonparametric tests 652 –3 Mann–Whitney U-test 652 Wilcoxon matched pairs test 653 latent variable 688 Lautamo, T. 493 Law, K. 279 Lawson 581 learning statistics difficulties 5 –7 research 4 Lees-Haley, P. Lesher, K. 629 level 688

11/14/13 8:52 AM



levels of measurement 688 Levene’s test 376, 688 Levine, T. R. 494 likelihood ratio chi-square 591–2, 688 Likert questionnaires 36 –7 limitations of statistics 10–12, 13 Lindfors, O. 526 Lindholm, B. W. 334 line graph 688 linear association or relationship 688 linear model 688 Ling, J. 55 Linley, P. A. 68 Lipsey, M. W. 576 LISREL 688 Livianos, L. 95, 279 loading 688 log likelihood 688 log-linear methods 589 – 613 analysis 608–9, 689 computer analysis 612 –13 differences between Pearson and likelihood ratio chi-square 591–2 goodness-of-fit 590 interactions 590 likelihood ratio chi-square 591–2 models 590 natural logarithm 591 Pearson chi-square 590, 591–2 research examples 611 see also analysis of complex contingency tables logarithm 6, 689 Loghmani, A. 175 logistic regression procedure 640–4, 689 backwards elimination analysis 642 –3 logit 625, 635 – 6 López-Rolón, A. 82 Lounsbury, J. W. 114, 456 Louvet, E. 154 Louw, J. 294 Lowe, P. 382 MacCabe, J. H. 278 – 9 Mack, M. G. 175 Maguire-Jack, K. 472 main effects model 592–3, 594–9, 602–3, 689 Maiscuilo, L. A. 252 manifest variable 689 Mann–Whitney U-test 246–8, 506, 652, 689 computer analysis 250–2 effect size 492 table of significance 676 – 8 MANOVA see multivariate analysis of variance marginal totals 689 Marwitz, J. H. 629 matched sets 284 –5 matched-subjects design 168, 689 matching 167 – 8 mathematical ability 5 –7 mathematics anxiety 5 – 6 matrix 689 Matthews, N. L. 214 Mauchly’s test 689

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 705

Index 705

maximum likelihood method 689 Maxwell, A. E. 217 McCoy, K. D. 397 McFadden’s r2 622, 623 McGorry, P. 39 McKiernan, A. 214, 294 –5 McLemore, C. 162 McNemar test 212, 242, 689 McSweeney, M. 252 mean 689 mean deviation 51 mean square 689 mean, median and mode 46 –50 arithmetic mean 46 –7 comparison 50 median 47 – 8 mode 48 – 9 measure of dispersion 689 measurement theory interval/equal-interval measurement 23, 25, 26 nominal categorisation 23, 24 –5, 26 ordinal (rank) measurement 23, 25, 26 ratio measurement 23 – 4, 25, 26 measurement types 22 – 6 measurement theory 23 – 6 nominal/category measurement 22 score/numerical measurement 22, 24 median 47–8, 689 mediator variables 413–14, 541–3, 689 Meeten, F. 55, 95, 236, 279 Mercer, S. H. 162 meta-analysis 495 –514 calculator 512 comparison of studies 510 computer analysis 512 –13 difficulties 496 –7 effects of different characteristics of studies 499 –500 example 506 – 9 first steps in meta-analysis 501–5 key points 512 objectives 496 other measures of effect size 498 – 9 Pearson correlation coefficient as effect size 498 reporting results 510–11 research examples 511 Meta-Analyst 512 Meta-Stat 512 Meyer, C. 95, 278 Mitchell, R. R. 162 Mitsumatsu, H. 230 MIX 512 mixed ANOVA 689 mixed designs and repeated measures 689 fixed vs random effects 339 risks in related subjects designs 349 –50 mode 48–9, 689 model 590, 643 model building 10 moderator variables and effects 413–14, 540–61, 689 ANOVA approach 555 – 8 calculation: identifying moderator effects using ANOVA approach 556 – 8

11/14/13 8:52 AM

706

Index

moderator variables and effects (continued) calculation: identifying moderator effects using hierarchical multiple regression approach 549 –55 computer analysis 560–1 hierarchical multiple regression approach 545 –55 key points 559 research design issue 548 research examples 559 statistical approaches 545 Morris, P. E. 68 Motes, M. A. 439 – 40 Mulrine, H. M. 350 multicollinearity 453, 617, 689 multifactorial ANOVA 332 –3 multimodal 689 multinomial logistic regression 614 –31 change in –2 log likelihood 624 computer analysis 630–1 discriminant function analysis 618 –19 dummy variables 617 findings 627 key points 629 pattern of variables 616 prediction 625 –7 prediction accuracy 621–2 predictors 622 –5 reporting findings 628 research examples 628 – 9 score variables 616 uses 618 –19 Wald statistic 627 worked example 620–1 multiple control variables 417 first-order partial correlation 417 second-order partial correlation 417 zero-order correlation 417 multiple correlation see multiple regression and multiple correlation multiple items to measure same variable 407 multiple regression and multiple correlation 388, 444–59, 689 computer analysis 457 – 8 hierarchical selection 449 key points 456 literature example 454 –5 multicollinearity 53 prediction and 453 regression equations 447 –-9 reporting results 454 research design issues 449, 450–1 research examples 455 – 6 selection 449 –50 setwise selection 449 stepwise selection 449, 450, 451–2, 457 – 8 theory 445 –51 multiple responses 30 multiplication rule 220, 222 multivariate 689 multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) 370–85, 689 combining dependent variables 373 computer analysis 383 –5 discriminant function analysis and 379 – 80 key points 382

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 706

reporting findings 381 research examples 382 vs several ANOVAs 373 two stages 374 –5 using 376 – 81 multivariate tests 376 – 8 Munford, M. B. 454 –5 Munro, N. 175 Murphy, K. R. 576 Murray, R. A. 350 Mutsvunguma, P. 192 Myors, B. 576 Nagelkerke’s R2 622, 623, 644, 689 Nair, U. S. 420 Napierian logarithms see natural logarithms Napolitano, M. A. 420 natural logarithms 591, 635, 636, 689 Poisson distribution 636 negative (–) values 6, 52, 81 nested model 689 Neuman–Keuls test 327 Nicholas, M. K. 456 Niemeier, J. P. 629 nominal categories 91–3 nominal categories/numerical scores 93 – 4 compound histogram 93 – 4 crosstabulation tables 94 nominal categorisation 23, 24 –5, 26 nominal (category) data 32 – 6 bar charts 34, 35 – 6 frequencies 32 percentage frequencies 33 – 4 pie diagrams 34 –5 simple frequencies 33 nominal variables see category variables nonparametric statistical tests 13, 241–8, 689 effect size 492 related samples 241– 6 unrelated samples 246 – 8 see also large-sample formulae for nonparametric tests nonparametric statistics see ranking tests nonparametric tests for three or more groups 654 – 9 computer analysis 658 – 9 Friedman three or more related samples test 656 –7, 658 – 9 Kruskal–Wallis three or more unrelated conditions test 654 – 6, 658 – 9 non-recursive relationships 462 normal curve 12, 60–1, 689 research design issue 61 Norman, G. J. 130 null hypothesis 146 – 8 number of factors 430–1 numeric variables 689 numerical indexes 45 numerical mean see arithmetic mean numerical score data 36 – 8 bands of scores 37 – 8 histogram 37 – 8 numerical scores 89 – 91 scattergram 89 – 91

11/14/13 8:52 AM

Index 707



oblique factors 689 oblique rotation 430 observed power 574 –5 odds 689 odds ratio 635, 689 Oltmanns, T. F. 114 one-tailed test 689 – 90 one-tailed vs two-tailed significance testing 232 –7 computer analysis 237 further requirements 235 – 6 key points 237 research examples 236 theory 233 –5 ordinal data 690 ordinal (rank) measurement 12 –13, 23, 25, 26 orthogonal 690 orthogonal factors 690 orthogonal rotation 430 Otgaar, R. 55 outcome variable 690 outliers, identifying 49, 690 output window 690 over-identified model 467, 690 Oyebode, J. 646 paired comparisons 690 Palmieri, P. A. 472 parameters 140, 690 parametric 690 parametric tests 239, 690 Parker, A. 115, 559 Parsons, T. D. 582 part correlation 690 partial correlation 411–22, 690 calculation 419 calculation: partial correlation coefficient 415 calculation: statistical significance of partial correlation 416 –17 computer analysis 421–2 interpretation 416 key points 420–1 multiple control variables 417 research design issue 413 research examples 418, 420 student example 419 –20 suppressor variables 417 –18 theory 413 –14 participant 690 Passmore, J. 192 –3 PASW Statistics 690 path analysis 460–75 computer analysis 473 –5 generalisation 466 –7 key points 473 path coefficients 463 – 6 reporting results 471–2 research design issue 467 research examples 468 –71, 472 theory 461–7 path coefficients 463 – 6 path diagram 690 pathway 690 pattern of variables 616

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 707

Pearson, J. 214 Pearson chi-square 590, 591–2, 686 Pearson correlation coefficient 99, 104 – 6, 148 –50, 501, 535 – 6 calculation 104 – 6, 149 –50 critical values 149 as effect size 490, 498 extended table of significance 660–2 research examples 114 –15 statistical power analysis 571, 572, 573 statistical significance of 149 –50, 154 see also correlation coefficients Pechey, R. 440 Pedhazur, E. J. 459, 475 percentage frequencies, calculation 33 – 4 percentiles 66 –7 Perlman, D. 295 Peters, M. 68 phi 690 pictogram 36 pie diagrams 34 –5 Pillai’s trace 376, 378 pivot table 690 planned vs a posteriori (post hoc) comparisons 329–30, 690 Plomin, R. 95, 261 point-biserial correlation 690 point estimates 530 Poisson distribution 636 populations 690 see also samples and populations post hoc statistical power analysis 573 –5 post hoc test 690 Potter, G. G. 420 Powell, B. 439 power 687, 690 see also statistical power analysis Power, M. J. 278 Pradat-Diehl, P. 154 prediction 625 –7 accuracy 621–2 see also regression predictors 622 –5 pre-test/post-test design 339 – 40 principal component analysis 690 probability 218 –23 calculation: addition rule 221 calculation: multiplication rule 222 implications 221 key points 223 principles 219 –20 regression to the mean 219 repeated significance testing 221 significance testing across different studies 221 probability distribution 690 promax 690 pseudo r2 statistics 622–3, 644 quantitative research 690 quartimax 690 questionnaire/survey project 476 – 84 Ramos, F. 82 random effects 339

11/14/13 8:52 AM

708

Index

random samples 137, 138 – 40 computer analysis 141–2 standard error 139 randomisation 690 range 50, 690 rank measurement (ordinal) 23, 25, 26 ranking tests 238 –52 calculation: Mann–Whitney U test 246 – 8 calculation: sign test 242 –3 calculation: Wilcoxon matched pairs test 244 –5 computer analysis 250–2 key points 250 nonparametric statistical tests 241– 8 parametric tests 239 research examples 249 theory 239 – 41 three or more groups of scores 249 Rastle, K. 511 ratio data 690 ratio measurement 23 – 4, 25, 26 ratio scores 12 –13 reciprocal relationships 462 recode 690 regression 120–32, 444 –59, 536 –7 calculation 126 –7 calculation: confidence intervals for predicted score 536 –7 computer analysis 131–2 equations 124 – 9 formula 644 –5 key points 130 line 122 –3, 124 –5 to the mean 219 research design issues 125, 128 research examples 130 standard error 128 – 9 see also multiple regression and multiple correlation regression coefficient 690 regression equations 124 – 9, 447 – 9 least squares solutions 124 Rehman, H. 192 –3 related factorial design 690 related measures designs 167, 690 related research designs 14 related samples sign test 242 –3 Wilcoxon matched pairs test 243 – 6 related t-test (correlated t-test) 166 computer analysis 177 – 8 relationship between significance and confidence intervals 533 – 6 calculation: confidence intervals for population mean based on single sample 533 calculation: confidence intervals for unrelated t-test 534 calculation: related t-test 534 –5 calculation: Pearson correlation coefficient 535 – 6 relationships between variables 86 – 97 computer analysis 96 –7 diagrammatic and tabular presentation 88 key points 95 nominal categories 91–3 nominal categories/numerical scores 93 – 4 numerical scores 89 – 91 research examples 95

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 708

reliability in scales and measurement 515–28, 690 agreement between raters 522 –5 alpha reliability 519 –21 calculation: kappa coefficient 524 –5 calculation: split-half reliability 519 computer analysis 527 – 8 internal consistency of scales and measurements 517 item analysis using item–total correlation 517 –18 key points 526 research examples 526 split-half reliability 518 –19 repeated measures ANOVA 690 repeated measures designs 167, 691 repeated significance testing 221 reporting findings 381, 396, 645 results 439, 454, 471–2, 510–11, 581, 610, 628 significance levels see significance level reporting research hypothesis 479 – 80 project 477 – 9 research methods and statistical efficiency 421 residual 462, 594, 608–9, 691 residual sum of squares 691 Ridenour, T. A. 397 Rienecke Hoste, R. 95, 278 Rippeth, J. D. 582 risks in related subjects designs 349 –50 Robins, T. H. 611 Roche, B. 261 Rohling, M. 82 –3 Rohmer, O. 154 Rojo, L. 95, 279 Rosenthal, J. A. 227 Rosenthal, R. 514 rotated or unrotated factors 429–30, 691 Rothbard, N. 39 rounding errors 192 Rowe, M. L. 230 Roy’s largest root 376, 378 Rudner, Lawrence M. 512 Ruggeri, K. 7 Ruscio, J. 261 Rypma, B. 439 – 40 sample size 8 – 9 samples 20, 691 development of sampling 7 – 8 samples and populations 135 – 42 computer analysis 141–2 confidence intervals 140 inferential statistics 136 key points 140 random samples 138 – 40 theory 136 – 8 sampling distribution 691 Saraydarian, L. 214 saturated model 594, 601, 691 scattergram 691 computer analysis 118 –19 crosstabulation (contingency) tables 91 frequencies 90 overlaps 90 regression line 90 Schau, C. 5

11/14/13 8:52 AM



Scheffé test 329, 330–2, 691 Schimmack, U. 582 Schlauch, R. C. 295 Schneider, M. K. 629 Schreurs, B. G. 39 Schruijer, S. G. L. 321–2 Schulenberg, S. E. 193 Schwarzer, Ralf 512 score/numerical measurement 22, 24 score variables 14 scores 46 –50 central tendency 46 logistic regression 616, 640, 691 see also shapes of distributions of scores scree test 431, 691 Sedlmeier, P. 511 Sefl, T. 611 Selbæk, G. 526 select cases 691 semi-partial correlation 690 sequential entry 688 setwise selection 449 Shaffer, H. J. 397 Shafran, R. 68, 249 shapes of distributions of scores 58 –70 computer analysis 69 –70 distorted curves 62 – 4 histograms and frequency curves 59 – 60 key points 68 normal curve 60–1 other frequency curves 64 –7 research examples 67 – 8 Shepherd, A. M. 39 Sherry, P. 611 Siegel, S. 252 Sierra, P. 95, 279 sign test 241, 242–3, 246, 691 extended table of significance 670–2 Signal, T. L. 350 significance level reporting 224–31, 691 APA recommended practice 229 computer analysis 231 key points 230 research examples 226 – 8, 230 shortened forms 225 – 6 significance testing 8, 109, 133 –252 across different studies 221 chi-square 196 –217 one-tailed vs two-tailed 232 –7 probability 218 –23 ranking tests 238 –52 samples and populations 135 – 42 significance levels 224 –31 standard error 157 – 64 statistical significance of correlation coefficient 143 –56 t-test: correlated/related scores 165 –78 t-test: unrelated/uncorrelated scores 179 – 95 simple logistic regression 634–6, 691 Simpson, S. 582 Singhal, A. 294 Skancke, R. H. 526 skewness 62–3, 691 negative skew 62

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 709

Index 709

positive skew 62 research examples 67 – 8 see also testing for excessively skewed distributions Skinner, B. F. 39 Skipper, Y. 193 Slinker, B. K. 349, 353, 369, 459 Sliter, M. T. 559 Smith 581 Smith-Bell, C. A. 39 sort cases 691 Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient 109–13, 152–3, 241, 691 calculation: with no tied ranks 112 –13 calculation: with/without tied ranks 110–11 research examples 114 statistical significance 152 –3, 154 table of significance 663 –5 see also correlation coefficients Spenelli, D. 82 Spengler, P. M. 629 sphericity 691 Spini, D. 39 split-half reliability 518–19, 691 calculation 519 Sprung, J. M. 559 SPSS 15, 691 ANCOVA 367 – 9 binomial logistic regression 647 – 8 chi-square 215 –17 confidence intervals 539 correlated ANOVA 296 –7 correlation coefficients 116 –19 Cronbach’s alpha and kappa 527 – 8 crosstabulation and compound bar charts 96 –7 data entry basics 27 – 8 descriptive statistics 56 –7 discriminant function analysis 398 – 9 exploratory factor analysis 441–2 frequencies 69 –70 Friedman test 658 – 9 Kruskal–Wallis test 658 – 9 log-linear analysis 612 –13 MANOVA 383 –5 meta-analysis 512 mixed design ANOVA 351–3 moderator variables 560–1 multinomial logistic regression 630–1 multiple comparison tests 335 – 6 one-tailed vs two-tailed significance testing 237 one-way analysis of variance 280–1 partial correlation 421–2 path analysis 473 –5 random samples 141–2 ranking tests 250–2 regression 131–2 related (correlated) t-test 177 – 8 reliability in scales and measurement 527 – 8 scattergrams 118 –19 standard deviation and z-scores 84 –5 standard error 163 – 4 statistical significance 231 statistical significance of correlation coefficient 155 – 6 stepwise multiple regression 457 – 8

11/14/13 8:52 AM

710

Index

SPSS (continued) tables and diagrams 40–3 two-way analysis of variance 323 – 4 unrelated t-test 194 –5 variance ratio (F-ratio) test 262 –3 spurious correlation, third or confounding variables, suppressor variables see partial correlation spurious relationships 462 square root of a number 6 squared Euclidean distance 691 squaring a number 6 standard deviation 54, 71–85, 186–8, 691 calculation 74 –5 calculation: converting score into z-score 77 calculation: table of standard normal distribution 79 – 80 computer analysis 84 –5 estimated standard deviation 76 key points 83 research examples 82 –3 standard normal distribution 78 – 81 theoretical background 72 – 6 z-score 76 –7 z-score: important feature 82 z-score: use 77 – 8 see also standard error standard entry 691 standard error 128–9, 157–64, 186–8, 530–1, 691 calculation 161 computer analysis 163 – 4 confidence interval 129 estimated standard deviation and standard error 159 – 62 key points 162 random samples 139 research examples 162 sampling distribution 159 t-test 170, 187 theory 158 – 9 standard normal distribution 78 – 81 calculation 79 – 80 standardisation, moderator effects 546 –7 standardised coefficients or weights 691 Stasiewicz, P. R. 295 statistical approaches to finding moderator effects 545 statistical efficiency and research methods 421 statistical inference see statistical significance of correlation coefficient statistical power analysis 562 – 85 calculating power 577 – 81 computer analysis 583 –5 effect size 571–3 key points 582 reporting results 581 research design issues 567 – 8, 571–3 research examples 582 Type I and II errors 564, 566, 567 – 9 types and limitations 573 –5 using 575 –7 statistical significance 488 – 9 statistical significance of correlation coefficient 143 –56 alternative hypothesis 146, 147 calculation: Pearson correlation coefficient 149 –50 computer analysis 155 – 6 key points 154

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 710

null hypothesis 146 – 8 Pearson correlation coefficient 148 –50 population 144, 146 research design issues 148 research examples 154 Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient 152 –3 theory 144 – 6 Type I error 151–2 Type II error 151–2 statistics 19 –28, 140 computer analysis 27 – 8 data exploration techniques 20–1 descriptive statistics 20, 31 inferential statistics 20 key points 26 measurement types 22 – 6 samples 20 variables and measurement 21–2 statistics and analysis of experiments 401– 8 checklist 402 –7 key points 408 Patent Stats Pack 402 research design issues 403, 407 special cases 407 Statistics Software for Meta-Analysis 512 stepwise discriminant function analysis 396 stepwise entry 691 stepwise multiple regression 451–2 computer analysis 457 – 8 stepwise selection 449, 450 Stoeber, J. 420 Stoll, O. 420 Straus, M. 418 Student t-test see t-test students and statistics 2 –3 sum of squares 266, 691 sunflowers 90 Sung, L. 130 suppressor variables 417 –18 Survey of Attitudes Toward Statistics 7 syntax 691 systematic reviews 496 Szostak, H. 78 t-test and correlation coefficient 13 development 8 effect size 490–1 extended table of significance 666 – 8 meta-analysis 506 table of significant values for multiple t-tests 682 – 4 t-test: correlated/related scores 165 –78 calculation 172 – 4 cautionary note 174 –5 computer analysis 177 – 8 degrees of freedom 170, 171 dependent and independent variables 168 key points 176 related (correlated) t-test 166, 177 – 8 repeated measures designs 167 research design issues 167 – 8 research examples 175 theory 169 –74

11/14/13 8:52 AM

Index 711



t-test: unrelated/uncorrelated scores 179 – 95 calculation 188 – 91 cautionary note 192 computer analysis 194 –5 key points 193 Mann–Whitney U-test 192 research examples 192 –3 rounding errors 192 standard deviation and standard error 186 – 8 theory 181–5 Tabachnick, B. G. 385, 400, 443, 459 tables and diagrams 29 – 43 computer analysis 40–3 nominal (category) data 32 – 6 numerical score data 36 – 8 using 39 see also relationships between variables Tatham, R. L. 385, 400 Taylor, J. S. 511 Teissedre, F. 114 Ternier-Thames, N. 611 test–retest reliability 691 Testa, M. 629 testing for excessively skewed distributions 649 –51 skewness 649 –50 standard error of skewness 650–1 Thompson, P. C. 611 three-variable example 509 – 609 data components 606 – 8 equal frequencies model 600–1 frequencies 599 – 600 log-linear analysis 608 – 9 main effects model 602 –3 saturated model 601 two-variable interactions 604 – 6 Tinsley, H. E. A. 528 Todaro, L. 611 Törmäkangas, K. 493 Touliatos, J. 334 Towl, G. 212 Tracey, T. J. 611 Trafimow, D. 213 transformation 691 Tremont, G. 83 trends in data 9 –10 Troster, A. I. 582 two-tailed test 691 two-variable example 592 – 9 equal frequencies model 592, 593 – 4 interactions 593 main effects model 592 –3, 594 – 9 saturated model 594 two-way relationships 462 Type I error 151–2, 373, 691 statistical power analysis 564, 566, 567 – 9 Type II error 151–2, 691 statistical power analysis 564, 566, 567 – 9, 571 Tyson, P. 279 under-identified model 467, 691 unique variance 692 univariate 692 unplanned comparisons 692

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 711

unrelated research designs 14 unrelated samples 246 – 8 Mann–Whitney U-test 246 – 8 unstandardised coefficients or weights 692 uses of statistics 3 Vallat-Azouvi, C. 154 value label 692 van den Berg, M. J. 350 van Kampen, R. 55 van Kleef, G. A. 322 van Middendorp, H. 114 vanSchaik, P. 55 variability 50–5 calculation: variance using computation formula 53 interquartile range 49, 50 mean deviation 51 standard deviation 54 using negative (–) values 52 variance 51– 4 variable label 692 variable name 692 Variable View 692 variables 29 – 43 calculation: percentage frequencies 33 – 4 calculation: slices for pie diagram 34 computer analysis 40–3 errors to avoid 39 key points 40, 55 and measurement 21–2 raw data 30 research design issue 30 statistics 30 tables and diagrams 32 – 9 using graphs and tables 39 see also averages, variation and spread variance 45, 265–6, 692 estimate 54, 692 variance analysis 253 – 408 analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) 354 – 69, 372 analysis of variance (ANOVA) 264 – 81 analysis of variance (ANOVA): correlated scores/repeated measures 282 – 97 analysis of variance (ANOVA): mixed design 337 –53 analysis of variance (ANOVA): multiple comparisons 326 –36 analysis of variance (ANOVA): one-way unrelated/ uncorrelated ANOVA 264 – 81 analysis of variance (ANOVA): two-way for unrelated/ uncorrelated scores 298 –325 discriminant function analysis 386 – 400, 618 –19 multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) 370– 85 statistics and analysis of experiments 401– 8 variance ratio test 255 – 63 variance–covariance matrix 16, 692 variance ratio test 255–63, 692 calculation 258 – 60 computer analysis 262 –3 key points 261 research examples 261 theory and application 257 – 60 Varimax 692

11/14/13 8:52 AM

712

Index

Vassari, M. 526 Vazire, S. 114 Vescio, T. K. 493 Vista, A. 261 Wagner, U. 468 –72 Wald statistic 627, 692 Walker, W. 629 Wang, M.-T. 559 Ward, T. 420 Warren, C. S. 115, 559 Wasco, S. 611 Wegener, D. T. 350 weights 692 Weiss, D. J. 528 West, S. G. 548, 550, 554, 555, 561 Whiteford, H. 39 Wickett, J. C. 95 Wickham, L. H. 68 Wilcoxon matched pairs test 241, 243 – 6, 653 computer analysis 250–2 table of significance 673 –5 Wilcoxon signed-rank test 692 Wilk, S. L. 39 Wilkes, S. 175

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 712

Wilks’ lambda 376, 378, 392–3, 692 Williamson, C. B. 611 Wilson, D. B. 576 Wilson, K. 279 Windsor, M. A. 611 within-subjects design 692 Woods, S. P. 582 Wright, L. 366 Wyrick, D. L. 322 Yates’s correction 200, 692 Yildirim, I. 334 Yutrzenka, B. A. 193 z-scores 76–8, 81, 168–9, 502–5, 692 calculation: converting score into z-score 77 computer analysis 84 –5 important feature 82 research examples 82 –3 use 77 – 8 Zamani, A. 175 Zick, A. 468 –72 Ziegler, R. H. 559 Zimprich, D. 7 Zoccolotti, P. 82

11/14/13 8:52 AM

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 713

11/14/13 8:52 AM

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 714

11/14/13 8:52 AM

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 715

11/14/13 8:52 AM

Z15_HOWI0749_06_SE_IDX.indd 716

11/14/13 8:52 AM