Handbook for Working with Children and Youth: Pathways to Resilience Across Cultures and Contexts [1 ed.] 9781452214658, 9781412904056

The Handbook For Working With Children & Youth: Pathways To Resilience Across Cultures and Contexts examines lives l

334 22 43MB

English Pages 553 Year 2005

Report DMCA / Copyright

DOWNLOAD FILE

Polecaj historie

Handbook for Working with Children and Youth: Pathways to Resilience Across Cultures and Contexts [1 ed.]
 9781452214658, 9781412904056

Citation preview

Handbook for

WORKING ^CHILDREN and

Youm

Handbookfor

WORKING ^CHILDREN Pathways to Resilience Across Cultures and Contexts

EDITOR

MICHAEL UNGAR Dalhousie University SAGE Publications ^55' Thousand Oaks • London • New Delhi

Copyright © 2005 by Sage Publications, Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher. For information: Sage Publications, Inc. 2455 Teller Road Thousand Oaks, California 91320 E-mail: [email protected] Sage Publications Ltd. 1 Oliver's Yard 55 City Road London EC1Y ISP United Kingdom Sage Publications India Pvt. Ltd. B-42, Panchsheel Enclave Post Box 4109 New Delhi 110 017 India

Printed in the United States of America on acid-free paper Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication data Handbook for working with children and youth : pathways to resilience across cultures and contexts / edited by Michael Ungar. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 1-4129-0405-6 (cloth) 1. Resilience (Personality trait) in children. 2. Resilience (Personality trait) in adolescence. 3. Resilience (Personality trait) in children—Cross-cultural studies. 4. Resilience (Personality trait) in adolescence—Cross-cultural studies. 5. Social work with children. 6. Social work with youth. I. Title: Pathways to resilience across cultures and contexts. II. Ungar, Michael, 1963BF723.R46H357 2005 362'.7—dc22 2004028633

05

06

07

08

Acquiring Editor: Editorial Assistant: Production Editor: Typesetter: Copy Editor: Indexer: Cover Designer:

09

10 9

8 7

6

5 4

Jim Brace-Thompson Karen Ehrmann Sanford Robinson C&M Digitals (P) Ltd. Linda Gray Karen A. McKenzie Michelle Kenny

3

2 1

CONTENTS Acknowledgments

ix

Foreword

xi

James

Garbarino

Introduction: Resilience Across Cultures and Contexts Michael

χν

Ungar

PART 1: THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES

1

1.

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations Jo Boyden and Gillian Mann

2.

Culture and Ethnic Identity in Family Resilience: Dynamic Processes in Trauma and Transformation of Indigenous People Laurie D. McCubbin and Hamilton I. McCubbin

27

Lessons Learned From Poor African American Youth: Resilient Strengths in Coping With Adverse Environments Joyce West Stevens

45

3.

3

4.

Gendered Adaptations, Resilience, and the Perpetration of Violence Jane F Gilgun and Laura S. Abrams

5.

The Theory of Resilience and Its application to Street Children in the Minority and Majority World Jacqueline McAdam-Crisp, Lewis Aptekar, and Wanjiku Kironyo

71

Beyond Resilience: Blending Wellness and Liberation in the Helping Professions Isaac Prilleltensky and Ora Prilleltensky

89

6.

7.

8.

Community-Based Child Welfare for Aboriginal Children: Supporting Resilience Through Structural Change Cindy Blackstock and Nico Trocmé

105

Beetles, Bullfrogs, and Butterflies: Contributions of Natural Environment to Childhood Development and Resilience Fred H. Besthorn

121

PART 2: METHODOLOGICAL CHALLENGES IN RESILIENCE RESEARCH 9. 10.

57

133

Methodological Challenges in the Study of Resilience William H. Barton

135

Qualitative Resilience Research: Contributions and Risks Michael Ungar and Eli Teram

149

11.

Psychosocial Health in Youth: An International Perspective John C. LeBlanc, Pam J. Talbot, and Wendy M. Craig

165

12.

Resilience and Weil-Being in Developing Countries Laura Camfield and Allister McGregor

189

13.

The International Resilience Project: A Mixed-Methods Approach to the Study of Resilience Across Cultures Michael Ungar and Linda Liebenberg

211

PART 3: INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS

227

14.

Israeli Youth Cope With Terror: Vulnerability and Resilience Zahava Solomon and Avital Laufer

15.

Overcoming Adversity With Children Affected by HIV/AIDS in the Indigenous South African Cultural Context Philip Cook and Lesley du Toit

16.

Bent But Not Broken: Exploring Queer Youth Resilience Marion Brown and Marc Colbourne

17.

Psychosocial Functioning of Children From Monogamous and Polygamous Families: Implications for Practice Alean Al-Krenawi and Vered Slonim-Nevo

229

247 263

279

18.

Strengthening Families and Communities: System Building for Resilience Barbara J. Friesen and Eileen Brennan

19.

Professional Discourse of Social Workers Working With At-Risk Young People in Hong Kong: Risk or Resilience? Kwai-Yau Wong and Tak-yan Lee

313

Resilient Youth in North East India: The Role of Faith-Based Organizations in Communities Affected by Violence Jerry Thomas and George Menamparampil

329

Alternative Approaches to Promoting the Health and Weil-Being of Children: Accessing Community Resources to Support Resilience Ken Barter

343

Respecting Aboriginal Families: Pathways to Resilience in Custom Adoption and Family Group Conferencing Nancy MacDonald, Joan Glode, and Fred Wien

357

Social and Cultural Roots of Russian Youth Resilience: Interventions by the State, Society, and the Family Alexander V. Makhnach and Anna I. Laktionova

371

20.

21.

22.

23.

295

24.

Intercepts of Resilience and Systems of Care Mary I. Armstrong, Beth A. Stroul, and Roger A. Boothroyd

387

25.

Youth Civic Engagement: Promise and Peril Scot D. Evans and Isaac Prilleltensky

405

26.

Resilience in the Palestinian Occupied Territories Toine van Teeffelen, Hania Bitar, and Saleem Al-Habash

417

27.

Resiliency and Young African Canadian Males Wanda Bernard and David Este

28.

Violence Prevention Programming in Colombia: Challenges in Project Design and Fidelity Luis F. Duque, Joanne Klevens, Michael Ungar, and Anna W. Lee

433

455

Author Index

473

Subject Index

491

About the Contributors

501

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

T

his book wouldn't exist without the inspiration of some very special, and often ignored, individuals whom I have met during years of travel in countries like Pakistan, Malaysia, Egypt, Tanzania, Colombia, and Israel. I also owe much to children who I came to know in correctional facilities, community mental health clinics and child welfare agencies. Together these children taught me about what it takes to survive. This book, by pushing the bounds of accepted scholarship on resilience, honors their wisdom. More practically, though, this book owes much to the world-class mentors and colleagues whom I have counted as friends these past many years and whose work has helped to inform my own. Many of them are contributors; many others continue with their work, some recognized for their contributions, others largely unrecognized. Though too numerous to name, I hope they will see in this book reflections of what they have taught me during our many discussions. Closer to home, I would also like to acknowledge the help and patience of the research assistants and teaching assistants who have helped with the production of this work and the research projects on which it is based. These people

include Linda Liebenberg, Janus Siebrits, Andrea Gregus, Anna Lee, Rhonda Brophy, Susanna Steinitz, and Nora Didkowsky, among others. I am also indebted to the editorial staff at Sage for their invaluable assistance, most notably Jim Brace-Thompson, who saw in this project an opportunity to contribute to this field of study. Bringing this work to completion has also been greatly assisted by my family, who have joined me often in my rambles around the world in search of the inspiration for this volume. Cathy, my partner, and our two children, Scott and Mèg, have always been there for me, even when commitments to writing may have meant time away from them. Lastly, this work would never have appeared without the financial support of various granting agencies, including The Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and the Nova Scotia Health Research Foundation, as well as supplementary funding from the Canadian International Development Agency. Well-funded research has made it possible for me to enjoy the luxury of collaborations and innovation. I am indebted to those who contribute their time and leadership to those agencies.

ix

FOREWORD JAMES GARBARINO

R

eading through the manuscripts that constitute the chapters for this book causes me to reflect on what I have learned from almost three decades of experience as a researcher, educator, author, program consultant, and legal expert witness dealing with issues of resilience in the lives of children and youth. 1 have learned that human beings are indeed generally adaptive and resourceful but that resilience is not unlimited, automatic, or universal. Under conditions of numerous serious threats experienced in hostile environments ("risk accumulation"), no child may escape unscathed, no matter how well equipped the child may be temperamentally. Every child has limits. Much is made in the scientific literature and the popular press of resilience. Although it is defined in numerous ways, resilience generally refers to an individual's ability to bounce back from adverse experiences, to avoid long-term negative effects, or otherwise to overcome developmental threats. Every one of us knows someone whose life is a testament to resilience. The concept of resilience rests on a key research finding: Although experiencing any specific negative influence increases the odds of a particular negative outcome, most children escape severe harm. I have seen and heard resilience at work in youth prisons (Garbarino, 1999), in inner-city neighborhoods and early childhood programs (Garbarino, Dubrow, Kostelny, & Pardo, 1992), in war zones and refugee camps (Garbarino, Kostelny, & Dubrow, 1991), in families (Garbarino & Bedard, 2001), and in schools (Garbarino & deLara, 2002). However, as the concept of resilience has been promoted in ever-wider circles, there has

been a parallel concern that the concept may easily be misused or misunderstood. Four such limitations are of particular importance in our conceptual toolbox as we look at the lives of children and youth coping with adversity. First, we must remember that resilience is not absolute. Virtually every youth has a breaking point. Research conducted by psychiatrist Bruce Perry on the impact of trauma and deprivation on brain development leads him to assert that kids are "malleable" rather than "resilient," in the sense that each threat costs them something. What is more, as psychologist Patrick Tolan (1996) points out, in some environments, virtually all youth demonstrate negative effects from exposure to highly stressful and threatening environments. In his Chicago data, for example, none of the African American adolescent males facing a combination of highly dangerous and threatening low-income neighborhoods, coupled with abusive families, was resilient at age 15. In this case, resilience was measured by a child having completed a two-year period during which he or she was neither more than one grade level behind in school nor requiring professional mental health services to deal with psychological problems. These data, however, leave unanswered the question of how these youths perceived themselves. Some of them may well have defined their status as "normal" or "unaffected." Regardless of these selfperceptions, Tolan's data do provide a picture of their ability to cope with some important social realities of day-to-day functioning in America. Avshalom Caspi, Terrie Moffitt, and their colleagues (Caspi et al., 2002), provide another example in research that focused on the role xi

xii · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH of a genetically based difference in crucial neurotransmitters (chemicals in the brain that influence the processing of information and arousal). When the MAOA gene is "off," the child does not have the same level of the enzyme that enables these neurotransmitters (e.g., norepinephrine, serotonin, and dopamine) than when it is "on." If children have the MAOA gene turned off and are abused, about 85% develop a chronic pattern of aggression, violating the rights of others, and antisocial behavior (and can be diagnosed with "conduct disorder" if one is of a clinical bent). If kids are abused and the gene is turned "on" the figure is only about 40%. If the gene is turned off and the child is not abused, the rate of conduct disorder is about 20% (and 20% if the gene is on and there is no abuse). But beyond this childhood reality of almost total effect (85% of the children are diagnosable with conduct disorder), does the combination of early maltreatment and MAOA vulnerability lead to an adult life dominated by negativity? Are children resilient? An ecological perspective would predict that the answer to both questions, indeed to virtually all developmental questions, is "it depends." The effects of the childhood double whammy or the " o f f gene and the experience of abuse are much less dramatic when the researchers look at whether or not an individual is convicted of a violent crime and whether or not the individual demonstrates symptoms of antisocial personality disorder in adulthood. These results seem to indicate that although the pathway into conduct disorder in childhood is almost a given for children who get the double whammy of being abused and having the MAOA vulnerability, the pathway from there into an antisocial life in adulthood is subject to other influences. Second, we must remember that resilience in gross terms may obscure real costs to the quality of an individual's inner life. Some people manage to avoid succumbing to the risk of social failure as defined by poverty and criminality but nonetheless experience real harm in the form of diminished capacity for successful intimate relationships. Thus, even apparent social success—performing well in the job market, avoiding criminal activity, and creating a family—may obscure some of the costs of socially successful coping. The inner lives of

these individuals may be fraught with emotional damage—to self-esteem and the capacity for intimacy, for example. Although resilient in social terms, these individuals may be severely wounded souls. This has long been evident in comparing the resilience of boys versus girls. Boys who succumb to the accumulation of risk have long been prone to act out in explicitly antisocial behavior (juvenile delinquency), whereas girls have been more likely to respond with self-destructive behavior and internalized symptoms such as stomachaches, nightmares, and wretchedly low self-esteem. Does this mean girls are more resilient than boys? A simple accounting of social success variables might lead us to think so. However, if we take into account the full range of harm, we can see that such an answer would be wrong. Kids adapt: for better and for worse. Third, we must always be vigilant lest cultural bias and ethnocentrism cloud our assessments of resilience. Just as some individuals are socially successful yet spiritually and emotionally afflicted, so the reverse is true as well: Some individuals who are apparently social failures are actually spiritual and psychological successes. This is particularly true when there are cultural differences involved in the assessment of resilience. Sternberg (2004) and his colleagues have provided reliable and excellent evidence that although some cognitive abilities and processes are universally part of the foundation for intelligence, social and cultural context go a long way toward defining the meaning and manifestation of competence in the real lives of children and youth. This insight has several implications for our understanding of resilience. For one thing, it highlights the importance of knowing the ecology of the child or youth so that the meaning of "competence" and "coping" used in any assessment of resilience is situationally valid. Also, it warns us against the simplistic application of standards of competence from one ecological niche to another. Rather, as Sternberg so clearly demonstrates, it impels us to see the world through the eyes of the child and youth in any effort to assess resilience. What is more, it argues for "dynamic" assessments that illuminate the child's ability to learn to demonstrate competence in new settings rather than simply

Foreword · xiii taking as definitive assessments conducted at one time (and perhaps in one place). Indeed, this approach on assessment is wholly consistent with the very concept of resilience, which is not about the immediate destabilizing effects of trauma and other adverse experiences but, rather, the child's ability to learn how to move from these insults to a position of health and success (in terms that are meaningful culturally and phenomenologically). Fourth, we must not forget that "resilient" does not necessarily mean "morally superior." The youth who demonstrates resilience has extraordinary attributes and resources that the nonresilient child does not have. Being unable to protect oneself against the accumulation of risk factors does not constitute moral turpitude. Some environments are too much for anyone, and to use the concept of resilience as a basis for moral judgment in these settings may be inappropriate and unfair. I have seen this first-hand when testifying as an expert witness in youth homicide trials. In one case, although seeking to discredit my expert testimony bearing on the role of trauma in shaping youth behavior, the prosecutor used the concept of resilience in precisely this manner. In his cross-examination, he asked why the defendant was not as successful as other kids in difficult situations. His exact words were, "What's wrong with this boy that he is not resilient?" That's not fair and it's not good science. It leads to judging and blaming in ways that are themselves unethical and bad science. The burdens imposed on children who live with violence, particularly when accompanied by an accumulation of other risk factors are sometimes too heavy for

anyone to carry without psychological back breaking. Starting with compassion for this brings head and heart together. To study resilience, one should adopt a fundamental humility about oneself and one's culture and society and simultaneously a respect for the human strength of others. The chapters in this book take these four cautions seriously and offer a convincing demonstration that resilience is indeed "a many splendored thing."

REFERENCES Caspi, Α., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I., Taylor, Α., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851-854. Garbarino, J. (1999). Lost boys: Why our sons turn violent and how we can save them. New York: Free Press. Garbarino, J., & Bedard, C. (2001). Parents under siege: Why you are the solution and not the problem in your child's life. New York: Free Press. Garbarino, J., & deLara, E. (2002). And words can hurt forever: How to protect adolescents from bullying, harassment, and emotional violence. New York: Free Press. Garbarino, J., Dubrow, N., Kostelny, K., & Pardo, C. (1992). Children in danger: Coping with the consequences of community violence. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Garbarino, J., Kostelny, K., & Dubrow, N. (1991). No place to be a child: Growing up in a war zone. New York: Lexington Books. Sternberg, R. (2004). Culture and intelligence. American Psychologist, 59, 325-338. Tolan, P. (1996). How resilient is the concept of resilience? Community Psychologist, 29(4), 12-15.

INTRODUCTION: RESILIENCE ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS MICHAEL UNGAR

L

ike all works on resilience, this one too is a cultural artifact, the result of a shared set of beliefs, values, and ideologies found among a group of academics, child advocates, clinicians, and other specialists who share an interest in children's unique ways of protecting themselves when growing up amid adversity. Although I share with contributors to this volume the common goal of better understanding children, our multiple standpoints make me reticent to assert that there is any one pathway to health common to children globally. Instead, I must be satisfied with a collage of competing truths, each a vibrant local account of what we have come to think we know about children's well-being. The more we dialogue across social, cultural, and linguistic barriers, the more convinced I am of a plurality of possible ways to account for children's resilience. The best place to begin this exploration of pathways to resilience is close to home. I live on the East Coast of Canada. I am a white, heterosexual male. I earn a good income. I am able-bodied. The risks my family and I confront in our daily lives are negligible compared with those of many other families and their children living less than a mile from my home. Of course, the latchkey children across the street who come home after school to an empty house, who spend hours with video games, suffer their own risks that come with being emotionally neglected. Fortunately, as a community we have provided services and structures to, at the very least, meet these children's needs. There are schools they can attend, guidance counselors who check in

on them, government-funded social workers to investigate claims of neglect against their parents. There are also community programs, perennially underfunded, but nevertheless available. There are police, doctors, and emergency services close at hand. Farther from my home, there are children who face far different challenges. They still confront the same acute risks of my neighbors, experiencing episodes of abuse or the disruption of divorce. But their worlds provide more chronic stressors as well. For example, the African Nova Scotian communities a little north of where I live and across the harbor have experienced systematic discrimination and underservicing that have been an unfortunate part of their deep-rooted history. Their schools have historically been underfunded more than those in my neighborhood. Their access to health care is compromised by the poverty that results from prejudice. They are more likely to be the target of police checks and incarceration. These same prejudices led to the outright dislocation of the African Nova Scotian community in the 1960s to make way for a bridge over the harbor. Combined, these intrusions have taken their toll. Of course, I could widen the circle further. I could speak about street children in other parts of my community, youth who couch surf, drift between shelters, or when weather permits, populate the streets of every major city and small town in Canada. I could go further and paint simplistic pictures from my travels in Aboriginal communities in Canada and describe the challenges they face: the legacy of residential xv

xvi · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH schools that were a cultural genocide that has contributed to epidemics of substance abuse and suicide. I could move beyond my national borders and look to the United States with its structural inequities, the multiple risks of biological, psychological, emotional, and social factors confounding children's development. But why stop there? I could look overseas to the Middle East, Asia, South America, Africa, and even Eastern and Western Europe where the politics of hatred and prejudice, war, poverty, disability, and marginalization due to gender, sexual orientation, race, and ethnicity all combine to disadvantage children. I could easily paint a picture of a world of children at risk. This monochromatic view of children and families presents us with a singular and "thin" description of children's lives. Seldom do we hear accounts from children themselves. This is unfortunate because a quieter, less articulated version of children's lives speaks of resilience. It is a much more hopeful vision, one embraced by the authors of the chapters in this volume. If we look, we can find within each population of at-risk children aspects of healthy functioning that may or may not have been overlooked.

DISCOVERING RESILIENCE In the middle to late 1900s, a growing number of researchers such as Werner and Smith (1982), Rutter (Rutter, Maughan, Mortimore, & Ouston, 1979), Garmezy (1976), and Murphy and Moriarty (1976) began to structure longitudinal studies in Western contexts that found that an inconsistent and unpredictable number of children from at-risk populations presented with remarkably good mental and physical health outcomes despite the multiple disadvantages of structural, familial, and individual stressors. That body of work has become the basis for a burgeoning field of research into resilience among children and adults. It has provided clinicians, policymakers, child advocates, and researchers a different way of thinking about populations at risk. It has shown that some individuals do survive incredible hardship and that the uniqueness of their solutions may be invisible to outsiders to those lives. However, even as we have come to notice the health to be found among at-risk individuals, our definition of this

resilience has tended to assume a minority-world bias, that of the small, privileged part of our world that lives in relative affluence in Western democracies. In particular, most resilience literature comes from the Western-trained psychological and social service community. Within this narrow community, resilience has come to mean the individual capacities, behaviors, and protective processes associated with health outcomes despite exposure to a significant number of risks. Without risk, there is no resilience, only health of a different order. Although a good start, and a paradigmatic shift from a focus on the etiology of disease to the "etiology" of health, we have ignored the bias inherent in what we assume to be health indicators. A volume such as this, that places side by side so many different understandings of children's well-being, is a challenge to this homogenizing health discourse. This broadening of our perspective is analogous to what the theory of resilience has done to psychopathologizing discourses of well-intentioned professionals who nonetheless stigmatize at-risk populations with labels of dysfunction. This volume seeks to open to debate what is and is not a sign of health, the variety in the pathways children travel to well-being, and the theoretical and methodological challenges accounting for this plurality of perspectives internationally. This new ground can be charted, however, only because of the debt we owe to the resilience pioneers who shook us up with their vision of health hidden amid danger. Caution is needed when speaking of resilience. The discourse of resilience can be (has been?) co-opted by proponents of a neoconservative agenda that argue if one person can survive and thrive, then shouldn't the responsibility for success be on all individuals within populations at risk to do likewise? Are services really needed, or should people themselves be expected to follow the lead of the "invulnérables" and surmount their difficult life circumstances? It is a familiar twist on the "anyone can be president" myth. It denies the very real structural constraints on children's lives. Not all children have the constellation of capacities to succeed. Much less would there be the capacity of our society as it is structured to provide places for all children if they were to succeed. After all there is only one president every four years.

Introduction · xvii More invisibly, resilience research and interventions based on a resilience framework have presented models of successful growth and adaptation that are biased toward Western conceptions of healthy functioning, ignoring the arbitrariness in their selection of outcome variables. The bulk of the resilience literature is based on a Eurocentric view of the world. This view is a product of a philosophical enlightenment that has taken place over the last 400 years during which the world became a knowable, measurable, and predictable place. Arguably, the complexity of resilience, the myriad ways individuals, families, and communities overcome adversity, cannot be so simplified as to generate a single set of principles generalizable from one contextually specific study to the next. This is not to say we cannot embrace the tools of scientific inquiry in different contexts, seeking the gold standard of external validity for each population under study. We can assert, "For this one group of people, sharing these qualities, what our research shows is likely to be true." But to go further, to speak globally, when we are bound to act only locally, requires that we overstep the bounds of reasonableness. If we learn anything from working crossculturally, it is that to speculate on the commonality of people's experiences is to tumble into an abyss of uncertainty. Claims of external validity are now being challenged by those formerly marginalized by the process of research (minority groups, women, people with disabilities or illness). They are calling for authenticity, relevance, and the re-presentation of people's localized truths. Although I might be fairly certain that what I know about health may be relevant to the neglected children who live next to me, I am less certain of the veracity of what I know about what makes a child resilient when I encounter communities further afield, such as those of Canada's First Nations or those in countries that are less economically developed, although with cultural traditions and indigenous health practices far more ancient than my own. At a time when we are increasingly open to critical engagement between those marginalized and the elites who hold power over them, there is a need when studying resilience to understand the multiple pathways that children, their caregivers, and communities travel toward health. This book

is intended to broaden our understanding of how children, youth, and the adults who care for them sustain resilience in diverse cultures and contexts. In the process, it challenges the individualizing discourse of health, showing that resilience is embedded not only in psychological factors but also in the structures that support children's access to the resources they need to sustain well-being.

A POPULAR THEORY A burgeoning interest in the study of resilience has resulted in a fascination with lives lived well despite adversity. Television talk shows and bookstore shelves are full of tales of those who have survived well. They provide a picture of individuals who encounter any of a host of challenges and then marshal personal and social resources to overcome them. Eric Weihenmayer (2001), for example, in his biography of his life as a visually impaired mountain climber, demonstrates incredible resourcefulness and determination as he learns to conquer more and more difficult peaks, eventually reaching the summit of Mt. Everest, a metaphoric as much as a physical accomplishment. In a different vein, The Girl in the Picture: The Kim Phuc Story (Chong, 2000), documents the life of a 9-year-old girl badly burned during a napalm attack in Vietnam in 1974. A newspaper photo of Phuc shortly after the attack not only helped to end the war by raising awareness of what was happening but also brought her much-needed medical attention even as she was being exploited as a tool for government propaganda. Her story, too, is one of survival. To understand these lives lived well, one cannot, however, overlook the cultural, social, and structural forces at play. Each was provided with very real resources that contributed to survival. Each had access to opportunities. Each also had the intelligence and temperament to exploit those opportunities. A broad developmental perspective on resilience that can fully account for how children become resilient in multiple contexts and across cultures has yet to be fully articulated. A number of works from Western authors, such as Combrinck-Graham's (1995) Children in Families at Risk, Walsh's (1998) Strengthening

xviii · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Family Resilience, Luthar's (2003) Resilience and Vulnerability: Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities, and Greene's (2003) Resiliency, have alluded to the trajectories of at-risk children's growth and the protective factors that promote resilience. Each has contributed to our thinking about what creates healthy individuals and families. However, as helpful as these works have been, they have not demonstrated the plurality of pathways and the cultural embeddedness of how health is realized by children and families growing up under adversity. We need to take an approach more typical of McCubbin (McCubbin, Fleming, etal., 1998; McCubbin, Thompson, Thompson, & Fromer, 1998; McCubbin, Thompson, Thompson, & Futrell, 1999), Werner and Smith (1992, 2001), Glantz and Johnson (1999), and Johnson-Powell and Yamamoto (1997), all of whom have made more explicit the need for contextual and cultural specificity when studying, intervening, or theorizing resilience as a developmental process in at-risk populations.

THE LOCAL AND THE UNIVERSAL There are many branches to this unwieldy tree that makes up the field of resilience research. Fields as diverse as developmental psychology, international development, refugee studies, criminology, and child and youth care, among others, have shown an acceptance of the concept of resilience and produced bodies of literature congruent with its usage. We are now coming to understand perilous development in cultures under stress (Johnson-Powell & Yamamoto, 1997), just as we are the neurological markers of children who cope better with stress (Denenberg, 1999; Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000). However, this knowledge is seldom sufficiently contextualized to make it relevant to those whose worlds it purports to represent. Recipients of interventions based on theories unquestioningly embedded in a dominant Western psychological discourse of disease and psychopathology are routinely suspicious of concepts and interventions created by those well-intentioned "others" beyond their borders (temporal, geographic, and sociocultural) (Swartz, 1998). As Rapoport (1997) explains,

In the smaller scale societies in developing [sic] countries, the impingement of modern values has produced a spectrum of responses from reaction against what they perceive as alien to enthusiastic acceptance of what they perceive as modern and advantageous. There is still little known about how and why families respond in the way they do. Even less is known about the process of reconciling local traditional values with global values, (p. 75) A construct as complicated as resilience requires a critical deconstruction, with special emphasis placed on the social locations of those who hold the theory to be true. What then is the value of this thing we call resilience? If it is a product of discourse, a socially and contextually specific idea that is open for interpretation by those who use it around the world, then what can it offer health professionals and researchers? How can we say anything meaningful about children's pathways through adversity that will resonate with "truth" for everyone globally if resilience remains a concept controlled by Western professionals? These questions are answerable. In fact, if we turn to philosophers such as Hacking (1999), who has sought to understand how ideas such as health become accepted as true, we see that realities are fixed through their simple dayto-day ritualization, the everyday way in which lives are lived. Hacking illustrates his point with a critical examination of what are termed "paper crime waves," the excessive focus on a public event that exceeds the relative importance of the phenomenon to most people's lives. This media hyperbole leads people to exaggerate the occurrence of the event and gives rise to panic. In North America, we see this occurring with youth crime, which continues to decrease despite public perceptions to the contrary. Hacking cautions us, however, from treating such panics as simply social constructions. Such a simplistic and dismissive understanding, he says, overlooks the complexity of the relationships involved and the intricate power plays between individuals and institutions that sustain the definition of the problem. Opening up the concept of resilience to a similar critique helps us to avoid simplistic explanations that all expressions of children's survival are social constructions and therefore

Introduction · xix equally valid pathways to health. Promoting such social relativism is not the intention here. A population of children at some risk may sustain health in many different ways that reflect their access to the resources they need to create health. But the views of the dominant culture, frequently that found in the minority Western world, cannot be so easily dismissed as a paper tiger. Instead, we need to move beyond the dualism, a view of resilience as all about local context or all about grand metatheories that reflect the bias of their expert proponents. It is this same middle path that has been well charted by others such as Swartz (1998), who has examined mental health in the context of Southern Africa. To Swartz, the contextualization of mental health phenomena is essential because biomedical explanations of disorder alone are inadequate to account for people's experiences of illness worldwide. Mental health must be understood as more than an intrapsychic or psychiatric phenomenon. The difficulty will always be balancing the emic and etic perspectives of those who experience illness and those who diagnose and treat it. As Swartz observes, creating an antiracist psychiatry requires that we see beyond the empiricism of psychological theory. We need a new language that more than translates ideas across cultures but that relays differing constructions of what health means and how it is expressed. Specific to the problem of translation (in particular its failure), Swartz explains, If we hold the empiricist view, the task of translating is simply finding the appropriate words in different languages for feeling states. If on the other hand we hold the hermeneutic/ constructionist view, the task of translating is more complex: we have to consider the extent to which the act of translation implies the construction of a particular reality, (p. 29) None of this, however, opens the door to a complete relativism or an overprivileging of indigenous medicine. According to Swartz (1998), mental illness does exist in developing countries, is recognized by people's communities as a sign of illness, leads to stigmatization, and cannot always be cured through the use of indigenous methods. Naïveté under the guise of

cultural sensitivity simply re-creates the dualism between the minority and majority worlds, without appreciating any shared realities whatsoever. A better balance is required. In the chapters that follow, we move back and forth between the specific and the universal, between local knowledge and pan-global understandings. This dialogue is meant to draw a line in the sand, to find the ever-shifting place where local truths compete with global truths in a healthy dialectic. When it comes to children's survival, we need to understand their accounts of their experiences while aggregating what we know collectively to offer others at risk some signposts on an effective pathway to health. The challenge is to provide these signposts without privileging a Eurocentric understanding of resilience as universal. Of course, some aspects of resilience are so ubiquitous as to appear universal: We agree in most cultures and contexts not to do violence to one another (except in ways justified by the need for self-defense); we agree to share food with loved ones; we form attachments; we seek power over our lives and a position of recognition in our communities. These are all essential elements of resilience that appear in global studies on health. We can assert with confidence such truths as "universal by consent" (see Leonard, 1997). We can also simultaneously strive to show tolerance for a polyocular view of the world, encouraging transcultural exchanges that help us to see the varying degrees of relevance of many of our commonly held beliefs about what makes people healthy when exposed to risk. The juxtaposition in this book of differing theories, definitions, and interventions concerned with resilience demonstrates this ambivalence between the cold certitude of the enlightenment and the free-for-all of the postmodern. If we are to understand resilience better, we must open to scrutiny what we know and how we practice based on that knowledge. Health data are never neutral. One would, for example, hardly know that teenagers are acting more responsible sexually now than in any time since statistics have been gathered. The teen birth rate in the United States has dropped to 42.9 births per 1,000 women aged 15 to 19 (Childtrends, 2003). The drop has been seen in all racial and ethnic groups and in all regions of

xx · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH the United States. Furthermore, teens' self-reports of sexual activity have also declined slightly. Strange, then, that there is still a moral panic about teenagers and sexuality. Stranger still is that the rate of teen births during the 1950s and 1960s was twice what it is today, given perceptions that sexuality was more controlled during both those decades. What we fail to understand in a media awash with worry is that interventions and a widening social safety net are having the effect we anticipated. Clearly, in the context of where I live, we have the technologies required to help children grow up safer and to prevent their exposure to risk.

Is RESILIENCE RESEARCH FLAWED? If we are to make the study of resilience a legitimate and fundable endeavor, one that can offer a counterpoint to the study of disease and psychopathology, we will need to address its shortcomings. Critics contend that the concept of resilience may be nothing more than a tautology, a simplistic way of saying that whatever makes you stronger must necessarily be good. There is also the danger of identifying resilience in individuals we have already arbitrarily designated as successful by the design of our inquiry into their lives. If, for example, a child remains in school despite population-wide risks associated with dropping out, then we might argue the child is resilient. But what does such a distinction, the laying on of the label resilient, add to our understanding about children and health? We already, after all, have ample theories to explain why some children drop out and why others do not. With all the problems discussed above, it is not surprising to find some researchers abandoning the construct of resilience altogether. Tarter and Vanyukov (1999) characterize resilience as lacking heuristic value or practical usefulness because of its nonlinearity and failure to predict epigenetic trajectories through life. Their argument is ecological: "Successful or poor adjustment does not ultimately reside in some abstruse property of the person such as resilience but instead emanates from the interaction between the person's phenotype and environment" (p. 99).

Arguably, what the study of resilience as an overarching concept adds is the possibility to weave a tapestry of health-related phenomena that offers a paradigmatically different position from which to examine children at risk. Resilience researchers and clinicians look to those who succeed for clues to successful development rather than focusing on those who succumb to risk. When we investigate what makes someone strong instead of what causes weakness, we are more likely to identify that which bolsters health. Alleviating illness gets us only halfway to resilience. We might interrupt the course of a disease, but we fail to understand how individuals sustain health. This shift in focus is imperative if we are to study health rather than disorder. As Glantz and Sloboda (1999) explain, despite criticisms of resilience research, "It should not be discarded because it encourages an important focus on a real and important phenomenon" (p. 113). In our pursuit of the etiology of health, we encounter the multiple forces at play in the lives of those who survive and thrive. For example, in Canada, it is noteworthy that some children don't drop out of school when facing the combined threats of decreased job prospects or the systemic prejudice encountered by visible minorities, most notably Aboriginal and African Canadian youth. However, it is even more remarkable that children don't drop out when they must dodge bullets to get to school or resist the pull into street gangs and the money and status they bring when growing up in the poorest communities of Medellin, Colombia. Shifting our focus to health, we are given culturally embedded clues to survival strategies. Collectively, these strategies can help us understand where best to invest our limited social and financial capital. Because good interventions and policies tend to be built on enlightened science, it has fallen to researchers to demonstrate what resilience is and how it is nurtured. The problems of arbitrariness in what is measured and what is used as health outcomes and the difficulties of accounting for social and cultural variability are all aspects of resilience research that are not insurmountable. Even the skeptics are encouraging a muchneeded debate. I would agree with Glantz and Sloboda (1999) who write the following:

Introduction · xxi Unfortunately, the concept of resilience is heavily laden with subjective often unarticulated assumptions and it is fraught with major logical, measurement, and pragmatic problems. . . . We share many of these concerns. We find there is great diversity in the use of the concept; it is used variously as a quality, a trait, a process, or an outcome. We have identified few attempts to assess resilience in which measurement problems do not cloud or eclipse the findings. There is no consensus on the referent of the term, standards for its application, or agreement on its role in explanations, models, and theories. In sum, the problems and inconsistencies in measurements, findings, and interpretations in the published literature raise serious questions about the utility and heuristic value of the concept of resilience, (pp. 110-111) These are not necessarily arguments for abandoning the term, which, as Glantz and Sloboda (1999) note, is still much needed. They do, however, push us to find a heuristically useful understanding of resilience that is helpful for comprehending the way children live their lives day-to-day. There are many different hypotheses about what sustains resilience. Frequently, they are not well articulated, hidden beneath the fog of a dominant discourse that is more concerned with illness than health. For example, Loeber and Farrington (2000) note in their review of the factors contributing to juvenile delinquency: Some children engage in minor delinquent acts for excitement, adventure, or other emotions common among children. For these children, offending may be considered as part of the context of child development in which youngsters learn prosocial behaviors by trial and error, (p. 742) Although it is certain that for some of these children, these early offenses are "stepping stones in pathways to serious, violent, and chronic offending" (p. 743), for many others, these events do not predict future negative outcomes. It is intriguing that Loeber and Farrington find such problematic behaviors to be part of normative development in some contexts. The problem, as they explain, is that "currently we have few tools to distinguish between those young children who will continue with their problem behavior and

those who will not" (p. 746). What is refreshing is that Loeber and Farrington can at least see the complex negotiations for health of the subjects in their study. If we are to understand healthy coping among children at risk of becoming delinquents, then we must look for patterns of health-seeking behavior that co-occur with their problems. Thus, we can see what Glantz and Sloboda (1999) mean in concrete terms when they invite us to "postulate the interaction of positive and negative influences leading to variable outcomes" (p. 114). In the case of children who act like delinquents, it is a difficult distinction to make between those who are engaged in risk-taking behaviors as a way to further their positive growth and those whose delinquency puts them on a course to more serious problems. To categorically say that risk-taking socially deviant behaviors are all bad, or all good, overlooks the variability in children's pathways to health. An example such as this enters us into the realm of Saussure's (1978) signs and signifiers: We can no longer be entirely certain that any set of behaviors signifies either health or illness. Empiricism does not necessarily become obsolete, however. Instead, our attention is called to the contextual specificity and need to account better for the meaning those being investigated hold for the phenomena under study. The more complex and mixed method our designs, the more likely we are to achieve a theory that accounts for the multiplicity of competing understandings of health (and illness). As Massey, Cameron, Ouellette, and Fine (1998) note in their studies with youth, at least three problems face resilience researchers: values, context, and trajectory. Resilience researchers have suggested that resilience can be conceptualized as increased selfesteem, decreased depression, and improvements in one's social competence, sense of coherence, or sense of empowerment. These multiple indicators of wellbeing complicate a simple conceptualization of thriving. In many cases the values implied by these indicators suggest outcomes harmonious with the lived experience of our participants, although in other cases there may be disagreement between the values of researcher and those of the researched [italics added], (p. 339)

xxii · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH In practice, one can see how these research problems get expressed in instances where children are resistant to interventions or behave in ways that challenge cultural norms. A remarkably diverse collection of studies have found that resistance is not all bad, and in fact, the children and families who challenge authority are often those who maintain health better than the passive victims of structurally exploitive educational and social welfare systems (see Ungar, 2004). Resisting hegemony has its value, although one is less likely to be seen as resilient. There is a fine balance observable in the lives of at-risk children and youth between conformity and resistance, each contributing to the definitional ceremony of becoming known as resilient to one's peers, caregivers, and community (see Bowman, 2001, for an example of how Palestinian families strengthen their collective identity through acts of resistance and personal sacrifice). Studies of lives lived well, such as those provided in this volume, highlight this tension, providing a caution to our nomothetic tendencies to categorize children without attention to the contexts in which labels are worn.

CULTURE AND CONTEXT The construct of resilience has relevance globally, although pathways to health must be understood as contextually specific. Take, for example, instances where children experience substantial social upheaval. In 1996, a postSoviet Russia adopted a new set of laws to govern families that have significantly changed the relationship between children and parents and families and State institutions. In this world of ambiguity and shifting norms, there have been unforeseen challenges as both bureaucrats and professionals are reluctant to pass to parents the State powers they enjoyed before perestroïka. Not surprisingly, contemporary Russian parents are also not prepared to rear their children independent of the State's sharing responsibility and authority over children (Butler & Kuraeva, 2001). From my standpoint, where the boundaries between State and family are more clearly defined, I find it difficult to understand the Russian family's dilemma. This blinder to my

Russian counterpart's more collectivist orientation would, of course, bias any research I design from my cultural standpoint that might overemphasize individualism. But how much does any systemic risk such as that found in Russia compromise the health of children? Should it be considered a risk factor at all? Markowitz (2000) shows that despite the changes Russian children have experienced over the past decade and a half, remarkably few have noticed how different their lives are from that of their parents. Instead, amid the chaos, adolescents have taken up the challenge to design a life that works well for them, one that emphasizes "challenge and adventure" (p. 216). In a world of constant change, anything becomes possible. As the value placed on authority breaks down in school and community, as evidenced by the dissolution of organizations for children sponsored by the Communist party, the effect has been to leave a cultural vacuum that is more noticeable to adults than children. Yet despite the absence of these formal collectivist organizations, Russian teens still desire the same close connection to their families and a few close friends that were common a generation ago. One must therefore exercise caution assessing Russian children as more at risk now as a result of the socioeconomic turmoil experienced by their caregivers. This trend toward greater understanding of health in context has been evolving for decades. More than 30 years ago, we saw novel approaches to studies of people's experiences that challenged racial bias. Ladner (1971) observed in her landmark study of 30 young black women: We can observe differences between racial and social class groups regarding, for instance, the time at which the female is considered to be ready to assume the duties and obligations of womanhood. Becoming a woman in the low-income Black community is somewhat different from the routes followed by the white middle-class girl. The poor Black girl reaches her status of womanhood at an earlier age because of the different prescriptions and expectations of her culture. There is no single set of criteria for becoming a woman in the Black community; each girl is conditioned by a diversity of factors depending primarily upon her opportunities, role models, psychological

Introduction · xxiii disposition and the influence of the values, customs and traditions of the Black community, (p. 11) We know that how children address the "maturity gap" (Moffitt, 1997) between their status as children and their participation in their communities as adults is an important component of how children negotiate their way to healthy adult identities. That Ladner (1971) opens to debate the cultural bias of normative behavior in American culture problematizes the whole notion of what is and is not measured as risk and resilience in the West. By her work, Ladner shakes the foundations of what we assume to be healthy adolescent behavior. If teenage pregnancy needs to be reconsidered as a sign of risk, then the entire psychological enterprise of arguing what is health and illness tumbles like a house of cards. What we see in its place is the tentative negotiated agreement that defines what is a healthy pathway to resilience and what is not for each specific context. This is similar territory to that charted by other feminist authors in the late 1970s and early 1980s. Gilligan (1982) showed us the different developmental pathways for girls' moral development, and members of the Stone Center such as Surrey (1991) and Miller (1976) challenged Erikson's stage theory of development. In both cases, these authors make the distinction that what we believe about development and, ultimately, about health is influenced by the dominant culture that has privileged male ways of classifying the world as normative or nonnormative. We no longer need to think of independence and autonomy as the signs of healthy growth. Instead, if we take the lead from these feminist theorists, we see that growth in connection is a better description of how girls develop. Interestingly, this has also been shown to be an accurate description of how healthy males develop as well. Osherson (1992) found among young men an expressed desire to find connections with their fathers and children. Perhaps we must follow Ladner's (1971) lead. As she notes, It is simply a question of whether or not the values, attitudes, behavior and systems of belief which govern the dominant white middle class

should be the criteria by which Black people, most of whom have never been allowed to assimilate into the American mainstream, should be evaluated, (pp. 267-268) Substitute the phrase "Black people" with any other group that does not count itself among the privileged white middle class and one quickly sees the shallowness of assuming any definitive construction of healthy functioning that can be evaluated outside the context in which it is experienced. The bulk of resilience research, although itself contesting the irony of trying to say something meaningful about health from studies of illness, has avoided looking critically at how wellness is culturally embedded and expressed.

CONSTRUCTIONS OF RISK AND RESILIENCE First things first. There is a good news story everywhere we turn. As much as professions such as social work, psychology, and psychiatry, and the general public hooked on CNN, want to imagine the world a more dangerous place, our children more at-risk than ever before, and life in general miserable, there remains much to be hopeful about. This does not minimize the staggering impact that people globally experience from HIV-AIDS, the effect of war and community-wide epidemics of violence, or the lack of human rights for the many who are dispossessed. In each instance, however, there remains an alternate story, one that is much more full of hope. There is a fine line when studying resilience: One can simply ignore the bad or, alternately, be too realistic, embedded in an empiricism that is itself biased by the numbers it reports. Instead, we may be better off to appreciate that the construction of problems is dialogical. As Houston and Griffiths (2000) have shown, at some level, risk is socially constructed, dependent for its identification on a discursive process that names what we experience as a risk to our well-being. Take, for example, two perspectives on violence. Michael Moore's Academy Awardwinning documentary Bowling for Columbine is a disturbing and humorous look at the culture of fear in America today, a fear that is far out of

xxiv · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH proportion to the risks people face. Instead, according to Moore, the fear that is seeded by the media and nurtured through people's collective beliefs makes Americans constantly afraid for their safety. Their response, a liberal access to guns, has ironically made the very people trying to protect themselves 10 times more likely to be killed by a firearm than in Canada and 50 times more likely than people living in Japan. In contrast, we can examine a very different perspective on violence. Accounts from Bogota, Colombia, one of the most violent places on earth, with homicide rates that have been as high as 4,000 per year in a city of 3 million, show that for most people the experience of violence is still a relatively rare event. Duque and his colleagues (Duque, Klevens, & Ramirez, 2003) have found that few people in Bogota say they experience extreme forms of violence, despite very well-founded perceptions by outsiders that their communities are dangerous. Which is the better account of the risks associated with violence? Whose view of their personal security, the American or Colombian, is the most accurate? Risk and resilience are never phenomena that are simply objective fact. They are entangled in the collective ideologies of people and their communities. If we look closely at the risk and resilience literature, there are quiet discourses that tell a different story about the health status of at-risk populations and the unique mechanisms that promote well-being. For example, in the West, we are coming to understand that our efforts to mitigate all risk in children's lives might inadvertently be removing meaningful rites of passage through which children experience manageable risk. Perhaps we would do well to remember "that which doesn't kill you makes you stronger." Improving children's well-being is never as simple as removing risk from children's lives (Ungar, 2002). We need to listen better. This volume is about providing a space for voices that are less often heard and, indeed, have been all but absent from discussions on resilience. Clearly, the contributors to the following chapters argue that resilience is not an individual characteristic alone. Nor is it only a process. Resilience occurs when the personal meets the political, when the

resources we need for health are available so we can realize our potential. Resilience is as much a quality of my family, community, and culture as it is something inside me or a process I engage in. It is only because of a Western psychological discourse that we think more about the individual than the communal. Even when we acknowledge the agentic qualities of a child who thrives, we mustn't overlook the access that child experiences to health resources, including a collective discourse that defines the child's pattern of coping as resilient. We need a communitization of health, understanding health as a communal experience. A well-resourced community, a caring family, meaningful roles for individuals, rites of passage, social equality, and access to education and health care are some of the conditions necessary for the individual to experience health. Strictly speaking, these factors are independent of the individual. And yet, they also result from the actions of healthy individuals who provide for others who are more vulnerable. To say "I" am resilient is to be mistaken. The / of which we speak is a cultural artifact, a perspective that is social and historical, relational and constructed. Instead, we might better say, "There is resilience in this child and his or her community, family, and culture." Resilience is simultaneously a quality of the individual and the individual's environment. To the extent that a child accesses communal health resources and finds opportunities to express individual resources, so too will resilience be experienced. The implication of this way of thinking is that pathways to resilience must be adaptive and provide individuals with ways to negotiate for the health resources that are available. These resources can be diverse and include anything from attachments to others, self-efficacy, and a healthy sexual identity to safety and security and access to health care, food, and shelter. As this volume illustrates, children, youth, and adults globally enjoy differential access to these resources and exploit opportunities to overcome adversity in many different ways. However, the interplay between what is available and what is used is complicated. Simplistically, the provision of an opportunity that addresses risk is insufficient to change behavior unless the complexity of the problem and the construction of

Introduction · xxv the solutions by those involved are appreciated. This may be obvious, and yet, volumes of work on program fidelity emphasize the correct implementation of programs more than their contextualization.

CONTEXTUAL SENSITIVITY IN RESILIENCE RESEARCH Luthar, Cicchetti, and Becker (2000) make a similar point in their analysis of the field of resilience research, which according to them has become quite muddled. They argue that studies of protective processes, for example, need to be contextually sensitive to understand the nature of the protective function each plays in different settings. Protective processes are not onedimensional but interact with the settings in which they appear. As such, a single protective process such as staying in school or remaining attached to one's family through times of crisis may either stabilize an individual's health (preventing further exposure to risks associated with dropping out or becoming a street kid), enhance his or her health (build competence, both academically and socially), be a reactive way to counter a specific threat (the more a child is in school, the less time he or she has to get into trouble), or simply be a direct and proportional response to a threatening environment (staying in school and staying connected to one's kinship network increases chances to find employment and community acceptance later in life). Any single factor associated with resilience in any particular context will protect against risk only in ways meaningful to those whose lives are affected. We are embracing this ambiguity more often. As Kaplan (1999) notes, A major limitation of the concept of resilience is that it is tied to the normative judgments relating to particular outcomes. If the outcomes were not desirable, then the ability to reach the outcomes in the face of putative risk factors would not be considered resilience. Yet it is possible that the socially defined desirable outcome may be subjectively defined as undesirable, while the socially defined undesirable outcome may be subjectively defined as desirable. From the subjective point of view, the

individual may be manifesting resilience, while from the social point of view the individual may be manifesting vulnerability, (pp. 31-32) The onus is on the research community, in partnership with study participants, to look closer at what is a risk factor, a protective factor, or an outcome associated with resilience (see Rutter, 2001). It is no longer good enough to arbitrarily decide the definition of these aspects of resilience without the contextualization required to authenticate the meaning of the terms with those who participate in research. Even quantitative designs can do this, although mixedmethod designs that employ rigorous qualitative work may be more amenable to the task. There is reason to be cautiously optimistic that we are getting better at embracing the ambiguity of the multiple pathways people travel to health. Take, for example, the relatively unknown work by Morgan (1998). He examined the relationship between behavioral outcomes, as measured by the level of privileges attained, and resiliency factors in residential treatment among 92 children aged 7 to 15. Hypothesizing that an internal locus of control would be related to behavior associated with resilience, Morgan found instead a null finding. There was no significant correlation between children's pattern of internality or externality, whether they behaved in ways that showed they exercised self-control or defied authority. As Morgan explains, It seems logical to suggest that, since internal locus of control is related to more successful outcomes in resiliency studies, that it may also be related to more successful, i.e. better level scores. It must be stressed that this remains only a conjecture, however, since it seems possible to also imagine the opposite direction of this relationship, that an internal locus of control, since it may suggest more of a sense of empowerment, may cause these children to, in fact, question and rebel against a well-defined set of rules and expectations precisely because they see themselves as having more options, as being more capable of effecting change compared to those children who are more externally oriented . . . who may just simply go along with the structure of the program because they feel rather powerless to change or manipulate the system, (p. 44)

xxvi · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Given the lack of significant findings, Morgan (1998) listens to his data speak and concludes that "a more resilient child might actually have poorer level scores if the nature of resiliency was to cause a child to fight a system" (p. 100). In other words, the contextual specificity of the residential setting makes an attribute like internality a potential threat to resilience despite the hypothesized link between internality and resilience. Clearly, the idea that resilience is an individual characteristic is no longer tenable. A wave of new resilience researchers is showing that resilience is as much a characteristic of the environment as it is an individual's capacity to exploit opportunities in that environment (see Knox, 2000). Take, for example, the behavior of students who cut classes. The attribution by educators might be that these are wrongful acts on the part of students. But it also may be that students are resisting a system that has failed to educate them in a way amenable to their style of learning. Fallis and Opotow (2003) talk about the incongruity between adult expectations to attend school and the boredom students who skip class say they experience: "For students, boring connotes something missing in their education, conveys a deep sense of disappointment, and casts class cutting as a coping mechanism for classes that fail to engage" (p. 108). Here again, as in earlier examples, the interplay of context, the power to define one's world (discursive empowerment), and behavior or outcomes is intricate and not always evident outside the specific context in which research takes place. If we broaden our scope to Colombia again, we can find similar discursive resistance documented by researchers like Felsman (1989) who, based on 300 semistructured interviews with street children under 16, heard them account for ganglike behavior in nonpathologizing ways: "Although the gallada's [gangs of street youth] involvements in crime and violence must be recognized and contended with, it must first be realized that these children do not band together to fight and steal; rather, they band together to meet primary physical and emotional needs not being addressed elsewhere" (p. 66). A host of other researchers are pushing us to redefine risk and resilience within a contextually specific discourse. Martineau (1999), for

example, examined the intertextuality of resilience studies through a meta-analysis of the language used in research reports. She finds that obscured behind the well-meaning intentions of teaching resilience is a call for disadvantaged children and youth to conform to the behavioral norms of the dominant society (associated with social and school success) by overcoming or being invulnerable to the systemic distresses and adversities of their everyday lives, (p. 3) This indictment of the system supports a notion of health as intricately linked to liberation. This overtly politicized understanding of resilience is neither common nor well documented. In part, this is because the dominant discourse of resilience researchers has to date been the psychologizing discourse of Western mental health researchers. Critically, Martineau argues: The resiliency discourse imposes prescribed norms of school success and social success upon underprivileged children identified as at risk. The effect is that non-conforming individuals may be pathologized as non-resilient. Emphasis remains wholly on the individual and thus, individualism is a dominant ideology embedded in the mainstream resiliency discourse, (pp. 11-12) Such studies reveal that the pathways children navigate toward healthy lifestyles are far from fixed, despite discursive hegemony that tells us what is and is not normative development. Our capacity to appreciate localized discourses of resilience has hindered our fuller understanding of how people navigate pathways to health when seriously compromised by adversity. We have tended to predetermine outcomes, blinding ourselves to the indigenous, and often everyday, occurrences of resilience. It is this more contextually sensitive, indeed everyday, understanding of resilience that is the next challenge for resilience researchers. As McCubbin and his colleagues (McCubbin et al., 1999) have explained in regard to African American families, Notwithstanding . . . negative realities, most minority families go on with the ordinary business

Introduction · xxvii of everyday living: raising and educating their children and grandchildren, caring for the elderly and infirm, celebrating birthdays and anniversaries, attending family reunions, and finding ways to make ends meet. As deeper understanding of resiliency is sought, perhaps a closer look at the ordinary might be instructive. For many racial and ethnic minority families, the ability to do the "ordinary" is in itself an extraordinary achievement. Daily functioning amidst negative realities requires a high level of motivation, commitment, tenacity and creativity, (p. 32)

A NEW UNDERSTANDING OF RESILIENCE If resilience is going to be understood, it is going to be a messy affair. As Genero (1998) explains, Studying resiliency within a context of relationships, variables such as class, gender, sexuality, and culture cannot be relegated to a background status. Nor can differences in the conceptual meaning of relationships, family, and cultural groups be ignored. . . . Although the use of both qualitative and quantitative data-gathering techniques may be desirable, it seems that an interdisciplinary, multimethod approach may generate the most exciting data. (pp. 44-45) Sadly, this mixed-methods approach is seldom employed. Worse, we as a research community have hesitated to complicate our studies with the numerous confounding variables we would require to situate resilience within the context of each population studied. Take for example a study by D'Abreu, Mullis, and Cook (1999) of street children in Brazil and the relationship between the quality and quantity of social support and the child's ability to adapt to street life, find food, and form attachments. Remarkably, their study found that there was no relationship between the dependent and independent variables. The results, which were based solely on quantitative data gathering, leave more questions unanswered than answered. We are left with the vaguely disconcerting thought that the research team approached its work with the certainty that they would find something without understanding

the localized constructions of the concepts indigenous to the children themselves. This openness to multiple constructions of core research terms by participants is not meant to create an epistemological vacuum typical of extreme positions in postmodernism. Not all truths will be equally privileged, but all truths may have a constituency of one and often more. The researcher who wants to understand individual and collective constructions of concepts such as resilience among marginalized populations will need to enter the world of discourse analysis. In theoretically murky spaces, the goal of inquiry must be partially consultation rather than consensus building or, perhaps worse, ethnocentric and premature generalization. This volume is a step forward in this regard. It is an attempt to bring together well-considered arguments about what creates resilience in at-risk populations of children around the world. In one way, it seeks a tentative consensus, a tacit agreement that we can all speak about resilience as if the term is held in common. It is, of course, not. Instead, if one scratches the surface, one will find in these chapters a plurality of understandings of how we understand children's health, how we study it, and how we intervene to promote well-being amid a wide range of challenging life circumstances. The result is a pastiche, a collection of related bits brought together in a stunning collage. To bring some order to this enterprise, the book is divided into three sections, theory, research methods, and finally, practice. Each section is introduced briefly below with an overview of the chapters it contains. Contained in this collection, then, are many perspectives not common among resilience researchers or among those who have tried to apply the concept of resilience to practice with at-risk populations across cultures and contexts. Venturing into uncharted territory has necessarily meant bringing together scholars and practitioners, advocates and community organizers, researchers and policymakers, both known and unknown, to the field. If this volume appears at moments slightly uneven in its presentation, it is because it has pulled together such a divergent group of authors under the same cover. The result is a collection of voices that have been more or less privileged. Together, however, we

xxviii · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH demonstrate that resilience is more than an individual set of characteristics. It is the structures around the individual, the services the individual receives, the way health knowledge is generated, all of which combine with characteristics of individuals that allow them to overcome the adversity they face and chart pathways to resilience. Perhaps in the juxtaposition of our work, the embracing of our own diversity as authors, we will move a little closer to identifying something that we all hold in common, a deeper understanding of what makes children healthy when growing up facing multiple risk factors. In that moment of synergy, there is the possibility that our appreciation of our diversity will give us a glimpse of that which is universal even as we celebrate the local.

IMPLICATIONS FOR THEORY In the first section of this volume, contributors examine how resilience is theorized and the need, if any, for change. Each chapter offers some less common perspective on the construct of resilience, pushing our conceptualization of the foundations of resilience-related research and practice. This work follows in the wake of others who have hinted at the need to look beyond the skewed, subjective bias of researchers. Take, for example, those who have challenged us to see beyond our dichotomous thinking that classifies communities as either high or low risk. Moving beyond the dichotomous thinking of inner-city human ecologies that overlook aspects of health, a number of researchers have explored the strengths of these communities, which are the direct result of the chaos people experience living there (Nelson & Wright, 1995). In such instances, the steeling effect of community stressors may actually enhance capacities that are invisible to outsiders. Theoretically sound resilience research and interventions are similar. They must demonstrate an understanding of the warp and weave of communities that knit themselves together to achieve health. A good example was provided by Brodsky (1996) who studied 10 African American single mothers living in high-risk

communities. Brodsky demonstrated that a psychological sense of community (PSOC), normally associated with measures of resilience, was in this case shown to be negatively associated with health among the women. Although interveners might bring with them as outsiders to these women's lives the belief that fostering community is a way to address the community's problems, they would actually be putting these particular women's sense of well-being more at risk because of the myopia of the theory from which they operate. Brodsky shows that the negative aspects of a PSOC far outweigh any well-intentioned outsider's positive connotation of the concept. She explains that given the atmosphere in those communities, resources, and the nature of community membership, the women in her study talked about how they maintain boundaries, avoid personal investments with others, hold common symbols of independence, and avoid emotional connections with others outside their families as strategies to protect themselves and their families from the chaos around them. It was, according to the women, a successful way to maintain health and security. This volume strives to explore similarly different perspectives on the theory of resilience. The intention is to contribute a broader, more contextually sensitive way of conceptualizing health in at-risk populations.

The Chapters The section begins with Boyden and Mann's (Chapter 1) global perspective on children's coping in majority-world contexts. Their work challenges our notions of what makes a child resilient, suggesting that although resilience may be a useful metaphor, we must attend far more to the specific cultural contexts in which children survive and thrive. Specifically, we must look as much at the meaning that children and their communities create for the social and developmental threats they encounter as at the political systems that do or do not meet children's needs. Boyden and Mann's extensive experience in countries as diverse as Thailand, Nepal, Tanzania, and the Balkans helps to show the plurality of understandings among children of what sustains their health under adverse circumstances.

Introduction · xxix Next, Laurie McCubbin and Hamilton McCubbin (Chapter 2) broaden our understanding of family resilience by focusing on aspects of culture and ethnic identity as important components of their family resilience model. The authors draw from research on families, trauma, and resilience and from anthropology, sociology, and feminist studies to reveal the importance of culture and ethnic identity as critical components of a relational perspective of family resilience in the face of trauma. The authors' ancestral ties to and immersion in Polynesian culture lay a foundation for their systematic study of resilience.

resilience through nonconventional lives lived in the street. Street children in both the minority and majority world are discussed in detail, with a rich number of examples from research and interventions. McAdam-Crisp and her colleagues show both intrigue and puzzlement regarding the capacities of these children to overcome adversity. Under their scrutiny, risk chains that might be thought to compromise children (including child labor, leaving home, and even prostitution) are shown in contextually specific instances to provide some of these children with a way to survive better than they would off the street.

In the next chapter, Joyce West Stevens (Chapter 3) examines whether the notion of risk and resilience can inform our understanding of marginalized populations, specifically African American youth in inner-city communities challenged by unsafe environments, economic problems, and few social resources. Concluding that both constructs are useful, West Stevens proposes that an ecological framework that focuses attention on the intersection of persons, process, and social context will best help us to understand the lives of these marginalized youth. Learning from these youth, she shows that they can educate us as to what youth need to survive adverse environments and that youth are helped most when attributes associated with resilience are supported. In Jane Gilgun and Laura Abrams's chapter (Chapter 4), the authors propose that resilience be understood as the expression of cultural and gendered norms. Challenging the dominance of what is and is not a sign of healthy functioning, they show through a study with violent individuals that processes associated with interpersonal violence can in some instances contribute to an "awkward but effective" way that vulnerable poorly resourced individuals experience health. Resilience is thereby understood as something individuals strive for even if their path is deviant or disordered when judged by others. In their chapter, Gilgun and Abrams analyze perpetrators' adaptations to adversity, looking at their gendered beliefs, roles, and strategies. In the next chapter, Jacqueline McAdamCrisp, Lewis Aptekar, and Wanjiku Kironyo (Chapter 5) provide a framework for understanding street children and their ways of sustaining

Ira Prilleltensky and Ora Prilleltensky (Chapter 6) move the discussion to the level of the professional intervening to promote wellness and resilience in marginalized populations, such as people with disabilities. They show that we need change in how we have understood the role of the psychologist, social worker, and other helping professionals. Interventions must necessarily be more influential in the sphere of social justice. Their argument is that wellness and resilience cannot "take hold" unless both are rooted in "socially just communities and processes." Both psychological and political influences are needed to create change at the level of individuals and their communities. To date, this critical and transformational focus has not been a large part of professional discourse by those employed to help vulnerable children, youth, and families. In the next chapter, Cindy Blackstock and Nico Trocmé (Chapter 7) use their examination of the reasons behind the disproportionate removal of Aboriginal children from their families as a way to argue that risk to children comes from structural forces outside children's immediate families. Using data from the Canadian Incidence Study on Reported Child Abuse and Neglect, they provide evidence to support an argument that we must necessarily address structural barriers such as inequitable service access if we are to create culturally appropriate conditions for children to overcome risks associated with maltreatment. In the last chapter of this section, Fred Besthorn (Chapter 8) advances our understanding of the conditions necessary for resilience even further. He offers a novel understanding of

xxx · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH one aspect of resilience that has been largely overlooked by researchers: the child's interaction with his or her natural environment. Besthorn shows that the ontological and epistemological insularity of the human development and resilience fields, embedded as they are in Western modernity, have caused us to ignore the importance of our experiences with the natural world and their contribution to health outcomes.

IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH Achieving a more contextually relevant understanding of resilience will require methods for research that reflect advances in health research already underway in the social sciences. Although beyond the scope of this volume to explore fully the debate over which methods are most appropriate to what kind of research, the bias of most contributors to this volume is toward some type of mixed-method approach to resilience research. The need for contextualization of concepts and instruments, combined with the need for "thickly" described lives to see resilience in lives lived under adversity, makes it most likely that as valuable as quantitative approaches can be, they require a qualitative component as well (Galambos & Leadbeater, 2000). The nuts and bolts of how one actually integrates these two approaches, however, remains hotly debated (Miller & Crabtree, 2000). Although debate continues, it appears that multiple-method designs, or at the very least the aggregated findings from studies within diverse research paradigms, both qualitative and quantitative, will provide the most comprehensive picture of resilience. As Tashakkori and Teddlie (2003) argue in their discussion of mixed-method approaches to research, such research is mixed in many or all stages of the study (questions, research methods, data collection and analysis, and the inference process). . . . Therefore, one of the assumptions of such research is that it is indeed possible to have two paradigms, or two worldviews, mixed throughout a single research project, (p. 11) The result is much more diversity in the data and what they can tell us. The mixed-method

approach routinely combines different paradigms. It is not simply that one does a focus group, a form of qualitative inquiry, to validate an instrument. The researcher goes further, combining both realist or value-neutral perspectives with the value-engaged constructionist and subjective position of complementary research paradigms (Greene & Caracelli, 2003). In particular, mixed-method designs are noted for their contribution to cross-cultural research where cultural distance demands researchers deconstruct their standpoint vis-à-vis research participants and the culture under study (Moghaddam, Walker, & Harré, 2003). None of these design advances, however, lets us avoid the thorny issues that complicate resilience research when it is attentive to contextual variation. The best these new methods do is allow us to engage more effectively in the debate. Critics from both research paradigms, quantitative and qualitative, recognize that they will have to do far more with the tools they have available if they are to adapt their methods to myriad different contexts (see, e.g., Glantz & Sloboda, 1999). The complexity of our research is likely to get worse the better we get at documenting resilience-related phenomena. This is not a field for those who like order. It is more like reading a great epic novel where one is never, indeed if ever, quite certain what a single event means to the overall experience of a character. Furthermore, if this complexity makes research difficult, the need to account for multiple constructions of health across global contexts makes it positively unwieldy. This is very similar to Cohen and his colleagues (Cohen, Cimbolic, Armeli, & Hettler, 1998) who have characterized the field of research on "thriving," principally concerned with adults who have suffered traumatic life events, as "unsophisticated" because of the methods employed and in particular the inability of researchers who as of yet have failed to account for the variability in how participants define benefits or gains associated with health. Under the weight of such critiques, researchers are obligated to embrace complexity and account for localized constructions of health phenomena. In this endeavor, either set of methods can become the principal tool when enhanced by the other. Far from a distant goal,

Introduction · xxxi such complementarity in design is now evident in many forums. As Madill and her colleagues (Madill, Jordan, & Shirley, 2000) observe, there has been a shift in tolerance for alternative epistemological orientations to research among psychologists, a shift easily seen in nursing, social work, medicine, and other disciplines as well. Maddill et al. observe that few psychologists today espouse a strict logical positivist in which knowledge is understood to be demonstrated through its direct correspondence with observed events and research methods modelled on those of the hard sciences. In practice, a range of post-positivist epistemologies are utilized which recognize an element of interpretation and metaphor in the production of social scientific theories and findings, (p. 1) Interpretation? Metaphor? These are landscapes of conflict under less ideal circumstances. The contributors to this section navigate these same epistemologically troubling waters, providing bridges to collaboration across research paradigms.

The Chapters It's appropriate, then, to begin this reflection on methods with a chapter by William Barton (Chapter 9). Barton critiques resilience research to date, acknowledging that its greatest strength has been shifting our attention to health and the ecological context in which that health occurs. However, there remain daunting methodological and theoretical challenges. Although he shows the potential value of both quantitative and qualitative methods and reviews many exemplary studies in both research traditions, he concludes that a symbolic interactionist approach to resilience is the most needed at this juncture because we still have not entirely comprehended the missing piece in most developmental theory, including resilience theory, that being the mechanism that best explains peoples' successful behavior despite exposure to risk. Quantitative designs that accomplish this have tended to be longitudinal and expensive and risk attrition in their sample. What's more, they may overlook the meaning and cultural specificity needed to understand a health-related construct such as

resilience. Although qualitative studies bring with them their own limitations, most notably a lack of generalizability (in preference for transferability and other criteria of authenticity), Barton argues we need more of them before we can carry on with quantitative work on this theme. In the next chapter, Eli Teram and I (Chapter 10) look specifically at the contributions qualitative research can make to resilience research that is more politicized, ideological, and culturally grounded. However, as we point out, although qualitative methods offer the hope that they can make transparent the researcher's bias, perhaps preventing research from being misappropriated by neoconservatives who would have us believe resilience is something inside a child, just waiting for the child to express, qualitative research too can be problematic. Although qualitative methods might be useful to resilience research, interpretative challenges abound. The chapter concludes with an examination of how the better integration of qualitative methods can make resilience research more politically relevant and informing of public policy. Taking a very different approach, John LeBlanc, Pam Talbott, and Wendy Craig (Chapter 11) look at the limits of epidemiological data, exploring the problems inherent in research that has tried to answer the question, "How are the world's children and youth faring with regard to their psychosocial health?" Their work highlights both the complexities and shortcomings of an epidemiological perspective, discussing the difficulties of measuring a multidimensional construct such as resilience and health. Using international data, they examine what we do know about the psychosocial health of youth while demonstrating both the strengths and limitations of the research that produced those findings. In the next chapter, Laura Camfield and Allister McGregor (Chapter 12) explore how we research well-being in majority-world contexts. Specifically, they offer indicators of how research on well-being is seeking to integrate both objective and subjective approaches to how we understand experiences of poverty and how those experiences are reproduced. They show that it is important that we bring together a range of academic disciplines if we are to

xxxii · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH understand how people achieve a sense of themselves as resilient, a proxy term in Camfield and McGregor's work for happy. Resilience is something people create for themselves and that they rely on structures around them to create. However, there is typically a gap between people's own evaluations of their lives and what others see as their material wellbeing. Methodologically, Camfield and McGregor raise a number of important questions regarding the obligation of researchers to examine the nature of people's own constructions of their poverty and disadvantage. Rather than being dismissive of what these people have to say, their chapter argues people's appraisals are not the product of misinformation or poor judgment but elaborate schemes reflecting people's aspirations and strategies to survive or perhaps even thrive. The following chapter by Linda Liebenberg and I (Chapter 13) details how one goes about researching resilience across cultures and contexts. Our experience designing and implementing the International Resilience Project, a multisite, mixed-method interdisciplinary study in 14 sites globally, provides an account of the real-world problems researchers encounter working cross-culturally. There is little, however, written to guide researchers in the specifics of how to conduct such work. In this chapter, the nittygritty of the perils and pitfalls of such collaborations are explored, with specific details provided regarding how to host a meeting to design a multisite study and the negotiations necessary to find consensus on how children's health should be studied.

IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE Well-considered theory regarding the nature of resilience generated from innovative and rigorous research methods can inform different practice applications. Approaching interventions to ameliorate conditions that threaten children and youth's well-being from an understanding of resilience orients clinical treatment, programming, community work, and policy initiatives in potentially novel directions. With few exceptions, however, these initiatives are seldom grouped under the heading "building resilience"

(see Ungar, 2004; Walsh, 1998). They are, nevertheless, increasingly informed by the burgeoning literature on resilience. Practice that builds resilience is multidimensional. Resilience is not an individual trait. Huang (2003) puts it succinctly: "We need to integrate the impact of disparities into our mental health care of children." In fact, interventions that address singular dimensions of health such as self-esteem or issues of child labor can actually result in negative consequences for children. There is, for example, evidence that antibullying programs in school that show children the negative aspects of bullying actually create pro-bullying attitudes in children who feel powerless and are seeking ways to increase their self-esteem (Rahey & Craig, 2002). Sutton, Smith, and Swettenham (1999) argue that approaches to violence prevention in schools that fail to recognize aspects of health among bullies will simply not work: It is important to realize that some bullying children do have power, and that they can misuse this power in ways advantageous to them (in some circumstances). For some, this power takes a social rather than physical form, and such bullies are undoubtedly skilled at achieving interpersonal goals. They would probably not see their behavior as incompetent or maladaptive, and there is evidence that it often is not. (p. 133) Although we would evidently prefer that children not bully others in their efforts to feel powerful, we cannot intervene assuming that they are incompetent. Such ungrounded, biased approaches to intervention obscure the health-sustaining behaviors of troubled children. Only when bullies are offered socially desirable outlets for their particular competencies will we see changes in their behavior. A more holistic understanding of the way children sustain health would help to avoid erroneous program design. What we now understand about resilience requires us to think more broadly about any type of interventions that address children's exposure to risk. Once one shifts focus and understands how children in different contexts search for resilience, one understands that intended outcomes from interventions result only when our actions are grounded on the knowledge of the culture

Introduction · xxxiii and context of those with whom we intervene. But how grounded? How specific do we need to be when we tailor interventions? There is after all a lack of homogeneity even among marginalized populations. As Dupree, Spencer, and Bell (1997) discuss in relation to self-esteem in black adolescent boys in the United States, a case-bycase and situation-specific approach is required if one is to promote resilience in children in unique circumstances. Although they speak of generic processes such as encouraging "helpseeking strategies and greater social mobility" (p. 258), which will enable many to survive in their home environments, they caution that the mistake we make is to not fully appreciate the unique constellation of resources available to support these strategies. Because resilience is becoming an increasingly complex term to understand, problematizing our discourse on health, we see that interventions at the level of policy are as necessary as interventions targeting individuals and families. Not surprising, Queiro-Tajalli and Campbell (2003) link organizing strategies and community development with the growth in resilience among specific populations. Resilient communities mobilize to secure resources, one aspect of this mobilization being to find others who will provide mutual support and develop a movement of resistance that fosters structural change. These are lofty goals, but they are just as integral to a resilience-based approach to practice as the more immediate treatment goals of direct practice and prevention programs. As Tully (2002) writes, "There is a constant dynamic interrelationship between the development of public policy (as a response to human need), human behavior (as defined and supported by public policy), and resilience (as a positive coping mechanism related to adapting to ongoing change)" (p. 330). When it comes to interventions, one size no longer fits all. As Crowel (2003) has pointed out, it is no longer possible to speak of evidencebased practice when we move interventions into ethnically diverse communities. Instead, we might more accurately speak only of "promising practices" that may be judged to fit with a particular population at risk. In this section, a diverse group of authors from around the world offer their examples of

interventions to build resilience with at-risk populations. The result is a pastiche of writing, varied but coherent when viewed from afar. There is much to advance our thinking here, with many different interventions advanced as multiple pathways through the swampy chaos of lives lived under adversity.

The Chapters The section begins with work by Zahava Solomon and Avital Laufer (Chapter 14) from Israel who relate the findings of a study of responses by Jewish Israeli children to terror. Surveying almost 3,000 adolescents, aged 13 to 15, from 11 schools in Israel, Solomon and Laufer report that on the whole, Israeli teens cope well with the terror they experience, demonstrating resilience both in the way they deal with the trauma and in how they use that exposure to trauma as an opportunity for growth. Embedded in this study are many clues for those who choose to intervene with youth exposed to war and violence, including evidence that social support, ideological commitment, and religiosity all play a role in both mitigating the effects of violence and enhancing psychological growth and coping. However, as Solomon and Laufer explain, the relationship between these factors is complex, with healthenhancing effects being different for youth with different experiences and different beliefs. The next chapter approaches the problem of intervention differently. Philip Cook and Lesley du Toit (Chapter 15) examine children's resilience when confronted with the challenge of HIV/AIDS in communities in South Africa. They discuss the Circles of Care, a community capacity-building project, as an example of culturally grounded action research and a way of supporting child and community resilience. Highlighting the fit between the project and indigenous African cultural values, beliefs, and practices, they show that children's resilience depends on the promotion of healthy human development resulting from child, peer, family, and social factors embedded in each child's social ecology. Marion Brown and Marc Colbourne (Chapter 16) next look at a very different context and the survival strategies of youth there. They examine

xxxiv · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH the experiences of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth (LGB) who cope with homophobia and heterosexism. Brown and Colbourne show that the behaviors of this population, frequently assumed to be signs of psychic distress, mental illness, or other social problems, have seldom been recognized as the legitimate, indeed at times, only ways these youth can respond to and resist their oppression. Resilience is found when these youth manage to navigate around the prejudice they experience. Brown and Colbourne, through discussion of a youth project targeted at meeting the needs of LGB youth, provide rich examples of resilience among this population as they engage in ongoing negotiations between themselves and their environments. Despite a societal context full of problems, both structural and ideological, and precarious family environments, these youth regularly locate the personal and community resources they need to succeed. Many of the chapters also look at families, their form and function, as one factor in the healthy development of children. Alean Al-Krenawi and Vered Slonim-Nevo (Chapter 17) report on a study of 352 Bedouin children from Israel from both monogamous and polygamous families. Theirs is an interesting story to tell, with Al-Krenawi and Slonim-Nevo hypothesizing that children from polygamous families would report more problems psychologically, socially, educationally, and within their families than children from monogamous families. Their intent is to show that different family forms affect the well-being of children differently. They show that although polygamy may once have been a family form that functioned well, the current reality of urbanization and consumerism that is changing Bedouin society seems to be making polygamous families less successful in terms of securing their children's well-being. Romanticism aside, culturally specific family forms appear to need to adapt as children's broader social environments change as well. The theme of family functioning as a factor in resilience among children is elaborated on in the next chapter by Barbara Friesen and Eileen Brennan (Chapter 18). Friesen and Brennan explore the resilience-building mechanisms found within families and the broader ecological and specifically community contexts within

which families live. Their work details the need for stable supportive communities that provide for the cultural continuity of those living there. In exploring these themes, they examine the child-community relationship, as influenced by the child-family relationship, documenting programs that build the structural conditions for resilience through quality child care arrangements, after-school and mentoring programs, and community-centered, youth development programs. Next Kwai-yau Wong and Tak-yan Lee (Chapter 19) examine youth at risk in Hong Kong in the 1990s and report on a significant innovation in service delivery that involved the adoption of a screening mechanism in the school system for early identification and intervention. This mechanism was adapted from one used with Canadian youth. While reporting on the Understanding the Adolescent Project (UAP), Wong and Lee identify the inadequacies inherent in its theoretical framework. In particular, they explore the lack of a culture-specific dimension to the UAP. A critical read of the literature, both Western and Chinese, provides a cultural lens through which to reexamine the UAP, offering educators, social workers, and mental health counselors on the front lines with at-risk youth in Hong Kong an alternative professional discourse focused on prevention and health from a uniquely Chinese perspective. Continuing to elaborate on this theme of resilience as dependent as much on social structures as on individual characteristics, Thomas and Menamparampil's chapter on the young people of North East India (Chapter 20) explores the complicated patterns of coping among youth who face the dual threats of poverty and armed militancy. Although their lives are difficult, youth in this remote part of India demonstrate a constellation of coping strategies aided by structural conditions that facilitate their development. In particular, Thomas and Menamparampil discuss one exemplar of community development for youth, a faith-based organization, Don Bosco, which has as its mission to educate and train youth who are marginalized. Although seldom discussed in the literature on resilience, Thomas and Menamparampil argue that faith-based organizations seed youth resilience through programs as wide

Introduction · xxxv ranging as literacy, peace education, vocational schools, and workshops on self-esteem. In the next chapter, Ken Barter (Chapter 21) moves the discussion from how community agencies contribute to resilience to what government services can and should do to promote health in at-risk children. His work discusses barriers to promoting protective factors in the lives of children who receive child protection services. Barter suggests alternatives to the formal bureaucratic support provided by government systems that might better strengthen protective factors and positive outcomes for children. His work demonstrates from a Canadian perspective that an important part of marginalized children's pathways to resilience includes pathways through the formal service delivery systems mandated to protect them. His message is that we have alternatives that work, that build community, and that address the systemic barriers, such as poverty and social injustice, that affect children adversely. As an example of alternatives to standard practices in child welfare, Nancy MacDonald, Joan Glode, and Fred Wien (Chapter 22) examine in their chapter approaches to family and children's services rooted in Aboriginal traditions in Canada. In particular, they explore custom adoptions and family group conferencing as ways of avoiding Eurocentric child welfare practices, such as adopting children out to white families, that have contributed to cultural genocide. Their work, focused on the Mi'kmaq communities of Nova Scotia, looks at how provincial, territorial, and federal levels of government in Canada have or have not changed legislation and implementation of child welfare laws to protect the rights of Aboriginal children. Their work is an example of how structures and policies (a) set the conditions to add to the risks faced by marginalized populations or (b) are designed to provide the conditions for resilience to emerge. They argue that child justice and child welfare bodies that understand the importance of using interventions that reflect traditional worldviews of Aboriginal peoples are likely to be those that are most successful in sustaining the well-being of these children. From child welfare, we move to education. Alexander Makhnach and Anna Laktionova (Chapter 23) explore resilience among contemporary Russian youth with a special focus on

both child protection and educational forces that influence children's positive outcomes in a society in transition. They review the personality characteristics found among Russian youth and families that contribute to resilience and explore the role played by communities and social institutions to strengthening qualities associated with resilience among youth. In particular, they look at how the Russian educational system is providing children and families with the interventions required to sustain them through this time of social change. Shifting the focus back to the minority world, Mary Armstrong, Beth Stroul, and Roger Boothroyd (Chapter 24) compare the closely related constructs of systems of care and resilience. Their argument, like those of their colleagues who precede them, is that these concepts intersect. Resilience depends on structural conditions, in particular the way formal and informal systems respond to children's needs. They present a historical overview of the resilience literature and then contrast it with the similarities and differences in what we understand about systems of care for children in need. Their work points to the benefits for policymakers, planners, and researchers examining systems of care and child resilience to be aware of advances in both fields and the potential of each body of literature to inform policy, system planning, and research efforts. Broadening our perspective further, Scotney Evans and Isaac Prilleltensky (Chapter 25) write about youth civic engagement (YCE) and its potential impact on the well-being of youth. Their argument is based on a contention that neither wellness nor resilience can be fully understood or promoted without accounting for power differentials between youth and adults and the broader social structures that adults control. However, there needs to be caution in how this engagement is undertaken. Evans and Prilleltensky explain that youth can just as easily be manipulated or cast as token agents of social control by adult-engineered systems that abuse the idealism of youth. Without a power analysis, Evans and Prilleltensky argue, YCE may pose more perils than promise. In a very different context, these same challenges are addressed in the chapter by Toine van Teeffelen, Hania Bitar, and Saleem Habash

xxxvi · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH (Chapter 26) who detail both the adversities Palestinians face and the consequences of those adversities to the mental health of youth growing up in the Occupied Territories. Unlike many other accounts of Palestinian youth, however, this chapter examines evidence of the resilience to be found among Palestinian youth. After discussing personal, familial, and cultural resources that sustain resilience, van Teeffelen, Bitar, and Habash consider how a development organization, the Palestinian Youth Association for Leadership and Rights Activation (PYALARA), can create conditions that bolster resilience among youth. PYALARA provides both social interventions such as mentoring programs and different forms of youth-oriented media to give youth a voice in the political decision-making process. The next chapter by Wanda Thomas Bernard and David Este (Chapter 27) examines the context within which young African Canadian males navigate through the challenges they face and the personal, family, community, and spiritual resources they use to overcome the systemic prejudice and structural disadvantage they experience. Bernard and Este examine these challenges to young African Canadian males, particularly in the areas of education and employment. They then apply Hill's (1998) resiliency model developed for African Americans to the African Canadians. Their chapter concludes with reflections on resilience collected from 30 males who discuss both their experiences of success and marginalization. Concluding the book is a chapter by Luis Duque and his colleagues (Chapter 28) who bring us fully back to the challenge this volume addresses: What does resilience look like across cultures and contexts, and how do we promote it? In this chapter, Duque and his coauthors explore the Early Prevention of Aggression Project that was implemented in Medellin, Colombia, based on design elements from Canada and the United States. The chapter examines the challenges faced when projects in majorityworld settings that seek to create the structural conditions for resilience to emerge are adapted from minority-world contexts where the bulk of the scholarship and implementation has taken place. Specifically, the chapter reviews findings from the first phases of an evaluation of

outcomes, with a special focus on how well the project was implemented and fidelity to the model when it is employed in communities far more dangerous and far less resourced than those in more economically developed nations. Far from showing that "one size fits all," the chapter shows that different settings provide children very different challenges to health that require different measures to promote resilience. It is an appropriate place to conclude the book, with the argument for a more contextual understanding of resilience, one that acknowledges the structural, as well as the personal factors that foster resilience, demonstrated on a large scale in a context far beyond that normally discussed in the resilience literature. Combined, this and all the other accompanying chapters demonstrate the necessity for the field of resilience theory, research, and practice to broaden its focus and cultural embeddedness if it is to continue itself to "survive and thrive."

REFERENCES Bowman, G. (2001). The two deaths of Basem Rishmawi: Identity constructions and reconstructions in a Muslim-Christian Palestinian community. Identities-Global Studies in Culture and Power, 8(1), 47-81. Brodsky, Α. Ε. (1996). Resilient single mothers in risky neighborhoods: Negative psychological sense of community. Journal of Community Psychology, 24(4), 347-363. Butler, A. C , & Kuraeva, L. G. (2001). Russian family policy in transition: Implications for families and professionals. Social Service Review, 75(2), 195-224. Childtrends. (2003). Facts at a glance. Retrieved January 18, 2005, www.childtrends.org/Files/ FAAG2003.pdf Chong, D. (2000). The girl in the picture: The Kim Phuc story. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Penguin. Cohen, L. H., Cimbolic, K., Armeli, S. R„ & Hettler, T. R. (1998). Quantitative assessment of thriving. Journal of Social Issues, 54(2), 323-335. Combrinck-Graham, L. (Ed.). (1995). Children in families at risk. New York: Guilford Press. Crowel, R. (2003). Improving access to care and outcomes for children from diverse racial and

Introduction · xxxvii ethnie backgrounds and their families. Plenary address, 16th Annual Research Conference, "A System of Care for Children's Mental Health: Expanding the Research Base," University of South Florida, Tampa. D'Abreu, R. C , Mullis, A. K., & Cook, L. R. (1999). The resiliency of street children in Brazil. Adolescence, 34(136), 745-751. Denenberg, V. H. (1999). Commentary: Is maternal stimulation the mediator of the handling effects in infancy? Developmental Psychobiology, 34(1), 1-3. Dupree, D., Spencer, M. B., & Bell, S. (1997). African American children. In G. Johnson-Powell & J. Yamamoto (Eds.), Transcultural child development: Psychological assessment and treatment (pp. 237-268). New York: John Wiley. Duque, L. F., Klevens, J., & Ramirez, C. (2003). Overlap and correlates of different types of aggression among adults: Results from a crosssectional survey in Bogata, Colombia. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 191-201. Fallis, R. K., & Opotow, S. (2003). Are students failing school or are schools failing students? Class cutting in high school. Journal of Social Issues, 59(1), 103-120. Felsman, J. K. (1989). Risk and resiliency in childhood: The lives of street children. In T. F. Dugan & R. Coles (Eds.), The child in our times: Studies in the development of resiliency (pp. 56-80). New York: Brunner/Mazel. Galambos, N. L., & Leadbeater, B. J. (2000). Trends in adolescent research for the new millennium. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(3), 289-294. Garmezy, N. (1976). Vulnerable and invulnerable children: Theory, research, and intervention. Journal Supplement Abstract Service, A.P.A. Genero, N. (1998). Culture, resiliency and mutual psychological development. In H. McCubbin, E. A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson & J. A. Futrell (Eds.), Resiliency in African-American families (pp. 31-48). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women's development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Glantz, M. D„ & Johnson, J. L. (Eds.). (1999). Resilience and development: Positive life adaptations. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Glantz, M. D., & Sloboda, Z. (1999). Analysis and reconceptualization of resilience. In M. D. Glantz &

J. L. Johnson (Eds.), Resilience and developmentPositive life adaptations (pp. 109-128). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Greene, R. R. (Ed.). (2003). Resiliency. Washington, DC: NASW Press. Greene, J. C , & Caracelli, V. J. (2003). Making paradigmatic sense of mixed methods practice. In A. Tashakkori & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research (pp. 91-110). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Hacking, I. (1999). The social construction of what? Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Hill, R. (1998). Enhancing the resilience of African American families. Journal of Human Behaviour in the Social Environment, 1(2/3), 49-61. Houston, S., & Griffiths, H. (2000). Reflections on risk in child protection: Is it time for a shift in paradigms? Child and Family Social Work, 5(1), 1-10. Huang, L. N. (March, 2003). Improving access to care and outcomes for children from diverse racial and ethnic backgrounds and their families. Plenary address, 16th Annual Research Conference, "A System of Care for Children's Mental Health: Expanding the Research Base," University of South Florida, Tampa. Johnson-Powell, G., & Yamamoto, J. (Eds.). (1997). Transcultural child development: Psychological assessment and treatment. New York: John Wiley. Kaplan, H. B. (1999). Toward an understanding of resilience: A critical review of definitions and models. In M. D. Glantz & J. L. Johnson (Eds.), Resilience and development: Positive life adaptations (pp. 17-84). New York: Kluwer/Plenum. Knox, L. M. (1998). The ecology of resilience in the inner-city: Redefining resilience in the lives of high-risk inner-city youth. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Austin. Ladner, J. A. (1971). Tomorrow's tomorrow: The black woman. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books. Leonard, P. (1997). Postmodern welfare: Reconstructing an emancipatory project. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Loeber, R., & Farrington, D. P. (2000). Young children who commit crime: Epidemiology, developmental origins, risk factors, early interventions, and policy implications. Development and psychopathology, 12(A), 737-762. Luthar, S. S. (Ed.). (2003). Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the context of childhood adversities. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

xxxviii · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71(3), 543-562. Madill, Α., Jordan, Α., & Shirley, C. (2000). Objectivity and reliability in qualitative analysis: Realist, contextualist and radical constructionist epistemologies. British Journal of Psychology, 91(1), 1-20. Markowitz, F. (2000). Coming of age in post-soviet Russia. Chicago: University of Illinois Press. Martineau, S. (1999). Rewriting resilience: A critical discourse analysis of childhood resilience and the politics of teaching resilience to "kids at risk. " Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia, Vancouver. Massey, S., Cameron, Α., Ouellette, S., & Fine, M. (1998). Qualitative approaches to the study of thriving: What can be learned? Journal of Social Issues, 54(2), 337-355. McCubbin, Η. I., Fleming, W. M., Thompson, A. I., Neitman, P., Elver, K. M., & Savas, S. A. (1998). Resiliency and coping in "at risk" AfricanAmerican youth and their families. In Η. I. McCubbin, E. A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson, & J. A. Futrell (Eds.), Resiliency in AfricanAmerican families (pp. 287-328). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McCubbin, Η. I., Thompson, Ε. Α., Thompson, A. I., & Fromer, J. E. (1998). Resiliency in Native American and immigrant families. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McCubbin, Η. I., Thompson, Ε. Α., Thompson, A. I., & Futrell, J. A. (Eds.). (1999). The dynamics of resilient families. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Miller, J. B. (1976). Toward a new psychology of women. Boston: Beacon Press. Miller, W., & Crabtree, B.F. (1994). Clinical research. In Ν. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 340-352). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Moffitt, T. E. (1997). Adolescents-limited and lifecourse-persistent offending: A complimentary pair of developmental theories. In T. P. Thornberry (Ed.), Developmental theories of crime and delinquency (pp. 11-54). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. Moghaddam, F. M., Walker, B. R., & Harré, R. (2003). Cultural distance, levels of abstraction, and the advantages of mixed methods. In A. Tashakkori & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods

in social and behavioral research (pp. 111-134). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Morgan, R. H. (1998). The relationship between resiliency factors and behavioural outcomes of children in residential treatment centers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, School of Social Services, Fordham University. Murphy, L. B., & Moriarty, A. E. (1976). Vulnerability, coping, and growth from infancy to adolescence. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Nelson, N., & Wright, S. (Eds.). (1995). Power and participatory development. London: Intermediate Technology. Osherson, S. (1992). Wrestling with love: How men struggle with intimacy with women, children, parents and each other. New York: Fawcett Columbine. Queiro-Tajalli, I., & Campbell, C. (2003). Resilience and violence at the macro level. In R. R. Greene (ed.), Resiliency (pp. 217-240). Washington, DC: NASW Press. Rahey, L., & Craig, W. M. (2002). Evaluation of an ecological program to reduce bullying in schools. Canadian Journal of Counselling, 36(4), 281-296. Rapoport, R. N. (1997). Families as educators for global citizenship: Five conundrums of intentional socialization. International Journal of Early Years Education, 5(1), 67-77. Rutter, M. (2001). Psychosocial adversity: Risk, resilience and recovery. In J. M. Richman & M. W. Fraser (Eds.), The context of youth violence: Resilience, risk and protection (pp. 13-41). Westport, CT: Praeger. Rutter, M., Maughan, B., Mortimore, P., & Ouston, J. (1979). Fifteen thousand hours: Secondary schools and their effects on children. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Saussure, F. (1978). Course in general linguistics (C. Bally & A. Sechehaye, Eds.; W. Baskin, Trans.). London: Collins. Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. Α., (Eds.). (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Committee on Integrating the Science of Early Childhood Development; Board on Children, Youth and Families; National Research Council & Institute of Medicine. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Surrey, J. L. (1991). The "self-in relation": A theory of women's development. In J. V. Jordan,

Introduction · xxxix A. G. Kaplan, J. B. Miller, I. P. Stiver, & J. L. Surrey (Eds.), Women's growth in connection (pp. 51-66). New York: Guilford Press. Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999). Socially undesirable need not be incompetent: A response to Crick and Dodge. Social Development, 8(1), 132-134. Swartz, L. (1998). Culture and mental health: A southern African view. Cape Town, South Africa: Oxford University Press. Tarter, R. E„ & Vanyukov, M. (1999). Re-visiting the validity of the construct of resilience. In M. D. Glantz & J. L. Johnson (Eds.), Resilience and development: Positive life adaptations (pp. 85-100). New York: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum. Tashakkori, Α., & Teddlie, C. (Eds.). (2003). Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Tully, C. T. (2003). Social work policy. In R. R. Greene (Ed.), Resiliency (pp. 321-336). Washington, DC: NASW Press.

Ungar, M. (2002). Playing at being bad: The hidden resilience of troubled teens. Lawrencetown Beach, Nova Scotia, Canada: Pottersfield Press. Ungar, M. (2004). Nurturing hidden resilience in troubled youth. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University of Toronto Press. Walsh, F. (1998). Strengthening family resilience. New York: Guilford. Weihenmayer, E. (2001). Touch the top of the world: A blind man's journey to climb farther than the eye can see. New York: Dutton. Werner, Ε. E., & Smith, R. S. (1982). Vulnerable but invincible: A longitudinal study of resilient children and youth. New York: McGraw-Hill. Werner, E. E„ & Smith, R.S. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY: Cornel University Press. Werner, E. E„ & Smith, R. S. (2001). Journeys from childhood to midlife: Risk, resilience, and recovery. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

PART 1 THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES

1 CHILDREN'S RISK, RESILIENCE, AND COPING IN EXTREME SITUATIONS Jo BOYDEN GILLIAN MANN

CHALLENGES TO CHILDREN'S WELL-BEING Adversity comes in many forms, as a result of social or political strife, individual acts of omission or commission, environmental calamities, and many other causes. Due to their youthfulness and, specifically, their lack of social power, children and adolescents are often among the most severely affected by these adverse circumstances. Poverty, armed conflict, forced migration, family problems, environmental degradation, and exploitation, all rising to unprecedented levels, have deepened concern internationally for the protection of children and for the promotion of their health and well-being. 1

With the nearly universal ratification of the UN Convention on the Rights of the Child (CRC), the protection of children exposed to adversity has now become one of the central priorities of childhood interventions internationally. The convention provides a comprehensive global framework for supporting children in both chronic and episodic conditions of stress. Modern policy has clearly embraced the ethical and moral view that children have a right to special consideration and that children exposed to exceptionally harsh situations merit the greatest concern. But on what terms do we extend such consideration? The logical outcome of public sector austerity and the gradual dismantling of state structures—or the absence of such

AUTHORS' NOTE: We would like to express our gratitude to the Andrew W. Mellon Foundation, which generously provided the funds that have made this chapter and the research that underpins it possible. We are also very grateful to Jo de Berry and Andy Dawes for their constructive comments on early drafts of this chapter and especially to William Myers who contributed many important insights that challenged us to become far bolder and more creative in our thinking. 3

4 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES structures in the first place—is that children and their families and communities cannot necessarily count on the promises made in international treaties. Moreover, due to the sheer scale of some childhood problems—for example, children in Africa orphaned by AIDS or those experiencing armed political strife—affected populations are frequently forced to rely on their own individual capacities to cope. To say this is not to exonerate callous governments that choose to ignore their responsibilities toward children, but to be pragmatic about the immediate prospects for large numbers of children and to focus policy attention on the challenges that lie ahead. Although there are many structural and practical obstacles to the development of effective measures for children, there is evidence that shortcomings in policy and practice are also the result of erroneous conceptualization of problems and their solutions, inadequate empirical evidence to support specific interventions, and unquestioned assumptions about children's development and their relative capacities and vulnerabilities. Indeed, recent research in the social sciences and experience in dealing with children in stressful situations, as will be discussed in this chapter, are providing new insights that challenge much conventional wisdom about how to assist affected children. Because it is increasingly clear that many notions of childhood and of childhood vulnerability, development, and well-being are contextually constructed (see Ungar, 2004), serious doubt is being cast on the relevance of many traditional prescriptions for protecting children, especially interventions imposed from outside the child's social and cultural context. At the same time, new insights and ideas that could be more helpful have not been widely disseminated or evaluated. Under increasingly difficult circumstances globally, the CRC's demand that children's best interests be a major criterion for actions concerning them has raised disturbing doubts and questions regarding how to define and deliver what is most appropriate and effective for young people. Scholars and practitioners in different parts of the world have been trying to find out what effects misfortune has on children's social, psychological, and emotional well-being and

to provide appropriate psychological and social care and support. However, child protection remains an uncertain art, beset by challenges and disputation at the methodological, conceptual, theoretical, and practical levels. The present chapter explores some of the issues and controversies pertinent to a discussion of children's vulnerability, resilience, and coping in situations of extreme hardship, highlighting problems and gaps in existing research and recommending areas for further theoretical development and field research. It makes the case for a dynamic, contextualized view of misfortune and suggests that children's experiences of adversity are mediated by a host of internal and external factors that are inseparable from the social, political, and economic contexts in which children live. It also calls for greater attention to children's own understandings of their experience. Throughout, it asserts the need for research, policy, and programmatic interventions to consider carefully the reality of children's lives in order to improve the effectiveness of interventions designed to assist them.

CHILD DEVELOPMENT, "RISK," AND "RESILIENCE" IN THE LITERATURE In this section, we examine some of the key concepts and theories that inform discussions of development, well-being, risk, and resilience in children who are exposed to adversity. Despite the global application of these concepts, the most systematic and influential body of information on child development and well-being and on the factors that mediate risk and resilience during childhood is found in research with children in the United States and Europe. Up until the early 1980s, scholarly understandings of child development were dominated by the work and ideas of Jean Piaget, whose emphasis was on the uniformities of children's development and the ways in which the individual child makes sense of the "generic" world (Rogoff, 1990). The Piagetian model stresses that individual children actively construct knowledge through their actions in the world. Learning takes place when a child is required to reconcile his or her expectations of the world and his or her actual experience of that world.

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations · 5 This constructivist approach is still important. However, in the last 25 years psychologists and anthropologists interested in child development in diverse cultural contexts have challenged Piaget's view of the child as a solitary, independent individual whose interaction with the world leads him or her to spontaneously develop general skills and strategies that can then be applied across logically similar problems (Cole, Gay, Glick, & Sharp, 1971; Rogoff, 1990). These scholars have become increasingly attracted to the sociocultural approach of Lev Vygotsky (1978) and the work of later theorists such as Barbara Rogoff (1990), Michael Cole (1996), and Jacqueline Goodnow (1990). The main tenet of this more recent perspective is that all psychological phenomena originate through interpersonal interaction and hence social and cultural context provide the framework for how children learn to think, speak, and behave. 2

This focus on the importance of social interaction to child development is reflected in the methodology implicit in Vygotskyian theory, in which activity, rather than the individual, is the basic unit of analysis. "Activity" from this perspective includes not only the task at hand but also people, interpersonal relations, goal-directed behavior, and shared understandings. In this view, individuals are active agents in their own environment; they engage with the world around them, and in some senses, create for themselves the circumstances of their own development. The central aspect of this approach is the notion that through participation in cultural activities, and with guidance from more skilled peers, siblings, and adults, children can learn to think and to develop new skills and more mature approaches to problem solving. It is generally assumed that all communities establish ways of helping children to build connections between their current knowledge and those skills and understandings they are capable of acquiring (Rogoff, 1990). This stress on the importance of activity, relationships, and interaction provides a useful framework within which to explore the influence of social and cultural context on child development. The ways in which children in diverse settings learn to respond to adversity and extreme hardships are critical components of this developmental process. Consciously or not, caregivers

and others structure children's learning environments to support boys and girls to acquire the knowledge, skills, and experience they need to function successfully in their community. The classification of certain experiences or circumstances as "risky" or "dangerous" is thus not a straightforward, universal given. Boys and girls of different ages and different abilities in different contexts will understand and make meaning of their experiences in different ways. These understandings will be influenced not only by their individual genetic heritage and physical and biological maturity but also, and more particularly, by the social, cultural, economic, and political environment in which they live. In assessments of the forces that undermine children's development and well-being, research in the minority world tends to focus on children who confront severe family and personal difficulties, such as recurrent ill-health, maltreatment, family separation and divorce, chronic poverty, and parental mental illness and unemployment (Garmezy & Rutter, 1983; Werner & Smith, 1998). Indeed, the death of a parent is highlighted as one of the more immediately traumatizing events for a child, linked with later psychic disorder, notably depression. This research originated in the field of psychopathology and responded initially to concerns among parents, welfare professionals, and other adults about a perceived rise in childhood problems, such as school failure, juvenile crime, and attempted and actual suicide among young people (Fraser, 1997). One of the objectives of the research was to identify factors in children's lives that increase risks for such behaviors and, insofar as these studies aimed to serve policy, establish how structural reform or service provision could prevent or reduce these risks (Garmezy, 1983). Initial concerns focused on exploring a possible correlation between stressful life experiences in children and a range of psychiatric disorders. Today, the focus for many scholars has shifted to the determination of the factors that enable children to remain competent in the face of adversity. Although studies of children in the minority, industrialized world focus primarily on intrafamilial risk, research on young people in the majority world tends to address major societal events and situations, such as armed conflict, mass murders, famine, or mass displacement.

6 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES Such catastrophic events are generally identified in this research as being "beyond the normal" range of human experience because they cause disturbance and upheaval, not just at a personal and familial level but throughout society (Ager, 1996; De Vries, 1996). They threaten family and community coping, destroy social and cultural institutions, and distort social norms and values (De Vries, 1996). There is a particular concern that such experiences overwhelm children psychologically, undermining their development, coping, and future adaptation in adulthood. Children's individual responses to adversity have been described in the research in terms of "risk" and "resilience." Risk refers to variables that increase individuals' likelihood of psychopathology or their susceptibility to negative developmental outcomes (Goyos, 1997). Some risks are found internally; they result from the unique combination of characteristics that make up an individual, such as temperament or neurological structure. Other risks are external; that is, they result from environmental factors, such as poverty or war, which inhibit an individual's healthy development. Despite the apparently devastating odds, however, not all children exposed to risks and adversities develop problems later on. In the literature, these children are deemed resilient. As Schaffer (1996) notes, "Whatever stresses an individual may have encountered in early years, he or she need not forever more be at the mercy of the p a s t . . . . children's resilience must be acknowledged every bit as much as their vulnerability" (p. 47). Historically, the notion of resilience entered the health sciences from applied physics and engineering, where it refers to the ability of materials to "bounce back" from stress and resume their original shape or condition. A rubber ball is an example. The term seems to have been first used in medicine to characterize the recovery of patients from physical traumas such as surgery or accidents. Somewhat later, it was adopted into psychology, first for the study of children of mentally ill mothers. It is now understood to indicate an individual's capacity to recover from, adapt, and remain strong in the face of adversity. Hence, the literature ascribes resilience to three kinds of phenomena: (a) good outcomes despite high-risk status, (b) sustained competence under threat, and (c) recovery from trauma (Masten, Best, & Garmezy, 1990).

Resilience is recognized as depending on both individual and group strengths and is highly influenced by supportive elements in the wider environment. These positive reinforcements in children's lives are often described as "protective factors" or "protective processes." They operate at different levels and through different mechanisms—individual, family, communal, institutional, and so on—and frequently correlate with and complement one another. Their effects are shown only in their interaction with risk. Although it is understood in the literature that risk and resilience are not constructed the same way in all societies, it is generally accepted that the interaction of risk and protective factors plays an important role in the social and psychological development of boys and girls in all contexts. The concepts of risk, resilience, and protective factors have now come to form the bedrock of research on children who live with adversity, although, as we argue below, they are not without their problems and limitations.

KEY MECHANISMS OF RISK AND RESILIENCE Research has identified several processes or mechanisms at the individual, family, and wider environmental levels that have been shown to have a significant influence on risk and resilience in children. For example, a healthy, strong child is likely to be more resilient emotionally and psychologically than one who is physically weak or sick. Likewise, gender has been found to have an important effect on the way in which children respond to adversity (Werner & Smith, 1998), although the literature tends not to provide consistent patterns linking gender with coping, resilience, or vulnerability. Among other individual attributes in children, age, temperament, sense of humor, memory, reasoning, perceptual competencies, sense of purpose, belief in a bright future, and spirituality have all been found to have a significant impact on resilience (see, e.g., Bernard, 1995; Garbarino, 1999). These protective factors shape to a large extent the strategies that children use to manage stressful situations and to defend themselves against painful experiences or low self-esteem.

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations · 7 Some children are better able to manage stress because of disposition or temperament. Thus, protective factors such as resourcefulness, curiosity, a goal for which to live, and a need and ability to help others are largely matters of temperament and coping style. Generally, children who are able to remain hopeful about the future, are flexible and adaptable, possess problemsolving skills, and actively try to assume control over their lives are likely to be less vulnerable than those who passively accept the adversity they face (Punamaki, 1987). Socially competent children, capable of lateral thinking and problem solving can enhance their coping by identifying alternatives to their current circumstances and devising creative solutions. The capacity to engage in critical thinking can also help to shield a child from simplistic interpretations of experience that are self-defeating (Garbarino, Kostelny, & Dubrow, 1991). Personal history also influences coping (Garbarino, 1999). Children who have experienced approval, acceptance, and opportunities for mastery are far more likely to be resilient than those who have been subjected to humiliation, rejection, or failure. The literature points not just to children's own inner resources and competencies but also to their interpersonal relationships as essential factors mediating risk and resilience. Thus, the presence of at least one supportive adult can have an enormous impact on a child's resilience (see, e.g., Ressler, Boothby, & Steinbock, 1988; Werner & Smith, 1992). Family members and significant others can play a major role in helping children interpret, "process," and adjust to, or overcome, difficult life experiences (Dawes, 1992). Acting as mentors, adults can provide models of and reinforcement for problem solving, motivation, and other coping skills (McCallin & Fozzard, 1991; Punamaki 1987; Richman & Bowen, 1997; Turton, Straker, & Mooza, 1990). Developmental psychology has long emphasized the early bonding between mother and infant and overall quality of nurture within the primary caregiving unit as absolutely fundamental to well-being, especially in younger children. Today, however, it is increasingly recognized that in many societies the mother is only one of several caregivers and that, consequently, children's attachments may be quite dispersed (Mann, 2001).

The centrality of emotional attachment to and support from a significant reference person is revealed in its absence. In the late 1990s in Sierra Leone, children who were separated from their families following abduction by the rebel forces or whose parents had been killed or deserted them during the war faced many grave difficulties. Without a caring guardian to take their side, children expressed feelings of being branded and were susceptible to discrimination and hostility. Fostering by extended family members does not always resolve their problems, as one adolescent girl emphasized: You have to be humble to the aunt and uncle and show them respect. You must not be proud. Because you don't have mother, you don't have father so you have no other choice but to be humble. If you do good things you never get praised— they always shout on you and put you down. (15-year-old girl, Makeni, Sierra Leone, quoted in Boyden, Eyber, Feeny, & Scott, 2004, p. 58) Such sentiments have been commonly reported by separated and orphaned children in numerous situations, including Liberia (Tolfree, 2004), Sudan (Vraalsen, in press), Sri Lanka (Galappatti, 2002), Tanzania (Mann, 2002), Malawi (Mann, 2003a), and elsewhere. Boys and girls in these circumstances described how difficult their lives were without the love and protection provided by at least one close adult. In many villages in South Sudan, children argued that those orphaned boys and girls who live with extended families are just as vulnerable as those who live entirely without adult or family care (Vraalsen, in press). Not only do supportive relationships with family and nonparental adults help to protect children from the negative effects of stressful situations, there is considerable evidence that social support from peers can greatly enhance children's resilience. This is clearly recognized by the Maasai, who live in an area of Kenya and Tanzania that is prone to severe drought and famine. The Maasai have a strong sense of clan, family, and community identity in which suffering is traced historically to the difficulties faced by the Maasai as a people. Traditional Maasai risk management strategies reflect a close familiarity with hunger and other hardships, such that

8 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES young morans (teenage boys who are learning to become warriors) must always travel and eat in pairs for mutual support and protection (Boyden et al., 2004). In this context, boys learn from a young age not only the importance of teamwork and cooperation but also how to provide material, emotional, and physical assistance to peers. In addition to the confidence they build in their own ability to confront future challenges, they learn to trust that they are not alone and that others can assist them in times of need. Positive peer relationships provide children with an arena of support outside the family in which they can experiment, develop attitudes, skills, and values, and learn to share, help, and nurture one another. These relationships become especially important during middle childhood and adolescence and both mitigate the negative effects of adversity and contribute to a child's sense of self-esteem. This process may in turn enhance the development of other protective factors, such as a sense of competence and an ability to form other meaningful relationships, empathize, and feel a sense of belonging. In short, friendships provide children, like adults, with opportunities to be themselves and to feel good about who they are—processes that help to build resilience. This truth was evident in research with children affected by HIV/AIDS in Malawi, where girls between the ages of 8 and 14 years described the opportunity to sit with friends and "share secrets" as one that made them feel "strong" and "less alone" (Mann, 2003a). Neighborhoods and institutions such as schools and organized community groups can supplement protective factors at the individual and family levels by providing a supportive context for children. In industrialized countries, the state is fundamental in this regard, aiding children through a range of interventions, including food subsidies, housing, and social insurance. Today, specialized state-run institutions of childhood—child care and leisure centers, schools, and so on—complement the traditional roles and functions of the family. During crises, they may replace family altogether. In contrast, most poor countries do not have the resources for widespread state support to communities, families, or children living in adversity. Where high birth rates, early mortality, and educational wastage are pressing problems,

birth spacing, health, and education services take priority over social services and welfare policies. In this context, it is often a bonus if government assistance programs exist at all. Where they do, they frequently suffer heavily from underinvestment, poor outreach, high transaction costs, or corruption. Experience demonstrates that in the absence of state intervention, child protection often relies on the mobilization efforts of civil society and participation from various community groups. These informal protective processes may include collective activities (such as joint labor on community projects or labor exchanges on farms) and institutional support (e.g., credit supplied by money lenders, remedies offered by traditional healers, or spiritual guidance provided by religious officials). Sometimes crisis itself can lead to the development of protective processes that enhance resilience in children. In Milange, Mozambique, following the devastation of war, reconciliation and forgiveness was achieved by acknowledging and celebrating the return of former combatants, who put the war behind them by partaking in ceremonies and confessionals (Gibbs, 1994). Reconstruction was achieved through the management of everyday activities such as building houses and planting fields in which children— including former child combatants—played an integral role. The church and local healers played a major part in the reconstruction, focusing and reaffirming the process. On the other hand, in impoverished and AIDS-affected areas of southern Kenya, assistance for struggling families from the village clan committee and other sources is said to be on the decline. Whereas once there existed a broad network of neighbors and other village members to ensure that no children would go hungry, nowadays, individual or familial selfhelp strategies are favored over collective ones. One elderly woman recalled how at one time "a child would belong to the community." This is no longer the case, she said (Boyden et al., 2004).

THE LIMITATIONS OF EXISTING RESEARCH Much of the literature on risk and resilience in childhood is based on longitudinal studies that

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations · 9 cover the life span well into adulthood and amplify important individual differences between children. As such, that literature has made a major contribution to our understanding of the personal, familial, and broader environmental influences on children's well-being and development in adverse situations. In so doing, it has vital implications for children throughout the world and should be widely disseminated among practitioners and policymakers. However, even after decades of learning and experience in this field in some countries, we still do not know the extent to which these protective factors are universal across cultures. We also do not know enough about the effects on children of exposure to adversities of different kinds or the ways in which children respond to and deal with these experiences. A review of the existing research evidence on childhood adversity indicates several major shortcomings. Before commencing a discussion of some of the most significant debates and issues associated with the topic, we highlight the problems with existing research. Limitations of Terminology. First , although the idea of resilience has for some purposes proven itself a useful way of imaging human ability to thrive in the face of adversity, it suffers several important limitations, both conceptual and cultural. Early writers employing the term conscientiously proposed it not as an explanation but as a temporary convenience until such time as scientific theories of natural human resistance to psychological stress could be properly formulated (Anthony, 1987). With time, however, this makeshift construct gradually came to be treated as confirmed fact by all but a few theoretical researchers. A fully featured scientific theory to explain unexpectedly positive human response to adversity still lies in the future. As might be expected of an impressionistic metaphor imported into psychology from the natural sciences, it has been impossible to define resilience with the precision necessary to confirm it through rigorous scientific research. A variety of sometimes incompatible concepts and definitions are currently in use. The term was at first commonly characterized as a trait of individuals—children were classified as "vulnerable" or "resilient"—and much of the lay literature is still in that vein. However, as 3

research and careful reflection (Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000; Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000) demonstrated this concept to be untenable, the definitional focus became both more collective, centered on families and other groupings and more abstract, portraying resilience as a "dynamic developmental process" (Yates, Egeland, & Sroufe, 2003) or the transaction between individuals and their environment. It seems increasingly clear that resilience cannot be directly observed and measured and, in fact, is only inferred from observations based on the related constructs of risk and "positive adaptation" (Luthar, 2003). This raises the question of whether anything like resilience actually exists in nature; perhaps it is only a conceptual artifact. There is no question that some children submitted to severe stress do survive better than expected, but it is not clear that one needs a mediating factor such as resilience to explain it. This doubt is now a matter of debate. Some researchers have suggested that the issue can be tested by statistically sorting the direct effects of "protective" variables from interactions that might suggest a mediating factor (i.e., resilience). At least some analysis of this type finds that the direct, context-specific effects of protective factors can explain virtually all positive adaptation, which suggests that the notion of a general quality of resilience is superfluous (Wyman, 2003). More analysis of this type is required, but some initial findings imply that resilience is perhaps an idea we do not need to explain why some children thrive in harsh conditions and others do not. Limited Assumptions. Second, ideas of resilience call on subthemes taken for granted in contemporary Europe and North America that are not so readily accepted by equally sophisticated intellectual systems elsewhere, including in science. A good example is the concept of self. From early on, resilience was conceived as completely or partially involving self-integration or self-organization. This, of course, requires some sort of self to be integrated or organized, perhaps the sort of core being envisaged in 20thcentury Freudian or humanistic approaches to psychology. The problem is that selfhood has never been shown to exist apart from the construct of it, and other intellectually sophisticated

10 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES cultures and systematic philosophies—such as Buddhism and much of Hinduism, for example— deny that self really exists. They do not deny that we experience a sense of inner self, but they consider that experience to be the most fundamental of all human illusions. There is no objective reason to consider the assertion of independent selfhood more rational than the assertion of no-self. Interestingly, some of today's leading researchers in neurology and evolutionary psychology hold to one or another version of the view that the idea of self is a sort of interpretive illusion compiled from a wide variety of neurological stimuli, perhaps originating from the entire body rather than the brain alone. The idea of risk, as used in much of the riskand-resilience literature, is also culturally and normatively loaded, as is the notion of competence, which is seen as the basis for resilience by highly influential researchers such as Garmezy and Masten. Even as used in the social science literature with a certain amount of care, these are not terms that always enjoy easy transit between cultural contexts. Even the negative conceptualization of adversity as hostile factors exogenous to the individual—the essential setting for the emergence of resilience—is regarded by a sizable number of non-Western cultures and philosophies as partial, shortsighted, and naive. Instead, they see connections between what we perceive and what we create. Rogoff (2003) notes that this objection is a difficult point for Westerners, including many social scientists, because schemes "separating the individual and the world are so pervasive in the social sciences that we have difficulty finding other ways to represent our ideas" (p. 49). Culture is not an entity that influences individuals. Instead, people contribute to the creation of cultural processes and cultural processes contribute to the creation of people. Thus, individual and cultural processes are mutually constituting rather than defined separately from each other, (p. 51) As we argue below, there is a case to be made that adversity is as much a matter of perception as of situational fact, and many societies deliberately create painful and even potentially dangerous situations for children—such as in some rites of passage or forms of apprenticeship—to

promote their development by teaching them to embrace discomfort as opportunity rather than turning it into adversity. That approach puzzles and horrifies some individuals, who might consider it abuse rather than nurture. The issue is not who is right or wrong, of course, but the degree to which the notion of resilience calls on elements of contemporary Western culture not necessarily shared elsewhere, including in the sciences. Although we use the term resilience here in recognition of its utility as a device for indicating a state that many of us recognize intuitively, we do not regard it as a sound theoretical construct. Indeed, we maintain that resilience may, following further enquiry, appear to be a sensible construct only in certain very limited cultural and intellectual contexts. Limited Scholarship. A third difficulty with the risk-and-resilience research is that scholarship in this field has been limited largely to children and childhood in the industrialized, minority world. Some scholars have gone to considerable lengths in these studies to include children from diverse cultural and economic backgrounds. Nevertheless, inherent in much of the literature on risk and resilience is a view of childhood that is informed by the context of white middle-class family life, not necessarily by the very different realities that shape the lives of children in other parts of the world. The underlying assumption is that children in the minority world have grown up the "right way" and that children everywhere should be raised in the same manner—that is, in the nuclear family, with two parents, without social or economic responsibility, and so on. The implication of this assumption is that this context is the benchmark against which "healthy" childhood is measured. However, ethnographic evidence from diverse cultures suggests that there is no single, uniform approach to child rearing. Multiple developmental pathways exist throughout the world. In accordance with the insights first advanced by Vygotsky (1978), many now recognize that the well-being of children is influenced by the material, social, and cultural aspects of the specific environment in which they live, despite what the literature presumes. The focus of this literature on the minority world and on the kinds of adversities thought

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations · 11 to prevail in industrialized countries limits its explanatory scope. As noted, the most systematic information currently available tends to deal with often chronic personal and family problems that occur within the private domain of the home. Hence, despite the overwhelming evidence concerning the structural causes of most childhood adversities, the most extensive research on risk in childhood concerns far more immediate and personal circumstances, in particular loss of and separation from parents, especially mothers. In the majority world, children tend to face additional and sometimes far greater threats to their well-being that are not adequately addressed in this literature. For example, large numbers of children throughout the globe are routinely exposed to major societal catastrophes that affect entire communities, such as famine, forced displacement, and "ethnic cleansing." Children's responses to these and other adverse situations have been underreported in the literature and are not well understood generally. Limited Interpretation. Finally, there is the additional problem that much of what we know about childhood experience from the research is based on adult interpretation and supposition. Most of the studies on children's vulnerability and resilience draw on researchers' preconceived ideas about what constitutes adversity or risk for children. Often, adults (parents, teachers, and others who are close to children) are used as respondents. The result is that, in many cases, we do not have accurate information on children's own perceptions. This is problematic given that there is emerging evidence that children do not share the same understanding of risk and adversity as do adults. The privileging of adult perceptions over children's experiences has sometimes meant that, in practice, resilience is conceived of more as the absence of pathology rather than the presence of personal agency in children. Thus, in many of the studies of war-affected and displaced children, resilience is tantamount to the lack of trauma or psychiatric disorder; the notion that children's own resourcefulness may promote their mental health is, in many cases, entirely foreign. In contrast, the psychological literature does envision children's personal attributes as contributing significantly to resilience, embracing

the idea of children as resourceful. Nevertheless, much of the discussion of children's competence in practice focuses on the rather passive notion of protective factors, not the idea of children actively managing and even in some instances improving their situation.

THE SOCIAL AND POLITICAL CAUSES OF RISK AND RESILIENCE The factors that determine whether and how a person comes to experience adversity operate at numerous levels. Practical experience has shown us that there are significant disparities between groups and categories of children in terms of their exposure to risk and survival, coping, and well-being and that such disparities normally have structural causes relating to discrepancies in social power. Often, these structural threats at the macro level are transmitted through successive generations of a population, community, or family and are seldom actually within the control of the individual they affect. Caste is one structural configuration that permanently disadvantages some people, for the status, classification, and indeed, much of the fate of over 100 million dalits ("untouchables") in India are determined at conception and cannot be changed even after death. As a child grows up, this structural vulnerability is then compounded if the child happens to be a girl (and thereby suffers intrahousehold inequities), lives in a rural area (with limited or no access to land and basic services), and has a disability (attracting social stigma). Although each of these factors is no doubt a hindrance in terms of isolation, it is through their interaction and accumulation within the life of an individual child that he or she is rendered susceptible to developmental disruption and psychological, social, or emotional distress. Structural disadvantage also applies at the micro level. Children who are distinguished by social attributes such as gender, ethnicity, or religion and by personal attributes such as temperament, physique, or cognitive ability tend to be valued and treated very differently within families and communities. Gender is one of the most striking and enduring examples of difference within childhood. Girls are generally

12 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES stronger biologically than boys, as evidenced by their higher survival rates after birth. Yet many societies have powerful gender preferences, and these preferences have differential effects on the life chances and well-being of boys and girls. In plough agriculture in Asia and Europe, for example, there is an apparent partiality toward boys (Robertson, 1991) because daughters have to be given dowries and are "lost" to parents as a support in old age. In African hoe agriculture, on the other hand, families have good productive as well as reproductive reasons to welcome girls and good political reasons (such as perpetuation of a lineage) to want boys. In most societies where there are major distinctions, baby and young girls are at far greater risk of exposure to adversity than boys. But as children grow older, less is known about the nature and influence of the risks faced by both genders. There is an assumption in the literature that the burdens, deprivations, and dangers of girlhood are more extreme, but this assumption remains to be proven. We simply do not know enough about how boys and girls of different ages in different social, political, and cultural contexts experience and interpret risk. During times of trouble, distinctions based on gender, ethnicity, and physical ability grow, sometimes with very serious consequences for children who are the least valued. For example, perpetuity of the social group is very important in many contexts of enduring hardship where mutual interdependence is strong and the individual cannot survive alone. In such settings, durability of the family group is commonly a greater priority than the relative well-being of individual offspring. Children in certain categories may be considered surplus to requirements and abandoned (Engle, Castle, & Menon, 1996). Recent research among displaced families in the Western Upper Nile region of Sudan has shown that disabled children are more likely than their able-bodied peers and siblings to die as a result of being left behind when families are forced to flee their villages (Vraalsen, in press). Likewise, in other extreme situations, children may be considered a good that can be exchanged or traded for income or used to forge links with political or economic allies. This practice is not uncommon in parts of Burma, where desperate families may receive

money in exchange for "lending" girls as young as 12 to "employers" in Thailand. Such girls often end up working in the sex industry (Mann, 2000). Indeed, gender preference directly affects survival in some settings. In South Asia, gender discrimination is a major determinant of demographic distortion, with fewer than expected females in the population relative to males (Drèze & Sen, 1995). This pattern is linked to female infanticide, the abortion of female fetuses, and gender discrepancies in the allocation of food and health care. Differences between groups of children often have explicitly political origins. Indeed, children's rights advocates have for some time been gathering evidence pointing to the political causes of much childhood deprivation and suffering. They have shown that the State can imperil children just as much as protect and nurture them, sometimes under the guise of protection. They have uncovered instances not just of inaction by the State but, more seriously, of acts of commission. Often, the State actively victimizes certain categories of children, as in the case of racist policies that discriminate against specific religious, cultural, or ethnic groups in the provision of services, access to resources, and so on. Hence, historically, apartheid policies in South Africa produced major disparities among ethnic groups in terms of youth and child morbidity and mortality, literacy, employment, personal security, and civil and political rights. Variations in patterns of resilience and coping at the group level are also a function of cultural beliefs about childhood and child development. As indicated, ethnographic evidence from several parts of the world and recent research in the tradition of cultural psychology suggest that childhood is a diverse, shifting category shaped by cultural and social context. Thus, although it may be true that all children have certain basic needs and vulnerabilities in a very general sense, differentiation between societies in definitions of childhood and understandings of and approaches to child development produce very real differences in terms of children's experiences, attitudes, and behavior (Bronfenbrenner, 1986, 1996; Cole, 1992; Wilson, 1998;Woodhead, 1998, p. 17). Particular societies have their own ideas about the capacities and vulnerabilities of children, the ways in 4

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations · 13 which they learn and develop, and those things that are good and bad for them. These ideas affect approaches to child socialization, learning, discipline, and protection and, hence, to a significant degree, circumscribe children's adaptation, resilience, and coping (Dawes & Donald, 1994; Super & Harkness, 1986; Woodhead, 1998). In other words, the social arrangements, child development goals, and child-rearing practices of the communities in which children live play a fundamental part in determining the different capabilities and susceptibilities that children develop.

diagnostic category—namely, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Use of the term trauma is very conscious in this context, for it indicates an emotional wound or shock resulting from exposure to an event or situation that causes substantial, lasting damage to the psychological development of a person, often leading to neurosis. PTSD was first identified as a syndrome in American veterans of the Vietnam War and has subsequently been identified by the World Health Organization (WHO, 1992) as the most severe psychiatric disorder and primary stress resulting from a catastrophe.

Selective neglect in the family, discrimination in the community, political oppression in national government, and pronounced inequity in international relations are all societal factors undermining children's well-being and development that policymakers have the power to do something about. The question is how to identify which groups and categories of children are the most susceptible and to find ways both of reducing risk among them and providing support to affected children. The problem is that research on risk and resilience in children seldom reveals group distinctions in a meaningful way that can be addressed by policy. On the other hand, policymakers are often reluctant to engage with issues that have political, social, or cultural roots, preferring to depoliticize adversity by defining it as a problem of family or individual pathology. The concern, then, is how research can more effectively capture these kinds of distinctions between groups and categories of children and establish their impact on children's well-being. Equally important is to identify the kinds of policies and practice that will prevent such inequities between groups of children.

Many find the concept of trauma useful in that it highlights how major disasters have potential to undermine children and adults psychologically (see also Solomon & Laufer's work on Israeli children in Chapter 14 of this volume). However, as we have suggested, there are numerous conceptual and methodological problems concerning the definition and measurement of life events and of psychiatric disorders (Garmezy & Rutter, 1983). For example, it is surprising to learn that of those children who suffer serious or prolonged psychological or emotional distress in conflict zones, a significant proportion have not experienced a major misfortune despite the catastrophic circumstances in which they live (Ressler, Tortorici, & Marcelino, 1992). Sometimes the most devastating situations are those involving insidious hardships and deprivations, such as constant humiliation, social isolation, or poverty related to loss of livelihood. For instance, in Dar es Salaam, Congolese refugee children between the ages of 7 and 13 years reported that discrimination and public humiliation by Tanzanian adults and children was so distressing for them that they would often prefer to remain at home than to endure the taunts of neighbors and others (Mann, 2003b). These ongoing stressors can impair the coping resources of children as well as the capacity of the community to support and protect those who care for them. Researchers looking for traumatic responses to situations identified in advance as highly stressful could miss such important subtleties.

BEYOND TRAUMA: THE SOCIAL EFFECTS OF ADVERSITY We have suggested that there is a focus among some scholars on the psychological and emotional effects of highly stressful experiences. This concern with catastrophic events and situations is surely appropriate given their pernicious impact on individuals and societies throughout the world. Many label the most stressful experiences "traumatic" and link them with one specific

As it happens, there has been much controversy surrounding disorders such as PTSD. A number of mental health experts do not accept PTSD as a valid diagnostic category in relation

14 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES to children in particular. Some point out that it is misapplied in many situations of chronic adversity where children continue to be exposed to stress. There is nothing "posttraumatic" about their experiences. Some also argue that the symptoms associated with the syndrome do not only occur in response to major stressful life events (Richman, 1993). Others say that symptoms characteristic of the syndrome such as bed-wetting and nightmares do not constitute "sickness" but a normal physiological reaction to shock. Still others highlight that these kinds of medicalized accounts of human responses to misfortune detract from the political, economic, and social nature of much of the adversity in the world today (Bracken, Giller, & Summerfield, 1995). They are critical of medical approaches that emphasize individual psychopathology and individualized therapeutic care in clinical settings. In fact, whatever one's views on PTSD, it is evident that this kind of diagnosis overlooks many important aspects of human experience during times of great hardship. Take, for example, the cultural bereavement experienced by many of those who are forcibly displaced by armed conflict or civil strife (Eisenbruch, 1991). Research by Armstrong, Boyden, Galappatti, and Hart (2004) with Tamil children aged 9 to 16 in the east of Sri Lanka has revealed a broad array of responses to the adverse conditions in which they live. These boys and girls have experienced displacement, armed violence, and impoverishment, loss of access to education and health care, and many other war-related risks. One of the key criteria of well-being distinguished by these children was "moving well with people," an expression used very positively with reference to a person who is hospitable, kind, polite, and generally relates well to others. The children are aware that not all their peers experience well-being and that there are boys and girls in their midst who in their view suffer impaired social and cognitive functioning. As they see it, impaired social functioning is expressed mainly in terms of solitary, antisocial behavior, symbolized by a desire to be left alone; unwillingness to play; lack of interest in, or interaction with, friends; and an inability to show affection. Cognitive functioning is judged in relation to enthusiasm for learning and sports, and attendance and performance at school.

These Tamil children explained their most frequent and distressing response to adversity as "thinking too much," a condition that can in more severe cases cause constant and intense headaches or heart pain. Most commonly, they associated this condition with the loss, disappearance, or death of a loved one or with fear of abduction and forced recruitment into the military. Other fears and anxieties—about snakebites, the proximity of armed military personnel, elephant attacks, and drowning—were similarly pervasive but seemingly less intense and did not appear to result in "thinking too much" or a somatic effect. During adversity, anxieties and fears of this nature can play a very important part in children's vulnerability as much as in their coping and resilience yet do not figure in PTSD diagnoses. Similarly, such a diagnosis says very little about children's actual functioning on a daily basis. In the Tamil areas of Sri Lanka affected by conflict, social trust outside the immediate family has been largely destroyed because neighbors have developed opposing loyalties and inform on each other to ensure their own survival. Children have few social or institutional resources to turn to for support, and mothers stand out as one of the few sources of love and protection for the young. Tamil children in the East have developed a whole host of coping mechanisms to deal with this corrosive social environment and a range of strategies to avoid abduction and other risks. These include limiting friendships to a few close and trusted peers (often cousins), restricting social visits to nearby houses, avoiding family conflict whenever possible, not discussing family problems with neighbors, being careful not to attract attention in school, staying away from school and in homes during the day; and sleeping in the forest at night. Such strategies are crucial to personal functioning. Insofar as children in middle childhood and adolescence tend to attach a great deal of significance to personal relationships, especially to friendships with peers; social approval and acceptance among peers and in the community at large are vital factors in their well-being. It is therefore not surprising to learn that for children of this age, the experience of adversity is very much mediated by its effects on their social world. In other, words, children gauge the

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations · 15 impacts of adversity not just in terms of psychic pathology but through the constraints it puts on their social networks. Unfortunately, it transpires that children who confront adversity are often denigrated and excluded by others, as was evident in a study of child poverty in India, Belarus, Kenya, Sierra Leone, and Bolivia (Boyden et al., 2004). That study revealed that the way in which poverty undermines an individual's social interactions and relationships with others can be far more important to children than having to go without food or other commodities. Thus, in rural Bolivia, despite knowing full well that chronic shortages of water have a significant effect on livelihoods and on the survival and health of both humans and livestock, children highlighted above all the humiliation of being unable to wash and therefore being labeled smelly, dirty, and poor. These children acknowledged that frequently they are themselves the main instigators of abuses directed at others due to their poverty. In fact, one of the worst consequences of being thought of as "poor" is the associated shame, social exclusion, and susceptibility to teasing, bullying, and humiliation by peers.

CHILDHOOD ADVERSITY CONTEXTUALLY DEFINED One of the strongest criticisms of universalized psychiatric diagnoses such as PTSD is that they seriously underestimate the differences between cultural groups in understandings of and responses to stressful events (Bracken et al., 1995). Undoubtedly, humans have a limited repertoire of responses to stressful life events, and feelings and symptoms will recur across social and cultural boundaries (Parker, 1996). Similarly, intelligence, temperament, good parenting, and family relationships early in life appear to be important contributors to resilience in all cultures and contexts. Nevertheless, children's worldview and mental health are very much influenced by local meanings given to misfortune. As we have suggested, these meanings in turn depend on other concepts—for example, ideas about causality in adversity, well-being, sickness, healing, personhood, identity, and the like (Bit, 1991; Bracken, 1998; Le

Vine, 1999; Parker, 1996; Shweder & Bourne, 1982; Summerfield, 1991, 1998). As critics of universalized diagnostic categories argue, even though certain symptoms of acute distress may occur across cultures and social groups, this does not suggest that their meaning is the same in all settings (Bracken et al., 1995; Parker, 1996). Thus, meaning is a profoundly important mediating factor in children's experiences of adversity, and yet it has been largely ignored in the literature. Indeed, it is our contention that how children respond to adversity cannot be understood without reference to the social, cultural, economic, and moral meanings given to such experiences in the contexts they inhabit. The contextual nature of adversity is illustrated by research on child abuse and neglect across cultures conducted over 20 years ago by Jill Korbin (1981), an anthropologist. Korbin cites examples of practices such as punishments (severe beatings) to impress a child with the necessity of adherence to cultural rules and harsh initiation rites (genital operations, deprivation of food and sleep, and induced bleeding) that to many outsiders would most likely appear abusive. Indeed, some of these practices have been denounced by children's rights advocates as abhorrent. But Korbin goes on to make the sobering point that many practices in the minority world that are accepted as "normal," such as isolating infants and small children in rooms or beds of their own at night or allowing them to cry without immediately attending to their needs or desires are at odds with the childrearing philosophies of most cultures, for whom such behaviors are likewise considered just as "abusive." 5

Even the meaning of death varies significantly, depending on cultural and religious views about whether human existence is irrevocably constrained by the live body, which in turn hinges on ideas about the soul, reincarnation, ancestral spirits, and so on. Views about death and well-being in many parts of the world are built on the notion of congruence, not merely in the functioning of mind and body but also between the human, natural, and spirit worlds. Any or all of these dimensions may play a part in explanations of the cause of misfortune, definitions of suffering, and ways of dealing with distress. In such systems, illness and adversity

16 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES are often caused by the intervention of powerful social, natural, and supernatural agents rather than individual pathology. Well-being is dependent on, and vulnerable to, the feelings, wishes, and actions of others, including spirits and dead ancestors (Lock & Scheper-Hughes, 1990). Hence, sickness is often portrayed as being caused by the witchcraft of neighbors, the forces of nature, or deities. On the other hand, spiritual and supernatural entities may also be perceived as providing protection for children and, indeed, are sometimes thought to be more effective in this regard than individual parental behavior or family circumstances (Engle et al., 1996). These kinds of beliefs sometimes account for approaches to child protection that families regard as appropriate, adaptive, and beneficial but that outsiders perceive as risky or neglectful behavior. For example, among the Acholi in Gulu, northern Uganda, the life of an individual who has died is said to continue in the world of ancestral spirits. Many young people in the area have been abducted and made to fight by the Lord's Resistance Army (LRA). Symptoms of severe emotional and psychological distress in young former combatants are taken to indicate that they are cen, "mad," and have become possessed by the spirits of the people they have killed (Boyden, 2002; Jareg & Falk, 1999). Former child combatants are thought of as in some way "contaminated," and it is held that the "spirit might come out at any time," influencing the person who has been possessed to behave unpredictably or uncontrollably and possibly even to harm others. However, at the same time, because so many of these children were abducted and forcibly recruited, there is a strong will to forgive, reintegrate, and reconcile. That said, before young former abductees are accepted back into their families and communities, proof is required that they are remorseful about acts of violence they have committed and determined to mend their ways. Acceptance tends to be conditional on the performance of rites of cleansing and atonement in which the spirit leaves the body of the possessed person. It has been observed that in many cases these young people appear calmer and more controlled following reintegration. To give another example, a study by Rousseau, Said, Gagné, and Bibeau (1998) of

unaccompanied Somali boys in exile in Canada produced quite unexpected findings. These boys were found to be far more resilient than anticipated, given the many severe hardships that they had experienced. Their resilience and coping was attributed to the fact that they had already become accustomed to long periods of separation from their families and communities prior to exile. This familiarity was due to the traditional pastoral nomadic practice of sending young boys away to tend herds, a practice that enables boys to learn self-sufficiency and autonomy and to acquire status in their communities as proto-adults. Hence, in this particular context, exile and separation from family were viewed not as forms of deprivation or loss but as having certain positive attributes. This evidence suggests that the degree to which stressful situations can be defined objectively as "traumatic" or "outside the realm of normal human experience" is limited. Phinney (1996) argues that the degree of psychological mastery children have in difficult situations to a significant extent reflects the degree to which a culture endorses active management of adversity by encouraging children to develop skills in communication, problem solving, and self-management of behavior. Thus, some societies think of misfortune as a matter of chance or fate, passively accepting and succumbing to events. Others actively train children to become resilient and to cope with unpredictable and painful situations. In the latter case, children may be encouraged to engage in activities that pose at least moderate risk to health and safety, with the aim of developing physical strength, endurance, confidence, dexterity, and selfdiscipline. Inuit children in Canada, for example, are taught to deal with a dangerous and often unpredictable Arctic environment, continuously tested in all spheres of knowledge and competence relating to the world around them and expected to experiment with uncertainty and danger (Briggs, 1986). They learn that the world is made up of problems to be solved: The ability to discover these problems, observe them actively and accurately, and analyze the implications of exposure to hazardous situations is a highly valued quality in Inuit society. In some African societies, learning to be resilient is institutionalized in formal rites of passage. For a

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations · 17 male, initiation into adulthood may involve circumcision or a trial of strength, in which boys become men by passing exacting tests of performance in combat, survival, economic pursuits, and procreativity (Gilmore, 1990). From this research and experience, it is apparent that vulnerability, resilience, and coping in children are not merely functions of health, sickness, or pathological behavioral reactions but also of beliefs and values (Gibbs, 1994; Masten et al., 1990). Dealing with distressing experiences involves making sense of those experiences; assimilating and processing fear, grief, or anger; and finding ways of adapting to, overcoming, or removing difficulties. Although these may be intensely personal processes, individuals engage with misfortune not as isolated beings but in socially mediated ways that are shared (Bracken, 1998; Kleinman & Kleinman, 1991; Reynolds-White, 1998). Crisis, suffering, grief, healing, and loss are all patterned by the social and cultural meanings they manifest. Hence, the "developmental appropriateness" of children's experiences, the "harmfulness" or "benefits" of their environment cannot be separated from the cultural context in which they are developing, the values and goals that inform their lives and their prior experiences of learning skills and ways of thinking. (Woodhead, 1998, p. 13) Children grow and flourish in a whole host of different environments and under a whole variety of circumstances, and what is adaptive in child development is very much a product of these specific settings (Dawes & Donald, 1994).

CHILDREN'S EXPERIENCES: INDIRECT AND COMPLEX EFFECTS ON WELL-BEING There is a view, prevalent in much of the global discourse surrounding child development, child protection, and children's rights, that exposure to misfortune has a direct and automatic deleterious effect on children's development and wellbeing. We have noted that it is common for children so exposed to be regarded as traumatized. This outlook is particularly strong in the literature on war-affected children and is

applied especially to children in early childhood, which is thought to be the most critical period of development, when children are most susceptible to harm that has long-term consequences (Schaffer, 2000). Such a perspective seems, on the surface at least, quite compelling, for as adults, we tend to hold the commonsense view of children (especially young children) as frail and dependent. However, studies of children affected by conflict and displacement in Uganda (De Berry, 2004) and Nepal (Hinton, 2000) have shown that children exposed to many and varied risks are not all inherently vulnerable. Similarly, research from the Balkans has highlighted how vulnerability in childhood does not necessarily preclude ability (Swaine, 2004). Many children are highly adaptable and able to adjust; some show greater personal resilience even than adults (Palmer, 1983). A few authors have suggested that a minority of children can even gain socially, emotionally, or psychologically from exposure to unfavorable conditions (Dawes, 1992; Ekblad, 1993; Garmezy, 1983; Zwi, Macrae, & Ugalde, 1992). For example, in their longitudinal research with children on the island of Kauai in Hawaii, Werner and Smith (1998) found that children in families under stress who are required to attend to family needs become more committed and responsible citizens when they grow up than those raised in more secure circumstances. And in Bhutanese refugee camps in Nepal, it was discovered that, through their conscious caregiving strategies, children were able to have a significant positive impact on the psychological and emotional worlds of adults (Hinton, 2000). Indeed, there is considerable anecdotal evidence that during adversity, children, both boys and girls, often bear the prime responsibilities within the family as caregivers of incapacitated adults or younger siblings, prime earners of family income, and so on. These kinds of findings have led many researchers and practitioners to shift the focus away from pathology and to search for the forces that might protect children from risk and promote their resilience. It is now accepted by many that the psychosocial outcome of exposure to adversity varies from individual to individual and population to population and is mediated by an array of personal, family, and

18 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES broader environmental factors or processes that interact with each other in a dynamic manner. These processes produce either a heightened probability of negative outcome in children's development and well-being or prevent, or reduce, risk. The complex interplay of risk and protective factors can be observed in many cases of child labor, for example. Because children's work is often regarded as securing the transition to adulthood and because earning a wage raises an individual's status within the household, many children gain a powerful sense of independence and self-esteem from work that to expert observers is evidently menial, exploitative, and even dangerous. Thus, despite the risks, children engaged in hazardous work are likely to be buffered psychologically and emotionally if their occupation is socially valued and their work recognized as contributing to family maintenance and integration (Woodhead, 1998). On the other hand, a child whose family does not approve of his or her work or whose job is denigrated by his or her community will likely find it much harder to maintain a sense of self-worth and is at far greater risk of being overwhelmed psychologically by the experience. Thus, the value placed on children's work and the opportunities it affords have a very direct effect on coping and resilience. Hence, children's well-being is mediated and influenced by protective processes at different levels and is highly dynamic and changeable. These processes may operate in different ways by altering exposure to risk, for example, or by reducing negative chain reactions that contribute to the long-term effects of exposure and so on (Rutter, 1987). From the child labor example, we can see that protective processes can "provide resistance to risk and foster outcomes marked by patterns of adaptation and competence" (Garmezy, 1983, p. 49). That said, protective processes are changeable according to situation and context and can in themselves, under certain conditions, become a source of risk. This point is made by Apfel and Simon (1996), who identify a number of individual attributes in children—including resourcefulness, curiosity, intellectual mastery, flexibility in emotional experience, access to autobiographical memory, a goal for which to live, and the need and ability to help others—as contributing to

their resilience. They stress how those attributes that may support well-being in some situations can, under different circumstances, have the opposite effect—they can increase vulnerability. Failure to achieve a desired goal during times of stress, for instance, can lead to loss of hope, selfreproach, and suicidal tendencies. Similarly, children who are highly motivated and driven to achieve may seem well equipped to master difficult situations. Yet these same children can feel inadequate and unsure of themselves when confronted with overwhelming circumstances over which they have no control or influence. Thus, although many children do remain competent in the face of adversity, research and experience warn against presuming such children to be invulnerable. In fact, evidence suggests that the effects of stress are cumulative in that children who are exposed to several stressful events and circumstances are at particular risk of becoming overwhelmed emotionally and psychologically. Furthermore, children who appear resilient in the short term may not be so in the longer term, whereas children who seem more vulnerable initially sometimes grow in competence and resourcefulness (see, e.g., Rutter, 1990). Moreover, competent behavior and effective coping should not be seen as indicating high levels of self-esteem or happiness, for people who have successfully overcome adversity may still experience depression, difficulties with relationships, and so forth (Garmezy, 1993). This evidence highlights the need to recognize that concepts such as resilience and coping should be applied with extreme caution even at the empirical level, let alone the theoretical; their use should not be taken to imply that children who appear to have adapted successfully to difficult situations suffer no ill effects. Nor should they be regarded as fixed states. The challenge in this regard is to identify ways in which resilience and coping in children can best be supported while also being mindful of the psychological and emotional costs to children and of the need to minimize these.

CHILDREN AS SOCIAL AGENTS Most children throughout the world are heavily reliant on the nurture and support of adults or

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations · 19 elder siblings, without which they would fail to thrive, or perish. This fact emphasizes the virtue of making a distinction between children and adults, because such a distinction serves to protect the vulnerable and to ensure the survival and healthy development of all. Indeed, it is striking that most modern policies are based on the premise that adults are the ones best equipped to define what is good and bad for children and also the ones responsible for child protection measures. Certainly, adults have a moral obligation toward the young that includes protection against adversity. This obligation, in regard to the State at least, has now been enshrined in international law through the CRC. Nevertheless, there is considerable evidence globally that adult society is failing children badly. Indeed, many children suffer as a consequence of actions by the very adults—parents, teachers, religious leaders, state officials—who have the greatest obligation toward them. In fact, society is structurally defined to confer on children minority status and in this way constrains their power and agency. However, conceptualizing children as helpless and dependent on adults in times of crisis is not necessarily the most effective way of supporting children's coping and resilience. This is not to deny that some children suffer long-term and highly debilitating psychological and emotional distress and that many need considerable support, specialist care, or both. It is merely to point out that children are not simply the products of adult beliefs, training, investment, and intervention but social agents in their own right. Even those children who are especially troubled can contribute to their own protection, if only in the smallest ways. For example, orphaned and separated Congolese children in Dar es Salaam reported forming alliances with kindhearted Tanzanian adults who agreed to allow the children to sleep in safety outside their homes in exchange for running occasional errands on behalf of the household head (Mann, 2003b). Although labeling children "victims" affords an appropriate emphasis on their suffering and highlights the fact that responsibility for their misfortune lies with others, it tends to characterize children as passive and defenseless in the face of adversity. Viewing children as helpless means that their own efforts to cope are often not

seen as legitimate or, indeed, even recognized at all. This lack of acknowledgment of the validity of children's own strategies can undermine their ability to act on their situation. It is vital to acknowledge that if overcoming stressful life events involves beliefs, feelings, competencies, and actions, children's own perspectives on adversity and the strategies they employ for their own protection are critical to coping and resilience. As we have stated, children do not always understand, experience, or respond to misfortune in the same way adults do. For instance, during the conflict in the Balkans, many Kosovar Albanian parents married their daughters off early so as to protect them from rape, trafficking, and other violations (Swaine, 2004). These girls, however, were unhappy with this strategy because it often resulted in their separation from cherished friends and family members at a time when they were needed most. Moreover, in many cases it restricted them to a lifetime of loneliness, domestic drudgery, and abusive or unhappy marriages. This and countless other examples show that disregarding children's perspectives can result in misplaced interventions that do not address children's real problems or concerns and may even increase their suffering. It is now apparent that supporting children in situations of adversity requires the perspective not just that children need special protection but that they have valid insights into their wellbeing, valid solutions to their problems, and a valid role in implementing those solutions. Such an approach acknowledges children not merely as beneficiaries of intervention by adults or as future societal assets but as competent social actors. For adults to better understand children's perspectives, we must temper adult expertise with some humility and allow children to explain and interpret their childhoods. This is not an easy task, for adults sometimes judge children's coping strategies—being streetwise, for example, or assuming the role of freedom fighter during civil strife—to be detrimental to their well-being. But this reality implies the need for new approaches to planning and policy development that involve more effective consultation and collaboration with children. It requires their inclusion in a broad range of civic processes, especially the identification of policy

20 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES need and impact, and the governance of childhood institutions. It implies also the need for research methods and methodologies that are participatory and child centered and give proper scope for children's testimony. The CRC provides for such an approach, although it is seldom translated effectively into policy and practice, which have tended to employ a far more paternalistic outlook. If children's participation in their own protection is to become a reality, appropriate fora and mechanisms must be developed. There is considerable scope for children to become more involved in the management and implementation of existing institutions and interventions run by adults. But there is also ample opportunity for greater engagement in collective action and mutual support with peers. To suggest that children have a valid role to play in their own protection is not to imply that they should take on the full complement of adult responsibilities or that they be treated as adults. Rather, it is to argue that children should have substantially more opportunities to participate in policy and action than they do currently. It highlights the need to work alongside and "with" rather than "for" children.

CONCLUSION In this chapter, we have argued that the term resilience provides a useful metaphor for the empirical observation that some children, possibly the majority, are surprisingly able to adjust to or overcome situations of serious adversity. Many of these more competent boys and girls appear to remain resourceful in the long term and to adapt well in adulthood; some even find themselves caring for younger siblings and adults more vulnerable than themselves. This quality and the factors that contribute to it are surely worthy of extensive exploration and analysis. Moreover, the ability to isolate and ameliorate risk and enhance protective factors in the life of a developing child is key to effective intervention. If we are to better protect children, we urgently require more information about what renders them vulnerable or resilient, what circumstances are amenable to intervention and change, and how best to assist them.

For interventions to effectively address the actual needs and concerns of children, this information must be grounded in both sound theory and appropriate empirical evidence from a broad range of settings. The existing research into risk and resilience in children confronting adversity goes some way toward addressing these requirements by stressing, for example, the significance of personal traits of the individual child, family circumstances, and peer and institutional support. Nevertheless, we have noted that this literature also embodies certain shortcomings, not least the fact that it makes very little use of children's own understandings and perspectives across cultures and of their active contributions to their well-being, coping, and very survival. We suggest that given the present state of the art, use of the term resilience cannot be taken to imply a fully-fledged theory about how children deal with adversity, for the concept does not stand up to rigorous scientific interrogation, especially when translated across cultural domains. This point matters a great deal because, according to the logic laid down by Vygotsky, culture is not a mere variable in human cognition but a major generative force: It is the lens through which we view the world, learn skills in survival and coping, and interpret and respond to our experiences. If core notions like those of personhood, death, well-being, and so on vary across cultures and if these and other similar notions really do shape the way in which humans address adversities, then scholars need to develop theoretical constructs that have far greater explanatory reach globally. This may mean abandoning some of the long-cherished ideas of the social sciences, such as the dichotomous conceptualization of the individual and the world he or she inhabits. Such expanded constructs should take account of the highly dynamic and mediated nature of human responses to misfortune and the complexity of meanings attached to this experience in different contexts. What at one point in history, in one setting, and for one child may be a hazard, at another time and in another setting and for a different child may be an important stimulus to learning and competence: The factors that mediate risk and resilience may have different effects in each child at different phases during that child's

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations · 21 life. These new constructs should also aid understanding of the effects on children of different kinds of risks, because it cannot be assumed that boys and girls deal with individual or intrafamilial adversities in the same way as they cope with major societal upheavals like war. This recognition implies a shift in emphasis away from the intrapsychic functioning of the individual, generic child and from consideration of that child as an isolated unit of analysis toward greater consideration of structural forces that mediate the well-being of whole groups or categories of children. Thus, there is a great deal more research needed in this field, especially in majorityworld contexts and with children whose lives do not conform to the image perpetuated by much of the existing literature. These children—workers, caregivers, household heads, sex workers, freedom fighters, and so on—have much to teach us in terms of broadening our understanding of well-being and coping in extremely difficult situations. Given the complexity of the issues under consideration and the diversity of children's lives in different circumstances, it is crucial that we do justice to these children's experiences and perspectives. Doing so means increasing our knowledge while avoiding simplistic policy recommendations aimed at reducing risk or enhancing protective factors. It is time to engage with the reality of children's lives in different settings and support their very different and diverse skills and capacities.

NOTES 1. For the purpose of this chapter, a child is defined in accordance with the UN Convention on the Rights of the Child as any individual below the age of 18. 2. Vygotsky articulated his ideas in the early 20th century, but North American and European researchers did not take up his ideas until much more recently. 3. We draw heavily on ideas shared with us by William Myers during personal communications for this and the following point. 4. Charles Super and Sara Harkness (1992) highlight the importance of cultural specificity through the concept of "developmental niche," which they use

to explain how children's needs and development are mediated and expressed in particular ways in particular cultural and social settings. 5. As it happens, some of the most widely accepted ideas about what is detrimental and what is beneficial to children turn out to be founded on a particular ideology or set of interests and therefore have little logic in terms of children's well-being. One has only to contrast internationally promoted attitudes and assumptions toward children's labor force work with those toward children's unpaid household work to comprehend how completely modern attitudes reflect the social and historical context from which they are derived. Why is drudgery that is unpaid household work acceptable and even good for children, whereas drudgery in the paid labor force is unacceptable and bad? Just as paid and unpaid labor force work share many characteristics, so labor force work and housework are often hard to differentiate in terms of children's effort, safety and risk factors, intellectual stimulation, and hours worked. Yet in most policy, domestic work is still considered appropriate for children, whereas labor force work is not. Even if a practice was identified as abusive to children, to exhort families or communities to behave differently would not necessarily foster children's well-being. This is because, as indicated, the effects of adversity on children are determined not merely by the objective nature of an act or situation so much as by children's subjective experience of that situation.

REFERENCES Ager, A. (1996). Children, war and psychological intervention. In S. Carr & J. F. Schumaker (Eds.), Psychology and the developing world (pp. 66-73). Westport, CT: Greenwood. Anthony, Ε. J. (1987). Risk, vulnerability, and resilience: An overview. In E. J. Anthony & B. J. Cohler (Eds.), The invulnerable child (pp. 3-48). New York: Guilford Press. Apfel, R., & Simon, B. (1996). Psychosocial interventions for children of war: The value of a model of resiliency. Medicine and Global Survival. Retrieved December 9, 2004, from www.ippnw.org/MGS/V3Apfel.html. Armstrong, M., Boyden, J., Galappatti, Α., & Hart, J. (2004). Piloting methods for the evaluation of psychosocial programme impact in Eastern Sri Lanka. Report for USAID, Oxford. Retrieved

22 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES December 9, 2004, from www.rsc.ox.ac.uk/ PDFs/ npilotingmethods04.pdf Bernard, B. (1995). Fostering resilience in children (ERIC Digest No. EDO-PS-95). Retrieved December 9, 2004, from resilnet.uiuc.edu/library/ benard95.html Bit, S. (1991). The warrior heritage: A psychological perspective of Cambodian trauma. El Cerrito, CA: Seanglim Bit. Boyden, J. (2002, September). Unpublished field notes: Interviews with former abducted child combatants, Gulu, Uganda. Country Review of Uganda Programme, Save the Children Denmark. Boyden, J., Eyber, C, Feeny, T., & Scott, C. (2004). Children and poverty. Part II. Voices of children: Experiences and perceptions from Belarus, Bolivia, Sierra Leone, India and Kenya (Children and Poverty Series). Richmond, VA: Christian Children's Fund. Bracken, P. (1998). Hidden agendas: Deconstructing post traumatic stress disorder. In P. Bracken & C. Petty (Eds.), Rethinking the trauma of war (pp. 38-59). London: Save the Children Fund UK and Free Association Press. Bracken, P., Giller, J. E., & Summerfield, D. (1995). Psychological responses to war and atrocity: The limitations of current concepts. Social Science and Medicine, 40(&), 1073-1082. Briggs, J. L. (1986, September). Expecting the unexpected: Canadian Inuit training for an experimental lifestyle. Paper presented at the Fourth International Conference on Hunting and Gathering Societies, Ontario, Canada. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723-742. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1996). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Cole, M. (1992). Culture in development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Human development: An advanced textbook (731-789). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J., & D. Sharp. (1971). The cultural context of learning and thinking: An exploration in experimental anthropology. London: Methuen.

Dawes, A. (1992, November). Psychological discourse about political violence and its effects on children. Paper presented at the meeting of the Mental Health of Refugee Children Exposed to Violent Environments, Refugee Studies Programme, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK. Dawes, Α., & Donald, D. (1994). Understanding the psychological consequences of adversity. In A. Dawes & D. Donald (Eds.), Childhood and adversity: Psychological perspectives from South African research (pp. 1-27). Cape Town: David Philip. De Berry, J. (2004). The sexual vulnerability of adolescent girls during civil war in Teso, Uganda. In J. Boyden & J. de Berry (Eds.), Children and youth on the front line: Ethnography, armed conflict and displacement. Oxford & New York: Berghahn Books. De Vries, M. (1996). Trauma in cultural perspective. In B. Van der Volk, A. McFarlane, & L. Weisaette (Eds.), Traumatic stress: The effects of overwhelming experience on mind, body and society (pp. 398-413). London: Guildford Press. Drèze, J., & Sen, A. (1995). Gender inequality and women's agency. In J. Drèze & A. Sen (Eds.), India: Economic development and social opportunity (pp. 140-178). Delhi: Oxford University Press. Eisenbruch, M. (1991). From post-traumatic stress disorder to cultural bereavement: Diagnosis of Southeast Asian refugees. Social Science and Medicine, 33(6), 673-680. Ekblad, S. (1993). Psychosocial adaptation of children while housed in a Swedish refugee camp: Aftermath of the collapse of Yugoslavia. Stress Medicine, 9, 159-166. Engle, P., Castle, S., & Menon, P. (1996). Child development: Vulnerability and resilience. Social Science and Medicine, 43(5), 621-635. Fraser, M.W. (Ed.). (1997). Risk and resilience in childhood: An ecological perspective. Washington, DC: NASW Press. Galappatti, A. (2002). Caring for separated children: An approach from Sri Lanka. Colombo, Sri Lanka: Save the Children Norway. Garbarino, J. (1999, November). What children can tell us about the trauma of forced migration. Seminar presented at the Refugee Studies Programme, University of Oxford, Oxford. Garbarino, J., Kostelny, K., & Dubrow, N. (1991). No place to be a child: Growing up in a war zone. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations · 23 Garmezy, N. (1983). Stressors of childhood. In N. Garmezy & M. Rutter (Eds.), Stress, coping and development in children (pp. 43-84). New York: McGraw-Hill. Garmezy, N. (1993). Children in poverty: Resilience despite risk. Psychiatry, 56(1), 127-136. Garmezy, N., & Masten, A. (1991). The protective role of competence indicators in children at risk. In E. M. Cummings (Ed.), Life span developmental psychology: Perspectives on stress and coping (pp. 151-176). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Garmezy, N., & Rutter, M. (Eds.). (1983). Stress, coping and development in children. New York: McGraw-Hill. Gibbs, S. (1994). Post-war social reconstruction in Mozambique: Reframing children's experience of trauma and healing. Disasters, 18(3), 268-276. Gilmore, D. (1990). Manhood in the making: Cultural concepts of masculinity. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Goodnow, J. J. (1990). The socialization of cognition. In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology (pp. 259-286). New York: Cambridge University Press. Goyos, J. M. (1997). Identifying resiliency factors in adult "Pedro Pan" children: A retrospective study. Doctoral dissertation submitted to Ellen Whiteside McDonnell School of Social Work, Barry University, Miami, FL. Hinton, R. (2000). "Seen but not heard": Refugee children and models of coping. In C. PanterBrick & M. Smith (Eds.), Abandoned children (pp. 199-212). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Jareg, E., & Falk, L. (1999, April-May). Centre-based and community based psychosocial projects for war-affected children. Draft Report for Redd Barna and Red Barnet, Save the Children Norway and Save the Children Denmark, Oslo. Kleinman, Α., & Kleinman, J. (1991). Suffering and its professional transformation: Toward an ethnography of interpersonal experience. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 15(3), 275-301. Korbin, J. (Ed.). (1981). Child abuse and neglect: Cross-cultural perspectives. Berkeley: University of California Press. Le Vine, P. (1999, July). Assessing "detachment" patterns and contextual trauma across cultures (trauma detachment grid). Seminar presented

at the Refugee Studies Centre, University of Oxford, UK. Lock, M., & Scheper-Hughes, N. (1990). A critical reinterpretive approach in medicinal anthropology: Rituals and routines of discipline and dissent. In T. Johnson & C. Sargent (Eds.), Medical anthropology: Contemporary theory and method (pp. 47-72). New York: Praeger. Luthar, S. S. (2003). The culture of affluence: Psychological costs of material wealth. Child Development, 74, 1581-1593. Luthar, S. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). The construct of resilience: Implications for interventions and social policies. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 857-885. Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71(3), 543-562. Mann, G. (2000). Separated children on the ThaiBurma border: A field research report. Unpublished paper, Refugee Studies Centre, University of Oxford, UK. Mann, G. (2001). Networks of support: A literature review of care issues for separated children. Stockholm: Save the Children Sweden. Mann, G. (2002). "Wakimbizi, Wakimbizi": Congolese children's perspectives of life in Dar es Salaam, Tanzania. Environment & Urbanization, 14(2), 115-122. Mann, G. (2003a). Family matters: The care and protection of children affected by HIV/AIDS in Malawi. Stockholm: Save the Children Sweden. Mann, G. (2003b). Not seen or heard: The lives of separated refugee children in Dar es Salaam. Stockholm: Save the Children Sweden. Masten, Α., Best, Κ. M., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contributions to the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2, A25-AAA. McCallin, M., & Fozzard, S. (1991). The impact of traumatic events on the psychosocial wellbeing of Mozambican refugee women and children. Geneva: International Catholic Child Bureau. Palmer, O. J. (1983). The psychological assessment of children (2nd ed.). New York: John Wiley. Parker, M. (1996). Social devastation and mental health in North East Africa. In T. Allen (Ed.), In search of cool ground: War, flight and homecoming in North East Africa (pp. 262-273). London: James Curry.

24 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES Phinney, J. S. (1996). When we talk about American ethnic groups, what do we mean? American Psychologist, 51(9), 918-927. Punamaki, R.-L. (1987). Content of and factors affecting coping modes among Palestinian children. Scandinavian Journal of Development Alternatives, 6(1), 86-98. Ressler, E., Boothby, N., & Steinbock, D. (1988). Unaccompanied children: Care and protection in wars, natural disasters, and refugee movements. New York: Oxford University Press. Ressler, E., Tortorici, J., & Marcelino, A. (1992). Children in situations of armed conflict: A guide to the provision of services. New York: UNICEF. Reynolds-White, S. (1998). Questioning misfortune. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Richman, J. M., & Bowen, G. L. (1997). School failure: An ecological-interactional-developmental perspective. In M. W. Fraser (Ed.), Risk and resilience in childhood (pp. 95-116.). Washington: NASW Press. Richman, N. (1993). Annotation: Children in situations of political violence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(8), 1286-1302. Robertson, A. F. (1991). Beyond the family: The social organisation of human reproduction. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford & New York: Oxford University Press. Rousseau, C , Said, T. M., Gagné, M-J., & Bibeau, G. (1998). Resilience in unaccompanied minors from the north of Somalia. Psychoanalytic Review, 85(4), 615-637. Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57(3), 316-331. Rutter, M. (1990). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. In J. E. Rolf, A. Masten, D. Cicchetti, K. Nuechterlein, & S. Weintraub (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 181-214). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Schaffer, H. R. (1996). Social development. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Schaffer, H. R. (2000). The early experience assumption: Past, present, and future. International Journal ofBehavioural Development, 24(1), 5-14.

Shweder, R., & Bourne, E. (1982). Does the concept of the person vary cross-culturally? In A. Marsella & G. White (Eds.), Cultural conceptions of mental health and therapy (pp. 158-199). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Reidal. Summerfield, D. (1991). Psychological effects of conflict in the Third World. Development in Practice, 1(3), 159-173. Summerfield, D. (1998). The social experience of war and some issues for the humanitarian field. In P. Bracken & C. Petty (Eds.), Rethinking the trauma of war (pp. 9-37). London & New York: Save the Children Fund UK, Free Association Books. Super, C , & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptualisation at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 9, 545-569. Super, C , & Harkness, S. (1992). Cultural perspectives on child development. In D. Wagner & H. Stevenson (Eds.), Cultural perspectives on child development. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. Swaine, A. (with Feeny, T.). (2004). A neglected perspective: Adolescent girls" experiences of the Kosovo conflict of 1999. In J. Boyden & J. de Berry (Eds.), Children and youth on the front line: Ethnography, armed conflict and displacement (pp. 63-86). Oxford & New York: Berghahn Books. Tolfree, D. (2004). Whose children? Separated children's protection and participation in critical situations. Stockholm: Save the Children Sweden. Turton, R., Straker, G., & Mooza, F. (1990). The experiences of violence in the lives of township youth in "unrest" and "normal" conditions. South African Journal of Psychology, 21(2), 77-84. Ungar, M. (2004). Nurturing hidden resilience in troubled youth. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Vraalsen, P. (in press). Separated children in South Sudan: A report of a participatory study. Nairobi: UNICEF. Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Werner, E., & Smith, R. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY, & London: Cornell University Press. Werner, E., & Smith, R. (1998). Vulnerable but invincible: A longitudinal study of resilient children and youth. New York: Adams, Bannister, Cox.

Children's Risk, Resilience, and Coping in Extreme Situations · 25 Wilson, Ε. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New York: Knopf. Woodhead, M. (1998). Is there a place for work in child development? Implications of child development theory and research for interpretation of the UN Convention on the Rights of the Child, with particular reference to Article 32, on Children, Work and Exploitation. London: Open University and Radda Barnen. World Health Organization. (1992). Psychosocial consequences of disasters: Prevention and management. Geneva: World Health Organization. Wyman, P. (2003). Emerging perspectives on context specificity of children's adaptation and resilience.

In S. S. Luther (Ed.), Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the context of childhood adversities (pp. 293-317). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Yates, T., Egeland, B., & Sroufe, A. (2003). Rethinking resilience: A developmental process perspective. In S. S. Luthar (Ed.), Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the context of childhood adversities (pp. 243-266). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Zwi, Α., Macrae, J., & Ugalde, A. (1992). Children and war. The Kangaroo, 7(1), 46-57.

2 CULTURE AND ETHNIC IDENTITY IN FAMILY RESILIENCE Dynamic Processes in Trauma and Transformation of Indigenous People LAURIE D . MCCUBBIN HAMILTON I. MCCUBBIN

T

he convergence of two generations of behavioral scientists, a psychologist and a family scientist, both of indigenous ancestry and immersed in the cultural context of their ancestors, inspired this chapter to give meaning to and advance understanding of family resilience and resilient behavior in response to trauma. In setting aside the strategy to present sweeping stereotypic generalizations about family resilience across cultures, the authors draw from ethnic identity and cultural studies in psychology, family science, feminist studies, and anthropology to explicate the dynamics of resilience in indigenous families and one of Polynesian origin. Native Hawaiian families, the indigenous people of the Hawaiian Islands

in the Pacific Ocean, are viewed as a unique social context from which scientists can extract and reveal the influential role of culture and ethnicity involved in adaptation and growth, the central outcomes of the resilience process. In so doing, the authors broaden the spotlight on the central research and clinical issues in understanding and promoting resilience from a crosscultural perspective. Interest in resiliency in children and families has flourished in the past three decades. It is a topic of prime interest to social and behavioral scientists because of the apparent role that resilience plays in understanding both individual and family developmental transitions as well as recovery from trauma or under conditions

AUTHORS' NOTE: The authors would like to acknowledge Dr. Marilyn Ann McCubbin, Professor, School of Nursing and Dental Hygiene, University of Hawaii at Manoa, and Mrs. Ann Tom of the Center on the Family, University of Hawaii at Manoa, for their critical review and editorial comments on the chapter. 27

28 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES that favor personal and family deterioration or dysfunction. Predictably, knowledge about successful adaptation in the face of traumatic if not catastrophic conditions also strengthens the conceptual base needed to guide and frame both treatment- and prevention-oriented interventions for children and families at risk or those rendered dysfunctional in the face of such adversity. Fortunately, both psychologists and family scientists have embraced the challenge of designing and conducting research that enhances the development of theories attempting to uncover the reasons why some families from different cultural and ethnic backgrounds are more resilient than others. Some families are better able to negotiate their way through both transitions and situational traumas and better able to cope, adjust, adapt, and even thrive on life's hardships, whereas other families faced with similar if not identical traumatic events give up, are easily exhausted, or deteriorate or give up altogether in the face of these difficult moments in the family's life course. When we consider that most families do not self-destruct or even deteriorate to the point of requiring therapy or treatment and that most do recover from adversity, it seems reasonable that theories and research would be advanced to explain and predict the behaviors of these resilient families, as well as the motivators, if not the cause, of these constructive behaviors. Yet both fields, psychology and family science, are dominated by theories about failure and dysfunction and the treatment of such conditions. If we are to make a serious commitment to prevention, family preservation, and the promotion of family well-being, it seems reasonable that this agenda be driven best by research and theories that have validity in explaining why families predisposed to, vulnerable to, or assaulted by life's hardships and traumas emerge resilient, succeed, and even thrive in the process. Across cultures, the interventions, programs, and policies aimed at enhancing the well-being of families should be guided by theories and research focused on the family competencies and abilities that promote resilience. The push for resilience research on children and families of different ethnic backgrounds and cultures has been a recent but compelling agenda. This situation is propelled by the rapid

and continuous growth in the United States in the number of persons and households of different and multi-ethnic backgrounds. Data from the 2000 U.S. census, which introduced a new system of reporting race, providing a list of races and asking respondents to check all that apply, and the National Health Interview, which uses a similar system, provide ample evidence of the changing ethnic profile of the total population with increasing numbers of Asians and those of Hispanic origins. The confluence of immigrations and an increase in interethnic marriages throughout the world, including countries such as the United Arab Emirates and Kuwait in the Middle East, the Philippines, and Vietnam, to name a few, affirm the gradual but distinct emerging profile of ethnic diversity.

RESILIENCE IN FAMILIES: AN EVOLVING CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK Over the past three decades, attempts to explain the variability in family behavior in response to stress have clustered around three bodies of theory building and related research. The research by Reuben Hill (1949) advanced the ABCX thesis that family resistance to the impact of stressors and avoidance of a family crisis could be explained by understanding the stressor (A), the resources available to and used by families (B), the family's definition of the stressor (C), and the outcome of family crisis (X). The research and theory building by McCubbin and Patterson (1983), nearly four decades later, focused on explaining the variability in family systems in responding to and recovering from a family crisis. To emphasize the recovery phase of family behavior, the double ABCX model of adaptation of families emerged with an emphasis on "postcrisis" factors (the ABCX model was viewed as focused on "precrisis"), such as the pile up or accumulation of life events and changes (AA); the family's rebuilding of protective resources that were depleted and the use of family recovery resources, inclusive of family coping (BB); the family's appraisal of the situation focused on balancing of demands and resources (CC); and family adaptation (XX), reflecting the outcome of family change and recovery from a crisis situation.

Culture and Ethnic Identity in Family Resilience · 29 This model evolved into the FAAR framework, or family adjustment and adaptation response, representing the integration of the Hill ABCX and the McCubbin and Patterson double ABCX into a full model with an added focus on the family processes (McCubbin & Patterson, 1983a, 1983b). This emphasis on the dynamic processes of both adjustment and adaptation inspired family scholars to examine the role of family typologies (core family patterns of functioning) as core family competencies in shaping outcomes, adjustment, and adaptation. The typologies of regenerative family systems (with core strengths in hardiness and coherence), versatile family systems (with core strength in bonding and flexibility), rhythmic family systems (with core strengths in family time and routines and the valuing of both), and traditionalistic family systems (with core strengths in traditions and celebrations) emerged in the literature as both core protective factors and recovery factors across the family life cycle (McCubbin, Thompson, Pirner, & McCubbin, 1988).

in functioning as well as the growth of family members. The resilience model with its emphasis on growth in the face of trauma and crises is depicted in Figure 2.1. By definition, the resiliency model, as well as its predecessors, is a contextualized and developmental framework; the family and family members are seen as an integral and interacting part of the larger social ecology of nature, community, society, nation, and the world, over time. In general, because the family is a system, each domain of family life has an effect on each of the other domains. From a process perspective, in crisis situations, particularly when faced with major traumas or catastrophes, the family's numerous and substantial hardships call for substantive changes in the family system, including roles, goals, value, rules, priorities, boundaries, and overall patterns of functioning. These changes are necessary to achieve balance and harmony across the domains of family functioning.

The resiliency model of family adjustment and adaptation (McCubbin & McCubbin, 1993; McCubbin, McCubbin, Thompson, & Thompson, 1995), referred to in this chapter as the resiliency model, was a natural evolution of earlier theory building and research with a dedicated commitment to explaining the variability in family behavior in the course of recovery when faced with traumatic life events and catastrophes. This deliberate and planful shift in our commitment and emphasis on postcrisis and family recovery flows from the refinement in theory and research rendering clarity in distinction between protective factors and resilience (McCubbin, 2003). Beauvias and Oetting (1999) made a distinction between the two concepts by first defining protective factors as capabilities and processes that increase the chances of family prosocial behaviors and norms in the face of stressors and strains. Thus, to determine the effects of protective factors is to focus on the degree to which they foster prosocial behaviors and reduce the risks, thus avoiding a family crisis. Prosocial behavior can be considered protective when it reduces negative behaviors or symptoms such as depression or anxiety. Prosocial behavior can also be considered positive when it promotes adjustments, stability, and harmony

In addition, families may take advantage of a crisis situation and choose to remain imbalanced and in the state of disharmony to bring about more substantial changes in the family's patterns of functioning. New patterns of family functioning may be introduced to reestablish harmony and balance. For example, in the face of the trauma of a spouse losing a job held for 18 years, a career position the spouse expected to retire from, accompanied by a deterioration in family income and harmony, a family may struggle with the decision to have the other parent return to the work or require one or both parents to take on multiple lower-paying jobs just to survive. These changes alone will force changes in other patterns of family functioning in an effort to bring about harmony and balance. The newly unemployed spouse or significant other will be asked to take on more domestic responsibilities along with added child care responsibilities, both of which may have been the point of prior conflicts, tensions, and resentments. It is also true that an older child in the same family may be called on to take on more responsibilities at home or as a wage earner, thus pulling the child away from school, individual developmental tasks, and other social growth-producing situations and relationships.

ο

Figure 2.1

Conflicted Community Relationships & Nature

Conflicted Structure & Functions

Family Resiliency Model

Unbalance

Conflicted Development Wellbeing Spirituality

Disharmony

Pile up of Stressors & Strains

Conflicted Interpersonal Relationships

Crisis Trauma Catastrophe

TIME

FAMILY CRISIS PHASE

Values Expectations

Beliefs Hopes

Disrupted and Incongruent Patterns of Functioning

Positive Community Relationships & Nature Balance

Positive Structure & Functions

Retained Patterns Restored Patterns Modified Patterns Newly Instituted Patterns

Positive Development Wellbeing Spirituality

Harmony

Manageability Comprehensibility Meaningfulness

Coherence

Positive Interpersonal Relationships

Values Expectations

TIME

FAMILY GROWTH PHASE

Changed and Congruent Patterns of Functioning

Community and Family Social Support

Family Coping

Family Problem-Solving Communication

Family Hardiness

Beliefs Hopes

Culture(s)

Ethnic Identity(ies)

Culture(s) Ethnic Identity(ies)

Family Schema Changed and Congruent

Family Schema Conflicted & Incongruent

FAMILY ADAPTATION AND RECOVERY PHASE >-

Culture and Ethnic Identity in Family Resilience · 31 The family system's rules, roles, and responsibilities will in all likelihood change, which may also create additional pressures adding to the imbalance and disharmony. In situations involving this challenge to the family's internal harmonizer and thermostat—that is, the family's established patterns of functioning—the family will, in all likelihood, experience a condition of maladjustment and resulting condition of family crisis in addition to the crisis created by the initial transition of job loss. Family crises have been conceptualized as a continuous condition of disruptiveness, disorganization, or incapacitation in the family social system (Burr, 1973) accompanied by family trialand-error efforts to restore harmony and balance. Consistent with Reuben Hill's (1949) original definition of family crisis, within the resiliency model, a family in crisis does not necessarily carry the stigmatizing pejorative value judgment that the family unit has failed, is dysfunctional, or in need of professional treatment for such a malady. It is important to reiterate that family crises are not necessarily brought about by the family's being victimized or traumatized by events beyond its control. Families may enter into an active process of inducing or exacerbating a crisis to bring about transformation and changes in the family's established patterns of functioning, which some members may view as more desirable or needed. Accompanied by family efforts to change its established patterns of functioning as part of restoring balance and harmony, the system enters into the adaptation or growth phase of the resiliency model. With the presentation of core concepts of the resiliency model, and the availability of more comprehensive and complete description of the resiliency model in other publications (McCubbin, McCubbin, & Thompson, 2002), we will focus the remainder of the chapter on the key elements of the resiliency model that reveal the application of the framework to the study of families of different ethnicities, cultures, and social contexts.

FAMILY ADAPTATION AND TRANSFORMATION The resiliency model with its adaptation and transformation phase emerged from studies of

war-induced family crises (McCubbin, Boss, Wilson, & Lester, 1980; McCubbin & Dahl, 1976), the study of families faced with chronic stressors and illnesses (Kosciulek, McCubbin, & McCubbin, 1993; McCubbin & McCubbin, 1987, 1989), the study of family transitions and changes over the life cycle (McCubbin & Lavee, 1986; McCubbin et al., 1988; Olson et al., 1983), the study of Native Hawaiian, Filipino, Asian American and African American families faced with both normative and nonnormative stressors and crises (McCubbin & McCubbin, 1988; Thompson, McCubbin, Thompson, & Elver, 1995), the study of African American families faced with relocations into foreign countries with the added risk of war (McCubbin, 1995), and the study of African American families involved in treatment programs for their sons determined to be youth offenders requiring residential treatment (McCubbin, Fleming, et al., 1995). The dynamic nature of family resilience and transformation, depicted in Figure 2.1 may be characterized in the following narrative. Families in crisis situations are characterized, in part, by an imbalance and disharmony, a condition precipitated by a traumatic situation and fostered by the inadequacy of or the problematic nature of the family's patterns of functioning. "Families in crisis" is defined as the system's fundamental inability to achieve balance and harmony along four interrelated dimensions of family life: (a) interpersonal communication and emotional relationships; (b) individual member and family development, well-being, and spirituality; (c) family structure and function; and (d) community relationships and nature (McCubbin, McCubbin, & Thompson, 1996). The crisis situation pushes the family to initiate change and transformation in the family's patterns of functioning. The family's ability to bounce back and transform itself— that is, to achieve balance and harmony across its four dimensions of family life—depends on the effectiveness of the family's cluster of six core competencies: 1. The family system's competencies and ability to manage the accumulation and reduction of co-occurring or residual historic stressors and strains (e.g., initial stressor, normative transitions, situation demands, consequences of family

32 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES efforts to cope, family and social ambiguity due to the lack of experience and guidelines, or conflict with existing guidelines; conflict with new patterns of functioning that don't fit; conflicts with family norms, rules, values, and beliefs; and conflict in patterns of functioning) 2. The family system's competencies in mobilizing its member strengths and capabilities (e.g., intelligence, knowledge, personality, health, sense of mastery, self-esteem, sense of coherence, ethnic identity, and cultural practices) and its collective strengths and capabilities (e.g., organization, hardiness, communication, problem solving, traditions, celebrations, bonding, flexibility, routines, and support) to (a) rebuild those strengths that may have been diminished or that may have deteriorated in the face of trauma and its impact; (b) activate and focus its protective and recovery resources to create and implement new patterns of functioning, to change, and to stabilize old patterns; render legitimacy to the new and changed patterns; and resolve conflicts with the old patterns; and (c) maintain emotional stability during the process 3. The family system's competencies to mobilize the community strengths and resources (e.g., emotional support, esteem support, network support, altruism, honest feedback) and to cultivate, change, and improve on community resources and strengths (e.g., family and individual programs, policies, rules, guidelines, regulations, mission, and resources)—to bear on and be tailored to meet the needs of the challenged family and to aid the family in its efforts to achieve harmony with the community 4. The family system's competencies in modifying, creating, and cultivating changes in the family's schema needed to guide behaviors as well as legitimize changes in the family's patterns of functioning and, in so doing, minimize conflict, maximize congruency between the family's schema and instituted behaviors, and promote a sense of manageability, comprehensibility, and meaningfulness 5. The family system's competencies in positive problem solving and coping focused on achieving balance and harmony among the core dimensions of family life while promoting

emotional stability and changes in the family's patterns of functioning and schema 6. The family's system's competencies in creating shared meaning (e.g., purpose, legitimacy, direction), as well as behavior, that will be congruent with the family's schema (e.g., ethnic identity, culture, beliefs, values, rules, priorities, expectations, relationship with nature, and convictions).

CULTURE AND RESILIENCE In the case of ethnic minority families, the identification of resilience factors, inclusive of ethnic identity and culture, emerge as central themes of resilience research. These efforts are accompanied by a line of inquiry focused on how ethnic identity and culture, independently or in combination with other moderating or mediating factors, will have the greatest impact and value in promoting recovery. The influence of culture on family life has been documented in the family literature. A comprehensive review by Tseng and Hsu (1991) reveals that, over time, culture has influenced family functioning in a great variety of ways: marriage forms, choice of mates, postmarital residence, the family kinship system and descent groups, household and family structure, the primary axis of family obligations, familycommunity dynamics, and alternative family formations (Berkner, 1972; Ishisaka, 1992; Li, 1968; Miller, 1969; Mokuau, 1992; Pelzel, 1970; Tseng & Hsu, 1986). Historically, the family has been the conduit for cultural transmission, providing a natural atmosphere for traditions, beliefs, and values to be passed from generation to generation, and it has evolved throughout the ages to keep culture and ethnic heritage alive. In turn, the family's traditions, an important element in the process of family resilience, have given families a sense of stability and support from which they draw comfort, guidance, and a means of coping with the problems of daily life. The establishment of culture in the resilience process—that is, the recovery from trauma— has been grounded in the literature, albeit on a limited basis. To demonstrate the importance of

Culture and Ethnic Identity in Family Resilience · 33 culture to family resilience, a brief synopsis of studies of the trauma of "end-of-life decisions" would be appropriate. The end of life with its accompanying decisions, considered a traumatic family crises, offers the family scientist and clinician a vivid set of examples of the subtle but potentially profound influence of culture on family and individual resilience. For a full and expanded discussion of end-of-life decisions, the work of Gwen Yeo and Nancy Hikoyeda (2000), from which the following synopsis is drawn, deserves full review and analysis. African Americans draw heavily from a religious doctrine that heaven is not of this earth and a transcendent soul rises to heaven on death (Mouton, 2000). This core religious belief, Lincoln and Miyama (1991) argue, emerges as a direct relationship between slavery and the notion of a "divine rescue." This deference to and dependence on a power greater than humans to relieve African Americans from the conditions of suffering is accompanied by "a belief in God's power to conquer all and a resilient hope that a miracle will happen" (Mouton, 2000, p. 74). Interestingly, studies of preference for lifesustaining care reveal that African Americans, compared with Caucasians, Hispanics and Asians, were more likely to choose life-sustaining treatment even in the face of futility or low expected quality of life. Furthermore, African Americans appear less likely than any other ethnic group to trust health care providers, communicate treatment preference, and participate in organ donation. Levy (1985) documented how medicine proceeded with incomprehensible and seeming unethical practices in the treatment of African Americans, all justified on the notion that African Americans were inferior to Caucasians. Hispanic families, faced with the same challenges of defining the role of religious beliefs, trust of health care providers, and choice of life-sustaining care, responded with notable differences that have implications for identifying factors in resilience and predicting family behavior. The Hispanic population is increasing significantly with about 22 million reported in 1990 and 31 million in the year 2000. The number of Hispanic elderly was expected to increase by nearly 4% each year from 1990 to the year of 2050 (U.S. Bureau of Census, 1993).

Keeping in mind that the category of Hispanics encompasses several racial subgroups—including Mexican Americans, Puerto Ricans, Cubans, Central Americans, and South Americans—we need to exercise caution in our generalizations about this group even though the members share commonalities such as language, religion, and tradition of family relations. Cuellar (1990) emphasized the importance of four cultural themes that promote resilience among Hispanics: jerarquismo (respect for hierarchy), personalismo (trust building over time based on the display of mutual respect), espiritismo (belief in good and evil spirits that cannot affect health and well-being), and presentismo (emphasis on the present and not the past or future). A qualitative study in a rural town in northern New Mexico (Rael & Korte, 1988) revealed the common practice of holding a vigil over an older family member with a terminal illness. They believe that dead family members continue to watch over the living family members and thus are prayed to for continued support and strength. In general, the rituals support the cultural perspective that death is a natural part of the cycle of life and life is only a temporary gift from God. Talamantes, Lawler, and Espino (1995) found that older Puerto Rican, Cuban, and Mexican American women caring for a terminally ill family member depend on their faith in God to cope with the hardships of pending loss and the death of a loved one. A saying such as Dios es Grande, God is great, is offered as testament to the value attached to their faith in coping with illness and death. Fe, or spirituality, was also found to be an important coping resource among older Hispanic women. The vast majority of the respondents to a survey found that Fe would help in healing and coping with life's problems, such caregiving responsibilities. Mexican Americans, in a qualitative study, were concerned about God's wanting a "whole body back." They believed that the soul remained in or near the body for up to 9 days and would feel an incision or insult to the body; thus, they were more likely to limit the practice of organ donations or autopsies. Asian/Pacific Islanders constitute a census category in the United States and thus are often treated as a unified group. In fact, more than 30 countries of origin are clustered together to

34 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES form this "homogenized" grouping of races. They span over half the globe and represent literally hundreds of language and ethnic subgroups, many vastly different from one another in cultural ideology, ethnic identity, and traditions, particularly about death and death decisions. Furthermore, even within culturally defined beliefs, which have a history spanning thousands of years, there may be marked differences in those beliefs across Asian groups. For example, although Buddhist traditions are viewed as having a deep history of practice and basic doctrines are similar, there are definite differences in rituals and practices, particularly those beliefs related to reincarnation and the role of ancestral spirits. Koenig (1997) points out that Chinese and Southeast Asian Buddhists believe in the important influence of ancestral spirits, whereas Japanese American Buddhists tend to honor their ancestors but do not attribute supernatural powers to them. "Little data are available on decision-making about death in the Native Hawaiian culture, especially outside the state of Hawaii" (Yeo & Hikoyeda, 2000, p. 119). According to Braun and Nichols (1996) current Native Hawaiian beliefs are influence by Native Hawaiian traditions as well as Christianity. As reported, some Hawaiians, particularly elders believed that talking about death will bring on death, but many Native Hawaiians make their wishes known to their ohana (family) and loved ones. In a study, Braun (1998) gathered end-of-life attitudes from five ethnic populations, including Native Hawaiians. It is striking that the vast majority, three fourths of the Hawaiians invited to participate in the study, refused. The few who did participate, being primarily Christians, revealed great respect for the traditional Hawaiian values of family, mutual cooperation and support, and collaborative decision making. They felt they had little control over medical decision making that affected them, which resulted in a low level of trust in physicians. Of all the ethnic groups, the Native Hawaiians were the most inclined to endorse preparation for death by making decisions about organ donations—that is, not to be organ donors: They did not believe in organ donation, for it was more appropriate to return the body to God, its maker, as it is. Native American Indians and Alaskan Natives, two other indigenous groups, make up

a small but ever-increasing component of the U.S. population. There are more than 300 federally recognized tribes, 100 state historical tribes, several dozen tribes with no formal recognition, and about 200 Alaskan Native villages (U.S. Bureau of Census, 1993). As Mason and Trible (1982) report, more than 150 languages are spoken by these diverse groups. Recognizing that beliefs and cultural customs vary within and among Native American tribes, there is one commonality in views regarding death and dying. Native American Indians and the Alaskan Natives view death as a natural and accepted part of life; life and death are seen as a unity in a cyclical process with nature (Lewis, 1990). Lombardi and Lombardi (1982), drawing attention to the harmonious relationship between the Native American Indians and the laws of nature, report that "Native Americans thus comprehend the harmony of the endless cycle creation and re-creation: Their interred bodies return nourishment to the earth; the earth makes the plants grow; the plants feed the animals; the animals feed humanity" (p. 36). The heterogeneity of traditional beliefs, values, and rituals can be discovered in ethnographic reports on the Lakota Sioux of South Dakota (Brokenleg & Middleton, 1993), the Tanacross Athabaskans of Alaska (Simeone, 1991), the Comanche of Oklahoma (Wallace & Hoebel, 1952), and Canadian Indians (Kaufert & O'Neil, 1991). One of the most interesting accounts reveals the beliefs of the Navajo people of Arizona, New Mexico, and Utah. The Navajo have been characterized as fearful of death and the dead, reluctant to touch the body of the dead for fear the spirit of the deceased might contaminate them. They believe that ghosts of the dead might return to their homes and harass the living to avenge past wrongs. The names of the dead are not spoken, and afterlife is an uninviting ambiguous world (French & Schwartz, 1976; Kluckhohn & Leighton, 1946). In the contemporary context, particularly the heath care system and its emphasis on patient autonomy (i.e., patient has control over body and mind) and self-determination (i.e., the right to decide), there exists an inherent conflict with Native American values and beliefs (McCabe, 1994). The goal of recent legislation was to

Culture and Ethnic Identity in Family Resilience · 35 increase patient participation in end-of-life decision making, thus expecting agencies and professionals to fully inform the patients of the good and bad. Carrese and Rhodes's (1995) report on their qualitative study of 34 Navajo informants revealed that the traditional Navajo believe that thought and language shape reality and influence events. Thus, positive language helps to maintain or to restore health, and negative language may be harmful to the patients. The disclosure of risk, providing of negative medical information, such as the disclosure of risk in informed consent, is by definition, a contradiction to traditional Navajo ways. Cooperation and consensus within the family system is central to the total process of end-oflife decision making in Native American and Alaskan families. End-of-life decisions are not made by the patient without consulting the family. McCabe (1994) highlighted the importance of beneficence (i.e., doing what is good for another) as central to the Navajo way of life of giving help or aid for the good of the culture without expecting anything in return. There is no hierarchical or vertical line of decision making; instead there is a horizontal line—all concerned individuals are involved. The degree to which indigenous families are able to recover from a trauma-induced crisis, such as the loss of a loved one, depends to some degree on the cultural beliefs and values embedded in the family system and the degree to which they, in turn, shape the family's collective behavior. There is little doubt, however, that even with the diversity of beliefs and practices across ethnic groupings, particularly among indigenous peoples, cultural beliefs and practices play an important role, although with varying impact, on the family's resilience over time.

ETHNIC IDENTITY AND RESILIENCE The concept of ethnic identity, the second key factor in explaining the variability in family resilience, deserves more in-depth consideration by both qualitative- and quantitative-oriented behavioral scientists than it has in the past. The central thesis of ethnic identity in family resilience stems from the core argument that different social categories such as race and ethnicity

shape an individual's or a family's identity as well as its social location in society. Thus, a family system that is racially coded Hawaiian in our society will usually face situations and have experiences that are significantly different from those of a family that is racially coded Asian or Caucasian. Similarly, a family that is racially coded Asian and that has ample financial and educational resources at its disposal will usually face situations and have experiences that are significantly different from a family that is racially coded as Hawaiian. The central point is that a family's identity is likely to be largely determined by its social location in a given society. In addition, identity is the formulation of a person's social, cultural, and historical matrix. Finally, a family's experience will influence, but not entirely determine, the formation of its cultural identity. Mohanty (1993) argued, "Identities are ways of making sense of our experiences." They are "theoretical constructions that enable us to read the world in specific ways" (p. 56). Moya (2000) advanced a realistic (versus postmodernism or essentialist) perspective and renders clarity to the central role of identity. She argues that an individual's understanding of himself or herself and the world will be mediated, more or less accurately, through his or her cultural identity (Moya, 2000, p. 86). She goes on to present the thesis that one's cultural identity is not fixed or absolute and is constantly being evaluated depending on the social contexts (social location consisting of race, class, gender, and sexuality) in which one lives over time: According to the realistic theory of identity, identities are not self-evident, unchanging, and uncontestable, nor are they absolutely fragmented, contradictory, and unstable. Rather identities are subject to multiple determinations and to a continual process of verification that takes place over the course of an individual's life through her interaction with the society she lives in. It is through this process of verification that identities can be (and often are) contested and that they can (and often do) change, (p. 84) Ethnic identity is acknowledged as a critical component of one's sense of identity (Roberts, Phinney, Masse, & Chen, 1999). Ethnic identity focuses on attitudes and beliefs about belonging

36 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES to an ethnic group, a process that evolves over time and through stages (Phinney, 1990; Tajfel & Turner, 1986). Phinney (1990,1992) proposed three stages of ethnic identity development: (a) identity diffusion/foreclosure, characterized by lack of exploration of one's identity; (b) moratorium, which consists of exploration of one's identity; and (c) identity achievement, where one has explored in depth and made commitment to one's ethnic identity. The importance of ethnic identity for persons belonging to minority groups is established in Phinney and Alipuria's (1990) study of ethnic identity among Asian American, African American, Mexican American or Hispanic, and white American college students. African Americans scored the highest on ethnic identity search, followed by Mexican Americans, Asian Americans, and whites, respectively. Ethnic identity has greater importance for minority students than for the majority students, as predicted. Ethnic importance was significantly related to ethnic identity search overall and for the three minority groups separately; however, it was less important for whites. Ethnic importance was also significantly related to ethnic identity commitment. The study also demonstrated the possible relationship between self-esteem and ethnic identity development. This finding of a positive relationship with ethnic identity and self-esteem has been affirmed across investigations for African American and Latino adolescents. Caucasian ethnic identification was also positively related to self-esteem. It is important to note, however, when the American identity measure was given to African Americans and Latinos, there was no relationship with self-esteem (Phinney, Cantu, & Kurtz, 1997). An investigation of 243 Native Hawaiian adolescents confirmed that ethnic identity predicted psychological well-being (self-acceptance and personal growth). The study confirmed that ethnic identity could serve as a protective factor in reducing symptoms of psychological distress and promoting well-being (McCubbin, 2003). Surprisingly, in explaining the variability in ethnic identification, the author discovered a positive correlation between the accumulation of Native Hawaiian stressors (i.e., racial discrimination) and increased ethnic identity. The pressures on Hawaiian youth, including discrimination,

comparative racial backgrounds of peers, and the strong emphasis on Hawaiian language, dance, and traditions, appear to foster and deepen an adolescent's sense of Hawaiian identity. As the author concluded, the stressors raise the consciousness of youth in regard to historical discrimination, a process that may increase their sense of belonging to this social/ethnic group (McCubbin, 2003). In this investigation, ethnic identity was positively related to higher levels of self-acceptance and personal growth and to lower levels of depression and anxiety, again affirming its protective value.

FAMILY SCHEMA AND RESILIENCE Family schema is introduced as a central dimension of family life with a function to represent the family's shared worldview inclusive of the family culture and ethnic identity. The concept of family schema has been traced to the general literature on the psychology of schemata. A family schema may be defined as a generalized structure of shared values, beliefs, goals, expectations, and priorities shaped and adopted by the family unit over time, thus formulating a generalized informational structure against and through which information and experiences are compared, sifted, analyzed, and processed. A family schema develops over time and evolves into an encapsulation of experiences that serves as a framework used to guide family behavior and patterns of functioning (Martin & Halverson, 1981; Segal, 1988). The dynamic interaction between the family schema and the family's patterns of functioning involves the family's evaluation of information leading to the acceptance or rejection of information as being irrelevant, conflictual, or congruent with the family's schema of values, beliefs, goals, expectations, and priorities. In addition, the family, guided by this analysis as a vital step in problem solving, decides on whether to introduce, change, or maintain the family's pattern of functioning. Over time, with the introduction and processing of experiences, the family unit creates a family schema that becomes selfimposed, stable, and to some degree, rigid. Not only is a family's schema highly resistant to change, but it plays a major and highly influential

Culture and Ethnie Identity in Family Resilience · 37 role in shaping and evaluating family meanings, its definition of the situation, the coping strategies employed, and the degree to which newly instituted patterns of functioning need to be cultivated to facilitate family adaptation (McCubbin & McCubbin, 1988; McCubbin & McCubbin, 1987, 1993). Once a family schema is shaped and quietly adopted by the family system, family patterns will then be guided, if not governed, by that schema or successive schema. Once a schema is shaped, adopted, and used to interpret phenomena and to guide family behavior, there is no such thing as family functioning in the absence of a schema. The development of family schémas may be viewed as a seemingly undetectable integration of the schemata of its individual members, adopted, and employed to shape family behavior, which would be upheld and maintained as long as it is successful for the family unity and its members. The family's schema is not likely to be doubted or questioned until the family faces a crisis or a series of crisis-producing situations that place the schema or parts of the schema in question. Alternative schémas or modification in or prioritization of elements within the family's schema (e.g., values, beliefs, goals, expectations) are then introduced and tested by the family to determine their acceptability and congruency with the family's adopted behaviors. This process of testing, rejecting, substituting, and modifying a family schema may be referred to as schema transformation. Family schémas gain their importance in family functioning by virtue of their role in guiding and legitimizing family behaviors and patterns of functioning and in the development of family meanings along with promotion of a sense of meaningfulness and comprehensibility. This aspect of family appraisal involves the creation of shared understandings and the facilitation of family resilience in the face of trauma and catastrophes. The family's meanings— shaped by the family's schema of values, beliefs, culture, ethnic identity and expectations—are often reflected in brief or meaningful phrases such as "God's will" or "God will make things pono (Hawaiian for making things right)" used to encourage understanding and acceptance of adversity that cannot be explained.

The crisis situation pushes the family to initiate change in and transformation of the family's schema. The family's ability to bounce back and transform itself—that is, to achieve balance and harmony—calls for changes in the family schema that will facilitate the achievement of congruency between the family's schema and the family's new patterns of functioning. Family scientists (McCubbin, McCubbin, etal., 1995) have introduced a typology of family strategies and processes involved in the family's efforts to modify its worldview and influence and legitimize the family's adopted patterns of functioning and meaning to foster family coherence (see Figure 2.1) and make family life and functioning more comprehensible, manageable, and meaningful. The typologies associated with the promotion of change and congruency with family patterns of functioning all focused on building family coherence include the following: •









Spiritualization: The process of framing the family crisis situation and changes in the family's patterns of functioning as part of the recovery process through an emphasis on spiritual beliefs and practices Temporalization: The process of framing the family crisis situation and change in the family's new and modified patterns of functioning as part of the recovery process through emphasizing the long- and short-term value and benefits derived from the situation Naturalization: The process of framing the family crisis situation and change as part of the family's "natural" recovery process in which the natural order of things and predictable elements of life is emphasized Prioritization: The process of framing the family crisis situation and change in the family's patterns of functioning as part of the recovery process involving a reexamination and reprioritization of values, beliefs, and expectations, which may vary from family member to family member Collectivation: The process of framing the family crisis situation and changes in the family's pattern of functioning as part of the recovery process through an emphasis on what is beneficial to the collective, the whole family, the family's relationship to the community, and

38 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES the total of relationships, with an added emphasis on the "we" as more important than the "I" • Culturation and multiculturation: The process of framing the family crisis situation and changes in the family's patterns of functioning as part of the recovery process through the clarification, affirmation/reaffirmation, integration, and adaptation of the family's ethnic/multi-ethnic and cultural/multicultural history and practices • Acculturation: The process of framing the family crisis situation and changes in the family's pattern's functioning as part of the recovery process grounded in the selective and strategic assimilation, modification, and adaptation of the values, beliefs, and practices of the majority group of people.

THE HAWAIIAN FAMILY: VULNERABILITY AND RESILIENCE Scholars are reminded of the at-risk status of indigenous peoples. For example, Hawaiians are overrepresented in mortality rates of 26.4 (per 1,000) for infectious disease (versus 13 for all races), 29.0 for diabetes (versus 9.8 for all races), 46.1 for strokes (versus 35.1 for all races), 183.9 for cancer (versus 132 for all races), and 273.0 for heart attacks (versus 198 for all races) (McCubbin & McCubbin, 1997). These alarming statistics set the stage for a more comprehensive look at the survival and resilience of these indigenous people. In an investigation of the functioning and well-being of Native Hawaiian families of preschool age children, McCubbin, McCubbin, and Thompson (1996) confirmed the importance of ethnic identity for individual family members and also as part of family schema in shaping the family's identity, as a resilience factor. Embedded in a measure of family ethnic schema (i.e., Hawaiian values, beliefs, expectation), ethnic identity emerged as a critical recovery factor in shaping the outcome of family resilience and adaptation (i.e., family well-being and functioning). Using a path model to identify the direct and indirect influence of the resilience factor of family schema (shared ethnic identity—Native Hawaiian), the investigators (McCubbin et al.,

1996) confirmed that family schema (including shared ethnic identity) was a significant and director predictor of other resilience factors of family's sense of coherence (i.e., family comprehensibility, manageability, and meaningfulness) and family problem-solving communication (i.e., high-affirming communication and lowincendiary communication). In turn, family sense of coherence (Le., comprehensibility, manageability, and meaningfulness) had a direct and positive relationship with the resilience factors of family problem-solving communication (i.e., highaffirming communication and low-incendiary communication), the latter of which had a direct positive relationship with family adaptation (i.e., family well-being and functioning). These findings (McCubbin et al., 1996) bring the constructs of culture and ethnic identity to center stage—as integral and vital competencies in the study of individuals and families of different ethnic and cultural backgrounds. These observations also place importance on the relatively unknown, suppressed, or ignored variable of family schema as a critical resilience factor in family life and underscore its direct and indirect bearing on the family's resilience and course of family functioning following a traumatic event. In addition, although not documented in this investigation, the findings suggest that a resilience factor may well be the family's capability and competency in modifying and transforming its schema in the process of family behavioral changes and adaptation. This proposition and principle has been advanced by Tedeschi and Calhoun (1995) as fundamental to the process of growth in the aftermath of trauma. Family scholars engaged in the study of families under stress and family resilience share assumptions about family functioning in the face of stressors. The central commonality is that families engage in a roller-coaster course of adaptation. The course of family response and behavior over time follows a predictable pattern, beginning with the family in a stable state punctuated by the impact of a traumatic event or cluster of events that sends the family spiraling downward, reflecting family disorganization, disorientation, and dysfunction, followed by the processes of family recovery and resilience. The trajectory downward (crisis) and upward

Culture and Ethnic Identity in Family Resilience · 39 and bouncing back (resilience) depends on the family's vulnerability due to the pile up of stressors and strains and the strengths and adaptability of the family's recovery factors (i.e., individual, family, community recovery factors or competencies), the most salient of which are family hardiness, community and family social support, family coping, and family problem-solving communication. The concept of a "roller-coaster" course of adaptation is legitimized by the observation that families engage in a trial-and-error process to find the optimum "fit." In this search for fit, the family may adopt structures and behavioral changes that may not be accepted or congruent with the family's schema, thus spiraling the family downward again after a short recovery, moving the family back into a crisis state, starting the trajectory upward once again. This up-anddown cycle may repeat itself over time. Family resilience, the process of bouncing back and adaptation following a family crisis, involves the process of restructuring and making changes in rule, boundaries, and patterns of functioning. To effect posttraumatic growth, Tedeschi and Calhoun (1995) argue that schema change will accompany the behavioral and pattern modification. Essentially, the established schema before the crisis will in all likelihood be disrupted and disorganized, producing the family's attempt to create a more useful and congruent schema (i.e., affirming and complementary to family behaviors and pattern change) that will promote the family's sense of coherence (i.e., comprehensibility, meaningfulness, and manageability). Thus, family growth, we argue, is possible because of change in schémas. Tedeschi and Calhoun (1995, p. 81) carry this point further in concluding that "growth is change in schémas" (see Figure 2.1). The Native Hawaiian family, the Ohana (family) of the Kanaka Maoli or true people, for example, views resilience as a relational process. According to Marsella, Oliveira, Plummer, and Crabbe (1995) the Native Hawaiian family would best be viewed in an ecological context in which the family member (Kamaaina), the family unit (Ohana), nature (Aina), and the spiritual forces of the world (uhane, akua, aumakua) are viewed as interconnected and interdependent. The family is not

only an integral part of the social fabric of society but also of the consciousness or mind of the Native Hawaiian. All these elements internal and external to the family unit are united and inseparable from the larger society, nature, and spiritual forces in the world. Within this relational and family ecological perspective (McCubbin et al., 1996), the concept of Lokahi or harmony takes on a relational meaning involving the land, spiritual energy, and individuals and the family unity. A family knows, Marsella et al. (1995) argue, when Lokahi is achieved because the family unit experiences a general state of well-being characterized by the presence of energy (Mana), which is interdependent with the family unit, the individual members, the spirit, the social ecology, and nature—all as one in unity. In the Native Hawaiian context, this ideal state may be referred to as Pono or Ma 'e, Ma 'e. This is the state that the family strives to achieve through seeking a balance and harmony closely related to the unity of the individual, family, nature, and the spiritual worlds. When this state is achieved, there is arguably optimum health, well-being, and functioning. McCubbin, Fleming, etal. (1995) point to the importance of culturally based resilience factors, including the placement of the "group or family" above self, investment in others through acts of altruism, commitment to conservation in the preservation of the land (Aina), and reverence and respect for the gods, rituals, and prayer. Cross's (1995) sensitive portrayal of the common elements of the Hawaiians with those of the Native American families faced with trauma and oppression also underscores the importance of a relational point of view in describing the resilience in indigenous families. Only through an understanding of the holistic and complex relationship that come into play in achieving harmony do we come to appreciate that the goal of families is to thrive, not just survive. Of importance to the study of trauma and indigenous families, Cross (1995) calls our attention to the vital roles that these families play in teaching future generations about resilience and how to build these competencies for use in their futures. Families cultivate a learning environment and thus a set of learning experiences that facilitate what Cross

40 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES called the sixth sense about where indigenous individuals and families are welcome and where they are not. Parents and siblings teach children to recognize the "subtle clues that spell danger." Family members interpret oppressive events from the media for young children and in so doing transmit information that cushions the assaults of the mainstream media. As adults, we learn to cope with and manage the dynamics of racial differences and pass on our strategies to our children. In the context of family life, resilience is enhanced through the family process of self-talk and story-telling, acts through which knowledge is transmitted about managing life events and managing change. In this way, family members, young and old alike, learn proven strategies for using resources and adapting to change. In story-telling, families pass on stories of their lives, their skills, and in so doing, "we parent for resilience" (Cross, 1995). As McCubbin, McCubbin, et al. (1995) concluded, "When the family system focuses on achieving harmony, resilience is advanced by contributing to the balance among these forces" (p. 43).

CHALLENGES AND OPPORTUNITIES Culture, by definition, is the sum total of knowledge passed on from generation to generation within a given society. Culture provides "meaning systems" in that it generally structures cognitive reality for an entire society (D'Andrade, 1984). Of importance to family resilience—the process of bouncing back from dramatic change, trauma, or catastrophe—D'Andrade (1984) affirms that culture, particularly its cultural meaning systems, has several key functions, the first of which is to enable the family within society to represent the world symbolically to its members and to persons outside of the family. In addition, culture has a constructive function of creating cultural entities that provide explanations of the world by way of rituals, scripted patterns of behavior, and rules to follow. Cultural meaning systems have both a directive function to guide persons in their behavior and a evocative function of creating rules for how to feel, by defining what a situation means. With these functions in mind, it is

unequivocal that culture plays a key, although complex, role in the family's process of recovery and resilience. Ethnic identity, gains the same prominence both for individual members and the family system, for it gives social and psychological meaning and serves as a basis for belonging to a larger group beyond the family. In the context of family resilience, ethnic identity serves to shape the "group's" sense of who they are as a collective unit in a larger society. Predictably, identity, and ethnic identity in particular give the individual and family unit a basis on which to explain behavior and interpret the social meanings of experiences. Ethnic identity plays a key role in the resilience process, for it has a significant part in shaping an individual's self-esteem and self-efficacy; it shapes the family's sense of viability and function as well as worth and confidence, all of which are essential foundations for resilience. Even with a long history of knowledge about the anthropologist's view of ethnicity, culture, and behavior, as well as the psychologist's understanding of identity, we are novices approaching a crossroads in research and theory building to explain how culture and ethnic identity—under what circumstances and for what groups—come to promote resilience and family resilience in particular. This chapter offers but a glimpse of the elements or recovery factors that shape the resilience process. We have only scratched the surface of the dynamic processes involved and how they work together to accomplish the family's recoverability and adaptation following trauma. One of the critical issues common to both resilience factors, culture and ethnic identity, is the reality of the proliferation of multicultural families with multicultural identities. Furthermore, culture and identities are constituted in different historical contexts. For example, the Native American living in the 1940s with the experience of World War II might experience his or her ethnic identity very differently from the Native American in the 21st century. The social cultural meanings attached to each person's ethnicity are so different as to render meaningless the project of describing one Native American in terms of the other. Consequently, in the current era of interracial marriages, cultural and ethnic

Culture and Ethnie Identity in Family Resilience · 41 selected health professionals. In M. S. Harper (Ed.), Minority aging: Essential curricula content for selected health and allied health professionals (DHHS Publication No. HRS P-DV-90-4, pp. 365-413). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. D'Andrade, R. G. (1984). Cultural meaning systems. In R. A. Shweder & R. A. Le Vine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 88-119). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. French, J., & Schwartz, D. R. (1976). Terminal care at home in two cultures. In P. J. Brink (Ed.), Transcultural nursing: A book of readings (pp. 247REFERENCES 255). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Hill, R. (1949). Families under stress. New York: Beauvias, F, & Oetting, Ε. (1999). Drug use, resilience, Harper & Row. and the myth of the golden child. In M. Glantz Ishisaka, H. A. (1992). Significant differences & J. Johnson (Eds.), Resilience and developbetween Pacific-Asian and Western cultures. ment: Positive life adaptations (pp. 101-106). In J. Fischer (Ed.), East-west directions: Social New York: Plenum Press. work practice, tradition, and change. Honolulu: Berkner, L. K. (1972). The stem family and the develUniversity of Hawaii, School of Social Work. opment cycle of the peasant household: An eighKaufert, J. M., & O'Neil, J. D. (1991). Cultural mediteenth-century Austrian example. American ation of dying and grieving among Native History Review, 77, 398-418. Canadian patients in urban hospitals. In D. R. Braun, K. ( 1998). Surveying community attitudes on endCounts & D. A. Counts (Eds.), Coping with the of-life options. Honolulu: University of Hawaii, final tragedy: Cultural variations in dying and Center on Aging. grieving (pp. 231-251). Amityville, NY: Braun, K., & Nichols, R. (1996). Cultural issues in Baywood. death and dying. Hawaii Medical Journal, 55, Kluckhohn, C , & Leighton, D. (1946). The Navaho 260-264. (Rev. ed.). Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Brokenleg, M., & Middleton, D. (1993). Native Koenig, B. (1997). Cultural diversity in decisionAmericans: Adapting, yet remaining. In D. P. making about care at the end of life. In Institute Irish, K. F. Lundquist, & V. J. Nelsen (Eds.), of Medicine (Ed.), Approaching death: Improving Ethnic variations in dying, death, and grief care at the end of life (pp. 363-382). Washington, (pp. 101-112). Washington, DC: Taylor & DC: National Academy Press. Francis. Kosciulek, R., McCubbin, Μ. A„ & McCubbin, H. I. Burr, W. (1973). Theory construction and the sociol(1993). A theoretical framework for family ogy of the family. New York: John Wiley. adaptation to head injury. Journal of RehabilitaCarrese, J. Α., & Rhodes, L. A. (1995). Western tion, 59, 40-45. bioethics on the Navajo reservation: Benefit or Levy, D. R. (1985). Caucasian doctors and African harm? Journal ofAmerican Medical Association, American patients: Influence of race on the doc274, 826-829. tor-patient relationship. Pediatrics, 75, 639-643. Cross, T. L. (1995). Understanding family resiliency Lewis, R. (1990). Death and dying among the from a relational worldview. In Η. I. McCubbin, American Indians. In J. K. Parry (Ed.), Social E. A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson, & J. E. work practice with the terminally ill: A transculFromer (Eds.), Resiliency in ethnic minority tural perspective (pp. 23-31 ). Springfield, IL: families: Native and immigrant American Charles C Thomas. Families (Vol. 1, pp. 143-157). Thousand Oaks, Li, Y. Y. (1968). Ghost marriage, shamanism, and CA: Sage. kinship behavior in rural Taiwan. In folk religion Cuellar, J. B. (1990). Hispanic American aging: and the world view in the Southwestern Pacific. Geriatric education curriculum development for

identity categories may be neither stable nor internally homogeneous, thus presenting unique challenges to the theorist, research scholar, and clinicians who are called on to understand this complexity, predict behavior, prevent crises, and facilitate recovery and resilience. The decade ahead presents numerous challenges for all who have a commitment to serving families in need and promoting their growth in an era of rapid social and technological change.

42 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES Tokyo: Keio University, Keio Institute of Cultural and Linguistic Studies. Lincoln, C. E., & Miyama, L. H. (1991). The African American church in the African American experience. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Lombardi, E, & Lombardi, G. S. (1982). Life and death: The cycle is timeless. Happy Camp, CA: Naturegraph. Marsella, A. J., Oliveira, J. M., Plummer, C. M., & Crabbe, Κ. M. (1995). Native Hawaiian (Kanaka Maoli) culture, mind, and well-being. In H. I. McCubbin, E.A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson, & J. E. Fromer (Eds.), Resiliency in ethnic minority families: Native and immigrant American Families (Vol. 1, pp. 93-113). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119-1134. Mason, S. M., & Trible, J. E. (1982). American Indian and Alaska Native communities. In L. R. Snowden (Ed.), Reaching the underserved: Mental health needs of neglected populations (pp. 143-163). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McCabe, M. (1994). Patient Self-Determination Act: A Native American (Navajo) perspective. Cambridge Quarterly of Healthcare Ethics, 3, 419^*21. McCubbin, H. I. (1995). Resiliency in AfricanAmerican families: Military families in foreign environments. In H. I. McCubbin, E. A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson, & J. Fromer (Eds.), Resiliency in ethnic minority families: Native and immigrant American families (Vol.1, pp. 67-97). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McCubbin, H. I., Boss, P., Wilson, L., & Lester, G. (1980). Developing family invulnerability to stress: Coping patterns and strategies wives employ in managing family separations. In J. Trost (Ed.), The family and change (pp. 186173). Vàsterâs, Sweden: International Library Publishing. McCubbin, H. I., & Dahl, Β. B. (1976). Prolonged family separation in the military: A longitudinal study. In H. I. McCubbin, B. Dahl, & E. Hunter (Eds.), Families in the military system (pp. 146176). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McCubbin, H. I., Fleming, M., Thompson, A. I., Neitman, P., Elver, K., & Savas, S. (1995). Resiliency and coping in "at risk" African American youth and their families. In H. I.

McCubbin, E. A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson, & J. Fromer (Eds.), Resiliency in ethnic minority families: Native and immigrant American families (Vol. 1, pp. 287-328). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McCubbin H. I., & Lavee, Y. (1986). Strengthening army families: A family life cycle stage perspective. Evaluation and Program Planning, 9, 221-231. McCubbin, H. I., & McCubbin, L. D. (1997). Hawaiian American families. In M. K. DeGenova (Ed.), Families in a cultural context: Strengths and challenges in diversity (pp. 237263). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield. McCubbin, H. I., & McCubbin, M. A. (1988). Typologies of resilient families: Emerging roles of social class and ethnicity. Family Relations, 37, 247-254. McCubbin, H., McCubbin, M., & Thompson, A. (1992). Resiliency in families: The role of family schema and appraisal in family adaptation to crises. In T. Brubaker (Ed.), Families in transition (pp. 86-100). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McCubbin, H. I., McCubbin, Μ. Α., & Thompson, A. I. (1996). Family assessment: Resiliency, coping and adaptation-inventories for research and practice. Madison: University of Wisconsin System. McCubbin, Η. I., McCubbin, Μ. Α., & Thompson, A. I. (2002). Family measures: stress, coping and resilience. Honolulu, HI: Kamehameha Schools. McCubbin, Η. I., McCubbin, M., Thompson, Α., & Thompson, E. (1995). Resilience in ethnic families: A conceptual model for predicting family adjustment and adaptation. In Η. I. McCubbin, E. A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson, & J. Fromer (Eds.), Resiliency in ethnic minority families: Native and immigrant American families (Vol. 1, pp. 3-48). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McCubbin, Η. I., & Patterson, J. M. (1983a). The family stress process: The double ABCX model of adjustment and adaptation. In Η. I. McCubbin, M. Sussman, & J. Patterson (Eds.), Social stress and the family: Advances and developments in family stress theory and research (pp. 26^47). New York: Haworth. McCubbin, Η. I., & Patterson, J. M. (1983b). Family transitions: Adaptation to crisis: A double ABCX model of family behavior. In D. Olson & B. Miller (Eds.), Family studies review yearbook (pp. 87-107). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Culture and Ethnie Identity in Family Resilience · 43 McCubbin, H. I., Thompson, A. I., Pirner, P., & McCubbin, M. A. (1988). Family types and strengths: A life cycle and ecological perspective. Edina, MN: Burgess International. McCubbin, L. D. (2003). Resilience among Native Hawaiian adolescents: Ethnic identity, psychological distress and wellbeing. Doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin-Madison. McCubbin, Μ. Α., & McCubbin, Η. I. (1987). Family stress theory and assessment: The T-double ABCX model of family adjustment and adaptation. In Η. I. McCubbin & A. I. Thompson (Eds.), Family assessment inventories for research and practice (pp. 3-34). Madison: University of Wisconsin-Madison. McCubbin, Μ. Α., & McCubbin, Η. I. (1989). Theoretical orientations to family stress and coping. In C. R. Figley (Ed.), Treating stress in families (pp. 67-89). New York: Brunner/ Mazel. McCubbin, Μ. Α., & McCubbin, Η. I. (1993). Families coping with illness: The resiliency model of family stress, adjustment and adaptation. In C. Danielson, B. Hamel-Bissell, & P. Winstead-Fry (Eds.), Families, health and illness (pp. 121-143). New York: Mosby. Miller, L. (1969). Child rearing in the kibbutz. In J. G. Howells (Eds.), Modern perspectives in international child psychiatry (pp. 141-162). Edinburgh, Scotland: Oliver & Boyd. Mohanty, S. P. (1993, Spring). The epistemic status of cultural identity: On beloved and the postcolonial condition. Cultural Critique, 24, 41-80. Mokuau, N. (1992). A conceptual framework for cultural responsiveness in the health field. In J. Fischer (Ed.), East-west directions: Social work practice, tradition, and change (pp. 118-130). Honolulu: University of Hawaii, School of Social Work. Mouton, C. P. (2000). Cultural and religious issues for African Americans. In K. Braun, J. Pietsch, & P. Blanchette (Eds.), Cultural issues in end-oflife decision making (pp. 71-82). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Moya, P. (2000). Postmodernism, "realism," and the politics of identity: Cherrie Moraga and Chicana feminism. In P. M. Moya & M. R. HameaGarcia (Eds.), Reclaiming identity: Realist theory and the predicament of postmodernism (pp. 67-101). Berkeley: University of California Press.

Olson, D. H., McCubbin, H. I. Barnes, H., Larsen, Α., Muxen, Α., & Wilson, L. (1983). Families: What makes them work. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Pelzel, J. (1970). Japanese kinship: A comparison. In M. Freedman (Ed.), Family and kinship in Chinese society (pp. 78-191). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Phinney, J. S. (1990). Ethnic identity in adolescents and adults: Review of research. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 499-514. Phinney, J. S. (1992). The Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure: A new scale for use with diverse groups. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7(2), 156-176. Phinney, J. S., & Alipuria, L. L. (1990). Ethnic identity in college students from four ethnic groups. Journal of Adolescence, 13, 171-183. Phinney, J. S., Cantu, C. L., & Kurtz, D. A. (1997). Ethnic and American identity as predictors of self-esteem among African American, Latino and white adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26(2), 165-185. Rael, R., & Korte, A. O. (1988). El ciclo de la vida y muerte: An analysis of death and dying in a selected Hispanic enclave. In S. R. Applewhite (Ed.), Hispanic elderly in transition: Theory, research, policy and practice (pp. 189-202). Westport, CT: Greenwood. Roberts, R., Phinney, J. S., Masse, L. C , & Chen, R. (1999). The structure of ethnic identity of young adolescents from diverse ethnocultural groups. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19(3), 301-322. Segal, Z. (1988). Appraisal of the self-schema construct in cognitive models of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 147-162. Simeone, W. E. (1991). The northern Athabaskan potlatch: The objectification of grief. In D. R. Counts & D. A. Counts (Eds.), Coping with the final tragedy: Cultural variations in dying and grieving (pp. 157-167), Amity ville, NY: Baywood. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. (1986). The social identity of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. Austin (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Talamantes, Μ. Α., Lawler, W. R, & Espino, D. V. (1995). Hispanic American elders: Caregiving norms surrounding dying and the use of hospice services. Hospice Journal, 10(4), 35^49. Tedeschi. R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (1995). Trauma and transformation: Growing in the aftermath of suffering. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

44 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES Thompson, Ε. Α., McCubbin, Η. I., Thompson, A. I., & Elver, Κ. (1995). Vulnerability and resiliency in Native Hawaiian families under stress. In Η. I. McCubbin, E. A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson, & J. Fromer (Eds.), Resiliency in ethnic minority families: Native and immigrant American families (Vol. 1, pp. 115-131), Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Tseng, W. S., & Hsu, J. (1986). The family in Micronesia. In W. S. Tseng & C. A. Less (Eds.), Culture and mental health in Micronesia. Honolulu: University of Hawaii, John A. Burns School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry. Tseng, W. S., & Hsu, J. (1991). Culture and family: Problems and therapy. New York: Haworth.

U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1993). We the... American Pacific Islanders. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Wallace, E., & Hoebel, E.A. (1952). The Comanches: Lords of the south plains. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. Yeo, G., & Hikoyeda, N. (2000). Cultural issues in end-of-life decision making among Asians and Pacific Islanders in the United States. In K. Braun, J. Pietsch, & P. Blanchette (Eds.), Cultural issues in end-of-life decision making (pp. 101-126). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

3 LESSONS LEARNED FROM POOR AFRICAN AMERICAN YOUTH Resilient Strengths in Coping With Adverse Environments JOYCE WEST STEVENS

B

ecause of the many social problems in modern American society, scholars have suggested that America is a nation at risk. Although this claim may seem hardly credible for the richest and most powerful nation in the world, empirical data suggest that a significant number of young Americans suffer the consequences of social ills that jeopardize future possibilities for meeting the challenges of a participatory democracy. Poverty, homelessness, HIV/AIDS, child maltreatment, adolescent parenting, substance abuse, school dropout, suicide, imprisonment, delinquency, and violence have all been identified as social problems that threaten the psychological, social, and physical health of American youth and the integrity of American society (McWhirter, McWhirter, McWhirter, & McWhirter, 1998). Of special concern is that many social problems disproportionately afflict people of color. As such, the most vulnerable populations have the least social and economic resources, thereby limiting genuine prospects for social mobility. Accordingly, scholars contend that young African American males fueled by

their inability to claim a legitimate stake in society become involved in illegal drug use during early adulthood. What is more, once drug careers have developed, they are easily maintained by a drug infrastructure that promotes violence and profiteering (Gibbs, 1988; Stevens, 2001; Wallace, 1999; Wilson, 1987). Reasonably, some scholars have therefore raised questions concerning the utility of a risk construct, suggesting that it represents a deficit model that "blames the victim" (Swadener & Lubeck, 1995). Arguably, when risks are viewed as a consequence of problems in society as a whole, there is little need to "blame the victim." Furthermore, despite adverse social conditions, some youth—perhaps all too few in such circumstances—nonetheless, do exhibit a power of endurance and competence that enables them to transcend environs replete with risks (Ford, 1994; Garmezy, 1993; Jarrett, 1995; Stevens, 1997; Wallace, 1999; Williams & Kornblum, 1985, 1994). Thus, I suggest elsewhere that resilience and risk are functional constructs that help clarify the intersection of social context 45

46 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES and behavioral responses (Stevens, 2002). Given the vagaries of life, mundane circumstances are never all bad or all good but, rather, a mixture of both favorable and unfavorable conditions. Notwithstanding, I make use of the at-risk concept to refer to those circumstances that are heavily weighted on the side of being unfavorable or toxic and, as such, pose grave threats to safety and well-being. Although there are several models of risk (cumulative, additive, interactive), being at risk is ordinarily understood to indicate exposure to circumstances of harm or jeopardy in environs in which individuals and families are situated. Resilience on the other hand is achieving a good outcome from harsh circumstances as well as the management of risks to avoid misfortune or harm. In this chapter, I suggest that the concepts of both risk and resilience have veritable utility for understanding marginalized youth in innercity communities where many neighborhoods are characterized by unsafe environments, economic impoverishment, and depleted social resources. First, I contend that the ecological framework is a conceptual tool that brings into focus the intersection of persons, process, and social context and thus clarifies the manner in which the concepts of risk and resilience are operative in lived experience. Second, I will argue that the gravity of present-day challenges that economically disadvantaged youth face necessitates change in how we understand the nature of their achievements and strengths. Hence, I propose that practitioners can learn a great deal from resilient inner-city African American youth and their families about what is needed to succeed in adverse environments. Based on research, attributes of resilience that play a part in self-affirmation and social mobility will be discussed. Last, I suggest that youth are helped best by supporting their attributes of resilience.

RESEARCHING RESILIENCE: A CULTURAL ETHNIC STANDPOINT The interpretation of ethnographic or qualitative data may be conducted from a standpoint epistemology—that is, from the lived experiences of the research participants themselves.

This interpretative method requires the researcher to work from the outside to the center (Collins, 1986,1990; Denzin, 1997; hooks, 1984). A standpoint text starts from the lived experiences of those considered to be "other" or outside mainstream society. This form of reporting narrative renders data less objective and abstract (Smith, 1989). In this regard, respondents' texts reflect intersections of class, ethnicity, and gender, yielding a multivocal text. All things considered, a multivocal text is created when the researcher is located inside the cultural narrative and carries on extensive dialogue as if a member of a valued cultural group. As a researcher, it has been a humbling learning experience and a privilege to enter the lives of African American female research participants to gain knowledge of their "ways of knowing" how to cope with adverse conditions. Moreover, as an African American female, I have the privilege of being situated inside the historical cultural narrative of the "black experience"; I too am marginalized in the larger societal context as "other." The situated experiences of ethnic outsiders are manifest concretely within a societal hierarchy distinguished by the social status of racial/ethnic devaluation and oppression. At the same time, however, my cultural ethnicity is one I share with research participants; hence, I am positioned as an "insider." Recognizably, a researcher's insider knowledge can be both an asset and a liability (Kanuha, 2000). Being all too ready with assumptions, an insider can presume to know about who research participants are and how they live their lives, minimizing an investigator's objectivity. For sure, there are limitations regarding my insider position by virtue of class and lack of personal knowledge with reference to the problems under investigation. At the same time, however, these personal characteristics could allow for objectivity and, as such, minimize bias. Indeed, the one most important lesson I learned from study participants over and over again is that resiliency exists in the direst of circumstances. Significantly, this lesson served as a caution to rethink notions of what it means to be at risk and resilient and how the two constructs operate in the lives of poor black females. For my purposes here, qualitative and quantitative findings from research studies of economically disadvantaged inner-city black females

Lessons Learned From Poor African American Youth · 47 that include studies of late-aged (17-19 years) pregnant and nonpregnant girls and early-aged (11-14 years) middle school girls provide the data for theorizing about how indigent but resilient black youth deal with adverse environments. Study samples were characterized by social indices suggesting that both groups were from impoverished environments. In the middle school study, a clinical intervention to improve school performance and adjustment was tested in a quasi-experimental design. The clinical group intervention sessions were audiotaped and used as qualitative data. Girls referred to the program displayed at-risk behaviors such as poor academic performance, excessive tardiness, absences, and school suspensions because of fights with peers. The pregnancy research was conducted to contrast and compare pregnant and nonpregnant females with regard to their self-perceptions and perceptions of the surroundings in which they lived. This research has been reported extensively elsewhere, most recently in the text Smart & Sassy: The Strengths of Inner City Black Girls (Stevens, 2002). Because research participants were all female, I use corresponding pronouns. As one would expect, the value.of qualitative research such as this was that participants' narratives are contextualized, with the ordinary manner in which most people engage in discourse under study. Information is processed from within the multileveled social contexts in which we find ourselves grounded. The open-ended adolescent pregnancy study questionnaire that was used elicited narrative accounts about a girl's family, neighborhood, peers, and school. By doing this, stories were contextualized naturally so as to construct meaning and complexity in how I understood these girls' thinking and behavior. This process is called contexting (Hall, 1971).

A THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK FOR RISK AND RESILIENCE Like others (Brunswick, 1999; Spencer, 1995), I draw on the ideas of Bronfenbrenner (1979) to consider an ecological perspective in my theorizing about at-risk social contexts, risk behaviors, and resiliency among African Americans. Briefly stated, Bronfenbrenner suggests that

an ecological system includes nested contexts within systemic domains of bio-psycho-socialhistorical influences that affect individual development. Namely, social contexts are the "nested contexts" of an individual's life and include a person's family, school, peers, church, neighborhood activities, and sociocultural structural conditions. The nested contexts within systemic domains are multileveled, bidirectional (reciprocal), and transactional such that the activities and persons within a given context affect the individual, who in turn has a reciprocal impact on the same social context. Consider abstractly Bronfenbrenner's systemic domains as concentric circles represented by numerical sequencing from center to outer spheres. Each circle represents a different level of abstraction: (1) ontogenetic (the individual person); (2) microsystem (family-kin, neighborhood activities, groups, and institutions); (3) mesosystem (larger community institutions, groups, and organizations); (4) exosystem (national institutions and social structures of health, welfare, business industries, financial centers, mass media); (5) macrosystem (societal attitudes and ideologies that induce structural strain); and (6) chronosystems (aspects of time, patterning of environmental events and transactions over the life course, sociocultural historical conditions, and structural strain). At this sixth level, structural strain refers to the structural barriers that block access to opportunities, privilege, and power over time. An illustrative example might be the perpetuity of "the old boys' network" of white AngloSaxon protestant males who have garnered prime opportunities for amassing wealth in high-status careers. Characterized by transactional intersubjective processes within contexts and between contexts, the ecological perspective is context dependent. Risk and resilience when interpreted in this light are operational within a transactional model of process and social context. What one experiences is conspicuously the result of intersubjective processes between individuals and institutions that make judgments regarding what is adaptive or maladaptive. Institutional intersubjectivity may be reflected, for example, in how well social policies and social policy formulation are tailored to meet the needs of individuals (Crossley, 1996). However, we can just as easily ignore the higher

48 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES levels of policy and look only at individuals and their maladaptive lifestyles and behaviors, a position that returns us once again to blaming the victim rather than understanding the intersubjectivity of people's experiences. Individual maladaptation may manifest itself as a lack of compassion (empathy), a lack of a shared sense of care for others and affinity with them (mutuality), a seeking of recognition in viciously destructive behaviors, a lack of assertion, or the affirmation of the self through acts of violence. In this regard, it has been well documented (Anderson, 1999; Bolland, 2001; Bourgois, 1995; Ratner, 1993; Sterk, 1999) that maladaptive lifestyles (e.g., drug kingpins) pose threats (risks) to the quality of life in inner-city communities. This is, of course, not debatable. However, we may need to question, from an ecological point of view, the role that social forces play in making these behaviors more desirable to some populations of marginalized individuals. Benjamin (1990) has argued that intersubjective attunements of recognition and assertion are operationalized reflexively in that they are mirrored and validated through others. Given this, I have chosen to focus this psychosocial inquiry on the following: (a) the nature of a girl's intersubjective transactions within and between social contexts; (b) the makeup of a girl's contexting or construction of meaning regarding self and surroundings; (c) the nature, content, and attitudinal perception of those social contexts in which a girl is deeply rooted; (d) the nature, content, and attitudinal perception of the social contexts in which a girl seeks to meet immediate needs; and (e) the nature, content, and attitudinal perception of the social context a girl seeks or desires to negotiate to meet future needs. Practically, this five-point query provides information that depicts portraitures about a girl's self-organization and self-regulation related to her intersubjective responses of empathy, assertion, and recognition and her attitudinal perceptions of others and various social contexts, and a girl's perception of the significance of a range of social contexts in meeting immediate needs and future goals. Perceptibly, this kind of inquiry yields rich data about risk and resiliency within the contexts of the adolescent's lived experiences. Thus, participants'

evaluative perceptions reveal judgments about favorable and unfavorable ecological conditions that lead to or inhibit the formation of resiliency. I claim two basic assumptions that undergird my theorizing in this way. Significantly, the first assumption is the commonplace understanding that human beings by nature are relational and grounded in social context. Equally important, my second assumption is that adolescents commonly covet a relationship with an older adult— one that offers consistent emotional attunement, guidance, and direction and that validates personal and collective experiences. A practitioner, for example, often builds on this normative and expected role modeling function required by adolescents. A primary relationship with a significant adult has been empirically documented as a capacity-enhancing attribute of resilient adolescent girls. Undeniably, a practitioner's relationship with adolescents can offer an intersubjective experience of fundamental consequence. The following illustrative examples, taken from my research, explore the intersubjective world of African American girls and the interface between them as individuals and the multilevel ecology of the social and political factors that affect their well-being.

ILLUSTRATIVE EXAMPLES Seminal ideas about person, process, and contexts begin to take hold when conducting research in an inner-city neighborhood middle school. Long hours were spent at the school. By chance, I became a participant observer, studying the varied intersubjective transactions among stakeholders (i.e., students, administrators, teachers, and auxiliary staff) that took place in the general surroundings of the school. My observations led me to understand that the school staff was generally not supportive of the girls in the study and did not see them in a good light. The girls' behaviors generated insensitive gossip among school staff, the content of which denigrated and devalued the girls. Importantly, the girls were well aware that they were viewed negatively by school personnel. The following description, illustrates how recognition, empathy, and assertion operate within two contextual fields among a group of middle school adolescent

Lessons Learned From Poor African American Youth · 49 girls. The clinician who worked with the girls in the intervention study program was a social work intern. The study girls were viewed by the school staff as displaying loud, boisterous, and brassy behaviors. Teachers complained that the girls argued and talked back to them. The girls were all given the label of conduct disorder by the school social worker, and the teachers were in agreement with this labeling. Interestingly, the girls in turn saw their teachers as not earning their respect, contemptuous of their scholastic efforts, and not invested in their learning. It was hard to disagree once immersed in the school ecology. Some of the teachers in the school seemed so overwhelmed by what they identified as "too many discipline problems" that their commitment to teaching was compromised by a failure to deal effectively with the many problems students presented. Likewise, the teachers were accurate in their observations of the girls as loud and argumentative. Eventually, the girls accepted responsibility for their confrontational behaviors, admitting that although their actions were defiant, it was a way they had managed to cope with feelings of being "put down." The girls questioned whether their being black was the reason they were treated disapprovingly by their teachers, who were white. From the girls' perspective, being argumentative or "speaking up" displayed selfaffirmation and provided protection against feelings of powerlessness and devaluation. In another realm, the girls used a very different set of coping strategies. In particular, they were in a quandary about what to do when adult males in their neighborhoods approached them sexually. Such advances offended the girls; unresponsive and frightened, they felt violated. What is more, the girls' unspoken disinterest provoked rude insults from the men that shocked, angered, and scared them even more. The intern helped the girls to appreciate the varied contextual complexities of their neighborhood and school. In addition, she clarified the girls' healthy need for self-affirmation (assertion) and their failed attempts to gain the respect (recognition) desired from the school setting. When engaged in this manner, the girls were willing to disclose feelings of helplessness and vulnerability and the social and personal devaluation they felt in both social contexts. As affective disclosures

were acknowledged, intersubjective processes of empathy, recognition, and assertion were strengthened to the degree that self-awareness of affinity (mutuality) intensified the bond between them and their caregivers. Moreover, the girls had obtained from each other sympathetic understanding (empathy), respect (recognition), and affirmation (assertion); supported by their peer group experience, the girls enhanced their sense of self-efficacy in other social contexts. As the intern observed, in school, the girls were outspoken and seemingly self-assured, whereas in the context of their neighborhood streets, they were indeed intimidated and angered but silent. Clearly, the girls' neighborhood presented them with multiple risks—in one case, sexual harassment and seduction by older men, in the other, a school climate that devalued them and their behavior. Through the group intervention, the girls were helped to evaluate distinctly diverse social contexts, deciding on the appropriateness of their responses within each. The purpose of the intervention was to enhance selforganization and self-regulation to aid the development of more elaborate and meaningful responses in different situations. In addition, the intent was to improve understanding of the best way to respond to varied social contexts. The intern enabled the girls to understand the need to choose different behaviors, assessing the best fit between their behavior and the demands of each context. The intern clarified the meaning of appropriate behaviors, when it was proper to turn up the verbal volume (i.e., to be self-affirming by being outspoken, blunt, candid, and loud when rejecting unwanted sexual advances) and when it was fitting and proper to turn down vocalizations (i.e., a quieter vocal style of self-affirmation in the school environment). Furthermore, clinicians empathically clarified developmental issues underlying each circumstance. For example, developmentally, adolescent girls undergo many physical changes and have many concerns about body image. Girls at this age naturally wish to be seen as attractive. The girls were confused by wishes to be sexually attractive and the anger they experienced at the vulgar lasciviousness of the older men. The social work intern validated the girls' normal developmental issues as well as pointedly

50 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES expressed indignation about the "indecent behavior of the men." Observably, the girls' natural self-confidence and capacity to discern threats to their well-being were characteristics associated with resilience that were enhanced by the intern's interventions. As this example illustrates, even though the individual is the focal point of Bronfenbrenner's (1979) ecological perspective, the personprocess-context paradigm allows for a biopsycho-social-historical systemic assessment of social contexts. The adolescent pregnancy research participants no less than the middle school intervention study participants demonstrated these same qualities of mutuality, empathy, recognition, and assertion in everyday life. One part of the adolescent pregnancy research hypothesized that nonpregnant girls are more likely than pregnant girls to develop links with their communities beyond their immediate families and peers. Such linkages are represented by engagement in church, community, employment, and educational environments. Findings supported this hypothesis. Nonpregnant girls differed from the pregnant girls at a < .05 level of significance on critical behavioral, psychological, educational, and situational variables. Unquestionably, both cohorts displayed what now are identified as resilient behaviors; however, nonpregnant girls consistently exhibited a certain individuality that demonstrated stability and hardiness in engagements with nonpeer and nonkin social contexts, such as church, social clubs, jobs, and college. Surprisingly, study results revealed that nonpregnant girls were more likely to express a strong sense of care and nurturance for others, whereas pregnant girls, although soon to be parents, were less likely to express this same care and nurturance (expressed mutuality and empathy as shared affinity with others). In view of the theoretical model set forth, it could be argued that nonpregnant girls were more likely to have sophisticated contexting experiences than their counterparts because of their involvement in varied social settings. One might conclude that the nonpregnant girls' diverse contextual experiences provided opportunities for a wide range of occurrences where intersubjective (self-relatedness) processes of empathy, recognition, and assertion could be played out.

Bearing in mind the distinction between the elements of a social context that includes form, intersubjectivity, and content, the adolescent pregnancy research participants provided rich qualitative data about their neighborhoods and families, including information about a vicinity's or a kin network's composition and organization, its self-relatedness (intersubjective processes), and its substance or essence (content and quality of life). Importantly, the distinction between context structure, context intersubjectivity, and context content is necessary because critics (Delgado, 2000; Swadener & Lubeck, 1995) of the at-risk construct mistakenly confound form, process, and content. Notwithstanding, the ecological model of context is holistic, intersectional, and transactional, making it possible to differentiate interrelated characteristics of a particular social context's elements of form, content, and process. Hence, the model can integrate the at-risk concept with specific process and, contextually, aspects of resilience and protective mechanisms. It is not a matter of either assets or risks as the more important focus for study when examining urban neighborhoods or urban families but, rather, the degree to which both are present. If a social context has a greater elemental presence of risks than assets, then it is likely that the social context poses greater dangers than protections of safety and support. On the face of it all, context elements seem equally consequential, but on closer examination, this may not be the case. Consider for example an economically disadvantaged, single-parent family of five living in substandard rental housing (form) that flagrantly poses dangers to physical safety, yet the family is strong in its history, values, and ethos (content). In the best possible way, the discourse within the family sensitively reflects empathy, care, respect, affirmation, and affinity (intersubjectivity) among members. And yet, there is a great deal of variety in both the risks this family faces and the strengths they bring to bear on coping with their adversity. On the positive side, the eldest daughter graduated from high school, is employed part-time, and attends college and a local Catholic Church. The mother, however, collects disability and is no longer employed. Other factors that must be considered include that the family's neighborhood is ethnically homogeneous, its occupants African American.

Lessons Learned From Poor African American Youth · 51 The neighborhood lays claim to economic and social resources, most of which are liquor stores, small groceries, beauty shops and barbershops, and a local YMCA recently renovated with a gym and swimming pool. Most housing in the neighborhood is substandard with the exception of a two-floor, walk-up public housing complex. A storefront church has been conspicuously present to neighborhood dwellers with its loudspeaker service broadcasts for several years; in addition, there is an established Catholic Church that has served the neighborhood for four generations. Drug gang profiteers have commandeered two old neighborhood buildings for selling and smoking crack cocaine. Unlike the more established services just noted, this threat to the community has been a part of the neighborhood for just two years. On the face of it, the illustration can make no claim with reference to existent risks. It does not suggest that poverty, single-parent households, or the recent infiltration of drug lords automatically mean those in this neighborhood are at risk. Although, empirically, the three demographic indices are indeed associated with neighborhood risk, descriptively, the neighborhood context element discussed thus far is one of form. What is known is a little something about physical structures of social, economic, and religious resources that are conceivably assets to the neighborhood, its families, and their children. What is not known is information about the neighborhood's substance (content), its perceptions of how neighborhood stakeholders view their environs as an identified entity, especially regarding risks and assets. Importantly, other unrevealed information is the intersubjectivity of the neighborhood, stakeholders' self-relatedness in the daily praxis of the life of the neighborhood. Although the structure of neighborhood ecologies seems transparently visible for risk and asset identification, without additional data about remaining context elements of content and intersubjectivity, a risk and asset assessment is incomplete.

DISCOVERING THE STRENGTHS OF INNER-CITY AFRICAN AMERICAN GIRLS Often, clinician biases take place when assessing various contextual domains in which African

American adolescent girls live. For example, if a girl resides in a drug- and crime-ridden neighborhood, the hazardous environment in which she lives may contextually prejudice the clinician. Thus, the clinician's assessment is likely to emphasize what may appear to be obvious social and psychological pathologies rather than the girl's less obvious strengths. Too often, African American girls' demonstrated strengths in assertively dealing with toxic inner-city neighborhoods go unevaluated altogether or are assessed negatively. But it is precisely within the self-relatedness of the nested contexts where a girl is embedded, be they hazardous or risk-free, that her strength and resilience are developed and fostered in meeting maturational challenges. Admittedly, my understanding of this subject was developed from my investigation of adolescent black girls' self-narratives and reports about themselves and the social ecologies in which they were situated. Some of what I discovered about the girls was refreshingly new information, whereas other findings simply confirmed what other studies have already told us. Both studies referred to earlier clearly show that resilient African American girls display a personal hardiness—a focused commitment to follow events through—and a strong sense of self-efficacy. They tend to develop coping strategies that make it possible for them to achieve good outcomes despite misfortune as well as steering clear of avoidable dangerous situations (Stevens, 2002). Lest we overlook the abundant evidence, poor African American inner-city families do develop particular strategies to nurture resilient capacities in their children. Jarrett (1995) has defined such families as "defended families." Accordingly, defended families make available role models in or outside the familial network, restrict peer relationships even within the kin group, and participate in social ecologies outside neighborhood or community boundaries. Furthermore, families protect their offspring from risk environments by enculturating principled values and a future orientation. Such is the case that protected families who exhibit psychosocial strengths, have a strong work ethic, are achievement oriented, have a sense of autonomy and responsibility, and are likely to postpone birthing and marriage until vocations are established and

52 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES jobs secured. Interestingly, protected families go so far as to even restrict relationships with extended family members who share similar values and behaviors. Thus, protected families are ideally situated to support their offspring in meeting developmental challenges through the use of strategies that have proved successful in supporting adolescents in at-risk environments (Hill, 1997; McAdoo, 1997). African American adolescent girls display various discernible strengths in meeting challenges of maturation that are context dependent. These include a range of core psychological strengths, behaviors, and actions that black, poor, inner-city girls show when dealing with perceived and actual threats to safety and wellbeing in varied social ecologies. Specifically, resilient adolescent females are likely to do the following: •







• • • • •

• • •

Have strong attachments to social and religious institutions that provide communal selfrelatedness—institutions that serve as havens for identity exploration and the development of leadership skills Select appropriate role models as a way to formulate principles and standards that will guide present and future behavior Demonstrate efforts in making changes in environmental situations to accommodate more positive and favorable peer affiliations Seek self-experiences for the development of cultural flexibility or bicultural competency when moving beyond neighborhood or community boundaries Confront racially devalued situations without feeling self-blame Assume responsibility for their behavior Resist collusion in racial denigration Discriminately appraise their social context for experiences of self-efficacy Exhibit a stance and attitude of candidness, courage, and assertiveness in response to racial/ethnic devaluation and life's daily hassles in general. Manifest a discriminating capacity in evaluating the behavior of others Demonstrate a capacity for care, loyalty, and nurturance in relation to others Have self-expectancies for social mobility to "improve one's lot in life"

The resilient attributes identified here were drawn from girls' actual experiences in living out their lives. Such traits can be strengthened and sustained when girls are served by social workers. Even though the girls who displayed the above strengths were dealing with their lives ably, they were on a journey of maturation and could benefit from services that aid in their development. To best serve girls with backgrounds similar to those in my research will require rethinking the ways in which we practice helping. Once again, bearing in mind the person-processsocial context paradigm, renewed services demand working with clinical participants contextually and intersubjectively. I conclude with a brief discussion of practice implications based on the argument set forth in this chapter.

PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS

Working Contextually and Intersubjectively Working contextually and intersubjectively, the therapist is not limited by the physical characteristics of traditional therapy (i.e., professional office space) or by the therapeutic material being subject to analytical scrutiny. Rather, development is seen in a state of fluctuation that orders complexity and informs how structure and pattern arises from many different aspects of young people's lives. Plainly, what happens inside the boundaries of the therapeutic relationship as well as what takes place peripherally becomes the playing field for therapeutic work. In the therapeutic field, both conscious and unconscious elements bear on the adolescent's past, present, organized, and organizing experiences (rigid and flexible). All such elements in this gestalt make it possible to explore unhealthy and healthy aspects of self-relatedness (Orange, Atwood, & Stolorow, 1997). In working intersubjectively and contextually, the core strengths of African American inner-city girls delineated here can be put to effective use in the clinical relationship. For example, when doing clinical work, empirically demonstrated strength-based characteristics could be used as assessment guidelines or therapeutic goals with girls who require the building

Lessons Learned From Poor African American Youth · 53 up of their capacities to meet maturational challenges more effectively. In other words, when therapeutic work is completed, a client will have developed a set of psychosocial skills to live life more fully. In particular, based on the discussion earlier, it is hoped that an adolescent girl at the end of therapy will have a more developed capacity to exercise self-efficacy appropriately in different contexts. Working intersubjectively and contextually, successful and effective clinical work strengthens the adolescent girl in a number of ways. She has a more developed capacity for self-reflection; makes pragmatic and judicious use of coping abilities; has an awareness of her gifts, talents, and strengths; and demonstrates a strengthened capacity for assessment of self and others within varied social contexts. Social work clinical interventions take place in a relational matrix of self-relatedness, a mutually relational, culturally nuanced space where the clinician joins with the adolescent as an empathie insider. In so doing, the clinical relationship grows to be one of collaboration, and a significant piece of the clinical work becomes contexting. Recall that contexting is defined as the enhancement of the adolescent's capacity to develop meaning and complexity in thinking and behavior. Thus, the adolescent girl's self-journey within the therapeutic relationship becomes one of identity exploration and self-discovery as the clinical work addresses mutually agreed-on issues, problems, and concerns. In undertaking this learning journey collaboratively with the client, the clinician communicates to the adolescent girl quite simply the nature of the work that they will accomplish together. For instance, she or he might say, We can work together as we both learn who you believe yourself to be here and now—how you want to grow to be what you want to be and learning what you want for the future and how you hope to accomplish what you want. In our work together, you may experience many different kinds of feelings—anger, pain, joy—as we try to figure out answers to these questions. Such is the case that the therapeutic journey is one where the adolescent has the opportunity to create meaning construction and meaning synthesis. The clinician and adolescent girl are

engaged in a therapeutic relational matrix consisting of person-process-context.

Cultural Implications Commonly, social workers understand the need for cultural awareness when working with different ethnicities and/or racial groups. Our contextual worlds are systemically influenced by culture. Recall my second assumption that the clinical work takes place in a relational matrix of intersubjectivity or self-relatedness, a mutually relational shared space, within a contextual field influenced by culture. This second assumption, I believe, speaks directly to issues of culture, ethnicity, and racial or cross-cultural differences. The clinician tries to understand the adolescent, emphasizing who she is and the social context that surrounds her. Nonetheless, I wonder if social work efforts to help poor adolescents in inner-city communities are as successful as they might be. Many of the students I teach do not seek clinical jobs in inner cities. I am led to believe that many clinical social workers feel that inner-city adolescents are not necessarily good candidates for clinical work. Students complain that the many social problems in inner-city communities compromise effective clinical work. Also, most novices feel that they do not have the training and cultural competency required to work with adolescents of color. When working intersubjectively and contextually, however, such cultural competency can be achieved without extensive training. Certainly, when the clinician assumes the role of empathie insider, he or she has a cultural standpoint or some fundamental level of cultural awareness. However, even when the therapist is different from the client, creating a space for intersubjectivity through recognition that the client and clinician are contextually embedded in similar surroundings may help create a sense that they both belong to one larger community and hold much in common. Still, when working intersubjectively and contextually, clinician biases, misperceptions (i.e., cultural, theoretical, professional, or personal) do transpire. Both anecdotal and empirical evidence suggests that in cross-race clinical dyads, practitioners with immature racial/ethnic identities themselves are not as likely to help

54 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES clients achieve positive therapeutic outcomes (Carter, 1997; Helms, 1990). Obviously, when racial/ethnic biases exist, empathie failures happen. The clinician is obligated to examine why failures in empathy have occurred. Misapplication of clinical principles and human behavior theory may also result when racial/ethnic biases occur in the therapeutic relationship. Effective clinical work demands that clinicians learn to manage tensions inherent in the nature of sameness and differences in the human condition (Dean, 2001). To be tmfhfully empathie, the clinician must be able to recognize difference in sameness. We like to think that empathie individuals are uniquely attuned to the mental and emotional state of another. Certainly, empathy is an imaginative process that involves mutual recognition of the other (i.e., I can perceive that the other is like myself), characterized by an attitude of care and understanding. Empathy involves both imagination and perspective taking and, thus, allows an individual to imagine himself or herself living in the context of the other. Dean (2001) has argued, however, that crosscultural competence, social work's standard for cultural practices, is a concept deeply flawed. She challenges social workers to adopt a postmodern view of "not knowing" and embrace a model of cultural noncompetence. Such a model, Dean asserts, enables the clinician to accept his or her lack of competence rather than striving to achieve a false sense of cultural competence. I agree with Dean in that the notion of cultural competence suggests a goal that is unrealizable. In my own clinical work, not knowing means I am poised, alert, and open to learning all that I can in the context of a subject-to-subject relationship. When I assume an attitude of not knowing, I am simultaneously engaged in "desiring to know." Thus, I am open to learning from myriad perspectives, one of which is the adolescent girl herself, who is, in fact, the expert witness regarding her life experiences. For that reason, I am humbled to be accepted and invited to participate in her world (Stevens, 1998). It is a world with which I want to become acquainted. Practically speaking, absolutism, certainty, or expertness embodied in concepts of either "knowing" or "competence" applied to cultural practices infer power, dominance, and superiority, all elements of the therapeutic relationship that

distance us from our subjectivity. I recognize that the objectification of clinical work belies working intersubjectively. I suggest that clinicians must have sufficient humility to enter the client's world. Understanding, acceptance, and a nonjudgmental attitude—elementary components of sound clinical practice—underscore this humility (Stevens, 1998). Moreover, in desiring to know, there is a certain willingness on my part to "be known." Consequently, I can be open and (cautiously) transparent with a client.

Self-Disclosure and Engagement When Working Intersubjectively and Contextually The clinician, when working intersubjectively and contextually, commits to the likelihood of therapist self-disclosure. Unquestionably, clinician self-disclosure has limitations, and the appropriateness of sharing personal information must be determined in advance. Clearly, the purpose of any shared material always serves the interest of the client and the therapeutic process; it stands to reason that any personal information shared should be sufficiently innocuous as to do no harm but still be of help to the client. The clinician's therapeutic position of openness, transparency, and humility reduces instances of cultural or personal bias and hence empathie failures. Not surprisingly, when empathie inquiry is sustained through clarification, interpretation, or any such therapeutic intervention, self-disclosure can advance the therapeutic work. The point to make here is that uninterrupted empathie inquiry is the crucible of working intersubjectively and contextually. Moreover, the use of nontraditional practices strengthens the clinician's role of empathie insider and offers opportunities for augmenting knowledge of the adolescent's social contexts.

REFERENCES Anderson, E. (1999). Code of the street: Decency, violence, and the moral life of the inner city. New York: W. W. Norton. Benjamin, J. (1990). An outline of intersubjectivity: The development of recognition. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 7(Suppl.), 33^16.

Lessons Learned From Poor African American Youth · 55 Bolland, J. M. (2001). In search of a few hundred good kids: Three months in the life of a community-based survey research study. Families in Society, 82(1), 76-96. Bourgois, P. I. (1995). In search of respect: Selling crack in el barrio. New York: Cambridge University Press. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Brunswick, A. F. (1999). Structural strain: An ecological paradigm for studying African American drug abuse. In M. R. De La Rosa, B. Segal, & R. Lopez (Eds.), Drugs and society: Conducting research with minority populations: Advances and issues (pp. 5-19). New York: Haworth Press. Carter, R. T. (1997). The influence of race and racial identity in psychotherapy. New York: Lexington Books. Collins, P. H. (1986). Learning from the outsider within: The sociological significance of black feminist thought. Social Problems, 33(6), S14-S23. Collins, P. H. (1990). Black feminist thought. Boston: Unwin Hyman. Crossley, N. (1996). Intersubjectivity: The fabric of social becoming. London: Sage. Dean, R. G. (2001). The myth of cross-cultural competence. Families in Society, 82(6), 623-663. Delgado, M. (2000). New arenas for community social work practice with urban youth. New York: Columbia University Press. Denzin, Ν. K. (1997). Interpretive ethnography. Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage. Ford, D. Y. (1994). Nurturing resilience in gifted young black youth. Roper Review, 17(2), 80-85. Garmezy, N. (1993, February). Children in poverty: Resilience despite risk. Psychiatry, 56, 127-136. Gibbs, J. T. (1988). Young black males in America: Endangered, embittered, and embattled. In J. T. Gibbs (Ed.), Young, black, and male in America: An endangered species (pp. 1-35). Dover, MA: Auburn House. Hall, Ε. T. (1971). Beyond culture. Garden City, NY: Doubleday Anchor Books. Helms, J. E. (1990). Black and white racial identity: Theory, research, and practice. New York: Greenwood Press. Hill, R. B. (1997). The strengths ofAfrican American families: Twenty-five years later. Washington, DC: R&B.

hooks, B. (1984). From margin to center. Boston: South End Press. Jarrett, R. L. (1995). Growing up poor: The family experience of socially mobile youth in lowincome African American neighborhoods. Journal ofAdolescent Research, 10(1), 111-134. Kanuha, V. K. (2000). "Being native" versus "Going native": Conducting research as an insider. Social Work, 45(5), 439^47. McAdoo, H. P. (1997). Black families (3rd ed.). Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage. McWhirter, J. J., McWhirter, Β. T, McWhirter, A. M., & McWhirter, Ε. H. (1998). At risk youth: A comprehensive response. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Orange, D. M., Atwood, G. E., & Stolorow, R. D. (1997). Working intersubjectively: Contextualism in psychoanalytic practice. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press. Ratner, M. S. (Ed.). (1993). Crack pipe as pimp: An ethnographic investigation of sex-for-crack exchanges. New York: Lexington Books. Smith, D. E. (1989). Sociological theory: Methods of writing patriarchy. In R. A. Wallace (Ed.), Feminism and sociological theory (pp. 34-64). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Spencer, M. B. (1995). Old issues and new theorizing about African-American youth: A phenomenological variant of ecological systems theory. In R. L. Taylor (Ed.), African-American youth: Their social and economic status in the United States (pp. 37-70). Westport, CT: Praeger. Sterk, C. E. (1999). Fast lives: Women who use crack cocaine. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Stevens, J. W. (1997). Opportunity outlook and coping in poor urban African American late age female adolescent contraceptors. Smith College Studies, 67(3), 456-476. Stevens, J. W. (1998). A question of values in social work practice: Working with the strengths of black adolescent females. Families in Society, 79(3), 288-296. Stevens, J. W. (2001). The social ecology of the co-occurrence of substance use and early coitus among poor urban black female adolescents. Substance Use and Misuse, 36(4), 421—446. Stevens, J. W. (2002). Smart and sassy: The strengths of inner city black girls. New York: Oxford University Press. Swadener, Β. B., & Lubeck, S. (Eds.). (1995). Children and families "at promise": Deconstructing the

56 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES discourse of risk. Albany: State University Press of New York. Wallace, J. M. (1999). Explaining race differences in adolescent and young adult drug use: The role of racialized social systems. In M. R. De La Rosa, B. Segal, & R. Lopez (Eds.), Drugs and society: Conducting research with minority populations: Advances and issues (pp. 21-36). New York: Haworth Press.

William, T. M., & Kornblum, W. (1985). Growing up poor. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. William, T. M., & Kornblum, W. (1994). The uptown kids. New York: Putnam. Wilson, J. W. (1987). When work disappears. New York: Knopf.

4 GENDERED ADAPTATIONS, RESILIENCE, AND THE PERPETRATION OF VIOLENCE JANE F. GILGUN LAURA S . ABRAMS

D

evelopmental psychopathologists seek to identify processes that lead to adaptive and maladaptive outcomes among high-risk groups, with the goal of contributing to interventions that can promote resilience and decrease risks (Luthar, 2003; Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000). Researchers, however, have given little attention to understanding the points of view of people who respond to their adverse life circumstances in ways that outsiders would define as maladaptive (Ungar, 2004). Soliciting the points of view of research participants helps us to understand the gendered, social, and cultural contexts to which individuals adapt. These concerns are important to human service professions that build on the premise of starting where the clients are. This chapter is based on research with adolescents and adults, both women and men, who have experienced adversities and whose behaviors led them to involvement with the law, primarily for violent acts, which are, from almost any point of view, a maladapted outcome. In the

study we discuss here, we developed a conceptual model that we applied to our analysis of two cases: an adolescent young man who was abusive to his girlfriend who was also the mother of his baby and an adult woman who engaged in sexual activities with an adolescent young woman. Our perspectives were constructivist and interpretive (Benner, 1994; Patton, 2002) in that we wanted to represent the points of view of the persons we interviewed. In our analysis, we show that the young man and adult woman view their illegal behaviors as logical and effective ways to enhance their own well-being and sometimes the well-being of loved ones, although usually they are aware that their behaviors contravene social norms and their own moral values. We contend that these views are fairly typical of people whom social and legal institutions consider "deviant" and even criminal. The elements of our conceptual model are human agency, resilience, schema theory, gendered adaptation, feminist and masculinity theory, and gendered interpretations of female 57

58 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES and male violence. We view these elements as interconnected and forming a viable theory of violence as gendered adaptations. Our method is deductive qualitative analysis (Gilgun, in press a, in press b), which by definition begins with a conceptual model that researchers can put to many different uses, such as hypothesis testing, pattern matching, or as a guide to exploring poorly understood topics. Deductive qualitative analysis (DQA) is different from many other ways of doing qualitative research in that the procedures call for developing a conceptual model before entering the field. Many forms of qualitative research, taking the lead from the work of Straus and his colleagues (Charmaz, 2000; Clarke, 2003; Glaser, 1978; Glaser & Strauss, 1967; Strauss & Corbin, 1998), guide researchers toward research that begins in an open-ended, atheoretical way. DQA builds on the traditions of analytic induction (Cressey, 1953; Gilgun, in press, b; Znaniecki, 1934), a form of qualitative research that originated in the Chicago School of Sociology in the early part of the 20th century. Analytic induction begins with a loose conceptual framework but claims to be inductive, which is actually a contradiction in terms, although many of the ideas on which analytic induction is based are important to DQA as well (see Gilgun, in press b, for an extended discussion of DQA compared with other forms of qualitative research).

A WORKING DEFINITION OF VIOLENCE For the purposes of this chapter, we contrast interpersonal violence with healthy interpersonal relationships. Mutuality, reciprocity, and the promotion of the best interests of others are widely recognized qualities characteristic of healthy relationships (Seifert, Hoffnung, & Hoffnung, 2000). Violence, on the other hand, not only bypasses mutuality and reciprocity and undermines the best interests of others, but it includes behaviors that hurt others psychologically and emotionally and often physically as well. Individuals involved in mutual and reciprocal relationships contribute to the well-being and quality of life for others. They respect others' autonomy and freedom of choice. They provide comfort in times of stress, and they sometimes

consciously give up something they want to promote the well-being of others and to nurture the continuation of the relationship. If they inadvertently hurt others, they apologize, seek forgiveness, and make efforts to repair the relationship and to change their own behaviors so as not to harm again. Such behaviors are mirror images of our concept of violence. They appear, on the surface at least, to mimic behavior we might associate with resilience. Relationships are, of course, fraught with possibilities of hurting others, whether intentionally or not. Our definition of violence assumes differential power, which can stem from attributes such as gender, physical strength, social status, experience, age, and maturation. Ideologies, such as who is entitled to what and who has rights to do what, grow from and structure these attributes. We believe that individuals who choose to take advantage of the power they have over others have a limitless number of cultural themes and practices that they use to guide and justify their behaviors but that they transform these in individualized, idiosyncratic ways (Gilgun & McLeod, 1999). In this chapter, we look primarily at age and gender to understand how these individuals engage in processes associated with the use of interpersonal violence as an awkward but effective means of experiencing health-related phenomena. In mutual, reciprocal relationships between generational equals, there is ongoing negotiation of issues such as understanding what the other person wants, how to reconcile differing wants and desires, and how to decide what actually does promote the well-being of the other (Gottman, 2001). However, this is not the case in crossgenerational interaction. We view older people as having particular responsibilities for recognizing and respecting generational differences, even if it means putting aside one's own powerful desires. Nor is it the case for relationships in which there are differences in power between men and women or where violence is expressed in gender-typical ways. We view males as more likely to commit overtly aggressive acts, whether physical, verbal, or psychological—although there are exceptions, as in child sexual abuse and incest where some perpetrators insist that

Gendered Adaptations, Resilience, and the Perpetration of Violence · 59 they act out of love and caring (Gilgun, 1995)— and for women, with some exceptions, to be more subtle and indirect in how they undermine the well-being of others (Crick, Grotpeter, & Bigbee, 2002). Even more confusing, perpetrators can experience emotional and sexual gratification from violence, often associated with a sense of power and control over others. Given these patterns, we propose that current understandings and definitions of violence are gendered—that is, biased toward more typically "male" styles of violence that connote and denote physical and verbal aggression. Finally, we believe there can be major differences between how outsiders interpret behaviors and how the people concerned interpret their own behaviors. Professionals may have great difficulty shifting perspectives so that they can connect to and understand the points of view of clients. For example, the research and theory on which we have drawn for our analysis is based on perspectives of "outsiders"—that is, the points of view of researchers and rarely those of the researched. Insiders' views on the meaning of their behaviors, including those that outsiders view as maladaptive, will contribute to the effectiveness of interventions that professionals craft in their efforts to enhance resilience processes and decrease the effects of risks. A number of chapters in this work, for example, achieve just such a link between understanding tied to research and better-informed interventions. Finally, contemporary thought on human agency emphasizes the idea of choice, but the range of options is restricted by the time in which people live, the settings in which they develop, and the people and ideologies to which they are exposed (Giddens, 1987). Social locations provide contexts and interpretive filters through which individuals make decisions about how to respond to challenging life circumstances (Abrams, 2003).

RESILIENCE Resilience represents capacities for coping with, adapting to, and overcoming adversities as well as competence in developmental tasks (Masten & Coatsworfh, 1998). Resilience is widely recognized as a "manifestation of positive outcomes"

after "exposure to adversity" (Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000, p. 857). People in high-risk situations and who exhibit prosocial, adaptive outcomes are said to have had the capacities to marshal sufficient resources to help them deal effectively with risks. When people do not have capacities to marshal resources in high-risk situations, then maladaptive behaviors occur, including violent acts. A person may be competent, prosocial, and well adapted in one domain, such as work, but have maladapted outcomes in other domains, such as interpersonal relationships and emotion regulation (Gilgun, 1996a, 1996b, 1999, 2002a). Although resilience is associated with prosocial adaptations to adversities, some researchers have pointed out that definitions of competence and resilience may vary across gender, age, socioeconomic status, and ethnicity (Abrams, 2002; Burton, Allison, & Obeidallah, 1995; Ungar, 2004). For example, children who live in dangerous neighborhoods may develop adaptive coping strategies that are maladaptive in more secure settings (Garcia Coll et al., 1996), whereas people who come from backgrounds of privilege and physical security may develop a sense of entitlement that is maladaptive in other settings (Gilgun, 1996b; Gilgun & McLeod, 1999). Many young people and their parents in resource-poor environments may aspire to a "revised American dream" in their definitions of successful outcomes (Burton et al., 1995). Moreover, resilience can have a gendered component; that is, resilience processes and outcomes may be different for women and men in gendered societies.

ADAPTATION Adaptation can be broadly defined as responses to the demands of the environment (Ashford, LeCroy, & Lortie, 2001; Siegelman & Rider, 2003). The meaning of the term encompasses the mutuality and reciprocity of interactions between people (and other organisms) and their environments (Germaine & Gitterman, 1996). Environments shape human growth and development, and human beings shape their own environments. As Germaine and Gitterman (1996) point out, all organisms require resources from their environments to survive and develop.

60 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES How individuals do this depends on the resources available to them, their capacities to respond to these resources, and the goals they value. Adaptation is a key concept in Piaget's theory of cognitive development. Piaget's theory is constructivist in that it is based on the idea that human beings actively construct their knowledge of themselves, others, and the world. Adaptations are mediated through schémas that people construct based on their interactions with environments (Berk, 2003). Schémas can be thought of as mental representations or internalized working models that help people make sense of their experiences through assimilation and accommodation. In assimilation, individuals fit new experiences into existing schémas, and in accommodation, they modify and transform their schémas to incorporate new information. Contemporary cognitive scientists generally have come to the conclusion that the activation of mental representations and their associated behaviors and motivations are almost entirely nonconscious. Most thoughts, emotions, moods, and behaviors occur automatically in response to a person's perceptions of environmental cues and are outside of conscious awareness (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999). These automatic mechanisms bypass conscious choice and appraisal. The cues that activate these mental representations are analogous to "buttons being pushed." According to Bargh and Chartrand (1999), "In whatever way the start button is pushed, the mechanism subsequently behaves in the same way" (p. 476).

GENDERED ADAPTATIONS This chapter is about "gendered adaptations" to adverse circumstances. To say that human adaptation is a "gendered" process means that how individuals construct themselves, choose to act, and develop their operant worldview is influenced to varying extents by gendered schémas derived from larger social norms and expectations, stereotypes, and power relationships. This focus on gender is not intended to diminish other significant forces that shape cognitive scripts and adaptation processés, such as ethnicity and culture, class, family background, or personal

experiences. Rather, with the understanding that the very meaning of violence is underscored by gendered assumptions (e.g., violence toward others is generally viewed as a "male" attribute, whereas violence toward the self is typically seen as a "female" attribute), we believe in the importance of analyzing perpetrators' adaptations to adverse circumstances through the lens of their gendered beliefs, roles, and strategies. The concept of gendered adaptations draws on theories found in feminist psychology, masculinities studies, and related genres of literature. Feminist psychology, for example, illustrates how dominant societal constructs of "femininity" and expectations related to gender roles shape women's social and psychological adaptation strategies. Feminist psychologists Brown and Gilligan (1992) argue that women are socialized into a more "relational" stance than their male counterparts. This means that women are more attuned to the intricate world of human relations and that self-esteem is gained primarily by virtue of connections with others. Gilligan (1982) stated that, gradually, women learn "not only to define themselves in the context of human relationships but also judge themselves in terms of their ability to care" (p. 17). This "care" orientation continues throughout the life cycle and becomes more solidified when a woman takes on mothering roles. According to Gilligan (1982), this emphasis on relation and care eventually becomes problematic because Western, white, Anglo-Saxon culture does not typically value or reward these traditionally female qualities. Moreover, women's socialization toward relationships as a primary gauge of self-worth sets the stage for a variety of internal conflicts for adolescent and adult women, including issues of selfdoubt, authenticity, and autonomy. Building on feminist psychology, we assume that women tend to manage processes of assimilation and accommodation of internalized messages about their gender roles and their tendencies to privilege narratives of care and relation above and beyond those of "autonomy" or "achievement." These are not merely individual scripts, but rather, they are derived from positions and discourses of masculinity and femininity that circulate through larger social arenas and institutions. Responding to adverse circumstances,

Gendered Adaptations, Resilience, and the Perpetration of Violence · 61 women, particularly those socialized in dominant culture norms, tend to harm themselves rather than perpetrate violence or harm toward others. Turning anger or rage inward, against the self, is a traditionally gendered adaptation strategy for women. A woman facing adverse economic or emotional circumstances might also draw on traditionally gendered survival skills such as selling one's body, becoming dependent, or seeking care and protection from others. The decision making that leads to these outcomes may not be based on conscious processes but may be an outcome of automatic schémas. 1

Women's Aggressive Behaviors When women are aggressive toward others, there is increasing evidence that the forms the aggression takes are gendered—that is, filtered through and influenced by broader social discourses that provide constructions of "womanhood" and "femininity." For example, Crick and colleagues (Crick & Dodge, 1994, 1996; Crick et al., 2002; Crick et al., 1999) have conducted research on "female" forms of aggression, which they call "relational aggression." Focusing on school-age and adolescent girls, they defined relational aggression as behaviors that harm others through damage, or the threat of damage, to relationships or feelings of acceptance, friendship, or group inclusion. These behaviors include acts such as giving someone the "silent treatment" to punishing him or her to get one's own way, using social exclusion as a form of retaliation, or threatening to end a friendship unless the friend complies with a request (Crick et al., 1999). These are forms of violence that are neither physically nor verbally assaultive but are, at least from perpetrators' points of view, meant to be gentle, kind, and loving. As mentioned earlier, some perpetrators of incest and child sexual abuse view their experiences in this way (Gilgun, 1995; Gilgun & McLeod, 1999). Both women and men may experience sexual behaviors with persons unable to give consent—because of age, ability, social status, and power—in these ways.

Men's Gendered Adaptations Until recently, social theory did not directly address men's psychological development and

adaptation. Rather, researchers assumed that men's psychology was the "norm" and left the gendered nature of experience unspecified. Masculinities theory, rooted in sociology and in criminology, presents understandings of gendered social and psychological adaptation that both complement and extend the feminist frame. These ideas also help to illustrate the gendered aspects of violence and the perpetration of crime. Masculinities theory offers contextualized views of male behaviors and adaptations in its premise that there is no universal, fixed masculine identity. Rather, throughout the life course, men adopt a range of gendered and culturally patterned responses to their social environments (Connell, 1987,1995; Goodey, 1997; Messerschmidt, 2000). Connell (1987) identified a hierarchy of possible masculinities, in which the traditionally held understanding of masculinity as the stoic, emotionally unexpressive, competitive breadwinner role assumes dominance. He called this traditional masculine response "hegemonic masculinity" and suggested that this identity has become the normative benchmark against which other masculine responses are measured. Alternative masculine responses, including more "effeminate" masculinities, homosexual, bisexual, and transgendered men, and others, become subordinated in relation to hegemonic masculinity. "Subordinate masculinities" are aberrations from the ideal and are, in many cases, negated by the dominant culture as viable or attractive masculine alternatives (Connell, 1995). Building on Connell's construction of hegemonic masculinity, other scholars (Gilgun & McLeod, 1999; Majors & Billson, 1992; Messerschmidt, 2000; West & Zimmerman, 1987) suggest that the resources men have at their disposal for expressing their maleness play a significant role in determining their individual responses or adaptations to their social environments. In the absence of resources allowing for expression of alternate, or subordinated, masculine traits (nurturing, cooperation, nonviolent conflict resolution, etc.), men compensate with excessive demonstrations of the one form of masculinity that is most influential, obvious, and valued—hegemonic masculinity. Men whose masculine experience is limited primarily to the expression of maleness as power and

62 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES aggression are unlikely to develop masculine identities that value cooperation and communication. On the contrary, their dominant gender expression is likely to fit into the framework of violence, power, and competition. Majors and Billson (1992), for example, observed the phenomenon of subordinated masculinities in their study of African American young men and their adoption of "cool pose"— a style of behaving and speaking that for some African American men allows them to negotiate their diminished power status in relation to the dominant culture. Although the marginalization they experience as African Americans robs them of many of the opportunities and resources needed to effectively fulfill hegemonic masculine ideals of power and success, they nonetheless strive to embody these very ideals through different channels, such as aggression, or streetwise knowledge and behaviors. Thus, they assimilate and perhaps accommodate into their inner representations or schémas, representations of self, others, and how the world works based on their interpretations of their experiences and the contexts in which they live their lives. Majors and Billson (1992), along with other masculinity theorists (Kersten, 1990; LoPresto & Deluty, 1987; Messerschmidt, 2000) identify the potential for violent or aggressive behavior as expressions of masculine scripts—which are similar to schémas—among marginalized groups of men as a means to conform to the structures of hegemonic masculinity even in their subordinated status. Anderson (1990) also suggests that violence and the perpetration of criminal acts can be seen as gendered adaptation strategies for survival in low-income communities with high rates of unemployment and social disorganization. Men, therefore, choose from a set of gendered adaptation strategies that might vary significantly according to their available resources, their class and race positions, and their contextual frames of reference. In response to adverse circumstances or distress, men incorporate into their self-schemas values related to violence and the perpetration of crimes as a means to maintain or fulfill hegemonic male expectations— such as power, control, or making "quick money." Although women might adopt similar schémas and associated behaviors, their

physical and sexual enactments of violence are typically viewed as more of a transgression of gender expectations than a conforming position, unlike the case with men.

METHOD This is not a traditional research report, nor is it a traditional theoretical discussion but, rather, the application of a theoretical model to two cases for the purposes of illustrating how the key concepts of our model play themselves out in individual lives. This approach is characteristic of deductive qualitative analysis (Gilgun, in press a, in press b). Our goal was to show how individuals act out cultural themes and practices and even invent new ways of doing so. We developed the two cases from ethnographic life history approaches, meaning that our goal was to examine individual lives and interpretations within the contexts of social, cultural, and historical themes and practices (Chambers, 2000; Denzin, 1989a, 1989b; Goldstein, 1994). As a prime research method of the Chicago School of Sociology, a seat of interactionist theorizing, life histories are a method of choice for examining how larger social forces and ideologies, based on gender, age, and social class, influence human lives (Gilgun & McLeod, 1999; Tierney, 2000). Gilgun's contribution to the present chapter is based on life history qualitative research she has conducted for more than 18 years with people who have had exposure to risk factors that predict violent behaviors, such as being abused and neglected in childhood, witnessing violence, espousal of violence-positive ideologies, and being discouraged from feeling and expressing emotions (Gilgun, 1990, 1991, 1992, 1994, 1995, 1996b, 1999, 2002b; Gilgun & McLeod, 1999; Gilgun, Klein, & Pranis, 2000; Gilgun & Reiser, 1990). Most had committed felony-level violence, such as child sexual abuse, physical abuse of children, woman battering, burglary, armed robbery, attempted murder, and murder and were in prison. Some of the women were convicted of felonies, but most had not. The ethnicity of the sample was European American, African American, Latino, and American Indian.

Gendered Adaptations, Resilience, and the Perpetration of Violence · 63 Abram's work draws on an ethnographic study of young males 14 to 17 years old incarcerated for 4 to 6 months in secure correctional facilities. That study involved both qualitative interviewing and observation. These young men were diverse in ethnicity and included white, Hmong, Native American, Latino, and African American participants. All were repeat offenders, and the majority grew up in working-class, poor, and urban environments of the Twin Cities, Minnesota. The first wave of this project involved over 100 observations and a series of in-depth interviews with 12 youthful offenders who volunteered to participate in the study. These interviews were conducted jointly by Abrams and a male research assistant (Abrams, Kim, & Anderson, in press). Although these two studies had different goals and research questions, we found that our data complement one another in several ways. Specifically, bringing together the youth and adult perspectives on violence and resilience as gendered adaptations provides insight into useful and innovative concepts in understanding adaptation to adverse circumstances and to the development of violent and criminal behaviors. ANALYSIS For our analysis, we chose two cases: an adolescent young man who was in a juvenile correctional facility for physically assaulting his girlfriend and a woman who had abused her authority to engage in a sexual relationship with an adolescent young woman.

Elijah Description Elijah is a 16-year-old, African American male sentenced to serve 6 months in the county juvenile correctional facility for a probation violation involving a physical assault on his baby's mother, who is also his current girlfriend. His prior record includes possession of a weapon, fighting, and curfew violations. Elijah also has a history of selling and using marijuana, although he was never caught for these crimes. Elijah was raised primarily by his biological mother and stepfather. His biological father deserted the family when Elijah was very young,

was addicted to crack cocaine, and spent time in and out of jail in Chicago. His stepfather and mother both use alcohol and marijuana openly, and his stepfather has a history of selling drugs and periodic incarceration. His older brother, whom he considered to be his closest friend, fathered five children by the age of 20 and was incarcerated for a 4-year sentence at the time of the interview. Elijah revealed that his mother and stepfather used force for discipline, including punching and whipping with a belt, but he didn't consider the behavior to be abusive. Elijah's family lived in one of the poorest, transient neighborhoods in the metropolitan Twin Cities area. This neighborhood is known for crime, drugs, and gang activity. He claimed that his family struggled with having enough food and maintaining a stable apartment. After fathering a son at age 15, Elijah sought to make quick money by selling marijuana and having sex with older women for cash or material goods. While he involved himself in these sexual exchanges, he still maintained a primary relationship with his baby's mother, Monica, whom he still considered his "girlfriend." On the day that he was arrested, he stated that he was upset over news of his brother's extended prison time and jealous because he saw Monica talking with a male peer at school. He said that he confronted Monica about the flirtation and she joked with him about having a crush on this other person. When they got into a verbal argument about it, the conversation escalated to a physical altercation, with Monica throwing the first punch, and Elijah proceeding to hit her and choke her until she threatened to call the police. According to Elijah, he ended up calling the police, not her. He was booked and sentenced technically for a probation violation. Analysis Elijah's version of his crime is imbued with several distinct and competing cognitive schémas. In his first interview (out of a series of five interviews over 6 months), he explained that he physically attacked Monica because she provoked him and disrespected him by "going for his face." Later in the same interview, he added that while he was hitting and choking her, what he was really feeling underneath it all was hurt.

64 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES Interviewer: Did you feel good when you pushed her? Elijah:

No. I was hurt. I was hurt, but I didn't really express my hurtness. I was hurt when I smacked her.

He also stated in a subsequent interview that he acted impulsively and compared his behavior with someone "who was on drugs . . . I was acting brain dead." This impulsive behavior fits into the framework of an automatic schema activated when he feels hurt or disrespected and results in the use of physical force. At the same time that he could articulate why he used physical force against Monica, he also espouses a set of moral standards that include a strong prohibition against ever "hitting a woman." He claims that he learned these values from his mother and his stepfather. "Dad said to me, if you love a girl, don't put your hand on her." To assimilate the reality of his assault against Monica into his espoused moral frame, he convinced himself that Monica knew that he didn't want to hit her. "Each time I hit her, she knew I didn't want to hit her." Because of this overriding value, he wished that he had made a different choice and fought the "dude" that was flirting with Monica, not Monica herself. However, he made a conscious choice not to fight "the dude" because he had marijuana with him at the time. At several points during the interview series, he also talked about his ethic of treating women with respect and the importance of equality between men and women in relationships. He claimed to have a perfectly "equal" relationship with Monica because they both show each other so much love and they share the responsibility of a child. His professed morality with regard to women is a competing schema that he did not activate in the instance that he assaulted Monica. What Elijah doesn't say explicitly but what emerged in the analysis of the transcripts was that his gender/morality schema with regard to not "hitting a woman" can be overridden by a feeling of disrespect, which, for him, automatically triggers violence. The time that he hit Monica back, he was reacting to being hit "in the face," which he interpreted as a sign of disrespect. Elijah described a very similar incident

that occurred with a female friend a few years earlier when he hit her and pushed her "down the stairs" because she "pointed in his face" after he told her not to. Elijah's violent responses illustrate his gendered adaptation to feelings of "disrespect" and his consistent need to prove himself or to "save face" in these interactions. This gendered version of violence, for him, works on many levels—in fights with other boys, in his drugdealing activities, and in regard to his own beliefs about what it means to have power. In his own words, "When you have power, people pay you respect." He associates selling drugs with making oneself powerful because of the money, which also gives one respect. "I liked it [selling marijuana] cuz I was getting respect, and I liked it cuz I was getting all the money." These ideas about power and disrespect fit neatly into his contextual urban frame of reference, even though they have the potential to contradict his professed moral frame with regard to hitting a woman. In his response to adverse family circumstances, Elijah learned to attend to his survival needs by making quick money through selling marijuana, which represents a masculinized response to adverse economic circumstances in his community. In addition, he also earned money and material goods by trading sex with older women, representing a more transgressive, or feminized gendered survival strategy. To reconcile this gender transgression, he draws a clear line between his exchanges with older women and what female prostitutes do: He doesn't negotiate money beforehand, he doesn't "pass" diseases, and he gets pleasure as well out of the sexual exchange. In this sense, he understands the exchange to be still "in his power," even though the women are older and he is bartering a service for them. Elijah can be viewed as having resilient traits because he consistently strives to feel powerful and strong in relation to others and to meet his and his baby's material needs. Drawing on his physical strength, athletic ability, and charismatic personality, Elijah tends to craft situations to his advantage to meet his needs. According to Elijah, "It's a great feeling for you to control yourself, be power yourself than for other people to be power over you or controlling you."

Gendered Adaptations, Resilience, and the Perpetration of Violence · 65 His method of achieving his goal of being "on top" through fighting, sexual exchange, or selling drugs would be viewed as maladaptive from a clinical or social work perspective. However, in his social context, Elijah's willingness to go after his goals and to provide for his family can also be seen as adaptive to the circumstances that he faces and as an acknowledgment of the limited resources available to him. Appreciating Elijah's behavior as adaptive is difficult unless we look at this research through a broader social, political, and structural lens. This work makes clear the need to understand the context in which behavior occurs rather than predetermining which behaviors are and are not associated with resilience.

Caron Description Caron, 34, was sexually involved for 2 years with a young woman named Tina who was a member of a church youth group that Caron directed. Caron, a college graduate who had a professional career in the arts, had been married for 10 years and had no children. She worked at the church as a part-time youth leader. She had grown up in this church, where her mother had been president of the women's auxiliary for many years and where she herself had been active in youth groups in high school. Caron had known Tina for 2 years before the sexual involvement, which began when Tina was 16. The sexual part of their relationship ended when Tina talked to a church counselor, who told the minister. The minister fired Caron and sent a letter to the congregation about the sexual abuse. Caron sought treatment with a psychiatrist who advised her to self-report her abuse to the police. She did. The police did not bring charges because, according to Caron, Tina was 18 and Caron was seeking the therapy that social services would have recommended. Caron's husband steadfastly supported his wife. He did not know about Caron and Tina's sexual involvement until Caron was fired. He was the youth director at the church. He subsequently resigned from his job. A year after the public disclosure of the sexual abuse, Caron and her husband invited

Tina to live with them because she was having trouble at home for disobedience, staying out after curfew, and using drugs. Tina lived with the couple for several months until she returned home. Caron said there was no resumption of their sexual activity, although she remained in love with Tina. From Caron's point of view, she and Tina both grew psychologically and emotionally through their relationship. Tina was the first person Caron was open with about her deepest thoughts and feelings. In her relationship to Tina, Caron experienced deep love, commitment, and authenticity. Analysis Whereas Elijah had competing schémas about how to treat women, Caron had one rock solid schema about her commitment to Tina, although she was aware that others had different views. She was unswerving in her representation of her relationship as moral. She said, "To become involved with Tina was more a moral, morally right to me, because I understood what was happening, and I felt right about it." She knew she was contravening what "society" thought, what her church thought, what Tina's mother would think, and what her "job was telling her." She must have had a sense of what her husband would have thought because she didn't tell him. As she said, she knew "the moral stance out there is, you know, to say no." Conventional morality, according to Caron, meant "I should say no to Tina, and I should tell her to bug off and leave me alone and basically abandon her." Furthermore, Caron believed that "not becoming involved with Tina because of someone else's feeling is not a moral stance." She said she looked very hard for reasons not to become sexually involved with Tina: "I couldn't give her enough reasons morally or enough reasons for her development to say no. I didn't find any at the time. I looked. I looked for them like crazy but I couldn't find any." She cast this sexual relationship in terms of caring, a stereotypical female way of behaving. She recognized that she was paying the consequences, which included public exposure, firing, and her grandmother's phoning her up to

66 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES tell her that she didn't want Caron to come to her funeral. Caron viewed involvement with Tina as a courageous, caring moral act because not to be sexual with Tina would have hurt Tina emotionally. She linked her commitment to engage in a sexual relationship with Tina to her own sense of abandonment, which she saw as arising from her own mother's leaving home with a man who was not her husband when Caron was about 14. Caron said she could identify completely with Tina, whose own family was not there for her. She said that Tina wanted to be sexual with her and to turn Tina away would have hurt her. Caron said she would never hurt Tina. She linked her commitment to not abandoning Tina to her own religious beliefs: I didn't want to abandon her. I will take shit to kingdom come before I will bail out.... In some ways that is very, very strong to my religious point of view. It's very strong that you be there for people. It is very strong that that's the spiritual connection and understanding of who Jesus Christ was. He didn't bail out. He didn't go when he . . . and he didn't maybe have his self-protection up either... when it really comes down to the story. And so religiously and morally, i t . . . i t . . . yeah, that's where I'm at. That Caron made sense of her relationship with Tina in terms of a religion and religious language in which she was steeped shows how people build their schémas and make their adjustments to their environments in terms of the resources to which they are exposed. This is an instance where an individual transformed significant religious principles and stories into guidelines for behaviors that others find deeply troubling. Her unshakable schema not to abandon Tina appeared to be based on her interpretation of her own mother's abandonment. Caron had a difficult time stepping away from her own inner representations and seeing Tina as different. Her schema about abandonment was so strong, even in her teen years, that she was unable to reconcile with her mother. Her mother made many efforts to soften the impact of her abandonment on Caron, efforts that Caron repelled. For example, Caron's mother left a note for Caron

the night she ran off with another man. Caron could not remember the content. When she returned late at night, she woke Caron up. Caron does not remember her response. Her mother made special efforts to reconnect with Caron, such as taking her alone on a camping trip. Caron remained unresponsive. What Caron did connect with was her mother's acts that Caron interpreted as personalized rejection. Caron clearly remembered her mother phoning her home collect while with the other man. Caron wanted to speak to her mother then but her mother hung up when the phone operator told her that the party she wished to speak to (her husband) was not there. Caron was unresponsive to her mothers' overtures for reconnection and forgiveness. Although Elijah's assaults of women clearly fall within conventional definitions and understandings of violence, some people may have trouble labeling Caron's sexual involvement with Tina as violence. We contend that conventional definitions of violence are gendered— that is, biased toward stereotypically male behaviors. The definition of violence in this chapter, however, clearly labels Caron's behaviors as violent. There are generational and maturational differences between Caron and Tina. Caron knew Tina was dealing with many personal issues. A mature response would have been to recognize Tina's vulnerability and gently refuse her sexual advances. Caron's responses to Tina appear to be almost the opposite of relational aggression, which Crick and colleagues (Crick & Dodge, 1994, 1996; Crick e t a l , 2002; Crick etal., 1999) find to be characteristic ways that girls show aggression. In her own terms, Caron sacrificed a great deal to ensure that Tina experienced inclusion, acceptance, and friendship. On the other hand, she perpetrated relationship aggression on her own mother, to whom she gave the silent treatment and whom she excluded from her life. Because of Caron's confused schémas about abandonment, hurt, and commitment and her schémas about conventional and nonconventional moral action regarding how to care for and protect others, she was unable to use ideas about generational differences and their inherent power differentials in her decision making and

Gendered Adaptations, Resilience, and the Perpetration of Violence · 67 subsequent interpretations of her sexual behaviors with Tina. Caron said she sought counseling about her relationship with Tina before it became sexual. She discussed the counseling in terms of lesbianism. She made no mention about whether the counselor also brought up issues related to generational differences, power, and the vulnerability of troubled teenagers. Caron's strong care orientation is obvious in her account. In her terms, a refusal to hurt Tina is a moral stance of caring, a stance that Gilligan (Gilligan, 1982) links to women's ways of being moral people. Caron saw the sexual relationship with Tina as part of a "whole relationship" of caring and commitment. Her caring schema was so strong that when Tina began expressing doubts about their relationship, Caron urged Tina to go into therapy or talk to anyone she wanted about it. She was willing to risk public exposure if this is what Tina wanted. At the time of the interviews, she seemed willing and almost proud of being able to bear the consequences of expressing her love for Tina in sexual ways. In summary, Caron was an otherwise conventional, middle-class college graduate, who had a 10-year marriage preceded by a 5-year relationship to the same man, had strong ties to her church, and was very successful in the arts community. In these areas, she was highly adapted to a secure, positive environment, whereas Elijah was highly adapted to his financially insecure, unpredictable, and sometimes dangerous environment. In the one area that others would consider maladapted—her sexual relationship with Tina—Caron herself saw in heroic terms and even mythic terms that made enormous sense to her. She implicitly linked the price she paid to the martyrdom of Jesus. She certainly would have labeled her behavior as prosocial and highly competent.

DISCUSSION The ideas of gendered adaptations, schémas, resilience, and our definition of violence were helpful in our analysis. We were able to see for ourselves that behaviors that outsiders might define as maladaptive, individuals themselves

can define as adaptive. Both Caron and Elijah would define themselves as competent and even possibly resilient. We also saw that Caron could also fit outsiders' definitions of competent, resilient, and prosocial in her behaviors in several aspects of her functioning. The notion of schémas contributed to our analysis. Caron had some apparently unshakable and at least partially conscious schémas about abandonment, commitment, care, and self-sacrifice. These schémas can be linked both to conventional female socialization and to Christian moral values. Although she was aware of conventional morality in regard to her behaviors toward Tina, she thought them irrelevant to her situation. Elijah, on the other hand, did have strong values and schémas about not hurting women, but when his pride was hurt, other mechanisms took over—his schémas about what to do when disrespected. Elijah represents a lot of other men in terms of his gendered adaptations. We do not know how typical Caron is because we know little about women's ways of being violent and aggressive. Our definition of violence, as far as we know, is novel. Although we incorporated many wellaccepted ideas about power, gender, and age, we also linked violence to definitions of healthy relationships that are widely accepted as having qualities of mutuality, reciprocity, and promoting the best interests of others, all elements associated with resilient individuals. We also found that understandings and definitions of violence are gendered; that is, both denotatively in definitions and connotatively in meanings, the term violence evokes images of physical and verbal aggression, qualities linked to male gender roles much more than female ones. We would like to expand definitions of violence to include "female" styles of perpetrating violence, although we certainly recognize that men are much more likely to commit lethal forms of violence than women, another outcome of gendered cultural themes and practices. We hope that our definition will provoke creative discussions, new understandings, and more effective responses to violence. We have found it challenging to drop our own schémas and link to the schémas of others, especially when their behaviors contravene our

68 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES moral values. We believe, however, that researchers, practitioners, and policymakers must make these connections to others. If we don't, our responses will not be effective. Starting where clients are includes connecting to what they value. The challenge that we hope to make is to ask how social workers and other mental health care professionals can work effectively with people whose schémas and behaviors are adaptive in their own contexts but maladaptive in the larger society. How can we integrate this knowledge into our practice interventions and strategies for populations known to be vulnerable to maladaptive behaviors? Finally, these cases illustrate how adaptation and resilience have gendered dimensions. This finding is particularly relevant for researchers who tend to conceptualize resilience as sets of universal, prosocial adaptations. Future research is needed, both qualitative and quantitative studies, to understand resilience processes for men and women, people of varying age groups, differing socioeconomic statuses, and social contexts.

NOTE 1. This theory has been shown to have several limitations for women of color and women of diverse social class backgrounds. For a review essay on this theory and its critiques, see Abrams (2002).

REFERENCES Abrams, L. S. (2002). Rethinking girls "at-risk:" Gender, race, and class intersections and adolescent development. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 6(2), 47-64. Abrams, L. S. (2003). Contextual variations in young women's gender identity negotiations. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27(1), 64-74. Abrams, L. S., Kim, K., & Anderson, B. (in press). Paradoxes of treatment in juvenile corrections. Child and Youth Care Forum. Anderson, E. (1990). Streetwise: Race, class and change in an urban community. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Ashford, J. B., LeCroy, C. W., & Lortie, K. L. (2001). Human behavior in the social environment (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Bargh, J., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54(1), 462-479. Benner, P. (1994). Interpretive phenomenology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Berk, L. E. (2003). Child development (6th ed). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Brown, L. M., & Gilligan, C. (1992). Meeting at the crossroads. New York: Ballantine Books. Burton, L. M., Allison, K. W., & Obeidallah, D. (1995). Social context and adolescence: Perspectives on development among inner-city African-American teens. In L. J. Crockett & A. C. Crouter (Eds.), Pathways through adolescence: Relation to social contexts (pp. 119-138). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Chambers, E. (2000). Applied ethnography. In Ν. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (2nd ed., pp. 851-869). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Charmaz, K. (2000). Grounded theory: Objectivist and constructivist methods. In Ν. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (2nd ed., pp. 509-535). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Clarke, A. E. (2003). Situational analyses: Ground theory mapping after the postmodern turn. Symbolic Interaction, 26(4), 553-576. Connell, R. (1987). Gender & power. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Connell, R. (1995). Masculinities. London: Polity Press. Cressey, D. (1953). Other people's money. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 74-101. Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67, 993-1002. Crick N. R., Grotpeter, J. K., & Bigbee, M. A. (2002). Relationally and physically aggressive children's intent attributions and feelings of distress for relational and instrumental peer provocations. Child Development, 73(4), 1134-1142. Crick, N. R„ Werner, N. E., Casas, J. E, O'Brien, Κ. M., Nelson, D. Α., Grotpeter, J. K., & Markon, K. (1999). Childhood aggression and gender: A new look at an old problem. In D. Bernstein

Gendered Adaptations, Resilience, and the Perpetration of Violence · 69 (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 45, pp. 75-141). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Denzin, Ν. K. (1989a). Interpretive biography. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Denzin, Ν. K. (1989b). Interpretive interactionism. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Garcia Coll, C , Lamberty, G., Jenkins, R., McAdoo, H. P, Crnic, K., Wasik, Β. H., & Vazquez Garcia, H. (1996). An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child Development, 67, 1891-1914. Germaine, C. B., & Gitterman, A. (1996). The life model of social work practice: Advances in theory and practice (2nd ed.). New York: Columbia University Press. Giddens, A. (1987). Central problems in social theory: Action, structure and contradiction in social analysis. Berkeley: University of California Press. Gilgun, J. F. (1990). Factors mediating the effects of childhood maltreatment. In M. Hunter (Ed.), 77;e sexually abused male: Prevalence, impact, and treatment (pp. 177-190). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Gilgun, J. F. (1991). Resilience and the intergenerational transmission of child sexual abuse. In M. Q. Patton (Ed.), Family sexual abuse: Frontline research and evaluation (pp. 93-105). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Gilgun, J. F. (1992). Hypothesis generation in social work research. Journal of Social Service Research, 15, 113-135. Gilgun, J. F. (1994). Avengers, conquerors, playmates, and lovers: A continuum of roles played by perpetrators of child sexual abuse. Families in Society, 75, 467-480. Gilgun, J. F. (1995). We shared something special: The moral discourse of incest perpetrators. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 265-281. Gilgun, J. F. (1996a). Human development and adversity in ecological perspective. Part 1 : A conceptual framework. Families in Society, 77, 395-402. Gilgun, J. F. (1996b). Human development and adversity in ecological perspective. Part 2: Three patterns. Families in Society, 77, 459-576. Gilgun, J. F. (1999). Mapping resilience as process among adults maltreated in childhood. In H. I. McCubbin, E. A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson, & J. A. Futrell (Eds.), The dynamics of resilient families (pp. 41-70). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Gilgun, J. F. (2002a). Completing the circle: American Indian Medicine Wheels and the promotion of resilience in children and youth in care. Journal of Human Behavior and the Social Environment, 6(2), 65-84. Gilgun, J. F. (2002b). Social work and the assessment of the potential for violence. In T. N. Tiong & I. Dodds (Eds.), Social work around the world (Vol. 3, pp. 58-74). Berne, Switzerland: International Federation of Social Workers. Gilgun, J. F. (in press a). Deductive qualitative analysis and family theory-building. In V. Bengston, P. Dillworth Anderson, K. Allen, A. Acock, & D. Klein (Eds.), Sourcebook of family theory and methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Gilgun, J. F. (in press b). Qualitative research and family psychology. Journal of Family Psychology. Gilgun, J. E, Klein, C , & Pranis, K. (2000). The significance of resources in models of risk. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 627-646. Gilgun, J. E, & McLeod, L. (1999). Gendering violence. Studies in Symbolic Interactionism, 22, 167-193. Gilgun, J. E, & Reiser, E. (1990). Sexual identity development among men sexually abused in childhood. Families in Society, 71, 515-523. Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women's development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. Glaser, B. (1978). Theoretical sensitivity. Mill Valley, CA: Sociology Press. Glaser, B., & Strauss. A. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory. Chicago: Aldine. Goldstein, H. (1994). Ethnography, critical inquiry, and social work practice. In E. Sherman & W. J. Reid (Eds.), Qualitative research in social work (pp. 42-51). New York: Columbia University. Goodey, J. (1997). Boys don't cry: Masculinities, fear of crime, and fearlessness. British Journal of Criminology, 37(3), 401-418. Gottman, J. D. (2001). The relationship cure. New York: Crown. Kersten, J. (1990). A gender specific look at patterns of violence in juvenile institutions: Or are girls really "more difficult to handle"? International Journal of the Sociology of Law, 18, 473-493. LoPresto, C , & Deluty, R. (1987). Consistency of aggressive, assertive, and submissive behavior in male adolescents. The Journal of Social Psychology, 128(5), 619-632.

70 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES Luthar, S. (2003). Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the context of childhood adversities. New York: Cambridge University Press. Luthar, S., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). The construct of resilience: Implications for interventions and social policies. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 857-885. Majors, R., & Billson, J. M. (1992). Cool pose. New York: Lexington Books. Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). The development of competence in favorable and unfavorable environments: Lessons from research on successful children. American Psychologist, 53, 205-220. Messerschmidt, J. (2000). Nine lives: Adolescent masculinities, the body, and violence. Boulder, CO: Westview. Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research and evaluation methods (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Seifert, K. L., Hoffnung, R. J., & Hoffnung, M. (2000). Life-span development. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Siegelman, C. K., & Rider, E. A. (2003). Life-span human development (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson. Strauss, Α., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Tierney, W. G. (2000). Undaunted courage: Life history and the postmodern challenge. In Ν. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 537-553). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Ungar, M. (2004). Nurturing hidden resilience in troubled youth. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. West, C , & Zimmerman, D. (1987). Doing gender. Gender and Society, 1(2), 125-151. Znaniecki, F. (1934). The method of sociology. New York: Farrar & Rinehart.

5 THE THEORY OF RESILIENCE AND ITS APPLICATION TO STREET CHILDREN IN THE MINORITY AND MAJORITY WORLDS JACQUELINE MCADAM-CRISP LEWIS APTEKAR WANJIKU KIRONYO

I

n the 1950s, children who had run away from home to the streets were classified in the DSM-III-R as having a mental disorder, a perception that focuses on a child's deficits as opposed to strengths (Demoskoff & Lauzer, 1994). Although this classification is no longer used, the idea is still prevalent that the behavior of a child that results in his or her presence on the street must be maladaptive. The media, police, courts, social workers, and the public perpetuate this sensationalist image of deviance globally (Aptekar, 2000; Le Roux, 1998; Scheper-Hughes & Hoffman, 1998). However, those of us who work with these children have a different story to tell. Since the early 1980s, a number of studies have examined the lives of street children from a strengths perspective

(Aptekar, 1994; Ennew, 1994; McAdam, 1995; Norman, 2000; Panter-Brick, 2002). There has been a growing interest in the situation of street children in both the minority and majority worlds. Street children throughout the world are now readily identifiable as a group "at risk." Yet in the face of adversity, many display a great deal of resilience. In this chapter, we examine the situation of street children in the minority and majority worlds within the framework of resilience theory. Each of us has worked with street children in a variety of cultural contexts, and through these experiences, we have become intrigued and puzzled with the capacity of some of these children to overcome adversity. We have been equally curious to understand the lack of capacity among others who, in comparison, 1

71

72 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES seem to have experienced fewer risks but who appear to be more vulnerable to the risks they encounter. Furthermore, we are interested in knowing the etic or emic nature of resilience theory and practice across cultures. As Poortinga (1997) states, Behaviour is emic, or cultural specific, to the extent it can only be understood within the cultural context within which it occurs; it is etic, or universal, in as much as it is common to human beings independent of culture, (p. 348)

explore the relationship between risk and resilience in this population. Our goal, then, is to better understand street children globally through the application of the theory of resilience to their lives. Although we advocate that each child needs to be seen in context, we hope to provide a broad enough framework for understanding resilience for those defined as street children across a variety of diverse social and cultural contexts.

THE CHILDHOOD OF STREET CHILDREN For example, although street children in both minority and majority world contexts are considered to be at risk, those in the majority world seem more often to possess a sense of hope, gratitude, and resilience. Although lacking in material wealth, these children possess a wealth of strength that seems foreign to street youth in the minority world. As Aptekar (1988) notes, "These children are not the emotionally injured 'runaways' whom I had work with in North America" (p. xiii). Typically, the term street children conjures up images of deviant maladjusted children who suffer from a range of psychological disorders. Visible on the city streets throughout the world, street children are far from a homogeneous group. Consequently, the adversities that street children experience and the way they sustain their well-being vary globally. To begin, then, the term street children is examined in relation to the concept of childhood, including the expectations of how children should behave and the levels of risk we assume they are competent to handle. Next, we examine the various ways street children are defined and where they are found globally. We then explore the concept of resilience as a theoretical framework for examining street children's lives. Specifically, street children who are confronted by war or high rates of AIDS in their communities (and possibly their own infection) and girls who live on the street are examined in more detail because of the unique circumstance of these children. Factors proposed to enhance or limit resilience will be highlighted and program-based solutions introduced. Research on street children in both the minority and majority worlds will be drawn on along with the authors' collective experience to

The UN Convention on the Rights of the Child (CRC) provides a benchmark from which the situation of street children can be measured. However, there have been many reservations about a unified set of principles and its application globally because of the diverse ways childhood is understood across cultures (Aptekar & McAdam-Crisp, in press; Scheper-Hughes & Sargent, 1998). Childhood as a developmental stage is obvious by a child's physical size. Yet there is a great deal of variety regarding how children are raised and what constitutes "good" child-rearing practices (Whiting, 1963). Olson's (1981) work with Turkish people, as discussed by Ratner (1999), provides an insightful example of how our values shape what we expect of children and how children are treated. [Olson] traces the low incidence of child abuse to the prevalent belief that life is unpredictable and subject to the vagaries of natural and supernatural forces, which transcend human will. Since humans have neither the power nor responsibility to control, they do not seek to control their children. Nor do they set expectations for children's physical and emotional capabilities. Caretakers accept and indulge children's behaviors. As a result, most misbehaviors of children are not punished but tolerated as childish naughtiness. Thus, parents' benevolent treatment of their children is mediated by beliefs about the causes of events, the power and responsibilities of people, and the capacity of children, (p. 8) Definitions of adolescence as a defined stage of one's childhood are also controversial across cultures. For example, the Gurage shoeshine

Theory of Resilience and Its Application to Street Children · 73 boys seen working in the streets of Addis Ababa, Ethiopia, are an accepted norm for adolescence and not thought to be a sign of a child at risk (Veale, Adefrisew, & Lalor, 1992). Consequently, removing these children from the street would threaten their acceptance within their cultural grouping. Likewise, in East and Central Africa, it is common for rural girls to be married by the age of 14. Of course, we do not mean to imply that there are not some inherent risks with both these examples of adolescents moving quickly into adultlike roles. Instead, we wish to point out that because the concept of childhood and adolescence is not universally defined, it is equally difficult to define risk and resilience across cultures. As Black (1993) notes, "The idea of a protracted period of time in the early part of life in which youngsters enjoy special protection, nurture and dependency is relatively recent historically and largely confined to the industrialized societies" (p. 15). The concept of childhood and the value society places on this stage of development are extremely diverse; thus, so are the various ways children cope with this stage of development.

STREET CHILDREN: A DEFINITION The term street children has been criticized as demeaning of children's experiences (Aptekar, 1988; Dallape, 1988; Hutz & Roller, 1999). Dallape (1996) notes that the term is "offensive and gives a distorted message" (p. 283). The term has been used to refer to children in a variety of circumstances, creating confusion about who these children are and what kinds of risk brought them to the streets. For years, UNICEF has referred to street children as "on" and " o f the street. Children "on" the street are those who have continual contact with their families, whereas children " o f the street have occasional contact with their families. A third category, which is the smallest, refers to children who have no contact with their families because of the death of their parent or parents, abandonment by their families, or family conflicts that necessitated the child's leaving home (Barker & Rnaul, 1991; Blanc, 1994; McAdam, 1995). The first two categories account for approximately 90% of all street children, who are commonly

referred to as "working street children." Many of these children combine work and school (Lusk, 1992). The latter group is a more "pure" definition of street children, accounting for those who are potentially at the greatest risk because of the extensive amount of time they spend on the street (Aptekar, 2000; ScheperHughes & Hoffman, 1998). No matter how these street children are categorized, they are seen in streets throughout the world, selling candies, newspapers, magazines, and tissues, as well as begging and stealing. Street children do not form a homogeneous group, nor do their life circumstances remain constant. Their involvement on the street varies, as does their family contact. Children may be represented in one or more categories at different times of their lives and careers in the streets. For this reason, other researchers have used their own definitions to name these youth (see Aptekar, 1988; Cosgrove, 1990; Lusk, 1992). Definitions in general can help to establish a context, but they do not fully provide a qualitative understanding of the relationship between the risk and resilience factors operating in these children's lives. They neglect to speak to the amount of time a child spends on the street, a child's source of livelihood or income, and what constitutes a responsible caregiver (PanterBrick, 2002). Furthermore, a child's developmental stage and gender is not considered. As Kironyo notes, she has seen children in Nairobi, Renya, as young as 2 years old on the street. The mother will sit at a corner of a shop and instruct the children who to beg from. Restrictive definitions also neglect the protective factors that can influence a child's presence on the street and decrease the cumulative effects of risk. For example, children are far more likely to stay in school if their parents value their children's education. Parents who are illiterate often do not understand the value of school. Therefore, they discourage study time because it cuts into the time children could be working on the streets and contributing to the family income. Conventional definitions of street children also do not acknowledge a child's own interpretation of risk. In both the minority and majority world, children have a reason for leaving home and being in the streets. Although reasons may differ across cultures, it needs to be acknowledged

74 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES that becoming a child of the streets is a form of coping. For example, McAdam-Crisp knew a 12-year-old girl in Addis Ababa who was living with her mother's friend because her mother had died. Prostitution was a common form of income generation in this home. The girl left, and because she had nowhere to go, she ended up in the streets. It was difficult for this girl to get services because, technically, she had a home. Leaving such an unhealthy environment would better be considered a strength and warrant services. We may wonder how often a child's sense of personal well-being is ignored as a result of well-meaning adults determining what is appropriate for the child, with decisions often dictated by strict funding criteria that identify the types of interventions available to service at-risk children. The complex realities of street life demand that children are understood as individuals within a social and cultural context that often perpetuates their existence in the streets. As in other chapters in this volume, most specifically Boyden and Mann's (Chapter 1), we, too, find it important to acknowledge children's own ways of sustaining resilience even when their choices are misunderstood by their caregivers.

STREET CHILDREN IN THE GLOBAL CONTEXT Estimates of the number of street children vary from 30 to 170 million (Barker & Knaul, 1991). The number is expected to increase to 800 million by the year 2020 (Save the Children Canada, 2004, March 1). Of these children, 90% live in the majority world, where 50% of the population is under the age of 15. These numbers are not surprising given that poverty is one of the main factors that accounts for the number of street children in the majority world, where children on the street range in age from 5 to 18 years (Aptekar, 1988; Aptekar & Ciano, 1999; Aptekar, Maphalala, Dlamini, Makhanya, 6 Magagula, 1998; Ennew, 1994; Le Roux, 1996; Muchini & Nyandiya-Bundy, 1991; Veale, 1996; Veale et al., 1992). Given the complexity of the problem, an accurate count of street children is hard to determine because of their transient nature, the lack of resources to

count these children, and the lack of agreement as to what defines a street child. Furthermore, estimates of the number of street children do not include those children under the age of 5 who are on the streets, most often with their mothers. In the majority world, there are a greater number of male street children: 90% in many African countries, approximately 80% in Jamaica and other Caribbean countries, and 75% worldwide (Aptekar, 1994, 2000; Aptekar & McAdam-Crisp, in press). There are various reasons for these gender differences. Veale (1996) observed that there were no street girls in Khartoum, Sudan, because of a strong Islamic presence that makes it inappropriate for girls to work in the street. The large proportion of boys on the street in the majority world is further explained by changing family structures, along with other factors that have resulted in a great number of families headed by mothers alone. Aptekar and Ciano (1999) note that boys are socialized by their mothers to cope with the necessity of surviving in a very limited economic environment by becoming independent at a far earlier age than Western culture deems appropriate. Consequently, many boys are expected to contribute to the incomes of their families. Paradoxically, mothers teach girls how to cope with the vagaries of poverty by staying at home and learning household tasks. Children are on the streets in the majority world for many reasons, including structural adjustment programs, rural to urban migration, changing family structures, AIDS, and civil and regional conflicts, along with problems in the immediate family, all of which contribute to the number of street children (Aptekar, 2000; Barker & Knaul, 1991; Ennew, 1994; McAdam, 1995; Scheper-Hughes & Sargent, 1998). As Kilbride, Suda, and Njeru (2000) note, "The phenomenal growth of the international economy with the demands of the global economy for competitive prices has served to pressure local markets for cheap labor, often including children as laborers" (p. 3). This, coupled with urban migration, means that many children who formerly worked in rural settings now work in the urban centers, making their work more visible. By comparison, children in the minority world are often forced out of their homes because of a dysfunctional family environment

Theory of Resilience and Its Application to Street Children · 75 that can include high levels of conflict; physical, emotional, or sexual abuse; and/or substance abuse (Ayerst, 1999). Although poverty may play a part in these children coming to the street, this is not always the case (Bagley, 1985; Webber, 1991). The reasons children end up in the streets are diverse, and as Webber (1991) points out, "all the partially true explanations ignore the economic, social, and moral foundation on which faltering families, failing schools, and inept social and correctional services totter" (p. 35). For these reasons, a discussion of the resilience of street children needs to incorporate the micro- and macrolevel issues that affect the social, cultural, and political circumstances of a specific environment.

STREET CHILDREN WITHIN THE FRAMEWORK OF RESILIENCE Approaches to examine resilience have relied primarily on a systemic approach for deducing risk in relation to protective factors, an approach that assumes a predictable relationship between risk and protective factors (Ungar, 2004). As Werner and Smith (2001) note in their work, "As the number of risks factors or stressful life events increase, more protective factors were needed to counterbalance the negative aspects in the lives of those vulnerable children and to ensure a positive developmental outcome" (p. 58). This denotes a potential quantifiable relationship between the risks children face and the protective factors present in their lives. In essence, if A, B, and C are present in the face of adversity, a positive outcome will result. The inverse relationship is also true. Although this deductive approach is invaluable, there are still many questions regarding the qualitative interactions of these different factors within various cultural contexts. Kirby and Fraser (1997) express the complex nature of identifying this process: No single event produces a negative outcome. Rather, interactional processes shape behaviours and problems over time. The separation of risk process from risk trait remains a major challenge for practitioners and researchers who work with troubled children and who seek to design more effective social programs, (p. 13)

Examining the situation of street children globally, it is difficult to unilaterally define what might be considered protective factors for children who experience diverse sociocultural factors. In addition, there are instances where resilience occurs despite very few protective factors (Higgins, 1994). Of course, children are not invincible to continual risks that result in life-threatening or traumatic experiences or both. However, the ability to deal with adverse situations is a dynamic process that varies throughout a person's life and in relation to the various situations that he or she encounters. Consequently, a child who is resilient in one situation may not be in another. Our approach assumes that street children have the ability to actively construct their own realities that are a reflection of their cultural values. However, we do not postulate that the culture with which a street child identifies is the same hegemonic culture that excludes them. Consider the story of a girl we will call Laura, with whom McAdam-Crisp worked in the mid1980s in Victoria, British Columbia, Canada.

Laura's Story Laura was a "high-risk youth" in the minority world. As a teenager, she moved between the street and various foster homes; she got pregnant and gave birth just after her 14th birthday. This was the first of four children who were either apprehended at birth or taken into care later. I began working with Laura when she was 12 until shortly after she decided to put her first child up for adoption (who had been apprehended at birth). She was just about 15 years old at the time. While she was pregnant, in an attempt to "rehabilitate" and improve the prospects for herself and her baby, I read her stories of teens who had become pregnant. This was my attempt to create an educational forum and opening for discussion between us. In response to one of these stories, Laura told me she had no idea who the father was and commented that she remembered the first time she had sex and her last partner, but all the ones in between were a blur. Laura defied the social standards of what was expected of a child of this age. Whatever Laura's abilities or more commonly discussed disabilities were, they have facilitated her coping over the years.

76 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES Now, fifteen years later, I continue to see Laura on the streets. She frequents the street kitchens and shelters and hangs out with the street people. Who am I to judge her experience as lacking in resilience? Unlike many of the other girls I worked with during my career, she is not dead. If this is used as an indicator of resilience, then she succeeded. For the most part, Laura just wanted to be able to live her life and resented being hassled by professionals.

Although a primarily social constructionist approach is taken here, a more deductive approach is not necessarily dismissed by the way we have come to understand Laura's life and the lives of other street youth. A deductive approach is drawn on, not to define hierarchical constructs of interaction, but to propose one kind of explanation for the interactions that affect children and contribute to their becoming attached to street-based lives. For example, Bowlby's (1969) work on attachment indicates that children deprived of maternal attachment are more likely to develop maladaptive behaviors in the future. Although Bowlby's theory places a great deal of emphasis on the early years, different child care practices that exist in the majority world may consequently affect a child's resilience. For example, mothers who beg in the street have their babies strapped to their backs, which may act as a psychological buffer against other risks encountered on the street. This is also observed by Werner and Smith (2001) who showed that in a group of children deemed to be high risk, those who had a consistent attachment to a primary caregiver did better in life. Furthermore, in the majority world, large, extended families provide nurturance and support for a child even if that family is homeless. We acknowledge the presence of quantifiable indicators, such as mother-child attachment, that have been well studied in minority-world contexts that could act as protective factors, but we also acknowledge that these may not be quantifiable across cultures or between individuals. Nevertheless, the development of some guiding principles that recognize the potential intersection of quantifiable indicators and the

quality of the experience for the child may help in understanding better the plight of street children and lead to the development of appropriate policies and practices.

STREET CHILDREN AND THE RISK PROCESS Interactions of psychosocial factors within the context of culture affect the experience of street children in relation to risk and resilience. An overview of the potential risk to street children in both the minority and majority world will be discussed briefly. We then examine specific areas that pose potentially greater risk to street children. These include exposure to war, AIDS, and the special issues that affect girls. To illustrate the confluence of factors that pose multiple risks to street children, we will use an illustration that comes from Kironyo's experience in Nairobi with a boy named Moses. Moses' story details the evolution of what we term a risk process for a street child and the difficulties involved in providing support to the child.

Moses' Story A few years ago, after moving to a new neighborhood, I was approached by a small boy who asked if he could wash my car. Looking at Moses, he seemed far too small to adequately complete such a task. I agreed and watched as he quickly took a stool, stood in front of the car, and started washing. He, in fact, did a good job. Upon completion, he asked for a fee that was one quarter of what would normally be paid for such a task. Out of concern, I asked where he was from and discovered he was from the Kibera slums where he lived with his mother. He said that his father has sent him, his mother, his eight brothers, and one sister away from home after marrying a younger wife. The children moved in with their grandmother, but the father's brothers (the children's uncles) were threatened by the fact that this might result in the boy's mother and her children inheriting the grandparent's land. The family then moved to the Kibera slums, and the mother started roasting maize for income. Because the mother was unable to feed

Theory of Resilience and Its Application to Street Children · 77 her children on her meager earnings, each was instructed to find a way to support the family. Moses was making his contribution. I suggested that he might go to school and spoke to his mother about this option. A school placement was established. I paid the school fees for the first term, but for the second term, I gave his mother the money. This was done to allow the mother to take on the responsibility of having her child go to school. She decided that there were other more pressing priorities than the school fees. Because the school fees were not paid, Moses was sent away by the school and started doing odd jobs for my neighbors. I followed up with Moses when I saw him on the street, and he told me the money for the school fee was used to buy food and pay the house rent. He did not wish to go to school while there was no food to eat. Later, Moses joined a group of street boys in central Nairobi whereupon his mother attempted to get him arrested and put in the Kabete remand home. While parking my vehicle in the city center, I saw a boy helping me to park and realized that it was Moses. I told him to come see me and we would discuss the issue of schooling. I found another school for him, but he said it was too far to walk. Moses returned to the streets where he sniffed glue and petrol, a pastime often engaged in to numb the physical pain of hunger. Two years later, I met him and told him that he was welcome to visit my home. He took me up on my offer and visited regularly. To help, I found a job for him; he had learned a number of skills while in the street. Moses could construct a bicycle from scraps of metal, a chair from various scraps of wood, and make a bed out of almost anything. He was extremely creative. Through this process, he revealed to me that being in the streets was not worth the trouble because the police constantly harassed him. It is often easier for older youth to leave the street and enter the informal job market. Skills learned while on the street are useful because the informal economy is street based.

Globally, street children like Moses are exposed to a number of dangers such as beatings, rape, and sexually transmitted diseases like AIDS, along with other health problems. Those children who remain in the street are more likely to

become involved in criminal activity, prostitution, and drug trafficking. However, this varies between urban settings and the way urban space is negotiated. For instance, the larger the urban setting and the greater the disparity between the rich and poor, coupled with a lack of social supports, the more risks there are to street children. A lack of social support to protect and provide for children coupled with extreme poverty in the majority world means that children are more visible in urban spaces. These children are more likely to be abducted, recruited as child soldiers, or become part of the human traffic circuit. Often, these children are maimed, because they will consequently make more money begging. In the majority world, children can remain in the streets for many years. However, in the minority world, this is not necessarily the case. Street children in the minority world often move between various care facilities, the street, and youth detention. Often, they return to the street in reaction to the breakdown of a care arrangement, and the cycle repeats. Boys typically partake in petty crime and the selling of drugs to support themselves, often resulting in their arrest. Girls, on the other hand, are more prone to prostitution and are therefore less visible on the streets or in the justice system. In the minority world, street children are more likely to suffer from high levels of depression, suicidal tendencies, and alcohol and drug problems (Smollar, 1999). This street cycle in both the minority and majority worlds is complicated by the concept of a risk chain, which results when a child's psychological and physical development creates vulnerabilities that in a given setting may increase the child's exposure to risk. For example, Erikson (1963) affirms a youth's need for belonging. Maslow (1954) and others have defined the need to belong as a significant process in our growth. This need provides a sense of support or enhanced community, identified in the literature as a protective factor. When a child feels disenfranchised or unconnected, he or she may gravitate toward the street to gain a sense of belonging and community through contact with informal street families. The experience on the street compels youth to rely on their street friends for support, which in turn strengthens the bonds between these children, creating a

78 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES circular effect. This can result in children becoming more entrenched in street life in their efforts to survive and thrive. That said, the concept of a risk chain might work differently in the majority world where working street children are less likely to be involved in illegal activity, drugs, or alcohol (Aptekar, 1988; Scheper-Hughes & Hoffman, 1998). In fact, these children are part of a growing urban economy that supports many of the families of the urban poor. Removing these children from the street often has negative consequences, because lack of income from the street means less money for food, medical costs, and school fees for those children who remain at home—many of whom are girls (Barker & Knaul, 1991; Boyden & Mann, 2000; McAdam, 1995). The supposed risk chain in the majority world may have a much more positive outcome because it reinforces a child's evolving self as a supportive member of the community and family. Although those classified as working street children can become involved in deviant behavior, those who meet the purer definition of street children are more likely to be involved in deviant activities as a way of coping. As Scheper-Hughes and Hoffman (1998) note, Although they represent the smallest number of those who are labelled "street kids," these truly homeless children are quite visible; and because of what they have to do to defend themselves on the street, they fuel the negative stereotypes of the "dangerous" and "uncontrollable" street kids, (p. 361) Research shows that the primary risks to street children do not come from living on the streets but from police action. In many places in the world, street children have been killed for no more than petty crimes (Aptekar, 2000; Aptekar & McAdam-Crisp, in press; Black, 1993; Ennew, 1994; Scheper-Hughes & Hoffman, 1998; Tierney, 1997). This is illustrated by the case of Simon.

The Case of Simon Aptekar and Ciano (1999) have written about Simon, a Kenyan child, 15 years of age, who was

murdered by a police reservist. He was shot five times at point-blank range, kicked into the gutter, and then spat on. He had stolen a signal lens from a parked car, nothing more. What about this boy aroused such anger? It appears that the reservist construed a scenario about street children that did not include loving parents or good character. Simon, like street children in nearly all cultures in the world, was treated with such fury because he received the moral judgment given to those who violate the norms that cultures give to acceptable behavior for children. Street children do this by not living under the same roof as their parents, by working instead of going to school, and by assuming the right to enjoy the fruits of their work as they chose (such as consuming alcohol or drugs). Simon was a street child with loving parents, who attended his funeral. They said he was a good boy who contributed what he earned from working on the streets to his family. He often stayed at home, but even when he didn't, he kept in touch with his family. The problem was that from the perspective of the police he was viewed as a delinquent and therefore was killed. We would suggest that Simon's behavior said far more about the resilience of street children, although theories of resilience have largely overlooked the way such behavior contributes to health for the child and his or her family.

STREET CHILDREN, WAR, AND A I D S Street children are most typically referred to as those who have suffered from prolonged deprivation or family violence at home and/or are poor and enter the streets to earn money or to escape abusive situations. This group forms the smallest cohort of street children, about 10% globally. However, the increasing number of civil and regional conflicts and the rampant spread of AIDS have changed the face of the world's street children. For example, in Rwanda, the number of children on the street increased significantly following the genocide in 1994 (Ministry of Labour and Social Affairs, 1997). Veale and Dona (2003) report that of the street children they surveyed, 87% came to the street following the genocide, and approximately 42%

Theory of Resilience and Its Application to Street Children · 79 of these children were living without family. This is significantly higher than the number of children who meet the stereotypical definition of a street child. Rwandan street children, like other children of war, do not have a history of living on the street. They are there because of displacement and the death of or separation from their parents (McAdam, 1997). Similar findings were noted in a comparative study in Ethiopia, in which 20% of all children had been orphaned in the Tigray region, an area that has been affected by the civil war between Eritrea and Ethiopia over the last 30 years, compared with only 4% to 8% of children being orphans in other regions (Veale et al., 1992). Aptekar's research with those displaced from Eritrea in 1991 further illustrates this point. When Eritrea gained independence in 1991, all those who were deemed to be Ethiopian were forced to leave. Some were taken in trucks, and others left on foot for Ethiopia, a journey that required them to pass through the Danakil Depression, one of the hottest places on earth and where there is little vegetation or water. Whether they went by truck or by foot, many people witnessed the death of friends and family. Many of the survivors, young children at the time, now live on the streets in Addis Ababa, Ethiopia's capital. Are they victims of war, street children, or both? Herein lies the problem of defining these youth as street children based solely on the observation that they now live on the street. These examples illustrate that it is far more important to understand a child's attachment to the street as the result of a multidetermined process in which the outcome—street life—is an adaptation to a number of different risks and may, by necessity, be the only healthy option for the child to overcome adversity.

may result, such as increased aggression and other symptoms such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (for a more detailed discussion of PTSD see Solomon and Laufer's work on Israeli children in Chapter 14). Given this risk process in which one risk piles up on another, it seems reasonable that AIDS has changed the life circumstances for many of the world's street children. For instance in 1989, ChildHope documented that Brazil had the third largest number of AIDS cases worldwide after the United States and France and that between 2% and 10% of street youth were HIV positive in San Paulo and Rio de Janeiro, Brazil; Khartoum, Sudan; and New York (Barker, 1989). Today, not only has the number of street children who are HIV positive risen, the number of children in the street who have been orphaned because of AIDS-related deaths of their parents has also risen. UNICEF (2003) reports, "Although HIV/AIDS has reached almost every part of the world, no other region has been harder hit than sub-Sahara Africa, home to nearly three quarters of the world's people living with HIV/AIDS" (p. 7). This includes approximately 11 million children under the age of 15 who have been orphaned by AIDS. Traditionally, many orphans are taken in by their extended family (UNICEF, 2003; Velis, 1995). However, the AIDS epidemic has resulted in too many orphans who cannot all be supported by their extended families. These children are often unaware of what is happening and, thus, are more vulnerable to the inherent risks associated with living on the street. Kironyo translates the words of one such orphaned youth from the Maji Mazuri Centre in Nairobi, Kenya, to illustrate this point.

Despite some positives, however, a life on the street solves only some problems. It does not provide the resources to address the high rates of trauma many of these children have experienced. Many have witnessed the death of their parents, and many have also been physically wounded. Consequently, along with poverty these children may exhibit a range of behaviors such as sleep disturbances, recurring memories, and extreme sadness in relation to the trauma they have experienced. If these qualitative differences are not addressed, more extreme psychological problems

I learned to survive the hard way after my mother died of AIDS before joining the Maji Mazuri Centre. I had experienced a lot of suffering by the age of 17. My mother died when I was very young. I cannot remember my age then. The problems started when she fell ill. My brother and sister went through a lot of pain, loneliness, anger as we watched our mom cough and lose weight. We did not know what she was suffering from; we only knew she was sick. We never knew our dad, nor did we have any other relatives. After mom died, we were thrown out of the shanty house we

2

80 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES rented; we begged for food after being kicked out; the little things we owned were taken away by the landlord due to rent arrears. We used to cry a lot. I could not really comprehend what was happening. We have never been to school, even when mom was alive. We ended up on the streets. Different people in the streets frequently raped us. We did not understand what was happening to us. Other people told us their stories. I had no idea this was the same thing that was happening to me. It was difficult having to beg every day for food, and life had to go on. I had to survive. We got involved in drugs and could not control ourselves. I was preoccupied with what to eat and where to sleep, not school, as it was not a priority.

phenomenon creating a public perception that it is not a problem. The lower numbers of street girls in the public view by day is compensated for by those who have been recruited as prostitutes, most of whom are on the streets at night (Agnelli, 1986; Aptekar, 1994; Tacon, 1981). Anecdotal information in this area demands more attention, as does childhood prostitution into which girls are primarily recruited. Kim's story is just one example of the pathways to the street for girls and the prevalence of prostitution, which they must turn to for survival.

AIDS will undoubtedly change the number of children who are forced to live in the streets to survive. Both the presence of war and AIDS will influence the percentage of abandoned children who are on the street, greatly increasing their numbers over time. This growing population of abandoned children also has meant many more girls on the streets as well as boys. Thus, it has become critical to understand the risks street life poses to this group of abandoned street children without family resources, in particular for girls, who are forming a disproportionately large percentage of such street children without kinship attachments.

1 [McAdam-Crisp] met Kim in a group home in which I worked in the early 1990s in Victoria, B.C. Kim had been placed because of difficulties at home between her mom's boyfriend and herself. Kim was always doing something for the other residents in the home. She was attractive, fun to be around, and had a bright sunny disposition at the age of 15. Because this was only a transitional arrangement, Kim left the home to be placed in a more permanent arrangement. Three months later while working downtown in Vancouver, B.C., I bumped into Kim, walking along the street with a male youth. She was wearing a short black skirt and high-heeled black boots; her peaches-andcream complexion was marred with a large scar down the side of her face. The scar, the clothing and the area were a strong indication that Kim was likely involved or becoming involved in Vancouver's prostitution circuit. I offered to help her return to Victoria and she agreed. A month later, the social worker phoned me to ask if I had heard from Kim; she was AWOL and assumed to be back on the streets in Vancouver. This is only one story of the many young girls with whom I have worked over the years who end up returning to the streets. Frequently, the draw is a boyfriend who affirms a girl's need for love and belonging. However, many of these so-called boyfriends are also pimps.

STREET GIRLS An examination of the profile of street children shows the complex nature of the intersection of various factors related to gender and other risks in minority and majority world contexts. In the minority world, more girls than boys are recorded as runaways, a potential indicator of the number of street-involved youth. In Canada in 2001, 53,434 children were recorded as runaways; of this group, a total of 31,981 were girls—10,000 more girls than boys (Royal Canadian Mounted Police, 2001). Although this information is revealing, it does not necessarily provide an accurate number because many runaways return home, only to run away again and, as such, are recorded twice. Despite this fact, the greater number of street girls in the minority world is most concerning because it is an invisible

Kim's Story

Many of these young girls have been sexually abused prior to these experiences or are soon abused on the street (Bagley, 1985).

Theory of Resilience and Its Application to Street Children · 81 When we look closely at the situation for girls in the majority world, the prospects of abuse are equally daunting. Girls are protected from the street for fear of sexual abuse, and if a girl is on the street, it means she has been compelled to move there against the gendered expectations of her culture. Aptekar and Ciano (1999) note in their study on gender differences among street youth in Kenya that 80% of street girls have been sexually abused. Unlike boys, these girls are not connected to their families of origin, and their time on the street is often compounded by continual sexual abuse. The story of Jocelyn, a street girl in Nairobi, Kenya, is offered as a contrast to Kim's story.

Jocelyn's Story One evening, I (Aptekar), along with my female research partner, stopped to talk to several street boys who were living at the end of a deserted alley near an open sewer just outside the city center. They lived in two shacks they had constructed from rubble. To enter, we had to bend low to pass through the doorway. To our surprise, we found that two of the occupants were female. Jocelyn, who appeared to have just reached puberty, lay covered in rags, a jar of inhalant in her hand. Through glazed eyes, she barely greeted us. We inferred that she didn't need our attention because she was being "protected" by several of the older street boys. The next day, we returned to the site, and thanks to the skill of the female researcher (and promises of financial assistance), we were able to take Jocelyn to a center that serves street girls where she had been treated well before. Presumably, because she was cooperating with us, thus violating the behavioral expectations put on street girls by street boys, upon arrival at the center, none of her peers, some of whom she knew, greeted her. Over the next several days, we were able to get some idea of Jocelyn's background. Jocelyn had been raised by her mother in Mathare, a large slum area on the outskirts of Nairobi. She had three older brothers. When sales of her mother's illegal beer were good, Jocelyn's brothers went to school while Jocelyn stayed at home to help with chores and her mother's business. Because her mother was not always present, she was abused several times by men who came

to buy beer. Her mother, Jocelyn said, "never helped me." Over time, she began to make friends with older boys, one of whom she liked enough to accept his invitation to live with him in the alley. Before long, she was in a similar position as she had been at home. Because many of the boys found her attractive, they paid her male friend for the right to enjoy Jocelyn's "company." With nowhere to turn, Jocelyn began a habit of taking inhalants to cope. We tried to get Jocelyn's mother to help her daughter but could not locate her. We were told that she had been caught by the police and was in prison. We were eventually able to find her, but Jocelyn by that time had returned to the streets. Our forays into the alley to find her were met with increasing hostility; once she threw stones at us. The next time we looked for her, we could not gain entry to her shack and we never saw her again. The path that Jocelyn appeared to be traveling was a common one for street girls in Nairobi.

In both the majority and minority worlds, the circumstances that result in girls leaving home are different than those for boys. The risk of sexual abuse and assault on the street will often result in girls' staying home even when conditions are poor. When a girl chooses to leave home, it is often because problems have intensified. Therefore, how a girl uses street life to bolster resilience to stressors at home may be more complicated than for boys. On the street, the risks girls face may expose them to additional stressors that compound the challenges they faced at home, making it unclear if life on the street is part of a pathway to resilience or a step backward in terms of the degree of vulnerability that girls experience. Whereas in Western countries it is assumed that children are on the streets because of family dysfunction, in the majority world, this is likely to be true for street girls but not street boys.

FACTORS THAT MAY ENHANCE AND LIMIT RESILIENCE FOR STREET CHILDREN From studying the lives of street children, we have learned that it is presumptuous to assume

82 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES that a Western-style definition of risk naturally equates to a traumatic experience. As our work has shown, working street children may actually fair better than their fellow age-mates when confronted with adversity. A lack of understanding of the relationship between risk and resilience in the lives of marginalized children has often resulted in forcing these children to get off the street, an act that further jeopardizes the economic situation of their family and potentially decreases their and their families' resilience (Boyden & Mann, 2000). Taking into account a child's subjective experience within his or her social and cultural context allows us to gain a better understanding of the intersection of risk and resilience factors associated with street life. Furthermore, just because a child is in the street does not mean that he or she is the victim of abuse or neglect, or a delinquent ready for reeducation. This is the case for both boys and girls, although the probability of risk for girls is likely higher. An example, the story of two working street girls in Ethiopia, is illustrative of the risk and resilience factors operating in their lives at one and the same time. Although girls are at an increased risk of sexual abuse and consequently early pregnancies, neither of these girls experienced this problem. These girls were careful to remain together and never started working before 10 in the morning and left the street before dark. Furthermore, as a consequence of their time on the street and the skills they gained, these girls were later able to obtain a job at a restaurant. As Lalor (1999) comments, many girls in Ethiopia have self-imposed curfews to avoid sexual assault, decreasing their potential risks. Female street children abide by the same rules. Viewing street children only in terms of their vulnerabilities does not do justice to the resilience of many who, in the face of adversity, have developed the ability to cope. Street children are social actors who develop a specific "microculture" that comes from balancing what they need to do to survive with the larger society's reactions to their status. Often, they do this quite successfully, much to public surprise. In fact, street children are often viewed as more resilient than their stay-at-home counterparts (Aptekar & Stocklin, 1997). In a study of street

children in Guatemala, the living conditions on the street were often better than those at home (Connelly, 1990). Studies in South Africa have noted that street children ate better and avoided the daily abuse they faced at home (Hickson & Gaydon, 1989; Scharf, Powell, & Thomas, 1986; Swart, 1990). In Brazil, street children had a higher degree of intelligence and were less likely to abuse drugs than their stay-at-home counterparts who lived in poverty (Lusk, 1992; Oliveria, Baizerman, & Pellet, 1992). Leaving home and fending for themselves on the street is seen as an indicator of resilience. In Bogota, Colombia, street children were found to immerse themselves in a network of caring and supportive friendships (Tyler, Holliday, Tyler, Echeverry, & Zea, 1987; Tyler, Tyler, Echeverry, & Zea, 1992). These patterns hold for many street boys and girls in the majority world who have been found to have developed coping strategies that allow them to function at least as well as equally impoverished counterparts who pass less time in public view. These coping strategies include finding a niche in the economic market, which gives them sufficient income to eat and clothe themselves. They are also able to find and take advantage of programs that serve them, being sufficiently informed about their physical health to maintain their well-being, form close friendships with peers, and keep up some form of connection with their families of origin.

STREET CHILDREN: PROGRAM-BASED SOLUTIONS FOR ENHANCING RESILIENCE With increasing awareness regarding the plight of street children, there has been a growing demand for programs that can address the needs of street children in both the minority and majority world. A key objective is to decrease the number of street children and enhance a child's potential for survival and future prospects. Services for street children are predominantly located in urban centers where street children are most often located. As an exemplar of these programs, the Undugu Society of Kenya is a well-known and well-established model for street children. This program, like many others, offers services through small primary centers

Theory of Resilience and Its Application to Street Children · 83 that provide food, recreation, personal hygiene facilities, life skills, education, job training, and in some cases, temporary shelter. Their model provides children with services within their own environment while also facilitating supportive relationships with center staff. By providing for the children's basic needs, including a sense of belonging, these programs successfully bolster a child's resilience. However, there are drawbacks to a model of service that provides paid relationships to adults that may be less authentic than naturally occurring relationships that have been developed on the street over an extended period of time. In the minority world in particular, the creation of trusting relationships between children and adults is complicated by a structure of formal care that often results in a child's feeling a greater sense of rejection and alienation, because the breakdown of many out-of-home placements is one of the reasons children take to the street in the first place. Coming from a background of abuse, these children are often more comfortable in the presence of peers who affirm their view of the world as a dangerous place when interacting with adults. In the majority world, structures are different and the need for services greater. However, success may be more a matter of "anything is better than nothing" than an indication of well-run programs with adequately trained staff (McAdam, 1997). In Rwanda, following the 1994 genocide, there was a proliferation of new programs with different approaches for street children. This was also partly because many trained personnel had been killed in the genocide. Many programs succeeded. However, this was not necessarily an indication of their ability to care for children but, rather, an indication of the need for resources regardless of their quality. Often, staff members are less than adequately trained, although they may appear quite charismatic and able to engage with the youth (Ennew, 1994). Not surprisingly, such individuals also need to be carefully supervised, because they do not always have the best of intentions. As in the minority world, those who have been granted the privilege of protecting children may at times be the ones who abuse them. Ensuring that best practices are realized and that services are tailored to the needs of the

children requires that accurate data be gathered on the children to assess present and potential future risks as well as relevant protective factors. Acquiring accurate data is not easy. In the minority world, street workers in cooperation with the police often walk the streets at night to gain a more accurate understanding of who is on the streets and what they are doing. As mentioned, many street girls involved in prostitution are not on the streets during the day. In fact, they may not necessarily be on the street at night either but, instead, work out of clubs. Realizing this, McAdam-Crisp, while living in Nairobi, frequented a nightclub known for its prostitution as part of the fieldwork for her master's thesis. Young girls would come to the club, enter the washroom, and change into more seductive outfits. A woman handing out condoms in the washroom would guard their clothes for a small price. Often, there was a priest at the entrance to the club handing out information on other options available to these girls. Many programs for these girls have been developed based on perceived rather than defined need. This is apparent in a number of programs that provide food and shelter. As mentioned before, most street children have parents and a home to go to but are on the streets because of circumstances related to their families' economic hardship. For this reason, it may be more appropriate to use a family-centered model of intervention rather than one that merely views the child individually as being at risk. For example, in the Rwandan capital, Kigali, there were significantly more services for street children than in smaller communities, which may have actually led to children's migration away from their families and into situations of greater risk. Offering a more community-based family model may help these children gain the support they need in their home communities. A family-centered approach does, however, present a number of potential problems. Although street children were reunited with their families, many children complained that their parents only took them back based on the promise of monetary benefits. Many programs provided material support, such as fixing a roof or buying a bed, as incentives for parents to accept responsibility for their children. More positively, income-generating activities have been used to support the needs of the entire

84 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES family, not just the child at risk. The development of income-generation activities has been acclaimed by many (Boyden & Mann, 2000; McAdam, 1995). However, such employment opportunities are limited. In areas where these schemes are operating, boys are traditionally trained in the area of woodwork, carpentry, and metal work and girls in hairdressing and dressmaking. Although these are all worthy endeavors to fully enhance the resilience of children, children are then expected to fend for themselves once they have completed their program. Because of a weak job market in most communities from where these children come, many are not able to get jobs, resulting in further exposure to potential risk factors. Programs that address the needs of working street children need to be designed based on a market assessment. A number of innovative programs that incorporate this have proven to be successful. For example, Peter Dalglish, the founder of Street Kids International, and others facilitated the development of a bicycle courier program for street boys in Khartoum, Sudan, in the late 1980s. Building broader community support, including among business people, police, and politicians, has also proven to be a successful means of advocating for street children in a number of cultural contexts. In Brazil, for example, large economic shifts and the brutal death of a number of street children over the last decade have resulted in the development of a number of initiatives that involve various levels of the community (Rizzini, Rizzini, Munoz-Varga, & Galeano, 1994; Scheper-Hughes & Hoffman, 1998). These efforts focus on increasing the community awareness about the plight of street children and advocating for policy changes to secure their welfare. Many involve children in the political process, which increases children's abilities to develop successful coping strategies and a better-defined role in their communities. Community-based models are one way of supporting street children. However, they do not fully address the needs of children who are truly orphaned, particularly girls who are on the street as a result of abuse. Although institutionalized care is often criticized regarding its overuse, it cannot be fully eliminated (Tolfree, 1995). In the minority world, a formalized structure of care is in place that includes both foster care

and group homes, in addition to an overused juvenile justice system. However, Anglin (2002) notes that these care facilities lack many of the essential elements that create a more resilient environment for children. In the majority world, because of the increasing number of children who are on the street as a result of war and AIDS, the opposite situation exists, with resilience-based models needing to develop more options for housing children. Models that promote the fostering of children in majority world contexts have been developed with some success (Dona, Kalinganire, & Muramutsa, 2001). Efforts to create an environment in which street children can enhance their resilience means creating an environment of relational elements on which children can draw as protective factors. This does not include the Westernized version of counseling as a therapeutic technique to address the trauma many street children have experienced. Such formal relationships are too hierarchical and tend to identify the adult as the one who knows all and the child as the one who is receiving knowledge. Counseling should be used as a means of facilitating growth through an interactive process that promotes the development of relationships that are more equal. In this regard, we agree with observations by Prilleltensky and Prilleltensky in this volume (see Chapter 6) that helping professionals need to become more socially and politically active in how they intervene. In the minority world, one way to accomplish this flattening of the hierarchy and involvement in the promotion of social justice is through the provision of food. As a community-based family counselor, McAdamCrisp did most of her work with youth in restaurants and coffee shops, which provided a nurturing and a nonthreatening forum for informal counseling. Techniques such as drama, dance, and music are often more culturally appropriate for building relationships in the majority world. Kironyo has conducted monthly youth camps for the past 15 years. At these camps, youth from the Mathare valley, a large slum on the outskirt of Nairobi, learn basic life skills through a process of drama and interactive workshops. A peer-mentoring model is also used to strengthen and create relationships within the group and between group members and staff, thus promoting the group as a whole.

Theory of Resilience and Its Application to Street Children · 85 Often, the mothers of the children are also present at the workshop, which strengthens the family as a unit. These initiatives show that the process of developing programs that can enhance resilience involves recruiting the appropriate staff, providing them the necessary training, and providing a venue where street children have access to their home communities and in which they feel comfortable and supported. Programs need to be community based, thus building on and strengthening the resources children already possess within their surrounding community. This needs to be coupled with educational information, such as life skills and employment training. Programs also work best when they provide AIDS education, health information, family planning, and socialization skills. Furthermore, as has been shown, an emphasis on the relational process at the core of these programs is essential to their success in mitigating risk and enhancing children's access to protective factors.

CONCLUSION The situation of street children is a global concern. The definition of a street child is context specific, and although there are some commonalities between street children in the minority and majority world, these cannot necessarily be generalized. The growing number of these children and their distribution throughout the world demands that resources at both the international and national level be used to address both micro and macro issues that influence their lives. The way we examine street children needs to be constructive and should recognize each child's individual experience within the context of his or her environment. Presently, we know far more about street children in the minority world than their counterparts in the majority world. Therefore, arguably, we need far more research to be done on a global scale. In accomplishing this research, we will need to be sensitive to the many contextually specific conditions in which street children live. For example, as has been detailed in this chapter, there are probably more similarities in the reasons girls end up on the street globally than boys. Girls therefore are at a greater risk when living on the street but remain more invisible.

Furthermore, the term street children needs to be deconstructed to understand children in relation to their social and cultural contexts to better understand their ability to sustain resilience. Such knowledge, generated collaboratively, will help us to design more successful programs. This process will increase our understanding of the diverse sociocultural factors that influence resilienceenhancing processes and provide information on the generalizability of resilience theory and practices for street children globally. This information can facilitate the development of policies and practices that are inherently strength based to enhance the resilience of street children in both the minority and majority worlds.

NOTES 1. The term minority world refers to the Western or developed world where the minority of children live, and the majority world refers to children in the developing or underdeveloped world. 2. The term PTSD is used hesitantly because there is a great deal of controversy over its application to children; it was originally coined to describe the symptoms of American Vietnam veterans and, as such, may not be applicable for children, especially those of another culture. Aptekar notes that PTSD as listed in the DSM-IV-R is less likely to occur in some cultures and that, depression, phobia, and somatic complaints are more common. For this reason DESNOS (disorder of extreme stress not otherwise classified), a condition referred to in the DSM, may be a more appropriate term for describing the progressive psychological impact of trauma as experienced by these street children. Symptoms include difficulty modulating anger and controlling impulses, a feeling of being victimized and/or victimizing others, and an inability to trust others. Amnesia, disassociation, and somatization are common. DESNOS is considered to be more common among people who have had repeated exposure to trauma.

REFERENCES Agnelli, S. (1986). Street children: A growing urban tragedy. London: Weidenfield & Nicholson. Anglin, J. P. (2002). Staffed group homes for children and youth: Constructing a theoretical framework

86 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES for understanding. Leicester, UK: University of Leicester. Aptekar, L. (1988). Street children of Call London: Duke University Press. Aptekar, L. (1994). Street children in the developing world: A review of their condition. CrossCultural Research, 28(3), 195-224. Aptekar, L. (2000, April). A world view of street children in the year 2000. Paper presented at the Program for Street Children Symposium, UNICEF, Jyvaskyla, Finland. Aptekar, L., & Ciano, L. (1999). Street children in Nairobi, Kenya: Gender differences and mental health. In R. W. Larson (Ed.), Developmental issues among homeless and working street youth: New directions in childhood development (pp. 35-46). San Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass. Aptekar, L., Maphalala, T. P., Dlamini, G., Makhanya, J., & Magagula, S. (1998). The newly emerging problem of street children in Swaziland. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 2, 123-141. Aptekar, L., & McAdam-Crisp, J. (2005). Street children, a global view of. In C. B. Fisher & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Encyclopedia of applied developmental science (pp. 1065-1069). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Aptekar, L., & Stocklin, D. (1997). Growing up in particularly difficult circumstances: A crosscultural perspective. In J. Berry, P. R. Dasen, & T. S. Saraswathi (Eds.), Handbook of crosscultural psychology: Basic process ofhuman development (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 377^412). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Ayerst, S. (1999). Depression and stress in street youth. Adolescence, 34(\35), 567-575. Bagley, C. (1985). Child sexual abuse: A child welfare perspective. In K. Levitt & B. Wharf (Eds.), The challenge of child welfare (pp. 66-93). Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press. Barker, G. (1989). Fact sheet on street children and AIDS/HIV. New York: ChildHope USA. Barker, G., & Knaul, F. (1991). Exploited entrepreneurs: Street and working children in developing countries (Working paper number 1). New York: ChildHope USA. Black, M. (1993). Street and working children: Innocenti Global Seminar summary report. Florence, Italy: UNICEF International Child Development Centre.

Blanc, C. S. (1994). Urban children in distress global predicaments and innovative strategies. Florence, Italy: UN Children's Fund. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1). New York: Basic Books. Boyden, J., & Mann, G. (2000). Children's risk, resilience and coping in extreme situations: Background paper to the consultation on children in adversity. Oxford, UK: Refugee Studies and Centre for Child-Focused Anthropological Research. Connelly, M. (1990). Adrift in the city: A comparative study of street children in Bogota, Colombia and Guatemala City. In N. Boxhill (Ed.), Homeless children: The watcher and the waiter (pp. 129— 149). New York: Haworth. Cosgrove, J. (1990). Towards a working definition of street children. International Journal of Social Work, 33, 185-195. Dallape, F. (1988). An experience with street children. Nairobi: Undugu Society of Kenya. Dallape, F. (1996). Urban children a challenge and an opportunity. Childhood, 3, 263-294. Demoskoff, L., & Lauzer, J. (1994). Working with street youth: A resource manual. Vancouver, Canada: Watari Research Association. Donâ, G., Kalinganire, C , & Muramutsa, F. (2001). My child is yours and ours: The Rwandan experience of foster care for separated children. Kigali, Rwanda: UNICEF. Ennew, J. (1994). Street and working children a guide to planning. London: Save the Children UK. Erikson, E. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton. Hickson, J., & Gaydon, V. (1989). "Twilight children": The street children in Johannesburg. Journal of Multicultural Counselling and Development, 17, 85-95. Higgins, G. (1994). Resilient adults overcoming a cruel past. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Hutz, C. S., & Roller, S. H. (1999). Methodological and ethical issues in research with street children. In M. Rafaelli & R. W. Larson (Eds.), Homeless and working youth around the world: Exploring developmental issues (pp. 59-70). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Kilbride, P., Suda, C , & Njeru, E. (2000). Street children in Kenya: Voices of children in search of a childhood. Westport, CT: Bergin & Garvey. Kirby, L., & Fraser, M. W. (1997). Risk and resilience in childhood. In M. W. Fraser (Ed.), Risk and

Theory of Resilience and Its Application to Street Children · 87 resilience in childhood: An ecological perspective (pp.10-33). Washington, DC: National Association of Social Workers. Lalor, K. (1999). Street children: A comparative perspective. Child Abuse and Neglect, 2J(8), 759-770. Le Roux, J. (1996). Street children in South Africa: Findings from interviews on the background of street children in Pretoria, South Africa. Adolescence, 27(122), 423-431. Le Roux, J. (1998). Is the street child phenomenon synonymous with deviant behaviour. Adolescence, 33(\32), 915-925. Lusk, M. (1992). Street children of Rio de Janeiro. International Social Worker. 35, 293-305. Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper & Row. McAdam, J. (1995). Evaluation framework for the development of an alternative form of education for marginalized youth in developing countries: A case study Nairobi, Kenya. Unpublished master's thesis. University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada. McAdam, J. (1997). The social reintegration of youth in difficult circumstances: Street children strategies for solutions. Kigali: UN Development Program, Rwanda. Ministry of Labour and Social Affairs. (1997). Enquête sur les problèmes d'intégration sociale des jeunes en situation difficile dans la ville de Kigali [Inquiry into the problems of social integration of the young people in a difficult situation in the city of Kigali.] Kigali, Rwanda. Muchini, B„ & Nyandiya-Bundy, S. (1991). Struggling to survive: A study of street children in Zimbabwe. Harare, Zimbabwe: UNICEF. Norman, E. (2000). Resiliency enhancement putting the strength perspective into social work practice. New York: Columbia University Press. Oliveria, W., Baizerman, M., & Pellet, L. (1992). Street children in Brazil and their helpers: Comparative views on aspiration and the future. International Journal of Social Work, 35, 163-176. Panter-Brick, C. (2002). Street children, human rights, and public health: A critique and future direction. Annual Review of Anthropology, 31, 147-171. Poortinga, Y. H. (1997). Towards convergence? In J. Pandey (Ed.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 347-387). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Ratner, C. (1999). Three approaches to cultural psychology. Cultural Dynamics, 11(1), 7-31. Rizzini, I., Rizzini, I., Munoz-Varga, M., & Galeano, L. (1994). Brazil: A new concept of childhood. In C. S. Blanc (Ed.), Urban children in distress (pp. 55-99). Florence, Italy: UN Children's Fund International Child Development Centre. Royal Canadian Mounted Police. (2001). Canada's missing children annual report. Ottawa: National Missing Children Services. (Available at www .ourmissingchildren.ca/en/publications/2000/ 2001-annualrep_e.pdf) Save the Children Canada. (2004, March 1). Street children, www.savethechildren.ca/en/whatwedo/ isstrchi.html Scharf, W., Powell, M., & Thomas, E. (1986). Stollerstreet children of Cape Town. In S. Burman & R. Reynolds (Eds.), Growing up in divided society: The context of childhood in South Africa (pp. 262-287). Johannesburg: Raven Press. Scheper-Hughes, N., & Hoffman, D. (1998). Brazilian apartheid: Street kids and the struggle for urban space. In N. Scheper-Hughes & C. Sargent (Eds.), Small wars the cultural politics of childhood (pp. 352-388). Berkeley: University of California Press. Scheper-Hughes, N., & Sargent, C. (Eds.). (1998). Small wars the cultural politics of childhood. Berkeley: University of California Press. Smollar, J. (1999). Homeless youth in the United States: Description and developmental issues. In M. Rafaelli & R. W. Larson (Eds.), Homeless and working youth around the world: Exploring developmental issues (pp. 47-58). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Swart, J. (1990). Malunde the children of Hillbrow. Cape Town: Witwatersrand University Press. Tacon, R. (1981). My child now: An action plan on behalf of the children without families. New York: UNICEF Document. Tierney, N. (1997). Robbed of humanity: Lives of Guatemalan street children. Saint Paul, MN: Pangaea. Tolfree, D. (1995). Roofs and roots. Aldershot, UK: Arena. Tyler, F, Holliday, M., Tyler, S., Echeverry, J., & Zea, M. (1987). Street children and play. Children's Environments Quarterly, 4(4), 13-17. Tyler, F., Tyler, S., Echeverry, J., & Zea, M. (1992). Making it on the streets of Bogota: A

88 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES psychosocial study of street youth. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs, 117, 395-117. Ungar, M. (2004). A constructionist discourse on resilience multiple contexts, multiple realities among at-risk children and youth. Youth and Society, 35(3), 341-365. UNICEF. (2003, November). Africa's orphaned generation. New York: United Nations Children Fund. Veale, A. (1996). An empirical and conceptual analysis of street children in Sudan and Ethiopia. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland. Veale, Α., Adefrisew, Α., & Lalor, K. (1992). Study of street children in four selected towns of Ethiopia. Addis Abba, Ethiopia: Ministry of

Labour and Social Affairs UNICEF, Ethiopia, University College Cork. Veale, Α., & Donâ, G. (2003). Street children and political violence: A socio-demographic analysis of street children in Rwanda. Child Abuse and Neglect, 27(3), 253-269. Velis, J.-P. (1995). Blossoms in the dust: Street children in Africa. Paris: UNESCO. Webber, M. (1991). Street kids: The tragedy of Canada's runaway. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Werner, Ε. E., & Smith, R. S. (2001). Journey from childhood to midlife: Risk, resilience, and recovery. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Whiting, B. (Ed.). (1963). Six cultures: Studies of child rearing. New York: John Wiley.

6 BEYOND RESILIENCE Blending Wellness and Liberation in the Helping Professions ISAAC PRILLELTENSKY ORA PRILLELTENSKY

R

esilience typically implies the ability to cope with family and social adversity (Prilleltensky, Nelson, & Peirson, 2001). Although the adversity is deplored by helping professionals, they usually limit themselves to working with the family and consider the social problems to be beyond their scope. If all of us followed this reasoning, nobody in the helping professions would enact practices that challenge injustice. Instead, we would resign ourselves to deal with the victims of injustice, hoping to steel our clients before the next blow. But an increasing number of helpers are growing uncomfortable with the idea that all they can do is react to environmental assaults—they want to prevent them. Furthermore, they want to redefine resilience as the ability to not only cope with conditions related to adversity and injustice but also to challenge their very existence. Indeed, helping professionals are struggling to promote a social justice agenda. Counselors,

psychologists, and social workers realize that their caring work is constantly undermined by conditions of injustice. At least for helpers working with marginalized populations, the injustice encountered by their clients has the power to undermine their caring work. Youth workers, for example, frequently do their utmost to empower young people and to instill in them a sense of control, only to realize early in the course of counseling that the environment in which marginalized youth live is much more powerful than the most sophisticated psychological intervention. A growing number of professionals understand that caring in the proximal sense is insufficient in the absence of caring in the distal sense. Proximal caring is expressed within the confines of the counseling session, whereas distal caring is manifested in work to promote justice in the community. Without the latter, the former has meager chances of success. Without

89

90 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES distal caring, in the form of challenging and changing unjust environments, proximal caring remains a humane but somewhat inadequate answer to the plight of the poor and the disadvantaged. Research has repeatedly demonstrated the effects of noxious environments on mental health (Carr & Sloan, 2003; McCubbin, Labonte, Sullivan, & Dallaire, 2003). From this perspective, promoting resilience has much to do with promoting social justice. Helpers in the mental health field face a gap between their understanding of unhealthy environments and their ability to do something about them. Whereas the level of critique tends to be quite complex, the level of social justice practice tends to be quite embryonic. Critical psychologists, like other groups of critically oriented helping professionals in allied fields of practice, have been creating alternatives that go beyond the status quo and its critique. In this chapter, we introduce some lessons from critical psychology, a movement that promotes wellness and liberation at the same time. Here we recommend several steps for blending caring work with justice work in efforts to mitigate the risks that confront marginalized populations. To illustrate the application of these recommendations, we will discuss them in the context of people with physical disabilities. There is commonality in the critique of counseling psychology put forth by Vera and Speight (2003) and Lewis, Lewis, Daniels, and D'Andrea (2003), of social work put forth by Mullaly (2002), and of psychology put forth by critical psychologists (Nelson & Prilleltensky, in press; Pare & Lamer, in press; Prilleltensky & Nelson, 2002; Sloan, 2000). Vera and Speight (2003) synthesize the shortcomings of an approach that pays lip service to cultural diversity and social justice but falls short of articulating emancipatory ways to practice. They enumerate the barriers to acting, not just thinking, justly. They point out that multicultural competencies must go beyond the recognition of oppression: A caring and competent practitioner ought to enact alternatives that not only identify but also, and primarily, reduce oppression. Helping professionals have differing degrees of critical awareness. Some of them are indifferent to how their profession promotes the societal status quo. Others, in turn, are painfully aware

of how their professions blame victims for their misfortune. However mindful, the latter group is at a loss when it comes to creating alternatives. In the case of counseling, Vera and Speight perform an invaluable service for those who may be unfamiliar with psychology's support for an unjust state of affairs (Prilleltensky, 1994). They adroitly summarize the unwitting alliance between counseling psychology and the societal status quo. In this chapter, we heed their call for aligning our practice as helping professionals with the principles of social justice. We believe that progress can be made by (a) stressing the synergy of diverse values, (b) stressing the synergy between wellness and liberation, (c) learning from existing critiques within psychology and other fields, (d) promoting role reconciliation between the helping professional as healer and agent of change, and (e) adopting psychopolitical validity as a new measure for the evaluation of our social justice agenda. These five initiatives to make psychological interventions more influential in the sphere of social justice set the conditions for a broader and more contextually relevant environment in which wellness can take hold. As we will show, the roots of wellness (and resilience) are firmly anchored in the ground of socially just communities and processes.

INTERDEPENDENT VALUES No single value is comprehensive enough to address the entire range of human needs. Therefore, we judge values such as social justice, caring and compassion, and cultural diversity on their synergistic qualities, not on their isolated merits (James & Prilleltensky, 2002; Prilleltensky, 2001). Vera and Speight (2003) correctly point out that multicultural competence without social justice is insufficient. Table 6.1 organizes human needs and values into three separate spheres of wellness and liberation: personal, relational, and collective. If we concentrate solely on relational values such as cultural diversity and democratic participation, we run the risk of neglecting both personal and collective needs. Similarly, the historical focus of psychology on self-determination and health meant that little or no attention was paid to democratic participation,

Beyond Resilience · 91 cultural diversity, sense of community, or social justice (Fox & Prilleltensky, 1997). Vera and Speight are justifiably alarmed that if we concentrate on celebrating diversity without attending to power inequality and social injustice, we will undermine wellness and liberation, for they cannot exist but in the synergy created by the composite of values. Historically, there is a propensity to concentrate on single values. Such proclivity is largely determined by dominant political and cultural ideologies. During conservative times, personal values of self-determination tend to be extolled, whereas principles of equality and justice come to the fore during progressive eras (Levine & Levine, 1992). It is our job to diagnose the mood of the times and realize what values we're missing from the equation. There is little doubt that psychology has absorbed the Zeitgeist of the last three decades and concentrated on individual remedies for social maladies (Albee, 1990; Cushman, 1990; Fox & Prilleltensky, 1997; Prilleltensky, 1994; Sampson, 1983; Sarason, 1981). As Boyden and Mann show in Chapter 1, the preponderance of resilience research and theory that is focused more on the psychological development of individuals than on the social and cultural context in which individuals live exemplifies this trend. As a result, we have neglected social justice and support for marginalized communities at our peril. However, there is also the current risk because our values extol respect for diversity above all else, even though cultural diversity cannot exist in the absence of social justice. All the values presented in Table 6.1 are codependent and interdependent. Extreme reliance on a single value undermines the existence of that very value, for it cannot thrive in the absence of others. We must be forever vigilant about what values are being privileged and what values are being ignored. There cannot be justice in the absence of compassion, and there cannot be compassion in the absence of justice. Striking a balance among values for personal, relational, and collective wellness and liberation is our most pressing task as professionals and citizens. The values of self-determination, and social justice in particular, have been severely undermined for many people with disabilities. So long as the problems they encounter in their

daily living are attributed to the impairment itself, efforts to enhance wellness are conceptualized and enacted at the individual level alone. Those who require assistance with daily living often have to fight for control over what services they will receive, their mode of delivery, and who will assist them with the most intimate selfcare tasks. The inability to carry out physical tasks unassisted is often taken as deficiency in the ability to make important decisions about one's life. Combined, such threats to control threaten individuals' capacities to overcome the multiple adversities they face coping with a disability, threatening their capacity to experience themselves as both resilient (for overcoming adversity) and well (for sustaining a quality of life). Unfair distribution of power has implications not only for how independence is defined (in primarily physical terms) but for how it is actually enacted in various medical and rehabilitation settings. Much of the work carried out by counselors and occupational and physical therapists is focused on patients' ability to independently carry out activities of daily living or to come to terms with their inability to do so. Whereas most people would prefer to be as independent as they can in self-care, it is critical that this is not regarded as necessary for autonomous adult or child functioning. I, Ora, am reminded of a patient I worked with who had to negotiate with one of his treating therapists to convince the therapist that it was pointless for the patient to attend a breakfast group that had as its goal to make him capable of preparing his own morning meal. A stroke had left this man with significant physical impairments, although his cognitive functioning remained relatively intact. It was very clear to him that he would not be attending to his own breakfast at home given the time and energy that this required of him. Given the emphasis placed on physical rehabilitation, convincing his therapist of this was no easy task. The therapist insisted the man needed to learn this skill, overlooking the man's capacity to make judgments on his own course of rehabilitation. Making such decisions on behalf of others is what truly robs people of dignity and control over their lives. Resilience stems, in part, from the capacity and opportunity to understand the role of adversity

Κ)

Protection of physical and emotional health and resistance to unhealthy personal, relational, and societal forces

Emotional and physical well-being

Promotion of ability of children and adults to pursue chosen goals in life without undue oppression

Mastery, control, self-efficacy, voice, choice, skills, growth and autonomy

Definition

Needs addressed

SOURCE: Adapted from Prilleltensky and Nelson (2002).

Health

Self-determination and personal growth

Personal

Identity, dignity, self-respect, self-esteem, acceptance

Promotion of people's ability to define themselves individually and collectively and to resist political, ideological, and cultural domination

Respect for human diversity

Participation, involvement, and mutual responsibility

Promotion of fair processes whereby children and adults can have meaningful input into decisions affecting their lives

Collaboration and democratic participation

Relational

Wellness and Liberation

Personal, Relational, and Collective Domains of Wellness and Liberation

Values

Domains

Table 6.1

Sense of community, cohesion, formal support

Promotion of vital community structures that facilitate the pursuit of personal and communal goals

Support for community structures

Economic security, shelter, clothing, nutrition, access to vital health and social services

Promotion of fair and equitable allocation of bargaining powers, obligations, and resources in society; and resistance to forces of exploitation and domination

Social justice

Collective

Beyond Resilience · 93 in one's life and the role of individuals and groups to challenge systems of inequity and discrimination. Coping without challenging these systems may result in accepting the unacceptable.

WELLNESS AND LIBERATION The helping professions have traditionally concerned themselves with wellness, health, and well-being. Under the aegis of the medical model, psychology and psychiatry conceptualized problems in living in intrapsychic terms. Mental health, wellness, and most recently, positive psychology became choice metaphors. They all conjure images of people enjoying life, worry free and healthy. This is a most worthy goal, which we fully support. But as with any single value, wellness cannot stand by itself. Unless it is supported by fairness and equality, it is bound to fall. An extensive body of research documents the ill effects of inequality and disempowerment on health and wellness (Kawachi, Kennedy, & Wilkinson, 1999; Kim, Millen, Irwin, & Gersham, 2000; Marmot, 1999). The impact of poverty, marginalization, exclusion, exploitation, and injustice is just as deleterious on the body as it is on the soul (I. Prilleltensky, 2003a). To ignore this evidence is to pretend that our psychological interventions can be potent enough to undo the damage of structural inequality—inequality often expressed in deficient health services and employment opportunities for the poor. We can afford to be humbler. Our psychological interventions are not that powerful. Wellness is a positive state of affairs, brought about by the simultaneous satisfaction of personal, relational, and collective needs. To meet these needs, we have to attend to power dynamics operating at micro, meso, and macro levels of analysis (Nelson & Prilleltensky, in press). Thus, wellness is intricately linked to empowerment. Empowerment, in turn, does not take place only at the personal level. Relational and collective empowerment support personal empowerment and vice versa (Kieffer, 1984; Lord & Hutchison, 1993). Power equalization must take place at all these levels if wellness is to be a resource available to those

marginalized by disability and other concurrent risk factors. Liberation needs wellness as much as wellness needs liberation from oppressive forces. Liberation, like freedom, has two aims: liberation from and liberation to (Fromm, 1960). Whereas the former strives to eliminate oppression and abuse at the personal, relational, and collective levels, the latter seeks to pursue wellness for self and others. People with disabilities have long struggled to attain wellness and liberation at the same time. They have claimed that disability is not a personal tragedy that requires medical solutions but, rather, a social issue requiring social intervention. They have decried the medical model of disability that regarded the problem as residing solely within the disabled individual. The focus on bodily abnormality meant that medically driven solutions were called for. Treatment was designed, implemented, and evaluated by a host of professionals, with the disabled individual having little input regarding the process. What could not be cured had to be rehabilitated, and what could not be rehabilitated had to be accepted. Psychological theories focused on the need to adjust to one's misfortune and make the best of a tragic and limited life. Those who did not despair despite their disability were often perceived as being in a state of denial (Oliver, 1996; Olkin, 1999) or, more positively, resilient, to use the word in the shallowest of ways. People with disabilities have argued that it is society, rather than the impairment itself, that is the source of their disablement. The Union of the Physically Impaired Against Segregation in 1976 declared: In our view, it is society which disables physically impaired people. Disability is . . . imposed on top of our impairments by the way we are unnecessarily isolated and excluded from full participation in society. Disabled people are therefore an oppressed group in society. (Barton, 1998, p. 56) Proponents of this alternative social model of disability have demonstrated the multiple ways in which people with disabilities are socially and economically disadvantaged. Being historically excluded from mainstream schooling,

94 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES many did not attain the necessary skills to further their education and make them competitive within the job market. Some encounter discriminatory attitudes and a lack of willingness to make simple accommodations within the workplace. Those who require assistive devices, attendant care, or both often come up against paternalistic policies designed to retain professional control over resources. Physical barriers have also been a source of exclusion; public spaces were historically designed with ablebodied people in mind. A shortage of affordable accessible housing and inaccessible public transportation further marginalize people with disabilities (Barton, 1998; Morris, 1993; Olkin, 1999; Oliver, 1996). In Ora's research on women with physical disabilities and motherhood, most participants reported that they did not envision that they would lead a life similar to nondisabled peers (O. Prilleltensky, 2003, 2004a, 2004b). One participant who spent most of her childhood in an institution described the difficulty in imagining an adult life beyond that setting: "You didn't see kids there leaving, or getting married, or having kids . . . they just left and you never heard from them again" (O. Prilleltensky, 1998, p. 118). At the time of the participants' birth some four decades ago, most of their parents were encouraged to institutionalize them (although few did), were told to expect little in the way of progress and growth, and were generally painted a grim picture of life with a disability. Not surprisingly, few parents expected that their children would lead typical adult lives and some ignored or actively discouraged their daughters' emergent sexuality. Oliver (1990), a disabled academic in the United Kingdom, was one of the first people to talk about the social versus the individual model of disability. Along with other disability activists, he argued that the very term disability is about exclusion and disadvantage. For example, Oliver suggested an alternative format to a disability survey conducted by the Office of Population Census and Surveys (OPCS) in the United Kingdom. Whereas the standard version focuses on the impairment as the source of limitation, Oliver's version shifts the focus to disabling barriers and attitudes. Consider the following examples:

OPCS:

"Can you tell me what is wrong with you?"

Oliver:

"Can you tell me what is wrong with society?"

OPCS:

"Do you have a scar, blemish, or deformity which limits your daily activities?"

Oliver:

"Do other people's reactions to any scar, blemish, or deformity you may have limit your daily activities?"

OPCS:

"Does your health problem/disability make it difficult for you to travel by bus?"

Oliver:

"Are there any transport or financial problems which prevent you from going out as often or as far as you would like?"

The political action and struggle of disabled people around the world has resulted in significant progress. No longer willing to put up with inadequate resources and professional control, people with disabilities have collectively fought for economic, legislative, and social gains. In the United States, the formation of "independent living movements" in the 1960s and 1970s has been associated with greater individual autonomy as well as more political and economic freedom (White, in press). The legislation of the American with Disabilities Act in 1990 has ensured that many of the aforementioned gains are not contingent on people's goodwill but are enforceable by law. For example, it is illegal to discriminate against a worker based on disability status, to hold a civic gathering at an inaccessible venue, or to fail to accommodate the needs of a disabled patient at a health clinic. Although there is still a long way to go, there is little doubt that these practical gains in legislation, economic resources, and social participation do go a long way toward the enhancement of wellness. Furthermore, the new focus on disabling societal barriers and systematic powerlessness has done much to improve the self-esteem and well-being of people with disabilities (Morris, 1993; Oliver, 1990; Shakespeare, 1998; White, in press). Combined, these changes go a long way toward creating the conditions in which people with disabilities can achieve health. These structural changes

Beyond Resilience · 95 contribute to an individual's being seen by others and himself or herself as resilient. Consider the following quote of a disabled activist in the United Kingdom who describes the impact that the social model of disability has had on her life: My life has two phases: before the social model of disability, and after it. Discovering this way of thinking about my experiences was the proverbial raft in stormy seas. . . . For years now this social model has enabled me to confront, survive, and even surmount countless situations of exclusion and discrimination.... It has played a central role in promoting disabled people's individual selfworth, collective identity, and political organization. I don't think it is an exaggeration to say that the social model has saved lives. (Crow, 1996, pp. 206-207) It is worth reexamining the concept of resilience in light of the empowering experiences of persons with disabilities. The claim can be made that Crow and other activists became more resilient precisely because they challenged the status quo and not because they learned how to cope with it. In fact, related research on empowerment demonstrates that participating in social actions enhances sense of control, a key component of resilience and mental health (Kieffer, 1984; Prilleltensky, Nelson, & Peirson, 2001).

INSULARITY AND ACTION Helping professionals cannot afford to ignore critiques such as this that are occurring in a number of related fields. The field of critical psychology has been struggling with how to promote a social justice agenda in ways that parallel the concerns raised by Vera and Speight (2003) in counseling and by Mullaly (2002) in social work (Fox & Prilleltensky, 1997; Prilleltensky & Nelson, 2002; Sloan, 2000). Prilleltensky and Nelson (2002), for instance, proposed means of promoting a social justice agenda in psychology. They made specific recommendations for working critically in school, health, counseling, clinical, work, and community settings. Community psychology has also been highly influential in fostering social change, prevention, cultural diversity, and

empowerment for the last four decades (Nelson & Prilleltensky, in press; Newbrough, 1992, 1995; Prilleltensky, 2001; Rappaport, 1987). Disciplinary boundaries and the insularity that results sometimes prevent fruitful explorations of similar agendas. Psychology in particular cannot afford to ignore critiques of the helping professions and the societal status quo mounted by people with disabilities (Oliver, 1990), by consumer/survivors of the psychiatric system (Nelson, Lord, & Ochocka, 2001), by sexual minorities (Kitzinger, 1997), and by other disciplines (Fox & Prilleltensky, 1997). As psychologists, our ability to see beyond our own psychological glasses is limited. Just as we need to expand our definitions of wellness to incorporate other cultural perspectives, we need to listen to critiques of psychology raised by nonpsychologists. But the problem of insularity goes beyond critique: It affects action as well. We should heed Audre Lorde's dictum: "The master's tools will never dismantle the master's house." People with disabilities did not achieve the rights they did because of professionals. Often, it is in spite of professionals that people with disabilities and other marginalized groups make progress toward wellness and liberation (Oliver, 1990). If we are to make progress toward social justice, we need to create alliances with the people we wish to help (Nelson, Prilleltensky, & MacGillivary, 2001). Much can be learned from social movements and consumers' movements in their efforts to declassify homosexuality as an abnormality, to obtain access to pubic buildings and transportation, or to overcome the stigma of mental illness (Nelson & Prilleltensky, in press). These actions, we claim, will not materialize until counselors reconcile their roles as healers with their role as change agents.

ROLE RECONCILIATION If helpers respond to the call for action, as we hope they do, they will pretty soon face a dilemma: how to reconcile their various roles as professional helpers on one hand and agents of social change on the other. Hitherto, we have not articulated how these two sets of knowledge, practices, and roles work in synergy for the

96 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES

Figure 6.1

Professional Helper

Critical Professional Praxis

Critical Agent

Specialized professional knowledge for wellness

Knowledge for wellness and liberation

Critical knowledge for liberation

Ameliorative practice for wellness

Practice for wellness and liberation

Transformative practice for liberation

Insider role for wellness

Role Role for for wellness wellness an and d liberation liberation

Outsider role for liberation

Knowledge, Practice, and Roles for Critical Professional Praxis in Mental Health Practice

SOURCE: Adapted from Prilleltensky and Prilleltensky (2003b). promotion of wellness and liberation. Here we propose ways of melding professional and critical praxis (Prilleltensky, 2001; Prilleltensky & Nelson, 2002; Prilleltensky & Prilleltensky, 2003a, 2003b). Our challenge is to find ways of reconciling the two sets of skills and aims. From the perspective of the professional helper, whether a psychologist, social worker, or other helping professional, being a critical practitioner means seeking answers to three important questions:

3. How does our insider role as wellness promoter in the helping system inform our outsider role as social critic? From the perspective of the social change agent, the critical practitioner needs to address the following issues: 1. How does our knowledge of inequality and injustice inform our counseling work?

1. How does our special knowledge of wellness inform our social justice work?

2. How does our transformative practice in society inform our ameliorative work in the helping system?

2. How does our ameliorative practice inform our transformative practice?

3. How does our outsider role as social critic inform or relate to our insider role?

Beyond Resilience · 97 We argue that reconciling these diverse roles would promote the dual goals of wellness and liberation, both equally important contributions to the resilience experienced by individuals and their communities. Whereas the former is the primary domain of the professional helper, the latter is the main concern of the critical change agent (Nelson & Prilleltensky, in press). Ora's work on women with disabilities and motherhood (O. Prilleltensky, 2004a, 2004b) provides some practical examples of this reconciliation of roles. For example, the professional helper informed by a critical perspective can encourage girls and young women with disabilities to explore the impact of negative societal messages pertaining to sexuality and disability. This process of conscientization can result in de-blaming and may also lay the foundation for taking a stand against oppression. At the same time, transformative work in the community can be directed at changing restrictive and oppressive concepts of female sexuality and motherhood. Narrow conceptions of motherhood limit the scope of available resources for women who are confronted with the adversity associated with having a disability. Better, we think, to understand that different types of mothering require different types of resources. An expanded notion of motherhood (to include women with disabilities) would naturally lead to a wider definition of acceptable resources. Wellness and liberation exist in a dialectical relationship. Without liberation, many oppressed people cannot experience wellness, and without wellness, there is no superordinate goal for liberation. Our objective is to blend the two so that our various roles and skills attend to emancipation and quality of life at the same time. Figure 6.1 describes the amalgamation of knowledge, practices, and roles of the professional helper on one hand and the critical agents of change on the other. The argument can be made that professional helpers cannot research or know in-depth all aspects of wellness and liberation. We agree that interdisciplinary research and action is vital. But it is entirely possible to have interdisciplinary research and action that supports the status quo. This is why we need critical knowledge of how power and inequality play a role in counseling and mental health (Habermas, 1971). If we were

to stay at the level of individual wellness alone and were not to consider the impact of inequality, disadvantage, and oppression or were to leave these political domains to others, we would not be as effective as we might in our individual work because we would obviate the role of power in mental health. There is a need to incorporate critical insights into our daily working routine. The type of knowledge we pursue has been well articulated by Aristotle and recently revived by Flyvbjerg (2001). Phronesis is the type of practical knowledge that combines scientific understanding with political wisdom. It is an applied type of knowledge that seeks understanding in context—contexts that are perpetually suffused by power differentials and inequality. What we seek, in Habermas's words, is knowledge for emancipation. With respect to practice, we need to articulate how the various roles would be manifested in the actual day-to-day practice of helpers and community workers. Prilleltensky and Nelson (2002) and Murray et al. (2001) have proposed ways of blending the transformative role with the ameliorative task. For us, transformation refers to system change, whereas amelioration refers to individual or reformist change that leaves the sources of the problem unaffected. There are in fact many ways to advance the transformative impulse and critical knowledge in the helping professions (Prilleltensky & Prilleltensky, 2003b). Some potential avenues include the following: •









Creating awareness among colleagues about how power differentials get enacted in interactions with clients seeking counseling Forming research and action groups in the workplace to explore how practices may be more empowering of clients Increasing political literacy of community members to empower them to scrutinize the practices of helping professionals Establishing practices that enable participation of clients, patients, and community members in the management of human services Connecting with poor communities and partnering with them in raising the level of public health, advocating for more resources, protesting tobacco advertising, boycotting sexist advertising and others.

98 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES As insiders within the health and helping system, psychologists and other professionals face many barriers and limitations. Although they may be aware of many oppressive policies and practices, they may be constrained in their ability to act. Outside critics, in turn, may feel free to point to shortcomings but may not have the inside knowledge of how systems work or why some practices that may seem unnecessary from the outside may be well justified from the inside. Whereas the pull for the professional helper is for amelioration, wellness, and the prevention of institutional unrest, the pull for the critical change agent is for transformation, liberation, and disruption of unjust practices. For critical professional praxis to emerge, these two roles need to exist in tension and synergy, not in opposition. If wellness and liberation are to emerge, we need specialized knowledge as much as political knowledge, ameliorative therapies as much as social change, and people working inside the system as much as people confronting it.

PSYCHOPOLITICAL VALIDITY How can we make sure that our research and action live up to the ideals presented by Vera and Speight (2003), Mullaly (2002), Prilleltensky and Nelson (2002), and others? This is a question of importance to critical practitioners concerned with the promotion of social justice in the mental health field. To address this concern, I, Isaac, have recently suggested the introduction of psychopolitical validity as a tool for the promotion of wellness and liberation (I. Prilleltensky, 2003b, in press). This type of validity is built on two complementary sets of factors, psychological and political: hence, psychopolitical. This combination refers to the psychological and political influences that interact to promote wellness, perpetuate oppression, or generate resistance and liberation. Psychopolitical factors help explain suffering and well-being. At the same time, this combination of terms denotes the need to attend to both sets of factors in our efforts to change individuals, groups, and societies. As a result, we propose two types of psychopolitical validity:

(a) epistemic and (b) transformational. Whereas the former refers to using psychology and politics in understanding social phenomena, the latter calls on both sets of factors to make lasting social changes. We pay equal attention to psychological and political factors. Psychological factors refer to the subjective life of the person, informed by power dynamics operating at the personal, interpersonal, family, group, and cultural levels. Political factors, in turn, refer to the collective experience of individuals and groups, informed by power dynamics and conflicts of interest at the interpersonal, family, group, community, and societal levels. In both sets of factors, we emphasize the role of power in the subjective or collective experience of people and groups. Psychopolitical validity, then, derives from the concurrent consideration and interaction of power dynamics in psychological and political domains at various levels of analyses. Hence, we can talk about psychopolitical validity when these conditions are met. When this type of analysis is applied to research, we talk about epistemic psychopolitical validity. When it is applied to social interventions, we talk about transformational psychopolitical validity. To illustrate these concepts, we refer you to Tables 6.2 and 6.3, respectively. To understand issues of well-being, oppression, and liberation at the personal, relational, and collective domains, we turn our attention to Table 6.2. Each cell in the table refers to issues of power and their manifestation in political and psychological spheres. Needless to say, this table is not exhaustive or inclusive of all fields in the helping professions. Rather, it concentrates on the priorities of wellness and liberation, two issues we regard as crucial. Table 6.2 may be used to guide our commitment to emancipatory research. Furthermore, it may be used as an accountability device. We can monitor the extent to which we study the priority areas described in the table. In a sense, these guidelines serve the function of a vision—a vision of what type of research we need to pursue. Epistemic validity depends on the incorporation of knowledge on oppression into all research and action in mental health. This means accounting for power dynamics operating at

Beyond Resilience · 99 Table 6.2

Guidelines for Epistemic Psychopolitical Validity Domains

Concerns

Collective

Relational

Personal

Well-being

Accounts for role of political and economic power in economic prosperity and in creation of social justice institutions.

Studies the role of power in creating and sustaining egalitarian relationships, social cohesion, social support, respect for diversity, and democratic participation in communities, groups, and families.

Studies role of psychological and political power in achieving selfdetermination, empowerment, health, personal growth, meaning, and spirituality.

Oppression

Explores role of globalization, colonization, and exploitation in suffering of nations and communities.

Examines the role of political and psychological power in exclusion and discrimination based on class, gender, age, race, education, and ability. Studies conditions leading to lack of support, horizontal violence, and fragmentation within oppressed groups.

Studies role of powerlessness in learned helplessness, hopelessness, selfdeprecation, internalized oppression, shame, mental health problems, and addictions.

Liberation

Deconstructs ideological norms that lead to acquiescence and studies effective psychopolitical factors in resistance.

Studies acts of solidarity and compassion with others who suffer from oppression.

Examines sources of strength, resilience, solidarity, and development of activism and leadership.

SOURCE: Adapted from I. Prilleltensky (in press).

psychological and political levels in efforts to understand phenomena of interest. The following questions might guide the pursuit of epistemic psychopolitical validity: 1. Is there an understanding of the impact of global, political, and economic forces on the issue at hand? 2. Is there an understanding of how global, political, and economic forces as well as social norms influence the perceptions and experiences of individuals and groups affected by the issue at hand?

3. Is there an understanding of how the cognitions, behaviors, experiences, feelings, and perceptions of individuals, groups, and entire communities perpetuate or transform the forces and dynamics affecting the issue at hand? 4. Is there an appreciation of how interactions between political and psychological power at the personal, relational, and collective levels affect the phenomena of interest? Table 6.3 integrates levels of intervention with key concerns for mental health: well-being, oppression, and liberation. This is a vision of

100 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES Table 6.3

Guidelines for Transformational Psychopolitical Validity Domains

Concerns

Collective

Relational

Personal

Well-being

Contributes to institutions that support emancipation, human development, peace, protection of environment, and social justice.

Contributes to power equalization in relationships and communities. Enriches awareness of subjective and psychological forces preventing solidarity. Builds trust, connection, and participation in groups that support social cohesion and social justice.

Supports personal empowerment, sociopolitical development, leadership training, and solidarity. Contributes to personal and social responsibility and awareness of subjective forces preventing commitment to justice and personal depowerment when in position of privilege.

Oppression

Opposes economic colonialism and denial of cultural rights. Decries and resists role of own reference group or nation in oppression of others.

Contributes to struggle against in-group and out-group domination and discrimination, sexism, and norms of violence. Builds awareness of own prejudice and participation in horizontal violence.

Helps to prevent acting out of own oppression on others. Builds awareness of internalized oppression and role of dominant ideology in victim blaming. Contributes to personal depowerment of people in position of privilege.

Liberation

Supports networks of resistance and social change movements. Contributes to structural depowerment of privileged people.

Supports resistance against objedification of others. Develops processes of mutual accountability.

Helps to resist complacency and collusion with exploitative system. Contributes to struggle to recover personal and political identity.

SOURCE: Adapted from I. Prilleltensky (in press).

preferred interventions. We would show high degrees of commitment and accountability to the extent that we pursue these interventions. As a monitoring system, Table 6.3 helps to keep track of our actions. Are we intervening primarily at the personal level? Do we focus too much on oppression to the neglect of liberation and wellbeing? Have we neglected the collective domain? Whereas epistemic validity refers to our understanding of psychopolitical dynamics of oppression, transformative validity demands changes toward liberation at personal, interpersonal, and

structural domains. The following questions attend to transformative validity: 1. Do interventions promote psychopolitical literacy? 2. Do interventions educate participants on the timing, components, targets, and dynamics of best strategic actions to overcome oppression? 3. Do interventions empower participants to take action to address political inequities and social injustice within their relationships, settings,

Beyond Resilience · 101 communities, and states and at the international level? 4. Do interventions promote solidarity and strategic alliances and coalitions with groups facing similar issues? 5. Do interventions account for the subjectivity and psychological limitations of the agents of change? Explicit political aims have often been advocated for but infrequently acted on in mental health. Transformative validity may serve to remind us that political literacy and social change have to be part of all interventions. We seek not only to ameliorate social conditions but also to alter the configurations of power that deprive citizens of their rights (Prilleltensky & Nelson, 2002). In so doing, we create the conditions for resilience to be nurtured and to flourish. Our worry is that we too easily psychologize the successful growth of individuals, those with or without disabilities. We have failed to investigate the conditions—social, political, and structural—that must necessarily exist to support wellness and resilience. Using the benchmarks of epistemic and transformative psychopolitical validity, we believe, offers the field of mental health (researchers and practitioners alike) a way in which to conceptually broaden the scope of their work to account for a nonindividualizing health discourse. That discourse has been thus far lukewarm to a more contextual understanding of health phenomena.

CONCLUSION People affected with physical disabilities and psychosocial problems are better off when they demonstrate resilience as evidenced by successful ways of coping. But resilience must go beyond being a phrase limited to understanding how individuals cope with adversity. It must entail a challenge to the very structures that create disadvantage, discrimination, and oppression. This is not to pile more responsibilities on people who already experience challenges in their lives. Rather, it is a call to action for people with and without disabilities and for those who advocate with them for a more caring and just

society to create the conditions for resilience to be experienced. Their own participation, along with mental health and community workers, in challenging injustice can do much to enhance resilience. Professionals cannot stand back and hope that personal resilience will emerge from their therapeutic interventions alone. Community change, not just personal change; political change, not just psychological change; and justice, not just caring, are all urgently needed.

REFERENCES Albee, G. W. (1990). The futility of psychotherapy. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 11, 369-384. Barton, L. (1998). Society, disability studies and education: Some observations. In T. Shakespeare (Ed.), The disability reader (pp. 53-64). London: Cassell. Carr, S„ & Sloan, T. (Eds.). (2003). Poverty and psychology: From global perspective to local practice. New York: Kluwer/Plenum. Crow, L. (1996). Including all of our lives: Renewing the social model of disability. In J. Morris (Ed.), Encounters with strangers: Disability and feminism (pp. 206-226). London: Women's Press. Cushman, P. (1990). Why the self is empty: Toward a historically situated psychology. American Psychologist, 45, 599-611. Flyvbjerg, B. (2001). Making social science matter: Why social inquiry fails and how it can succeed again. New York: Cambridge University Press. Fox, D., & Prilleltensky, I. (Eds.). (1997). Critical psychology: An introduction. London: Sage. Fromm, E. (1960). Escape from freedom. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Habermas, J. (1971). Knowledge and human interests. Boston: Beacon. James, S., & Prilleltensky, I. (2002). Cultural diversity and mental health: Towards integrative practice. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 1133-1154. Kawachi, I., Kennedy, B., & Wilkinson, R. (Eds.). (1999). The society and population health reader: Income inequality and health. New York: New Press. Kieffer, C. (1984). Citizen empowerment: A developmental perspective. Prevention in Human Services, 5(2-3), 9-36. Kim, J. K., Millen, J. V., Irwin, Α., & Gersham, J. (Eds.). (2000). Dying for growth: Global

102 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES inequality and the health of the poor. Monroe, ME: Common Courage Press. Kitzinger, C. (1997). Lesbian and gay psychology: A critical analysis. In D. Fox & I. Prilleltensky (Eds.), Critical psychology: An introduction (pp. 202-216). London: Sage. Levine, M., & Levine, A. (1992). Helping children: A social history. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Lewis, J., Lewis, M., Daniels, J., & D'Andréa, M. (2003). Community counseling: Empowerment strategies for a diverse society. Pacifica Grove, CA: Thomson Learning. Lord, J., & Hutchison, P. (1993). The process of empowerment: Implications for theory and practice. Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health, 12(1), 5-22. Lorde, A. (1984). The master's tools will never dismantle the master's house. In Sister outsider: Essays and speeches (pp. 110-113). Santa Cruz, CA: Crossing Press. Marmot, M. (1999). Introduction. In M. Marmot & R. Wilkinson (Eds.), Social determinants of health (pp. 1-16). New York: Oxford. McCubbin, M., Labonte, R., Sullivan, R., & Dallaire, B. (2003). Mental health is our collective wealth. Discussion paper submitted to Federal/ Provincial/Territorial Advisory Network on Mental Health and Health Canada. (Available from Saskatchewan Population Health and Evaluation Research Unit) Morris, J. (1993). Independent lives? Community care and disabled people. London: Macmillan. Mullaly, B. (2002). Challenging oppression: A critical social work approach. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Oxford University Press. Murray, M., Nelson, G., Poland, B., Matycka-Tyndale, E., Ferris, L., Lavoie, F, Cameron, R., & Prkachin, K. (2001). Training in community health psychology. Report to Canadian Institutes of Health Research. (Available from Dr. Michael Murray, Division of Community Health, Faculty of Medicine, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St John's, Newfoundland, Canada, A1B 3V6) Nelson, G., Lord, J., & Ochocka, J. (2001). Shifting the paradigm in community mental health: Towards empowerment and community. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University of Toronto Press. Nelson, G., & Prilleltensky, I. (Eds.), (in press). Community psychology: In pursuit of wellbeing and liberation. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

Nelson, G., Prilleltensky, I., & MacGillivary, H. (2001). Building value-based partnerships: Toward solidarity with oppressed groups. American Journal of Community Psychology, 29, 649-677. Newbrough, J. (1992). Community psychology in the post-modern world. Journal of Community Psychology, 20, 10-25. Newbrough, J. (1995). Toward community: A third position. American Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 9-37. Oliver, M. (1990). The politics of disablement. New York: St. Martin's Press. Oliver, M. (1996). A sociology of disability or a disablist sociology? In L. Barton (Ed.), Disability and society (pp. 18—42). London: Longman. Olkin, R. (1999). What therapists should know about disability. New York: Guilford Press. Pare, D., & Lamer, G. (Eds.), (in press). Critical knowledge and practice in psychology and psychotherapy. New York: Haworth Press. Prilleltensky, I. (1994). The morals and politics of psychology: Psychological discourse and the status quo. Albany: State University of New York Press. Prilleltensky, I. (2001). Value-based praxis in community psychology: Moving towards social justice and social action. American Journal of Community Psychology, 29, 747-778. Prilleltensky, I. (2003a). Poverty and power. In S. Carr & Τ Sloan (Eds.), Psychology and poverty (pp. 19—H). New York: Kluwer/Plenum. Prilleltensky, I. (2003b). Understanding and overcoming oppression: Towards psychopolitical validity. American Journal of Community Psychology, 31, 195-202. Prilleltensky, I. (in press). The role of power in wellness, oppression, and liberation: The promise of psychopolitical validity. Journal of Community Psychology. Prilleltensky, I., & Nelson, G. (2002). Doing psychology critically: Making a difference in diverse settings. London: Palgrave Macmillan. Prilleltensky, I., Nelson, G., & Peirson, L. (Eds.). (2001). Promoting family wellness and preventing child maltreatment. University of Toronto Press. Prilleltensky, I., & Prilleltensky, O. (2003a). Reconciling the roles of professional helper and critical agent in health psychology. Journal of Health Psychology, 8, 243-246.

Beyond Resilience · 103 Prilleltensky, I., & Prilleltensky, O. (2003b). Towards a critical health psychology practice. Journal of Health Psychology, 8, 197-210. Prilleltensky, O. (1998). Motherhood in the lives of women with disabilities. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Toronto. Prilleltensky, O. (2003). A ramp to motherhood: The experiences of mothers with disabilities. Sexuality and Disability, 21, 21—47. Prilleltensky, O. (2004a). Motherhood and disability: Children and choices. New York: Palgrave/ Macmillan. Prilleltensky, O. (2004b). My child is not my carer: Mothers with physical disabilities and the wellbeing of children. Disability and Society, 19(3), 209-223. Rappaport, J. (1987). Terms of empowerment/ exemplars of prevention: Toward a theory for

Community Psychology. American Journal of Community Psychology, 15, 121-144. Sampson, Ε. E. (1983). Justice and the critique of pure psychology. New York: Plenum. Sarason, S. B. (1981). Psychology misdirected. New York: Free Press. Shakespeare, T. (Ed.). (1998). The disability reader: Social science perspectives..London: Cassell. Sloan, T. (Ed.). (2000). Critical psychology: Voices for change. London: Macmillan. Vera, Ε. M., & Speight, S. L. (2003). Multicultural competence, social justice, and counseling psychology: Expanding our roles. The Counseling Psychologist. White, G. (in press). Ableism. In G. Nelson & I. Prilleltensky (Eds.), Community psychology: In pursuit of wellbeing and liberation. New York: Palgrave/Macmillan.

7 COMMUNITY-BASED CHILD WELFARE FOR ABORIGINAL CHILDREN Supporting Resilience Through Structural Change CINDY BLACKSTOCK NICO TROCMÉ

• "~W" ~W~ elp Me," wrote Richard Cardinal in living on-reserve increased 71.5% nationally between 1995 and 2001 (McKenzie, 2002). I I his own blood while the 17-year-old Overall, we estimate that there may be as many -A. -M- Métis boy committed suicide after as three times more Aboriginal children in the spending 13 years moving in and out of 28 foscare of child welfare authorities now than were ter homes, group homes, and shelters in Alberta placed in residential schools at the height of (Obomsawin, 1986). Although Cardinal's death those operations in the 1940s (Blackstock, drew attention to the significant overrepresenta2003). This is particularly concerning because tion of Aboriginal children in state care, 20 years information suggests that many Aboriginal later, the problem has become far more serious, children resident off-reserve continue to be with Aboriginal children representing approxiplaced in non-Aboriginal homes (British mately 40% of the 76,000 children and youth Columbia Children's Commissioner, 1998). placed in out-of-home care in Canada (FarrisMoreover, as the UN Committee on the Rights Manning & Zandstra, 2003). Although there is a of the Child notes in its concluding remarks to lack of information on placement trends for Canada in 2003, Aboriginal children continue Aboriginal children off-reserve due to variances to face significant and disproportionate levels of in provincial data collection mechanisms, risks in other areas, such as education, youth Department of Indian Affairs year-end data sugjustice, health, and poverty. In keeping with the gest that the number of status Indian children 1

2

105

106 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES committee's concern for Aboriginal children, over one third of the concluding observations for Canada make specific mention of Aboriginal children (UN Committee on the Rights of the Child, 2003a). The reasons for the disproportionate removal of Aboriginal children from their families are poorly understood. Furthermore, much of the existing resiliency literature places the child as the primary locus of analysis versus exploring the implications of cultural, community, and family resiliency as central factors. This chapter draws from a number of sources to examine some of these mechanisms. We begin by making the point that diverse Aboriginal Nations have demonstrated resiliency for thousands of years prior to the arrival of colonial powers and certainly by surviving through myriad traumas brought on by colonization. We further discuss how residential schools, out-of-community foster care, and adoptive placements have historically shaped Aboriginal communities' experience of, and relationship with, child welfare services. We then present a profile of the contemporary experience of Aboriginal children and families who come into contact with the child welfare system, through an analysis of data from the Canadian Incidence Study on Reported Child Abuse and Neglect (Trocmé et al., 2001). We have structured our argument to demonstrate that the risks posed to Aboriginal children were, and are, often the result of structural decisions made by those outside their communities. In the process, generations of children suffered severe and long-lasting threats to their wellbeing, both psychologically and physically. Consistent with Aboriginal holistic approaches and structural social work theory, we believe that child, family, and community resiliency are interdependent, and thus, culturally based family interventions must be coupled with culturally based community development approaches to redress structural challenges to the safety of Aboriginal children. Finally, we discuss how culturally based community development frameworks could better address some of the current structural barriers, including inequitable service access, and the implications of systemic causal factors on child maltreatment assessment and response.

HISTORICAL CONTEXT Carbon-dated evidence suggests that Aboriginal peoples have lived on these lands now known as Canada for over 10,500 years (Muckle, 1998), raising over 525 generations of children before child welfare and social work were even founded. These emotional, physical, cognitive, and spiritual ways of knowing and being have guided the resilient development of hundreds of generations of Aboriginal children who were healthy, proud, contributing members of society . . . living safely at home in their communities. Yet consistent with patterns of colonialism, today, this knowledge is too often viewed as ancillary to the "legitimate" knowledge of the child welfare system and to child resiliency. As the history below describes, EuroWestern-based social work in Canada frequently embodies an unearned arrogance expressed through statute, funding regimes, and social policies that directly regulate and shape the way in which Aboriginal peoples (and Aboriginal child welfare agencies) can care for their children. The first colonists arrived on the eastern shores of what is now Canada in the 1490s. Reports indicate that initial contact between Aboriginal peoples and the colonial powers were primarily mutually beneficial because the relationship was centered on trade activity, but this rapidly changed as colonial aspirations moved to settlement, resource extraction, and the elimination of Indian peoples from the land (Royal Commission on Aboriginal Peoples [RCAP], 1996). The impacts of colonization on Aboriginal peoples cannot be underestimated. RCAP estimates that the population of Aboriginal peoples in Canada decreased 80% from the time of contact to confederation as a result of intentional and unintentional introduction of disease, bounty hunting, and starvation. Some peoples such as the Beothuck in Newfoundland became extinct. This prolific loss of life was coupled with forced displacement from traditional lands and the assignment of Aboriginal peoples to small reserves where maintenance of traditional sustenance was often not possible. The result was an erosion of communal cultural knowledge and ways of life that had sustained generations of Aboriginal children and the introduction of multigenerational grief and trauma and displacement.

Community-Based Child Welfare for Aboriginal Children · 107 Beginning in the 1800s, the government of Canada aided by the Christian churches strengthened its assimilation efforts through the operation of residential schools for Indian children (Milloy, 1999). The primary objective of these schools was to eliminate any vestige of Aboriginality, replacing it with Euro-Western culture, knowledge, and spirituality. Because Indian parents seldom voluntarily sent their children to these often distant schools, the government of Canada amended the Indian Act to force Indian parents to send their child(ren) aged 5 to 15 years to the schools. The penalty for failing to comply was incarceration and fines that often could not be paid because, typically, Aboriginal peoples were living in abject poverty. The conditions at the schools were abysmal; they were built of the cheapest possible materials, employed by untrained staff, and often overcrowded because of government financial inducements to increase enrollment. Sexual and physical abuses were prevalent as were preventable deaths from disease (Milloy, 1999). These conditions were known to the Canadian government as early as the 1890s (Milloy, 1999; RCAP, 1996). In fact, Dr. P. H. Bryce, chief medical health officer for the government of Canada, found in 1907 that the death rate at the schools from preventable disease was a shocking 24% per annum, increasing to 46% if the children were tracked over a 3-year period (RCAP, 1996.) Bryce's report was released to the government and published in the media; however, the government's response to the report was to eliminate the chief medical health officer position (RCAP, 1996). The schools continued to operate under these conditions for decades, with many schools opening cemeteries on school grounds to bury the children (Milloy, 1999). Generations of children attended these schools. Separated from family, culture, and traditional teachings, the impact was devastating at the personal, kinship, and community levels (Fornier & Crey, 1997). Children in residential schools did not experience healthy parental role modeling and as a result had a diminished capacity as adults to care for their own children (Bennett & Blackstock, 2002). Although the schools began closing in the 1940s, it took over 50 years for the last residential school to close in Saskatchewan in 1996—making it a very recent

experience for many Aboriginal people (Department of Indian and Northern Affairs Canada [INACj, 2003).

ABORIGINAL CHILD WELFARE IN CANADA The division of constitutional powers in Canada is such that the provincial and territorial governments carry the legal mandate and responsibility for providing child welfare services (Sinclair, Bala, Lilies, & Blackstock, 2004). The provincial and territorial governments have responsibility for funding child welfare services off-reserve, whereas the federal government retains responsibility under the Indian Act to fund child welfare services provided on-reserve to status Indian children. Up until the mid 1950s, the only "child welfare" service provided to Aboriginal families and their children was residential school placement. Advocacy efforts by social workers lead to the expansion of provincial child welfare jurisdiction on reserves. The nature and extent of child welfare services provided to Aboriginal families resident on reserves varied according to the province or territory and local practice. It was not atypical for Aboriginal children to be placed in residential schools by child welfare authorities up until the early 1970s, nor was it unusual for child welfare services on-reserve to be devoid of prevention and family support, relying instead on removal as the only response to child maltreatment (Aboriginal Justice Inquiry, 2001). Although there are incidents where interventions by child welfare authorities were experienced as positive by Aboriginal peoples, the overall impact of child welfare involvement with Aboriginal services has been discouraging. Social workers deprived of the information, skills, and resources to address the poverty, disempowerment, multigenerational grief, and loss of parenting knowledge defaulted to a practice of mass removals known as the 60s scoop (Aboriginal Justice Inquiry, 2001). The RCAP (1996) notes Department of Indian Affairs statistics indicating that over 11,000 status Indian children were placed for adoption between the years of 1960 and 1990. This statistic does not

108 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES include children for whom Indian status had not been recorded or nonstatus children. In some cases, buses were hired to remove large numbers of children from reserves, often placing them in distant non-Aboriginal families. As the removals took place, there was very little effort by child welfare authorities to address structural risk factors such as multigenerational trauma, poverty, unemployment, and substandard housing conditions, which were resulting in disproportionate rates of child abuse and neglect. There also was very little consideration of the influence of Euro-Western child welfare legislation or social workers' values and beliefs on their child welfare decision making and planning for Aboriginal children and families (Union of BC Indian Chiefs, 2002). The 60s scoop, coupled with a growing movement within First Nations and Aboriginal communities to stem the tide of children and youth being placed outside their communities, motivated the development of First Nations child and family service agencies (Blackstock, 2003). The number of First Nations child and family agencies expanded in the early 1990s when the federal government lifted a moratorium on the development of Aboriginal child agencies serving on-reserve residents and implemented a national funding formula known as Directive 20-1 Chapter 5 (with the exception of Ontario, which operates under a separate funding agreement, and agencies that had funding agreements that predated Directive 20-1). Directive 20-1 Chapter 5 (the Directive) provides funding for on-reserve child welfare services only and requires that First Nations agencies work pursuant to provincial and territorial child welfare statutes; First Nations operating under their own child welfare jurisdiction therefore are not eligible for funding under this arrangement. It is important to emphasize that the federal government will not fund services to First Nations children and families off-reserve, so many of these agencies are in the difficult position of serving only on-reserve residents, deferring off-reserve services to provincial and territorial child welfare agencies, which may or may not offer culturally based services. A further complication of the Directive 20-1 funding regime is that funding levels are not linked to the content of provincial and territorial child 3

welfare statutes, meaning that as provinces and territories change their legislation, there is no concordant review of funding levels to ensure that adequate resources are provided to First Nations child welfare agencies to meet new statutory responsibilities. A national review conducted in June of 2000 found that, on average, First Nations child and family service agencies receive 22% less funding per child than their provincial equivalents, despite the documented higher child welfare needs on-reserve (MacDonald & Ladd, 2000). The Directive, although facilitating the development of over 100 First Nations child and family service agencies serving on-reserve communities, has been broadly criticized for its inequitable funding levels compared with provincial child welfare providers and its emphasis on supporting child removal and placement versus allocating resources to support families and communities to safely care for their children at home (MacDonald & Ladd, 2000). First Nations child and family service agencies have, despite the barriers, been very successful in ensuring that children are cared for in the community whenever possible, and when placement outside of community is required, steps are taken to ensure that the child has access to cultural and linguistic services and to family whenever possible. Clearly, when culturally based structural supports are provided to Aboriginal children and families at risk, significant and sustained positive outcomes in child and family well-being can be expected. Furthermore, as the practices of First Nations child and family service agencies become known, they have increasingly been recognized for the outstanding quality and innovation in service delivery. Some of these practices are discussed in detail in the chapter by MacDonald, Glode, and Wien in this volume (see Chapter 22). In parallel to the development of on-reserve agencies, off-reserve child welfare agencies have started to develop in a number of provinces. In some cases, First Nations child welfare agencies basically extend their mandates off-reserve, whereas other agencies are developed to meet the needs of Aboriginal people living off reserves. One of the most progressive movements is the Manitoba Aboriginal Justice Inquiry Child Welfare Initiative, which allows residents of

Community-Based Child Welfare for Aboriginal Children · 109 Manitoba to choose which of four culturally based child welfare authorities they wish to be serviced by (Northern First Nations, Southern First Nations, Métis, or Mainstream). In this province where over 70% of the children in care are Aboriginal, 86% of families are choosing their culturally based authority (personal conversation with Elsie Flette, CEO of the Southern First Nations Child Welfare Authority, Winnipeg, Manitoba, February 2004). This model is very respectful of the cultural identity of clients and will be an important model to monitor over time.

Nations, Métis, and Inuit. In addition, there were questions to determine if the child had status pursuant to the Indian Act and whether the parent lived on-reserve. Data on Aboriginal status were not collected in the Quebec portion of the CIS-98 (N= 2,309) and were missing on a further 10 cases. Because the Aboriginal status of each investigated child was determined by the status of the biological parent(s) who were living with the child, children who did not reside with a biological parent (N = 225) were also excluded. Finally, unsubstantiated reports (N = 1,969) were excluded from the analysis, leaving a sample of 3,159 cases where maltreatment had been substantiated or remained suspected.

A PROFILE OF ABORIGINAL CHILDREN RECEIVING CHILD WELFARE SERVICES To date, there has been very little statistical information available about Aboriginal children and families receiving child welfare services (Blackstock, Clarke, Cullen, D'Hondt, & Formsma, 2004). The 1998 Canadian Incidence Study of Reported Child Abuse and Neglect (CIS-98) (Trocmé etal., 2001) was the first national study to examine the profile children and families coming into contact with the child welfare system. Although the scope of the 1998 study does not allow for national estimates specific to the subset of Aboriginal children, the sample of Aboriginal children included in the study nevertheless represents the best source of data currently available. The material presented in this chapter is drawn from two previous analyses of this data set (Blackstock, Trocmé, & Bennett, 2004; Trocmé, Knoke, & Blackstock, 2004). The data point to the importance of a broader conceptualization of child maltreatment, one that highlights the critical role that extended family and community supports can play in assisting children, young people, and families at risk of maltreatment. The CIS-98 collected information directly from child welfare investigators on cases of reported child abuse or neglect. A multistage sampling design was used to track child maltreatment investigations conducted in 51 randomly selected sites, including 3 First Nations child and family service agencies, from October to December 1998. Data on Aboriginal heritage included three Aboriginal groupings: First

Aboriginal Cultural Identification Of victims of all cases of suspected or substantiated maltreatment in CIS-98 (614 of 3,149 children), 19% were Aboriginal (see Table 7.1). According to the 1996 Statistics Canada Census figures, 5% of Canadians 15 years of age or younger were classified as Aboriginal, clearly indicating a possible overrepresentation of Aboriginal children involved with child welfare authorities. Approximately two thirds (64%) of Aboriginal children were classified as First Nations, many of whom lived off-reserve, a distribution similar to the 1996 Census Canada estimates indicating that 69% of Aboriginal children under 16 were classified as "North American Indian." The relatively large proportion of First Nations children within the Aboriginal service population is consistent with child-in-care data from British Columbia, indicating that 87% of the Aboriginal children in care are First Nations (status and nonstatus), with Métis and Inuit children representing 12% and 1%, respectively (Ministry for Child and Family Development, 2002). Similar figures are reported in Manitoba where First Nations children are overrepresented among other Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal children in the child welfare system (Stevens, 2003). 4

5

FORMS OF MALTREATMENT Well over half (61%) the Aboriginal cases of substantiated or suspected maltreatment primarily

6

110 · THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES Table 7.1

Aboriginal Status and Visible Minority Background for Cases of Substantiated and Suspected Maltreatment, 1998 CIS" (#=3,159)

Number of Investigations 614

Aboriginal

Percentage of All Cases

Percentage of Aboriginal Cases (excluding non-Aboriginals)

19%

First Nations on-reserve

120

27%

First Nations status, off-reserve

150

34%

First Nations nonstatus, off-reserve

14

3%

Métis

37

8%

Inuit

42

10%

Other

78

18%

Unsure/no answer Non-Aboriginal

173 2,114

67%

431

14%

Other minority Total

3,159

a. Unweighted sample, excludes (1) investigations in Quebec (N = 2,309), where aboriginal and ethnic identity data were not collected; (2) cases of children not residing with a biological parent (N = 225); (3) unsubstantiated investigations (N = 1,969); and (4) other eligible cases where Aboriginal and ethnic identity data were missing (N =10).

involved some form of neglect, whereas neglect was found in only half as many non-Aboriginal cases (Table 7.2). Most cases were categorized as failure to supervise or failure to protect, resulting in either physical harm or risk of physical harm to the child. There was no differentiation between failure to protect and failure to supervise in CIS primary form of maltreatment reporting categories, so it not possible to determine what proportion of these cases were failure to protect, which suggests a more conscious decision to not meet a child's basic needs versus failure to supervise, which suggests a passive decision to neglect the child's needs. The larger proportion of neglect cases is not surprising given the impacts of residential schools in separating children from parental and community systems of care (Earle & Cross, 2001; Indian Residential Schools Survivor Society, 2002). This, coupled with the high incidence of failure to protect or failure to supervise cases, suggests that a depoyment of resources to specifically address neglect and its related undercurrents of poverty, inadequate housing,

and substance misuse may be advised as a means of decreasing the numbers of Aboriginal children in the child welfare system. This type of investment would be consistent with First Nations' request that child welfare be positioned within a community development framework that considers some of the etiological drivers of child maltreatment, such as poverty, social isolation, racism, and socioeconomic exclusion (Blackstock, Clarke, et al., 2004; MacDonald & Ladd, 2000). Non-Aboriginal cases were much more likely to involve physical abuse, most notably punishment-related abuse, which accounted for 35% of cases involving visible minority families and 22% of non-Aboriginal families. Sexual abuse was not noted as often in Aboriginal cases, a finding consistent with the analysis of Dr. Kathleen Earle of the secondary data from the Department of Health and Human Services archived data at Cornell University, indicating that sexual and physical abuse rates were lower for Indian and Alaskan Native children in the United States (Earle & Cross, 2001).

Community-Based Child Welfare for Aboriginal Children · 111 Table 7.2

Primary Form of Maltreatment, by Aboriginal Status, for Substantiated or Suspected Maltreatment, 1998 CIS" (N = 3,159) Aboriginal

Non-Aboriginal

Other Visible Minority

Physical abuse, punishment

8%

22%

35%

Physical abuse, other

8%

12%

11%

Sexual abuse

5%

10%

5%

41%

17%

19%

7%

5%

4%

11%

9%

9%

Emotional maltreatment

7%

11%

6%

Exposure to domestic violence

9%

14%

13%

Total

614

2,114

431

Failure to supervise child at risk of physical harm Physical neglect (failure to provide adequate physical care) Other neglect (failure to supervise child at risk of sexual abuse, medical neglect, failure to provide treatment, educational neglect, emotional neglect, and abandonment)

a. Unweighted sample, excludes (1) investigations in Quebec (TV = 2,309), where aboriginal and ethnic identity data were not collected; (2) cases of children not residing with a biological parent (N = 225); (3) unsubstantiated investigations (N= 1,969); and (4) other eligible cases where Aboriginal and ethnic identity data were missing (N= 10). Chi square = 244.31; df= 8; ρ < .001.

Child Characteristics Aboriginal children were on average slightly younger (mean age = 7.33 years) than their nonAboriginal counterparts (mean age = 7.93 years; F = 4.38; N

VIII OPERATIONAL SERVICES

VII RECREATIONAL SERVICES

II SOCIAL SERVICES

V

CHILD

/ [ 1

: !

FAMILY

EDUCATIONAL SERVICES

VI VOCATIONAL SERVICES

HEALTH SERVICES

Y v

/

\

S U ABUSE BSTANCE

!

\

SERVICES

Figure 24.1

Systems of Care Framework

of families and youth, and attention to cultural differences. Calls for increased investment of resources in children's mental health services reflect the current reality of insufficient service capacity to meet the needs (Stroul, Pires, & Armstrong, 2001). In identifying needed improvements in services and service systems, many have arrived at similar conclusions to those reached by earlier reformers—that children with emotional disorders and their families need a range of comprehensive, individualized, coordinated services and supports; that all key partners must come together to plan for and deliver these services; that families must be full partners; and that cultural competence in service delivery is critical. These are the fundamental elements of the systems of care concept and philosophy that emerged in the 1980s. This concept continues to offer a framework for system reform in children's mental health, although the field's understanding of the concept and how it is

implemented has changed through ongoing system development activities and will continue to evolve as reforms progress. The term systems of care has been interpreted as referring to a "model," and people have tried to "replicate" it, to "operationalize" it, to measure it, to evaluate it, and to compare it to "traditional" services. In addition, a number of inaccurate interpretations of the meaning of the systems of care concept have emerged over time. Stroul (2002) clarified the concept by emphasizing that first and foremost, systems of care are a range of treatment services and supports guided by a philosophy and supported by an infrastructure. In another effort to clarify the meaning of the systems of care concept, Friedman and Hernandez (2002) recently wrote that developing a system of care is neither a specific nor a simple intervention and that it could be seen as a general statement of "policy" indicating a desire to establish a complex system targeted at a specific

Intercepts of Resilience and Systems of Care · 391 Table 24.2

Mental Health Dimensions

Nonresidential services Prevention Early intervention Assessment Outpatient treatment Home-based services Day treatment Crisis services Residential services Therapeutic foster care Therapeutic group care Therapeutic camp services Independent living services Residential treatment Crisis residential services Inpatient hospitalization Other essential services Case management Respite services School-based mental health services Behavioral aides Mental health consultation

population of children and families based on a widely agreed-on set of principles and values. Hernandez and Hodges (2003) wrote that systems of care may be better thought of as a cluster of organizational change strategies based on a set of values and principles intended to shape policies, regulations, funding mechanisms, services, and supports. These interpretations emphasize the complexity of the system of care concept. Stroul (2002) emphasized that it is essential to recognize that developing a system of care is a multifaceted, multilevel process, involving (a) making changes in state policies, financing mechanisms, training, and other structures and processes to support systems of care; (b) making changes at the local system level needed to plan, implement, manage, and evaluate the system; and (c) making changes at the service delivery level to provide a broad array of effective, stateof-the-art treatment services and supports to children and families in an individualized and coordinated manner.

CONSTRUCT OF RESILIENCE Leaders in the field of resilience research such as Luthar, Cicchetti, and Becker (2000a), Masten (2001), and Rutter (1990) have all proposed that the construct of child resilience includes two essential factors: (a) the presence of serious threats to adaptation or development and (b) the achievement of positive adaptation and good outcomes. The need to use a developmental perspective is a common theme in theoretical frameworks for child resilience. A developmental perspective takes into account the child's developmental level and functioning, the multiple levels of influence on a child's developmental pathways, and reciprocity between the risk and protective factors and the child's adjustment. Resilience in children occurs through normal human adaptive processes, including the development of cognition, regulation of behavior, and interactions with caregivers and the environment (Masten, 2001).

Key Elements Risk Factors As noted above, the concept of resilience includes the judgment that serious threats to child development are present. The phenomenon of resilience emerged from the study of risk factors in disciplines such as epidemiology and developmental psychopathology (Cicchetti & Toth, 1997; Masten, Morison, Pellegrini, & Tellegen, 1990; Rutter, 1990). Risk factors describe those circumstances that increase the likelihood that a child will experience negative outcomes and problem behaviors. There is considerable agreement that risk factors can be found within the child, the family, the neighborhood, and in societal structures. Rutter (1979) conducted a study to identify family risk factors for child psychiatric disorders and found that more than one risk factor needed to be present to affect child outcomes. He also found that the presence of two risk factors resulted in a fourfold increase in risk for psychiatric disorder, and so on. In a study using data from the Ontario Child Health Study, risk factors from the child's environment as well as child attributes classified as protective factors

392 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS were examined to determine which protective factors, holding risk constant, predict absence of child psychiatric disorders (Rae-Grant, Thomas, Offord, & Boyle, 1989). For both children (4- to 11 -year-olds) and adolescents (12- to 16-yearolds), the risk factor with the highest relative odds for presence of a child psychiatric disorder was family problems. The National Institute of Mental Health (Hann & Borek, 2001) recently convened an expert panel to conduct an extensive literature review of family risk factors for children's externalizing behavior problems. The evidence-based, malleable risk factors identified were lower levels of parental engagement, greater use of invalidation, and harsh and inconsistent discipline. Better understanding is needed regarding how risk factors and risk processes operate in person-environment interactions. There is some evidence, for example, that boys are more vulnerable to stressors in the first decade of life; in the second decade, girls are more susceptible to risks; and males are more vulnerable in the third decade (Werner & Smith, 1992). Protective

Factors

The other core characteristic of child resilience is the process that mediates the relationship between stress and competence. There is disagreement regarding the nature of these processes, except for the belief that they are related to the presence of protective factors or mechanisms. There are two basic models to explain the interaction between stressors (i.e., risk factors) and protective factors—the main effect and the buffering effect. Substantial main effects have been found for parenting qualities, intellectual functioning, socioeconomic status, and positive selfperceptions (Masten, 2001). Kirby and Fraser (1997) reviewed three variations of the buffering effect model. First, protective factors may operate as a buffer to risk factors, reducing the possible negative effects of a stressor. Second, protective factors break the cycle of a chain of risk factors. Finally, the presence of a protective factor may prevent the initial onset of risk. Rak and Patterson (1996) reviewed several studies and identified a number of protective factors within the child and the family. At the child level, protective factors included an active

approach to problem solving, the ability from infancy on to gain the positive attention of others, an ability to be alert and autonomous, the tendency to seek out novel experiences, and an optimistic view even in the face of distressing experiences. At the family level, protective factors included the age of the opposite sex parent, consistent nurturing during the first year of life, alternative caretakers who step in when parents are not present, a multi-age network of relatives, the presence of sibling caretakers, and structure and rules during adolescence.

Recent Clarifications Luthar and Zelazo (2003), as well as numerous other authors, point out the ambiguity and lack of clarity regarding the construct of resilience because theorists and researchers use the term in varying ways. Their position is that resilience is a process or phenomenon rather than an individual trait or personal attribute. The belief is that the term should not be used as an adjective describing a person but rather as a description of positive adaptation or child trajectories (Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000b). There are at least two reasons for defining resilience as a process. First, we do not want to "blame the victim," with the victim being either the child or the family, or both. Second, we do not want to infer that some children are unable to overcome adversity.

INTERCEPTS OF THE SYSTEMS OF CARE AND RESILIENCE CONSTRUCTS Given that both constructs developed during the same historical period, it is not surprising that their theoretical foundations and many assumptions are similar. On the other hand, there are a number of differences between the two concepts.

Theoretical Grounding in Systems Theory Based on a review of the seminal literature for both concepts, it is evident that both systems of care and child resilience use social systems theory as their theoretical foundation. Social systems theory recognizes that different parts of

Intercepts of Resilience and Systems of Care · 393 a whole, functioning entity are interrelated and interdependent (Bertalanffy, 1981). Performance of any one part not only affects other parts but also may depend on those parts for its survival (Berrien, 1968; Robbins, Chatterjee, & Canda, 1998). Systems theory encompasses the individual as well as his or her social environment. From a systems perspective, the goal of helping professions is to help individuals perform life tasks, alleviate distress, and achieve aims and positions that are important to them. Systems that may help people are informal or natural systems, formal systems, and social institutions. There are at least two areas where we can identify the influence of systems theory on both systems of care and child resilience. First, both concepts recognize and incorporate the belief that a child lives within and is affected by the social environment. The roles of the family, the community, and social institutions, especially schools, in the promotion of child competence are acknowledged by both constructs. Second, both concepts are premised on the belief that child characteristics and behavior affect the systems in which they are engaged. In other words, in a system, each component can operate as either a dependent or an independent variable. A number of studies have demonstrated that a reciprocal relationship exists between the quality of parenting and a child's personality and behavior (Crockenberg, 1981; Crowley & Kazdin, 1998; Grolnick & Ryan, 1989; Steinberg, Elmen, & Mounts, 1989). Using a system of care framework, McDonald, Grégoire, Poertner and Early (1997) developed a model of caregiving for children with emotional problems. Their research found only three latent variables that made a significant direct contribution to caregiver stress. The latent variable with the highest contribution to caregiver stress was the child's externalizing problem behaviors.

Ecological Approach One type of systems theory, ecology systems theory, constructs the individual system as part of a larger ecological system, with which the individual must negotiate so as to accommodate, adjust, and survive. Ecological systems theory views individuals as constantly adapting in an interchange with many different aspects of their environment. Adaptation is believed to be

reciprocal; people are able to develop through change and are supported by the environment (Payne, 1991). The family ecological model is one theoretical framework used to explain the relationship between families and the contributing factors to their well-being (Bronfenbrenner, 1986). Rather than focusing on intrafamilial processes and characteristics, the focus is on external factors that may contribute to a family's capacity for parenting. Bronfenbrenner identified three external systems that affect the family: mesosystems, exosystems, and chronosystems. A mesosystem, for example, refers to the influences that operate between the primary settings in which child development takes place, such as home and school. It assumes that effects occur in both directions; that is, school affects home, and home affects what occurs in school. Exosystem models identify the influences in settings in which parents function but where children do not enter, such as the parents' work environment. Chronosystem models analyze the dynamic relationships between changes and continuities over time, both within the person and within the environment. Both systems of care and child resilience use an ecological approach. For example, the explicit goal of systems of care is that children will be able to live and function well at home with their family, in school, and their own community (Brannan, Baughman, & Reed, 2002). In addition, systems of care take an ecological approach to child development. A child is viewed ecologically as an individual who interacts with others in a number of life domains. Early research studies on child resilience were focused on the personal characteristics and attributes of children. In the last two decades, we have seen a shift to a more comprehensive focus on three sets of factors: attributes of children, of families, and of wider social environments (Luthar et al., 2000a). Today, there is general agreement that both risk and protective factors and processes can be identified at multiple levels; studies of child resilience identify and examine risk and protective factors and processes in all areas of a child's life (Fraser & Galinksy, 1997; Werner & Smith, 1992; Wyman, Sandler, Wolchik, & Nelson, 2000). Many of the chapters in this volume do likewise.

394 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS

Multilevel Perspective and Causation As is indicated by the term systems of care, inherent in this framework is the recognition that families, communities, and social institutions affect at-risk children both positively and negatively. Values and principles specify that both treatment and support services should be community based, that families should be integrally involved in all aspects of service delivery, and that services should be coordinated among those agencies and systems that share responsibility for children with emotional disorders. Through a review of several research studies, Kirby and Fraser (1997) proposed a multilevel model of common risk and protective factors for many childhood problems. At the system level, common risk factors are poverty; lack of education, employment options, or both; and racial discrimination. Protective factors include opportunities for education, employment, and achievement. At the family and neighborhood level, risk factors include child abuse or neglect, parental conflict (believed to be a barrier to effective parenting), parental mental health and substance abuse problems, and poor parenting skills. Social support for parents and youth, the presence of a caring, supportive adult, positive parent-child relationship, and effective parenting skills are identified as protective factors. At the individual child level, risk factors include biomedical problems and gender (a marker for certain conditions). Individual-level protective factors are easy temperament, self-efficacy, competence in normative roles, self-esteem, and intelligence. The assumption is that individual, family, and neighborhood protective factors and processes can mediate risk factors at any level.

Target Population: Children at Risk Another convergence of both concepts is their roots in child psychopathology, with its focus on children who are at risk for a variety of psychosocial problems. According to Cicchetti and Garmezy (1993), growth of the developmental perspective in child psychopathology led to a growing interest in resilience. There was recognition that an understanding of the mechanisms and processes that lead to positive outcomes in the face of adversity increases

knowledge of both normal and abnormal development (Luthar, Burack, Cicchetti, & Weisz, 1997). The systems of care concept was developed to address the needs of children with serious emotional disturbances and their families, although its application has been expanded to include children at risk.

Strengths-Based Focus Despite the shared roots in psychopathology, a focus on strengths can be found in both concepts. Although not part of the early writings on systems of care, the systems of care values and principles created a service-planning process characterized as "individualized service planning" or a "wraparound approach." In the wraparound process, both the child and the family are active members of the treatment planning team. The first phase of service planning has to do with the identification of both strengths and challenges in the child and family's life. The assumption is that strengths are present in every family and that child and family strengths should be used in the development and implementation of the child's service plan (Burchard, Bruns, & Burchard, 2002). As noted, child resilience likewise assumes the presence of strengths, referred to as protective factors and processes. Several theories have been proposed to explain how protective factors increase resilience in children. Rutter (1987) distinguished between protective factors and protective processes. He defined protective factors as those variables and mechanisms that modify a person's response to a risk situation, and protective processes as successful engagement with risk that involves a change from risk to adaptation. Rutter (1990) identified four mediating mechanisms in protective processes: (a) mechanisms that directly reduce the impact of risk exposure, (b) mediating factors that stop or reduce the impact of risk chains, (c) the development of a child's self-esteem, and (d) turning points and the opening up of new opportunities. More recently, Rutter added four more protective mechanisms: (a) protective processes that reduce sensitivity to risk, (b) an increase of positive chain reactions, (c) compensatory positive experiences that counter the effects of risk, and (d) positive cognitive processing of negative events (Rutter, 1995). A related phenomenon in child resilience is the

Intercepts of Resilience and Systems of Care · 395 ability of some children to actively generate and create experiences that foster competence (Masten et al., 1990). Both Murphy and Moriarty (1976) and Werner (1993) in their landmark studies identified the ability of resilient children to evoke help and positive responses, as well as their capacity to identify resilient caregivers. The shared focus on strengths has parallels with new understandings in related fields that approach human functioning with assumptions of active human agency, prevention, and health promotion (Windle, 1999). In the field of health care, the salutogenic model emphasizes the concept of coherence, a person's belief that the environment is structured and predictable, that resources are available to meet whatever challenges are present, and that the challenges are worthy of engagement (Antonovsky, 1990). This approach to well-being contends that the strength of resistant resources is far more important than strength avoidance. Healing, a related concept from the field of holistic health, is described as a process that makes the patient better able to cope with new situations rather than as simply the return to the state of health previous to the disease (Dubos, 1990).

Challenge of Definitional Ambiguity and Drift Over time, a number of inaccurate interpretations of the meaning of the systems of care concept have emerged. Some of the "myths and misconceptions" about systems of care include the following: • • • • • • •

They are primarily designed to improve service coordination and integration. They do not focus on clinical interventions but mostly focus on system infrastructure. The philosophy is primarily focused on family involvement and cultural competence. They are different from and/or do not involve evidence-based interventions. No "traditional" services are included in them. They primarily involve providing "wraparound" services. They place greater value on nonprofessional service providers and natural supports than on other clinicians, providers, and treatment modalities.

However, systems of care do involve clinical interventions, and they involve "traditional" services such as outpatient, inpatient, and residential treatment, as well as more recently developed service modalities, such as home-based services, therapeutic foster care, multisystemic therapy (MST), intensive case management, and others, many of which have an emerging evidence base from research in community settings (Burns & Hoagwood, 2002; Burns, Hoagwood, & Mrazek, 1999). Wraparound is an approach to planning and providing highly individualized services and supports of all types that is used extensively within systems of care. Using the wraparound approach leads to the development of a comprehensive, holistic, individualized service plan for a child and family that brings to bear all the needed treatment services and supports. The wraparound approach is also an element of the systems of care concept and philosophy. Furthermore, systems of care involve highly trained clinicians of all disciplines, as well as paraprofessionals, families as providers, and other creative staffing strategies to meet different needs. Systems of care involve all these things. Many theorists and researchers have similarly identified issues related to definitional ambiguity regarding child resilience. A recent review identified major areas of concern (Luthar et al., 2000a). First, there is little consensus regarding definitions. For example, the term resilience is used in reference to at least three groups: (a) at-risk children who show better than expected outcomes, (b) youth who maintain positive adaptation despite stressful experiences, and (c) children who demonstrate good recovery from a traumatic event or situation. A second discrepancy is in the conceptualization of resilience as a personal trait versus a dynamic process. In addition, there is little consensus regarding major terms such as protective factors or vulnerability. For example, the term protective factor is used to reference both main effect models and those involving interactive processes.

COMPLEXITY OF THE CONSTRUCTS Research on both systems of care and resilience is challenged methodologically by limitations in the precision of measurement. With respect to

396 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS the study of resilience, Luthar and her colleagues (Luthar et al., 2000a) note that children exposed to various types of adversity are often treated analytically as a homogeneous group. Researchers frequently fail to take into account potentially important aspects of the adversity children experience, such as severity, duration, or protective factors, that might explain variability in child outcomes. Other investigators argue the need for increased complexity. Curtis and Cicchetti (2003), for example, indicate that research on resilience has focused almost exclusively on psychosocial variables as explanatory factors in differential outcomes. They argue that researchers need to include potential biological factors such as neural plasticity as potential predictors of resilience. Similarly, Friedman and Hernandez (2002) note the complexity associated with systems of care studies and the challenge facing evaluators in "determining how to describe and measure it" (p. 68). There are a number of reasons for this. First, as Manteuffel, Stephens, and Santiago (2002) explain, communities develop programs tailored to their needs. Second, studying systems of care is complex because the interventions provided to children served within the same system of care are intended to be individualized (Farmer, 2000). Third, Farmer also noted that different service providers provide interventions to children at different times. Fourth, the children and adolescents served by local systems are as diverse as are the environments in which the systems are located (Friedman & Hernandez, 2002). These multiple sources of variability, many by design, create enormous challenges for researchers attempting to determine which systems of care components are most effective and for whom.

Lack of Definitional Consensus and Clarity Research on both systems of care and resilience share a lack of definitional consensus, clarity, and operationalization related to both the independent and dependent variables frequently examined. Despite the existence of a set of guiding principles (Stroul & Friedman, 1986, 1996), there exists no single set of agreed-on, operationally defined criteria for

determining whether or not a system of care exists. As a result, variability exists regarding the degree to which existing systems of care conform to these guiding principles, which are likely partly responsible for variability in the relative effectiveness of these systems of care. A study conducted by Brannan et al. (2002) found that funded systems of care were more likely to adhere to the guiding principles compared with unfunded child mental health systems. However, they also found meaningful variability across sites in their adherence to these principles as well as principles in which sites' adherence was generally lacking. A challenge to researchers is that "communities develop programs according to their own unique circumstance within the system-of-care framework" (Manteuffel et al., 2002, p. 17). Rosenblatt (1998) characterized this issue as "the problem of the independent variable: what is a system of care?" (p. 330). Similar challenges exist in identifying and measuring the dependent or outcome variables. Because of the varied goals within a systems of care framework, researchers have employed a variety of child-level outcome measures across various domains and using different measures (Rosenblatt, 1998). The domains most relevant for systems of care research are probably best articulated by Hoagwood, Jensen, Petti, and Burns (1996) as (a) child symptoms and diagnoses, (b) child adaptive functioning, (c) consumer perspectives such as satisfaction, (d) environments, and (e) systems outcomes. One approach in systems of care research has been to include a wide variety of measures across these domains in their attempts to assess system effectiveness. A problem with this approach was noted by Boothroyd, Banks, Evans, Greenbaum, and Brown (2004), who indicate that given that these individually tailored interventions have goals for children and families that can focus on a wide range of outcomes such as improving child functioning, improving family communication skills, or increasing the family's social support network, the use of a broad array of outcome measures results in children and families being assessed on a number of measures for which their intervention was not intended to have an impact. This decreases the likelihood that positive changes will be observed for the group.

Intercepts of Resilience and Systems of Care · 397 There exists wide variation in the definitions and operationalization among researchers studying resilience. Researchers focus on various domains to identify children who are resilient or show competence. These domains include areas such as clinical symptoms, academic attainment, and personality traits (Heller, Larrieu, Dimperio, & Boris, 1999). Even when researchers focus within the same domains, the standard used to define resilience often differs. For example, high school graduation might define competence in one study, whereas aboveaverage grades might be the standard in another. The general recommendation is that risk factors and competence should be assessed in similar domains (Luthar & Zelazo, 2003). For example, the resilience or competence of children who are at risk because of factors such as low parental educational attainment levels or having learning disabilities should be examined in terms of some form of academic or school success. In terms of the measurement of risk (and protective) factors, Windle (1999) argued that research on resilience lacks a conceptual framework as various investigators assess risk using a broad range of characteristics spanning from low birth weight to poverty to child maltreatment to psychiatric disorders and substance abuse. In summary, researchers in both systems of care and resilience face challenges in terms of having agreed-on definitions and indicators of the independent variables (i.e., risk and protective factors and system of care) as well as the dependent variables of interest (resilience/ competence and child outcomes).

DIVERGENCE OF THE TWO CONSTRUCTS There are also areas where the two constructs lack agreement with one another and have different foci or areas of emphasis.

System Level Versus Individual Child Level For child resilience, the starting point and focus is children at risk. In this regard, child resilience has stayed close to its roots in developmental psychopathology. For systems of care,

the focus has been, and continues to be, both the child at risk and system-level factors and processes. As noted earlier, the concept of systems of care was developed to address systemic issues and problems, such as insufficient funds and a lack of trained, qualified staff, in efforts to serve children with serious emotional problems and their families. Research on systems of care and research on resilience share a multilevel perspective (i.e., person, family, environment); however, historically, both areas of inquiry evolved into this multilevel perspective from opposite ends of the continuum (Luthar et al., 2000a; Masten etal., 1990; Rosenblatt, 1998; Rosenblatt & Woodbridge, 2003; Werner, 1993; Werner & Smith, 1992). Systems of care research began with various system redesign and integration strategies such as alternative funding strategies, multidisciplinary treatment teams, and crossagency memoranda of understandings (MOUs) (Rosenblatt & Woodbridge, 2002). As noted by Farmer (2000), the early focus of these system redesign strategies was clearly on system-level changes (see, e.g., Behar, 1985; Burchard & Clarke, 1990). Over time, however, the focus shifted in response to increasing demands for individual-level outcomes as measures of system effectiveness (Farmer, 2000). Given this, increased emphasis emerged within systems of care research focused on provider behaviors and child and family outcomes. For example, a recent study of children's in-home emergency psychiatric services that used a systems of care framework identified outcomes at the family level (increases in social supports, caregiver selfefficacy, and family adaptability and cohesion), at the child level (higher self-esteem, higher levels of functioning at home and school), and at the service delivery level (reduced provider burnout, higher levels of provider cultural competence) (Evans, Boothroyd, & Armstrong, 1997). Despite this multilevel research approach, it is unclear whether system-level change is a necessary condition to affect child-level outcomes (Farmer, 2000), although some investigators argue that researchers should focus on individual-level outcomes, examining processes and factors such as therapeutic alliance (Bickman, Noser, & Summerfelt, 1999; Bickman, Smith, Lambert, & Andrade, 2003).

398 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS In contrast, the study of resilience historically began at the individual level—attempting to determine why children exposed to adversity differed with respect to their subsequent "competence" or "resilience" (Luthar etal., 2000a; Masten & Garmezy, 1985; Werner & Smith, 1992). As Luthar et al. (2000a) discuss, over time, researchers expanded their search for explanatory protective factors and mechanisms, moving away from simply examining qualities associated with the child to a broader focus on aspects of their families and the characteristics of the environments in which they resided. For example, a recent study of resilience in children with serious emotional problems focused on the role of parental social supports and their relationship with family wellbeing, quality of parenting, and the child's resilience (Armstrong, 2003). This book, and the questions it raises, takes this exploration even further, looking at broader systems of care, cultural factors, and other aspects of the environment that influence individual outcomes, the traditional focus of resilience research.

Formal Versus Informal Supports and Services Perhaps because the initial target population for systems of care was children and youth with serious emotional disturbances, systems of care tend to emphasize treatment supports and services offered by providers, both agency-based and individual practitioners. Although systems of care opened up the service array from inpatient and outpatient services to a broader array of community-based services and supports, and encouraged in-home and school-based services, the base continues to be human service providers. The introduction of individualized service planning included some emphasis on the use of informal supports, but implementation has been challenging. For example, one of the stated values of the wraparound process is that natural supports should outnumber professionals at service-planning meetings. However, a recent study that included observations of 72 serviceplanning meetings found that about 60% of the team meetings included no natural supports (Walker, Koroloff, & Schutte, 2003). Only 7 meetings had more than one natural support person who attended the planning meeting.

The construct of child resilience, on the other hand, tends to be "blind" to the distinction between formal and informal support services. The schema of risk and protective factors and processes, for example, tends to emphasize characteristics and attributes of the community, the family, and the child and is silent about the role of the formal service delivery network (for more on this, see Barter's and MacDonald, Glode, and Wein's contributions detailing communitybased approaches to child welfare in Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal settings, Chapters 21 and 22, respectively).

Developmental Tasks Versus Here and Now A review of the framing literature for both systems of care and child resilience indicates that the importance of developmental tasks and critical periods in a child's formation is stressed much more in child resilience. The construct of child resilience has its roots in developmental psychopathology, and many early writings on child resilience emphasize the importance of using a developmental perspective for understanding adaptation in children (Cicchetti & Garmezy, 1993; Masten etal., 1990; Sroufe & Rutter, 1984). Child adaptation is viewed within the context of developmental periods. Characteristics of a developmental perspective, including holism, directedness, differentiation, and the coherence of an individual's development, are presented as the framework for understanding child resilience (Sroufe & Rutter, 1984). The developmental perspective is used to explain why resilience is not static and that new vulnerabilities, new strengths, or both may emerge during developmental transitions throughout the life course (Cicchetti & Garmezy, 1993). In a discussion of how to operationalize social competence, Luthar et al. (1997) emphasize that competence equates with behaviors that indicate success at meeting expectations associated with a specific developmental stage.

Action Versus Theory Since its inception, the concept of system of care has been action oriented because it was developed as an organizing framework for the

Intercepts of Resilience and Systems of Care · 399 delivery of children's mental health services. The focus was, and continues to be, on "How?"— how to organize services and supports so that these youth have positive outcomes, are able to live and grow as members of their family and school community, and can become productive young adults who can make a contribution to their community? For child resilience, the operative question has been and continues to be "Why?"—why do some children who experience high levels of trauma overcome the odds?

Family and Youth Involvement One of the guiding principles for systems of care has been that families should be full participants in the planning and delivery of services (Stroul & Friedman, 1986). Child resilience does not address the issue of family or youth partnerships, probably because its focus is not on intervention.

Importance of Culture and Context As noted earlier, the core values of systems of care are that services should be community based, child centered and family focused, and culturally competent. The final value, cultural competence, specifies that agencies and the services they provide, need to be responsive to cultural, racial, and ethnic differences (Stroul & Friedman, 1986). Although implementation has been challenging, the importance of culture in the planning and provision of a service network has been, and continues to be, a consistent theme in the systems of care literature (IsaacsShockley et al., 1996; Stroul & Friedman, 1996). The construct of child resilience has placed less emphasis on culture. For example, research studies typically have not included race and culture as a variable to be examined. Some investigators have begun to more closely examine issues of culture within the context of child resilience (McCubbin, McCubbin, Thompson, & Thompson, 1998). In their chapter in this volume (Chapter 2), McCubbin and McCubbin describe how the resiliency model of family stress, adjustment, and adaptation (McCubbin & McCubbin, 1993) is particularly well suited for identifying and understanding how sociocultural

factors influence children's and families' responses to stressful events. Ungar and his colleagues, in this volume and elsewhere (see Chapter 13; Ungar, Lee, Callaghan, & Boothroyd, in press), describe an international study of resilience among adolescents facing war, violence, cultural disintegration, and structural inequalities across five continents. Central to this study is an innovative approach to crosscultural research in efforts to better understand the phenomenon of resilience.

FUTURE INTERCEPTS Facing the challenge of definitional ambiguity has resulted in the recognition of the complexity of both systems of care and child resilience. Both theoretical frameworks may benefit from embracing the system theory's concept of equifinality, meaning that many trajectories can lead to child competence. There is a growing body of research supporting the premise that child development and well-being depends on reciprocal transactions between the child and his or her environment over time. Researchers in both systems of care and child resilience need to understand the multiple trajectories that can lead to positive outcomes, such as school readiness and successful transition to adulthood. Another potential intercept is between individualized care planning and protective processes. From the child resilience literature, it is now clear that efforts to promote competence in children at risk need to focus on strategies that protect or restore the effective functioning of basic adaptational processes (Masten, 2001). Although one of the assumptions of a wraparound approach is to build on child and family strengths, an additional benefit would be to consciously seek and facilitate protective processes and mechanisms in a child's life, including making use of natural turning points, such as high school graduation or entering middle school, creating new opportunities for a child, and providing compensatory experiences that can counteract previous traumas. In the area of treatment services and supports, a reciprocal transaction could benefit both concepts. Systems of care have tended to place less emphasis on the informal and naturally

400 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS occurring protective factors and mechanisms in a child's life, including natural helpers, turning points, and normal developmental and adaptational processes. Individualized care planning, as well as system-level and organizational-level reforms, could make better use of these concepts. Child resilience, on the other hand, could pay greater attention to the formal service system as a protective factor. The study of child resilience could benefit from embracing the systems of care value placed on culture and context. A family's cultural heritage often includes traditions, values, and beliefs that can play a protective role for children at risk. Cultural institutions, such as churches and social organizations, need to be viewed as protective factors and included in every child's assessment. Both areas of inquiry could profit from applying and sharing advances in multilevel analytic strategies to simultaneously examine person, family, and community influences on the outcomes of children served in systems of care as well as in interventions that promote child competence in response to traumatic events. In this chapter, we have attempted to highlight the founding principles of the systems of care movement in the United States over the past 20 years and to summarize the research base that has emerged. In addition, we presented a brief historical overview of the resilience literature and then contrasted the similarities and differences in the constructs of systems of care and child resilience. We concluded by highlighting the areas in which we believe policymakers, planners, and researchers examining systems of care or child resilience can benefit through increased awareness of each other's work as well as through joint policy, planning, and research efforts.

REFERENCES Antonovsky, A. (1990). The Salutogenic Model of Health. In C. Swencionis (Ed.), The healing brain: A scientific reader (pp. 231-243). New York: Guilford Press. Armstrong, M. I. (2003). An empirical study: A model of the pathways between social support, family wellbeing, parenting quality, and child

resilience. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John's, Newfoundland. Behar, L. (1985). Changing patterns of state responsibility: A case study of North Carolina. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 14(3), 188-195. Berrien, F. K. (1968). General and social systems. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Bertalanffy, L. V. (1981). A systems view of man. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Bickman, L., Noser, K., & Summerfelt, W. T. (1999). Long-term effects of a system of care on children and adolescents. Journal of Behavioral Health Services & Research, 26(2), 185-202. Bickman, L., Smith, C. M., Lambert, E. W., & Andrade, A. R. (2003). Evaluation of a congressionally mandated wraparound demonstration. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 12(2), 135-156. Boothroyd, R. Α., Banks, S. M., Evans, M. E., Greenbaum, P. E., & Brown, E. (2004). Untangling the web: An approach to analyzing the impacts of individually tailored, multi-component treatment interventions. Mental Health Service Research, 6(4), 143-153. Brannan, A. M., Baughman, L. N., & Reed, E. D. (2002). System-of-care assessment: Cross-site comparison of findings. Children's Services: Social Policy, Research, and Practice, 5(1), 37-56. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723-742. Burchard, J. D., Bruns, Ε. J., & Burchard, S. N. (2002). The wraparound approach. In K. Hoagwood (Ed.), Community treatment for youth: Evidence-based interventions for severe emotional and behavioral disorders (pp. 69-90). New York: Oxford University Press. Burchard, J. D., & Clarke, R. T. (1990). The role of individualized care in a service delivery system for children and adolescents with severely maladjusted behavior. Journal of Mental Health Administration, 77(1), 48-60. Burns, B. J., & Hoagwood, K. (2002). Community treatment for youth: Evidence-based interventions for severe emotional and behavioral disorders. New York: Oxford University Press. Burns, B. J., Hoagwood, K., & Mrazek, P. J. (1999). Effective treatment for mental disorders in

Intercepts of Resilience and Systems of Care · 401 children and adolescents. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 2(4), 199-254. Cicchetti, D., & Garmezy, N. (1993). Prospects and promises in the study of resiliency. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 497-502. Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1997). Transactional ecological systems in developmental psychopathology. In J. R. Weisz (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology: Perspectives on adjustment, risk, and disorder (pp. 317-349). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Crockenberg, S. B. (1981). Infant irritability, mother responsiveness, and social support influences on the security of infant-mother attachment. Child Development, 52(3), 857-865. Crowley, M. J., & Kazdin, A. E. (1998). Child psychosocial functioning and parent quality of life among clinically referred children. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 7(2), 233-251. Curtis, W. J., & Cicchetti, D. (2003). Moving research on resilience into the 21st century: Theoretical and methodological considerations in examining the biological contributors to resilience. Development and Psychopathology, 15(3), 773-810. Dubos, R. (1990). Self-healing: A personal history. In C. Swencionis (Ed.), The healing brain: A scientific reader (pp. 135-146). New York: Guilford Press. Evans, M. E., Boothroyd, R. Α., & Armstrong, M. I. (1997). Development and implementation of an experimental study of the effectiveness of intensive in-home crisis services for children and their families. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 5(2), 93-105. Farmer, Ε. M. Z. (2000). Issues confronting effective services in systems of care. Children and Youth Services Review, 22(8), 627-650. Fraser, M. W., & Galinksy, M. J. (1997). Toward a resilience-based model of practice. In M. W. Fraser (Ed.), Risk and resilience in childhood (pp. 265-276). Washington, DC: NASW Press. Friedman, R. M., & Hernandez, M. (2002). The national evaluation of the Comprehensive Community Mental Health Services for Children and Their Families Program: A commentary. Children's Services: Social Policy, Research, and Practice, 5(1), 67-74. Friesen, B. J., & Huff, B. (1996). Family perspectives on systems of care. In B. A. Stroul (Ed.), Children's mental health. Creating systems of

care in a changing society (pp. 41-68). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles associated with children's self-regulation and competence in school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(2), 143-154. Hann, D. M., & Borek, N. (Eds.). (2001). Taking stock ofriskfactors for child/youth externalizing behavior problems. Rockville, MD: Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Mental Health. Heller, S. S., Larrieu, J. Α., Dimperio, R., & Boris, N. W. (1999). Research on resilience to child maltreatment: Empirical considerations. Child Abuse & Neglect, 23(A), 321-338. Hernandez, M., & Hodges, S. (2003). Building upon the theory of change for systems of care. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 11(1), 19-26. Hoagwood, K, Jensen, P. S., Petti, T., & Burns, B. J. (1996). Outcomes of mental health care for children and adolescents: A comprehensive conceptual model. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(8), 1055-1063. Isaacs-Shockley, M., Cross, T., Bazron, B., Dennis, K, & Benjamin, M. (1996). Framework for a culturally competent system of care. In B. A. Stroul (Ed.), Children's mental health: Creating system of care in a changing society (pp. 23^10). Baltimore: Brookes. Kirby, L. D., & Fraser, M. W. (1997). Risk and resilience in childhood. In M. W. Fraser (Ed.), Risk and resilience in childhood: An ecological perspective (pp. 10-33). Washington, DC: NASW Press. Luthar, S. S., Burack, J. Α., Cicchetti, D., & Weisz, J. R. (1997). Preface. In J. R. Weisz (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology: Perspectives on adjustment, risk, and disorder (pp. xv-xxi). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000a). The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 72(3), 543-562. Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000b). Research on resilience: Response to commentaries. Child Development, 71(3), 573-575. Luthar, S. S., & Zelazo, L. B. (2003). Research on resilience: An integrative review. In S. S. Luthar

402 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS (Ed.), Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the context of childhood adversities (pp. 510550). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Manteuffel, B., Stephens, R. L., & Santiago, R. (2002). Overview of the national evaluation of the Comprehensive Community Mental Health Services for Children and Their Families program and current findings. Children's Services: Social Policy, Research, and Practice, 5(1), 3-20. Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American Psychologist, 56(3), 227-238. Masten, A. S., & Garmezy, N. (1985). Risk, vulnerability, and protective factors in developmental psychopathology. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Advances in clinical child psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 1-52). New York: Plenum Press. Masten, A. S., Morison, P., Pellegrini, D., & Tellegen, A. (1990). Competence under stress: Risk and protective factors. In S. Weintraub (Ed.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 236-256). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. McCubbin, H. I., McCubbin, Μ. Α., Thompson, A. I., & Thompson, E. A. (1998). Resiliency in ethnic families: A conceptual model for predicting family adjustment and adaptation. In I. Hamilton & M. McCubbin (Eds.), Resiliency in Native American and immigrant families (pp. 3-47). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McCubbin, Μ. A„ & McCubbin, Η. I. (1993). Families coping with illness: The resiliency model of family stress, adjustment and adaptation. In C. Danielson, B. Hamel-Bissel, & P. Winstead-Fry (Eds.), Families, health and illness (pp. 21-63). New York: Mosby. McDonald, T. P., Grégoire, T. Κ., Poertner, J., & Early, T. J. (1997). Building a model of family caregiving for children with emotional disorders. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 5(3), 138-148. Murphy, L. B., & Moriarty, A. E. (1976). Vulnerability, coping, and growth from infancy to adolescence. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. New Freedom Commission on Mental Health. (2003). Achieving the promise: Transforming mental health care in America (Publication No. SMA-03-3831). Rockville, MD: Department of Health and Human Services.

Payne, M. S. (1991). Systems and ecological perspectives. In M. S. Payne (Ed.), Modern social work theory: A critical introduction (pp. 137-156). Chicago: Lyceum Books. President's Commission on Mental Health. (1978). Report of the sub-task panel on infants, children and adolescents. Washington, DC: Author. Rae-Grant, N., Thomas, B. H., Offord, D. R., & Boyle, M. H. (1989). Risk, protective factors, and the prevalence of behavioral and emotional disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(2), 262-268. Rak, C. E, & Patterson, L. E. (1996). Promoting resilience in at-risk children. Journal of Counseling and Development, 74(4), 207-221. Robbins, S. P., Chatterjee, P., & Canda, E. R. (1998). Systems theory. In Contemporary human behavior theory: A critical perspective for social work. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Rosenblatt, A. (1998). Assessing the child and family outcomes of systems of care for youth with severe emotional disturbance. In A. Duchnowski (Ed.), Outcomes for children and youth with emotional and behavioral disorders and their families (pp. 329-362). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Rosenblatt, Α., & Woodbridge, M. W. (2003). Deconstructing research on systems of care for youth with EBD. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 77(1), 27-37. Rutter, M. (1979). Protective factors in children's responses to stress and disadvantage. In M. W. Kent & E. Rolf (Ed.), Primary prevention of psychopathology: Vol. 3. Social competence in children (pp. 49-74). Hanover, NH: University Press of New England. Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57(3), 316-331. Rutter, M. (1990). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. In S. Weintraub (Ed.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 181-214). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Rutter, M. (1995). Psychosocial adversity: Risk, resilience and recovery. South African Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 7(2), 75-88. Sroufe, L. Α., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55(1), 17-29.

Intercepts of Resilience and Systems of Care · 403 Steinberg, L., Elmen, J. D., & Mounts, N. S. (1989). Authoritative parenting, psychosocial maturity, and academic success among adolescents. Child Development, 60(6), 1424-1436. Stroul, B. A. (2002). Issue brief—System of care: A framework for system reform in children's mental health. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Child Development Center, National Technical Assistance Center for Children's Mental Health. Stroul, Β. Α., & Friedman, R. M. (1986). A system of care for severely emotionally disturbed children and youth. Washington, DC: Georgetown University, CASSP Technical Assistance Center. Stroul, Β. Α., & Friedman, R. (1996). The system of care concept and philosophy. In B. A. Stroul (Ed.), Children's mental health: Creating systems of care in a changing society (pp. 591-612). Baltimore: Brookes. Stroul, Β. Α., Pires, S. Α., & Armstrong, Μ. I. (2001). Health care reform tracking project: Tracking state health care reforms as they affect children and adolescents with behavioral health disorders and their families—2000 State Survey (No. 183). Tampa: Research and Training Center for Children's Mental Health, Department of Child and Family Studies, Division of State and Local Support, Louis de la Parte Florida Mental Health Institute, University of South Florida. Ungar, M., Lee, A. W, Callaghan, T, & Boothroyd, R. A. (in press). An international collaboration to study resilience in adolescents across cultures. Journal of Social Work Research and Evaluation. U.S. Congress, Office of Technology Assessment. (1986). Children's mental health: Problems and

services: A background paper. Washington, DC: Author. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2001). Mental health: Culture, race, and ethnicity—A supplement to Mental Health: A report of the Surgeon General. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Office, Office of the Surgeon General. U.S. Public Health Service. (2000). Report of the Surgeon General's Conference on children's mental health: A national action agenda. Washington, DC: Author. Walker, J. S., Koroloff, N., & Schutte, K. (2003). Implementing high-quality collaborative individualized service/support planning: Necessary conditions. Portland, OR: Portland State University, Research and Training Center on Family Support and Children's Mental Health. Werner, Ε. E. (1993). Risk, resilience, and recovery: Perspectives from the Kauai Longitudinal Study. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 503-515. Werner, Ε. E., & Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Windle, M. (1999). Critical conceptual and measurement issues in the study of resilience. In J. L. Johnson (Ed.), Resilience and development: Positive life adaptations (pp. 161-176). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Wyman, P. Α., Sandler, I. N., Wolchik, S., & Nelson, K. (2000). Resilience as cumulative competence promotion and stress protection: Theory and intervention. In R. P. Weissberg (Ed.), The promotion of wellness in children and adolescents (pp. 133-184). Washington, DC: CWLA Press.

25 YOUTH CIVIC ENGAGEMENT Promise and Peril SCOT EVANS ISAAC PRILLELTENSKY

R

ecent interest in youth civic engagement (YCE) parallels the growing attention being paid to social capital (Putnam, 2000; Putnam & Feldstein, 2003). Like social capital, YCE poses promises as well as perils. The promise of YCE lies in enhanced wellness for participating youth, for their interpersonal connections, and for the community at large. The perils lie in accentuating the virtues of participation at the expense of changing structural inequalities and power differentials that ultimately undermine the goods associated with democratic participation (Nelson & Prilleltensky, 2004). These threats may prove unfounded if YCE addresses inequality, injustice, and power differentials. However, if YCE is primarily about supporting the structures that uphold the status quo, we should proceed with caution. Participation can easily create an impression of progress, when in fact such engagement may reinforce only models of charity as opposed to models of justice. Such is the case with many social capital efforts that have proliferated in response to Putnam's calls to revive community (Putnam, 2000; Putnam & Feldstein, 2003). In a thinly veiled

cautionary note, Muntaner, Lynch, and Davey Smith (2003) have characterized the flock around social capital as "Communitarians of the world unite! ignoring the class, gender, and race structure" (p. 292). Our condemnation may be premature. After all, it can be argued that before youth can engage in any kind of social justice, they have to learn how to participate. Young people first need to experience what it feels like to move beyond the negative roles of "clients" or "adultsin-waiting" into the empowering roles of participants and valued "community members" (Ungar, Langlois, & Hum, 2004). Perhaps, it can be argued, what we are witnessing today is the first developmental phase of a long process designed to engage youth in civic action. However, before we render a judgment on the promises and perils of civic engagement, we first offer criteria for what might constitute a positive or negative outcome from YCE. Our criteria are based on the achievement of two desirable outcomes: wellness and resilience. We then continue with a three-part argument to demonstrate the advantages and disadvantages 405

406 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS of youth becoming involved in their communities. In the first part of this chapter, we show that as youth master the skills of participation, they are able to move beyond the reinforcement of unjust social structures. In the second part, we examine the strengths of a YCE approach. Then in the third part, we turn our attention to shortcomings of the YCE movement to address injustice and power inequalities. As we will show, although engagement is a part of wellness and resilience leading to social justice, YCE is not isomorphic with positive outcomes. History is replete with cases of people becoming engaged in social movements that support discrimination and exclusion of the "other."

WELLNESS AND RESILIENCE Wellness is a positive state of affairs, brought about by the simultaneous and balanced satisfaction of personal, relational, and collective needs. Cowen (1991, 1994, 1996), a leading theorist of wellness, defined the construct as the positive end of a hypothetical adjustment continuum an ideal we should strive continually to approach. . . . Key pathways to wellness, for all of us, start with the crucial needs to form wholesome attachments and acquire age-appropriate competencies in early childhood. Those steps, vital in their own right, also lay down a base for the good, or not so good, outcomes that follow. Other cornerstones of a wellness approach include engineering settings and environments that facilitate adaptation, fostering autonomy, support and empowerment, and promoting skills needed to cope effectively with stress. (Cowen, 1996, p. 246) Although Cowen asserts that health and wellness derive from multiple sources, internal and external to the child, including opportunities for empowerment, his definition is psychocentric in its focus on the individual and family. A broader view of health has been proposed in the Canadian federal government report Mental Health for Canadians: Striking a Balance (Epp, 1988). According to the Epp report, health involves not only individual well-being but equality and social justice as well. We concur with Wiley and Rappaport (2000), who argue

that neither wellness nor resilience can be explained in the absence of a power analysis. Wellness emerges from the synergistic interaction of multiple forces that affect how needs at three levels are met (Nelson & Prilleltensky, 2004; Prilleltensky & Nelson, 2002; Prilleltensky, Nelson, & Peirson, 2001a, 2001b). At the personal level, individuals have to meet needs for a sense of control, hope, optimism, physical and psychological growth, stimulation, health, meaning, and spirituality. At the next level, relationships, individuals need to satisfy requirements for mutual respect, appreciation for diversity, caring, and compassion. Finally, at the third level, communities seek to promote a fair and equitable distribution of power and resources, democratic means to make decisions, adequate access to health services, decent housing and employment, a clean environment, accessible transportation, and food security (Nelson & Prilleltensky, 2004; Prilleltensky & Nelson, 2002). Although not an exhaustive list, combined, these needs represent some of the basic requirements of wellness. Maximal wellness may be said to occur when both individuals and their communities as a whole benefit from the satisfaction of their needs at all levels. For example, as a private citizen, the resident of a community derives tangible benefits from access to universal health care, high-quality schools, and safe communities. Communities, as collective entities, benefit from institutions that promote participation, employment, and health and from individuals who support these healthenhancing entities (Nelson & Prilleltensky, 2004; Putnam, 2000). But how do we meet these needs? The fulfillment of needs depends on individual and collective values, resources, programs, and policies. Values are primordial because they determine priorities for the generation and distribution of resources, programs, and policies. Parallel to these three levels of wellness, values may be organized along a continuum that ranges from the personal to the collective (Prilleltensky & Nelson, 2002). At one end, people require freedom and self-determination to exercise control over their lives. In the middle of the range, people require respect, participation, and a sense of community. This is reflected in the values of solidarity and fraternity. At the collective

Youth Civic Engagement: Promise and Peril · 407 end, communities that promote justice, fairness, and equality are those most likely to meet the health needs of their members. Individual and collective values serve people best when they achieve equilibrium. Too much emphasis on self-determination diminishes fraternity and solidarity, whereas too much emphasis on the collective thwarts individual uniqueness. A delicate balance is required among values for personal, relational, and collective wellness. Resources, which translate into programs and policies such as day care, health care, unemployment insurance, and public education, are often determined by the values of the dominant class. When self-determination is heralded as the ultimate value and individualism reigns in resource distribution, policies typically emphasize the need for people to solve their own problems, in large disregard for the social conditions that lead to problems in the first place. In contrast, when the collective is privileged over all other values, personal sacrifice is bound to ensue (Prilleltensky, 1997, 2001). Unlike wellness, which is a satisfactory state of affairs, resilience is associated with the ability to cope under adverse circumstances. This is the case when needs at one or more levels of wellness are inadequately met. In such situations, the individual, family, or community is called on to cope under less than ideal circumstances. As we note below, various coping and compensating mechanisms have been shown to support processes and outcomes of resilience. Resilience is not a personality characteristic, nor is it a static or permanent state of affairs; rather, it is a dynamic process, associated with—but not identical to—personality features. Furthermore, as this volume makes abundantly clear, it is a quality found at all levels of analysis, from the personal to the relational to the collective. Resilience and wellness are theoretically linked, but distinct. Under conditions of adversity, resilience must precede the promotion of wellness. Under optimal circumstances, health and wellness are more readily achieved. But wellness, as much as resilience, relies on values, resources, programs, and policies influenced by dynamics of power. Those in power usually impose their values and will onto the people, determining priorities that suit their particular interests. Values do not exist in a political vacuum, nor do

organizations that support children and youth. It is our contention, therefore, that neither concept, wellness or resilience, can be fully grasped without accounting for power differentials.

BENEFITS OF YOUTH CIVIC ENGAGEMENT FOR PERSONAL WELLNESS One of the important developmental and protective factors to consider related to YCE is selfefficacy—the perception that one can achieve desired goals through one's action (Bandura, 1989). To foster development, youth need opportunities to be efficacious and to make a difference. They have a need for "mattering" (Eccles & Gootman, 2002). Opportunities to do things that make a real difference build self-efficacy. Like Cowen (1996), we believe that wellness and self-efficacy hinge on experiences and feelings that promote competence and skills. Children and youth can develop mastery and a sense of control in family, school, and community settings. As they mature into youth and young adulthood, they may also expand their competencies through participation in social and civic affairs (Pancer & Pratt, 1999). It is important to young people's definition of self as resilient to experience opportunities that not only enhance their personal skills and competencies but also their political competence (Ungar, 2004). Competencies and self-efficacy develop through participation in different settings, especially when children and youth have a voice in the governance of those settings and can influence decision-making processes. As personal and political competence increase, so too does their sense of control over the institutions and structures in which they find themselves. Although many settings provide children and youth with opportunities for participation, opportunities that develop political competence, power, and self-determination are often limited. Community participation and prosocial activities also appear to offer young people valuable opportunities to work on important developmental tasks. Researchers have documented how opportunities for meaningful involvement contribute to the development of protective factors (Benson, 1997; Catalano, Berglund, Ryan, Lonczak, & Hawkins, 2002; Lerner, 2004; Lerner

408 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS & Benson, 2003; Scales & Leffert, 1999). With sufficient opportunities for involvement in meaningful activities, and adequate supports, young people may overcome negative experiences and even thrive. Ungar (2004), in his research with high-risk teens, found that "experiences that enhance capacities, promote selfdetermination, increase participation, and distribute power and justice" (p. 285) have the potential to promote wellness-enhancing alternate discourses youth associate with experiences of resilience. Ongoing exposure to positive experiences, settings, and people enhances the acquisition of assets that youth assert they need for healthy development. Adolescents who spend time in communities that offer rich developmental opportunities experience less risk and show evidence of higher rates of positive development in a variety of domains, including school achievement, employment, family life, relationships, and life satisfaction in general (Eccles & Gootman, 2002; Lerner, 2004). As young people enter adolescence, they have a need to experience control over events and a say in decisions that affect their lives. Evidence suggests that a strong sense of self, confidence in their ability to cope with challenges, and experiences of task completion are all protective factors (Rutter, 1987). Mastery over a difficult situation reinforces efforts to take action, which, in turn, precipitates positive chain reactions, including social acceptance. Acceptance, in turn, leads to new opportunities and expanded roles within the community (Bandura, 1989; Rutter, 1987; Wyman, Sandler, Wolchik, & Nelson, 2000). Evans (2004) witnessed just such a positive chain reaction as it was unfolding. In a study of youth empowerment, he documents the story of a group of young people who, after learning how much check-cashing outlets in their neighborhood were charging their customers, was eager to spread the information among members of their community. These teenagers from lowincome families researched neighborhood economic issues as part of a summer youth civic engagement program. The youth reported feeling empowered (and energized) by the information they gained. When they shared the information with others, they were treated as experts, which

in turn contributed to their self-efficacy. After the first public presentation of the results of the research the youth themselves had conducted, they were asked to present to other groups, businesses, and organizations from their community and beyond. One group participant described the experience as follows: At first I got up there and was thinking that they weren't going to be interested, we're a bunch of kids that don't really know what adults have to go through. People were really listening and saying yeah, that's true. They were really listening to us and saying like "Wow that's wild 313% [interest rate on money paid]. I can't believe that these youth really know stuff like that; they know more than I know." They were really asking us questions. Man that's really touching people. (Evans, in press) These young people have been rewarded for their work. They have been invited to share their knowledge across their community, to lead a class at their school on the subject, and to present their material to a national youth organization in the nation's capital. Opportunities like this, that stretch and challenge youth with demanding tasks, have been shown to protect against current and future adversity (see Masten, Best, & Garmezy, 1990; Prilleltensky etal., 2001b; Rutter, 1987; Werner, 1995). In addition to skills, control, and self-efficacy, voice and decision-making power are also correlated with positive developmental outcomes. Voice and choice define our sense of agency and contribute to positive psychosocial development. Simply put, young people feel important and part of something bigger than themselves—part of a community. Young people speak of embracing opportunities to contribute in a variety of settings. These opportunities, it seems, appear to be reinforcing. The more youth experience opportunities to have a voice, the more they find their voice and want to contribute. They also begin to see how much value their voice can have for their community (Catalano et al., 2002). One young person described how it felt to be included in organizational decisions as follows: It makes you feel like a person, like you're an equal. Teens can have a good idea or an opinion

Youth Civic Engagement: Promise and Peril · 409 and it's important. Adults need to know. It makes you feel important to know that you can have a say. It's important to have a say so cause we are the one's who will be in charge soon. (Evans, in press). Accounts of youth experiences like this demonstrate that power and control are determinants of voice and choice. At times, power and control are accomplished through collaborative means, whereas at other times, they are achieved through conflict. In either case, they support voice and choice (Prilleltensky et al., 2001a, 2001b). Intellectual, social, and cognitive skills are also correlated with resilience (Garmezy, 1985; Masten etal., 1990; Werner, 1995). Handling complex interpersonal and social situations requires thinking on different levels. Developing these capacities is a gradual and ongoing process that requires extensive experience and exposure to community conflict (Clark, 1988; Keating, 1990). Unfortunately, we often expect young people to develop these competencies without providing in vivo opportunities in a supportive climate. Studies support the notion that opportunities for participation and problemsolving promote responsibility and lead to positive developmental outcomes for young people (Catalano etal., 2002; Rutter, 1987). Experiences of this sort foster empowerment, perceptions of control, and self-efficacy (Lord & Hutchison, 1993; Prilleltensky et al., 2001ba; Rutter, 1987). Empowering opportunities often grow from voluntary structured activities and service to others. Studies indicate that participation in well-designed activities during nonschool time is associated with development of positive identity, increased initiative, positive relationships with diverse peers and adults, better school achievement, reduced rates of dropping out of school, reduced delinquency, and more positive outcomes in adulthood (Barber, Eccles, & Stone, 2001; Youniss & Yates, 1997; Youniss, Yates, & Su, 1997). Werner and Smith (1992) found that participants in their study of a birth cohort of children born on the Hawaiian Island of Kauai in 1955 who were the most resilient as adults tended to have taken on various kinds of helping responsibilities as adolescents, whether this

was paid work or a more informal contribution such as caring for ailing family members. As in Werner and Smith's (1992) study, community service, volunteering, and service learning have all been associated directly or indirectly with a wide range of positive developmental outcomes (see Scales & Leffert, 1999, for a thorough review of this literature). The learning benefits and potential positive outcomes are magnified when the activities take place in quality settings and when an intentional reflective component is built into the structure of the experience (Eyler & Giles, 1999). Furthermore, there is some evidence to suggest that young people can benefit by looking more critically at the broader society and at the barriers facing their families and communities in regard to control over the services they require (LewisCharp, Yu, Soukamneuth, & Lacoe, 2003). Therefore, as we would expect, youth organizing and civic activism offer new ways of working with young people. Young people benefit by learning how to participate in a group action process, build consensus, and set aside personal interests to consider those of the collective (LewisCharp et al., 2003). With this approach, youth are assets and agents capable of transforming their toxic environments, not simply individuals who need to develop resilience and resistance (Ginwright & James, 2002).

BENEFITS OF YOUTH CIVIC ENGAGEMENT FOR COLLECTIVE WELLNESS Adolescents are potential agents of change in their own lives and in their communities. Through meaningful civic and political involvement, young people can develop the skills and capacities that foster resilience and help transform communities at the same time. Youth can play important roles in educating, organizing, and taking action on issues of social justice. Families, schools, neighborhoods, and community- and faith-based organizations can facilitate youth and community development by creating opportunities for teenagers to play meaningful roles, influence decisions, help others, and partner with adults in addressing the root causes of suffering in their communities (Lerner, 2004).

410 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS For example, John (2003) describes the role of youth in establishing the Devon Youth Council in England. Among other things, the council was charged with promoting the UN Convention on the Rights of the Child. This successful initiative led to the establishment of similar councils throughout England and other countries. In related work, John (2003) reports on the remarkable efforts of the Children's Parliament in rural Rajasthan, India. With help from a social work institute, the parliament was set up to influence government policies affecting children, from literacy to the hiring and firing of teachers, to access to potable water. The many contributions reported by John make it abundantly clear that children can readily surpass our current expectations of them. Another notable contribution of a youth movement is the work of Free the Children. Started by Craig Kielburger, a Canadian teen in the early 1990s, Free the Children seeks to liberate young laborers from bondage in India. Since its inception, however, its mandate has grown to include the construction of schools in developing nations and other humanitarian projects. Completely run by young people, this organization is another exemplar of what youth can accomplish and contribute to the community at large. In the United States, a recent study of marginalized youth engaged in civic activism by Lewis-Charp and colleagues (2003) witnessed the impact youth organizing groups can have on their communities. In one case example, youth organizing groups helped to close down a cement plant that was a problem for those living nearby, created a recreational skate park for teens, and initiated the creation of a sexual discrimination policy for their school district. One of these local groups was also able to secure funds for a cleanup of the Bronx River and for the development of the Bronx greenway. As these examples illustrate, young people, if given the opportunity and support, can help change communities. These examples also demonstrate that youth can play meaningful leadership roles at all levels of their community, from families to schools and civic organizations. If given active roles on committees, governing boards, and other decision-making bodies, young people can learn how to work effectively, take responsibility for

important decisions, and find their voice and power. Through participation in social and civic affairs, young people have an opportunity to develop and expand their competencies (Pancer & Pratt, 1999). Youth who are involved in these institutions are not only less likely to violate social norms but also more likely to reinforce community norms through their participation (Youniss et al., 1997).

CHALLENGES TO MEANINGFUL PARTICIPATION A major barrier to the healthy development of young people is the absence of opportunities to participate meaningfully in the contexts that affect their lives. This is especially the case for disadvantaged and marginalized youth—those who have most to gain from participation. Just as young people are becoming ready and able to contribute to community, they are being denied the opportunities and supports they need for full participation (Ginwright & James, 2002). Well-meaning attempts often relegate youth to token participants, with no power and no preparation for their role as decision makers. For example, we are aware of a school board charter that was recently changed to create two positions for high school students on the board. These positions, however, did not come with voting privileges. Students can have a say but have no power to influence decisions. Other organizations are also creating slots on governing councils for young people but often neglect to prepare them to serve effectively in these roles. The culture of these bodies and the structure of the meetings remain largely adult centered. In addition, meetings are held at locations and times inconvenient for youth. Furthermore, the content and format of meetings is not adjusted to meet the needs of youth participants. Organizations that want increased youth participation in decision making must be willing to alter their processes so that youth can play an authentic role (Lewis-Charp et al., 2003). If our aim is to promote youth engagement in civic matters, for their personal development and for community well-being, we must do more to better facilitate full participation by youth.

Youth Civic Engagement: Promise and Peril · 411 NEGATIVE ENGAGEMENT Although we have been discussing youth participation as a desirable event in young people's lives, many risks are faced by youth who join fanatical groups that meet their needs for belonging through indoctrination into belief systems that promote hatred. History is replete with examples of young people joining fascist groups or religious extremists bent on ethnic cleansing. The bonding created by such organizations decimates any shred of possible bridging across ethnic, religious, or sexually diverse groups. We raise this caution because we do not wish to romanticize involvement in civic engagement. Like other associations, engagement may establish negative dynamics that can potentially damage self-esteem and exclude young people from full participation. Joining a civic association is only the first step. What happens once a young person has joined depends on many factors, including leadership, sense of community, and social support. In Bowling Alone, Putnam (2000) describes the perils of people bonding together to create a sense of belonging at the expense of providing bridging opportunities for others to join in. Although a sense of cohesion is a desirable quality in communities, it can breed exclusion. When exclusion is combined with intolerance, dangerous outcomes are possible, as in discrimination, oppression, exploitation, and in more extreme instances, extermination. These are not exaggerated claims. The many ethnic wars that have taken place over the last century and that continue today prove that this is no idle threat. The ultimate question is, engagement for what? In the absence of freedom, respect for diversity, equality, and justice, the bonding generated by civic associations may lead to dogmatism, racism, sexism, and xenophobia.

CHALLENGES TO THE IDEALISM OF Y C E As noted in the introduction to this chapter, the promise of YCE is threatened by the peril of idealism. Unless the engagement we promote for youth includes a critical analysis of the power dynamics that exclude them from full

participation, the peril may outweigh the promise. We should remember that many of the injustices perpetrated against the poor and the marginalized are carried out by the very institutions we want youth to join: schools, local governments, and social services. How can we ensure that the type of engagement we foster in youth is different from mere reinforcement of the status quo? These doubts should cause us to pause (Nelson & Prilleltensky, 2004). Unless engagement is accompanied by social change, YCE, as much as social capital, can limit its contribution to the promotion of person-centered capacities that are ultimately undermined by the presence of overwhelming environmental and social problems facing youth. In their extensive review of programs for positive youth development, Catalano et al. (2002) recognize an extensive list of positive personal, relational, and collective outcomes. However, there is no mention of injustice, inequality, or power differentials. Most of the outcomes may be safely designated as "apolitical." Of 25 evaluated programs with strong research designs, nineteen effective programs showed positive changes in youth behavior, including significant improvements in interpersonal skills, quality of peer and adult relationships, self-control, problem solving, cognitive competencies, self-efficacy, commitment to schooling, and academic achievement. Twenty-four effective programs showed significant improvements in problem behaviors, including drug and alcohol use, school misbehavior, aggressive behavior, violence, truancy, high risk sexual behavior, and smoking. (Catalano et al., 2002, Executive Summary section, final para.) As can be seen, most of the positive outcomes reported deal with personal and interpersonal skills, none of which call for critical thinking or sociopolitical development. This is not surprising, given that by definition, positive youth development programs have been characterized by the following features: 1. Promote bonding 2. Foster resilience 3. Promote social competence

412 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS 4. Promote emotional competence 5. Promote cognitive competence 6. Promote behavioral competence 7. Promote moral competence 8. Foster self-determination 9. Foster spirituality 10. Foster self-efficacy 11. Foster clear and positive identity 12. Foster belief in the future 13. Provide recognition for positive behavior 14. Provide opportunities for prosocial involvement 15. Foster prosocial norms (Catalano et al., 2002) Although the last two features address social norms, the scope of the programs reviewed by Catalano and his colleagues is both narrow in its focus and decidedly apolitical.

CONCLUSION There is some evidence that civic activism can be a pathway to well-being and resilience for youth and for communities (Ginwright & James, 2002; John, 2003; Lerner, 2004; Lewis-Charp et al., 2003; Morsillo & Prilleltensky, in press). However, much more attention has been paid to the personal skills that accrue through YCE. Opportunities for participation also hold the potential to contribute to community well-being in three primary ways: (a) analyzing power in social relationships, (b) promoting social change, and (c) acting collectively (Ginwright & James, 2002). Traditional after-school and youth development organizations and programs can better foster both individual youth and community wellness by shifting their activities toward youth organizing and civic activism. This shift will not be easy however. There are powerful internal and external barriers for organizations to do this. Many adults lack the critical awareness of how social and political factors influence positive outcomes and regard political activism as incongruous with organizational purpose.

How do we enable positive individual and community outcomes, and how do we thwart negative ones? How do we prevent co-optation of YCE, and how do we merge the lessons of participation with the insights of injustice? Efforts are under way to merge hitherto fragmented roles: the helping role with the change agent role, the ameliorative role with the transformative role, the clinical role with the community builder role, and the caring role with the justice role (Prilleltensky & Nelson, 2002; Prilleltensky & Prilleltensky, 2003). For as long as we envision the contribution of YCE as merely ameliorative, enhancing personal capacities without linking competencies to social justice, YCE will fare no better than many programs that limit their contributions to person-centered outcomes. Arguably, resilience, like health, is not a strictly individual experience but one that depends on social and political structures to be achieved. Territorialism, parochialism, and acquired ignorance have prevented the creation of new roles for helpers and youth workers that might address this more politicized understanding of power and participation. It is unacceptable to delegate social change to politicians alone who, in the United States, have been unable to provide universal health care and ratify the UN Convention on the Rights of the Child; in Australia and Canada, have been unable to improve the quality of life for Aboriginal people; and who the world over, ignore the plight of youth and the poor. Adults working with youth, and youth themselves, need to join hands in learning how to address personal, relational, and collective wellness at the same time. Splitting roles into "fixers" and "changers" is inconceivable. Humans and societies require integrative roles for citizens. It is a chimera to believe that once adults and youth "put their house in order" they will be in a position to contribute to the common good. The common good cannot wait. In fact, part of putting their own house in order is to enhance collective well-being. The personal good is inextricably tied to the common good. YCE cannot afford to go the path of personal skills only. Personal and relational wellness are essential but insufficient parts of overall health. Without collective wellness, and without YCE actively contributing to it, personal and

Youth Civic Engagement: Promise and Peril · 413 relational wellness are bound to suffer. Youth workers and allied professionals have a responsibility to merge strategies for personal and collective well-being. It is interesting to note the stark contrast between multiple personal outcomes and scant community outcomes in youth programs (Catalano et al., 2002; Lerner, 2004). This discrepancy reflects the very strategies and aims of initiatives targeting youth. Judging from the available evidence, most youth programs designed to improve positive and civic development concentrate on personal, cognitive, and social skills to the detriment of political understanding of the conditions that lead to youth exclusion, discrimination, and poverty. Most programs reviewed by Lerner (2004) and Catalano et al. (2002) look remarkably didactic, person-centered, and wedded to charity models of well-being. Few programs strive to challenge the status quo (Morsillo & Prilleltensky, in press) or address injustice. In our programs and in our general interactions with youth, it seems clear that the aim should be to support young people in building capacities and to create opportunities for youth to work alongside adults to address harmful conditions. In so doing, we simultaneously foster resilience and promote human and community development by equipping youth with skills and by providing them with opportunities to use those skills in ways to challenge inequality. This is a dynamic, experiential, and self-reinforcing process. Youth gain skills, a sense of belonging, and a deeper understanding of themselves and their world through social action. Youth are more inclined to act as they develop skills, interpersonal competencies, and sociopolitical awareness. But programs are only one way to foster participation and social conscience. We need to look beyond programs and services as we create ways to build capacities and opportunities for healthy development. Developmental theory might suggest that programs and services should be the fallback position and a sure sign that the natural facilitation of development has broken down (Kegan, 1982). Parents and families surely play a crucial role. However, the community, not the family alone (as Winnicott, 1965, proposed), may be the most important

holding environment for thriving (Kegan, 1982; Lerner, 2004). As a culture, we need to do a better job supporting the developing young person as he or she strives toward selfsufficiency, competence, identity, and political agency. Adults across a community can look for ways to give young people the opportunity to have a voice in public contexts and in the decisions that affect their lives. We agree with McKnight (1995) who suggests a community vision where the marginalized are not treated as clients but instead are "incorporated into community to experience a network of relationships, work, recreation, friendship, support, and the political power of being a citizen" (p. 169). The aim is to create communities where resources facilitate personal power and control as well as collective wellbeing. It is incumbent, therefore, on adults to join with youth to create more supportive structures to confront injustice and oppression.

REFERENCES Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44(9), 1175-1184. Barber, B. L., Eccles, J. S., & Stone, M. R. (2001). Whatever happened to the jock, the brain, and the princess? Young adult pathways linked to adolescent activity involvement and social identity. Journal of Adolescent Research, 16(5), 429-455. Benson, P. L. (1997). All kids are our kids: What communities must do to raise caring and responsible children and adolescents. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Catalano, R. F., Berglund, M. L., Ryan, J. A. M., Lonczak, H. S., & Hawkins, J. D. (2002, June 24). Positive youth development in the United States: Research findings on evaluations of positive youth development programs. Prevention and Treatment, 5, Article 15. Retrieved March 29, 2004, from www.journals.apa.org/prevention/ volume5/pre0050015a.html Clark, R. M. (1988). Critical factors in why disadvantaged students succeed or fail in school. New York: Academy for Educational Development. Cowen, E. (1991). In pursuit of wellness. American Psychologist, 46, 404-408.

414 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS Cowen, E. (1994). The enhancement of psychological wellness: Challenges and opportunities. American Journal of Community Psychology, 22, 149-179. Cowen, E. (1996). The ontogenesis of primary prevention: Lengthy strides and stubbed toes. American Journal of Community Psychology, 22, 235-249. Eccles, J. S., & Gootman, J. A. (2002). Community programs to promote youth development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Epp, J. (1988). Mental health for Canadians: Striking a balance. Ottawa, Ontario, Canada: Minister of Supplies and Services. Evans, S. D. (in press). Youth sense of community: Voice and power in community contexts. Journal of Community Psychology. Eyler, J., & Giles, D. (1999). Where's the learning in service-learning? San Francisco, CA: JosseyBass. Garmezy, N. (1985). The NIMH-Israeli high-risk study: Commendation, comments, and cautions. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 11(3), 349-353. Ginwright, S., & James, T. (2002). From assets to agents of change: Social justice, organizing, and youth development. In B. Kirshner (Ed.), Youth participation: Improving institutions and communities (pp. 27^16). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. John, M. (2003). Children's rights and power. London: Jessica Kingsley. Keating, A. E. (1990). Adolescent thinking. In S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 54-89). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kegan, R. (1982). The evolving self: problem and process in human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Lerner, R. (2004). Liberty: Thriving and civic engagement among America's youth. London: Sage. Lerner, R. M., & Benson, P. L. (2003). Developmental assets and asset-building communities: Implications for research, policy, and practice. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Lewis-Charp, H., Yu, H. C, Soukamneuth, S., & Lacoe, J. (2003). Extending the reach of youth development through civic activism: Outcomes of the Youth Leadership for Development Initiative. Oakland, CA: Social Policy Research Associates. Lord, J., & Hutchison, P. (1993). The process of empowerment: Implications for theory and practice. Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health, 12(1), 5-22.

Masten, A. S., Best, Κ. M., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2(4), 425-444. McKnight, J. (1995). The careless society: Community and its counterfeits. New York: Basic Books. Morsillo, J., & Prilleltensky, I. (in press). Social action with youth: Interventions, Evaluation, and psychopolitical validity. Journal of Community Psychology. Muntaner, C , Lynch, J., & Davey Smith, G. (2003). Social capital and the third way in public health. In R. Hofrichter (Ed.), Health and social justice (pp. 285-304). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Nelson, G., & Prilleltensky, I. (2004). Community psychology: In pursuit of liberation and wellbeing. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. Pancer, S. M., & Pratt, M. (1999). Social and family determinants of community and political involvement in Canadian youth. In M. Yates & J. Youniss (Eds.), Community service and civic engagement in youth: International perspectives. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Prilleltensky, I. (1997). Values, assumptions, and practices: Assessing the moral implications of psychological discourse and action. American Psychologist, 52(5), 517-535. Prilleltensky, I. (2001). Value-based praxis in community psychology: Moving towards social justice and social action. American Journal of Community Psychology, 29(5), 747-777. Prilleltensky, I., & Nelson, G. (2002). Doing psychology critically: Making a difference in diverse settings: Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Macmillan. Prilleltensky, I., Nelson, G., & Peirson, L. (Eds.). (2001a). Promoting family wellness and preventing child maltreatment. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University of Toronto Press. Prilleltensky, I., Nelson, G., & Peirson, L. (2001b). The role of power and control in children's lives: An ecological analysis of pathways toward wellness, resilience and problems. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 11(2), 143-158. Prilleltensky, I., & Prilleltensky, O. (2003). Reconciling the roles of professional helper and critical agent in health psychology. Journal of Health Psychology, 8, 243-246. Putnam, R. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Youth Civic Engagement: Promise and Peril · 415 Putnam, R., & Feldstein, L. (2003). Better together: Restoring the American community. New York: Simon & Schuster. Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57(3), 316-331. Scales, P. C , & Leffert, N. (1999). Developmental assets: A synthesis of the scientific research on adolescent development. Minneapolis, MN: Search Institute. Ungar, M. (2004). Nurturing hidden resilience in troubled youth. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University of Toronto Press. Ungar, M., Langlois, M., & Hum, M. (2004, May). Mistaken identities: Canadian youth and their search for a place in their communities. Keynote presentation to the International Conference on Youth Empowerment: A Cross-Cultural Exchange, Hong Kong. Werner, Ε. E. (1995). Resilience in development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4(3), 81-85. Werner, Ε. E., & Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY: Cornel University Press.

Wiley, Α., & Rappaport, J. (2000). Empowerment, wellness, and the politics of development. In D. Cicchetti, J. Rappaport, I. Sandler, & R. Weissberg (Eds.), 77ie promotion of wellness in children and adolescents (pp. 59-100). Washington, DC: Child Welfare League of America. Winnicott, D. W. (1965). Maturational processes and the facilitating environment. New York: International Universities Press. Wyman, P. Α., Sandler, I., Wolchik, S., & Nelson, K. (2000). Resilience as cumulative competence promotion and stress protection: Theory and intervention. In D. Cicchetti, J. Rappaport, I. Sandler, & R. P. Weissberg (Eds.), The promotion of wellness in children and adolescents (pp. 133-184). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Youniss, J., & Yates, M. (1997). Community service and social responsibility in youth. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Youniss, J., Yates, M., & Su, Y. (1997). Social integration: Community service and marijuana use in high school seniors. Journal of Adolescent Research, 12(2), 245-262.

26 RESILIENCE IN THE PALESTINIAN OCCUPIED TERRITORIES TOINE VAN TEEFFELEN Η ΑΝΙΑ BlTAR SALEEM AL-HABASH

A

focus on resilience is an extremely useful way of looking at the mental health of children and adults in the Occupied Palestinian Territories (OPT: includes the West Bank, Gaza, and East-Jerusalem). On the one hand, Palestinians are dealing with a broad range of adversities and psychological stressors that, especially during the latest intifada (Palestinian uprising), have tested them and their health to the limit; on the other hand, they possess individual, social, and national assets that help the majority continue to live on despite the many problems they face. In this chapter, we detail both the adversities Palestinians face and the consequences of those adversities to their mental health. We also examine evidence of the resilience of Palestinian youth in the OPT. In particular, we consider how a development organization, the Palestinian Youth Association for Leadership and Rights Activation (PYALARA), a Palestinian youth nongovernmental organization (NGO) in the West Bank, creates conditions that bolster resilience among Palestinian youth.

This discussion will show that resilience is as dependent on how, structurally, organizations respond to the needs of youth, providing them opportunities for meaningful participation in their communities, as it is the individual qualities each youth has when confronting adversity.

THE CHALLENGES FACED Palestinians in the West Bank, Gaza, and EastJerusalem presently find themselves in a crisis of dramatic proportions. Strict border closures and a system of Israeli checkpoints and roadblocks, initiated during the first half of the 1990s, have reached an unprecedented level during the second intifada (from September 2000 to the present). Travel has become full of obstacles and hazards—not only traveling from the OPT into Israel but also between Palestinian cities or districts inside the West Bank and Gaza. As a result, there has been a steep decline 417

418 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS in employment opportunities, trade movements, and the provision of services. In a recent study, the International Labour Organization (Unemployment, Poverty Grips, 2004) noticed an expanded unemployment rate of 35.3%, "a number which would be even higher if women confined to their homes by necessity and not by choice were included." In their report on poverty, Christian Aid (2003) indicated that almost three quarters of Palestinians in the OPT now live on less than $2 a day, the official UN poverty line. The majority of Palestinians in the OPT— over 3 million—are young, below 24 years of age, and confronted with little hope that their situation will get better in the near future. There has been a tendency among youth and adults to focus on the deficits they routinely experience. They can barely meet others outside their community, so important for adolescents who want to enjoy a normal social life, engage in social experimentation and adventuring, or test their competencies. Youth also know that they will face difficulty in finding appropriate vocational or academic study opportunities after high school. Furthermore, given the stagnation in the search for political solutions to the PalestinianIsraeli conflict, we observe present-day Palestinian youth to be generally pessimistic about the duration of the occupation or whether a viable Palestinian state will ever emerge. The recent Israeli policy of building the separation wall/ fence, the political-military measure that attracted a great deal of international attention in 2003 and 2004, is generally perceived as an instrument of annexation, making the establishment of a viable Palestinian state impossible. Many Palestinians, including the youth, talk daily of their struggle to confront a policy of national fragmentation and "silent transfer" (the emigration of Palestinians from the OPT to countries abroad as a way to cope with the economic and social obstacles they face in the OPT). Youth perceive that, gradually, Israel is trying to test the resolve of Palestinians to induce them to give up on their project of nation building. Although youth experience their individual opportunities to be very limited and the political track to be blocked, they also face a great many, often recurrent, crises in daily life. From September 28, 2000, to July 20, 2004, 3,209 Palestinians were killed as a result of the violence,

an average of over 2.3 per day. From that total, an estimated 23.5% were children (age 18 and under) (Palestine Monitor, 2004). The number of unarmed people killed so far in the West Bank and Gaza during the second intifada is over 2,000. Thousands of Palestinian youth have become emotionally handicapped as a result of clashes with the army. In a study conducted among 10- to 19-year-olds in the OPT in 2003, the Gaza Community Mental Health Program discovered that 94.6% had experienced a funeral, 83.2% had witnessed shooting incidents, 62% had seen a relative being hurt or killed, and 36% had been teargassed (quoted in Shahade, 2004). A psychosocial assessment of Palestinian children in August 2002 by Arafat and Boothby (2003) showed 9 3 % "not feeling safe and exposed to attack" (p. 5), and almost half of the children "personally experienced violence owing to the ongoing Palestinian-Israeli conflict o r . . . witnessed an incident of such violence befalling on an immediate family member" (p. 5). "One out of five children (21 % ) . . . had to move out of their homes, temporarily or permanently, overwhelmingly for conflict related reasons" (p. 5). Witnessing relatives or friends being imprisoned, especially when they are older and carry authority, is an additional stressor on young people. During the direct occupation of the Palestinian cities by the Israeli army, from spring 2002 on, tens of thousands of arrests have been made, and Palestinian youth have been imprisoned or seen their fathers or brothers being imprisoned. Such violent happenings cause fear and anxiety in children. During intifada clashes, a great many children and youth were forced to lay on the ground for several hours in their homes or to hide in kitchens and stairwells to avoid stray bullets. Even then, many witnessed bullets coming into the homes, family members escaping death, or soldiers entering and sometimes occupying their homes for the purpose of using them as lookout posts. Some young people have witnessed the demolition of their houses after they were considered illegally built or in retaliation for a family member being involved in violent action against the Israelis or for the purposes of Israeli construction projects (e.g., the building of settlement roads or the separation wall/fence).

Resilience in the Palestinian Occupied Territories · 419 Less dramatically, but also significantly affecting people's mood, are the humiliations people routinely witness or experience at checkpoints. People aimlessly waiting at checkpoints for many hours has become commonplace. Each community has countless stories about soldiers mocking them or asking people to perform irrelevant and humiliating tasks in order to pass. Related to these barriers to free movement, a stressor that usually goes unobserved but that has a pervasive influence on Palestinians is the continuous uncertainty experienced organizing daily life. Whole communities often do not know in the early morning what the day will bring, whether the children and youth can go to school, which route to take to work, or whether there will be work tomorrow. During the months of December 2002 to February 2003, it appeared to be a purposeful policy of the military to keep the inhabitants of Palestinian cities such as Bethlehem uncertain about whether a curfew would be installed that day or lifted, with word coming only the morning on which the curfew was to take effect. This uncertainty led to stressful situations inside families who did not know whether to work or send their children to school. "You cannot plan for tomorrow," has become a common complaint heard in the OPT. These obstacles should be viewed in a more general context of personal and collective vulnerability. Due to the violence, there is a basic lack of safety in public life, and many parents do not allow their children to leave home in the evening. The lack of safety is also keenly felt at the economic level. The OPT do not have an institutional safety net associated with welfare states, such as unemployment or retirement allowances. What many experience in such a risky situation is an accumulation of adversities in daily life, such as when a child has a handicap, a father is unemployed, and family members cannot reach the places they need to go, including medical and educational facilities, because of hazards and restrictions when traveling. Characteristic of Palestinian life in the OPT is an accumulation of interlocking obstacles that make nearly all aspects of daily life an uphill struggle, a continuing experience of exponential risks resulting from multiple stressors (Small & Memmo, 2004). Although everybody is at risk, there are those whose exposure to multiple risks is particularly

acute, including those in lower socioeconomic strata, the refugee camps, on the peripheries of the West Bank, in the Gaza Strip as a whole (with its large refugee camps), near the Israeli settlements (where clashes with settlers may be frequent), and near the separation wall/fence. Groups at special risk include the disabled, who are often already secluded from public life but are now further affected by financial constraints and travel obstacles. Girls and young women are also affected disproportionately; they cannot easily leave home alone because of unsafe streets and traditional conventions against their travel in the late afternoon and evening. Arguably, political and military circumstances affect them more than they affect boys and young men.

MENTAL HEALTH ISSUES How does this situation of interlocking adversities affect the mental health and behavior of young people in the OPT? First, it has been documented by mental health organizations and psychologists that the emotional behavior of individual youth display many direct symptoms of trauma and anxiety, such as nightmares and bed-wetting, increased aggressiveness and hyperactivity, and a decrease in attention span and capacity to concentrate (Arafat & Boothby, 2003). World Vision, a leading foreign aid organization working in the OPT, speaks in a recent report about the psychosocial syndromes and trauma that Palestinian children face (Albina, 2002). That report includes results from an unpublished study by Tamar Lavi from Tel Aviv University that showed a 70% prevalence rate of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) among Palestinian children from the West Bank and Gaza Strip, compared with a 20% to 60% prevalence of such syndromes in international studies on children in war zones (Albina, 2002). It has been suggested by Palestinian spokesperson Hanan Ashrawi that the term PTSD is not appropriate in the case of the OPT, because Palestinians do not face just posttraumatic stress disorders but rather ongoing trauma disorders (personal communication, March 2004). PYALARA's own experiences as a youth development NGO in the OPT point to how the

420 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS reality of risk and vulnerability negatively affects youths' sense of self-efficacy, especially for the estimated 95% of youth who are not actively involved in the present intifada (unlike the previous one during the end of the 1980s and the beginning of the 1990s in which many youth participated, largely because of its nonviolent character). PYALARA staff members hear youth in workshops say frequently that they do not feel in control of their lives, nor do they experience control over the general political developments around them or over the events that take place at the level of their family and community. It is very common to hear youth implicitly or explicitly speak about their lack of power; for instance, they feel subject to political "conspiracies," or they say that "it doesn't really matter what we are doing, the Israelis do what they want anyway" or that the Palestinian National Authority is "not doing anything." Moreover, although they often see democracy at work in other societies, including Israel, they don't see many opportunities to have their own voices heard. Against this background of powerlessness and frustration, one problematic health response is the withdrawal by many youth from active participation in their families and communities. Many youth are inactive, staying at home, watching television (several music video satellite stations have become available in the Arab world, attracting huge youth audiences). Youth lack both a role and a voice. The result is a kind of generalized frustration turning inward—not an explosion but an implosion. Their withdrawal is sometimes a reaction to the gravity of events. After prolonged periods of curfew in the Palestinian cities, it was not uncommon to observe people being confused, not concentrating and rather forgetful, overcome by an overall loss of orientation as they ventured to reconstruct regular routines. Another specific response to the political situation is a compensatory identification with suicide bombers, as documented by the Gaza Community Mental Health Program (quoted in Shahade, 2004). This identification results from the absence of powerful and inspiring persons in youths' social environments. Because Arab society and culture place special importance on a strong father figure, children and youth are

deeply affected by the diminished authority of fathers who have become unemployed, humiliated at checkpoints by young Israeli soldiers, or put in custody. At the national level, until his recent death, Yasser Arafat, considered by many to be the father of the Palestinian national movement for five decades, was imprisoned in his headquarters in Ramallah, making him a leader without the physical opportunity to lead. The Palestinian police, part of the Palestinian Authority, have become a symbol of powerlessness as well, because they are usually perceived to comply with Israeli army commands. In this void of leadership, the suicide bomber takes on the role of a substitute, seemingly powerful figure, expressing in a single, spectacular and deadly act the frustration and rage of those victimized. A small but significant number of parents (5%-8%) reported that "their children have become fixated on thoughts of death and revenge" (Arafat & Boothby, 2003, p. 6). Withdrawal, anger, and feelings of revenge go hand in hand with a pervasive despair about both the larger political context and the hassles of daily life. Feelings of hopelessness have deepened. In the streets, it is common to hear expressions such as, "For Palestinians yesterday is always better than today." Informally, many youth are ambiguous about the official Palestinian "story" of a progressive struggle leading to independence. Even though people in general subscribe to the legitimacy of Palestinian national rights and regard Palestinian spokespersons as expressing their collective rights in the conflict with Israel, it is also often said that the struggle goes from one "debacle" to another. Such pronouncements speak to people's sense of being abandoned by their leaders and misguided by social institutions. Overall, the mental health situation of youth and adults is characterized by a pervasive sense of the obstacles faced rather than a focus on opportunities. It is difficult to challenge this view when it appears grounded in a reality that indeed effectively blocks development, personally and economically. "Nothing is easy here," people say. Many identify with the painting of the Palestinian artist Suleiman Mansour that depicts the Palestinian people's identity as an old man carrying the globe on his back. Daily talk is full of images of being "closed up" or

Resilience in the Palestinian Occupied Territories · 421 about how one is living in a "coffin," "cage," or "prison." The visibility of the newly constructed and well-guarded separation wall/fence adds to this sense that one's life is buttressed, immobile. "Traveling [from the southern or middle West Bank to the northern part] to a city like Jenin is in people's experience almost like traveling to the moon," a development worker recently commented to one of the authors. The constant awareness of one's confinement and a sense of waiting without knowing the future generate feelings of boredom among youth, especially outside school hours and on holidays. Diaries written by older students in the OPT (see the compilation by Atallah & van Teeffelen, 2004) display a remarkable swing between, on the one hand, a basic nervousness, sense of vulnerability, and uncertainty because of the hazards of daily life, and on the other, routine boredom of living in an environment that lacks the presence of spatial and temporal "horizons."

SOURCES OF RESILIENCE Given this gloomy picture, Palestinians continue to possess a range of significant resources, a resilience that presents a balance to the threats to their mental health and a way out of their desperate situation. After all, the academic and developmental interest in concepts such as resilience has precisely sprung from a need to correct the pervasive image of helpless and traumatized victims that puts responsibility for change on psychosocial professionals or policymakers, with few expectations of the affected persons themselves (Newman, 2004). To their own misfortune, Palestinians have long been confronting, in the media and politics, the bipolar stereotypes of victimized refugees and victimizing terrorists (van Teeffelen, 2004). Under the gaze of an international audience, their resilience is overlooked. Resources for resilience among Palestinians do exist, however, and include social opportunities, supportive networks, and spiritual, cultural, or value-based strengths. Of these, their national and cultural identity is the most ubiquitous resource they possess. Palestinians have as cultural traits stubbornness and the enduring capacity to cope, a capacity

perhaps founded on the traditional mentality of the peasant (the majority of Palestinians before the 1948 war were peasants) who are strongly attached to their lands. Rooted in a centuries-old history of collective resilience (and resistance) forged during confrontations with rulers and landlords and of fighting subjugation by successive military and political rulers, talk of despair is neutralized by a spoken, or unspoken, collective will not to have their spirits broken. Palestinians will say, "While we suffer more, the Israelis suffer too, and this will in the end break their spirit rather than ours." This spirit, however, is not one-dimensional, limited to one or two expressions of emotion (anger and despair, for instance) but rather can manifest in a range of interpretative and emotional repertoires helpful for coping with the uncertainties of life as it is. Combined with this is a willingness to accept suffering while celebrating at the level of the human spirit, warmth and a certain "lightness" characteristic of Mediterranean culture. Visitors to Palestine may be struck by the ability of Palestinians to laugh and enjoy the quiet rhythm of life interrupted by impatient but friendly bursts of social interaction. Much of Palestinian cultural resilience is a way of coping with suffering, a survival strategy that allows them to "hang on." In local discourse, this daily strategy for living is sometimes called sumud or steadfastness, a concept close to but not exactly the same as resilience. Stretched over a period of three decades, the Palestinian lawyer Raja Shehadeh (1982, 1992, 2003) has written a series of diaries in which he gave personal meaning to sumud, or what he initially called "the third way"—neither allowing oneself to be subjugated by the occupation (through withdrawal or inactivation) nor making a choice for armed struggle. The third way was to stay put, not to leave the country or to become resigned to the occupation but to remain attached to living a normal life. As a concept, sumud has not been unproblematic. It initially was employed as a top-down concept by Arab and Palestinian leaders to express politically a desired steadfastness during the national struggle. But staying put became a sometimes impossibly rigid demand, not linked to the demands of daily life, when for instance youth were advised to stay in the country while

422 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS study or work possibilities elsewhere were much better—possibilities that would in fact allow them later to provide a greater contribution to the national cause. Also, the sumud concept—sometimes metaphorically likened in Palestinian arts and popular culture to the ineradicable desert cactus or the long-living olive tree with its roots deep in the soil—was criticized for not being flexible enough to meet the challenges of modern life and identity. This life is after all characterized by mobility, by "routes" rather than "roots" (Woodward, 2002). Perhaps the term resilience has more "flexible" connotations than sumud can provide. Yet somehow sumud captures the essential elements of endurance and refusal to give up without which no account or analysis of Palestinian society can be valid. A factor relevant to the cultural sumud of Palestinians is the importance of religion as a source of faith, spiritual commitment, guidance, and consolation. Observers during the intifada have often equated the increase in Islamic religious observance with an increase in political radicalism and despair, resulting in support for militant paramilitary groups like Hamas and Islamic Jihad. Yet such observations, although satisfying media stereotypes of Islam, give reductive meaning to a complex phenomenon. During uncertain and seemingly hopeless times, many persons and families consider faith as a beacon for spiritual orientation in a world lacking leadership and values. For instance, in the diaries of Palestinian youth (Atallah & Van Teeffelen, 2003), several Christian and Moslem girls at a private school in Bethlehem mentioned as their main inner source of strength the inspiring example of religious personalities, Palestinian and non-Palestinian, who devoted themselves to the improvement of the world. In this sense, religion may well contribute to an inner, spiritual resilience. At the same time, it is also true that religion can contribute to a crippled form of resilience when it incorporates youth into hierarchical relations that block initiative and independence and when religion feeds fanaticism. To what extent religion contributes to mental health is therefore a complex and contradictory subject that certainly, in the Palestinian context, awaits study and research. Resilience is further supported by educational opportunities. In the study by Arafat and

Boothby (2003), education was felt to be the single most important resource for healthy development. Despite the odds of gaining access to education, when it is available at all, 96% of Palestinian children saw education as their main means to improve their situation. Commitment to education is very strong among Palestinian families for historical reasons. After losing much of their land in 1948, Palestinian families inculcated values of education into their children because education was perceived as a way of opening horizons for alternative routes in life. It helped to develop their resilience, a resilience understood not only as psychic strength and the capacity to endure adversities over a prolonged period of time ("steeling" one's self to adversity, as it is appropriately described in the resilience literature; Small & Memmo, 2004) but also in terms of tactical inventiveness, problem-solving capacities, and survival techniques that enable one to meet the requirements of an adventurous journey through life. Understood this way, the resilient person should not only be steadfast but also make detours before coming on course again to find solutions that ensure survival (Cyrulnik, 2002). When referring to flexible problem-solving skills and tactical resilience, we should not only think about the influence of formal organizations for youth such as the educational system but also of informal daily survival techniques such as supportive sociality, which includes sharing the many tricks needed to cross checkpoints, the informal and impromptu release of thoughts and feelings between family members at the morning break, and the pervasive dark humor that helps anybody, including youth, rise above their situation and look at it in a lighter way. In our experience, mutually supportive discussion about the small moments of daily life is a major factor in explaining Palestinian resilience, combining and valorizing the influence of cultural values and social networks. Crucial supportive networks for social resilience are the extended family and the community. The family is considered a moral pillar in Palestinian society and takes a central place in Palestinian popular culture. Traditionally, and now most especially in the refugee camps, the Palestinian families' ability to endure, successfully recover from wars, and reorganize the

Resilience in the Palestinian Occupied Territories · 423 social fabric after dislocation is critical to the survival of its members (Sayigh, 1976). In Palestinian society it is still common—even though to a somewhat lesser extent than in the past, because of globalization and the influence of the media—to take care of each other, emotionally and financially; to create a feeling of togetherness during feasts and celebrations; and to lend each other support during times of crisis such as curfews or illness in the family. Hospitality, generosity, and sociability are values that underpin social life to an extent unknown in the West. A friend of ours said that after she had a car accident, she was so warmly and promptly surrounded by families and friends that for some moments she completely forgot about the accident! Social occasions on the balcony and in the garden or strolls in the street in good weather are common and form natural resting points in Palestinian life, providing relaxation as well as moments to reenergize the spirit and experience a gay, uplifting friendliness. This spirit endured even during the few hours of permitted movement following long curfews in Palestinian cities. As people rushed to buy their amenities, it was still common to see people supporting each other by giving walkers a ride or making purchases for the elderly. In addition, it has to be noted that it is uncommon to see persons begging on Palestinian streets. The effective welfare safety net is not created by formal institutions but by extended families. Based on values and practices of mutuality, there is an informal involvement in neighborhood or community voluntary work, the last being another traditional custom among Palestinians, perhaps, we speculate, rooted in ancient practices of cooperative work among the peasantry.

Palestinian-Israeli conflict (proven a failure after almost 7 years), the role of the nongovernmental organizations started to evolve as an important component in the development of Palestinian society into a proto-state. The collapse of the peace process and the reescalation of violence and conflict during the second intifada have meant that NGOs have had to change their course once again, providing opportunities for dealing with the emergency situation resulting from the violence. With regard to youths' role in these NGOs, they organized and facilitated leisure activities, training and workshops, nonviolent actions, and community work. Specific activities have included the following:

Guided by these principles, a great many Palestinian NGOs have contributed over the years to enhancing the resilience of youth. Although the history of Palestinian NGOs in relation to the Palestinian National Authority is beyond the scope of this chapter, it should be noted that prior to the signing of the Oslo agreement,' the role of NGOs in the OPT was more focused on relief and aid for crisis-affected Palestinian citizens, in addition to acting as referral organizations substituting for the lack of a ruling government. After a new future was envisioned with the semiresolution of the

Community

Activation and Awareness • • • • •

Raising

Summer camps Discussion meetings Club activities Local journeys International journeys

Skills Related •

Skill-enhancing courses and workshops for youth (arts, computer, etc.)

Expressive, • • • • • •

"Voice"

Activities

Diary writing International computer exchanges Counseling workshops Youth media Theater and film Participation in international meetings Actions



Voluntary community work (e.g., rebuilding demolished houses or supporting patients) • Advocacy campaigns for issues such as people's right and access to education • Support to victims of violence • Nonviolent direct or indirect actions in protest against the occupation or the separation wall/ fence A major task of youth development NGOs in Palestine has been to look for ways to bring

424 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS about healing through activities such as those listed above, counter despair, and challenge negative thinking, all part of efforts to strengthen sumud. PYALARA, as one of the organizations founded to address these issues, has developed an innovative model of intervention and social action in which media, counseling techniques, and the establishment of new social relationships among youth have been strategically combined to create a deeper and broader health effect.

THE STORY OF P Y A L A R A PYALARA was established in 1999, initially with the aim to serve a Palestinian youth audience as a youth media and communicationsoriented NGO. It was an initiative by a group of young journalists who were determined to give Palestinian youth a voice through the establishment of a youth paper. This paper became The Youth Times, a 24-page monthly bilingual newspaper produced by and for youth. Before the establishment of The Youth Times, only a few young people could publish their writing in the daily newspapers in the OPT. During several brainstorming sessions with young people from different Palestinian locations, it became clear to the founders of The Youth Times that young people needed to have a louder voice in their community. Hindering this expression was a Palestinian media that lacked cultural identity, professionalism, and a comprehensive vision about its role as a tool for change. Moreover, the print media were politically one-sided, boring, semiofficial, and did not meet the social, political, educational, and entertainment needs of their readers. In this context, The Youth Times was a fruit hard to harvest. People were not accustomed to a youth paper by and for youth. At the time, the Palestinian media did not feature any youth papers, pages, or magazines, and those that were available in the shops—youth magazines from the Arab world—were all made by adults and usually directed themselves toward young people in a patronizing manner. Circumstances have obliged The Youth Times to adopt a much broader view of the needs of Palestinian youth. A few months after PYALARA's establishment as a registered NGO in October

2000 with the intent to produce both print and television reports about issues of concern to young people in the OPT, the second intifada erupted. A sense of paralysis, uncertainty, fear, and helplessness prevailed throughout the territories. The energized, active, and enthusiast youth associated with PYALARA suddenly felt powerless and lost their sense of direction. In response, a youth conference organized by PYALARA, Palestinian Youth Vision, and the YWCA took place in Jerusalem in November 2000 with more than 200 young Palestinians in attendance. The conference was titled "The Role of Young People in Times of Conflict." Youth participants mapped out their role for the stressing times ahead. The ideas and recommendations agreed on constituted the action plan on which PYALARA started to strategize its role during the second intifada. In the presence of Bertrand Bainvel, UNICEF's program officer, PYALARA's members shared their deep feelings of anxiety, frustration, and despair. "I am really scared," said Lana, a 14year-old from Jerusalem. "When I was going to PYALARA's office, the Israeli soldiers bodysearched me, my school b a g . . . . They humiliated me to the extent that I wished I had anything with me to defend myself." She started crying and continued: "I was ready to do a real crazy thing at that moment." Nisreen, a 15-yearold, gently interrupted: "What would you gain if you do something stupid? . . . You should control yourself and think of better ways to express your anger." The level of frustration and helplessness expressed at the meeting was great. The young people participating repeatedly said that they felt terrible about being unable to do anything about the escalation of violence and the worsening political situation. We had witnessed this frustration daily before the meetings. Those who could make it through checkpoints, invasions, and curfews were coming to our offices to talk, some of them expressing their frustration through writings, whereas others could not speak or write at all, so fearful had they become. Those youth who made it to PYALARAsupported meetings came together to find a way out of their situation by helping others far more affected by the invasions and curfews than they were to express themselves. Saleem Habash,

Resilience in the Palestinian Occupied Territories · 425 managing editor of PYALARA's The Youth Times recounts, At the start of the Intifada when Mohammed al-Dura was killed [a small Gaza boy killed while in the arms of his father, an event accidentally filmed and repeatedly shown on all Palestinian and Arab TV stations], we realized that most young people were unable to express themselves and that this would lead to an explosion. I wanted to spread the word that youth could do something. The initiative of what later became the We Care project evolved from these brainstorming sessions. Young people and PYALARA subsequently advocated and lobbied with the Jerusalem office of UNICEF to support a new initiative. UNICEF gave the green light for launching a project that aimed to alleviate the pressure that Palestinian adolescents experience. A cadre of Palestinian university students (specializing in sociology, psychology, and social work) were trained to become mentors and psychosocial supports for their peers. The training included psychosocial intervention methods, conflict resolution techniques, communication, and leadership skills. After training, the mentors of the We Care project headed to the field to meet with school pupils in a selected number of underprivileged areas in the West Bank and Gaza. In pairs, they conducted a series of workshops aimed at alleviating the pressures of the crisis affecting their peers. Through a series of 8 to 10 youth-toyouth sessions, youth mentors helped adolescents by listening to their problems, giving them advice, providing them with information, and making them aware that there were people who cared about them. In the beginning, the young people and mentors had to deal with the psychological barriers between them and create trust and a sense of mutual solidarity. The fact that the mentors were only a few years older than the pupils greatly facilitated interaction. After the ice was broken between the mentors and school pupils, the mentors encouraged the pupils to relieve their inner feelings about the problems they were suffering. The issues that the young people related to the mentors at first had most to do with the political situation— about how the violence affected them; how

some of them faced nightmares, nervousness, sadness, or anger over the loss of a relative or a friend; concerns about their inability to socialize due to the inability to travel; and general feelings of fear, insecurity, despair, and revenge. After young people expressed their problems and feelings, they discussed with the mentors ways to cope with these problems. Several youth talked about how they reacted violently to their feelings. Wael, a 13-year-old from the Qalandia refugee camp near Ramallah, felt relieved only when he could throw stones at the jeep of soldiers patrolling the streets adjacent to the camp. When asked why he threw stones at a fully protected jeep, Wael said, "Those Israelis have to know that we will not allow them to steal more Palestinian land." In another of the We Care sessions, a 16-year-old girl from the Nablus area said that she envied the boys who could throw stones at Israeli jeeps and soldiers. "I wish to have the courage to go to the streets and also throw stones at the occupiers." Despite the fact that less than 1 % of Palestinian youth resort to stone throwing, face-to-face interviews conducted at the time by PYALARA revealed that those youngsters who expressed their anger and frustration against the occupation by throwing stones, by demonstrating, burning tires, or writing slogans in the streets were in fact less prone to think of suicidal acts or bombings. Interviews also showed that the more silent and less expressive the youth were, the more inclined they were to carrying out suicide bombings. Interestingly, after a number of sessions with We Care mentors, many of the young people showed indifference to the political problems they were facing, at least as they had first perceived and understood them. Once given a platform on which to express themselves, they chose instead to focus on normal teen problems, such as difficulties with relationships between boys and girls; sexual problems; social problems (such as domestic violence and violence in school, early marriage, and the use of drugs); problems related to their local communities including social, mterpolitical, and family problems; or studyand work-related problems, all complicated by the political realities of their lives but nevertheless common to teenagers globally. Mentors were asked not to give any prescriptions for action. Many of the students described

426 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS the mentors as "having become our friends." Fifteen-year-old Mohammed from Ar-Ram, an area near Jerusalem well-known for drug abuse, violence, and prostitution and an area particularly badly affected by the intifada, told us, "The mentors were like my friends. I could tell them anything. They did not solve any of my problems, but helped me in solving them." Hana from Ramallah said that working with PYALARA and being involved in its activities had given her the self-confidence and self-esteem to become active in her society. "When I started to attend the We Care sessions with PYALARA, I started to feel that there were people who cared about me and my problems." Before attending, she said her life was "black," but that that blackness began to decrease the more she became involved with PYALARA. Crucial to the success of the We Care sessions was helping youth to find roles and activities in specific fields such as journalism or community work, which made them perceive themselves as proactive participants in the political process, which boosted their self-confidence. We Care thus carried young Palestinians from a state of isolation and frustration into a more nourishing environment that not only catered to their needs for inclusion and power but also channeled their energies in a creative and sustainable manner. In summary, the We Care project addressed a need to make available a refuge for youth where they could find people who listened to them, gave them appropriate advice, and assisted them to realize their potential. But beyond helping individual kids, the project also had a broader social dimension. It followed the long-valued tradition of volunteer work among Palestinians, encouraged hand-in-hand cooperation between universities and schools, between NGOs and ministries, between older and younger youth, and between those empowered and those less advantaged adolescents. Despite these many positive outcomes, the approach was criticized nonetheless. The bulk of the critique was focused on a single issue that reflected the widespread acceptance of traditional mental health models of caring, which relied on experts. How could young people talk with their peers effectively about their problems, especially during such a severe situation, without any real professional help? The youths'

enthusiastic endorsement of the project was an effective answer to its critics. The dogmatic assertions by the experts only hardened the determination of PYALARA staff and the youth they served to continue their work. Even though the level of interaction between the mentors and adolescents was high, some of the youth demanded more sessions as well as a private environment more conducive to talk about personal problems. In 2002, a youth-toyouth hotline was established after an intensive training of the mentors. Many calls were received from young people seriously affected by the intifada. The callers felt that it was easier for them to speak to an "invisible friend" whom they could tell anything without being shy. As a tool to listen to youth, the hotline does not provide prescriptions for how callers can solve their problems. Instead, operators are taught to help youth express their anger in healthy ways. In case of serious problems, callers are referred to more professional helpers. Through both these initiatives, it has become apparent that the frustrations felt in general by young Palestinians are multiplied in the case of those who also cope with physical disability in a society where few specialized resources are available. Eighteen-year-old Hamdan has never been able to walk without support. His early childhood was a profoundly unhappy time because his family lacked understanding of his particular needs. According to him, "I was always alone, always crying; I didn't feel human." A chance encounter in 2000 led to an invitation from PYALARA for Hamdan to attend a journalism course in Jordan, which Hamdan felt "was the first time people treated me with respect." Since then, he has been an active participant in all activities at PYALARA's office north of Jerusalem, traveling from his home in Bethlehem along a route made tortuous by the Israeli Army roadblocks. In early 2002, Hamdan negotiated with PYALARA to take responsibility for the production of a four-page special feature in The Youth Times focusing on disability issues. He explained his motivation for this as follows: "I don't want others to live a hard life like me. I have a message in my life, which I must offer. Even if I die, I must paint a smile on handicapped people's faces." With the confidence and skills acquired through activities with PYALARA, Hamdan has taken a

Resilience in the Palestinian Occupied Territories · 427 lead role in local initiatives to support disabled young people in his area. He explains that "it has helped to make me part of society."

A Televised Voice Participants in the We Care groups moved from reflection to action as they became more involved with PYALARA and each other. Groups were provided with an opportunity to share their ideas in The Youth Times and to make use of PYALARA's weekly television program Alii Sotak [Speak Up] to voice their issues and concerns. It has to be emphasized that there continues to be very little attention paid in the Palestinian media, schools, and among caregivers to the health and lifestyle questions Palestinian youth face. PYALARA's work has brought a new level of awareness to the experience of Palestinian youth. Until this work began, it was uncommon to find the perspectives of teenagers represented during political discussions conducted in the media or in public places, even though youth have been the main street actors in the intifada and are the main victims of restrictions on mobility prevalent in the OPT. Established in 1999, PYALARA was selected by UNICEF to help celebrate, for the first time in Palestine, the International Broadcasting Day for Children. A series of television episodes were produced by young Palestinians that became the base for regular television programming by 2002. Young people now not only present but also produce the televised episodes. Alii Sotak has become a weekly 2-hour program broadcast by Palestine TV that tackles both light and serious issues of concern for youth. According to estimates based on polls conducted by the Palestine Bureau of Statistics, over the period of its 5-year existence Alii Sotak has been regularly viewed by as many as 300,000 people. The relatively high number of viewers, combined with the 90,000 to 100,000 readers of The Youth Times demonstrates how collectively oriented Palestinian society is. Episodes are watched and the newspapers read in a communal setting with multiple family members and visitors present or among peers at school or university. The television programming by PYALARA is owned by the youth themselves, as is their newspaper. Through both mediums, although

most especially the television, PYALARA participants are able to reach almost every household in the West Bank and Gaza. Even more important, they are also able to address the concerns of youth who because of a handicap or because of family restrictions and social conventions are unable to leave their homes. Participants in PYALARA's production work routinely comment on the impact their participation has had on their lives. Ahmed, a 17-yearold from Jerusalem, considers PYALARA's media as a door to his future, allowing him to express his worries and those of his peers. Eighteen-year-old Lana said that her experience producing a television program helped her to deal better with the bitter outcomes of the Palestinian-Israeli conflict, making her experiences of value to others: "Through Alii Sotak, I can express myself. I can communicate with my peers from all over Palestine. I can share their pains and touch their souls." This same impact is felt by media consumers. In research by Birzeit University's Department of Mass Communication (2004), 77.3% of young people who read The Youth Times said that they thought that it both activated and empowered Palestinian youth. Almost 80% believed that it raised their awareness of their rights and responsibilities, and 30% felt that the newspaper changed their views and attitudes toward a variety of issues. The results reflect PYALARA's commitment to focus on issues relevant to youth, producing programming and journalistic accounts that can make even the most modest individuals heroes and heroines in their underprivileged communities. For example, when a group of young girls from Biddo near Ramallah tackled the issue of early marriage, their whole village watched the special TV episode they produced. Such initiatives make adolescents role models for their peers who watch the shows or read what they write in The Youth Times.

Saleem Habash, Age 22: My Experience In 1997 I got to know about a new idea of a newspaper for young people—all made by young people. Since I loved writing, and I could not find a place to publish my writings but my notebooks and computer, I was excited to get involved. At that time, during the years after the

428 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS Oslo agreement was signed, it was a duty to help develop Palestinian society. A whole new bunch of creative and fruitful ideas were evolving, and the process of development was being remolded into a more vibrant momentum. At that time I was in high school, a 15-yearold who had lots of energy but nowhere to express myself or a forum to speak through. I started getting involved with the preparatory meetings held in the Jerusalem Times office. Afterward, I became a correspondent for The Youth Times. / remember the first time I had an article published in the newspaper, with my personal photo beside it, I was returning back from school, and people were pointing at me saying that they had seen my picture in the newspaper. After that, I became very active with The Youth Times and PYALARA. In 2001, I became a PYALARA staff and then managing editor for The Youth Times. My experience with PYALARA has changed a lot about who I am without changing my principles and beliefs. PYALARA has given me the space and tools to grow, yet without being forced into a certain shape. Having the tools for change, you end up in a cycle of creating change: not only do you create change amongst others, but further, they exert change within your life as well; it is a cycle. Being involved with PYALARA has given me the strength to become an activist in my society and an advocate for the rights of children and young people. There is not just one experience that makes all the difference, which teaches the person fully and makes him or her to become more resilient. Rather, the experiences between the ups and downs are the ones that really educate the individual and make of him or her a new person. I remember, once, I was excited to cover a musical festival in Ramallah. We contacted the organizers and convinced them to give us a press pass to enter and view all the shows. Apparently, mine was taken away because I was, then, too young to be at the show. I became almost furious about this unjust treatment. It did not stop right there, but I was urged to write about what happened, and I did. I wrote about it in the paper from all my heart and soul. Everyone read it, and I felt that although I missed being treated like the other journalists, I was the gatekeeper for the freedom of the press.

Now, everything is different. My role has developed greatly. I am responsible for urging other young people to write and express themselves. The uniqueness of PYALARA stems from the fact that we do it differently. Everything is developmental; we develop ourselves to help develop other young people. PYALARA has played a major role in my life. Having worked with PYALARA has urged me to become a journalist and has unleashed various sides of my personality that have not been touched before. Even in the darkest days, I found my shelter in PYALARA.

Lessons Learned Our experience with PYALARA has shown that promoting resilience requires the abandonment of an overly medical and individualistic approach to health promotion. We must look instead for a holistic way to intervene that places emphasis on the context in which social relationships take place. Furthermore, disadvantaged youth need opportunities to become fully involved participants in their society. Youth need a voice, a respected role, and a meaningful cause to sustain health, especially during bewildering times such as this intifada. Such involvement helps them to develop psychological protection, selfesteem, and confidence. Through the act of tackling issues collectively, youth develop a sense of competence and political and self-efficacy. What PYALARA demonstrates is that participation by youth should not be a form of tokenism but should involve youth at the very first steps in the development of programming. They should be involved in designing, implementing, and monitoring the projects in which they are engaged. In fact, We Care and the youth media projects have all been youth initiatives in which adults played only a distant role. PYALARA's success also teaches us that successful trauma projects do more than provide skills training and discussion groups. Any training PYALARA gives is always linked to action and field implementation. Any investment put in training and empowering young persons helps them to become leaders, providers of services, and role models for their peers. In this way, PYALARA guarantees a sustained engagement

Resilience in the Palestinian Occupied Territories · 429 with, and the participation of, groups of young people even as institutions build their capacity through the efforts of the youth. Resilience is realized through the structures provided, specifically how support is provided and accessed and resources and opportunities for personal and collective development organized. The development of peer-to-peer relationships as part of projects promoting resilience has been shown to be particularly helpful, especially when older competent youth such as university students support younger youth at school. These mentoring relationships can sometimes provide better support than family members to youth in crisis (Newman, 2004), especially in a traditionally hierarchical society such as that of the Palestinians. We fully endorse Gilligan's (1999) plea to make use of nonprofessional mentors who, with only a modest allowance, can offer quality time, enthusiasm, and commitment to their younger peers. Furthermore, when these mentors do not come from the community where school students live, they may in fact be trusted with more intimate information than is the case of those who are part of the local community. In a sense, these mentors are both indigenous and external members of the youths' communities (Gilligan, 1999), combining the advantages of both. This is even more the case for youth who help to operate the hotline at PYALARA's offices. In PYALARA's experience, youth media constitute a strategic social resource for the development of resilience, for several reasons. First, a healthy youth media help youth to express themselves and to relay in creative ways the issues they face. Resilience is greatly enhanced when traumatized youth have the opportunity to express themselves through genres with which they are familiar. PYALARA's media projects give room to various forms of expression, including music, popular song, text, and poetry, as well as more standard types of journalistic reporting. Second, making and watching media reports about situations or events that leave traumatic scars (such as long curfews and being a witness to violence in the streets or at school) help youth to better understand their problems. Furthermore, a vibrant youth media helps to put youth issues into a broader context so that it becomes possible for youth to look at

and understand those issues with perspective and depth. Exemplary cases of coping, resilient, or transformative behavior in response to adverse or challenging situations, documented as part of these media expressions, inspire viewers and readers alike. Third, making media products gives participating youth active roles that are interesting, challenging, prestigious, and skill based. These roles include journalist, cameraman, soundman, interviewer, program producer, editor, and writer. By fulfilling such roles, youth demonstrate their interests and abilities and develop their skills and a sense of competence and self-esteem. Skill development is enhanced through exposure to the work of other media professionals as well. In fact, recently, local journalists of the satellite station AlJazeera, several of them female, have become role models for PYALARA participants. These journalists have reported, not without danger, about daily events from the West Bank and Gaza, inspiring PYALARA's youth to do the same. Enacting media-related roles is a source of self-worth and prestige for youth and provides opportunities to participate in a modern and adventurous-looking vocation. Their role as journalists also makes it possible for them to approach decision makers and experts for their opinions on issues that affect youth. Fourth, media projects provide publicity to particular youth issues and thus allow local community projects to find a much larger following nationwide. Fifth, developing media products about youth helps Palestinian youth to consider how they want to be seen even because images of them are being broadcast locally and abroad by others. Because Palestinian youth are well aware of the bipolar images of violent activism and helpless suffering that others have of them, critical reflection through the production of their own images has helped to open up space for more nuanced portrayals of youth in Palestine. The combination of peer-to-peer activities and media projects is characteristic of the overall philosophy of PYALARA. Combined, the effect is particularly felicitous: The first type of activities help youth to work through issues on an interpersonal, face-to-face level, and the second, media-based interventions make it possible to disseminate the perspectives of youth to

430 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS a much wider public, leading to activism and empowerment among young people. This dual approach builds on traditional sources of cultural resilience in Palestinian society such as sumud while also contributing to a climate in which can be negotiated cultural notions of steadfastness and community. Like many other Palestinian NGOs, PYALARA works hard at making itself a nurturing "family" for the youth who participate, providing a sense of belonging and participation in a supportive group conducive to personal growth. Of course, the focus of PYALARA is not just the youth themselves. Much energy is expended to network with the larger community at local, district, and national levels. PYALARA's projects provide opportunities for youth to relate directly with decision makers, schools, educational and health services, churches, mosques, professionals, businesses, music groups, and community clubs. Media and social interventions open up avenues for youth who want to relate to various community services and activities while simultaneously making community institutions responsive to the needs of youth. This "community capacity building," whereby communities are identified and mobilized to support common goals, is increasingly seen as facilitating resilience among children (Kretzman & McKnight, 1993). Thus, PYALARA's emphasis on promoting children's rights does not unfold in an abstract, decontextualized, or rhetorical way. In all its projects, PYALARA focuses on the local concrete community issues faced by youth, linking these local concerns to issues of general rights and international law to build among youth a sense of (injustice as experienced in their own daily lives. This focus on rights has been especially important because it has helped to anchor PYALARA's work to moral values relevant to daily living. In the Palestinian context, resilience is not the same as adaptation to an unjust system such as an occupation, nor does an emphasis on resilience mean that the need to pursue larger social change is forgotten (see the critical remarks about resilience as an approach in Small & Memmo, 2004). Rather, youth are encouraged to examine their lives from a perspective of equity, justice, and human rights. Such a perspective cannot be guaranteed under conditions

of occupation and ongoing Israeli noncompliance with principles of international law.

NOTE 1. Signed on May 4, 1994, outlining the first stage of Palestinian autonomy—in Gaza and Jericho—including Israeli redeployment and the establishment of a Palestinian self-governmental authority. Israel remained in control of the settlements, military locations, and security matters. The stipulated interim period ended on May 4, 1999.

REFERENCES Albina, M. (2002, October 8). Palestinian children show signs of trauma. Report prepared for World Vision, Jerusalem. Retrieved January 13, 2005, from www.reliefweb.int/w/rwb.nsf/s/D4EDE 6E172BBCB1FC1256C4C00517A78 Arafat, C , & Boothby, N. (2003). A psychosocial assessment of Palestinian children. Jerusalem: Secretariat of the National Plan of Action for Palestinian Children (NPA) and Save the Children. Atallah, S., & van Teeffelen, T. (2004). Diaries from Palestine 2000-2004: The wall cannot stop our stories. Bethlehem: Terra Sancta School/Sisters of St Joseph. Birzeit University's Department of Mass Communication. (2004). The impact of youth media on Palestinian youth: The Youth Times as a case study. Unpublished manuscript. Christian Aid. (2003). Losing ground: Israel, poverty and the Palestinians. London: Author. Cyrulnik, B. (2002). Les Vilains Petits Canards [Ugly ducklings]. Paris: Editions Odile Jacob. Gilligan, R. (1999). Enhancing the resilience of children and young people in public care by mentoring their talents and interests. Child and Family Social Work, 4, 187-196. Kretzman, J., & McKnight, J. (1993). Building communities from inside out. Chicago: ACTA. Newman, J. (2004, March). Protection through participation. Paper presented to the Voices Out of Conflict: Young People Affected by Forced Migration and Political Crisis conference, London. Palestine Monitor. (2004, July). [Fact sheet]. Retrieved January 13, 2005, from www.palestine monitor.org/new_web/palestinian_killed.htm

Resilience in the Palestinian Occupied Territories · 431 Sayigh, R. (1979). Palestinians: From peasants to revolutionaries. London: Zed Press. Shahade, A. (March 29,2004). A quarter of Palestinian children . . . Seek martyrdom (Translated by T. van Teeffelen). Al-Wasat supplement, Al-Hayat, Jerusalem. Retrieved in English translation January 13, 2005, from www.geocities.com/raph__ co/press/children Shehadeh, R. (1982). The third way: A journal of life in the West Bank. London: Quartet. Shehadeh, R. (1992). The sealed room. London: Quartet. Shehadeh, R. (2003). When the Bulbul stopped singing: A diary of Ramallah under siege. London: Profile Books.

Small, S., & Memmo, M. (2004). Contemporary models of youth development and problem prevention: Toward an integration of terms, concepts, and models. Family Relations, 53(1), 3-11. Unemployment, poverty grips Palestinian workers [Press release]. (2004). Geneva: International Labour Organization, Department of Communication. Retrieved January 13, 2005, from www.ilo .org/public/english/bureau/inf/pr/2004/ 24.htm van Teeffelen, T. (2004). (Ex)communicating Palestine: From bestselling terrorist fiction to real-life personal accounts. Studies in the Novel, 36(3), 438-458. Woodward, K. (2002). Understanding identity. London: Arnold.

217 RESILIENCY AND YOUNG AFRICAN CANADIAN MALES WANDA BERNARD DAVID ESTE

I

n the preface to his book "Nurturing Young Black Males", Ronald Mincy (1994b) recollects about growing up in his neighborhood, poignantly sharing how he and his brothers eluded behaviors that would have hindered their future as African Americans: The Patteson Projects provided many opportunities for us (young Black males) to make other choices (other than becoming young absent fathers) that would ruin our long-term prospects. Cocaine trafficking was rising and many of our friends were committing petty crimes. Violence was already increasing in our neighborhoods, (p. 2) My mother forbade us to "hang out" at night. On warm nights I would see a group of boys or young men hanging out under my window, laughing, smoking, drinking or gambling. My brothers and I wanted to join them but if we were not in the house before dark the punishment was swift, sure, and humiliating, (p. 2) Today, most of the young men who regularly hung out below my window are dead, physically debilitated, or incarcerated. My brothers and I were

spared this fate by my mother's convictions, courage, and wisdom, (pp. 2-3) Mincy's experiences substantiate a critical question that serves as a catalyst for academics, practitioners, policymakers, and families interested in youth resiliency; that is, what factors or forces enable some youth to overcome hostile environmertts and become productive contributors to society, whereas others remain trapped, destined to become members of what Glasgow (1981) describes as the underclass? Of particular concern are minority groups such as young African American males as described by Lee (1994): Social and economic indicators for Black male youth in America today provide a profile of a group whose quality of life is in serious jeopardy. It has become increasingly apparent that adolescent Black males are confronted with a series of obstacles in their attempts to attain academic, professional, and personal success, (p. 33) Mincy (1994b) was writing during a period when grave concerns emerged that African 433

434 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AI> American males were at high risk: "Young black men are disproportionately represented among labor force non-participants, victims, perpetrators of violence, general assistance recipients, and inmates of prisons and mental health institutions" (p. 196). Numerous writers have stressed the need to develop, implement, and evaluate programs and services to foster the social and economic empowerment of young black males. Several studies over the last 15 years (Barbarin, 1993; Brodsky, 1999; McCubbin etal., 1998; Reynolds, 1998) have focused on African American youth and their families who, despite living in a socially and economically marginalized society, have achieved success. Hill (1998) provides an insightful list of questions to investigate resiliency within African American communities and specific populations within the community: • Why do sizeable numbers of black teenage mothers have successful outcomes as adults? • Why do black adolescents living in inner cities have lower rates of smoking cigarettes, drinking alcohol, and drug abuse than white adolescents living in the suburbs? • Why are single black parents likely to have more positive developmental outcomes for their children than single white parents? (p. 50) Adolescent resiliency persistently garners attention by researchers. In comparison with their American counterparts (see Chapter 3), however, there is little literature that encapsulates the challenges of young African Canadian males. Increasingly, members of this group must cope with severe forms of "everyday racism," defined by Essed (1991) as follows: A process in which (a) socialized racist notions are integrated into meanings that make practices immediately definable and manageable, (b) practices with racist implications become in themselves familiar and repetitive, and (c) underlying racial and ethnic relations are actualized and reinforced through these routine or familiar practices in everyday situations, (p. 52) Racism and discrimination toward young African Canadian males is even more prevalent

CONTEXTS in Canadian institutions such as the educational, legal, and social service systems where members of this group are likely to be victims of racial profiling. Institutional or systemic racism is present when the established policies, rules, and regulations reflect and support differential treatment of various groups within the organization or society (James, 2003) and are used to maintain social control and the status quo of the dominant group: It refers to the way in which the rooted inequalities of society operate to justify the allocation of racial groups to particular categories and class sites. It explains how the ideas of inferiority and superiority based on socially selected physical characteristics, and which are found in society's norms and values, operate to exclude racial minority group members from accessing and participating in major social and cultural institutions, (pp. 137-138) In its comment on racism in Canada, the African Canadian Legal Clinic said: As a result of the anti-Black racism that pervades Canada's body politics, racism is entrenched in Canadian institutions, policies, and practices, so much that its institutionalized and systemic forms are either functionally normalized or rendered invisible to dominant White society. This contemporary form of racism nonetheless replicates the historical and de facto substantive conditions and effects of spatial segregation, economic disadvantage, and social division. It involves discrimination in the immigration and refugee system, the criminal justice system, employment, education, health, and other spheres in society. (Smith, Lawson, Chen, Parsons, & Scott, 2002, p. 21) This chapter is divided into three sections. The first establishes the context within which young African Canadians navigate and the challenges they may face as adults in Canadian society, particularly in the areas of education and employment. In the second section, definitions and conceptions of resiliency and other critical aspects are provided along with a summary of Hill's resiliency model developed for African Americans. The chapter concludes with reflections on resiliency from 30 African

Resiliency and Young African Canadian Males · 435 Nova Scotian males and the factors that contributed to their success in a society that continues to marginalize people of African descent. Through their stories, the strength of individuals, families, friends, and community organizations is realized and the significance of resiliency revealed.

THE CANADIAN CONTEXT Service providers, educators, and researchers have become increasingly aware of the suffering by young African Canadian males triggered by the manifestation of everyday and institutional racism that in turn severely limits educational and employment opportunities. Este and Bernard (2003) maintain: Poverty, high unemployment, underemployment, and lack of education and marketable skills are symptomatic of the reality of the social, economic, and political exclusion experienced by African Canadians. The unemployment rate for Canadians of African origin is one and a half times higher than that for the total population. This community has the lowest rate of selfemployment of all racial groups, (p. 326) Issues confronting the African Canadian community are not recent developments but rooted in historical experiences since their arrival in Canada during the 17th and 18th centuries. Although the majority either attempted to integrate into the mainstream society or expressed a desire to do so, the presence of the "color line," described by scholars such as Walker (1980) and Tulloch (1975) as subtle racism, effectively excluded the majority of African Canadians from becoming members of the broader society. When they protested their secondary status, they were battling racist attitudes that could not be destroyed. Rejected by and alienated from the dominant group, African Canadians turned inward and created their own institutions that resulted in the formation and consolidation of a distinct culture (Walker, 1980, 1995). Canadian governments have invested minimally in African Canadians. In addition to a dramatic downsizing of government and the public

sector, economic policies have focused on decreased government intervention and increased power for big business. There appears to be no understanding of the need to redress systemic discrimination with effective policies and procedures. Este and Bernard (2003) describe the economic status of African Nova Scotians as follows: There is a lack of support for Black businesses and the low business-participation is tied to high unemployment. Although the youth unemployment rate in Canada is high, the rate for African Canadian youth is even higher, particularly for males, who continue to drop out (or be pushed out) of public school at an alarming rate (Dei, Mazzucca, Mclsaac, & Zire, 1997). No business sector exists in the African Nova Scotian community to assist youth who are unemployed and unskilled, (p. 326) Bernard and Bernard (2002) argue that the history of marginalization and oppression, in addition to the systemic barriers to social and economic resources and power, has had a devastating impact on the emotional and psychological well-being of African Canadian people. Christensen (1998) refers to these phenomena as the cycle of unequal access and argues that extraordinary interventions are required if the cycle of psychological trauma that results from such limitations is to be broken. Christensen further contends that this cycle leads to low selfesteem, a sense of hopelessness, internalization of oppression and racism, anger, anxiety, and the destruction of self and others. Mensah (2002), in his work Black Canadians: History, Experiences, Social Conditions, states the following: Given the high unemployment rate for Canadian Blacks, together with their acute underrepresentation in high occupations, it is hardly surprising that they have relatively lower employment incomes for both full- and parttime workers and lower average annual income. Moreover . . . excepting the province of Quebec and the Northwest Territories, Blacks have higher unemployment rates than the average person in all Canadian provinces and territories, (p. 146)

436 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS A recent report by Statistics Canada (Milan & Tran, 2004) titled "Blacks in Canada: A Long History" presents recent data that describe the employment and economic status of African Canadians: Unemployment rates in 2001 were substantially lower than they were in 1991, but rates for Blacks were higher than those for all prime working age adults. In 1991, Canadian-born and foreign-born Blacks of prime working age both had a 12.5% age standardized unemployment rate. . . . the unemployment rate of Canadian-born Blacks dropped more than that of foreign-born Blacks. In 2001, Canadian-born Blacks had a 7.9% unemployment rate compared with 9.6% for foreignborn Blacks, (p. 7) Flegel (2004) maintains that limited job opportunities for black youth in Montreal contribute to the high levels of poverty as age and racial discrimination result in disproportionately high levels of unemployment in the workforce (p. 49). Unemployment rates convey only a partial picture of the situation for African Canadians; there is no statistical information on the number who are underemployed. For those who are employed, some critical questions emerge, such as what types of jobs they are obtaining, whether the work is full-time or parttime, and what opportunities exist for occupational advancement. Highly educated newcomers, in particular, are subjects of this phenomenon when the educational credentials acquired in their country of birth or another nation are not recognized in Canada. Milan and Tran (2004) also speculate about the reasons why African Canadians have lower employment rates and employment income and higher rates of unemployment: According to the Ethnic Diversity Survey [2003], Blacks are more likely to feel that they have been discriminated against or treated unfairly because of their ethnicity, culture, race, skin colour, language, accent or religion. Nearly one-third (32%) of Blacks aged 15 and over said they had had these experiences sometimes or often in the past five years compared with 20% of all visible minorities and 5% of those who were not a visible minority. Another 17% of Blacks rarely reported

these experiences, compared with 15% for all visible minorities and 5% of those who were not a visible minority, (p. 7) Several writers (e.g., Bernard & Bernard, 2002; Mensah, 2002) strongly contend that despite their origins, African Canadians are marginalized in terms of income and occupational status. In their description of the realities of African Nova Scotians, Este and Bernard (2003) claim that the impact of exclusion is farreaching: "African Nova Scotians are at greater risk for major health problems such as diabetes and hypertension; family and social problems, including violence and abuse; identity and selfesteem problems; and mental health challenges" (p. 326). The same authors assert that systemic racism is manifested by the overrepresentation of African Canadians in mandated services such as child welfare and by underrepresentation in voluntary services such as counseling.

African Canadian Youth and the Education System Learning to read and write are basic skills that are needed if one is to work and be a fully productive citizen. These skills are not taught to most black males. Educational systems fail to impart or inspire learning in black males of all ages, (bell hooks 2004, pp. 40-41) One of the obvious societal institutions where antiblack racism is prevalent is the education system. This is not a new phenomenon for people of African descent; historically, African Canadians were either denied access to educational opportunities or the education provided was not equivalent to that of white children. Segregated schools operated in Ontario and Nova Scotia well into the 1950s and 1960s, respectively. However, as indicated in the Black Learners Advisory Committee Report on Education ( 1994), members of the African Canadian community did not passively accept the status quo: Many lessons have been learned as one examines the history of education and the Black community in Nova Scotia. The lessons also demonstrate centuries of incredible fortitude as Black Nova Scotians, especially in their role as parents, struggled

Resiliency and Young African Canadian Males · 437 in vain to gain access to an education system that prepared the dominant population for a wide variety of roles in society while excluding Black children, (p. 1) Scott (2004) quotes a parent who describes how teachers treat African Canadian students: Teachers take it out on the Black kids. If they do well, they are wrong. If they do it right, they are wrong. When the kids do good, nobody believes them. My son did a great assignment. The teacher did not believe that he did [it] himself. His selfesteem dropped, (p. 8) Several writers (Braithwaite & James, 1996; Henry, 1994; Kelly, 1998) have examined the experiences of African Canadian youth in the school system. Henry (1994), in her book dealing with the Caribbean Diaspora in Toronto, maintains that adolescents from this community encounter challenges that may affect their educational experience, including adjusting to family reunifications, family expectations, a heterogeneous society, and the school system. Based on interviews and discussions with Caribbean immigrants, Henry contends that the most pressing issues for Caribbean students are the structural and attitudinal behaviors that prevail: "Systemic racism and the differential treatment of Caribbean students, administrators, and other students is a significant problem that directly contributes to the lack of achievement and high dropout rates in some regions" (p. 134). She states that the following practice is one of several examples of the structural and systemic barriers for Caribbean students: Differential treatment begins when students are assessed for placement into the system. This issue has a long history and sufficient time has already elapsed for the building of stereotypes about Caribbean and especially Jamaican students. They are already perceived to be slow learners and are often routinely put back by at least one year. (p. 134) Regardless of their origin, Henry's words are applicable to African Canadian adolescents in general. An example of the attitude of white teachers and students is revealed in the following

comment by a student involved in Kelly's (1998) study that focused on the experiences of African Canadian students in Edmonton: Last year, I had a teacher for social studies, and I said, "Let's do something about Black people," and he got mad at me. And all these [classmates] were saying I hate teachers . . . and that I am racist because I asked for a class on Black stuff, (p. 132) In a more recent document, Anti-Black Racism in Canada: A Report on the Canadian Government's Compliance With the International Convention on the Elimination of All Forms of Racial Discrimination prepared by the African Canadian Legal Clinic (Smith et al., 2002), issues and practices are identified that perpetuate antiblack racism in educational institutions: "Institutional and individual racist practices that push Black students out of schools occurs in subtle and overt ways, done through teacher attitudes that convey low expectations and disrespect for African Canadian students and their culture" (p. 6). One of the most serious concerns centers on the educational achievements of African Canadian students and the number of students who are forced out of the school system: A Toronto Board of Education study .. . [over] two decades showed that Black students were second to Aboriginal people in being most highly represented in basic levels of programs of study and a 1991 school survey of a Toronto area Board of Education revealed that African Canadian youth were not accumulating credits as well as other students. (Smith et al., 2002, p. 7) The report compiled by the African Canadian Legal Clinic provides statistical information related to the achievement of African Canadian students: The . . . survey showed that 36% of Black students were at risk of dropping out or failure to gather sufficient credits to graduate in six years. This compared with 20% for Whites and 18% for Asians,. . . high school students who had enrolled in 1987 also showed that by 1991, 42% of African Canadian students compared to 33% of the overall population had dropped out of school. (Smith et al., 2002, p. 7)

438 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS These statistics echo the concerns raised in 1992 in Toronto: The African Canadian community has been crying out in anguish over the poor performance of its youth in the Ontario school system. The dropout rate, the truancy rate, the failure rate, the basic streaming rate—all these pointed inexorably to the fact, where Black kids are concerned, something is terribly wrong. (Towards a New Beginning, 1992, p. 77) A participant in Dei's (1993) study provided reasons for the school dropout rate of African Canadian youth: I know of students who drop out because when they look at the issue of educational aspiration and how that is linked to employment, and they look within society and see that their own people are not attaining jobs—they are unemployed, and they themselves have got an education—they feel that the educational system is not just, and regardless of whether or not they have an education, they will not gain employment... they've reached the point that they do feel "pushed out." (p. 51) In the view of both parents and students, a combination of practices and behaviors within the school system contributes to the negative experiences of African Canadian students, particularly males. Henry (1994) summarized the factors to "include low self-esteem; the lack of Black teachers as role models; the persistent invisibility of Black studies and Black history within the curriculum" (p. 124). The report by the African Canadian Legal Clinic (Smith et al., 2002) included similar factors and additional practices such as the following:

were significantly overrepresented (18%) in the lower "basic" academic stream. More than 1 in 3 black students (36%) were at risk of leaving high school without a diploma based on their marks and credit accumulation in the core subjects of English and math (quoted from Simmons & Plaza, 1998, p. 103). The absence of role models and teachers who are sensitive to the educational concerns of the African Canadian community is usually highlighted as another major deficiency of the education system. James and Braithwaite (1996) quote UN Ambassador Stephen Lewis in his letter to Bob Rae, Premier of Ontario in 1992, where he identified this concern, "Where are the visible minority teachers? Why are there so few role models?" Solomon (1996) quotes S. H. King whose work centers on the lack of African American teachers and stresses the importance in having instructors with this background to serve as an inspiration to young males who are often led to believe that they are not able to excel academically: African American teachers are of critical importance, not just because children need to see that teachers of colour exist or that people of colour can assume leadership positions. They are needed because of their many other roles, perspectives, and practices, (p. 217) The exclusion of African heritage programs and curriculum is frequently cited as a factor that contributes to the lack of connection between African Canadian students and the education system. From the perspective of African Canadians, the curriculum is too Eurocentric, it negates black history and culture, and it imparts a very negative message that their worldview is not valued:

Streaming . . . is the process whereby African Canadian youth are pushed into high school vocational programs rather than encouraged and supported to enter the academic stream. This practice is based on the belief that Black students do not possess the intellectual ability to succeed in the more academic program, (p. 2)

The balance and perspective effectively maintains the status quo and perpetuates a worldview that places Caucasian achievement at the centre and the achievements, beliefs, and cultural practices of other people, including Black people and other people of colour, at the margin. (Henry, 1994 p. 141)

According to the Royal Commission on Learning (Begin & Caplan, 1995), black students, who make up 9% of the Toronto population,

In her personal reflections on being an African Canadian student, Kong (1996) quotes James Walker:

Resiliency and Young African Canadian Males · 439 In the Anglo-dominated schools they [Black students] have been taught that the heroes are white, the accomplishments have been attained by whites, the nation was built by whites, all of which leaves Blacks as intruders, or at best, hangers-on in a flow of history that ignores them. (p. 63) The omission of the contributions by people of African descent to Canadian society negatively affected Kong's racial and national identity. Ironically, African Canadians who are considered a "visible minority" group within the curriculum become the "invisible people." Roberts-Fiati (1996) contends that giving validation to the world of African Canadian children and youth in the classroom can have a positive impact on these individuals and that teachers should make a concerted effort to incorporate material that reflects the lives and experiences of these children in the class. The hostile environments of schools and classrooms present additional obstacles for African Canadian youth in their quest to obtain a quality education. Dei (1996) stresses that for black students, "The low teacher expectations represent deeply held beliefs about people who are non-white" (p. 46). He also notes that students in his study stated that certain teachers made fun of black students, thereby making them "feel dumb." Some black students also suffer after being told by teachers that they will "never amount to much" or the best program for them would be the vocational education route, thus facilitating and supporting the streaming process. As part of the Racism, Violence, and Health Project, African Canadian parents who attended the first annual community forum in Calgary explicitly spoke about the racism experienced by school-aged children: The perception is that a Black person is not bright enough. The teacher will block your child from entering the matriculation stream. It is up to the parents to take the responsible position to help your child succeed. Our children are placed in vocational schools. We do not know the system so our kids are affected. We keep telling our children that they have to rise above the racism by students and teachers.

I took the children out of the school system and teach them at home. The white kids are not suspended when bullying takes place. Only Black kids are suspended and are always supposed to be guilty. (Este, 2003, p. 11) The parents further described how their children are affected by their experiences at school: Our children experience racism at schools and we as parents are left to pick up the pieces. The psychological damage to our children is immense. We need to educate people about the harmful effects of racism, (p. 9) 1 am deeply concerned about the welfare of young school-aged children and what they experience at school. There is definitely a lack of role models for these children and this may hurt their motivation, (p. 7) Summarizing the experiences of African Canadian youth in the education system, James and Braithwaite's (1996) comments capture the essence of the range of issues: Indeed, racism is a theme of Black students' school experience and the source of their disenchantment with it. It is an integral part of the course content, classroom atmosphere, discipline procedures.... It is the most serious barrier to their progress in that it affects their self-esteem and sets up limitations around them in the form of expectations which, as self-fulfilling prophecy, they meet. (p. 19) Education in Western society is still viewed as the pathway to employment opportunities and economic security. The message that they must work hard to succeed in Canadian society has been repeatedly imparted to many black youth. James and Braithwaite (1996) comment on this phenomenon: "Most [Black youth] believed that education was important to their success in this society and that racism and discrimination were merely Obstacles' that they would be able to overcome through their high level of education" (p. 21). However, as Flegel (2002) remarks, "An increasing number of youths are wondering whether the pursuit of post-secondary education has any worth given the possibility that they will not be able to find jobs" (p. 41).

440 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS Flegel (2002) highlights a major finding from the study titled "The Evolution of the Black Community in Montreal: Change and Challenge," which was that a black university graduate is more likely to be unemployed than a black person who has not graduated from high school (p. 41). For African Canadian students and their parents, the struggle for an education system that addresses the needs of the youth will continue to be a protracted one. As a result, some members of this community have advocated for the development of African-centered or Africentric schools that would foster the social and academic learning of all students.

Racial Profiling The past three decades have been marked by expressions from African Canadian communities about police surveillance and attacks on African Canadian community leaders. In particular, the African Canadian community has expressed concern on several occasions about such issues as over-policing as well as police harassment and brutality. (Smith, 2004, p. 3) There are numerous definitions for racial profiling. Fredrickson and Siljander (2002) define it as follows: Racial profiling is a term that is generally understood to mean enforcement action on the part of police officers that is motivated more by racial bias than by any reasonable suspicion or probable cause that may exist under the circumstances, (p. 15) Gold (2003) also describes this practice: Racial profiling is thus profiling (i.e., identification of target criminals) based on one characteristic: race. It is an attempt to identify previously undetected criminals based upon the single factor of race (p. 394) It describes a practice in which a person's race or ethnicity influences police decisions to stop citizens, search them, or make an arrest, (p. 391) Wortley and Tanner (2003), who have written extensively on the practice of racial profiling in Toronto, provide this definition of the term:

Racial profiling . . . is typically defined as a racial disparity in police stop-and-search practices, racial differences in customs searches at airports and border crossings, increased police patrols in racial minority neighbourhoods and undercover activities, or sting operations that selectively target particular ethnic groups, (pp. 369-370) Other factors that would be taken into consideration include age (young), dress (hooded sweatshirt, baggy pants), time of day (late evening), and geography (in the wrong neighborhood). In a study conducted by the Committee to Stop Targeted Policing in 2000 in Toronto, it was found that two of three interviewees (of the 167 interviews, many of them were African Canadians from low-income neighborhoods) reported they were assaulted or threatened by police, which ranged from being beaten, slapped, punched, and maced. Threats included threats of death (37%). Other intimidation tactics included police demanding names and identification of people who had done nothing wrong (79%), being harassed (74%), being threatened with arrest (59%), being searched without good cause (54%), being issued false tickets for jaywalking (49%), being arrested on false or improper charges that were eventually thrown out (35%), and being photographed on the street without their consent. Both James (1998) and the authors of the report published by the African Canadian Legal Clinic (Smith et al., 2002) present highlights from the 1995 study titled Report of the Commission on Systemic Racism in the Ontario Criminal Justice System that depicted the daily lived experience of African Canadians. The study noted the following: •

Police stop blacks twice as often as whites, particularly black males. • The pattern of differential treatment extends from policing on the streets into halls of justice where blacks are detained more often and for longer periods. Whites are less likely (23%) to be detained before trial than blacks (30%), particularly if they are up on a drug charge (10%, against 31% for blacks). (Commission on Systemic Racism, 1995, p. 6) The material also cites statistics by Wortley that reinforce the manifestation of antiblack

Resiliency and Young African Canadian Males · 441 racism within the criminal justice system. Wortley reported: •

42.7% black males report being stopped by police in the past 2 years compared with 22.1 % whites and Asians. • 28.7% black males report being stopped twice in the past 2 years compared with whites and Asians. (Smith et al, 2002, p. 7) A quote by one of James's (1998) research participants clearly illustrates a typical experience for the African Canadian male: Once we were stopped by the police because they said that we fit the descriptions of robbery suspects. The description was that the robbers were Black. The cops asked us to open up our bags and they took everything out. (p. 167) James contends that young black men in Canada are portrayed as potential criminals who should be feared. He also adds that the characterization of these men, combined with the perception that they are listless or hanging out on the streets, contributes to the assumption that they, indeed, are potential criminals. Kelly (1998) cites two African Canadian male students whose words express the reality most black men in Canada experience or fear in relation to police: One time I was pretty much pissed off. I had come from a party and got dropped off and then I hear this car screech to a halt. I felt a hand and this cop putting me into this car. They kept saying, "I know you're lying...." Then they heard over the radio that they had caught the person. They didn't apologize; they just took off the cuffs and let me go. (p. 18) One time I was walking with a Caucasian girl and a cop pulled up . . . beside me and said, "Are you okay, Miss? Is this guy giving any trouble to you?" (pp. 18-19) The participants in Kelly's study also stated that if they walked into a store, someone watched them to make sure they were not stealing. Hence, a strong stereotype exists that perpetuates the notion that black youth, especially males, are troublemakers or "criminals" (Kelly, 1998).

In a recent report released by the Ontario Human Rights Commission (2003), the following conceptualization of racial profiling was articulated: "It includes any action undertaken for reasons of safety, security, or public protection that relies on stereotypes about race, colour, ethnicity, ancestry, religion, or place of origin, rather than on reasonable suspicion, to single out an individual for greater scrutiny or different treatment" (p. 6). This report describes the effects of racial profiling, including compromising the futures of African Canadians, creating mistrust of institutions, feeling alienated and having a diminished sense of citizenship, a negative impact on communities, changes in behavior of community members, the unseen toll, the physical effects, and the cost to society from the practice of this behavior. Many African Canadians quoted in the report commented on their lack of trust with societal institutions due to racial profiling: I do not go to the police when I have a problem. I will not do so in the future either. However, if there is a problem that absolutely requires police assistance and I can request help on the phone anonymously, so they can't see that I'm Black, then I will. (p. 27) Now I feel very afraid for my two boys. I'm afraid for them to go out. I'm scared when they go out with Black friends. They're like a magnet. It's not fair that four Black kids can't walk around, (p. 25) Others felt excluded and that they did not belong in Canadian society: [Being stopped because I was driving a car registered to a union] tells me I'm not good enough to work for a union, because I am Black. And this made me feel less than a human being. And this shows that my contribution to Canadian society is not valued, (p. 31) Examples of the experiences of African Canadian youth and their treatment by the police force in Toronto are provided by James (1998): I think that if you are Black and wearing a suit, they think that you did something illegal to get the suit. They don't think that Black people have money, (p. 166)

442 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS A bald head is a message. If you are white and you have a bald head, you are probably seen as a skinhead. If you are Black and have a bald [head], you will probably be [seen as] a gang member, (p. 166) The following is typical for African Canadian males: There was a robbery in the area. The cop pulled me over and all of a sudden a whole swarm of cops came. It was eight cops. I was with three guys. The cops said that we fit the description of the robbers who were five in number but we fit the description of three. They surrounded us and asked us to lie on the ground and they had their guns out. (p. 167) In the fall of 2002, the Toronto Star published a series of articles that examined the treatment of African Canadians by the police and legal system. The data assessed information on 480,000 incidents where an individual was charged with a crime or ticketed for a traffic offense. It also reviewed the total population of criminal charges (approximately 800,000) from 1996 to 2002. Some of the salient findings included the following: •

Although only 8.1% of Toronto's population, African Canadians comprise 34% of the drivers charged with out-of-sight traffic offenses (e.g., driving without a license, driving with a suspended license, and driving without proper insurance). • After being taken into custody, African Canadians were held for court appearance 15.5% of the time compared with 7.3% for whites. • For cocaine possession (over 2,000 cases), 41.5% of African Canadians were released at the scene compared with 63% of the whites. (Smith, 2004, p. 73) Smith (2004) concluded that based on the Star's evidence, racial profiling is an "alarming reality." Wortley and Tanner (2003) report on their recent study that provides some insight regarding racial profiling and the experiences of African Canadian high school students in Toronto:

Over 50% of the black students in the study (survey sample of approximately 3,400 high school students) reported that they had been stopped and questioned by police on two or more occasions in the two previous years, compared to only 23% of whites, 11% of Asians, and 8% of South Asians. Similarly, over 40% of black students claimed that they had been physically searched by the police in the two previous years compared to only 17% of their white and 11% of their Asian counterparts, (p. 371) This type of treatment undoubtedly contributes to the mistrust of African Canadians toward societal institutions such as the police. It is important to stress that racial profiling is not limited to the legal and educational systems but extends to other societal domains. Kelly (1998) maintains that shopping malls are sites where African Canadian youths are also profiled: The students perceive that the mall security guards often seek them out as a group to ask them to "move on" or to remove articles of clothing that signify perceived gang membership and therefore potential violence, (p. 18) One of Kelly's participants commented: They just look at y o u . . . the way you dress, and stuff like that.... Black people dress different from White people.... and the way we dress they always look at us and say that we are in a gang.... It's like we walk around as friends but they take it the wrong way. We just move with the crowd, (p. 20) Barlow (2001) argues that racial profiling will continue to exist as long as cultural stereotypes about criminal offenders, particularly those responsible for violence and drugs, are linked with race (p. 13). Consequently, there is a need for strong and effective antiracism initiatives to deal with the pain caused by racial profiling and to eradicate this severe form of systemic racism.

RESILIENCY: A CONCEPTUAL REVIEW Within the literature, a number of definitions are associated with the term resiliency. At a basic

Resiliency and Young African Canadian Males · 443 level, the ability to bounce back, recover, or successfully adapt in the face of obstacles and adversity is a common theme. For example, McGloin and Widom (2001) refer to resiliency as describing "those [individuals] who demonstrate a good outcome in spite of high risk, sustained competence under stress, and recovery from trauma" (p. 1021). Similarly, Garmezy (1991) maintains that the qualities of resilience include the tendency to rebound, recoil, or spring back and the power of recovery. Fraser, Richman, and Galinsky (1999) describe aspects of resilience as follows: •

Overcoming the odds—being successful despite exposure to high risk • Sustaining competence under pressure— adapting successfully to high risk • Attitudes (such as optimism bias), beliefs (such as commitment to conventional lines of action), and dispositional characteristics (such as an easygoing temperament) (p. 137)

A detailed definition of resilience is provided by Walsh (2003): It [resilience] involves dynamic processes fostering positive adaptation within the context of significant adversity. These strengths and resources enable individuals and families to respond successfully to crises and persistent challenges and to recover and grow from these experiences,. . . resilience involves key processes over time that foster the ability to struggle well, surmount obstacles, and go on to live and love life fully, (p. 1) Michael Rutter (cited in Hill, 1998), a British psychiatrist who is one of the leading resilience writers, stresses that resilience should be viewed as a positive phenomena—healthy responses to stressful circumstances or risk situations at various points of time in an individual's life. He contends that resilience can be enhanced in response to situations involving stress or risk or by facilitating the operation of protective mechanisms or processes. The literature describing the characteristics of resilient youth is rich. McWhirter, McWhirter, McWhirter, and McWhirter (2003) provide the following attributes:





• • •

An active approach to life's problems, including a proactive problem-solving perspective that enables the child to negotiate emotionally hazardous experiences An optimistic tendency to perceive pain, frustration, and other distressing experiences constructively The ability to gain positive attention from others both in the family and elsewhere An ability to be alert and autonomous with a tendency to seek novel experiences Competence in school, social, and cognitive areas (p. 82)

Rak, Patterson, and Lewis (1996) describe the personal characteristics of resilient children as (a) an active evocative approach toward problem solving that enables the children to negotiate an array of emotionally dangerous experiences, (b) an ability from infancy on to gain the positive attention of others, (c) exhibiting an optimistic view of their experiences even when encountering difficult situations, and (d) having the ability to maintain a positive vision of a meaningful life. However, several other factors contribute to the resiliency of youth. Within the literature, there appears to be consensus that the family milieu is a critical influence. Lee (1996) describes the significance of the family in adolescent development: The family is the chief socializing influence on adolescents. Home atmosphere, parental involvement, and family relationships shape adolescent personality and instill modes of thought and behavior important for impending adult life. Important role modeling from parents and other family members can promote socially responsible behavior and contribute to the acquisition of values and ethics, (p. 34) A positive family environment, including lack of physical crowding, consistently enforced rules with strict but fair supervision, and wellbalanced discipline, contributes to the development of resilient adolescents (Rak et al., 1996). Fraser and his colleagues (1999) claim that families may require resources such as parenting, training, financial, and other forms of support that enhance their ability to contribute to the resiliency of their adolescents. An important goal

444 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS of intervention at the family level should be the enhancement of coping abilities by the parents, which, ideally, will lead to the reduction of stress in their lives (Smith & Carlson, 1997). Writers such as Werner (1984) and Rutter (1990) contend that there also are individuals such as teachers, school counselors, coaches, mental health workers, clergy, and good neighbors in the environment who may contribute to resilient adolescents' positive outcomes. Such persons are well situated to work with youth and to convey the message that these adolescents will succeed in life.

HILL'S RESILIENCY FRAMEWORK Several frameworks provide insight and explanations related to resiliency at the individual, family, and community levels. A summary of Hill's (1998) comprehensive framework is presented here along with a discussion that incorporates the work of other writers that complement his work. Following the work of Rutter (1990), Hill's conceptual framework identifies protective mechanisms or processes that can increase the resiliency of low-income individuals and families of color. These protective mechanisms are described at three levels—individual, family, and community. Although Hill's work is based on the experiences of African Americans, it may also be useful in helping to understand the process of resiliency associated with young African Canadian males.

and concern about the welfare for others . . . Resilient children and youth place a high priority on satisfying the wishes and desires of their parents and other family members, (p. 53) Social

Competencies

Hill (1998) maintains that youth who are resilient exhibit strong social competencies and that self-esteem or self-concept is an important factor in resilient youth. In direct relation to black children, he states, "Most contemporary research in this field [resiliency] reveals that the self-esteem of black children is often equal to and sometimes higher than the self-esteem of white children" (p. 54). Those youth who are able to resist negative influences from their peers in areas such as poor school performance, substance abuse, and delinquent activities have more positive outcomes than do less resilient youth. Promoting Academic

Orientation

Not surprisingly, Hill (1998) asserts that individuals who display a strong commitment to academic success have more resilient outcomes. He contends that these youth spend more time than less resilient youth completing their homework, attending classes on a regular basis, responding to questions from their teachers, and engaging in a variety of extracurricular activities.

Family Level Individual Level At the individual level, Hill (1998) explicates internalizing positive values, enhancing social competencies, and fostering academic orientation as three protective mechanisms that foster resiliency.

Like other writers (Lee, 1994; Mincy, 1994), Hill (1998) asserts that the family plays a critical role in fostering resiliency in African American children and youth through the following five processes. Instilling Family Values

Internalizing Positive Values Hill's (1998) examination of the research articulating the strengths of people of color showed that resilient youth are more likely to have positive values: Respect for family, high regard for the elderly, strong religious orientation, personal responsibility

Parents who inculcate their children with strong family values such as respect for parents and reverence for the elderly while stressing and demonstrating the importance of children, the provision of mutual support and the need for reciprocity in social relations, are those parents most likely to establish a strong foundation on which resilience in children is based (Hill, 1998).

Resiliency and Young African Canadian Males · 445 Communications

and Social

Interactions

According to Hill (1998), primary protective mechanisms for facilitating resilience in African American families are effective communication patterns and positive interactions. Mincy (1994) also contends that these are critical processes. Control and Discipline. Mincy (1994) emphasizes the importance for African American parents to maintain control and discipline over their children. Hill (1998) describes what is required: "Discipline goes beyond correcting undesirable behavior to providing emotional nurturance for strong character development. Control also involves monitoring the in-home and outof-home activities of children" (p. 56). Provision of Academic

Support

The provision of supports to enhance the academic achievements of African American children is another element critical to their resiliency. According to Hill (1998), these may encompass frequent contact with their children's teachers, attending meetings with teachers, and serving as a volunteer for school-related activities. Such activities send a clear message that the parents are active participants in ensuring that their children succeed academically. Garibaldi (1992) asserts that there is an urgent need for African American male students, their teachers, and their parents to work together in a cooperative manner. He suggests the following strategies to encourage academic achievement: •

ability to perform successfully in the classroom. More specifically, parents need to acknowledge and, if possible, reward their sons' academic accomplishments and stress the value of learning. • Parents need greater interaction with the teachers in order to find out how their sons are performing and areas where they require assistance. • Teachers need to learn how to communicate effectively with the parents of young African male students and share with the parents their children's academic strengths as well as weaknesses. • There is an urgent need to change the negative perceptions teachers possess related to the academic capabilities of young African American males, (pp. 8-9)

Students who perform well should not be ostracized, ridiculed, intimidated, physically assaulted, or belittled by their peers. • There is a need to minimize the social and psychological stresses that academically talented African American students must confront on a daily basis. • Teachers must challenge the young Black intellectually and . . . provide them with immediate, continuous, and appropriate reinforcement as well as positive feedback for their academic accomplishments. • Parents must motivate, encourage, and reinforce their sons so that they will use their talents and

Flexible Family Role Another mechanism for facilitating resiliency in African American families is to have flexible family roles. Hill (1998) argues that the adaptability of roles in these families is demonstrated by the following: equalitarian patterns in work and household activities, shared parenting, surrogate parenting, and the functionality of single-parent families. In relation to the latter, Hill states, "Studies consistently reveal that children of black female-headed families have higher educational aspirations, higher rates of college attendance, and lower levels of high school dropouts, anti-social behavior, and substance abuse than children reared in white femalebased families" (p. 56). He also contends that the value of taking care of each other when assistance is required is another value within African American families. Este and Bernard (2003) maintained that in the context of African Nova Scotian families, there appears to be this type of reverence for the elderly who have survived a hostile environment where educational and employment opportunities were limited.

Community Protective Mechanisms Hill (1998) identifies two processes that contribute to resiliency. These include using the talents of individuals who are labeled (e.g., as "low

446 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS income") and enhancing the role of mediating structures such as informal and formal groups. Congruent with the strengths perspective advocated by Salabeey (1997), Hill (1998) recognizes that in economically challenged communities, all residents possess talent, capacity, and strength. The use of these assets is an important contribution to the facilitation of resiliency of inner-city children and youth. Informal groups such as neighbors, extended families, and peers are viewed as mechanisms to buffer or reduce the impact of stressful circumstances or negative risk factors such as racism, poverty, and limited education. According to Hill (1998) and Mincy (1994), more formal groups such as schools, social services, and youth-serving organizations are instrumental in fostering resiliency. In particular, from historical and contemporary perspectives, African American churches are viewed as a major contributor to enhancing the resiliency of African American families and children: "The historical role of churches as major contributors to the stability and resilience of African American families has been widely documented. Black churches provide regular social and economic support to black individuals at all stages of their life cycle" (Hill, 1998, p. 59). Writers such as Walker (1979, 1995), Hill (1981), and Pachai (1990) make clear that the black church was a pillar of strength within these communities. The following excerpts from Walker (1979) and Hill (1981), respectively, reinforce this viewpoint: The Black church has harboured and succoured a distinct view of life that is rich and satisfying to its members. It is, of course, a Christian definition of the meaning of life, a gospel oriented definition that has survived despite the increasing materialization of mainstream society. If Blacks hold values that are distinct from white society's, those distinctions can be traced to the central importance of the Gospel in the Black community life, as preserved and transmitted by the Black church. (Walker, 1979, pp. 86-87) Early in the 19th century, when some religious groups—particularly the Quakers—undertook to combat slavery by the Underground Railway movement, Blacks began to come into Canada,

first in small numbers, later in thousands. Their churches, which had been an important part of their life before they fled from the USA, were quickly transplanted to Canadian soil and carried on their ministry there. The earliest and most important institutions in all Black Upper Canadian communities were the churches. (Hill, 1981, p. 130)

Summary Conceptually, and from a practice perspective, it is necessary to examine adolescent resiliency as interactions between the individual and his or her environment. Walsh (2003), in capturing this viewpoint, states, "Resilience came to be viewed in terms of an interplay of multiple risk and protective processes over time involving individual, family, and large sociocultural influences" (p. 2). Fraser et al. (1999) echo Walsh's words: Although resilience is ipso facto an individual response, it is not an individual trait. It is conditioned on both individual and environmental factors. It must be viewed ecologically. . . . Resilience emerges from heterogeneity of individual and environmental influences that conspire to produce exceptional performance in the face of significant threat, (p. 138) The reflections of 30 African Nova Scotian males and their experiences living in a society where racism prevails on a daily basis constitute the balance of this chapter. These narratives clearly illustrate the interplay between individual and environmental factors that not only enable these men to survive in a hostile and demeaning environment but that also facilitate their success. The lessons extracted from their experiences may be helpful for young African Canadian males who are challenged on a regular basis in Canadian society.

AFRICAN NOVA SCOTIAN MEN'S REFLECTIONS ON RESILIENCY The critical questions explored next in this chapter are these: What are the strategies that enable black boys to survive in societies where they are expected to fail? What conditions

Resiliency and Young African Canadian Males · 447 enable them to move beyond survival to achieve success? We report here on exploratory research conducted by Bernard (1996) with African Canadian men. A reflexive analysis of that data is used to capture reflections on resiliency of African Canadian boys as self-defined after their journey into manhood. We begin with a description of the research study methodology.

The Research Study The research, Survival and Success: As Defined by Black Men in Sheffield, England, and Halifax, Canada (Bernard, 1996), was a participatory action research project. It involved two groups of black men, called research working groups (RWGs), one in each site, in an exploratory study of the strategies they used to survive and succeed in societies where they were expected to fail. Beginning from the standpoint of black men in Halifax and how they defined success, the research used a number of data-gathering methods. There were 20 individual interviews and focus groups with 10 men in each site. In addition, a conference in each site allowed for a wider community of black men and their allies to be involved in the research. This chapter reports only on the experiences of the African Canadian men who participated. Also included are data gathered in follow-up focus groups with the Halifax participants. A thematic analysis of the data was initially done by the RWGs and further developed by focus group and conference participants, thus allowing for inquiry audits and member checks (Lather, 1991). These authors conducted a secondary reflective analysis of the data. We report here on the survival strategies that emerged in the research. Black men in the two sites shared the experiences of living with everyday racism (Essed, 1991) and also shared the following survival strategies that not only helped them to survive but also to succeed: (a) positive racial identity, (b) having positive role models and mentors, (c) having a strong racial and political consciousness, (d) having positive personal values and supportive relationships with family and friends, (e) the ability to set and to work toward goals, (f) attaining education and marketable skills, (g) finding and maintaining employment or creating self-employment, and (h) spirituality.

A secondary analysis of the data was done to further understand those strategies that were specifically used by the men as they negotiated the journey through adolescence to manhood. The following strategies were identified: (a) positive racial identity; (b) having positive role models and mentors; (c) having strong supportive relationships with parents, grandparents, and extended family and community; and (d) getting an education. The men's stories are used to explore the factors that helped them make positive choices as they confronted the many challenges and barriers that black boys have to overcome to become positive, productive men in Canadian society. We begin with a discussion of what survival and success means to these men, then move on to discuss those strategies that helped them to get there.

Defining Survival and Success As one of our participants stated, "It is a wonder that we have survived at all, considering all of the obstacles that we have had to overcome. So many doors have been closed to us." Yet, despite the obstacles, black men have survived and do succeed, as they self-define it. Although we recognize that there are both positive and negative habits of survival, our focus is on positive habits or strategies. Scott (1991) asserts that positive habits include taking action to transcend, empower, love, and confront injustice. The men who have managed to survive and are perceived as successful have used a variety of strategies, which we call strategies of survival. These are strategies of mind, will, feeling, and action and are evidenced in the workplace, in schools, and in the black and women's liberation movements (Scott, 1991), as well as in the home and in social interactions. This exploratory investigation into strategies that black men use to survive began with a definition of a "successful black man" as one who had achieved or was working toward a set goal (or set of goals) using positive, constructive means that he maintained over time. Furthermore, a successful black man was considered to be one who had overcome obstacles, had managed to survive, and used his success to help bring others along. Black men were invited to tell their stories of survival and success, to

448 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS explore the strategies that helped them not only to cope but also to overcome individual and systemic barriers and to achieve a modicum of success. These men tell a story of survival at the most basic level. One man says, Survival means being able to wake up everyday, going out and trying to make it, without letting the stress of racism and racial pressure get you down. . . . part of surviving is dealing with racism every day of your life with your dignity intact. Another says, Survival... is not to be suppressed by all the negative connotations that society imposes on black men. . .. surviving means reclaiming the ability to be self-sufficient and claiming a structure that will enable black people to have a voice. Black men also comment on the need not only to survive but to succeed, and they define what they mean by success as indicated above. One participant said, "Success is not about how much money you have, the type of car you drive, or the type of work you do! It is about how you live your life." Similar themes emerged at the conference, "Black Men Surviving the 90's," where David Divine gave a keynote address titled "Successful Black Men." According to Divine (1994), Keys to success are not related to money or influence or sexual conquest, but to common, takenfor-granted values such as respect, time for self and others, integrity, being open to others, and loving oneself and others. A measure of one's success is the degree of positive influence that you bring to someone else's life, as well as your own. (p. 23) Being successful is partly determined by how to give back to the community. One participant clearly articulates this point: "When a black man makes it, he must not, should not, and cannot forget where he came from." Black men are seen as successful if they are using their success to help uplift the race. As one man said, "Even the smallest action can work wonders for someone's soul, in building someone's ability to dream and to carry out those dreams" and "successful

black men must give back and be visible in the community." These may appear to be lofty ideals for black boys to aspire to. One might ask how any young person could reach such goals. In the next section, we share the strategies that black adolescent males discussed during this study, beginning with the development of a positive racial identity.

Positive Racial Identity The majority of the participants in the study indicated that the development of a positive racial identity created by connecting with African principles and beliefs and with others in the Diaspora was a survival strategy used by black men. This includes the development of love and respect for oneself and others, a positive value system, and a connection with a black, African community and culture. The necessity of having a positive sense of self as a black person was also stressed in the focus groups, where participants argued that the maintenance of this, once success in white society is achieved, is vital to collective identity and survival. For many, developing a positive sense of self and a positive racial identity is the very essence of survival as Africans in the Diaspora. Having a strong sense of who they are is a necessary first step to being able to cope with everyday racism and racial profiling. One man tells it this way: "Our major source of strength is embracing our African roots, history, and culture. . . . it is important to know our history. . . . if we don't know our history it is difficult to plan our future." Another says, My parents taught me to love and respect myself and others. However, they also taught me that I could never forget that I am black, and therefore that I must be strong in order to survive. That is one of the most essential things about being black. Finally, one of the participants said: I was taught at an early age to be proud of being a black Nova Scotian. Growing up in the 60s and hearing James Brown sing, "Say it loud—I'm black and I'm proud," brought new meaning to

Resiliency and Young African Canadian Males · 449 black pride here in Nova Scotia. It gave me a very solid foundation from which to fight and resist the racism that was all around me. Today I pass on the same message to my children. Clearly, having a positive racial identity is a strategy that helps black boys negotiate their space and location in society as they journey to manhood. This is consistent with Hill's (1998) thesis that enhancing social competencies is a core skill for young, poor people of color to master to achieve success and resiliency. The impact of negative images and stereotyping can be overwhelming, and some internalize these messages, despising themselves and rejecting their ethnicity. However, the black men participating in this research suggest that the development of a positive racial identity—that is, a positive perception and acceptance of oneself as a black person, an African-descended person— is a vital survival strategy.

Positive Role Models and Mentors Equally important in the study was the presence of role models and mentors. Linked to the development of positive identity is one's access to a wide range of roles with which to identify. Moving through the life cycle, young people are exposed to a range of people and institutions as they define their individual identities. Black young people have limited access to positive black images and role models, whether in the education system, in society, or through the media and mass culture. Yet the accessibility and availability of positive role models, mentors, or both was identified as a successful survival strategy in this study. Black men want role models who they can emulate; however, they need to be within one's reach. As one Halifax participant said, "A role model is someone whose principles I can copy." The emphasis here is on principles, as stressed by the respondent, suggesting that the values and morals that one displays are most important. The young men in our study recognized the importance of having positive role models and mentors in their lives and are committed to the principle of giving back to the next generation. The significance of role models and mentors is illustrated in some of the men's stories.

I looked at our forefathers from slavery. They had to bear the brunt of the struggle.... I looked at the black men [in my community] who were still out there doing things, making changes while maintaining a positive outlook, and I saw them progress. . . . they help brothers like me keep going, keep moving forward. Another says, "The opportunity to see different black men that are making a living and providing for their families . . . made me want to strive to be all that I could be." And another asserts, "I think of the endless contributions that have been made and are still being made . . . it gives me strength and pride . . . and helps me to keep going. Finally, the story of this man summarizes the important role that black men play in helping black youth resist and survive. Black men have taught me how to cope with and deal with life . . . they also showed me that I could succeed, as I modeled myself after people who were positive success stories. I was able to disregard the negative men in my life. Having access to role models and mentors was clearly a positive strategy for resilience for these young African Canadian men. This is consistent with Hill's (1998) assertion that having formal and informal community supports serves as a protective mechanism. Similarly, Werner (1984) and Rutter (1990) emphasize the significance of coaches, teachers, and other community members as sources of support for at-risk youth.

Family and Community Equally important is the role of family, extended family, and community, which was identified by all the participants. It is an extra bonus when one's parent(s) can also be role models and mentors. All participants in this study stressed the importance and significance of family and community support. The role of mothers in particular was seen as central in recounting contributions made to the individual and collective survival of African Canadian men. However, the role of fathers and the issue of father responsibility were also noted as a

450 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS significant factor that helped black men to succeed. In addition, the role of grandparents and extended family and community supports were identified as positive factors that helped adolescent boys navigate their way through to adulthood. Some of the men talked about the significance of their parents and grandparents: My parents and grandparents were all strong, stable people. I could count on them to be there . .. and to challenge me on the negative things.... I did not want to do anything bad to disgrace them, or lose their support and respect. My mother provided me with all the positive advice and experiences possible.... She was a good role model as a parent... I never got into trouble with the law, thanks to my mother . . . I was always more afraid of disappointing her than I was scared of the police. I had such respect for her that I would never do anything to bring her shame or distress. . . . My father was there even when I did not know it. His presence, though limited, made me realize how much we counted on him in the family for all sorts of things. Now that I am a father myself, I realize some of the things that he did, he did because he wanted to protect us from harm. He died before I could thank him for his contributions to my survival, but I tell my children about him every day in my words and my actions. Some of the strengths and ability to make positive choices can be traced back to early teachings in the family. My grandmother influenced me greatly by teaching me the value of life is something we find in ourselves . . . and that helped me to always make good choices. My grandparents . . . paved . . . a pattern of lifestyle that I could follow. My grandmother taught me about human emotions; everyone can cry. My grandfather taught me to be strong. These values have taught me to say no to certain things instead of yes and kept me out of trouble. These views are consistent with what other researchers such as Hill (1998), Lee (1994), and Rak etal. (1996) have stressed, that flexible family roles and family and community supports help facilitate resiliency.

The notion of communities sharing responsibility for child rearing and helping young black people negotiate the barriers on their journeys also emerged in this research. For example, one participant who grew up in a small semi-urban community said, "Black communities always offer a sense of comfort, support, and l o v e . . . . the community has been an integral part of the black man's life, a place where love is not denied." Another offered a similar perspective: People from the community offered a lot of support, encouragement, and guidance . .. some who were able to see the potential in me and others . . . when every parent in the community had high expectations of their children.... There were so many community-based groups and organizations that gave us a place to develop our skills and to grow and mature. For many black youth, their homes and communities are not only sites of love, resistance, and support, they are also the first teachers. In Halifax in particular, the role of the Church and church-led organizations has been instrumental in the survival of the community in general and for African Nova Scotian young men in particular. Of the participants, 85% stated that the Church has been instrumental in the survival of black men and black people in general in Nova Scotia. All participants in Halifax believe that the Church also has a role in the future survival of black men; furthermore, this role has to be more proactive and radical in the spiritual and social development of black men and the black community as a whole.

Education as Key to Survival In addition to the role of the Church and community, participants noted the importance of both formal and informal education. This is similar to Hill's (1998) finding that formal and informal groups in low-income communities are vital to their members' survival and success. A responsive education system is seen as one of the most important influences on black youth because education is key to survival. However, the findings in this study indicate that black men's experiences in education have been

Resiliency and Young African Canadian Males · 451 largely negative. Participants repeatedly defined their experience as one of miseducation because of Eurocentric curriculum and racist educational practices. Although the majority of this sample have attained higher levels of education, most have done so at great personal sacrifice. Many returned to education after leaving school early, and those who stayed did so in an atmosphere that discouraged and demotivated them. As black men in this study reflected on their educational journey, they shared stories about the people who helped influence them and helped to keep them in school. For many, the involvement of their parents was key. One man explains: My parents encouraged us and showed us the advantages of a good education in preparing for . . . one's future,. . . they influenced me to achieve an education . . . to develop leadership qualities and responsibilities . . . and to be the best that I could be. Another man further elaborates on the theme of informal education: My survival, and that of my brothers and sisters depended on a good solid foundation, which was provided by our family. Our parents instilled in all of us positive values, strong moral values, and a sense of responsibility . . . thus providing us with a better opportunity to survive.... we all did well in school because we knew we had to, as tough as it was to constantly fight the negativity. Hill (1998) also maintains that resiliency is more readily attained when parents help facilitate an academic orientation. In this study, education was seen as a tool for empowerment, a survival strategy that could not be taken from black men, even though the literature is replete with examples of the many challenges that black youth experience in education. So how is it that some black youth survive? In addition to the supports provided by parents, siblings, and positive role models and mentors, many of the black men in this study shared stories of the positive influences that some of their teachers had been. For some, it was simply having someone who believed in them, whereas for others, it was being motivated

by teachers who appeared to care. This is consistent with Werner's (1984) and Rutter's (1990) thesis that teachers have the potential to be contributing factors in youth's resiliency. The following stories are illustrative: Early in my life I did have one teacher that was influential. She showed that she was concerned and it didn't matter what my color was. She was the first white woman that helped me to understand the ability that I had to do well if I applied myself and filtered out the negativity. I recall a white male teacher who was positive and encouraging. He helped me to believe in myself and to understand that I could achieve goals if I set them and stayed on target regardless of what others said or did. For some others, the drive to succeed is motivated by the negativity one experiences from those in authority. For example, one participant talked about the difficult transition from segregated to integrated schooling. I was at the top of the class in my local community school where all my teachers and classmates looked like me. But when I went to the integrated high school, I quickly learned to hate school. I remember a teacher telling me that I was going nowhere, and that is when I decided to hit the books and prove him wrong.

DISCUSSION African Canadian men are victims of cultural pain and disenfranchisement brought on by institutional, systemic, individual, and cultural racism and sexism. This is a shared phenomenon, based on their everyday experiences and realities. Those who survive and succeed do so against incredible odds. They are not alone. The stories in this chapter reveal some of the influences of family and community that help black boys successfully negotiate their passage to manhood. What are the implications for families, social workers, and health care professionals who are working with at-risk African Canadian youth today? Communities could offer "rites of passage" programs that help black youth navigate

452 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS their way through life transitions. Such programs would help empower youth and their caregivers as they create collective strategies of resistance and resilience. Parents and grandparents could apply the Africentric principles in their daily lives. Living the principles of Africentricity could serve as a buffer from the harsh realities of a racist and race-conscious society. The lessons learned from these men's reflections could serve young black men today as they negotiate their space in society. The value of positive racial identity, strong role models and mentors, formal and informal education, and family and community supports could be developed as road maps for young African Canadian youth as they face today's challenges. We have identified problems in the education system, the legal system, and the society at large, all of which create barriers for African Canadian youth. The struggle to challenge these structural barriers needs to continue. More allies are needed in the struggle. Social workers and health care professionals could advocate for structural change within their agencies and within the wider community. African people need to reclaim their space in society. Africentricity allows us to step back to our origins, in order to move forward.

REFERENCES Barbarin, O. A. (1993). Coping and resilience: Exploring the inner lives of African American children. Journal of Black Psychology, 19, 478^192. Barlow, H. (2001). Driving while black: Observations on the practice and future of racial profiling in the U.S. Journal ofIntergroup Relations, 38(3), 3-15. Begin, M., & Caplan, G. (1995). For the love of learning: Report of the Royal Commission on Learning. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Royal Commission on Learning. Bernard, C , & Bernard, W. T. (2002). Learning from the past/visions for the future: The black community and child welfare in Nova Scotia. In B. Wharf (Ed.), Community work approaches to child welfare (pp. 116-130). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Broadview Press.

Bernard, W. T. (1996). Survival and success: As defined by black men in Sheffield, England and Halifax, Canada. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK. Black Learners Advisory Committee. (1994). BLAC report on education—redressing inequity: Empowering black learners: Vol. 1. Summary. Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada: Author. Braithwaite, K., & James, C. (Eds.). (1996). Educating African Canadians. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: James Lorimer. Brodsky, A. (1999). Making it: The components and process of resilience among urban, AfricanAmerican single mothers. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 69(2), 148-160. Christensen, C. P. (1998). Social welfare and social work in Canada: Aspects of the black experience. In V. D'Oyley & C. James (Eds.), Re/Visioning: Canadian perspectives on the education of Africans in the late 20th century (pp. 36-55). North York, Ontario, Canada: Captus Press. Commission on Systemic Racism in the Ontario Criminal Justice System. (1995). Report of the Commission on Systemic Racism in the Ontario Criminal Justice System. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Queen's Printer for Ontario. Dei, G. S. (1993). Narrative discourses of black/ African parents and the Canadian public school system. Canadian Ethnic Studies, 3, 49-64. Dei, G. S. (1996). Listening to voices: Developing a pedagogy of change from the narratives of African Canadian students and parents. In K. Braithwaite & C. James (Eds.), Educating African Canadians (pp. 32-57). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: James Lorimer. Divine, D. (1994). Successful black men. In W. T. Bernard (Ed.), Black men surviving the 90's conference proceedings (pp. 12-26). Sheffield, UK: University of Sheffield. Essed, P. (1991). Understanding everyday racism: An interdisciplinary theory. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Este, D. (2003). Racism, violence, and health project: Report on the community forum held January 29, 2003. Unpublished report. Este, D., & Bernard, W. T. (2003). Social work practice with African Canadians: An examination of the African Nova Scotian community. In A. AlKrenawi & J. Graham (Eds.), Multicultural social work in Canada: Working with diverse ethno-racial communities (pp. 306-337). Don Mills, Ontario, Canada: Oxford University Press.

Resiliency and Young African Canadian Males · 453 Flegel, P. (2002, February). Challenges to Canadian multiculturalism: The case of the black Montreal. Canadian Issues, pp. 39-41. Flegel, P. (2004). Intersecting oppressions in urban Montreal: A hip-hop perspective. Canadian Diversity, 3(1), 47-49. Fraser, M., Richman, J., & Galinsky, M. (1999). Risk, protection, and resilience: Toward a conceptual framework for social work practice. Social Work Research, 23, 131-142. Fredrickson, D., & Siljander, R. (2002). Racial profiling: Eliminating the confusion between racial and criminal profiling and clarifying what constitutes discrimination and persecution. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Garibaldi, A. (1992). Educating and motivating African American males to succeed. Journal of Negro Education, 61(4), 4-11. Garmezy, N. (1991). Resilience in children's adaptation to negative life events and stressed environments. Pediatric Annals, 20(9), 459-466. Glasgow, D. (1981). The black underclass: Poverty, unemployment and entrapment of ghetto youth. New York: Random House. Gold, A. (2003). Media hype, racial profiling, and good science. Canadian Journal of Criminology and Criminal Justice, 45(3), 391-399. Henry, F. (1994). The Caribbean diaspora in Toronto: Learning to live with racism. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University of Toronto Press. Hill, D. (1981). The freedom seekers: Blacks in early Canada. Agincourt, Ontario, Canada: Book Society of Canada. Hill, R. (1998). Enhancing the resilience of African American families. Journal of Human Behaviour in the Social Environment, 1(2/3), 49-61. hooks, bell. (2004). We real cool: Black men and masculinity. New York: Routledge. James, C. E. (1998). Up to no good: Blacks on the streets and encountering police. In V. Satzewich (Ed.), Racism and social inequality in Canada (pp. 157-176). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Thompson. James, C. E. (2003). Seeing ourselves: Exploring ethnicity, race, and culture (3rd ed.). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Thompson. James, C. E., & Braithwaite, K. (1996). The education of African Canadians: Issues, contexts, expectations. In K. Braithwaite & C. E. James (Eds.), Educating African Canadians (pp. 13-31). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: James Lorimer.

Kelly, J. (1998). Under the gaze: Learning to be black in white society. Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada: Fernwood. Kong, Ν. H. (1996). Confronting a history of exclusion: A personal reflection. In K. Braithwaite & C. E. James (Eds.), Educating African Canadians (pp. 58-68). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: James Lorimer. Lather, P. (1991). Getting smart: Feminist research and pedagogy with/in the post modern. New York: Routledge. Lee, C. (1994). Adolescent development. In R. Mincy (Ed.), Nurturing young black males (pp. 33^14). Washington, DC: Urban League of America. McCubbin, H., Fleming, W., Thompson, Α., Neitman, P., Elver, K, & Savas, S. (1998). Resiliency and coping in "at risk" African-Canadian youth and their families. In H. McCubbin, E. Thompson, A. Thompson, & J. Futrell (Eds.), Resiliency in African American families (pp. 287-328). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McGloin, J. M., & Widom, C. S. (2001). Resilience among abused and neglected children grown up. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 1021-1038. McWhirter, J., McWhirter, B., McWhirter, E., & McWhirter, R. (2003). At-risk youth: A comprehensive response (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Brooks/ Cole-Thompson Learning. Mensah, J. (2002). Black Canadians: History, experience, social conditions. Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada: Fernwood. Milan, Α., & Tran, K. (2004, Spring). Blacks in Canada: A long history. Canadian Social Trends, 72, Statistics Canada Catalogue No. 11-008-2.7. Mincy, R. (1994a). Conclusions and implications. In R. Mincy (Ed.), Nurturing young black males (pp. 187-203). Washington, DC: Urban League of America. Mincy, R. (1994b). Why this book? In R. Mincy (Ed.), Nurturing young black males (pp. 1-5). Washington, DC: Urban League of America. Ontario Human Rights Commission. (2003). Paying the price: The human cost of racial profiling: Inquiry report. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Author. Pachai, B. (1990). Beneath the clouds of the promised land: The survival of Nova Scotia's blacks: Vol. 2, 1800-1989. Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada: Black Educators' Association. Rak, C , Patterson, L., & Lewis, E. (1996). Promoting resilience in at-risk children. Journal of Counseling and Development, 74(4), 368-373.

454 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS Reynolds, A. (1998). Resilience among black urban youth: Prevalent, intervention effects, and mechanisms of influence. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 68, 84-100. Roberts-Fiati, G. (1996). Assessing the affects of early marginalization on the education of African Canadian children. In K. Braithwaite & C. E. James (Eds.), Educating African Canadians (pp. 69-80). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: James Lorimer. Rutter, M. (1990). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. In J. Rolf, A. S. Masten, D. Cicchetti, K. H. Nuechterlein, & S. Weintraub (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 181-214). New York: Cambridge University Press. Salabeey, D. (1997). The strengths perspective in social work. New York: Longmans. Scott, J. (2004). English language and communication issues for African and Caribbean immigrant youth in Toronto. Retrieved January 14, 2005, from http://ceris.metropolis.net/Virtual%20 Library/ education/scottl.html Scott, Y. K. (1991). The habit of surviving. New York: Ballantine Books. Simmons, Α., & Plaza, D. (1998). Breaking through the glass ceiling: The pursuit of university training among African-Caribbean migrants and their children in Toronto. Canadian Ethnic Studies Journal, 30(3), 99-120. Smith, C. (2004). Crisis, conflict, and accountability. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: African Canadian Community Coalition on Racial Profiling. Smith, C , & Carlson, B. (1997). Stressing, coping, and resilience in children and youth. Social Science Review, 71(2), 231-256. Smith, C. C , Lawson, E., Chen, M., Parsons, M., & Scott, S. (2002). Anti-black racism in Canada:

A report on the Canadian government's compliance with the international convention on the elimination of all forms of racial discrimination. African Canadian Legal Clinic. Retrieved January 14, 2005, from www.aclc.net/antiba_ table.html Solomon, R. P. (1996). Creating an opportunity structure for Blacks and other teachers of colour. In K. Braithwaite & C. E. James (Eds.), Educating African Canadians (pp. 216-233). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: James Lorimer. Towards a new beginning: The report and action plan of a four-level government/African Canadian community working group. (2003). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: City of Toronto. Tulloch, H. (1975). Black Canadians: A long line of fighters. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: NC Press. Walker, J. W., St. G. (1979). Identity: The black experience in Canada. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Ontario Educational Communications Authority. Walker, J. W., St. G. (1980). A history of blacks in Canada: A study guide for teachers and students. Hull, Quebec, Canada: Minister of State for Multiculturalism. Walker, 3. W., St. G. (1995). African Canadians. In P. Magocsi (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Canada's peoples (pp. 139-176). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University of Toronto Press. Walsh, F. (2003). Family resilience: A framework for clinical practice. Family Process, 42(1), 1-18. Werner, Ε. E. (1984). Resilient children. Young Children, 40(1), 68-72. Wortley, S., & Tanner, J. (2003). Data, denials, and confusion: The racial profiling debate in Toronto. Canadian Journal of Criminology and Criminal Justice, 45(3), 367-389.

28 VIOLENCE PREVENTION PROGRAMMING IN COLOMBIA Challenges in Project Design and Fidelity Luis

F. DUQUE

JOANNE KLEVENS MICHAEL UNGAR ANNA W . LEE

F

or the past several years, violence has ranked first as the major cause of mortality in Colombia (Colombia Ministerio de Salud, 1994a) and is one of the most frequent causes for hospitalization, emergency care, and disability, generating nearly 25% of the burden of disease (Colombia Ministerio de Salud, 1994b). Although the burden for disease for intentional injury at the world level is 3.4% (World Health Organization. 2000), this figure stands at 44.8% in the case of Medellin, Colombia's second largest city with 2 million inhabitants. If one counts all types of violent injuries, the figure rises to 56.6% (Londono, Grisales, Fernandez, & Cadena, 1999). In other words, in Medellin, the healthy years lost for injuries resulting from intentional violence are nearly equal to that for all other causes of death

or injury combined. Of the 10 Colombian cities with the largest population, Medellin has the second highest rate of violent deaths (Colombia. Instituto Nacional de Medicina Legal y Ciencias Forenses, 1999, 2000), currently amounting to 60 homicides per 100,000 inhabitants per year. Of all kidnappings in the world, 60% occur in Colombia, and 1 of every 5 kidnappings in the world takes place in the province of Antioquia (whose capital is Medellin). Unlike what many people believe, violence generated by leftist or rightist armed groups does not constitute the major violence problem in Colombia. It is estimated that only between 5% and 20% of total homicides in the country can be attributed to the armed insurgence (Colombia. Instituto Nacional de Medicina Legal y Ciencias Forenses, 2000; Comision de Estudios sobre la 455

456 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS Violencia, 1987). Guerrilla violence accounts for only one third of all violence-related costs in the country; two thirds of these attacks are the result of urban violence related to daily conflicts or organized crime (Colombia. Departamento Nacional de Planeacion, 1998).

VIOLENCE PREVENTION IN DEVELOPING WORLD CONTEXTS Several longitudinal studies have been conducted to determine what factors predict violence and delinquency. Still others have examined factors that prevent violence. However, almost all these have taken place in developed countries with the resources to support alternatives to violence, as well as the structural supports to deliver programs and ensure their fidelity. A program's fidelity refers to the degree of adherence the program shows to the principles and procedures of the program model as it is implemented across different settings. Early violence prevention programs, then, are almost exclusively designed to fit contexts that are different from communities with levels of violence unheard of in the developed world. To date, the majority of these types of programs have been experimental or have been implemented among limited subgroups of the populations in the United States and Canada. The best known exception is the Head Start project initiated in 1965, which has included over 15 million children from many different cultural backgrounds in United States and which has undergone notable changes since its beginning (General Accounting Office, 1997). Such programs typically target one or two behaviors such as prevention of future violence and criminality, alcohol abuse, or drug abuse, or improved prosocial behavior and academic performance. Few have simultaneously targeted multiple behavioral problems, measured impact, or shown successful outcomes (Shonkoff & Meisels, 2000). Positive outcomes, as other chapters in this volume demonstrate, are associated with resilience among those vulnerable to the influence of violence. These health-enhancing behaviors have the potential to influence the transgenerational transmission of violence by addressing personal,

familial, and community factors that propel cycles of violence. The challenge is to conceptualize a violence prevention program that can adapt to the exigencies of a developing country facing the enormous challenges brought about by violence. If such a program is to have a meaningful impact on the ability of children in the developing world to overcome the adversity they experience related to their exposure to violence, then the program must be adaptable to the contexts in which the majority of the world's children live. For the past 5 years, community and academic stakeholders in Medellin have designed and implemented the Early Prevention of Aggression Project, which is, as far as we know, the first such initiative undertaken on a large scale in a Latin American country. The project targeting individual and family risk factors seeks the early identification of children with high probabilities of developing aggression in public schools and day care centers and works with their teachers and families to prevent violent behavior. Its efforts are intended to create the conditions children aged 3 to 9 need to grow up psychologically well and without the threat of violence. As cornerstones of everyday resilience (Masten, 2001), it is reasonable to assume that violencefree homes and communities, the provision of alternative coping strategies and resources that make violence unnecessary among children and their caregivers, and education about violence and its impact that sensitizes educators and their communities to the need to address the root causes of violence would combine to create the conditions necessary for more children to achieve positive outcomes associated with resilience. These outcomes would be expected even if other adversities associated with living in a country struggling to develop economically persist. In this chapter, we explore the Early Prevention of Aggression Project design, and we address some of the challenges it has faced in its implementation. Specifically, this chapter explores some of the findings from the first stages of a longitudinal evaluation of the project currently underway. It addresses shortcomings in the fidelity of the project and raises questions regarding expectations that violence prevention programs like it can be imported from developed country contexts. More broadly, experiences implementing the project

Violence Prevention Programming in Colombia · 457 attest to the barriers that professionals and communities encounter when attempting to create structural supports such as safer communities that are foundational to children's resilience.

THE PROBLEM OF AGGRESSION IN CHILDREN Several studies, including two recent Colombian studies, reveal that the population of serious aggressors globally (those involved in theft, armed assault, or sexual assault) is small (Duque & Klevens, 2000; Duque, Klevens, & Ramirez, 2003a, 2003b; Farrington, 1995; Farrington & West, 1993), although they generate a high volume of victims. Studies conducted in various developed countries also reveal that aggression and antisocial behavior in children is one of the best predictors of aggression and criminality in adulthood, particularly among male children, who present with more serious and earlier manifestations (Brame, Nagin, & Tremblay, 2001; Keenan & Shaw, 1994; Loeber, 1982; Nagin & Tremblay, 1999; Olweus, 1979). It has been reported that approximately 14% to 17% of all children have behavior problems (Campbell, 1995). The proportion found in Itagiii, a municipality next to Medellin, Colombia, is 2 3 % (Duque & Klevens, 2001). Depending on the measurement of "behavior problems" used and the time of application, the population under study (high risk versus community samples), time of follow-up, and study design (prospective or retrospective), between 30% and 84% of children with these behavior problems in preschool persist with behavior problems through adolescence and adulthood (Campbell, 1995; Farrington, 1992; Farrington & Maughan, 1999; Kratzer & Hodgins, 1997; Stattin & Magnusson, 1991). There is a significant continuity between childhood aggression and adolescent aggression (Brame et al., 2001) and adult violence (Eron & Huesmann, 1990; Farrington, 1991; Thornberry & Krohn, 2003). Aggression in a child generates a "snowball" effect (Patterson, Capaldi, & Bank, 1991; Patterson, DeBaryshe, & Ramsey, 1989; Pepler & Slaby, 1996). His or her behavior generates rejection from parents, who resort to increasingly more severe disciplinary methods. It also produces rejection from teachers, which, coupled

with the child's impulsiveness and hyperactivity, hampers learning, elevating the risk of failure and school dropout. Likewise, it causes rejection among schoolmates (Coie & Kupersmidt, 1983). These aggressive children, once isolated, tend to seek out peers with similar problems (Cairns & Cairns, 1994). During adolescence, these children have a higher risk of early use of alcohol and drugs, alcohol-related problems, premature and promiscuous sexuality, possession of weapons, affective and occupational instability, and violence against women (Caspi, Elder, & Bern, 1987; Farrington, 1995). Whether these problems are consequences of early aggression or other underlying common risk factors remains to be determined. They do underscore, however, the need to intervene early and comprehensively. Arguably, sustainable interventions that target these multiple negative behavioral outcomes need to be developed and evaluated for developing countries such as Colombia.

Risk Factors for the Appearance or Persistence of Early Aggression The appearance of early aggression and a high persistence of aggression in a subgroup of children lead some to believe that its roots are found in early childhood or prior to birth. Factors that might be significant prior to birth include a family history of criminality, which becomes particularly important when coupled with an inadequate environment for upbringing (Cadoret, Yates, Troughton, Woodworm, & Stuart, 1995), "in-uterus" exposure to alcohol (Streissguth, Sampson, & Barr, 1999), perinatal complications (Kandel & Mednick, 1991), and certain temper traits (Chess & Thomas, 1992). Gender is perhaps one of the most influential factors in violent behavior. Males, particularly youths, predominate in both victims and aggressors of all types of violence, with the exception of verbal or indirect violence. Such predominance appears during the preschool years (Tremblay et al., 1999) and cannot be attributed completely to biological factors but, instead, must also be accounted for by children's socialization processes (Kruttschnitt, 1994; Pepler & Slaby, 1996). The family is the first place in which the child observes and learns his or her initial behavior. Families with children having behavior problems

458 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS are characterized by high levels of conflict, stress (Campbell, 1995; Loeber & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1986), and violence (Klevens, Restrepo, Roca, & Martinez, 2001; Tolman & Bennett, 1990). Parents of aggressive children tend to use coercive upbringing strategies more frequently (Rothbaum & Weisz, 1994). Maltreatment is, in particular, one of the most frequent factors singled out as a determinant of aggression, although the majority of maltreated children do not become aggressors themselves (Widom, 1989). A greater tendency to tolerate the child's coercive behavior (e.g., disobedience, fits, or aggression toward others) and ignore prosocial behaviors has also been observed in these families (Patterson et al., 1991). Both child abuse and witnessing family violence (Klevens et al., 2001), as well as a lack of clarity in norms, poor parental supervision, and a family history of crime and interpersonal violence are factors associated with aggression in Colombian studies of violent behavior (Duque, Klevens, & Ramirez, 2003a, 2003b). Some studies, including those conducted in Colombia, have reported that the perception of having a mother overwhelmed by problems is significantly more frequent among delinquent populations (Klevens et al., 2001; Klevens & Roca, 1999). This might constitute another factor that could disrupt interactions between a mother and her son and her capacity to supervise or correct him adequately. It has been reported that in communities of low socioeconomic status, family traits account for 53% of the variation in aggressive behavior, whereas in high-socioeconomic status communities, family characteristics are responsible for a mere 3 % of the variance (Pagani, Boulerice, & Tremblay, 1997). Retrospective studies of resilient men in Colombia compared with men who became involved in crime or substance abuse showed that resilient men perceived stronger support from families and described their mother as a strong person who had taken charge of the situation, introducing stability amid life stressors, and stressing the importance of education and work; they also showed greater degrees of control and coherence in their lives and had fewer and more selective friends. Resilient men also reported less arbitrary and physical punishments and more affection, acceptance, and supervision from their caregivers than did aggressive persons (Klevens, Restrepo, & Roca, 2000; Klevens &

Roca, 1999). Another Colombian study on the interrelationships between individual, family, peer, and societal factors has shown the importance of family in promoting resilience. The availability of drugs, low familial contact, and low identification with one's heritage were offset by a close parent-child relationship, thereby leading to less marijuana use. In addition, all cultural and ecological factors related to violence were buffered by a close parent-child mutual attachment. Important to our discussion here, the protective effect of family appeared to be more important in Colombia than among youth in the United States (Brook et al., 1998; Brook, Brook, De la Rosa, Whiteman, & Montoya, 1999).

Prevention Possibilities Fortunately, there exist various effective alternatives to prevent or modify the course of early aggression. In a systematic review of literature on successful experiences for early prevention conducted for the Medellin Violence Prevention Program (Klevens, Tremblay, & Corporacion Presencia Colombo Suiza, 2000), 21 programs were found that complied with the following parameters: (a) based on a sample of 50 or more children under 12 years of age, (b) used a control group for comparison, and (c) using standardized instruments, measured results in terms of aggression or antisocial behavior at least 1 year following the intervention. Unfortunately, as noted above, all these programs took place in developed countries. There is a dearth of research on violence prevention efforts in developing countries. Seven types of activities were identified through this review, with the following results: 1

1. Interventions promoting cognitive skills generate a high impact when carried out during preschool age and when combined with the development of other types of skills in the child, such as planning. It is not clear if parental involvement in educational activities constitutes an essential component. 2. Workshops for children that consolidate social interaction skills are effective for children with behavior problems as well as for high-risk children, but the cognitive content should be adapted to children under the age of 7.

Violence Prevention Programming in Colombia · 459 3. Workshops for parents in the contingent and consistent management of child behavior reveal 60% to 75% reductions on a short-range basis in aggressive and antisocial behavior among children aged 3 to 8. The impact is lower in children whose families are low income or without social support and in which there exists violence or intrafamily conflict. These workshops alone will not generalize the results to other contexts such as school, and hence intervention in two environments is recommended: family and school. 4. Training of educators in the contingent and consistent management of behavior of the child decreases problems in the classroom, but it will not suffice to decrease the incidence of delinquency. 5. Home visits during the early years of life are effective, particularly when they succeed in modifying the course of life of the mother and mobilizing her support network. 6. Administration of medication to aggressive children contributes to the performance of children with hyperactivity and attention disorder in such a way that they can profit more from school activities or any other type of intervention. 7. Linking high-risk children with mentors seems to have a positive impact, but existing evidence is still weak given the limited number of evaluations conducted on this intervention. A review of the literature reveals other results that can be expected from these types of programs (Barnett, 1995, 1998; Karoly et al., 1998; Kellermann, Fuqua-Whitkey, Rivara, & Mercy, 1998; Miller, 1994; Webster-Stratton, 1991; Yoshikawa, 1995): •



Decreased criminal activity and enhanced social behavior: Projects in which this effect was measured revealed positive results in the sense that, once reaching adolescence or adulthood, children who participated in the early prevention of violence projects had a lower incidence and less serious violations of legal norms and better social behavior standards than did children in control groups. Better cognitive and emotional development: A favorable effect was observed on children's

IQ during the intervention or immediately following it; however, this effect tends to fade a few years after the intervention is completed. Only some very high-quality interventions conducted during the early years tend to present positive, long-lasting effects in IQ level. • Improved educational results: In the majority of the projects, groups participating in the programs showed better academic performance, less repetition of grade levels, fewer dropouts, and a higher high school graduation rate than did the comparison groups. These effects are even higher when the intervention has a longer duration and when it takes place at the preschool and elementary school level. Given the multiple causes of the early appearance of aggression and antisocial behavior, combined interventions are recommended to modify various factors simultaneously in the child's different contexts and expand their influence over several years (Tremblay & Craig, 1995; Wasserman & Miller, 1998). Such multilevel intervention addressing individual, familial, community, and cultural factors associated with the risks that predict violence in children is typical of scores of other initiatives targeted at enhancing functioning among populations under stress. As other chapters in this volume demonstrate, however, we have seldom understood the complexity of multilevel intervention in contexts other than those of developed countries. Despite the fact that many communities in developed countries may resemble those in the developing world in terms of systemic prejudice, marginalization, and poverty, arguably, the proximity of these communities to others of privilege makes their situation distinct from that of environments such as Medellin, which lacks national resources equal to those of the developed nations on which it seeks to model its interventions.

THE MEDELLIN VIOLENCE PREVENTION PROGRAM One of the authors (Duque) directed the team that designed and initiated the implementation of the Program for Peaceful Coexistence in Medellin, funded by the city with an international loan. This program had six components: (a) promoting nonviolence among children and youth,

460 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS (b) bringing justice closer to communities, (c) social communication as a promoter of nonviolence, (d) violence surveillance, (e) institutional modernization, and (f) community participation in monitoring and evaluation (Duque, 2000). The Early Prevention of Aggression Project, designed by the second author (Klevens), was one of the projects in the first component, "Promoting Nonviolence Among Children and Youth," of the Program for Peaceful Coexistence. At the center of the Early Prevention Project are interventions for children in public schools and day care centers. By 2005, the project should reach half of all children aged 4 to 9 in the city of Medellin—that is, nearly 107,000 children in 452 schools and 56 day care centers located in marginal areas, with high rates of violence and scarce social and economic resources. With this project, the municipality hopes to prevent aggression and delinquency, use of psychoactive substances, and low school performance and dropout in children from these communities, to ensure social and economic progress. The Early Prevention of Aggression Project was designed in response to the alarming violence figures for the municipality of Medellin (Duque, 2000). To our knowledge, this project is the first of its kind to be conducted on a large-scale basis in Latin America or other developing country—hence, the importance of establishing the effectiveness of this program with a child population. In the following sections, we will describe the original design, the changes introduced to the design, and the problems observed in the preliminary evaluation to extract some of the lessons learned along the way.

Design and Implementation of the Project The Early Prevention of Aggression Project, once designed, went through extensive consultations with local experts and municipal servants to adjust the design to the local context. Through the various discussions with these participants, it became clear that there was discomfort with the theoretical base (conductive social learning) instead of a psychodynamic orientation, which was the orientation further adopted by the secretary of education of the Medellin municipality. There was also discomfort with a foreign intervention model. Unfortunately,

there were no rigorously evaluated Colombian experiences or experiences based in psychodynamic theory from which to draw. The Early Prevention of Aggression Project was proposed to do two things: (a) train teachers to teach and promote prosocial skills in the classroom and manage children's behavior contingently, consistently, and nonviolently and (b) provide support and skills to parents of children already manifesting behavior problems to help them manage their child's behavior contingently, consistently, and nonviolently. In this way, the total population of children in selected schools and classrooms participates in the project, but only those children with behavior problems receive additional support in their homes. For the school intervention, a manual was adapted from existing sources (McGinnis & Goldstein, 1990, 1997; Shure, 1994; Slaby, Roedell, Arezzo, & Hendrix, 1995; Slaby, Wilson-Brewer, & Dash, 1994; Sobel, 1983) to provide teachers with a basic understanding of the strategies proposed and activities for the classroom. Teachers volunteered to participate in the program. They were trained by faculty members of the University of Antioquia and a nongovernmental organization whose mandate is to address violence. In the initial design, groups of 10 to 12 teachers would attend a 2-hour workshop facilitated by a trained professional once a week for 17 consecutive weeks. During this time, the professional trainer would visit each teacher in his or her group twice a month and provide on-the-job feedback. After the 17 weeks of training, the trainer would continue to meet monthly with each teacher's group for 6 more months to provide support and troubleshoot problems. The purpose of the training was to encourage teachers to do the following: •

Establish clear norms and routines as well as clear and predictable procedures in their classrooms to create a predictable, safe, and nurturing school climate • Modify the physical and programmatic environment of their classrooms and schools to reduce opportunities for conflict and promote opportunities for cooperation and sharing • Shape prosocial behaviors and strategies for the pacific resolution of conflicts

Violence Prevention Programming in Colombia · 461 •

Favor encouragement of positive behavior over punishment for the negative • Teach nearly 40 prosocial skills, which include courtesy norms, understanding of one's feelings and those of others, assertive communication, and strategies for the peaceful management of conflict

The manual prepared for the teachers included a series of specific activities to be conducted. It was also expected that teachers would address aggression in students in a contingent and consistent manner and acquire the habit of shaping prosocial behavior in children during all classroom and playground activities. In fact, some of these modifications in the classroom and school have been nothing short of a cultural shift in emphasis among some educators. The project's message was that teachers can greatly affect the patterns of violence among students and that their work will create conditions that buffer children's exposure to violence and offer alternatives that may seed resilience. The goal, then, was that the project would influence (a) the incidence of aggressive behaviors among students, (b) the initiation of psychoactive substance (alcohol, illicit drugs) and tobacco use, (c) the initiation of risky and delinquent behaviors, and (d) school performance and dropout.

Determining Level of Risk As part of an ecological intervention, simultaneously with work done in the schools with educators, support was to be provided for the families of children with more serious behavior problems. These children were identified through an instrument adapted and normed for the Medellin population, known as the COPRAG, a measure of children's prosocial and aggressive behavior (Agudelo et al., 2002). The instrument, which is completed by parents or teachers of the children, includes 45 questions using a 3-point Likert scale, covering four subscales: prosocial skills, proactive aggression, reactive aggression, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. One additional question asks about the child's school performance. To develop this measure, 53 items representing conceptual definitions for "prosocial behavior" and "aggressive behavior" were selected from

Tremblay et al.'s (1996) National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth, Achenbach's (1991) Child Behavior Checklist, and Correa and Olaya's (1999) Pediatric Symptom Checklist. Other scales were developed from work by Apodaca, Lopez, and Etxebarria (1998). Items in English were translated and back translated to ensure comparability. The original 53-item measure (filled out by teachers) was tested in a sample of 83 school-aged children from three different schools in Bello (part of metropolitan Medellin but excluded from the intervention). The revised measure was applied to another sample of students (N = 714) from 20 schools in Medellin. Resulting data were factor-analyzed using principal components factor analysis. A five-factor solution was selected based on eigenvalues, the proportion of variance explained by the factor, and the meaningfulness of the items in each scale. The resulting factors appeared to represent aggression (14 items), prosocial behavior (11 items), hyperactivity and attention deficit (8 items), depression and anxiety (7 items), and indirect aggression (5 items). The scale appeared to be internally consistent with a Cronbach's alpha of .934, for the complete measure, ranging from .88 to .937 for each of the subscales. The subscales were significantly correlated with each other, with coefficients ranging from .076 (between prosocial behavior and indirect aggression) to .67 (between physical aggression and indirect aggression). A score of 30 on the aggression subscale, which corresponded to the 90th percentile, was considered the cutoff point for aggressive behavior, while a score of 31, which corresponded to the 25th percentile, was taken as the cutoff point for lack of prosocial behavior. To establish interrater reliability, the primary caregiver of a subsample (from the previous sample) of children (n = 165), was also asked to respond to the measure. Concordance between teachers and caregivers was low, with a kappa of 0.24. However, the teachers' reports were strongly associated with caregivers' reports of a perceived "behavior problem" for the child. Information on domestic violence, family alcohol and drug use, and parent discipline techniques as well as the child's social information processing deficits was also gathered. The Conflict Tactics Scale (Strauss, 1979) was proposed to measure violence among adults in the

462 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS family, and a modified version was proposed to measure violence toward the index child. A measure for social information-processing deficits was designed based on work by Slaby and Guerra (1988) and Dodge and Coie (1987). Twelve pictures depicting social interactions were developed. Separate pictures were made for girls and boys so that the characters in each picture were of the same gender as the respondent. Three vignettes represented accidental situations (e.g., a boy rushes by and knocks things off your desk), three represented prosocial situations (e.g., you are on the floor and another child asks you if you need help), three represented ambiguous situations (e.g., you ask a child sitting on a bench with a ball if he'll lend you his ball and he says "no"), and three represented aggressive situations (e.g., a child cuts in line in front of you). Children were asked (a) to describe the problem, and responses were coded on the number of hostile interpretations given; (b) what they would do in this situation, with probing for alternatives by interviewers; and (c) what the consequences would be if they chose to carry out the most aggressive response they had proposed. Shortly after the beginning of the academic calendar year, teachers participating in the project completed the COPRAG instrument on all students in their classes. COPRAG test score determination was calculated by university faculty members rather than by teachers, to avoid stigma in the school environment.

Family-Based Interventions for Children at Highest Risk of Aggression The second component of the proposed intervention consisted of training professionals to act as advisers to families of those children identified with scores at or above the 90th COPRAG test percentile. The design called for weekly home visits and monthly parent group workshops during 1 year, with the parents advancing to a new learning module once they showed that they could apply the knowledge gained in a daily situation. Based on adult learning principles (Knowles, 1980), family advisers were expected to facilitate critical analysis among parents of their interaction and communication patterns with their child, including their

discipline and supervision techniques, and model these practices for parents. The manual for family advisers adapted material from various sources (Dinkmeyer & McKay, 1989; Garber, Garber, & Spizman, 1987; Patterson, 1976; Slaby et al., 1995) that was judged to be socially and culturally appropriate in these communities. Parents were to be encouraged to (a) clearly communicate their expectations with regard to positive or negative behaviors expected of their child, (b) adequately supervise the child's behavior, (c) praise the child's positive behavior, and (d) provide negative consequences, with no aversion ("time-out" or loss of privileges) in response to aggressive behavior. The institutions in charge of the different components of the intervention (School of Humanities and Social Sciences of the University of Antioquia for elementary school teacher training, Colombian Society for Psychoanalytic Care (ODRES) for preschool teacher training, and the Catholic Bolivarian University for training of family advisers) were selected competitively after a public announcement. The intervention as described was presented to officials from the Secretary of Education Office and various universities in Medellin. At this presentation, it became evident that the social cognitive model on which the intervention is based was not the theoretical orientation accepted by the participants, who favored a more psychodynamic approach. Thus, the objectives of the project were reformulated so that instead of reducing aggressive behavior and promoting prosocial behavior, it would promote insight into current child-rearing and child management practices among teachers and parents. A psychodynamic orientation (i.e., assumption that aggression is innate and that promoting insight will lead to behavior change) was adopted for the project implementation (Dominguez, 2002). Further changes included modification of the 2-week training of trainers that we proposed reduced to 2 days by the municipality because of costs. The quantitative measures proposed to collect data for follow-up and evaluation were deemed too burdensome and culturally inappropriate and were replaced with qualitative data (interviews and field diaries). In addition, the institutions selected for training the family advisers negotiated with the municipality a

Violence Prevention Programming in Colombia · 463 reduction in the number of home visits (from weekly to two meetings overall) and parent group workshops (from monthly to six sessions in total). To replace the home visits and workshops families were to receive, family advisers offered each family two family counseling sessions at the advisers' offices. The institutions training the teachers also negotiated reductions in their component: instead of 17 workshops spaced over 17 weeks, they would do 10 concentrated in 10 days, with no bimonthly on-thejob training sessions, although they would do bimonthly workshops. As the implementation began, family advisers were allowed to include families with children with scores less than the 90th percentile on the COPRAG, once they completed service for the children falling within the 90th percentile and above, to use available funds in their contract. During the first year of intervention, the institutions in charge of training teachers adapted the manual to give it a psychodynamic orientation. Finally, in 2003, a group of consultants recommended that the crux of the intervention shift from children and families to schools to deal with culture and school climate instead of individual behavior (Montoya, Montoya, Pardo, & Alvarez, 2003). To date, the project has been implemented continuously for 3 years with the following community coverage (see Table 28.1).

Evaluation In total, 8,900 children from child care centers and schools participated in the first cohort of implementation in 2001. Of these, 579 scored above the 90th percentile on the COPRAG and were initially eligible for the family-based Table 28.1

Phase Pilot, 2001 Second, 2003 Third, 2004 Total

Coverage of the Early Prevention of Aggression Project, Medellin, Colombia Teachers and Caregivers

Schools and Day Care Centers

361

57

1,394

163

338

46

1,778

266

intervention. Because there was room in the budget to fund interventions with more families, 1,853 additional families received direct support from the project. To examine the effectiveness of the project, 339 youth were selected from the first cohort of the project in 2001 and are now being studied by the University of Antioquia and Dalhousie University. Of these 339 children, 115 scored above the 90th percentile on the two aggression subscales of the COPRAG, 154 between the 75th and 89th percentiles, and 107 below the 75th percentile. A control group of 339 youth was matched by age, gender, and socioeconomic status to those children in the intervention cohort. Initial data from this evaluation show great variation in program participation during implementation of the project. Although some sites had shown great enthusiasm and included the participation of directors, teachers, and caregivers, others implemented the program only partially or hardly at all. Similar variations of participation were found among the families who received the additional support and training. Thus, based on the qualitative data collected, schools, day care centers, teachers, caregivers, and families were classified as A (good), Β (fair), and C (poor or not participating). The classification criteria and breakdown of ratings are as detailed in Tables 28.2 and 28.3. Implementation in day care centers and with day care workers was very positive: Educators working with preschool children adhered to the program closely, with all 138 educators from 22 day care centers participating well in most aspects of the intervention. However, the poor participation rates of elementary school teachers were disconcerting; 56% demonstrated poor levels of participation. Our results contrast sharply with expected levels of participation for programs on which the Medellin Project were based. For example, in their report on rates of participation for a comparable program, Tremblay and his colleagues (Tremblay, Pagani-Kurtz, Masse, Vitaro, & Pihl 1995; Vitaro, Brendfen, & Tremblay, 1999) report that 87% of the teachers in the schools involved completed the initial rating of aggression among their students. The differences in implementation may be explained by variation in expectations by school

464 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS Table 28.2 Fidelity

Fidelity Criteria for the Early Prevention of Aggression Project School or Day Care Center

Teacher, Day Care Giver

Principal trained and committed > 80% teacher participation Accomplished three program phases

Attended 9-10 of 10 training sessions

> 1 parent attended 9-10 of 10 training sessions

50%-79% teacher participation Accomplished two or three program phases

Attended 8 of 10 training sessions

> 1 parent attended 6, 7, or 8 of 10 training sessions

Less than 50% teacher participation Accomplished two or three program phases

Attended < 7 of 10 training sessions

> 1 parent attended 5 or fewer of 10 training sessions

Table 28.3

Family

Fidelity to the Early Prevention of Aggression Project Fidelity A

Β

C

Total

4

7

22

33

Percentage

12

21

67

100

Number

22

2

0

24

Percentage

92

8

0

100

Number

26

9

22

57

Percentage

46

16

39

100

Number

72

19

132

223

Percentage

32

9

59

100

Number

138

0

0

138

Percentage

100

0

0

100

Number

210

19

132

361

58

5

37

100

531

513

1.398

2.442

22

27

57

100

Institutions Schools Day care centers Total institutions

Number

Teachers Teachers Day care givers Total teachers

Percentage Families

Number Percentage

and day care administrators regarding their staff's participation. In the elementary schools, only those teachers who wanted to participate were trained, meaning that the project may have been implemented in only one or two classrooms within each school. In contrast, all the teachers in the preschools were trained, along with support staff. This degree of saturation appears to

have made a significant difference in the quality of project implementation. The reluctance among schoolteachers to participate may, however, be related to the overcrowded conditions in many teachers' classrooms, with teachers responsible for 40 to 45 students each. Day care group sizes are much smaller. Furthermore, no incentives were given to the teachers to add these

Violence Prevention Programming in Colombia · 465 violence prevention activities to their already full workload. Even more troubling, however, is that of the 2,242 families involved in the more intensive intervention, only 22% were considered to have participated at a "good" level. Another 2 1 % had "fair" levels of participation, and the majority (57%) showed "poor" levels of participation. These numbers also contrast with reports of participation by Tremblay (Tremblay et al., 1995; Vitaro et al., 1999) who found that 35.9% of families of aggressive children in the program they initiated refused to participate in the intervention, and another 8.4% dropped out before the end of the intervention. Although this is a notably high rate of poor participation, our experience in a developing-world context shows that families are even more likely than families in developed countries to avert participation in such programs. Staff members who were tasked with contacting families report that the main barrier they experienced in engaging parents in the project was finding an adult caregiver of the identified child with whom to work. Frequently, caregivers were absent from their homes during the hours that family advisers were willing and able to visit the families. Advisers were limited in the number of hours they could safely move around the communities, given the level of violence present in the communities in which they provided outreach. In addition, as noted earlier, the implementation team was allowed to conduct only a 2-day workshop to explain the project in general to those professionals contracted through partner institutions to train teachers. This gave the implementation team only enough time to present the empirical support for the project and its content. Team members could not discuss the theoretical orientation in detail or use strategies to better engage staff and families.

Lessons Learned Implementing a large-scale violence prevention project in a community such as Medellin where there are significant stressors necessarily requires adaptation. Addressing risk through structural change and community-wide support is a noteworthy ambition, but the practicalities of implementation in a developing-world

context make success less ensured. Looking back, ideally, we would have begun with a pilot project to test the feasibility and efficacy of the proposed intervention in this new setting, with members of the core pilot project team doing the intervention to ensure initial fidelity and philosophical congruence rather than taking this intervention to such a large scale and hoping that third parties would implement it appropriately. Although a pilot phase was proposed, pressure by the municipality to increase coverage forced us to carry out the work initially with 57 schools (hardly a "pilot"). We also learned the importance of identifying and working with allies. As previously explained, the two institutions hired to train the teachers and day care workers did not agree with the social cognitive orientation from the start. Finding partners who shared the theoretical orientation of the proposed project probably would have ensured better rates of participation and fidelity. In addition, we feel strongly that trainers would have needed at least 2 weeks (instead of 2 days) of training to become adequate project instructors. Better skill development and a lengthier orientation for trainers would have increased their buy-in to the project goals as well as increased the likelihood that they acquired the skills needed to model and provide appropriate feedback to the teachers they trained. There were, of course, reasons why the project was implemented so quickly. Although we enjoyed ample community and institutional participation, culminating in an initially large number of project sites, it was still a very long and drawn-out process that seriously delayed implementation and ran the risk of failing altogether when the government changed and threatened to end the project. Given the adjustments made to the project during the multiple discussions with high levels of participation by community members and school administrators, we expected that the project was feasible and acceptable in the local context in which it would be implemented. However, such contextualization did not occur, and many of the better aspects of the intervention were watered down or lost during the constant give and take among stakeholder groups. Initial outcomes that have been documented show that these problems have influenced the

466 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS project's effectiveness. Qualitative analyses of field diaries suggested that teachers had changed in their ways of thinking about children's aggressive behavior and had clear ideas of how to intervene in conflict situations, but their actions remained unchanged. We believe that it is possible that, given the psychodynamic orientation of the revised intervention, trainers spent a great deal of time promoting "insight" or awareness of inappropriate teaching practices but did not model or practice the specific skills that were to be implemented as part of the original project design. Even if teachers had wanted to do the intervention in the ways intended, they may not have gained the skills needed to do so during their training. The most important lesson learned is that a large city, such as Medellin, is able to invest substantial sums of money and resources in a long-term preventive intervention and is able to persist in its implementation despite all the difficulties we have observed in these first few years of a project such as ours. The Peaceful Coexistence Program was designed and planned with the participation of 500 experts from 80 entities: experts from the Mayor's Office, officials from various government offices (Police Department, Army, Forensic Medicine, the Attorney General's Office, Child Services, etc.), representatives from private foundations and nongovernmental organizations, academics and researchers from the main universities, and community and church leaders. This ample participation in the initial design of the Peaceful Coexistence Program may have contributed to its sustainability. Support from the mayor elected in 2000 who was deeply committed to social change and who made available sufficient resources to fund the project through the negotiation of a loan from the Inter-American Development Bank also ensured the project's long-term viability. Although a project of this magnitude is a huge undertaking for any community, it is particularly so for one plagued by an epidemic of violence such as Medellin. This intervention is allowing us to investigate the impact of an early violence prevention program on children in a developing country. The structure, having borrowed program elements from developed countries, will allow us to compare findings

across settings. As such, the project has shown many strengths from which we can learn much about what is required to conduct violence prevention work in the developing world. The project has been implemented in very dangerous neighborhoods. It has managed to include an evaluation component. There has been a high degree of local commitment by the municipality and business and education leaders, as well as an international team of experts and researchers who assist with the project. However, even with these strengths, demonstrating the effectiveness of the project over time will be difficult. The validity of many of the items borrowed for inclusion in the COPRAG has not been established in Spanish. Finally, like many such programs, issues of fidelity in program implementation have raised concerns about measures of the program's effectiveness. These drawbacks, not unique to this program, highlight the difficulties in implementing comprehensive violence prevention programs in developing countries and the challenges of demonstrating their effectiveness. Although initial participation rates are disappointing, we feel the work is still valuable, not only for those children whose caregivers do participate fully but to investigate what are feasible and effective practices for other developing countries, countries in transition, and even economically developed countries that have communities with high rates of violence. In this way, efforts to implement this project are enhancing our understanding of the cultural specificity necessary to implement violence prevention programs that create the structural conditions necessary to create and sustain resilience among child populations in the developing world.

NOTE 1. Programs reviewed included Abikoff (1991); Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group (1999a, 1999b); Durlak, Furhman, and Lampman (1991); Graziano and Diament (1982); Greenberg, Kusche, Cook, and Quamma (1995); Guerra, Eron, Huesmann, Tolan, and VanAcker (1996); Hawkins etal. (1992); Hinshaw, Klein, and Abikoff (1998); Kazdin (1987, 1997); Kazdin, Siegel, and Bass (1992); Kolvin et al. (1981); Lochman (1992); Long, Forehand, Wierson, and Morgan (1994); McCarton

Violence Prevention Programming in Colombia · 467 etal. (1997); Miller (1994); O'Donnell, Hawkins, Catalano, Abbott, and Day (1995); Olds et al. (1998); Schweinhart, Weikart, and Lamer (1986); Seitz, Rosenbaum, and Apfel (1985); Soloman, Watson, Deluchi, Schaps, and Battistich (1997); Spencer et al. (1996); Strayhorn and Weidman (1991); Tierney and Grossman (1995); Tolan and MacKay (1996); Tonry and Farrington (1995); Tremblay, Masse, Pagani, and Vitaro (1996); Wasserman and Miller (1998); Webster-Stratton (1985, 1991); Webster-Stratton and Hamond (1997); Webster-Stratton, Kolpacoff, and Hollinsworth (1988).

REFERENCES Abikoff, H. (1991). Cognitive training in ADHD children: Less to it than meets the eye. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 205-209. Achenbach, T. (1991). The child behavior checklist. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont. Agudelo, S. L. M., Giraldo.C. Α., Gaviria, L. M. B., Sandoval, C. C. Α., Rodriguez, Μ. Α., Gomez, C. J. E, Gallon, L. Α., et al. (2002). Caracteristicas de las familias y escuelas relacionadas con los comportamientos agresivos y prosociales en ninos y nirias de 3 a 11 anos. [Characteristics of the families and schools related to the aggressive and prosocial behaviors in children 3 to 11 years old]. Medellin, Colombia: Universidad de Antioquia, Colciencias, CES. Apodaca, P., Lopez, F., & Etxebarria, I. (1998). Conducta prosocial en preescolares. [Prosocial conduct in pre-schoolers]. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 82, 45-61. Barnett, W. S. (1995). Long-term effects of early childhood programs on cognitive and school outcomes. The Future of Children, 5, 25-50. Barnett, W. S. (1998). Long-term cognitive and academic effects of early childhood education on children in poverty. Preventive Medicine 27, 204-207. Brame, Β., Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2001). Developmental trajectories of physical aggression from school entry to late adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 503-512. Brook, J. S., Brook, D. W., De la Rosa, M., Duque, L. F, Rodriguez, E., Montoya, I., & Whiteman, M. (1998). Pathways to marijuana use among adolescents: Cultural/ecological, family, peer and

personality influences. Child Adolescence and Psychiatry, 37, 759-766. Brook, J. S., Brook, D. W., De la Rosa, M., Whiteman, M., & Montoya, I. (1999). The role of parents in protecting Colombian adolescents from delinquency and marijuana use. Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine, 153, 457-465. Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Woodworth, G., & Stuart, M. A. (1995). Genetic-environmental interactions in the genesis of aggressivity and conduct disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 916-924. Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways of youth in our times. New York: Cambridge University Press. Campbell, S. B. (1995). Behavior problems in preschool children: A review of recent research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 113-149. Caspi, Α., Elder, G., & Bern, D. (1987). Moving against the world: Life-course patterns of explosive children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 308-313. Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1992). Dynamics of individual behavioral development. In M. D. Levine, W. B. Carey, & A. C. Crocker (Eds.), Developmentalbehavioral pediatrics. Philadelphia: Saunders. Coie, J., & Kupersmidt, J. (1983). A behavioral analysis of emerging social status in boys' groups. Child Development, 54, 1400-1416. Colombia. Departamento Nacional de Planeacion. (1998). La paz: El desafio para el desarrollo [La Paz: The challenge for development]. Bogota, Colombia: Tercer Mundo Edit. Colombia. Instituto Nacional de Medicina Legal y Ciencias Forenses. Centro de Referencia Nacional sobre Violencia. (1999). Lesiones de causa externa, Colombia 1998 [Injuries of external origin, Colombia 1998]. Bogota, Colombia: Author. Colombia. Instituto Nacional de Medicina Legal y Ciencias Forenses. Centro de Referencia Nacional sobre Violencia (2000). Forensis 1999 Datos para la vida. Instituto Bogota, Colombia: Author. Colombia. Ministerio de Salud. (1994a). La Salud en Colombia. Diez Anos de Informacion [Health in Colombia. Ten years of information]. Santa Fe de Bogota, Colombia: Litografia ARCO. Colombia. Ministerio de Salud (1994b). La Carga de la Enfermedad en Colombia [The load of the

468 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS disease in Colombia]. Santa Fe de Bogota, Colombia: Litografia ARCO. Comision de Estudios sobre la Violencia. (1987). Colombia: Violencia y democracia [Colombia: Violence and democracy]. Bogota, Colombia: Universidad Nacional de Colombia. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1999a). Initial impact of the fast track prevention trial for conduct problems: I. The high risk sample. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 631-647. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1999b). Initial impact of the fast track prevention trial for conduct problems: II Classroom effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 67, 648-657. Correa, M. L., & Olaya, A. (1999). Validacion del Pediatric Symptom Checklist para detectar riesgos psicosociales en escolares [Validation of the Pediatric Symptom Checklist to detect psychosocial risks in students]. Unpublished masters thesis, University of Antioquia, Medellin, Colombia. Dinkmeyer, D., & McKay, G. D. (1989). STEP (Systematic Training for Effective Parenting): The parent's handbook. Circle Pines, MN. American Guidance Service. Dodge, Κ. Α., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children's peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146-1158. Dominguez, F. (2002). Observaciones al proceso de aplicacion del modelo de prevencion temprana de la agresiôn. Pautas de educaciôn y crianza [Observations of the process of application of the model of early prevention of aggression. Guidelines for education and child-rearing]. Medellin, Colombia: Secretaria de Educaciôn, Alcaldia de Medellin. Duque, L. F. (2000). El Programa de Convivencia Ciudadana de Medellin [Program for peaceful coexistence in Medellin]. Medellin, Columbia: Cargraphics. Duque, L. E, & Klevens, J. (2000). La Violencia en Itagui, Antioquia (I): Prevalencia y Distribution [Violence in Itagui, Antioquia (I): Prevalence and distribution]. Biomédica, 20, 151-68. Duque, L. E, & Klevens, J. (2001). La Violencia en Itagui, Antioquia (II): Factores asociados [Violence in Itagui, Antioquia (I): Associated factors]. IQEN, 6(\\), 161-170.

Duque, L. R, Klevens, J., & Ramirez, C. (2003a). Cross sectional survey of perpetrators, victims, and witnesses of violence in Bogota, Colombia. Journal of Epidemiology Community Health, 57, 355-360. Duque, L. E, Klevens, J., & Ramirez, C. (2003b). Overlap and correlates of different types of aggression among adults: Results from a crosssectional survey in Bogota, Colombia. Aggressive Behaviour, 29, 191-201. Durlak, J. Α., Furhman, T, & Lampman, C. (1991). Effectiveness of cognitive-behavior therapy for maladapting children: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 202-214. Eron, L. D., & Huesmann, L. R. (1990). The stability of aggressive behavior. In M. Lewis & S. M. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (pp. 147-156). New York: Plenum. Farrington, D. P. (1991). Childhood aggression and adult violence: Early precursors and later life outcomes. In D. J. Pepper & Κ. H. Rubin (Eds.), The development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp. 5-29). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Farrington, D. P. (1992). Criminal career research in the United Kingdom. British Journal of Criminology, 32, 521-536. Farrington, D. P. (1995). The Twelfth Jack Tizard Lecture. The development of offending and antisocial behavior from childhood: Key findings from the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 360, 929-964. Farrington, D. P., & Maughan, B. (1999). Criminal careers in two London cohorts. Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 9, 91-106. Farrington, D. P., & West, D. J. (1993). Criminal, penal and life histories of chronic offenders: Risk and protective factors and early identification. Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 3, 492-523. Garber, S. W„ Garber, M. D., & Spizman, R. F. (1987). Good behavior. New York: St. Martin's Press. General Accounting Office. (1997). Head Start: Research provides little information on impact of current program (GAO/HEHS-97-59). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Graziano, A. M., & Diament, D. M. (1982). Parent behavioral training: An examination of the paradigm. Behavior Modification, 16, 3-38. Greenberg, M. T, Kusche, C. Α., Cook, E. T., & Quamma, J. P. (1995). Promoting social competence in school-aged children: The effects of

Violence Prevention Programming in Colombia · 469 the PATHS curriculum. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 117-136. Hawkins, J. D., Catalano. R. K, Morrison, D. M., O'Donnell, J., Abbott, R. D., & Day, L. E. (1992). The Seattle Social Development Project: Effects of the first four years on protective factors and problem behaviors. In J. McCord & R. E. Tremblay (Eds.), Preventing antisocial behavior: Interventions from birth to adolescence (pp. 139-161). New York: Guilford Press. Hinshaw, S. P., Klein, R. J., & Abikoff, H. (1998). Childhood attention deficit disorder: Nonpharmacologic and combination treatments. In P. Nathan & J. Gorman (Eds.), A guide to treatments that work (pp. 3-24). New York: Oxford University Press. Kandel, E., & Mednick, S. A. (1991). Perinatal complications predict violent offending. Criminology, 29, 519-530. Karoly, L. Α., Greenwood, P. W., Everingham, S. S., Hoube, J„ Kilburn, M. R„ Rydell, C. P., Sanders, M., et al. (1998). Investing in our children: What we know and don't know about the costs and benefits of early childhood interventions. Santa Monica, CA: RAND. Kazdin, A. E. (1987). Treatment of antisocial behavior in childhood: Current status and future directions. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 187-203. Kazdin, A. E. (1997). Parent management training: Evidence, outcomes and issues. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1349-1356. Kazdin, A. E., Siegel, T. C , & Bass, D. (1992). Cognitive problem solving skills training and parent management training in the treatment of antisocial behavior in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 733-747. Keenan, K., & Shaw, D. S. (1994). The development of aggression in toddlers: A study of low-income families. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 22, 53-77. Kellermann, A. L., Fuqua-Whitkey, D. S., Rivara, F. P., & Mercy, J. (1998). Preventing youth violence: What works. Annual Review of Public Health, 19, 271-292. Klevens, J., Restrepo, O., & Roca, J. (2000). Some factors for explaining resilience among young men in Colombia. Revista de Salud Publico, 2, 165-172. Klevens, J., Restrepo, O., Roca, J., & Martinez, A. (2001). Risk factors for adult male criminality in

Colombia. Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 11, 73-85. Klevens, J., & Roca, J. (1999). Nonviolent youth in a violent society: Resilience and vulnerability in the country of Colombia. Violence and Victims, 14, 311-322. Klevens, J., Tremblay, R., & Corporation Presencia Colombo Suiza. (2000). Prevencion temprana de la violencia: Estrategias de Intervention [Early prevention of violence: Strategies for intervention]. Medellin, Colombia: Alcaldia de Medellin. Knowles, M. (1980). The modern practice of adult education: From pedagogy to androgogy. Chicago: Follett. Kolvin, I., Garside, R. F , Nicol, A. R., MacMillan, Α., Wolstenhome, R, & Leitch, I. M. (1981). Help starts here. New York: Tavistock. Kratzer, L., & Hodgins, S. (1997). Adult outcomes of child conduct problems: A cohort study. Abnormal Child Psychology, 1, 65-81. Kruttschnitt, C. (1994). Gender and interpersonal violence. In A. J. Reiss & J. A. Roth (Eds.), Understanding and preventing violence: Vol. 3. Social influences (pp. 293-376). Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Lochman, J. E. (1992). Cognitive-behavioral intervention with aggressive boys: Three-year follow up and preventive effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 48, 1181-1209. Loeber, R. (1982). Stability of antisocial behavior: A review. Child Development, 53, 1431-1446. Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1986). Family factors as correlates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and delinquency. In N. Tonry & M. Norris (Eds.), Crime and justice: An annual review of research (Vol. 7, pp. 29-150). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Londono, J. L., Grisales, H., Fernandez, S. J., & Cadena, E. (1999). Anos de vida saludables perdidos por la poblacion de Medellin. [Lost years of healthy life among the population of Medellin]. Rev. Facultad Nacional de Salud Publico, 77(1), 63-92. Long, P., Forehand, R., Wierson, M., & Morgan, A. (1994). Does parent training with young noncompliant children have long term effects? Behavioral Research Therapy, 32, 101-107. Masten, A. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American Psychologist, 56(3), 227-238. McCarton, C. M., Brooks-Gunn, J., Wallace, I. E, Bauer, C. R., Bennett, F. C , Berbaum, J. C ,

470 · INTERVENING ACROSS CULTURES AND CONTEXTS Broyles, S., et al. (1997). Results at age 8 years of early intervention for low birth weight premature infants. The infant health and development program. JAMA, 277, 126-132. McGinnis, E., & Goldstein, A. P. (1990). Skill-streaming in early childhood. Teaching prosocial skills to the preschool and kindergarten child. Champaign, IL: Research Press. McGinnis, E., & Goldstein, A. P. (1997). Skill-streaming the elementary school child. Champaign, IL: Research Press. Miller, L. S. (1994). Preventive interventions for conduct disorder: A review. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 3, 405-419. Montoya, E. M., Montoya, M., Pardo, V. M., & Alvarez, B. (2003). Una memoria reflexiva sobre el proceso de asistencia técnica en la implementation de un modelo de prevention temprana de la agresividad, pautas de education y crianza 2000-2003 [A reflective memory on the process of technical attendance in the implementation of a model of early prevention of aggressiveness, guidelines for education and childrearing 2000-2003]. Medellin: Corporation Colombo Suiza. Nagin, D., & Tremblay, R. E. (1999). Trajectories of boys' physical aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically violent and nonviolent juvenile delinquency. Child Development, 70, 1181-1196. O'Donnell, J., Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. E, Abbott, R. D., & Day, L. E. (1995). Preventing school failure, drug use, and delinquency among low income children: Long term intervention in elementary schools. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 65, 87-100. Olds, D., Henderson, C. R., Cole, R., Eckonode, J., Kilzman, H., Luckey, D., Pettit, L., et al. (1998). Long-term effects of nurse home visitation on children's criminal and antisocial behavior. JAMA, 280, 1238-1244. Olweus, D. (1979). Stability of aggressive reaction patterns in males. A review. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 852-875. Pagani, L., Boulerice, B., & Tremblay, R. E. (1997). The influence of poverty on children's classroom placement and behavior problems. In G. Duncan & J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), Consequences of growing up poor (pp. 311-339). New York: Russell Sage. Patterson, G. R. (1976). Living with children: New methods for parents and teachers. Champaign, IL: Research Press.

Patterson, G. R., Capaldi, D., & Bank, L. (1991). An early starter model for predicting delinquency. In D. J. Peplar & Κ. H. Rubin (Eds.), The development and treatment of childhood aggression. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E. (1989). A developmental perspective of antisocial behavior. American Psychologist, 41, 432-444. Pépier, D. J., & Slaby, R. G. (1996). Theoretical and developmental perspectives on youth and violence. In L. D. Eron, J. H. Gentry, & P. Schlegel (Eds.), Reason to hope: A psychological perspective on violence and youth (2nd ed., pp. 27-58). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Rothbaum, E, & Weisz, J. (1994). Parental caregiving and child externalizing behavior in non-clinical samples: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 55-74. Schweinhart, L. J., Weikart, D. P., & Larner, M. B. (1986). Consequences of three preschool curriculum models through age 15. Early Child Research Quarterly, 7(33), 5"J, 37-38. Seitz, V., Rosenbaum, L. K., & Apfel, Ν. H. (1985). Effects of a family support intervention: A ten-year follow up. Child Development, 56, 376-391. Shonkoff, J. P., & Meisels, S. M. (Eds.). (2000). Handbook of early childhood intervention (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Shure, M. (1994). Raising a thinking child: Help your young child resolve conflicts and get along with others. New York: Pocket Books. Slaby, R. G., & Guerra, N. G. (1988). Cognitive mediators of aggression in adolescent offenders: Assessment. Developmental Psychology, 24, 580-588. Slaby, R. G., Roedell, W. C, Arezzo, D., & Hendrix, K. (1995). Early Violence Prevention: tools for Teachers of Young Children. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children. Slaby, R. G., Wilson-Brewer, R., & Dash, K. (1994). Aggressors, victims and bystanders. Newton, MA: Education Development Center. Sobel, J. (1983). Everybody wins: 393 non-competitive games for young children. New York: Walker. Soloman, D., Watson, M. S., Deluchi, K. L., Schaps, E., & Battistich, V. (1997). Enhancing children's prosocial behavior in the classroom. American Journal of Educational Research, 24, 527-554.

Violence Prevention Programming in Colombia · 471 Spencer, T., Biederman, J., Willens, T., Harding, M., O'Donnell, D., & Griffin, S. (1996). Pharmacotherapy of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder across the life cycle. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 409^32. Stattin, H., & Magnusson, D. (1991). Stability in change in criminal behaviour up to age 30. British Journal of Criminology, 31, 327-346. Strauss, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics Scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75-88. Strayhorn, J. M., & Weidman, C. S. (1991). Follow up one year after parent-child interaction training: Effects on behavior of preschool children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 138-143. Streissguth, A. P., Sampson, P. D., & Barr, H. M. (1999). Neurobehavioral dose-response effects of prenatal alcohol exposure in humans from infancy to adulthood. Annals of New York Academy of Sciences, 562, 145-158. Thornberry T. P., & Krohn M. D. (Eds.). (2003). Taking stock of delinquency. An overview of findings from contemporary longitudinal studies. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Tierney, J. P., & Grossman, J. B. (1995). Making a difference: An impact study of big brothers/big sisters. Philadelphia: Public/Private Ventures. Tolan, P. H., & MacKay, M. M. (1996). Preventing serious antisocial behavior in inner-city children: An empirical based family intervention program. Family Relations, 45, 148-155. Tolman, R. M., & Bennett, L. W. (1990). A review of quantitative research on men who batter. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 5, 87-118. Tonry, M., & Farrington, D. P. (Eds.). (1995). Building a safer society: Strategic approaches to crime prevention. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Tremblay, R. E., & Craig, W. M. (1995). Developmental crime prevention. In M. Tonry & D. P. Farrington (Eds.), Building a safer society: Strategic approaches to crime prevention (pp. 151-236). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Tremblay, R. E., Japel, C , Pérusse, D., Boivan, M., Zoccolillo, M., Montplaisir, J., & McDuff, P. (1999). The search for age of "onset" of physical aggression: Rousseau and Bandura revisited. Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 9, 24-39.

Tremblay, R. E., Masse, L. C , Pagani, L., & Vitaro, F. (1996). The Montreal prevention experiment. In R. de V. Peters & R. J. McMahon (Eds.), Preventing Childhood Disorders, Substance Abuse & Delinquency. Thousand Oaks, CA. Sage. Tremblay, R. E., Pagani-Kurtz, L., Masse, L. C , Vitaro, E, & Pihl, R. O. (1995). A bimodal preventive intervention for disruptive Kindergarten boys: Its impact through mid-adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 560-568. Vitaro, F., Brendfen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (1999). Prevention of school dropout through the reducation of disruptive behaviors and school failure in elementary school. Journal of School Psychology, 37, 205-226. Wasserman, G. Α., & Miller, L. S. (1998). The prevention of serious and violent juvenile offending. In R. Loeber & D. P. Farrington (Eds.), Serious and violent juvenile offenders: Risk factors and successful interventions (pp. 197-247). Thousand Oaks, CA. Sage. Webster-Stratton, C. (1985). The effects of father involvement in parent training for conduct problem children. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 26, 801-810. Webster-Stratton, C. (1991). Annotations: Strategies for helping families with conduct disordered children. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 32, 1047-1062. Webster-Stratton, C , & Hamond, M. A. (1997). Treating children with early onset conduct problems: A comparison of child and parent training interventions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 93-19. Webster-Stratton, C , Kolpacoff, M., & Hollinsworth, T. (1988). Self administered video tape therapy for families with conduct problem children. Comparison of two cost-effective treatments and control group. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 558-566. World Health Organization. (2000). World health report 2000: Statistics annex. Geneva: Author. Widom, C. (1989). The cycle of violence. Science, 244, 160-166. Yoshikawa, H. (1995). Long-term effects of early childhood programs on social outcomes and delinquency. The Future of Children, 5, 51-75.

AUTHOR INDEX

Abed, Y.. 230 Aber, J. L., 231, 232, 233, 234, 316 Abou-Saleh, M., 279 Abrams, L. S., 59, 63 Abu-Baker, K„ 290 Abu-Dagga, S., 281 Adams, B., 289 Adams, P., 347 Adams, R., 353 Adams, S. E., 301, 302 Adefrisew, Α., 73 Adelman, H. S., 324 Adnopoz, J., 298 Ager, Α., 6, 192 Agnelli, S., 80 Agudelo, S. L. M., 461 Ai, A. L., 239 Ajdukovic, D., 234 Ajdukovic, M., 234 Akers, R. I., 137 Al-Ajeel, M., 231 Al-Asfour, Α., 231 Albee, G. W., 91 Albina, M., 419 Albright, L., 302 AlGazali, L., 279 Al-Haj, M., 281 Ali, S., 251 Alipuria, L. L., 36 Alkire, S., 191 Alkon, Α., 302 Al-Krenawi, Α., 279, 280, 281, 282, 288, 289, 290 Al-Krenawi, S., 281 Allison, K. W., 59 Allport, G. W„ 239 Allwood, Μ. Α., 231 AlSadawi, N., 280 Altman, I., 125 Alvarez, B., 463 Ama, S., 302 Amir, M., 232 Anastas, J. W., 264, 266 Angermeyer, M. C., 195

Anderson, B., 63 Anderson, E., 48, 62 Anderson, J., 113 Anderson, L., 316 Andrade, A. R., 397 Andrews, E, 192 Andrews, G., 235 Anglin, J. P., 84 Angold, Α., 306 Anthony, E. J., 9, 136, 137, 150 Antonovsky, Α., 232, 282, 395 Apfel, R., 18, 155, 340 Apodaca, P., 461 Appleby, G. Α., 264, 266 Aptekar, L., 71, 72, 73, 74, 78, 80, 81, 82 Arafat, C., 418, 419, 420, 422 Arambasic, L., 230 Arezzo, D., 460 Armeli, S. R., xxx, 150 Armstrong, M., 14 Armstrong, M. I., 390, 397, 398 Arnesen, T., 192 Arnold, R., 305 Arthur, M. W„ 137,212 Ashford, J. B., 59 Ashirova, Ο. K, 383 Atallah, S., 421, 422 Atwood, G. E., 52 Auerbach, J. G., 281 Ayerst, S., 75 Babineau, N., 269 Bachar, E., 232 Bachman, J. G., 176 Bagley, C., 75, 80, 267, 268 Baines, C., 350 Baizerman, M., 82 Baker, A. M., 230, 232 Baker, E. L., 303 Bala, N., 107 Baldwin, A. L., 297 Baldwin, C., 297 Baldwin, M. N., 283

474 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Bandura, Α., 407, 408 Bank, L., 457 Banks, C , 268, 269 Banks, S. M., 213, 396 Barbarin, Ο. Α., 434 Barber, B., 166 Barber, B. L., 409 Bardhan, P., 191 Bargh, J., 59 Barker, G., 73, 74, 78, 79 Barlow, H„ 442 Barnett, W. S., 459 Baron, R. M., 286, 288 Barr, Η. M., 457 Bartelt, D. W, 142 Barter, K., 344, 349, 350, 352 Barton, L., 93, 94 Barton, W. H., 213, 214 Basanez, M., 190 Bates, J. E., 302 Batts, K. R., 233 Baughman, L. N., 393 Bazron, B., 387 Beardslee, W. R., 298 Beauvias, E, 29 Beavon, D., 113 Beck, A. J., 141 Beck, A. T., 168 Beck, T., 199 Becker, B„ xxv, 9, 122, 151, 213, 391, 392 Becker, E., 237 Begin, M., 438 Begun, A. L., 348 Behar, L., 397 Beier, S. R., 303 Beinashowitz, J., 316 Bell, S., xxxiii, 154, 216 Bell-Dolan, D., 231 Bern, D., 457 Benard, B., 324, 325, 348 Benjamin, J., 48 Benjamin, M., 388 Benner, P., 57 Bennett, L. W., 458 Bennett, M., 107, 109 Benson, P., 304, 305 Benson, P. L., 407, 408 Berger, R., 239 Bergin, A. E., 240 Berglund, M. L., 167, 304, 407 Bergman, L. R., 139 Berk, L. E., 59 Berkner, L. K., 32 Bernard, B., 6 Bernard, C , 435, 436

Bernard, W. T., 435, 436, 445, 447 Bernstein, J., 232 Berrien, F. K., 393 Bertalanffy, L. von., 393 Best, K. M.,6, 315, 408 Besthorn, F. H., 123, 124, 126 Bettelheim, B., 237, 239 Bevan, P., 203 Bezmenov, P. V., 381 Bibeau, G., 16 Bickman, L., 397 Bierman, J. M., 137 Bigbee, Μ. Α., 59 Biju, M., 330 Billick, S., 281 Billson, J. M., 61,62 Bin-Othman, S., 279 Bishop, D. S., 283 Bishop, S., 281 Biswas-Diener, R., 190, 198, 201 Bit, S., 15 Bixler, R., 127 Black, M., 73, 78 Blackstock, C , 105, 107, 108, 109, 110, 111, 116 Blanc, C. S., 73 Blankemeyer, M., 300 Blankenship, Κ. M., 219 Blum, R„ 268 Blumber, Α., 198 Blumberg, S. J., 196 Blumenfeld, W. J., 266 Blumer, H., 143 Bodrova, V., 373 Boehnke, K., 151 Boettcher, Α., 281 Bogat, G. Α., 303 Bok, S., 332, 340 Bolger, N., 286 Bolland, J. M., 45 Bonne, O., 232 Bontempo, A. N., 303 Boothby, N., 7, 418, 419, 420, 422 Boothroyd, R., 186, 212, 396, 397, 399 Borek, N., 296, 300 Boris, N. W., 397 Borisova, L. G., 374 Bosley, P. J., 125 Boss, P., 31 Boulerice, B., 458 Bourgois, P. I., 48 Bourne, E., 15 Bowen, G. L., 7 Bowlby, J., 76 Bowling, Α., 194 Bowman, G., xxii

Author Index · 475 Boyden, J., 7, 8, 14, 15, 16, 78, 82, 84 Boyle, M. H., 392 Bracken, P., 14, 15, 17 Bradburn, Ν. M , 195 Bradley, J. Α., 281 Bradley, R. H., 300, 302, 303 Braithwaite, K., 437, 438, 439 Brame, Β., 457 Brannan, A. M., 393, 396 Braun, K., 34 Braverman, M. T., 322 Brazelton, T. B., 297, 298, 299, 300, 301 Bremner, D., 230 Brendfen, M., 463 Brennan, Ε. M., 302, 303 Breton, M„ 305 Brickman, P., 197 Briggs, J. L., 16 Brink, S., 116 Brodsky, A. E., xxviii, 296, 434 Brokenleg, M., 34 Bronfenbrenner, U., 12, 47, 50, 167, 254, 295, 301, 393 Brook, D. W., 458 Brook, J. S., 458 Brooks-Gunn, J., 137, 167, 304 Broude, G. J., 281 Brown, E., 396 Brown, J. D., 198 Brown, Κ. M„ 221 Brown, L. M., 60 Brown, S., 302 Browne, J. P., 202 Bruns, Ε. J., 138, 394 Brunswick, A. E, 47 Bryant, S., 301 Buckner, J. C., 236 Bullis, M., 155 Burack, J. A„ 394 Burchanal, M. R., 302 Burchard, J. D., 138, 394, 397 Burchard, S. N., 138, 394 Burgert, W., 281 Burghardt, S., 347, 352 Burke, B., 265 Burns, B. J., 395, 396 Burr, W., 31 Burton, L. M., 59 Buston, K., 266 Butler, A. C., xxii Butollo, W., 232, 234 Bybee, D., 144 Caddell, J. M., 233 Cadena, E., 455

Cadenhead, C., 301 Cadoret, R. J., 457 Cairns, B. D., 150, 457 Cairns, E., 230, 250 Cairns, R. B., 150, 457 Calhoun, L. G., 38, 39, 232, 234, 321 Calkins, J., 301 Callaghan, T., 186,212, 399 Caiman, K. C , 203 Cameron, Α., xxi, 155, 215 Campbell, C , xxxiii Campbell, S. B., 457, 458 Canda, E. R., 126, 393 Canetti, L., 232, 283, 288 Cantu, C. L., 36 Capaldi, D., 457 Capizzano, J., 301, 302 Caplan, G„ 438 Caputo, T., 154 Caracelli, V. J., xxx Carlson, B., 123, 444 Carlson, E., 301, 323 Carniol, B., 351 Carr, S., 90 Carrese, J. Α., 35 Carrigan, K., 368 Carson, R., 125, 128 Carta, J. J., 321 Carter, R. T., 54 Carter-Pokras, O., 281, 289 Carver, C. S., 321 Caspi, Α., 170, 457 Cass, V. C , 267 Castle, S., 12 Catalano, R. E, 136, 137, 167, 168, 304, 324, 407, 408, 409, 411, 412, 413 Cawood, N., 302 Chaleby, K., 280, 281 Chamberland, C , 214, 272, 348, 349 Chambers, E., 62 Chambers, R., 191 Chan, L. K., 320 Chan, W. T., 320 Chandler, G. E., 153 Chandler, M., 117 Charmaz, K., 58 Chartrand, T. L„ 60 Chatterjee, P., 393 Chawla, L., 127 Cheatham, Α., 158 Chen, M., 434 Chen, R., 35 Chen-Gal, S., 237 Cherian, V. I., 279 Chess, S., 457

476 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Cheung, J. C. K., 320 Cheung, S. K., 314 Chimienti, G., 234 Choi, P. Y. W., 320, 321, 323, 324 Choi, S. C , 196 Christensen, C. P., 435 Christopher, J. C , 191, 196 Chung, M. J., 300 Ciano, L., 74, 78, 81 Cicchetti, D., xxv, 9, 57, 59, 122, 151, 213, 391, 392, 394, 396, 398 Cimbolic, K., xxx, 150 Claes, M., 377 Clair, S., 301 Clark, D. Α., 191 Clark, D. E., 301 Clark, P., 316 Clark, R. M., 409 Clarke, A. E., 58 Clarke, R. T., 397 Clarke, S„ 109, 110, 111 Clausen, J. Α., 140 Clayton, S. L., 300 Coates, D., 197 Coatsworth, J. D., 59, 298, 301, 322 Cobb, E., 123 Cohen, E., 299 Cohen, L. H., xxx, 150, 232, 240 Cohen, R. D., 239 Cohen-Silver, R., 233 Cohler, B. J., 150 Coie, J., 457, 462 Cole, M., 5, 12 Cole, R. E., 297 Collins, P. H.. 46 Combrinck-Graham, L., xvii Compton, S. N., 306 Conger, K. J., 296, 298 Conger, R. D., 296, 298 Connell, J. P., 142, 305, 316 Connell, R., 61 Connelly, M., 82 Connor, 116 Connors, E. W, 362 Conrad, Α., 123 Conway, J. E, 344, 345 Cook, D. J., 152 Cook, L. R., xxvii Cook, P., 251 Cooke, D. J., 213 Cooke, M., 113 Cooke, W., 366 Cooley, C. H., 143 Cooper, H., 304 Corbière, D., 362

Corbin, J., 58, 154 Cornell, J., 125 Cornell, S., 117 Cornwall, Α., 159 Correa, M. L., 461 Cortes, D. E., 215 Corwyn, R. R, 300 Cosgrove, J., 73 Costa, P. T., 198 Costello, E. J., 306 Coulton, C , 305 Cowen, E. L., 151, 212, 406, 407 Crabbe, Κ. M., 39, 361 Crabtree, B. R, xxx, 158 Craig, J. G., 377 Craig, W. M., xxxii, 459 Cressey, D., 58 Crey, E., 107 Crichlow, W., 359, 360 Crick, N. R., 59, 61, 66 Crockenberg, S. B., 393 Crooks. C , 128 Cross, Α., 110 Cross, T., 387 Cross, T. L„ 39, 40, 359, 361 Crossley, N., 47 Crow, L., 95 Crowe. P. Α., 302 Crowel, R., xxxiii Crowley, M. J., 393 Cuellar, J. B., 33 Cullen, J., 109 Culp, A. M., 300 Culp, R. E., 300 Cummins, R., 190, 191, 196, 197, 200, 201, 202, 204 Curtis, W. J., 396 Cushman, P., 91 Cyrulnik, B., 422 D'Abreu, R. C , xxvii D'Ambrosio, R., 155 Dahl, Β. B., 31 Daiute, C , 221 Dallaire, B., 90 Dallape, R, 73 Dalrymple, J., 265 D'Andréa, M., 90 Daniels, J., 90 Dapic, R., 239 Dash, K., 460 D'Augelli, Α., 267, 268 Davey Smith, G„ 405 Davies, L., 281 Dawes, Α., 7, 13, 17, 249, 250

Author Index · 477 Dean, R. G., 54 De'Andrade, R. G., 40 DeAntoni, C , 155 DeBaryshe, B. D., 457 De Berry, J., 17 Dei, G. S., 435, 438, 439 Deisher, R. W., 268 Dekel, R., 234 De la Rosa, M., 458 Delgado, M., 50 DeLongis, Α., 235 Deluty, R., 62 Denenberg, V. H., xviii Dennis, K., 388 Denour, A. K., 232 Denzin, Ν. K., 46, 62 Derogatis, L., 283 Derr, V., 125, 128 De Vries, M., 6 D'Hondt, J., 109 D'Hondt, W., 279 Diener, C., 196 Diener, E., 190, 195, 196, 198, 200, 202 Diener, M., 200 Diez-Medrano, J., 190 Dimperio, R., 397 Dinkmeyer, D., 462 Divine, D., 448 Dlamini, G., 74 Dmitrieva, T. B., 379 Dodge, K., 302 Dodge, Κ. Α., 60, 66, 462 Doggett, M., 194 Dominguez, E, 462 Domoskoff, L., 71 Dona, G., 78, 84 Donald, D., 13, 17, 249, 250 Dorjahn, V. R., 279 Dreze, J., 12 Dryfoos, J. G., 136, 137, 252 Dube, E. M., 273 DuBois, D. L., 304 Dubos, R., 395 Dubrovina, I. V., 384 Dubrow, N., 7, 238 Duckworth, M. R, 301 Dupree, D., xxxiii, 154, 216 Duque, L. F., xxiv, 457, 458, 460 DuRant, R. H., 301 Durakovic-Belko, E., 239 DuRant, R. H., 268 Dusa, G. S., 317 Dutta, D., 331 Dwairy, M., 290

Dworkin, J., 152 Dyregrov, Α., 233, 234, 235 Eapen, V., 279 Earle, K., 110 Earls, E, 305 Early, T. J., 393 Easterlin, R. Α., 195 Ebert, L., 233 Eccles, J., 142, 166, 167, 304, 407, 408, 409 Eccles, T. Α., 267 Echeverry, J., 82 Edelman, R, 303 Egeland, B., 9, 137, 301,323 Eisenberg, N., 169 Eisenbruch, M., 14 Ekblad, S., 17 Elder, G. H., 140, 457 Eliason, M., 264 El-Islam, M. G., 280, 281 Elmen, J. D., 393 El-Saraaj, E., 230, 235 Elver, K., 31 Emmons, R. Α., 195 Engle, R, 12, 16 Enikolopov, S. N., 376 Ennew, J., 71, 74, 78, 83 Epp, J., 406 Epstein, M. H., 149 Epstein, Ν. B., 283 Erbaugh, J., 168 Erikson, E., 77, 267 Eron, L. D., 457 Espino, D. V, 33 Essed, R, 434, 447 Este, D., 435, 436, 439, 445 Este, D. C., 301 Etxebarria, L, 461 Evans, M. E., 397 Evans, R, 350 Evans, S. D., 408 Eyber, C., 7 Eyler, J., 409 Fabricant, M. B., 347, 352 Fagen, D. B., 151, 212 Fairbanks, L. Α., 231 Falk, L., 16 Fallis, R. K., xxvi Farmer, Ε. M. Z., 295, 396, 397 Farmer, T. W., 295 Farrington, D. R, xxi, 457 Farris-Manning, C., 105, 365, 366 Farrow, J. Α., 268 Farwell, N., 231

478 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Feeny, T., 7 Feikema, R. J., 305 Felce, D., 196, 201 Feldstein, L., 405 Felsman, J. K., xxvi, 153 Fenko, A. B., 373 Ferguson, N., 230 Fernandez, S. J., 455 Ferrari, M., 152 Fine, M., xxi, 153, 155, 215, 221 Firminger, Κ. B., 298 Fisher, C. B., 176 Fisher, R. H., 232 Fitzpatrick, R., 193 Flay, B., 302 Flegel, R, 436, 439, 440 Fleming, M., 31, 39 Fleming, W. M., xviii Fletcher, R. H., 168 Fletcher, S. W., 168 Flora, B. L., 154 Flowers, P., 266 Floyd, M., 127 Flyvbjerg, B., 97, 152 Folkman, S., 235 Fong, Α., 302 Fontaine, P., 360 Foran,T, 115 Ford, D. Y, 45 Forehand, R., 300 Formsma, J., 109 Former, S., 107 Fortenberry, J. D., 267 Foster, T., 142 Fox, D., 91,95 Fozzard, S., 7 Fraser, M. W., 5, 75, 122, 135, 137, 138, 142, 150, 249, 343, 345, 346, 348, 349, 392, 393, 394, 443, 446 Frederick, C , 231 Frederickson, D., 440 French, F. Ε., 137 French, J., 34 French, S., 268 Frey, B. S., 192 Friedman, R. M., 138, 388, 389, 390, 396, 399 Friesen, B. J., 387 Fromm, E., 93 Frye, M., 265, 266 Fu, V. R., 300 Fuqua-Whitkey, D. S., 459 Furrer, C , 304 Furstenberg, F. F., 137, 140 Futrell, J. Α., xviii, 122 Futterman, D., 268

Gagne, M. J., 16 Galaguzova, M. Α., 379 Galambos, N. L., xxx, 151 Galappatti, Α., 7, 14 Galasso, L., 303 Galeano, L., 84 Galinsky, M. J., 348, 349, 393, 443 Gambone, M. Α., 142, 305 Garbarino, J., 6, 7, 153, 156, 230, 233, 234, 237, 238 Garber, M. D., 462 Garber, S. W., 462 Garcia-Coll, C , 59, 300 Garibaldi, Α., 445 Garmezy, N., xvi, 5, 6, 10, 13, 17, 18, 45, 122, 249, 316, 321, 348, 394, 398, 408, 409, 443 Garofalo, R., 268 Garratt, A. M., 193 Gaventa, J., 159 Gavrilovic, J., 233, 235 Gay, J., 5 Gaydon, V., 82 Geertz, C , 140 Genero, N., xxvii Gergen, K. J., 143, 200 Gergen, M., 200 Germaine, C. B., 59 Gersham, J., 93 Gibbs, J. T., 45 Gibbs, S., 8, 17 Gibson, P., 268 Giddens, Α., 59, 199 Gilat, L, 238 Gilbert, G. T., 196, 197, 200 Giles, D., 409 Gilgun, J. F., 58, 59,61,62, 141, 143, 150, 154, 155, 214, 219 Giller, J. E., 14 Gilligan, C , xxiii, 60, 67, 155 Gilligan, R., 429 Gilmore, D., 17 Gil-Rivas, V., 233 Ginwright, S., 409, 410, 412 Ginzburg, K., 234 Gitterman, Α., 59 Gjestad, R., 233 Glantz, M. D., xviii, xx, xxi, xxx, 122, 150 Glaser, B., 58 Glasgow, D., 433 Glick, J., 5 Glodich, Α., 301 Glueck, E., 140 Glueck, S., 140

Author Index · 479 Goffin, S. G., 350, 351 Gold, Α., 440 Goldman, S. Κ., 138 Goldstein, A. P., 460 Goldstein, Η., 62 Goldstein, Η. M., 300 Golembiewski, R. T., 198 Goodey, J., 61 Goodman, J., 127 Goodman, R., 169 Goodnow, J., 5 Gootman, J. Α., 142, 304, 407, 408 Gordon, E. W., 137, 141, 143, 316 Gorsuch, R. L., 240 Gottfredson, M., 137 Gottman, J. D., 58 Gould, S. J., 125 Goyos, J. M., 6 Graham, C., 201 Graham, J., 151, 350 Graham, J. R., 279, 280, 281, 288, 290 Grealish, E. M., 138 Green, Κ. E., 168 Greenbaum, C. W., 282 Greenbaum, P. E., 396 Greenberg, J., 237 Greene, J. C , xxx Greene, R. R., xviii, 122, 123 Greenspan, S. I., 297, 298, 299, 300, 301 Greenwood, D. J., 159 Grégoire, T. K., 393 Gribbons, B., 303 Grietens, H., 373 Griffin, S., 195 Griffiths, H., xxiii Grinnell, R. M., 138 Grisalis, H., 455 Grolnick, W. S., 393 Groopman, J., 144 Grossman, Α., 268 Grossman, F. Κ., 316 Grossman, J. Β., 303 Grotberg, Ε., 321 Grotpeter, J. K., 59 Gruen, R., 235 Guba, E. G., 138, 139, 154 Guerra, N. G., 213, 462 Guetzloe, E., 316 Gupta, L., 233 Guttman, G., 128 Guy, Κ. Α., 346 Habermas, J., 97 Hacam, B., 231 Hacking, I., xviii, 155

Hackney, C. H , 239 Hadi, E, 230, 230 Hagey, J., 114 Haggerty, R., 122 Hale, D. D. C., 301 Hall, E., 280 Hall, E. T., 47 Hallam, L., 194 Hallis, D., 233 Halman, L., 190 Halverson, C. E, 36 Hamer, D. H., 195 Hamilton, Μ. Α., 142 Hamilton, S. E, 142 Hann, D. Α., 296, 392 Hann, D. M., 392 Harel.Y., 176 Harkness, S., 13 Harre, R., xxx Hart, J., 14 Hart, R., 125 Harter, S., 169 Harvey, M., 127 Hasenfeld, Y„ 343, 350 Hauser, S. T., 151, 155, 219 Hawkins, J. D., 136, 137, 167, 212, 304, 324, 407 Hazarika, S., 329 Headey, B., 197, 198 Heerwagen, J., 123 Heller, S. S„ 397 Hellinckx, W., 373 Helms, J. E., 54 Hemmings, Α., 157 Henderson, S., 237 Hendrix, K., 460 Hendy, H , 128 Henry, E, 437, 438 Herb, L., 240 Herbert, E„ 128 Herbert, M. D., 351 Herek, G., 264 Hernandez, M., 390, 391, 396 Herrenkohl, T. L., 139 Hershberger, S., 268 Herzog, T. E, 125, 127 Hettler, T. R., xxx, 150 Hewson, D., 235 Heymann, J., 301 Hickey, Α., 193 Hickson, J., 82 Higgins, G., 75 Hikoyeda, N., 33, 34 Hill, R., 28, 31,434, 443, 444, 445,446,449, 450, 451

480 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Hill, R. B., 52 Hinton, R., 17 Hirata, J., 195 Hirschi, T., 137 Ho, J. Κ. M., 314 Hoagwood, K., 395, 396 Hobart, J., 193 Hodges, S., 391 Hodgins, S., 457 Hoebel, Ε. Α., 34 Hoffman, C. C , 151 Hoffman, D., 71, 73, 78, 84 Hoffnung, M., 58 Hoffnung, R. J., 58 Hoh, Y, 317 Hoke, L., 298 Holliday, M., 82 Holloway, Β. E., 304 Holman, Ε. Α., 233 Holtzman, W. H., 300 hooks, B., 46, 265, 266 Hooper-Briar, K., 347, 349, 351 Houghton, T. G., 298, 300 Houston, S., xxiii Hsu, J., 32, 360 Huang, B„ 139, 239 Huang, D., 303 Huang, L. N., xxxii, 154 Hudson, W. W, 283 Huesmann, L. R., 457 Huff, B., 387 Hughes, D. L., 152 Hum, M., 405 Hunt, S., 194 Hunter, A. J., 153 Huntsinger, C. S., 320 Huntsinger, P. R., 320 Hurtig, M., 345 Husain, S.A., 231 Hutchinson, P., 93 Hutchison, P., 409 Hutz, C. S., 73 Ife, J., 345,351 Inglehart, R., 190, 195 Iovchuk, Ν. M., 381 Irby, M., 142 Ireland, M., 193 Irwin, Α., 93 Isaacs-Shockley, M., 387, 399 Ishisaka, Η. Α., 32 Israel, Β. Α., 152 Israeli, R., 235 Itskowitz, R., 235 Izzeldin, Α., 279

Jacobs, Β. K., 362 Jacobson, L., 144 Jacobvitz, D., 137 James, C , 437 James, C. E., 434, 438, 439, 440, 441 James, S., 90 James, T., 409, 410, 412 Janoff-Bulman, R., 197 Jareg, E., 16 Jarrett, R. L., 45, 51 Jeffries, S. H., 305 Jenkinson, C , 193 Jensen, P. S., 396 Jensen, S., 321 Jew, C. L., 168 John, M., 410, 412 John, O. P., 376 Johnson, J. L., xviii, 122 Johnson-Powell, G., xviii, 154, 216 Johnston, L. D., 176 Johnston, P., 358, 359 Jones, L., 224 Jordan, Α., xxxi Jordan, Β. K., 233 Jose, P. E., 320 Joyce, C. R. B., 193 Jung, C , 125 Kafetsios, K., 224 Kagan, D. M., 321 Kagitcibasi, C , 196 Kahn, P., 124, 125, 127 Kahneman, D., 195, 198, 200 Kaistaniemi, L., 151 Kalinganire, C , 84 Kalt, J., 117 Kandel, E., 457 Kane, T. J., 302, 303 Kann, L. K., 171 Kanuha, V. K., 46 Kaplan, G. Α., 239 Kaplan, Η. B., xxv, 151 Kaplan, R., 125, 128 Kaplan, S., 125 Kaplan-De-Nour, Κ. Α., 283 Karoly, L. Α., 459 Karpukhina, Ο. I., 374 Kashy, D. Α., 286 Katcher, Α., 127, 128 Kaufert, J. M., 34 Kaufman, J., 298, 299 Kaukiainen, Α., 151 Kawachi, I., 93 Kazdin, A. E., 301, 393 Keating, A. E., 409

Author Index · 481 Kebede, Κ., 203 Keedwell, P., 193 Keenan, K„ 457 Kegan, R., 413 Keller, G., 306 Kellerman. A. L., 459 Kellert, S., 124, 125, 126, 128, 129 Kelly, D., 302 Kelly, J., 212, 437,441,442 Kemmis, S., 158 Kennedy, B., 93 Kenny, D. Α., 286, 288 Kerestes, G., 232 Kersten, J., 62 Kertzmann, J., 251 Kessel. S., 268 Khalifeh, I., 234 Kharitonova, Ν. K., 380 Kieffer, C , 93, 95 Kilbride, J., 289 Kilbride, P., 74, 289 Kilian, R., 195 Kim, J. K., 93 Kim, K., 63 Kim, K. S., 303 Kim, U., 196 Kind, P., 192 King, Α., 171, 176 Kirby, L., 75 Kirby, L. D., 137, 345, 346, 348, 392, 394 Kisthardt, W., 138 Kitayama, S., 191, 196, 376 Kitzinger, C., 95 Klaw, E. L„ 303 Klein, C., 62 Kleinman, Α., 17 Kleinman, J., 17 Klevens, J., xxiv, 154, 155, 457, 458 Klingemann, H. D„ 195 Klingman, Α., 233, 235 Kluckhohn, C , 34 Knaul, E, 73, 74, 78 Knezevic, G., 233 Knitzer, J., 299, 301 Knoke, D., 109 Knowles, M., 462 Knox, L. M., xxvi Knutson, K., 253 Kobb, J. Α., 304 Koenig, B., 34 Koenig, H. G., 240 Kolko, D. J., 2144 Koller, S. H., 73, 155 Kolobe, Τ. Η. Α., 300 Kon, I. S., 377, 378

Kong, Ν. H., 438 Korbin, J., 15, 305 Korenman, S., 281, 289 Kornblum, W., 45 Koroloff, N., 398 Korolyova, Ε. V., 380 Korte, A. O., 33 Kosciulek, R., 31 Kostelny, K., 7, 230, 233, 234, 237, 238 Kotchick, Β. Α., 300 Kratzer, L., 457 Kretzman, J., 430 Kroger, J., 168 Krohn, M. D., 457 Kroll, I. T., 264 Kruttschnitt, C., 457 Kubisch, A. C., 305 Kulenovic, Α., 239 Kumpfer, K., 321 Kuo, F„ 127 Kupersmidt, J., 457 Kuraeva, L. G., xxii Kurtz, D. Α., 36 Kuterovac, G., 234 Kutervac-Jagodic, G., 230 Kvale, S., 159 Labonte, R., 90 Lacoe, J., 409 Ladd, P., 108, 110, 116, 117 Ladner, J. Α., xxii, xxiii Lagerspetz, Κ. M. J., 151 Lai, M. E, 320 Laird, Ν. M., 169 Laird, R. D., 302 Lalonde, C., 117 Lalor, K., 73, 82 Lam, C.W., 314 Lambert, E. W., 397 Lamping, D., 193 Landsverk, J. Α., 212 Langlois, M., 405 Lamer, G., 90 Larocque, G., 114 Larrieu, J. Α., 397 Larsen, R. J., 195 Larson, R„ 152, 196 Lather, P., 447 Lau, J., 316, 317, 318, 319, 321, 323 Laurendeau, M., 214, 272, 348, 349 Laurie, B., 212 Lauzer, J., 71 Lavee, Y., 31 Laverack, G. R., 221 Lavi, T., 231,233, 234

482 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Lawler, W. R., 33 Lawrence, E., 127 Laws, S., 224 Lawson, C. W., 198, 202 Lawson, E., 434 Lawson, Η. Α., 347, 351 Layard, R., 190 Lazarus, R. S., 235 Leadbeater, B., 281 Leadbeater, B. J., xxx, 151 Lecic, T. D., 233 LeCroy, C. W., 59 Lee, A. W„ 186, 212, 399 Lee, C, 303, 433, 443, 444, 450 Lee, T. Y, 320 Leffert, N., 142, 304, 306, 408, 409 Leighton, A. H., 169 Leighton, D., 34 Leng, M., 193 Leonard, R, xix Leplege, Α., 194 Lerner, R. M., 167, 176, 297, 304, 407, 408, 409,412,413 Le Roux, J., 71,74 Lesko, N., 155 Lester, G., 31 Leung, Κ. K, 320 Levin, M., 159 Levine, Α., 91 Levine, M., 91 Le Vine, P., 15 Levinovski, L., 234 Levi-Strauss, C , 125 Lev-Wiesel, R., 232 Levy, D. R., 33 Lewis, E., 443 Lewis, J., 90 Lewis, M., 90 Lewis, R., 34 Lewis-Chaφ, H„ 409, 410, 412 Li, Y. Y, 32 Liaw, F. R., 320 Lichtman, R. R., 198, 200 Lifschitz, M., 235 Lightfoot, C , 151 Lightman, E., 279, 281, 288 Lilies, H., 107 Limber, S. P., 324 Lin, C. Y. C , 300 Lincoln, C. E., 33 Lincoln, Y. S„ 138, 139, 154 Linden, C. W., 301 Lindenbach, I., 195 Litrownik, A. J., 212

Littlechild, W., 360, 368 Liu, S. C , 320 Llabrem M., 230 Lobel, T, 238 Lobig, U., 195 Lochhead, C , 344 Lock, M., 16 Lockwood, D., 199 Loeber, R., xxi, 170, 457, 458 Lombardi, E, 34 Lombardi, G. S., 34 Lonczak, H. S., 167, 304, 407 Londono, J. L., 455 Long, J. V. E, 137, 140 Lopez, E, 461 LoPresto, C , 62 Lord, J., 93, 95, 409 Lortie, K. L„ 59 Lotman, U. M., 372 Louw, 249 Low, B. S., 279 Low, J., 159 Lubeck, S., 45, 50 Lucas, L. M., 304 Lucas, R. E., 195 Luijkx, R., 190 Lukes, S., 191, 194 Lundberg, Α., 127 Lusk, M., 73, 82 Luthar, S. S., xviii, xxv, 9, 57, 59, 122, 137, 150, 151,213,215, 303, 391, 392, 393, 394, 395, 396, 397, 398 Lykken, D., 195, 198 Lynch, J., 405 MacDonald, L. M., 193 MacDonald, R., 108, 110, 116, 117 MacGillivary, H., 95 Maclver, D. J., 177 Mackin, J. R., 304 Macksoud, M. S., 231, 232, 233, 234 MacLennan, K., 302 MacQueen, K, 263 Macrae, J., 17 Madill, Α., xxxi Magagula, S., 74 Magnus, Κ. B., 151, 212 Magnusson, D., 139, 457 Majors, R., 61, 62 Makhanya, J., 74 Mallon, G. P., 273 Mandell, B. R., 347 Mann, G., 7, 8, 12, 13, 19, 78, 82, 84, 224 Manteuffel, B., 396 Maphalala, T. P., 74

Author Index · 483 Marcelino, Α., 13 Margadant-van Arcken, M., 127 Markowitz, R, xxii, 219 Marks, E., 280 Markus, H. R., 144, 191,196, 376 Marmot, M., 93 Marquis, R, 194 Marsella, A. J., 39, 361 Marshall, C , 138 Marshall, N. L., 302 Martin, C. L., 36 Martineau, S., xxvi, 155-156, 219 Martinez, Α., 458 Martinez, J. C , 300 Maruna, S., 141 Maslow, A. EX., 77 Mason, S. M., 34 Masse, L. C , 35, 463 Massey, S., xxi, 155, 156, 215, 216 Masten, Α., 6, 10, 17, 59, 135, 150, 249, 295, 296, 298, 301, 315, 322, 324, 391, 392, 395, 397, 398, 399, 408, 409, 456 Masters, K. S., 240 Maughan, B., xvi, 457 Maurice, L. B., 362 Mayan, M., 159 Mburugu, E., 289 McAdam, J., 71, 73, 74, 78, 79, 83, 84 McAdam-Crisp, J., 72, 74, 75, 78 McAdoo, H. P., 52, 300 McBride, C , 114 McCabe, M., 34, 35 McCallin, M., 7 McCollam, Κ. M., 151 McCubbin, Η. I., xviii, xxvi, 28, 29, 31, 37, 38, 39, 40, 122, 150, 360, 361, 399, 434 McCubbin, L. D., 36 McCubbin, Μ. Α., 29, 31, 37, 38, 40, 90, 360, 399 McDermott, Β. M., 281 McDonald, T. P., 393 McDowell, L., 194 McGee, H., 193, 202 McGinnis, E., 460 McGloin, J. M., 443 McGrath, J., 141 McGregor, J. Α., 193, 198, 202 Mchedlov, M. P., 374 Mcintosh, D. M., 233, 240 McKay, G. D., 462 McKelvey, R., 281 McKenzie, B., 105 McKnight, J., 251, 413, 430 McLeod, J. D., 306 McLeod, L., 58, 59, 61, 62 McMillan, J. H., 317

McMillen, D. P., 124 McMillen, J. C , 232, 234 McPherson, S. E., 240 McRae, R. R., 198 McTaggert, R., 158 McWhirter, A. M., 45 McWhirter, B. T., 45, 443 McWhirter, Ε. H., 45, 443 McWhirter, J. J., 45, 443 McWhirter, R., 443 Mead, G. H., 143 Meares, P. Α., 298 Mednick, S. Α., 457 Meisels, S. M., 456 Melton, G. B., 324 Memmo, M., 419, 422, 430 Menamparampil, G., 330, 331 Mendelson, M., 168 Menon, P., 12 Mensah, J., 435, 436 Menzel, P., 192 Mercy, J., 459 Messerschmidt, J., 61, 62 Mettalic, R, 362 Michalos, A. C , 192, 194, 200, 201 Michie, C , 152, 213 Middleton, D., 34 Mihalik, G. J., 264 Milan, Α., 436 Milgram, Ν. Α., 230, 235, 236 Milgram, R. M., 230 Millen, J. V., 93 Miller, D. B., 300 Miller, J. B., xxiii Miller, J, E., 281 Miller, J. Y., 136, 137,324 Miller, L., 32 Miller, L. S., 459 Miller, W., xxx, 158 Milloy, J., 107 Milofsky, E. S., 140 Mincy, R., 433, 444, 445, 446 Minkler, M., 158 Minkovitz, C. S., 300 Mirsky, L., 363, 364 Mitchell, R. C , 351 Miyama, L. H., 33 Mock, J., 168 Moffitt, T. E., xxiii, 170 Moghaddam, F. M., xxx Mohanty, S. P., 35 Mokuau, N., 32 Monson, R. R., 169 Montgomery, L., 281, 289 Montoya, Ε. M., 463

Montoya, L., 458 Montoya, M., 463 Moore, R., 125 Mooza, F., 7 Morgan, D. L., 158,214 Morgan, R. H., xxv, xxvi Morgan, S. P., 137 Moriarty, A. E., xvi, 395 Morison, P., 391 Morris, J., 94 Morris, P. Α., 301 Morrison, G. M., 212 Morsillo, J., 412, 413 Mortimore, P., xvi Mott, J. Α., 302 Mould, J. W., 351 Mounts, N. S., 393 Mouton, C. P., 33 Mowbray, C. T., 298 Moya, P., 35 Mpofu, E., 152 Mrazek, P. J., 395 Mroueh, M., 230 Muchini, B., 74 Muckle, R., 106 Mukanoheli, E., 233 Muldoon, O. T., 231 Mulhall, P. F., 302 Mullaly, B., 90, 95, 98 Mullis, A. K., xxvii Munoz-Varga, M., 84 Muntaner, C., 405 Munthali, Α., 251 Muramutsa, E, 84 Murch, R. L., 232 Murphy, B., 345 Murphy, J. M., 169 Murphy, L. B., xvi, 395 Murray, M., 97 Myers, R., 248 Nabhan, G., 125, 127 Nader, K. O., 231, 235 Nadjiwan, S., 116 Nagin, D. S., 170, 457 Nalchajyan, Α. Α., 376 Narayan, D., 191 Nash, J. K., 138 Nasr, J. Α., 234 Nawegahow, D., 362 Neheegahow, N., 362 Neiger, B., 321 Nelson, G., 89, 90, 93, 95, 96, 97, 98, 101,214, 344, 405,406,412 Nelson, K., 347, 349, 393, 408, 411

Nelson, N., xxviii Nesselroade, J. R., 151 Newbrough, J., 95 Newell, C., 194 Newman, D. L., 170 Newman, J., 421, 429 Newton, R. R., 212 Neysmith, S., 350 Ng, A. M. C., 313 Nichols, R., 34 Nikitina, L. E., 372, 377, 382, 384 Nishat, S., 331, 332, 333 Nistico, H., 196 Nixon, W., 127 Njeru, E., 74 Nord, E., 192 Norman, E., 71, 122, 123 Noser, K., 397 Nsamenang, Α., 250 Nukariya, K., 152 Nuruis, P., 144 Nuttall, Ε. V., 230 Nuttall, R. L., 230 Nyandiya-Bundy, S., 74 Oates, J., 362 Obeidallah, D., 59 Obomswain, Α., 105 Oborne, D. W., 317 O'Boyle, C., 193, 202 O'Brien, P., 141 Ochocka, J., 95 O'Connor, M., 159 Offord, D. R., 392 Oishi, S., 190, 195, 196 Olaya, Α., 461 Oliveira, J. M., 39, 361 Oliver, M., 93, 94, 95 Oliveria, W., 82 Olkin, R., 93, 94 Olweus, D., 457 Oetting, E., 29 Ohsako, T., 152 O'Malley, K., 193 O'Malley, P. M., 176 O'Neil, J. D., 34 Opotow, S., xxvi Orange, D. M., 52 Orenstein, Α., 268 Orian, G., 123 Orr, D., 127 Osherson, S., xxiii Oswald, Α., 190, 192 Ouellette, S., xxi, 155, 215 Ouston, J., xvi

Author Index · 485 Ouweneel, P., 196 Owuamanam, D. O., 279 Oyserman, D., 144, 298 Pachai, B., 446 Pagani, L., 458 Pagani-Kurtz, L., 463 Painter, D., 151-152 Palfrey, J., 268 Palmer, O. J., 17 Palmer, S., 366 Pancer, S. M., 407, 410 Pandiani, J. Α., 213 Pankhurst, Α., 203 Panter-Brick, C , 71, 73 Papageorgiou, V., 230 Pardo, V. M., 463 Parducci, Α., 20 Pare, D., 90 Pargament, Κ. I., 239, 240 Park, C , 232, 240 Parker, M , 15 Parsons, M., 434 Passmark, L., 300 Patterson, G. R., 457, 458, 462 Patterson, J. M., 28, 29 Patterson, L. E., 316, 392, 443 Patterson, S., 363 Patton, M. Q., 57 Paul, D., 366 Paul, V., 368 Payne, M. S., 393 Peirson, L., 89, 95, 214, 272, 344, 348, 349, 406 Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., 302 Pellegrini, D., 391 Pellet, L., 82 Pelzel, J., 32 Pendergrast, R. Α., 301 Pépier, D. J., 457 Perin, C., 128 Perrin, S., 231 Pervin, L. E, 376 Peters, Α., 268, 271 Peters, R. D. V., 305 Peterson, C., 239 Petesch, P., 191 Petrova, A. S., 373, 376 Petrunka, K., 305 Petti, T., 396 Pettinato, S., 201 Pettit, G. S., 302 Pettit, J., 191 Pharr, S., 264, 266 Philippov, V. M., 382 Phillips, D. Α., xviii, 301

Phinney, J. S., 16, 35, 36 Pianta, R. C., 322, 323, 324 Picketts, J., 153 Pierce, G. R., 235 Pihl, R. O., 463 Pilkington, N., 267, 268 Pinel, E. C , 196 Pinto, J., 192 Pires, S. Α., 390 Pirner, P., 29 Pittman, K., 142 Plank, S. B., 177 Plaza, D., 438 Plummer, C. M., 39, 361 Plummer, D., 266 Poertner, J., 393 Pollard, J. Α., 137,212 Polozhiy, B. S., 379 Poortinga, Y. H., 72 Porche, M. V., 266 Posner, J. K., 302 Poulin, M., 233 Powell, M., 82 Powell, S„ 232 Pranis, K., 62, 362 Pratt, M., 407, 410 Predovic, M., 128 Presnyakova, L., 376 Priebe, S., 233 Prihozhan, A. M., 384 Prilleltensky, I., 89, 90, 91, 93, 95, 96, 97, 98, 99, 100, 101, 344, 345, 346, 351, 405, 406, 407, 408, 409, 411, 412, 413 Prilleltensky, O., 94, 96, 97, 412 Puhakka, T., 239 Pulkingham, J., 344 Punamâki, R. L., 7, 230, 235, 237, 238, 239 Putnam, R., 405, 406, 411 Pyle, R., 127, 128 Pynoos, R. S., 231 Queiro-Tajalli, I., xxxiii Quinn, M. M., 151 Qouta, S., 230, 233, 234, 235 Racusin, G., 298 Rae-Grant, N., 392 Rael, R., 33 Rahav, G., 230 Rahey, L., xxxii Rains, P., 156 Rajani, 252 Rak, C. E, 392, 443, 450 Rak, C. L., 316 Ramirez, C., xxiv, 457, 458

486 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Ramier, M., 302 Ramphele, M., 250 Ramsey, Ε., 457 Rapoport, R. N., xviii, 213 Rapp, C , 138 Rappaport, J., 95, 406 Ratanapojnard, S., 125 Ratner, C , 72 Ramer, M. S., 48 Raudenbush, S. W, 305 Ray, L. D., 159 Rean, Α. Α., 374, 375, 378, 384 Reasoner, R. W., 317 Redefer, L., 127 Redelmeier, D. Α., 195 Reed, D. F., 317 Reed, E. D., 393 Regehr, C , 159 Reiser, E., 62 Remafedi, G., 268 Resnick, M., 268 Resnick, M. D., 139 Ressler, E„ 7, 13 Restrepo, O., 458 Reynolds, Α., 434 Reynolds-White, S., 17 Rhodes, J., 303 Rhodes, L. Α., 35 Riazi, Α., 193 Rice, Ε P., 378 Richards, P. S., 239 Richardson, G. E., 321, 322 Richardson, J., 302 Richman, J. M., 7, 135, 142, 150. 443 Richman, N., 14 Rider, Ε. Α., 59 Rigsby, L. C , 142, 144 Rivara. F. P., 459 Rizzini, I„ 84 Robbins, S. P., 393 Roberts, R., 35 Roberts, R. E., 239 Roberts-Fiata, G., 439 Robertson, A. E, 12 Robertson, L., 212, 281 Roca, J., 154, 155, 458 Rockwood, K., 151 Rodwell, M. K., 154 Roedell, W. C , 460 Roehlkepartain, E. C , 304 Roffman, J., 303 Rogler, L. H., 215 Rogoff, B., 4, 5, 10 Ronen, T., 230 Rosenblatt, A„ 396, 397

Rosenfeld, W. D., 303 Rosen-reynoso, M., 266 Rosenthal, R., 144 Rosner, R., 232 Ross, D. P., 344 Ross, J. M., 239 Rossiter, Α., 353 Roth, J., 304 Roth, J. L., 167 Rothbaum, E, 458 Rousseau, C , 16 Rumyantseva, P. V., 375 Russell, I. T., 193 Ruta, D. Α., 193, 202 Rutter, M„ xvi, xxv, 5, 13, 18, 122, 136, 142, 143, 151, 298, 317, 391, 394, 398, 408, 409, 444, 449, 451 Ryan, C , 268 Ryan, J. A. M., 167, 304, 407 Ryan, R. M., 393 Ryff, C. D., 196 Sagor, R„ 316, 318 Sagy, S., 282 Said, T. M., 16 Saigh, P. Α., 230 Salabeey, D., 446 Saleebey, D., 124, 126, 136. 138 Salmivalli, C , 151 Saltmarshe, D., 193, 198 Saltz, E., 144 Sampson, Ε. E., 91 Sampson, P. D., 457 Sampson, R. J., 296, 305 Sanders, G. L., 264 Sanders, G. S., 239 Sandler, I., 408 Sandler, I. N., 393 Sandvik, E., 200 Santiago. R., 396 Sarason, B., 235 Sarason, I. G., 235, 236 Sarason, S. B., 91 Sargent, C, 72, 74 Saric, Z., 232 Saussure, E, xxi Savin-Williams, R. C , 263, 267, 268, 269, 273 Sayegh, Μ. Α., 267 Sayigh, R., 423 Scales, P. C , 142, 304, 306, 408, 409 Schachter, C. L., 158 Schaffer, H. R., 6, 17 Scharf, W, 82 Schene, P., 349 Scheper-Hughes, N., 16, 71, 72, 73, 74, 78, 84, 200

Author Index · 487 Schimmack, U., 190, 195 Schkade, D. Α., 198 Schlenger, W. Ε., 233 Schmidt, J. Α., 219 Schofield, G., 155 Schonell, M., 235 Schorr, L. B., 344, 345, 346, 349, 352 Schram, B., 347 Schulenberg, J. E., 176 Schultz, R., 155 Schuster, Μ. Α., 235, 239 Schutte, K., 398 Schwartz, C. E., 191, 198, 200 Schwartz, D. R., 34 Schwartzwald, J., 232, 233, 234, 235, 282 Schwarz, N„ 195, 196, 198 Scott, C., 7 Scott, S., 434 Scott, Y. K., 437, 447 Searles, H., 123, 126 Sebba, R., 126, 128 Seccombe, K., 306 Segal, Z., 36 Segalavich, J. H., 305 Seidlitz, L., 200 Seidman, E., 152 Seifert, K. L., 58 Seigel, L., 298 Seligman, M., 317 Selyavina, L. K., 383 Sen, Α., 12, 197, 198, 199, 203 Sesma, Α., Jr., 304 Shah, M. K., 191 Shahade, Α., 418 Shakespeare, T., 94 Shalev, A. Y, 232 Shalev, Υ Α., 283 Shanahan, M. J., 306 Shao, L., 195, 196 Sharma, B., 152 Sharma, J. M., 149 Sharma, N., 152 Sharp, D., 5 Shaw, D. S., 457 Shehadeh, R., 421 Shek, D. T. L., 314, 315, 320, 321, 323 Shelina, S. L., 372, 373 Shelly, J., 159 Shema, S. J., 239 Shen, E., 158 Shepard, P., 125 Sherbakova, A. M., 381 Sherbourne, C. D., 193 Sherrod, L., 122 Shevchenko, U. S., 376

Shillington, R. E., 344 Shilshtein, E. S., 378 Shimoni, R., 301 Shirley, C., xxxi Shonkoff, J. P., xviii, 301, 456 Shure, M., 460 Shweder, R., 15, 191 Siegelman, C. K., 59 Siljander, R., 440 Silbereisen, R. K., 150 Silva, J. Α., 152 Silva, P. Α., 170 Silver, R. C., 240 Silverman, D., 154 Simeone, W. E., 34 Simmons, Α., 438 Simon, B., 18, 155, 340 Sinclair, M., 107 Sirgy, J. M., 200 Siverson-Hall, B., 303 Sjaastad, J. E., 281 Slaby, R. G., 457, 460, 462 Slavens, G., 301 Sloan, T., 90, 95 Sloboda, Z., xx, xxi, xxx, 150 Slone, M., 233, 238 Slonim-Nevo, V., 282, 288 Smale, G. G., 350 Small, S., 419, 422, 430 Smith, C., 123 Smith, C. C., 434, 437, 438, 440, 441, 442, 444 Smith, C. M., 397 Smith, D. E., 46 Smith, Ε. M., 232 Smith, H. E., 195 Smith, P., 231 Smith, P. K., xxxii, 151, 215 Smith, R. S., xvi, xviii, 5, 6, 7, 17, 75, 76, 122, 136, 137, 140, 142, 249, 303, 316, 324, 392, 393, 398, 409 Smith, T. J., 142 Smokowski, P., 123 Smollar, J., 77 Snaith, R. P., 193 Sobel, D., 125, 127 Sobel, J., 460 Sobkin, V. S., 373, 374 Sobol, A. M„ 169 Solokhina, Τ. Α., 372 Solomon, R. P., 438 Solomon, Z., 231, 232, 233, 234, 235, 236, 239, 282 Song, L. D., 137, 141, 143, 316 Soraski, H., 239 Soukamneuth, S., 409

488 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Speight, S. L., 90, 91,95, 98 Spencer, M. B., xxxiii, 47, 154, 216, 316 Spencer, P., 283 Spencer, R., 266 Spitalny, K. C , 303 Spizman, R. F., 462 Sprangers, M. A. G., 191, 196, 198, 200 Sroufe, Α., 9 Sroufe, L. Α., 137, 301, 323, 398 Stalker, C. Α., 158 Starfield, B., 281, 289 Stattin, H., 457 Stein, E., 239 Steinberg, L., 393 Steinbock, D., 7 Stephens, R. L„ 396 Stepney, P.. 353 Sterk, C. E., 48 Stevens, H., 109 Stevens, J. W., 45, 46, 47, 51, 54 Stocklin, D„ 82 Stoiber, K. C , 298, 300 Stolorow, R. D., 52 Stone, B., 302 Stone, D., 302 Stone, M., 166 Stone, M. R., 409 Stoolmiller, M., 296, 298 Story. M., 268 Stouthamer-Loeber, M., 458 Strack, R, 195, 198 Straker, G., 7 Strauss, Α., 58, 154 Strauss, Μ. Α., 461 Strawbridge, W. J., 239 Streissguth, A. P., 457 Stretzler, J., 216 Stroebe, M., 200 Stroube, W., 200 Stroul, Β. Α., 138, 388, 389, 390, 391, 396, 399 Stuart, Μ. Α., 457 Stutzer, Α., 192 Stuvland, R., 231, 234 Su, Y, 409 Suda, C , 74 Sue, D., 216 Sue, D. W., 216 Suh, Ε. M., 190, 195, 196 Suleiman, R., 230 Sullivan, A. M., 155 Sullivan, R., 90 Sullivan, W, 127, 138 Suis, J., 200 Summerfelt, W. T., 397 Summerfield, D., 14, 15

Super, C , 13 Surrey, J. L., xxiii Sutton, J., xxxii, 151, 215 Swadener, Β. B., 45, 50 Swaine, Α., 17 Swart, J., 82 Swartz, L., xviii, xix, 219 Swatling, S., 298 Swenson, C. C , 214 Swettenham, J., xxxii, 151, 215 Tacon, R., 80 Tajfel, Ft., 36 Tajima, Ε. Α., 139 Takanishi, R., 214 Talamantes, Μ. Α., 33 Tank, V, 315 Tanner, J., 440, 442 Tarter, R. E., xx Tashakkori, Α., xxx Taylor, A„ 127 Taylor, E., 198 Taylor, J. M., 155 Taylor, S. E., 200 Teague, T. L., 324 Tebes, J. K., 298 Teddlie, C , xxx Tedeschi, R. G., 38, 39, 232, 234, 321 Tellegen, Α., 195, 198, 249, 391 Templeton, J., 166 Tennant, C , 235 Teram, E., 153, 155, 156, 158 Ternowetsky, G., 344 Terr, L. C , 235 Terry, K., 144 Tesser. Α.. 198 Thabet, Α. A. M., 230, 231, 233, 234 Thoits, P., 151 Thomas, Α., 457 Thomas, Β. H., 392 Thomas, E., 82 Thomas, J., 330, 332, 333 Thompson, Α., 193 Thompson, A. I., xviii, 29, 31, 38, 122, 360. 399 Thompson, Ε. A„ xviii, 29, 31, 122, 360, 399 Thornberry, T. P., 457 Tierney, J. P., 303 Tierney, N., 78 Tierney, W G„ 62 Tineo, W., 299 Todis, B., 155 Tolan, P. H., 142 Tolfree, D., 7, 84 Tolman, D. L., 266 Tolman, R. M., 458

Author Index · 489 Tolstikh, Ν. Ν., 384 Tortorici, J., 13 Toth, S. L., 391 Toubiana, Y. H., 235, 236 Tout, K., 301, 302 Toutant, C , 156 Tran, K., 436 Tremblay, R. E., 170, 268, 457, 458, 459, 461,463,465 Tremble, B., 268 Trew, K., 231 Triandis, H. C , 196 Trible, J. E., 34 Trimble, S., 125, 127 Trocme, N., 109, 344 Troiden, R. R., 267 Troughton, E., 457 Tseng, W. S., 32, 216, 360 Tudor-Smith, C , 176 Tulloch, H., 435 Tully, C. T., xxxiii Turner, J., 36 Turner, R. H., 143 Turton, R., 7 Tversky, Α., 200 Tyler, Ε, 82 Tyler, S., 82 Ugalde, Α., 17 Ulrich, R., 125 Ungar, M., xxii, xxiv, xxxii, 4, 57, 59, 75, 122, 141, 143, 153, 155, 156, 157, 158, 168, 186, 212, 214, 264, 320, 321, 399, 405, 407, 408 Uribe, V., 268 Vaillant, C. O., 140 Vaillant, G. E., 137, 140 Valentine, J. C., 304 Valiente, C., 169 Vandell, D. L., 302 VanDenBerg, J. E., 138 Van de Velde, R, 298 Vandewiele, M., 279 Vandivere, S., 301, 302 Vanistendael, S., 122 Van Soest, D., 301 van Teeffelen, T., 421,422 Vanyukov, M., xx Vattathara, T., 330 Veale, Α., 73, 74, 78, 79, 252 Veenhoven, R., 190, 194, 196 Vera, E. M., 90, 91, 95, 98 Verghese, B. G., 330 Verma, S., 152 Versage, E. M., 298

Vitaro, E, 463, 465 Vizek-Vidovic, V., 230, 233 von Eye, Α., 150 Vostanis, P., 230, 231, 233, 234 Vostroknutov, Ν. V, 378 Vraalsen, P., 7 Vygotsky, L., 5, 10, 384 Wagner, E. H., 168 Wagner, M. M., 300 Waintrup, M., 155 Walker, B. R., xxx Walker, J. S., 398 Walker, J. W, 435, 446 Wallace, E., 34 Wallace, J. M., 45, 176 Waller, M., 363 Wallerstein, N., 158 Walsh, D. J., 322, 323, 324 Walsh, E, xvii, xxxii, 122, 443, 446 Walton, J. R., 230, 231, 236 Ward, C. H., 168 Ware, H., 279, 289 Ware, J. E., 193 Wasserman, G. Α., 459 Waysman, M., 232, 233, 235, 282 Wearing, Α., 197, 198 Webber, M., 75 Webster-Stratton, C., 300, 459 Weick, Α., 138 Weihenmayer, E., xvii Weinberg, E, 264 Weinberg, R. Α., 176 Weinger, S., 281,289 Weinreb, M. L., 316 Weisenberg, M., 233, 234 Weisz, J. R., 394, 458 Werner, Ε. E., xvi, xviii, 5, 6, 7, 17, 75, 76, 122, 136, 137, 140, 142, 249, 303, 316, 321, 324, 348, 392, 393, 395, 398, 408, 409, 444, 449, 451 West, C., 61 West, D. J., 457 Wheatley, T. P., 196 Wheeler, L., 200 White, D. R., 279 White, G., 94 White, S., 191 Whiting, B., 72 Whitman, M., 458 Whitney, S. D., 139, 140 Widom, C. S., 443, 458 Wiley, Α., 406 Wilkin, D., 194 Wilkins, G., 127, 128 Wilkinson, R., 93

490 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH William, T. M., 45 Willms, J. D., 344, 345, 346 Wilson, E. O., 12, 124, 125 Wilson, J. W., 45 Wilson, L., 31 Wilson, T. D., 196 Wilson-Brewer, R., 460 Windle, M., 395, 397 Winfield, L. Α., 321 Winnicott, D. W., 413 Withey, S., 192 Wittrup, I., 289 Wohlwill, J., 125 Wolchik, S., 393, 408 Wold, B., 176 Wolf, R. C , 268 Wolin, S., 122, 319, 322 Wolin, S. J., 122,319, 322 Wood, G., 202 Wood, J.V., 198, 200 Woodbridge, M. W., 397 Woodhead, M., 12, 13, 17, 18 Woodward, K., 422 Woodworth, G., 457 Work,W, 151,212 Worrell, E C , 316 Wortley, S., 440-441, 442 Wortman, C. B., 240 Wright, S., xxviii Wu, R., 302 Wyman, R, 9 Wyman, P. Α., 151, 212, 393, 408 Yamamoto, J., xviii, 152, 154, 216 Yartsev, D. V., 377

Yastrebov, V. S., 372 Yates, M., 409 Yates, T., 9 Yates, W. R., 457 Yellin, Ε. M., 151 Yeo, G., 33, 34 Yoon, G., 196 Yoshikawa, H., 299, 301, 459 Young, T, 362, 365, 367 Youniss, J., 409, 410 Yu, H. C , 409 Yule,W., 231 Zahr, L. K., 230 Zandstra, M., 105, 365, 366 Zansky, S. M., 303 Zaslow, M. J., 214, 301 Zea, M., 82 Zehr, H., 363 Zeidner, M., 235 Zelazo, L. B„ 392, 397 Zemanek, M., 128 Zhou, Q., 169 Ziebland, S., 193 Zigler, E., 151,303 Zimet, G. D., 267 Zimmerman, D., 61 Ziv, A. M., 235 Zivcic, I., 230, 234 Znaniecki, E, 58 Zonderman, A. B., 198 Zuzul, M., 232 Zvicic, I., 234 Zwi, Α., 17

SUBJECT INDEX

Aboriginal youth abuse of. See Maltreatment of Canadian Incidence Study on Reported Child Abuse and Neglect (CIS) of, 106, 109, 110, 113, 117 colonization and, 106, 360 community-based child welfare services for, 106, 107-117, 358-369 culture/context and, 109, 359-362, 367, 369 customary care and, 365-369 Department of Indian Affairs and, 105, 107 EuroAVestern influence on, 106-108, 360-361 family and, 111-114, 360-361 family group conferencing (FGC) and, 363-365, 367, 369 first Nations and, 108, 110, 116, 117, 357-360, 362, 366 history of, 106-107, 358-360 in non-Aboriginal families, 108, 359, 366-367 language of, 365-367 legislation affecting, 107-109, 358, 360, 362, 364, 366, 368 maltreatment of, 109-110 (table), 111 (table), 112 (tables), 113 (table), 114 (table), 115 (table), 117, 357-361 racism and, 358, 366 resilience of, 106, 116-117 restorative justice for, 361-363 risks faced by, 105, 107-108, 110, 114, 116 Royal Commission on Aboriginal Peoples and (RCAP) and, 106, 107 socioeconomic factors of, 107, 110, 113-114, 116, 359 state care of, 105-108, 358-362 statistics on, 109-116, 359 versus non-Aboriginal, 357-358 Adaptation by men, 61-62, 64-65 by women, 61, 65 context and, 64 defined, 59 developmental perspective of, 398 ecological systems theory and, 393

gendered, 60-62, 64, 67-68 masculinities theory and, 61-62 schémas and, 60 versus maladaptive behavior, 67. See also Maladaptive behavior Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities, xviii Adversity childhood, 3, 5-6 conceptualization of, 10 contextual nature of, 15-17, 151. See also Cultural context coping mechanisms for, 14 gender and, 6 perception and, 10 positive effects of, 17 poverty. See Poverty social effects of, 13-15 street children and. See Street children See also Posttraumatic stress disorder African American youth assertion by, 49-50 Canadian. See African Canadian youth case study of, 63-65 community and, 50-51 culture/context and, 47-54 end-of-life decisions and, 33 empathy in, 49-50 families of, 51-52, 154 female, 47-52 interventions for, 47-53 mutuality in, 49-50 problems of, 45, 47, 50, 62, 434 recognition and, 49-50 research on, 46-54, 212 resilience of, 51-52, 433^134 socioeconomic status of, 433 stressed, 212 working with, 52-53 See also Resiliency framework. Hill's African Canadian youth Africentricity of, 452 context and, 435-444

492 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH education of, 436^40, 450-451 family/community and, 449—450 positive racial identity of, 448—449, 452 problems of, 435^136, 451 program suggestions for, 451^152 racial profiling of, 440—142 racism toward, MA-AM, 451 research on, 434, 436-438, 441-442, 446-451 role models/mentors for, 449 socioeconomic status of, 435-436, 439^440 survival and success for, 447—448 See also Resiliency framework, Hill's Aggression, in children, 456^161 AIDS. See HIV/AIDS Alaskan Natives. See Native American youth Alternative approaches advocacy and, 351 collaboration and, 349-352 community capacity building and, 352 ecological approach to, 347-351. See also Ecological perspective family-centered, 351-352 innovation in, 350 multideterminism and, 348 need for, 343-353 outreach and, 350-352 past practices and, 348 prevention/early intervention in, 350-352 rights/needs of youth and, 351 suggestions for, 352-353 Americans with Disabilities Act, 94 Anatomy of Hope: How People Prevail in the Face of Illness, The, 144 Anderson, Perry, 199 Asian/Pacific Islanders, 33-34. See also Hawaiian youth At-risk youth African American. See African American youth child psychopathology and, 394, 397, 400 defining, 217 mental health care of, 154 models of, 137 outcomes for, 57 out-of-home placements for, 153 psychosocial health of. See Psychosocial health See also specific ethnic group; Risk; Street children Balkans, 17, 19 Bangladesh, 190, 202 Bedouins. See Polygamous families, children of Biophelia, 124-125. See also Natural environment Black Canadians: History, Experiences, Social Conditions, 453 Bosnia, 231

Bowling Alone, 411 Bowling for Columbine, xxiii Brazil, 84, 155 Bryce, Dr. P.H., 107 Buddhists, 34 Bullying, 151 Canadian youth African. See African Canadian youth economic factors and, 344-345 families and, 345 gay/lesbian, 269-272. See also Lesbian/gay/bisexual youth homeless, 75-76, 80. See also Street children problems of, 344-345 research on, 171 screening mechanism for, 315 services for, 343-345, 348 society and, 344 See also Aboriginal youth; African Canadian youth; Alternative approaches; International Resilience Project Caste, 11 Centers for Disease Control, 170, 171 Challenge model, 249 Chicago School of Sociology, 62 Child abuse/neglect, 15. See also specific ethnic group Child care, 301-302 Child development adolescence and, 72-73 adversity and, 3, 5-6, 15 aggression and. See Aggression attachment and, 7, 76 Bronfenbrenner's theory of, 295-296 communities and, 301 cultural differences in, 10, 12-13, 16-17, 20, 72-73, 248 family/community influences on, 296 (figure), 443. See also Family resilience identity formation and, 267 natural environment and. See Natural environment needs, 248, 297-301 normal, 215 normative, xxi, xxiii, xxvi parenting and, 297-301 Piaget's theory of, 4-5, 60 psychosocial health and. See Psychosocial health research, 4-6, 11, 60, 295-296 social ecology of, 254-256 socialization in, 256 sociocultural approach to, 5 Child labor, 18

Subject Index · 493 Children, as social agents, 18-21. See also Youth civic engagement Children in Families at Risk, xvii Circles of Care project background of, 248 community and, 247, 249-252 culture and, 249, 256 description of, 249-252, 256, 260, 247 gender and, 259-260 goals of, 249-251,261 identifying vulnerable children for, 258-260 key informant interviews in, 251 mapping rights for, 253-254 results of, 256-258, 260-261 rights-based approach to, 252-25 (figure), 256-257 triple A model of, 247-248, 250-254, 256, 260-261 See also HIV/AIDS Columbian youth Early Prevention of Aggression Project for, 456, 460-461 evaluation of project for, 463 (table), 464 (tables), 465 family and, 458, 462-463 interventions for, 456, 459-466 lessons learned from project for, 465—466 program for Peaceful Coexistence for, 459^*66 resilient men and, 458 violence prevention programs for, 455^156, 459^166 See also Developing countries, resilience in Communities after school activities and, 302-303 caring, 301-303 child care and, 301-302 child development and, 301. See also Child development comprehensive community initiatives (CCIs), 305 examples of programs for, 305-306 importance of, 301, 306, 412-413 interventions in, 137, 302-306 mentoring in, 303-304 neighborhoods, 301-306 problems in, 301 psychological sense of (PSOC), xxviii resources in, 305-306, 343-344 suggestions for, 305 youth development in, 304-306 See also specific ethnic group Communities that Care model, 137 Compensatory model, 249 Competence, 10, 322, 323, 395, 397-399 Conformity, xxii, xxvi, 156

Congolese youth, 19 Context. See Cultural context Critical psychology. See Psychology, critical Cultural competence, 54 Cultural context assumptions about, 9-10, 149 contexting and, 47 contextual sensitivity and, xxv-xxvii, 35, 152-155,211-213,216, 225 importance of, xxii-xxiii, 15-17, 20, 142, 152, 212-213, 216, 225, 321, 324, 399 individuals and, 10 of risk, 10 research and, 151-158,211 trend toward, xxii Cultural embeddedness, xviii, xxiii Cultural identity. See Cultural context; Family resilience, ethnic identity and Cultural psychology, 191 Culture, defined, 40 Death, 15-16, 33-35 Deductive qualitative analysis (DQA), 58 Depression, 169 Developing countries, resilience in agency and structure in, 199 comparison gap and, 201 "false consciousness" and, 198-199 "focusing illusion," and, 198 happiness and, 190 homeostatic theory and, 201, 204 multiple discrepancy theory and, 201 objective versus subjective evaluations and, 191, 199 poverty and, 190, 198, 201-202, 204 programs for, 458—459 quality of life and. See Quality of life Research Group on Wellbeing in Developing Countries (WeD), 189, 201-203 research on, 189-190, 201-204, 458 resource profiles approach (RPA) and, 201-204 "response shift" and, 199-200 social comparison and, 200-201 subjective well-being (SWB) and. See Quality of life, subjective well-being and the "gap" and, 190-191, 198-199, 204 See also specific country Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 166, 231 Disabilities, people with as a social issue, 93-95, 101 independent living movement by, 94 institutionalization of, 94 laws covering, 94

494 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH problems of, 93-94 resilience of, 93, 95, 101 social versus individual model of disability and, 94-95 values of, 91 wellness/liberation of, 93 Earle, Dr. Kathleen, 110 Ecological perspective, 47, 50, 154, 345-346, 348, 393, 461 Ethiopia, 73, 74, 79, 81, 202-204 Ethnic identity and identity, 35 central thesis of, 35 changeable nature of, 35-36 development, 36 ethnocentricity and, 194 family resilience and, 28, 35-36, 38, 40-41 importance of for minorities, 36, 38, 360 self-esteem and, 36 See also specific ethnic group Family resilience ABCX model of, 28-29 collective social control and, 296-297 communities and. See Communities core competencies for, 31-32 culture and, 32-35, 38, 40, 306, 360 developmentally appropriate experiences and, 299-300 end-of-life decisions and, 33 ethnic identity and, 28, 35-36, 38, 40-41. See also Ethnic identity FAAR framework of. 29 family crises and, 28-31, 38-39, 347 family schema and, 36-39 identity and, 35, 40 interventions for, 298-299, 306, 444 limit setting, structure and expectations and, 300-301 models of, 29-30 (figure), 31, 360 parenting and, 296-301, 306, 393 physical protection, safety and regulation and, 297-298 precrisis versus postcrisis factors and, 28, 29, 39 prosocial behavior and, 29 protective factors and, 29, 38. See also Protective factors relationships, nurturing, and, 298-299 research on, 27-38 socioeconomic factors and, 29, 31, 306 stress and, 347, 444 typologies of, 29, 37-38 variability of, 29

Family science, 27, 28, 37 Feminist psychology, 60 Gender adaptation and, 60-62, 64, 67-68 adversity and, 6 circles of Care project and, 259-260 Russian youth and, 373-374 street children and, 74, 75, 77, 80-81, 85 violence and, 58-61, 64, 66, 67-68 Girl in the Picture, The: The Kim Phuc Story, Groopman, Dr. Jeremy, 144 Hacking, Ian, 155 Hawaiian youth at-risk status of, 38 end-of-life decisions and, 34 ethnic identity of, 27, 36-39 family schema of, 33-34, 38, 39 lokahi and, 39 research on, 17, 137, 140, 409 resilience of, 38-40, 122, 137, 409 self esteem and, 36 stressors of, 36, 38 Head Start, 456 Health communal experience of, xxiv data, xix defined, 166, 406 empiricist view of, xix etiology of, xx hermeneutic/constructionist view of, xix meaning of, xviii-xix, xxi polyocular view of, xix promoting, 347-350. See also Alternative approaches psychosocial. See Psychosocial health resources, xxiv understanding, xxi See also Well-being; Wellness Helping professions as critical practitioners, 96 (figure) as social change agents, 96-101 phronesis and, 97 power/inequality and, 97 promoting social justice, 89-90, 95-101 proximal caring by, 89-90 psychopolitical validity and. See Psychopolitical validity roles of, 95-98 suggestions for, 97 wellness/liberation and, 96-101. See also Liberation; Wellness High-risk youth. See At-risk youth Hispanic youth, 33, 212

Subject Index HIV/AIDS. 78-80, 247-248. See also Circles of Care Hong Kong youth assets of, 316-318, 322-323 at-risk, 313-315, 321 culture and, 319-320 (figure), 322 (table), 323-324 initiatives for, 313-316 (figure), 317-325 juvenile delinquency among, 314-315 mental health of, 314 suicide in, 314-315 Understanding the Adolescent Project (UAP) for, 314-319, 321-322 (table), 323-324 Ideology, 237-239 India, 190, 198-199, 329. See also Indian youth Indian youth All Assam Students Union (AASU) of, 330 characteristics of, 320, 330-333, 336, 340-341 community support for, 334-335, 341 culture and, 330, 331 Don Bosco Youth Mission and Educational Services (DBYES) for, 336-338, 341 economic prospects for, 335 faith and religion and, 334-340 family support for, 334 female, 333-334 initiatives for, 335-341 North Eastern Regional Youth Commission (NERYC) for, 336-340 problems of, 329-334 resourcefulness of, 340 responsibility of, 335 violence and, 332-333, 339-340 Individualism, xxvi, 156 Institutionalization, 153 International Classification of Diseases (ICD), 166, 168 International Resilience Project child and Youth Resilience Measure (CYRM) in, 219-221, 223 community involvement in, 215 constraints and opportunities encountered during the, 222-223 design of, 158, 211-217, 223-224 dialogue on methods phase of, 216-223 domains studied in, 217-219 ethical issues in, 221-222, 223-224 fieldwork documents in, 223 lessons learned from, 224-225 qualitative and quantitative methods for, 213-215, 219-221,225 sharing and understanding phase of, 216

Interventions, xxxii-xxxiii, 156. See also specific ethnic group Inuit youth. See Native American youth Ireland, 231 Israeli/Palestinian conflict, 155, 229-231, 417-418, 423. See also Israeli youth research study; Palestinian youth Israeli youth research study child Posttraumatic Stress Reaction Index (CPTS-RI) for, 231 description of, 230, 233-235 fear and, 241 ideological commitment in, 237-239, 241 posttraumatic growth in, 232-233, 240-241 posttraumatic symptoms in, 232-236 religious conviction and, 239-241 results of, 231, 232, 233-236, 238-241 social support and the, 235-237 terror management theory and the, 237 See also War, exposure to Juvenile delinquency, xxi, 314—315 Kenya, 73,76-78, 79,81,83 Kuwait, 231 Latinos. See Hispanic youth Lesbian/gay/bisexual (LGB) youth challenges facing, 263, 267-269 community and, 274 contexts of, 263-264, 266-267 disclosure by, 268 discrimination of, 268 family and, 265-266, 268, 270, 273-274 heterosexism and, 264-269, 273, 275 HIV and, 270. See also HIV/AIDS homogenizing of the, 267 homophobia and, 264-270, 273, 273, 275 initiatives for, 275, 269-274 research on, 267-268 resilience and, 269, 272-275 social opportunities for, 271-272 societal constructs and, 264, 266 sexism and, 265-266 truth in being in, 272-273 Liberation goals of, 93 personal, relational, and collective domains of, 92 (table) promotion by helping professions, 90, 96-98. See also Helping professions psychQpolitical validity and, 98-101. wellness and, 93, 96, 97. See also Wellness Locus of control, xxv Lord's Resistance Army (LRA), 16

496 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Maasai community, 7-8 Maladaptive behavior adaptation and, 67. See also Adaptation attachment and, 76 context and, 65, 67 defining, 59 examples of, 48, 57, 59 researching, 57, 142-143 street children and, 71. See also Street children See also Adaptation; Violence Marxist theory, 199 Medical model, 137, 138 Mental health, xix, 214. See also Systems of care Mental Health for Canadians: Striking a Balance, 406 Mentoring, 303-304, 425^126, 429, 449 Millennium Development Goals, 191 Moore, Michael, xxiii-xxiv National Health Interview, 28 National Institute of Mental Health, 392 Native American youth, 16, 34-35, 40 Natural environment adolescence and the, 125 biophilia theory and the, 124-125 childhood development and the, 125-129 healing nature of the, 128 importance of the, 121, 125-129 research on the, 127-129 resilience and the, 123-125, 127-129 risk and the, 123 Nazi concentration camps, 237 Nepal, 17, 215 Nigeria, 190 Normative development. See Child development, normative Nurturing Young Black Males, 433 Occupied Palestinian Territories (OPT), 417-419, 423-424. See also Israeli/Palestinian conflict; Israeli youth research study; Palestinian youth Office of Population Census and Surveys (OPCS), 94 Outcomes. See Resilience, research on, outcomes Palestinian youth activities for, 423 association for Leadership and Rights Activation (PYALARA), 417, 419-420, 424^130 characteristics of, 421—422 community and, 430 cultural identity of, 421

disabilities and, 426 education of, 422 family and, 422-423, 430 initiatives for, 417, 423-426, 428, 430, 427-430 mental health issues and, 419-422 mentors for, 425-426, 429 problems of, 418-426 religion and, 422 research on, 231, 235, 418 rights of, 430 self-efficacy of, 420 sources of resilience for, 421-424 socioeconomic status of, 418^119 Speak Up television program for, 427 sumud of, 421—422 The Youth Times and, 424-428 UNICEF and the, 424-425, 427 violence and, 418^420, 424-426 We Care project for, 425-426, 428 Peru, 201, 202 Phronesis, 97 Piaget, Jean, 4—5 Polygamous families, children of Bedouin-Arab culture and, 280-282 economic status of, 281, 287, 289 family life of, 279-282 interventions for, 289-291 mediating factors for, 284, 286 (figure), 287-288 pathogenic versus salutogenic perspective of, 282 problems of, 279, 282, 287-290 psychosocial and familial functioning of, 284-285 (table), 286 (figure) 288 (table), 289 research on, 279-288 results of study on, 283-284 (table), 285 (table), 286 (figure), 287 (table), 288 (table) sociodemographic characteristics of, 283-284 (table), 287 (table) versus children of monogamous families, 283-289 Possible selves, 144 Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) controversy surrounding, 13-15 defined, 13 first identification of, 13 in children, 13-14, 79,419 limitations of, 14 war and, 230-233, 236-237, 239, 241. See also War, exposure to Poverty, 15, 74-75, 78-79, 190, 198, 249, 344. See also specific ethnic group

Subject Index · 497 Protective factors additive/interactive models of risk and, 348 competence, 322, 323, 395 defined, 250 examples of, 6-8, 137, 250, 316-317, 348, 392, 394 family, 295, 392 importance of, 323, 394 levels of, 272 main effect versus buffering effect of, 392 model of, 250, 394 promoting, 347 religion, 239-240 research on, 141-142, 213, 346. See also Resilience, research on skill-based, 323 social support, 235-237 socioeconomic, 392 versus resilient factors, 346 versus risk factors, 137, 346, 348, 392, 394-395. See also Risk, factors See also Resilience mechanisms Protective processes, xxv, 6-8, 18, 123, 394. See also Protective factors Psychology, 27, 28, 90, 95 Psychopolitical validity defined, 98 epistemic, 98-99 (table), 101 transformational, 98, 100, 101 (table) wellness/liberation and, 98-101. See also Liberation; Wellness Psychosocial health across countries and cultures, 171-185 caring and, 182-183 (figure), 184 changes over time in, 170-182, 184 competence and, 176-178 (table) confidence and, 180-182 (table), 183 (figure) connectedness and, 182, 184 (figures) connection and, 177-179 (table), 180 (figure), 181 (table, figure) data on, 169-184 defining, 165-167 development assets of, 167, 394 epidemiology and, 165-166 five Cs and, 167-169, 172-185 health Behavior in School-Aged Children Survey (HBSCS) on, 171-172, 176, 177, 184 longitudinal surveys on, 171, 173-176 measuring, 165-175 (table), 176, 182, 185-186 monitoring the Future project on, 175-176 "person-in-environment" perspective of, 167 positive youth development (PYD) model of, 167-171, 185-186 programs for, 168

resilience and, 167-168, 186 Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System (YRBSS) on, 170-172 Public child-serving organizations, problem of, 346-347 Qualitative research advantages of, 140-141, 155, 156, 214 context and, 152-158 examples of, 46-47, 138, 141, 151-155 "hidden corridor curriculum," and, 157 interpretation and, 46 limitations of, 155-157, 214 model of mediation and, 158 need for, xxx, 143, 144, 151, 155 participatory action, 158-159 policy and, 157-159 psychosocial health and, 165 validity of, 138-139, 158-159 versus quantitative, 141, 158, 214 youth participation in, 152-158 See also International Resilience Project; Resilience, research on Quality of life (QoL) cultural differences and, 194-197 different perspectives of, 191-194 domains of, 192 global versus profile measure of, 192, 195-197 happiness and, 194-197, 201 health-related, 192-194 holistic nature of, 194 life satisfaction and, 195-197 material circumstances and, 196-197 measures of, 192-197 research, 192-195, 201-204 social/environmental factors and, 194 subjective well-being (SWB) and, 192, 194-198, 201-202 Racial profiling, 440-441 Resilience and Vulnerability: Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities, xviii Resilience, defined, 6, 20, 59, 122, 135, 150, 153, 168, 264,316,443 Resilience, defining as a process, 407 by characteristics of, 443 by research, xvi consensus on, xxvii, 135, 391, 395 cultural context and, 321. See also Cultural context difficulty of, xxvii, xxxiii, 9, 59, 142, 150. 316, 392, 395, 397 essential elements of, xix, xxi, 6, 135 evolution of, 121-123, 136

498 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH heuristics and, xxi importance of, xx individual versus environment and, xxiv, xxvi local versus universal view of, xviii-xx new understanding of, xxvii-xxviii, 9, 20, 89, 321, 392 preventive-promotive theory of, 316 terminology and, 9 theory of, xxviii, 316 wellness and, 406-407. See also Resilience, defined; Wellness Resilience, developmental perspective of, xvii-xviii, 252-254, 295, 391, 398 Resilience, discourse of, xvi, xviii, xxvi Resilience, factors influencing. See Protective factors; Resilience mechanisms Resilience, family. See Family resilience Resilience, measuring, xx, 59, 168, 396. See also Resilience, research on Resilience mechanisms changeable nature of, 18, 20, 122, 123 community support, 8, 14, 16 emotional attachment, 7, 14 ethnicity, 11-12, 35-36 examples of, 6-8, 11-12, 14, 15, 123, 136, 343 macro versus micro, 11-12 physical attributes, 6, 11-12 political/societal, 12-13, 16-17 relationships, 7-8, 14-15, 324 resourcefulness, 11, 340 self-righting, 322 social, 13-15 strengths-based, 394-395 See also Protective factors Resilience, models of, 29-30 (figure), 31, 249-250, 360 Resilience, research on adult interpretation and, 11 assumptions and, 9-10 bias in, xvi-xvii, xxii, xxvi causality and, 139 challenges of, xxx-xxxi, 136 child-focused, 151 context and. See Cultural context cross-cultural, 152-155, 211 cross-sectional studies, 138-140 domains of, 397 early, xvi, 6, 122, 136-137, 398 ecological approach to, 136-137, 154, 213, 461 ethnographic, 138 flaws in, xvi, xx-xxii, 150-152 future of, xxx-xxxi, 144, 399^100 goal of, 135 importance of, xx, 20, 27-28

in developing countries, 189-190, 201-204, 458. See also Developing countries interdisciplinary approach to, xxvii-xxviii, 27 interpreting data for, 46 limitations of, 8-11, 141-143, 148, 150-152, 211, 213-214, 396, 397 longitudinal studies, 8-9, 138, 140, 142, 213 methodological variations of, xxvii, xxx, 138-141, 213-215 multivocal texts, use of, 46 outcomes, xxv, 142-145, 148, 151, 211, 344 participatory, 152-159 phenomological, 138, 154 qualitative. See Qualitative research quantitative, xxx, 46-47, 138, 141, 142, 144, 150, 151,213,214 sociocultural context and, 136-137, 139, 150 strengths perspective of, 138 symbolic interactionism and, 143-144 terminology and, 9 theoretical framework and, xxviii, 47-48 validity of, xxviii, 138-139, 158-159, 213 versus systems of care, 397—400. See also Systems of care Western/Euro influence on, 151-152, 155, 211, 360 See also International Resilience Project; Qualitative research Resilience, stages of, 122 Resilience theory, xxviii, 47—48, 72. See also Ecological perspective Resiliency framework, Hill's, 445—446 Risk constructions of, xxiii-xxv contextual effects and, 123 defined, 6 defining, xxvi, 10, 150 factors, xvi, 137, 139, 142, 213, 345-348, 391-392, 394, 397 internal versus external, 6 management, xxiv, 346 models, 46, 394 negative outcomes and, 122-123, 136, 345-348 predictive, 345-346 social/political causes of, 11-13 street children and. See Street children theoretical framework of, 47-48 traits, 123 understanding, 346 variability of response to, 17-18, 21 versus protective factors, 137, 346, 348, 392, 394 Russian youth characteristics of, 372-375 community and, 380-381

Subject Index · 499 culture and, 372, 374-377 education of, 382-385 family and, 372, 377-381 gender-based differences in, 373-374 interventions for, 380-385 problems of, 371-376, 379, 384-385 research on, 373-379, 384 school of Custodial Parents and, 381 service recommendations for, 381 television and, 376 values of, 373-375 Rwanda, 78-79, 83 Search Institute, 167, 169, 304 Schémas, 60, 67-68, 33-34, 36-39 School-based programs, 323-325. See also specific ethnic group Searles, Harold, 126 Self, concept of, 9-10 Self-disclosure, therapist, 54 Sen, Amartya, 198 Shehadeh, Raja, 421 Smart & Sassy: The Strengths of Inner City Black Girls, 47 Social justice, 89-91, 93, 95-101 Social relativism, xix Socioeconomic factors, 344. See also Poverty; specific ethnic group Somalia, 16 South Africa, 247-248. See also Circle of Care Street children assumptions about, 71 community support for, 84 culture/context and, 72-74, 81, 85 defining, 73-74, 85 examples of, 74-78 families of, 83-84 gender differences in, 74, 75, 77, 80-81, 85 minority versus majority world and, 74-75, 77-78, 80, 81, 82, 84, 85 numbers of, 74, 80, 85 problems of, 74-75, 77-81, 83 programs for, 76, 82-85 protective factors and, 75, 84 psychosocial factors and, 76 reasons for, 74-75, 79 research on, 71-85 resilience of, 72-75, 78, 81 risk and, 73-79, 81 strengths of, 81 war and, 78-79. See also War, exposure to working, 78, 82 Strengthening Family Resilience, xvii-xviii

Strengths perspective, 136, 138, 446 Stress cultures under, xviii cumulative effects of, 18 family, 347, 444. See also Family resilience neurological markers in children with, xviii social support and, 235 youth, 36, 38, 212 See also Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) Subjective well-being. See Quality of life, subjective well-being and Sudan, 74 Systemic domains, 47 Systems of care action orientation of, 398-399 background for, 387-388 culture/context and, 399 defined, 388, 390-391 defining, 396-397 ecological approach to, 393 examples of, 395 family and, 394, 399 framework for, 388-390 (figure), 394, 397 improving, 388-391, 397, 399-400 mental health dimensions of, 391 (table) misconceptions about, 395 outcomes and, 396, 397 research, 396-398 services and support, 398 strengths-based focus of, 394—395 systems theory and, 392-393 values and principles of, 388-389 (table), 394 versus child resilience, 392-401 wraparound approach of, 395, 398, 399 Thailand, 202 Thompson, E.P., 199 Trauma, defined, 13 Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMMS), 177 Uganda, 16, 17 U.N. Committee on the Elimination of Racial Discrimination, 116 U.N. Convention on the Rights of the Child (CRC), 3, 4, 19, 20, 72, 105, 116, 117, 189, 247,368,410,412 U.N. Development Programme (UNDP), 189, 190 UNICEF, 73 Union of the Physically Impaired Against Segregation, 93 Universal by consent, xix U.S. Census, 28, 33, 34 U.S. National Institute of Mental Health, 214

500 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH Values collective, 90 domains of, 90, 92 (table) interdependent, 90-93 of people with disabilities, 91 of researchers, xxi, 216 personal, 90 relational, 90 single, 91 systems of care, 388-389 (table), 399 wellness and, 406—407 Victims, children as, 19 Violence age and, 58-59 case studies about, 63-67 defined, 58, 67 gender and, 58-61, 64, 66, 67-68 masculinity and, 62 morality and, 65-67 perspectives on, xxiii-xxiv relationships and, 58 research on, 57-58, 62-63, 156-157 women's aggressive behaviors and, 61 See also War, exposure to Vygotskyian theory, 5, 20 War, exposure to ideological commitment and, 237-239 pathogenic effects of, 230-231 posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and, 230-233, 236-237, 239, 241 psychic numbing and, 231 religious conviction and, 239-240 salutogenic effects of, 232-233 social support and, 235-237 See also Israeli youth research study; Palestinian youth Weinberg, George, 264 Well-being, 3-4, 17-18, 347-350, 406. See also Health; Wellness

Wellness defined, 406 defining, 93 empowerment and, 93, 406—407 fairness/equality and, 93 importance of, 138, 351 liberation and, 93, 96, 97. See also Liberation maximal, 406 needs and, 406 personal, relational, and collective domains of, 92 (table), 406^410 promotion, 90, 96-98, 138, 351. See also Helping professions psychopolitcal validity and, 98-101 resilience and, 406^107 social justice and, 93 values and, 406^407 Western culture, influence of, 4, 9-10, 106-108, 151-152, 155, 360-361 Wilson, E.O., 124 World Health Organization (WHO), 13, 166, 171 Yazzie, Honorable Robert, 363 Youth civic engagement (YCE) benefits of, 407-409, 411-413 collective wellness and, 409-410, 412 examples of, 408-410 idealism and, 411—412 negative engagement and, 411 power/control and, 408—412 promises of, 405, 411, 412 potential problems with, 405, 410—412 self-efficacy and, 407-408 social capital and, 405, 411-412 voice and, 408—410 wellness and, 406^110, 412-413. See also Wellness Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System (YRBS), 17

ABOUT THE EDITOR

Michael Ungar is both a social worker and marriage and family therapist with experience working directly with children and adults in mental health, educational, and correctional settings. Now Associate Professor in the School of Social Work at Dalhousie University, Halifax, Canada, he continues to supervise and consult extensively with educators, guidance counselors, and other professionals in Canada, the United States, and overseas. He has conducted many workshops internationally on resilience-related themes relevant to the treatment and study of at-risk youth and has published dozens of

peer-reviewed articles on resilience and work with children and their families. He is also the author of two books: Playing at Being Bad, a book for parents, and Nurturing Hidden Resilience in Troubled Youth, for family therapists. Dr. Ungar holds numerous research grants from national funding bodies and is a collaborator on several international research projects as well. Currently, he leads a study titled Methodological and Contextual Challenges Researching Childhood Resilience: An International Collaboration that includes researchers from 10 countries on five continents.

501

ABOUT THE CONTRIBUTORS

Laura S. Abrams is Assistant Professor of Social Work in the School of Social Work at the University of Minnesota, Twin Cities. She is interested in social context influences on youth identity, self-perceptions, and risk behaviors. Her current project is an ethnographic study of cultures of correctional institutions for youthful male offenders. This project explores how these institutions construct masculine identities, how offenders view their treatment, and how these institutions prepare youth for transitions home. Saleem Al-Habash was born in 1982 and is now the managing editor of the Youth Times. He was one of the founding members of the Palestinian Youth Association for Leadership and Rights Activation (PYALARA), where he has volunteered and worked since 1998. He is completing his bachelor degree in journalism and political Science from the University of Birziet and is a freelance writer for the Al-Ayyam daily newspaper. He lives in Ramallah. Alean Al-Krenawi is Associate Professor in and Chairman of the Spitzer Department of Social Work at Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel. His area of research is multicultural social work and mental health with particular interest on indigenous peoples. He has published over 50 peer-reviewed journal articles in the area of social work and mental health with the Arab societies in the Middle East, over 20 book chapters, and several books. His most recent project is the preparation of a book on Islam and social work. Lewis Aptekar is currently Professor of Counselor Education at San Jose State University. Some of his academic awards include Fulbright scholarships (Colombia, Swaziland, Honduras), a position as Nehru Visiting Professor, and a

scholarly residency at the Bellagio Rockefeller Foundation Study and Conference Center (Italy). His has written two books, Street Children ofCali and Environmental Disasters in Global Perspective, and more than 50 articles and chapters. He is currently working on a manuscript titled Human Rights and Survival in an Ethiopian Refugee Camp. Mary I. Armstrong has over 20 years experience in children's behavioral health, public sector managed care, children's health insurance, and child welfare and social services. She is currently Assistant Professor and Director of the Division of State and Local Support, Department of Child and Family Studies at the Louis de la Parte Florida Mental Health Institute, University of South Florida. She is responsible for the administration of the Division of State and Local Support and of evaluation and research activities. Her current activities include a national study of public sector managed care and its effects on children with serious emotional problems, child welfare privatization, the impact of welfare reform on the adolescent daughters of enrollees, and financing mechanisms for systems of care. During 2002, she was appointed as a member of the national Outcomes Roundtable for Children and Families. She has many publications in both professional journals and textbooks. Ken Barter is Professor and former Chair in Child Protection with the School of Social Work, Memorial University of Newfoundland. His research has been in the field of public child welfare, child protection, administration, and social work. He has published numerous book chapters, journal articles, research reports, and conference proceedings and presented to audiences at provincial, national, and international conferences. 503

504 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH William H. Barton is Professor and Director of Research Services at the Indiana University School of Social Work in Indianapolis. Following 8 years of postdoctoral research at the Institute for Social Research and the Center for the Study of Youth Policy, both at the University of Michigan, he joined the faculty of the Indiana University School of Social Work where he has worked since 1993. He teaches courses in juvenile justice policy, program evaluation research methods, and the philosophy of science. His research interests include juvenile justice, delinquency prevention and youth development issues, and the applied methodologies of program evaluation and needs assessment. In addition to several journal articles and book chapters, his publications include two books—Reforming Juvenile Detention (coedited with Ira Schwartz) and Closing Institutions for Juvenile Offenders (with Denise Gottfredson). Wanda Thomas Bernard is Associate Professor and Director of the School of Social Work, Dalhousie University. She has a particular interest in antiracist and anti-oppressive social work theory and practice. Much of her professional study and work pursuits have focused on race, racism, and racial uplift. She and her colleague, David Este, are currently undertaking a major research project on the intersection of violence, racism, and health, a 5-year, multisite, national project funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health. Fred H. Besthorn is Associate Professor of Social Work at the University of Northern Iowa, Cedar Falls, Iowa. He has written extensively on the development of a framework for integrating deep ecological awareness with social work policy and strengths-based practice. This involves research on the relationship between environmental degradation and its social, economic, and spiritual impact on disadvantaged populations. He is the creator of the Global Alliance for a Deep-Ecological Social Work. This unique organization unites social workers around the world sharing a commitment to incorporating deep environmental awareness into traditional social work practice and the founder of Earth Consciousness: The Journal of Environmental Social Work and Human Services—the first

online journal of its kind devoted exclusively to the creative works of social workers and human service professionals who care deeply for the natural world. Hania Bitar is the founder and Director General of the Palestinian Youth Association for Leadership and Rights Activation (PYALARA). In 1997, she established and edited the Youth Times—the first Palestinian youth newspaper— which developed into a 24-page monthly with an outreach of 200,000 youth readers. With a background in literature from the Catholic University (USA), she started her professional career as a teacher of English language at Bethlehem University and at the YAWCA in Jerusalem. She was awarded a Fulbright scholarship in 1992, was nominated as a Global Leader for Tomorrow in 2002 and Young Arab Leader in 2003. She is the author of several articles and has been a keynote speaker at a number of international conferences. Cindy Blackstock is Executive Director, First Nations Child and Family Caring Society and Codirector of the Center of Excellence for Child Welfare. A member of the Gitksan Nation, she has worked in the field of child and family services for over 20 years. Key research interests include exploring the etiological drivers of child maltreatment in First Nations communities and the role of the voluntary sector and philanthropic organizations in expanding the range of culturally and community-based responses to child maltreatment. Roger A. Boothroyd is Associate Professor in the Department of Mental Health Law and Policy at Louis de la Parte Florida Mental Health Institute at the University of South Florida. His background is in the field of educational psychology, specializing in measurement, evaluation, and research design. His current research interests include examining the impact of health care financing arrangements on members' access, outcomes and quality of care, welfare reforms and its effects on mothers and children, and research ethics. He has been a coprincipal investigator on grants funded by the National Institutes of Health and the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration.

About the Contributors · 505 Jo Boyden is a social anthropologist and senior research officer at the Refugee Studies Centre, University of Oxford. For many years, she worked internationally as a social development consultant to a broad range of development and humanitarian relief agencies, governmental, nongovernmental and intergovernmental. This involved a mix of primary and secondary research, advocacy, training, planning, monitoring, and evaluation. Drawing mainly on fieldwork in South Asia, she is currently researching children's and adolescent's experiences of armed conflict and forced migration. The focus of this research is the development of theory and empirical evidence regarding risk, resilience, and coping in childhood; young people's economic, political, and social roles and responsibilities; intergenerational relations; and social and cultural constructions of childhood and youth. Eileen M. Brennan is an applied psychologist with specialized training in human development. She has concentrated her recent scholarship on investigations of family support and the ways in which employed parents of children with emotional disorders fit work and family responsibilities together. She is Professor and Associate Dean of Social Work at Portland State University. For the last 10 years, she has also served as a principal investigator with the Research and Training Center on Family Support and Children's Mental Health for studies of work-life integration and of inclusion of children with mental health disorders in child care settings. She has written and presented widely on the topics of social support, family support, and work-life issues and is currently coauthoring a book on family support and worklife integration with Julie M. Rosenzweig. Marion Brown is a PhD student (social work) at Memorial University of Newfoundland and a lecturer at the School of Social Work, Dalhousie University. Her social work practice has focused on community-based programming with youth populations. Her research interests include exploring gender identity negotiations and constructs of aggressive behaviors among young women in residential care through qualitative methods.

Laura Camfield is Research Officer for the UK ESRC-funded Research Group on Well-Being in Developing Countries (WeD) at the University of Bath. She is a qualitative researcher who also uses quantitative and participatory techniques to explore people's experiences. Her primary research interest is conceptualizing and measuring quality of life, but she has also published on narrative approaches to chronic illness and disability. She completed her PhD on measuring the quality of life of people with dystonia. Since the mid1990s, she has also been engaged in research into the experiences of people living with multiple sclerosis, dystonia and intellectual disabilities, and the treatment and care of people with HIV/AIDS and imprisoned female foreign nationals. Marc Colbourne is a master of social work student in the School of Social Work, Dalhousie University. He worked for 10 years as Coordinator of Support Services at the Lesbian, Gay and Bisexual Youth Project and has recently begun a position with Canada World Youth, facilitating community development projects in Nova Scotia and Latin America. Philip Cook is the founder and current Executive Director of the International Institute for Child Rights and Development. The Institute is part of the Centre for Global Studies at the University of Victoria and a recognized Canadian leader in community-based, national, regional, and international applications of the UN Convention on the Rights of the Child. Since 1995, he has overseen the institute's growth as a leader in linking children's healthy development to broad issues of human development and participatory governance across diverse cultures and situational contexts. Wendy M. Craig is Associate Professor in the Department of Psychology at Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada. Her research program focuses on bullying and victimization and on the development of aggression in young females. In recognition of her work on bullying and victimization, she recently won an Investigator Award from the Canadian Institute of Health Research. She has published widely in the area, including topics of bullying and victimization, peer processes, sexual harassment, and

506 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH aggression in girls. Also, she is editor of a volume on childhood social development. Luis F. Duque is a physician with a background in public health who taught epidemiology and research methods for nearly 15 years at two Colombian universities. He has conducted research projects on the epidemiology of violence in several Colombian cities, and he directed the first National Household Survey on Psychoactive Substance Use in Colombia. He also directed the design and preparation of the City of Medellin's Early Violence Prevention Program and was its first Director. He was also formerly the President of the University of Antioquia, Dean of its National School of Public Health, a Deputy Minister of Health, and Director of the National Institute of Health. He currently works as a researcher of the National School of Public Health, University of Antioquia and is President of the Direction Council of the Colombian Health Association. Lesley du Toit is the Founder and Executive Director of the Child and Youth Care Agency for Development, a South African nongovernmental organization promoting children's full and healthy development. Her work has mainly been in the residential child care field and youth work where she has done direct care work, therapeutic work with troubled children, teaching and supervising of child care workers, curriculum development for degree programs in child and youth care, and more recently, policy and legislative reform for the South African Government from 1995 to 2001. She has received numerous national and international awards for her programming and policy work with children and has advised various national governments on issues of child welfare and HIV/AIDS. Dave Este is an Associate Professor in the Faculty of Social Work, University of Calgary. His teaching and research interests include social work practice with immigrants and refugees, management of nonprofit organizations, qualitative research methods, and mental health. Scot D. Evans is a doctoral student in the Community Research and Action program at Peabody College of Vanderbilt University. He has extensive practical experience in communitybased organizations as a youth development

worker, family counselor, youth program developer, program evaluator, and organizational consultant. Currently, his research is focused on the role of human service organizations in promoting social change, in particular one community-based human service organization that is attempting to shift its practice paradigm from amelioration to transformation. Scot's community work with Isaac Prilleltensky and others at Vanderbilt has attracted the attention of the local United Way who have asked them to begin working closely with four other communitybased organizations on a similar process of change. Barbara Friesen is Director of the Research and Training Center on Family Support and Children's Mental Health, Portland, Oregon, and a Professor of Social Work at Portland State University. She brings substantial experience to the understanding and improvement of services for children and their families through her role as the principal investigator of the Center's Family Participation Project. She has authored publications on a number of mental health topics, including community practice, family-centered services, prevention, professional training, family support, and family views of residential treatment. She has worked in both clinical and administrative positions in a number of mental health settings, including state institutions, adult and children's psychiatric day treatment programs, and therapeutic classrooms in the public schools. James Garbarino is Professor in and Director of the Department of Human Ecology at Cornell University in Ithaca, New York, as well as the Director of the acclaimed "Just for Kids!" program. He is an internationally recognized expert in issues dealing with child abuse, specializing in psychological maltreatment. His research focuses on the impact of violence and trauma in the family and community on child and youth development, and it examines interventions to deal with these effects. He has authored and edited numerous books; serves as a consultant to television, magazine, and newspaper reports on children and families; and has been a consultant or adviser to a wide range of national and international organizations concerned with the health and well-being of children, including the American Medical Association, the National

About the Contributors · 507 Science Foundation, and Childwatch International Research Network.

organizations establish community-based organizations like the one she directs.

Jane F. Gilgun is Professor in the School of Social Work at the University of Minnesota, Twin Cities. She has done research for many years and published widely in the areas of how persons overcome adversities, the meaning of violence to perpetrators, strengths-based assessments and child and family treatment programs, and capacities of parents adopting children with special needs. Her current research projects are on the development of violent behaviors and a project on parenting adoptive children with special needs. She has presented widely on resilience, violence, and treatment approaches that build on client strengths. She is the author of workbooks for children and their families where the children have a variety of adjustment issues associated with histories of adversity.

Joanne Klevens is a physician and former head of the Community Health Department of Javeriana University in Colombia. She currently works for the U.S. Centers for Disease Control, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. She, along with Luis F. Duque, directed the design of the Medellin Early Violence Prevention Project and conducted several research projects on violence epidemiology in Colombia.

Joan Glode is a Mi'kmaq woman from Nova Scotia, Canada. Joan has worked both on- and off-reserve in a variety of programs involving social services for both urban Aboriginal and on-reserve First Nations peoples. She worked as a Human Rights Officer with the Nova Scotia Human Rights Commission, as Executive Director of the Friendship Center in Halifax, and as a middle manager with the Department of Indian Affairs. She is now the Executive Director of Mi'kmaw Family & Children's Services, a mandated child welfare agency that also operates two Family Healing Centers that provide shelter and safety to women and children and outreach counseling to men. Wanjiku Kironyo is a social psychologist with a specialization in marriage and family counseling. She has taught at the University of Nairobi in the Department of Social Work. Through a student placement, she learned about the dire needs of women and children in the slums of Nairobi. This experience led to the development of the Maji Mazuri Center for social and economic development in the late 1980s of which she is Director. She also directed the African Family Development Program for 4 years and chaired the Breastfeeding Information Group. She has traveled widely giving lectures to formal and informal institutions in different parts of the world. She also helps various Kenyan-based

Anna I. Laktionova is Director of the Counseling Center at the Institute of Psychology and Psychotherapy, Moscow, where she works with adolescents. She is also a half-time senior researcher at the Moscow State Psychological and Pedagogical University Research Laboratory. Her work examines the psychological and social problems of orphans in Moscow. Concurrently, she is pursuing doctoral studies at this same university with research focused on coping mechanisms of youth, social adaptation of adolescents, and resilience. She worked previously as a high school vice principal. Avital Laufer is a health sociologist. Her doctoral dissertation examined violence among Israeli youth and won the Bar-Ilan President and Dean prize for excellence. Her studies focus on youth violence and the effects of terror on youth. She completed her postdoctoral studies at the Adler Research Center at Tel Aviv University. Currently, she is a lecturer at the College of Judea and Samaria in Israel. John C. LeBlanc is Assistant Professor of Pediatrics, Psychiatry, and Community Health and Epidemiology at Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada. He is based at the IWK Health Centre, a teaching hospital for children and women's health. He holds an IWK Health Centre Investigatorship award that allows him to devote 7 5 % of his time to research. His research interests are in the early identification of disruptive behaviors in children and the evaluation of school-based interventions to reduce these. Anna W. Lee is currently a postdoctoral associate in the Department of Neurobiology and Behavior at the Rockefeller University in

508 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN AND YOUTH New York. Her current research investigates how estrogen modulates adrenergic facilitation of reproductive behavior on a molecular level. Her excitement in research, however, lies in linking the brain and behavior, encompassing aspects from psychology, animal behavior, neuroscience, and biophysics. During her Ph.D. work at Dalhousie University, she was involved in the initial stages of the International ResiLience project. Lee Tak-yan is Associate Professor and concurrently Fieldwork Coordinator of the Department of Applied Social Studies, City University of Hong Kong. His research covers civic awareness and civic education, youth development indices, parenting styles and parent-child conflict, adolescent gambling behavior, sociocultural beliefs, moral values and behavior, and effectiveness of parent-teacher associations. He has recently published on topics including handling sibling conflicts, adolescent moral behavior, social worker performance planning, learning strategies as predictors for fieldwork outcomes and performance, youth empowerment, adolescents facing problems in the cyber era, and factors in successful relapse prevention of drug addicts. He has published over 50 journal articles, monographs, and book chapters.

Group since October 2000, where there are ongoing research projects developed and completed by this group. Nancy has developed and delivered elective courses for the BSW program called Social Work With Aboriginal Populations and Child Welfare With Aboriginal Peoples. Other courses include Cross Cultural Issues for Social Work Practice and Beginning Social Work Practice. She is a Planning Group Member of Aboriginal Women's Health and Healing Research Group. She is lead author of "Managing Institutional Practices to Promote and Strengthen Diversity: One School's Journey," in AntiOppressive Social Work Practice, edited byWes Shera and is lead author of "Respecting Aboriginal Families: Pathways to Resilience in Custom Adoption and Family Group Conferencing," in Pathways to Resilience, edited by Michael Ungar.

Linda Liebenberg is a South African doctoral student, now managing the International Resilience Project in Halifax, Canada. She has previously managed research projects relating to out-of-school youth in informal settlements surrounding Cape Town and women on farms in the West Coast/Winelands region of South Africa. With a background in psychology and sociology, her main focus is the use of visual methods in varied research contexts. Her interests also include mixed methodologies and how they relate to our understanding of women and children in developing countries.

Alexander V. Makhnach is a psychologist and Rector at the Institute of Psychology, Russian Academy of Sciences, Moscow, a leading institute of psychology in Russia. In 1994-1995 he worked on the development of the international program The Challenge of Foster Care under the auspice of Christian Solidarity International (Switzerland) and the Russian Ministry of Education that designed a system of foster families in Russia. From 1997 to 1998 he served as scientific secretary for the working group for development of the federal program Development of Philosophical, Clinical and Applied Psychoanalysis. More recently he has been on the Board of Experts and then as Scientific Vice-Director of the Assistance to Russian Orphans-ARO Program. He teaches basic skills in counseling, family therapy, foundations of supervision, and assessment in several state universities in Moscow and Tomsk. He is the author of more than 60 papers on issues such as psychodiagnostics, methodology of supervision, personality, and youth issues.

Nancy MacDonald is Assistant Professor in the School of Social Work, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia. Previously, she worked with Mi'kmaq Family and Children's Services of Nova Scotia and has more than 15 years of direct child welfare practice experience with the Mi'kmaq communities in Nova Scotia. She has been a member of the Mi'kmaq Health Research

Gillian Mann has a background in anthropology and education. She has worked for more than 10 years in the field of policy and programs for children living in adversity, as both a practitioner and a researcher. Her interests lie in children's social competencies and relationships at the household, family, and community levels. Recently, her work has focused on the experiences

About the Contributors · 509 of separated boys and girls in particular, including those children who live without their parents as a result of war, HIV/aids, or both. She is currently engaged in an ethnographic study of the cognitive and emotional development of Congolese refugee children living in Dar es Salaam, Tanzania. Jacqueline McAdam-Crisp has degrees in child and youth care and for the past 20 years has worked with children in need of protection in Canada, Kenya, Rwanda, and Ethiopia. Her MA work was done in Nairobi, Kenya, with the assistance of a Canadian International Development Agency (CIDA) award and in cooperation with Wanjiku Kironyo. Her research focused on the development of an alternative form of education for street youth. She is presently working on her PhD in human development at the Fielding Graduate Institute in Santa Barbara, California. Her research focuses on the experiences of children following situations of war and their coping abilities, mental health, and resiliency. Hamilton I. McCubbin is Professor at the Center on the Family, University of Hawaii at Manoa, and Director, Institute for the Study of Resilience in Families and Center on the Family, University of Wisconsin-Madison. Formerly the Chancellor and CEO, Kamehameha Schools, Honolulu, Hawaii, he has also held posts as the Dean of the School of Human Ecology and Child and Family Studies, University of WisconsinMadison, and as Head, Family Social Science, University of Minnesota. His scholarly interests include family stress, coping, and resilience with an emphasis on multi-ethic families, development, and change over the life cycle.

Laurie ("Lali") D. McCubbin is Assistant Professor, in the department of Educational Leadership and Counseling Psychology, Washington State University, in Pullman, Washington. Her research interests and expertise include risk and protective factors and resilience across the life span, cultural identity development, multicultural counseling, and career development. She is also Associate Director of the Stress, Coping and Resilience Project: Individuals, Families and Communities in collaboration with the University of Hawaii at Manoa.

Allister McGregor is Director of the UK ESRC-funded Research Group on Well-Being in Developing Countries (WeD) and is Senior Lecturer in the Department of Economics and International Development at the University of the Bath. The WeD research group is developing a conceptual and methodological framework for understanding the social and cultural construction of well-being in developing countries. The group is working with research partners in Bangladesh, Ethiopia, Peru, and Thailand to carry out detailed empirical research in rural and urban communities in each country. He has a disciplinary background in economics and social anthropology and has extensive experience of primary fieldwork in South and Southeast Asia. George Menamparampil, a Catholic priest and Salesian of Don Bosco, pioneered the nonformal education of the young in North East India and Bhutan. From 1988 to 1997 he was the adult adviser to MIJARC, an international youth organization based in Brussels. In this capacity, he conducted training programs for youth in grassroots and international forums. From 1998 to 2002, he was a member of the national youth animation team of the Salesians in India. Since March 2002, he has been based at Imphal in Manipur, India, offering a variety of services to underprivileged youth. Isaac Prilleltensky is the author or editor of five books in the field of community psychology. In addition to these books, he has published approximately 60 articles in refereed journals and 20 book chapters. He has been an invited speaker in many countries, including Argentina, Canada, England, Scotland, the United States, New Zealand, Australia, Venezuela, Spain, Cuba, Norway, and Israel. He is a member of the editorial board of several journals, including the Journal of Community Psychology and the Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology. He is a fellow of the American Psychological Association and of the Society for Community Research and Action. He is currently the Director of Graduate Studies in Human and Organizational Development at Vanderbilt University, where he also directs the doctoral program in Community Research and Action.

510 · HANDBOOK FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN-AND YOUTH Ora Prilleltensky is a psychologist, who from 2000 to 2002 taught in the counseling program at Victoria University in Melbourne, Australia. She now teaches counseling in the Department of Human and Organizational Development in Peabody College at Vanderbilt University. Ora is a mother with a physical disability and the author of Motherhood and Disability: Children and Choices published by Palgrave/MacMillan in 2004. Vered Slonim-Nevo is Associate Professor in the Spitzer Department of Social Work, Ben Gurion University of the Negev, Israel, where she has taught advanced clinical courses since 1989. Her research areas include family relations, polygamy, immigration, AIDS prevention, and treatment evaluation. She is also a clinical social worker who counsels families and individuals. Zahava Solomon is Professor of Psychiatric Epidemiology and Social Work at the Tel-Aviv University and the Head of the Adler Research Center for Child Welfare and Protection. She joined the Israeli Defense Force (IDF) in 1981 and served as Head of the Research Branch in the Medical Corps from 1981 to 1992. In 1990 she joined the Tel-Aviv University and since May 1997 has been Head of the Adler Center for the Study of Child Welfare and Protection. She is internationally known for her research on traumatic stress and especially on the psychological sequel of combat stress reactions, war captivity, and the Holocaust. She has published five books on psychic trauma-related issues and over 200 articles and more than 50 chapters. She was member of the DSM-4 Advisory Subcommittee for PTSD and has earned numerous Israeli and international awards and research grants. Joyce West Stevens is Professor Emeritus at Boston University School of Social Work. Prior to her tenure there, she spent many years in both the public and private sectors serving inner-city populations in child welfare, mental health, hospital, and private practice settings. Her investigative studies have been in the area of adolescent health-compromising behaviors, including adolescent pregnancy and substance abuse. Her book Smart and Sassy: the Strengths of Inner City Black Girls examines adolescent

developmental issues within the context of daily life, which she argues is necessary when providing direct services. She has been the recipient of both service and research grants and was Principal Investigator for the African American Women's Study, a 3-year NIDA-funded project that investigated the generational transmission of substance use and nonuse among African American women and daughter pairs. Beth A. Stroul is Vice-President and co-founder of Management & Training Innovations, Inc., a consulting firm in McLean, Virginia, and serves as a consultant in the area of mental health policy. She has completed numerous research, evaluation, policy analysis, and technical assistance projects related to service systems for children and adolescents with emotional disorders and their families. She has been a consultant to the National Technical Assistance Center for Children's Mental Health at Georgetown University since its inception in 1984. Her projects have included coauthoring a widely circulated monograph that presents a conceptual framework and philosophy for a system of care titled, A System of Care for Children and Adolescents With Severe Emotional Disturbances. She has published extensively in the field of children's mental health. Pamela J. Talbot is a graduate student in the master of science program in community health and epidemiology at Dalhousie University. She is focusing on mental health and mental health promotion as part of a holistic model of health. Currently, she is an honorary Killam scholar. Eli Teram is Professor, Faculty of Social Work, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada. He teaches policy, research, and organization and management courses in the master's and doctoral programs. His research interests relate to the organizational context of social work practice, including professional power and the processing of clients, teamwork, organizational control, and multiculturalism. His current research projects include an action-oriented study of ethical issues in social work practice (with Marshall Fine) and an exploration of the experience of childhood sexual abuse survivors with health professionals (with Candice Schachter and Carol Stalker). He is on the editorial board of the

About the Contributors · 511 Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health. He started his professional career working with street gangs in Israel. Jerry Thomas is a member of the Salesians of Don Bosco, a religious order working with youth. He has been working with youth in North East India for the past 20 years. He recently completed a youth survey for the Salesian province of Dimapur, and studies on youth work in Northeast India, on the ULFA—an insurgent group in Assam. His work has examined the impact of violence on youth. Currently as Director of the North Eastern Regional Youth Commission at Guwahati, he facilitates and coordinates youth activities in the 11 Catholic dioceses in the region. Nico Trocmé is Professor in the Faculty of Social Work, University of Toronto, and the Director of Centre of Excellence for Child Welfare (CECW). He is the principal investigator for the Canadian Incidence Studies of Reported Child Abuse and Neglect, a periodical survey of investigated maltreatment conducted in Canada. Additional research activities include pilot testing a national framework for tracking outcomes for children receiving child welfare services, analysis of child welfare service trends in Ontario, examination of rates of maltreatment related injuries, and an analysis of child welfare service responses to corporal punishment. Toine van Teeffelen is Director of Development at the Arab Educational Institute in Bethlehem and member of the board of Palestinian Youth Association for Leadership and Rights Activation (PYALARA). He studied sociology at the University of Rotterdam and social anthropology at the University of Amsterdam, completing his master's thesis with a study of Israeli anthropology.

In the late 1980s and the beginning of the 1990s, he conducted his PhD studies with a discourse analysis of the images of Palestine and Israel in Western popular literature. Articles based on this study appear in various international volumes and journals. He has also authored English and Dutch books about the PalestineIsrael conflict. Fred Wien is Professor in the School of Social Work, Dalhousie University. He served as the school's Director between 1981 and 1986 and on an acting basis more recently. He is also the Director of the Atlantic Aboriginal Health Research Program, established in the fall of 2002 to strengthen the health research capacity of Aboriginal people in Atlantic Canada. He served as Deputy Director of Research with the Royal Commission on Aboriginal Peoples with particular responsibility for managing the commission's research program in the area of employment and economic development. He has published many books and articles arising from research on the subject of the socioeconomic development of disadvantaged communities. Wong Kwai-Yau is a social work supervisor with the Boys' and Girls' Clubs Association of Hong Kong. His work experience is with children and youth, including gangs, developmental and preventive programs, and moral education. In addition, his work also addresses problems of child and family poverty. Recently, he and his colleagues started a pilot project, Project Chance, to eradicate child poverty among children in the association. Projects he has led were granted the Outstanding Program Award for his association and recognized by colleagues in the social work field. He has been actively involved in childhood and adolescent resilience work in Hong Kong.