Educational Psychology: Active Learning Edition [15 ed.] 0138124329, 9780138124328

Educational Psychology: Active Learning Edition uses 44 easy-to-read modules to explain how you can use educational psyc

136 93 42MB

English Pages 700 [889] Year 2024

Report DMCA / Copyright

DOWNLOAD FILE

Polecaj historie

Educational Psychology: Active Learning Edition [15 ed.]
 0138124329, 9780138124328

Table of contents :
Front Cover
Title Page
Copyright Page
Pearson’s Commitment to Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion
Dedication
About the Authors
Preface
Brief Contents
Contents
Special Features
Part I Setting the Stage, Meeting the Players
Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology
Teachers’ Casebook—Becoming a Great Teacher What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
Learning and Teaching Today
Inside Three Classrooms
A Multilingual First Grade
A Suburban Fifth Grade
Teaching Math to Students with Learning Disabilities
Students Today: Dramatic Diversity and Remarkable Technology
Confidence in Every Context
High Expectations for Teachers and Students
Teaching the Whole Child: Social and Emotional Learning
Research on Social and Emotional Learning
Paths: an Approach to Social and Emotional Learning
Living with Social-Emotional Trauma
Do Teachers Make a Difference?
Teacher–Student Relationships
The Cost of Poor Teaching
What Is Good Teaching?
Models of Good Teaching
Danielson’s Framework for Teaching
Teachingworks
The Class Model
Beginning Teachers
Module 1 Summary
The Role of Educational Psychology
Educational Psychology Today
Is It Just Common Sense?
Learning Styles
Answer Based on Research
Skipping Grades
Answer Based on Research
Students in Control
Answer Based on Research
Obvious Answers?
Using Research to Understand and Improve Learning
Correlation Studies
Experimental Studies
Abab Experimental Designs
Scientifically Based Research and Evidence-based Practices
Clinical Interviews and Case Studies
Ethnography
The Role of Time in Research
What’s the Evidence? Qualitative Versus Quantitative Research
Mixed Methods Research
Teachers as Researchers
Point/Counterpoint: What Kind of Research Should Guide Education?
Theories for Teaching
Supporting Student Learning
Module 2 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Becoming a Great Teacher: What Would They Do?
Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity
Teachers’ Casebook—Conversations About Race What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
Education Is Cultural
What Is Culture?
Your Cultural and Educational History
Meet Two Students
Cultural Intersections and Terminology
Intersectionality
Cultural Groups and Terminology
Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Discrimination
Stereotypes: the Good and the Bad
From Stereotypes to Prejudice
Stereotype Threat
From Prejudice to Discrimination
Module 3 Summary
Economic and Social Class Differences
Social Class and Socioeconomic Status
Poverty and Social Inequality
Poverty and Academic Outcomes
Health, Environment, and Stress
Low Expectations—Low Academic Self-Concept
Peer Influences and Resistance Cultures
Home Environment and Resources
Summer Setbacks and School Disruptions
Tracking and Poor Teaching
Extreme Poverty: Homeless and Highly Mobile Students
Guidelines: Teaching Students Who Live in Poverty
Module 4 Summary
Ethnicity and Race in Teaching and Learning
Defining Ethnicity and Race
Ethnic and Racial Identity
Multidimensional and Flexible Ethnic Identities
Racial Identity Development
Ethnic and Racial Differences in School Achievement
The Legacy of Racial Inequality
Racism and Discrimination
Module 5 Summary
Gender and Sexual Orientation in Teaching and Learning
Sex and Gender
Gender Identity
Gender Roles
Gender Bias and Sexism in Curriculum and Media
Gender Bias in Teaching
Guidelines: Avoiding Gender Bias in Teaching
Point/counterpoint: Should Girls and Boys Be Taught Differently?
Sexual Orientation
Discrimination Based on Gender Identity and Sexual Orientation
Module 6 Summary
Creating Culturally Welcoming Classrooms
Culturally Relevant Pedagogy
Diversity in Learning
Social Organization
Cultural Values and Learning Preferences.
Sociolinguistics
Cultural Discontinuity
Lessons for Teachers: Teaching Every Student
Know Yourself
Guidelines: Culturally Relevant Teaching
Know Your Students
Respect Your Students
Teach Your Students
Module 7 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—conversations About Race: What Would They Do?
Part II Development: Similarities and Difference
Cluster 3 Cognitive Development
Teachers’ Casebook—Symbols and Cymbals What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
A Definition of Development
Three Questions Across the Theories
What Is the Source of Development? Nature Versus Nurture
What Is the Shape of Development? Continuity Versus Discontinuity
Timing: Is It Too Late? Critical Versus Sensitive Periods
Beware of Either/Or
General Principles of Development
The Brain and Cognitive Development
The Developing Brain: Neurons
The Developing Brain: Cerebral Cortex
Brain Development in Childhood and Adolescence
Putting It All Together: How the Brain Works
Culture and Brain Plasticity
Point/counterpoint: Brain-Based Education
Neuroscience, Learning, and Teaching
Does Instruction Affect Brain Development?
The Brain and Learning to Read
Emotions, Learning, and the Brain
Lessons for Teachers: General Principles
Module 8 Summary
Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development
Influences on Development
Basic Tendencies in Thinking
Organization
Adaptation
Equilibration
Four Stages of Cognitive Development
Infancy: The Sensorimotor Stage
Early Childhood to the Early Elementary Years: The Preoperational Stage
Later Elementary to the Middle School Years: The Concrete-operational Stage
Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships: Helping Families Care for Preoperational Children
Guidelines: Teaching the Concrete-Operational Child
High School and College: Formal Operations.
Do We All Reach the Fourth Stage?
Guidelines: Helping Students Use Formal Operations
Some Limitations of Piaget’s Theory
The Trouble with Stages
Underestimating Children’s Abilities
Cognitive Development and Culture
Information Processing, Neo-piagetian, and Neuroscience Views of Cognitive Development
Module 9 Summary
Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Perspective
The Social Sources of Individual Thinking
Cultural Tools and Cognitive Development
Technical Tools in a Digital Age
Psychological Tools
The Roles of Language and Private Speech
Private Speech: Vygotsky’s and Piaget’s Views Compared
The Zone of Proximal Development
Private Speech and the Zone
The Role of Learning and Development
Limitations of Vygotsky’s Theory
Implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories for Teachers
Piaget: What Can We Learn?
Understanding and Building on Students’ Thinking
Activity and Constructing Knowledge
Vygotsky: What Can We Learn?
The Role of Adults and Peers
Assisted Learning
An Example Curriculum: Tools of the Mind
Reaching Every Student: Teaching in the “Magic Middle”
Guidelines: Applying Vygotsky’s Ideas in Teaching
Cognitive Development: Lessons for Teachers
Module 10 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Symbols and Cymbals: What Would They Do?
Cluster 4 The Self, Social, and Moral Development
Teachers’ Casebook—Mean Girls: What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
Physical Development
Physical and Motor Development
Young Children
Elementary School Years
Guidelines: Dealing with Physical Differences in the Classroom
The Adolescent Years
Early and Later Maturing
Play, Recess, and Physical Activity
Cultural Differences in Play
Exercise and Recess
Reaching Every Student: Physical Activity and Students with Disabilities
Challenges in Physical Development
Obesity
Body Image and Disordered Eating
Guidelines: Supporting Positive Body Images in Adolescents
Module 11 Summary
Bronfenbrenner: the Social Context for Development
The Importance of Context and the Bioecological Model
Families
Parenting Styles
Culture and Parenting
Attachment
Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships: Connecting with Families
Divorce
Guidelines: Helping Children of Divorce
Peers
Cliques
Crowds
Peer Cultures
Friendships
Popularity
Who Is Likely to Have Problems with Peers?
Aggression
Relational Aggression
Media, Modeling, and Aggression
Video Games and Aggressive Behavior
Reaching Every Student: Teacher Support
Guidelines: Dealing with Aggression and Encouraging Cooperation
Academic and Personal Caring.
Supporting Social and Emotional Learning.
Teachers and Child Abuse
Mandated Reporting.
What Can Teachers Do?
Module 12 Summary
Identity and Self-Concept
Erikson: Stages of Psychosocial Development
The Preschool Years: Trust, Autonomy, and Initiative
The Elementary and Middle School Years: Industry Versus Inferiority
Guidelines: Encouraging Initiative and Industry
Adolescence: the Search for Identity
Identity and Technology
Beyond the School Years
Guidelines: Supporting Identity Formation
Racial and Ethnic Pride
Self-Concept
The Structure of Self-Concept
How Self-Concept Develops
Self-Concept and Achievement
Sex Differences in the Self-Concept of Academic Competence
Self-Esteem
Point/Counterpoint: What Should Schools Do to Encourage Students’ Self-Esteem?
The Dangers of Perfectionism
Module 13 Summary
Understanding Others and Moral Development
Theory of Mind and Intention
Moral Development
Kohlberg’s Theories of Moral Development
Criticisms of Kohlberg’s Theory
Moral Judgments, Social Conventions, and Personal Choices
Moral Versus Conventional Domains
Implications for Teachers
Beyond Reasoning: Haidt’s Social Intuitionist Model of Moral Psychology
Moral Behavior and the Example of Cheating
Who Cheats?
Dealing with Cheating
Personal/Social Development: Lessons for Teachers
Module 14 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Mean Girls: What Would They Do?
Cluster 5 Learner Differences and Learning Needs
Teachers’ Casebook—Including Every Student: What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
Language and Labeling
Impairments, Disabilities, and Handicaps
Person-first/identity-first Language
Possible Biases in the Application of Labels
Intelligence
What Does Intelligence Mean?
Intelligence: One Ability or Many?
Another View: Gardner’s Multiple Intelligences
What Are These Intelligences?
Critics of Multiple Intelligences Theory
Gardner Responds
Multiple Intelligences: Lessons for Teachers
Another View: Sternberg’s Successful Intelligence
Neuroscience and Intelligence
Measuring Intelligence
Binet’s Dilemma
What Does an Iq Score Mean?
Group Versus Individual IQ Tests
The Flynn Effect: Are We Getting Smarter?
Guidelines: Interpreting IQ Scores
Intelligence and Achievement.
Sex Differences in Intelligence and Achievement
Heredity or Environment?
Learning to Be Intelligent: Being Smart About IQ
Emotional Intelligence
What Is Emotional Intelligence?
Module 15 Summary
Creativity: What It Is and Why It Matters
Assessing Creativity
Ok, But So What: Why Does Creativity Matter?
What Are the Sources of Creativity?
Creativity and Cognition
Creativity and Diversity
Creativity in the Classroom
Guidelines: Applying and Encouraging Creativity
Learning Styles: Proceed with Caution
Learning Styles/Preferences
Cautions About Learning Styles
Why Are Learing Styles/Preferences So Popular?
Beyond Either/Or
Module 16 Summary
Individual Differences and the Law
Idea
Least Restrictive Environment
Individualized Education Program
The Rights of Students and Families
Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships: Productive Conferences
Section 504 Protections
Response to Intervention
Module 17 Summary
Students with Learning Challenges
Neuroscience and Learning Challenges
Students with Specific Learning Disabilities
Student Characteristics
Teaching Students with Learning Disabilities
Students with Hyperactivity and Attention Disorders
Definitions
Treating ADHD with Drugs
Point/Counterpoint: Pills or Skills for Children with ADHD?
Alternatives/Additions to Drug Treatments
Lessons for Teachers: Learning Disabilities and ADHD
Students with Communication Impairments
Speech Impairments
Language Disorders
Students with Emotional or Behavioral Disorders
Trauma
Guidelines: Disciplining Students with Emotional Problems
Death by Suicide
Substance Use Disorder
Prevention
The Stigma of Mental Health Issues
Students with Intellectual Disabilities
Guidelines: Teaching Students with Intellectual Disabilities
Students with Health and Sensory Impairments
Cerebral Palsy and Multiple Disabilities
Seizure Disorders (Epilepsy)
Other Serious Health Concerns: Asthma, Sickle Cell Disease, and Diabetes
Students with Visual Impairments
Students Who are Deaf
Autism Spectrum Disorders
Interventions
Module 18 Summary
Students Who Are Gifted and Talented
Who Are These Students?
What Is the Origin of These Gifts
What Problems Do Students Who Are Gifted Face?
Identifying Students Who Are Gifted and Talented
Recognizing Gifts and Talents
Teaching Students with Gifts and Talents
Acceleration
Methods and Strategies
Module 19 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Including Every Student: What Would They Do?
Cluster 6 Language Development, Language Diversity, and Immigrant Education
Teachers’ Casebook—Communicating and Creating Community in the Classroom What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
The Development of Language
What Develops? Language and Cultural Differences
The Puzzle of Language
When and How Does Language Develop?
Sounds and Pronunciation
Vocabulary and Meaning
Grammar and Syntax
Pragmatics: Using Language in Social Situations
Metalinguistic Awareness
Emergent Literacy
Building a Foundation
When Problems Persist
Emergent Literacy and Language Diversity
Languages and Emergent Literacy
Bilingual Emergent Literacy
Guidelines: Supporting Language and Promoting Literacy
Diversity in Language Development
Dual-Language Development
Second-Language Learning
Benefits of Bilingualism
Language Loss
Signed Languages
What Is Involved in Being Bilingual?
Basic Communication and Academic Language
Guidelines: Promoting Language Learning
Dialect Differences in the Classroom
Dialects
Dialects and Pronunciations
Dialects and Teaching
Genderlects
Module 20 Summary
Teaching Immigrant Students and English Learners
Immigrants and Refugees
Classrooms Today
Four Student Profiles
Generation 1.5: Students in Two Worlds
Bilingual Education and English Learners
Two Approaches to Learning English
Point/Counterpoint: What Is the Best Way to Teach English Learners?
Research on Bilingual Education
Visual Strategies
Literature Response Groups
Bilingualism for All: Two-way Immersion
Sheltered Instruction
Affective and Emotional/social Considerations
Dealing with Trauma.
Working with Families: Using the Tools of the Culture
Guidelines: Providing Emotional Support and Increasing Self-Esteem for Students Who Are Els
Funds of Knowledge and Welcome Centers
Student-Led Conferences
Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships: Welcoming All Families
Special Challenges: Students Who Are English Learners with Unique Learning Needs and Gifts
Students Who Are English Learners with Disabilities
Reaching Every Student: Recognizing Giftedness in Bilingual Students
Module 21 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—communicating and Creating Community in the Classroom: What Would They Do?
Part III Learning and Motivation
Cluster 7 Behavioral Views of Learning
Teachers’ Casebook—Sick of Class: What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
Understanding Learning
Ethical Issues
Goals
Strategies
Learning Is Not Always What It Seems
Early Explanations of Learning: Contiguity and Classical Conditioning
Guidelines: Applying Classical Conditioning
Operant Conditioning: Trying New Responses
Types of Consequences
Reinforcement
Punishment
Neuroscience of Reinforcement and Punishment
Reinforcement Schedules
Extinction
Antecedents and Behavior Change
Effective Instruction Delivery
Cueing
Module 22 Summary
Putting It All Together: Applied Behavior Analysis
Methods for Encouraging Behaviors
Reinforcing with Teacher Attention
Selecting Reinforcers: The Premack Principle
Guidelines: Applying Operant Conditioning—Using Praise Appropriately
Shaping
Contingency Contracts, Token Reinforcement, and Group Consequences
Contingency Contracts.
Guidelines: Applying Operant Conditioning—Encouraging Positive Behaviors
Token Reinforcement Systems
Group Consequences
Handling Undesirable Behavior
Negative Reinforcement
Positive Practice Overcorrection
Reprimands
Response Cost
Social Isolation
Some Cautions About Punishment
Guidelines: Applying Operant Conditioning—Using Punishment
Module 23 Summary
Current Applications: Functional Behavioral Assessment, Positive Behavior Supports, and Self-Management
Discovering the “Why”: Functional Behavioral Assessments
Reaching Every Student: Positive Behavior Supports
Self-Management
Goal Setting
Monitoring and Evaluating Progress
Self-Reinforcement
Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships: Applying Operant Conditioning: Student Self-Management
Challenges and Criticisms
Beyond Behaviorism: Bandura’s Challenge and Observational Learning
Enactive and Observational Learning
Learning and Performance
Criticisms of Behavioral Methods
Behavioral Approaches: Lessons for Teachers
Point/Counterpoint: Should Students Be Rewarded for Learning?
Module 24 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Sick of Class: What Would They Do?
Cluster 8 Cognitive Views of Learning
Teachers’ Casebook—Remembering the Basics: What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
Elements of the Cognitive Perspective
The Brain and Cognitive Learning
The Importance of Knowledge in Cognition
General and Specific Knowledge
Declarative, Procedural, and Self-Regulatory Knowledge
Cognitive Views of Memory
Sensory Memory
Capacity, Duration, and Contents of Sensory Memory
Perception
The Role of Attention
Attention and Multitasking
Point/Counterpoint: What’s Wrong with Multitasking?
Attention and Teaching.
Guidelines: Gaining and Maintaining Attention
Working Memory
A Model of Working Memory
The Central Executive
The Phonological Loop
The Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad
The Episodic Buffer
Capacity of Working Memory
The Duration and Contents of Working Memory.
Cognitive Load and Retaining Information in Working Memory
Two (or Three) Kinds of Cognitive Load
Cognitive Load: Lessons for Teachers
Retaining Information in Working Memory
Levels of Processing Theory
Forgetting
Individual Differences in Working Memory
Developmental Differences
Individual Differences
Is Working Memory Really Separate?
Module 25 Summary
Long-Term Memory
Capacity and Duration of Long-Term Memory
Contents of Long-Term Memory: Explicit (Declarative) Memories
Propositions and Propositional Networks
Images
Two Are Better Than One: Words and Images
Concepts
Prototypes, Exemplars, and Theory-Based Categories
Teaching Concepts
Schemas
Episodic Memory
Guidelines: Interviewing Young Students and Eyewitness Testimony
Contents of Long-Term Memory: Implicit Memories
Retrieving Information in Long-Term Memory
Spreading Activation
Reconstruction
Forgetting and Long-Term Memory
Individual Differences in Long-Term Memory
Module 26 Summary
Teaching for Long-Lasting Knowledge: Basic Principles and Applications
Constructing Declarative Knowledge: Making Meaningful Connections
Elaboration
Organization
Imagery
Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships: Organizing Learning
Context
Desirable Difficulty
Effective Practice
Reaching Every Student: Make It Meaningful
Mnemonics
If You Have to Memorize . . .
Lessons for Teachers: Declarative Knowledge
Development of Procedural Knowledge
Automated Basic Skills
Domain-Specific Strategies
Guidelines: Helping Students Understand and Remember
Module 27 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Remembering the Basics: What Would They Do?
Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes
Teachers’ Casebook—Uncritical Thinking: What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
Metacognition
Metacognitive Knowledge and Regulation
Individual Differences in Metacognition
Lessons for Teachers: Developing Metacognition
Metacognitive Development for Younger Students.
Metacognitive Development for Secondary and College Students (Like You)
Learning Strategies
Being Strategic About Learning
Deciding What Is Important
Summaries
Underlining and Highlighting
Taking Notes
Visual Tools for Organizing
Retrieval Practice: Powerful but Underused
Reading Strategies
Applying Learning Strategies
Appropriate Tasks
Valuing Learning
Effort and Efficacy
Reaching Every Student: Teaching How to Learn
Module 28 Summary
Problem Solving
Identifying: Problem Finding
Defining Goals and Representing the Problem
Focusing Attention on What is Relevant
Understanding the Words
Understanding the Whole Problem
Translation and Schema Training: Direct Instruction in Representing Problems
Translation and Schema Training: Worked Examples
Worked Examples and Embodied Cognition
The Results of Problem Representation.
Searching for Possible Solution Strategies
Algorithms
Heuristics
Anticipating, Acting, and Looking Back
Factors That Hinder Problem Solving
Some Problems with Heuristics
Guidelines: Applying Problem Solving
Expert Knowledge and Problem Solving
Knowing What Is Important
Memory for Patterns and Organization
Procedural Knowledge
Planning and Monitoring
Guidelines: Becoming an Expert Student
Module 29 Summary
Critical Thinking and Argumentation
What Critical Thinkers Do: Paul and Elder’s Model
Teaching Critical Thinking
Applying Critical Thinking in Specific Subjects
Point/Counterpoint: Should Schools Teach Critical Thinking and Problem Solving?
Thinking Critically About Online Sources
Argumentation
Two Styles of Argumentation
Lessons for Teachers
Teaching for Transfer
The Many Views of Transfer
Teaching for Positive Transfer
What is Worth Learning?
Lessons for Teachers: Supporting Transfer
Stages of Transfer for Strategies
Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships: Promoting Transfer
Bringing It All Together: Teaching for Complex Learning and Robust Knowledge
What Is Robust Knowledge?
Teaching for Robust Knowledge
Practice
Worked Examples
Analogies
Integrating Multiple Texts
Self-Explanations
Module 30 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Uncritical Thinking: What Would They Do?
Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning
Teachers’ Casebook—Necessity Is the Mother of Inventive Teaching: What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
Cognitive and Social Constructivism
Constructivist Views of Learning
Cognitive Constructivism
Social Constructivism
How Is Knowledge Constructed?
Knowledge: Situated or General?
Common Elements of Learner-Centered Teaching
Complex Learning Environments and Authentic Tasks
Social Negotiation
Multiple Perspectives and Representations of Content
Self-Awareness and Reflection About the Knowledge Construction Process
Student Ownership of Learning
Designing Constructivist Learning Environments
Facilitating Deep Learning in a Constructivist Classroom
Scaffolding
Advance Organizers
Facilitating Through Asking and Answering Deep Questions
Guidelines: Facilitating Deep Questioning
Inquiry-Based Learning
Examples of Inquiry
Problem-Based Learning
Does Inquiry-Based Instruction Improve Learning?
Point/Counterpoint: Are Teaching Approaches to Support Inquiry- and Problem-Based Learning Effective?
Being Smart About Inquiry Learning
Cognitive Apprenticeships
Module 31 Summary
Collaboration and Cooperation
Learning in Groups
Goals of Group Work
What Can Go Wrong: Misuses of Group Learning
Learning Through Cooperation
Effects of Cooperative Learning
Teacher’s Role in Cooperative Learning
Designing Cooperative Learning Tasks
Highly Structured, Review, and Skill-Building Tasks
ILL-structured, Conceptual, and Problem-Solving Tasks
Social Skills and Communication Tasks
Giving and Receiving Explanations
Setting Up Cooperative Groups
Group Composition
Assigning Roles
Examples of Cooperative Learning Techniques
Reciprocal Questioning
Jigsaw
Constructive/Structured Controversies
Reaching Every Student: Using Cooperative Learning Wisely
Guidelines: Using Cooperative Learning
Module 32 Summary
Designing Interactive Digital Learning Environments
Technology and Learning
Technology-Rich Environments
Mobile Learning
Virtual Learning Environments
Immersive Virtual Learning Environments
Games
Computational Thinking and Coding
Media Use, Digital Citizenship, and Media Literacy
Developmentally Appropriate Activities
Digital Citizenship
Critical Media Literacy
Guidelines: Supporting the Development of Media Literacy
Blending and Flipping: Technology-Powered Pedagogy
Dilemmas of Constructivist Practice
Module 33 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Necessity Is the Mother of Inventive Teaching: What Would They Do?
Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation
Teachers’ Casebook—Failure to Self-Regulate: What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
Social Cognitive Theory
A Self-Directed Life: Albert Bandura
Beyond Behaviorism
Triadic Reciprocal Causality
Modeling: Learning by Observing Others
Elements of Observational Learning
Attention
Retention
Production
Motivation and Reinforcement
Observational Learning in Teaching
Directing Attention
Fine-Tuning Already-Learned Behaviors
Strengthening or Weakening Inhibitions
Teaching New Behaviors
Arousing Emotion
Guidelines: Using Observational Learning
Agency and Self-Efficacy
Self-Efficacy, Self-Concept, and Self-Esteem
Sources of Self-Efficacy
Self-Efficacy in Learning and Teaching
Guidelines: Supporting Self-Efficacy
Teachers’ Sense of Efficacy
Module 34 Summary
Self-Regulated Learning: Skill and Will
How Does Self-Regulation Develop?
Knowledge
Motivation
Volition
Point/Counterpoint: Are “Grittier” Students More Successful?
Developmental Changes in Self-Regulation
A Social Cognitive Model of Self-Regulated Learning
Reaching Every Student: Technology and Self-Regulation
Self-Regulation of Emotions
Social and Emotional Learning
Teacher Stress and Self-Regulation
Guidelines: Encouraging Emotional Self-Regulation
Teaching Toward Self-Efficacy and Self-Regulated Learning
Complex Tasks
Agency and Control
Self-Management
Self-Evaluation
Collaboration
Bringing It All Together: Theories of Learning
Module 35 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Failure to Self-Regulate: What Would They Do?
Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching
Teachers’ Casebook—Motivating Students When Resources Are Thin: What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
What Is Motivation?
Meeting Some Students
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation
What You Already Know About Motivation
Needs and Self-Determination
Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs
Self-Determination: Need for Competence, Autonomy, and Relatedness
Self-Determination in the Classroom
Need-Supportive Versus Need-Thwarting Classrooms
Guidelines: Supporting Self-Determination and Autonomy
Supportive Relationships as Motivators
Needs: Lessons for Teachers
Module 36 Summary
Goals and Goal Orientations
Goal Setting: What Am I Trying to Do?
Making Goals Work: Feedback, Framing, and Commitment
Goal Orientations: Why Am I Trying to Do This?
Wait—Are Performance Goals Always Bad?
Work-Avoidance Goals
Contextual Influences on Goal Orientation
Social Goals
Goals: Lessons for Teachers
Expectancies, Values, and Costs
Expectancy for Success
Task Value
Perceived Cost
Expectancy, Value, and Cost: Lessons for Teachers
Attributions and Beliefs About Ability
Attributions in the Classroom
Teacher Attributions Trigger Student Attributions
Mindsets About Ability
Ability Mindsets in the Classroom
Mindsets About Failure
Attributions and Mindsets: Lessons for Teachers
Guidelines: Encouraging a Healthy Mindset
Module 37 Summary
How Do You Feel About Learning? Interest, Curiosity, and Emotions
Tapping Interests
Two Kinds of Interests
Catching and Holding Interests
Curiosity: Novelty and Complexity
Point/Counterpoint: Does Making Learning Fun Make for Good Learning?
Guidelines: Building on Students’ Interests and Curiosity
Flow
Emotions
Emotion and the Body
Achievement Emotions
Emotions in the Classroom
Anxiety in the Classroom
Reaching Every Student: Coping with Negative Emotions
Guidelines: Coping with Test Anxiety
Interest, Curiosity, and Emotions: Lessons for Teachers
Module 38 Summary
Strategies to Encourage Motivation to Learn
Why Am I Learning This? Is It Valuable, Useful, Relevant?
Creating Authentic Tasks
Increasing Perceived Relevance and Utility
Will I Enjoy This?
Enhancing Curiosity, Interest, and Intrinsic Value
Supporting Autonomy
Can I Do It? Building Confidence and Positive Expectations
What Do I Need to Do to Succeed? Providing Structures That Support Progress
Do I Belong in This Classroom? Creating Inclusive Opportunities
Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships: Supporting the Sociocultural Foundations of Motivation
Module 39 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Motivating Students When Resources Are Thin: What Would They Do?
Part IV Teaching and Assessing
Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments
Teachers’ Casebook—Bullies and Victims What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
The What and Why of Supportive Classroom Organization
The Basic Task: Gain Their Cooperation
The Basic Goals: Access, Time, Relationships, and Self-Management
Access to Learning
More Time for Learning
Management Means Relationships
Management for Self-Management
Creating a Positive Learning Environment
Relevant Research Results
Routines and Rules Required: In-Person Learning
Routines and Procedures
Guidelines: Establishing Class Routines for In-Person Learning
Rules
Rules for In-Person Teaching in Elementary School
Rules for In-Person Teaching in Secondary School
Consequences
Who Sets the Rules and Consequences?
Routines and Rules Required: Remote Learning
Guidelines: Establishing Routines and Rules for Remote Learning
Planning Spaces for Learning
Personal Territories and Seating Arrangements.
Interest Areas
Guidelines: Designing Learning Spaces
Getting Started: The First Weeks of Class
Effective Managers for Elementary Students
Effective Managers for Secondary Students
Module 40 Summary
Maintaining a Good Environment for Learning
Encouraging Engagement
Guidelines: Keeping Students Engaged
Prevention Is the Best Medicine
Withitness
Overlapping and Group Focus
Movement Management
Student Social Skills as Prevention
Caring Relationships: Connections with School
Teacher Connections
School Connections and Belonging
Creating Communities of Care for Adolescents
Guidelines: Creating Caring Relationships
Module 41 Summary
Dealing with Discipline Problems
Stopping Problems Quickly
If You Impose Penalties
Guidelines: Imposing Penalties
What About School Suspensions and Zero Tolerance?
Point/Counterpoint: Is Zero Tolerance a Good Idea?
Reaching Every Student: Restorative Justice
Teacher-Imposed Penalties Versus Student Responsibility
Bullying and Cyberbullying
Victims
Why Do Students Bully?
What Can Teachers Do? Bullying and Teasing
Cyberbullying
Special Challenges with High School Students
Guidelines: Handling Potentially Explosive Situations
The Need for Communication
Message Sent—Message Received
Empathetic Listening
When Listening Is Not Enough: I-Messages, Assertive Discipline, and Problem Solving
I-Messages
Assertive Discipline
Confrontations and Negotiations
Diversity: Culturally Responsive Management
Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships: Classroom Organizationand Management
Module 42 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Bullies and Victims: What Would They Do?
Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student
Teachers’ Casebook—Reaching and Teaching Every Student: What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
Research on Teaching
Characteristics of Effective Teachers
Clarity and Organization
Enthusiasm and Warmth
Knowledge for Teaching
Teacher Expectations
Two Kinds of Expectation Effects
Sources of Expectations
Do Teachers’ Expectations Really Affect Students’ Achievement?
Lessons for Teachers: Communicating Appropriate Expectations
Guidelines: Avoiding the Negative Effects of Teacher Expectations
The Goal: Ambitious Teaching
The First Step: Planning
Research on Planning
Learning Goals
An Example of State-Level Goals: The Common Core
Classrooms Goals for Learning
Flexible and Creative Plans—Using Taxonomies
The Cognitive Domain
The Affective Domain
The Psychomotor Domain
The Cognitive Demands of Learning Objectives: Webb’s Depth of Knowledge
Planning from a Constructivist Perspective
Guidelines: Using Learning Objectives
Module 43 Summary
Teaching Approaches
Research on Teaching Strategies
Explicit Teaching and Direct Instruction
Rosenshine’s Six Teaching Functions
Why Does Direct Instruction Work?
Evaluating Direct Instruction
Guidelines: Effective Direct Instruction
Independent Work and Homework
Independent Work
Homework
Point/Counterpoint: Is Homework Valuable?
Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships: Homework
Questioning, Discussion, Dialogue, and Feedback
Kinds of Questions
Asking Authentic Questions
Fitting the Questions to the Students—And Waiting
Responding to Student Answers
Group Discussion and Quality Talk
Lessons for Teachers: Fitting Teaching to Your Goals
Guidelines: Productive Group Discussions
Putting It All Together: Understanding by Design
Differentiated Instruction
Within-Class and Flexible Grouping
The Problems with Ability Grouping
Flexible Grouping
Guidelines: Using Flexible Grouping
Differentiated Instruction in Inclusive Classrooms
Assistive Technology
Reaching Every Student: Differentiation with Universal Design for Learning
Module 44 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Reaching and Teaching Every Student: What Would They Do?
Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing
Teachers’ Casebook—Giving Meaningful Grades What Would You Do?
Overview and Objectives
Basics of Assessment
Measurement and Assessment
Formative, Interim, and Summative Assessment
Assessing the Assessments: Reliability and Validity
Reliability of Test Scores
Validity
Absence of Bias
Classroom Assessment: Testing
Interpreting Any Test Score
Norm-Referenced Test Interpretations
Criterion-Referenced Test Interpretations
Using the Tests from Textbooks
Selected-Response Testing
Using Multiple-Choice Tests
Writing Multiple-Choice Questions
Constructed Responses: Essay Testing
Guidelines: Writing Multiple-Choice Test Items
Constructing Essay Tests
Evaluating Essays
Assessing Traditional Testing
Module 45 Summary
Formative and Authentic Classroom Assessments
Informal Assessments
Exit Tickets
Journals
Involving Students in Assessments
Authentic Assessments: Performances and Portfolios
Performances
Portfolios
Evaluating Portfolios and Performances
Scoring Rubrics
Guidelines: Creating Portfolios
Reliability and Validity
Guidelines: Developing a Rubric
Diversity and Bias in Performance Assessment
Assessing Complex Thinking
Classroom Assessment: Lessons for Teachers
Grading
Norm-Referenced Versus Criterion-Referenced Grading
Effects of Grading on Students
The Value of Failing?
Retention in Grade
Point/Counterpoint: Should Children Be Held Back?
Grades and Motivation
Beyond Grading: Communicating with Families
Guidelines: Using Any Grading System
Module 46 Summary
Standardized Testing
Types of Scores
Measurements of Central Tendency and Standard Deviation
The Normal Distribution
Percentile Rank Scores
Grade-Equivalent Scores
Standard Scores
Interpreting Standardized Test Reports
Discussing Test Results with Families
Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships: Conferences and Explaining Test Results
Accountability and High-Stakes Testing
Making Decisions
What Do Teachers Think?
Documented Problems with High-Stakes Testing
New Directions: PARCC and SBAC
Using High-Stakes Testing Well: Lessons for Teachers
Reaching Every Student: Helping Students with Disabilities Prepare for High-Stakes Tests
Guidelines: Preparing Yourself and Your Students for Testing
Teacher Accountability and Evaluation
Value-Added Models
So How Should We Evaluate Teachers?
A Broader Approach: Measures of Teacher Effectiveness
Module 47 Summary
Key Terms
Connect and Extend to Licensure
Teachers’ Casebook—Giving Meaningful Grades: What Would They Do?
Licensure Appendix
Glossary
References
Name Index
Subject Index

Citation preview

Educational Psychology Active Learning Edition Fifteenth Edition

Anita Woolfolk The Ohio State University, Emerita

Ellen L. Usher Mayo Clinic College of Medicine and Science

Content Development: Alicia Santamaria Reilly Content Management: Brooke Nethers Content Production: Janelle Rogers Product Management: Drew Bennett Product Marketing: Krista Clark Rights and Permissions: Jenell Forschler Cover Designer: Carie Keller Please contact https://support.pearson.com/getsupport/s/ with any queries on this content Cover Image by Robert M. Pratt Copyright © 2024, 2023, 2019 by Pearson Education, Inc. or its affiliates, 221 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030. All Rights Reserved. Manufactured in the United States of America. This publication is protected by copyright, and permission should be obtained from the publisher prior to any prohibited reproduction, storage in a retrieval system, or transmission in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise. For information regarding permissions, request forms, and the appropriate contacts within the Pearson Education Global Rights and Permissions department, please visit www.pearsoned.com/permissions/. Acknowledgments of third-party content appear on the appropriate page within the text. PEARSON is exclusive trademark owned by Pearson Education, Inc. or its affiliates in the U.S. and/or other countries. Unless otherwise indicated herein, any third-party trademarks, logos, or icons that may appear in this work are the property of their respective owners, and any references to thirdparty trademarks, logos, icons, or other trade dress are for demonstrative or descriptive purposes only. Such references are not intended to imply any sponsorship, endorsement, authorization, or promotion of Pearson’s products by the owners of such marks, or any relationship between the owner and Pearson Education, Inc., or its affiliates, authors, licensees, or distributors. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Educational psychology Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Control Number: 2023000005

ScoutAutomatedPrintCode

ISBN 10: ISBN 13:

0-13-812432-9 978-0-13-812432-8

Pearson’s Commitment to Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion Pearson is dedicated to creating bias-free content that reflects the diversity, depth, and breadth of all learners’ lived experiences. We embrace the many dimensions of diversity, including but not limited to race, ethnicity, gender, sex, sexual orientation, socioeconomic status, ability, age, and religious or political beliefs. Education is a powerful force for equity and change in our world. It has the potential to deliver opportunities that improve lives and enable economic mobility. As we work with authors to create content for every product and service, we acknowledge our responsibility to demonstrate inclusivity and incorporate diverse scholarship so that everyone can achieve their potential through learning. As the world’s leading learning company, we have a duty to help drive change and live up to our purpose to help more people create a better life for themselves and to create a better world. Our ambition is to purposefully contribute to a world where: • Everyone has an equitable and lifelong opportunity to succeed through learning.

• Our educational products and services are inclusive and represent the rich diversity of learners.

• Our educational content accurately reflects the histories and lived experiences of the learners we serve.

• Our educational content prompts deeper discussions with students and motivates them to expand their own learning (and worldview).

Accessibility

Contact Us

We are also committed to providing products that are fully accessible to all learners. As per Pearson’s guidelines for accessible educational Web media, we test and retest the capabilities of our products against the highest standards for every release, following the WCAG guidelines in developing new products for copyright year 2022 and beyond.

While we work hard to present unbiased, fully accessible content, we want to hear from you about any concerns or needs with this Pearson product so that we can investigate and address them.

You can learn more about Pearson’s commitment to accessibility at https://www.pearson.com/us/accessibility.html

Please contact us with concerns about any potential bias at https://www.pearson.com/report-bias.html For accessibility-related issues, such as using assistive technology with Pearson products, alternative text requests, or accessibility documentation, email the Pearson Disability Support team at [email protected]

Anita Woolfolk Hoy To my brothers, Eric and Robert Pratt—two of the smartest, kindest, most talented people I have ever known. I am grateful for their presence in my life. They are my parent’s best gift to me. Eric is a dedicated engineer, responsible for designing important parts on several airplanes. He perfected each design with a sense of personal responsibility for the well-being of everyone who would fly in those planes. Robert is the artist who created the amazing sculpture on the cover of this book. As a friend said when she saw it, “I am struck by the beauty of the hands uplifting the child. I envision the hands of parents, grandparents, family, and teachers. My son was in kindergarten this year and had the most wonderful teacher. When I look at this sculpture, I see her hands with ours, lifting his up.” Ellen L. Usher With gratitude to members of the P20 Motivation & Learning Lab. Our decade of thinking, laughing, and learning together enriched my understanding of what it means to teach and learn. You made my intellectual life and scholarship infinitely better. From Both of Us: In memory of Albert Bandura 1925–2021 He forever changed the fields of social and clinical psychology, learning, motivation, personality, and moral reasoning. As undergraduate and graduate students, we read his books and studies. As professors, we taught his theories. As researchers, we grounded our work on motivation, self-efficacy, and self-regulation in his research and scholarship. We have never known an educational psychology that did not include his thinking, nor will we ever. A lifetime of thanks to you, Professor Bandura, our professional inspiration and guide.

About the Authors So you will know your authors a bit better, here is some information. Anita Woolfolk Hoy was born in Fort Worth, Texas, where her mother taught child development at TCU and her father was an early worker in the computer industry. She is a Texas Longhorn—all her degrees are from the University of Texas, Austin, the last one a PhD. After graduating, she was a psychologist working with students in elementary and secondary schools in 15 counties of central Texas. She began her career in higher education as a professor of educational psychology at Rutgers University and then moved to The Ohio State University in 1994. Today she is Professor Emerita at Ohio State. Anita’s research focuses on motivation and cognition, specifically, students’ and teachers’ sense of efficacy and teachers’ beliefs about education. For many years she was the editor of Theory Into Practice, a journal that brings the best ideas from research to practicing educators. She is a Fellow of both the American Psychological Association and the American Educational Research Association and has served as President of Division 15 (Educational Psychology) of APA and Vice President for Division K (Teaching & Teacher Education) of AERA. Anita also has collaborated with Nancy Perry, University of British Columbia, to write the second edition of Child Development (Pearson, 2015) and with her husband, Wayne Hoy, to complete the fifth edition of Instructional Leadership: A Research-Based Guide to Learning in Schools (Pearson, 2020). Ellen L. Usher spent her early childhood in Roswell, Georgia, where she had diverse educational experiences that included attending a private nature-based school and public elementary and middle schools. She went to high school in rural South Carolina and urban Atlanta. A lifelong Francophile, she earned her bachelor’s in foreign language education and began her professional career teaching French to elementary school students in Atlanta Public Schools. Inspired by her own favorite former teachers, Ellen transitioned to teaching fifth and sixth grades while pursuing a master’s degree in middle grades education from Oglethorpe University. After earning her PhD in Educational Studies from Emory University, Ellen began her career in higher education at the University of Kentucky in 2007, where she was director of the P20 Motivation and Learning Lab—an intergenerational, interdisciplinary team of researchers engaged in projects that explore human motivation in a variety of teaching and learning contexts. In 2022, she joined the Mayo Clinic College of Medicine and Science where she is an education scientist and professor. Ellen is a Fellow of the American Psychological Association and past Chair of the Motivation in Education Special Interest Group of the American Educational Research Association.

v

Preface Many of you reading this book are enrolled in an educational psychology course as part of your professional preparation for teaching, counseling, speech therapy, nursing, or psychology. The material in this text should be of interest to everyone who is concerned about education and learning, from the nursery school volunteer to the instructor in a community program for adults learning English. No background in psychology or education is necessary to understand this material. It is as free of jargon and technical language as possible, and many people have worked to make this edition clear, relevant, and interesting. The text maintains the new, unique format that was created for the previous Active Learning Edition. If you didn’t see that text, this one is probably unlike any textbook you have ever encountered. It is divided into 47 easy-to-read modules. Research in educational psychology points to several reasons why this format will help you learn. First, we have known for years that learning is faster and more permanent if you study smaller chunks over a longer period instead of trying to jam more learning into your brain in a short period of time. Also, we know that learning is deeper and more meaningful if you act on what you read—connect and apply your understanding before you get too far away from the new information. Third, we know that motivation is higher when goals are specific, the task is moderately challenging, and it can be accomplished with reasonable time and effort. Fourth, more frequent testing and checking your understanding early let you correct misconceptions and relearn, so you do not remember the wrong information. Finally, we know that your lives are full, and your assignments are many, so shorter, more manageable readings fit better into your schedule. Mastering these active learning modules will help you become more self-regulating and in charge of your own academic life. So welcome to a better, more research-based way to learn educational psychology—our favorite subject! The 15th edition of Educational Psychology continues to emphasize the educational implications and applications of research on child development, cognitive science, learning, motivation, teaching, and assessment in diverse contexts. Theory and practice are not separated in the text but are considered together to solve the everyday problems of teaching. To help you explore the connections between research and practice, these pages include a wealth of examples, lesson segments, case studies, guidelines, and even practical tips from experienced teachers. Our goal is to provide knowledge and skills so you can build a solid foundation for a sense of teaching efficacy in every context and for every student. As you read this book, we believe you will see the immense value and usefulness of educational psychology. The field offers unique and crucial knowledge to any who dare to teach and to all who love to learn. Since the last edition appeared, there have been exciting developments in the field, and they are reflected in the upcoming clusters. We have also addressed the ways in which the COVID-19 pandemic has changed educational practices and outcomes for many teachers and learners around the world.

New Content in the 15th Edition Across the book, there is increased coverage of a number of important topics. Some of these include: • A bold approach to face head-on the issues of identity, race, and privilege in teaching. After you explore the field of educational psychology in Cluster 1, in the next cluster, we ask you to examine yourself and your students. Who are you? Who are they? We consider diversity in today’s classrooms. Portraits of students in

vi

Preface

• • • •

educational settings make diversity real and human. In a number of other clusters, there are new exercises asking readers to “Put Yourself in Their Place” as a way to develop empathy for many students and situations. Increased coverage of the brain, neuroscience, and teaching, emphasized in Cluster 3 and also integrated into several other clusters. Increased coverage of the impact of technology and interactive learning environments on the lives of students and teachers today. New sections in several clusters on social and emotional learning (SEL) and trauma-informed teaching. New “What Would You Do?” cases and new responses from teachers around the United States and Canada, including a case that asks expert teachers what they learned from the shifts to remote instruction during COVID-19 and what they will keep doing as schools get back to “normal.”

Key content changes in each cluster include: • Cluster  1: To help build your foundation for a sense of teaching efficacy, we have added a new casebook about becoming a great teacher in a world filled with online and in-person “advice.” There are responses from expert teachers and even an expert student teacher. In addition, we include new information on social and emotional learning (SEL) and the effects of trauma on students—two topics that will be addressed in several other clusters. Also, the section on good teaching now includes the widely used CLASS model. • Cluster  2: This cluster begins with a new case that could happen in any school, “Conversations About Race.” We explore the ways that education is cultural and then ask you to examine your own educational and cultural history—Who are you? What brought you here? To help you think through these important questions, there is new coverage of intersectionality, stereotypes, prejudice, and poverty as well as expanded coverage of racial and gender identity, sexual orientation, and creating inclusive classrooms. • Cluster 3: New information on the brain, synaptic plasticity, and implications for teaching. Also, there is greater critical analysis of Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s theories. • Cluster  4: Updated information on early and late maturation in puberty, play, childhood obesity, disordered eating, culture and parenting, aggression, social and emotional learning, child abuse and mandated reporting, identity and technology, and dealing with cheating. • Cluster 5: New sections on terms and labeling, multiple intelligences, emotional intelligence, problems with learning styles, ADHD, trauma-informed teaching, seizure disorders and other serious health concerns, and autism spectrum disorders. • Cluster  6: New information on language diversity, Native American languages, and an expanded section on affective and emotional/social considerations for immigrant student and language learners, including dealing with trauma. • Cluster  7: Expanded coverage of the neuroscience of reinforcement and punishment, effective instruction delivery, cautions in using time out for parents and teachers, reasons for problem behaviors, and using positive behavior supports for tiers 1, 2, and 3 prevention of problems. • Cluster  8: Updated coverage of the brain and cognitive learning, multitasking, working memory and cognitive load, concept teaching, effective practice, and teaching implications of cognitive learning theories. In addition, there is a new section on children and eye-witness memory, including guidelines for interviewing students. • Cluster  9: New sections on how teachers can support student metacognition, using visual strategies and drawing to learn, worked examples and embodied cognition, deliberate practice, thinking critically about online sources, and integrating

vii

viii Preface













multiple sources of information. Updated coverage of problem solving and argumentation. Cluster  10: A new casebook asking about new approaches to teaching and learning that emerged during COVID-19 to facilitate meaningful learning through remote and hybrid instruction. Updated discussion of the elements of constructivist learning and teaching, scaffolding, deep questioning, collaboration, learning in a digital world, immersive learning environments, blending and flipping classrooms, and computational thinking. New sections on constructive/structured controversy, mobile learning, and media citizenship. Cluster 11: Updated coverage of social cognitive theory, self-efficacy and agency, teacher self-efficacy, self-regulated learning, grit, and teacher stress. New sections on self-regulation of emotions as relevant for both teachers and learners. Cluster  12: Cluster is reorganized around motivational components (e.g., needs, values, goals, beliefs) and the antecedents and outcomes associated with them. Updated treatment of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, self-determination, goals, school belonging, mindsets, and curiosity. New sections on social goals and achievement emotions. Completely revised section on strategies to encourage motivation to learn. Cluster  13: Cutting across this cluster is the challenge of creating supportive in-person and remote learning environments. New sections on rules and routines for managing remote learning including a “Welcome to Remote Learning” letter to students from an expert teacher (Anita’s daughter). Updated and expanded material on positive teacher connections, zero tolerance, bullying and cyberbullying, restorative justice, and culturally responsive classroom management. Cluster  14: Updated research on teacher expectations, differentiated teaching, and homework, as well as new sections on ambitious teaching, Webb’s Depth of Knowledge, asking essential and authentic questions, and giving feedback. There also are new sections on Quality Talk and Universal Design for Learning. Cluster  15: New sections on feedback and teacher evaluation. Updated material on selected-response testing, scoring rubrics, grading, retention in grade, and value-added modeling.

A Crystal Clear Picture of the Field and Where It Is Headed The 15th edition maintains our renowned lucid writing style presenting accurate, up-to-date coverage of the foundational areas within educational psychology: learning, development, motivation, teaching, and assessment. We add intelligent examinations of emerging trends in the field and society that affect student learning, such as student diversity, inclusive teaching, social and emotional learning, education and neuroscience, and technology.

Learning Management System (LMS)-Compatible Assessment Bank, and Other Instructor Resources

Preface

With this new edition, all assessment types— quizzes, application exercises, and licensure exam practice—are included in LMS-compatible banks for the following learning management systems: Blackboard, Canvas, D2L, and Moodle. These packaged files allow maximum flexibility to instructors when it comes to importing, assigning, and grading. Assessment types include: LMS-COMPATIBLE ASSESSMENT BANK

• Learning Outcome Quizzes: Each cluster learning outcome is the focus of a Learning Outcome Quiz that is available for instructors to assign through their learning management system. Learning outcomes identify cluster content that is most important for learners and serve as the organizational framework for each cluster. The higher-order, multiple-choice questions in each quiz will measure your understanding of cluster content, guide the expectations for your learning, and inform the accountability and the applications of your new knowledge. Each multiple-choice question includes feedback for the correct answer and for each distractor to help guide students’ learning. • Application Exercises: Each cluster provides opportunities for students to apply what they have learned through Application Exercises. One Application Exercise is available for each Learning Outcome within the cluster. The exercises require students to watch short videos, read scenarios, or think about situations and then answer open-ended questions. When used in the LMS environment, a model response written by experts is provided after students submit the exercise. This feedback helps guide students’ learning and can assist the instructor in grading. • Cluster Tests: Suggested test items are provided for each cluster and include questions in multiple-choice and short-answer/essay formats. Some items (lower-level questions) simply ask students to identify or explain concepts and principles they have learned. But many others (higher-level questions) ask students to apply those same concepts and principles to specific classroom situations—that is, to actual student behaviors and teaching strategies. The lower-level questions assess basic knowledge of educational psychology. But ultimately, it is the higher-level questions that can best assess students’ ability to use principles of educational psychology in their own teaching practice.

ix

x Preface

Additional Text Features With an unswerving emphasis on educational psychology’s practical relevance for teachers and students in classrooms, the text is filled with current issues and debates, examples, lesson segments, case studies, and practical ideas from experienced teachers. Point/Counterpoint sections in each cluster present two perspectives on a controversial question related to the field; topics include debates on the kinds of research that should guide education (p. 25), should girls and boys be taught differently? (pp. 70–71), brainbased education (pp. 98–99), the self-esteem movement (pp. 168–169), pills or skills for students with ADHD (pp. 220–221), the best way to teach English learners (pp. 270–271), using rewards to encourage student learning (pp. 322–323), what’s wrong with multitasking? (pp. 337–338), teaching critical thinking and problem solving (p. 405), problem-based education (pp. 434–435), are “grittier” students more successful? (pp. 488–489), the value of trying to make learning entertaining (p. 537), zero tolerance (pp. 585–586), the value of homework (pp. 629–630), and holding children back (pp. 676–677). Guidelines appear throughout each cluster, providing concrete applications of theories or principles discussed. See, for example, pages 55, 69, 80, 110, 113, 126, 135, 141, 147, 152, 164, 196, 205, 225, 230, 255, 262, 278, 295, 304, 307, 313, 339, 355, 366, 398, 400, 430, 449, 459, 476, 481, 495, 517, 533, 538, 544, 564, 567, 571, 575, 580, 584, 593, 614, 621, 626, 636, 640, 661, 671, 678, and 691. Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships sections offer specific guidelines for involving all families in their children’s learning—especially relevant now, when demand for parental involvement is at an all-time high and the need for cooperation between home and school is critical. See, for example, pages 108, 146, 211, 280, 319, 360, 411, 551, 600, 630, and 687. Teachers’ Casebook sections present students with realistic classroom scenarios at the beginning of each cluster and ask “What Would You Do?”—giving students the opportunity to apply all the important topics of the Cluster to these scenarios via application questions. Students may then compare their responses to those of veteran teachers appearing at the end of each cluster. See, for example, pages 83, 181, 285, 463, 504, 553, and 603. Reaching Every Student sections present ideas for assessing, teaching, and motivating ALL of the students in today’s inclusive classrooms. See, for example, page 152. Lessons for Teachers are succinct and usable principles for teaching based on the research. See, for example, page 101. Put Yourself in Their Place experiences develop empathy by asking students to imagine how they would feel in different situations. See pages 62, 103, 255, 274, and 594. Stop and Think activities give students firsthand experience with the concept being discussed, as on pages 15, 39, 42, 162, 168, 308, 390, 451, 522, and 559.

Supplementary Materials Many supplements to the textbook are available to enhance readers’ learning and development as teachers. INSTRUCTOR’S MANUAL The Instructor’s Manual is provided as a Word document and includes resources to assist professors in planning their course. These resources consist of suggestions for learning activities, supplementary lectures, group activities, and additional media resources. These have been carefully selected to provide opportunities to support, enrich, and expand on what students read in the textbook. ®

POWERPOINT SLIDES PowerPoint®slides are provided for each cluster and highlight key con-

cepts and summarize the content of the text to make it more meaningful for students. Often these slides also include questions and problems designed to stimulate discussion and to encourage students to elaborate and deepen their understanding of cluster topics. Note: All instructor resources—LMS-compatible assessment bank, instructor’s manual, and PowerPoint slides—are available for download at http://www.pearsonhighered.com.

Preface

Acknowledgments From the initial draft of this book to this most recent revision, many people have supported the project. Without their help, this text simply could not have been written. Many educators contributed to this project. Nancy Perry, our colleague from the University of British Columbia, contributed to the early planning for this edition and the redesign of several Teachers’ Casebook questions; recruited and collaborated with Canadian teachers for the Casebooks; worked with her student Silvia Mazabel to research Clusters 3, 4, and 5; and drafted the first several sections of Cluster 4. Nancy’s keen eye and deep knowledge of the field made this edition more current and inclusive. In addition to working with Nancy, Silvia expertly completed research for multiple clusters, kept a sharp eye on the final versions of those clusters, and ensured that the references in the clusters and the final bibliography were accurate. In previous editions, before she became a coauthor, Ellen L. Usher contributed her remarkable scholarship and delightful writing to revise Clusters 2 and 11. Carol Weinstein wrote the section in Cluster 13 on spaces for learning. Michael Yough (Oklahoma State University) reviewed several clusters, including Cluster 6, “Language Development, Language Diversity, and Immigrant Education.” Cluster 6 was also improved by suggestions from Alan Hirvela, The Ohio State University. Jerrell Cassady, Ball State University, provided invaluable guidance for Cluster 12, “Motivation in Learning and Teaching.” The portraits of students in Clusters 1, 2, and 6 were crafted by Nancy Knapp (University of Georgia). As we made decisions about how to revise this edition, we benefited from the ideas of colleagues around the country who took the time to complete surveys, answer our questions, and review clusters. Special thanks to Krystal Lira, Kimberly Alberts, and Alexandra Lee (Michigan State University) and Tony Perez and Arianna White-Levatich (Old Dominion University) for their input on content, structure, and flow for the 15th edition. We thank Jennifer Burris, Anastacia Cole, Candice Hargons, Sara Kuhl, and Jaylene Patterson (University of Kentucky) and Xiao-Yin Chen (University of Georgia) for their insightful input on specific sections related to privilege and diversity. And once again, we are grateful for the astute comments and wise guidance on those topics from H. Richard Milner, a groundbreaking scholar and friend. For their revision reviews, thanks to Alyssa Gonzalez-DeHass, Florida Atlantic University; Carla M. Firetto, Arizona State University; Judy K. Hughey, Kansas State University; Kurt Kowalski, California State University San Bernardino; and Marie K. Iding, University of Hawaii. Many classroom teachers across the country and around the world contributed their experience, creativity, and expertise to the Teachers’ Casebook. We have thoroughly enjoyed our association with these master teachers, and we are grateful for the perspective they brought to the book: AIMEE FREDETTE • 2nd-Grade Teacher, Fisher Elementary School, Walpole, MA ALLAN OSBORNE • Assistant Principal, Snug Harbor Community School, Quincy, MA AMANDA ROMANO • 6th-Grade Teacher, Galileo School for Gifted Learning, Sanford, FL AMY POCHODYLO • High School Science Teacher, Buckeye Valley High School, Delaware, OH ANNE LINERS • 9th–12th-Grade Student Teacher, South High School, Fort Minneapolis, MN ASHLEY RICHARDSON • 6th–8th-Grade Dean of Instruction and Discipline, Lansing Charter Academy, Lansing, MI BARBARA PRESLEY • Transition/Work Study Coordinator—High School Level, BESTT Program (Baldwinsville Exceptional Student Training and Transition Program) C. W. Baker High School, Baldwinsville, NY

xi

xii Preface CARLA S. HIGGINS • K–5 Literacy Coordinator, Legend Elementary School, Newark, OH CHRISTEN DILLON • K-12th-Grade Music Teacher, Model Laboratory School at Eastern Kentucky University, Richmond, KY CORALIE HAFNER • Primary/1st-Grade Teacher, Olde Orchard Alternative Elementary, Columbus, OH CORY HILTY • 5th-Grade Teacher, Tyler Run Elementary School, Powell, OH DAN DOYLE • History Teacher, Grade 11, St. Joseph’s Academy, Hoffman, IL DANIELLE HARTMAN • 2nd-Grade Teacher, Claymont Elementary School, Ballwin, MO DEMETRICK TENSLEY • 9th–12th-Grade Business Education & Computer Science Teacher, Seneca High School, Seneca, SC DONNIE PIERCEY • 5th-Grade Teacher and 2021 Kentucky State Teacher of the Year, Stonewall Elementary School, Lexington, KY DR. NANCY SHEEHAN-MELZACK • Art and Music Teacher, Snug Harbor Community School, Quincy, MA INDIA CHAMBERS • 1st–8th-Grade Director of Academics, Northside Preparatory Academy, Cincinnati, OH JACALYN D. WALKER • 8th-Grade Science Teacher, Treasure Mountain Middle School, Park City, UT JANE W. CAMPBELL • 2nd-Grade Teacher, John P. Faber Elementary School, Dunellen, NJ JENNIFER L. MATZ • 6th-Grade Teacher, Williams Valley Elementary, Tower City, PA JENNIFER PINCOSKI • Learning Resource Teacher, K–12, Lee County School District, Fort Myers, FL JESSICA N. MAHTABAN • 8th-Grade Math Teacher, Woodrow Wilson Middle School, Clifton, NJ JOLITA HARPER • 3rd-Grade Teacher, Preparing Academic Leaders Academy, Maple Heights, OH KAREN BOYARSKY • 5th-Grade Teacher, Walter C. Black Elementary School, Hightstown, NJ KATHRYN ALBRECHT LARSEN • Junior High School Principal, Pleasant Grove Junior High, Pleasant Grove, UT KATIE CHURCHILL • 3rd-Grade Teacher, Oriole Parke Elementary School, Chicago, IL KATIE PIEL • Kindergarten–6th-Grade Teacher, West Park School, Moscow, ID KEITH J. BOYLE • English Teacher, Grades 9–12, Dunellen High School, Dunellen, NJ KELLEY CROCKETT • Professor and Former Elementary School Teacher, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, and Fort Worth, TX Meadowbrook Elementary School, Fort Worth, TX KELLY L. HOY • 5th-Grade Humanities Teacher, Katherine Delmar Burke School, San Francisco, CA KELLY MCELROY BONIN • High School Counselor, Klein Oak High School, Spring, TX KYLE BROWN • 3rd-Grade Teacher, Hjorth Road Elementary, Surrey, British Columbia, Canada. LAUREN ROLLINS • 1st-Grade Teacher, Boulevard Elementary School, Shaker Heights, OH LINDA GLISSON AND SUE MIDDLETON • 5th-Grade Team Teachers, St. James Episcopal Day School, Baton Rouge, LA LINDA SPARKS • 1st-Grade Teacher, John F. Kennedy School, Billerica, MA LISA WHITE • 2nd-/3rd-Grade Combined and 3rd-/4th-Grade Combined Elementary Teacher, Moody Elementary School, Port Moody, British Columbia, Canada LORI HALL • 9th-Grade Math Teacher, Monroe High School, Monroe, NC

Preface

LOU DE LAURO • 5th-Grade Language Arts Teacher, John P. Faber School, Dunellen, NJ LUCY ORDAZ SANCHEZ • 7th- and 8th-Grade Family and Consumer Science Teacher, Dixon Middle School, Provo, UT M. DENISE LUTZ • Technology Coordinator, Grandview Heights High School, Columbus, OH MADYA AYALA • High School Teacher of Preperatoria, Eugenio Garza Lagüera, Campus Garza Sada, Monterrey, N. L. Mexico DR. MARI ANN BANKS • Equity Officer, City Schools of Decatur, Decatur, GA MARIA GALLO AVILA • 5th-Grade Math and Science Teacher, Stone Lakes Elementary School, Stonebrook, FL MARIA SOISSON • 6th-Grade Mathematics Teacher, Walnut Springs Middle School, Westerville, OH MARIANNE ROBINSON • 4th–5th and 6th–7th-Grade Teacher, Meadowbrook Elementary/Scott Creek Middle, Coquitlam Edmonton, Alberta, Canada MARIE HOFFMAN HURT • 8th-Grade Foreign Language Teacher (German and French), Pickerington Local Schools, Pickerington, OH MELISSA CHO • 3rd- and 4th-Grade Teacher, Royal Heights Elementary School, Surrey, British Columbia, Canada MELISSA ROY • 10th-Grade Reading and English Teacher Lake Brantley High School, Altamonte Springs, FL MELODI STERNER • 6th -7th-Grade Life Science and Skills for Adolescents Teacher, Riverwatch Middle School, Suwanee, GA MICHAEL YASIS • 5th-Grade Teacher, L. H. Tanglen Elementary School, Minnetonka, MN MIRIAM MILLER • English Language Learning Teacher Educator, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada MONIKA STAHLSTROM • 3rd–7th-Grade Teacher, East Kensington Outdoor Learning School, Surrey, British Columbia, CA NANCY SCHAEFER • Grades 9–12 Teacher, Cincinnati Hills Christian Academy High School, Cincinnati, OH NICOLE MACPHERSON • Kindergarten–12th-Grade District Learning Support Consultant, Burnaby School District, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada PAM GASKILL • 2nd-Grade Teacher, Riverside Elementary School, Dublin, OH PATRICIA A. SMITH • High School Math Teacher, Earl Warren High School, San Antonio, TX PAUL DRAGIN • English as a Second Language, Grades 9–12 Teacher, Columbus East High School, Columbus, OH PAULA COLEMERE • Special Education Teacher—English, History, McClintock High School, Tempe, AZ ROBBY HOVEL • 11th- and 12th-Grade AP Psychology Teacher, Lyman High School, Longwood, FL SARA VINCENT • Special Education Teacher, Langley High School, McLean, VA THOMAS NAISMITH • Science Teacher, Grades 7–12, Slocum Independent School District, Elkhart, TX TIFFANY AL-LATEE • Kindergarten–1st-Grade Teacher, KIPP Columbus Primary, Columbus, OH VALERIE A. CHILCOAT • 5th-/6th-Grade Advanced Academics, Glenmount School, Baltimore, MD VALERIE FIELDS • 11th–12th-Grade AP Psychology Teacher, Eagle's Landing Christian Academy, McDonough, GA VANESSA SAUER • 2nd–3rd-Grade Teacher Royal Heights Elementary School, Surrey, British Columbia, Canada VICKIE MORIARITY • 7th-Grade English and Language Arts Teacher, Bath County Middle School, Owingsville, KY

xiii

xiv Preface On this edition, we worked with an outstanding editorial group. Their intelligence, creativity, sound judgment, style, and enduring commitment to quality can be seen on every page of this text. Rebecca Fox-Gieg and Brooke Nethers, Content Analysts in Teacher Education, guided the project from reviews to completion. Their patience, persistence, and expertise made all the difference. Alicia Reilly was the outstanding developmental editor with the perfect combination of vast knowledge, organizational ability, and creative thinking. The text features, embedded videos, and excellent pedagogical supports would not exist without her tireless efforts. Karin Kipp focused her keen copyeditor's eye on the manuscript. Janelle Rogers kept all aspects of the project moving forward with amazing skill, grace, artistic vision, and good humor. Ajay Prasanna V was the senior production project manager. His attention to detail, keen eye, and instant answers to questions were remarkable. Somehow these wonderful colleagues brought sanity to what could have been chaos and fun to what might have been drudgery. What a talented and creative group—we are honored to work with them all. Finally, I (Anita) want to thank my family and friends for their kindness and support during the long days and nights that I worked on this book. To my family, Bob, Eric, Suzie, Lizzie, Wayne K., Marie, Kelly, and the newest member, Amaya—you are amazing. And, of course, to Wayne Hoy, my friend, colleague, inspiration, passion, husband— you are simply the best. I (Ellen) would like to thank Anita for inviting me to collaborate on this book. I have so much appreciation for the energy and time that such an effort requires. You are a kind and patient collaborator. Thanks also to members of the Kentucky P20 Motivation & Learning Lab, including two exceptional undergraduate students—Licia Henneberg and Olivia Huffman—who offered extensive help with literature recommendations, searches, and reference keeping. —ANITA WOOLFOLK HOY AND ELLEN L. USHER

Brief Contents PART I SETTING THE STAGE, MEETING THE PLAYERS

1

Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

2

Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

2 34

PART II DEVELOPMENT: SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCES

3

Cognitive Development

4

The Self, Social, and Moral Development

132

5

Learner Differences and Learning Needs

184

6

Language Development, Language Diversity, and Immigrant Education 246

86

PART III LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 7

Behavioral Views of Learning

8

Cognitive Views of Learning

9

Complex Cognitive Processes

288 328 372

10

Constructivism and Interactive Learning

11

Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

12

Motivation in Learning and Teaching

PART IV

418 466

506

TEACHING AND ASSESSING

13

Creating Supportive Learning Environments

14

Teaching Every Student

15

Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

556

606 650

xv

Contents Preface

vi

PART I SETTING THE STAGE, MEETING THE PLAYERS Cluster  1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

2

Teachers’ Casebook—Becoming a Great Teacher: What Would You Do?

2

Overview and Objectives

3

Learning and Teaching Today Inside Three Classrooms

4 5

A MULTILINGUAL FIRST GRADE 5 • A SUBURBAN FIFTH GRADE TO STUDENTS WITH LEARNING DISABILITIES 5

5 • TEACHING MATH

Students Today: Dramatic Diversity and Remarkable Technology Confidence in Every Context High Expectations for Teachers and Students Teaching the Whole Child: Social and Emotional Learning

6 7 8 8

RESEARCH ON SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL LEARNING 9 • PATHS: AN APPROACH TO SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL LEARNING 9 • LIVING WITH SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL TRAUMA 10

Do Teachers Make a Difference? TEACHER–STUDENT RELATIONSHIPS

10 11 • THE COST OF POOR TEACHING

11

What Is Good Teaching? Models of Good Teaching DANIELSON’S FRAMEWORK FOR TEACHING MODEL 13

12 12 12 • TEACHINGWORKS

13 • THE CLASS

Beginning Teachers

15

Module 1 Summary

16

The Role of Educational Psychology Educational Psychology Today Is It Just Common Sense?

18 18 18

LEARNING STYLES 19 • ANSWER BASED ON RESEARCH 19 • SKIPPING GRADES 19 • ANSWER BASED ON RESEARCH 19 • STUDENTS IN CONTROL 19 • ANSWER BASED ON RESEARCH 19 • OBVIOUS ANSWERS? 20

Using Research to Understand and Improve Learning

20

CORRELATION STUDIES 20 • EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES 21 • ABAB EXPERIMENTAL DESIGNS 22 • SCIENTIFICALLY BASED RESEARCH AND EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICES 22 • CLINICAL INTERVIEWS AND CASE STUDIES 22 • ETHNOGRAPHY 23 • THE ROLE OF TIME IN RESEARCH 23 • WHAT’S THE EVIDENCE? QUALITATIVE VERSUS QUANTITATIVE RESEARCH 23 • MIXED METHODS RESEARCH 23 • TEACHERS AS RESEARCHERS 24

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: What Kind of Research Should Guide Education? Theories for Teaching Supporting Student Learning

xvi

25 26 27

Module 2 Summary

29

Key Terms

30

Connect and Extend to Licensure

30

Teachers’ Casebook—Becoming a Great Teacher: What Would They Do?

31

Contents

Cluster  2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

34

Teachers’ Casebook—Conversations About Race: What Would You Do?

34

Overview and Objectives

35

Education Is Cultural What Is Culture?

37 37

Your Cultural and Educational History Meet Two Students Cultural Intersections and Terminology

39 41 42

INTERSECTIONALITY

42 • CULTURAL GROUPS AND TERMINOLOGY

42

Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Discrimination

43

STEREOTYPES: THE GOOD AND THE BAD 43 • FROM STEREOTYPES TO PREJUDICE STEREOTYPE THREAT 45 • FROM PREJUDICE TO DISCRIMINATION 46

44 •

Module 3 Summary

47

Economic and Social Class Differences Social Class and Socioeconomic Status Poverty and Social Inequality Poverty and Academic Outcomes

47 48 49 50

HEALTH, ENVIRONMENT, AND STRESS 51 • LOW EXPECTATIONS—LOW ACADEMIC SELFCONCEPT 52 • PEER INFLUENCES AND RESISTANCE CULTURES 52 • HOME ENVIRONMENT AND RESOURCES 53 • SUMMER SETBACKS AND SCHOOL DISRUPTIONS 53 • TRACKING AND POOR TEACHING 54

Extreme Poverty: Homeless and Highly Mobile Students

54

GUIDELINES: Teaching Students Who Live in Poverty

55

Module 4 Summary

56

Ethnicity and Race in Teaching and Learning Defining Ethnicity and Race Ethnic and Racial Identity

56 56 57

MULTIDIMENSIONAL AND FLEXIBLE ETHNIC IDENTITIES

57 • RACIAL IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT

Ethnic and Racial Differences in School Achievement The Legacy of Racial Inequality RACISM AND DISCRIMINATION

58

59 62

63

Module 5 Summary

64

Gender and Sexual Orientation in Teaching and Learning Sex and Gender Gender Identity Gender Roles Gender Bias and Sexism in Curriculum and Media Gender Bias in Teaching

64 65 65 66 67 68

GUIDELINES: Avoiding Gender Bias in Teaching

69

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Should Girls and Boys Be Taught Differently?

70

Sexual Orientation Discrimination Based on Gender Identity and Sexual Orientation

72 73

Module 6 Summary

74

Creating Culturally Welcoming Classrooms Culturally Relevant Pedagogy Diversity in Learning

75 76 77

SOCIAL ORGANIZATION 77 • CULTURAL VALUES AND LEARNING PREFERENCES 77 • SOCIOLINGUISTICS 78 • CULTURAL DISCONTINUITY

79

xvii

xviii Contents Lessons for Teachers: Teaching Every Student KNOW YOURSELF

79

79

GUIDELINES: Culturally Relevant Teaching KNOW YOUR STUDENTS

80

81 • RESPECT YOUR STUDENTS

81 • TEACH YOUR STUDENTS 81

Module 7 Summary

82

Key Terms

82

Connect and Extend to Licensure

83

Teachers’ Casebook—Conversations About Race: What Would They Do?

83

PART II DEVELOPMENT: SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCES

Cluster  3 Cognitive Development

86

Teachers’ Casebook—Symbols and Cymbals: What Would You Do?

86

Overview and Objectives

87

A Definition of Development Three Questions Across the Theories

88 89

WHAT IS THE SOURCE OF DEVELOPMENT? NATURE VERSUS NURTURE 89 • WHAT IS THE SHAPE OF DEVELOPMENT? CONTINUITY VERSUS DISCONTINUITY 90 • TIMING: IS IT TOO LATE? CRITICAL VERSUS SENSITIVE PERIODS 90 • BEWARE OF EITHER/OR 90

General Principles of Development

90

The Brain and Cognitive Development The Developing Brain: Neurons The Developing Brain: Cerebral Cortex Brain Development in Childhood and Adolescence Putting It All Together: How the Brain Works CULTURE AND BRAIN PLASTICITY

91 91 94 95 96

97

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Brain-Based Education

98

Neuroscience, Learning, and Teaching

99

DOES INSTRUCTION AFFECT BRAIN DEVELOPMENT? 100 • THE BRAIN AND LEARNING TO READ 100 • EMOTIONS, LEARNING, AND THE BRAIN 100

Lessons for Teachers: General Principles

101

Module 8 Summary

102

Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development Influences on Development Basic Tendencies in Thinking

103 104 104

ORGANIZATION 104 • ADAPTATION

104 • EQUILIBRATION

105

Four Stages of Cognitive Development

105

INFANCY: THE SENSORIMOTOR STAGE 106 • EARLY CHILDHOOD TO THE EARLY ELEMENTARY YEARS: THE PREOPERATIONAL STAGE 107 • LATER ELEMENTARY TO THE MIDDLE SCHOOL YEARS: THE CONCRETE-OPERATIONAL STAGE 108

GUIDELINES: Family and Community Partnerships: Helping Families Care for Preoperational Children

108

GUIDELINES: Teaching the Concrete-Operational Child

110

HIGH SCHOOL AND COLLEGE: FORMAL OPERATIONS STAGE? 112

111 • DO WE ALL REACH THE FOURTH

GUIDELINES: Helping Students Use Formal Operations

113

Some Limitations of Piaget’s Theory THE TROUBLE WITH STAGES 113 • UNDERESTIMATING CHILDREN’S ABILITIES COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT AND CULTURE 114

113 114 •

Contents

Information Processing, Neo-Piagetian, and Neuroscience Views of Cognitive Development

115

Module 9 Summary

115

Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Perspective The Social Sources of Individual Thinking Cultural Tools and Cognitive Development

116 117 118

TECHNICAL TOOLS IN A DIGITAL AGE

119 • PSYCHOLOGICAL TOOLS

119

The Roles of Language and Private Speech

119

PRIVATE SPEECH: VYGOTSKY’S AND PIAGET’S VIEWS COMPARED

120

The Zone of Proximal Development PRIVATE SPEECH AND THE ZONE

121

121 • THE ROLE OF LEARNING AND DEVELOPMENT

122

Limitations of Vygotsky’s Theory

122

Implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories for Teachers Piaget: What Can We Learn? UNDERSTANDING AND BUILDING ON STUDENTS’ THINKING KNOWLEDGE 123

122 122

123 • ACTIVITY AND CONSTRUCTING

Vygotsky: What Can We Learn? THE ROLE OF ADULTS AND PEERS

124 124 • ASSISTED LEARNING

124

An Example Curriculum: Tools of the Mind Reaching Every Student: Teaching in the “Magic Middle”

125 125

GUIDELINES: Applying Vygotsky’s Ideas in Teaching

126

Cognitive Development: Lessons for Teachers

127

Module 10 Summary

127

Key Terms

128

Connect and Extend to Licensure

129

Teachers’ Casebook—Symbols and Cymbals: What Would They Do?

129

Cluster  4 The Self, Social, and Moral Development

132

Teachers’ Casebook—Mean Girls: What Would You Do?

132

Overview and Objectives

133

Physical Development Physical and Motor Development

134 135

YOUNG CHILDREN

135 • ELEMENTARY SCHOOL YEARS

135

GUIDELINES: Dealing with Physical Differences in the Classroom THE ADOLESCENT YEARS

136 • EARLY AND LATER MATURING

135

136

Play, Recess, and Physical Activity CULTURAL DIFFERENCES IN PLAY

137

138 • EXERCISE AND RECESS

138

Reaching Every Student: Physical Activity and Students with Disabilities Challenges in Physical Development OBESITY

139 • BODY IMAGE AND DISORDERED EATING

139 139

140

GUIDELINES: Supporting Positive Body Images in Adolescents

141

Module 11 Summary

142

Bronfenbrenner: The Social Context for Development The Importance of Context and the Bioecological Model Families

142 142 144

PARENTING STYLES

144 • CULTURE AND PARENTING

145 • ATTACHMENT

145

GUIDELINES: Family and Community Partnerships: Connecting with Families DIVORCE

146

146

GUIDELINES: Helping Children of Divorce

147

xix

xx Contents Peers CLIQUES 148 POPULARITY AGGRESSION AGGRESSION

148 • CROWDS 148 • PEER CULTURES 148 • FRIENDSHIPS 148 • 149 • WHO IS LIKELY TO HAVE PROBLEMS WITH PEERS? 150 • 150 • RELATIONAL AGGRESSION 151 • MEDIA, MODELING, AND 151 • VIDEO GAMES AND AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR 151

Reaching Every Student: Teacher Support

152

GUIDELINES: Dealing with Aggression and Encouraging Cooperation ACADEMIC AND PERSONAL CARING

152

153 • SUPPORTING SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL LEARNING

Teachers and Child Abuse MANDATED REPORTING

153

155

155 • WHAT CAN TEACHERS DO?

155

Module 12 Summary

157

Identity and Self-Concept Erikson: Stages of Psychosocial Development

158 158

THE PRESCHOOL YEARS: TRUST, AUTONOMY, AND INITIATIVE 159 • THE ELEMENTARY AND MIDDLE SCHOOL YEARS: INDUSTRY VERSUS INFERIORITY 159

GUIDELINES: Encouraging Initiative and Industry ADOLESCENCE: THE SEARCH FOR IDENTITY BEYOND THE SCHOOL YEARS 163

161

162 • IDENTITY AND TECHNOLOGY

163 •

GUIDELINES: Supporting Identity Formation RACIAL AND ETHNIC PRIDE

164

165

Self-Concept

165

THE STRUCTURE OF SELF-CONCEPT 165 • HOW SELF-CONCEPT DEVELOPS SELF-CONCEPT AND ACHIEVEMENT 167

166 •

Sex Differences in the Self-Concept of Academic Competence Self-Esteem

167 168

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: What Should Schools Do to Encourage Students’ Self-Esteem? 168 THE DANGERS OF PERFECTIONISM

169

Module 13 Summary

170

Understanding Others and Moral Development Theory of Mind and Intention Moral Development

170 171 171

KOHLBERG’S THEORIES OF MORAL DEVELOPMENT THEORY 172

171 • CRITICISMS OF KOHLBERG’S

Moral Judgments, Social Conventions, and Personal Choices MORAL VERSUS CONVENTIONAL DOMAINS

173 • IMPLICATIONS FOR TEACHERS

Beyond Reasoning: Haidt’s Social Intuitionist Model of Moral Psychology Moral Behavior and the Example of Cheating WHO CHEATS?

176 • DEALING WITH CHEATING

173 174

175 176

177

Personal/Social Development: Lessons for Teachers

178

Module 14 Summary

178

Key Terms

179

Connect and Extend to Licensure

180

Teachers’ Casebook—Mean Girls: What Would They Do?

181

Cluster 5 Learner Differences and Learning Needs

184

Teachers’ Casebook—Including Every Student: What Would You Do?

184

Overview and Objectives

185

Language and Labeling

187

Contents

Impairments, Disabilities, and Handicaps Person-First/Identity-First Language Possible Biases in the Application of Labels

187 188 189

Intelligence What Does Intelligence Mean?

189 189

INTELLIGENCE: ONE ABILITY OR MANY?

190

Another View: Gardner’s Multiple Intelligences

190

WHAT ARE THESE INTELLIGENCES? 191 • CRITICS OF MULTIPLE INTELLIGENCES THEORY 191 • GARDNER RESPONDS 192

Multiple Intelligences: Lessons for Teachers Another View: Sternberg’s Successful Intelligence Neuroscience and Intelligence Measuring Intelligence

193 193 194 194

BINET’S DILEMMA 194 • WHAT DOES AN IQ SCORE MEAN? 195 • GROUP VERSUS INDIVIDUAL IQ TESTS 195 • THE FLYNN EFFECT: ARE WE GETTING SMARTER? 195

GUIDELINES: Interpreting IQ Scores INTELLIGENCE AND ACHIEVEMENT

196

196

Sex Differences in Intelligence and Achievement HEREDITY OR ENVIRONMENT?

197

198

Learning to Be Intelligent: Being Smart About IQ Emotional Intelligence WHAT IS EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE?

198 198

199

Module 15 Summary

200

Creativity: What It Is and Why It Matters Assessing Creativity OK, But So What: Why Does Creativity Matter? What Are the Sources of Creativity?

201 202 203 203

CREATIVITY AND COGNITION

204 • CREATIVITY AND DIVERSITY

204

Creativity in the Classroom

204

GUIDELINES: Applying and Encouraging Creativity

205

Learning Styles: Proceed with Caution Learning Styles/Preferences

207 207

CAUTIONS ABOUT LEARNING STYLES POPULAR? 208

207 • WHY ARE LEARING STYLES/PREFERENCES SO

Beyond Either/Or

208

Module 16 Summary

208

Individual Differences and the Law IDEA

209 209

LEAST RESTRICTIVE ENVIRONMENT 210 • INDIVIDUALIZED EDUCATION PROGRAM THE RIGHTS OF STUDENTS AND FAMILIES 211

210 •

GUIDELINES: Family and Community Partnerships: Productive Conferences

211

Section 504 Protections Response to Intervention

212 213

Module 17 Summary

214

Students with Learning Challenges Neuroscience and Learning Challenges Students with Specific Learning Disabilities

215 215 216

STUDENT CHARACTERISTICS

217 • TEACHING STUDENTS WITH LEARNING DISABILITIES

Students with Hyperactivity and Attention Disorders DEFINITIONS

219 • TREATING ADHD WITH DRUGS

219

220

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Pills or Skills for Children with ADHD? ALTERNATIVES/ADDITIONS TO DRUG TREATMENTS

218

221

220

xxi

xxii Contents Lessons for Teachers: Learning Disabilities and ADHD Students with Communication Impairments SPEECH IMPAIRMENTS

222 • LANGUAGE DISORDERS

221 222

223

Students with Emotional or Behavioral Disorders TRAUMA

223

224

GUIDELINES: Disciplining Students with Emotional Problems DEATH BY SUICIDE 227 • SUBSTANCE USE DISORDER THE STIGMA OF MENTAL HEALTH ISSUES 228

225

227 • PREVENTION

228 •

Students with Intellectual Disabilities

229

GUIDELINES: Teaching Students with Intellectual Disabilities

230

Students with Health and Sensory Impairments

230

CEREBRAL PALSY AND MULTIPLE DISABILITIES 230 • SEIZURE DISORDERS (EPILEPSY) 231 • OTHER SERIOUS HEALTH CONCERNS: ASTHMA, SICKLE CELL DISEASE, AND DIABETES 232 • STUDENTS WITH VISUAL IMPAIRMENTS 232 • STUDENTS WHO ARE DEAF 233

Autism Spectrum Disorders INTERVENTIONS

233

234

Module 18 Summary

234

Students Who Are Gifted and Talented Who Are These Students?

236 236

WHAT IS THE ORIGIN OF THESE GIFTS FACE? 237

237 • WHAT PROBLEMS DO STUDENTS WHO ARE GIFTED

Identifying Students Who Are Gifted and Talented RECOGNIZING GIFTS AND TALENTS

238

238

Teaching Students with Gifts and Talents ACCELERATION

240 • METHODS AND STRATEGIES

240 240

Module 19 Summary

240

Key Terms

241

Connect and Extend to Licensure

241

Teachers’ Casebook—Including Every Student: What Would They Do?

242

Cluster  6 Language Development, Language Diversity, and Immigrant Education

246

Teachers’ Casebook—Communicating and Creating Community in the Classroom: What Would You Do?

246

Overview and Objectives

247

The Development of Language What Develops? Language and Cultural Differences

249 249

THE PUZZLE OF LANGUAGE

249

When and How Does Language Develop?

250

SOUNDS AND PRONUNCIATION 250 • VOCABULARY AND MEANING 250 • GRAMMAR AND SYNTAX 252 • PRAGMATICS: USING LANGUAGE IN SOCIAL SITUATIONS 252 • METALINGUISTIC AWARENESS 252

Emergent Literacy BUILDING A FOUNDATION

253 253 • WHEN PROBLEMS PERSIST

254

Emergent Literacy and Language Diversity LANGUAGES AND EMERGENT LITERACY

254

254 • BILINGUAL EMERGENT LITERACY

255

GUIDELINES: Supporting Language and Promoting Literacy

255

Diversity in Language Development Dual-Language Development

256 256

SECOND-LANGUAGE LEARNING LANGUAGE LOSS 258

257 • BENEFITS OF BILINGUALISM

257 •

Contents

Signed Languages What Is Involved in Being Bilingual? Basic Communication and Academic Language

259 259 260

GUIDELINES: Promoting Language Learning

262

Dialect Differences in the Classroom Dialects

263 263

DIALECTS AND PRONUNCIATIONS

263 • DIALECTS AND TEACHING

264

Genderlects

264

Module 20 Summary

265

Teaching Immigrant Students and English Learners Immigrants and Refugees Classrooms Today Four Student Profiles

266 267 268 268

Generation 1.5: Students in Two Worlds Bilingual Education and English Learners

269 270

TWO APPROACHES TO LEARNING ENGLISH

270

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: What Is the Best Way to Teach English Learners?

270

RESEARCH ON BILINGUAL EDUCATION 272 • VISUAL STRATEGIES 272 • LITERATURE RESPONSE GROUPS 272 • BILINGUALISM FOR ALL: TWO-WAY IMMERSION 272

Sheltered Instruction Affective and Emotional/Social Considerations DEALING WITH TRAUMA

274 274

276

Working with Families: Using the Tools of the Culture

278

GUIDELINES: Providing Emotional Support and Increasing Self-Esteem for Students Who Are ELs FUNDS OF KNOWLEDGE AND WELCOME CENTERS

279 • STUDENT-LED CONFERENCES

278 280

GUIDELINES: Family and Community Partnerships: Welcoming All Families

280

Special Challenges: Students Who Are English Learners with Unique Learning Needs and Gifts Students Who Are English Learners with Disabilities Reaching Every Student: Recognizing Giftedness in Bilingual Students

281 281 282

Module 21 Summary

283

Key Terms

284

Connect and Extend to Licensure

285

Teachers’ Casebook—Communicating and Creating Community in the Classroom: What Would They Do?

285

PART III LEARNING AND MOTIVATION Cluster  7 Behavioral Views of Learning

288

Teachers’ Casebook—Sick of Class: What Would You Do?

288

Overview and Objectives

289

Understanding Learning Ethical Issues

291 292

GOALS

292 • STRATEGIES

292

Learning Is Not Always What It Seems

292

Early Explanations of Learning: Contiguity and Classical Conditioning

293

GUIDELINES: Applying Classical Conditioning

295

xxiii

xxiv Contents Operant Conditioning: Trying New Responses Types of Consequences REINFORCEMENT

296 • PUNISHMENT

296 296

297

Neuroscience of Reinforcement and Punishment Reinforcement Schedules EXTINCTION

298 299

300

Antecedents and Behavior Change EFFECTIVE INSTRUCTION DELIVERY

300 301 • CUEING

301

Module 22 Summary

301

Putting It All Together: Applied Behavior Analysis Methods for Encouraging Behaviors

302 303

REINFORCING WITH TEACHER ATTENTION PRINCIPLE 304

303 • SELECTING REINFORCERS: THE PREMACK

GUIDELINES: Applying Operant Conditioning—Using Praise Appropriately SHAPING

304

305

Contingency Contracts, Token Reinforcement, and Group Consequences CONTINGENCY CONTRACTS

306

306

GUIDELINES: Applying Operant Conditioning—Encouraging Positive Behaviors TOKEN REINFORCEMENT SYSTEMS

308 • GROUP CONSEQUENCES

307

309

Handling Undesirable Behavior

310

NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT 311 • POSITIVE PRACTICE OVERCORRECTION 311 • REPRIMANDS 311 • RESPONSE COST ISOLATION 312 • SOME CAUTIONS ABOUT PUNISHMENT 312

312 • SOCIAL

GUIDELINES: Applying Operant Conditioning—Using Punishment

313

Module 23 Summary

314

Current Applications: Functional Behavioral Assessment, Positive Behavior Supports, and Self-Management Discovering the “Why”: Functional Behavioral Assessments Reaching Every Student: Positive Behavior Supports Self-Management

315 316 317 318

GOAL SETTING 318 • MONITORING AND EVALUATING PROGRESS SELF-REINFORCEMENT 319

318 •

GUIDELINES: Family and Community Partnerships: Applying Operant Conditioning: Student Self-Management

319

Challenges and Criticisms Beyond Behaviorism: Bandura’s Challenge and Observational Learning

320 320

ENACTIVE AND OBSERVATIONAL LEARNING

320 • LEARNING AND PERFORMANCE

Criticisms of Behavioral Methods Behavioral Approaches: Lessons for Teachers

320

321 321

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Should Students Be Rewarded for Learning?

322

Module 24 Summary

323

Key Terms

324

Connect and Extend to Licensure

325

Teachers’ Casebook—Sick of Class: What Would They Do?

325

Cluster 8 Cognitive Views of Learning

328

Teachers’ Casebook—Remembering the Basics: What Would You Do?

328

Overview and Objectives

329

Elements of the Cognitive Perspective

330

Contents

The Brain and Cognitive Learning The Importance of Knowledge in Cognition

330 331

GENERAL AND SPECIFIC KNOWLEDGE 332 • DECLARATIVE, PROCEDURAL, AND SELF-REGULATORY KNOWLEDGE 332

Cognitive Views of Memory Sensory Memory

332 333

CAPACITY, DURATION, AND CONTENTS OF SENSORY MEMORY 334 • PERCEPTION THE ROLE OF ATTENTION 335 • ATTENTION AND MULTITASKING 337

335 •

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: What’s Wrong with Multitasking? ATTENTION AND TEACHING

337

338

GUIDELINES: Gaining and Maintaining Attention

339

Working Memory

340

A MODEL OF WORKING MEMORY 340 • THE CENTRAL EXECUTIVE 341 • THE PHONOLOGICAL LOOP 341 • THE VISUO-SPATIAL SKETCHPAD 342 • THE EPISODIC BUFFER 342 • CAPACITY OF WORKING MEMORY 342 • THE DURATION AND CONTENTS OF WORKING MEMORY 342

Cognitive Load and Retaining Information in Working Memory

343

TWO (OR THREE) KINDS OF COGNITIVE LOAD 343 • COGNITIVE LOAD: LESSONS FOR TEACHERS 343 • RETAINING INFORMATION IN WORKING MEMORY 344 • LEVELS OF PROCESSING THEORY 345 • FORGETTING 345

Individual Differences in Working Memory DEVELOPMENTAL DIFFERENCES

346

346 • INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES

346

Is Working Memory Really Separate?

347

Module 25 Summary

347

Long-Term Memory Capacity and Duration of Long-Term Memory Contents of Long-Term Memory: Explicit (Declarative) Memories

349 349 349

PROPOSITIONS AND PROPOSITIONAL NETWORKS 349 • IMAGES 350 • TWO ARE BETTER THAN ONE: WORDS AND IMAGES 350 • CONCEPTS 351 • PROTOTYPES, EXEMPLARS, AND THEORY-BASED CATEGORIES 351 • TEACHING CONCEPTS 352 • SCHEMAS 352 • EPISODIC MEMORY 354

GUIDELINES: Interviewing Young Students and Eyewitness Testimony Contents of Long-Term Memory: Implicit Memories Retrieving Information in Long-Term Memory SPREADING ACTIVATION MEMORY 357

356 • RECONSTRUCTION

355 355 356

357 • FORGETTING AND LONG-TERM

Individual Differences in Long-Term Memory

357

Module 26 Summary

358

Teaching for Long-Lasting Knowledge: Basic Principles and Applications Constructing Declarative Knowledge: Making Meaningful Connections

359 359

ELABORATION

359 • ORGANIZATION

360 • IMAGERY

360

GUIDELINES: Family and Community Partnerships: Organizing Learning CONTEXT

361 • DESIRABLE DIFFICULTY 361 • EFFECTIVE PRACTICE

Reaching Every Student: Make It Meaningful MNEMONICS

360

361

362

363

If You Have to Memorize . . . Lessons for Teachers: Declarative Knowledge Development of Procedural Knowledge AUTOMATED BASIC SKILLS

365 • DOMAIN-SPECIFIC STRATEGIES

364 365 365 366

GUIDELINES: Helping Students Understand and Remember

366

Module 27 Summary

368

Key Terms

368

Connect and Extend to Licensure

369

Teachers’ Casebook—Remembering the Basics: What Would They Do?

370

xxv

xxvi Contents

Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

372

Teachers’ Casebook—Uncritical Thinking: What Would You Do?

372

Overview and Objectives

373

Metacognition Metacognitive Knowledge and Regulation Individual Differences in Metacognition Lessons for Teachers: Developing Metacognition

375 375 375 376

METACOGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT FOR YOUNGER STUDENTS 376 • METACOGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT FOR SECONDARY AND COLLEGE STUDENTS (LIKE YOU) 377

Learning Strategies Being Strategic About Learning

379 380

DECIDING WHAT IS IMPORTANT 380 • SUMMARIES HIGHLIGHTING 381 • TAKING NOTES 382

381 • UNDERLINING AND

Visual Tools for Organizing Retrieval Practice: Powerful but Underused Reading Strategies Applying Learning Strategies APPROPRIATE TASKS 386 • VALUING LEARNING

383 385 385 386 386 • EFFORT AND EFFICACY

387

Reaching Every Student: Teaching How to Learn

387

Module 28 Summary

387

Problem Solving Identifying: Problem Finding Defining Goals and Representing the Problem

388 389 390

FOCUSING ATTENTION ON WHAT IS RELEVANT 390 • UNDERSTANDING THE WORDS 390 • UNDERSTANDING THE WHOLE PROBLEM 390 • TRANSLATION AND SCHEMA TRAINING: DIRECT INSTRUCTION IN REPRESENTING PROBLEMS 392 • TRANSLATION AND SCHEMA TRAINING: WORKED EXAMPLES 392 • WORKED EXAMPLES AND EMBODIED COGNITION 393 • THE RESULTS OF PROBLEM REPRESENTATION 395

Searching for Possible Solution Strategies ALGORITHMS 395 • HEURISTICS

395

395

Anticipating, Acting, and Looking Back Factors That Hinder Problem Solving SOME PROBLEMS WITH HEURISTICS

396 397

397

GUIDELINES: Applying Problem Solving

398

Expert Knowledge and Problem Solving

399

KNOWING WHAT IS IMPORTANT 399 • MEMORY FOR PATTERNS AND ORGANIZATION 399 • PROCEDURAL KNOWLEDGE 399 • PLANNING AND MONITORING

400

GUIDELINES: Becoming an Expert Student

400

Module 29 Summary

402

Critical Thinking and Argumentation What Critical Thinkers Do: Paul and Elder’s Model Teaching Critical Thinking

403 403 403

APPLYING CRITICAL THINKING IN SPECIFIC SUBJECTS

404

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Should Schools Teach Critical Thinking and Problem Solving? Thinking Critically About Online Sources Argumentation TWO STYLES OF ARGUMENTATION

Teaching for Transfer The Many Views of Transfer

407 • LESSONS FOR TEACHERS

405 405 406

408

408 408

Contents

Teaching for Positive Transfer

409

WHAT IS WORTH LEARNING? 410 • LESSONS FOR TEACHERS: SUPPORTING TRANSFER 410 • STAGES OF TRANSFER FOR STRATEGIES 410

GUIDELINES: Family and Community Partnerships: Promoting Transfer

411

Bringing It All Together: Teaching for Complex Learning and Robust Knowledge What Is Robust Knowledge? Teaching for Robust Knowledge

412 412 412

PRACTICE 412 • WORKED EXAMPLES 412 • ANALOGIES TEXTS 413 • SELF-EXPLANATIONS 413

413 • INTEGRATING MULTIPLE

Module 30 Summary

413

Key Terms

415

Connect and Extend to Licensure

415

Teachers’ Casebook—Uncritical Thinking: What Would They Do?

416

Cluster  10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

418

Teachers’ Casebook—Necessity Is the Mother of Inventive Teaching: What Would You Do?

418

Overview and Objectives

419

Cognitive and Social Constructivism Constructivist Views of Learning

420 421

COGNITIVE CONSTRUCTIVISM

421 • SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIVISM

422

How Is Knowledge Constructed? Knowledge: Situated or General? Common Elements of Learner-Centered Teaching

423 423 424

COMPLEX LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS AND AUTHENTIC TASKS 425 • SOCIAL NEGOTIATION 425 • MULTIPLE PERSPECTIVES AND REPRESENTATIONS OF CONTENT 425 • SELF-AWARENESS AND REFLECTION ABOUT THE KNOWLEDGE CONSTRUCTION PROCESS 426 • STUDENT OWNERSHIP OF LEARNING 427

Designing Constructivist Learning Environments Facilitating Deep Learning in a Constructivist Classroom SCAFFOLDING 427 • ADVANCE ORGANIZERS ANSWERING DEEP QUESTIONS 429

427 427

429 • FACILITATING THROUGH ASKING AND

GUIDELINES: Facilitating Deep Questioning

430

Inquiry-Based Learning

431

EXAMPLES OF INQUIRY 431 • PROBLEM-BASED LEARNING INSTRUCTION IMPROVE LEARNING? 433

432 • DOES INQUIRY-BASED

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Are Teaching Approaches to Support Inquiry- and Problem-Based Learning Effective? BEING SMART ABOUT INQUIRY LEARNING

434

435

Cognitive Apprenticeships

436

Module 31 Summary

437

Collaboration and Cooperation

438

Learning in Groups

439

GOALS OF GROUP WORK

439 • WHAT CAN GO WRONG: MISUSES OF GROUP LEARNING

439

Learning Through Cooperation EFFECTS OF COOPERATIVE LEARNING

440 442 • TEACHER’S ROLE IN COOPERATIVE LEARNING

Designing Cooperative Learning Tasks

442

442

HIGHLY STRUCTURED, REVIEW, AND SKILL-BUILDING TASKS 442 • ILL-STRUCTURED, CONCEPTUAL, AND PROBLEM-SOLVING TASKS 443 • SOCIAL SKILLS AND COMMUNICATION TASKS 443 • GIVING AND RECEIVING EXPLANATIONS 444

Setting Up Cooperative Groups GROUP COMPOSITION

444 • ASSIGNING ROLES

444 445

xxvii

xxviii Contents Examples of Cooperative Learning Techniques RECIPROCAL QUESTIONING CONTROVERSIES 447

446 • JIGSAW

446

446 • CONSTRUCTIVE/STRUCTURED

Reaching Every Student: Using Cooperative Learning Wisely

448

GUIDELINES: Using Cooperative Learning

449

Module 32 Summary

450

Designing Interactive Digital Learning Environments Technology and Learning Technology-Rich Environments

451 451 452

MOBILE LEARNING 452 • VIRTUAL LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS VIRTUAL LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS 454 • GAMES 455

453 • IMMERSIVE

Computational Thinking and Coding Media Use, Digital Citizenship, and Media Literacy DEVELOPMENTALLY APPROPRIATE ACTIVITIES CRITICAL MEDIA LITERACY 459

457 • DIGITAL CITIZENSHIP

456 457 458 •

GUIDELINES: Supporting the Development of Media Literacy

459

Blending and Flipping: Technology-Powered Pedagogy Dilemmas of Constructivist Practice

460 461

Module 33 Summary

462

Key Terms

462

Connect and Extend to Licensure

463

Teachers’ Casebook—Necessity Is the Mother of Inventive Teaching: What Would They Do?

463

Cluster  11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

466

Teachers’ Casebook—Failure to Self-Regulate: What Would You Do?

466

Overview and Objectives

467

Social Cognitive Theory A Self-Directed Life: Albert Bandura Beyond Behaviorism Triadic Reciprocal Causality

469 469 470 470

Modeling: Learning by Observing Others Elements of Observational Learning

472 473

ATTENTION 473 • RETENTION REINFORCEMENT 474

473 • PRODUCTION

473 • MOTIVATION AND

Observational Learning in Teaching

474

DIRECTING ATTENTION 474 • FINE-TUNING ALREADY-LEARNED BEHAVIORS 475 • STRENGTHENING OR WEAKENING INHIBITIONS 475 • TEACHING NEW BEHAVIORS 475 • AROUSING EMOTION 475

GUIDELINES: Using Observational Learning

476

Agency and Self-Efficacy Self-Efficacy, Self-Concept, and Self-Esteem Sources of Self-Efficacy Self-Efficacy in Learning and Teaching

477 477 478 480

GUIDELINES: Supporting Self-Efficacy

481

Teachers’ Sense of Efficacy

482

Module 34 Summary

483

Self-Regulated Learning: Skill and Will How Does Self-Regulation Develop?

484 485

KNOWLEDGE

485 • MOTIVATION

486 • VOLITION

486

Contents

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Are “Grittier” Students More Successful?

488

DEVELOPMENTAL CHANGES IN SELF-REGULATION 490

A Social Cognitive Model of Self-Regulated Learning Reaching Every Student: Technology and Self-Regulation Self-Regulation of Emotions SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL LEARNING

493 • TEACHER STRESS AND SELF-REGULATION

490 492 493 494

GUIDELINES: Encouraging Emotional Self-Regulation

495

Teaching Toward Self-Efficacy and Self-Regulated Learning Complex Tasks Agency and Control Self-Management Self-Evaluation Collaboration

496 496 497 498 499 500

Bringing It All Together: Theories of Learning

500

Module 35 Summary

502

Key Terms

503

Connect and Extend to Licensure

503

Teachers’ Casebook—Failure to Self-Regulate: What Would They Do?

504

Cluster  12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

506

Teachers’ Casebook—Motivating Students When Resources Are Thin: What Would You Do?

506

Overview and Objectives

507

What Is Motivation? Meeting Some Students Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation What You Already Know About Motivation

509 510 511 512

Needs and Self-Determination Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs Self-Determination: Need for Competence, Autonomy, and Relatedness

513 513 515

SELF-DETERMINATION IN THE CLASSROOM 515 • NEED-SUPPORTIVE VERSUS NEED-THWARTING CLASSROOMS 516

GUIDELINES: Supporting Self-Determination and Autonomy SUPPORTIVE RELATIONSHIPS AS MOTIVATORS

517

518

Needs: Lessons for Teachers

518

Module 36 Summary

518

Goals and Goal Orientations Goal Setting: What Am I Trying to Do? Making Goals Work: Feedback, Framing, and Commitment Goal Orientations: Why Am I Trying to Do This?

519 520 521 522

WAIT—ARE PERFORMANCE GOALS ALWAYS BAD? 523 • WORK-AVOIDANCE GOALS CONTEXTUAL INFLUENCES ON GOAL ORIENTATION 524

Social Goals Goals: Lessons for Teachers Expectancies, Values, and Costs Expectancy for Success Task Value Perceived Cost Expectancy, Value, and Cost: Lessons for Teachers

524 •

524 525 525 526 526 527 527

xxix

xxx Contents Attributions and Beliefs About Ability Attributions in the Classroom Teacher Attributions Trigger Student Attributions Mindsets About Ability Ability Mindsets in the Classroom Mindsets About Failure Attributions and Mindsets: Lessons for Teachers

527 528 529 530 530 531 532

GUIDELINES: Encouraging a Healthy Mindset

533

Module 37 Summary

533

How Do You Feel About Learning? Interest, Curiosity, and Emotions Tapping Interests

535 535

TWO KINDS OF INTERESTS

535 • CATCHING AND HOLDING INTERESTS

536

Curiosity: Novelty and Complexity

536

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Does Making Learning Fun Make for Good Learning?

537

GUIDELINES: Building on Students’ Interests and Curiosity

538

Flow Emotions

539 540

EMOTION AND THE BODY 540 • ACHIEVEMENT EMOTIONS 541 • EMOTIONS IN THE CLASSROOM 542

Anxiety in the Classroom Reaching Every Student: Coping with Negative Emotions

542 543

GUIDELINES: Coping With Test Anxiety

544

Interest, Curiosity, and Emotions: Lessons for Teachers

545

Module 38 Summary

545

Strategies to Encourage Motivation to Learn Why Am I Learning This? Is It Valuable, Useful, Relevant?

546 547

CREATING AUTHENTIC TASKS

547 • INCREASING PERCEIVED RELEVANCE AND UTILITY

547

Will I Enjoy This? ENHANCING CURIOSITY, INTEREST, AND INTRINSIC VALUE

548 548 • SUPPORTING AUTONOMY 548

Can I Do It? Building Confidence and Positive Expectations What Do I Need to Do to Succeed? Providing Structures That Support Progress Do I Belong in This Classroom? Creating Inclusive Opportunities

549 549 550

GUIDELINES: Family and Community Partnerships: Supporting the Sociocultural Foundations of Motivation

551

Module 39 Summary

552

Key Terms

552

Connect and Extend to Licensure

553

Teachers’ Casebook—Motivating Students When Resources Are Thin: What Would They Do?

553

PART IV TEACHING AND ASSESSING Cluster  13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

556

Teachers’ Casebook—Bullies and Victims: What Would You Do?

556

Overview and Objectives

556

The What and Why of Supportive Classroom Organization The Basic Task: Gain Their Cooperation The Basic Goals: Access, Time, Relationships, and Self-Management

558 559 560

ACCESS TO LEARNING 560 • MORE TIME FOR LEARNING 560 • MANAGEMENT MEANS RELATIONSHIPS 561 • MANAGEMENT FOR SELF-MANAGEMENT 562

Contents

Creating a Positive Learning Environment Relevant Research Results Routines and Rules Required: In-Person Learning ROUTINES AND PROCEDURES

562 563 563

563

GUIDELINES: Establishing Class Routines for In-Person Learning

564

RULES 565 • RULES FOR IN-PERSON TEACHING IN ELEMENTARY SCHOOL 565 • RULES FOR IN-PERSON TEACHING IN SECONDARY SCHOOL 565 • CONSEQUENCES WHO SETS THE RULES AND CONSEQUENCES? 566

566 •

Routines and Rules Required: Remote Learning

567

GUIDELINES: Establishing Routines and Rules for Remote Learning

567

Planning Spaces for Learning

570

PERSONAL TERRITORIES AND SEATING ARRANGEMENTS

570 • INTEREST AREAS

570

GUIDELINES: Designing Learning Spaces

571

Getting Started: The First Weeks of Class

572

EFFECTIVE MANAGERS FOR ELEMENTARY STUDENTS SECONDARY STUDENTS 573

572 • EFFECTIVE MANAGERS FOR

Module 40 Summary

573

Maintaining a Good Environment for Learning Encouraging Engagement

574 574

GUIDELINES: Keeping Students Engaged

575

Prevention Is the Best Medicine

575

WITHITNESS 576 • OVERLAPPING AND GROUP FOCUS 576 • MOVEMENT MANAGEMENT 576 • STUDENT SOCIAL SKILLS AS PREVENTION 576

Caring Relationships: Connections with School TEACHER CONNECTIONS 578 • SCHOOL CONNECTIONS AND BELONGING COMMUNITIES OF CARE FOR ADOLESCENTS 579

578 579 • CREATING

GUIDELINES: Creating Caring Relationships

580

Module 41 Summary

581

Dealing with Discipline Problems Stopping Problems Quickly If You Impose Penalties

582 582 583

GUIDELINES: Imposing Penalties

584

What About School Suspensions and Zero Tolerance?

585

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Is Zero Tolerance a Good Idea?

585

Reaching Every Student: Restorative Justice Teacher-Imposed Penalties Versus Student Responsibility Bullying and Cyberbullying VICTIMS 588 • WHY DO STUDENTS BULLY? TEASING 590 • CYBERBULLYING 592

586 587 588

590 • WHAT CAN TEACHERS DO? BULLYING AND

Special Challenges with High School Students

593

GUIDELINES: Handling Potentially Explosive Situations

593

The Need for Communication Message Sent—Message Received Empathetic Listening When Listening Is Not Enough: I-Messages, Assertive Discipline, and Problem Solving

594 594 595

I-MESSAGES 596 • ASSERTIVE DISCIPLINE NEGOTIATIONS 597

596

596 • CONFRONTATIONS AND

Diversity: Culturally Responsive Management

597

GUIDELINES: Family and Community Partnerships: Classroom Organization and Management

600

xxxi

xxxii Contents Module 42 Summary

601

Key Terms

602

Connect and Extend to Licensure

603

Teachers’ Casebook—Bullies and Victims: What Would They Do?

603

Cluster  14 Teaching Every Student

606

Teachers’ Casebook—Reaching and Teaching Every Student: What Would You Do?

606

Overview and Objectives

607

Research on Teaching Characteristics of Effective Teachers

608 609

CLARITY AND ORGANIZATION

609 • ENTHUSIASM AND WARMTH

609

Knowledge for Teaching Teacher Expectations

610 610

TWO KINDS OF EXPECTATION EFFECTS 611 • SOURCES OF EXPECTATIONS 611 • DO TEACHERS’ EXPECTATIONS REALLY AFFECT STUDENTS’ ACHIEVEMENT? 612 • LESSONS FOR TEACHERS: COMMUNICATING APPROPRIATE EXPECTATIONS 613

GUIDELINES: Avoiding the Negative Effects of Teacher Expectations The Goal: Ambitious Teaching

615

The First Step: Planning Research on Planning Learning Goals

615 616 616

AN EXAMPLE OF STATE-LEVEL GOALS: THE COMMON CORE LEARNING 618

617 • CLASSROOMS GOALS FOR

Flexible and Creative Plans—Using Taxonomies THE COGNITIVE DOMAIN DOMAIN 620

614

619 • THE AFFECTIVE DOMAIN

618 620 • THE PSYCHOMOTOR

The Cognitive Demands of Learning Objectives: Webb’s Depth of Knowledge Planning from a Constructivist Perspective

620 620

GUIDELINES: Using Learning Objectives

621

Module 43 Summary

622

Teaching Approaches Research on Teaching Strategies Explicit Teaching and Direct Instruction

624 624 624

ROSENSHINE’S SIX TEACHING FUNCTIONS 625 • WHY DOES DIRECT INSTRUCTION WORK? 625 • EVALUATING DIRECT INSTRUCTION 625

GUIDELINES: Effective Direct Instruction

626

Independent Work and Homework INDEPENDENT WORK 627 • HOMEWORK

627 628

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Is Homework Valuable?

629

GUIDELINES: Family and Community Partnerships: Homework

630

Questioning, Discussion, Dialogue, and Feedback

631

KINDS OF QUESTIONS 632 • ASKING AUTHENTIC QUESTIONS 633 • FITTING THE QUESTIONS TO THE STUDENTS—AND WAITING 633 • RESPONDING TO STUDENT ANSWERS 634 • GROUP DISCUSSION AND QUALITY TALK 634

Lessons for Teachers: Fitting Teaching to Your Goals

635

GUIDELINES: Productive Group Discussions

636

Putting It All Together: Understanding by Design

637

Differentiated Instruction Within-Class and Flexible Grouping THE PROBLEMS WITH ABILITY GROUPING

639 639 640 • FLEXIBLE GROUPING

640

Contents

GUIDELINES: Using Flexible Grouping

640

Differentiated Instruction in Inclusive Classrooms ASSISTIVE TECHNOLOGY

641

641

Reaching Every Student: Differentiation with Universal Design for Learning

642

Module 44 Summary

643

Key Terms

645

Connect and Extend to Licensure

645

Teachers’ Casebook—Reaching and Teaching Every Student: What Would They Do?

646

Cluster  15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

650

Teachers’ Casebook—Giving Meaningful Grades: What Would You Do?

650

Overview and Objectives

651

Basics of Assessment Measurement and Assessment

653 653

FORMATIVE, INTERIM, AND SUMMATIVE ASSESSMENT

654

Assessing the Assessments: Reliability and Validity RELIABILITY OF TEST SCORES

654

655 • VALIDITY 655 • ABSENCE OF BIAS

656

Classroom Assessment: Testing Interpreting Any Test Score

657 657

NORM-REFERENCED TEST INTERPRETATIONS INTERPRETATIONS 658

658 • CRITERION-REFERENCED TEST

Using the Tests from Textbooks Selected-Response Testing USING MULTIPLE-CHOICE TESTS

659 659 660 • WRITING MULTIPLE-CHOICE QUESTIONS

660

Constructed Responses: Essay Testing

660

GUIDELINES: Writing Multiple-Choice Test Items CONSTRUCTING ESSAY TESTS

661

662 • EVALUATING ESSAYS

663

Assessing Traditional Testing

663

Module 45 Summary

664

Formative and Authentic Classroom Assessments Informal Assessments

665 665

EXIT TICKETS

665 • JOURNALS

665 • INVOLVING STUDENTS IN ASSESSMENTS

Authentic Assessments: Performances and Portfolios PERFORMANCES

666 • PORTFOLIOS

666

668

Evaluating Portfolios and Performances SCORING RUBRICS

666

668

668

GUIDELINES: Creating Portfolios RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY

669

670

GUIDELINES: Developing a Rubric DIVERSITY AND BIAS IN PERFORMANCE ASSESSMENT

671 671

Assessing Complex Thinking Classroom Assessment: Lessons for Teachers

671 672

Grading Norm-Referenced Versus Criterion-Referenced Grading Effects of Grading on Students THE VALUE OF FAILING?

674 • RETENTION IN GRADE

675

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Should Children Be Held Back? Grades and Motivation Beyond Grading: Communicating with Families GUIDELINES: Using Any Grading System

673 673 674 676 677 677 678

xxxiii

xxxiv Contents Module 46 Summary

680

Standardized Testing Types of Scores

681 681

MEASUREMENTS OF CENTRAL TENDENCY AND STANDARD DEVIATION 681 • THE NORMAL DISTRIBUTION 682 • PERCENTILE RANK SCORES 683 • GRADE-EQUIVALENT SCORES 683 • STANDARD SCORES 683

Interpreting Standardized Test Reports DISCUSSING TEST RESULTS WITH FAMILIES

685 687

GUIDELINES: Family and Community Partnerships: Conferences and Explaining Test Results Accountability and High-Stakes Testing MAKING DECISIONS 689 • WHAT DO TEACHERS THINK? WITH HIGH-STAKES TESTING 689

687 688

689 • DOCUMENTED PROBLEMS

New Directions: PARCC and SBAC Using High-Stakes Testing Well: Lessons for Teachers Reaching Every Student: Helping Students with Disabilities Prepare for High-Stakes Tests GUIDELINES: Preparing Yourself and Your Students for Testing

691 691

Teacher Accountability and Evaluation VALUE-ADDED MODELS 693 • SO HOW SHOULD WE EVALUATE TEACHERS? APPROACH: MEASURES OF TEACHER EFFECTIVENESS 694

689 690

693 694 • A BROADER

Module 47 Summary

695

Key Terms

696

Connect and Extend to Licensure

696

Teachers’ Casebook—Giving Meaningful Grades: What Would They Do?

697

Licensure Appendix A-1 Glossary

G-1

References

R-1

Name Index Subject Index

N-1 S-1

Special Features TEACHERS’ CASEBOOK: WHAT WOULD YOU DO? Becoming a Great Teacher: What Would You Do? Becoming a Great Teacher: What Would They Do? Conversations About Race: What Would You Do? Conversations About Race: What Would They Do? Symbols and Cymbals: What Would You Do? Symbols and Cymbals: What Would They Do? Mean Girls: What Would You Do? Mean Girls: What Would They Do? Including Every Student: What Would You Do? Including Every Student: What Would They Do? Communicating and Creating Community in the Classroom: What Would You Do? Communicating and Creating Community in the Classroom: What Would They Do? Sick of Class: What Would You Do? Sick of Class: What Would They Do? Remembering the Basics: What Would You Do? Remembering the Basics: What Would They Do? Uncritical Thinking: What Would You Do? Uncritical Thinking: What Would They Do? Necessity Is the Mother of Inventive Teaching: What Would You Do? Necessity Is the Mother of Inventive Teaching: What Would They Do? Failure to Self-Regulate: What Would You Do? Failure to Self-Regulate: What Would They Do? Motivating Students When Resources Are Thin: What Would You Do? Motivating Students When Resources Are Thin: What Would They Do? Bullies and Victims: What Would You Do? Bullies and Victims: What Would They Do? Reaching and Teaching Every Student: What Would You Do? Reaching and Teaching Every Student: What Would They Do? Giving Meaningful Grades: What Would You Do? Giving Meaningful Grades: What Would They Do?

2 31 34 83 86 129 132 181 184 242 246 285 288 325 328 370 372 416 418 463 466 504 506 553 556 603 606 646 650 697

GUIDELINES Teaching Students Who Live in Poverty Avoiding Gender Bias in Teaching Culturally Relevant Teaching Family and Community Partnerships: Helping Families Care for Preoperational Children Teaching the Concrete-Operational Child Helping Students Use Formal Operations Applying Vygotsky’s Ideas in Teaching Dealing with Physical Differences in the Classroom Supporting Positive Body Images in Adolescents Family and Community Partnerships: Connecting with Families Helping Children of Divorce Dealing with Aggression and Encouraging Cooperation Encouraging Initiative and Industry Supporting Identity Formation

55 69 80 108 110 113 126 135 141 146 147 152 161 164

xxxv

xxxvi Special Features Interpreting IQ Scores Applying and Encouraging Creativity Family and Community Partnerships: Productive Conferences Disciplining Students with Emotional Problems Teaching Students with Intellectual Disabilities Supporting Language and Promoting Literacy Promoting Language Learning Providing Emotional Support and Increasing Self-Esteem for Students Who Are ELs Family and Community Partnerships: Welcoming All Families Applying Classical Conditioning Applying Operant Conditioning—Using Praise Appropriately Applying Operant Conditioning—Encouraging Positive Behaviors Applying Operant Conditioning—Using Punishment Family and Community Partnerships: Applying Operant Conditioning: Student Self-Management Gaining and Maintaining Attention Interviewing Young Students and Eyewitness Testimony Family and Community Partnerships: Organizing Learning Helping Students Understand and Remember Applying Problem Solving Becoming an Expert Student Family and Community Partnerships: Promoting Transfer Facilitating Deep Questioning Using Cooperative Learning Supporting the Development of Media Literacy Using Observational Learning Supporting Self-Efficacy Encouraging Emotional Self-Regulation Supporting Self-Determination and Autonomy Encouraging a Healthy Mindset Building on Students’ Interests and Curiosity Coping With Test Anxiety Family and Community Partnerships: Supporting the Sociocultural Foundations of Motivation Establishing Class Routines for In-Person Learning Establishing Routines and Rules for Remote Learning Designing Learning Spaces Keeping Students Engaged Creating Caring Relationships Imposing Penalties Handling Potentially Explosive Situations Family and Community Partnerships: Classroom Organization and Management Avoiding the Negative Effects of Teacher Expectations Using Learning Objectives Effective Direct Instruction Family and Community Partnerships: Homework Productive Group Discussions Using Flexible Grouping Writing Multiple-Choice Test Items Creating Portfolios Developing a Rubric Using Any Grading System Family and Community Partnerships: Conferences and Explaining Test Results Preparing Yourself and Your Students for Testing

196 205 211 225 230 255 262 278 280 295 304 307 313 319 339 355 360 366 398 400 411 430 449 459 476 481 495 517 533 538 544 551 564 567 571 575 580 584 593 600 614 621 626 630 636 640 661 669 671 678 687 691

Special Features

POINT/COUNTERPOINT What Kind of Research Should Guide Education? Should Girls and Boys Be Taught Differently? Brain-Based Education What Should Schools Do to Encourage Students’ Self-Esteem? Pills or Skills for Children with ADHD? What Is the Best Way to Teach English Learners? Should Students Be Rewarded for Learning? What’s Wrong with Multitasking? Should Schools Teach Critical Thinking and Problem Solving? Are Teaching Approaches to Support Inquiry- and Problem-Based Learning Effective? Are “Grittier” Students More Successful? Does Making Learning Fun Make for Good Learning? Is Zero Tolerance a Good Idea? Is Homework Valuable? Should Children Be Held Back?

25 70 98 168 220 270 322 337 405 434 488 537 585 629 676

xxxvii

Cluster 1

vvoe/Shutterstock

Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

Teachers’ Casebook: Becoming a Great Teacher What Would You Do? You are committed to being a great teacher, but teaching is a huge job. Fortunately, you’re not alone. Every day, educators and researchers from around the globe post new guides and ideas for effective instruction. You enjoy using social media, online resources, and popular books to improve your teaching. You’re drawn to the wisdom of those who are obviously great teachers. You bookmark these resources for activity ideas, innovative approaches, and tips for reaching your ever-changing student population. Also, you just feel that staying current in your professional knowledge is important. On occasion, the advice you hear challenges your long-held beliefs about teaching and learning. But it sometimes feels overwhelming or contradictory. 2

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

Critical Thinking • What makes someone a great teacher? How are “best” practices determined? • How do you evaluate the quality of others’ advice about teaching and learning? • What would lead you to conclude that someone else’s advice is simply a trend versus a sound educational practice? • What kinds of research findings would convince you to change your practice?

Overview and Objectives Like many students, you may begin this course with a mixture of anticipation and wariness. Perhaps you are required to take educational psychology as part of a program in teacher education, speech therapy, nursing, or counseling. You might have chosen this class as an elective. Whatever your reason for enrolling, you probably have questions about teaching, schools, students—or even about yourself—that you hope this course could answer. We have written the 15th edition of Educational Psychology with questions such as these in mind. In this first cluster, we begin with the state of education in today’s world. Teachers have been both criticized as ineffective and lauded as the best hope for young people. Do teachers make a difference in students’ learning? What characterizes good teaching—how do truly effective teachers think and act? What do they believe about students, learning, and themselves? When you are aware of the challenges and possibilities of teaching and learning today, you can appreciate the contributions of educational psychology. After a brief introduction to the world of the teacher, we turn to a discussion of educational psychology itself. How can principles identified by educational psychologists benefit teachers, therapists, parents, and others who are interested in teaching and learning? What exactly is the content of educational psychology, and where does this information come from? Finally, we consider an overview of a model that organizes research in educational psychology to identify the key student and school factors related to student learning (J. Lee & Shute, 2010). Our goal is for you to become a confident and competent beginning teacher so that by the time you have completed this cluster, you should be able to: 1.1

Describe the challenges facing teachers today, including increasing student diversity, requirements of the Every Student Succeeds Act, the continuing impacts of testing and accountability for teachers and students, and the emphasis on social and emotional learning.

1.2

Discuss the essential features of effective teaching, including different frameworks describing what good teachers do.

1.3

Describe the methods used to conduct research in the field of educational psychology and the kinds of questions each method can address.

1.4

Recognize how theories and research in development and learning are related to educational practice.

3

4 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

OUTLINE Teachers’ Casebook—Becoming a Great Teacher: What Would You Do? Overview and Objectives MODULE 1: Educational Psychology for Today’s Teachers Learning and Teaching Today Inside Three Classrooms Students Today: Dramatic Diversity and Remarkable Technology Confidence in Every Context High Expectations for Teachers and Students Teaching the Whole Child: Social and Emotional Learning Do Teachers Make a Difference? What Is Good Teaching? Models of Good Teaching Beginning Teachers MODULE 2: Research and Theory in Educational Psychology The Role of Educational Psychology Educational Psychology Today Is It Just Common Sense? Using Research to Understand and Improve Learning Theories for Teaching Supporting Student Learning Cluster 1 Review Key Terms • Connect and Extend to Licensure • Teachers’ Casebook— Becoming a Great Teacher: What Would They Do?

MODULE 1

Educational Psychology for Today’s Teachers

Learning Objective 1.1 Describe the challenges facing teachers today, including increasing student diversity, requirements of the Every Student Succeeds Act, the continuing impacts of testing and accountability for teachers and students, and the emphasis on social and emotional learning. Learning Objective 1.2 Discuss the essential features of effective teaching, including different frameworks describing what good teachers do.

Learning and Teaching Today For the first 14 editions of this book, I (Anita) focused on traditional, in-person teaching. In several clusters of this 15th edition, we will be making a distinction between in-person learning and remote learning. If you were involved with schools in any way in 2020 and 2021 as a student, sibling, teacher, parent, or family member, then you know why we will be making this distinction. Most teachers and students had to quickly learn how to “Zoom,” assuming their technologies allowed remote learning. Classes were converted to hybrid, online, or other versions to protect everyone from the spread of COVID-19. But you might be surprised to know that hybrid learning (in-person meetings combined with remote online teaching) and completely online learning were increasing even before schools had to close because of the pandemic (Pulham & Graham, 2018). Before the pandemic, Ted Cross and Laura Polk (2018) claimed that “online education is an integral part of the 21st century” (p. 1). So we will be looking at research on both in-person and online teaching and learning strategies in many clusters. Expanding your knowledge and skills to include remote

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

learning will prepare you for whatever the future throws at you, which brings us to our favorite topic. We invite you to join us. Welcome to our favorite topic: educational psychology—the study of development, learning, motivation, teaching, and assessment in and out of schools. We believe this is one of the most important courses you will take to prepare for your future as an educator in the classroom or the consulting office, whether your “students” are children or adults learning how to read or individuals discovering how to improve their diets. In fact, some evidence shows that new teachers who have coursework in development and learning are twice as likely to stay in teaching (National Commission on Teaching and America’s Future, 2003). This may be a required course for you, so let us make the case for educational psychology, first by stepping into classrooms today.

Inside Three Classrooms To begin our examination of good teaching, let’s step inside the classrooms of three outstanding teachers. The three situations are real. Our colleague Carol Weinstein worked with the first two teachers (Weinstein & Romano, 2015). The third teacher became an expert at helping students with disabilities master specific learning strategies. Most of the 25 students in Viviana’s class have recently emigrated from the Dominican Republic; the rest come from Nicaragua, Mexico, Puerto Rico, and Honduras. Even though the children speak little or no English when they begin school, by the time they leave in June, Viviana will have helped them master the normal first-grade curriculum for their district. She accomplishes this by teaching in Spanish early in the year to aid understanding and then gradually introducing English as the students are ready. Viviana does not want her students segregated or labeled as disadvantaged. She encourages them to take pride in their Spanish-speaking heritage and uses every available opportunity to support their developing English proficiency. Both Viviana’s expectations for her students and her commitment to them are high. She has an optimism that reveals her dedication: “I always hope that there’s somebody out there that I will reach and that I’ll make a difference” (Weinstein & Romano, 2015, p. 15). For Viviana, teaching is not just a job; it is a way of life.

A MULTILINGUAL FIRST GRADE.

A SUBURBAN FIFTH GRADE. Ken teaches fifth grade in a suburban school in central New Jersey. Students in the class represent a range of racial, ethnic, family income, and language backgrounds. Ken emphasizes “process writing.” His students complete first drafts, discuss them with others in the class, revise, edit, and “publish” their work. The students also keep daily journals and often use them to share personal concerns with Ken. They tell him of problems at home, fights, and fears; he always takes the time to respond in writing. Ken also uses technology to connect lessons to real life. Students learn about ocean ecosystems by using a special interactive software program. For social studies, the class plays two simulation games that focus on history. One is about coming of age in Native American cultures, and the other focuses on the colonization of America. Throughout the year, Ken is very interested in the social and emotional development of his students; he wants them to learn about responsibility and fairness as well as science and social studies. This concern is evident in the way he develops his class rules at the beginning of the year. Rather than specifying do’s and don’ts, Ken and his students devise a “Bill of Rights” for the class, describing the rights of the students. These rights cover most of the situations that might need a “rule.” TEACHING MATH TO STUDENTS WITH LEARNING DISABILITIES. The eighth-grade prealgebra class had 11 students with disabilities—nine with learning disabilities. While her co-teacher taught the math lesson, Joan Hamilton provided explicit instruction in learning strategies. For example, as the math teacher explained a problem, Joan drew a circle on the overhead projector, with the example problem in the middle, and then made notes on the outside of the circle about the steps to solve the problem. Students created their own graphic organizers following Joan’s lead. The next day during the homework discussion, students

5

6 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

Reflective Thoughtful and inventive. Reflective teachers think back over situations to analyze what they did and why and to consider how they might improve learning for their students.

returned to their circle graphic organizers to review and ask questions. The teachers retaught the steps of solving the problem as needed. Most of the students “made it through the material and on to algebra with a basic understanding of the concepts and of how to structure their notes and ask questions” (Hallahan et al., 2019, p. 130). What do you see in these three classrooms? The teachers are confident and committed to their students. They must deal with a wide range of student characteristics: different languages, different home situations, and different abilities and learning challenges. They must adapt instruction and assessment to students’ needs and teach their students “how to learn.” They must make the most abstract concepts, such as ecosystems, real and understandable for their particular students. The whole time these experts are navigating through the academic material, they also are taking care of the emotional needs of their students, propping up sagging self-esteem, and encouraging responsibility. If we followed these teachers from the first day of class, we would see that they carefully plan and teach the basic procedures for living and learning in their classes. They can efficiently collect and correct homework, regroup students, give directions, distribute materials, and deal with disruptions—and do all of this while also making a mental note to find out why one of their students is so tired. Finally, they are reflective—they constantly think back over situations to analyze what they did and why and to consider how they might improve learning for their students. And what about those students in classrooms today?

Students Today: Dramatic Diversity and Remarkable Technology Who are the students in American classrooms today? Here are a few statistics about the United States.

• About 25% of U.S. children under 18 have at least one immigrant parent, but in some states such as California, the number is closer to 50% (Urban Institute, 2019). By 2045, half of the U.S. population will be members of some minority group, with about 25% being Hispanic (Frey, 2018).

• Almost 15 million children—about 21% of all children—live in poverty, defined in 2020 by the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services as an income of $26,200 for a family of four ($32,750 in Alaska and $30,130 in Hawaii). And in the public schools, just over half the students qualify for free or reduced-cost lunches—a rough indicator of poverty (Child Trends Databank, 2019). The United States has the seventh-highest rate of child poverty among the 41 economically advantaged countries of the world; only Spain, Mexico, Bulgaria, Turkey, Israel, and Romania are worse. Iceland and the Scandinavian countries have the lowest rates of child poverty (Blazier, 2017).

• The poverty rate for Black and American Indian children in the United States is about 30%, while the rate for Hispanic children is about 24% (Children’s Defense Fund, 2020a).

• About 18% of American children have a mild to severe developmental disability such as speech and language impairments, intellectual disabilities, cerebral palsy, or autism. This number has been increasing since the early 2000s. More than half of these children spend most of their time in general education classes (Zablotsky et al., 2019).

• In 2018, for children ages birth to 17, 23% had parents who were divorced or separated, 8% were living with someone who had an alcohol or a drug problem, 7% had a parent who had served time in jail, and 7% lived with someone with a mental illness (Children’s Defense Fund, 2020b). It is clear that American society and schools are more diverse than ever. In contrast, because of the effects of mass media, these diverse students share many similarities today, particularly the fact that most are far more technologically literate than their teachers. For example:

• In 2017, infants to eight-year-olds spent an average of a little more than 2 hours each day with screen media, particularly mobile devices (Rideout, 2017). For 9- to 12-year-olds,

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

7

screen time outside school jumped to almost 5 hours, and for teens their average time is an amazing 7-plus hours. Smartphone ownership is up dramatically, as you probably know. In 2019, 19% of eight-year-olds owned smartphones. By age 18, that number was 91%, with ownership more than 80% from age 14 on (Common Sense Media, 2019). Today the numbers probably have increased. These statistics are dramatic but a bit impersonal. As a teacher, counselor, recreational worker, speech therapist, or family member, you will encounter real children. In this book, you will meet many individuals such as Josué, a bright first-grader whose first language is Spanish and who is struggling to care about learning to read in a language that offers only “run, Spot, run”; Alex, an 11-year-old who has created 10 languages and 30 or 40 alphabets; Jamie Foxx, a very bright third-grade student in a small Texas town whose teacher rewards him for working hard all week by letting him do stand-up comedy for the class on Fridays; Tracy, a failing high school student who does not understand why her study strategies are not working for her; Felipe, a fifth-grade boy from a Spanish-speaking family who is working to learn school subjects and make friends in a language that is new to him; Ternice, an outspoken Black girl in an urban middle school who is hiding her giftedness; Trevor, a second-grade student who has trouble with the meaning of symbol; Maya, the head of a popular clique and tormentor of the outcast Jasmine; Eliot, a bright sixth-grade student with severe learning disabilities; and Jessie, a student in a rural high school who just doesn’t seem to care about her sinking grade-point average or school in general. If your students are from low-income families, it is likely that their learning suffered more than that of students from higher-income families in the wake of the COVID pandemic. These students from less advantaged backgrounds might have more catching up to do, but it all depends on these individuals and their specific experiences (Kuhfeld et al., 2020). Even though students in classrooms are increasingly diverse in race, ethnicity, language, and economic level, teachers are much less diverse—the percentage of White teachers is increasing (now about 80%), while the percentage of Black teachers is falling, down to about 7%. Clearly, all teachers should know and be able to work effectively with all their students. Several clusters in this book are devoted to understanding these diverse students. In addition, many times within each cluster, we will explore student diversity and inclusion through research, cases, and practical applications.

Confidence in Every Context Schools are about teaching and learning; all other activities are secondary. But teaching and learning in the contexts just described can be challenging for both teachers and students. This book is about understanding the complex processes of development, learning, motivation, teaching, and assessment so that you can become a capable and confident teacher. Throughout this text, we—Anita Woolfolk and Ellen Usher—will share our professional and personal experiences. We see this book as an ongoing conversation with you. To learn more about us, see the “About Your Authors” section on page v. Here is the first example of many more shared experiences, this time from Anita: Much of my own research has focused on teachers’ sense of efficacy, defined as a teacher’s belief that he or she can reach even difficult students to help them learn. This confident belief appears to be one of the few personal characteristics of teachers that predict student achievement (Çakıroğlu et al., 2012; Klassen & Tze, 2014; Woolfolk Hoy, in press; Zee & Kooman, 2016). Teachers with a high sense of efficacy work harder and persist longer even when students are difficult to teach, in part because these teachers believe in themselves and in their students. Also, they are less likely to experience burnout and more likely to be satisfied with their jobs (Fernet et al., 2012; Fives et al., 2007; Klassen & Chiu, 2010). If you would like to take the teacher self-efficacy survey that I developed, go to my website for the measure and scoring instructions (https://u.osu.edu/hoy.17/research/instruments/#Sense). I have found that prospective teachers tend to increase in their personal sense of efficacy as a consequence of completing student teaching. But sense of efficacy may decline after the first year as a teacher, perhaps because the support that was provided during student teaching is gone (Taylor et al., 2019; Woolfolk Hoy & Burke-Spero, 2005). Teachers’ sense of efficacy is

Teachers’ sense of efficacy Teachers’ beliefs about how capable they are of reaching even the most difficult students and helping them learn.

8 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology higher in schools where the other teachers and administrators have high expectations for students and the teachers receive help from their principals in solving instructional and management problems (Capa, 2005). Efficacy grows from real success with students, not just from the cheerleading of professors and colleagues. Any experience or training that helps you succeed in the day-to-day tasks of teaching will give you a foundation for developing a sense of efficacy in your career. This book was written to provide the knowledge and skills that form a solid foundation for an authentic sense of efficacy in teaching.

High Expectations for Teachers and Students Teachers and students are affected by the expectations and requirements of their school districts, which are themselves influenced by state and national education policies. For example, in 2002, the No Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act became a federal law. NCLB required that all students reach proficiency by the end of the 2013–2014 school year, based on standardized test scores. You probably noticed—that did not happen. For a while, NCLB dominated education. Testing expanded. Schools and teachers were penalized if they did not perform. Federal money could be taken away, teachers and principals could be fired, and schools could be converted to charter schools or closed. As you can imagine or may have experienced yourself, such high-stakes penalties pushed teachers and schools to “teach to the test” or worse. The curriculum narrowed, and much time was spent on drill and practice—many teachers we worked with said teaching just wasn’t fun anymore (Davidson et al., 2015; Meens & Howe, 2015; Strauss, 2015). All in all, NCLB requirements were widely criticized as “blunt instruments, generating inaccurate performance results, perverse incentives, and unintended negative consequences” (Hopkins et al., 2013, p. 101). In general, math achievement did increase with NCLB, but over time student engagement declined—and student engagement is a powerful link to learning. Students can’t learn what they ignore (Markowitz, 2018). In 2015, NCLB was replaced with the Every Student Succeeds Act (ESSA). ESSA dropped the requirement for proficiency for all students by a certain date and returned most control to the states to set standards and develop interventions. For example, schools must test the same subjects in the same grades, as specified in NCLB, and at least 95% of students must participate in the testing. But the local districts now can decide when to test, whether to break one big test into several smaller tests, and even how to find better tests that really capture important student learning. At least one additional measure of school quality such as school climate and safety or student engagement must be included, along with measures of progress toward English language proficiency for English learners (Korte, 2015). Even after ESSA, many excellent teachers still believe they are spending too much time preparing for tests and not enough time supporting student learning in subjects not tested, such as social studies, art, music, physical education, and technology (Cusick, 2014). Which leads to another high expectation for today’s teachers—teaching the whole child.

Social and emotional learning (SEL) The process of integrating thinking, emotion, and behavior into teaching and learning so that adults and children develop skills to be aware of themselves and others, learn to manage their own and others’ emotions and behavior, make responsible decisions, and build positive relationships.

Teaching the Whole Child: Social and Emotional Learning In the preceding paragraphs, you may have noticed an interest in non-academic outcomes such as school climate or student engagement and worries about subjects left behind such as art, music, or physical education. These concerns are consistent with a larger emphasis on social and emotional learning (SEL) and teaching the whole child, concerns shared by Viviana, Ken, and Joan, the expert teachers we described earlier. Social and emotional learning is: the process of integrating cognition, emotion, and behavior into teaching and learning such that adults and children build self- and social awareness skills, learn to manage their own and others’ emotions and behavior, make responsible decisions, and build positive relationships . . . (Brackett et al., 2019, p. 144).

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

Social and emotional learning gained attention in 1994 when a group of educators, researchers, and psychologists formed the Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning (CASEL). Their mission is to be “a trusted source for knowledge about high-quality, evidence-based social and emotional learning” and to encourage schools “to educate the whole child, equipping students for success in school and in life” (CASEL, 2020; http://casel.org). Since the founding of CASEL, interest in SEL has grown. Programs and interventions for schools and classrooms have expanded—see the CASEL website for many excellent examples (http://casel.org). Today, many psychologists believe schools can promote students’ mental health and academic learning by incorporating these SEL programs and practices (Schonert-Reichl, 2019). In fact, Educational Psychologist, a premier journal in our field, devoted an entire issue to this topic in 2019, with articles on theory, research, school interventions, assessment, teaching practices, social justice, and neurobiology (Wentzel, 2019). Does SEL make a difference? There is good evidence that SEL can have positive effects on students’ academic achievement and social behaviors (Hart et al., 2020). For example, Rebecca Taylor and her colleagues (2017) published a meta-analysis (an integration and summary of many individual studies) on the effects of 82 different SEL programs involving 97,000 students from kindergarten through high school. Students were followed from at least six months and up to 18 years after the programs ended. The results of the meta-analysis were impressive. In the eight studies that measured academic achievement, 3.5 years after the programs ended, the average achievement of students in SEL programs was 13 percentile points higher than that of students in the control groups. (We explain percentiles in Cluster 15, but for now, let’s just say this is pretty good.) In other studies, students in SEL programs had lasting decreases in behavior problems, less emotional distress, less drug use, and increases in high school and college graduation rates. In a more recent review of research, Stephanie Jones and her colleagues (2019) concluded that individual SEL interventions show the largest gains for students with the greatest number of risks and needs. When what is measured in the studies is closely matched to the goals of the program, effects are more positive as well. By the way, this matching principle is important in many areas of teaching. School achievement tests and classroom tests should measure the learning goals and objectives taught. Don’t teach one thing and test another. Upcoming clusters in this book will describe how social and emotional learning is related to the brain, cognitive development and learning, social development, motivation, and teaching. For now, let’s consider one example of SEL in action.

RESEARCH ON SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL LEARNING.

PATHS: AN APPROACH TO SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL LEARNING. SEL interventions can be as small as kernels, strategies that teach one skill, such as deep breathing to manage anger; larger classroom practices and kits that teach many skills, such as PATHS (Promoting Alternative THinking Strategies); or even more extensive, whole-school programs that engage students, parents, teachers, and administrators, such as RULER, which stands for Recognizing, Understanding, Labeling, Expressing, and Regulating emotions (Brackett et al., 2019; Jones et al., 2017, 2019). Let’s consider middle-level curriculum interventions, with PATHS as an example, because these are interventions you could encounter as a teacher. The PATHS curriculum is used in more than 3,000 kindergarten through sixth-grade classrooms in the United States and another 500 around the world (Domitrovich et al., 2019). The goal of PATHS is to develop students’ social and emotional skills in self-control and emotional regulation, attention, communication, and problem solving. The curriculum has modules for pre-K through grade 5. Each module includes teaching resources such as a curriculum manual, instructor’s manual, posters and other visual aids (charts, stickers, cards, etc.), puppets for young children, novels for older children, and family communication materials (https://pathsprogram.com/overview). Let’s look inside the curriculum for grade 5. One component is centered around four novels: Bridge to Terabithia by Katherine Paterson, Maniac Magee by Jerry Spinelli, Number the

Meta-analysis An integration and summary of many individual studies to synthesize the outcomes into one result that characterizes the findings from the studies.

9

10 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology Stars by Lois Lowry, and Hatchet by Gary Paulsen. In the lesson on the first three chapters of Maniac Magee, the students read the chapters aloud and then discuss such questions as “Why was Amanda Beale suspicious of Jeffrey Magee?” (Amanda’s family is Black, and Jeffrey is White, so maybe he is not from her part of town. Jeffrey wants to borrow one of Amanda’s dearest possessions, a book. Will he return it?) Students might talk about inequality, racism, trust, sharing, or empathy. After the lesson, students may journal and then share their ideas about such topics as “Amanda kept her books in a suitcase to protect them—they were her most important possessions. What would you put into a suitcase—what are your most important possessions? Why?” Other lesson follow-ups might introduce the idea of character analysis, so the PATHS curriculum supports academic goals as well (https://pathsprogram .com/grade-5). Explore the PATHS website for more information. As you will see in future clusters, some applications of SEL have been criticized for viewing the social skills of students of color through a deficient lens, overemphasizing self-regulation and underemphasizing student agency to resist injustice and avoiding the difficult issues that true social awareness and empathy would open up for students. One goal of SEL should be to create caring communities that tackle courageous conversations about injustice, hate, violence, and inequality, not just anger management or decision making (Simmons, 2019). Cluster 4 explores these possibilities. LIVING WITH SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL TRAUMA. Almost half of all elementary school students in the United States have experienced or witnessed traumatic events (Koslouski & Stark, 2021). An entire issue of the American Psychologist was devoted to adverse childhood experiences and trauma (Portwood et al., 2021). What kinds of traumas do students experience? The last few years provide so many examples: hurricanes and tornadoes that rip through communities, floods and fires that destroy homes, lockdowns from pandemic viruses, undrinkable water, police brutality replayed endlessly in videos, protests and counterprotests, attacks on the U.S. Capitol, televised images of collapsing buildings, and mass shootings, including in many schools. But trauma has always been in children’s lives in the form of child abuse, divorce, deaths, parental drug and alcohol addictions, bullying, food insecurity, community violence, medical procedures, and even serious accidents. Teachers may not be aware that many students show up in their classrooms straining under the weight of “invisible backpacks” filled with traumatic burdens. Students living in poverty, students facing racism every day, students with disabilities, and immigrant students are especially vulnerable (Santiago et al., 2018). Because trauma can affect so many aspects of teachers’ and students’ lives, including brain development and learning, we will revisit this topic in several upcoming clusters. The educational challenges, health care inequalities, and economic devastation that came with the coronavirus pandemic certainly set the stage for increased trauma among students and teachers. No matter what policies your school or the government adopts, what trends affect schools, or what traumas your students experience, capable and confident teachers will be required. But is that true? Do teachers really make a difference? Good question.

Do Teachers Make a Difference? We usually consider school achievement to be an outcome—a goal for our students. But doing well in school can also cause other outcomes. In fact, there is some evidence that education and doing well in school are related to well-being and to high IQ scores in adulthood (Ritchie & Tucker-Drob, 2018; Tomsaik et al., 2019). So education can make a difference, but what about teaching, and how would you decide? Perhaps one of your teachers influenced your decision to become an educator. Even if you had such a teacher, and we hope you did, one of the purposes of educational psychology in general and this text in particular is to go beyond individual experiences and testimonies, powerful as they are, to examine larger groups. The results of many large-group studies speak to the power of teachers in the lives of students, as you will see next.

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology TEACHER–STUDENT RELATIONSHIPS. Bridgett Hamre and Robert Pianta (2001) monitored all the children who entered kindergarten one year in a small school district and continued to do so in that district through the eighth grade. The researchers concluded that the quality of the teacher–student relationship in kindergarten (defined in terms of level of conflict with the child, the child’s dependency on the teacher, and the teacher’s affection for the child) predicted several academic and behavioral outcomes through the eighth grade, particularly for students with many behavior problems. Even when the gender, ethnicity, cognitive ability, and behavior ratings of the student were accounted for, the relationship with the teacher still predicted aspects of school success. So students with significant behavior problems in the early years are less likely to have problems later in school if their first teachers are sensitive to their needs and provide frequent, consistent feedback. Of course, forming positive relationships with challenging students isn’t easy. When students act out, teachers can respond negatively, and the cycle of student behavior problems and teacher conflict continues. Forming positive relationships with students also means breaking the cycle of conflict by teaching the students better ways to deal with anger and frustration—the role of social and emotional learning again (de Jong et al., 2018). The connection between teacher relationships and student outcomes is widespread. Pianta’s research team has documented the importance of teacher–student relationships in many other studies that followed students for years (e.g., Ansari et al., 2020). Daniel Quin (2017) reviewed 46 studies, including studies that followed students over time, and reached similar conclusions—better teacher–student relationships predicted student engagement. In Germany, secondary students who reported higher levels of teacher support also were more satisfied with school, and in the United States, positive teacher relationships predicted social-emotional development and reading achievement for elementary school students (Aldrup et al., 2018; Rucinski et al., 2018). And as you will learn in Clusters 13 and 14, teacher warmth is one element of effective teaching (Sandilos et al., 2019). Positive relationships with students are important for teachers as well. Remember our expert teacher Ken, who always took the time to respond to students’ personal concerns shared in their journals? He would not be surprised that teachers who have close relationships with their students experience high levels of accomplishment and that teachers who report more conflict in student relationships feel more emotional exhaustion and burnout (Corbin et al., 2019). So evidence is mounting for a strong association between the quality of teacher–student relationships and important outcomes for both students and teachers.

Several years ago, in a widely publicized study, researchers examined how students are affected by having several effective or ineffective teachers in a row (Sanders & Rivers, 1996). They looked at fifth-graders in two large metropolitan school systems in Tennessee. Students who had highly effective teachers for third, fourth, and fifth grades scored at the 83rd percentile on average on a standardized mathematics achievement test in one district and at the 96th percentile in the other district (99th percentile is the highest possible score). In contrast, students who had the least effective teachers 3 years in a row averaged at the 29th percentile in math achievement in one district and 44th percentile in the other—a difference of more than 50 percentile points in both cases! Students who had average teachers or a mixture of teachers with low, average, and high effectiveness for the 3 years had math scores between these extremes. Sanders and Rivers concluded that the best teachers encouraged good to excellent gains in achievement for all students, but lowerachieving students were the first to benefit from good teaching. The effects of teaching were cumulative and residual; that is, better teaching in a later grade can partially make up for less effective teaching in earlier grades but cannot erase all the deficits traced to poor teachers (Hanushek et al., 2005; Rivkin et al., 2001). Effective teachers who establish positive relationships with their students appear to be a powerful force in those students’ lives. Students who have problems seem to benefit the most from good teaching. So an important question is “What makes a teacher effective? What is good teaching?”

THE COST OF POOR TEACHING.

11

12 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Teacher Professionalism (IV, A2) Begin your own development by reading educational publications. One widely read periodical is Education Week. You can access it online at https://www.edweek .org.

What Is Good Teaching? Educators, psychologists, philosophers, novelists, journalists, filmmakers, mathematicians, scientists, historians, policy makers, and parents, to name only a few groups, have examined this question; there are hundreds of answers. And good teaching is not confined to classrooms. It occurs in homes and hospitals, museums and sales meetings, therapists’ offices, and summer camps. In this book we are primarily concerned with teaching in classrooms, but much of what you will learn applies to other settings as well. So is good teaching science or art? The application of research-based theories or the creative invention of specific practices? Is a good teacher an expert explainer (“a sage on the stage”) or a great coach (“a guide by the side”)? These debates have raged for years. In your other education classes, you probably will encounter criticisms of the scientific, teacher-centered sages. You will be encouraged to be inventive, student-centered guides. But beware of either/or choices. Teachers must be both knowledgeable and inventive. They must have some basic research-based routines for managing classes, but they must also be willing and able to break from the routine when the situation calls for change. They must know the research on student development, and they also need to know their own particular students, who are unique combinations of cultures, genders, and geographies. Personally, we hope you all become teachers who are both sages and guides, wherever you stand. Another answer to “What is good teaching?” involves considering what different models and frameworks for teaching have to offer. We look at them next.

Models of Good Teaching We will briefly examine three frameworks to help answer the question “What is good teaching?” Another reason to consider these models is that when you become a teacher, you might be evaluated based on one of these approaches or something like them—teacher evaluation is a very hot topic these days! In fact, the Every Student Succeeds Act requires teacher evaluation systems to use multiple measures of effectiveness. We will look at Charlotte Danielson’s Framework for Teaching, the high-leverage practices identified by TeachingWorks at the University of Michigan, and the CLASS framework developed by Robert Pianta and his colleagues (2008b). The Framework for Teaching was first published in 1996 and has been revised three times since then, most recently in 2013 (see http://danielsongroup.org for information about Charlotte Danielson and the Framework for Teaching). According to Danielson (2013):

DANIELSON’S FRAMEWORK FOR TEACHING.

The Framework for Teaching identifies those aspects of a teacher’s responsibilities that have been documented through empirical studies and theoretical research as promoting improved student learning. While the Framework is not the only possible description of practice, these responsibilities seek to define what teachers should know and be able to do in the exercise of their profession. (p. 1) Danielson’s Framework has four domains or areas of responsibility: Planning and Preparation, Classroom Environment, Instruction, and Professional Responsibilities. Each domain is further divided into five or six components, making a total of 22 components for the entire framework. For example, Domain 1: Planning and Preparation, is divided into six components:

1a Demonstrating knowledge of content and pedagogy 1b Demonstrating knowledge of students 1c Setting instructional objectives 1d Demonstrating knowledge of resources 1e Designing coherent instruction 1f Designing student assessments

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

When the Framework is used for teacher evaluation, each of these 22 components is further divided into elements (76 in all), and several indicators are specified for each component. For example, component 1b, demonstrating knowledge of students, includes the elements describing knowledge of:

• Child and adolescent development. • The learning process. • Students’ skills, knowledge, and language proficiency. • Students’ interests and cultural heritage. • Students’ special needs. Indicators of this knowledge of students include the formal and informal information about students that the teacher gathers when planning instruction, the students’ interests and needs the teacher identifies, the teacher’s participation in community cultural events, opportunities the teacher has designed for families to share their cultural heritages, and any databases the teacher has for students with special needs (Danielson, 2013). The evaluation system further defines four levels of proficiency for each of the 22 components—unsatisfactory, basic, proficient, and distinguished—with a definition, critical attributes, and possible examples of what each level might look like in action. Two examples of distinguished knowledge of students are a teacher who plans lessons with three different follow-up activities designed to match different students’ abilities and a teacher who attends a local Mexican heritage event to meet members of her students’ extended families. Many other examples are possible, but these two give a sense of distinguished knowledge of students (component 1b). You can see that it would take extensive training to use this framework well for teacher evaluation. When you become a teacher, you may learn more about this concept of good teaching because your school district is using it—this framework forms the basis of teacher evaluation in several states. For now, be assured that you will gain knowledge and skills in all 22 components in this text. For example, you will gain knowledge of students (component 1b) in Clusters 2 through 6. TEACHINGWORKS. TeachingWorks is a national project based at the University of Michigan that is dedicated to improving teaching practice. Project members working with experienced teachers have identified 19 high-leverage teaching practices, defined as the basic fundamentals of teaching—actions that are central to teaching and useful across most grade levels, academic subjects, and teaching situations (see Table 1.1 on the next page). Again, you will develop skills and knowledge about all of these practices in this text. (For a more complete description of the 19 high-leverage practices, see https://library.teachingworks .org/curriculum-resources/high-leverage-practices/.) When you compare the high-leverage practices in Table 1.1 with the Danielson components listed earlier, do you see similarities and overlaps?

An important conception of good teaching is based on a large-scale program of longitudinal research by Robert Pianta and his colleagues (Allen et al., 2013; Crosnoe et al., 2010; Hafen et al., 2012; Jerome et al., 2009; Luckner & Pianta, 2011; Pianta et al., 2008a, 2008b). Pianta’s work has identified three domains of teacher–student interactions in the classroom that are related to development and learning for preschool through high school students, regardless of where the students live or their families’ incomes, as you can see in Table 1.2 on the next page. Using these three domains, the researchers have also developed a set of classroom observation instruments called CLASS—for the Classroom Assessment Scoring System (https://teachstone.com/class). Schools in your district might use these instruments for class observation or professional development. The affective domain in Pianta’s model is teacher emotional support, similar to teacher warmth and enthusiasm identified in early research on teacher effectiveness and described in Cluster 14. The cognitive domain is instructional support, which includes different dimensions, depending on the age of the student. For example, language modeling is a dimension of instructional support through early elementary school, but content understanding, THE CLASS MODEL.

13

14 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

Table 1.1 TeachingWorks 19 High-Leverage Teaching Practices These practices are based on research evidence, the wisdom of practice, and logic. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19.

Leading a group discussion Explaining and modeling content, practices, and strategies Eliciting and interpreting individual students’ thinking Diagnosing particular common patterns of student thinking and development in a subject-matter domain Implementing norms and routines for classroom discourse and work Coordinating and adjusting instruction during a lesson Specifying and reinforcing productive student behavior Implementing organizational routines Setting up and managing small-group work Building respectful relationships with students Talking about a student with parents or other caregivers Learning about students’ cultural, religious, family, intellectual, and personal experiences and resources for use in instruction Setting long- and short-term learning goals for students Designing single lessons and sequences of lessons Checking student understanding during and at the conclusion of lessons Selecting and designing formal assessments of student learning Interpreting the results of student work, including routine assignments, quizzes, tests, projects, and standardized assessments Providing oral and written feedback to students Analyzing instruction for the purpose of improving it

Source: Reprinted with permission from TeachingWorks (2014), High-leverage practices. Retrieved from http://www.teachingworks.org/ work-of-teaching/high-leverage-practices.

Table 1.2 A Model of Teacher–Student Interactions in Early Elementary Classrooms ASPECT OF TEACHING

DOMAIN OF TEACHER–STUDENT INTERACTIONS

DIMENSIONS

DEFINITIONS AND EXAMPLES

Affective

Emotional Support

Positive Climate

Warmth, mutual respect, positive emotional connections between teacher and students

Negative Climate (negative predictor of learning)

Disrespect, anger, hostility

Teacher Sensitivity

Consistency and effectiveness in responding to students’ academic and emotional needs

Regard for Students’ Perspectives

Activities that encourage student autonomy and emphasize students’ interests, motivations, and points of view

Concept Development

Activities and discussion that promote higher-order thinking skills and cognition

Quality of Feedback

Consistency in providing specific, process-oriented feedback and back-and-forth exchanges to extend students’ learning

Language Modeling

Teachers’ modeling more complex language for students in conversations with them and encouraging student talk

Cognitive

Instructional Support

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

ASPECT OF TEACHING Behavioral

DOMAIN OF TEACHER–STUDENT INTERACTIONS Classroom Organization

DIMENSIONS

DEFINITIONS AND EXAMPLES

Behavior Management

Teachers’ effectiveness in monitoring, preventing, and redirecting misbehavior

Productivity

How consistently learning is maximized with clear activities and routines, teacher preparation, efficient transitions, and minimal disruptions

Instructional Learning Formats

How well materials, modalities, and activities are used to engage students in learning

15

Source: Based on Brown et al. (2010). Improving classroom quality: Teacher influences and experimental impacts of the 4Rs program. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 153–167 and Gregory et al. (2017). My teaching partner-secondary: A video-based coaching model. Theory into Practice, 56(1), 38–45.

analysis, and inquiry become important in the later grades. Quality of feedback is included for every grade. The third domain is classroom organization, which includes behavioral concerns such as classroom and lesson management, with clear activities and routines that make more time for student learning and are really engaging—we explore these critical organizational dimensions of teaching in depth in Cluster 13. Is all this talk about expert teachers and effective teaching making you a little nervous? Viviana, Ken, and Joan are experts at the science and art of teaching, but they have years of experience. What about you?

Beginning Teachers Stop & Think Imagine walking into your first day of teaching. List the concerns, fears, and worries you have. What assets do you bring to the job? What would build your confidence to teach? Beginning teachers everywhere share many concerns, including maintaining classroom discipline, motivating students, meeting students’ different needs, evaluating students’ work, dealing with parents, getting along with other teachers, being evaluated by supervisors, and dealing with paperwork and lesson planning (Brichinall et al., 2019; Melnick & Meister, 2008). Many teachers also experience what has been called “reality shock” when they take their first job because they really cannot ease into their responsibilities. On the first day of their first job, beginning teachers face the same tasks as teachers with years of experience. Student teaching, while a critical element, does not really prepare prospective teachers for starting off a school year with a new class. If you listed any of these concerns in your response to the Stop & Think question, you shouldn’t be troubled. They come with the job of being a beginning teacher (Armstrong, 2018; Borko & Putnam, 1996). With experience, hard work, and good support, seasoned teachers can focus on their students’ needs and judge their own success by looking at their students’ accomplishments. One experienced teacher described the shift from concerns about yourself to concerns about your students in this way: “The difference between a beginning teacher and an experienced one is that the beginning teacher asks, ‘How am I doing?’ and the experienced teacher asks, ‘How are the children doing?’” (Codell, 2001, p. 191). But here is one study that ought to give you confidence, even as a beginner: When Linda Graham and her colleagues (2020) analyzed CLASS observation data from 80 elementary grade teachers, they concluded, “Beginning teachers are doing as well or better than teachers with more years of experience, but . . . the overall quality of teaching could be higher” (p. 8). So enter your teaching career with confidence and strive to get even better

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Teacher Professionalism (IV, A1) Your professional growth relies on your becoming a member of a community of practice. The national organizations listed here have hundreds of affiliations and chapters across the country, with regular conferences, conventions, and meetings to advance instruction in their areas. Take a look at their websites to get a feel for their approaches to issues related to professionalism.

• • • • •

National Council of Teachers of English (http://ncte.org) International Literacy Association (http://reading.org) National Science Teachers Association (http://www.nsta.org) National Council for the Social Studies (http://www .socialstudies.org) National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (http://www .nctm.org)

16 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

Table 1.3 Advice for Student Teachers from Their Students The students in Ms. Amato’s first-grade class gave this advice as a present to their student teacher on her last day. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13.

Teach us as much as you can. Give us homework. Help us when we have problems with our work. Help us to do the right thing. Help us make a family in school. Read books to us. Teach us to read. Help us write about faraway places. Give us lots of compliments, like “Oh, that’s so beautiful.” Smile at us. Take us for walks and on trips. Respect us. Help us get our education.

Source: Nieto, Sonia, Affirming diversity: The sociopolitical context of multicultural education, 4th ed., © 2004. Reprinted and Electronically reproduced by permission of Pearson Education, Inc. Upper Saddle River, New Jersey.

every year. Our goal in writing this book is to give you the foundation for becoming an expert as you gain experience. One thing experts do is listen to their students. Table 1.3 shows some advice a first-grade class gave to their student teacher: It looks like the students know about good teaching, too. We began this cluster claiming that educational psychology is the one of the most important courses you will take. OK, maybe we are a bit biased—we have been teaching the subject for a combined total of more than 50 years! So let us tell you more about our favorite topic.

Module 1 Summary Learning and Teaching Today (pp. 4–11) What are classrooms like today? About 25% of U.S. children under 18 are living in immigrant families. By 2045, half of the U.S. population will be members of some minority group, with about 25% being Hispanic. Around 21% of American children currently live in poverty. More than half of school-age students with disabilities receive most of their education in general education classrooms. Even though students in classrooms are increasingly diverse in race, ethnicity, language, and economic level, teachers are much less diverse—the percentage of White teachers is increasing, while the percentage of Black teachers is falling. This book is about understanding the complex processes of development, learning, motivation, teaching, and assessment so that you can become a capable and confident teacher with a high but authentic sense of efficacy. What are NCLB and ESSA? The No Child Left Behind Act (NCLB) of 2002 required extensive standardized achievement testing. The law also required all students in the schools to reach full proficiency in these subjects by the end of the 2013–2014 school year; it didn’t happen. Largely because there were major penalties for schools that did not perform well, several negative consequences followed in the wake of the tests—teaching to the test, narrowing the curriculum to a few subjects, driving teachers out of the classroom. The Every Student Succeeds Act (ESSA), which replaced NCLB, returned most of the control to the states. Testing in the same grades and subjects still takes place, but states and local schools decide when and how to test and how to intervene in the lowest-performing schools. The states must use evidence-based practices to intervene in

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

failing schools. ESSA also supports the establishment of teacher education academies outside colleges and universities. What is SEL––social-emotional learning? SEL is learning to understand and manage emotions, set and achieve positive goals, feel and show empathy for others, establish and maintain positive relationships, and make responsible decisions. The Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning (CASEL) provides resources and information for educators. There is good evidence that SEL can have positive effects on students’ academic achievements and social behaviors. Teachers can introduce specific strategies or larger curriculum programs such as PATHS. Even-more-comprehensive curricular programs can support both academic and social-emotional objectives. Important social and emotional concerns include the traumatic events that many students have experienced or witnessed. What evidence is there that teachers make a difference? Several studies speak to the power of teachers in the lives of students. The first found that the quality of the teacher–student relationship in kindergarten predicted several aspects of school success through the eighth grade. The second study found similar results for students from preschool through fifth grade, a finding confirmed by almost 100 students in countries around the world. The third study examined math achievement for students in two large school districts as they moved through third, fourth, and fifth grades. Again, the quality of the teacher made a difference: Students who had three high-quality teachers in a row were way ahead of peers who spent 1 or more years with less-competent teachers. In a study that followed children from third through fifth grades, two factors helped children with lower skills in mathematics begin to close the achievement gap: higher-level (not just basic skills) instruction and positive relationships with teachers.

What Is Good Teaching? (pp. 12–16) Teachers must be both knowledgeable and inventive. They must be able to use a range of strategies, and they must also be capable of inventing new strategies. They must have some basic research-based routines for managing classes, but they must also be willing and able to break from the routine when the situation calls for change. They must know the research on student development, and they also need to know their own particular students, who are unique combinations of culture, gender, and geography. What are some research-based models of effective teaching? Charlotte Danielson describes a Framework for Teaching, which has 22 components organized into four domains or areas of teaching responsibility: Planning and Preparation, Classroom Environment, Instruction, and Professional Responsibilities. This framework is the basis for a widely used system of teacher evaluation. TeachingWorks, a national project based at the University of Michigan and dedicated to improving teaching practice, has identified 19 high-leverage teaching practices, defined as actions that are central to teaching and useful across most grade levels, academic subjects, and teaching situations. Finally, a program of large-scale, longitudinal research has identified three aspects of classroom climate that are related to the development and learning of preschool and elementary school students. These three dimensions are consistent with the characteristics of teachers identified in earlier research on teaching and cover affective, behavioral, and cognitive dimensions. The affective dimension is teacher emotional support, similar to teacher warmth and enthusiasm identified in early research. The cognitive dimension is instructional support, which includes concept development (activities and discussions that promote students’ higher-order thinking) and quality feedback that is specific and focused on the learning process. The third dimension is classroom organization, which includes behavioral concerns such as classroom and lesson management with clear activities and routines that make more time for learning and really engage students. What are the concerns of beginning teachers? Learning to teach is a gradual process. The concerns and problems of teachers change as they grow in their ability. During the beginning years, attention tends to be focused on maintaining discipline, motivating students, accommodating differences among students, evaluating students’ work, dealing with parents and paperwork,

17

18 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology being evaluated by supervisors, and getting along with other teachers. Even with these concerns, many beginning teachers bring creativity and energy to their teaching and improve every year. The more experienced teacher can move on to concerns about professional growth and effectiveness in teaching a wide range of students.

MODULE 2

Research and Theory in Educational Psychology

Learning Objective 1.3 Describe the methods used to conduct research in the field of educational psychology and the kinds of questions each method can address. Learning Objective 1.4 Recognize how theories and research in development and learning are related to educational practice.

The Role of Educational Psychology A quick look at history shows that educational psychology and teaching have been closely linked since the beginning. The founders of psychology in the United States believed education was an important arena for investigation (Alexander, 2018). At Harvard in 1890, William James founded the field of psychology and developed a lecture series for teachers titled Talks to Teachers about Psychology (it’s worth a read, by the way). These lectures were given in summer schools for teachers around the country and then published in 1899. James’s student, G. Stanley Hall, founded the American Psychological Association. Teachers helped him collect data for his dissertation about children’s understandings of the world. Hall encouraged teachers to make detailed observations to study their students’ development—as his mother had done when she was a teacher. Hall’s student John Dewey founded the Laboratory School at the University of Chicago and is considered the father of the progressive education movement. Another of William James’s students, E. L. Thorndike, wrote the first educational psychology text in 1903 and founded the Journal of Educational Psychology in 1910 (Berliner, 2006; Hilgard, 1996; Pajares, 2003). We go way back!

Educational Psychology Today Educational psychology The discipline concerned with teaching and learning processes; applies the methods and theories of psychology and has its own as well.

What is educational psychology today? The view generally accepted is that educational psychology is a distinct discipline with its own theories, research methods, problems, and techniques. Educational psychologists do research on learning and teaching and, at the same time, work to improve educational policy and practice (Anderman, 2011). To understand as much as possible about learning and teaching, educational psychologists examine what happens when someone (a teacher or parent or software designer) teaches something (math or weaving or dancing) to someone else (student or coworker or team) in some setting (classroom or theater or gym) (Berliner, 2006). So educational psychologists study child and adolescent development; learning and motivation—including how people learn different academic subjects such as reading or mathematics; social and cultural influences on learning; teaching and teachers; and assessment, including testing (Alexander & Winne, 2006). But even with all this research on so many topics, are the findings of educational psychologists really that helpful for teachers? After all, most teaching is just common sense, isn’t it? Let’s take a few minutes to examine these questions.

Is It Just Common Sense? In many cases, the principles set forth by educational psychologists—after spending much thought, time, and money for research—sound pathetically obvious. People are tempted to say, and usually do say, “Everyone knows that!” Consider these examples.

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

Students have different learning styles that are dominated by particular senses (visual, auditory, etc.), and they will learn best when they receive information in their preferred learning style.

LEARNING STYLES.

Commonsense Answer. Of course, we are all different. Some of us are visual learners, and some have to hear information to learn. Most people can describe their own learning style and learn best using that style. ANSWER BASED ON RESEARCH. This is one of the most persistent myths in education— a “zombie” belief that just won’t die. In fact, Kelly Macdonald and her colleagues (2017) found that 93% of the general public and 76% of educators believed this myth even though study after study has shown that students do not learn more when taught in their preferred style (Pashler et al., 2009, Willingham et al., 2015). There are two underlying truths that encourage the persistence of this myth: People do have a preference for how to receive information, and teachers do achieve better results if they present information in multiple sensory modes. So this myth is made up of a seed of facts, some emotional bias, and just plain wishful thinking that a simple key to good teaching exists. Unfortunately, what you prefer—say, ice cream and cake as your dinner every night—is not always good for you. People simply do not learn best when taught in their preferred style (Scudellari, 2015). OK—we sense your skepticism. In Cluster 5, we will dig deeper into the question of learning styles.

Should a school encourage exceptionally bright students to skip grades or to enter college early?

SKIPPING GRADES.

Commonsense Answer. No! Very intelligent students who are several years younger than their classmates are likely to be social misfits. They are neither physically nor emotionally ready for dealing with older students and would be miserable in the social situations that are so important in school, especially in the later grades. Maybe. In the report A Nation Empowered: Evidence Trumps the Excuses Holding Back America’s Brightest Students, the authors note, “Extensive research has indicated that acceleration has positive effects on the academic as well as the affective lives of students,” but “decisions about individual students must be based on more than research” (Assouline et al., 2015, p. 2). One example of positive long-term effects is that mathematically talented students who skipped grades in elementary or secondary school were more likely to go on to earn advanced degrees and publish widely cited articles in scientific journals (Park et al., 2013). Whether acceleration is the best solution for a particular student depends on many specific individual characteristics, including the intelligence and maturity of the student as well as the other available options. For some students, moving quickly through the material and working in advanced courses with older students can be a very positive experience (Kretschmann et al., 2014). See Cluster 5 for more on adapting teaching to students’ abilities. ANSWER BASED ON RESEARCH.

Does giving students more control over their own learning— more choices—help them learn? STUDENTS IN CONTROL.

Commonsense Answer. Of course! Students who choose their own learning materials and tasks will be more engaged and thus learn more. Not so fast! Sometimes giving students more control and choice can support learning, but many times it doesn’t. For example, giving lower-ability students choice in learning tasks sometimes means the students just keep practicing what they already do well instead of tackling tougher assignments. This happened when hairdressing students were given choices. The lower-ability students kept practicing easy tasks such as washing hair but were reluctant to try more difficult tasks such as giving permanents. When they developed portfolios to monitor their progress and received regular coaching

ANSWER BASED ON RESEARCH.

19

20 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology and advice from their teachers, the students made better choices—so guided choice and some teacher control may be useful in some situations (Kicken et al., 2009). OBVIOUS ANSWERS? Years ago, Lily Wong (1987) demonstrated that just seeing research results in writing can make them seem obvious. She selected 12 findings from research on teaching. She presented six of the findings in their correct form and six in exactly the opposite form to both college students and experienced teachers. Both the college students and the teachers rated about half of the wrong findings as “obviously” correct. Jeanne Oakes has an idea about why “obvious answers” are so powerful: “Everybody’s firsthand schooling experience often makes research seem irrelevant—that it unnecessarily documents what everybody already knows or unhelpfully contradicts what’s obviously true” (2017, p. 94). Paul Kirschner and Joren van Merriënboer (2013) made a similar point when they challenged several “urban legends” in education, including the assertion that learners (like the hairdressing students just described) know best how to learn. These current, strongly held beliefs about students as self-educating digital natives who can multitask, have unique learning styles, and always make good choices about how to learn have no strong basis in research, but still they are embraced. You might have thought that educational psychologists spend their time discovering the obvious. The preceding examples point out the danger of this kind of thinking. When a principle is stated in simple terms, it can sound simplistic. A similar phenomenon takes place when we see a professional dancer or athlete perform: The well-trained performer makes it look easy. But we see only the results of the training, not all the work that went into mastering the individual movements. And bear in mind that any research finding—or its opposite—may sound like common sense. The issue is not what sounds sensible but what is demonstrated when the principle is put to the test in research—our next topic.

Using Research to Understand and Improve Learning Stop & Think

Descriptive studies Studies that collect detailed information about specific situations, often using observation, surveys, interviews, recordings, or a combination of these methods.

Correlations Statistical descriptions of how closely two variables are related.

Quickly list all the different research methods you can think of.

Educational psychologists design and conduct many different kinds of research studies. But before we explore some of the main methods in those studies, let’s take a moment to emphasize critical thinking about research in general. In this book and many others, you will encounter studies and research claims. Sometimes the claims will seem contradictory. Being a critical consumer of research, not just in your profession but also in the general media, is important. How strong is the evidence for a claim? Did the researchers study just a few people or many, over a short time or longer? Did what was assessed match what was taught or the goals of the program? Are the students in the study similar to yours? Is the school and community context like your situation? Research results can give you ideas to try, new concepts—tools to think with. But you must bring your own inventiveness and clear thinking to the process. OK, with that caution, let’s examine a common research method that involves simply describing events in a particular situation, called (you guessed it) descriptive studies. Often, the results of descriptive studies include reports of correlations. You will encounter many correlations in the coming clusters, so let’s take a minute to examine this concept. A correlation is a number that indicates both the strength and the direction of a relationship between two events or measurements. Correlations range from +1.00 to –1.00. The closer the correlation is to either +1.00 or –1.00, the stronger the relationship. For example, the correlation between adult weight and height is about .70 (a strong relationship); the correlation between adult weight and number of languages spoken is about .00 (no relationship at all).

CORRELATION STUDIES.

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

21

Figure 1.1 Correlations Do Not Show Causation When research shows that landscaped lawns and school achievement are correlated, it does not show causation. Community wealth, a third variable, may be the cause of both school achievement and landscaped lawns.

School achievement

ds to le a

C o rrelat

ti o n

leads to

r re l a

Landscaped lawns

to ds lea

Correlation

Co

ion

Community wealth

School achievement

Landscaped lawns

Correlation Faulty Assumption

Positive correlation A relationship between two variables in which the two increase or decrease together. Example: calorie intake and weight gain.

More Likely Assumption

Negative correlation

The sign of the correlation tells the direction of the relationship. A positive correlation indicates that the two factors increase or decrease together. As one gets larger, so does the other. Weight and height are positively correlated because greater weight tends to be associated with greater height. A negative correlation means that increases in one factor are related to decreases in the other; for example, the less you pay for a theater or concert ticket, the greater your distance from the stage. It is important to note that correlations do not prove cause and effect (see Figure 1.1). For example, weight and height are correlated—but gaining weight obviously does not cause you to grow taller. Knowing an individual’s weight simply allows you to make a general prediction about that person’s height. Educational psychologists identify correlations so they can make predictions about important events in the classroom. A second type of research—experimentation—allows educational psychologists to go beyond predictions and actually study cause and effect. Instead of just observing and describing an existing situation, the investigators introduce changes and note the results. First, several comparable groups of participants are created. In psychological research, the term participants (also called subjects) generally refers to the people being studied—such as teachers or ninth-graders. One common way to make sure that groups of participants are essentially the same is to assign each person to a group using a random procedure. Random means each participant has an equal chance of being in any group. Quasi-experimental studies meet most of the criteria for true experiments, with the important exception that the participants are not assigned to groups at random. Instead, existing groups such as classes or schools participate in the experiments. In experiments or quasi-experiments, for one or more of the groups studied, the experimenters change some aspect of the situation to see whether this change or “treatment” has an expected effect. The results in each group are then compared, often using statistics. When differences are described as statistically significant, it means that they probably did not happen simply by chance. For example, if you see p < .05 in a study, this indicates that the result reported could happen by chance less than 5 times out of 100, and p < .01 means less than 1 time in 100. Several studies we will examine attempt to identify cause-and-effect relationships by asking questions such as this: If some teachers receive training in how to teach spelling using word parts (cause), will their students become better spellers than students whose teachers did not receive training (effect)? This actually was a field experiment because it took place in real classrooms and not in a simulated laboratory situation. In addition, it was a quasi-experiment because the students were in existing classes and had not been randomly assigned to teachers, so we cannot be certain the experimental and control groups were the

A relationship between two variables in which a high value on one is associated with a low value on the other. Example: height and distance from top of head to the ceiling.

Experimentation Research method in which variables are manipulated and the effects recorded.

EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES.

Participants/subjects People or animals studied.

Random Without any definite pattern; following no rule.

Quasi-experimental studies Studies that fit most of the criteria for true experiments, with the important exception that the participants are not assigned to groups at random. Instead, existing groups such as classes or schools participate in the experiments.

Statistically significant Not likely to be a chance occurrence.

22 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology same before the teachers received their training. The researchers handled this by looking at improvement in spelling, not just final achievement level, and the results showed that the training worked (Hurry et al., 2005). ABAB EXPERIMENTAL DESIGNS. The goal of ABAB designs is to determine the effects of a therapy, teaching method, or other intervention by first observing the participants for a baseline period (A) and assessing the behavior of interest, trying an intervention (B) and noting the results, removing the intervention and going back to baseline conditions (A), and finally reinstating the intervention (B). This form of design can help establish a cause-and-effect relationship (Plavnick & Ferreri, 2013). For example, a teacher might record how much time students are out of their seats without permission during a weeklong baseline period (A). The teacher then tries ignoring those who are out of their seats but praising those who are seated, again recording how many are wandering out of their seats for the week (B). Next, the teacher returns to the baseline conditions of just recording how many students are out of their seats with no comments to any of them (A) and then reinstates the praise-and-ignore strategy (B). In the classic study that first tested this intervention, the praise-and-ignore strategy proved effective in increasing the time students spent in their seats (C. H. Madsen et al., 1968). Laws and policies from the U.S. government today emphasize experimental and quasiexperimental research, often called scientifically based research.

The No Child Left Behind Act emphasized scientifically based research, and the Every Student Succeeds Act (ESSA) requires “evidence-based” interventions in failing schools. In fact, we use the term “evidence-based” 10 times in this cluster and many more times in this book. What does this mean? Robert Slavin (2020) explains:

SCIENTIFICALLY BASED RESEARCH AND EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICES.

Evidence of effectiveness is defined as evidence from rigorous experiments in which students experiencing experimental programs are compared over significant periods (say, a semester or more) to those using traditional control methods in terms of gains on valid measures of achievement or other outcomes. Ideally, students, teachers, and/or schools are assigned at random to experimental or random treatments. . . .but at a minimum, experimental and control students are well matched at pretest on measures such as achievement and demographic variables. (p. 22) What Works Clearing House A service of the U.S. Institute of Educational Studies that reviews the existing research on different programs, products, practices, and policies in education in order to provide educators with the information they need to make evidence-based decisions.

Where can teachers and schools find evidence-based practices (besides in this book, of course)? The U.S. Institute of Education Sciences (IES) has established a What Works Clearinghouse with a series of Practice Guides that contain recommendations from experts about various challenges that educators face—guides to action based on strong evidence from large-scale, well-designed research studies (https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/practiceguides). Each of the Practice Guides describes several strategies and rates the evidence supporting their use as “strong,” “moderate,” or “minimal.” In the upcoming clusters, we will explore several of these guides, for example, Improving Mathematical Problem Solving in Grades 4 through 8 in Cluster 9 (Woodward et al., 2018). Educational psychologists also conduct other kinds of research—read on. Jean Piaget pioneered an approach called the clinical interview to understand children’s thinking. The clinical interview uses open-ended questioning to probe responses and to follow up on answers. Questions go wherever the child’s responses lead. Here is an example of a clinical interview with a 7-year-old. Piaget is trying to understand the child’s thinking about lies and truth, so he asks, “What is a lie?”

CLINICAL INTERVIEWS AND CASE STUDIES.

What is a lie?—What isn’t true. What they say that they haven’t done.—Guess how old I am.—Twenty. No, I’m thirty.—Was that a lie you told me?—I didn’t do it on purpose.— I know. But is it a lie all the same, or not?—Yes, it is the same, because I didn’t say how old you were.—Is it a lie?—Yes, because I didn’t speak the truth.—Ought you be punished?—No.—Was it naughty or not naughty?—Not so naughty.—Why?—Because I spoke the truth afterwards! (Piaget, 1965, p. 144)

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

Researchers also may employ case studies. A case study investigates one person or situation in depth. For example, years ago Benjamin Bloom and his colleagues conducted in-depth studies of highly accomplished concert pianists, sculptors, Olympic swimmers, tennis players, mathematicians, and neurologists to try to understand what factors supported the development of outstanding talent. The researchers interviewed family members, teachers, friends, and coaches to build an extensive case study of each of these highly accomplished individuals (B. S. Bloom et al., 1985). Some educators recommend case study methods to identify students for gifted programs because the information gathered is richer than just test scores. ETHNOGRAPHY. Ethnography, borrowed from anthropology, involves studying the naturally occurring events in the life of a group to understand the meaning of these events to the people involved. In educational psychology research, ethnographies might study how students from different cultural groups are viewed by their peers or how teachers’ beliefs about students’ abilities affect classroom interactions. In some studies the researcher uses participant observation, actually participating in the group, to understand the actions from the perspectives of the people in the situation. Teachers can do their own informal ethnographies to understand life in their classrooms.

Many things that psychologists want to study, such as cognitive development (Cluster 3), happen over several months or years. Ideally, researchers would study the development by observing their participants over many years as changes occur. These are called longitudinal studies. They are informative but time-consuming, expensive, and not always practical: Keeping up with participants over several years as they grow up and move can be impossible. As a consequence, much research is cross-sectional, focusing on groups of students at different ages. For example, to study how children’s conceptions of numbers change from ages 3 to 16, researchers can interview children of several different ages rather than follow the same children for 14 years. THE ROLE OF TIME IN RESEARCH.

WHAT’S THE EVIDENCE? QUALITATIVE VERSUS QUANTITATIVE RESEARCH. Here is a distinction you will encounter in your journey through educational psychology: the contrast between qualitative research and quantitative research. These are large categories and, like many categories, a bit fuzzy at the edges, but here are some simplified differences:

Qualitative Research. Case studies and ethnographies are examples of qualitative research. This type of research uses words, dialogue, events, themes, and images as data. Interviews, observations, and analysis of transcripts are key procedures. The goal is to explore specific situations or people in depth and to understand the meaning of the events to the people involved in order to tell their story. Qualitative researchers assume that no process of understanding meaning can be completely objective. They are more interested in interpreting subjective, personal, or socially constructed meanings. Quantitative Research. Both correlational and experimental types of research generally are quantitative because measurements are taken and computations are made. Quantitative research uses numbers, measurements, and statistics to assess levels or sizes of relationships among variables or differences between groups. Quantitative researchers try to be as objective as possible in order to remove their own biases from their results. One advantage of good quantitative research is that results from one study can be generalized or applied to other, similar situations or people. Many researchers now are using mixed methods or complementary methods to study questions both broadly and deeply. These research designs are procedures for “collecting, analyzing, and ‘mixing’ both quantitative and qualitative methods in a single study or series of studies to understand a research problem” (Creswell, 2015, p. 537). A special issue of the journal Contemporary Educational Psychology (2019) described three basic ways of combining methods. First, a researcher collects both quantitative and qualitative data at the same time and then merges and integrates the data in the analyses. In

MIXED METHODS RESEARCH.

23

Case study Intensive study of one individual or one situation.

Ethnography A descriptive approach to research that focuses on life within a group and tries to understand the meaning of events to the people involved.

Participant observation A method for conducting descriptive research in which the researcher becomes a participant in the situation in order to better understand life in that group.

Qualitative research Exploratory research that attempts to understand the meaning of events to the participants involved using such methods as case studies, interviews, ethnography, participant observation, and other approaches that focus on a few people in depth.

Quantitative research Research that studies many participants in a more formal and controlled way using objective measures such as experimentation, statistical analyses, tests, and structured observations.

24 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology the second approach, the researcher collects quantitative data first—for example, from surveys or observation instruments—and then follows this by performing in-depth qualitative interviews of participants selected on the basis of their quantitative responses or observed actions. Often the goal here is to explain or look for causes. Finally, the sequence can be reversed—the researcher first conducts interviews or case studies to identify research questions and then collects quantitative data as guided by the qualitative findings. Here the goal may be to explore a situation deeply (McCrudden et al., 2019). My colleagues and I (Ellen talking this time) used mixed methods to study the sources of self-efficacy for students in grades 1 to 6 who lived in a high-poverty rural area of Central Appalachia. We found that different sources supported the self-efficacy of students in mathematics compared to science and that the gender of the student mattered as well (Usher et al., 2019). In the final analysis, the methods used—quantitative, qualitative, or a mixture of both— should fit the questions asked (Bobbit Nolen, 2020). Different approaches to research can ask different questions and provide different kinds of answers, as you can see in Table 1.4. You can find reports of the findings from all types of studies in journals that are referenced in this book. For years Anita was editor of the Theory Into Practice journal (http://tip.ehe.osu.edu). For a great overview of the past 50 years in educational research and practice, see the Special 50th Anniversary issue of Theory Into Practice (Gaskill, 2013). You might find some ideas for your own teaching and try them out—do your own action research, described next. Research also can be a way to improve teaching in one classroom or one school. The same kind of careful observation, intervention, data gathering, analysis, inquiry, close listening to students, and viewing the classroom through their eyes that occurs in research projects can be applied in any classroom to answer questions such as “Which writing prompts seem to encourage the most creative writing in my class?” “When does Kenyon seem to have the greatest difficulty concentrating on academic tasks?” “Would assigning task roles in science groups lead to more equitable participation of girls and boys

TEACHERS AS RESEARCHERS.

Table 1.4 What Can We Learn? Different approaches to research can ask and answer different questions. RESEARCH METHOD

PURPOSES/QUESTIONS ADDRESSED

EXAMPLE

Correlational

To assess the strength and direction of the relation between two variables; to make predictions

Is the average amount of homework completed weekly related to student performance on unit tests? If so, is the relation positive or negative?

Experimental

To identify cause-and-effect relations; to test possible explanations for effects

Will giving more homework cause students to learn more in science class?

ABAB Experiment

To identify the effects of a treatment or intervention for one or more individuals

When students record the number of pages they read each night, will they read more pages? If they stop recording, will their amount of reading return to the previous levels?

Case Studies

To understand one or a few individuals or situations in depth

How does one boy make the transition from a small rural elementary school to a large middle school? What are his main problems, concerns, issues, accomplishments, fears, supports, and so on?

Ethnography

To understand experiences from the participants’ points of view: What are their meanings?

How do new teachers make sense of the norms, expectations, and culture of their new school, and how do they respond?

Mixed Methods

To ask complex questions involving causes, meanings, and relations among variables; to pursue both depth and breadth in research questions

Based on a study of 20 classrooms using quantitative observational instruments, select the 5 classes with the fewest behavior problems and the 5 with the most problems late in the year. Next, interview those teachers and their students, and analyze videotapes made the first weeks of school to answer the question “Did the effective and ineffective teachers differ in how they established rules and procedures in their classes?”

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

in the work?” This kind of problem-solving investigation is called action research. By focusing on a specific problem and making careful observations, teachers can learn a great deal about both their teaching and their students. Meghan Manfra (2019) says that action research lets us conceptualize teaching as inquiry—teachers continually learn and improve through research and reflection on their own practices. So, after all this discussion about different approaches to research, you might be wondering, “Enough already—what kind of research should guide education?” Not a simple question. The Point/Counterpoint takes a deeper dive.

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: What Kind of Research Should Guide Education? Policies in both health care and the treatment of psychological problems have emphasized evidence-based practices (McHugh & Barlow, 2010). Is this right for education?

Point Yes, research should be scientific; educational reforms should be based on solid evidence. According to Robert Slavin (2020), tremendous progress has taken place in fields such as medicine, agriculture, transportation, and technology because these fields base their practices on scientific evidence. Randomized clinical trials and replicated experiments are the sources of the evidence. Until recently, however, teachers, principals, and superintendents were more likely to choose instructional programs for their schools by asking friends in another district or listening to sales representatives for commercial packages (Dagenais et al., 2012). But thanks to recent government policies that require evidence-based interventions for failing schools, educators are turning to proven programs that have been developed and tested using rigorous quantitative research. Not only is scientifically based research identifying promising practices, there are also websites available for educators to explore and compare the programs:

• Evidence for ESSA (https://www.evidenceforessa.org) lets you locate K–12 programs that meet ESSA standards in reading, math, social-emotional learning, attendance, science, and writing. • What Works Clearinghouse (https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/) has K–12 programs and practices in many areas including for students with disabilities, English learners, behavior programs, and subject areas such as science or math. • Best Evidence Encyclopedia (http://www.bestevidence.org) has full-scale academic reviews of research on programs in specific areas such as reading, math, and science.

Counterpoint Experiments and controlled studies are not the only or even the best source of evidence for education. David Olson (2004) disagrees strongly with Slavin’s position. He claims that we cannot use medicine as an analogy to education. “Treatments” in education are much more complex and unpredictable than administering one drug or another in medicine. And every educational program is changed by classroom conditions and the way it is implemented. Patti Lather, Anita’s colleague at Ohio State, says, “In improving the quality of practice, complexity and the messiness of practice-in-context cannot be fantasized away. To try to do so yields impoverishment rather than improvement. That loss is being borne by the children, teachers, and administrators in our schools” (Lather, 2004, p. 30). David Berliner (2002) makes a similar point: Doing science and implementing scientific findings are so difficult in education because humans in schools are embedded in complex and changing networks of social interaction. The participants in those networks have variable power to affect each other from day to day, and the ordinary events of life (a sick child, a (Continued)

25

Action research Systematic observations or tests of methods conducted by teachers or schools to improve teaching and learning for their students.

26 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

messy divorce . . .a birthday party, alcohol abuse, a new principal, a new child in the classroom, rain that keeps the children from a recess outside the school building) all affect doing science in school settings by limiting the generalizability of educational research findings. Compared to designing bridges and circuits or splitting either atoms or genes, the science to help change schools and classrooms is harder to do because context cannot be controlled. (p. 19)

Beware of Either/Or

Design-based research Practitioners identify research questions based on problems of practice; then researchers gather and analyze the data to address those problems.

Qualitative research tells us specifically what happened in one or a few situations. Conclusions can be applied deeply but only to the issue that was studied. Quantitative research can tell us what generally happens under certain conditions. Conclusions can be applied more broadly. Complex problems in education require a whole range of methods for study as well as input from both researchers and educators. Educators must help researchers target the most important problems that need evidence-based solutions. Some current proponents of evidence-based interventions in education suggest that we benefit from the knowledge and wisdom of both practitioners and researchers. Design-based research does just that. Practitioners identify research questions based on problems of practice. Researchers then bring their time and talent to gather and analyze the data to address those problems (Scanlan, 2015).

Theories for Teaching

Principle Established relationship between factors.

Theory Integrated statement of principles that attempts to explain a phenomenon and make predictions.

Hypothesis/hypotheses A prediction of what will happen in a research study based on theory and previous research.

As we saw earlier, the major goal of educational psychology is to understand what happens when someone teaches something to someone else in some setting (Berliner, 2006; Schwab, 1973). Reaching this goal is a slow process. There are very few landmark studies that answer a question once and for all. There are so many different kinds of students, teachers, tasks, and settings; and besides, human beings are pretty complicated. To deal with this complexity, research in educational psychology examines limited aspects of a situation—perhaps a few variables at a time or life in one or two classrooms. If enough studies are completed in a certain area and findings repeatedly point to the same conclusions, we eventually arrive at a principle. This is the term for an established relationship between two or more factors— between a certain teaching strategy, for example, and student achievement. Another tool for building a better understanding of the teaching and learning processes is theory. The commonsense notion of theory (as in “Oh well, it was only a theory”) is “a guess or hunch.” But the scientific meaning of theory is quite different. A theory in science is a set of interrelated concepts, definitions, assumptions, and generalizations used to describe and explain data and to make predictions (Hoy & Adams, 2016). Educational psychologists have developed explanations for the relationships among many variables and even whole systems of relationships. There are theories to explain how language develops, why goals affect motivation, and, as noted earlier, how people learn. You will encounter many theories of development, learning, and motivation in this book. Theories are the beginning and ending points of the research cycle. In the beginning, theories provide the research hypotheses to be tested (predictions about what will happen) or the questions examined. For example, Piaget’s theory might suggest the hypothesis that instruction cannot teach young children to think more abstractly, whereas Vygotsky’s theory might suggest the competing hypothesis that instruction will be effective. Of course, at times, psychologists don’t know enough to state hypotheses, so they just ask research questions. An example question might be “Is there a difference in the Internet usage of male and female adolescents from different ethnic groups?” Research is a continuing cycle that involves:

• Clear specification of hypotheses, problems, or questions based on current theories. • Systematic gathering and analysis of all kinds of information (data) about the questions from well-chosen research participants in carefully selected situations.

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

27

• Interpretation and analysis of the data gathered using appropriate methods to answer the questions.

• Modification and improvement of explanatory theories based on the results of those analyses.

• Formulation of new and better hypotheses based on the improved theories . . . and on and on. This empirical process of collecting data to test and improve theories is repeated over and over. Empirical means “based on data.” When researchers say that identifying an effective antibiotic or choosing a successful way to teach reading is an “empirical question,” they mean that you need data and evidence to make the call. Constructing decisions from empirical analyses protects psychologists from developing theories based on personal biases, rumors, fears, faulty information, or preferences (Mertler & Charles, 2005). Good research is self-correcting. If predictions do not play out or if answers to carefully formulated questions do not support current best understandings (theories), then the theories have to be changed. You can use the same kind of systematic and self-correcting thinking in your work with students. Few theories explain and predict perfectly. In this book, you will see many examples of educational psychologists taking different theoretical positions and disagreeing on the overall explanations of such broad topics as learning and motivation. Because no one theory offers all the answers, it makes sense to consider what each has to offer. So why, you may ask, is it necessary to deal with theories? Why not just stick to principles? The answer is that both are useful. Principles of classroom management, for example, will give you help with specific problems. A good theory of classroom management, on the other hand, will give you a new way of thinking about discipline problems; it will give you cognitive tools for creating solutions to many different problems and for predicting what might work in new situations. A major goal of this book is to provide you with the best and the most useful theories of development, learning, motivation, and teaching—those theories that have solid evidence behind them. Although you might prefer some theories to others, consider them all as ways of understanding the challenges teachers face. We began this cluster by asserting that educational psychology is our favorite topic as well as a key source of knowledge and skills for teaching. We end this cluster with one more bit of evidence for our enthusiasm: Educational psychology will help you support student learning—the goal of all teaching.

Supporting Student Learning In an article in the Educational Psychologist, a major journal in our field, Jihyun Lee and Valerie Shute (2010) reported sifting through thousands of studies of student learning conducted over the course of 60 years, seeking to identify those that had direct measures of student achievement in reading and mathematics. Then they narrowed their focus to studies with strong effects. About 150 studies met all their rigorous criteria. Using the results from these studies, Lee and Shute identified about a dozen variables that were directly linked to K–12 student achievement. The researchers grouped these factors into two categories: student personal factors and school and social-contextual factors, as you can see in Table 1.5. When we read this article, we were pleased to see that our favorite subject, educational psychology, provides a base for developing knowledge and skills in virtually every area except principal leadership. (For that subject, you have to consult a book Anita wrote with her husband on principals as instructional leaders—Woolfolk Hoy & Hoy, 2020.) As you can see in Table 1.5, this text should help you become a capable and confident teacher who can get students engaged in the classroom learning community—a community that respects its members. This book will guide you toward becoming a teacher who helps students develop into interested, motivated, self-regulated, and confident learners. As a consequence, you will be able to set high expectations for your students, rally the support of parents, and build your own sense of efficacy as a teacher.

Empirical Based on systematically collected data.

28 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

Table 1.5 Research-Based Personal and Social-Contextual Factors That Support Student Achievement in K–12 Classrooms STUDENT PERSONAL FACTORS

EXAMPLES

WHERE IN THIS TEXT

Engaging Students’ Behavior

Make sure students attend classes, follow rules, and participate in school activities.

Clusters 5–7, 13

Engaging Students’ Minds and Motivations

Design challenging tasks, tap intrinsic motivation, support student investment in learning, and nurture student self-efficacy and other positive academic beliefs.

Clusters 2, 3, 4, 10, 11, 12

Engaging Students’ Emotions

Connect to student interest, pique curiosity, foster a sense of belonging and class connections, diminish anxiety, and increase enjoyment in learning.

Clusters 4, 5, 6, 10, 12

Cognitive Strategies

Directly teach knowledge and skills that support student learning and deep processing of valuable information (e.g., summarizing, inferring, applying, and reasoning).

Clusters 7–9, 14

Metacognitive Strategies

Directly teach students to monitor, regulate, and evaluate their own cognitive processes, strengths, and weaknesses as learners; teach them about when, where, why, and how to use specific strategies.

Clusters 7–9, 11

Behavioral Strategies

Directly teach students strategies and tactics for managing, monitoring, and evaluating their actions, motivations, effects, and environments, such as skills in:

Clusters 7–15

Student Engagement

Learning Strategies

time management test taking help seeking note taking homework management

SOCIAL-CONTEXTUAL FACTORS

EXAMPLES

WHERE IN THIS TEXT

Academic Emphasis

Set high expectations for your students, and encourage the whole school to do the same; emphasize positive relations with the school community.

Clusters 11–13

Teacher Variables

If possible, teach in a school with the positive qualities of collective efficacy, teacher empowerment, and sense of affiliation.

Clusters 1, 11, 13

Principal Leadership

If possible, teach in a school with the positive qualities of collegiality, high morale, and clearly conveyed goals.

See Woolfolk Hoy and Hoy (2020).

School Climate

Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

SOCIAL-CONTEXTUAL FACTORS

EXAMPLES

WHERE IN THIS TEXT

Parental Involvement

Support parents in supporting their children’s learning.

Clusters 3–6, 12

Peer Influences

Create classroom and school norms that honor achievement, encourage peer support, and discourage peer conflict.

Clusters 4, 10, 13, 15

Social-Familial Influences

Source: Based on Lee, J., & Shute, V. J. (2010). Personal and social-contextual factors in K–12 academic performance: An integrative perspective on student learning. Educational Psychologist, 45, 185–202.

Module 2 Summary The Role of Educational Psychology (pp. 18–29) What is educational psychology? Educational psychology has been linked to teaching since it began in the United States more than a century ago. The goals of educational psychology are understanding and improving the teaching and learning processes. Educational psychologists develop knowledge and methods; they also use the knowledge and methods of psychology and other related disciplines to study learning and teaching in everyday situations. Educational psychologists examine what happens when someone/something (a teacher or parent or computer) teaches something (math or weaving or dancing) to someone else (student or coworker or team) in some setting (classroom or theater or gym). What are the research methods in educational psychology? Correlational methods identify relationships and allow predictions. A correlation is a number that indicates both the strength and the direction of a relationship between two events or measurements. The closer the correlation is to either +1.00 or –1.00, the stronger the relationship. Experimental studies allow researchers to detect causes, not just make predictions. Experimental studies should help teachers implement useful changes. Instead of just observing and describing an existing situation, the investigators introduce changes and note the results. Quasi-experimental studies meet most of the criteria for true experiments, with the important exception being that the participants are not assigned to groups at random. Instead, existing groups such as classes or schools participate in the experiments. In ABAB experimental designs, researchers examine the effects of treatments on one or more people, often by using a baseline/intervention/baseline/intervention approach. Clinical interviews, case studies, and ethnographies look in detail at the experiences of a few individuals or groups. If participants are studied over time, the research is called longitudinal. No matter what method is used, results from the research are used to further develop and improve theories so that even-better hypotheses and questions can be developed to guide future research. What is the difference between qualitative and quantitative research? A general distinction between qualitative and quantitative research exists. These are large categories, and, like many categories, they are a bit fuzzy at the edges. Qualitative methods such as case studies and ethnographies use words, dialogue, events, themes, and images as data. Their goal is to explore specific situations or people in depth and to understand the meaning of the events to the people involved in order to tell their stories. Quantitative research uses numbers, measurements, and statistics to assess levels or sizes of relationships among variables or differences between groups— correlational and experimental research are examples. Different types of research can answer different questions. Today many researchers are using mixed methods to study questions both broadly and deeply. There are three basic ways of combining methods. First, a researcher collects both quantitative and qualitative data at the same time and then merges and integrates the data in the analyses. Second, the researcher collects quantitative data first, from, for example, surveys or observation instruments, and then follows this by performing in-depth qualitative interviews of selected participants. Often the goal is to explain or look for causes. Finally, the sequence can be reversed—the researcher first conducts interviews or case studies to identify research questions and then collects quantitative data as guided by the qualitative findings. Here the goal may be to explore a situation thoroughly.

29

30 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology Scientifically based research, which is more consistent with quantitative research, systematically uses observations or experiments to gather valid and reliable data; involves rigorous and appropriate procedures for gathering and analyzing the data; is clearly described so that it can be repeated by others; and has been rigorously reviewed by appropriate independent experts. Evidence-based practices, the kind that must be used to intervene in failing schools under the ESSA, are grounded in the results of systematic, rigorous research. When teachers or schools make systematic observations or test methods to improve teaching and learning for their students, they are conducting action research. Distinguish between principles and theories. A principle is an established relationship between two or more factors—between, for example, a certain teaching strategy and student achievement. A theory is an interrelated set of concepts that is used to explain a body of data and to make predictions. The principles from research offer several possible answers to specific problems, and the theories offer perspectives for analyzing almost any situation that may arise. Research is a continuing cycle that involves clear specification of hypotheses or questions based on good theory, systematic gathering and analyzing of data, interpretation and analysis of the data gathered using appropriate methods to answer the questions, modification and improvement of explanatory theories based on the results, and the formulation of new, better questions based on the improved theories. What key factors support student learning? A synthesis of about 150 studies of student learning found two broad categories of influence: student personal factors and school and social-contextual factors. Educational psychology provides a base for developing knowledge and skills in virtually every area except principal leadership.

Cluster 1 Review Key Terms Action research Case study Correlations Descriptive studies Design-based research Educational psychology Empirical Ethnography Experimentation Hypothesis/hypotheses Meta-analysis Negative correlation Participant observation Participants/subjects

Positive correlation Principle Qualitative research Quantitative research Quasi-experimental studies Random Reflective Social and emotional learning (SEL) Statistically significant Teachers’ sense of efficacy Theory What Works Clearinghouse

Connect and Extend to Licensure Multiple-Choice Questions 1.

Novice teachers face numerous tasks and scenarios with which they have little prior experience. For teachers

currently entering the field, which of the following is not a challenge they are apt to encounter? A.

Students who may exhibit superior technology skills as compared to their teachers

Cluster 1

2.

3.

B.

An increasingly diverse population of students and families

C.

Inadequate resources to ensure the safety of their students while using technology in the classroom

D.

Students who face the challenges associated with living in poverty

Both students and teachers work harder and persist longer when they have a high sense of efficacy. Which of the following does not enhance self-efficacy in both students and teachers? A.

Formal school relationships that focus solely on skills

B.

Day-to-day success in achieving tasks

C.

High expectations from those in the environment

D.

Assistance from more knowledgeable partners

All the students in Ms. Clare’s third-grade class engage in weekly test reviews. Ms. Clare believes that these reviews will enhance student retention when standardized testing occurs in the spring. Which of Ms. Clare’s students will have his or her scores reported separately under the Every Student Succeeds Act (ESSA)? A.

Susan Frasier, who was recently identified with having a learning disability

B.

Brendan Kincaid, who must wear corrective lenses in order to read

C.

Miranda Ruiz, whose English is excellent even though her parents moved to the United States from Mexico only 10 years ago

D.

Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

31

Lauren Stone, who is a member of the third grade’s cohort of students who are gifted and talented

Constructed-Response Questions Case Sandra Chapman was determined to add to her repertoire of teaching skills as she entered her second year of teaching. Her first year as a high school teacher proved to be more of a challenge than she had expected. Her school, located in the heart of the city, drew students from all walks of life and economic circumstances. Last year, she initially hoped that all of her students would master the history curriculum that she had inherited, but by midyear several of her students were not attending class on a regular basis. In an effort to increase attendance, she took points off students’ grades when they missed class and intentionally ignored them when they returned. She believed that by not taking an interest in where they were, she would not reinforce their “skipping” behavior. She also thought that by continually reminding students of how much they did not know, she would encourage them to study. Sadly, these methods did not work well, and attendance only further declined. She is now in the process of designing some new strategies. 4.

Identify the methods Sandra Chapman used to encourage attendance, and explain why these methods might have been unsuccessful.

5.

What advice would you offer Sandra Chapman as she prepares to develop new methods?

What Would They Do? Teachers’ Casebook: Becoming a Great Teacher At the beginning of this cluster, we asked you to think critically about what makes a great teacher and what sources of information might help. Now, as you consider these expert teachers’ responses to the case, we again suggest that you think critically. Do the following responses reflect (or contradict) any of the concepts discussed in this cluster? Do the suggestions and solutions described here make sense in light of what you have learned in this or other classes about best practices? How do these ideas align with or challenge your personal philosophy of teaching?

Anne Liners • 9th–12th-Grade Student Teacher South High School, Fort Minneapolis, MN I believe the most important part of being a reflective teacher is recognizing what your students need. If you’re putting in the effort to listen, kids are pretty open about what is or isn’t working for them. One of the strongest characteristics of good teachers is that they can effectively read their classrooms to see if the students are engaged, excited, bored, exhausted, confused, or antsy, and then use what they see to adapt

their lessons and objectives. New research or cutting-edge approaches are effective only when they actually enhance the experiences and learning of students. A good teacher’s loyalty is not necessarily to the latest studies and best-selling books (although those can help!) but instead to the well-being of the young people they are responsible for. The master teachers who have mentored me have been very forthcoming about the fact that they know they aren’t able to effectively implement every possible new idea—no one is. Instead, they pick the research they can apply using their strengths, implement that research, then evaluate the effectiveness of the techniques based on the students’ responses. Always keeping the practical realities of students in mind helps me define my own goals and practices.

India Chambers, ED.S. • 1st–8th-Grade Director of Academics Northside Preparatory Academy, Cincinnati, OH Great teachers have a genuine love for children and dedicate their practice, time, and knowledge base to helping their scholars shatter that glass ceiling. Great teachers devote their efforts to tangibly closing the achievement gap and

32 Cluster 1 Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology tapping into the untapped talents of their students. They embrace the promise and potential in every learner they encounter even through difficult times. When evaluating the quality of others’ advice about teaching and learning, I base my evaluation upon the realities and practices in their classrooms. The same advice given should be clearly evident and observed in that individual’s classroom. That would help me discern whether the teachers really “know” what they are talking about and if the given advice is even practical. Best practices are determined by the realities of each teacher’s classroom and what has been proven by research to work for the population being served by the educator. It is helpful to research instructional strategies, interventions, and practices that have been shown by research studies to work for students who are similar demographically to the students in your classroom. Research that would convince me to change my practice include the studies that show substantial improvement depicted in data. These data would reflect quantitative analysis, trends, and correlations that prove the proposed action tested in the study positively impacted student outcomes.

Mari Ann Banks, PH.D. • Equity Officer City Schools of Decatur, Decatur, GA There is no singular representation of a great teacher. Good teachers come in all shapes, sizes, and dispositions, but the good become great by doing a few key things: 1. Great teachers identify the needs of their students, meet them where they are, and then help them develop the courage and skills to go further than they ever thought they could. 2. Great teachers know they will always be students themselves—and they are happy about that. They work to keep abreast of groundbreaking changes in the field and integrate valuable new knowledge into their praxis. 3. Great teachers implement culturally relevant, critical care for their students. They don’t just “love all students” or claim that they “don’t see color.” Instead, they care by unmasking the hidden (and overt) curriculum that is so prevalent throughout the United States. They expose and unveil various forms of privilege, expand their curriculum to include multiple voices and perspectives, and show directly and by personal example how to make systemic change. When evaluating the quality of teachers’ advice and ideas, I make sure to look at their results. Not just test scores and grades of the advisor ’s students—in fact, that’s the last thing I find important. Instead, I look to see what types of relationships they have established with students. Do students grow under their care? Are students challenged to think deeply about issues? Do students feel safe enough to ask questions about the way the world works, and, if so, are they encouraged to find the answers? If the answer to all of these is yes—that’s the advice I want to hear. When faced with too much information or conflicting

information, this is also how I decide in whom I will place my trust and energy.

Kathryn Larsen • Junior High School Principal Pleasant Grove Junior High, Pleasant Grove, UT

• What makes someone a great teacher? How are “best” practices determined? Great teachers foster belonging, equity, and unity in their classrooms. Many use best practices to accomplish this. Best practices are determined through both research and a genuine understanding of the students in your classroom. What works well with one group of students may not work the next year with another group of students. Every group has its own culture. When looking to research to help determine best practices, it is important to find seminal research that is supported by additional research. I have found that the results of the best education researchers are not contradictory. Instead, their work supports the work of many others.

• How do you evaluate the quality of others’ advice about teaching and learning? Try it for yourself! If you get advice from a trusted colleague, modify it to fit the needs of your own students and then go for it. This is where action research comes in. Implement the advice, modify it, try it again, and keep refining your practice. If the individual who gave you the advice is a trusted friend and colleague, ask that person to help you refine your practice. Teaching is very personal. What works for others may not work for you. However, by making small shifts to others’ advice about teaching and learning, you will figure out what works for you and your students. Ask for student input when evaluating the quality of the advice you have received from others. Students know what works, but we rarely give them the autonomy and the voice they deserve to take part in evaluating teaching and learning.

• What would lead you to conclude that someone’s advice was simply a trend versus a sound educational practice? If I have gone through the action research process using advice from a colleague and am not seeing a pattern of positive results, higher levels of student achievement, and more engagement, I would know that I need to continue to modify that advice to make it work in my own classroom. I may also need to reconsider whether that advice should become a permanent part of my own practice. A practice you can trust becomes sound educational practice.

• What kinds of research findings would convince you to change your practice? I am especially drawn to research that is conducted by teacherresearchers who are still in the classroom. The credibility this gives to the researcher is invaluable. When I read a researchbased book about literacy practices written by a teacherresearcher who is working with her or his own students, I am more convinced that these same practices will work in my own classroom. The research findings from teacher-researchers feel very authentic and trustworthy, and I am more likely to change my own practice as a result.

Cluster 1

Donnie Piercey • 5th-Grade Teacher and 2021 Kentucky State Teacher of the Year Stonewall Elementary School, Lexington, KY When I reflect back on my own time in school—before I became a teacher—there are a few things that still stand out to me about what made some of my favorite teachers great: They just seemed to run their classrooms differently. They weren’t satisfied with the mold that other teachers before them created; rather, they wanted their classrooms to feel different, to be different. They took time to tailor-make lessons and experiences just for us (based on the time that they spent with us at recess or during brief conversations in between classes). They weren’t afraid to establish a relationship with their students to let them know that they care.

Learning, Teaching, and Educational Psychology

33

Trends in education often look like one teacher, school, or district copying something that they saw others do or say that they heard was successful. This could be after-school programs, a textbook curriculum, or even a piece of technology that was purchased. While this definitely doesn’t mean that all of these borrowed ideas are bad for schools and students (many are great), the danger comes when schools look at others before trying to creativity figure out how best to serve their own students first. I’ve always placed a lot of value on the importance of teacher and student creativity, especially when a teacher’s idea is grounded in sound educational practice.

Cluster 2

Andrew, Age 5

Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Teachers’ Casebook: Conversations About Race What Would You Do? A video is widely circulated on social media of an unarmed Black teenager being injured by police over the weekend in a town not far from your school. On Monday morning, as students are settling into class, you overhear a conversation about what happened. One student, whose dad is a police officer and whose backpack sports a “Blue Lives Matter” tag, remarks that what the teen was doing was wrong. Another student responds, “Well, my parents have always taught me that I’d better watch out for the police because I’m Black. Black lives matter too, don’t they?” Students around the room begin to chatter. One girl sighs audibly, “Why does everything have to be about race? Can’t we just agree that all lives matter?” Her comment immediately reminds you of how upset one of your colleagues became earlier this year when she found her “Black Lives Matter” bumper sticker defaced in the school parking lot. Perhaps this is a teachable moment. 34

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Critical Thinking • How could you use this as an opportunity to talk about race in your classroom? How would you design the classroom space for difficult conversations about race? What would be essential for you to do in order to set up the classroom for productive and courageous discursive interactions? • If you knew that students of color were upset by White students’ comments, how would you respond? • When, if ever, is it a good idea to bring up group-based identity and the systems of privileges, advantages, and disadvantages those identities might bring with them? • What are your fears about having conversations about race (or other social identities)?

Overview and Objectives Who are you? How have the various facets of your cultural and social identities shaped your own experiences and views? Who are your students? How will their cultural backgrounds affect their learning, development, and motivation in your class? Nearly all aspects of educational psychology— from how brains work to how you can create a positive workflow in your classroom—are better understood when we are able to recognize the ways in which our cultural and social backgrounds have uniquely shaped who we are. The cultural composition of classrooms in the United States and around the world is constantly evolving. Frank Pajares, who was my (Ellen in this cluster) advisor in graduate school and one of the wisest educational psychologists I know, speaking at a meeting of the American Educational Research Association, observed that “the critical questions in education involve matters that cannot be settled by universal prescription. They demand attention to the cultural forces that shape our lives” (Pajares, 2000, p. 5). I believe he is right. In this cluster, we explore the ways in which culture forms the fabric of our society. We will start with a look inward. Understanding yourself and your own frame of reference is key to understanding others. Then we will meet two individuals whose stories bring the statistics on cultural diversity and intersectionality to life. We will consider the development of culturalbased stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination as they affect teaching and learning. With a broad conception of culture as a basis, we then examine three dimensions of every teacher’s and student’s identity: social class, race/ ethnicity, and gender and sexual orientation. Then we turn to a consideration of culturally responsive education, an approach to teaching that embraces diversity, and we look at ways to create culturally welcoming classrooms.

35

36 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

The last section presents four general principles for teaching every student. By the time you have completed this cluster, you should be able to: 2.1 Describe the meaning of culture and discuss how cultural diversity in American education today is related to learning and teaching. 2.2 Discuss what defines social class and socioeconomic status, including how SES differences relate to school achievement. 2.3 Explain how race, ethnicity, prejudice, discrimination, and stereotype threat might affect student learning and achievement in schools. 2.4 Describe the development of gender, gender identity, and sexual orientation and discuss their roles in teaching and learning. 2.5 Define multicultural education and apply research on diversity to the creation of culturally relevant classrooms.

OUTLINE Teachers’ Casebook—Conversations About Race: What Would You Do? Overview and Objectives MODULE 3: Education and Culture Education Is Cultural What Is Culture? Your Cultural and Educational History Meet Two Students Cultural Intersections and Terminology Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Discrimination MODULE 4: Social and Diversity Economic Economic and Social Class Differences Social Class and Socioeconomic Status Poverty and Social Inequality Poverty and Academic Outcomes Extreme Poverty: Homeless and Highly Mobile Students MODULE 5: Ethnic and Racial Diversity Ethnicity and Race in Teaching and Learning Defining Ethnicity and Race Ethnic and Racial Identity Ethnic and Racial Differences in School Achievement The Legacy of Racial Inequality MODULE 6: Gender Identify and Sexual Orientation Gender and Sexual Orientation in Teaching and Learning Sex and Gender Gender Identity Gender Roles Gender Bias and Sexism in Curriculum and Media Gender Bias in Teaching Sexual Orientation Discrimination Based on Gender Identity and Sexual Orientation MODULE 7: Diversity and Teaching Creating Culturally Welcoming Classrooms Culturally Relevant Pedagogy

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

37

Diversity in Learning Lessons for Teachers: Teaching Every Student Cluster 2 Review Key Terms • Connect and Extend to Licensure • Teachers’ Casebook— Conversations About Race: What Would They Do?

MODULE 3

Education and Culture

Learning Objective 2.1 Describe the meaning of culture and discuss how cultural diversity in American education today is related to learning and teaching.

Education Is Cultural Classrooms are culturally diverse places. Classrooms and schools have their own cultures, too. Throughout this text we take a broad interpretation of culture and cultural diversity. We begin with a look at the meaning of “culture.” Then we invite you to explore your own cultural and educational backgrounds. This exploration will be important for how you relate not only to the rest of your studies in education but also to your future students. We will then consider social groups and identities and the cultural significance they hold. We will specifically examine social class, race, ethnicity, gender, and sexual orientation because they reflect some of the most researched social categories in education.

What Is Culture? There are many definitions of culture. Most include some or all of the following: the knowledge, skills, rules, norms, practices, traditions, self-definitions, institutions (e.g., educational, legal, communal, religious, political), language, and values that shape and guide beliefs and behaviors in a particular group of people as well as the art, literature, folklore, and artifacts produced and passed down to the next generation (Banks & Banks, 2016; Cohen, 2009, 2010). Cultures have scripts—programs for living—that are communicated to members. This communication is for the most part tacit—understood or implied without being explicitly stated. Cultural boundaries are socially constructed and fluid. Cultural and social groups can be defined along regional, ethnic, religious, racial, gender, social class, or other lines. These lines are sometimes drawn to maintain or resist systems of power (Causadias, 2020). For example, the Black Lives Matter movement arose in resistance to a dominant culture built on the foundation of White supremacy in the United States. Each of us is a member of many groups, so we all are influenced by many different cultures. Sometimes these influences are incompatible or even contradictory. For example, if you are a feminist but also a Roman Catholic, you might have trouble reconciling the two different cultures’ beliefs about the ordination of women as priests. Your personal belief will be based, in part, on how strongly you identify with each group. Many different cultures thrive within every modern country. In the United States, students growing up in a small rural town in the Great Plains are part of a cultural group that is quite different from that of students in a large Northeastern urban center or students in a Texas suburb. Within those small towns in the Great Plains, the child of a convenience store clerk grows up in a different culture than does the child of the town doctor or dentist. Individuals of African, Asian, Hispanic, Native American, or European descent have distinctive histories and traditions even though great cultural variety exists among members of these groups. People living within a particular country may share common experiences and values, but many aspects of their lives are shaped by their different cultural affiliations and increasingly by their social networks that have no geographical boundaries. Members of different cultural groups are treated differently by the larger cultural structures within society.

Culture The knowledge, values, attitudes, and traditions that guide the behavior of a group of people and allow them to solve the problems of living in their environment.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® The Larger Community (IV, B1, 3) Familiarize yourself with the predicted changes in the U.S. population over the next several decades. How are those changes likely to affect education? What can schools and teachers do to adjust positively to those changes?

38 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Figure 2.1 Culture as an Iceberg Just as most of the iceberg is out of sight and below the waterline, so are most cultural differences invisible to others and even out of conscious awareness. The out-of-awareness differences are often the causes of misunderstandings and conflicts.

Heroes and heroines Cooking and foods Literature Myths Music Dress Games Language Notions of modesty Holiday customs

Childrearing beliefs Understanding of the natural world Rules of eye contact

Roles of men and women

Explanations of disease Beliefs about effort and ability Conception of justice and fairness Conception of cleanliness

Beliefs about social mobility

Definition of insanity Roles in relation to age, sex, class, occupation, kinship, and so forth Nature of friendship

Facial expressions

Preference for competition or cooperation Definitions of showing respect

Sense of time Conceptions of learning and teaching

Concept of personal space

AND MUCH, MUCH MORE…

Culture has been compared to an iceberg. A small part of the iceberg is visible; the rest is hidden and often unknown. The visible signs of culture, such as costumes and marriage traditions, reflect only a small portion of the differences among cultures, as you can see in Figure 2.1. Many aspects of our cultural selves exist “below the surface.” Many of our cultural affiliations are not readily visible to others. In addition, your own cultural “database”—the basis for your assumptions, norms, and actions—might not be visible even to you. I once gave a research presentation in France and was worried when I saw that no one was nodding along as I spoke. Was it my accent? My message? I realized that cultural rules for appropriate interpersonal behaviors differ. In some groups, listeners give a slight affirmative nod of the head and perhaps an occasional “uh-huh” to indicate they are listening carefully. But members of other cultures listen without giving acknowledgment or with eyes downcast as a sign of respect. In some cultures, high-status individuals initiate conversations and ask the questions, and low-status individuals only respond. In other cultures, the pattern is reversed. These are the elements of culture beneath the surface—they are implicit, unstated beliefs about acceptable ways of thinking, being, and doing (Kahneman, 2011; Sheets, 2005). I was unaware of this culture-based expectation I held of my audience. Cultural influences are widespread and pervasive. Some psychologists have shown that culture can determine even how a group defines intelligence (Nisbett, 2009). For example, physical grace is essential in Balinese social life, so the ability to master physical movements is a mark of intelligence in that culture. Manipulating words and numbers is important in Western

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

societies, so in these cultures such skills are indicators of intelligence (Gardner, 2011). Even symptoms of psychological disorders are affected by culture. In industrialized cultures where cleanliness is emphasized, people with obsessive-compulsive disorders often become obsessed with cleaning their hands, whereas in Bali, where social networks are emphasized, people with obsessive-compulsive disorders often become obsessed with knowing all the details about the lives of their friends and family—their social network (Lemelson, 2003).

Your Cultural and Educational History To better understand educational psychology and human development, let’s begin by exploring your own cultural and educational background. More than 100 years ago, father of American psychology William James, whom you met in Cluster 1, famously observed that “introspection is what we have to rely on first, foremost, and always.” Stop & Think So let’s conduct a little introspective research (we might call this “me” search). Look back at your life. How would you describe your educational and cultural background? What were your formal and informal educational experiences like? Which cultural groups were you part of growing up? Which have you chosen for yourself? In what ways have you been advantaged? In what ways have you been disadvantaged? How does this affect you now? I recently asked my undergraduate students to conduct their own autobiographical research project about their educational background (we called it a “me” search project). To help them reflect, I provided a few guiding questions (see the list in Table 2.1 on the next page). We also spent some time discussing how talking openly about our individual, social, and cultural backgrounds can be an emotionally vulnerable thing to do. I encouraged students to share with others only those details about their experiences that they felt comfortable sharing. The activity was mostly designed to help students reflect introspectively on the circumstances of their lives that contributed to who they have become. How would you answer the questions in Table 2.1? Your own experiences and circumstances, whether you selected them or not, have shaped the way you see yourself and others. They are also part of your cultural background. Your social and educational experiences have helped you build a rich cultural “database.” As my students selected details about their educational autobiographies to share with others in the class, many used words like “normal” or “typical” to describe their schooling experiences. However, as they listened to their classmates, they were asked to write down at least one aspect of each classmate’s cultural or educational experience that was different from their own. Here are a few of the distinctions my students noticed as they listened to one another:

• “He is from a big family and did not move, which is the opposite of my small, fractured family and many moves.”

• “She was raised in an affluent community outside of Chicago—she had more opportunities for growth than I had. My dad drank our money away.”

• “I never went to a predominantly White school like he did.” • “I grew up with no ethnic minority students in my school system. I missed out on a valuable learning experience.”

• “She went to an all-girls high school, which was probably a very difficult experience.” • “He has lived in three different states throughout his life, and I’ve never left Kentucky. My family has lived in the same house since my parents got married.”

• “She became super close friends with one of her teachers, which I wouldn’t have even imagined possible in my experience.”

• “Christian farm life must be so different from my non-Christian suburban life. I have never even been on a farm before.”

39

40 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Table 2.1 Your Educational Autobiography REFLECTIVE QUESTIONS • What was your family’s educational background? What was your household income? Whom have you lived with? • How would you describe your parents’ social class and status? • Where have you lived? How would you describe your home(s) and neighborhood(s)? Where have you traveled? • What were the values, norms, and expectations in your home? • What learning tools (e.g., books, computers, games, people) were available to you at school and at home? • What were your formal schooling experiences like? What kinds of schools did you attend? What kinds of classes were you in (or not in)? How did you feel at school? • What were your informal learning experiences (e.g., faith groups, sports, travel, music, camps, work) like? • How were your teachers? Who or what has been your most influential teacher? • How would you describe your peers and classmates? • What was the most typical aspect of your education? What was most atypical? • In what areas do you feel less educated than you would like to be? Why? • How have you felt different from others? When, where, and by whom did you feel most and least accepted? • How much trust did you feel in people in your community (e.g., peers, teachers, neighbors)? • How would you describe your own cultural background? How has it affected you? • How were you as a learner? Where has learning been easiest? Where has it been most difficult? • How would you describe your academic motivation? • How has your educational experience influenced what you want out of life? • What social or cultural events, ceremonies, or rituals did you take part in? • How would you describe the health (e.g., physical, psychological) of your family members? • Who were the members of the “in” and “out” groups in your schools and communities? Which were you in? • How would you describe your interactions with members of other racial groups? • Were you a part of any social group for which you had to qualify? • Were you a member of any selective groups (e.g., girls only, advanced students, special education, varsity athletics)? • Did you have financial or social resources that gave you additional educational opportunities? • Do you or does someone close to you (a) speak more than one language, (b) play a musical instrument, (c) participate in a civic organization, (d) have a private tutor, (e) read extensively, or (f) have an advanced degree? • How much choice did you or your parents have in where you went to school, what classes you were in, and who your teachers were? • What resources (e.g., books, transportation, high-speed Internet, family members who attended college, a trusted adult confidant, a personal bank account, a job opportunity, a private tutor) were available to you? • What was your relationship to the natural environment? How often did you spend time outdoors? • What job(s) have you held? How and why did you get those job(s)? What have they taught you? • In what capacity have you had to work with others toward a common goal? • Where have you felt that the doors to opportunity were open to you? Where have the doors felt closed to you? Why? • What did you learn, whether directly or indirectly, about groups of people who were different from you?

• “He had advanced musical opportunities from a pretty young age. He has been very well trained by world-class musicians.”

• “Being from a poorer area and school district shaped her educational experience. Unlike me, she learned more from her informal education than from her formal education due to the low funding.” What did you notice? I hope you can see that no single educational or cultural experience exists. We can all find commonalities and differences. Becoming more aware of your own social and cultural upbringings and those of others will help you become a more culturally conscious teacher. This awareness will increase the likelihood that you honor cultural differences when you plan your instruction. Later in this cluster, we will provide specific examples of ways you can do this. For now, let’s meet two students who provide a more in-depth look at cultural diversity.

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Meet Two Students In Cluster 1, you read some statistics about students in America and saw that classrooms are becoming more diverse. But teachers do not work with statistics; they work with students— unique individuals. In this section, Nancy Knapp from the University of Georgia invites us to meet two individuals. These students are not specific people; they are composites of the characteristics of real people Nancy has known and taught. The names and schools are fictional, but the descriptions of these students’ lives are very real. Ternice Mattox is a seventh-grader who lives with her mother and three younger siblings in a large city in the Northeast. Her mother works the 7:00 to 3:00 shift at a dry-cleaning plant and then cleans offices some nights and weekends to make ends meet, so Ternice gets her brothers and sister up and ready for school every day, feeds them dinner when they get home, and makes sure they do their homework at night; she has been doing this since she was 10. School hasn’t ever been very hard for Ternice. In elementary school she usually got B’s, but she never really liked school until sixth grade, when her English teacher “let you write about whatever you wanted, even your own life; and she didn’t count off for every mistake right away, but let you work with her and with the other kids until you had a final copy you could be really proud of.” Ternice found out that she really enjoyed writing and was good at it, too. She talked so much in English class that Anthony Bailey criticized her about “actin’ so White.” Ternice got mad; it really bothered her. She and Anthony kind of “talk,” and she likes him a lot. Her teacher wants her to take some tests to see whether she can get into the program for students with gifts and talents, but Ternice is not so sure. Even if she got in, she’s afraid she wouldn’t know anyone; almost all the kids in “gifted” are White, and the few Black kids are from another part of town. Besides, her friends, especially Anthony, might not like it. Her mama wants her to try and says there’s no telling where she can go from there, but Ternice doesn’t want to go anywhere that’s away from all her friends. Still, she wishes she could have more classes like her English class last year. Jessie Kinkaid is a junior at Red Falls High School in Wisconsin. She lives with her mother, who works as a doctor’s receptionist, in a small house in town. Her father lives just outside town with his second wife and Jessie’s 3-year-old half-brother, so she sees him pretty often. Jessie is in the vocational track at school and mostly makes Cs with a few Ds. Once in a while, she fails a course, but she’ll have enough credits to graduate by the end of next year, which is all she really cares about. Her family and consumer sciences teacher says she has a real flair for cooking and wants Jessie to bring up her grades so that she can apply to chef’s school. Jessie likes to cook and knows she’s good at it, but she doesn’t see any point in getting more schooling. She’s graduating only to please her parents; she knows what she’s going to do with her life. After graduation she’s going to get a job in town somewhere for a couple years to save up some money, and then she’ll marry Walter Aiken. She and Walt have been dating since she was a freshman and he was a junior. Walt started this year at UW–Platteville to get a degree in animal science, and they plan to wait until he is finished before they get married. Then they’ll move into the small house on the Aikens’ farm until Walt’s dad is ready to retire, probably in another 3 or 4 years. Then Walt will take over the farm, and they’ll move into the large farm house; Jessie hopes they’ll have at least one child by then. So Jessie doesn’t see any point in worrying about her grades, as long as they’re good enough for her to graduate. Her father agrees that it would be foolish to waste time and money on extra schooling she’ll never use. Jessie’s mother, who left school at 17 to marry, is the one urging Jessie to think about continuing her education. She says she just wants Jessie to “keep all her options open.” Ternice and Jessie are just two students, and there are millions more—unique collections of abilities and experiences. They use different vocabularies, have different ethnic and racial backgrounds, and live in different kinds of communities. Some come from families in poverty, others from families with power and privilege—but all face challenges in their education. For the remainder of the cluster, we look at several dimensions of cultural differences in schools today.

41

42 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Cultural Intersections and Terminology As we discuss cultural differences, we need to keep several important points and terms in mind. First, we will focus sections of this cluster, on social class, ethnicity, race, gender, and sexual orientation because much of the available research has focused on these categories. Of course, real people are not just African American or middle class or male; they are complex beings and members of many groups, just like Ternice and Jessie, the two students you just met. Stop & Think Stop here for a moment and think of three to five groups (gender, sexual orientation, class, ethnicity, religion, socioeconomic status, age, etc.) to which you belong. You might use the identity wheel pictured in Figure 2.2 to help. How do the intersections of your memberships influence your interactions with others? How might your own cultural heritage and chosen cultural affiliations affect how you see yourself and what you do? What have been the major advantages of your group affiliations or social identities? What, if any, disadvantages have you experienced?

Intersectionality Our overlapping, intersecting social identities (gender, sexual orientation, class, ethnicity, religion, socioeconomic status, age, etc.) that shape each and every one of us in unique ways.

The fact that you belong to many social groups results in a fascinating tapestry of cultural intersectionality. Intersectionality refers to our overlapping, intersecting social identities (gender, sexual orientation, class, ethnicity, religion, socioeconomic status, age, etc.) that shape each of us in unique ways (Rosenthal, Cultural and Gender Differences in the Classroom (III, B) 2016). Individuals who are members of multiple social groups that have been disempowWhat are the sources of posered by others can experience additional psychological, educational, and health risks. sible miscommunication among As Audre Lorde, an African American author who dedicated her career to confronting students and teachers in the racism, sexism, homophobia, and classism, once said, “There is no such thing as a classroom because of cultural or single-issue struggle because we do not live single-issue lives” (Lorde, 1982). For example, gender differences? Identify steps students who are in gender and sexual minority groups are at greater risk of homelessness, a teacher can take to minimize such problems. and this risk increases if they are also members of an ethnic minority group (Tierney & Ward, 2017). As you read on, think of your own examples of how such cultural intersectionalities might influence learning and teaching. Figure 2.2 Cultural Identity Wheel Intersectionality also means that knowing that a person is a People belong to many cultural and social groups. How member of a particular cultural group does not define what that would you describe yourself in each category? Which person is like. As you just saw, you belong to many social and of these groups is most important to you? Which do cultural groups—some visible and some invisible. Even members you think about most often? of the same cultural group can have widely different behaviors. This means that we must resist the tendency to predict or explain a person’s behavior from their membership in a particular group. Religious The reasons for human behavior are much too complicated! Neighborhood beliefs Consider, for example, how many explanations there might be for why a student in your class consistently arrives late. It might be that the student has a job before school, must walk a Gender Race long distance, or, like Ternice, is responsible for getting younger Family Peer siblings to school. Maybe the student dreads school. Why, then, status groups Physical Abilities do we use these cultural categories to label people at all? traits INTERSECTIONALITY.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II®

National origin

Sexual identity Ethnicity

Work experience

Source: Image created by Ellen Usher.

Social class Age

Political affiliation

The identity wheel in Figure 2.2 offers some broad categories behind which are many more terms people use to describe their cultural affiliations and social group memberships. As we will explore throughout the book, language is a powerful cultural tool. Behind the words we use are concepts that have been socially agreed upon, sometimes through force and domination. For example, the concept of “nationality” has often been decided based on who conquered a given territory or people. Similarly, the concept of “race” has been redefined over centuries using an assortment of arbitrarily selected physical features, including skin pigmentation.

CULTURAL GROUPS AND TERMINOLOGY. Geographic location

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Neuroscientist Lisa Feldman Barrett (2020) observed that we tend to “treat the labeled boxes as if they’re part of nature when in fact we build them. . . . We, as a culture, choose the features of discrimination and draw dividing lines that magnify the differences between the group we call ‘us’ and the group we call ‘them’” (pp. 121–122). When labels are used in ways that show an inclination toward or against members of a certain group, the label is considered biased and can cause harm. Consider how children might feel when the term “illegal alien” (versus “immigrant” or “refugee”) is used to describe them. As an educator, you should strive to use bias-free language. This can be challenging because language is dynamic and fluid, changing over time and among groups. Also, the labels that in-group members use to describe themselves might differ from those used by out-group members. For example, “Latinx” has been used increasingly as a gender-inclusive alternative to “Latino” or “Latina” to refer to people in the United States with ethnic roots in Latin America or Spain. However, a recent study found that only 25% of Hispanic-identifying people had heard of the term and only 3% actually use it (Noe-Bustamante et al., 2020). How should you determine which labels to use? What will you teach your students about labels? There is no set of rigid rules for the “correct” terms to use. However, some terms are considered oppressive or obsolete and are therefore always unacceptable. The American Psychological Association has published guidelines for bias-free language. One overarching recommendation is that it is best “to use the terms that individuals and/or communities use to describe themselves, their experiences, and their practices” (APA, 2019, p. 131). However, if the terms that people use to describe themselves are considered stigmatizing, it is best to avoid using them. Remember that language holds great power. When in doubt, respect people’s humanity. We will all make mistakes, but we can apologize and use the more appropriate term next time. In this book, we will make linguistic decisions as responsibly as we can, knowing that some readers will prefer that we had used different terminology and that language is always evolving. You can read more specific recommendations about language related to race, ethnicity, gender, socioeconomic status, disability, and sexual orientation here: https://apastyle.apa.org/style-grammar-guidelines/bias-free-language.

Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Discrimination Now that you have explored your own cultural background and reflected on the many intersecting cultural groups to which you belong, let’s look at how you have learned to understand people in social and cultural groups similar to and different from your own. We will specifically examine the human tendency to categorize people and things and what can happen when those categorizations become too rigid and lead to cultural conflicts, prejudice, and discrimination. Understanding this process will help you develop more culturally supportive practices and support your students’ social development. The social and cultural information that your mind has been sifting through during your lifetime—your “database”—permits you to quickly categorize things, people, and places according to the common features that you have learned about them. As you will learn in later clusters, your mind works more efficiently by grouping things together based on their common features rather than by treating things individually. We develop schemas—organized bodies of knowledge—about objects, events, and actions. STEREOTYPES: THE GOOD AND THE BAD.

Stop & Think

List three traits most characteristic of:

College freshmen Politicians Athletes Buddhists Members of the National Rifle Association

43

44 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity Stereotype A widely held, often oversimplified, schema that organizes knowledge or perceptions about a category.

Prejudice Prejudgment or irrational generalization about an entire category of people.

Bias A prejudicial preference or action.

From a very young age, we also form schemas, or stereotypes, about groups of people. For the Stop & Think activity on the previous page, your mind consulted your experiential database to help furnish you with a list of several traits most characteristic of college freshmen, politicians, athletes, Buddhists, and members of the National Rifle Association. That list would reveal your stereotypes about each group. These stereotypes organize what you know, believe, and feel about the group. Nothing is inherently wrong with stereotyping. Throughout your life, your mind has been busy gathering and categorizing data to help you understand the world around you and make predictions about what will happen. Stereotypes are based on your own social and cultural experiences. This includes your exposure to media such as television, films, books, and the Internet. You passively consume a lot of information each day and are not always aware of the massive number of implicit associations your brain is making along the way (Greenwald & Lai, 2020). Anytime you encounter a new situation—even as you read the words in this textbook—your mind will refer back to your own experiences to help you make sense of it. This is a natural starting place. But what if some of that information is incomplete or, worse, inaccurate or biased? You probably also know that stereotypes can be problematic. One reason for this is that stereotypes are oversimplified (e.g., all Californians are relaxed). Another is that stereotypes are based on limited data (I will not meet every Californian or learn everything about each one of them). Even seemingly positive stereotypes can lead to problems. For example, Asians and Asian Americans have been stereotyped as “model minority” students—quiet, hardworking, passive students who universally succeed (Yi et al., 2020). These stereotypes can reinforce conformity and stifle assertiveness. They also set up harmful social comparisons between Asian-heritage students and students of other ethnicities. Stacey Lee and her colleagues have described another stereotype confronting Asian Americans: They are seen as perpetual foreigners (Lee et al., 2017). No matter how many decades their families have lived in America, even fourth- or fifthgeneration Asian American students might not be perceived as “real” Americans (J. Lee & Zhou, 2015). In fact, researchers have shown that teachers tend to refer to these students as “Asian,” not as “Asian American” or “American. ” Too often, students who are the target of teachers’ cultural stereotypes take them to heart and feel pressured, burdened, or even invisible. FROM STEREOTYPES TO PREJUDICE. When people think stereotypically about social groups, they tend to make evaluations about two group characteristics: warmth (Are members of this group friendly? Trustworthy?) and competence (Are people in this group capable? Assertive?). For example, older adults are often stereotyped as high in warmth but low in assertiveness. How would you rate each group in the previous Stop & Think activity on these two dimensions? According to psychologist Susan Fiske (2018), your evaluation of social groups leads you hold certain feelings toward people within them. For instance, people admire those whom they judge to be high in warmth and competence, but they feel pity or contempt for those who lack these qualities. Research has shown that these feelings about a group (e.g., admiration, contempt, pity) can quickly give way to prejudice—rigid and unfair generalizations about an entire category of people. For example, you are prejudiced against people who are overweight if you think they are lazy, feel disgusted when you see them, and refuse to date them (Aboud et al., 2012). Likewise, bias refers to a prejudicial preference or action. Like stereotypes, prejudice and bias can be positive or negative; that is, you can have positive as well as negative irrational beliefs about a group. They can be directed toward individuals of any group categorization, such as race, ethnicity, religion, politics, age, geographic location, language, sexual orientation, gender, ability, or appearance. How do our prejudices develop? Prejudice starts early. Humans have a tendency to divide the social world into two categories: us and them, or the in-group and the out-group. We tend to see members of

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

45

the out-group as inferior to and different from us but similar to each other—“they all look alike.” Even in infancy, children are more likely to accept a toy from people familiar to them (Rhodes & Baron, 2019). Results from studies around the world show that prejudice based on ethnic group membership begins by age 4 or 5 (Aboud et al., 2012; Anzures et al., 2013). In-group and out-group prejudices might be based on race, religion, gender, age, ethnicity, sexual orientation, or even athletic team membership. Another source of prejudice is that people tend to make favorable attributions to themselves and less favorable attributions to others, thus assuming their own superiority. For example, people who have more (money, social status, prestige) might justify their privilege by claiming it was earned entirely by their individual efforts and skills. This can lead to blaming the victims: People who live in poverty or women who are raped are seen as causing their problems by their behaviors—“they got what they deserved.” What follows can be the myth of meritocracy—the idea that all people are on equal playing fields and that success is just a matter of working hard. This overlooks the many unearned advantages or disadvantages people have by virtue of their life circumstances, such as when or where they were born. Emotions play a part as well. When things go wrong, we look for someone or some whole group to blame. For example, after the global coronavirus outbreak, some people vented their anger by attacking innocent Asian Americans (Lee & Waters, 2021). But prejudice is more than a tendency to form in-groups, a self-justification, or an emotional reaction—prejudice is also the product of a large storehouse of information and values that we gather throughout our lives. Children learn both explicitly and implicitly about traits and characteristics that are (or are not) valued by their families, peers, and teachers and by the broader world around them. The media also perpetuates stereotypical messages about who is (and is not) successful. For years, the “powerful” and “smart” characters presented in books, films, television, and advertising were European Americans and men. People of different ethnic and racial backgrounds were seldom the “heroes” (Ward, 2004). This is changing. In 2020, the United States elected its first African American, southeast Asian American, and female Vice President; its first female Secretary of the Treasury; and its first African American Secretary of Defense. Even so, seeing historically oppressed groups acknowledged favorably by the media is not a given. Negative portrayals in the media of women, racial minorities, and certain religious minority groups have been amplified in some media outlets in recent years and by some elected officials. The Southern Poverty Law Center reported a surge in White nationalism, hate speech, racism, and far-right extremism during the 2016 and 2020 presidential campaigns (see Costello, 2016, https://www.splcenter.org). How does this affect young learners and, particularly, those in minoritized groups? When learners feel that they must constantly monitor the environment for possible stereotypes and prejudice, their academic performance can be undermined—a concept referred to as stereotype threat. The term stereotype threat refers to an “apprehensiveness about confirming a stereotype” (Aronson, 2002, p. 282). The basic idea is that when individuals are in situations in which a stereotype applies, they bear extra emotional and cognitive burdens—the possibilities of confirming the stereotype, either in the eyes of others or in their own eyes. So, when girls are asked to solve complicated mathematics problems or when African Americans take the SATs, for example, they are at risk of confirming widely held stereotypes that girls are inferior to boys in mathematics or that African Americans score lower on the SATs than do other ethnic groups. Believing the stereotype is not necessary: All that matters is that the individual is aware of the stereotype and cares about performing well enough to disprove its unflattering implications. Steven Spencer and his colleagues (2016) pointed out that “every individual is potentially vulnerable to stereotype threat, because every individual has at least one social

STEREOTYPE THREAT.

Stereotype threat The emotional and cognitive burdens that can result from heightened awareness that your performance in an academic situation might confirm a stereotype that others hold about you.

46 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity identity that is targeted by a negative stereotype in some given situation” (p. 415). The impact seems to be worse for learners who strongly identify with the group or activity under threat (“I am proud to be African American,” “Science is really important to me!”) (Appel & Kronberger, 2012; Huguet & Régner, 2007). For example, studies have shown that stereotype threat has worsened the performance of students from lower-SES backgrounds, older adult test takers, elite athletes, White male college students who were very strong in mathematics but were told that Asian students performed much better on a particular test, and school-age boys who believed they were inferior to girls (Hartley & Sutton, 2013; Spencer et al., 2016). How, exactly, does the threat of confirming a stereotype affect learners? Experiencing a psychological threat can (a) prevent individuals from performing at their best on tests and assignments; (b) interfere with attention, working memory, and learning in the subject (e.g., math); and (c) decrease connections to and valuing of that subject (Spencer et al., 2016). Thus, stereotype threat might be one cause, but not the sole cause, of performance differences between some groups (Nadler & Clark, 2011). Students who experience stereotype threat are less likely to feel a sense of belonging and connection in the context where the threat is “in the air.” When they feel disconnected, motivation and engagement suffer (Thoman et al., 2013). They might lose interest, develop self-defeating strategies to avoid looking stupid, or feel anxious in testing situations. (Read more about these in Cluster 12.) Experiencing persistent threats can lead to negative, long-term consequences such as withdrawal of effort or even dropping out of school. As soon as students psychologically disengage from school, they are unlikely to exert the effort needed for real learning. One way that teachers can help reduce these effects is by developing a curriculum that provides rich counterevidence to stereotypical messages and by affirming students in their diverse identities (Spencer et al., 2016). As we have seen, prejudice consists of rigid, irrational beliefs and feelings (usually negative) about an entire category of people. The third element of prejudice is a tendency to act, called discrimination. Discrimination refers to unequal treatment of particular groups of people. Clearly, many Americans face prejudice and discrimination in subtle or blatant ways every day. Mistreatment of unarmed Black men by the police is an example of discrimination with life-and-death consequences. However, ample evidence of discrimination can be found in childhood and adolescence, from the playground to the classroom to the principal’s office. Although prejudice and discrimination come from many places—peers, media, social encounters outside of school—evidence suggests that perceiving discrimination from a teacher may be the most damaging for educational outcomes (Benner & Graham, 2013). As noted earlier, teachers are often unaware of their prejudices. Yet these prejudices can affect teachers’ expectations for their students and how they interpret students’ behaviors. In her book Discrimination in Childhood and Adolescence, psychologist Christia Brown (2017) shows that even young children are highly attuned to whether they are welcomed and included by others at school. Perceiving that one is being unfairly treated can have negative psychological, physical, academic, and social consequences in both the short and the long terms. Feeling undervalued, overlooked, or excluded makes staying focused and motivated at school difficult. In addition, being the target of others’ prejudices and discrimination can lead learners to feel that they must constantly monitor the environment for further prejudice and discrimination, causing immense stress and undermining academic achievement. In the sections that follow, we will look more closely at how members of different cultural groups experience school. Specifically, we focus on economic and social class, ethnicity and race, gender and sexual identities, and how learners’ experiences are related to learning, development, and motivation. We will also share evidence about how groupbased stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination can alter learners’ self-beliefs, motivation, emotions, and learning.

FROM PREJUDICE TO DISCRIMINATION.

Discrimination Treating unfairly or acting unfairly toward particular categories of people.

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

47

Module 3 Summary Education Is Cultural (pp. 37–39) What is culture, and how does cultural diversity affect learning and teaching? There are many conceptions of culture, but most include the knowledge, skills, rules, traditions, beliefs, and values that guide behavior in a particular group of people: Culture is a program for living. The differences between cultures might be very obvious, tip-of-the-iceberg characteristics, or they might be very subtle, below-the-surface differences. When subtle cultural differences meet, misunderstandings and conflicts are common. These conflicts can happen when the values and competencies of the dominant, mainstream culture are used to determine what is considered “normal” or appropriate behavior in schools. In these cases, children who have been socialized in different cultures might be perceived as acting inappropriately, not following the rules, or being rude and disrespectful.

Your Cultural and Educational History (pp. 39–43) Why is it important to understand your own cultural background? Everyone is a member of many cultural groups, defined in terms of geographic region, nationality, ethnicity, race, gender, social class, and religion. Membership in a particular group does not determine behavior or values but makes certain values and kinds of behaviors more likely. Wide variations exist within each group. You met two individuals, Ternice and Jessie, who embody that diversity. What terms should I use when describing my own and others’ cultures? Examining the different social groups to which we belong can serve to guide the terms we use. However, labels that show a prejudicial evaluation for or against a group should be avoided. When in doubt, use the terms that individuals in the group would prefer you to use when describing their group and its practices. Remember that intersectionality refers to the overlapping cultural identities that people have. These might shape individuals in unique ways that no single group would have done.

Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Discrimination (pp. 43–46) What are the differences among prejudice, discrimination, and stereotype threat? Prejudice is a rigid and unfair generalization—a prejudgment or attitude—about an entire category of people. Prejudice might target people in particular racial, ethnic, religious, political, geographic, or language groups, or it might be directed toward the gender or sexual orientation of an individual. Discrimination is unequal treatment of or actions toward particular categories of people. Stereotype threat is the extra emotional and cognitive burden that your performance in an academic situation might confirm a stereotype that others hold about you. It is not necessary that the individual even believe the stereotype. All that matters is that the individual is aware of the stereotype and cares about performing well enough to disprove its unflattering implications. In the short run, the fear that you might confirm a negative stereotype can induce test anxiety and undermine performance. Over time, experiencing stereotype threat might lead to disidentification with schooling and academic achievement.

MODULE 4

Social and Diversity Economic

Learning Objective 2.2 Discuss what defines social class and socioeconomic status, including how SES differences relate to school achievement.

Economic and Social Class Differences Even though most researchers would agree that social class is one of the most meaningful cultural dimensions in people’s lives, those same researchers have great difficulty defining social class (Liu et al., 2004; Macionis, 2019). Different terms are used—social class, socioeconomic status, economic background, wealth, poverty, capital, and privilege. Some people consider only

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Economic Conditions/Socioeconomic Status (SES) (IV, B2) Be aware of the possible effects of SES on student achievement. Consider what steps teachers can take to minimize those effects.

48 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity economic differences; others add considerations of power, influence, mobility, educational attainment, occupational status, control over resources, access to opportunities, and prestige.

Social Class and Socioeconomic Status

Socioeconomic status (SES) Relative standing in a society based on income, power, background, and prestige.

Most researchers identify five general social class distinctions in U.S. society: upper, upper middle, middle, working, and lower classes. The main characteristics of these five groups in the United States are summarized in Table 2.2. These are rough approximations; people’s lived experiences might not fall neatly within these categorical lines. Across the past five decades, the number of American adults living in middle-income households has steadily decreased as the number of wealthy and poor households in America has increased (Horowitz et al., 2020a). The richest 5% of families in the United States have seen the most rapid growth in wealth. The trends reflect growing income inequality in the United States, which outpaces that of other countries. Another way to assess people’s standing in society commonly used in research by sociologists and psychologists is by combining variations in wealth, power, control over resources, and prestige into an index called socioeconomic status (SES). SES is usually ascribed to people by researchers; different formulas for determining SES might lead to different categorizations (Macionis, 2019; Sirin, 2005). No single variable, not even income, is an effective measure of SES. Rather, SES refers to a composite ranking of these various measures of social inequality. You might be thinking that your own social standing does not fit into any of these rigid categories. One reason is that levels of wealth, power, and prestige are not always consistent. Some people—for instance, university professors—are members of professions that are reasonably high in terms of social status and prestige but that provide considerably less wealth or power (believe me). Other people have political power even though they are not wealthy, or they might be members of the elite social register in a town even though their family money is long gone.

Table 2.2 Selected Characteristics of Different Social Class Memberships UPPER CLASS

UPPER MIDDLE CLASS

MIDDLE CLASS

WORKING CLASS

LOWER CLASS

33%

20%

Percentage of U.S. population

5%

Income

$239,000 to billions

$134,000–$238,000

$55,000–$134,000

$30,000–$55,000

Below $27,000

Occupation/Source of Money

Family money, “old money,” investments, CEO

Corporate, professional, income earned in some way

White-collar, skilled blue-collar

Blue-collar

Minimum wage, unskilled labor

Education

Home-schooled, tutors, prestigious private schools and colleges

Prestigious colleges and graduate schools

High school, college, or professional school

High school; about 20% go to college

High school

Home Ownership

Several homes, private jets for transportation

At least one home

Usually own home

About half own a home

About 40% own a home

40–45%

Health Coverage

Full

Full

Usually

Limited

Uncommon

Neighborhoods

The most exclusive

Exclusive or comfortable

Comfortable

Modest

Deteriorating, least desirable areas

Afford Children’s College

Easily

Usually

Seldom

Rarely

Uncommon

Political Power

National (perhaps international), state, local

National, state, local

State or local

Limited

No

Note: All descriptions are rough categorizations. Many exceptions can occur within each category according to individuals and context. For example, income markers differ by number in household and geographic region. In very expensive areas such as San Francisco, it may take at least $150,000 for a family of three to be in the lower middle class. Source: Information from Macionis, J. J. (2019). Sociology (17th ed.). Pearson; Gorski, P. (2013). Reaching and teaching students in poverty: Strategies for erasing the opportunity gap. Teachers College Press.

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Most people are generally aware of their social class; that is, they perceive that some groups are above them in the social class hierarchy and some are below (Diemer et al., 2013). In fact, people quickly evaluate small social cues (e.g., physical appearance, language, behaviors) to determine what social class others are in relative to them (Kraus et al., 2017). These subjective evaluations, in turn, guide people’s beliefs, behaviors, and well-being, thereby reinforcing actual social class boundaries. Students (and teachers) may even show a kind of “classism” (like racism or sexism), believing that they are superior to members of lower social classes and avoiding association with them. For example, Marissa, a member of the most popular and privileged clique in her high school, described the “grits”—the least popular group—as follows: Grits are poor. I think they mostly live in the country. We—[quickly correcting herself] some of my friends call them hicks or rednecks. I guess most live on the Hill—that’s over on the west side of town. It’s the slums. Grits smoke, do drugs, dress grungy. They have those hick accents. They usually get bad grades. They don’t like school so I think they drop out a lot. They don’t really fit in. They are troublemakers. I don’t see them much; they aren’t in any of my classes. (Brantlinger, 2004, pp. 109–110) Author Isabel Wilkerson would say that Marissa is describing a “caste” system of social hierarchy in the United States (Wilkerson, 2020). “Caste” is defined as the “invisible structure that created and maintains hierarchy and inequality” based on our many intersectional cultural group memberships, including class, wealth, power, race, gender, immigration status, and other cultural identities (pp. 69–70). This invisible structure becomes reinforced in our policies, structures, and habitual ways of thinking. A caste “affix[es] people to certain roles based upon what they look like and what they historically have been assigned to or the characteristics and stereotypes by which they have been categorized” (p. 71).

Poverty and Social Inequality Experiencing poverty can amplify other disadvantages related to a person’s social standing. In 2019, about 14.5% of Americans under the age of 18 lived below the federal poverty line of a $26,172 annual income for a family of four (Semega et al., 2020). If we add children living in low-income families (earning about $48,000 for a family of four), then 44% of all children in the United States lived in low-income or poor families, and 30 million students qualified to receive free or reduced-price lunches at school in 2016 (Jiang et al., 2016; U.S. Department of Agriculture, 2016). The absolute number of children living in poverty is similar for non-Hispanic White children (3.2 million), Latino children (4.4 million), and African American children (3.0 million). However, these numbers are misleading. The rate of poverty is much higher for African American, Latino, and Native American children—30% of African American and 24% of Latino children lived in poverty in 2018, whereas 11% of Asian and 9% of nonHispanic White children were poor (Children’s Defense Fund, 2020). Contrary to many stereotypes, the absolute number of poor children living in rural and suburban areas is greater than that in central cities. But poverty rates are high in urban schools. Childhood poverty rates declined steadily during the period of economic expansion in the United States between 2010 and 2020 (Semega et al., 2020); however, the COVID-19 pandemic is exacerbating social and economic inequalities, which are worse for Latino, African American, and indigenous children and families (Bauer et al., 2020; Benner & Mistry, 2020). Even before the pandemic, wealth gaps between the upper class and other income groups in the United States had been widening over the previous two decades; White households are approximately 10 times wealthier than Black households and 8 times wealthier than Hispanic households (Killewald et al., 2017). Discriminatory practices (e.g., lending practices, segregation, school districting) and institutionalized racism continue to put African Americans at a disadvantage when it comes to accumulating wealth.

49

50 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Poverty and Academic Outcomes Living in poverty can affect learners’ development and academic outcomes. The average correlation between SES and achievement tests is moderate, about .30 to .40 (Sackett et al., 2009; Sirin, 2005). In general, students of all ethnic groups with high SES show higher average levels of achievement on test scores and stay in school longer than students with low SES, and this difference widens with the student’s age, from 7 to 15 (Cutuli et al., 2013). And the longer the child is in poverty, the stronger the impact is on achievement. For example, even when we take into account parents’ education, the chance that children will be held back in school or placed in special education classes increases by 2% to 3% for every year the children live in poverty (Ackerman et al., 2004). Figure 2.3 shows the trends in reading achievement from third through eighth grade for students in several income risk groups (homeless/highly mobile, free lunch, reduced-cost lunch) compared both to the national average for those grades and to students who were never a part of any of these risk groups (labeled “general” in the figure). You can see that the growth rates are similar but that these groups begin at different levels in third grade. This is one reason that early interventions (preschool and primary school) are especially important for students placed at risk. One troubling trend is that the educational inequalities are growing between children from privileged families (income at the 90th percentile) and children from poor families (income at the 10th percentile). These inequalities were 30% to 40% greater for children born

Figure 2.3 Trends in Reading Achievement from Third Through Eighth Grade for Students in Several Income Risk Groups

240 Risk group

Reading achievement

General

Reduced meals National norm

220

Free meals HHM 200

180

3

4

5

6

7

8

Grade Note: General = students who were never a part of any of the risk groups; HHM = homeless/highly mobile. Source: Cutuli, J. J., Desjardins, C. D., Herbers, J. E., Long, J. D., Heistad, D., Chan, C-K, Hinz, E., & Masten, A. S. (2013). Academic achievement trajectories of homeless and highly mobile students: Resilience in the context of chronic and acute risk. Child Development, 84, p. 851. Reproduced with permission of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

in 2001 compared to children born in 1976. These increasingly dramatic income differences between the wealthy and the poor have led to greater segregation of low-income children in lower-quality schools. Economic resources available to wealthy children allow them to reap tremendous advantages from informal learning opportunities, such as travel, specialized summer camps, and tutoring services—resources that poor families can’t afford (Berliner, 2013; Reardon, 2011). This has led some researchers to refer to differences in the educational performances of rich and poor students as an “opportunity gap” rather than an achievement gap (Gorski, 2013; Milner, 2010, 2015). When it comes to the effects of poverty on academic achievement, however, it is important to remember that correlation is not causation. In fact, determining how and why poverty affects children’s performance can be challenging. Researchers have studied many related factors, such as family and environmental stress, availability of resources, the types of educational investments parents make, school and neighborhood characteristics, and cultural beliefs and expectations (Diemer et al., 2020; Duncan et al., 2017). Figure 2.4 provides some examples; others include poor health care, dangerous home environments, interruptions in schooling, and exposure to violence, overcrowding, homelessness, and discrimination. In turn, academic difficulties can lead to low-paying jobs—and another generation born into poverty. Numerous scholars have offered other possible explanations (Ehrlich, 2020; Gorski, 2013; Jensen, 2016). Let’s take a closer look at some of them. HEALTH, ENVIRONMENT, AND STRESS. The negative effects of poverty begin even before a child is born. Families in poverty have less access to good prenatal and infant health care and nutrition. More than half of all adolescent mothers receive no prenatal care at all. Poor mothers and adolescent mothers are more likely to have premature babies, and prematurity is associated with many cognitive and learning problems. Children in poverty are more likely to be exposed to both legal drugs (nicotine, alcohol) and illegal drugs (cocaine, heroin) before birth. Children whose mothers take drugs during pregnancy can have problems with organization, attention, and language skills. Poor children are four times as likely to experience chronic stress due to evictions, lack of food, overcrowding, or utility disconnections. Increased stress is related to increased school absences, decreased attention span and concentration ability, problems with memory and thinking, reduced motivation and effort, increased depression, and reduced neurogenesis (growth of new brain cells) (Blair & Raver, 2016; Evans & Kim,

Figure 2.4 Mechanisms by Which Poverty Affects Children’s Educational Outcomes Family and Environmental Stress • • • • •

Threats to basic needs Inability to buy goods/services Stress/depression Greater household hostility Exposure to environmental hazards

Culture • • • • •

Norms, behaviors, and values Beliefs and worldview Cultural scripts Structural and economic factors Neighborhood/community context

How Might Poverty Affect Child Outcomes? Resources and Investments Caregivers influence children via: • Skills, preferences, and values • Availability of financial resources • Learning and enrichment opportunities • Decisions made on behalf of children

Developmental Factors • • • • •

Brain development Cognitive and emotional control (e.g., executive functions) Cumulative or snowballing effects Timing and duration of poverty Anxiety, internalized oppression

Source: Duncan, G. J., Magnuson, K., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2017). Moving beyond correlations in assessing the consequences of poverty. Annual Review of Psychology, 68(1), 413–434.

51

52 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity 2013; Jensen, 2013). In the early years, children in poverty experience higher levels of stress hormones than do children in middle-class and wealthy families. High levels of these hormones can interfere with the flow of blood in the brain, decrease the development of synaptic connections, and deplete the body’s supply of tryptophan, an amino acid that calms impulsive and violent behaviors (Hudley & Novak, 2007). As they grow, poor children breathe more polluted air and drink more contaminated water—recall the scandal surrounding the water in Flint, Michigan. Poor children are at least twice as likely as non-poor children to have lead poisoning, which is associated with lower school achievement and long-term neurological impairment (Evans, 2004). Economically disadvantaged children and particularly those who also experience maltreatment or discrimination are at increased risk of obesity, high blood pressure, and other inflammatory diseases (Boyce et al., 2021). Exposure to these and other environmental stressors during critical periods of development can affect many aspects of neurological, cognitive, and emotional development (Hyde et al., 2020). Poor students are also at risk of being stigmatized by their teachers and peers. For example, students from homes with limited resources might wear older clothes or be less familiar with certain books and school activities. Their teachers and other students might in turn believe that these students are not bright. The teacher might avoid calling on them, assuming they don’t know the answer, set lower standards, place them in lower-ability groups, and accept poor work. Even without awareness, teachers and peers can send social “signals” that relay a message to students lower in socioeconomic standing that they are less valued (Piff et al., 2018). Thus, low expectations become institutionalized, and the educational resources provided to these children are minimal and inadequate (Borman & Overman, 2004). Low expectations, along with a lower-quality educational experience, can lead children to internalize others’ negative evaluations and develop a sense of learned helplessness, described in Cluster 12 (Ruck et al., 2019). Students with low SES, particularly those who also encounter racial discrimination, might decide that school is a dead end. Without a high school diploma, these students find few rewards awaiting them in the work world. Many available jobs barely pay a living wage. In his 2015 book Rac(e)ing to Class, Richard Milner describes the ways in which the intersection of poverty and race has led to chronic inequalities in the educational opportunities of poor students of color.

LOW EXPECTATIONS—LOW ACADEMIC SELF-CONCEPT.

Students who attend schools where most of their peers are from middle- and high-income families are 68% more likely to attend college compared to students in schools where most of the students are from low-income homes. Even after controlling for many possible causes, Gregory Palardy (2013) concluded that peer influences are the strongest predictors of this difference in college attendance. Students in high-poverty schools are much less likely to have friends who plan to attend college and much more likely to have friends who drop out of school. Some researchers have suggested that certain students from low-SES backgrounds might cope with the inequities around them by becoming part of a resistance culture. To members of this culture, making it in school means selling out and trying to act “middle class.” To maintain their identity and their status within the group, these students might cope by rejecting the behaviors that would make them succeed in school—studying, cooperating with teachers, and even coming to class (Bennett, 2011; Ogbu, 1987, 1997). This is an understandable reaction from students who might view such attempts as futile in an oppressive or discriminatory system (Cokley et al., 2011). John Ogbu linked identification with a resistance culture to poor Latino American, Native American, and African American groups, but similar reactions have been noted for poor White students both in the United States and in England and also in high school students in Papua New Guinea (Woolfolk Hoy et al., 2002).

PEER INFLUENCES AND RESISTANCE CULTURES.

Resistance culture Group values and beliefs about refusing to adopt the behaviors and attitudes of the majority culture.

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

This is not to say that all or even most students with low SES resist achievement. Many young people are high achievers in spite of either their economic situation or negative peer influences (O’Connor, 1997). And we should not forget that some aspects of schooling— competitive grading, public reprimands, stressful testing and assignments, and repetitive work that is too hard or too easy—can (and probably should) encourage resistance in all students (Okagaki, 2001). Focusing solely on students’ resistance (or their lack of resilience) is a way of blaming students for their lower achievement; instead, educators should focus on making school an inclusive place that does not invite resistance from anyone (Stinson, 2006). One way to do this is by realizing how our own cultural backgrounds, socialization, and rigid stereotypes “might hinder our abilities to connect with low-income families, or any families, in the most authentic, open way” (Gorski, 2013, p. 59). In Gorski’s words, educators need to become “equity literate.” HOME ENVIRONMENT AND RESOURCES. Families in poverty seldom have access to highquality preschool care for their young children—the kind of care that enhances cognitive and social development (Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Vandell, 2004). Poor children read less and spend more time watching television; they have less access to books, computers, the Internet, libraries, and trips—opportunity gaps again (Kim & Guryan, 2010). Even the ways in which parents talk with their children can vary by SES (Rowe, 2018). Again, these are averages and do not apply to all low-income families. Many families provide rich learning environments for their children and help them develop considerable strengths by coping with adversity (Frankenhuis & Nettle, 2020). When parents of any SES level support and encourage their children—by reading to them, providing books and cognitively stimulating toys, taking the children to the library, making time and space for learning—the children tend to become better, more enthusiastic readers (Cooper et al., 2010). Home and neighborhood resources seem to have the greatest impact on children’s achievement when school is not in session. SUMMER SETBACKS AND SCHOOL DISRUPTIONS. Children in poverty begin school about 6 months behind in reading skills compared to those from wealthier homes, but the skill difference between the groups grows to almost 3 years by sixth grade. This gap in reading skills between students in poverty and middle-class students has been increasing since the early 1970s. One explanation for this growing gap is that the children from poorer homes, and especially those whose first language in not English, lose ground over the summer, when school is not in session. Even though both groups make comparable achievement gains during the school year, every summer vacation creates about a 3-month reading achievement gap between poor and advantaged children (Kim & Guryan, 2010; Kim & Quinn, 2013). One study suggested that the four summer vacations between second and sixth grade accounted for 80% of the achievement differences between poor and advantaged students (Allington & McGill-Frazen, 2003, 2008). This truly is a case of the rich getting richer. Wealthier children have greater access to books all the time but especially over the summer. They read more, and the more children read, the better readers they become—volume of reading matters. The good news is that quality summer reading programs for low-income families and their children can be effective in helping students improve their reading skills (Kim & Quinn, 2013). The COVID-19 pandemic has widened the economic and opportunity gaps between students whose families face economic hardships and those who are financially stable. One immediately apparent gap emerged when schools closed to in-person instruction and instructional delivery moved online. Suddenly the classroom became the living room, bedroom, or kitchen. Resources available in most school districts, such as reliable access to learning materials, regular meal service, and transportation, were disrupted. Many families—including some teachers—lack sufficient resources, technological equipment, and access to Internet connectivity to participate in high-quality remote instruction. In some school districts, students have been alternating between remote and in-person learning

53

54 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity for many months, with no clear end in sight. In schools that have been unable to deliver online instruction to some or all students, teachers have been doing their best to get materials to students to learn at home. Student absenteeism is at an all-time high; by one estimate, 3 million U.S. students did not attend school in 2020–2021 (Korman et al., 2020). Scholars have projected that students will experience disproportionate learning loss as a function of their SES and prior achievement levels (Kuhfield et al., 2020). Stories abound about the ways in which the pandemic has highlighted and exacerbated economic inequalities and opportunity gaps. These gaps are worse for ethnic minority students, English language learners, students with disabilities, and students experiencing homelessness. A final explanation for the lower achievement of many students who live in low-SES environments is that these students are more often placed in lower academic tracks, and they are therefore actually taught differently (Oakes, 2005). If they are tracked into “low-ability,” “general,” “practical,” or “vocational” classes, they might be taught to memorize and to learn more passively than their more advantaged peers. Middleclass students are more likely to be encouraged to think and be creative in their classes. Even if they are not tracked, students from low-income families are more likely to attend schools with inadequate resources and less effective teachers (Evans, 2004; Goldhaber et al., 2018). For example, in high-poverty schools, more than 50% of math teachers and more than 60% of science teachers are inexperienced or teaching outside their subject area expertise; they were not trained for the subjects they are teaching (Jensen, 2009). When students with low SES receive a substandard education, this gives them inferior academic skills and limits their life chances, beginning with not preparing them for higher education (Knapp & Woolverton, 2003). Having effective teachers improves students’ short- and long-term learning, school attendance, and graduation rates.

TRACKING AND POOR TEACHING.

Extreme Poverty: Homeless and Highly Mobile Students According to the National Center for Children in Poverty, nearly 10% of U.S. children who are 9 years old and younger live in deep poverty—their family income is less than half of the federal poverty line (Nguyen et al., 2020). When families live in extreme poverty, they sometimes lack even a stable home. In the 2018–2019 school year, nearly 1.4 million students were homeless in the United States (National Center for Homeless Education, 2021). That represents about 2.7% of all enrolled students. Those experiencing homelessness in adolescence were more likely Black, Hispanic, lesbian, gay, bisexual, or transgender, partly because of increased exposure to victimization and abuse (Morton et al., 2018; Tierney & Ward, 2017). Students who are homeless or who move very often are at additional risk of a range of physical and mental health problems and social and learning difficulties (Herbers et al., 2020). For example, even after taking many other risk factors and income levels into account, researchers found that students who moved three or more times during a school year were 60% more likely to repeat a grade (Cutuli et al., 2013). According to the National Center on Family Homelessness, 75% of elementary students and 85% of high school students who were homeless performed below grade level in reading and math. Homelessness and high mobility contribute to chronic risks and problems in school, problems that are difficult to overcome. Even with such risks, many of these students are resilient. Cutuli and his colleagues (2013) analyzed the math and reading test scores of more than 26,000 students from third through eighth grades and found that 45% of the homeless and highly mobile students achieved in the average-or-better range across time in spite of their challenges. The researchers concluded that factors such as effective parenting, student self-regulation skills (see Cluster 11), academic motivation (see Cluster 12), and the quality of teaching and teachers’ relationships with students (see this whole book) support resiliency for these students. The early years in school are particularly important. Homeless students who develop reading and self-regulation skills in the early grades are more likely to be successful throughout school (Buckner, 2012). For more ideas about quality teaching for students who live in poverty, see the Guidelines: Teaching Students Who Live in Poverty on the next page.

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

GUIDELINES Teaching Students Who Live in Poverty Educate yourself about the effects of poverty on student learning. Examples 1. 2.

Read articles from good journals. Seek diverse sources to inform you. For example: a. Eric Jensen’s (2013) Engaging Students with Poverty in Mind: Practical Strategies for Raising Achievement b. Paul Gorski’s (2013) Reaching and Teaching Students in Poverty: Strategies for Erasing the Opportunity Gap

Set and maintain high expectations for all students. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Guard against feeling sorry for students, excusing poor work, and expecting less. Replace pity with empathy based on solid knowledge of your students. Communicate to students that they can improve and succeed with good effort. Engage higher-order thinking skills that validate students’ intellectual capacities. Provide constructive criticism because you believe your students can do quality work. Add challenging subjects and Advanced Placement classes.

Develop caring relationships with your students (see the 2015 Center for Promise report “Don’t Quit on Me”). Examples 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Use inclusive language—“our class,” “our projects,” “our school,” “our efforts.” Talk to students outside class. Make a point to identify their interests and abilities. Attend sports or other events in which your students participate. Create a class Welcome Center for families (see Cluster 6). Help a student connect with a mentor, tutor, and/or coach.

Build learning and self-regulation skills as part of the curriculum. Examples 1. 2.

Teach students how to organize work, focus attention, or seek appropriate help. Include conflict management and social problem-solving skills in lessons.

Notice health problems. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Notice who seems to be absent or tardy often. Check to see whether some students struggle to hear the class discussions. Can they see from the back of the room? Model healthy eating and physical activity.

Assess student knowledge; start where they are, but don’t stay there (Milner, 2010). Examples 1. 2.

Use short, ungraded assessments that target the learning objectives for each unit. Differentiate instruction based on results (see Cluster 14).

Source: Jensen, E. (2013). Engaging students with poverty in mind: Practical strategies for raising achievement. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.; Gorski, P. (2013). Reaching and teaching students in poverty: Strategies for erasing the opportunity gap. New York, NY: Teachers College Press; Center for Promise. (2015). Don’t quit on me: What young people who left school say about the power of relationships. Washington, DC: America’s Promise Alliance.

55

56 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Module 4 Summary Economic and Social Class Differences (pp. 47–55) What is SES, and how does it differ from social class? Social class reflects a group’s prestige and power in a society. Most people are aware of the social class that they share with similar peers. Sociologists use the term SES for variations in wealth, power, control over resources, and prestige. Socioeconomic status is determined by several factors—not just income—and often overpowers other cultural differences. No single variable is an effective measure of SES, but most researchers identify four general levels of SES: upper, middle, working, and lower classes. The main characteristics of these different SES levels are summarized in Table 2.2. What is the relationship between SES and school achievement? Socioeconomic status and academic achievement are moderately correlated. Students of all ethnic groups with high SES show higher average levels of achievement on test scores and stay in school longer than students with low SES. The longer the child is in poverty, the stronger the impact is on achievement. Why is there a correlation between SES and school achievement? Students with low SES might experience inadequate health care, teachers’ lowered expectations of them, low self-esteem, learned helplessness, participation in resistance cultures, school tracking, understimulating home environments, and summer setbacks. This last, striking finding is that children with low SES lose academic ground outside school over the summer, whereas children with higher SES continue to advance.

MODULE 5

Ethnic and Racial Diversity

Learning Objective 2.3 Explain how race, ethnicity, prejudice, discrimination, and stereotype threat might affect student learning and achievement in schools.

Ethnicity and Race in Teaching and Learning The United States truly is a diverse society. The National Center for Education Statistics projects that by fall 2029, more than half of the school-age population will be African American, Asian, Latino/a, or from other ethnic groups (National Center for Education Statistics, 2020). Before we look at the research on ethnicity and race in school settings, let’s understand how people come to understand their own and others’ ethnic and racial group membership.

Defining Ethnicity and Race Ethnicity A cultural heritage shared by a group of people.

Race A socially constructed category based on appearances and ancestry.

Ethnicity usually refers to a group’s shared, common cultural characteristics such as history, homeland, language, traditions, or religion. We all have some ethnic heritage, whether our background is Italian, Ukrainian, Hmong, Chinese, Japanese, Navajo, Hawaiian, Puerto Rican, Cuban, Hungarian, German, African, or Irish—to name only a few. Race is defined as a socially constructed category of people who share certain physical characteristics (e.g., skin color or hair texture) that members of a society have considered important ( Macionis, 2019). There are no biologically pure races. For any two humans chosen at random, an average of only 0.012% (about one-hundredth of 1%) of the alphabetic sequence of their genetic codes is different due to race (Myers, 2005). Both ethnicity and race are therefore primarily social constructions that gain meaning within particular social or political contexts (Lee & Bean, 2010). At the individual level, they become part of our identity—how we understand ourselves and interact with others. At the group level, both ethnicity and race are often institutionally imposed labels involving educational, economic, political, and power structures (Macionis, 2019). In this book we use the labels ethnicity, race, or race/ethnicity based on which label seems most appropriate to the findings being discussed. Sociologists sometimes use the term minority group to label a group of people that receives unequal or discriminatory treatment. But this practice of referring to people as

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

57

minorities because of their racial or ethnic heritage has been criticized because doing so is misleading and has negative historical connotations (APA, 2019; Milner, 2010). For example, in certain places such as Chicago and Mississippi, African Americans are in the numeric majority, and European Americans are in the numeric minority. Unless otherwise specified, when we refer to ethnic minority groups, we do so using the entire U.S. population as a reference group. We often use the terms White, Black, and Latino instead of European American, African American, and Hispanic, respectively, because the former terms are more inclusive of students with diverse national origins (e.g., Canada, Haiti, or the Dominican Republic) and various immigration statuses. As we noted earlier in the cluster, teachers should be mindful of their own use of these terms when addressing their students’ racial/ethnic heritages. (For more information on terminology, see Christia Brown’s 2017 book about students’ perceptions of discrimination and the link to the APA’s guidelines for reducing bias provided earlier.)

Ethnic and Racial Identity At some point during their development, young people begin to understand themselves as members of racial and ethnic groups. Young children often make little or no distinction between their own race and their ethnic identity—the two are inextricably intertwined. Moreover, although a group may share an ethnicity, its members can include people of different races or vice versa. Therefore, researchers have suggested that terms such as racialethnic-cultural identity may be more appropriate when attempting to accurately represent the psychological experiences of young people (Umaña-Taylor et al., 2014). Ethnic-racial identity generally refers to how people think and feel about their own ethnic and racial groups and the significance and meaning they ascribe to them (Seaton et al., 2018; Umaña-Taylor et al., 2014). Identity evaluations might include how important or central one’s ethnicity or race is to their life, how group members see themselves (private regard) or perceive others seeing them (public regard), or a sense of pride (see Rivas-Drake et al., 2014). Children may begin with an unexamined racial or ethnic identity, either because they have not explored at all or because they have accepted the identity labels that have been encouraged by others around them. European American adolescents are less likely to have examined their own ethnic-racial identity given their membership, particularly if most of their experiences have put them in the numeric majority. Early adolescents who have ethnically diverse friends engage in their own ethnic-racial identity exploration, which is an important part of healthy identity development (Rivas-Drake et al., 2017). You will learn more about identity development processes in Cluster 4. At the beginning of this cluster, you explored your own complex cultural identity. How would you describe your ethnic and racial identity? For many students in the United States, ethnic-racial identity is multidimensional and intersects with other identities such as nationality (Parker et al., 2015). For example, a Pew Research Center survey revealed that two thirds of Hispanic adults consider their Hispanic background to be part of their racial background. However, when asked for their race on the U.S. Census, many Hispanic adults identify more with the country or region of their ancestors (Cuba, Mexico, the Dominican Republic, the Caribbean, etc.) than with a general notion of Hispanic or Latino. Others highlight different aspects of their racial identities, such as their African heritage or Caribbean roots (Noe-Bustamante et al., 2020). Members of other ethnic and cultural groups respond in similar ways. Adolescents in immigrant or refugee families might be more motivated to identify strongly with their nationality or origin—for instance, with their Chinese, Syrian, Japanese, Filipino, Vietnamese, or Moroccan heritage in certain situations; in other situations, they might identify with their new country or with broader ethnic groups such as Asian or Black (Seaton et al., 2018; Verkuyten, 2016). Similarly, students from indigenous communities might view their tribal identity as central in some contexts but identify with colonial (i.e., White) norms in others (Galliher et al., 2017; Jones & Galliher, 2015). Determining an ethnic-racial identity may be even more complicated for biracial or multiracial adolescents. The parent they live with, the makeup of their neighborhood, their appearance, and their experiences of discrimination or support can influence how these adolescents identify themselves. Some psychologists think that these challenges help

MULTIDIMENSIONAL AND FLEXIBLE ETHNIC IDENTITIES.

Ethnic-racial identity The beliefs, feelings, significance, and meaning people have about their ethnicity or race.

58 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity multiracial youth develop stronger and more complex identities, but other psychologists argue that the challenges present an extra burden in an already-tough process (Herman, 2004; Ritchey, 2015). Perhaps the outcome depends in part on the support adolescents receive when facing the challenges. One thing is certain: The complexity of identity makes clear how limiting labels can be, particularly labels that are imposed by others who have more power. Teachers should understand that their students might be navigating multiple cultures and identities (Fuligni & Tsai, 2015). Incorporating instructional strategies that encourage students to reflect on their own and others’ ethnic-racial identities can offer needed support (Branch, 2020). Perhaps the best advice is to try to understand that many of your students will be constructing complex identities that allow them to move in and out of several cultures and languages. Get to know the individual students and respect their growing identities by creating an “identity-safe” learning environment (Spencer et al., 2016). Richard Milner (2003, 2015) has pointed to the importance of racial identity development and awareness, especially in teaching. To reckon with how we and our students have come to understand ourselves and others as members of different races is helpful for moving toward more racially just and equitable education. The process of racial identity development is also helpful for understanding people’s race-related beliefs, behaviors, and experiences. Let’s examine one model that has focused on Black racial identity development. William Cross (1991; Cross & Cross, 2007; Cross et al., 2012) devised a framework that specifically addresses African American racial identity and consciousness—that is, the process of becoming Black. The process, which he called nigrescence, has five stages:

RACIAL IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT.

Nigrescence The process of developing a Black racial identity.

• Pre-Encounter: At this stage, Cross says that an African American’s attitude might range from ignoring race to feeling neutral about race and to actually being anti-Black. African Americans at this stage might adopt certain beliefs of White Americans, including the tendency to see “Whiteness” as superior. Some level of self-hate is a possible consequence. At the pre-encounter stage, people value other aspects of their identities, such as religion, profession, or social status.

• Encounter: This stage is often triggered by encounters with overt, covert, or institutional racism. For instance, when an African American is followed around in an upscale store, is assaulted by police, or sees news reports about such assaults, then his or her eyes are opened to the reality that race matters in society. The individual becomes attuned to their Blackness.

• Immersion/Emersion: Cross sees this stage as a transition—an in-between state that might cause people to be anxious about “becoming the ‘right kind’ of Black person” (Cross, 1991, p. 202). In response to encounters with discrimination, the individuals fill their lives with symbols of Blackness; they buy books about Black experiences and socialize mainly with other African Americans, for example. They are eager to understand their racial heritage more deeply.

• Internalization: Individuals are firmly connected to and secure in their sense of racial identity. They don’t worry about what friends or outsiders think—they are confident in their own standards of Blackness.

• Internalization-Commitment: This stage is very closely connected with internalization. The main difference is an individual’s continued interest in and commitment to Black affairs. Such individuals chart their lives to connect to their Black racial identity; for example, a painter might dedicate his life to painting images of Black life or a researcher might dedicate her life to studying the educational experiences of African Americans. No single model describes identity development for all members of a race group living in United States. For example, the stages in the nigrescence model might not be appropriate for understanding the identity development of first-generation, U.S.-born African students or for students with both a Latino and a Black heritage such as that of some Dominican Americans (DeWalt, 2013). Similar racial identity development models have been explored for individuals of other racial backgrounds. For example, Janet Helms (1995, 2014) put forth a model describing stages of White identity development. White people have historically held power over

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Table 2.3 Privileges Often Afforded by Being White in America ARE YOU ABLE TO ... • Browse leisurely in a store without being watched closely by an employee? • Congregate in public spaces with other members of your race without being perceived as threatening? • Observe more positive than negative portrayals of your race group in the media? • Experience failures in work or school without fearing that people will attribute them to your race? • Take classes on just about any topic that are taught by people who look like you? • Choose health care professionals who are members of your race? • Share the same race with most of your immediate colleagues and high-level leaders in your field? • Call the police when a crime has been committed without fear that your race will affect how you will be treated? • Walk around in your residential community without being asked if you really live there? • Go to any salon or barbershop and find someone who can appropriately style and care for your hair? • Go through your daily life without fear of being viewed as “out of place” because of your race? • Offer your opinions without an expectation that they reflect the views of your entire racial group? • Be an expert or highly skilled in something without it being attributed to your race? • Find numerous books and films that feature lead characters who look like you? • Receive an education that will highlight the traditions and accomplishments of people of your race? • Interact with others without being asked what country you are from? Source: Statements created with input from Jennifer Burris, Xiao-Yin Chen, Anastacia Cole, Candice Hargons, and Jaylene Patterson.

indigenous people and people of color in the United States. Consequently, their healthy identity development requires, in part, that they come to terms with this history and the unearned privileges that Whiteness has given them (Helms, 2020). Whitney Dow, a filmmaker and founder of “The Whiteness Project,” recalled being asked by a seventh-grade student what he had learned about his own racial identity after working on the project with his African American collaborator, Marco Williams: “It was like suddenly getting x-ray glasses. . . . [I realized that] the most day-to-day manifestation of my White privilege is not being forced to think about the fact that I am White. I think that for most people who aren’t White, they don’t have that luxury” (PBS, 2017). Some White people have difficulty recognizing the privileges and affordances of being born White. Table 2.3 provides a list that may be helpful in understanding some of these advantages. For Black individuals and members of other racial and ethnic minority groups who have been historically oppressed or marginalized in the United States, having strong positive feelings about one’s own group seems to be important for good mental health as well as for engagement and success in school (Cook & Cook, 2014; Steinberg, 2005). How central or salient people’s race is to how they view themselves might also be related to how they handle discrimination (Brown, 2017). Ultimately, the importance of your racial identity depends on several contextual factors. For example, researchers studied the effect of the sociopolitical context of the Black Lives Matter movement on children’s racial identity development between 2014 and 2016. Black and multiracial children who were interviewed at each time point reported that their racial identity had become more important to them, whereas White children did not (Rogers et al., 2021). Parents, peers, and media also affect children’s developing racial identities (Huguley et al., 2019; Williams et al., 2020). Teachers who understand the complexity of students’ racial identity development and expose students to diverse social models who have developed their own healthy racial identities are better able to support their students’ healthy development (DeCuir-Gunby, 2009; Spencer et al., 2016).

Ethnic and Racial Differences in School Achievement A major concern in schools is that some ethnic groups consistently achieve below the average for all students (Matthews et al., 2010; Uline & Johnson, 2005). This pattern of results tends to hold for all standardized achievement tests, but the gaps have been narrowing since about the 1980s and are smaller than the gaps seen between wealthy and poor students (Raudenbush, 2009; Reardon, 2011). For example, as you can see in Figure 2.5, on

59

60 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Figure 2.5 Scores on the National Assessment of Educational Progress in FourthGrade Mathematics The following figures show the math achievement scores of American fourth-graders between 1990 and 2019. The top figure compares White and Hispanic students. The bottom figure compares White and African American students. SCALE SCORE 500 320 300 280 260

*

**

240 220* * 220 200

**

180 220* *

*

248 249

*

*

*

*

*

*

*

*

*

*

*

*

229* 231

160 140 120 100 80 0

20

*

*

*

*

*

90

19 18 17 19

ASSESSMENT YEAR White

Hispanic

Accommodations permitted

Accommodations not permitted

Significant difference ( p < .05) * Significantly different from 2019 ( p < .05).

SCALE SCORE 500 320 300 280 260

220 240 * 220

*

**

*

200 180

* 160 188

*

**

*

*

*

*

*

*

*

248 249

*

223 224

*

140 120 100 80 0

32

*

*

*

*

*

90

*

*

25 25 17 19

ASSESSMENT YEAR White

Black

Accommodations not permitted

Accommodations permitted

Significant difference ( p < .05) * Significantly different from 2019 ( p < .05). Note: Black includes African American, and Hispanic includes Latino. Race categories exclude Hispanic origin. Score gaps are calculated based on differences between unrounded average scores. Source: U.S. Department of Education, 2019 National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) Report Card: Mathematics. Retrieved from https://www.nationsreportcard.gov/mathematics/nation/groups/?grade=4.

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

the National Assessment of Educational Progress in mathematics, the gap between scores of White and Black fourth-graders has narrowed from 34 points in 1996 to 25 points in 2019 (U.S. Department of Education, 2019). The gap between White and Hispanic fourthgraders has narrowed from 25 in 1996 to 18 in 2019. These gaps are larger in eighth grade (32-point gap between White and Black students; 24-point gap between White and Hispanic students). Asian and Pacific Islander students scored the highest in math at both grade levels. Proponents of this notion of an “achievement gap” have been criticized for taking a narrow view and assuming that the scores of White, middle-class students are the norm that all other students must be compared to and measured by (Anyon, 2012) or that students who are underperforming are somehow to blame for their inadequacies (Zirkel & Pollack, 2016). Milner (2013, 2015) suggests that we think about other kinds of “gaps” such as teacher education and quality gaps, challenging curriculum gaps, affordable housing and health care gaps, school integration and funding gaps, and quality childcare gaps, digital divide gaps, wealth and income gaps, and employment gaps—all culminating in opportunity gaps for many students of color. For example, a case analysis of one high school in Berkeley, California, showed that school staff implemented science learning labs before and after school to decrease achievement gaps between White students and African American and Latino students. These opportunity structures were not actually equally accessible to all students and ended up widening rather than narrowing the learning gap between students (Zirkel & Pollack, 2016). Gloria Ladson Billings (2006) described the educational debt (based on decades of underinvestment and discrimination) that we owe students of color and students living in poverty. Other educational psychologists have pointed out that students of color often experience a discrepancy between their own needs and the opportunity structures provided by schools, which can result in a decreased sense of school belonging, academic motivation, and, in turn, performance (Gray et al., 2018). Opportunity gaps and educational debts lead to education completion gaps. Across the United States in 2018, about 89% of White students graduated from high school, compared to 79% of African American students, 81% of Hispanic students, 92% of Asian/Pacific Islander students, and 74% of American Indian and Alaska Native students (Hussar et al., 2020). But again, these are averages across all states—variations occur by state and district. For example, graduation rates for Hispanic students ranged widely from 65% in the District of Columbia to 90% in West Virginia (U.S. Department of Education, 2019). Be aware of the graduation rates in the district where you teach and the factors that might influence those rates. Although consistent differences still exist among groups on tests of academic abilities, most researchers agree that the reasons for these differences are mainly the legacy of discrimination, the product of cultural mismatches and language differences, or a result of growing up in poverty. Because many students from ethnic groups are also economically disadvantaged, separating the effects of these two sets of influences on school achievement is important (Milner, 2015; Shores et al., 2020). For example, one study found that learning and self-regulation skills (e.g., attentiveness, persistence, organization, learning independence) explain the literacy development of African American boys from kindergarten through fifth grade, even after taking into account the effects of the boys’ SES, home environment, and problem behaviors (Matthews et al., 2010). So early development of these learning skills can help close the opportunity gap, at least for African American boys and probably for others. Rather than focusing on achievement gaps, many educators have called for more research on the school-based assets that facilitate the growth and successes of African American and Latino students (Del Toro & Wang, 2020). Berry (2005) studied two middle-school-age African American boys who were successful in mathematics. In the lives of those students, Berry found support and high expectations from family and teachers; positive math experiences in preschool and elementary school; connections to church and athletic extracurricular activities; and positive identities as math students. One final theme characterized the successful African American boys: Their families had prepared them to understand and deal with a legacy of inequality in schools, our next topic.

61

62 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

The Legacy of Racial Inequality When we considered explanations for why students with low SES have trouble in school, we mentioned the limited educational opportunities and low expectations/biases of teachers and fellow students. This also has been the experience of many students from ethnic and racial minority groups. For example, in some areas of the South in 1924, Black students attended their own, separate schools for only 6 months out of the year because they were expected to work in the fields the other 6 months. White students attended school a full 9 months. The highest grade available for the Black students was eighth grade (Raudenbush, 2009). A minister in Topeka, Kansas, was upset about how far his young daughter had to travel just to get to her elementary school. Despite the fact that Linda Brown was accustomed to playing in her neighborhood with White, Black, and Native American children, the closest elementary school, just four blocks from her home, would not admit her because she was Black. Instead, Linda had to traverse a rail yard, cross a busy road, and then wait for a bus to take her across town to the school that served Black students. She recalled her misery on one bitter cold winter day that made her tears turn to ice. Linda wondered why she couldn’t just attend the school closer to home (Genzlinger, 2018). Her parents’ answer to this question, with the help of other concerned families, was to file a suit challenging the school policy. You know the outcome of the 1954 Brown v. the Board of Education of Topeka ruling: “Separate but equal” schools for Black children were declared inherently unequal. PUT YOURSELF IN THEIR PLACE

Legal segregation came to an end in 1954. Take a moment to imagine that you were living back then and that the child described in this section was your own. What would you do?

Almost 70 years later, pages of research have mostly shown that legally mandated integration is not a quick solution to the detrimental effects of centuries of racial inequality. White students left desegregated schools and neighborhoods as the number of students of color increased, leaving many urban schools today more segregated than they were before the Supreme Court ordered desegregation measures such as busing (Orfield et al., 2016). Schools in Los Angeles, Miami, Baltimore, Chicago, Dallas, Memphis, Houston, and Detroit have fewer than 11% White, non-Hispanic students. In fact, two-thirds of the schools that African American and Latino students attend are quite segregated, with high concentrations of students living in poverty, which means that racial segregation is often aligned with economic segregation (Ladson Billings, 2004; Mickelson et al., 2013; Shores et al., 2020). In school districts where Latino children make up at least 10% of student enrollment, Latino students are experiencing more segregation over time and are less likely to interact with White peers (Fuller et al., 2019). Parent and journalist Nikole Hannah-Jones (2016) described the tension she and her husband experienced when deciding whether to allow their daughter to attend a racially segregated, low-income public school in New York City. “Hadn’t we worked hard . . . precisely so that she would not have to go to the types of schools that trapped so many Black children?” This is the question many families face (see Hannah-Jones, 2015 episode on This American Life). Research suggests that the demographic composition of schools does indeed affect long-term psychological and academic outcomes (Graham, 2018). For example, economist Rucker Johnson (2019) found that Black adults who attended more racially desegregated schools as children lived longer, were more likely to attend college and live in racially integrated neighborhoods, and were less likely to be poor, to suffer from chronic health problems, and to go to jail than were those adults who had attended racially segregated schools. Similar research has shown that Asian American adolescents who have more cross-race friendships report better psychological well-being, particularly in contexts where anti-Asian prejudice was most prevalent (Liu et al., 2020). But even in “integrated” schools, too often students from historically disadvantaged ethnic or racial minority groups are resegregated in low-ability tracks due to teachers’ lower expectations about them (Kogachi & Graham, 2020; Tenenbaum & Ruck, 2007; Tyson, 2013).

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

63

Simply putting people in the same building does not mean that they will come to respect each other, become friends, or even experience the same quality of education (Johnson, 2019; Ladson Billings, 2004; Mickelson et al., 2013). For more information on how to understand and reduce ethnic and racial disparities in education, look at the report by the American Psychological Association’s Presidential Task Force on Educational Disparities (2012). One critical step in reducing disparities in educational outcomes is to acknowledge the roles that racism and discrimination play in education. RACISM AND DISCRIMINATION. Racial prejudice (racism) is pervasive in the United States. Although racism is not confined to one racial group, anti-Black racism has remained in the spotlight because of the killings of unarmed Black people such as Breonna Taylor and George Floyd, which sparked nationwide protests for racial justice in 2020. Even so, White and Black Americans differ in their beliefs about whether the United States should take further action to protect civil rights. More than 10,000 American adults were asked in September 2020 whether the country has done enough when it comes to giving Black people rights equal to those of White people. The perceptions were starkly different by race: 42% of White people responded that the country’s response has “been about right,” whereas only 5% of Black respondents chose this response (Horowitz et al., 2020b). More than 86% of Black respondents said the country had “not gone far enough.” Subtle, below-the-surface racism remains. Even though it may be outside conscious awareness, it can lead to discriminatory behavior. Researchers have demonstrated this in numerous ways. For example, in response to police killings of unarmed Black men, researchers created a video game that showed a series of White or Black men holding either a gun or a non-weapon such as a flashlight or wallet. Participants in the research were told to “shoot” whenever the person in the video game held a weapon. Race was not mentioned. Nevertheless, participants shot armed targets more quickly and more frequently when those targets were Black and more quickly and more frequently decided not to shoot unarmed targets when they were White (Greenwald et al., 2003). When the participants in another study were actual police officers, they were more likely to mistakenly shoot unarmed Black suspects compared with unarmed White suspects (Plant & Peruche, 2005). You might think that police officers and teachers are quite different. Think again. Researchers have used similar techniques to show that teachers are more likely to expect less of students from ethnic minority groups than they expect from students from ethnic majority groups. They are also more likely to disproportionately refer students from historically disadvantaged groups—and particularly Black students—for special education or disciplinary interventions (Morris & Perry, 2016; van den Bergh et al., 2010). In fact, teachers hold similarly pro-White, anti-Black biases in roughly the same proportion as members of the general public (Starck et al., 2020), and these biases have been shown to exacerbate achievement differences between their Black and White students (Chin et al., 2020). In a profession that prides itself on educating all learners equally, how and why does this happen? People tend to over-rely on rigid, possibly biased categories when they are under stress or have limited resources (Baumeister & Bargh, 2014). Teachers know these conditions well! Teachers, like police officers and all humans, can distort information to make it fit their schema better, especially if their stereotypes include prejudiced beliefs about a group. We notice information that confirms or agrees with our stereotypes—our schema—and miss or dismiss information that does not fit (Kahneman, 2011). This can lead to actions that discriminate against others who are members of social groups whom we have categorized less favorably. Although prejudice and discrimination come from many places—peers, media, social encounters outside of school—evidence suggests that perceiving discrimination from a teacher might be the most damaging for educational outcomes (Benner & Graham, 2013). Even if teachers are unaware of their prejudices, those very prejudices can affect the expectations teachers hold for their students and how they interpret students’ behaviors (Brown, 2017). For example, researchers at Yale asked teachers to watch videos featuring racially diverse preschool children. Some teachers were primed to expect the students to show “challenging behaviors,” whereas others were not. Teachers primed to look for misbehavior gazed longer at Black children—especially Black boys—than at White children, suggesting a

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Racial Bias (IV, B4) Describe the possible effects of racial discrimination and bias on minority students. What can teachers and schools do to address the lingering effects of this discrimination?

64 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity form of implicit bias. Both Black and White teachers showed similar patterns (Gilliam et al., 2016). This type of unconscious bias could be partly responsible for the disproportionate number of suspensions of Black students relative to their White peers (U.S. Department of Education, 2016). Another study found that non-Black high school teachers held lower expectations of Black students (particularly of boys) than did Black teachers (Gershenson et al., 2016). Implicit biases can have serious effects (even if unintentional) on students’ educational attainment. Clearly, many learners face prejudice and discrimination in subtle or blatant ways every day. Black and Hispanic students begin to lose out in science and math as early as elementary school. They are chosen less often for gifted classes and acceleration or for enrichment programs. They are more likely to be tracked into “basic skills” classes (Tyson, 2013). As they progress through middle school, high school, and college, their paths take them further and further out of the pipeline that produces our scientists. If they do persist and become scientists or engineers, they, along with women, will still be paid less than White employees for the same work (National Science Foundation, 2019; Shen, 2016). Hispanics, African Americans, and Native Americans make up about 35% of the U.S. population, but in 2019, about 8% of the doctorates awarded went to Hispanic students, 7% to African Americans, and fewer than 1% to Native Americans. In contrast, 38% of the doctorates were awarded to nonresidents of the United States (National Science Foundation, 2020). The families of racial and ethnic minority students often have to be vigilant about discrimination to protect their children from its adverse psychological, social, and academic consequences. Some evidence has shown that children who have been socialized to understand racism and discrimination are better equipped to handle them when they arise (Park et al., 2018; Yip et al., 2019).

Module 5 Summary Ethnicity and Race in Teaching and Learning (pp. 56–64) Distinguish between ethnicity and race. Ethnicity (culturally transmitted behavior) and race (a socially constructed categorization based on physical traits) are significant categories people use to describe themselves and others. Ethnic or racial minority groups (either numerically or historically marginalized) are rapidly increasing in population. How can differences in the ethnicity of teachers and students affect school performance? Conflicts can arise from differences between teachers and students in culture-based beliefs, values, and expectations. Cultural conflicts are usually about below-the-surface differences because when subtle cultural differences meet, misunderstandings are common. Students in some cultures learn attitudes and behaviors that are more consistent with school expectations. Differences in cognitive and academic abilities among ethnic groups are largely the legacy of racial segregation and continuing prejudice and discrimination.

MODULE 6

Gender Identify and Sexual Orientation

Learning Objective 2.4 Describe the development of gender, gender identity, and sexual orientation, and discuss their roles in teaching and learning.

Gender and Sexual Orientation in Teaching and Learning In this section we examine the development of gender identity and sexual orientation. We particularly focus on how people develop, how they are socialized, and the role of teachers in providing an equitable education for all students, regardless of their particular identities.

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

65

Sex and Gender The word gender usually refers to traits and behaviors that a particular culture judges to be appropriate for boys and girls, men and women. In contrast, sex typically refers to biological differences assigned at birth. As with other cultural labels, the terms used in this area can be complex and controversial (Hyde et al., 2019). An individual’s identity in terms of gender and sex is multifaceted. In this section, we examine the role of three important facets: gender identity, gender roles, and sexual orientation (Ruble et al., 2006). Gender identity is an individual’s self-identification as a gendered person. Although gender identity has historically been described in binary terms (i.e., male or female), it is better conceptualized along a continuum. This more inclusive view recognizes that physiologically and psychologically, gender is more like a mosaic than an either/or categorization (Hyde et al., 2019). It is therefore no surprise that many forms of gender expression exist. Gender roles are those behaviors and characteristics that the culture stereotypically associates with a binary (male/female) view of gender. Sexual orientation is also a multidimensional construct that can refer to people’s sexual identity, sexual attraction, and/or sexual behavior (APA, 2015; Wolff et al., 2017). Relations among these three elements are complex. For example, a young woman might identify herself as female (gender identity) but behave in ways that are not consistent with the stereotypical female gender role (play football or wrestle) and might be attracted to both men and women (e.g., bisexual). Let’s look at some of these facets in further detail.

Gender Identity Gender identity refers to one’s individual sense of being a woman, a man, both, or neither (Brown & Stone, 2016). Erikson and many other earlier psychologists thought that identifying one’s gender is straightforward; one simply realizes that they are a man or a woman and acts accordingly. But today, we know that an individual’s gender identity might be different from the sex assigned at birth. The term cisgender (pronounced “sis-gender”) is used to describe an individual whose gender identity matches the biological sex that they were assigned at birth. Transgender individuals often report feeling trapped in the wrong body: They experience themselves as female, whereas their biological sex is male, or vice versa (Lhamon & Gupta, 2016; Ruble et al., 2006). In a poignant self-disclosure, one of my undergraduate students recently told our class that they spent their childhood knowing that they were really a boy, not the girl they were raised as. The student described having the distinct feeling that “my insides don’t match my outsides.” Others might simply behave in gender-expansive, gender nonconforming, or gender atypical ways (Diamond, 2020). A young boy might surround himself with girls, play with dolls, and dress as girls do. He might identify with most girls more than he identifies with most boys and might therefore resist a binary gender label. Some children’s gender identity is formed early and remains relatively permanent. However, gender identity formation might take longer for others. Students who express themselves in gender-atypical ways might be vulnerable to stigma, discrimination, or low self-beliefs. For example, researchers in Holland asked seventh-grade students how gender typical they felt. Students who reported higher gender atypicality felt less confident in their academic capabilities than did gender-typical students (Vantieghem et al., 2014). Teachers can play an important role in supporting students during their process of gender identity formation (Fabes et al., 2019; Leaper & Brown, 2018). Three major ways that they can do this is by addressing harassment and bullying, making school spaces more gender inclusive (avoiding segregation by gender), and dealing respectfully with students’ records, names, pronouns, and privacy (see Beyond the Binary: Discussing Transgender and Gender Non- Conforming Identity in K–12 Schools, ADL, 2019).

Gender identity The sense of self along the female-to-male continuum or as neither female nor male (i.e., nonbinary).

Gender roles The behaviors and characteristics that the culture stereotypically associates with being a man or a woman.

Sexual orientation A multidimensional construct that refers to people’s sexual identity, sexual attraction, and/or sexual behavior.

66 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Gender Roles

Gender schemas Organized cognitive structures that include gender-related information that influences how children think and behave.

Gender roles are expectations about how people should behave in gender-conforming ways—in other words, in ways consistent with what is considered masculine or feminine. Gender roles vary by culture, time, and place. What was expected of women in the United States in the 1700s definitely has changed even though women generally still are the primary caregivers and are in charge of the home. When and how do children develop their understandings of masculine and feminine gender roles? As early as age 2, children are aware of gender differences. They know whether they are girls or boys and that Mommy is a girl and Daddy is a boy. By age 3 or so, many begin to believe that their sex cannot be changed. Biology plays a part in gender-role development: Very early, hormones affect activity level and aggression, with boys tending to prefer active, rough, noisy play. Play styles lead young children to prefer same-sex play partners with similar styles, so by age 4, children spend three times as much play time with same-sex playmates as with opposite-sex playmates; by age 6, the ratio is 11 to 1 (Halim et al., 2013; Hines, 2004). Biology is not the whole story, however. The fact that boys tend to play with boys and girls tend to play with girls might lead to a gender-segregation cycle by which binary gender-role norms are defined and reinforced (Martin et al., 2014). Boys and girls might be treated differently, too. Researchers have found that boys are given more freedom to roam the neighborhood and are allowed to attempt potentially dangerous activities earlier, such as crossing the street alone. Thus, independence and initiative seem to be encouraged more in boys than in girls. In fact, parents, peers, and teachers might reward behaviors that seem gender appropriate—gentle kindness in girls and strong assertiveness in boys (Brannon, 2002; Brown, 2014). Then there are the toys! Walk through most stores’ toy sections or order a child’s fast food meal and see what is offered for girls and boys. Dolls and kitchen sets for girls and toy weapons for boys have been with us for decades. Then came aisles of princess paraphernalia for girls and battle video games for boys. Are consumers partly to blame? Adults often select gender-typed gifts for children, and some fathers discourage their young sons from playing with “girl” toys (Brannon, 2002; Brown, 2014). When a large retailer announced recently that it would no longer segregate toys by gender, many parents were outraged. And let’s face it, gender socialization does not stop in childhood. I was recently helping a first-year student move into his co-ed dormitory on campus. His door was decorated with a hot-rod car with his name on it; his female dorm-mates had princess tiara name cards on their doors. What are the consequences of such environmental messages about gender? Through their interactions with family, peers, teachers, toys, media, and the broader social environment, children begin to form gender schemas, or organized networks of knowledge about what it means to be a man or a woman (Ward & Grower, 2020). Gender schemas help children make sense of the world and guide their behavior (see Figure 2.6). So a young girl whose schema for “girls” includes “girls play with dolls and not with trucks” or “girls can’t be scientists” will pay attention to, remember, and interact more with dolls than with trucks, and she might avoid science activities (Golombok et al., 2008; Leaper, 2002). Of course, these are averages, and individuals do not always fit the average. An individual girl might decide, for example, that the gender schema “trucks are for boys” doesn’t matter to her. She will play with the truck if it interests her (Bigler & Liben, 2007). By age 4, children have an initial sense of gender roles, and by 5 or so, they have developed a gender schema that describes what clothes, games, toys, behaviors, and careers are “right” for boys and for girls—and these ideas can be quite rigid (Ellemers, 2018; Halim et al., 2013). Even in this era of great progress toward equal opportunity, a preschool girl is more likely to tell you that she wants to become a nurse than to say that she wants to become an engineer. After she had given a lecture on the dangers of sex stereotyping in schools, a

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

67

Figure 2.6 Gender Schema Theory According to gender schema theory, children and adolescents use gender as an organizing theme to classify and understand their perceptions about the world. Influences processing of social information— attention, memory, etc. Society’s beliefs about the traits of females and males

Gender Schema Influences self-esteem (only behavior or attitudes consistent with gender schema are acceptable)

colleague of mine brought her young daughter to her college class. The students asked the little girl, “What do you want to be when you grow up?” The child immediately replied, “A doctor,” and her professor/mother beamed with pride. Then the girl whispered to the students in the front row, “I really want to be a nurse, but my mommy won’t let me.” Actually, this is a common reaction of young children. Preschoolers tend to have more stereotyped notions of sex roles than do older children, and all ages seem to have more rigid and traditional ideas about male occupations than about what occupations women should pursue (Woolfolk & Perry, 2015). Later, as children approach and enter puberty, they might become even more focused on behaving in “masculine” or “feminine” ways, as defined by their peer culture, social media, parents, or the environment. For example, in one study, children and early adolescents who were shown sexualized and nonsexualized images of girls rated the sexualized girls as more popular than the nonsexualized girls but also viewed the sexualized girls as not as athletic, smart, or nice (Stone et al., 2015). The sexualized gender stereotype was more strongly endorsed by girls and by older students, suggesting that exposure to sexualized images in the media increases as children get older and might be especially important to girls as they form their schemas about gender. Other research has shown that when parents (particularly mothers) see the usefulness and value of science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) fields, their children enroll in more STEM courses throughout high school (Rozek et al., 2015). Even small changes in the learning environment (e.g., when a computer science classroom contains stereotypical objects such as science fiction books versus plants) can invoke gender stereotypes that disadvantage some students. One study showed that just reading about a stereotypical computer science classroom caused adolescent girls to feel less like taking the class for fear they would not belong (Master et al., 2016). The bottom line is that so many factors, from biology to cultural norms, play roles in gender-role development. Beware of either/or explanations!

Gender Bias and Sexism in Curriculum and Media While I (Anita here) was reviewing this very page for a previous edition, riding crosscountry on a train, the conductor stopped beside my seat. He said, “I’m sorry, dear, for interrupting your homework, but do you have a ticket?” I had to smile at his (I’m sure unintended) sexism. I doubt that he had asked the same question of the man across the aisle who was writing on his legal pad. Like racial discrimination, messages of sexism and gender biases can be subtle, and they can and do appear in classrooms (see Brown & Stone, 2016; Leaper & Brown, 2014).

Gender biases Different views of men, women, or gender-nonconforming people often favoring one group over the other.

68 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Gender Bias (IV, B4) There has been much debate in the news media over possible gender bias in schools. What can you as a teacher do to reduce or eliminate gender bias and its effects?

Unfortunately, schools often foster sexism in several ways. Publishers have established guidelines to prevent gender bias in educational materials, but it still makes sense to check those materials for stereotypes, particularly given the slow turnover of books that populate school library shelves. For example, even though children’s books now have equal numbers of men and women as central characters, there still are more men in the titles and the illustrations, and the characters (especially the boys) continue to behave in stereotypical ways. Boys are aggressive and argumentative, and girls are expressive and affectionate. Girl characters sometimes cross gender roles to be more active, but boy characters seldom show “feminine,” expressive traits (Brannon, 2002; Brown, 2014). Also, virtual learning sites, social media sites, and online sources such as YouTube have not been carefully screened for gender, racial, ethnic, economic, religious, or age stereotypes and biases, and they can be sources of stereotyped messages (Henry, 2011; Ward & Grower, 2020). Digital games and even school materials often feature hypermasculine or hyperfeminine characters. One look at the body features of men and women in video combat games shows what unreal and unhealthy body images they promote. Another “text” that students read long before they arrive in your classroom is televised and online media. A content analysis of television commercials found that White male characters are more prominent than any other group. Even when only the actor’s voice can be heard, men are 10 times more likely to narrate commercials. And the same pattern of men as the “voice of authority” on television occurs in the United Kingdom, Europe, Australia, and Asia. Women are more likely to be depicted as dependent on men and often are shown at home (Brannon, 2002). The #MeToo movement has brought greater awareness to pervasive sexism in televised media and advertising. The documentary film Miss Representation (Newsom, 2011) offers numerous compelling (and rather shocking) examples of how mainstream media and culture depict girls and women in hypersexualized and underpowered positions. Similarly, the film The Mask You Live In explores how boys are often subjected to narrow definitions of masculinity that cause stress and dissonance (see http://therepresentationproject.org for more information, including an educator toolkit). Disclosure, a 2020 Netflix documentary, shows how unfavorably transgender individuals have been depicted in films and on television. How might these messages affect how people think about themselves and others?

Gender Bias in Teaching Quite a bit of research has examined teachers’ gendered treatment of students. You should know, however, that most of these studies have focused on White students. Emerging research points to the ways in which intersectionality provides a useful lens for understanding teachers’ different interactions with students of various gender and race groups. The Guidelines: Avoiding Gender Bias in Teaching on the following page provide ideas for teaching all your students effectively and empathetically. Let’s look at some related research findings. Many studies describe what seem like biases favoring boys. One of the best-documented findings of the past 30 years is that teachers have more overall interactions with boys than with girls; however, this includes more negative interactions with boys but not more positive interactions (Jones & Dindia, 2004). This is true from preschool to college. Teachers ask more questions of male students, give them more feedback (praise, criticism, and correction), and offer them more specific and valuable comments. Of course, these differences are not evenly distributed. Some boys, generally high-achieving White students, receive more than their share, whereas high-achieving White girls receive the least teacher attention. On average, teachers rate boys as more competent in mathematics than girls, unless girls are perceived as working harder, behaving better, and being more eager to learn than boys. Only then do teachers rate girls’ math competence similarly to that of boys (Robinson-Cimpian et al., 2014). Recent efforts have also examined how teachers’ implicit beliefs and attitudes might differentially affect girls and boys. For instance, some evidence has shown that girls pick up on the attitudes of their teachers. In one study, girls whose teachers had higher math anxiety scored lower on end-of-year math tests and were more likely to endorse the view that boys are better than girls in math (Beilock et al., 2010). Another study showed that middle school girls who perceived that their math teachers treated them differently because of their gender performed

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

GUIDELINES Avoiding Gender Bias in Teaching Check to see whether textbooks and other media you are using present an honest view of the options open to men, women, and gender-nonconforming individuals. Examples 1. 2.

Identify whether men, women, and gender-nonbinary people are portrayed in traditional or nontraditional roles at work, at leisure, and at home. Discuss your analyses with students, and ask them to help you find gender-role biases in other materials—magazine advertising, TV programs, and news reporting, for example.

Watch for any unintended biases in your own classroom practices. Examples 1.

2. 3.

Monitor whether you group students by gender for certain activities. Consider how being in gendered groups might feel to students who do not identify as boys or girls. Is the grouping appropriate? Monitor whether you call on one gender group more frequently for certain answers—boys for math and girls for poetry, for example. Consider the quality of your feedback to students. Monitor your metaphors. Don’t ask students to “tackle the problem.”

Look for ways in which your school might be limiting the options open to students with different gender identities or expressions. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Find out what curricular or career advice guidance counselors give to students. Look into whether a good sports program is accessible to all. See whether girls are encouraged to take advanced courses in science and mathematics and whether boys are encouraged to take English and foreign language classes.

Use gender-free language as much as possible. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Make sure you use inclusive terms (e.g., speak of “law-enforcement officer” and “mail carrier” instead of “policeman” and “mailman”). Be sure you name a committee “head” instead of a “chairman.” Discuss personal pronouns with your students. Make sure you are using the pronoun they would like you to use (e.g., her, him, they).

Provide counter-stereotypical gender role models. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Assign articles written by female research scientists or mathematicians. Have recent female graduates who are majoring in science, math, engineering, or other technical fields come to class to talk about college. Create electronic mentoring programs for students that connect them with genderdiverse adults who are working in areas of interest to the students.

Make sure all students have a chance to do complex, technical work. Examples 1. 2.

Experiment with collaborative groups to avoid gender-stereotypical work assignments (e.g., girls as the secretaries, boys as the scientists). Rotate jobs in groups or randomly assign responsibilities.

What if you witness gender bias as a student teacher? For ideas, see https://learningforjustice.org and search for “classroom resources.”

69

70 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity worse on subsequent math tests and reported decreased motivation for math in high school (McKellar et al., 2019). Teachers might not be aware of how their behaviors are influenced by their own attitudes, but through direct instruction, modeling, or differential interactions with boys and girls, they can have a powerful influence on both how students see themselves and how they perform (Gunderson et al., 2012). Not all biases in school favor boys. In the past 10 years in North America, Western Europe, Australia, and some Asian countries, educators have raised questions about whether schools are serving boys well. This concern is fueled by data from many countries that seem to show underachievement in boys. In fact, the underachievement of boys in school has been called “one of the most pressing educational equality challenges of current times” (Hartley & Sutton, 2013, p. 1716). More dramatic accusations include that schools are trying to destroy “boys’ culture” and force “feminine, frilly content” on boys. Boys are referred to special education services at greater rates than are girls, and these rates are even higher for boys who are members of ethnic or racial minority groups (Artiles, 2019). One possible explanation put forth for why boys struggle in school is that the expectations of schooling do not fit the way boys learn (Gurian & Stevens, 2005), particularly African American boys (Stinson, 2006). Another suggestion is that boys sabotage their own learning by resisting school expectations and rules to “display their masculinity and get respect” (Kleinfeld, 2005, p. B6). Some have suggested that boys need smaller classes, more discussions, better discipline, mentoring programs, and more men in their schools—90% of elementary teachers are women. One suggestion for making schools more effective for both boys and girls is single-sex classrooms. In 2008, The New York Times Magazine had a cover story about that topic (Weil, 2008), and in 2015 and 2016, The Atlantic reported on its “resurgence” (Anderson, 2015; Yap, 2016). During the past decade, many school districts across the United States have experimented with single-sex classrooms in core subjects such as English, science, and mathematics (Herron, 2013). Do gender-segregated approaches work? Read the Point/Counterpoint for the arguments.

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Should Girls and Boys Be Taught Differently? Point Proponents say that students are better off learning some things with their same-gender counterparts. Critics say that gender-segregated classrooms change little for learners and reinforce problematic stereotypes that could lead to greater gender disparities. Boys and girls have unique strengths and challenges and should be taught in different ways. Many educators paid attention when, in 2001, the No Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act began to allow public schools to offer same-sex classes, which led to the 2006 amendment to the Title XI regulation that removed the ban on single-sex public education. In the spirit of innovation, many schools took up the call and began catering instruction to the unique needs of boys and girls. After all, findings from the 2012 PISA test revealed that among 15-year-olds worldwide, boys were disproportionately overrepresented at the lowest and highest ends of achievement in math and reading, and girls generally reported lower self-confidence and higher anxiety in math than boys (OECD, 2015). It seemed logical to examine whether all students would benefit from gender-specific instruction (Bigler et al., 2014). Since then, thousands of school districts across the United States have implemented some degree of single-sex education (Klein et al., 2014). Some organizations, such as the Gurian Institute (http://www.gurianinstitute.com), have offered books and professional development workshops for schools focused on how best to educate each gender based on the core assumption that boys and girls have different learning “styles” (Gurian & Stevens, 2005). A staunch proponent of single-sex classrooms, Leonard Sax (2005) has suggested that these styles are rooted in fundamental biological differences between boys and girls

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

that require different instructional approaches. Single-sex classrooms can help girls develop their own strengths and boost their self-confidence, interests, and achievements, particularly in STEM disciplines. Boys, on the other hand, can focus on strengthening their literacy skills and cooperativeness. Anecdotal findings reported by Gurian and others, primarily in popular media, have shown gains for both genders (e.g., Gurian et al., 2009). Students and their families might have some choice in whether students attend a single-sex or a mixed-sex school, which could mean that such students are predisposed to respond well to the singlesex learning environment.

Counterpoint Girls and boys are more similar than different and should be taught together. Researchers who have looked critically at data used to support gender-segregated educational approaches have argued that these approaches come up short in fulfilling their promise of promoting gender equity in learning and psychological well-being. In their 2011 article, “The Pseudoscience of Single-Sex Schooling,” Diane Halpern and her colleagues noted that research studies on same-sex education have been “deeply misguided, and often justified by weak, cherry-picked, or misconstrued scientific claims rather than by valid scientific evidence” (p. 1706). For example, when researchers take into account students’ initial performance levels, many students’ achievement gains in single-sex classrooms disappear. These scholars argue that studies of single-sex schooling have lacked the statistical rigor of randomized assignment, and therefore findings could be due to sampling or researcher bias. In fact, Halpern and colleagues claim that separating boys and girls actually worsens outcomes by making gender differences (and, therefore, stereotypes) more noticeable. They point to research showing that when any group of humans is set apart from another (based on gender, eye color, or even T-shirt color), people develop intergroup biases. Single-sex education, they argue, minimizes the very opportunities needed for gender equity—namely, teaching boys and girls to work alongside each other. “Positive and cooperative interaction with members of other groups is an effective method for improving intergroup relationships” (p. 1707). But what about the evidence of biological differences between men and women? Are men really from Mars and women from Venus? Hyde (2005) conducted a meta metaanalysis (i.e., a review of 46 meta-analyses) summarizing hundreds of studies on sex differences in a range of human behaviors. Her findings provided evidence for what she called the “gender similarities hypothesis.” With some important exceptions, men and women are highly similar on numerous psychological and performance metrics. The magnitude of these differences fluctuates across the life span and in different contexts, which can lead researchers to overestimate the size and stability of sex differences and ignore the fact that gender is better treated as a continuum (Hyde et al., 2019). For example, much of the evidence on sex differences has come from studies with adults who have had years of socialization; these sex differences are not necessarily present in children. Maybe boys and girls are from the same planet after all . . . and should therefore be educated in similar ways.

Beware of Either/Or So, do single-sex schools or classrooms improve learning? The answer is “It depends.” One meta-analysis offers the most comprehensive answer to date, addressing some of the methodological problems identified by Halpern and her colleagues. Pahlke et al. (2014) examined results from 184 studies involving 1.6 million students in grades K–12 from 21 countries. They looked at whether students learning in single-sex schools versus those learning in co-educational schools displayed different performances and attitudes. To account for variable quality in research designs, the researchers ran separate analyses for studies that used random assignment and those that did not. Among studies that randomly assigned students to single-sex classrooms, only trivial differences emerged between students in the two settings. Both girls and boys performed marginally better in single-sex schools, but girls attending co-educational schools had higher educational aspirations than did girls attending same-sex schools. Furthermore, gender-nonbinary and gender-nonconforming students have been overlooked in studies that consider gender as a binary variable.

71

72 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity Both proponents and critics of single-sex schools generally agree that good education is never one-size-fits-all. Teachers must realize that there are no boy- or girl-specific teaching strategies—good teaching is good teaching, and inclusive approaches serve all students. Regrouping students by sex does not make teaching easier; in fact, it can make class management more difficult and can reinforce rigid gender stereotypes that are harmful (Fabes et al., 2019). The Guidelines: Avoiding Gender Bias in Teaching provide additional ideas about ways to prevent gender bias for all students in your classes. Giving careful attention to each student’s motivation and achievement might mean that different approaches are called for at different times. As William James (1899/2001) long ago reminded teachers: “One can draw no specific rules for all this. It depends on close observation in the particular case” (p. 55).

Sexual Orientation Researchers have often defined sexual orientation in terms of sexual attraction—“an internal mechanism that directs a person’s sexuality to females, males, or both, perhaps to varying degrees” (Savin-Williams & Vrangalova, 2013, p. 59). However, some people identify as asexual and may not experience sexual attraction to others (see The Asexual Visibility & Education Network, n.d.). Sexual orientation can also be defined in terms of how a person identifies or behaves (Wolff et al., 2017). Sexual orientation and gender identity are separate and distinct aspects of human development (Orr et al., 2015). For example, a person who identifies as a woman might be attracted to both men and women (i.e., a bisexual orientation). Some labels for sexual orientation include gay, lesbian, bisexual, asexual, and pansexual. Another frequently used category is “questioning,” which refers to being unsure of one’s sexual orientation, or “queer,” which some people use to define their more fluid sexual or gender identity (Robinson & Espelage, 2012). But sexual orientation labels, like identity labels, can be incomplete representations of how individuals refer to themselves, and some view sexual orientation on a continuum or prefer no label at all (Savin-Williams, 2016). This makes it somewhat difficult to assess the precise number of individuals of different sexual orientations within a population (Bailey et al., 2016). However, the Division of Adolescent and School Health at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention estimates that among U.S. adolescents, 84.4% identify as heterosexual, 2.5% as gay or lesbian, 8.7% as bisexual, and 4.5% as not sure (CDC, 2020). Quite a few identity-development models have included a description of how individuals come to understand their sexual orientation if they are not heterosexual. Generally, the models include the following or similar stages (Savin-Williams & Cohen, 2015).

• Feeling different—Beginning around age 6, the child might be less interested in the activities of other children who are the same sex. Some children might find this difference troubling and fear being “found out.” Others do not experience these anxieties.

• Feeling uncertain—In adolescence, as they feel attractions for peers of the same sex, students might be confused, upset, lonely, and unsure of what to do. They might lack role models and might try to change themselves by becoming involved in activities and dating patterns that fit heterosexual stereotypes.

• Acceptance—As adolescents or young adults, many individuals develop a sexual orientation and identify themselves as gay, lesbian, or bisexual. They may or may not make their sexual orientation public but might share the information with a few friends. One problem with phase models of identity development is that the identity achieved is assumed to be final. Actually, newer models emphasize that sexual orientation can be flexible, complex, and multifaceted: It can change over a person’s lifetime (Diamond, 2020). For example, people may have dated or married opposite-sex partners at one point in their lives but have same-sex attractions or partners later in their lives, or vice versa (Garnets, 2002).

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Discrimination Based on Gender Identity and Sexual Orientation Children and adolescents who express their gender or sexual orientation in nonconforming ways (that is, whose expressions are not typical of others their age) are more likely than their classmates to face discrimination by peers and adults (Brown, 2017; Brown & Stone, 2016; Horn, 2019). For example, a boy whose preferences or behaviors differ from the norms of masculinity is often teased, subjected to homophobic slurs, stigmatized as being too feminine, or—worse—physically assaulted. This is an affront to all gender groups. Youth who identify as lesbian, gay, transgender, or bisexual (often referred to under the more inclusive term sexual minority youth) are more likely to be victims of aggression at school, according to a meta-analysis (Toomey & Russell, 2016). National survey data have shown this, too. The 2019 National School Climate Survey asked more than 16,000 adolescents to report their experiences in the past year. Among sexual minority youth who took the survey, almost all (98.8%) had heard the word “gay” being used in a derogatory manner, 69% had been victims of verbal harassment or threats, and 26% were physically harassed or assaulted in the past year (Kosciw et al., 2020). Almost all reported feeling distressed over these incidents, and many said they did not feel safe at school because of their sexual orientation (59%) or gender expression (43%). Furthermore, two-thirds of students said that they had heard teachers or other school staff make negative remarks about gender expression, and more than half had heard teachers make homophobic remarks. These stressors can lead to academic disengagement and lower academic performance among gender-minority and sexual-minority youth (Poteat et al., 2014). The stress associated with such victimization is one reason sexual minority students are at greater risk than their heterosexual peers of missing school and attempting suicide (Robinson & Espelage, 2012). Transgender students are particularly susceptible to increased stress and suicide (dickey & Budge, 2020). Fortunately, educational programs and activities that are run by schools receiving U.S. federal assistance must abide by Title IX of the Education Amendments of 1972. Title IX was designed to protect the educational rights of all students and ensures them a safe and nondiscriminatory environment, regardless of their gender identity (Lhamon & Gupta, 2016). However, the Trump administration rolled back some of the protections for transgender and gender-nonbinary students. An executive order signed by the Biden administration has signaled that protections will once again be put into place under Title IX (Gupta et al., 2021). These shifts indicate that the struggle for equal rights continues for gender and sexual minority individuals. How can teachers create a safe learning environment for all students, regardless of their gender identity or sexual orientation? A teacher ’s ultimate goal, as one superintendent observed, should be to determine what a child needs “to feel safe, included, and supported” (Orr et al., 2015, p. ii). We talk about bullying and teachers’ roles in handling all types of bullying in Cluster 13. For now, let’s consider what you can do if students come to you with concerns about their gender identity or sexual orientation. Even though parents and teachers are seldom the first people to hear about an adolescent’s identity concerns, you can be prepared. If a student does seek your counsel, Table 2.4 on the next page provides some ideas for reaching out and creating a safe environment for gender and sexual minority youth. See more ideas from the 2019 Safe Space Kit: A Guide to Supporting Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, Transgender and Queer Students in Your School, published by GLSEN. In addition, you might find it helpful to consult Schools in Transition: A Guide for Supporting Transgender Students in K–12 Schools (Orr et al., 2015). Both resources are readily available online. By simply offering a nonjudgmental presence, teachers can help gender and sexual minority youth to develop a more positive and healthy identity (Riggle & Rostosky, 2012). They can also be advocates for school policies and protections that make school a safe place for all students (see Collins & Ehrenhalt, 2018, Best Practice for Serving LGBTQ Students, from https://www.learningforjustice.org). We have dealt with a wide range of cultural differences in this cluster. How can teachers provide an appropriate education for all of their students? We turn now to this question.

73

74 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Table 2.4 Reaching Out to Help Students of Diverse Sexual Orientations The GLSEN Safe Space Kit provides tips for youth-service providers in schools for creating a safe and supportive learning environment for LGBTQ youth. TIPS FOR CREATING A SAFE ENVIRONMENT FOR LGBTQ STUDENTS It is important to create and maintain an environment in which lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queeridentifying youth and youth questioning their own sexual orientation feel welcome and safe. Here are several steps you can take: MAKE YOUR SUPPORT VISIBLE Safe Space stickers, posters, or other LGBTQ-affirming materials in your classroom can signal to students that you are an ally. Also consider letting staff in your school know that you support LGBTQ individuals. LISTEN One of the best things that you can do is to invite students to express what is going on in their lives and to listen without judgment. AFFIRM Use stories, curricula, and words to convey to students that they are not alone. Some LGBTQ youth feel isolated and lack a supportive social network with whom they can discuss their sexual orientation. Allow students to self-identify and use terms that affirm their identity. CONNECT STUDENTS TO RESOURCES You do not have to be the expert. Refer distressed students to someone who is trained to deal with sexual identity development. This is a demonstration of care, not a dismissal of responsibility. You can also help interested students find LGBTQ-affirming organizations and clubs. INTERVENE If you see or hear about a student being verbally or physically harassed, take action immediately. It is important that targeted students know that you will intervene and that perpetrators know that any form of harassment is unacceptable. RESPECT CONFIDENTIALITY Be sure to let students who confide in you know that you won’t share their information unless they ask for your help. FOLLOW UP If a student confides in you, check in with them with acceptance and understanding and to see if there is anything further you may be able to do. Additional steps you can take: • Work on your own sense of comfort around issues of sexual orientation and gender identity. • Dispel myths around sexual orientation by knowing facts and sharing that information. • Work on setting aside your own personal biases to better serve students dealing with issues around sexual orientation and gender identity. Source: Adapted from GLSEN. (2019). Safe Space kit: A guide to supporting lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender and queer students in your school.

Module 6 Summary Gender and Sexual Orientation in Teaching and Learning (pp. 64–74) What are gender identity and gender roles, and how do they develop? Gender identity is a personal view of oneself along the female-to-male continuum or in a nonbinary way. Gender roles refer to a set of stereotypically masculine or feminine characteristics and behaviors. The differential behavior of parents and teachers toward male and female children and the larger social environment influence children’s understandings of their gender roles. Through their interactions with family, peers, teachers, and the environment in general, children begin to form gender schemas, or organized networks of knowledge about what it means to belong to a particular gender group. How are gender biases communicated? In children’s books, there are more males in the titles and the illustrations, and the characters (especially the boys) continue to behave in stereotypical ways. Girl characters sometimes cross gender roles to be more active, but boy characters seldom show “feminine,” expressive traits. Some overrepresentation of gender exists in television commercials and online media, too. Teachers interact more with boys in both positive and negative ways. Lately some educators have claimed that schools are not supportive of boys, and single-gender classrooms have been suggested as an answer. The research findings on the value of these classrooms are mixed.

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

75

What are the stages of developing one’s sexual orientation? Stages of developing a sexual orientation can follow a pattern from discomfort to confusion to acceptance, particularly for sexual minority (e.g., gay, lesbian, bisexual, and transgender) students. Some researchers contend that one’s sexual orientation is not always permanent and can change over the years.

MODULE 7

Diversity and Teaching

Learning Objective 2.5 Define multicultural education and apply research on diversity to the creation of culturally relevant classrooms.

Creating Culturally Welcoming Classrooms Picture a tenth-grade class in urban Philadelphia, a first-grade class in rural Kentucky, and an eighth-grade class in El Paso. Now picture the classrooms where you were educated (those that you thought about at the beginning of the cluster) and those you see on television. Classrooms are the daily gathering places for a unique blend of individuals who are poor and rich and everything in between, who hail from widely varying cultural heritages, who express their gender and sexual orientation in stereotypical and nonstereotypical ways, and who display a rich tapestry of differences in other important aspects of life, such as religion, family composition, political affiliation, and (dis)ability. How can teachers, who begin their work from their own unique cultural locations, create classrooms that can hold, protect, and even embrace cultural diversity? Some educators have noted that creating culturally compatible classrooms is the foundation of providing a multicultural education, defined as A process of comprehensive school reform and basic education for all students. It challenges and rejects racism and other forms of discrimination in schools and society and accepts and affirms the pluralism (ethnic, racial, linguistic, religious, economic, and gender, among others) that students, their communities, and their teachers reflect. (Nieto & Bode, 2012, p. 42) James Banks (2015) described five core dimensions of multicultural education. Teachers are likely to be most familiar with the content integration dimension, which involves using examples and content from a variety of cultures when teaching a subject. And because they believe that multicultural education is simply a change in content, some teachers assume that multicultural education is irrelevant for subjects such as science and mathematics. But multicultural education is more than a change in curriculum. To make education welcoming and inclusive for all students in all subjects, we must consider other dimensions as well. Teachers enact multicultural education by embracing an equity pedagogy, which means they diversify their teaching practices to meet the needs of a wide range of learners from different socioeconomic, cultural, and ethnic backgrounds. They also pay attention to the ways in which students’ cultural frames affect the knowledge construction process. Multicultural education also involves prejudice reduction efforts, which focus on teaching practices that help reduce negative attitudes and biases about different cultural groups. Finally, an empowering school culture and social structure are critical. The way the athletics and counseling programs are structured, the teaching method used, lessons about prejudice, perspectives on knowledge—these and many more elements contribute to a true multicultural education that empowers all people. Banks’s (2015) five-dimensional model is just one framework providing ways to create a multicultural learning environment. An examination of the alternative approaches to multicultural education is beyond the scope of an educational psychology text, but be aware that no general agreement exists about the “best” approach to use. Nevertheless, many educators have suggested that a culturally relevant and culturally sustaining pedagogical approach is beneficial in all classrooms.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Multicultural Education (III, B) Know the major dimensions of multicultural education. Describe how these dimensions influence each other.

Multicultural education Education that promotes equity in the schooling of all students and challenges racism and other forms of discrimination.

76 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

Culturally Relevant Pedagogy

Culturally relevant pedagogy Excellent teaching that supports students’ cultural identities by promoting academic success, developing/maintaining cultural competence, and developing a critical consciousness to challenge the status quo.

Many scholars have described methods for teaching in a culturally relevant and culturally sustaining way (Delpit, 1995, 2012; Emdin, 2016; hooks, 1994; Love, 2019; Milner, 2015; Siddle Walker, 2001; Stevenson, 2014). The work of Gloria Ladson Billings (1990, 1992, 1994, 1995, 2009) is a good example. For 3 years, she studied excellent teachers in a California school district that served an African American community. To select the teachers, she asked parents and principals for nominations. Parents nominated teachers who respected them, created enthusiasm for learning in their children, and understood their children’s needs to operate successfully in two different worlds—the home community and the White world beyond. Principals nominated teachers who had few discipline referrals, high attendance rates, and students with high standardized test scores. Ladson Billings was able to examine in depth eight of the nine teachers who were nominated by both parents and principals. Ladson Billings developed a conception of teaching excellence based on her research. She used the term culturally relevant pedagogy to describe teaching that rests on three propositions. 1. Students must experience academic success. “Despite the current social inequities and hostile classroom environments, students must develop their academic skills. The ways those skills are developed may vary, but all students need literacy, numeracy, technological, social, and political skills in order to be active participants in a democracy” (Ladson Billings, 1995, p. 160). 2. Students must develop/maintain their cultural competence. As they become more academically skilled, students still retain their cultural competence. “Culturally relevant teachers utilize students’ culture as a vehicle for learning” (Ladson Billings, 1995, p. 161). For example, one teacher used rap music to teach about literal and figurative meaning, rhyme, alliteration, and onomatopoeia in poetry. Another brought in a community expert known for her sweet potato pies. Follow-up lessons included investigations of George Washington Carver’s sweet potato research, numerical analyses of taste tests, marketing plans for selling pies, and research on the educational preparation needed to become a chef. In this way, pedagogies also serve to sustain cultural diversity. 3. Students must develop a critical consciousness to challenge the status quo. In addition to developing academic skills while retaining cultural competence, excellent teachers help students “develop a broader sociopolitical consciousness that allows them to critique the social norms, values, mores, and institutions that produce and maintain social inequities” (Ladson Billings, 1995, p. 162). For example, in one school, students were upset that their textbooks were out of date. They mobilized to investigate the funding formulas that allowed middle-class students to have newer books, wrote letters to the newspaper editor to challenge these inequities, and updated their texts with current information from other sources. Ladson Billings (1995) noted that many people have said her three principles “are just good teaching.” She agrees that she is describing good teaching but questions “why so little of it seems to be occurring in classrooms populated by African American students” (p. 159). Lisa Delpit (2003) also described three steps for teaching students that are consistent with culturally relevant pedagogy. First, teachers must be convinced of the inherent intellectual capabilities, humanity, and spiritual character of their students—they must believe in the children. She has provided many examples from schools around the United States where ethnically diverse students from low-income families are reading well above grade level and doing advanced math (Delpit, 2012). When scores are low, the fault is not in the students but in their education. Second, teachers must fight the foolishness of asserting that high test scores or scripted lessons are evidence of good learning and good teaching. Successful instruction is “constant, rigorous, integrated across disciplines, connected to students’ lived cultures, connected to their intellectual legacies, engaging, and designed for critical thinking and problem solving that is useful beyond the classroom” (Delpit, 2003, p. 18). Third, teachers must learn who their students are and what legacies they bring. Then,

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

students can explore their own intellectual legacies and understand the important reasons for academic, social, physical, and moral excellence—not just to “get a job” but also “for our community, for your ancestors, for your descendants” (p. 19). Drawing on the cultural strengths of and advantages of students whose cultures have been historically erased, such as those from indigenous communities, is one way to honor their knowledge and ways of life (Kana‘iaupuni et al., 2017). In the past, discussions about teaching low-income students and those from racial, ethnic, or linguistic minority groups have focused on remediating problems or overcoming perceived deficits. But a culturally sustaining approach conveys to students that they are fully seen within the particular struggles and oppressive forces that they face. In her book We Want to Do More Than Survive: Abolitionist Teaching and the Pursuit of Educational Freedom, Bettina Love (2019) emphasized that “Dark children cannot thrive without a community of love, refusal, protection, knowledge, and resource-sharing” (p. 53). Full acceptance also means acknowledging the role that racism and power have played in systems of oppression. This is not easy, particularly for White teachers for whom these ideas might be new. Becoming an “abolitionist” teacher, Love argues, means you are ready to lose something, you are ready to let go of your privilege, you are ready to be in solidarity with dark people by recognizing your Whiteness in dark spaces, recognizing how it can take up space if unchecked, using your Whiteness in White spaces to advocate for and with dark people. And you understand that your White privilege allows you to take risks that dark people cannot take in the fight for educational justice. (pp. 158-159)

Diversity in Learning How might classrooms be organized to reflect the diverse ways in which students learn? Let’s look at four aspects that teachers can consider to help them honor the cultural differences present in their classes: social organization, cultural values and learning preferences, sociolinguistics, and cultural discontinuity. These were first introduced by Roland Tharp (1989) and offer useful suggestions for teachers. SOCIAL ORGANIZATION. Social structure or organization refers to the ways people interact to accomplish a particular goal. For example, the social organization of Hawaiian society depends heavily on collaboration and cooperation. Children play together in groups of friends and siblings, with older children often caring for the younger ones. When cooperative work groups of four or five boys and girls were established in Hawaiian classrooms, student learning and participation improved (Okagaki, 2001, 2006). The teacher worked intensively with one group while the children in the remaining groups helped each other. But when the same structure was tried in a Navajo classroom, students would not work together. These children are socialized to be more solitary and not to play with children of the opposite sex. By setting up same-sex working groups of only two or three Navajo students, teachers encouraged them to help each other. Providing choices and variety in grouping structures creates greater flexibility in the social organization of the classroom. CULTURAL VALUES AND LEARNING PREFERENCES. As noted throughout this cluster, all learners (and teachers) have distinct values and preferences related to their diverse cultural backgrounds. For example, research has shown that some Hispanic American students are more oriented toward family and group loyalty. This might mean that these students prefer cooperative activities and dislike being made to compete with fellow students (Garcia, 1992; Vasquez, 1990). It might also mean that some students will behave in ways that reflect certain Latino values, such as these four identified by Dingfelder (2005):

Familismo: Tightly knit families. Discussing family problems or business might be seen as disloyal. Simpatia: Value of interpersonal harmony. Assertively voicing personal opinions or arguing might be seen as inappropriate.

77

78 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity Respeto: Respect for people in authority, for example, teachers and government officials. Personalismo: Valuing of close interpersonal relationships; discomfort with distant, cold, professional relationships. The learning approaches of African Americans might be inconsistent with teaching approaches in some schools. For example, some student learning is facilitated by taking a visual/global approach rather than a verbal/analytic approach; a preference for reasoning by inference or improvisation rather than by formal logic; a focus on people and relationships; a preference for energetic involvement in several activities simultaneously rather than routine, step-by-step learning; a tendency to approximate numbers, space, and time; and a greater dependence on nonverbal communication. Students of color who identify with their heritage cultures might respond better to open-ended questions with more than one answer as opposed to single, right-answer questions. Questions that focus on meaning or the “big picture” might be more productive than questions that focus on details (Bennett, 2011; Gay, 2010; Sheets, 2005). In many indigenous communities, humility and harmony are valued more than competition. Teaching and learning often take place through oral narrative forms that emphasize holistic, integrated, and “high context” ways of knowing that use cultural metaphors and imagery (Cajete, 2020). For example, research has shown that Navajo students preferred hearing a story all the way through to the end before discussing parts of the story. Teachers who stopped reading to ask comprehension questions seemed odd to these students and interrupted their learning process (Tharp, 1989). Also, these students sometimes show strong preferences for learning privately through trial and error rather than having their mistakes made public (Morgan, 2009). But as you saw earlier, there are dangers in stereotyping any group, especially in terms of their cultural values or learning preferences. A heated debate exists today about whether identifying group differences in learning preferences is a dangerous, racist (and/or sexist, colonialist) exercise. In our society, we are quick to move from the notion of “difference” to the idea of “deficits” and stereotypes (Christopher et al., 2014). The latter view assumes that White, middle-class culture is “normal” and valuable and can lead to the perpetuation of oppressive teaching approaches that silence or erase students’ ancestral and cultural roots (McIntosh, 2009; Paris, 2012; Sabzalian, 2019). The best advice for teachers is to be sensitive to individual differences in all your students and to make available alternative paths to learning. Never prejudge how a student will learn best based on assumptions about the student’s ethnicity, race, or gender. Get to know the individual and get to know their culture. Sociolinguistics The study of the formal and informal rules for how, when, about what, to whom, and how long to speak in conversations within cultural groups.

Pragmatics The rules for when and how to use language to be an effective communicator in a particular culture.

Participation structures The formal and informal rules for how to take part in a given activity.

Sociolinguistics is the study of “the courtesies and conventions of conversation across cultures” (Tharp, 1989, p. 351). The classroom is a special setting for communicating; it has its own set of rules for when, how, to whom, about what subject, and in what manner to use language. To be successful, students must know these communication rules; that is, they must understand the pragmatics of the classroom—when, where, and how to communicate. This is not such an easy task. As class activities change, rules change. Sometimes you have to raise your hand (during the teacher’s presentation), but sometimes you don’t (during story time on the rug or during an experiment in the lab). Sometimes asking a question (during discussion) is good, but other times (such as when the teacher is reprimanding you), asking a question isn’t so good. These differing activity rules are called participation structures, and they define appropriate participation for each class activity. Most classrooms have many different participation structures. To be competent communicators in the classroom, students sometimes have to read very subtle, nonverbal cues telling them which participation structures are currently in effect. For example, when the teacher moves to the whiteboard, students should look up and be ready for instructions. Some children are simply better than others at reading classroom situations because the participation structures of the school match the structures they have learned at home (Tyler et al., 2008). The communication rules for most school situations are similar to those in middle-class homes, so children from these homes often appear to be more competent SOCIOLINGUISTICS.

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

communicators. They know the unwritten rules. However, it seems that even students who speak the same language as their teachers might still have trouble communicating and so have trouble learning school subjects. What can teachers do? You can begin by making communication rules for activities clear and explicit. Do not assume students know what to do. Use cues to signal students when changes occur. Explain and demonstrate desired behavior. I have seen teachers show young children how to use their “inside voices,” “six-inch voices,” or “whisper voices.” One teacher said and then demonstrated, “If you have to interrupt me while I’m working with other children, stand quietly beside me until I can help you.” Be consistent in responding to students. If students are supposed to raise their hands, don’t call on those who break the rules. In these ways you will help students learn the rules of communication in the school context. CULTURAL DISCONTINUITY. What happens when students’ values, participation structures, and learning preferences conflict with those espoused in mainstream school culture? After all, a value-free learning environment does not exist (Christopher et al., 2014). Kenneth Tyler and his colleagues (2008) referred to this phenomenon as cultural discontinuity, which they defined as “a school-based behavioral process where the cultural value-based learning preferences and practices of many ethnic minority students—those typically originating from home or parental socialization activities—are discontinued at school” (p. 281). Some learners who arrive at school have difficulty finding their own values reflected in the school culture, which is predominantly based on White, middle-class norms. Consequently, some students “learn quickly that they are expected to divorce themselves from their culture in order to be academically successful,” as Christopher Emdin (2016) poignantly asserted in his book For White Folks Who Teach in the Hood (p. 13). What is the cost to students of being in a school culture with dissimilar values? “A loss of their dignity and a shattering of their personhood,” says Emdin. “Urban youth who enter schools seeing themselves as smart and capable are confronted by curriculum that is blind to their realities and school rules that seek to erase their culture” (p. 13). Such students learn that they must conform or else risk becoming invisible and devalued in the eyes of their teachers and peers.

Lessons for Teachers: Teaching Every Student To be effective, instruction should be coupled with humility and respect for cultural differences (Delpit, 2012). The set of cultural behaviors and values that works in one context might not work in another. That does not mean that one is correct and the other is not. How will you understand, support, and build on all the cultures of your students? Here are several general teaching principles to guide you in finding answers to these questions. See the Guidelines: Culturally Relevant Teaching on the next page for more ideas. We began this cluster with a bit of introspective “me” search. Doing more of this will help you to become familiar and at ease with your own racial and ethnic heritage, cultural and linguistic background, gender identity, and sexual orientation (Love, 2019). Developing an understanding of your own many identities is essential to your being able to create a safe space for students to do the same. Self-awareness also involves questioning the assumption that your values and experiences are the “norm” and therefore are the yardstick by which others should be measured. Scholars like Peggy McIntosh (2009) encourage all teachers—White teachers in particular—to explore their own varied identities. This means reflecting on the ways in which we, in our unique and intersecting racial, economic, ethnic, religious, gender, and sexual identities (to name just a few), have reaped advantages and disadvantages from the various systems of power around us. We would like to underscore this point: To varying degrees, all people face both unearned privileges and unearned disadvantages in their lives tied to the ways their identities are (or are not) valued by others in different contexts. Take a moment to return to the Stop & Think activities earlier in this cluster. How has some aspect of your identity advantaged you? How has some aspect of your identity led to disadvantages? How can this help you to understand your students’ experiences?

KNOW YOURSELF.

79

80 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

GUIDELINES Culturally Relevant Teaching Experiment with different grouping arrangements to encourage social harmony and cooperation. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Try “study buddies” and pairs. Organize heterogeneous groups of four or five. Establish larger teams for older students.

Provide a range of ways to learn material to accommodate a range of learning preferences. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Give students verbal materials at different reading levels. Offer visual materials—charts, diagrams, and models. Provide audio and video for listening and viewing. Set up activities and projects.

Teach classroom procedures directly, even ways of doing things that you thought everyone would know. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Tell students how to get the teacher’s attention. Explain when and how to interrupt the teacher if students need help. Show which materials students can take and which require permission. Demonstrate acceptable ways to disagree with or challenge another student.

Learn the meanings of your students’ different behaviors. Examples 1. 2.

Ask students how they feel when you correct or praise them. What gives them this feeling? Talk to family and community members and other teachers to discover the meanings of expressions, gestures, or other responses that are unfamiliar to you.

Emphasize meaning in teaching. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Make sure students understand what they read. Try storytelling and other modes that don’t require written materials. Use examples that relate abstract concepts to everyday experiences; for instance, relate negative numbers to being overdrawn in your bank account.

Get to know the customs, traditions, and values of your students. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Use holidays as a chance to discuss the origins and meanings of traditions. Analyze different traditions for common themes. Attend community fairs and festivals.

Help students detect racist, classist, sexist, and homophobic messages. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Analyze curriculum materials for biases. Make students “bias detectives,” reporting biased comments in the media. Discuss the ways that students communicate biased messages about each other, and decide what should be done when this happens. Capitalize on teachable moments. Discuss expressions of prejudice such as antiSemitism or homophobia when they arise in the media, in curricular materials, or in class.

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

We must learn who our students are and understand the legacies they bring (Delpit, 2003). Nothing you read in a cluster on cultural differences will teach you enough to understand the lives of all your students. If you can take other courses in college or read about other cultures, we encourage you to do so. But reading and studying are not enough. You will learn much more by getting to know your students. This means fully seeing your students as complex, developing, and culturally situated individuals. Get to know your students’ families and communities too. Elba Reyes, a successful bilingual teacher for children with special needs, describes her approach:

KNOW YOUR STUDENTS.

Usually I find that if you really want to know a parent, you get to know them on their own turf. This is key to developing trust and understanding the parents’ perspective. First, get to know the community. Learn where the local grocery store is and what the children do after school. Then schedule a home visit at a time that is convenient for the parents. The home environment is not usually as ladened with failure. I sometimes observed the child being successful in the home, for example, riding a bicycle or helping with dinner. (Bos & Reyes, 1996, p. 349) Try to spend time with students and parents on projects outside school. Ask parents to help in class or speak to your students about their jobs, their hobbies, or the history and heritage of their ethnic group. Don’t wait until a student is in trouble to have the first meeting with a family member. Watch for and listen to the ways that your students interact in large and small groups. Have students write to you, and write back to them. Eat lunch with one or two students. Spend some nonteaching time with them. From knowledge should come respect for your students’ learning strengths—for the struggles they face and the obstacles they have overcome. Love (2019) emphasizes the importance of celebrating the full selfhood of students, particularly students of color.

RESPECT YOUR STUDENTS.

Not just their culture, language, sexuality, or current circumstances, but their entire selves, past, present, and future. Their ancestors, their family members, their friends, their religion, their music, their dress, their language, the ways that they express their gender and sexuality, and their communities must all be embraced and loved. (p. 120) For a child, genuine acceptance is a necessary condition for developing self-esteem. Sometimes the self-image and occupational aspirations of some children actually decline in their early years in public school, probably because of the emphasis on majority culture values, accomplishments, and history. One solution is to engage in what Emdin (2016) calls “reality pedagogy—an approach to teaching and learning that has a primary goal of meeting each student on his or her own cultural and emotional turf” (p. 27). By inviting students to bring their group’s culture into the classroom (in the form of literature, art, music, or any other cultural knowledge), teachers can help students maintain a sense of pride in their cultural group. This integration of culture must be more than the “tokenism” of sampling ethnic foods or wearing costumes. Students should teach and learn with others about the socially and intellectually important contributions of various groups. Many excellent references provide background information, history, and teaching strategies for different groups of students (e.g., Banks, 2014; Emdin, 2016; Gay, 2010; Irvine & Armento, 2001; Ladson Billings, 1995). TEACH YOUR STUDENTS. The most important thing you can do for your students is teach them to read, write, speak, compute, think, and create—through constant, rigorous, culturally connected instruction (Delpit, 2003). A strong emphasis on academics and high expectations combined with caring support for students are key (Palardy, 2013). Beware of how hidden biases can creep in. Sometimes, in an attempt to be compassionate or to relieve the stress on students at risk, teachers give these students more positive feedback than they would privileged students. This well-intended but overly positive feedback can contribute to lowering expectations and reducing the academic challenge for these students (Harber et al., 2012). Too often, goals for students with low SES or from ethnic minority groups have focused

81

82 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity exclusively on basic skills. Students are taught words and sounds, but the meaning of the story is supposed to come later. And finally, teach students directly about how to be students. In the early grades, this could mean directly teaching the courtesies and conventions of the classroom: how to get a turn to speak, how and when to interrupt the teacher, how to whisper, how to get help in a small group, how to give an explanation that is helpful, how to respect difference when interacting with others. In the later grades, it might mean teaching the academic language and study skills that fit your subject. You can ask students to learn “how we do it in school” without violating the third principle—respect your students. Ways of asking questions around the kitchen table at home might be different from ways of asking questions in school, but students can learn both ways without deciding that either way is superior. And you can expand ways of doing it in school to include more possibilities.

Module 7 Summary Creating Culturally Welcoming Classrooms (pp. 75–82) What is multicultural education? Multicultural education is a field of study designed to increase educational equity for all students. According to the multicultural ideal, America should be transformed into a society that values diversity. James Banks suggests that multicultural education has five dimensions: integrating content, helping students understand how knowledge is influenced by beliefs, reducing prejudice, creating social structures in schools that support learning and development for all students, and using teaching methods that reach all students. What is culturally relevant pedagogy? Gloria Ladson Billings (1995, 2004) describes culturally relevant teaching that rests on three propositions: Students must experience academic success, develop/maintain their cultural competence, and develop a critical consciousness to challenge the status quo. This includes recognizing power structures and racism and the ways they have perpetuated inequalities. What are some aspects of diversity in learning? Teachers should pay attention to the social organization and participation structures in their classrooms and teach students directly how to be successful in these structures, strategically use information about students’ cultural values and learning preferences, avoid cultural discontinuities in class, and embrace themselves and their students as individuals with complex identities. These accomplishments can lead to respecting every student and teaching more effectively.

Cluster 2 Review Key Terms Bias Culturally relevant pedagogy Culture Discrimination Ethnic-racial identity Ethnicity Gender biases Gender identity Gender roles Gender schemas Intersectionality Multicultural education

Nigrescence Participation structures Pragmatics Prejudice Race Resistance culture Sexual orientation Socioeconomic status (SES) Sociolinguistics Stereotype Stereotype threat

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

83

Connect and Extend to Licensure Multiple-Choice Questions 1.

Socioeconomic status and school achievement are often correlated. Which one of the following statements is NOT true regarding the relationship between SES and levels of achievement? A.

The longer a child lives in poverty, the greater the impact on achievement.

B.

Children who are poor are no more likely to be kept back in school than children who are not.

C.

Students of all ethnic groups with high SES generally show higher levels of achievement on test scores and stay in school longer than students with low SES.

D. 2.

3.

D. 4.

Children who are poor are more likely to live in suburban and rural areas than to live in central cities.

Educators often assume that students are not bright because they have inadequate resources at home. This inadequacy manifests itself as a lack of familiarity with school-related activities. When this occurs, what is the likely outcome?

Damon’s high self-efficacy makes him think that testing is a waste of his time.

To prevent gender bias in his fourth-grade classroom, Mr. Bonner used gender-free language, provided positive role models, and ensured that all his students had opportunities to engage in various activities by rotating classroom jobs and activities. His school was also experimenting with single-sex classrooms. Next year, Mr. Bonner thought he might opt to teach in one of those classrooms. Which of the following statements concerning single-sex classrooms is NOT true? A.

Single-sex classrooms have positive effects on learning, motivation, and engagement when certain conditions are met.

B.

Teaching is not easier in a single-sex classroom than it is in a mixed-sex classroom.

C.

Teachers must use teaching strategies that are geared specifically for students of a particular sex.

D.

Students in single-sex classrooms are often less concerned about making positive impressions on peers.

A.

These students work harder to prove themselves to their teachers.

B.

Constructed-Response Questions

Teachers might have low expectations that have negative impacts on future academic success.

Case

C.

The students will perform poorly because they will never catch up with their peers.

D.

Teachers understand that not all students will be able to achieve academically.

Damon, an African American student in Diane Collins’s math class, pushed his math test away after a few minutes and proclaimed, “This is stupid. I don’t know why we even have to do this.” What is Ms. Collins most likely to think?

Paulo Nzambi moved from his home in Angola to the United States in the fifth grade. Although Paulo’s English and schooling were adequate in Angola, his teacher, Katie Wyant, worried about his social adjustment. His quiet demeanor and soft voice were, in many ways, the opposite of those of his male peers. Paulo appeared hesitant when interacting with her, as though he was unsure about how to behave. As the year progressed, Katie noticed that he had not made any progress in adjusting to the classroom. She decided she needed to be proactive in finding a solution.

A.

She should send Damon to the principal’s office for insubordination.

5.

B.

She might have made the test too difficult for her African American students, so she should make an easier test next time.

To acquire a better understanding of Paulo and make school a more positive experience, what three types of relationships would assist Paulo as well as Ms. Wyant?

6.

C.

Damon might be exhibiting performance-avoidance goals because he doesn’t want to look dumb.

What aspects of culturally relevant teaching might Katie Wyant employ to assist Paulo Nzambi in his transition to an American classroom?

What Would They Do? Teachers’ Casebook: Conversations About Race At the beginning of this cluster, we asked you to think critically about students who were disputing an incident involving an unarmed Black teenager and the police. As you now consider these teachers’ responses to that question, we again

suggest that you think critically. Do the following responses reflect (or contradict) any of the concepts discussed in this cluster? Do the suggestions and solutions described here make sense in light of what you have learned about best practices in this or other classes? How do these ideas align with or challenge your personal philosophy of teaching?

84 Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity Dr. Mari Ann Banks • System Equity Coordinator City Schools of Decatur, Decatur, GA The U.S. is a country scarred by its history of racism, so if we wish to be great teachers, it is essential that we either have or develop the ability to examine race with students and in our own praxis. Many of us have been indoctrinated with the belief that conversations about race are injurious and impolite. Nevertheless, the current racial climate shows us that the time for polite conversation that circumnavigates race is over. Black people are being murdered. We do much more harm in bypassing such subject matter than we will ever do by engaging students in thoughtful conversation about how we make it stop. In the U.S., race has always been the paisley elephant in the room, but it’s an elephant that only White people have had the privilege to ignore while the rest of us are crushed in its wake. It’s time for White people to shoulder their share of the weight. This requires active engagement. However, teachers need to be thoughtful and careful when having such conversations. That’s why it’s so important for us to develop our understanding of race and use appropriate discussion protocols with our students. For example, I might directly ask the Blue Lives Matter students, “What would it mean to ‘Blue’ lives if Black lives mattered?” Or I might provide the following journal prompt: “Police have chosen to serve and to risk their lives. Black people have no choice in being Black and do not want their lives to be at risk. Explore what these two statements mean together.” After students have had time to think privately, I would facilitate discussion of these topics using one or more protocols such as “Critical Friends” or “Courageous Conversations” to help guide discussions in a healthy manner.

Amy Pochodylo • High School Science Teacher Buckeye Valley High School, Delaware, OH This situation is not an unusual one, so thinking about it beforehand is key to avoiding a knee-jerk, emotional response. As a teacher, I have spent time considering my own identities and the privileges that come along with those identity groups, to help me understand my own biases and blind spots. Other important pre-work that I do with my classes is to establish norms. Ideally, these norms make the classroom a safe space for every learner, and address how to have conversations that respectfully consider a range of viewpoints. As a science teacher, I try to ground everything in data. Black Lives Matter should not be a political or controversial statement, and there is so much data to support that. I would present data on the racial differences in the use of deadly force or stops by police officers to demonstrate why policing is often at the center of the conversation around Black Lives Matter. I would also use data around environmental justice movements and race-based differences in healthcare outcomes. After spending time as a class analyzing these sets of data, I would have the class work together (or in small groups) to write a claim, evidence, [and] reasoning using the data to back up the claim that Black Lives Matter. My hope would

be that, by approaching this topic through the scientific practices used every day in the classroom, students could better understand the statement “Black Lives Matter” and that it isn’t explicitly political. My fear as a teacher around discussing group-based identities, including race, is that I have my own set of biases and identities that will certainly lead to blind spots in my own thinking and imperfect ways of leading conversations in my classroom. While I fear saying the wrong thing and marginalizing other identities, I also believe in the importance of having these conversations in the classroom, being open about my own shortcomings, and supporting my students as they learn.

Demetrick Tensley • 9th–12th Grade Business Education and Computer Science Teacher Seneca High School, Seneca, SC The topics of race and policing are ubiquitous in our society. As individuals of all ages are becoming more aware of intersectionality, these conversations will spill into our classrooms. For teachers, this can feel daunting. Some might hedge and think, “This is not a part of my curriculum,” and work to hide from these topics in hopes of escaping possible discomfort. That being said, the world is diversifying. The workforce will need our future graduates to be prepared to thrive in diverse teams. To establish a foundation for this end, we must cultivate an environment in our classrooms for discussing difficult topics. This situation provides an authentic opportunity to guide students in a discussion of these topics. This type of discussion will also help students to hone the essential skill of civil discourse. Teachers attempting to lead students through this type of discussion will need to take an unbiased stance, lay ground rules, and preserve healthy discussion. If teachers are uncomfortable or feel ill-prepared, school guidance counselors are an excellent resource. The prospect of a potentially contentious situation can cause fear. During preparation for a controversial discussion, I work to be both unbiased and prepared with facts/historical background. When dealing with issues at the intersection of race and policing, students can voice provocative opinions. This can make it difficult to maintain impartiality. I’ve found that in these situations, it is best to be prepared, listen, and look for commonalities. To continue daily toward the goal of students thriving in diverse group settings, I also try to have students complete components of the curriculum in diverse teams. Facilitating group work allows me to talk to students about the importance of understanding peers’ beliefs, differences, and the need for mutual respect.

India Chambers • Kindergarten through 8th Grade Director of Academics Northside Preparatory Academy, Cincinnati, OH The teacher must set an expectation that the classroom is a safe environment to express one’s self respectfully

Cluster 2 Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity

and to value others’ opinions. If this expectation is not in place, these difficult, uncomfortable learning conversations cannot take place. Once this expectation has been established, I would give students background stories of people around the world and have them write a journal entry as that person. This will allow them to explore other lives from a different perspective. For instance, one day we will read an excerpt from Anne Frank and note the realities of her world and experiences. Students would write a journal as Anne. Another day, they would write a journal entry as Trayvon Martin. Another day they would write it as a police officer who is trying their best to defend the community they serve while facing a difficult situation that could possibly cost someone their lives. We would do these exercises throughout the year to extend students’ perspectives and allow for them to develop empathy and respect for all persons. If I knew students of color were upset about White students’ comments, I would host a forum with a panel discussion with stakeholders from various backgrounds. Students, teachers, and parents would have the opportunity to ask questions that would be addressed by the panel. To acknowledge the differences and the realities of marginalized populations would be imperative, as would coming up with a solution that can take our differences and meld them into a beautifully blended school community. I’m not fearful of having conversations about race because I believe things resonate with people when they’re uncomfortable, and this forces them to act and make a mental shift. Race talks are uncomfortable, but extremely important to change mentalities.

Lucy Ordaz Sanchez • 7th and 8th Grade Family and Consumer Science Teacher Dixon Middle School, Provo, UT I would take this opportunity in a heartbeat! I would first play the line game—have students stand on both sides of a line of tape and ask random questions such as “Who has a dog?” Students who have a dog step on the line. Questions could be comical at first for students [to] understand how the game works and also get them to relax a little. Then I would get more serious with the questions. Students would be able to see that they do have things in common with other students they might think are too different, as both might step onto the line after being asked, for example, “Are your parents . . . divorced?” After a couple of rounds of this game I would set up the classroom for all of us to be sitting in a circle (circle has no sides and

85

symbolizes unity). I would start a discussion about the game to help them see that although they might look different, some of them have similar situations in their lives that they can relate to. In order for this activity to work I would have already spent time creating a safe environment in my classroom where students know that they are being heard, respected, valued, and seen for who they are. It is likely that some comments would trigger emotions and perhaps the desire to be hurtful or offensive, but before the discussion we would have set up some rules as a class to minimize those negative comments. My only fear in talking about race would be the reactions of the parents. I know students go home and sometimes discuss what their day was like at school. I believe that the students reflect and/ or believe what is being discussed at home and sometimes as adults we forget how our words, our thoughts and actions will impact the children/youth who are watching us.

Melissa Roy • 10th Grade Reading and English Teacher Lake Brantley High School, Altamonte Springs, FL The majority of students in my high school remedial reading classes belong to racial and ethnic minority groups. It would be more comfortable for me as a White teacher to ignore this issue and stick with academics, for I sometimes fear saying the wrong thing and offending my students of color. Given the current social climate in the United States, however, that would be the wrong decision. Though I may not understand what it feels like to be a minority, as the adult in the room, it is my responsibility to facilitate opportunities for students to develop understanding for and empathy toward one another. This issue is far bigger than my classroom and could have lasting repercussions for students who are members of marginalized groups. From day one I create a space in which respectful discourse among students is not only allowed but also expected, and I model what that looks like for them until they learn. I offer them daily opportunities to read about the world and develop their own evidencebased opinions of different topics of interest. We engage in Socratic seminars, Harkness discussions, and other activities designed to spark conversation about topics that impact their lives. Perhaps most importantly, I encourage students to share their stories so they can get to know each other on a deeper personal level. My hope is that this will help them understand those who look, live, and believe differently than they do. I believe the most significant lessons I can teach my students are to care for others, notice when injustice happens, and stand up for those who do not have a voice. This is how we change the world!

Cluster 3

Ying T. H., Age 12, Malaysia ICAF.org

Cognitive Development

Teachers’ Casebook: Symbols and Cymbals What Would You Do? The district curriculum guide calls for a unit on poetry, including lessons on symbolism in poems. You are concerned that many of your fourth-grade students may not be ready to understand this abstract concept. To test the waters, you ask a few students what a symbol is. “It’s sorta like a big metal thing that you bang together,” Tracy says as she waves her hands like a drum major. “Yeah,” Sean adds, “my sister plays one in the high school band.” You realize that they are on the wrong track here, so you try again: “I was thinking of a different kind of symbol, like a ring as a symbol of marriage or a heart as a symbol of love or. . . .” You are met with blank stares. Trevor ventures, “You mean like the Olympic torch?” “And what does that symbolize, Trevor?” you ask. Trevor replies, “Like I said, a torch,” but he thinks, “Duh!” 86

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

Critical Thinking • What do these students’ reactions tell you about children’s thinking? • How would you approach this unit? • What more would you do to “listen” to your students’ thinking so that you could match your teaching to their level of thinking? • How would you give your students concrete experiences with symbolism? • How will you decide whether the students are developmentally ready for this material?

Overview and Objectives What is going on with Trevor? In this cluster, you will find out. We begin with a definition of development and examine three basic questions. Psychologists have debated the answers to these questions for many years: nature versus nurture, continuity versus discontinuity, and critical versus sensitive periods for development. Next we look at general principles of human development that most psychologists affirm. To understand cognitive development, we begin by studying how the brain works and then explore the ideas of two of the most influential cognitive developmental theorists, Jean Piaget and Lev Vygotsky. Piaget’s ideas have implications for teachers about how their students think and what they can learn. We will consider criticisms of his ideas as well. The work of Lev Vygotsky, a Russian psychologist, highlights the important roles teachers and parents play in the cognitive development of the child. Vygotsky’s theory is becoming more and more influential in the field of child development. By the time you have completed this cluster, you should be able to: 3.1 Provide a definition of development that takes into account three

agreed-upon principles and describe three continuing debates about development, along with current consensus on these questions. 3.2 Summarize research on the physical development of the brain and

possible implications for teaching. 3.3 Explain the principles and stages presented in Piaget’s theory of

cognitive development, including criticisms of his theory. 3.4 Explain the principles presented in Vygotsky’s theory of development,

including criticisms of his theory. 3.5 Discuss implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky ‘s theories for teaching.

OUTLINE Teachers’ Casebook—Symbols and Cymbals: What Would You Do? Overview and Objectives MODULE 8: Development and the Brain: Some General Principles A Definition of Development Three Questions Across the Theories General Principles of Development

87

88 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development The Brain and Cognitive Development The Developing Brain: Neurons The Developing Brain: Cerebral Cortex Brain Development in Childhood and Adolescence Putting It All Together: How the Brain Works Neuroscience, Learning, and Teaching Lessons for Teachers: General Principles MODULE 9: Piagetian and Information Processing Theories Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development Influences on Development Basic Tendencies in Thinking Four Stages of Cognitive Development Some Limitations of Piaget’s Theory Information Processing, Neo-Piagetian, and Neuroscience Views of Cognitive Development MODULE 10: Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Perspective and the Implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories for Teachers Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Perspective The Social Sources of Individual Thinking Cultural Tools and Cognitive Development The Roles of Language and Private Speech The Zone of Proximal Development Limitations of Vygotsky’s Theory Implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories for Teachers Piaget: What Can We Learn? Vygotsky: What Can We Learn? An Example Curriculum: Tools of the Mind Reaching Every Student: Teaching in the “Magic Middle” Cognitive Development: Lessons for Teachers Cluster 3 Review Key Terms • Connect and Extend to Licensure • Teachers’ Casebook—Symbols and Cymbals: What Would They Do?

MODULE 8

Development and the Brain: Some General Principles

Learning Objective 3.1 Provide a definition of development that takes into account three agreed-upon principles, and describe three continuing debates about development, along with current consensus on these questions. Learning Objective 3.2 Summarize research on the physical development of the brain and possible implications for teaching.

A Definition of Development In the next few clusters, as we explore how children develop, we will encounter some surprising situations.

• Leah, a 5-year-old, is certain that rolling out a ball of clay into a snake creates more clay because “it’s longer.”

• A 9-year-old child in Geneva, Switzerland, firmly insists that being Swiss and being Genevan at the same time is impossible: “I’m already Swiss. I can’t also be Genevan.”

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

89

• Jamal, a very bright elementary school student, cannot answer the question “How would life be different if people did not sleep?” because he insists, “People HAVE TO SLEEP!”

• A 2-year-old brings his own mother to comfort a friend who is crying, even though the friend’s mother is available, too. What explains these interesting events? You will soon find out because you are entering the world of child and adolescent development. The term development in its most general, psychological sense refers to certain changes that occur in human beings (or animals) between conception and death. The term is not applied to all changes but, rather, to those that appear in orderly ways and remain for a reasonably long period of time. A temporary change caused by a brief illness, for example, is not considered a part of development. Human development can be divided into several different aspects: Physical development, as you might guess, deals with changes in the body. Personal development is the term generally used for changes in an individual’s identity and personality. Social development refers to changes in the way an individual relates to others. And cognitive development refers to changes in thinking, reasoning, and decision making. Many changes during development are simply matters of growth and maturation. Maturation refers to changes that occur naturally and spontaneously and that are, to a large extent, genetically programmed. Such changes emerge over time and are relatively unaffected by environment, except in cases of malnutrition or severe illness. Much of a person’s physical development falls into this category. Other changes are brought about through learning, as individuals interact with their environments. Such changes make up a large part of a person’s social development. But what about the developments of thinking and personality? Most psychologists agree that in these areas, both maturation and interaction with the environment (or nature and nurture, as they are sometimes called) are important, but they disagree about the amount of emphasis to place on each one. Nature versus nurture is one of three continuing discussions in theories of development.

Development Orderly, adaptive changes we go through between conception and death; these developmental changes remain for a reasonably long period of time.

Physical development Changes in body structure and function over time.

Personal development Changes in personality that take place as one grows.

Social development Changes over time in the ways we relate to others.

Three Questions Across the Theories

Cognitive development

Because there are many different approaches to research and theory, there also are continuing debates about key questions surrounding development.

Gradual, orderly changes by which mental processes become more complex and sophisticated.

Which is more important in development: the “nature” of an individual (heredity, genes, biological processes, maturation, etc.) or the “nurture” of environmental contexts (education, parenting, culture, social policies, etc.)? This debate has raged for at least 2,000 years and has accumulated many labels along the way, including “heredity versus environment,” “biology versus culture,” “maturation versus learning,” and “innate versus acquired abilities.” In earlier centuries, philosophers, poets, religious leaders, and politicians argued the question. Even in scientific explanations, the pendulum has swung back and forth between nature and nurture (Cairns & Cairns, 2006; Miller, 2016). Scientists now bring new tools to the discussion because they can map genes or trace the effects of drugs on brain activity, for example (Gottlieb et al., 2006). Today the environment is seen as critical to development, but so are biological factors and individual differences. In fact, some psychologists assert that behaviors are determined 100% by biology and 100% by environment—they can’t be separated (Miller, 2016). Current views emphasize complex coactions (joint actions) of nature and nurture. For example, a child born with a very easy going, calm disposition will likely elicit different reactions from parents, playmates, and teachers than will a child who is often upset and difficult to soothe; this shows that individuals are active in constructing their own environments. But environments shape individuals as well—if not, what good would education be? So today, the either/or debates about nature and WHAT IS THE SOURCE OF DEVELOPMENT? NATURE VERSUS NURTURE.

Maturation Genetically programmed, naturally occurring changes over time.

Coactions Joint actions of individual biology and the environment—each shapes and influences the other.

90 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development nurture are of less interest to educational and developmental psychologists. As a pioneering developmental psychologist said more than 100 years ago, the more exciting questions involve understanding how “both causes work together” (Baldwin, 1895, p. 77). Is human development a continuous process of increasing abilities, or are there leaps to new stages when abilities actually change? A continuous process would be like gradual improvements in your running endurance through systematic exercise. A discontinuous change (also called qualitative) would be like many of the changes in humans during puberty, such as the ability to reproduce—an entirely different ability. You can think of continuous or quantitative change like walking up a ramp to go higher and higher: Progress is steady. A discontinuous or qualitative change is more like walking up stairs: There are level periods, and then you ascend the next step all at once. Piaget’s theory of cognitive development, described in the next section, is an example of qualitative, discontinuous change in children’s thinking abilities. But other explanations of cognitive development based on learning theories emphasize gradual, continuous, quantitative change.

WHAT IS THE SHAPE OF DEVELOPMENT? CONTINUITY VERSUS DISCONTINUITY.

Sensitive periods Times when a person is especially ready to learn certain things or is responsive to certain experiences.

TIMING: IS IT TOO LATE? CRITICAL VERSUS SENSITIVE PERIODS. Are there critical periods during which certain abilities, such as language, need to develop? If those opportunities are missed, can the child still “catch up”? These are questions about timing and development. Many earlier psychologists, particularly those influenced by Freud, believed that early childhood experiences are critical, especially for emotional/social and cognitive development. But does early toilet training really set all of us on a particular life path? Probably not. More recent research shows that later experiences are powerful, too, and can change the direction of development. Most psychologists today talk about sensitive periods, not critical periods. There are “windows of opportunity”—times when a person is especially ready for or responsive to certain experiences (Scalise & Felds, 2017; Schunk, 2020). BEWARE OF EITHER/OR. As you might imagine, these debates about development proved too complicated to be settled by splitting alternatives into either/or possibilities (Miller, 2016). Today, most psychologists view human development, learning, and motivation as a set of interacting and coacting contexts, from the inner biological structures and processes that influence development, such as genes, cells, nutrition, and disease, to the external factors of families, neighborhoods, social relationships, educational and health institutions, public policies, time periods, historical events, and so on. So the effects of a childhood disease on the cognitive development of a child born in the 16th century to a poor family and treated by bloodletting or leeches will be quite different from the effect of the same disease on a child born in 2022 to a wealthy family and given the best treatment available for that time period. Throughout the rest of this book, we will try to make sense of development, learning, motivation, and teaching without falling into the either/or trap.

General Principles of Development Although there is disagreement about exactly how development takes place, there are a few general principles that almost all theorists would support. 1. People develop at different rates. In your own classroom, you will have a broad range of examples of different developmental rates. Some students will be larger, better coordinated, or more mature in their thinking and social relationships. Others will be much slower to mature in these areas. Except in rare cases of very rapid or very slow development, such differences are typical and should be expected in any large group of students. 2. Development is relatively orderly. People develop abilities in a logical order. In infancy, they sit before they walk, babble before they talk, and see the world through their own eyes before they can begin to imagine how others see it. In school, they will master addition before algebra, Harry Potter before Shakespeare, and so on. But “orderly” does not necessarily mean “linear” or “predictable”—people might advance, stay the same for a period of time, or even go backward.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

3. Development takes place gradually. Very rarely do changes appear overnight. A student who cannot manipulate a pencil or answer a hypothetical question may well develop this ability, but the change is likely to take time.

The Brain and Cognitive Development If you have taken an introductory psychology class, you have read about the brain and nervous system. You probably remember that the brain has several different areas and that certain areas are involved in particular processes. For example, the brain stem handles basic functions such as heart rate, breathing, and blood pressure as well as levels of arousal such as sleeping and wakeful attention. The feathery-looking cerebellum coordinates and orchestrates balance and smooth, skilled movements—from the graceful gestures of the dancer to the everyday action of eating without stabbing yourself in the nose with a fork. The cerebellum may also play a role in higher cognitive functions such as learning. The hippocampus is critical in recalling new information and recent experiences and helps establish information in long-term memory, while the amygdala directs emotions and aggression and is important for emotional memories. Almost all sensory signals pass through the thalamus to affect brain activity. Also, the thalamus is involved in our ability to learn new information, particularly if it is verbal. The corpus callosum connects the two hemispheres of the brain to allow communication between them for complex mental processing. The frontal lobe is the area that sets humans apart from other animals by enabling us to process information for planning, remembering, making decisions, solving problems, and thinking creatively. Rather than being a single organ, the brain is a “society of experts” that collaborate to affect what we think and do (Gluck et al., 2020; Schunk, 2020). Figure 3.1 shows the various regions of the brain. Advances in brain imaging techniques have allowed scientists remarkable access to the functioning brain. For example, computerized axial tomography (CAT) scans give three-dimensional images of the brain. Positron emission tomography (PET) scans can track brain activity amid different conditions. An electroencephalograph (EEG) measures electrical patterns in the brain, and event-related potential (ERP) uses EEG data to study the brain as people perform activities such as reading or learning vocabulary words. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) shows how blood flows within the brain when children or adults do different cognitive tasks. Finally, a new approach, near-infrared optical tomography (NIR-OT), uses infrared light through the scalp to assess brain activity. Table 3.1 on the next page summarizes what each of these techniques can and cannot do. Let’s begin our look at the brain by examining its tiny components: neurons, synapses, and glial cells.

The Developing Brain: Neurons A newborn baby’s brain weighs about 1 pound, barely one-third of the weight of an adult brain. But this infant brain has billions of neurons, the specialized nerve cells that accumulate and transmit information (in the form of electrical activity) in the brain and other

Figure 3.1 Regions of the Brain Cerebrum Parietal lobe

Corpus callosum

Basal ganglia

Frontal lobe Temporal lobe Hypothalamus Pituitary gland Amygdala

Thalamus

91

Computerized axial tomography (CAT) A technique that uses X-ray technology to provide enhanced, three-dimensional images of the part of the body scanned.

Positron emission tomography (PET) A method of localizing and measuring brain activity using computer-assisted motion pictures of the brain.

Electroencephalograph (EEG) A technique that uses electrodes attached to the scalp to measure electrical patterns in the brain created by neuron movements.

Event-related potential (ERP) Measurements that assess electrical activity of the brain through the skull or scalp.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) An MRI is an imaging technique that uses a magnetic field along with radio waves and a computer to create detailed pictures of the inside of the body. A functional MRI uses an MRI to measure the tiny changes that take place in the brain during brain activity.

Near-infrared optical tomography (NIR-OT) A technique that uses an optical fiber to transmit near-infrared light through the scalp and into the brain. Some of the light is reflected back, indicating blood flow and oxygenation in the blood, which reveals brain activity.

Occipital lobe Hippocampus Pons

Cerebellum

Neurons Nerve cells that store and transfer information.

Medulla oblongata Brain stem

92 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

Table 3.1 Brain Imaging Techniques Advances in brain imaging have led to greater understanding of how the brain functions. Each technique can give some kinds of information but not others. LIMITATIONS/WHAT IT CAN’T DO

COMMON NAME

WHAT IT DOES

EXAMPLE USES

CAT Scan

Uses X-ray technology to provide enhanced, three-dimensional images of the brain.

Locating and studying tumors or lesions in the brain.

Cannot use too often because of radiation exposure; does not give detailed information about brain activity.

Shows the extent of activity in different parts of the brain. A small amount of radioactive glucose is injected into the body and carried to the brain. Greater brain activity in various areas uses more glucose and shows up as brighter colors on computerized brain maps.

Studying how the brain works and which areas are more or less involved in different cognitive activities such as reading; diagnosing brain diseases such as tumors, strokes, and dementia.

Because radioactive injections are needed, cannot do many sessions; because there is a brief lag, does not capture fast-paced neural activity; tells more about where in general the activity takes place than about when it does.

Uses electrodes attached to the scalp to measure electrical patterns in the brain created by neuron movements. No drugs or radiation are required.

Studying sleep disorders, epilepsy, language disorders, and cognitive load (Clusters 5 and 8).

Does not provide either two- or three-dimensional pictures of the brain; reflects activity for the whole brain and cannot show specifically where activity is occurring.

Provides a calculation based on EEG data that reflects the brain’s response to a stimulus or event.

Studying sensory and cognitive activity, especially language, as well as visual problems and brain disorders.

Is good at assessing speed of neural activity but not at identifying location.

Shows momentby-moment blood flow within the brain associated with neural activity when children or adults do different cognitive tasks, revealing the amount of work done in certain areas. No radiation or injections are necessary.

Studying brain processes and structures related to perception, emotion, thinking, and action; diagnosing when to use drugs to treat strokes; mapping patients’ brains before surgery.

Has few limitations and has largely replaced PET scans, but there is a brief lag between changes in brain activity and the changes in blood flow picked up by the fMRI.

Uses an optical fiber to transmit near-infrared light through the scalp and into the brain. Some of the light is reflected back, indicating blood flow and oxygenation in the blood, which reveals brain activity.

Studying brain processes and changes during particular activities, social interactions, classroom learning. Not invasive, no chemicals or radiation are used; can be mobile and used over longer periods of time.

Has few limitations, but it can detect activity only a few centimeters into the brain where the light can penetrate.

Computerized Axial Tomography

PET Scan Positron Emission Tomography

EEG Electroencephalograph

ERP Event-Related Potentials

fMRI Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

NIR-OT Near-Infrared Optical Tomography

parts of the central nervous system. Neurons are a grayish color, so they sometimes are called the gray matter of the brain. One neuron has the information-processing capacity of a small computer. This means the processing power of one 3-pound human brain is likely greater than the power of all the computers in the world! Of course, computers do many things, like calculate square roots of large numbers, much faster than human brains can

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

93

(Anderson, 2020). These incredibly important neuron cells are Figure 3.2 A Single Neuron tiny; about 30,000 could fit on the head of a pin (Sprenger, 2010). Each neuron (nerve cell) includes dendrites that bring Scientists once believed that all the neurons a person would ever in messages and an axon that sends out messages. possess are present at birth, but now we know that the produc- This is a single neuron, but each neuron is in a network tion of new neurons, neurogenesis, continues into adulthood, with many others. especially in the hippocampus region (Scalise & Felde, 2017). Axon sends messages Neurons come in many shapes and sizes depending on to other cells their functions, but they all have a similar structure. These cells send out long arm- and branch-like fibers, called axons and den- Neuron Myelin cover on the axon accelerates drites, to connect with other neuron cells. The fiber ends from transmission of different neurons don’t actually touch; there are tiny spaces impulses between them, about one-billionth of a meter in length, called synapses. Neurons share information by using electrical signals and by releasing chemicals that jump across the synapses. Axons transmit information to muscles, glands, or other neurons; dendrites receive information and transmit it to the neuron Dendrites receive messages from cells themselves. Connections between neurons via these syn- other neurons aptic transmissions become stronger with use or practice and Synapse Dendrite weaker when not used. The neural pathways reinforced by use Axon form memory traces that are the end result of learning (Scalise & In the synapse, Felde, 2017; Schunk, 2020). So the strength of these synaptic conneurotransmitters nections is dynamic—always changing as learning occurs. This carry information is called synaptic plasticity, or just plasticity, a very important between neurons concept for educators, as you will see soon. Researchers have found that physical exercise plays a critical role in maintaining Neurotransmitters a healthy, plastic brain (Doidge, 2015; Dubinsky et al., 2013). Figure 3.2 shows these components of the neuron system. At birth, each of a child’s approximately 100 to 200 billion neurons has about 2,500 synNeurogenesis apses. However, the number of fibers that reach out from the neurons and the number of synapses between the fiber ends increase during the first years of life, perhaps into adoles- The production of new neurons. cence or longer. By ages 2 to 3, each neuron has around 15,000 synapses; children this age have many more synapses than they will have as adults. In fact, they are oversupplied with neurons and synapses they will need to adapt to their environments. However, only those Synapses neurons that are used will survive, and unused neurons, up to 40%, will be “pruned.” This The tiny spaces between pruning is necessary and supports cognitive development. Researchers have found that neurons—chemical messages are some developmental disabilities are associated with a gene defect that interferes with prun- sent across these gaps. ing (Siegler et al., 2020). Two kinds of overproduction and pruning processes take place. One is called experience-expectant because synapses are overproduced in certain parts of the brain dur- Synaptic plasticity ing specific developmental periods, awaiting (expecting) stimulation. For example, during The brain’s tendency to remain the first months of life, the brain expects visual and auditory stimulation. If a normal range somewhat adaptable or flexible. of sights and sounds occurs, then the visual and auditory areas of the brain develop. But children who are born completely deaf receive no auditory stimulation, and, as a result, the auditory-processing area of their brains becomes devoted to processing visual information. Plasticity Similarly, the visual-processing area of the brain for children blind from birth becomes de- The brain’s tendency to remain somewhat adaptable or flexible. voted to auditory processing (Siegler et al., 2020). Experience-expectant overproduction and pruning processes are responsible for general development in large areas of the brain and may explain why adults have difficulty with pronunciations that are not part of their native language. For example, the distinction between the sounds of r and l is important in English but not in Japanese, so by about 10 months of age, Japanese infants lose the ability to discriminate between r and l; those neurons are pruned away. As a result, Japanese adults learning these sounds require intense instruction and practice. Just think about the cognitive advantages and extra capacities of an infant growing up learning two languages (Broderick & Blewitt, 2015; Hinton et al., 2008)!

94 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

Glial cells The white matter of the brain. These cells greatly outnumber neurons and appear to have many functions such as fighting infections, controlling blood flow and communication among neurons, and providing the myelin coating around axon fibers.

Myelination The process by which neural fibers are coated with a fatty sheath called myelin that makes message transfer more efficient.

The second kind of synaptic overproduction and pruning is called experience-dependent. Here, synaptic connections are formed based on the individual’s experiences. New synapses are formed in response to neural activity in very localized areas of the brain. Examples are learning to ride a bike or use a spreadsheet. The brain does not “expect” these behaviors, so new synapses form in response to these experiences. Again, more synapses are produced than will be kept after “pruning.” Experience-dependent processes are involved in individual learning, such as mastering unfamiliar sound pronunciations in a second language you are studying. Stimulating environments may help in the pruning process in early life (experienceexpectant period) and also may support increased synapse development in adulthood (experience-dependent period) (Broderick & Blewitt, 2015; Cook & Cook, 2014). In fact, animal studies have shown that rats raised in stimulating environments (with toys, tasks for learning, other rats, and human handling) develop and retain 25% more synapses than rats who are raised with little stimulation. Even though the research with rats might not apply directly to humans, it is clear that extreme deprivation can have negative effects on human brain development. But extra stimulation—for example, flash cards, “educational” tablets, or baby brain games—will not necessarily improve development for young children who are already getting adequate or typical amounts (Berk, 2019). So spending money on expensive toys or baby education programs probably offers more stimulation than is necessary and might be harmful. Pots and pans, blocks and books, sand and water all provide excellent, appropriate stimulation—especially if accompanied by caring conversations with parents or teachers. Look back at Figure 3.2. It appears that there is nothing between the neurons but air. Actually, that is incorrect. The spaces are filled with glial cells, the white matter of the brain. There are trillions of these glial cells; they greatly outnumber neurons. Glial cells appear to have many functions, such as fighting infections, controlling blood flow and communication among neurons, and providing the myelin coating (see Figure 3.2) around axon fibers. Myelination, the coating of axon neuron fibers with an insulating, fatty glial covering, influences thinking and learning. This process is something like coating bare electrical wires with rubber or plastic. This myelin coating makes message transmission faster and more efficient and is important for brain functioning. For example, in multiple sclerosis the myelin coating degenerates, which leads to trouble with movements, coordination, vision, and speech. Myelination happens quickly in the early years but continues gradually into adolescence, with the child’s brain doubling in volume in the first year of life and doubling again around puberty. Myelination of the areas involved in judgment and decision making, the prefrontal regions, is not complete until early adulthood—more about this later (J. R. Anderson, 2020; Gluck et al., 2020).

The Developing Brain: Cerebral Cortex Let’s move from the neuron level to the brain itself, which amazingly is almost 80% water, with the rest being fat and protein (Schunk, 2020). The outer 1/8-inch-thick covering is the cerebral cortex—the largest area of the brain. The cerebral cortex is a thin sheet of neurons that is almost 3 square feet in area for adults. To get all that area in your head, the sheet is crumpled together, with many folds and wrinkles (J. R. Anderson, 2020). In humans, this area of the brain is much larger than in lower animals. The cerebral cortex accounts for about 85% of the brain’s weight in adulthood and contains the greatest number of neurons. The cerebral cortex thus allows for the greatest human accomplishments, such as complex problem solving and language. The cortex is the last part of the brain to develop, so it is believed to be more susceptible to environmental influences than other areas of the brain. Parts of the cortex mature at different rates. The region of the cortex that controls physical motor movement matures first, then the areas that control complex senses such as vision and hearing, and last, the frontal lobe, which controls higher-order thinking processes. The temporal lobes of the cortex that play major roles in emotions, judgment, and language do not develop fully until the high school years and maybe later (Berk, 2019; Siegler et al., 2020).

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

95

Different areas of the cortex seem to have distinct functions, as shown in Figure 3.3 A View of the Cerebral Figure  3.3. Even though different functions are found in particular areas of the Cortex brain, these specialized functions are quite specific and elementary. To accomplish This is a simple representation of the more complex functions such as speaking or reading, the various areas of the cor- left side of the human brain, showing tex must communicate and work together (J. R. Anderson, 2020). the cerebral cortex. The cortex is Another aspect of brain functioning that has implications for cognitive devel- divided into different areas, or lobes, opment is lateralization, or the specialization of the two hemispheres of the brain. each having a variety of regions with We know that each half of the brain controls the opposite side of the body. Dam- different functions. A few of the major age to the right side of the brain will affect movement on the left side of the body functions are indicated here. and vice versa. In addition, certain areas of the brain affect particular behaviors. Body movement Body sensation For most of us, the left hemisphere of the brain is a major factor in language pro- and coordination Parietal cessing, and the right hemisphere handles much of our spatial-visual information lobe and emotions (nonverbal information). For some left-handed people, the relation- Frontal Visual ship might be reversed, but for most left-handers, and for females on average, lobe cortex there is less hemispheric specialization altogether. The brains of young children show more plasticity (adaptability) because they are not as specialized or lateralized as the brains of older children and adults. Young children with damage to the left side of the brain are somewhat able to overcome the damage, which allows language development to proceed. Different areas of the brain take over the funcAuditory tions of the damaged area. But in older children and adults, this compensation cortex Occipital is less likely to occur after damage to the left hemisphere (J. R. Anderson, 2020; Temporal lobe lobe Ormrod, 2020). These differences in performance by the brain’s hemispheres, however, are more relative than absolute; one hemisphere is just more efficient than the other in performing certain functions. Language is processed “differently, but simultaneously” by the left and right hemispheres (Alferink & Farmer-Dougan, 2010, p. 44). Nearly any Lateralization task, particularly the complex skills and abilities that concern teachers, requires simultane- The specialization of the two ous participation of many different areas of the brain in constant communication with each hemispheres (sides) of the brain other. For example, the left side of the brain is where grammar and syntax are understood, cortex. but the right side is better at figuring out the meaning of a story or interpreting sarcasm, irony, metaphors, or puns, so both sides of the brain have to work together when you read or make sense of literature, films, and jokes. Remember, no mental activity is exclusively the work of a single part of the brain, so there is no such thing as a “right-brained student,” unless that individual has had the left hemisphere removed—a rare and radical treatment for some forms of epilepsy (Ormrod, 2020).

Brain Development in Childhood and Adolescence The brain continues to develop throughout childhood and adolescence. In infancy, children identify patterns in their world and in the language(s) spoken by the people who care for them. Infants learn—form neural connections and networks—by exploring, acting, and observing. They are self-directed in this adventure—which is a good thing because they have so much to learn. During this time a stimulating, responsive, and safe environment is a much better “teacher” than flashcards or structured lessons because young children follow their own interests and curiosities. In the elementary school years, children’s brains continue to grow. The different parts of the brain that support various processes such as perception, memory, and emotion become more networked and connected. These interconnections enable children to reflect on their feelings and thoughts—to think about their own thinking. Children also can add to their store of knowledge and hold more information in memory at one time. At this age they are ready to learn more vocabulary and grammar in their first language and also to learn a second language. But they still have limited attention spans, so longer lessons, activities, or directions should be divided into manageable and memorable pieces (McDevitt & Ormrod, 2020). In addition, all of the ideas in Cluster  11 for developing self-regulated learning can support elementary school students as their growing brains open new possibilities for understanding and controlling their own cognitive processes.

96 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development During adolescence, changes in the brain increase individuals’ abilities to control their behavior in both low-stress and high-stress situations, to be more purposeful and organized, and to inhibit impulsive behavior (Wigfield et al., 2006). But these abilities are not fully developed until the early 20s, so adolescents may “seem” like adults, at least in low-stress situations, but their brains are not mature. They often have trouble avoiding risks and controlling impulses. This is why adolescents’ brains have been described as “high horse power, poor steering” (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development [OECD], 2007, p. 6). One explanation for this problem with avoiding risks and impulsive behaviors looks to differences in the pace of development for two key components of the brain—the limbic system and the prefrontal cortex of the brain (Casey et al., 2008). The limbic system, which is involved with emotions and reward-seeking/novelty/risk-taking/sensation-seeking behaviors, develops earlier. The prefrontal lobe takes more time to develop and is involved with judgment and decision making. As the limbic system matures, adolescents become more responsive to pleasure seeking and emotional stimulation. In fact, adolescents appear to need more intense emotional stimulation than either children or adults, so these young people are set up for taking risks and seeking thrills. Risk taking and novelty seeking can be positive factors for adolescent development as young people courageously try new ideas and behaviors—and learning is stimulated (Luna et al., 2013). But their less mature prefrontal lobe is not yet good at saying, “Whoa—that thrill is too risky!” So in emotional situations, thrill seeking wins out over caution, at least until the prefrontal lobe catches up and becomes more integrated with the limbic system toward the end of adolescence. Then risks can be evaluated in terms of long-term consequences, not immediate thrills (Casey et al., 2008; Smith et al., 2012). In addition, there are individual differences: Some adolescents are more prone than others to engage in risky behaviors. Teachers can take advantage of their adolescent students’ intensity by helping them devote their energy and passion to areas such as politics, the environment, public service, or social causes (Price, 2005) or by guiding them to explore emotional connections with characters in history or literature. Connections to family, school, community, and positive belief systems help adolescents “put the brakes” on reckless and dangerous behaviors (McDevitt & Ormrod, 2020). Other changes in the neurological system during adolescence affect sleep; teenagers need about 9 hours of sleep per night, but many students’ biological clocks are reset, making it difficult for them to fall asleep before midnight. Some experts interviewed by Sumathi Reddy (2014a) have recommended that ideally, high school should start at 9:00 or even 10:00 in the morning—sounds good to us! Yet in many school districts, high school begins by 7:30, which makes 9 hours of sleep impossible to get, so students are continually sleep deprived. Research in neuroscience shows that sleep deprivation impairs the initial formation of memories for facts, so learning suffers. This means that losing sleep to cram for tests actually interferes with learning by shutting down the very parts of the brain needed to remember what you are studying (Scalise & Felde, 2017). Classes that keep students in their seats taking notes for the full period may literally “put the students to sleep.” With no time for breakfast and little for lunch, these students’ nutritional needs are often deprived as well (Sprenger, 2005).

Putting It All Together: How the Brain Works What is your conception of the brain? Is the brain a culture-free container that holds knowledge the same way for everyone? Is the brain like a library of facts or a computer filled with information? Do you wake up in the morning, download what you need for the day, and then go merrily on your way? Is the brain like a pipe that transfers information from one person to another—a teacher to a student, for example? Kurt Fischer—a developmental psychologist and Harvard professor—offers a different view, one based on neuroscience research. Knowing is actively constructing understandings and actions. Knowledge is based on our activities, and the brain is constantly changing. Experience shapes our brains, actually altering our neurons and their connections (Fischer, 2009). See Table 3.2 for some other myths and truths about the brain.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

Table 3.2 Myths and Truths About the Brain COMMON MYTHS

TRUTH

1. You use only 10% of your brain.

1. You use all your brain. That is why strokes are so devastating. Your brain is 2% of your body weight but uses 20% of available energy.

2. Listening to Mozart will make children smarter.

2. Listening to music won’t make you smarter, but learning to play a musical instrument is associated with increased cognitive achievement.

3. Some people are more “right brained,” and others are more “left brained.”

3. It takes both sides of your brain to do most things.

4. A young child’s brain can manage to learn only one language at a time.

4. Children all over the world can and do learn two languages at once.

5. You can’t change your brain.

5. Our brains are changing all the time.

6. Damage to the brain is permanent.

6. Most people recover well from minor brain injuries.

7. Playing games like Sudoku keeps your brain from aging.

7. Playing Sudoku makes you better at playing Sudoku and similar games. Physical exercise is a better bet to prevent decline.

8. The human brain is the biggest brain.

8. Sperm whales have brains five times heavier than those of humans.

9. Alcoholic beverages kill brain cells.

9. Heavy drinking does not kill brain cells, but it can damage the nerve ends called dendrites, and this causes problems with communicating messages in the brain. This damage is mostly reversible.

10. The adolescent’s brain is the same as that of an adult.

10. Critical differences between adolescents’ and adults’ brains exist, especially in the areas of judgment and risk assessment.

11. Students learn best with their preferred learning style.

11. Teaching according to a student’s preferred style does not improve learning.

12. You can learn while you sleep.

12. Sorry, no.

Source: Based on Aamodt & Wang (2008); Fischer, (2009); Freeman (2011); Holmes (2016); Macdonald et al. (2017); Schunk (2020).

All experiences sculpt the brain—play and deliberate practice, formal and informal learning (Dubinsky et al., 2013). Earlier you encountered the term that describes the brain’s capacity for constant change in neurons, synapses, and activity—plasticity. Cultural differences in brain activity provide examples of how interactions in the world shape the brain through plasticity. For example, in one study, when Chinese speakers added and compared Arabic numbers, they showed brain activity in the motor (movement) areas of their brains, whereas English speakers performing the same tasks had activity in the language areas of their brains (Tang et al., 2006). One explanation is that Chinese children are taught arithmetic using an abacus—a calculation tool that involves movement and spatial positions. These children retain a kind of visual-motor sense of numbers when they become adults (Varma et al., 2008). There also are cultural differences in how languages affect reading. For example, when they read, native Chinese speakers activate parts of their brains associated with spatial information processing, probably because the language characters used in written Chinese are pictures. But Chinese speakers also activate these spatial areas of the brain when they read English, demonstrating that reading proficiency can be reached through different neural pathways (Hinton et al., 2008). So, thanks to plasticity, the brain is ever changing, shaped by activity, culture, and context. We build knowledge as we do things, as we manipulate objects and ideas mentally and physically. As you can imagine, educators have looked for and disagreed about instructional applications of neuroscience research. See the Point/Counterpoint on the next page.

CULTURE AND BRAIN PLASTICITY.

97

98 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Brain-Based Education Educators are hearing more and more about brain-based education. Are there clear educational implications from the neuroscience research on the brain?

Point No, the implications are not clear. Early on, John Bruer, president of the James S. McDonnell Foundation, wrote articles that were critical of the brain-based education craze (Bruer, 1997, 1999, 2002). He noted that many so-called applications of brain research begin with solid science but then move to unwarranted speculation and end in a sort of appealing folk tale about the brain and learning. In the title of his 1997 article, he called that journey “a bridge too far.” He suggested that for each claim, the educator should ask, “Where does the science end and the speculation begin?” One claim that Bruer questioned was the notion of right-brain, left-brain learning, a popular idea that has been around for more than 30 years, in spite of the fact that neuroscientists keep debunking the claims. Even though different brain areas are specialized for different tasks, this specialization occurs at very fine levels of analysis, and forming visual images involves many parts of the brain. As we will see later in this book, imagery can be a good learning strategy—but not because it enlists the “underutilized” right brain in learning (Bruer, 1999). Kurt Fischer (2009), president of the International Mind, Brain, and Education Society, has lamented that many neuromyths, like those in Table 3.2 (blatantly wrong beliefs about how the brain and body work), still are widely accepted—zombie ideas that won’t die. No teacher doubts that the brain is important in learning. As Steven Pinker (2002), professor of psychology at Harvard University, observed, does anyone really think that learning takes place somewhere else, like the pancreas? But knowing that learning affects the brain does not tell us how to teach. All learning affects the brain; “this should be obvious, but nowadays any banality about learning can be dressed up in neurospeak and treated like a great revelation of science” (Pinker, 2002, p. 86). Virtually all of the socalled best practices for brain-based education are simple restatements of good teaching based on understandings of how people learn, not how their brains work. For example, we have known for more than 100 years that learning in many short practice sessions is more effective than learning in one long cramming session. Building more dendrites through short study sessions may be one of many reasons why the strategy works, but it does not offer teachers new strategies (Alferink & Farmer-Dougan, 2010). Have things changed lately? Jeffrey Bowers says no: “There are still no examples of EN [educational neuroscience] providing new insights to teaching in the classroom, . . . no examples of EN providing new insights to remedial instruction for individuals, and . . . no evidence that EN is useful for the diagnosis of learning difficulties” (Bowers, 2016a, p. 628). Bowers also said, “There are no examples of novel and useful suggestions for teaching based on neuroscience thus far” (2016b, p. 609), and Dougherty and Robey (2018) concluded, “The idea that neuroscience can have a direct impact in the classroom is a bit farfetched” (p. 401).

Counterpoint Yes, teaching should be brain-based. In their article “Applying Cognitive Neuroscience Research to Education” in the Educational Psychologist, Tami Katzir and Juliana Paré-Blagoev (2006) concluded, “When applied correctly, brain science may serve as a vehicle for advancing the application of our understanding of learning and development. . . . Brain research can challenge common-sense views about teaching and learning by suggesting additional systems that are involved in particular tasks and activities” (p. 70). One influential commonsense view to challenge is that brain functioning is fixed early in life. In fact, when adolescents were taught about neuroplasticity and the brain’s ability to change across the life span, their academic

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

achievement and self-concept were higher compared to their peers who did not receive that message (Howard-Jones et al., 2016). There is also promising work on very early detection of brain indicators for later risks of learning problems such as dyslexia. Early detection would allow early intervention (Thomas, 2019). If we are to guard against overstating the links between brain research and education, then we should not ask whether to teach neuroscience to preservice teachers but, instead, how best to do that teaching (Dubinsky et al., 2013). Several universities, including Harvard, Cambridge, Dartmouth, the University of Texas at Arlington, University of Minnesota, University of Southern California, Beijing Normal University, Southeast University in Nanjing, and Johns Hopkins are pioneering this process. They have established training programs for educators in brain-education studies (Dubinsky et al., 2013; Fischer, 2009; Wolfe, 2010). Other educational psychologists have called for a new professional specialty—neuro-educators (Beauchamp & Beauchamp, 2013). If you want to learn more about the brain and learning, many online sources provide courses and information for educators. Two good ones are the Science of Learning course offered by Future Learn (http://www.futurelearn.com/courses/science-of-learning) and Neuroscience & the Classroom: Making Connections, offered by Annenberg Learner (http://www.learner.org/ courses/neuroscience). In his presidential address for the First Conference of the International Mind, Brain, and Education Society, Kurt Fischer (2009) also noted that the primary goal of that organization is to ground education in solid research from biology, cognitive science, development, and education while preventing myths and popular misconceptions. Fischer makes the point that we can go from understanding how the brain works to understanding cognitive processes and then to developing educational practices. But jumping directly from knowledge about the brain to educational practices probably involves too much speculation—a bridge too far. Even enthusiastic supporters believe that educational neuroscience does not claim a direct link between measures of brain functioning and teaching practices. Instead, educational neuroscience and teaching should work together to develop and test different practices (Howard-Jones et al., 2016).

Beware of Either/Or Few educators have a background in neurobiology, and most neuroscientists can’t create concrete and useful applications of research on the brain because they just don’t know enough about the specifics of teaching in kindergarten to high school classrooms (Beauchamp & Beauchamp, 2013). Insights from studies of the brain can likely produce medical treatments for educational problems, such as drugs that act on the brain to help with attention-deficit disorder. Teaching approaches for students with attention-deficit disorder can be powerful, but they grow from educational psychology research, not from medical research (Bowers, 2016a). However, to ignore what we do know about the brain would be irresponsible. Brain-based learning offers some direction for educators who want more purposeful, informed teaching. At the very least, the neuroscience research is helping us understand why effective teaching strategies, such as distributed practice, work. The person in the best position to create, invent, and apply strategies is the teacher who understands both the way the brain works and the way children learn (Scalise & Felde, 2017). For more information, see the following podcast on understanding the brain: http:// www.oecd.org/edu/ceri/understandingthebrainthebirthofalearningscience.htm.

Neuroscience, Learning, and Teaching In 2017, Richard Mayer noted that only six articles out of 9,000 in the Journal of Educational Psychology database contain the words brain or neuroscience. Why so few? As you saw in the Point/Counterpoint, there has been vigorous debate between the enthusiastic educational advocates of brain-based education and the skeptical neuroscience researchers who caution that studies of the brain do not really address major educational questions yet.

99

100 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development DOES INSTRUCTION AFFECT BRAIN DEVELOPMENT? There are differences in brain activity associated with instruction. For example, the intensive instruction and practice provided to rehabilitate stroke victims can help them regain functioning by forming new connections and using new areas of the brain (Bransford et al., 2000; McKinley, 2011). In a dramatic example of how teaching can affect brain development, K. W. Fischer (2009) describes two children who each had one brain hemisphere removed as a treatment for severe epilepsy. Nico’s right hemisphere was removed when he was 3, and his parents were told he would never have good visual-spatial skills. However, with strong and constant support and teaching, Nico grew up to be a skilled artist! Brooke’s left hemisphere was removed when he was 11. His parents were told he would lose his ability to talk. Again, with strong support, he regained enough speaking and reading abilities to finish high school and attend community college. THE BRAIN AND LEARNING TO READ. Brain-imaging research is revealing interesting differ-

ences between skilled and less-skilled readers as they learn new vocabulary. For example, one imaging study showed that less-skilled readers had trouble establishing high-quality representations of new vocabulary words in their brains, as indicated by ERP measurements of electrical activity of the brain. When they later encountered one of the new words, less-skilled readers’ brains often didn’t recognize that they had seen the word before even though they had learned it in an earlier lesson. If words you have learned previously seem unfamiliar later, you can see why it would be hard to understand what you read (Balass et al., 2010). Reading is not innate or automatic—every brain has to be taught how to read (Frey & Fisher, 2010). Reading is a complex integration of the systems in the brain that recognize sounds, written symbols, meanings, and sequences and then connect those things with what the reader already knows. This has to happen quickly and automatically (Wolf et al., 2009). What are some strategies suggested? Use multiple approaches that teach sounds, spelling, meanings, sequencing, and vocabulary through reading, writing, discussing, explaining, drawing, and modeling. Different students may learn in different ways, but all students need practice in literacy. Finally, another clear connection between the brain and classroom learning is in the area of emotions and stress. Let’s step inside a high school math classroom described by Christine Hinton and her colleagues (2008, p. 91) for an example:

EMOTIONS, LEARNING, AND THE BRAIN.

Patricia, a high school student, struggles with mathematics. The last few times she answered a mathematics question she got it wrong and felt terribly embarrassed, which formed an association between mathematics . . . and negative emotions. . . . Her teacher had just asked her to come to the blackboard to solve a problem. This caused an immediate transfer of this emotionally-charged association to the amygdala, which elicits fear. Meanwhile, a slower, cortically-driven cognitive appraisal of the situation is occurring: she remembers her difficulty completing her mathematics homework last night, notices the problem on the board contains complicated graphs, and realizes that the boy she has a crush on is watching her from a front-row seat. These various thoughts converge to a cognitive confirmation that this is a threatening situation, which reinforces her progressing fear response and disrupts her ability to concentrate on solving the mathematics problem. In Cluster 7 you will learn about how emotions can become paired with particular situations, and in Cluster 12 you will see that anxiety interferes with learning, whereas challenge, interest, and curiosity can support learning. If students feel unsafe and anxious, they are not likely to be able to focus their attention on academics. But if students are not challenged or interested, learning suffers too. Keeping the level of challenge and support “just right” is not simple for teachers. And helping students learn to regulate their own emotions and motivation is an important goal for education (see Cluster 11). Simply put, learning will be more effective “if educators help to minimize stress and fear at school, teach students

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

emotional regulation strategies, and provide a positive learning environment that is motivating to students” (Hinton et al., 2008).

Stop & Think As a teacher, you don’t want to fall for overly simplistic “brain-based” teaching slogans. But obviously, the brain and learning are intimately related—this is not a surprise. So how can you be a savvy, “neuroscientific” teacher (Murphy & Benton, 2010)?

Lessons for Teachers: General Principles What can we learn from neuroscience? One overarching idea is that teachers and students should transform the notion of learning from “using your brain” to “changing your brain”—embrace the amazing plasticity of the brain (Howard-Jones et al., 2016). Here are some general teaching implications drawn from Brown et al. (2014), Driscoll (2005), Dubinsky, et al. (2013), Murphy and Benton (2010), Sprenger (2010), Scalise and Felde (2017), and Schunk (2020). 1. Human capabilities—intelligence, communication, problem solving, and so on— emerge from each person’s unique synaptic activity overlaid on his or her genetically endowed brain anatomy; nature and nurture are in constant activity together. The brain can place some limits on learning in the form of genetic brain anomalies in neural wiring or structure, but learning can occur via alternate pathways in the brain (as Nico and Brooke demonstrate). So there are multiple ways both to teach and to learn a skill, depending on the student. 2. Many cognitive functions are differentiated; they are associated with different parts of the brain. Using a range of modalities for instruction and activities that draw on different senses may support learning—for example, using maps and songs to teach geography. Using different modalities also helps students stay focused and engaged. Assessment should be differentiated, too. 3. The brain is relatively plastic, so enriched, active environments and flexible instructional strategies are likely to support cognitive development in young children and learning in adults. The brain is constantly changing at many levels, from cells to connections to remapping skills in new areas in response to an injury—think plasticity! 4. Changing the brain takes time, so teachers must be consistent, patient, and compassionate in teaching and reteaching in different ways, as Nico’s and Brooke’s parents and teachers could tell you. Don’t overwhelm the brain with a heavy cognitive load (see Cluster 8) that presents too much too quickly. Give students ways of managing the cognitive load using graphic organizers, visuals, tables, glossaries, notes, and other “external brain” tools. 5. Some learning disorders may have a neurological basis; neurological testing can assist in diagnosing and treating these disorders as well as in evaluating the effects of various treatments. 6. Learning from real-life problems and concrete experiences helps students construct knowledge and also gives them multiple pathways for learning and retrieving information. Knowledge learned should be clearly connected with situations in which it is useful. “Inert” knowledge that is memorized but not used is quickly forgotten. Results of both research on learning and research on the brain support using problem-based learning, simulations and role plays, active discussions, graphics, and visuals. 7. Our brains are wired to be curious as we attempt to make sense of all the incoming sensory information. Harnessing this curiosity can engage and motivate students. 8. The brain seeks meaningful patterns and connections with existing networks, so teachers should tie new information to what students already understand and help them form new connections. Use both concrete and abstract examples when possible, and ask students to think of their own examples. Information that is not linked to existing

101

102 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development knowledge will be easily forgotten. Help students recognize patterns by pointing out similarities and differences and teaching them how to detect patterns. 9. Because the brain naturally looks for patterns and makes predictions about what to expect, feedback is critical because feedback is a form of evidence that helps the brain correct and improve its predictions. 10. It takes a long time and extensive practice to build and consolidate knowledge. Changing the brain (learning) benefits from elaborating, extending, and applying concepts in different situations and academic subjects over time and revisiting the applications often. Probing questions, worked-out examples, annotated and carefully labeled charts and graphs, reflection, and asking students to explain new learning to themselves and each other all help. 11. Large, general concepts should be emphasized over small, specific facts so students can build enduring, useful knowledge categories and associations that are not constantly changing. 12. Stories should be used in teaching. Stories engage many areas of the brain—memories, experiences, feelings, and beliefs. Stories also are organized and have a sequence— beginning, middle, and end—which makes them easier to remember than unrelated or unorganized information. 13. Emotions and health affect learning—positive emotions can support learning and memory, whereas negative emotions can interfere. Emotions also are related to motivation, as you will see in Cluster  12. Stress, lack of sleep, poor nutrition, and lack of exercise all can affect the brain’s ability to learn. 14. Helping students understand how activity (distributed practice, problem solving, making connections, inquiry, etc.) changes their brain and how emotions and stress affect attention and memory can be motivating, leading to greater self-efficacy and self-regulated learning. (We talk more about this in Cluster 11.) One important message to students is that they are responsible for doing what it takes to change their own brains; they have to work (and play) to learn. For the rest of the cluster, we turn from the brain and cognitive development to examine several major theories of cognitive development, the first one offered by a biologist turned psychologist, Jean Piaget.

Module 8 Summary A Definition of Development (pp. 88–91) What are the different kinds of development? Human development can be divided into physical development (changes in the body), personal development (changes in an individual’s personality), social development (changes in the way an individual relates to others), and cognitive development (changes in thinking). What are three questions about development and three general principles?  For decades, psychologists and the public have debated whether development is shaped more by nature or nurture, whether change is a continuous process or involves qualitative differences or stages, and whether there are critical times for the development of certain abilities. We know today that these simple either/or distinctions cannot capture the complexities of human development. Coactions and interactions are the rule. Theorists generally agree that people develop at different rates, that development is an orderly process, and that development takes place gradually.

The Brain and Cognitive Development (pp. 91–102) What part of the brain is associated with higher mental functions? The cortex is a crumpled sheet of neurons that serves three major functions: receiving signals (such as visual or auditory signals) from the sense organs, controlling voluntary movements, and forming connections. The part of the cortex that controls physical motor movement develops or matures first, then the areas that control complex senses such as vision and hearing, and last, the frontal lobe, which controls higher-order thinking processes.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

What is lateralization, and why is it important?  Lateralization is the specialization of the two sides, or hemispheres, of the brain. For most people, the left hemisphere is the major factor in language, and the right hemisphere is prominent in spatial and visual processing. Even though certain functions are associated with particular parts of the brain, the various parts and systems of the brain work together to learn and perform complex activities such as reading and constructing understanding. What are some implications for teachers? Recent advances in both teaching methods and discoveries in the neurosciences provide exciting information about brain activity during learning and brain activity differences among people with varying abilities and challenges and from different cultures. These findings have some basic implications for teaching, but many of the strategies offered by “brain-based” advocates are simply good teaching. Perhaps we now know more about why these strategies work.

MODULE 9

Piagetian and Information Processing Theories

Learning Objective 3.3 Explain the principles and stages presented in Piaget’s theory of cognitive development, including criticisms of his theory.

Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget was a true prodigy. In fact, in his teens, he published so many scientific papers on mollusks (marine animals such as oysters, clams, octopuses, snails, and squid) that he was offered a job as the curator of the mollusk collection at the Museum of Natural History in Geneva. He told the museum officials that he wanted to finish high school first. For a while, Piaget worked in Alfred Binet’s laboratory in Paris developing intelligence tests for children. The reasons children gave for their wrong answers on these tests fascinated him. What were they thinking? That question intrigued him for the rest of his life. He continued to write until his death at the age of 84 (Green & Piel, 2010; Miller, 2016). During his long career, Piaget devised a model describing how humans go about making sense of their world by gathering and organizing information (Piaget, 1954, 1963, 1970a, 1970b). He studied a wide range of topics including the development of scientific and mathematical reasoning, moral judgments, language, drawing, cause and effect, concepts of time and number, and memory for past events, to name just a few. Piaget really started the field of cognitive development. He is still studied today because his insights about children’s thinking ring so true and provide such a good description of the development of thinking from infancy to adulthood (Siegler et al., 2020). Put Yourself in a Child’s Place Can you be in Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, and in the United States at the same time? Is this a difficult question for you? How long did it take you to answer? According to Piaget (1954), certain ways of thinking that are quite simple for an adult, such as the Pittsburgh question in Put Yourself in a Child’s Place, are not so straightforward for a child. For example, do you remember the 9-year-old child at the beginning of the cluster who was asked if he could be Genevan? He answered, “No, that’s not possible. I’m already Swiss. I can’t also be Genevan” (Piaget, 1965/1995, p. 252). Imagine teaching this student geography. He has trouble classifying one concept (Geneva) as a subset of another

103

104 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development (Switzerland). There are other differences between adult and child thinking. Children’s concepts of time may be different from your own. They may think, for example, that they will someday catch up to a sibling in age, or they might confuse the past and the future. Let’s examine why.

Influences on Development Cognitive development is much more than the addition of new facts and ideas to an existing store of information. According to Piaget, our thinking processes change radically, though slowly, from birth to maturity because we constantly strive to make sense of the world. Piaget identified four factors—biological maturation, activity, social experiences, and equilibration—that interact to influence changes in thinking (Piaget, 1970a). Maturation is the unfolding of the biological changes that are genetically programmed. Parents and teachers have little impact on this aspect of cognitive development, except to be certain that children get the nourishment, sleep, and care they need to be healthy. With physical maturation comes the increasing ability to act on the environment and learn from it. When a young child’s coordination is reasonably developed, for example, the child can discover principles about balance by walking on a low balance beam or a curb. Thus, physical and mental activity—exploring, testing, observing, and eventually organizing information—changes our thinking processes. Our cognitive development also is influenced by social transmission, or learning from others. Without social transmission, we would need to reinvent all the knowledge already offered by our culture. The amount people can learn from social transmission varies according to their stage of cognitive development. We’ll return to a discussion of the fourth influence on thinking, equilibration, in the next section. Maturation, activity, and social transmission all work together to influence cognitive development. How do we respond to these influences?

Basic Tendencies in Thinking Organization Ongoing process of arranging information and experiences into mental systems or categories. Ordered and logical network of relations.

Adaptation Adjustment to the environment.

Schemes Mental systems or categories of perception and experience.

As a result of his early research in biology, Piaget concluded that all species inherit two basic tendencies, or “invariant functions.” The first of these tendencies is toward organization—the combining, arranging, recombining, and rearranging of behaviors and thoughts into coherent systems. The second tendency is toward adaptation, or adjusting to the environment. ORGANIZATION. People are born with a tendency to organize their thinking processes into psychological structures—systems for understanding and interacting with the world. Simple structures are continually combined and coordinated to become more sophisticated and thus more effective. Very young infants, for example, can either look at an object or grasp it when it comes in contact with their hands. They cannot coordinate looking and grasping at the same time. As they develop, however, infants organize these two separate behavioral structures into a coordinated, higher-level structure of looking at, reaching for, and grasping the object. They can, of course, still use each structure separately (Miller, 2016; Siegler et al., 2020). Piaget gave a special name to these psychological structures: schemes. In his theory, schemes are the basic building blocks of thinking. They are organized systems of actions or thoughts that allow us to mentally represent or “think about” the objects and events in our world. Schemes can be very small and specific—for example, the sucking-through-a-straw scheme or the recognizing-a-rose scheme. Or they can be larger and more general—for example, the drinking scheme or the gardening scheme.

In addition to the tendency to organize psychological structures, people also inherit the tendency to adapt to their environment. Two basic processes are involved in adaptation: assimilation and accommodation. ADAPTATION.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

Assimilation involves trying to understand something new by fitting it into what we already know—by using our existing schemes. At times, we may have to distort the new information to make it fit. For example, the first time many children see a raccoon, they call it a “kitty.” They try to match the new experience with an existing scheme for identifying animals. Accommodation occurs when we must change existing schemes to respond to a new situation. If we cannot make new data fit any existing schemes, then we must develop more appropriate structures. We adjust our thinking to fit the new information, instead of adjusting the information to fit our thinking. Children demonstrate accommodation when they add the scheme for recognizing raccoons to their other systems for identifying animals. People adapt to their increasingly complex environments by using existing schemes whenever these schemes work (assimilation) and by modifying and adding to their schemes when something new is needed (accommodation). In fact, both processes are required most of the time. Even using an established pattern such as sucking through a straw requires some accommodation if the straw is of a different size or length than the type you are used to. If you have ever tried drinking juice from box packages, you know that you have to add a new skill to your sucking-through-a-straw scheme: Don’t squeeze the box, or you will shoot juice through the straw, straight up into the air, and into your lap. Whenever new experiences are assimilated into an existing scheme, the scheme is enlarged and changed somewhat, so assimilation involves some accommodation (Siegler et al., 2020). Sometimes, neither assimilation nor accommodation is used. If people encounter something that is too unfamiliar, they might ignore it. Experience is filtered to fit the kind of thinking a person is doing at a given time. For example, if you overhear a conversation in a foreign language, you probably will not try to make sense of the exchange unless you have some knowledge of the language. According to Piaget, organizing, assimilating, and accommodating can be viewed as a kind of complex balancing act. In his theory, the actual changes in thinking take place through the process of equilibration—the act of searching for a balance. Piaget assumed that people continually test the adequacy of their thinking processes in order to achieve that balance. Briefly, the process of equilibration works like this: If we apply a particular scheme to an event or situation and the scheme works, then equilibrium exists. If the scheme does not produce a satisfying result, then disequilibrium exists, and we become uncomfortable. This motivates us to keep searching for a solution through assimilation and accommodation, and thus our thinking changes and moves ahead. Of course, the level of disequilibrium must be just right or optimal—too little and we aren’t interested in changing, too much and we may be discouraged or anxious and not change. Piaget thought that interactions with peers the same age are more likely to provoke disequilibrium than interactions with older children or adults because children will accept the ideas of older people but will question and think deeply about the ideas of children their own age (Siegler et al., 2020).

EQUILIBRATION.

Four Stages of Cognitive Development Now we turn to the actual differences that Piaget hypothesized for children as they grow. Piaget believed that all people pass through the same four stages in exactly the same order. The stages are generally associated with specific ages, but these are only general guidelines, not labels for all children of a certain age. Piaget noted that individuals may go through long periods of transition between stages and that a person could show characteristics of one stage in one situation but show traits of a higher or lower stage in other situations. Therefore, remember that knowing a student’s age is never a guarantee that you will know how the child thinks (Orlando & Machado, 1996). Table 3.3 summarizes the stages.

105

Assimilation Fitting new information into existing schemes.

Accommodation Altering existing schemes or creating new ones in response to new information.

Equilibration Search for mental balance between cognitive schemes and information from the environment.

Disequilibrium In Piaget’s theory, the “out-ofbalance” state that occurs when an individual realizes that his or her current ways of thinking are not working to solve a problem or understand a situation.

106 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

Table 3.3 Piaget’s Stages of Cognitive Development

Sensorimotor Involving the senses and motor activity.

Object permanence The understanding that objects have a separate, permanent existence.

Goal-directed actions Deliberate actions toward a goal.

STAGE

APPROXIMATE AGE

CHARACTERISTICS

Sensorimotor

0–2 years

Learns through reflexes, senses, and movements—actions on the environment. Begins to imitate others and remember events; shifts to symbolic thinking. Comes to understand that objects do not cease to exist when they are out of sight—object permanence. Moves from reflexive actions to intentional activity.

Preoperational

Begins about the time the child starts talking to about 7 years old

Develops language and begins to use symbols to represent objects. Has difficulty with past and future—thinks in the present. Can think through operations logically in one direction. Has difficulties understanding the point of view of another person.

Concrete Operational

Begins about first grade to early adolescence, around 11 years old

Can think logically about concrete (hands-on) problems. Understands conservation and organizes things into categories and in series. Can reverse thinking to mentally “undo” actions. Understands past, present, and future.

Formal Operational

Adolescence to adulthood

Can think hypothetically and deductively. Thinking becomes more scientific. Solves abstract problems in logical fashion. Can consider multiple perspectives and develops concerns about social issues, personal identity, and justice.

The earliest period is called the sensorimotor stage because the child’s thinking involves seeing, hearing, touching, tasting, moving, and so on. During this period, infants develop object permanence, the understanding that objects exist in the environment whether they perceive the objects or not. This is the beginning of the important ability to construct a mental representation. As most parents discover, before infants develop object permanence, taking something away from them is relatively easy. The trick is to distract them and remove the object while they are not looking—“out of sight, out of mind.” The older infant who searches for the ball that has rolled out of sight is indicating an understanding that objects still exist even when they are not in view (Berk, 2019). A second major accomplishment in the sensorimotor period is the beginning of logical, goal-directed actions. Think of the familiar clear plastic container baby toy with a lid and several colorful items inside that can be dumped out and replaced. A 6-month-old baby is likely to become frustrated trying to get to the toys inside. An older child who has mastered the basics of the sensorimotor stage will probably be able to deal with the toy in an orderly fashion by building a “container toy” scheme: (a) get the lid off, (b) turn the container upside down, (c) shake the container if the items jam, and (d) watch the items fall. Separate, lower-level schemes have been organized into a higher-level scheme to achieve a goal. The child is soon able to reverse this action by refilling the container. Learning to reverse actions is a basic accomplishment of the sensorimotor stage. As we will soon see, however, learning to reverse thinking—that is, learning to imagine the reverse of a sequence of actions—takes much longer.

INFANCY: THE SENSORIMOTOR STAGE.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development EARLY CHILDHOOD TO THE EARLY ELEMENTARY YEARS: THE PREOPERATIONAL

By the end of the sensorimotor stage, the child can use many action schemes. However, as long as these schemes remain tied to physical actions, they are of no use in recalling the past, keeping track of information, or planning. For this, children need what Piaget called operations, or actions that are carried out and reversed mentally rather than physically. At the preoperational stage, the child is moving toward mastery but has not yet mastered these mental operations (which means thinking is preoperational). According to Piaget, the first type of thinking that is separate from action involves making action schemes symbolic. The ability to form and use symbols—words, gestures, signs, images, and so on—is thus a major accomplishment of the preoperational period and moves children closer to mastering the mental operations of the next stage. This ability to work with symbols to represent an object that is not present, such as using the word horse or a picture of a horse or even pretending to ride a broomstick horse, is called the semiotic function. In fact, children’s earliest use of symbols is in pretending—pretending to drink from an empty cup, showing that they know what the object is for. This behavior also shows that their schemes are becoming more general and less tied to specific actions. The drinking scheme, for example, can be used in playing “tea time.” During the preoperational stage, that very important symbol system, language, also rapidly develops. Between the ages of 2 and 4, most children enlarge their vocabularies from about 200 to about 2,000 words. As the child moves through the preoperational stage, the developing ability to think about objects in symbolic form remains somewhat limited to thinking in one direction only, or using one-way logic. “Thinking backward,” or imagining how to reverse the steps in a task, is very difficult for the child. Reversible thinking is involved in many tasks that are difficult for the preoperational child, such as the conservation of matter. Conservation is the principle that the amount or number of something remains the same even if the arrangement or appearance is changed, as long as nothing is added and nothing is taken away. You know that if you tear a piece of paper into several pieces, you will still have the same amount of paper. To prove this, you know that you can reverse the process by taping the pieces back together, but a child using preoperational thinking can’t think that way. Here is a classic example of difficulty with conservation: Leah, a 5-year-old, is shown two identical glasses, both short and wide in shape. Both have exactly the same amount of colored water in them. She agrees that the amounts are “the same.” The experimenter then pours the water from one of the glasses into a taller, narrower glass and asks, “Now, does one glass have more water, or are they the same?” Leah responds that the tall glass has more water because “It goes up more here” (she points to the higher level on the taller glass). Piaget’s explanation for Leah’s answer is that she is focusing, or centering, her attention on the dimension of height. She has difficulty considering more than one aspect of the situation at a time (height and width), or decentering. Thus, children at the preoperational stage have trouble freeing themselves from their own immediate perceptions of how the world appears. This brings us to another important characteristic of the preoperational stage. Preoperational children, according to Piaget, have a tendency to be egocentric, to see the world and the experiences of others from their own viewpoint. This is one reason it is difficult for preoperational children to understand that your right hand is not on the same side as theirs when you are facing them. The concept of egocentrism, as Piaget intended it, does not mean “selfish”; it simply means children often assume everyone else shares their feelings, reactions, and perspectives. For example, the 2-year-old at the beginning of this cluster who brought his own mother to comfort a distressed friend—even though the friend’s mother was available—was simply seeing the situation through his own eyes and providing for his friend what he would want for himself. Research has shown that young children are not totally egocentric in every situation, however. Children as young as age 2 use more details when describing a situation STAGE.

107

Operations Actions an individual carries out by thinking them through instead of literally performing the actions.

Preoperational The stage before a child masters logical mental operations.

Semiotic function The ability to use symbols— language, pictures, signs, or gestures—to represent actions or objects mentally.

Reversible thinking Thinking backward, from the end to the beginning.

Conservation Principle that some characteristics of an object remain the same despite changes in its appearance.

Decentering Focusing on more than one aspect at a time.

Egocentric Assuming that others experience the world the way you do.

108 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development to a parent who was not present than they provide to the parent who experienced the situation with them. So young children do seem quite able to take the needs and different perspectives of others into account, at least in certain situations (Flavell et al., 2002). And in fairness to young children, even adults can make assumptions that others feel or think like they do—consider all the politicians who believe “the people agree with me!” The Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships give ideas for working with preoperational thinkers and for guiding families in supporting the cognitive development of their children. LATER ELEMENTARY TO THE MIDDLE SCHOOL YEARS: THE CONCRETE-OPERATIONAL

Concrete operations Mental tasks tied to concrete objects and situations.

STAGE. Piaget coined the term concrete operations to describe this stage of “hands-on” thinking. The basic characteristics of the stage are the recognition of the logical stability of the physical world, the realization that elements can be changed or transformed and still conserve many of their original characteristics, and the understanding that these changes can be reversed.

GUIDELINES Family and Community Partnerships Helping Families Care for Preoperational Children Encourage families to use concrete props and visual aids whenever possible. Examples 1. 2.

When family members use words such as part, whole, or one half, encourage them to demonstrate using objects in the house such as cutting an apple or pizza into parts. Let children add and subtract with sticks, rocks, or colored chips. This technique also is helpful for early concrete-operational students.

Make instructions relatively short—not too many steps at once. Use actions as well as words. Examples 1. 2.

When giving instructions, such as how to feed a pet, first model the process, and then ask the child to try it. Explain a game by acting out one of the parts.

Help children develop their ability to see the world from someone else’s point of view. Examples 1. 2.

Ask children to imagine “how your sister felt when you broke her toy.” Be clear about rules for sharing or using materials. Help children understand the value of the rules, and work with them to develop empathy by asking them to think about how they would like to be treated. Avoid long lectures on “sharing” or being “nice.”

Give children a great deal of hands-on practice with the skills that serve as building blocks for more complex skills such as reading comprehension or collaboration. Examples 1. 2.

Provide cut-out letters or letter magnets on the refrigerator for building words. Do activities that require measuring and simple calculations—cooking, dividing a batch of popcorn equally.

Provide a wide range of experiences in order to build a foundation for concept learning and language. Examples 1. 2.

Take trips to zoos, gardens, theaters, and concerts; encourage storytelling. Give children words to describe what they are doing, hearing, seeing, touching, tasting, and smelling.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

Look at Figure 3.4 to see examples of the different tasks given to children to assess their understanding of conservation. According to Piaget, the ability to solve conservation problems depends on having an understanding of three basic aspects of reasoning: identity, compensation, and reversibility. With a complete mastery of identity, the child knows that if nothing is added or taken away, the material remains the same. With an understanding of compensation, the child knows that an apparent change in one direction can be compensated for by a change in another direction—that is, if the glass is narrower, the liquid will rise higher in the glass. And with an understanding of reversibility, the child can mentally cancel out the change that has been made. Leah apparently knew that it was the same water (identity), but she lacked compensation and reversibility, so she was still moving toward conservation. Another important operation mastered at this stage is classification. This operation depends on a student’s ability to focus on a single characteristic of objects in a set (e.g., color) and group the objects according to that characteristic. More advanced classification at this stage involves recognizing that one class fits into another. A city can be in a particular state or province and also be in a particular country, as you probably knew when we

Figure 3.4 Some Piagetian Conservation Tasks In addition to the tasks shown here, other tasks involve the conservation of number, length, weight, and volume. These tasks are all achieved during the concrete-operational period. Suppose you start with this

(a) conservation of mass

Then you change the situation to this

Roll out clay ball B

A

Which is bigger, A or B?

A

B

(b) conservation of weight

A

B

B

B

B

A

A

B

C

B

A

Which beaker has more liquid, B or C? A

A

Break candy bar B into B pieces

Which will weigh more, A or B?

When I put the clay back into the water beakers, in which beaker will the water be higher?

Pour water in beaker A into beaker C

(d) conservation of continuous quantity

(e) conservation of number

Roll out clay ball B

Take clay ball out of water and roll out clay ball B

(c) conservation of volume A

The question you would ask a child is

B

C

A B

Which is more candy, A or B?

Source: Woolfolk, A., & Perry, N. E., Child Development (2nd ed.), © 2015 by Pearson Education, Inc. Reproduced by permission of Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved.

109

Identity Principle that an individual or object remains the same over time.

Compensation The principle that changes in one dimension can be offset by changes in another dimension.

Reversibility A characteristic of Piagetian logical operations—the ability to think through a series of steps and then mentally reverse the steps and return to the starting point; also called reversible thinking.

Classification Grouping objects into categories.

110 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

Seriation Arranging objects in sequential order according to one aspect, such as size, weight, or volume.

asked you earlier about Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA. As children apply this advanced classification to locations, they often become fascinated with “complete” addresses such as Lee Jary, 5116 Forest Hill Drive, Richmond Hill, Ontario, Canada, North America, Northern Hemisphere, Earth, Solar System, Milky Way, Universe. Classification is also related to reversibility. The ability to reverse a process mentally allows the concrete-operational student to see that there is more than one way to classify a group of objects. The student understands, for example, that shapes can be classified by color and then reclassified by size or by the number of sides. Seriation is the process of making an arrangement of items in order from large to small or vice versa. This understanding of sequential relationships permits a child to construct a logical series in which A < B < C (A is less than B is less than C) and so on. Unlike the preoperational child, the concrete-operational child can grasp the notion that B can be larger than A but still smaller than C. In any grade you teach, knowledge of concrete-operational thinking will be helpful (see Guidelines: Teaching the Concrete-Operational Child). In the early grades, the students are moving toward this logical system of thought. In the middle grades, the system is in full flower, ready to be applied and extended by your teaching. Students in high school and even adults still commonly use concrete-operational thinking, especially in areas that are new or unfamiliar to them.

GUIDELINES Teaching the Concrete-Operational Child Continue to use concrete props and visual aids, especially when dealing with sophisticated material. Examples 1. 2.

Use timelines in history and three-dimensional models in science. Use diagrams to illustrate hierarchical relationships such as branches of government and the agencies under each branch.

Continue to give students a chance to manipulate and test objects. Examples 1.

2.

Set up simple scientific experiments such as the following involving the relationship between fire and oxygen: What happens to a flame when you blow on it from a distance? (If you don’t blow it out, the flame gets larger briefly because it has more oxygen to burn.) What happens when you cover the flame with a jar? Have students make candles by dipping wicks in wax, weave cloth on a simple loom, bake bread, set type by hand, or do other craft work that illustrates the daily occupations of people in the colonial period.

Make sure presentations and readings are brief and well organized. Examples 1. 2.

Assign stories or books with short, logical clusters, moving to longer reading assignments only when students are ready. Break up a presentation, giving students an opportunity to practice the first steps before introducing the next steps.

Use familiar examples to explain more complex ideas. Examples 1.

2.

Compare students’ lives with those of characters in a story. After the class reads Island of the Blue Dolphins (the true story of a girl who grew up alone on a deserted island), ask, “Have you ever had to stay alone for a long time? How did you feel?” Teach the concept of area by having students measure two schoolrooms that are different sizes.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

111

Give opportunities to classify and group objects and ideas on increasingly complex levels. Examples 1. 2.

Give students slips of paper with individual sentences written on each slip, and ask the students to group the sentences into paragraphs. Compare the systems of the human body to other kinds of systems: the brain to a computer, the heart to a pump. Break down stories into components from the broad to the specific: author, story, characters, plot, theme, place, time.

Present problems that require logical, analytical thinking. Examples 1.

2.

Discuss open-ended questions that stimulate thinking: “Are the brain and the mind the same thing?” “How should the city deal with stray animals?” “What is the largest number?” Use sports photos or pictures of crisis situations (Red Cross helping in disasters, victims of poverty or war, older adults who need assistance) to stimulate problem-solving discussions.

With the abilities to handle operations such as conservation, classification, and seriation, the student at the concrete-operational stage has finally developed a complete and very logical system of thinking, but this thinking is still tied to physical reality and the way things actually are. The student is not yet able to reason about hypothetical, abstract problems that involve the coordination of many factors at once. Such coordination is part of Piaget’s final stage of cognitive development, formal operations. At this stage the focus of thinking can shift from what is to what might be. Situations do not have to be experienced to be imagined. You met Jamal at the beginning of this cluster. Even though he is a bright elementary school student, he could not answer the question “How would life be different if people did not have to sleep?” because he insisted, “People HAVE TO SLEEP!” In contrast, the adolescent who has mastered formal operations can consider contraryto-fact questions. In answering, the adolescent demonstrates the hallmark of formal operations— hypothetico-deductive reasoning. The formal-operational thinker can consider a hypothetical situation (people do not sleep) and reason deductively (from the general assumption to specific implications, such as longer workdays, more money spent on energy and lighting, smaller houses without bedrooms, or new entertainment industries). Formal operations also include inductive reasoning, or using specific observations to identify general principles. For example, the economist observes many specific changes in the stock market and attempts to identify general principles about economic cycles from this information. Using formal operations is a new way of reasoning that involves “thinking about thinking” or “mental operations on mental operations” (Inhelder & Piaget, 1958). For example, the child using concrete operations can categorize animals by their physical characteristics or by their habitats, but a child using formal operations can perform “second-order” operations on these category operations to infer relationships between habitat and physical characteristics—such as understanding that the physical characteristic of thick fur on animals is related to their arctic habitats (Kuhn & Franklin, 2006). Abstract formal-operational thinking is necessary for success in many advanced high school and college courses. Most math is concerned with hypothetical situations, assumptions, and givens: “Let x = 10” or “Assume x 2 + y 2 = z 2 ” or “Given two sides and an adjacent angle. . . .” Work in social studies and literature requires abstract thinking, too: “What did President Wilson mean when he called World War I the ‘war to end all wars’?” “What HIGH SCHOOL AND COLLEGE: FORMAL OPERATIONS.

Formal operations Mental tasks involving abstract thinking and coordination of several variables.

Hypothetico-deductive reasoning A formal-operations problemsolving strategy in which an individual begins by identifying all the factors that might affect a problem and then deduces and systematically evaluates specific solutions.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Reasoning (II, A1) Be able to distinguish between inductive and deductive reasoning. Explain the role that each plays in the learning of concepts.

112 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development are some metaphors for hope and despair in Shakespeare’s sonnets?” “What symbols of old age does T. S. Eliot use in The Waste Land?” “How do animals symbolize human character traits in Aesop’s fables?” Stop & Think You are packing for a long trip but want to pack light. How many different three-piece outfits (pants, shirt, jacket) will you have if you include three shirts, three pairs of pants, and three jackets (assuming, of course, that they all go together in fashion perfection)? Time yourself to see how long it takes you to arrive at the answer.

Adolescent egocentrism Assumption that everyone else shares one’s thoughts, feelings, and concerns.

The organized, scientific thinking of formal operations requires that individuals systematically generate all possibilities for a given situation. If asked the preceding Stop & Think question, a person using formal operations can identify all 27 possible combinations. (Did you get it right?) A concrete-operational thinker might name just a few combinations, using each piece of clothing only once. Another characteristic of this stage is adolescent egocentrism. Unlike egocentric young children, adolescents do not deny that other people may have different perceptions and beliefs; adolescents just become very focused on their own ideas and thoughts. This leads to what Elkind (1981) calls the sense of an imaginary audience— the feeling that everyone is watching: “Everyone noticed that I wore this shirt twice this week!” “The whole class thought my answer was dumb!” You can see that social blunders or imperfections in appearance can be devastating if “everyone is watching.” Luckily, this feeling of being “on stage” seems to peak in early adolescence by age 14 or 15, although in unfamiliar situations we all might feel our mistakes are being broadcast for everyone to see. The ability to think hypothetically, consider alternatives, identify all possible combinations, and analyze their own thinking has some interesting consequences for adolescents. Because they can think about worlds that do not exist, they often become interested in science fiction. Because they can reason from general principles to specific actions, they often are critical of people whose actions seem to contradict their principles. Adolescents can deduce the set of “best” possibilities and imagine ideal worlds (or ideal parents and teachers, for that matter). This explains why many students at this age develop interests in utopias, political causes, and social issues. Adolescents also can imagine many possible futures for themselves and may try to decide which is best. Feelings about any of these ideals can be strong. DO WE ALL REACH THE FOURTH STAGE? Most psychologists agree that there is a level of thinking more sophisticated than concrete operations, but do we all reach that level? The first three stages of Piaget’s theory are forced on most people by physical realities: Objects really are permanent. The amount of water doesn’t change when poured into another glass. Formal operations, however, are not as closely tied to the physical environment. Being able to use formal operations may be the result of practice in solving hypothetical problems and using formal scientific reasoning—abilities that are valued and taught in literate cultures in high school and college. But even in these cultures, not all high school students or adults can perform Piaget’s formal-operational tasks (Shayer, 2003). Formal thinking might develop only in areas where adolescents and adults have experience or interest or take advanced classes (Piaget, 1974; Siegler et al., 2020). So expect many students in your middle school or high school classes to have trouble thinking hypothetically, especially when they are learning something new. Students sometimes find shortcuts for dealing with problems that are beyond their grasp; they may memorize formulas or lists of steps. These systems might be helpful for passing tests, but real understanding will take place only when students can go beyond this superficial use of memorization. The Guidelines: Helping Students Use Formal Operations on the next page will help you support the development of formal operations in your students.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

GUIDELINES Helping Students Use Formal Operations Continue to use concrete-operational teaching strategies and materials. Examples 1. 2.

Use visual aids such as charts and illustrations as well as somewhat more sophisticated graphs and diagrams, especially when the material is new. Compare the experiences of characters in stories to students’ experiences.

Give students opportunities to explore many hypothetical questions. Examples 1.

2.

Have students write position papers and then exchange these papers with the opposing side to debate topical social issues such as the environment, the economy, and national health insurance. Ask students to write about their personal visions of a utopia; write a description of a universe that has no sex differences; write a description of Earth after humans are extinct.

Give students opportunities to solve problems and reason scientifically. Examples 1. 2.

Set up group discussions in which students design experiments to answer questions. Ask students to justify two different positions on animal rights with logical arguments for each position.

Whenever possible, teach broad concepts, not just facts, using materials and ideas relevant to the students’ lives (Delpit, 1995). Examples 1. 2.

When discussing the Civil War, consider racism or other issues that have divided the United States since then. When teaching about poetry, let students find lyrics from popular songs that illustrate poetic devices, and then discuss how these devices do or don’t work well to communicate the meanings and feelings the songwriters intended.

Some Limitations of Piaget’s Theory Although most psychologists agree with Piaget’s insightful descriptions of how children think, many disagree with his explanations of why thinking develops as it does. THE TROUBLE WITH STAGES. Some psychologists have questioned the existence of four separate stages of thinking even though they agree that children do go through the changes that Piaget described (Mascolo & Fischer, 2005; Miller, 2016). One problem with the stage model is the lack of consistency in children’s thinking. For example, children can conserve number (the number of blocks does not change when they are rearranged) a year or two before they can conserve weight (the weight of a ball of clay does not change when you flatten it). Why can’t they use conservation consistently in every situation? Another problem with the idea of separate stages is that the processes may be more continuous than they seem. Changes seem like discontinuous, qualitative leaps when we look across longer time periods. The 3-year-old persistently searching for a lost toy seems qualitatively different from the infant who doesn’t miss a toy or search when the toy rolls under a sofa. But if we watched a developing child very closely and observed moment-to-moment or hour-to-hour changes, we might see gradual, continuous changes. The older child knows that the toy is under the sofa because her more developed memory allows her to remember seeing it roll there, whereas the infant can’t hold on to that memory. The longer you require children to wait before searching—the longer you make them remember the object—the older they have to be to succeed (Siegler et al., 2020).

113

114 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development Change can be both continuous and discontinuous, as described by a branch of mathematics called catastrophe theory. Changes that appear sudden, like the collapse of a bridge, are preceded by many slowly developing changes such as gradual, continuous corrosion of the metal structures. Similarly, gradually developing changes in children can lead to large changes in abilities that seem abrupt (Bjorklund, 2012; Siegler et al., 2020). In fairness, we should note that in his later work, even Piaget put less emphasis on stages of cognitive development and gave more attention to how thinking changes through equilibration (Miller, 2016). UNDERESTIMATING CHILDREN’S ABILITIES. It now appears that Piaget underestimated the cog-

nitive abilities of children, particularly younger ones. The problems he gave young children may have been too difficult and the directions too confusing. His subjects may have understood more than they could demonstrate when solving these problems. For example, work by Gelman and her colleagues (Gelman, 2000; Gelman & Cordes, 2001) shows that preschool children know much more about the concept of number than Piaget thought, even if they sometimes make mistakes or get confused. As long as preschoolers work with only three or four objects at a time, they can tell that the number remains the same even if the objects are spread far apart or clumped close together. Mirjam Ebersbach (2009) demonstrated that most of the German kindergartners in her study considered all three dimensions—width, height, and length—when they estimated the volume of a wooden block (actually, how many small cubes would make bigger blocks of different sizes). In other words, we might be born with a greater store of cognitive tools than Piaget suggested. Some basic understandings or core knowledge, such as the permanence of objects or the sense of number, may be part of our evolutionary equipment, ready for use in our cognitive development (Berk, 2019; Woodward & Needham, 2009). Finally, Piaget argued that the development of cognitive operations such as conservation or abstract thinking cannot be accelerated. He believed that children had to be developmentally ready to learn. Quite a bit of research, however, has shown that with effective instruction, children can learn to perform cognitive operations such as conservation. They do not have to naturally discover these ways of thinking on their own. Knowledge and experience in a situation affect the kind of thinking that children can do (Brainerd, 2003). One final criticism of Piaget’s theory is that it overlooks the important effects of a child’s cultural and social groups. Research across different cultures, however, has generally confirmed that Piaget was accurate about the sequence of the stages in children’s thinking. The difference is that in some cultures, the age ranges for the stages varied. Western children typically move to the next stage about 2 to 3 years earlier than children in non-Western societies (Siegler et al., 2020). But careful research has shown that these differences across cultures depend on the subject or domain tested and whether the culture values and teaches knowledge in that domain. For example, children in Brazil who sell candy in the streets instead of attending school appear to fail a certain kind of Piagetian task—class inclusion. (Are there more daisies, more tulips, or more flowers in the picture?) But when the tasks are phrased within concepts they understand—selling candy— then these children perform better than Brazilian children of the same age who attend school (Saxe, 1999). When a culture or context emphasizes a particular cognitive ability, children growing up in that culture tend to acquire that ability sooner. In a study that compared Chinese first-, third-, and fifth-grade students to American students in the same grades, the Chinese students mastered a Piagetian task that involved distance, time, and speed relationships about 2 years ahead of American students, most likely because the Chinese education system puts more emphasis on math and science in the early grades (Zhou et al., 2001). Even concrete operations such as classification might develop differently in different cultures. For example, when individuals from the Kpelle people of Africa were asked to sort 20 objects, they created groups that made sense to them—a hoe with a potato, a knife with an orange. The experimenter could not get the Kpelle to change their categories; they said this way of sorting is how a wise man would do it. Finally, the experimenter asked in desperation, “Well, how would a fool do it?” They promptly created the four neat classification piles the experimenter had expected—food, tools, and so on (Rogoff & Morelli, 1989).

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT AND CULTURE.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

115

Information Processing, Neo-Piagetian, and Neuroscience Views of Cognitive Development As you will see in Cluster 8, there are explanations for why children have trouble with conservation and other Piagetian tasks. These explanations focus on the development of information-processing skills, such as attention, memory capacity, and learning strategies. As children mature and their brains develop, they are better able to focus their attention, process information more quickly, hold more information in memory, and use thinking strategies more easily and flexibly (Berk, 2019; Siegler, 2004). One critical development is improvement in executive functioning. Executive functioning involves all those cognitive processes that we use to organize, coordinate, and perform goal-directed, intentional actions such as focusing attention, inhibiting impulsive responses, making and changing plans, organizing, and using memory to hold and manipulate information (Best & Miller, 2010; Morgan et al., 2019). As children develop more sophisticated and effective executive functioning skills, they are active in advancing their own development: They are constructing, organizing, and improving their own knowledge and strategies (Siegler et al., 2020). For example, one classic Piagetian task is to show children 10 daisies and 2 roses and then ask whether they see more daisies or more flowers. Young children see more daisies and jump to the answer “daisies.” As they mature, children are better at resisting (inhibiting) that first response based on appearances and can answer based on the fact that both daisies and roses are flowers. But even adults have to take a fraction of a second to resist the obvious, so inhibiting impulsive responses is important for developing complex knowledge throughout life (Borst et al., 2013). Some developmental psychologists have formulated neo-Piagetian theories that retain Piaget’s insights about children’s construction of knowledge and the general trends in children’s thinking but add findings from cognitive neuroscience and information processing theories about the role of attention, memory, and strategies (Bjorklund, 2018). Perhaps the best-known neo-Piagetian theory was developed by Robbie Case (1992, 1998). He devised an explanation of cognitive development suggesting that children develop in stages within specific domains such as numerical concepts, spatial concepts, social tasks, storytelling, reasoning about physical objects, and motor development. As children practice using the schemes in a particular domain (e.g., using counting schemes in the number concept area), accomplishing the schemes requires less attention and working memory space. The schemes become more automatic because the child does not have to “think so hard.” This frees up mental resources and memory to do more, so the child can combine simple schemes into more complex ones and invent new schemes when needed—assimilation and accommodation in action (Siegler et al., 2020). Kurt Fischer (2009) connected cognitive development in different domains to research on the brain. Even though children may follow different pathways as they master skills in speaking, reading, and mathematics, their growth patterns show a similar series of spurts, and they go through predictable levels of development. When learning a new skill, children move through three tiers—from actions to representations to abstractions. Within each tier, the pattern moves from accomplishing a single action to mapping or coordinating two actions together, such as coordinating addition and multiplication in math, to creating whole systems of understanding. At the level of abstractions, children finally move to constructing explanatory principles. This might remind you of sensorimotor, concrete operations, and formal operations in Piaget’s theory. For each skill level, the brain reorganizes itself, too. In this process, skills develop sooner at each level with the chance to practice and with quality support. Support and practice are keys in another explanation of cognitive development we discuss next—Vygotsky’s theory.

Module 9 Summary Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development (pp. 103–115) What are the main influences on cognitive development? Piaget’s theory of cognitive development is based on the assumption that people try to make sense of the world and actively create knowledge through direct experiences with objects, people, and ideas. Maturation, activity, social

Executive functioning All those processes that we use to organize, coordinate, and perform goal-directed, intentional actions, including focusing attention, inhibiting impulsive responses, making and changing plans, and using memory to hold and manipulate information.

Neo-Piagetian theories More recent theories that integrate findings about attention, memory, and strategy use with Piaget’s insights about children’s thinking and the construction of knowledge.

116 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development transmission, and the need for equilibrium all influence cognitive development. In response to these influences, thinking processes and knowledge develop through changes in the organization of thought (the development of schemes) and through adaptation—including the complementary processes of assimilation (incorporating into existing schemes) and accommodation (changing existing schemes). What is a scheme? Schemes are the basic building blocks of thinking. They are organized systems of actions or thought that allow us to mentally represent or “think about” the objects and events in our world. Schemes may be very small and specific (grasping, recognizing a square), or they may be larger and more general (using a map in a new city). People adapt to their environment as they increase and organize their schemes. As children move from sensorimotor to formal-operational thinking, what are the major changes? Piaget believed that young people pass through four stages as they develop: sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete-operational, and formal-operational. In the sensorimotor stage, infants explore the world through their senses and motor activity, and they work toward mastering object permanence and performing goal-directed activities. In the preoperational stage, symbolic thinking and logical operations begin. Children in the stage of concrete operations can think logically about tangible situations and can demonstrate conservation, reversibility, classification, and seriation. The ability to perform hypothetico-deductive reasoning, coordinate a set of variables, and imagine other worlds marks the stage of formal operations. How do neo-Piagetian and information processing views explain changes in children’s thinking over time?  Information processing theories focus on attention, memory capacity, learning strategies, and other processing skills to explain how children develop rules and strategies for making sense of the world and solving problems. Neo-Piagetian approaches also look at attention, memory, and strategies and at how thinking develops in different domains such as numbers or spatial relations. Research in neuroscience suggests that when learning a new skill, children move through three tiers—from actions to representations to abstractions. Within each tier, the pattern involves moving from accomplishing a single action to mapping or coordinating two actions together, such as coordinating addition and multiplication in math, to creating whole systems of understanding. What are some limitations of Piaget’s theory? Piaget’s theory has been criticized because children and adults often think in ways that are inconsistent with the notion of invariant stages. It also appears that Piaget underestimated children’s cognitive abilities; he insisted that children could not be taught the operations of the next stage but had to develop them on their own. Alternative explanations place greater emphasis on children’s developing information processing skills and the ways teachers can enhance their development. Piaget’s work is also criticized for overlooking cultural factors in child development.

MODULE 10

Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Perspective and the Implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories for Teachers

Learning Objective 3.4 Explain the principles presented in Vygotsky’s theory of development, including criticisms of his theory. Learning Objective 3.5 Discuss implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky ‘s theories for teaching.

Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Perspective Psychologists today recognize that culture shapes cognitive development by determining what and how a child will learn about the world—the content and processes of thinking. For example, cultures that prize cooperation and sharing teach these abilities early, whereas cultures that encourage competition nurture competitive skills in their children.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

The stages observed by Piaget are not necessarily “natural” for all children because to some extent, the stages reflect the expectations and activities of Western cultures, as the Kpelle people described earlier have taught us (Kozulin, 2003; Kozulin et al., 2003; Rogoff, 2003). A major spokesperson for this sociocultural theory (also called sociohistoric theory) was a Russian Jewish psychologist who was born the same year as Piaget—1896. Lev Semenovich Vygotsky was only 37 when he died of tuberculosis, but during his brief life he produced more than 100 books and articles. Some of the translations are now available (e.g., Vygotsky, 1978, 1986, 1987a, 1987b, 1987c, 1993, 1997). Vygotsky began studying learning and development to improve his own teaching. He went on to write about language and thought, the psychology of art, learning and development, and educating students with special needs. His work was banned in Russia for many years because he referenced Western psychologists in his writings. But in the past 50-plus years, with the rediscovery of his work, Vygotsky’s ideas have become major influences in psychology and education and have provided alternatives to many of Piaget’s theories (Gredler, 2012; Moll, 2014; Van Der Veer, 2007; Wink & Putney, 2002). Vygotsky believed that human activities take place in cultural settings and that they cannot be understood apart from these settings. Instead of being little scientists constructing understandings on their own, as Piaget described, Vygotsky believed that children construct the skills needed for living in their particular culture with the support of other people eager to help them learn. So one of Vygotsky’s key ideas was that our specific mental structures and processes can be traced to our interactions with others. These social interactions are more than simple influences on cognitive development—they actually create our cognitive structures and thinking processes. In fact, “Vygotsky conceptualized development as the transformation of socially shared activities into internalized processes” (John-Steiner & Mahn, 1996, p. 192). We develop by internalizing our culture. We will examine three themes in Vygotsky’s writings that explain how social processes form learning and thinking: the social sources of individual thinking; the role of cultural tools in learning and development, especially the tool of language; and the zone of proximal development (Berger, 2018; Gredler, 2012; Siegler et al., 2020).

Sociocultural theory Emphasizes role in development of cooperative dialogues between children and more knowledgeable members of society. Children learn the culture of their community (ways of thinking and behaving) through these interactions.

The Social Sources of Individual Thinking Vygotsky assumed that: Every function in a child’s cultural development appears twice: first, on the social level and later on the individual level; first between people (interpsychological) and then inside the child (intrapsychological). This applies equally to voluntary attention, to logical memory, and to the formation of concepts. All the higher functions originate as actual relations between human individuals. (Vygotsky, 1978, p. 57; our emphasis) In other words, higher mental processes, such as directing your own attention and thinking through problems, first are co-constructed during shared activities between the child and another, more skilled person such as a parent, older sibling, or teacher. Then these co-constructed processes are internalized by the child and become part of that child’s cognitive development (Gredler, 2009a, 2009b; Mercer, 2013). For example, children first use language in activities with others to try to regulate their own behavior (“No nap!” or “I wanna cookie.”). Later, however, the children can regulate their own behavior using private speech (“careful—don’t spill”), as you will see in a later section. So, for Vygotsky, social interaction was more than influence: It was the origin of higher mental processes such as problem solving. Other people guiding the child become mentors in an apprenticeship of thinking (Vygotsky, 2012). Consider this example: A six-year-old has lost a toy and asks her father for help. The father asks her where she last saw the toy; the child says “I can’t remember.” He asks a series of

117

Co-constructed process A social process in which people interact and negotiate (usually verbally) to create an understanding or to solve a problem. The final product is shaped by all participants.

118 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development questions—did you have it in your room? Outside? Next door? To each question, the child answers, “no.” When he says “in the car?” she says “I think so” and goes to retrieve the toy. (Tharp & Gallimore, 1988, p. 14) Who remembered? The answer is really neither the father nor the daughter but the two together. The remembering and problem solving were co-constructed—between people—in the interaction. But the child (and the father) may have internalized strategies to use the next time something is lost. At some point, the child will be able to function independently to solve this kind of problem. So, like the strategy for finding the toy, higher functions appear first between a child and a “teacher” before they exist within the individual child (Kozulin, 2003; Kozulin et al., 2003; Miller, 2016). Here is another example of the social sources of individual thinking. Richard Anderson and his colleagues (2001) studied how fourth-graders in small-group classroom discussions appropriate (take for themselves and use) argument stratagems that occur in the discussions. An argument stratagem is a particular form such as “I think [POSITION] because [REASON],” where the student fills in the position and the reason. For example, a student might say, “I think that the wolves should be left alone because they are not hurting anyone.” Another strategy form is “If [ACTION], then [BAD CONSEQUENCE],” as in “If they don’t trap the wolves, then the wolves will eat the cows.” Other forms manage participation, for example, “What do you think, [NAME]?” or “Let [NAME] talk.” Anderson’s research identified 13 forms of talk and argument that helped to manage the discussion, get everyone to participate, present and defend positions, and handle confusion. The use of these different forms of talking and thinking snowballed: After a useful argument was employed by one student, it spread to other students, and the argument stratagem appeared more and more in the discussions. Open discussions—students asking and answering each other’s questions—were better than teacher-dominated discussion for the development of these argument forms. Over time, these ways of presenting, attacking, and defending positions could be internalized as mental reasoning and decision making for the individual students. Both Piaget and Vygotsky emphasized the importance of social interactions in cognitive development, but Piaget saw a different role for interaction: He believed that interaction encourages development by creating disequilibrium—that is, cognitive conflict motivated change. As we saw earlier, Piaget believed that the most helpful interactions are those with peers because peers are on an equal basis and can challenge each other’s thinking. Vygotsky, on the other hand, suggested that children’s cognitive development is fostered by interactions with people who are more capable or advanced in their thinking—people such as parents and teachers. Of course, students can learn from both adults and peers, and today, computers can play a role in supporting communication across distances or in different languages.

Cultural Tools and Cognitive Development Cultural tools The real tools (computers, scales, etc.) and symbol systems (numbers, language, graphs) that allow people in a society to communicate, think, solve problems, and create knowledge.

Vygotsky believed that cultural tools, including technical tools (e.g., printing presses, plows, rulers, abacuses, graph paper—today, we would add mobile devices, computers, the Internet, real-time translators for mobile devices and chats, search engines, digital organizers and calendars, assistive technologies for students with learning challenges, etc.) and psychological tools (signs and symbol systems such as numbers and mathematical systems, Braille and sign language, maps, works of art, codes, and language) play very important roles in cognitive development. For example, as long as the culture provides only Roman numerals for representing quantity, certain ways of thinking mathematically—from long division to calculus—are difficult or impossible. But if a number system has a zero, fractions, positive and negative values, and an infinite quantity of numbers, then much more is possible. The number system is a psychological tool that supports learning and cognitive development—it changes the thinking process.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

This symbol system is passed from adult to child and from child to child through formal and informal interactions and teachings. TECHNICAL TOOLS IN A DIGITAL AGE. The use of technical tools such as calculators and spell checkers has been somewhat controversial in education. Technology is increasingly “checking up” on us. We rely on the spell checker in our word-processing program to protect us from embarrassment. But we also read student papers or text messages with spelling replacements that must have come from decisions made by the wordprocessing program—without a “sense check” by the writer. Is student learning harmed or helped by these technology supports? Just because students learned mathematics in the past with paper-and-pencil procedures does not mean that this is the best way to learn. In fact, the research on calculators has found that rather than eroding basic skills, calculator use has positive effects on students’ problem-solving skills and attitudes toward math (Ellington, 2003, 2013; Waits & Demana, 2000). There is a catch, however: For simple math problems, it probably is better to attempt recalling or calculating the answer first before turning to a calculator. Math fact learning and fluency in arithmetic are supported when students self-generate answers before resorting to calculators (Mao et al., 2017; Pyke & LeFevre, 2011).

Vygotsky believed that all higher-order mental processes such as reasoning and problem solving are mediated by (accomplished through and with the help of) psychological tools. These tools allow children to transform their thinking by enabling them to gain greater and greater mastery of their own cognitive processes; they advance their own development as they use the tools. Vygotsky believed that the essence of cognitive development is mastering the use of psychological tools such as language to accomplish the kind of advanced thinking and problem solving that could not be accomplished without those tools (Gredler, 2012). The process goes something like this: As children engage in activities with adults or more capable peers, they exchange ideas and ways of thinking about or representing concepts—drawing maps, for example, as a way to represent spaces and places. Children internalize these co-created ideas. Children’s knowledge, ideas, attitudes, and values develop through appropriating or “taking for themselves” the ways of acting and thinking provided by both their culture and other members of their group (Wertsch, 2007). In this exchange of signs and symbols and explanations, children begin to develop a “cultural tool kit” to make sense of and learn about their world (Wertsch, 1991). The kit is filled with technical tools, such as graphing calculators or rulers, directed toward the external world and psychological tools for acting mentally, such as concepts, problem-solving strategies, and (as we saw earlier) argument strategems. Children do not just receive the tools, however. They transform the tools as they construct their own representations, symbols, patterns, and understandings. These understandings gradually change as the children continue to engage in social activities and try to make sense of their world (Siegler et al., 2020). In Vygotsky’s theory, language is the most important symbol system in the tool kit and is the one that helps fill the kit with other tools.

PSYCHOLOGICAL TOOLS.

The Roles of Language and Private Speech Language is critical for cognitive development because it provides a way to express ideas and ask questions, the categories and concepts for thinking, and the links between the past and the future. Language frees us from the immediate situation so we can think about what was and what might be (Mercer, 2013). Vygotsky thought that: the specifically human capacity for language enables children to provide for auxiliary tools in the solution of difficult tasks, to overcome impulsive action, to plan a solution to a problem prior to its execution, and to master their own behavior. (Vygotsky, 1978, p. 28)

119

120 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development Vygotsky placed more emphasis than Piaget on the roles of learning and language in cognitive development. Vygotsky also believed that language in the form of private speech (talking to yourself) guides cognitive development.

Collective monologue Form of speech in which children in a group talk but do not really interact or communicate.

Private speech Children’s self-talk, which guides their thinking and actions. Eventually, these verbalizations are internalized as silent inner speech.

PRIVATE SPEECH: VYGOTSKY’S AND PIAGET’S VIEWS COMPARED. If you have spent much time around young children, you know that they often talk to themselves as they play. This can happen when the child is alone or, even more often, in a group of children—each child talks enthusiastically, without any real interaction or conversation. Piaget called this the collective monologue, and he labeled all of the children’s self-directed talk “egocentric speech.” He assumed that this egocentric speech is another indication that young children can’t see the world through the eyes of others, so they chat away without taking into account the needs or interests of their listeners. As they mature, and especially as they have disagreements with peers, Piaget believed children develop socialized speech. They learn to listen and exchange (or argue) ideas. Vygotsky had very different ideas about young children’s private speech. He suggested that rather than being a sign of cognitive immaturity, these mutterings play an important role in cognitive development because they move children in stages toward self-regulation: the ability to plan, monitor, and guide your own thinking and problem solving. First the child’s behavior is regulated by others using language and other signs such as gestures. For example, the parent says, “No!” when the child reaches toward a candle flame. Next, the child learns to regulate the behavior of others using the same language tools. The child says, “No!” to another child who is trying to take away a toy, often even imitating the parent’s voice tone. The child also begins to use private speech to regulate her own behavior, saying, “No” quietly to herself as she is tempted to touch the flame. Finally, the child learns to regulate her own behavior by using silent inner speech (Berk, 2019; Siegler et al., 2020). For example, in any preschool room you might hear 4- or 5-year-olds saying, “No, it won’t fit. Try it here. Turn. Turn. Maybe this one!” while they do puzzles. Around the age of 7, children’s self-directed speech goes underground, changing from spoken to whispered speech and then to silent lip movements. Finally, the children just “think” the guiding words. The use of private speech peaks at around age 9 and then decreases, although one study found that some students from ages 11 to 17 still spontaneously muttered to themselves during problem solving (McCafferty, 2004; Winsler & Naglieri, 2003). Vygotsky called this inner speech “an internal plane of verbal thinking” 1987c, p. 279)—a critical accomplishment on the road to higher-order thinking. This series of steps from spoken words to silent inner speech is another example of how higher mental functions first appear between people as they communicate and regulate each other’s behavior and then emerge again within the individual as cognitive processes. Through this fundamental process, the child is using language to accomplish important cognitive activities such as directing attention, solving problems, planning, forming concepts, and gaining self-control. Research supports Vygotsky’s ideas (Berk & Spuhl, 1995; Emerson & Miyake, 2003). Children and adults tend to use more private speech when they are confused, having difficulties, or making mistakes. Have you ever thought to yourself something like, “Let’s see, the first step is” or “Where did I use my glasses last?” or “If I read to the end of this page, then I can . . .”? You were using inner speech to remind, cue, encourage, or guide yourself. This internal verbal thinking is not stable until about age 12, so children in elementary school may need to continue talking through problems and explaining their reasoning in order to develop their abilities to control their thinking (Gredler, 2012). Because private speech helps students regulate their thinking, it makes sense to allow, and even encourage, students to use private speech in school. Teachers’ insisting on total silence when young students are working on difficult problems may make the work even harder for the students. Take note when muttering increases in your class—this could be a sign that students need help. Table 3.4 contrasts Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s theories of private speech. We should note that Piaget accepted many of Vygotsky’s arguments and came to agree that language could be used in both egocentric and problem-solving ways (Piaget, 1962).

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

121

Table 3.4 Differences Between Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories of Egocentric, or Private,

Speech

PIAGET

VYGOTSKY

Meaning and Purpose

Represents an inability to take the perspective of another and engage in reciprocal communication.

Represents externalized thought; its function is to communicate with the self for the purpose of self-guidance and self-direction.

Course of Development

Declines with age.

Increases at younger ages and then gradually loses its audible quality to become internal verbal thought.

Relationship to Social Speech

Negative; the least socially and cognitively mature children use more egocentric speech.

Positive; private speech develops out of social interaction with others.

Relationship to Environmental Contexts

No relationship.

Increases with task difficulty. Private speech serves a helpful self-guiding function in situations where more cognitive effort is needed to reach a solution.

Source: From “Development of Private Speech among Low-Income Appalachian Children,” by L. E. Berk and R. A. Garvin, 1984, Developmental Psychology, 20, p. 272. Copyright © 1984 by the American Psychological Association. Adapted with permission.

The Zone of Proximal Development According to Vygotsky, at any given point in development, a child is on the verge of solving certain problems—“processes that have not matured at the time but are in a period of maturation” (Vygotsky, 1998, p. 201). The child just needs some structure, demonstrations, clues, reminders, help with remembering details or steps, encouragement to keep trying, and so on. Some problems, of course, are beyond the child’s capabilities, even if every step is explained clearly. The zone of proximal development (ZPD) is the area between what the child already knows and what the child isn’t ready to understand or learn yet. The ZPD is the level of performance that the child could achieve with adult guidance or by working with “a more fully developed child” (p. 202). It is a dynamic and changing space as student and teacher interact and understandings are exchanged. This is the area where instruction can succeed. Kathleen Berger (2018, p. 52) called this area the “magic middle”—somewhere between “the boring and the impossible.” As you probably can guess, this zone varies from time to time and student to student. In some situations, a student may only move a bit, no matter how much support is provided, but another student can soar and benefit from more and more support. PRIVATE SPEECH AND THE ZONE. We can see how Vygotsky’s beliefs about the role of private speech in cognitive development fit with the notion of the ZPD. Vygotsky once noted that thoughts have movement—they unfold. But what happens when the unfolding gets stuck? Often, an adult uses verbal prompts and structuring to help a child get unstuck—to solve a problem or accomplish a task (Hermkes et al., 2018). We will see later that this type of support has been called scaffolding. This support can be gradually reduced as the child takes over the guidance, perhaps first by giving the prompts as private speech and finally as inner speech. As an example, think of the young girl described earlier who had lost her toy. Let’s move forward several years in her life and listen to her thoughts as an older student when she realizes that a schoolbook is missing. They might sound something like this:

“Where’s my math book? Used it in class. Thought I put it in my book bag after class. Dropped my bag on the bus. That dope Larry kicked my stuff, so maybe. . . .” The girl can now systematically search for ideas about the lost book without help from anyone else.

Zone of proximal development (ZPD) Developmental phase at which a child can master a task if given appropriate help and support from a more capable person.

122 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development Piaget defined development as the active construction of knowledge and learning as the passive formation of associations (Siegler, 2000). He was interested in knowledge construction and believed that cognitive development has to come before learning—the child has to be cognitively “ready” to learn. Children can memorize, for example, that Geneva is in Switzerland but still insist that they cannot be Genevan and Swiss at the same time. True understanding will take place only when the child has developed the operation of class inclusion—that one category can be included within another. But as we saw earlier, research has not supported Piaget’s position on the need for cognitive development to precede learning (Brainerd, 2003). In contrast, Vygotsky believed that learning is an active process that does not have to wait for readiness. He saw learning as a tool in development: Learning pulls development up to higher levels, and social interaction is a key in learning. In other words, what develops next is what is affected by learning (Bodrova & Leong, 2012; Gredler, 2012). This means that other people, including teachers, play significant roles in cognitive development. It does not mean that Vygotsky believed memorization is learning. When teachers try to directly communicate their understandings, the result can be a “meaningless acquisition of words” and “mere verbalization” (1987b, p. 356) that actually hides an understanding vacuum (Gredler, 2012). In Vygotsky’s words, the teacher “explains, informs, inquires, corrects, and forces the child to explain” (p. 216). We can sum up (and maybe oversimplify) the contrast between Piaget and Vygotsky on learning by saying that Piaget favored discovery learning and Vygotsky advocated guided discovery.

THE ROLE OF LEARNING AND DEVELOPMENT.

Limitations of Vygotsky’s Theory Vygotsky’s theory added important considerations by highlighting the roles of culture and social processes in cognitive development, but he may have gone too far. As you have seen in this cluster, we may be born with a greater store of cognitive tools than either Piaget or Vygotsky suggested. Some basic understandings, such as the idea that adding increases quantity, might be part of our biological predispositions, ready for use to guide our cognitive development. Young children appear to figure out much about the world before they have the chance to learn from either their culture or their teachers (Schunk, 2020; Woodward & Needham, 2009). The major limitation of Vygotsky’s theory, however, is that it consists mostly of general ideas: Vygotsky died before he could expand and elaborate on his ideas and pursue his research. His students continued to investigate his ideas, but much of that work was suppressed by Stalin’s regime until the 1950s and 1960s (Gredler, 2005, 2009b; Kozulin, 2003; Kozulin et al., 2003). A final limitation might be that Vygotsky did not have time to detail the applications of his theories for teaching even though he was very interested in instruction. As a result, most of the applications described today have been created by others—and we don’t even know whether Vygotsky would agree with them. It is clear that some of his concepts, like ZPD, have been misrepresented at times (Gredler, 2012).

Implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories for Teachers Piaget did not make specific educational recommendations, and Vygotsky did not have enough time to develop a complete set of applications. But we can glean some guidance from both men.

Piaget: What Can We Learn? Even though Piaget’s theory is not the overarching explanation of cognitive development anymore, his findings are still relevant in part “because he studied universal aspects of cognitive development, mainly children’s understanding of physics—from object permanence, to conservation, to time and space” (Bjorklund, 2018, p. 2298). Piaget believed that the main goal of education should be to help children learn how to learn and that education should

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

123

“form not furnish” the minds of students (Piaget, 1969, p. 70). Piaget has taught us that we can learn a great deal about how children think by listening carefully and by paying close attention to their ways of solving problems. If we understand children’s thinking, we will be better able to match teaching methods to children’s current knowledge and abilities; in other words, we will be better able to differentiate instruction. Even though Piaget did not design programs of education based on his ideas, his influence on current educational practice is huge. For example, the National Association for the Education of Young Children has guidelines for developmentally appropriate practice (DAP) that incorporate Piaget’s findings (Bredekamp, 2017; Copple & Bredekamp, 2008; Gestwicki, 2017). The students in any class will vary greatly in both their level of cognitive development and their academic knowledge. As a teacher, how can you determine whether students are having trouble because they lack the necessary thinking abilities or because they simply have not learned the basic facts? To do this, Case (1985) suggests you observe your students carefully as they try to solve the problems you have presented. What kind of logic do they use? Do they focus on only one aspect of the situation? Are they fooled by appearances? Do they suggest solutions systematically or by guessing and forgetting what they have already tried? Ask your students how they tried to solve the problem. Listen to their strategies. What kind of thinking is behind repeated mistakes or problems? Students are the best sources of information about their own thinking. An important implication of Piaget’s theory for teaching is what J. Hunt years ago (1961) called “the problem of the match.” Students must be neither bored by work that is too simple nor left behind by teaching they cannot understand. According to Hunt, disequilibrium must be kept “just right” to encourage growth. Setting up situations that lead to unexpected results can help create an appropriate level of disequilibrium. When students experience some conflict between what they think should happen (a piece of wood should sink because it is big) and what actually happens (it floats!), they may rethink the situation, and new knowledge may develop. Many materials and lessons can be understood at several levels and can be “just right” for a range of cognitive abilities. Classics such as Alice in Wonderland, myths, and fairy tales can be enjoyed at both concrete and symbolic levels.

UNDERSTANDING AND BUILDING ON STUDENTS’ THINKING.

Piaget’s fundamental insight was that individuals construct their own understanding; learning is a constructive process. At every level of cognitive development, you will also want to see that students are actively engaged in the learning process. In Piaget’s words: ACTIVITY AND CONSTRUCTING KNOWLEDGE.

Knowledge is not a copy of reality. To know an object, to know an event, is not simply to look at it and make a mental copy or image of it. To know an object is to act on it. To know is to modify, to transform the object, and to understand the process of this transformation, and as a consequence to understand the way the object is constructed. (Piaget, 1964, p. 8) For example, research in teaching mathematics indicates that students from kindergarten to college remember basic facts better when they have learned these facts using manipulative materials such as counting sticks, pattern blocks, fraction strips, or blocks versus using abstract symbols only (Carbonneau et al., 2013). Beware, however, of assuming that you can just give students manipulatives to work with and they will learn automatically. In one study, middle school students learned more science when teachers accompanied manipulatives with in-depth guidance about how to use them. Research in other subject areas has found similar results (Hushman & Marley, 2015; Marley & Carbonneau, 2014). This active experience, even at the earliest school levels, should not be limited to the physical manipulation of objects but should also include mental manipulation of ideas that arise out of class projects or experiments (Gredler, 2005, 2012). For example, after a social studies lesson on different jobs, a primary-grade teacher might show students a picture of a woman and ask, “What could this person be?” After answers such

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Implications of Piaget’s Theory (I, A2) The music, physical education, and art teachers in a rural preKto-8 school district work with students who characterize several of Piaget’s stages. How should these three teachers adjust their teaching from level to level over the course of a week?

124 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development as “teacher,” “doctor,” “administrative assistant,” “lawyer,” “sales representative,” and so on, the teacher could suggest, “How about a daughter?” Answers such as “sister,” “mother,” “aunt,” and “granddaughter” may follow. This should help the children switch dimensions in their classifications and center on another aspect of the situation. Next, the teacher might suggest, “American,” “jogger,” or “poet.” With older children, hierarchical classification might be involved: It is a picture of a woman, who is a human being; a human being is a primate, which is a mammal, which is an animal, which is a life form. All students need to interact with teachers and peers in order to test their thinking, to be challenged, to receive feedback, and to watch how others work out problems. Disequilibrium is often set in motion quite naturally when the teacher or another student suggests a new way of thinking about something. As a general rule, students should act on, manipulate, observe, and then talk and/or write about (to the teacher and each other) what they have experienced. Concrete experiences provide the raw materials for thinking. Communicating with others makes students use, test, and sometimes change their thinking strategies.

Vygotsky: What Can We Learn?

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Implications of Vygotsky’s Theory (I, A2) Make a list of scaffolding techniques that would be appropriate with different instructional levels and content areas. Think of scaffolding techniques that others used when you learned things outside of school (e.g., sports, hobbies).

Scaffolding Support for learning and problem solving. The support could be clues, reminders, encouragement, breaking the problem down into steps, providing an example, or anything else that allows the student to grow in independence as a learner. Teachers and students make meaningful connections between what the teacher knows and what the students know and need in order to help the students learn more.

Like Piaget, Vygotsky believed that the main goal of education was the development of higher mental functions, not simply filling students’ memories with facts. So Vygotsky probably would oppose educational curricula that are an inch deep and a mile wide or seem like “trivial pursuit.” As an example of this trivial pursuit curriculum, Margaret Gredler (2009a) described a set of materials for a 9-week science unit that had 61 glossary terms such as aqueous solution, hydrogen bonding, and fractional crystallization—many terms described with only one or two sentences. Higher mental functions can be developed through cultural tools and passed from one individual to another in at least three ways: imitative learning (one person tries to imitate the other), instructed learning (learners internalize the instructions of the teacher and use these instructions to self-regulate), and collaborative learning (a group of peers strives to understand each other, and learning occurs in the process) (Tomasello et al., 1993). Vygotsky was most concerned with the second type, instructed learning through direct teaching or by structuring experiences that encourage another’s learning, but his theory supports learning through imitation or collaboration as well. Thus, Vygotsky’s ideas are relevant for educators who teach directly, intentionally use modeling to teach, or create collaborative learning environments (Miller, 2016; Wink & Putney, 2002). That group pretty much includes all of us. THE ROLE OF ADULTS AND PEERS. Vygotsky believed the child is not alone in the world “discovering” the cognitive operations of conservation or classification. This discovery is assisted or mediated by family members, teachers, peers, and even software tools (Puntambekar & Hubscher, 2005). Most of this guidance is communicated through language, at least in Western cultures. In several other cultures, observing a skilled performance, not talking about it, guides the child’s learning (Rogoff, 1990). Some people have called this adult assistance scaffolding, which is taken from Wood, Bruner, and Ross (1976). The idea is that children use the help for support while they build a firm understanding that will eventually allow them to solve the problems on their own. Actually, when Wood and his colleagues introduced the term scaffolding, they were talking about how teachers set up or structure learning environments, but Vygotsky’s theory implies more dynamic exchanges that allow the teacher to support students in the parts of the task they cannot do alone—the interactions of assisted learning, as you will see next (Schunk, 2020).

Vygotsky’s theory suggests that teachers need to do more than just arrange the environment so that students can discover on their own. Students cannot and should not be expected to reinvent or rediscover knowledge already available in their cultures. Rather, they should be guided and assisted in their learning. Assisted learning, or guided participation, requires first learning from the students what is needed; giving information, prompts, reminders, and encouragement at the right times and in the right amounts; and gradually allowing the students to do more and more on

ASSISTED LEARNING.

Assisted learning Providing strategic help in the initial stages of learning, gradually diminishing that help as students gain independence.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

Table 3.5 Strategies to Provide Scaffolding and Assisted Learning • Model the thought process for the students: Think out loud as you solve the problem or outline an essay, for example. • Provide organizers or starters such as who, what, why, how, and what next? • Complete part of the problem. • Give hints and cues. • Encourage students to set short-term goals and take small steps. • Connect new learning to students’ interests or prior learning. • Use graphic organizers: timelines, charts, tables, categories, checklists, and graphs. • Simplify the task, clarify the purpose, and give clear directions. • Teach key vocabulary words, and provide examples.

their own. Teachers can assist learning by adapting materials or problems to students’ current levels; demonstrating skills or thought processes; walking students through the steps of a complicated problem; doing part of the problem (e.g., in algebra, the students set up the equation, and the teacher does the calculations, or vice versa); giving detailed feedback and allowing revisions; or asking questions that refocus students’ attention (Rosenshine & Meister, 1992). Cognitive apprenticeships (Cluster 10) are examples. Look at Table 3.5 for more ideas about strategies that you can use in any lesson.

An Example Curriculum: Tools of the Mind Deborah Leong and Elena Bodrova (2012) worked for years to develop a curriculum for preschool through second-grade children based on Vygotsky’s theory. In Russia, Dr. Bodrova had studied with colleagues of Vygotsky and wanted to bring his ideas to teachers. The result is the Tools of the Mind project that includes curriculum ideas for preschool, kindergarten, and students with special needs (see toolsofthemind.org). One key idea taken from Vygotsky is that as children develop mental tools such as strategies for focusing their attention, they cease being prisoners of their environment—having their attention “grabbed away” by any new sight or sound. They learn to control their attention. A second key idea is that play, particularly dramatic pretend play, is the most important activity supporting the development of young children. Through dramatic play children learn to focus their attention, control their impulses, follow rules, use symbols, regulate their own behaviors, and cooperate with others. So a key element of the Tools of the Mind curriculum for young children is play plans, created by the children themselves. Children draw a picture of how they plan to play that day and then describe their plan to the teacher, who may make notes on the page and thus model literacy activities. Plans become more complex and detailed as children become better planners. Figure 3.5 on the next page shows a simple play plan for pretending to be a doctor.

Reaching Every Student: Teaching in the “Magic Middle” Both Piaget and Vygotsky probably would agree that students should be taught in the magic middle (Berger, 2020), or the place of the “match” (Hunt, 1961)—where they are neither bored nor frustrated. Students should be put in situations where they have to reach to understand but where support from other students, learning materials, or the teacher is also available. Sometimes the best teacher is another student who has just figured out how to solve the problem because this student is probably operating in the learner’s ZPD. Having a student work with someone who is just a bit better at the activity would be a good idea because both students benefit in the exchange of explanations, elaborations, and questions. In addition, students should be encouraged to use language to organize their thinking and to talk about what they are trying to accomplish, as in Figure 3.5. Dialogue and discussion are important avenues to learning (Schunk, 2020; Wink & Putney, 2002). The Guidelines: Applying Vygotsky’s Ideas in Teaching on the next page give more ideas for applying Vygotsky’s insights.

125

126 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

Figure 3.5 “I’m Going to Listen to Their Heart” Play Plan Here a child draws a nurse and a client and then dictates to the teacher, “I’m going to listen to their heart.” The teacher might encourage the child to add a stethoscope to the drawing so that the child can talk through the plan and then remember it better.

Source: Tools of the Mind. https://toolsofthemind.org/?s=play+plans. Used by permission.

GUIDELINES Applying Vygotsky’s Ideas in Teaching Tailor scaffolding to the needs of students. Examples 1.

2.

When students are beginning new tasks or topics, provide models, prompts, sentence starters, coaching, and feedback. As the students grow in competence, give less support and more opportunities for independent work. Give students choices about the level of difficulty or degree of independence in projects; encourage them to challenge themselves but to seek help when they are really stuck.

Make sure students have access to powerful tools that support thinking. Examples 1.

2.

Teach students to use learning and organizational strategies, research tools, language tools (wikis, dictionaries, or computer searches), spreadsheets, and word-processing programs. Model the use of tools; show students how you use an appointment book or calendar app on your smartphone to make plans and manage time, for example.

Build on the students’ cultural funds of knowledge (N. Gonzalez et al., 2005; Moll et al., 1992). Examples 1.

2.

Identify family knowledge by having students interview each other’s families about their work and home knowledge (agriculture, economics, manufacturing, household management, medicine and illness, religion, child care, cooking, etc.). Tie assignments to these funds of knowledge, and use community experts to evaluate assignments.

Capitalize on dialogue and group learning. Examples 1. 2.

Experiment with peer tutoring; teach students how to ask good questions and give helpful explanations. Experiment with the cooperative learning strategies described in Cluster 10. For more information about Vygotsky and his theories, see tip.psychology.org/vygotsky.html.

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

Cognitive Development: Lessons for Teachers In spite of cross-cultural differences in cognitive development and the different theories of development, there are some convergences. Piaget, Vygotsky, and more recent researchers studying cognitive development and the brain probably would agree with the following big ideas: 1. Cognitive development requires both physical and social stimulations. 2. To develop thinking, students have to be mentally, physically, and linguistically active. They need to experiment with, talk about, describe, reflect on, write about, and solve problems. But they also benefit from teaching, guidance, questions, explanations, demonstrations, and challenges to their thinking. 3. Teaching students what they already know is boring for them. Trying to teach what the student isn’t ready to learn is frustrating and ineffective. 4. Challenge with support will keep students engaged but not fearful.

Module 10 Summary Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Perspective (pp. 116–122) According to Vygotsky, what are three main influences on cognitive development? Vygotsky believed that human activities must be understood within their cultural settings. He believed that our specific mental structures and processes can be traced to our interactions with others; that the tools of the culture, especially the tool of language, are key factors in development; and that the ZPD is where learning and development are possible. What are psychological tools, and why are they important? Psychological tools are signs and symbols such as numbers and mathematical systems, codes, and language that support learning and cognitive development. They change the thinking process by enabling and shaping thinking. Many of these tools are passed from adult to child through formal and informal interactions and teachings. Explain how interpsychological development becomes intrapsychological development.  Higher mental processes first appear between people as they are co-constructed during shared activities. As children engage in activities with adults or more capable peers, they exchange ideas and ways of thinking about or representing concepts. Children internalize these co-created ideas. Children’s knowledge, ideas, attitudes, and values develop through appropriating, or “taking for themselves,” the ways of acting and thinking provided by their culture and by the more capable members of their groups. What are the differences between Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s perspectives on private speech and its role in development? Vygotsky’s sociocultural view asserts that cognitive development hinges on social interaction and the development of language. As an example, Vygotsky describes the role of children’s self-directed talk in guiding and monitoring thinking and problem solving, whereas Piaget suggests that private speech indicates the child’s egocentrism. Vygotsky, more than Piaget, emphasized the significant role played by adults and more-able peers in children’s learning. This adult assistance provides early support while students build the understanding necessary to solve problems on their own later. What is a student’s ZPD? At any given point in development, there are certain problems that a child is on the verge of being able to solve and others that are beyond the child’s capabilities. The ZPD (zone of proximal development) is the place where the child cannot solve a problem alone but can succeed under adult guidance or in collaboration with a more advanced peer. What are two criticisms or limitations of Vygotsky’s theory?  Vygotsky may have overemphasized the role of social interaction in cognitive development: Children figure out quite a bit on their own. Also, because he died so young, Vygotsky was not able to develop and elaborate on his ideas. His students and others since have taken up that work.

Implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories for Teachers (pp. 122–127) What is the “problem of the match” described by Hunt? The “problem of the match” is that students must be neither bored by work that is too simple nor left behind by teaching they cannot understand. According to Hunt, disequilibrium must be carefully balanced to encourage growth. Situations that lead to unexpected results can help create an appropriate level of disequilibrium.

127

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Distinctions Between Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories (I, A2) Consider how two teachers—one based in Vygotskian theory and one based in Piagetian theory— might differ in their concepts of learning and teaching and the instructional techniques that they might prefer.

128 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development What is active learning? Why is Piaget’s theory of cognitive development consistent with active learning? Piaget’s fundamental insight was that individuals construct their own understanding: Learning is a constructive process. At every level of cognitive development, students must be able to incorporate information into their own schemes. To do this, they must act on the information in some way. This active experience, even at the earliest school levels, should include both physical manipulation of objects and mental manipulation of ideas. As a general rule, students should act, manipulate, observe, and then talk and/or write about what they have experienced. Concrete experiences provide the raw materials for thinking. Communicating with others makes students use, test, and sometimes change their thinking abilities. What is assisted learning, and what role does scaffolding play? Assisted learning, or guided participation in the classroom, requires scaffolding—understanding the students’ needs; giving information, prompts, reminders, and encouragement at the right time and in the right amounts; and then gradually allowing the students to do more and more on their own. Teachers can assist learning by adapting materials or problems to students’ current levels, demonstrating skills or thought processes, walking students through the steps of a complicated problem, completing part of the problem, giving detailed feedback and allowing revisions, or asking questions that refocus students’ attention.

Cluster 3 Review Key Terms Accommodation Adaptation Adolescent egocentrism Assimilation Assisted learning Classification Coactions Co-constructed process Cognitive development Collective monologue Compensation Computerized axial tomography (CAT) Concrete operations Conservation Cultural tools Decentering Development Disequilibrium Egocentric Electroencephalograph (EEG) Equilibration Event-related potential (ERP) Executive functioning Formal operations Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) Glial cells Goal-directed actions Hypothetico-deductive reasoning Identity

Lateralization Maturation Myelination Near-infrared optical tomography (NIR-OT) Neo-Piagetian theories Neurogenesis Neurons Object permanence Operations Organization Personal development Physical development Plasticity Positron emission tomography (PET) Preoperational Private speech Reversibility Reversible thinking Scaffolding Schemes Semiotic function Sensitive periods Sensorimotor Seriation Social development Sociocultural theory Synapses Synaptic plasticity Zone of proximal development (ZPD)

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

129

Connect and Extend to Licensure Multiple-Choice Questions 1.

2.

3.

Mr. Winstel was worried about his former star student, Ramon. As the seventh-grade year progressed, Ramon was frequently being called into the principal’s office for skateboard stunts that broke school rules and bordered on dangerous. Recently, Ramon’s parents contacted Mr. Winstel to alert him to the fact that Ramon had been skipping school to hang out with some older boys in the neighborhood. Which of the following statements would typically best describe what is happening with Ramon? A.

Ramon’s culture demands that boys of his age begin to engage in behaviors that reflect fearlessness.

B.

Ramon’s limbic system is maturing, but his prefrontal lobe has not yet caught up.

C.

Ramon is engaging in deviant behaviors as a cry for attention from his parents.

D.

Ramon is undergoing a period of synaptic pruning, which causes adolescents to engage in risk-taking behavior.

Miss McClintock discovered that five of the students in her class are developmentally advanced. All of the students’ language skills are exploding! Although many of the students still have trouble sharing, a few appear to understand that by sharing, everyone can be happy. Finally, there is one student who can solve conservation problems. According to Piagetian theory, in what stage are the students in Miss McClintock’s class?

4.

B.

Have students initially watch several commercials and take notes.

C.

Lecture students on the major persuasive techniques, and have a quiz to assess learning.

D.

Have students form groups to research persuasive techniques.

Research studies involving the brain and learning indicate that all but which one of the following statements are true? A. B.

There is no such thing as “left-brain” and “rightbrain” thinking. The production of new neurons continues into adulthood.

C.

Using different modalities for instruction and activities that draw on different senses may support learning.

D.

Pruning can pathways.

damage

heavily

used

cognitive

Constructed-Response Questions Case

A.

Formal operations

B.

Concrete operations

C.

Preoperational

When planning for instruction, Mr. Gething remembers that students should be neither bored nor frustrated. Although this makes sense to him, he is unsure how he could compensate for the diverse group of students he has in his second-period language arts class. Some students have difficulty with the English language, and other students plan to participate in the school’s annual Shakespearean play. He knows that by grouping students of mixed abilities, he could occasionally draw on the talents of his knowledgeable students to assist the less advanced students. He also understands that without guidelines, students might not accomplish anything.

D.

Sensorimotor

5.

Explain the theory of learning that Mr. Gething is initially drawing on, and identify the individual credited with it.

6.

What is the term for the assistance that more knowledgeable class members might provide to less advanced students in order to help the latter succeed? List some strategies that the more knowledgeable students might use to assist their peers.

In introducing students to persuasive advertising methods, which of the following approaches would be most apt to lead to student retention? A.

Determine what students already know about the topic, and connect new information to their prior knowledge.

What Would They Do? Teachers’ Casebook: Symbols and Cymbals Critical Thinking About Cases At the beginning of this cluster, we asked you to think critically about how to help students understand abstract concepts (symbols and cymbals). Now, as you consider these expert teachers’ responses to the case, we again suggest that you think critically. Do the following responses reflect (or contradict) any of the concepts discussed in this cluster? Do the suggestions and solutions described here make sense in light

of what you have learned in this or other classes about best practices? How do these ideas align with or challenge your personal philosophy of teaching?

Coralie Hafner • Primary/1st Grade Teacher Olde Orchard Alternative Elementary, Columbus, OH It is evident that these students are practical thinkers and have an understanding of basic, concrete facts. They recognize that a cymbal is a musical instrument due to personal connections (a sibling). They recognize connections that can be made between

130 Cluster 3 Cognitive Development objects (heart and love, torch to the Olympics, a ring to marriage) due to their relevance and use. It’s unclear whether they understand that items and pictures can embody the meaning of the intangible (love, commitment, triumph, perseverance). Begin with prior knowledge; the connection between things and ideas. I would show them pictures and we’d brainstorm concepts and ideas that they’re reminded of when they look at the pictures. I’d include graphics, landmarks, and sentimental items as we discuss how they connect to a concept or construct that is larger than the item itself. We’d then begin to talk about expressions and idioms (“Break a leg”) to build understanding that phrases have multiple meanings and interpretations. In order to gauge their readiness for learning, I would listen to their interactions and dialogue, providing opportunities for them to share and bring in items that represent something personal to them. I would offer concrete examples of symbolism through the use of sign language, graphics, photographs, emoticons, punctuation, labels on bottles, brochures, and math manipulatives like base 10 blocks and Unifix cubes. These items would help them begin to understand the connection between an object and what it represents. Research best practices on how to introduce the concept, build upon prior knowledge and how to navigate their thinking towards understanding. Once they build understanding, they will recognize the purpose for symbolism in poetry. Once students grasp symbolism, I would have them share their thinking and revelation with the other students for peer interaction and student-led discussions.

Amanda Romando • 6th Grade Teacher Galileo School for Gifted Learning, Sanford, FL The students’ responses are telling me that they are misinterpreting a multiple-meaning word, and require some scaffolding to begin understanding the concept of symbolism. First, I would take this as a teachable moment to address multiple-meaning words and homophones. I would give some examples and explain that many words require context in order to fully understand their meanings. I would give some examples, and then elicit some examples from the students. Then, moving on to the lesson at hand regarding symbols, I would backtrack a bit in order to help the students understand the meaning of a symbol. Most kids in upper elementary, middle school, or high school grades are well versed in texting and computer skills. I would ask them what they use emojis for when they are texting. I would ask them what types of emojis they use and what they mean by them. This is a relatable way for students to start understanding that pictures and tangible objects can stand for (or symbolize) abstract ideas and thoughts in a simple, widely understood way. After I am sure that my students understand the concept of a symbol, I would play a game of “Symbolism Pictionary” in order for them to explore the concept of symbolism. For

example, I would say a word like “school,” and the students would draw a quick picture of what comes to mind when they hear that word. Depending on my group of students, I might make it a game in order to encourage deeper thinking. Students could get bonus points for having unique answers, being able to clearly explain the reason they chose the symbol they did. From there, I would expand it to metaphorical and universal symbols, such as mountains to show overcoming adversity, or an owl to represent wisdom. As I proceeded with the unit, I would continue to refer back to these lessons on symbolism as we begin to analyze symbolic words and phrases in poetry. If students continue to struggle with the concept of symbolism after these lessons, it might help me decide that they are not quite ready for this material, and I would focus on teaching figurative language within the poetry unit.

Linda Glisson and Sue Middleton • 5th Grade Team Teachers St. James Episcopal Day School, Baton Rouge, LA To begin the lesson, I would have the students use a dictionary to define the word symbolism (root word—symbol) to discover that it means “something that stands for or represents something else.” I would then give them a brief “across the curriculum” exercise in ways they incorporate symbols and symbolism into their thinking every day. Examples follow. Social studies, American history: The American flag is just a piece of cloth. Why then do we recite a pledge to it? Stand at attention when it passes in a parade? What does it stand for? English, literature—fables and fairy tales: What does the wolf usually represent (stand for)? The lion? The lamb? Art: What color stands for a glorious summer day? Evil? Goodness and purity? I would continue with math symbols, scientific symbols, and music symbols and lead the students toward contributing other examples such as symbols representing holidays. I would then tell them about their own examples of symbolism that I had recorded. The students’ participation in and enthusiasm for the exercises would serve to determine whether they were ready for the material.

Dr. Nancy Sheehan-Melzack • Art and Music Teacher Snug Harbor Community School, Quincy, MA Even very young children can recognize symbols if the symbol is presented first and the explanation required second. A drawing of an octagon on a pole has always elicited the answer, “A stop sign,” whenever I have shown it. Children recognize symbols, but the teacher needs to work from their concrete knowledge to the more abstract concept, and there are a great many symbols in their daily life on which one can draw. Children as young as first-graders can recognize traffic sign shapes, letters of the alphabet, and numbers, and further can recognize that they stand for directions, sounds,

Cluster 3 Cognitive Development

and how many. When they talk about these very common symbols, they can also realize they all use them for the same meaning.

Valerie A. Chilcoat • 5th and 6th Grade Advanced Academics Teacher Glenmount School, Baltimore, MD Concrete examples of symbolism must come from the students’ own world. Street signs, especially those with pictures and not words, are a great example. These concrete symbols, however, are not exactly the same as symbolism used in poetry. The link has to be made from the concrete to the abstract. Silly poetry is one way to do this. It is motivating to the students to read or listen to, and it can provide many examples of one thing acting as another. This strategy can also be used in lower grades to simply expose children to poetry containing symbolism.

131

Karen Boyarsky • 5th Grade Teacher Walter C. Black Elementary School, Hightstown, NJ You can tell a lot about students’ thinking simply by interpreting their reactions. Knowing how to interpret students’ reactions is just as important as any other assessment tool you might use. In this case, it is clear that the students are confused about the concept of symbolism. This is a difficult concept even for many fifthgraders to understand and should be approached slowly. One approach to this topic would be to present students with pictures of familiar symbols, such as McDonald’s Golden Arches, the Nike Swoosh, or the Target logo. Students could attempt to explain what each of these symbols mean. A discussion about why manufacturers choose to use symbols instead of words would follow. Another approach would be to have the students interpret comparisons that use like or as. For example, “Sue is as pretty as a flower.” The teacher would guide the student to see that the author is using a flower to symbolize Sue’s looks.

Cluster 4

Eva Speshneva/Shutterstock

The Self, Social, and Moral Development

Teachers’ Casebook: Mean Girls What Would You Do? You have seen it before, but this year the situation in your middle school classroom seems especially vicious. A clique of popular girls has made life miserable for one of their former friends, Jasmine, who is now rejected. Jasmine committed the social sin of not fitting in—wearing the wrong clothes or hairstyles, not being pretty enough, or not showing an obvious interest in boys. To keep the status distinctions clear between themselves and Jasmine, the popular girls spread gossip about their former friend, often disclosing intimate secrets Jasmine revealed when she was still considered a close friend, which was only a few months ago. However, these girls are not passing notes or whispering in the hallways; they are using social media to humiliate Jasmine. First, they posted a long, heart-baring text from Jasmine to her former best friend, Maya. More recently, one of them used a cell phone to take a picture of Jasmine while she was changing her clothes after gym class and posted it on Instagram. Jasmine has been absent from school for three days since this latest incident.

132

Cluster 4 The Self, Social, and Moral Development

Critical Thinking • How would you respond to each of the girls? • What—if anything—would you say to your other students? • Are there ways you can address the issues raised by this situation in your classes?

Overview and Objectives Schooling involves more than cognitive development. As you think back on your years in school, what stands out? Perhaps highlights of academic experiences but also memories of sports, relationships with teachers and friends, and a developing sense about yourself and the kind of person you would like to become. In this cluster, we examine these latter experiences, which comprise personal, social, and moral development. We begin by looking at a basic aspect of development that affects all the others—physical changes as students mature. Then we explore Urie Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological theory and use it as a framework for examining three major influences on children’s personal and social development: families, peers, and teachers. Next, we explore ideas about how we come to understand ourselves by looking at self-concept and identity. Erik Erikson’s psychosocial theory provides one lens for viewing these developments. Finally, we consider moral development: What factors determine our views about morality? What can teachers do to foster such personal qualities as honesty and cooperation? Why do some students cheat in their academic work? How can we support students to make “good” choices? By the time you have completed this cluster, you should be able to: 4.1 Describe general trends, group differences, and challenges in physical

development throughout childhood and adolescence. 4.2 Discuss how the components of Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological model

influence development, especially the influences of families, parenting styles, peers, teachers, and technology. 4.3 Describe general trends and group differences in the development of

identity and self-concept. 4.4 Explain theories of moral development including those of Kohlberg,

Gilligan, Nucci, and Haidt and discuss how teachers can deal with one moral challenge for students—cheating.

133

134 Cluster 4 The Self, Social, and Moral Development

OUTLINE Teachers’ Casebook—Mean Girls: What Would You Do? Overview and Objectives MODULE 11: Physical Growth as a Context for Social and Moral Development Physical Development Physical and Motor Development Play, Recess, and Physical Activity Reaching Every Student: Physical Activity and Students with Disabilities Challenges in Physical Development MODULE 12: Bronfenbrenner’s Theory Bronfenbrenner: The Social Context for Development The Importance of Context and the Bioecological Model Families Peers Reaching Every Student: Teacher Support Teachers and Child Abuse MODULE 13: The Self Identity and Self-Concept Erikson: Stages of Psychosocial Development Self-Concept Sex Differences in the Self-Concept of Academic Competence Self-Esteem MODULE 14: Social and Moral Development Understanding Others and Moral Development Theory of Mind and Intention Moral Development Moral Judgments, Social Conventions, and Personal Choices Beyond Reasoning: Haidt’s Social Intuitionist Model of Moral Psychology Moral Behavior and the Example of Cheating Personal/Social Development: Lessons for Teachers Cluster 4 Review Key Terms • Connect and Extend to Licensure • Teachers’ Casebook—Mean Girls: What Would They Do?

MODULE 11

Physical Growth as a Context for Social and Moral Development

Learning Objective 4.1 Describe general trends, group differences, and challenges in physical development throughout childhood and adolescence.

Physical Development This cluster is about personal and social development, but we begin with a kind of development that is a basic concern of all individuals and families—physical development. Put Yourself in Their Place How tall are you? What grade were you in when you reached that height? Were you one of the tallest or shortest students in your middle or high school, or were you about average? Did you know students who were teased because of their physical appearance? How important was your physical development to your feelings about yourself?

Cluster 4 The Self, Social, and Moral Development

135

Physical and Motor Development For most children, at least in the early years, growing up means getting bigger and stronger and becoming more coordinated. It also can be a frightening, disappointing, exciting, and puzzling time. YOUNG CHILDREN. Preschool children are very active. Their gross-motor (large muscle) skills improve greatly during these early years. Between ages 2 and about 4 or 5, preschoolers’ muscles grow stronger, their brains develop to better integrate information about movements, their balance improves, and their center of gravity moves lower, so they are able to run, jump, climb, and hop. By age 2, most children stop “toddling.” Their awkward, wide-legged gait becomes smooth and rhythmic; they have perfected walking. During their third year, most children learn to run, throw, and jump, but these activities are not well controlled until age 4 or 5. Most of these movements develop naturally if a child has typical physical abilities and opportunities to play. Children with physical impairments, however, may need special training to develop these skills. And because they can’t always judge when to stop, many preschoolers need interludes of rest scheduled after periods of physical exertion (Berger, 2020; Thomas & Thomas, 2008). Fine-motor skills such as tying shoes or fastening buttons, which require the coordination of small movements, also improve greatly during the preschool years. Children should be given the chance to work with large paintbrushes, fat pencils and crayons, big pieces of drawing paper, oversized Legos, and soft clay or play dough to accommodate their current skills. During this time, most children will begin to develop a lifelong preference for their right or left hand. By age 5, about 90% of students prefer their right hand for most skilled work. More boys than girls are left-handed, and a small, indeterminant proportion of people are ambidextrous—using different hands for different tasks or even switching hands during the same task (Feldman, 2004; Hill & Khanem, 2009). Handedness is a genetically based preference linked to brain development and organization, so don’t try to change it. For example, some research indicates that left-handed children are more likely than their right-handed peers to develop advanced verbal and mathematical skills (Berk, 2019).

During the elementary school years, physical development is fairly steady for most children. They become taller, leaner, and stronger, making them better able to master sports and games. There is tremendous variation among children, however. A particular child can be much larger or smaller than average and still be

ELEMENTARY SCHOOL YEARS.

GUIDELINES Dealing with Physical Differences in the Classroom Address students’ physical differences in ways that do not call unnecessary attention to the variations. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Try to seat smaller students so they can see and participate in class activities, but avoid seating arrangements that are obviously based on height. Balance sports and games that rely on size and strength with games that reflect cognitive, artistic, social, or musical abilities, such as charades or drawing games. Don’t use and don’t allow students to use nicknames based on physical traits. Keep a good supply of tools and equipment (e.g., scissors, baseball gloves, field hockey sticks) for students who are left-handed.

Help students obtain factual information on differences in physical development. Examples 1. 2.

Set up science projects on sex differences in growth rates. Have readings available that focus on differences between early and late maturers. Make sure that you present the positives and the negatives of each. (Continued)

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Human Development (I, A2) Explain how development in one domain (e.g., physical, emotional) can affect development in other domains.

136 Cluster 4 The Self, Social, and Moral Development

3.

4.

Find out the school policy on sex education and on informal guidance for students. Some schools, for example, encourage teachers to talk to girls who are upset about their first menstrual period, whereas other schools expect teachers to send the girls to talk to the school nurse or counselor. Give the students models in literature or in their community of accomplished and caring individuals who do not fit the culture’s “ideal” physical stereotypes.

Accept that concerns about physical appearance will occupy much time and energy for adolescents. Examples 1. 2.

Encourage teens to focus more on what they do than on how they look. Deal with some of these issues through curriculum-related materials.

perfectly healthy. Children at this age notice physical differences and are not always the most tactful people (they are still learning norms for interacting politely and considerately). You may overhear comments such as “You’re too little to be in third grade. What’s wrong with you?” or “How come you’re so fat?” These comments more often reflect curiosity than cruelty. However, witnessing such comments signals an opportunity for teaching and modeling kind and inclusive behavior toward people who are physically different. The Guidelines for Dealing with Physical Differences in the Classroom can help with this. Puberty The physiological changes during adolescence that lead to the ability to reproduce.

Menarche The first menstrual period in girls.

Spermarche The first sperm ejaculation for boys.

THE ADOLESCENT YEARS. Puberty marks the beginning of sexual maturity. It is not a single event, but a series of changes involving almost every part of the body. The sex differences in physical development observed during the later elementary years become even more pronounced at the beginning of puberty. But these changes take time. The earliest visible signs of puberty in girls are the growth of nipples and budding of breasts at around age 9. On average, girls have their first menstrual period (called menarche) between ages 12 and 13, but the actual range is from age 10 to 16½, with some sociodemographic groups reporting earlier menstruation than others (e.g., Black girls in the United States, Indian and Pakistani girls in the United Kingdom, and urban compared with rural girls in China). At about the same time, boys’ testes and scrotums begin to grow larger. Boys have their first sperm ejaculation (called spermarche) between the ages of 12 and 14. Boys develop facial hair over the next several years, reaching their final beard potential by about age 18 or 19—although some boys take longer to develop their final facial hair. Less welcome changes in puberty are increases in skin oiliness, skin acne, and body odor. These trends reflect research conducted mainly in high-income countries (Brix et al., 2018; Kelly et al., 2017; Sun et al., 2012), but similar trends have also been observed in lowand mid-income countries, such as Nigeria (Irewole-Ojo et al., 2018) Girls reach their final height around ages 14 to 16, several years ahead of boys, so there is a time in middle school, as in late elementary school, when many girls are taller than their male classmates. This size discrepancy can give girls an advantage in physical activities, but some girls feel awkward about it and, as a result, downplay their physical abilities (Woolfolk & Perry, 2015). Most boys continue growing until about age 19, but both boys and girls can continue to grow slightly until about age 25 (Thomas & Thomas, 2008; Wigfield et al., 2006).

Psychologists have been particularly interested in examining the academic, social, and emotional implications for adolescents who mature early and those who mature later. For girls, maturing way ahead of classmates has been linked to some adverse outcomes in adolescence, especially when being larger and more “developed” than everyone else your age is not a valued cultural characteristic (Kelly et al., 2017; Mendle & Ferrero, 2012). For example, early maturation is associated with early sexual activity and teenage pregnancy, lower achievement in school, drug and alcohol abuse, emotional difficulties such as depression and anxiety during adolescence, and disordered eating. In mid- to later life, girls who mature early are at greater risk for heart disease and breast cancer. However, the timing of maturation affects girls in different ways, depending on the social context. Researchers have found fewer problems for early maturing Black girls, but EARLY AND LATER MATURING.

Cluster 4 The Self, Social, and Moral Development

studies of these girls are limited (DeRose et al., 2011; Stattin et al., 2011). Similarly, in a study of Native American (United States) and First Nations (Canada) girls, Melissa Walls and Les Whitbeck (2011) found that early maturating girls were more likely to abuse alcohol and drugs, but this association was influenced by social factors such as early dating and the attitudes of peers toward drugs. Maturing early can place girls in dating and friendship contexts where saying no to drugs is difficult. Later-maturing girls seem to have fewer problems, but they may worry that something is wrong with them, so adult reassurance and support are important for them too. Findings from research on early maturity in boys are mixed. Some research suggests early maturity is associated with greater social competence and popularity, whereas other research links early puberty in boys to higher involvement in crime and risky behavior (Klopack et al., 2020). The early maturing boy’s taller, broad-shouldered body type fits many cultural stereotypes for the male ideal, which might be an asset, but these boys also appear to be at greater risk for depression, victimization by bullies, eating disorders, early sexual activity, and abusing alcohol, illicit drugs, and cigarettes (Berk, 2019; Mendle & Ferrero, 2012; Westling et al., 2008). Here again, social context is important. Outcomes are contingent on such factors as peer group values, school experiences, parenting, and neighborhood organization/disorganization (Klopack et al., 2020). Boys who mature late may have a more difficult time initially because they are smaller and less muscular than the “ideal” for men (Harter, 2006). However, some studies show that in adulthood, men who matured later tend to be more creative, tolerant, and perceptive. Perhaps the trials and anxieties of maturing late teach these boys to be better problem solvers (Brooks-Gunn, 1988; Steinberg, 2014). The Guidelines: Dealing with Physical Differences in the Classroom presented earlier offer some suggestions for all students. The challenges of puberty may be amplified for gender nonconforming or nonbinary youth (Janika & Forcier, 2016). Many experience effects of chronic brain-body incongruence, lack of family and community support, discrimination, and marginalization, all of which can contribute to anxiety disorders, depression, low self-esteem, suicidal ideation, suicide attempts, and non-suicidal self-injury. However, recent research indicates a focus on early identification, family support, and well-timed interventions can mitigate these harms and offer long-term health benefits to these children and youth. Educators, along with medical and mental health professionals, need to be aware of sexual orientation and gender identity issues (SOGI) and be ready to offer support because these professionals might be a first point of contact for gender-diverse youth. The following resources can help: 1. The Community for Accredited Online Schools offers this teacher’s guide to creating inclusive classrooms that attend to SOGI: https://www.accreditedschoolsonline. org/education-teaching-degree/lgbtq-youth 2. The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention offers resources for friends/supporters, teachers and school administrators, and parents, guardians and family members: https://www.cdc.gov/lgbthealth/youth-resources.htm 3. The OK2BME website offers downloadable resources (including lesson plans) for elementary and secondary school teachers: https://ok2bme.ca/resources/ parents-educators/resources-for-schools-and-classrooms In summary, puberty can make adolescents vulnerable. All adolescents can benefit from knowing that there is a very wide range of individual differences in maturation. As a teacher, you need to be aware that some youth may require specialized attention and support during these stressful times. In addition to the challenges we have discussed in this section, we address obesity and disordered eating later in the cluster.

Play, Recess, and Physical Activity Play is spontaneous, self-initiated, and self-regulated activity (Edwards et al., 2020). When children view their activities as play, not work, they sustain their engagement for long periods of time without the need for external incentives. Play contributes to children’s cognitive, social-emotional, and physical development. For example, at every age play provides stimulation that supports brain development. In fact, some neuroscientists suggest that play might help in the important process of pruning brain synapses during childhood

137

138 Cluster 4 The Self, Social, and Moral Development (Pellis, 2006). Play excites the senses and creates opportunities for children to improve eyehand coordination as well as fine- and gross-motor coordination. Through play, children develop language, problem-solving, perspective-taking, and relationship skills (Hopkins et al., 2015; Lillard et al., 2013; Woolfolk & Perry, 2015). Piaget (1966) identified three types of play associated with his stages of cognitive development. Babies and toddlers engage in sensorimotor play. They are interested in the properties and functions of objects (e.g., What can this do? What sounds can it make?) and learn by exploring, sucking, pounding, shaking, and throwing. Preschool children engage in symbolic, or pretend, play. They are able to use one object to represent another, and activities become more purposeful and meaningful (Howard, 2019). They may create simple games with predictable rules, and they love to make-believe. Elementary school children also like fantasy, but their fantasy and rule-based play becomes more complex, signaling the development of logical thinking skills. They also are beginning to play more sophisticated games and sports, which can support language development, as well as teach cooperation, fairness, negotiation, and winning and losing. As children head toward adolescence, games and sports continue to be part of their physical and social development (Woolfolk & Perry, 2015). Play spaces correspond to Vygotsky’s ZPD, allowing children to experiment in a safe environment, try out new roles and behaviors, solve problems, and adapt to new situations (Chen & Fleer, 2016). Given that play is so important for children’s happiness and their social and cognitive development, teachers and schools might consider making learning more like play than work. Howard (2019) suggests this can be accomplished through more child-centered/ child-led activities that are open-ended, focus on processes more than products, and offer lots of choices (Hopkins et al., 2015; Lillard et al., 2013). CULTURAL DIFFERENCES IN PLAY. Play is a common experience for children all over the world. However, consistent with so many other topics, there are cultural differences in play. In some cultures, such as American or Turkish communities, adults, particularly mothers, often are play partners with their children. But in other cultures, such as East Indian, Indonesian, or Mayan, adults are not seen as appropriate play partners for children; siblings and peers are the ones who teach younger children how to participate in play activities (Callaghan et al., 2011; Vandermass-Peler, 2002). In some families and cultures, children spend more time helping with chores than playing. However, there are characteristic forms of play across cultures. Variations of tag and games that involve jumping rope or playing with marbles and balls are played in countries all over the world. Different materials and “toys” are used as available for play—everything from expensive toys and digital devices to sticks, rocks, and banana leaves. Children use what their cultures and communities provide to play. Also, teachers in the United States and Australia may place less emphasis on the value of play for children’s learning compared to teachers in other countries such as Norway, Sweden, New Zealand, and Japan, where a “play pedagogy” may be part of the curriculum (Lillemyr et al., 2011; Synodi, 2010). EXERCISE AND RECESS. Physical activity and participation in athletics have benefits for all students’ health, well-being, leadership skills, social relationships, brain development, and even learning. Because most of today’s children do not get much physical activity in their daily lives, schools have a role in promoting active play. There are good, academic reasons for recess and exercise: Exercise promotes blood flow and increased neurotransmitters in the brain in addition to improving mood and helping students focus their attention (Berger, 2018). Well-designed recess and exercise periods can support social and emotional learning (SEL) by helping students to exercise capacities for managing emotions, showing respect and empathy for others, and maintaining positive relationships with peers (London, 2019a). Students in Asian countries, who consistently outperform U.S. students on international reading, science, and mathematics tests, have more frequent recess breaks throughout the school day. One study of 11,000 elementary students found that those who had daily recess of 15 minutes or longer every day were better behaved in class than students who had little or no recess. This was true even after controlling for student gender and ethnicity, public or private school setting, and class size (Barros et al., 2009).

Cluster 4 The Self, Social, and Moral Development

Unfortunately, physical education (PE) time in the United States is being cut to allow for more academic time focused on test preparation (Ginsburg, 2007; Zhu et al., 2010). And some groups of students are more likely to miss out on exercise and recess than others (e.g., children in urban, low-income schools and students with disabilities). Rebecca London (2019a, 2019b) recommends schools guard against this “opportunity gap” by asking themselves three key questions: Is recess part of the daily schedule? Is recess withheld as a punishment for bad behavior or missed schoolwork? Has the school taken steps to make recess a safe, healthy, and inclusive experience?

Reaching Every Student: Physical Activity and Students with Disabilities Compared to their peers, students with disabilities often have limited opportunities for sports participation. This should not be the case! Regular physical activity for children with disabilities is associated with numerous benefits, such as controlling or slowing the progression of diseases and disabilities, improving overall health and well-being, and ameliorating the psychosocial impact of having a disability (Woolfolk & Perry, 2015). Participating in sports and physical activity creates opportunities for children and youth with disabilities to form friendships and develop positive self-concepts and self-esteem (Murphy et al., 2008). Consider the achievements of individuals who compete in the Paralympics. All children should have the opportunity to develop their athletic potential. “Inclusive playgrounds” are one way to ensure every child has access and is able to participate in activities on playgrounds. These places aim to go beyond the physical aspects of accessibility by creating spaces that promote social interaction and inclusion. Inclusive playgrounds, also known as “playgrounds for all,” exist in many regions of the world, including the United States, Australia, Hong Kong, and the United Kingdom (Wenger et al., 2020). One reason for concern about physical activity for children is the increase in childhood obesity, as you will see next.

Challenges in Physical Development Physical development is public. Everyone sees how tall, short, heavy, thin, muscular, or coordinated you are. As students move into adolescence, they feel self-conscious, as though everyone is evaluating them, and physical development is part of what is being evaluated. So physical development also has psychological consequences (Thomas & Thomas, 2008). Overweight and obesity among children and adolescents in developed countries have increased substantially in recent decades (Bray et al., 2019). In the United States, 18.5% of children and youth met criteria for obesity in 2015–2016 (Hales et al., 2017). See Figure 4.1 on the next page for the rates of obesity among high school students in U.S. states in 2019. Some groups of children and youth had higher rates of obesity than others (e.g., Black and Latino youth compared with White and Asian youth). Also, the rates of obesity for schoolage children are higher than for preschool children, signaling an important role for early prevention and intervention. The consequences of obesity are serious for children and adolescents: strain on bones and joints, respiratory problems, and a greater chance of heart problems, type 2 diabetes, and obesity as adults. Obesity also has negative effects on children’s play with friends or participation in sports. Children with obesity often are the targets of cruel teasing and might be less agile or athletic, which means they are not chosen as often by students or teachers for teams in sports. Over the long term, these children can experience depression and reductions in self-esteem (Bray et al., 2019). Like everything else involving children’s development, obesity has many interacting causes, including food insecurity, poor diet, genetic factors, increased hours sitting in front of screens (phones, computers, televisions, and tablets), and lack of exercise (Woolfolk & Perry, 2015). Children’s challenges with weight are on a continuum from minor concerns about overweight and body shape to disordered eating and other extreme efforts to control weight.

OBESITY.

139

140 Cluster 4 The Self, Social, and Moral Development

Figure 4.1 Percentage of High School Students in Selected States Who Had Obesity, 2019

12 12 12 16

15

14 15

14 17

13 10

13

10

18

15

18

23

18

17

15

15 17

21 22 23

17

15 17

15 14

15

13

15

16

16

14 14 13 13 14 13 12

16 14

15

CT DE MA MD NH RI VT NJ

2019 Not available

and the statements would still be true (but not equalities as specified in the problem above). Bill Wetta, a student at Ashland University, offered another solution that used both Arabic and Roman numerals. You can move one matchstick to make the first V an X. Then VI = II becomes XI = II, or eleven (in Roman numerals) equals 11 (in Arabic numerals). Another creative approach came from Ray Partlow, an educational psychology student in Newark, Ohio. He noted, “Simply remove a matchstick from the V from the left-hand side, and place it directly on top of the I, getting II = II.” Covering one matchstick with another opens up a whole new set of possibilities! Can you come up with any other solutions? Let us know. Be creative! We often apply heuristics automatically to make quick judgments; that saves us time in everyday problem solving. The mind can react automatically and instantaneously, but the price we often pay for this efficiency may be bad problem solving, which can be costly. Making judgments by invoking stereotypes leads even smart people to make dumb decisions. For example, we might use representativeness heuristics to make judgments about possibilities based on our prototypes—what we think is representative of a category. Consider this:

SOME PROBLEMS WITH HEURISTICS.

If I ask you whether a slim, short stranger who enjoys poetry is more likely to be a truck driver or an Ivy League classics professor, what would you say? You might be tempted to answer based on your prototypes of truck drivers or professors. But consider the odds. With about 10 Ivy League schools and 4 or so classics professors per school, we have 40 professors. Say 10 are both short and slim, and half of those like poetry— we are left with 5. But there are at least 3.5 million truck drivers in the United States. If only 1 in every 5,000 of those truck drivers were short, slim poetry lovers, we would have 700 truck drivers who fit the description. With 700 truck drivers versus 5 professors, it is 140 times more likely that our stranger is a truck driver (Myers & DeWall, 2018). Teachers and students are busy people, and they often base their decisions on what they have in their minds at the time—they use the availability heuristic. If instances of vivid events come to mind easily, we think they are common occurrences, but that is not necessarily the case; in fact, it is often wrong. For example, after you watch a few TV programs

Representativeness heuristic Judging the likelihood of an event based on how well the events match your prototypes—what you think is representative of the category.

Availability heuristic Judging the likelihood of an event based on what is available in your memory, assuming those easily remembered events are common.

398 Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

Belief perseverance The tendency to hold on to beliefs, even in the face of contradictory evidence.

Confirmation bias Seeking information that confirms our choices and beliefs while ignoring disconfirming evidence.

during “Shark Week” you may overestimate how many people actually are killed by sharks each year. Data may not support a judgment, but belief perseverance, or the tendency to hold on to our beliefs, even in the face of contradictory evidence, may make us resist change. The confirmation bias is the tendency to search for information that confirms our ideas and beliefs. You may have heard the saying “Don’t confuse me with the facts.” Most people seek evidence that supports their ideas more readily than they search for facts that might refute them. For example, once you decide to buy a certain car, you are likely to notice reports about the good features of the car you chose, not the good news about the cars you rejected. Our automatic use of heuristics to make judgments, our eagerness to confirm what we like to believe, and our tendency to explain away failure combine to generate overconfidence. Students usually are overconfident about how fast they can get their papers written; it typically takes twice as long as they estimate (Buehler et al., 1994). In spite of their underestimation of their completion time, they remain overly confident of their next prediction. The Guidelines: Applying Problem Solving give some ideas for helping students become good problem solvers.

GUIDELINES Applying Problem Solving Ask students if they are sure they understand the problem. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Can they separate relevant from irrelevant information? What type of problem is it? Can they sort different types of problems into the right categories? Encourage them to visualize the problem by diagramming or drawing it. Are there alternative representations? Ask them to explain the problem to someone else. What would a good solution look like?

Encourage attempts to see the problem from different angles. Examples 1. 2.

Suggest several different possibilities yourself, and then ask students to offer some. Give students practice in taking and defending different points of view on an issue.

Let students do the thinking; don’t just hand them solutions. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Offer individual problems as well as group problems, so that each student has the chance to practice. Give partial credit if students have good reasons for “wrong” solutions to problems. If students are stuck, resist the temptation to give too many clues. Let them think about the problem overnight. For complex problems with many interacting elements, give some explicit instructions about how to solve the problems before students jump into the process. As they get better at solving complex problems, skip the explicit instructions (Ashman et al., 2020)

Help students self-monitor and reflect on the problem-solving process (IES, 2018). Examples 1.

Provide a list showing the steps in the problem-solving process, for example: • • • • •

2.

Identify the goals of the problem and the information provided. Identify the problem type. Recall similar problems that could help solve this one. Create a visual representation. Solve the problem and check your solution.

Provide a reasonable number of prompts and questions that students can ask themselves, for example, select a few from the following: • •

What is the problem asking? What information is relevant to solve this problem and why?

Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

• • • • • 3.

When I solved this type of problem before, which visual representation did I use? How will I represent this new problem? What mathematical operations are involved? If I get stuck, can I try a different way? Does my solution make sense? How can I verify?

Model how to monitor and reflect on the problem-solving process: • •

Answer a few of the prompts out loud and give your reasons. Pick one prompt while reading the problem, one while solving the problem, and one reflecting back on the solution.

Teach heuristics. Examples 1. 2.

Use analogies to solve the problem of limited parking in the downtown area. How are other “storage” problems solved? Use the working-backward strategy to plan a party.

Expert Knowledge and Problem Solving Most psychologists agree that effective problem solving is based on having an ample store of knowledge about the problem area (Belland, 2011; Schoenfeld, 2011). To solve the matchstick problem, for example, you had to understand Roman and Arabic numbers as well as the concept of square root. You also had to know that the square root of 1 is 1. But what about expertise, really high levels of performance? Let’s take a moment to examine what is takes to be an expert. Experts know where to focus their attention. For example, expert baseball fans (I am told) pay attention to the moves of the shortstop to learn if the pitcher will throw a fastball, curveball, or slider. But those with little knowledge about baseball may never see the movements of the shortstop unless a hit is headed toward that part of the field (Bruning et al., 2011). In general, experts know what to pay attention to when judging a performance or product such as an Olympic high dive or a prize-winning chocolate cake. To nonexperts, most good dives or cakes look about the same, unless of course they “flop”!

KNOWING WHAT IS IMPORTANT.

The modern study of expertise began with investigations of chess masters (De Groot, 1965; Simon & Chase, 1973). Results indicated that masters can quickly recognize about 50,000 different arrangements of chess pieces. They can look at one of these patterns for a few seconds and remember where every piece on the board was placed. It is as though they have a “vocabulary” of 50,000 patterns. Michelene Chi (1978) demonstrated that third- through eighth-grade chess experts had a similar ability to remember chess piece arrangements. For all the masters, patterns of pieces are like words. If you were shown any word from your vocabulary store for just a few seconds, you would be able to remember every letter in the word in the right order (assuming you could spell the word). But a series of letters arranged randomly is hard to remember, as you saw in Cluster 8. An analogous situation holds for chess masters. When chess pieces are placed on a board randomly, masters are no better than average players at remembering the positions of the pieces. The master’s memory is for patterns that make sense or could occur in a game. So expertise in chess is based on extensive domainspecific knowledge of possible patterns and moves stored in long-term memory. A similar phenomenon occurs in other fields such as memory for patterns in electrical circuit diagrams, bridge hands, and computer programming (Anderson, 2020). There may be an intuition about how to solve a problem based on recognizing patterns and underlying principles and knowing the “right moves” for those patterns. For example, when asked to classify physics problems, novices grouped problems based on surface features such as “These are all about inclined planes.” Experts grouped problems based on the key principle involved, as in “These problems [which looked entirely different] all involve conservation of energy” (Anderson, 2020).

MEMORY FOR PATTERNS AND ORGANIZATION.

PROCEDURAL KNOWLEDGE. So we see that experts pay attention to important features and recognize patterns in order to represent a problem very quickly. What else? With the problem

399

400 Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

Deliberate practice Becoming expert in a skill that has clear criteria by focusing on the discrepancies between your current skill level and expert performance and working persistently to improve your performance, usually with guidance and feedback from an expert coach.

identified, experts know what to do next and they can do it. They have a large store of productions or if–then schemas about what action to take in various situations. The steps of understanding the problem and choosing a solution happen simultaneously and fairly automatically (K. A. Ericsson & Charness, 1999). Of course, this means that experts must have many, many schemas available. A large part of becoming an expert is simply acquiring a great store of domain knowledge or knowledge that is particular to a field. Experts’ rich store of knowledge is elaborated and well-practiced, so that it is easy to retrieve from long-term memory when needed. The right kind of practice is critical. Simple repetition is not enough. This is why golfers or tennis players can play often but never get much better. Developing expert performance requires deliberate practice. To engage in deliberate practice, you must be attempting to master a skill that has clear criteria for excellence. Second, you usually need a teacher or coach to analyze your performance and identify the discrepancies between your performance and the criteria for an expert performance. Identifying these discrepancies is the key to deliberate practice—you have to know what to practice. You have to be motivated to learn, receive feedback on your performance, monitor how well your performance matches the criteria for expert performance, and focus your practice on deviations from the criteria to get better. Deliberate practice is difficult, time-consuming, and not much fun (Anderson, 2020; Ericsson & Pool, 2016). So how long would you have to engage in this deliberate, focused, not-so-much-fun kind of practice to become an expert? In 2008, Malcolm Gladwell cited some of Anders Ericsson’s research on expert violinists and created the “ten-thousand-hour rule,” stating it takes 10,000 hours of practice to develop expertise. Gladwell was right that is takes a very long time to become an expert, but the time varies by fields, it helps to have some talent, the practice must be deliberate, and no amount of practice will guarantee to make you an expert in any given field. But even if you don’t reach world-class levels in a field through practice, you will continue to improve if you continue the right kind of practice (Anderson, 2020; Ericsson & Pool, 2016). Experts spend more time analyzing problems, drawing diagrams, breaking large problems down into subproblems, and making plans. A novice might begin immediately—writing equations for a physics problem or drafting the first paragraph of a paper—but experts plan out the whole solution and often make the task simpler in the process. As they work, experts monitor progress, so time is not lost pursuing dead ends or weak ideas (Schunk, 2020). We all can be experts in one area—studying. The Guidelines: Becoming an Expert Student provide ideas for you and your students.

PLANNING AND MONITORING.

GUIDELINES Becoming an Expert Student Be clear about your goals in studying. Examples 1. 2.

Target a specific number of pages to read and outline. Write the introduction section of a paper.

Make sure you have the necessary declarative knowledge (facts, concepts, ideas) to understand new information. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Keep definitions of key vocabulary available as you study. ?” Use your general knowledge. Ask yourself, “What do I already know about Build your vocabulary by learning two or three new words a day and using them in everyday conversation.

Find out what type of test the teacher will give (essay, short answer), and study the material with that in mind. Examples 1. 2.

For a test with detailed questions, practice writing answers to possible questions. For a multiple-choice test, use mnemonics to remember definitions of key terms.

Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

Make sure you are familiar with the organization of the materials to be learned. Examples 1. 2.

Preview the headings, introductions, topic sentences, and summaries of the text. Be alert for words and phrases that signal relationships, such as on the other hand, because, first, second, however, since.

Know your own cognitive skills and use them deliberately. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Use examples and analogies to relate new material to something you care about and understand well, such as sports, hobbies, or films. If one study technique is not working, try another—the goal is to stay involved, not to use any particular strategy. If you start to daydream, stand up from your desk and face away from your books, but don’t leave. Then sit back down and study.

Study the right information in the right way. Examples 1. 2.

3. 4.

Be sure you know exactly what topics and readings the test will cover. Spend your time on the important, difficult, and unfamiliar material that will be required for the test or assignment. Resist the temptation to go over what you already know well, even if that feels good. Keep a list of the parts of the text that give you trouble, and spend more time on those pages. Process the important information thoroughly by using mnemonics, forming images, creating examples, answering questions, making notes in your own words, and elaborating on the text. Do not try to memorize the author’s words—use your own.

Monitor your own comprehension. Examples 1. 2.

3.

Use questioning to check your understanding. When reading speed slows down, decide if the information in the passage is important. If it is, note the problem so you can reread or get help to understand. If it is not important, ignore it. Check your understanding by working with a friend and quizzing one another.

Manage your time. Examples 1. 2. 3.

4.

When is your best time for studying? Morning, late night? Study your most difficult subjects then. Study in shorter rather than longer blocks, unless you are really engaged and making great progress. Eliminate time wasters and distractions. Study in a room without a television or your roommate, then turn off your phone and stay off social media—maybe even off the Internet altogether. Use bonus time—take your educational psychology notes to the doctor’s office waiting room or laundry room. You will use time well and avoid reading old magazines.

Source: Based on ideas from University of Lynchburg. (2021). Study tips. Available at https://www.lynchburg.edu/academics/tutoringacademic-support/top-10-study-skills/; Sanger Study Center. (2021). Study smart, not hard. University of Texas and Wong, L. (2015). Essential study skills (8th ed.). Cengage.

So what can we conclude? Experts (a) know where to focus their attention; (b) perceive large, meaningful patterns in given information and are not confused by surface features and details; (c) hold more information in working and long-term memories, in part because they have organized the information into meaningful and interconnected schemas and procedures; (d) take a great deal of time to analyze a given problem; (e) have automatic procedures for accomplishing pieces of the problem; (f) are better at monitoring their performance; and (g) have deep, flexible knowledge that applies to new situations (Richey &

401

402 Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes Nokes-Malach, 2015). One consequence of developing expertise is that experts forget how difficult it was to learn something and how long it took. As a teacher, you will have to be sensitive about what it means for students to not understand. Sometimes the best teacher is another student who has just mastered the material, not an expert who cannot remember what it was like to not know. When the area of problem solving is fairly well defined and the criteria for excellent performances are clear, such as chess or physics or computer programming, then these characteristics of expert problem solvers hold fairly consistently. In these kinds of domains, even if students do not have the extensive background knowledge of experts, they can learn to approach the problem like an expert by taking time to analyze the problem, focusing on key features, using the right schema, and not trying to force old but inappropriate solutions on new problems (Belland, 2011). But when the problem-solving area is less well defined and has fewer clear underlying principles, such as problem solving in economics or psychology, then the differences between experts and novices are not as clear-cut (Ericsson & Pool, 2016).

Module 29 Summary Problem Solving (pp. 388–402) What is problem solving? Problem solving is both general and domain specific. Also, problems can range from well structured to ill structured, depending on how clear-cut the goal is and how much structure is provided for solving the problem. General problem-solving strategies usually include the steps of identifying the problem, setting goals, searching for possible solutions, anticipating possible consequences, acting, and finally looking back to evaluate the outcome. Both general and specific problem solving are valuable and necessary. Why is the representation stage of problem solving so important? To represent the problem accurately, you must understand both the whole problem and its discrete elements. Schema training may improve this ability. The problem-solving process follows entirely different paths, depending on what representation and goal are chosen. If your representation of the problem suggests an immediate solution, the task is done; the new problem is recognized as a “disguised” version of an old problem with a clear solution. But if there is no existing way of solving the problem or if the activated schema fails, then students must search for a solution. The application of algorithms and heuristics—such as means-ends analysis, working backward, analogical thinking, and verbalization—may help students solve problems. How can worked examples help students develop powerful schemas for problem solving? Worked examples help students manage cognitive load (overload) and avoid inefficient trial-and-error learning. Worked examples chunk some of the steps, provide cues and feedback, focus attention on relevant information, and make fewer demands on memory, so the students can use cognitive resources to understand instead of searching randomly for solutions. To get the most benefit from worked examples, however, students have to actively engage—just “looking over” the examples is not enough. Here, self-explanation and using multiple learning channels (vision, touch, movement, hearing) support engagement. Describe factors that can interfere with problem solving. Factors that hinder problem solving include functional fixedness or rigidity (response set). These disallow the flexibility needed to represent problems accurately and to have insight into solutions. Also, as we make decisions and judgments, we may overlook important information because we base judgments on what seems representative of a category (representativeness heuristic) or what is available in memory (availability heuristic), then pay attention only to information that confirms our choices (confirmation bias) so that we hold on to beliefs, even in the face of contradictory evidence (belief perseverance). What are the differences between expert and novice knowledge in a given area? Expert problem solvers have a rich store of declarative, procedural, and conditional knowledge. They organize this knowledge around general principles or patterns that apply to large classes of problems. They work faster, remember relevant information, and monitor their progress better than novices. Developing expert performance requires deliberate practice. To engage in deliberate practice, you must be attempting to master a skill that has clear criteria for excellence. Second, you usually need a teacher or coach

Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

403

to analyze your performance and identify the discrepancies between your performance and the criteria for an expert performance. One consequence of developing expertise is that experts forget how difficult it was to learn something and how long it took. As a teacher, you will have to be sensitive about what it means for students to not understand.

MODULE 30

Critical Thinking, Transfer, and Complex Learning

Learning Objective 9.4 Identify factors that influence students’ abilities to think critically and to form and support arguments. Learning Objective 9.5 Discuss how, why, and when knowledge learned in one situation might be applied to new situations and problems. Learning Objective 9.6 Explain what characterizes robust knowledge, how to recognize it, and how teaching can develop robust knowledge.

Critical Thinking and Argumentation Paul and Elder (2020) define critical thinking as “the art of analyzing and evaluating thought processes with a view to improving them” (p. 9). Critical thinking involves intentionally bringing your clearest thinking to shape your beliefs and direct your actions. Virtually every article I read before writing this section began with a claim about how essential critical thinking is today. Critical thinking skills are useful in almost every life situation—even in evaluating the media and political ads that constantly bombard us (Huber & Kuncel, 2016). When you see a group of gorgeous people extolling the virtues of a particular brand of sparkling water as they frolic in skimpy bathing suits, you must decide if sex appeal is a relevant factor in choosing a drink (remember Pavlovian advertising from Cluster 7).

What Critical Thinkers Do: Paul and Elder’s Model Paul and Elder (2014, 2020; Elder & Paul, 2012) suggest the model in Figure 9.7 on the next page as a way of describing what critical thinkers actually do. As you can see, at the center of critical thinking are the elements of reasoning, which entail drawing conclusions based on reasons. But to reason well—to think critically—we should apply the standards such as clarity, accuracy, logic, and fairness, as indicated in Figure 9.7. With practice in clear, accurate, logical (etc.) reasoning, we develop intellectual traits such as humility, integrity, perseverance, and confidence.

Teaching Critical Thinking Research results are clear: Critical thinking skills and the dispositions to apply those skills can be taught at all grade levels. So how would you do that in your classes? When Phillip Abrami and his colleagues (2015) analyzed over 340 interventions to teach critical thinking with students ages 6 through adulthood in primary through graduate schools, three elements emerged as effective: dialogue, authentic instruction, and mentorship.

• Dialogue: Teachers pose questions and encourage students to dialogue through wholeclass and small-group discussion, debates, Socratic dialogue, or written exchanges.

• Authentic instruction: Teachers focus the dialogue on problems that make sense to the students using role-plays, simulations, case studies, or ethical dilemmas, for example.

• Mentorship: One-to-one mentoring for students from teachers, coaches, or other adults also supports the development of critical thinking. No matter what approach you use to develop critical thinking, it is important to follow up with additional practice. One lesson is not enough. For example, if your class examined a particular historical document to determine if it reflected bias or propaganda, you should follow up by

Critical thinking Evaluating conclusions by logically and systematically examining the problem, the evidence, and the solution.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Thinking Skills (II, A1) A nearly universal goal of educational programs across the country is the development of thinking skills. Describe what a teacher can do to cultivate these skills in the classroom. Search for “teaching thinking skills” at edutopia.org for more ideas.

404 Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

Figure 9.7 Paul and Elder’s Model of Critical Thinking Critical thinkers routinely apply the intellectual standards to the elements of reasoning to develop intellectual traits. THE STANDARDS clarity accuracy relevance logic breadth

precision significance completeness fairness depth

must be applied to

THE ELEMENTS

as we learn to develop

purposes questions points of view information

inferences concepts implications assumptions

INTELLECTUAL TRAITS intellectual humility intellectual autonomy intellectual integrity intellectual courage

intellectual perseverance confidence in reason intellectual empathy fairmindedness

Source: Elder, L. & Paul, R. (2012). Critical thinking: Tools for taking charge of your learning and your life (3rd ed., p. 58). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. Reprinted and Electronically reproduced by permission of Pearson Education Inc. Upper Saddle River, New Jersey.

analyzing other written historical documents, contemporary advertisements, social media postings, web search results, or news stories. Unless thinking skills become overlearned and relatively automatic, they are not likely to be transferred to new situations (Mayer & Wittrock, 2006). Instead, students will use these skills only to complete the lesson in social studies, not to evaluate the claims made by friends, websites, politicians, car manufacturers, salespeople, or diet plans. Critical thinking could be useful in any subject. But many critical thinking skills are specific to a particular subject and serve to guide actions in that subject (Huber & Kuncel, 2016). For example, three critical thinking skills for studying history are:

APPLYING CRITICAL THINKING IN SPECIFIC SUBJECTS.

• Sourcing: Looking at the source of the document before reading and using that information to help interpret and make inferences about the reading. Is the source biased? Can I trust it?

• Corroboration: Making connections between the information in different texts and noting similarities and contradictions.

• Contextualization: Understanding the time, place, people, and culture that comprise the context for the event, with all the political and social forces that might be operating. Jeffrey Nokes and his colleagues (2007) investigated direct teaching of these three history critical thinking skills (versus no skills teaching) and using multiple readings (versus traditional texts). The multiple readings included historical fiction, excerpts from speeches, government documents, photographs, charts and historical data, and short sections from texts. Students who learned with multiple readings instead of traditional textbooks actually learned more history content, with or without applying the critical thinking skills, but the students who were taught and applied critical thinking skills to multiple readings learned the most. These students successfully used two of the three critical thinking skills, sourcing and corroboration. Contextualization

Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Should Schools Teach Critical Thinking and Problem Solving? The question of whether schools should focus on process or content, higher-order thinking skills or academic information has been debated for years. Some educators suggest that students must be taught how to think, while other educators assert that students cannot learn to “think” in the abstract. They must be thinking about something—some content. Should teachers focus on content knowledge or critical thinking?

Point Problem solving and critical thinking can and should be taught. In a special issue of Educational Psychology Review, Karen Murphy and her colleagues (2014) made this claim: “Perhaps one of the most important aims of formal education is to equip students with the ability to think critically and analytically about complex topics” (p. 561). This claim is not new, Murphy says. It goes back at least to philosophers before Socrates. But today educators and policy makers argue strongly for investing in programs and practices that teach critical thinking to children and adolescents. Closer to home for you, Peter Facione (2011) claims that critical thinking is related to GPA in college and to reading comprehension. There is evidence that attending college improves general critical thinking skills and dispositions even without specific interventions fostering critical thinking (Huber & Kuncel, 2016).

Counterpoint Critical thinking and problem-solving skills do not transfer. According to E. D. Hirsch, a vocal critic of critical thinking programs: But whether such direct instruction of critical thinking or self-monitoring does in fact improve performance is a subject of debate in the research community. For instance, the research regarding critical thinking is not reassuring. Instruction in critical thinking has been going on in several countries for over a hundred years. Yet researchers found that students from nations as varied as Israel, Germany, Australia, the Philippines, and the United States, including those who have been taught critical thinking continue to fall into logical fallacies. (1996, p. 136) A focus on general critical thinking skills is wasteful when so many of the important skills are specific to a particular domain and when general critical thinking skills tend to develop on their own (Huber & Kuncel, 2016).

Beware of Either/Or One clear message from current research on learning is that both subject-specific knowledge and learning strategies are important. Students today need to be critical consumers of all kinds of knowledge, but critical thinking alone is not enough. Students need the knowledge, vocabulary, and concepts to understand what they are reading, seeing, and hearing. The best teachers can teach both math content and how to learn math at the same time and can provide instruction in both history and how to critically assess history sources.

proved more difficult, perhaps because the students lacked the background knowledge to fill in contextual information. So critical thinking for specific subjects can be taught along with the subject, especially if you use the right materials. But as you can see in the Point/Counterpoint, educators don’t agree about the best way to foster critical thinking in schools.

Thinking Critically About Online Sources As you just saw, learning with multiple sources can be powerful, especially when students are taught ways to think critically about the information. What about sources from the Internet? As the teacher in the cluster-opening case lamented, not every student naturally

405

406 Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

Table 9.4 Evaluating Online Resources There are many checklists available for evaluating online resources. These guidelines are a good start. Who? Author Is the author an expert? Look for credentials, education, degrees, training, titles. Who sponsors the website? Is the sponsor an expert on the topic? Is the author biased? Anonymous? If the author is not named, beware! Why? What is the motivation of the author? Is the website trying to inform or explain? Trying to sell you something? Trying to persuade you with propaganda? Where? Publisher Is the publisher a scientific or academic journal? A government agency? A reliable news source? Trustworthy websites end in .org, .edu, .gov, or a web address you recognize. Check the contact information or “About Us” section. If the publisher is an organization that wants money or personal information, beware! When? How old is the post? When was the information posted? When was it last revised? Is the information accurate? Does the website list sources, references, support? Cross check the facts presented on other websites— look for confirmation from many other sources. If there are spelling or grammar errors, beware! Sources: Based on ideas from https://library.columbia.edu/libraries/undergraduate/evaluating_web.html.; https://guides.lib.berkeley.edu /evaluating-resources; https://www.loc.gov/rr/business/beonline/selectbib.html; and https://www.tech-talk.com/wp-content/uploads/2019/09 /cars_website_evaluation_checklist.pdf.

thinks critically about those sources: “If it’s on the web, it must be right—right?” Table 9.4 is a checklist for evaluating online sources. Effectively teaching students how to critically evaluate information from the Internet, or any source for that matter, is not easy. It is more than providing a checklist or set of guidelines. In a study of seventh- and eighth-grade students asked to evaluate online visual representations (charts, graphs, photographs, tables, maps, etc.), very few students mentioned validity or the trustworthiness of the source as criteria for judging the images. Students were more likely to judge the images based on whether they were clear or easy to read or provided a large amount of detail (Barzilai & Eilam, 2018). But the critical thinking skill of evaluating the source of information is, well, critical (Cho et al., 2017). Researchers had some success in training ninth-grade students to think critically about the source based on three dimensions: the author’s position (“who says what”), the author’s motivation (“why the author says it”), and the media quality (“where it was published”). The training involved group discussions focusing on pairs of selections from the Internet that contradicted each other. The students used the three dimensions of Who? Why? Where? to guide the discussions. Compared to a control group, the trained students gave lower ratings to less reliable links and increased the number of references they made to reliable sources such as scientific journals. Group discussions of and arguments about students’ conflicting ideas were key elements in the intervention—see the next section for more on argumentation. No matter what, students need direct teaching, modeling, and practice with many different assignments to think critically about online sources. Once is never enough—a theme throughout this book. For lesson plans about evaluating online resources, got to https:// www.learningforjustice.org and search for “evaluating online resources.”

Argumentation

Argumentation The process of debating a claim with someone else.

The ability to construct and defend a position is essential in mathematics, physical and social sciences, politics, persuasive writing, and critical thinking, to name just a few areas. Both Piaget and Vygotsky would agree that cognitive development is supported by social interactions, dialogue, challenging misunderstandings, and argument. Like critical thinking, argumentation—the process of constructing and critiquing arguments—is considered an important 21st-century skill (Asterhan & Schwarz, 2016). For example, learning science means coming to understand and accept ‘‘crazy ideas; for instance, the idea that a spinning

Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

Earth causes day and night rather than a moving Sun; the idea that air has mass; or the idea that plants get the majority of their mass from the air” (Osborne et al., 2019, p. 1073). Throughout the history of science, these crazy ideas were accepted only after repeated successful arguments based on evidence. Thus, the Next Generation Science Standards (NGSS) lists engaging in argument from evidence as one of the eight key practices that are the goals of science teaching. There are two styles of argumentation—disputative and deliberative. Disputative argumentation is debate. You must support your claims with evidence and understand your opponents’ positions so you can successfully refute their claims with evidence. It is a competitive process where the goal is to convince an opponent to switch sides. The basic question is who is right. With deliberative argumentation, the goal is to collaborate in comparing, contrasting, and evaluating alternatives, then arrive at a constructive conclusion. The basic question is which idea is right. Both kinds of argumentation are difficult. Students often concede right away or else they hold on to their initial beliefs without engaging in the argument. But true learning comes with considering, understanding, and refuting arguments and then improving knowledge based on a consideration of the evidence (Asterhan & Babichenko, 2015). Children are not good at disputative argumentation, adolescents are a bit better, and adults are better still but not perfect. Children don’t pay very much attention to the claims and evidence of the other person in the debate. Adolescents understand that their opponent in a debate has a different position, but they tend to spend much more time presenting their own position than they do trying to understand and critique their opponent’s claims. It is as if the adolescents believe “winning an argument” means making a better presentation, but they don’t appreciate the need to understand and weaken the opponent’s claims (Kuhn & Dean, 2004; Nussbaum, 2011). Children and adolescents focus more on their own positions because it is too demanding to remember and process both their own and their opponent’s claims and evidence at the same time—the cognitive load is just too much. In addition, argumentation skills are not natural. They take both time and instruction to learn (Kuhn et al., 2008; Udell, 2007). Students must learn to plan, evaluate how the plan is going, reflect on what the opponent has said, and change strategies as needed—in other words, use metacognitive knowledge and skills for argumentation. What about deliberative-collaborative argumentation? Christa Asterhan and Baruch Schwartz found that the deliberative approach was better than the disputative method for learning subject matter and for changing beliefs. Maybe the debate style of disputative argumentation focuses students on winning and on how they perform, so they are more resistant to change—and learning often requires change (Asterhan & Schwarz, 2016). A study by Jonathan Osborne and his colleagues (2019) on professional development for teachers demonstrated the possibilities of deliberative-collaborative argumentation in changing students’ argumentation skills. The teachers in the study learned to use six classroom practices to scaffold their students’ skill in engaging in argument from evidence. The practices were:

TWO STYLES OF ARGUMENTATION.

1. Ask: The teacher asks open-ended questions where the “best” answer is not obvious and conflicting answers from students are likely. 2. Press: The teacher encourages students to elaborate on their reasoning—“Tell me more.” “Why?” “How do you know?” 3. Link: The teacher relates one student response to another to highlight agreements or conflicts. 4. Claim/Explain: The students provide extended explanations and support their claims with evidence. 5. Co-construct: The students build on each other’s ideas, ask other students to elaborate or clarify their ideas. 6. Critique: When students learn to respectfully challenge and critique each other’s ideas, then the culture of the classroom supports reasoning and critical thinking.

407

408 Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes In a classroom where the teacher applies these practices, you would hear students saying things like “OK, I respectfully disagree with . . . because. . . .” or “I agree with . . . and also you could add. . . .” The teacher might be saying things like “Be more specific; what exactly . . .?” or “Who can explain why . . .?” Another study found that prompting students to consider evidence that was inconsistent with their positions by saying, “Not all of the evidence is going to support your side; if it doesn’t, see if you can deal with it” made a big difference in helping students develop their argumentation skills (Iordanou et al., 2019, p. 8). Finally, a meta-analysis of 71 studies of student interaction found that both children and adolescents learn more when they are asked to discuss information and come to agreement (Tenenbaum et al., 2020). It seems that to improve students’ debating and persuasion skills, to help them learn to argue, disputative argumentation is a good approach. But to learn subject matter, to argue to learn, deliberative-collaborative argumentation makes sense. With this approach teachers encourage students to discuss and dispute in order to construct the best understanding based on evidence (Nussbaum, 2020).

LESSONS FOR TEACHERS.

Teaching for Transfer Stop & Think Think back for a moment to one of your high school classes in a subject that you have not studied in college. Imagine the teacher, the room, the textbook. Now remember what you actually learned in class. If it was a science class, what were some of the formulas you learned? Oxidation reduction? Boyle’s law?

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Transfer of Learning Successful transfer of learning from the school to other contexts is evidence of superior instruction. What can teachers do to optimize transfer of knowledge and skills to the broader world?

Transfer Influence of previously learned material on new material; the productive (not reproductive) uses of cognitive tools and motivations.

If you are like most of us, you may remember that you learned these things, but you will not be quite sure exactly what you learned. Were those hours wasted? This question relates to the important topic of learning transfer. Let’s begin with a definition of transfer. Whenever something previously learned influences current learning or when solving an earlier problem affects how you solve a new problem, transfer has occurred. Shana Carpenter (2012) defines transfer simply as “the application of learned information to novel contexts” (p. 279). If students learn a mathematical principle in one class and use it to solve a physics problem days or weeks later in another class, then transfer has taken place. However, the effect of past learning on present learning is not always positive. Functional fixedness and response set (described earlier in this cluster) are examples of negative transfer because they are attempts to apply inappropriate strategies to a new situation. Transfer has several dimensions (Barnett & Ceci, 2002; Carpenter, 2012). You can transfer learning across subjects (math skills used in science problems), across physical contexts (learned in school, used on the job), across social contexts (learned alone, used with your family or team), across time periods (learned in college, used months or years later), across functions (learned for academics, used for hobbies and recreation), and across modalities (learned from watching the Home and Garden cable channel, used to create a reading corner in your classroom). Using an analogy to make a connection between a familiar topic and a way to solve a new type of problem is another kind of transfer (Jacobson et al., 2020; Sidney & Thompson, 2019). So transfer can refer to many different examples of applying knowledge and skills beyond where, when, and how you learned them.

The Many Views of Transfer Transfer has been a focus of research in educational psychology for over 100 years. After all, the productive use of knowledge, skills, and motivations across a lifetime is a fundamental goal of education (Goldstone & Day, 2012). Early work focused on specific transfer of skills and the general transfer of mental discipline gained from studying rigorous subjects such as Greek or mathematics. But in 1924, E. L. Thorndike demonstrated that no mental discipline benefit is derived from learning Greek. Learning Greek just helps you learn more Greek. So, thanks to Thorndike, you were not required to take Greek in high school. There is some recent evidence, however, that learning computer programming can

Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

Table 9.5 Kinds of Transfer DIRECT APPLICATION Definition

Automatic transfer of highly practiced skill

PREPARATION FOR FUTURE LEARNING Conscious application of abstract knowledge to a new situation Productive use of cognitive tools and motivations

Key Conditions

Extensive practice Variety of settings and conditions Overlearning to automaticity

Mindful focus on abstracting a principle, main idea, or procedure that can be used in many situations Learning in powerful teaching– learning environments

Examples

Driving many different cars Using the subway or bus system in your community

Applying KWL or READS strategies Applying procedures from math in designing a page layout for the school newspaper

improve students’ creative thinking, reasoning, and metacognitive skills in other areas. In the future, maybe more students will learn computer programming and benefit from transfer effects (Scherer et al., 2019). Today, researchers distinguish between the automatic, direct use of skills such as reading or writing in everyday applications and the thoughtful transfer of knowledge and strategies to arrive at creative solutions to problems (Bereiter, 1995; Bransford & Schwartz, 1999). Automatic transfer probably benefits from practice in different situations, but thoughtful transfer requires more than practice. Michelene Chi and Kurt VanLehn (2012) describe thoughtful transfer as involving two processes—initial learning and reusing or applying what was learned. For thoughtful transfer to succeed, students must first actually learn the underlying principle or concept, not just the surface procedure or algorithm. So essential to thoughtful transfer in the initial learning stage is mindful abstraction, which is the deliberate identification of a principle, main idea, strategy, or procedure that is not tied to one specific problem or situation but could apply to many. Such an abstraction becomes part of your metacognitive knowledge, available to guide future learning and problem solving. Table 9.5 summarizes the types of transfer.

Teaching for Positive Transfer Here is a great perspective on transfer from David Perkins and Gavriel Salomon (2012): Schools are supposed to be stopovers in life, not ends in themselves. The information, skills, and understandings they offer are knowledge-to-go, not just to use on site. To be sure, often Monday’s topics most conspicuously serve the Tuesday problem set, the Friday quiz, or the exam at the end of the year. However, in principle those topics are an investment toward thriving in family, civic, cultural, and professional lives. (p. 248) Years of research and experience show that students will not always take advantage of knowledge-to-go. They may (seem to) learn new concepts, problem-solving procedures, and learning strategies Monday, but they may not use them for the year-end exam or even Friday unless prompted or guided. For example, studies of real-world mathematics show that people do not always apply math procedures learned in school to solve practical problems in their homes or at grocery stores (Lave, 1988; Lave & Wenger, 1991). This happens because learning is situated—tied to specific situations. Because knowledge is learned as a tool to solve particular problems, we may not realize that the knowledge is relevant when we

409

410 Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes encounter a problem that seems different, at least on the surface—remember my students’ complaints about studying boat problems and being tested on airplane problems (Driscoll, 2005; Singley & Anderson, 1989). How can you make sure your students will use what they learn, even when situations change? WHAT IS WORTH LEARNING? First, you must answer the question “What is worth learning?” The learning of basic skills such as reading, writing, computing, cooperating, and speaking will definitely transfer to other situations because these skills are necessary for later work both in and out of school—writing job applications, reading novels, paying bills, working on a team, and locating and evaluating health care services, among other tasks. All later learning depends on positive transfer of these basic skills to new situations. Teachers must also be aware of what the future is likely to hold for their students, both as a group and as individuals. What will society require of them as adults? As a child growing up in Texas in the 1950s and 1960s, I studied nothing about or with computers, even though my father was a computer systems analyst, yet now I spend hours at my Mac each day. My mom encouraged me to take advanced math and physics instead of typing in high school. Those were great classes, but I struggle with typing every day at my computer— who knew? Undoubtedly, changes as extreme and unpredictable as these await the students you will teach. For this reason, the general transfer of principles, attitudes, learning strategies, self-motivation, time management skills, and problem solving will be just as important for your students as the specific transfer of basic skills.

For basic skills, greater transfer can also be ensured by overlearning, practicing a skill past the point of mastery. Many of the basic facts students learn in elementary school, such as the multiplication tables, are traditionally overlearned. Overlearning helps students develop automated basic skills, as we saw in Cluster 8. For higher-level transfer, students must first learn and understand. Students will be more likely to transfer knowledge to new situations if they have been actively involved in the learning process. Strategies include asking students to explain to themselves or each other the worked examples provided by the teacher or identify for each step in a problem solution the underlying principle at work. Students also can learn a new concept and then explain it to peers, discuss it in small groups, or make videos to be used in peer learning (Chi & VanLehn, 2012; Hoogerheide et al., 2014; Pai et al., 2015). Students should be encouraged to form abstractions that they will apply later so they know transfer is an important goal. Also, teachers can give students warm-up activities that practice prior knowledge relevant to new learning. For example, students can practice division with whole numbers using physical objects before the teacher starts to explain and demonstrate division with fractions. This provides an implicit analogy to transfer prior knowledge to learning the new material (Sidney & Thompson, 2019). Finally, one of the most powerful strategies for supporting transfer is retrieval practice through frequent testing and applying knowledge (Carpenter, 2012). Positive transfer is encouraged when skills are practiced under authentic conditions, similar to those that will exist when the skills are needed later. Students can learn to write by corresponding with email pen pals in other countries. They can learn historical research methods by studying their own family history. Some of these applications should involve complex, ill-defined, unstructured problems because many of the problems to be faced in later life, both in school and out, will not come to students complete with instruction manuals. One last kind of transfer is especially important for students—the transfer of the learning strategies we encountered earlier. Learning strategies are meant to be applied across a wide range of situations. LESSONS FOR TEACHERS: SUPPORTING TRANSFER.

Overlearning Practicing a skill past the point of mastery.

Too often, students learn strategies but never apply them beyond the lessons or assignments where they first studied the strategies (Schunk, 2020). Gary Phye described three stages in developing strategic transfer. In the acquisition phase, students should not only receive instruction about a strategy and how to use it but also rehearse the strategy and practice being aware of when and how they are using it.

STAGES OF TRANSFER FOR STRATEGIES.

Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

In the retention phase, more practice with feedback helps students hone their strategy use. In the transfer phase, students should be given new problems that they can solve with the same strategy, even though the problems appear different on the surface. To enhance motivation, teachers should point out to students how using the strategy will help them solve many problems and accomplish different tasks. These steps help build both procedural and self-regulatory knowledge—how to use the strategy as well as when and why (Phye, 1992, 2001). Some students will master productive learning and study strategies on their own, but all students can benefit from direct teaching, modeling, and practice with feedback. This is one important way to prepare all of your students for the future. The Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships give ideas for enlisting the support of families in encouraging transfer.

GUIDELINES Family and Community Partnerships Promoting Transfer Keep families informed about their child’s curriculum so they can support learning. Examples 1.

2. 3.

At the beginning of units or major projects, send a letter summarizing the key goals, a few of the major assignments, and some common problems students have in learning the material for that unit. Ask parents for suggestions about how their child’s interests could be connected to the curriculum topics. Invite parents to school for an evening of “strategy learning.” Have the students teach their family members one of the strategies they have learned in school.

Give families ideas for how they might encourage their children to practice, extend, or apply learning from school. Examples 1.

2. 3.

To extend writing, ask parents to encourage their children to write letters or emails to companies or civic organizations asking for information or free products. Provide a shell letter form for structure and ideas, and include addresses of companies that provide free samples or information. Ask family members to include their children in some projects that require measurement, halving or doubling recipes, or estimating costs. Suggest that students work with grandparents to do a family memory book. Combine historical research and writing.

Show connections between learning in school and life outside school. Examples 1. 2.

Ask families to talk about and show how they use the skills their children are learning in their jobs, hobbies, or community involvement projects. Ask family members to come to class to demonstrate how they use reading, writing, science, math, or other knowledge in their work.

Make families partners in practicing learning strategies. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Focus on one learning strategy at a time. Ask families to simply remind their children to use a particular strategy with homework that week. Develop a lending library of books, website resources, and DVDs to teach families about learning strategies. Give parents a copy of the Guidelines: Becoming an Expert Student on page 400, rewritten for your grade level.

411

412 Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

Bringing It All Together: Teaching for Complex Learning and Robust Knowledge We began this cluster by talking about complex cognitive skills and higher-order learning. Then we went on to explore metacognition and learning strategies, problem solving and expertise, critical thinking and argumentation, and transfer of learning to new situations. Even though research in educational psychology has examined these topics for years, lately, some researchers have focused specifically on complex learning and teaching for robust knowledge—the kind that experts have (Richey & Nokes-Malach, 2015). In many ways this current focus brings together much of what we have learned in this cluster.

What Is Robust Knowledge? The study of expertise points to three important characteristics of robust knowledge—it is deep, connected, and coherent. Deep knowledge is knowledge about underlying principles that allows experts to recognize the same principle-based features in seemingly different problems. For example, having robust knowledge in math enables students to see immediately that river current problems and airplane wind problems can be solved with the same underlying principles. Connected knowledge means many separate bits of information are linked—problem-solving steps are linked automatically within a problem, abstract principles are linked to specific features of a problem, concepts are linked to appropriate procedures, and principles are connected across different disciplines and domains. Coherent knowledge is consistent and has no contradictions. Experts are much better than novices at detecting inconsistencies in knowledge (Richey & Nokes-Malach, 2015). As teachers, we want all our students to develop robust knowledge.

Teaching for Robust Knowledge What are some instructional strategies that help students move from novice to expert skill— that help them develop robust knowledge that is deep, connected, and coherent? In the next cluster we explore several teaching approaches such as inquiry and problem-based learning that target the construction of robust knowledge. Here, let’s review five strategies that can be incorporated into most teaching approaches: practice, worked examples, analogies, integrating multiple texts, and self-explanation. You have read about practice in several clusters of this book. Overlearning, or practicing even after you can do a skill or procedure, makes performance smooth, fast, and automatic—with little drain at all on working memory. Retrieval practice or testing is better than restudying for remembering information. Practice can be very effective in developing procedural knowledge of how to do things, but it is not that beneficial by itself in helping students learn to do analogous things—solve new problems; form abstract concepts and deep, principle-based understandings; or connect knowledge across situations. In fact, if your students are very practiced at one procedure in math or art or writing, they may try to apply it even when that procedure or skill is inappropriate, so developing robust knowledge takes more than practice. To develop expertise, you need deliberate practice, that is, establish clear criteria for excellence, identify discrepancies between current performance and criteria, practice improving, fail, and try again until you reach the goals.

PRACTICE.

WORKED EXAMPLES. Worked examples can support the development of robust knowledge by managing cognitive load so that students’ working memory is not overwhelmed. This leaves enough working memory available to recognize and remember key features and deeper structures in the problem. Instead of trying to solve a problem by trial and error, students see the pathway and target their cognitive resources on learning. But worked examples and practice share some of the same obstacles to developing robust knowledge. Students

Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

can get better at doing the kind of problem in the examples, but not at doing different kinds of problems. One solution is to interleave (interweave or alternate) worked examples with practice problems and with other worked examples that have steps left out. This makes students think more deeply about what they are doing and why. But the best use of worked examples requires students to explain to themselves why each step in the example is necessary. ANALOGIES. To use analogies, students map the similarities or shared features between two examples, cases, problems, time periods, works of art, and so on. Using analogies can support transfer as students apply what they know to recognize similar processes at work in seemingly different situations. The students are also building robust conceptual knowledge by linking key features in the problems to underlying principles. One problem, however, is that novices may identify analogies based on superficial similarities that have nothing to do with deep structure or underlying principles—for example, “both paintings are blue” instead of “both paintings are examples of cubism.” The careful selection of cases or problems plus some teacher guidance are important here. INTEGRATING MULTIPLE TEXTS. We saw earlier that using multiple sources to study history led to better understanding (Nokes et al., 2007). Sarit Barzilai and her colleagues (2018) go even further to claim that “the ability to meaningfully and critically integrate multiple texts is vital for twenty-first-century literacy” (p. 973). We are bombarded with information from multiple sources every day, and the list goes way beyond books and television to speeches, videos and photographs, charts, social media, talk radio, the Internet—on and on. Without the ability to critically integrate information, students fall back on their own biases or are swayed by the most appealing or dramatic presentation. How do students learn to build robust and reliable knowledge from multiple sources that may present conflicting, complementary, or overlapping information? In a review of 21 studies of students from fifth grade through college, researchers found that text integration can be taught. In the studies, lessons about integrating texts occurred in different subjects, depending on grade level: language arts for younger students, history for secondary students, and science and social sciences for college students. The teachers in the studies used a variety of effective approaches including explicitly teaching students how to integrate information, modeling integration processes, collaborative student discussions based on multiple texts, using graphic organizers (tables, graphs, flow charts, timelines, maps), and having students practice individually. As students practice, teachers can provide reminders to integrate information. It helps to read and also write—that is, to put your integrated understanding into words. Finally, teachers need to spend some time explaining the value of being able to integrate information from multiple texts. Teach why, when, and how. Then model, provide practice with feedback, and try again—the usual suspects.

To build robust knowledge, the big winner is self-explanation. Explaining each step in a worked example, drawing a model, explaining to a peer (especially creating a drawing and explaining it to someone), providing evidence, telling why, justifying an answer—these self-explanations are better than detailed explanations by the teacher in building robust knowledge. You will learn in the next cluster that students who explain in a cooperative learning group learn more than students who receive explanations. Self-explanation encourages connections (why, what else, how, when . . .?) and coherence (does that make sense? Are there any contradictions in the explanation?). SELF-EXPLANATIONS.

Module 30 Summary Critical Thinking and Argumentation (pp. 403–408) What is critical thinking? Critical thinking skills include defining and clarifying the problem, making judgments about the consistency and adequacy of the information related to the problem, and drawing conclusions. No matter what approach you use to develop critical thinking, it is important to follow up activities with additional practice. One lesson is not enough— overlearning will help students use critical thinking in their own lives. Today, critical thinking

413

414 Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes about online sources has become truly critical. Direct teaching, teacher modeling and thinkalouds, and student discussions of conflicting information based on Who? (author and credentials), Why? (motivation and purpose), and Where? (published by trustworthy source) can support student critical analysis of online sources. What is argumentation? There are two styles of argumentation—disputative and deliberative. The heart of disputative argumentation is supporting your position with evidence and understanding and then refuting your opponent’s claims and evidence. It is a competitive process where the goal is to convince an opponent to switch sides. The basic question is who is right. With deliberative argumentation, the goal is to collaborate in comparing, contrasting, and evaluating alternatives, then arrive at a constructive conclusion. The basic question is which idea is right. Argumentation skills are not natural. They take both time and instruction to learn. It is especially difficult for children and adolescents to pay attention to, understand, and refute an opponent’s position with evidence (disputative argumentation). Deliberative argumentation also requires learning and practice.

Teaching for Transfer (pp. 408–411) What is transfer? Transfer occurs when a rule, fact, or skill learned in one situation is applied in another situation; for example, applying rules of punctuation to write a job application letter. Transfer also involves applying to new problems the principles learned in other, often dissimilar situations. What are some dimensions of transfer? Information can be transferred across a variety of contexts. Some examples include transfer from one subject to another, one physical location to another, or one function to another. These types of transfer make it possible to use skills developed in one area for many other tasks. Distinguish between automatic and thoughtful transfer. Spontaneous application of well-learned knowledge and skills is automatic transfer. Thoughtful transfer involves initial learning and reusing or applying what was learned. Essential in the initial learning stage is mindful abstraction, which is the deliberate identification of a principle, main idea, strategy, or procedure that is not tied to one specific problem or situation but could apply to many. Learning environments should support active constructive learning, self-regulation, collaboration, and awareness of cognitive tools and motivational processes. In addition, students should deal with problems that have meaning in their lives. Teachers can help students transfer learning strategies by teaching strategies directly, providing practice with feedback, and then expanding the application of the strategies to new and unfamiliar situations.

Bringing It All Together: Teaching for Complex Learning and Robust Knowledge (pp. 412–413) How do you recognize robust knowledge? In terms of perceiving and representing the problem, novices focus on surface features, whereas experts focus on the structure and larger concepts underlying the problem. Experts can recall from their long-term memory many important details about the problem domain, but novices rely mostly on what they can hold in their working memory, and they often get overwhelmed. In actually solving the problem, novices have to rely on general problem-solving strategies that take a long time, are filled with errors, and often fail. Experts, on the other hand, quickly and accurately apply the appropriate domain-specific strategy for that particular problem type. Finally, because novices’ knowledge is based on surface details of problems, they have no principles or conceptual knowledge to transfer to new situations, whereas the flexible, deep, connected, coherent knowledge of experts gives them abundant useful knowledge to transfer to new situations and problems. How can teaching develop robust knowledge? Five strategies that we have covered in this cluster can be incorporated into most teaching approaches: practice, worked examples, analogies, integrating multiple texts, and self-explanation. Each of these five strategies can be useful, but the big winner is self-explanation. Explaining each step in a worked example, drawing a model, explaining to a peer, providing evidence, telling why, justifying an answer—these selfexplanations are better than detailed explanations by the teacher in building robust knowledge.

Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

415

Cluster 9 Review Key Terms Algorithm Analogical thinking Argumentation Availability heuristic Belief perseverance CAPS Cmaps Concept map Confirmation bias Critical thinking Deliberate practice

Problem solving Production deficiency Representativeness heuristic Response set Retrieval practice/testing effect Schema-driven problem solving Transfer Verbalization Working-backward strategy

Embodied cognition Executive control processes Functional fixedness Heuristic KWL Learning strategies Means-ends analysis Metacognition Overlearning PARS Problem

Connect and Extend to Licensure Multiple-Choice Questions 1.

2.

3.

4.

“I think we should start walking on the path, and maybe we will get there before they notice we missed the bus.”

What higher-order knowledge can make the difference between how well and quickly students learn material? A.

Declarative

B.

Rote

C.

Metacognition

D.

Procedural

Knowing the importance of metacognition, Joanna Pappas decided she would try to focus her young students’ attention on their own thinking skills. Joanna knew by having her students “think” about their thinking they would eventually increase their metacognitive skills. Which one of the following strategies should Joanna employ? A.

Insight

B.

A KWL chart

C.

An algorithm

D.

Overlearning

Teachers often neglect to teach their students about when, where, and why they should use various strategies. A strategy is more apt to be retained and appropriately used when educators directly teach which type of knowledge?

“I think we should wait. If we walk on the path and our mothers come to get us, they won’t see us, and they’ll worry. Missing the bus is bad enough. If my mom can’t find me, she’ll be really mad!” In what type of problem solving are the two boys engaged? A.

Heuristic

B.

Schema-driven

C.

Algorithm

Constructed-Response Questions Case Karen Slagle walked away from her friends on the playground. She had just had an argument over who would win the spot as class president. “I know my brother will win. He has more friends and that equals more votes.” “Karen, you can’t be serious. What about the issues such as school policies and procedures?” “Those issues don’t matter. Regina Hoyt won because she was popular last year. The only real issue is popularity.” “What about the year before last when the captain of the football team lost because his campaign didn’t deal with anything but sports issues?”

A.

Declarative

B.

Procedural

C.

Self-regulatory

“That was a fluke. Look at our freshman year. That popular basketball player won.”

D.

Rote

5.

How is Karen Slagle’s argument an example of confirmation bias?

6.

In the current argument, Karen Slagle is not practicing critical thinking. What types of strategies do critical thinkers employ?

Fourth-graders Richard and Bruce sat on the stoop outside of the school. They had missed their bus and now had to make the decision whether to walk taking a shortcut or just wait until their mothers noticed they had not arrived home.

416 Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

What Would They Do? Teachers’ Casebook: Uncritical Thinking At the beginning of this cluster, we asked you to think critically about how to help students learn to evaluate the information they find on the Internet. Now, as you consider these teachers’ responses to that question, we again suggest that you think critically. Do the following responses reflect (or contradict) any of the concepts discussed in this cluster? Do the suggestions and solutions described here make sense in light of what you have learned in this or other classes about best practices? How do these ideas align with or challenge your personal philosophy of teaching?

Robert John Hovel Jr. • 11th- and 12th-Grade AP Psychology Teacher Lyman High School, Longwood, FL Knowing where your information is coming from is critical not only so the correct information is being utilized, but also so your own work is credible as well. For these students, all they have known is the Internet. Tracing information they are finding on the Internet back to where it actually came from is not only a great learning experience, it is also a very good life lesson to implement in class. My grandma always taught me to “consider the source” of where all my information, meaning rumors or otherwise, was coming from. This lesson can be brought back to considering where the information is found and considering if the “source” they are using is actually credible. Then, having them examine the credibility of the source, they can determine if it is a good source to use or not with your guidance. Furthermore, this can be brought to life in a larger, more realistic context with students in helping them learn about if information they hear is coming from a “credible source.” They can be taught to not listen to rumors and false information about people, whatever the age of the student, and hopefully glean a more authentic self from the learning process.

Northwest Tree Octopus site” out of the blue to show how real “fake” information can look in today’s world. I share Snopes .com with students to verify information. Finally, I constantly ask the questions “why” and “how” in my room to assess critical thinking. I ask for proof when opinions are offered, and I require students to tell me how the evidence they choose supports their opinions. These questions require [the] students to think critically, an incredibly challenging skill. I compare it to how sports announcers use commentary to share their thoughts about the facts. I created a video for this skill—You can access it on YouTube, by searching for “Vickie Moriarity What Does a Good Explanation Look Like?”

Paul Dragin • 9th–12th-Grade ESL Teacher Columbus East High School, Columbus, OH This common problem does not have an easy fix. For example, years ago, one of my students completed a research paper about 9/11, and the paper was filled with conspiracy theories that the student reported as fact. This dramatic example of undocumented, unsubstantiated research opened my eyes to the need to teach research methods far more explicitly to my students. Here is my general strategy to help ensure more quality research: After allowing the students to explore using Google and other common search engines to get some familiarity with their topic, I direct them to a reference database such as EBSCOhost or ProQuest, which, in our city, can be accessed with a library card via the public library website. Limiting the databases they can use reduces the likelihood that inaccurate and highly biased information makes its way into their reports. Demonstrating to them the difference between research-based, scholarly information and general information can go a long way in producing a product that demonstrates actual research.

Sara Vincent • High School Special Education Teacher Vickie Moriarity • 7th-Grade English/Language Arts Teacher Bath County Middle School, Owingsville, KY Writing research papers and arguments requires strong reading comprehension skills. Just because students can read fluently does not mean they understand what they are reading. To help students overcome this deficit, I teach them a strategy called Read, Cover-Up, Paraphrase. Students read a single sentence of text, cover it up, and paraphrase it in simpler language that a fifth grader might understand. It is astounding how many students struggle to do this. It may take between 3–12 times before many students can analyze a sentence; we often break longer sentences into chunks and then put them back together. After reading a paragraph this way, I have students write one sentence gists that answer the questions “who, what, where, when, why, and how.” I also emphasize the importance of knowing any biases of website creators. I teach students that .edu, .gov, and .org sites tend to be more trustworthy and teach them to question everything they read online. I show students the “Save the Pacific

Langley High School, McLean, VA The Internet is a useful tool for students, but it is filled with a plethora of bad information. Luckily, the teacher in the described scenario can fix the problem before the students’ submission of their final drafts. As an English teacher, I often encounter this problem but find that the majority of the students will comply if I set strict citation guidelines. After returning the first drafts to the students, I devote the next lesson to using credible sources rather than using less-reliable sources. I show examples of absurd claims from Internet websites. I also show the students how to find appropriate information. The students then exchange papers and complete a peer-editing lesson. In this lesson, students critique areas of their peers’ drafts in which the sources were weak or nonexistent. To create strict citation guidelines, I tell my students that they are not allowed to use any website that ends in “.com.” Instead, they only are allowed to use websites that end in “.edu” or “.gov,” or they can use online databases such as JSTOR. Choosing to use an inappropriate site results in automatic failure on the assignment. If teachers use these strict guidelines, their students are highly likely to choose credible websites.

Cluster 9 Complex Cognitive Processes

Paula Colemere • High School Special Education English and History Teacher McClintock High School, Tempe, AZ I usually teach my research unit after we have done persuasive reading and writing. Because I teach students how to evaluate information for bias during the persuasive unit, I know they have some prior knowledge before we tackle research. I show examples from scholarly journals, books, and general websites like Wikipedia. Then, as we discuss, I think aloud to model for students why I would or would not use a source. I typically do not allow my students to use any web sources and keep them to the sites that are preapproved in our library’s database. If I were to allow a web-based source, it would be limited to one. It is extremely helpful to students if you can show them sample research papers that are excellent, satisfactory, and poor. This way, they have an idea of what the finished product should look like. Another strategy would be to read a passage as a class and critically evaluate it together. Thinking critically is a skill that needs to be modeled and taught to kids. It is a mistake to assume they know how to do this on their own.

Jessica N. Mahtaban • 8th-Grade Math Teacher

level of support. An “I do, we do, you do” approach is probably the best way to assist students in the development and practice of these skills.

Lauren Rollins • 4th-Grade Teacher Boulevard Elementary School, Shaker Heights, OH The Internet is a fantastic resource when it is used properly. Unfortunately, because anyone can post information on the Internet, it is not always reliable or factual. With respect to evaluating information found on the Internet, I encourage my students to visit multiple sites with multiple viewpoints so they can weigh the relative merits of each. In this way, they practice their critical thinking skills. Using examples of “factual” information found on websites that contradict clearly accepted facts will teach the students that they must evaluate information in the context of what they know to be true. This will help them to understand that multiple sources, not just the Internet, should be used. In addition, I devote some time to teaching my students how to properly cite sources from the Internet and other sources. This is an important skill and also a necessary one so that they will not be suspected of plagiarism.

Woodrow Wilson Middle School, Clifton, NJ

Linda Sparks • 1st-Grade Teacher

The best way to show students how to evaluate information from websites is by modeling. I have a PowerPoint presentation that explains to students how to evaluate the authenticity of what they are reading on the Internet. Once the students become familiar with all the key points of how to evaluate websites, then I can show various websites on the Smart board, and as a class we can discuss and review the validity of the site.

John F. Kennedy School, Billerica, MA

We need to teach students how to articulate their thoughts by expanding on their ideas. Teachers can add, “Why or why not?” or “explain” to the end of questions. If students are exposed to higher-level thinking questions and learn how to ask and answer these questions, then they can think more critically about school subjects.

Jennifer Pincoski • K–12 Learning Resource Teacher Lee County School District, Fort Myers, FL Understanding that the Internet plays such a significant role in students’ lives and that it does provide reliable information, the class needs to be taught how to appropriately use the web for research. This involves teaching strategies on how to identify credible information, and providing ample opportunities for practice. To understand that not all information on the Internet is accurate, students need to see real examples—examples that are relevant to them. This could be as simple as exposing them to several different sites that post conflicting information about the same topic and then asking them to define how they would decide which information to believe. Once students recognize that they need to exercise discretion when retrieving information from the Internet, they can be taught HOW to do so. The teacher can provide a list of guiding questions that will help students critically evaluate their sources. The ultimate goal is for students to use the guiding questions independently and apply them across settings; however, in the beginning, students will require a much higher

417

Whenever I assign a new research project, I begin with a specific list of resource instructions. For example, I might state that I want two books, two magazine articles, and three websites. After the topic is picked, I have the students pull together their resources and come to me so that I can check them to make sure each student is headed in the right direction. It also gives me a better understanding of what they are researching and I can find out if they have any misconceptions about the project. I find I get better results this way, because the students know I am aware of what they are researching and have their lists of resources. (Even if they don’t use all their sources, I want them to see what is available for them.) I also am more prepared to teach specific writing skills. The first skill I teach before I assign a project as they practice reading articles is how to take that information and transfer it into their own words. Limiting the number of quotes is a way to encourage higher-level thinking processes. Many times, when given the chance, students are more creative than the writers of the articles they read.

Barbara Presley • High School Transition/Work Study Coordinator BESTT Program (Baldwinsville Exceptional Student Training and Transition Program), C. W. Baker High School, Baldwinsville, NY To me, discussion is key: whole-group discussion, small-group discussion, and one-to-one discussion, all the time being prepared to defend or criticize (with legitimate corroboration) topics raised. Students learn well from their mistakes as long as the correction is respectful and meaningful to them personally. They can’t learn to think critically until someone questions their premises and they have to defend their position—as long as the discussions are conducted without malice. They are gaining experience in critical thinking through the process of criticism, both given and received.

Cluster 10

Ester Y., Age 9, Virginia, USA ICAF.org

Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Teachers’ Casebook: Necessity Is the Mother of Inventive Teaching What Would You Do? Your school went through the stay-at-home period during the COVID-19 outbreak that required remote instruction off and on. Even when the schools reopened, a good number of parents were reluctant to send their children back due to health concerns. You had to plan remote instruction for these students while also continuing in-person teaching for the rest of the class. For a while, it was difficult to create opportunities for collaborative learning. Even with all the challenges, you discovered several new teaching and management approaches, taking advantage of the online format and resources. That difficult time opened up new possibilities for teaching and learning.

Critical Thinking • What are the critical features of effective technology-enhanced instruction? • As you plan for similar possibilities in the future, what online or hybrid strategies, resources, assignments, or assessments would you incorporate into your curriculum for the year?

418

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

• How would you get your students to be both independent and engaged when they were learning remotely? • How would you help your students collaborate with each other in a meaningful way, even when they are far apart?

Overview and Objectives For the past three clusters, we have analyzed different aspects of learning. We have considered behavioral, information processing, and cognitive science explanations of what and how people learn. We have examined complex cognitive processes such as metacognitive skills and problem solving. These explanations of learning focus on individuals and what is happening in their “head.” In this cluster, we expand our investigation of learning to focus on how the social context of learning is involved in the way learners construct meaning. Constructivism is a broad perspective that calls attention to two critical aspects of learning: social and cultural factors. Sociocultural constructivist theories have roots in cognitive perspectives but have moved well beyond these early explanations. We will explore a number of teaching strategies and approaches that are consistent with sociocultural constructivist perspectives— teacher facilitation, inquiry, problem-based learning, cooperative learning, and cognitive apprenticeships. We will also focus on the ways that digital technologies enable interactive learning to take place in online spaces and through blended learning approaches like the flipped classroom. By the time you have completed this cluster, you should be able to: 10.1 Explain different perspectives on constructivism as a theory of learning

and teaching. 10.2 Identify the common elements in most contemporary constructivist

theories. 10.3 Apply constructivist principles to classroom practice including using

inquiry, problem-based learning, and cognitive apprenticeships. 10.4 Appropriately incorporate collaboration and cooperative learning in

your classes. 10.5 Describe positive and negative influences of technology-mediated

instruction on how children and adolescents develop and learn.

OUTLINE Teachers’ Casebook—Necessity Is the Mother of Inventive Teaching: What Would You Do? Overview and Objectives MODULE 31: Constructivism and Teaching Cognitive and Social Constructivism Constructivist Views of Learning How Is Knowledge Constructed?

419

420 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning Knowledge: Situated or General? Common Elements of Learner-Centered Teaching Designing Constructivist Learning Environments Facilitating Deep Learning in a Constructivist Classroom Inquiry-Based Learning Cognitive Apprenticeships MODULE 32: Collaboration and Cooperation in Teaching and Learning Collaboration and Cooperation Learning in Groups Learning Through Cooperation Designing Cooperative Learning Tasks Setting Up Cooperative Groups Examples of Cooperative Learning Techniques Reaching Every Student: Using Cooperative Learning Wisely MODULE 33: Technology in Teaching and Learning Designing Interactive Digital Learning Environments Technology and Learning Technology-Rich Environments Computational Thinking and Coding Media Use, Digital Citizenship, and Media Literacy Blending and Flipping: Technology-Powered Pedagogy Dilemmas of Constructivist Practice Cluster 10 Review Key Terms • Connect and Extend to Licensure • Teachers’ Casebook—Necessity Is the Mother of Inventive Teaching: What Would They Do?

MODULE 31

Constructivism and Teaching

Learning Objective 10.1 Explain different perspectives on constructivism as a theory of learning and teaching. Learning Objective 10.2 Identify the common elements in most contemporary constructivist theories. Learning Objective 10.3 Apply constructivist principles to classroom practice including using inquiry, problem-based learning, and cognitive apprenticeships.

Cognitive and Social Constructivism Consider this situation: A young child who has never been to the hospital is in her bed in the pediatric wing. The nurse at the station down the hall calls over the intercom above the bed, “Hi, Naomi, how are you doing? Do you need anything?” The girl looks puzzled and does not answer. The nurse repeats the question with the same result. Finally, the nurse says emphatically, “Naomi, are you there? Say something!” The little girl responds tentatively, “Hello, wall—I’m here.” Naomi encountered a new situation—a talking wall. The wall is persistent. It sounds like a grown-up wall. She shouldn’t talk to strangers, but she is not sure about walls. She uses what she knows and what the situation provides to construct meaning and to act. Here is another example of constructing meaning. This time, Maria and her 9-year-old son, Isaac, co-construct understandings as they buy groceries: Isaac: (running to get a shopping cart) Do we need the big one?

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

421

Maria: We might—better too big than not big enough. Here is our list—where do we go first? Isaac: We need ice cream for the party! (Isaac heads toward frozen foods.) Maria: Whoa! What happened to the ice cream carton you left out on the kitchen counter? Isaac: It melted and it wasn’t out that long. I promise! Maria: Right and we may be in this store a while, so let’s start with things that won’t melt while we are shopping—I usually buy produce first. Isaac: What’s “produce”? Maria: Things that grow—fruits and vegetables “produced” by farmers. Isaac: Ok, the list says cucumbers. Here they are. Wait, there are two kinds. Which do you want? The little ones say “local.” What’s local? Maria: Local means from around here—close to us, close to our “location.” Isaac: Is local better? Maria: Maybe. I like to support our local farmers. Where are the small cucumbers from—look at the tiny print on the label. Isaac: Virginia—is that close to us? Maria: Not really—it is about a 6-hour drive from here. . . . Look at the knowledge being co-constructed about planning ahead, vocabulary, problem solving, and even geography. Constructivist perspectives of learning focus on how people make meaning, both on their own like Naomi and in interactions with others like Isaac.

Constructivist Views of Learning Constructivism is a broad and much-debated term. Actually, constructivism is more a philosophy about knowledge than a scientific theory of learning. Various constructivist perspectives are grounded in the research of Piaget, Vygotsky, the Gestalt psychologists, and the work of Bartlett, Bruner, and Rogoff. The philosophy of John Dewey and Jean Lave’s work in anthropology also are intellectual roots of constructivism. But even though many psychologists and educators use the term constructivism, they often mean very different things (J. Martin, 2006; McCaslin & Hickey, 2001; Phillips, 1997). Although there is no single constructivist theory, most constructivist perspectives do agree on two central ideas:

Central Idea 1: Learners are active in constructing their own understanding—they create knowledge by going beyond the information they are given (Chi & Wylie, 2014). Central Idea 2: Social interactions are important in this knowledge construction process (Bruning et al., 2011; Schunk, 2020). One way to organize constructivist views is to describe two forms of constructivism that match these central ideas: cognitive and social construction (Palincsar, 1998; Phillips, 1997). Cognitive constructivists focus on how individuals use information, resources, and even help from others to build understanding—see Central Idea 1. In contrast, social constructivists view the social environment as critical to learning and development; we learn by participating with others in activities that are meaningful in the culture—see Central Idea 2 (Dohn, 2016; Windschitl, 2002). Let’s look a bit closer. Many psychological theories include some kind of constructivism because these theories embrace the idea that individuals construct their own cognitive structures as they make sense of their experiences (Palincsar, 1998). Because these theories emphasize the development of personal knowledge, beliefs, self-concept, or identity, they sometimes refer to learners as individual constructivists or psychological constructivists; they all focus on the inner psychological life of people. When Naomi talked to the wall in the previous section, she was making meaning using her own individual knowledge and beliefs about how to respond when someone (or something) talks to you (Piaget, 1971; Windschitl, 2002). When children observe that most plants need soil to grow and then conclude that plants “eat dirt,” they are using what they know about how eating supports life to make sense of plant growth (Linn & Eylon, 2006).

COGNITIVE CONSTRUCTIVISM.

Constructivism/ Constructivist approach View that emphasizes the active role of the learner in building understanding and making sense of information.

422 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Radical constructivism Knowledge is assumed to be the individual’s construction; it cannot be judged right or wrong.

Using these standards, the most recent information processing theories are constructivist in nature because they are concerned with how individuals construct internal representations (propositions, images, concepts, schemas) that can be remembered and retrieved (Mayer, 2012; Schunk, 2020). Some psychologists, however, believe that information processing is “trivial” or “weak” constructivism because the individual’s only constructive contribution is to build accurate internal representations of the outside world, not to construct a unique and individual understanding (Derry, 1992; Garrison, 1995; Marshall, 1996; Windschitl, 2002). In contrast, Piaget’s psychological (cognitive) constructivist perspective is less concerned with “correct” representations and more interested in meaning as constructed by the individual. Piaget’s special focus was on logic and the construction of universal knowledge such as conservation or reversibility—concepts you learned about in Cluster 3 (Miller, 2016). Such knowledge comes from reflecting on and coordinating our own cognitions or thoughts, not from copying external reality. Piaget saw the social environment as an important factor in development, but he did not believe that social interaction was the main mechanism for changing thinking. At the extreme end of individual constructivism is the notion of radical constructivism. This perspective holds that each of us constructs meaning (knowledge) from our own experiences as we try to explain to ourselves what we perceive, but we have no way of understanding or “knowing” that the knowledge constructed by others or ourselves is “correct.” Learning for radical constructivists consists of replacing one construction with another that better explains the person’s current perceptions of reality (Hennessey et al., 2012). A difficulty with this position is that, when pushed to the extreme of relativism, all knowledge and all beliefs are equal because they are all valid individual perceptions. There are problems with this thinking for educators. First, teachers have a professional responsibility to emphasize some values, such as honesty or justice, over others, such as deception and bigotry. All perceptions and beliefs are not equal. Second, in many fields, there are “right” answers that have been reached by scientific consensus; students will have trouble learning if they hold on to personal misconceptions and naïve constructions (Moshman, 2021). As teachers, we ask students to work hard to learn. If learning cannot advance understanding because all understandings are equally good, then, as David Moshman (1997) noted, “We might just as well let students continue to believe whatever they believe” (p. 230). Also, it appears that some knowledge, such as counting and one-to-one correspondence (i.e., the idea that numbers correspond to specific quantities), is not constructed but innate. Knowing one-to-one correspondence is part of being human (Geary, 1995; Schunk, 2020). We have seen that in cognitive constructivism, learning means individually possessing knowledge (Central Idea 1). But much learning takes place in social contexts. From the perspective of social constructivism, learning is a socially mediated process in which learners, who belong to social groups, construct knowledge through interaction (Central Idea 2; Dohn, 2016; Mason, 2007). Vygotsky emphasized the second idea, that social interaction, cultural tools, and activity shape individual development and learning, just as Isaac’s interactions and activities in the grocery store with his mother shaped his learning about anticipating possible consequences (running out of space in the shopping cart and melted ice cream), the meaning of “produce” and “local,” and geography—“where is Virginia?” (Martin, 2006). By participating in a broad range of activities with others, learners appropriate the outcomes produced by working together; these outcomes could include both new strategies and knowledge. Appropriating means being able to reason, act, and participate using cultural tools—for example, using conceptual tools such as “force” and “acceleration” to reason in physics (Mason, 2007). Because Vygotsky’s theory relies heavily on social interactions and the cultural context to explain learning, most psychologists classify him as a social constructivist (Palincsar, 1998; Prawat, 1996). However, some theorists categorize him as a cognitive constructivist because he was primarily interested in development within the individual (Moshman, 1997; Phillips, 1997). In a sense, Vygotsky was both. One advantage of his theory of learning is that it gives

SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIVISM.

Appropriating Being able to internalize or take for yourself knowledge and skills developed in interaction with others or with cultural tools.

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

us a way to consider both the cognitive and the social: He bridges both camps. For example, Vygotsky’s concept of the zone of proximal development—the area in which a child can solve a problem with the help (scaffolding) of an adult or more able peer—has been called a place where culture and cognition create each other (Cole, 1998). Culture creates cognition when the adult uses tools and practices from the culture (language, maps, technology, stories, or music) to steer the child toward goals the culture values (reading, writing, traditions, dance). Cognition creates culture as the adult, child, and members of the community together generate new practices and problem solutions to add to the cultural group’s repertoire (Serpell, 1993). So people are both products and producers of their societies and cultures (Bandura, 2018). One way of integrating cognitive and social constructivism is to think of knowledge as both individually constructed and socially mediated (Windschitl, 2002).

How Is Knowledge Constructed? These two different perspectives on constructivism raise some general questions, create some tensions, and suggest different answers. One question concerns how knowledge is constructed. Table 10.1 provides three explanations. Notice that the sources of knowledge (first column) differ because each approach is based on a different assumption about how individuals come to “know.” By logical extension, therefore, the role of teachers and learners looks different for each approach.

Knowledge: Situated or General? A second question that cuts across many constructivist perspectives is whether knowledge is internal, general, and transferable, or bound to the time and place in which it is constructed. Psychologists who emphasize the social construction of knowledge and situated learning affirm Vygotsky’s notion that learning is inherently social and embedded in a particular cultural setting (Cobb & Bowers, 1999; Dohn, 2016; Schoor et al., 2015). What is true

Table 10.1 How Knowledge Is Constructed KNOWLEDGE SOURCE

ASSUMPTIONS ABOUT KNOWLEDGE

IMPLICATIONS FOR TEACHING

EXAMPLE THEORIES

External

• Knowledge is acquired by constructing a representation of the outside world. • Knowledge is accurate to the extent that it reflects the “way things really are” in the outside world.

Direct teaching, feedback, and explanation affect learning.

Information processing

Internal

• Knowledge is constructed by transforming, organizing, and reorganizing previous knowledge. • Knowledge is not a mirror of the external world, even though experience influences thinking and thinking influences knowledge.

Exploration and discovery are more important than direct teaching. Teacher serves as guide.

Piaget’s theory of cognitive constructivism

Both External and Internal

• Knowledge is constructed through social interactions and experience. • Knowledge reflects the outside world as filtered through and influenced by culture, language, beliefs, interactions with others.

Guided discovery, teaching, modeling, and opportunity for social interaction help the learner connect to prior knowledge, beliefs, and thinking, all of which affect learning.

Vygotsky’s theory of social constructivism

Source: Bruning, Schraw, and Norby (2011); Moshman (1982); and Schunk (2020).

423

Zone of proximal development Developmental phase at which a child can master a task if given appropriate help and support from a more capable person.

424 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning Community of practice Social situation or context in which ideas are judged useful or true.

Situated learning The idea that skills and knowledge are tied to the situation in which they were learned and that they are difficult to apply in new settings.

in one time and place—such as before the Middle Ages, when many accepted the “fact” that the earth was flat—becomes false in another time and place. Particular ideas may be useful within a specific community of practice, such as 15th-century navigation, but useless outside that community. What counts as new knowledge is determined in part by how well the new idea fits with current accepted practice. Over time, the current practice may be questioned and even overthrown, but until such major shifts occur, current practice will shape what is considered valuable and even what is considered knowledge. Situated learning emphasizes that learning in the real world is not like studying in school. It is more like an apprenticeship where novices, with the support of community members who serve as expert guides and models, take on more and more responsibility until they can function independently. Proponents of this view believe situated learning explains learning in factories, around the dinner table, in high school halls, in street gangs, in the business office, in fan fiction communities, in artistic endeavors, on the playground, and so on (Ito et al., 2020). Situated learning is often described as “enculturation,” or forming an identity within a particular community by adopting its norms, behaviors, skills, beliefs, language, and attitudes. The community might be mathematicians or online gamers or writers or students in your eighth-grade class or soccer players—any group that has particular ways of thinking and acting. Knowledge is viewed not as individual cognitive structures but rather as a creation of the community over time. The practices of the community—the ways of interacting and getting things done as well as the identities and tools the community has created—constitute the knowledge of that community. Learning means becoming more able to participate in those practices, take on those identities, and use the tools (Dohn, 2016; Greeno et al., 1996; Mason, 2007). At the most basic level, “situated learning emphasizes the idea that much of what is learned is specific to the situation in which it is learned” (Anderson et al., 1996, p. 5). Thus, some would argue, learning to do calculations in school may help students do more school calculations because the skills can be applied only in the context in which they were learned—namely, school (Lave, 1997; Lave & Wenger, 1991). But it also appears that knowledge and skills can be applied across contexts that were not part of the initial learning situation, as when you use your ability to read and calculate to do your income taxes, even though income tax forms were not part of your high school curriculum (Schunk, 2020). Whether it takes place in school or in everyday contexts out of school does not necessarily make learning isolated or irrelevant (Bereiter, 1997). As you saw in Cluster 9, a major question in educational psychology—and education in general—concerns the transfer of knowledge from one situation to another. How can you encourage this transfer from one situation to another? Help is on the way in the next section.

Common Elements of Learner-Centered Teaching

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Student-Centered Learning (II, A3) Many of the major initiatives to reform content-area curricula (e.g., science, mathematics) emphasize student-centered/ constructivist approaches to learning. Describe the major principles of these approaches, and explain how they differ from teacher-centered approaches.

Stop & Think What makes a lesson “student centered”? What experiences have you and your fellow students had where you were at the “center” of instruction? List the characteristics and features that put the student in the center of learning. We have looked at some areas of disagreement among the constructivist perspectives, but what about areas of agreement? All constructivist perspectives assume that knowing develops as learners, like Naomi and Isaac, try to make sense of their experiences. “Learners, therefore, are not empty vessels waiting to be filled, but rather active organisms seeking meaning” (Driscoll, 2005, p. 487). Humans construct mental models or schemas and continue to revise them to make better sense of their experiences. We are knowledge inventors, not copy machines or filing cabinets. Our constructions do not necessarily resemble external reality; rather, they are our unique interpretations, like Naomi’s friendly, persistent wall. This doesn’t mean that all constructions are equally useful or viable. Learners test their understandings against experience and the understandings of other people—they negotiate and co-construct meanings like Isaac did with his mother.

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

425

Constructivists share similar goals for learning. They emphasize knowledge in use rather than the storing of inert facts, concepts, and skills. Learning goals include developing abilities to find and solve ill-structured problems, critical thinking, inquiry, self-determination, and openness to multiple perspectives (Driscoll, 2005). Even though there is no single constructivist theory, many constructivist approaches recommend five conditions for learning: 1. Embed learning in complex, realistic, and relevant learning environments. 2. Provide for social negotiation and shared responsibility as a part of learning. 3. Support multiple perspectives, and use multiple representations of content. 4. Nurture self-awareness and an understanding that knowledge is constructed. 5. Encourage ownership in learning. (Driscoll, 2005; Marshall, 1992) When these conditions for learning are met, learners are not recipients but co-creators of their learning—learners become the center rather than their teachers. Table 10.2 on the next page shows how student-centered learning approaches lead to deeper learning than do traditional, teacher-centered approaches (Sawyer, 2006). As you read more about the five common elements of constructivist teaching, think about whether these elements were present in your own classes when you were a student. Before we discuss constructivist teaching approaches consistent with this view, let’s look more closely at these five conditions for learning. Constructivists believe that students should not be given stripped-down, simplified problems and basic skills drills but instead should encounter complex learning environments that deal with “fuzzy,” ill-structured problems. The world beyond school presents few simple problems or step-by-step directions, so schools should be sure that every student has experience solving complex problems. Complex problems are not just difficult ones; rather, they have many parts. They involve multiple, interacting elements and many possible solutions. There is no one right way to reach a conclusion, and each solution may bring a new set of problems. These complex problems should be embedded in authentic tasks and activities, the kinds of situations that students would face as they apply what they are learning in their everyday lives beyond school (Gutiérrez et al., 2017). Students may need support (scaffolding) as they work on these complex problems, with teachers helping them find resources, keeping track of their progress, breaking larger problems down into smaller ones, and so on. As they develop more expertise, learners can apply and modify their knowledge to fit different situations.

COMPLEX LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS AND AUTHENTIC TASKS.

SOCIAL NEGOTIATION. Many constructivists share Vygotsky’s belief that higher mental processes develop through social negotiation and interaction, so collaboration in learning is valued. A major goal of teaching is to develop students’ critical thinking and collaborative argumentation—the abilities to establish and defend their own reasoned positions while respecting the positions of others and working together to negotiate or co-construct meaning. To accomplish this exchange, students must talk and listen to each other, weigh evidence for and against their position, and integrate information to support their argument. This process involves “learning to argue and arguing to learn” (Nussbaum, 2021, p. 10). Collaborative practices and productive dialogue can help students build shared meaning by finding common ground and exchanging interpretations (Howe et al., 2019).

When students encounter only one model, one analogy, one way of understanding complex content, they often oversimplify as they try to apply that one approach to every situation. I (Anita) saw this happen in my educational psychology class when six students were presenting an example of guided discovery learning. The students’ presentation was a near copy of a guided discovery demonstration I had given earlier in the semester, but with some major misconceptions. My students knew only one way to represent discovery learning. Resources for the class should have provided multiple representations of content using different

Complex learning environments Problems and learning situations that mimic the ill-structured nature of real life.

Social negotiation Aspect of learning process that relies on collaboration with others and respect for different perspectives.

MULTIPLE PERSPECTIVES AND REPRESENTATIONS OF CONTENT.

Multiple representations of content Considering problems using various analogies, examples, and metaphors.

426 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Table 10.2 How Deep Learning Contrasts with Learning in Traditional Classrooms: Principles and Examples

LEARNING IN TRADITIONAL CLASSROOMS

BUT FINDINGS FROM COGNITIVE SCIENCE SHOW OTHER REQUIREMENTS FOR DEEP LEARNING

• Class material is not related to what students already know.

• Learners relate new understandings to what they already know and believe.

Teacher: “Igneous rocks are. . . .”

• Class material presented and learned as disconnected bits of knowledge. Teacher: “The definition of metamorphic rocks is. . . .”

• Lessons involve memorizing facts and doing procedures without understanding how or why. Teacher: “To divide fractions, invert and multiply. . . .” • Knowledge is owned by an authority (e.g., textbook). Learners have trouble understanding ideas that are not straight from the book or explained in the same way. Teacher: “What does your textbook say about . . .?” • Authorities and experts are the source of unchanging and accurate facts and procedures. Teacher: “Scientists agree. . . .”

• Learning is for reproduction instead of thinking about the purpose of learning and the best strategies for that purpose. Teacher: “This will be on the test.”

Teacher: “Have any of you seen granite countertops on TV home shows or maybe you have one in your house? What do they look like . . .?” • Learners integrate and interconnect their knowledge in expanding conceptual systems. Teacher: “We already have learned about two kinds of rocks. Janay brought rock samples from her trip to Colorado. We also learned last week about how the earth has changed over the centuries, with some ocean floors becoming land areas. Today we will learn about how marble and diamonds. . . .” • Learners search for patterns and recognize or invent underlying principles. Teacher: “Remind me what it means to divide. . . . Ok, so 3/4 divided by 1/2 means how many sets of what are in . . .?” • Learners evaluate new ideas, even if not in the text, and integrate them into their thinking. Teacher: “On TV yesterday there was a story about a new drug that is effective in curing 1 out of 8 cases of. . . . What is the probability of a cure?” • Learners understand that knowledge is socially constructed by people, so ideas require critical examination. Teacher: “Here are brief summaries of two positions about climate change. How can we assess the evidence for each position?” • Learners think about why they are learning, monitor their understanding, and reflect on their own learning processes. Teacher: “How could you use this concept in your own life? How can you tell if you are understanding it?”

Source: Based on Sawyer, R. K. (2006). The new science of iearning. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (p. 4). Cambridge University Press.

Spiral curriculum Bruner’s design for teaching that introduces the fundamental structure of all subjects early in the school years and then revisits the subjects in more and more complex forms over time.

analogies, examples, and metaphors. This idea is consistent with Jerome Bruner’s (1966) spiral curriculum, a structure for teaching that introduces the fundamental structure of all subjects—the “big ideas”—early in the school years and then revisits the subjects in more and more complex forms over time. Another example, the use of manipulatives in mathematics, allows students different ways to represent the quantities and processes in mathematics (Carbonneau et al., 2013). SELF-AWARENESS AND REFLECTION ABOUT THE KNOWLEDGE CONSTRUCTION PROCESS. Constructivist approaches emphasize making students aware of their own role in constructing knowledge. The assumptions we make, our beliefs, and our experiences shape what each of us comes to “know” about the world. Different assumptions and different experiences lead to different knowledge, as we saw in Cluster 2 when we explored the

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

427

role of cultural differences in shaping knowledge. If students are aware of the influences that shape their thinking, they will be more able to choose, develop, and defend positions in a self-critical way while respecting the positions of others. Reflection is also necessary for knowledge construction. To develop deep conceptual knowledge, students need to reflect by thoughtfully analyzing their own work and progress. STUDENT OWNERSHIP OF LEARNING. Constructivist approaches to learning emphasize students’ direct involvement in and ownership of their own learning. Student ownership does not mean that the teacher abandons responsibility for instruction. Rather, teachers honor the knowledge that students bring to school and support students’ agency and self-determination (Vossoughi et al., 2021). Students come into our classrooms filled with knowledge and beliefs about how the world works. Some of these preconceptions are right, some are partly right, and some are wrong. If learning opportunities do not begin with what the students “know,” then the students might learn what it takes to pass a test, but their knowledge and beliefs about the world will not change, nor will their sense of ownership over what they have learned (Hennessey et al., 2012). As we’ll see in Clusters 11 and 12, supporting ownership enhances students’ self-efficacy, interest, and intrinsic motivation. Because the design of teaching is a central issue in this book, we will spend the rest of this cluster discussing examples of ownership of learning and student-centered instruction.

Designing Constructivist Learning Environments Now that you understand the common elements of learner-centered, constructivist teaching approaches, how can you design environments that will put students at the center? Designing learning environments means translating our knowledge about learning and motivation into activities, assignments, assessments, and other resources for instruction (Belland et al., 2013). An interdisciplinary field often called the learning sciences encompasses research in psychology, education, computer science, philosophy, sociology, anthropology, neuroscience, and other fields that study learning and learning environments. Like educational psychologists, learning scientists are interested in the structures that support deep, robust knowledge construction in subjects like science, mathematics, and literacy.

Facilitating Deep Learning in a Constructivist Classroom

Learning sciences An interdisciplinary science of learning, based on research in psychology, education, computer science, philosophy, sociology, anthropology, neuroscience, and other fields that study learning.

In constructivist classrooms, teachers are facilitators and learning environment designers. Even though the student is at the center of constructivist perspectives of learning, this does not mean that teachers are irrelevant or obsolete. Let’s look at several other practices that teachers can use to facilitate students’ active construction of knowledge. Constructivist approaches include scaffolding to support students’ developing expertise. Scaffolding is “the practice of providing students with support for meaning making and independent thinking” (Muhonen et al., 2016, p. 143). Deep understanding requires that students grapple with problems beyond what they are able to do independently—that is, problems in their zone of proximal development. As you saw in Cluster 3, in Vygotsky’s theory, this is where learners need the support of others to construct new knowledge through assisted learning. In other words, higher-order thinking and learning have their origins in our social interactions (McCaslin & Vriesema, 2018). Look back at the grocery store conversation between Isaac and his mother. Notice how the mother used the melted ice cream on the kitchen counter—connecting to Isaac’s experience and knowledge—to scaffold Isaac’s understanding about the best method for grocery shopping.

SCAFFOLDING.

Scaffolding The practice of providing students with support for meaning making and independent thinking.

428 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning Scaffolding can be provided by teachers, peers, parents, and even multimedia resources. Scaffolding includes both motivational and cognitive support—helping students stay engaged and interested while also helping them move toward deeper learning (Belland, 2014; Wood et al., 1976). Motivational scaffolding includes recruiting the students’ interests and enthusiasm to engage with the learning activity, maintaining the students’ attention and redirecting when the students stray off task, supporting students’ self-efficacy, and helping students manage their emotions if learning gets frustrating. These motivational supports are important for all students, but especially for students with learning challenges (Radford et al., 2015). Cognitive scaffolding includes three characteristics (Radford et al., 2015; van de Pol et al., 2010). The teacher provides contingency support by constantly adjusting, differentiating, and tailoring responses to optimize students’ understanding. The teacher gradually withdraws support as the students’ understanding and skills deepen—a process known as fading. Eventually, the teacher transfers responsibility to students as they assume more and more responsibility for their own learning (van Merriënboer & Kirschner, 2013). During initial learning, teachers can begin with concrete examples and move to greater abstraction. For example, Emily Fyfe and her colleagues (2015) tested four ways of teaching math to firstthrough third-graders. The winning combination involved beginning with the most concrete representation (monkey and frog puppets who collected different numbers of stickers) and progressing to the most abstract (an addition equation with numbers and symbols). This fading technique helped all students learn more, even those who had greater prior knowledge in math. Figure 10.1 provides examples of different sources of support that might help scaffold students’ knowledge construction: materials, instructional support, interactive scaffolds, technology-mediated support, and motivational scaffolding. These methods often work in tandem (Belland, 2017; Palincsar et al., 2017). For example, Tzu-Jung Lin and her colleagues (2015) tested instructional supports for fourth-grade students’ learning, such as prompting, modeling, and reminding. They analyzed over 35,000 exchanges during students’ problembased discussions in small groups. They found that teachers’ prompts for relational thinking—encouraging logical reasoning, use of analogies, counterargument, alternative hypotheses, and elaborated clarifications—led to better student relational thinking. Once

Figure 10.1 Scaffolding Support to Help Learners Construct Meaning Teachers can use various strategies to support students’ deep learning. These involve selecting materials, using instructional approaches that facilitate understanding, designing interactive opportunities, using technology, and supporting student motivation. TYPES OF SCAFFOLDING TO HELP LEARNERS CONSTRUCT MEANING Material

Graphic organizers Number lines Infographics Models Manipulatives Tables Graphs Charts Images

Instructional

Modeling Questioning Task sequencing Fading support Schema building Worked examples Hints Prompts Advance organizers Demonstrations

Interactive

Technological

Motivational

Pairs Small groups Cooperative groups Peer tutors/coach Interviewing Discussion Argumentation Learning stations Reciprocal teaching

Intelligent tutor Online prompts Adaptive questions Online discussion Videos Leveled games Feedback

Choice Autonomy Personalization Self-efficacy Interest Relevance Goal setting Self-regulation

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

429

the teacher coached one student to think more deeply, the relational thinking spread to other members of the group—they scaffolded each other (interactive scaffolding). Other effective teacher supports included praising students for using cognitive strategies and prompting the group to stay on task, not interrupt each other, take turns, and be sure everyone contributed (i.e., motivational scaffolding). One way to scaffold both learning and motivation is to begin a lesson or activity with an advance organizer (Melrose, 2013). This is the provision of introductory material broad enough to encompass all the information that will follow. The organizers can serve three purposes: They direct your attention to what is important in the upcoming material, they highlight relationships among ideas that will be presented, and they remind you of relevant information you already have. Advance organizers fall into two categories, comparative and expository (Mayer, 1984). Comparative organizers activate (bring into working memory) already-existing schemas. They remind you of what you already know but may not realize is relevant to the new topic being discussed. A comparative advance organizer for a history lesson on revolutions might be a statement that contrasts military uprisings with the physical and social changes involved in the Industrial Revolution; you could also compare the common aspects of the French, English, Mexican, Russian, Iranian, Egyptian, and American revolutions (Salomon & Perkins, 1989). In contrast, expository organizers provide new knowledge that students will need to understand the upcoming information. In an English class, you might begin a large thematic unit on rites of passage in literature with a very broad statement of the theme and a brief analysis about why it has been so central in literature—something like “A central character coming of age must learn to know themself, often makes some kind of journey of self-discovery, and must decide what in the society is to be accepted and what should be rejected.” Such an organizer might precede reading novels such as The Adventures of Huckleberry Finn. The general conclusion of research on advance organizers is that they do scaffold student learning, especially when the material to be learned is quite unfamiliar, complex, or difficult—as long as two conditions are met (Langan-Fox et al., 2000; Morin & Miller, 1998). First, for the organizer to be effective, the students must understand it—ask students to paraphrase the organizer to check their understanding. Second, the organizer must really be an organizer: It must indicate relations among the basic concepts and terms that will be used in the upcoming lesson. Concrete models, diagrams, concept maps, charts, timelines, or analogies seem to be especially good organizers (Robinson, 1998; Robinson & Kiewra, 1995). ADVANCE ORGANIZERS.

To identify the teaching practices that best support students’ sensemaking, researchers reviewed studies conducted in classrooms of students from fourth grade through college in subjects as diverse as science, mathematics, history, and literature. The most frequent teacher practice shown to facilitate students’ sensemaking involved asking deep questions that generated productive discussion (Fitzgerald & Palincsar, 2019). To be effective, you first must be sure that all your students have the necessary basic facts and knowledge to think deeply about. Then you can identify questions that prompt students to reason about underlying principles and big ideas in the content, make reasoned arguments, and provide evidence (Pashler et al., 2007). As the next example shows, graphic organizers and deep questions can make this easier. Former high school teacher Doug Lombardi and his colleagues (2018) examined whether using an instructional scaffolding technique, called “model-evidence link,” would improve high school students’ ability to evaluate scientific evidence related to four science topics that have competing explanatory models—climate change, fracking and earthquakes, wetlands and land use, and the formation of the moon. Students were given a graphic organizer with two possible explanations about each phenomenon and four evidence statements, each with

FACILITATING THROUGH ASKING AND ANSWERING DEEP QUESTIONS.

Advance organizer A statement or tool that introduces and summarizes concepts to help students organize the information they will learn about.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Advance Organizers (II, A3) The advance organizer is an important element in many teachercentered/expository approaches to instruction. Be able to explain the role of the advance organizer in these approaches and identify the basic types of organizers.

430 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning corresponding “evidence text.” Students were then asked to complete a sentence frame to evaluate and explain how well each piece of evidence supported each model: Evidence #

strongly supports

Model #

because . . .

supports contradicts has nothing to do with

After evaluating the available evidence, students then rated the plausibility of each model from greatly implausible to highly plausible. The researchers found that this instructional scaffold helped make a complex and abstract topic more manageable and improved students’ ability to evaluate the connection between evidence and alternative explanations for scientific phenomena. It is not easy or natural to ask and answer deep questions. Students have to be supported as they learn these skills. Teachers can help improve the richness of students’ discussions by asking students to clarify their points, drawing attention to explicit strengths in students’ arguments, modeling target strategies, and checking for understanding (Wei et al., 2018). The Guidelines: Facilitating Deep Questioning give additional ideas.

GUIDELINES Facilitating Deep Questioning Encourage students to think aloud when speaking or writing their explanations as they study or discuss a topic. Examples 1. 2.

3.

Present a challenging story and ask students to think aloud to explain as they read the story, linking the story to personal experiences and prior knowledge. Have students respond to each other’s explanations and consider multiple explanations. “Are the reasons behind these explanations based on evidence? Are there other possible explanations?” Where appropriate, use deliberative argumentation (Cluster 9) to reach a collaborative consensus about what is a good explanation. “What is the argument here? How good is the evidence? Does the argument connect with scientific principles? What is missing or weak in the argument?”

Ask questions that require an explanation, not just reciting facts or repeating from the text, to answer. Examples 1.

2.

“How and why would the destruction of bees affect other life on our planet?” “How would the United States have changed if the other candidate had won the presidential elections in 1860, 1952, and 2020?” Provide models of good, deep questions and teach students to distinguish between deep questions that require explanations like those above and superficial questions that only require a factual answer such as “What insect pollinates flowers?” “Who were the two main presidential candidates in 1860, 1952, and 2020?”

Ask questions that challenge students’ prior beliefs and assumptions. Examples 1. 2.

Ask questions that highlight puzzling or paradoxical situations, such as “Why is it good for a forest to periodically experience fires?” Ask questions that make students defend their positions with facts and evidence. “What is the evidence for the position? Does this evidence rule out other possible explanations?”

Source: Based on Pashler, H., Bain, P., Bottge, B., Graesser, A., Koedinger, K., McDaniel, M., & Metcalfe, J. (2007). Organizing instruction and study to improve student learning (NCER 2007–2004). National Center for Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/Docs/PracticeGuide/20072004.pdf https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/Docs/ PracticeGuide/20072004.pdf and Nussbaum, E. M. (2021). Critical integrative argumentation: Toward complexity in students’ thinking. Educational Psychologist, 56(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2020.1845173

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

431

To repeat one of our favorite assertions, teachers are not irrelevant in constructivist classrooms. Rather, they are facilitators and learning environment designers. We have explored facilitation. Now let’s examine instructional designs that draw heavily from constructivist principles: inquiry and problem-based learning and cognitive apprenticeships.

Inquiry-Based Learning John Dewey described the basic inquiry learning format in 1910. Educators have developed many adaptations of this strategy, but the form usually includes the following elements (Dobber et al., 2017; Pedaste et al., 2015). An inquiry learning scenario typically begins with presentation of a puzzling event, question, or problem. The students then:

• Formulate hypotheses to explain the event or solve the problem, • Investigate the problem and test hypotheses through planning, observation, experimentation, and data analysis,

• Draw conclusions, and • Reflect on the original problem and the thinking processes needed to solve it. Often the final state includes sharing findings with others. Many approaches can fall under the umbrella of inquiry-based learning and are named according to their primary focus and scope (e.g., to solve an ill-structured problem, to complete a multi-stage project). This is a general picture of inquiry learning, but what is actually going on? Erin Furtak and her colleagues (2012) categorized the actual activities and processes in science inquiry as being procedural (hands-on, posing scientific questions, doing science procedures, collecting data, graphing or charting data), epistemic (i.e., related to building knowledge by drawing conclusions based on evidence, generating and revising theories), conceptual (connecting to students’ prior knowledge, eliciting students’ mental models and ideas), or social (participating in class discussions, arguing and debating ideas, giving presentations, working collaboratively). When the researchers analyzed 37 studies conducted from 1996 to 2006 that compared inquiry approaches with the traditional teaching of science, they found that the greatest impact on student learning came when the inquiry approach included epistemic activities or a combination of epistemic, procedural, and social activities. So having students collaborate to do hands-on scientific procedures, gather and represent data, draw conclusions, debate ideas, and make presentations was more effective than traditional teacher-centered approaches. Throughout these phases of inquiry, teacher guidance and scaffolding were important. Just letting the students work completely on their own doing experiments was not effective (Furtak et al., 2012). What kind of guidance? Teachers provide metacognitive support by helping students think and act like scientists and monitor their progress, conceptual support by providing information related to the topic, and social support by helping students develop equitable collaborative skills (Dobber et al., 2017). Learners who are young or inexperienced might need additional supports, such as constraints on the scope of the inquiry and frequent prompts or explanations. Even though inquiry-based learning puts students in the driver’s seat as investigators, teacher guidance is directly associated with greater learning (Lazonder & Harmsen, 2016). EXAMPLES OF INQUIRY. Let’s see how teachers might use the principles of inquiry-based learning to help elementary school students learn about communication (see Figure 10.2; Hapgood et  al., 2004; Palincsar et  al., 2001). The teacher first asks this general question: “How and why do humans and animals communicate?” Next, the teacher poses several specific focus questions: “How do whales communicate?” “How do gorillas communicate?” The focus questions have to be carefully chosen to guide students toward important understandings. One key idea in understanding animal communication is the relationships among the animals’ structures (e.g., large ears or echolocators), survival functions (e.g., find food, avoid predators), and habitat (e.g., listening while navigating in the dark). Thus, focus questions must address meaningful differences; questions about animals with the same

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Inquiry Learning (II, A2, 3) Inquiry learning is a studentcentered approach to learning that predates many “traditional” forms of instruction. Describe the basic structure of this approach to learning. What are its strengths and limitations? What roles does the teacher have?

Inquiry learning Approach in which the teacher presents a puzzling situation or question and students solve the problem by gathering data and testing their conclusions.

432 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Figure 10.2 A Model to Guide Teacher Thinking About Inquiry-Based Science Instruction The straight lines show the sequence of phases in instruction, and the curved lines show cycles that might be repeated during instruction. Share Findings: Public Reporting

Engage: Guesses, Claims, Hypotheses

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Discovery Learning (I, A1) Many teachers, especially in mathematics and science, believe that meaningful learning in their content areas is best supported by discovery learning. Be prepared to answer questions about the assumptions, techniques, strengths, and limitations of this instructional strategy.

Problem-based learning Students are confronted with a problem that launches their inquiry as they collaborate to find solutions and learn valuable information and skills in the process.

General Guiding Questions and Specific Focus Questions

Investigate: Identify Relationships

Evaluate: Create Explanations

Prediction

Source: Based on “Designing a community of practice: Principles and practices of the GisML community,” by A. S. Palincsar, S. J. Magnusson, N. Marano, D. Ford, and N. Brown. (1998). Teaching and Teacher Education, 14, p. 12. Adapted with permission from Elsevier.

kinds of structures or habitats would not be good focus points for inquiry (Magnusson & Palincsar, 1995). The next phase is to engage students in the inquiry, perhaps by playing different animal sounds and having students make guesses and claims about communication. Then the students conduct investigations—both firsthand (e.g., measuring the size of cats’ eyes and ears in relation to their bodies) and secondhand (e.g., consulting a website or veterinarian). The students begin to identify patterns. The curved lines in Figure 10.2 show that students might go through several cycles of investigating, identifying patterns, and reporting results before moving on to constructing explanations and making final reports. Notice that inquiry teaching involves plenty of scaffolding that allows students to learn content and process at the same time. In addition, students learned the inquiry process itself—how to solve problems, evaluate solutions, debate ideas, and think critically. PROBLEM-BASED LEARNING. One inquiry-based approach that grew out of research on expert knowledge in medicine is problem-based learning (Belland, 2011). In problem-based learning, students work in groups to confront a real-world, ill-structured problem that has no single correct solution (Belland et al., 2013; Lisette et al., 2014). The goal is for students to develop knowledge that is useful and flexible, not inert. Inert knowledge is information that is memorized but seldom applied (Whitehead, 1929). Project-based learning is similar to problem-based learning, but the projects extend over a longer period of time and may involve multiple problems along the way. The phases of problem-based learning are similar to those followed in inquiry. During the first phase, initial problem discussion, the students identify and analyze the problem based on the facts from the scenario and then determine what they already know. It soon becomes clear that the students will need more information, so they identify learning issues. These questions guide the next phase—individual research on the learning issues. After the individual research phase, students return to their groups to report their research results and collaborate to find solutions. As they suggest hypotheses, apply their new knowledge, and evaluate their problem solutions, they may recycle to research again if necessary and finally reflect

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

on the knowledge and skills they have gained. Throughout the entire process, students are not alone or unguided. Their thinking and problem solving are scaffolded by the teacher, online resources, models, coaching, expert hints, guides and organizational aids, or other students in the collaborative groups—so working memory is not overloaded. For example, as students work, they may have to fill in a diagram that helps them distinguish between “claims” and “reasons” in a scientific argument or create a shared document divided into four sections where they list facts, ideas, learning issues, and action plans (Hmelo-Silver, 2003, Hmelo-Silver et al., 2007). In true problem-based learning, the problem is real and the students’ actions matter. For example, during the 2010 Deepwater Horizon oil spill, many teachers used the problem as a springboard for learning. Their students researched how this spill compared to others in size, location, expense, causes, and attempted solutions. What could be done? How do currents and tides play a role? What locations, businesses, and wildlife are in the greatest danger? What will be the short-term and long-term financial and environmental impacts? What actions can students take to play a positive role? A number of teachers blogged about using the oil spill in problembased learning and collected resources for other teachers (see http://www.edutopia.org and search for “oil spill” to find more resources). Let’s look at these phases more closely as they might take place in an upper-level science class (Klein & Harris, 2007). 1. The cycle begins with an intriguing challenge to the whole class. For example, in biomechanics it might be “Assume you are a living cell in a bioreactor. What things will influence how long you live?” or “Your grandmother is recovering from a broken hip. In which hand should she hold the cane to help her balance?” The question is framed in a way that makes students bring to bear their current knowledge and preconceptions. 2. Next, students generate ideas to compile what they currently know and believe and what they need to know more about using individual, small-group, or whole-group brainstorming or other activities. 3. Students research the topic individually. Multiple perspectives are added to the process in the form of outside experts (live, on video, or from texts), websites, magazine or journal articles, or podcasts on the subject. 4. Students go deeper to research and revise. They consult more sources or listen to class lectures, all the while revising ideas and perhaps journaling about their thinking. 5. Students test their mettle by getting feedback from other students or the teacher about their tentative conclusions. Some formative (ungraded) tests might check their understanding at this point. 6. Students come back to their groups, discuss conclusions, and go public with their final conclusions and solutions in the form of an oral presentation, poster/project, or final exam. The teacher’s role in problem-based learning is to identify engaging problems and appropriate resources; orient students to the problem by describing objectives and rationales; organize the students’ work by helping them set goals and define tasks; support, coach, and mentor students as they gather information, craft solutions, and prepare artifacts (e.g., models, reports, videos, presentation slides, portfolios); and support student reflection on their own learning outcomes and processes (Arends & Kilcher, 2010). Does using inquiry projects or problem-based learning activities lead to greater achievement? Researchers have debated this question for years. Some research results say “yes” (Belland et al., 2013; Chen & Yang, 2019; Furtak et al., 2012; Loyens et al., 2015), and other results suggest that they do not (Jerrim et al., 2019). The mixed evidence is partly a question of what is being learned, what type of inquiry-based approach is being used, and how learning outcomes are being measured (Hung et al., 2019). Not every educational psychologist agrees that inquiry-based learning is valuable, at least for all students, as you can see on the next page in the Point/ Counterpoint: Are Teaching Approaches to Support Inquiry- and Problem-Based Learning Effective?

DOES INQUIRY-BASED INSTRUCTION IMPROVE LEARNING?

433

434 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Are Teaching Approaches to Support Inquiry- and Problem-Based Learning Effective? Teaching for inquiry- and problem-based learning is very appealing, but is it effective? Specifically, do inquiry- and problem-based teaching approaches lead to deep understanding and robust knowledge for most students?

Point Inquiry-based learning is overrated. Paul Kirschner and his colleagues were clear and critical in their article in the Educational Psychologist. Even the title of the article was blunt: “Why Minimal Guidance During Instruction Does Not Work: An Analysis of the Failure of Constructivist, Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based Teaching.” They argued: Although unguided or minimally guided instructional approaches are very popular and intuitively appealing, the point is made that these approaches ignore both the structures that constitute human cognitive architecture and evidence from empirical studies over the past half-century that consistently indicate that minimally guided instruction is less effective and less efficient than instructional approaches that place a strong emphasis on guidance of the student learning process. (Kirschner et al., 2006, p. 75) These respected researchers (and others more recently) cited decades of research demonstrating that unguided discovery/inquiry and problem-based learning are ineffective, especially for students with limited prior knowledge (Kalyuga, 2011; Klahr & Nigam, 2004; Tobias, 2010). A nationally representative study of 15-year-old science students in England similarly showed that, regardless of students’ prior achievement levels, “High levels of inquiry or unguided inquiry have no relationship with attainment at all” (Jerrim et al., 2019, p. 42). Louis Alfieri and his colleagues (2011) examined the results from 108 studies going back over 50 years and found that explicit teaching was more beneficial than unassisted discovery, especially for studies published in the most well-rated journals. Their conclusion: “Unassisted discovery generally does not benefit learning” (p. 12). Similar evidence from over 300 studies conducted between 1966 and 2016 showed that direct instruction led to higher academic achievement (Stockard et al., 2018). The researchers concluded that direct instruction works because it provides learners with clear, unambiguous examples that ensure understanding. Students do not waste time feeling confused or, worse, reaching false conclusions from having too little guidance. Also, you may remember from Cluster 5 that a thorough review by the leading researchers studying specific learning disabilities concluded, “There is little evidence that children with SLDs benefit from discovery, exposure, or constructivist instructional approaches” (Grigorenko et al., 2020, p. 48). But what about problem-based learning in particular? Much of the research on problem-based learning has taken place in medical schools, and results have been mixed (Hung et al., 2019). For example, one meta-analysis showed that, although students learning through problem-based instruction were better at practical medical skills, graduated sooner, and reported greater well-being, they had relatively small gains in their medical knowledge (Schmidt et al., 2009).

Counterpoint Approaches that focus on inquiry can be powerful tools for learning. When problembased learning was used in science classes in Grades K–8, students’ science achievement and conceptual development improved (Merritt et al., 2017). Second-grade students whose teachers were randomly assigned to implement project-based approaches in social studies and literacy showed more than five months of learning gains compared to their peers in

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

traditional learning formats (Duke et al., 2021). In a study of almost 20,000 middle school students in a large urban district who used inquiry-based materials, those who participated in inquiry learning had significantly higher passing rates on standardized tests. African American boys especially benefited from these methods (Geier et al., 2008). In studies of high school economics and mathematics, research favors problem-based approaches for learning more complex concepts and solving multistep word problems. Advanced Placement (AP) U.S. government and environmental science students who were randomly assigned to “Knowledge in Action” inquiry-based projects earned higher AP scores compared to students who received traditional instruction (Saavedra et al., 2021). Several other studies have pointed to increases in student engagement and motivation with inquiry learning (Hmelo-Silver et al., 2007), as long as the learning is supported and students have adequate background knowledge. In sum, Cindy Hmelo-Silver (2004; Hmelo-Silver et al., 2007) reviewed the research and found good evidence that problem-based learning supports the construction of flexible knowledge and the development of problem-solving and self-directed learning skills, but less evidence indicates that participating in problem-based learning is intrinsically motivating or that it teaches students to collaborate.

Beware of Either/Or The difference among effective and ineffective inquiry, project-based learning, and problembased learning seems to come down to completely unguided discovery versus guided, supported, and well-scaffolded learning. Alfieri and his colleagues (2011) concluded: [O]ptimal approaches should include at least one of the following: (a) guided tasks that have scaffolding in place to assist learners, (b) tasks requiring learners to explain their own ideas and ensuring that these ideas are accurate by providing timely feedback, or (c) tasks that provide worked examples of how to succeed in the task. (p. 13) But to make the matter a bit more complicated, evidence shows that the value of guidance and feedback can depend on the student’s prior knowledge or age. For example, in learning mathematics problem-solving strategies, students with little knowledge benefited from feedback as they explored possible solutions, but students with some knowledge benefited more from just exploring solutions independently without feedback and guidance (Fyfe et al., 2012). On the other hand, first-grade students learned certain basic mathematics reasoning skills better during unguided discovery learning via computer than from direct instruction—perhaps an example of the value of unstructured play for young children (Baroody et al., 2013). For an example of project-based learning successes, check out http://pblworks.org for videos and resources.

You don’t have to choose between inquiry and content-focused methods. The best approach in K–12 schools may be a balance of contentfocused and inquiry or problem-based methods (Lisette et al., 2014). For example, third- and fourth-grade students who were learning how to use the “control of variables strategy” when designing science experiments were randomly assigned to different teaching approaches: minimal guidance, some direct guidance, teacher modeling and direct guidance, or teacher modeling. Although all students improved, students who received direct guidance and modeling from their teachers performed the best (Martella et al., 2020). The bottom line is that inquiry learning can be effective for helping students learn to solve ill-structured problems if appropriate scaffolding and teacher facilitation are available. Teachers can select and limit the number of research materials for the individual research phase, provide models of questions and answers about the learning issues identified, offer guidance about the characteristics of a good solution, and give feedback along the way (Belland, 2017; Lisette et al., 2014).

BEING SMART ABOUT INQUIRY LEARNING.

435

436 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning Remember that strong inquiry learning approaches support learners’ motivation and identity development in addition to their learning—which should be the goal of all instruction. The Buck Institute for Education (2019) recommends that inquiry designs include support for student voice and choice and connect authentically to students’ “own concerns, interest, cultures, identities, and issues in their lives” (p. 3). Participatory designs that directly involve students foster the co-evolution of students’ learning and identities (Hand & Gresalfi, 2015). You should always encourage learners to connect what they are learning to their out-of-school interests, cultures, and funds of knowledge (Glazewski & Ertmer, 2020). Above all, teachers should remember that using clever projects is not helpful if content standards are unmet, learning is superficial, or students are not engaged in disciplinary thinking (Grossman et al., 2019). Another constructivist approach that relies on scaffolding and student participation is cognitive apprenticeships.

Cognitive Apprenticeships

Cognitive apprenticeship A relationship in which a less experienced learner acquires knowledge and skills under the guidance of an expert.

Over the centuries, apprenticeships have proved to be an effective form of education. By working alongside a master and perhaps other apprentices, young people have learned many skills, trades, and crafts. Knowledgeable guides provide models, demonstrations, and corrections as well as a personal bond that is motivating. The performances required of the learner are real and important and grow more complex as the learner becomes more competent (Collins, 2006; Hung, 1999; Linn & Eylon, 2006). With guided participation in real tasks comes participatory appropriation—students appropriate the knowledge, skills, and values involved in doing the tasks (Rogoff, 1995, 1998). In addition, both the newcomers to learning and the old-timers contribute to the community of practice by mastering and remastering skills—and sometimes improving these skills in the process (Lave & Wenger, 1991). Barbara Rogoff and her colleagues (2016) have suggested that, in contrast to the assembly-line model of learning common in our recent history of formal schooling, the archives of human history are full of culturally embedded forms of learning in the world beyond school. These forms often involve something Rogoff (2016) has called “Learning by Observing and Pitching In”—a type of apprenticeship known to indigenous communities in which learning takes place in families and communities with the goal of enabling learners’ increasing participation and contribution to the community over time. How can schools adapt some of the features of apprenticeships? Rather than (or in addition to) showing learners how to sculpt or dance or build a cabinet, apprenticeships could focus on cognitive objectives such as reading comprehension, writing, or mathematical problem solving. There are many cognitive apprenticeship models, but most share these six features:

• Students observe an expert (usually the teacher) model the performance. • Students get external support through coaching or tutoring (including hints, tailored feedback, models, and reminders).

• Students receive conceptual scaffolding, which is then gradually faded as the student becomes more competent and proficient.

• Students continually articulate their knowledge—putting into words their understanding of the processes and content being learned.

• Students ref lect on their progress, comparing their problem solving to an expert’s performance and to their own earlier performances.

• Students are required to explore new ways to apply what they are learning—ways that they have not practiced at the master’s side. As students learn, they are challenged to master more complex concepts and skills and to perform them in many different settings.

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

How can teachers provide cognitive apprenticeships? Developing activities that remove traditional hierarchies from learning is a good start. Promoting intergenerational learning or working with a community member with more expertise fosters “relational equity” and can provide many of the qualities of an apprenticeship (DiGiacomo & Penuel, 2018). At the STEAM Academy in Lexington, Kentucky, public high school students take part in community-based internships in which they learn a craft or a discipline. Some high schoolers spend part of their school day working in research labs or design studios on the University of Kentucky’s campus alongside undergraduate and graduate students and their professors. The internship culminates in a public presentation of findings. Projects have addressed diverse topics such as community recycling, radio production, horse grooming, organic gardening, architecture, and financial planning. Many levels of expertise are evident in these participatory apprenticeships, so students can move at a comfortable pace but still have the model of a master available. School-based apprenticeships can involve multi-age groups or community volunteers and parents who can demonstrate a variety of cultural practices. Another advantage of apprenticeships is that they help build skills in collaborative problem solving (Graesser et al., 2018). Key word—collaborative.

Module 31 Summary Cognitive and Social Constructivism (pp. 420–427) Describe two kinds of constructivism. Constructivism is more a philosophy about knowledge than a scientific theory of learning. Cognitive constructivists such as Piaget are concerned with how individuals make sense of their world based on individual knowledge and beliefs. Social constructivists such as Vygotsky believe that social interaction, cultural tools, and activity shape individual development and learning. By participating in a broad range of activities with others, learners appropriate the outcomes produced by working together; they acquire new strategies and knowledge of their world. In what ways do constructivist views differ about knowledge sources, accuracy, and generality?  Constructivists debate whether knowledge is constructed by mapping external reality, by adapting and changing internal understandings, or by an interaction of external forces and internal understandings. Most psychologists believe there is a role for both internal and external factors but differ in how much they emphasize one or the other. Also, there is discussion about whether knowledge can be constructed in one situation and applied to another or whether knowledge is situated—specific and tied to the context in which it was learned. What is meant by thinking as enculturation? Enculturation is a broad and complex process of acquiring knowledge and understanding consistent with Vygotsky’s theory of mediated learning. Just as our home culture taught us lessons about the use of language, the culture of a classroom can teach lessons about thinking by giving us models of good thinking, providing direct instruction in thinking processes, and encouraging practice of those thinking processes through interactions with others. What are some common elements in most constructivist views of learning? Even though there is no single constructivist theory, many constructivist approaches recommend complex, challenging learning environments and authentic tasks; social negotiation and co-construction; multiple perspectives and representations of content; understanding that knowledge is constructed; and student ownership of learning.

Designing Constructivist Learning Environments (pp. 427–437) What are some basic assumptions to guide the design of learning environments? Key assumptions to guide design are that experts develop deep conceptual knowledge, learning comes from the learner, creating learning environments is the responsibility of the school, students’ prior knowledge is key, and reflection is a critical component of learning. These common assumptions enable researchers from a variety of disciplines to address the same issues of learning from a variety of perspectives.

437

438 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning What is scaffolding?  Scaffolding involves making connections between teachers’ cultural knowledge and the everyday experience and knowledge of the student by providing supports for learning—prompts, cues, needed information, coaching, and so on. Scaffolding includes both motivational and cognitive support—helping students stay engaged and interested while also helping them move toward deeper learning. Motivational support includes supports such as connecting to student interests, redirecting attention, and coping with frustration. Cognitive supports include tailoring instruction, fading supports, and gradually transferring responsibility to students. How can teachers facilitate using advance organizers and deep questioning? Advance organizers facilitate instruction by providing introductory material broad enough to encompass all the information that will follow. The organizers can serve three purposes: They direct your attention to what is important in the upcoming material, they highlight relationships among ideas that will be presented, and they remind you of relevant information you already have. Teachers also can facilitate learning by training students in how to ask and answer deep questions when they read, listen to a lecture, or participate in class discussions. These questions prompt students to reason about underlying principles and big ideas in the content, make reasoned arguments, and provide evidence. What are some examples of inquiry methods and problem-based learning?  Inquiry approaches begin when the teacher presents a puzzling event, question, or problem. The students ask questions (only yes–no questions in some kinds of inquiry) and then formulate hypotheses to explain the event or solve the problem, collect data to test the hypotheses about casual relationships, form conclusions and generalizations, and reflect on the original problem and the thinking processes needed to solve it. Problem-based learning may follow a similar path, but the learning begins with an authentic problem—one that matters to the students. The goal is to learn math or science or history or some other important subject while seeking a real solution to a real problem. Describe features that most cognitive apprenticeship approaches share.  Students observe an expert (usually the teacher) model the performance; get external support through coaching or tutoring; and receive conceptual scaffolding, which is then gradually faded as the students become more competent and proficient. Students continually articulate their knowledge—putting into words their understanding of the processes and content being learned. They reflect on their progress, comparing their problem solving to an expert’s performance and to their own earlier performances. Finally, students explore new ways to apply what they are learning—ways that they have not practiced at the master’s side.

MODULE 32

Collaboration and Cooperation in Teaching and Learning

Learning Objective 10.4 Appropriately incorporate collaboration and cooperative learning in your classes.

Collaboration and Cooperation Even with all the concern today about test performance and international comparisons, schooling has always been about more than academic learning. Of course, academics are the prime directive, but the ability to collaborate to solve problems is a core 21st-century skill (Fiore et al., 2018). At the turn of the century, Elliot Aronson (2000) observed that: Most corporations are looking for employees who are not only good at the mastery of a particular set of academic skills but who also have the ability to work harmoniously with a wide variety of coworkers as a cooperative team, to demonstrate initiative and responsibility, and to communicate effectively. (p. 91)

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Aronson may not have realized that rapid digital advances would bring collaborative communities to our own fingertips. From Zoom to Facetime to Google, much of our interactive communication takes place online; we will examine this in detail later in this cluster. Big shifts in the types of interactive environments available to learners have not stopped researchers from being interested in what occurs when students work together in groups. The terms group work, collaboration, and cooperative learning often are used as if they mean the same thing. Certainly there is some overlap, but there are important distinctions as well.

Learning in Groups

439

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Characteristics of Cooperative Learning (II, A2) Many instructional strategies labeled as cooperative learning lack one or more qualities that are essential components of such techniques. List those essential qualities, and explain the role of each.

Stop & Think What comes to mind when you hear “group work”? In your own learning experience, when did group-based learning help you the most? When did it help the least? Many school activities can be completed in groups. Group work simply refers to several people working together on an assigned learning task—they may or may not be cooperating (Cohen & Lotan, 2014). For example, students can divide up the territory where they will be conducting a local survey and discuss how people feel about the plan to build a new entertainment complex that will bring more fun activities but more traffic. Or, to learn 10 new terms in a biology class, students can create a shared Google slideshow where they each define two terms with words and graphics to “teach” one another the definitions. Group work can be useful, but, as we will see, true cooperative learning requires much more than simply putting students in groups and dividing up the work. For example, Angela O’Donnell and Jim O’Kelly, two of Anita’s colleagues from Rutgers University, described a teacher who claimed to be using “cooperative learning” by asking students to work in pairs to write a paper, each taking one part. Unfortunately, the teacher allowed the students no time to work together and provided no guidance or preparation in cooperative social skills. Students got a grade for their individual part and a group grade for the whole project. One student received an A for his part but a C for the group project because his partner earned an F; he never turned in any work. So one student was punished with a C for a situation he could not control while the other was rewarded with a C for doing no work at all. This was not collaborative or cooperative learning—it wasn’t even group work (O’Donnell & O’Kelly, 1994). How does this experience align with your group-based learning experiences? Although the goals of group work can be to quickly accomplish a task by dividing labor, most teachers who assign group work hope that their students will collaborate with each other. Collaboration can be viewed as a philosophy about how to relate to others—how to learn and work (Panitz, 1996). Collaboration is a way of dealing with people that respects differences, shares authority, and builds on the knowledge that is distributed among other people. Collaborative learning has roots in the work of British teachers who wanted their students to respond to literature in more active ways as they learned. Cooperation, on the other hand, is a way of working with others to attain a shared goal (Gillies, 2003). Cooperative learning has American roots in the work of psychologists John Dewey and Kurt Lewin. You could say that cooperative learning is one way to collaborate in schools. Some scholars believe that learning to successfully collaborate is an important skill in itself and necessary for success in the future (Lawlor et al., 2018). Other educators claim that collaboration is a means for learning academic material—so we could say the two goals are learning to collaborate and collaborating to learn (Kuhn, 2015). Of course, teachers don’t have to choose. Both goals are valuable.

GOALS OF GROUP WORK.

Without careful planning and monitoring by the teacher, group interactions can hinder learning and reduce rather WHAT CAN GO WRONG: MISUSES OF GROUP LEARNING.

Collaboration A philosophy about how to relate to others—how to learn and work together.

Cooperation Way of working with others to attain a shared goal.

440 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning than improve social relations in classes (Gillies & Boyle, 2011; Kuhn, 2015). For example, if there is pressure in a group for conformity, interactions can be unproductive and unreflective. Without feedback about accuracy, misconceptions might be reinforced, or the worst, not the best, ideas may be combined to construct a superficial or even incorrect understanding (Asterhan et al., 2014). Students who work in groups but arrive at wrong answers may be more confident that they are right—a case of “two heads are worse than one” (Puncochar & Fox, 2004). Also, social or cultural status might interfere with group processes. The ideas of low-status students may be ignored or even ridiculed while the contributions of high-status students are accepted and reinforced, regardless of the merit of either set of ideas (Anderson et al., 1997; Gillies, 2014). Mary McCaslin and Tom Good (1996) mentioned several other possible disadvantages of group learning:

• Students often value the process or procedures over the learning. Speed and finishing early take precedence over thoughtfulness and learning.

• Socializing and interpersonal relationships may take precedence over learning. • Students may simply shift dependency from the teacher to the “expert” in the group; learning is still passive, and what is learned can be wrong.

• Status differences may be increased rather than decreased. Some students learn to “loaf” because the group progresses with or without their contributions. Others become even more convinced that they are unable to understand without the support of the group. Deanna Kuhn (2015) sums up the situation: “It is not enough to put individuals in a context that allows collaboration and expect them to engage in it effectively. Intellectual collaboration is a skill, learned through engagement and practice and much trial and error” (p. 51). Unless teachers pay careful attention to task design and support for cooperation, their students probably won’t benefit from collaborative activities (van Leeuwen & Janssen, 2019). What are the features of a truly cooperative learning environment? How can you ensure that all learners benefit? We turn to that next.

Learning Through Cooperation

Cooperative learning Situations in which elaboration, interpretation, explanation, and argumentation are integral to the activity of the group and where learning is supported by other individuals.

Throughout this book, you have read about different theories of learning that would support the use of cooperative learning, but for different reasons (O’Donnell, 2002, 2006). Information processing theorists point to the value of group discussion in helping participants rehearse, elaborate, and expand their knowledge. As group members question and explain, they have to organize their knowledge, make connections, and review—all processes that support information processing and memory. Advocates of a Piagetian perspective suggest the interactions in groups can create the cognitive conflict and disequilibrium that lead individuals to question their understanding and try out new ideas (Brown & Palincsar, 2018). By encountering people who hold opposing views, learners are left to reevaluate their own perspectives—or, as Piaget (1985) said, “to go beyond [their] current state and strike out in new directions” (p. 10). Those who favor a sociocultural constructivist approach, such as Vygotsky’s theory, suggest that social interaction is important for learning because higher mental functions such as reasoning, comprehension, and critical thinking originate in social interactions and are then appropriated and internalized by individuals (McCaslin & Vriesema, 2018). Students can accomplish mental tasks with social support before they can do them alone. Thus, cooperative learning provides the social support and scaffolding students need to move learning forward. To benefit from these dimensions of cooperative learning, groups must be cooperative—all members must participate. David and Roger Johnson (2009a), two of the founders of cooperative learning in the United States, define formal cooperative learning as “students working together, for one class period to several weeks, to achieve shared learning goals and complete jointly specific

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

441

Figure 10.3 Five Elements of Cooperative Learning Groups David and Roger Johnson (2009a) described five elements foundational to successful cooperative learning groups.

Positive interdependence

Promotive interaction

Individual accountability

Collaborative and social skills

Group processing

tasks and assignments” (p. 373). The Johnson brothers described five elements that define true cooperative learning groups, which are depicted in Figure 10.3. In true cooperative groups, group members experience positive interdependence. The members believe they can attain their goals only if the others in the group attain their goals as well, so they need each other for support, explanations, and guidance. Promotive interaction means that group members encourage and facilitate each other’s efforts. They usually interact face-to-face and close together, not across the room, but they also could interact remotely with others around the world. Even though they feel a responsibility to the group to work together and help each other, students must ultimately demonstrate learning on their own; they are held individually accountable for learning, often through individual tests or other assessments. Collaborative and social skills are necessary for effective group functioning. These skills include paying attention and listening even if you don’t agree, disagreeing and sharing your ideas respectfully, taking turns, doing your fair share, working with partners even if they are not your choice, asking for/providing help, giving constructive feedback, reaching consensus, involving every member, encouraging and praising, controlling emotions and frustrations, cheering up partners, and admitting mistakes, among other skills (Ladd et al., 2014a, 2014b). These skills do not necessarily come naturally, even for socially capable students. Often they must be taught and practiced before the groups tackle a learning task (see tips for promoting social and emotional learning in Clusters 4 and 11). Finally, members monitor group processes and relationships to make sure the group is working effectively and to learn about the dynamics of groups. They take time to ask, “How are we doing as a group? Is everyone working together? What should we do more or less of next time?” Engaging in these cooperative learning practices involves co-regulated learning—the process of distributing the responsibility for regulating learning, motivation, behaviors, and emotions to all members of the group (Hadwin et al., 2018). There is a lot to co-regulate in a group process (I am thinking of a long faculty meeting I had recently!). Co-regulation might involve helping each other clarify goals, follow procedures, manage disagreements, remain on task, check in on progress, seek tools and resources,

Co-regulated learning Process of sharing the regulation of learning, behaviors, motivation, and emotions among group members.

442 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning and, during remote meetings, monitor the Zoom chat, just to name a few (McCaslin & Vriesema, 2018). EFFECTS OF COOPERATIVE LEARNING. Cooperative learning has a long history in American education, moving in and out of favor over the years. Today, our evolving, interconnected society coupled with the complex problems we face require a steady commitment to cooperative learning and collaboration (Fiore et al., 2018). Cooperative learning approaches are now used “in schools and universities throughout most of the world in every subject area and from preschool through graduate school and adult training programs” (D. Johnson & R. Johnson, 2009a, p.  365). Research supports this wide use. Despite some inconsistencies, the majority of the studies on cooperative learning indicate that truly cooperative groups have positive effects—from preschool to college— on students’ empathy, tolerance for differences, feelings of acceptance, friendships, selfconfidence, awareness of the perspectives of others, higher-level reasoning, problem solving, decision making, essay writing, and even school attendance (Galton et al., 2009; Gillies & Boyle, 2011; Inns & Slavin, 2018; Kyndt et al., 2014; Roseth et al., 2008; Solomon et al., 2001). Experimental evidence has strengthened these conclusions. Consider one study in which fifth-graders from 36 classes were randomly assigned to a Wolf Reintroduction and Management Learning Unit taught either in a collaborative group work format or through direct instruction (Zhang et al., 2016). Students who worked in collaborative groups met to discuss the “‘big question’—whether the community should be permitted to hire professional hunters to kill wolves” (p. 201) and smaller questions related to it. Students assigned to direct instruction learned about wolves through whole-class discussion and activities. Students who learned collaboratively scored higher on the three outcomes related to decision-making skills: recognizing two sides of a dilemma, reasoning, and weighing the reasons by how important they were. About 80% of elementary school teachers and 62% of middle school teachers use some form of peer learning on a regular basis (Ladd et al., 2014b). However, some evidence suggests that students around the globe are not gaining strong skills in collaborative problem solving. The Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD, 2017) administered a computerized problem-solving simulation to test the collaborative problem-solving skills of 15-year-old students from 52 countries. Only 8% of students were top performers on the test. What can you do to help strengthen your students’ collaboration skills?

How can teachers encourage true cooperation and avoid the pitfalls of less cooperative group work? As with any instructional activity, to ensure successful cooperative learning, teachers have to plan before; monitor, support, and consolidate learning during; and reflect on learning after the cooperative learning activity, as shown in Table 10.3. We will examine the teacher’s role more closely next.

TEACHER’S ROLE IN COOPERATIVE LEARNING.

Designing Cooperative Learning Tasks Like so many other decisions in teaching, plans for using cooperative groups begin with a goal. What are students supposed to accomplish? What is the task? Is it a true group task— one that builds on the knowledge and skills of several students—or is the task more appropriate for individuals (Cohen, 1994; O’Donnell, 2006)? As we will see, tasks for cooperative groups can be more or less structured. HIGHLY STRUCTURED, REVIEW, AND SKILL-BUILDING TASKS. Highly structured tasks include work that has specific answers—drill and practice, applying routines or procedures, answering questions from readings, computations in mathematics, and so on. A relatively structured task such as reviewing previously learned material for an exam might be well served by a structured technique such as student teams achievement divisions (STAD), in which teams of four students compete to determine which team’s members can amass the greatest

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Table 10.3 The Teacher’s Role in Cooperative Learning Here are some examples of teacher competencies needed for successful cooperative learning. The teacher plans, monitors, supports, consolidates, and finally reflects. BEFORE THE ACTIVITY

DURING THE ACTIVITY

Plan/Design

Monitor

Support

Consolidate

Reflect

• Group composition • Goals/Tasks • Materials • Roles/Scripts • Instructions • Planned assessments

• Determining which interactions will lead to learning in this task • Watching for explanations, questions, challenges, shared information, elaboration, and affirmations

• Scaffolding and fading • Prompts, cues • Resources at different levels • Encouragement • Feedback • Praise • Guiding questions

• Class presentations • Compare/ contrast solutions • Whole-class discussion • Individual testing

Revisit learning goals and plans:

AFTER THE ACTIVITY

• What did monitoring reveal? • Was the support appropriate? • Did students learn? • What changes should be made for the next time?

Source: Based on Kaendler, C., Wiedmann, M., Rummel, N., & Spada, H. (2015). Teacher competencies for the implementation of collaborative learning in the classroom: A framework and research review. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 505–536.

improvement over previous achievement levels (Slavin, 2017). Praise, recognition, or extrinsic rewards can enhance motivation, effort, and persistence under these conditions, and thus increase learning. Focusing the dialogue by assigning narrow roles also may help students stay engaged when the tasks involve practice or review. Ill-structured complex tasks have multiple answers and unclear procedures, requiring problem finding and higher-order thinking, which you learned about in Cluster 9. These ill-structured problems are true collaborative tasks—they are likely to require the resources (knowledge, skills, problem-solving strategies, creativity) of all the group members to accomplish. These tasks require more and higher-quality interactions than routine tasks if learning and problem solving are to occur (Cohen, 1994; Gillies, 2004; Gillies & Boyle, 2011). As we have already seen, strategies that encourage extended and productive interactions are appropriate when the goal is to develop higher-order thinking, reasoning, and problem solving. In these situations, a tightly structured process, competition among groups for rewards, and rigid assignment of roles are likely to inhibit the richness of the students’ interactions and to interfere with progress toward the goal. Open-ended techniques such as reciprocal questioning (Brown & Palincsar, 2018) and Jigsaw (Aronson, 2000) should be more productive because, when used appropriately, they encourage more extensive interaction and elaborative thought in situations where students are being exposed to complex materials. In these instances, the use of rewards may divert the group from the goal of in-depth cognitive processing especially if the goal becomes achieving the reward as efficiently as possible rather than supporting all group members’ understanding (Roseth et al., 2019).

ILL-STRUCTURED, CONCEPTUAL, AND PROBLEM-SOLVING TASKS.

When the goal of collaborative learning is to enhance social skills or increase intergroup understanding and appreciation of diversity, the assignment of specific roles and functions within the group might support communication (Cohen, 1994; Kagan, 1994). In these situations, it can be helpful to rotate leadership roles so that all students have the opportunity to demonstrate and develop leadership skills. This structure enables all group members to distribute power and to experience the leadership capabilities of each individual (Miller & Harrington, 1993). Rewards probably are not necessary, and they may actually get in the way because the goal is to build community, a sense of respect, and responsibility for all team members. When the group’s goal is to reach consensus, students learn more (Tenenbaum et al., 2020).

SOCIAL SKILLS AND COMMUNICATION TASKS.

443

444 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning GIVING AND RECEIVING EXPLANATIONS. Students who ask questions, get answers, and attempt explanations are more likely to learn than students whose questions go unasked or unanswered. In fact, evidence shows that the more a student provides elaborated, thoughtful explanations to other students in a group, the more the explainer learns. (I, Ellen, never learned more about U.S. history than when I had to explain it to my fifth-grade students!) Yes, for deep learning, giving good explanations appears to be even more important than receiving explanations (O’Donnell, 2006). To explain an idea clearly, you have to organize the information, put it into your own words, think of examples and analogies (which connect the information to things you already know), and test your understanding by answering questions. These are excellent learning strategies (Fiorella & Mayer, 2016; Hoogerheide et al., 2019; King, 2002). Good explanations are relevant, timely, correct, and elaborated enough to help the listener correct misunderstandings; the best explanations tell why (Webb et al., 2002; Webb & Mastergeorge, 2003). For example, in a middle school mathematics class, students worked in groups on problems like this:

Find the cost of playing a video game for 20-minutes when the first minute costs $0.25 and each additional minute costs $0.11. The level of explanation and help students gave to their peers was significantly related to learning; the higher the level, the more the learning. At the highest level, the helper told how to solve the problem and why. For example, a helper explaining the problem above might say, “OK, it is 25 cents for the first minute, then there are 19 minutes left and each of those minutes costs 11 cents, so you multiply 11 cents by 19. That equals $2.09—then add 25 cents for the first minute so it costs $2.34.” A poor explanation might be just giving the solution, “11 times 19 plus 25,” or even just providing the answer—“I got $2.34.” If a helper says, “11 times 19,” then the receiver should say, “Why is it 19?” or “Why do you multiply by 11?” Asking good questions and giving clear explanations are critical, and usually these skills must be explicitly taught or modeled.

Setting Up Cooperative Groups How large should a cooperative group be? A meta-analysis of 71 studies showed that group size was unrelated to how much students learned during interactive activities with peers (Tenenbaum et al., 2020). However, the best choice probably depends on your learning goals. If the goal is for the group members to review, rehearse information, or practice, four to five or six students is about the right size. But if the goal is to encourage each student to participate in discussions, problem-solve, or collaborate at one computer, then groups of two to four members work best. Let’s look at a few other factors you might want to consider when setting up cooperative groups. In general, heterogeneous groups—those that are diverse in multiple ways—are best for supporting learning and social skills. Therefore, when setting up cooperative groups, it often makes sense to balance the group by what you know about your students (e.g., personality, ability, background, special skills). For instance, when quiet or shy students are outnumbered in a group, they tend to be left out of the discussions. By contrast, when there are only one or two assertive or outspoken students in the group, they tend to dominate. Establishing rules of engagement, such as “No person speaks twice until each person has spoken once,” can help distribute the talk time. If a group includes some students who are perceived as different or who are often rejected, then be sure that the group includes members who are patient and kind. One successful teacher interviewed by Gillies and Boyle (2011) put it this way:

GROUP COMPOSITION.

I also try to make sure that there are one or two people in the group who have the ability to be tolerant. At least the kid in question will know that, while the other group members may not be his best friends, they won’t give him a hard time. I try to put the least reactive kids in the group with the child in question. This year I’ve had a couple of girls who have been very good with difficult kids. They don’t put up with nonsense but they don’t over-react and are prepared to demonstrate some good social skills. (p. 72)

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Table 10.4 Possible Student Roles in Cooperative Learning Groups Depending on the purpose of the group and the age of the participants, having these assigned roles might help students cooperate and learn. Of course, students may have to be taught how to enact each role effectively, and roles should be rotated equitably so that students can participate in different aspects of group learning. ROLE

DESCRIPTION

Encourager

Encourages reluctant or shy students to participate

Supporter

Shows appreciation of others’ contributions, and recognizes accomplishments

Gate Keeper

Equalizes participation, and makes sure no one dominates

Coach

Helps with the academic content, explains concepts

Question Commander

Makes sure all students’ questions are asked and answered

Checker

Checks the group’s understanding

Taskmaster

Keeps the group on task

Recorder

Writes down ideas, decisions, and plans

Reflector

Keeps group aware of progress (or lack of progress)

Quiet Captain

Monitors noise level

Materials Manager

Picks up and returns materials

Source: Adapted from Cooperative learning by S. Kagan. Published by Kagan Publishing. Copyright © 1994 by Kagan Publishing.

Grouping students by ability level must also be done with care. Fourth- and fifth-grade students who were grouped homogeneously by reading ability performed worse on a small-group reading comprehension activity than did those grouped heterogeneously (Murphy et al., 2017). On the other hand, in some circumstances, homogeneous grouping might be helpful (Steenbergen-Hu et al, 2016). Whatever the case, teachers should select grouping practices according to their goals and monitor groups to make sure everyone is contributing and learning. ASSIGNING ROLES. Some teachers assign roles to students to encourage cooperation and full participation. Several roles are described in Table 10.4. If you use roles, be sure that they support learning. In all groups, and particularly those that focus on social skills, roles should support listening, encouragement, and respect for differences. In groups that focus on practice, review, or mastery of basic skills, roles should support persistence, encouragement, and participation. In groups that focus on higher-order problem solving or complex learning, roles should encourage thoughtful discussion, sharing of explanations and insights, probing, brainstorming, and creativity. Make sure that you communicate to students that the major purpose of the groups is to support their learning, not simply to perform their roles and follow the procedures. Roles are supports for learning, not ends in themselves (Woolfolk Hoy & Tschannen-Moran, 1999). Often, cooperative learning strategies include group reports to the entire class. If you have been on the receiving end of these class reports, you know that they can be deadly dull. One strategy is to give non-presenters meaningful roles to guide their listening. For example, audience members could assume an investigative listener role in which they predict and theorize during listening, summarize results, and/or relate their predictions and theories to the results in a “report out.” Research shows that using roles like these promotes class dialogue and conceptual understanding (Palincsar & Herrenkohl, 2002). In my classes (Ellen here), I ask audience members to be “supportive critics” (Brown & Palincsar, 2018, p. 414). I provide a sheet of paper or a Google doc on which students in the audience type questions and feedback for each presenter as they listen. This benefits both the listener and the presenter. Although cooperative learning has been found to enhance learning in a variety of cultural contexts, students from different cultural backgrounds may approach group work with different values related to processes such as communication, leadership, and interpersonal trust (Inns & Slavin, 2018). Not everyone will have the same ideas about what it means to work cooperatively. Modeling and setting clear guidelines can help.

445

446 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Figure 10.4 Question Stems to Encourage Dialogue in Reciprocal Questioning After participating in a lesson or studying an assignment on their own, students use these stems to develop questions, create and compare answers, and collaborate to create the best response. What is an everyday application of

?

in your own words?

How would you define

?

What are the advantages and disadvantages of ?

What do you already know about applies to

Explain why

.

influence

How does

?

?

What is the value of What are the reasons for

? ?

What are some arguments for and against

? Your second choice?

What is your first choice about What if Compare How would

? Why?

and why? and

based only on

be different if

Do you agree or disagree with this claim

.

? ? What is your evidence?

Examples of Cooperative Learning Techniques We have established that developing deep understanding of content when working in cooperative groups requires that all the group members participate in high-quality discussions that include interpretations, connections, explanations, and evidence supporting claims. We now turn to different cooperative learning structures that build in these strategies. In addition to these approaches, you might check out Spencer Kagan’s many cooperative learning structures and activity workbooks designed to accomplish different kinds of academic and social goals. Reciprocal questioning Students work in pairs or triads to ask and answer questions about lesson material.

Jigsaw classroom A learning process in which each student is part of a group and each group member is given part of the material to be learned by the whole group. Students become “expert” on their piece and then teach it to the others in their group.

Reciprocal questioning requires no special materials or testing procedures and can be used with a wide range of ages. After a lesson or presentation by the teacher, students work in pairs or triads to ask and answer questions about the material (King, 1990, 1994, 2002). The teacher provides question stems (see Figure 10.4), and then students are taught how to develop specific questions about the lesson material using the stems. Students create questions and then take turns asking and answering. This process has proved more effective than traditional discussion groups because it seems to encourage deeper thinking about the material as well as forming connections between the lesson and previous knowledge or experience.

RECIPROCAL QUESTIONING.

JIGSAW. Elliot Aronson and his graduate students invented the Jigsaw classroom when Aronson was a professor of social psychology and I (Anita) was a student at the University of Texas at Austin. Some of my friends worked on his research team. Aronson (2000) developed the approach “as a matter of absolute necessity to help defuse a highly explosive situation” (p. 137). The Austin schools had just been desegregated by court order. White, Black, and Latino students were together in classrooms for the first time. Hostility and turmoil ensued, with fistfights in corridors and classrooms. Aronson’s answer was the Jigsaw classroom. In Jigsaw, each group member is given a “piece” of the material (let’s call them pieces A, B, C, and D) to be learned by the whole group. Students become “experts” on their piece. Because students have to learn and be tested on every piece of the larger “ABCD puzzle,”

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

everyone’s contribution is important—the students truly are interdependent. Another version, Jigsaw II, adds expert consultation groups. Imagine that your class has five groups, each with four group members who are responsible for teaching about their “piece” (A, B, C, or D). In Jigsaw II, new “expert” groups are formed based on the particular piece of the puzzle being learned. For example, each original group sends its “As” to one new expert group, its “Bs” to another expert group, and so on. These experts confer to make sure they understand their assigned part and to plan ways to teach the information to their original learning group. Students then return to their original learning groups with greater expertise. In the end, students take an individual test covering all the material and earn points for their learning team score (Aronson, 2000; Slavin, 1995). More recent evidence has shown that Jigsaw methods sometimes activate competitive and individualistic tendencies that impede the goals of collaborative learning (Roseth et al., 2019). Therefore, teachers should be sure to emphasize the interdependence goal that all members learn all of the material. Assigning group members to check the accuracy of fellow members’ understanding can help monitor progress toward the group goal. Constructive conflict resolution is essential in classrooms because conflicts are inevitable and even necessary both for learning and for creating a peaceful and just society. In fact, David Johnson, whose ideas you’ve read about in this section, became interested in the topic of constructive conflict resolution after meeting students taking part in the lunch counter sit-ins in Greensboro, North Carolina, in protest of racial discrimination in 1961. His hope was “that racism would be ended most effectively through intervening in the socialization of the children, adolescents, and young adults” (Tjosvold et al., 2020, p. 351). Johnson helped teachers “stir up intellectual conflict” by selecting topics that have two well-documented positions (for and against) to engage students more deeply and maintain their interest (Johnson, 2015, p. 130). Piaget’s theory similarly tells us that developing knowledge requires disequilibrium—cognitive conflict. In fact, research over the last 40 years demonstrates that constructive/structured controversy— engaging cooperative groups in a discussion around a topic that involves a structured intellectual conflict—can lead to greater learning, open-mindedness, seeing the perspectives of others, creativity, motivation, engagement, and self-esteem (Johnson & Johnson, 2009b; Roseth et al., 2011). Let’s look at an example provided by D. W. Johnson and R. Johnson (2009b): In U.S. history, students are learning about the role of civil disobedience in society. The ultimate goal is for students to reach consensus and write a critical essay articulating the pros and cons of the issue. The teacher begins by explaining to students that “in numerous instances, such as in the civil rights and antiwar movements in the United States, individuals have wrestled with the issue of breaking the law to redress a social injustice” (p. 40). The class might briefly identify such instances such as the Black Lives Matter protests or the U.S. Capitol riots on January 6, 2021. The teacher raises the guiding question “Is civil disobedience in a democracy constructive or destructive?” (p. 40). One way to structure the lesson is by establishing four-person collaborative learning groups with two subgroup pairs. Each pair is tasked with developing and advocating for the best case possible for one side of the issue (e.g., the constructiveness or the destructiveness of civil disobedience in a democracy). After thorough research, each pair persuasively presents their side. Then the group engages in “spirited” open discussion, using as many facts as possible to support their position. Student pairs then reverse positions and present evidence from the opposing side. Finally, the group seeks consensus by creating a joint, integrated perspective on the issue. Along the way, the teacher can play devil’s advocate or use questions to deepen critical thinking. Final reports and tests cover the full range of pros and cons about the topic. Teachers using this technique should establish both academic objectives and social skills objectives. Table 10.5 provides strategies for successful interpersonal engagement during discussion of controversies.

CONSTRUCTIVE/STRUCTURED CONTROVERSIES.

447

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Forms of Cooperative Learning (II, A2) STAD and Jigsaw are just two of many cooperative learning techniques, each designed for certain instructional purposes. For cooperative learning activity ideas that are especially appropriate for older students, go to the Center for Teaching Excellence at Cornell University (http://teaching.cornell. edu) and search for “collaborative learning” or to the University of Tennessee at Chattanooga (http:// www.utc.edu) and search for “cooperative learning.”

Constructive/Structured controversy Students work in cooperative groups to research a particular controversy.

448 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Table 10.5 Strategies for Civil Discourse During Controversy or Conflict STRATEGY

HOW?

1. I am critical of ideas, not people.

• I challenge and refute the ideas of the other participants while confirming their competence and value as individuals. • I do not indicate that I personally reject them.

2. Separate my personal worth from criticism of my ideas

• Recognize that even when people critique my ideas, I am still valued.

3. Remember that we are all in this together, sink or swim.

• Focus on reaching the best decision possible, not on winning. • Listen to everyone’s ideas, even if I do not agree.

4. Encourage everyone to participate and to master all the relevant information.

• I want to support other people’s understanding of the issue. • Restate what someone has said if it is not clear.

5. Differentiate before I try to integrate.

• First, I bring out all ideas and facts supporting both sides and clarify how the positions differ. • Then I try to identify points of agreement and put them together in a way that makes sense.

6. Try to understand both sides of the issue.

• I try to see the issue from the opposing perspective to understand. • I change my mind when the evidence clearly indicates that I should do so.

7. Emphasize rationality in seeking the best possible answer, given the available data.

• I help everyone remember that we might not have all the data on the issue, but we can reason with what we have.

8. Follow the golden rule of conflict: I act toward opponents as I would have them act toward me.

• I want the opposing pair to listen to me, so I listen to them. • I want the opposing pair to include my ideas in their thinking, so I include their ideas in my thinking. • I want the opposing pair to see the issue from my perspective, so I take their perspective.

Source: Based on Johnson, D. W., & Jonson, R. T. (2009b). Energizing learning: The instructional power of conflict. Educational Researcher, 38(1), pp. 42–43. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X08330540

Reaching Every Student: Using Cooperative Learning Wisely Cooperative learning always benefits from careful planning, and meeting the needs of diverse learners requires extra attention and preparation. The Guidelines: Using Cooperative Learning give you ideas for using cooperative learning with all your students. For example, cooperative structures such as scripted questioning and peer tutoring depend on a balanced interaction between the person taking the role of questioner or explainer and the student who is answering or being taught. In these interactions, you want to see and hear explaining and teaching, not just telling or giving right answers. But many students with learning disabilities have difficulties understanding new concepts, so both the explainer and the student can get frustrated, and social rejection for the student with learning disabilities might follow. Because students with learning disabilities often have problems with social relations, it is not a good idea to put them in situations where more rejection is likely. So, when you are teaching new or difficult-to-grasp concepts, cooperative learning might not be the best choice for students with learning disabilities (Kirk et al., 2006; Smith, 2006).

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

GUIDELINES Using Cooperative Learning Fit group size and composition to your learning goals. Examples 1. 2.

3.

For social skills and team-building goals, use groups of two to five, common interest groups, mixed groups, or random groups. For structured, fact- and skill-based, practice-and-review tasks, use groups of four to six, mixed-ability such as high-middle and middle-low or high-low and middle-middle group compositions. For higher-level conceptual and thinking tasks, use groups of two to four; select members to encourage interaction.

Assign appropriate roles. Examples 1. 2. 3.

For social skills and team-building goals, assign roles to monitor participation and conflict; rotate leadership of the group. For structured fact- and skill-based, practice-and-review tasks, assign roles to monitor engagement and ensure low-status students have resources to offer, as in Jigsaw. For higher-level conceptual and thinking tasks, assign roles only to encourage interaction, divergent thinking, and extended, connected discourse, as in debate teams, or to assign a group facilitator. Don’t let roles get in the way of learning.

Make sure you assume a supporting role as the teacher. Examples 1. 2. 3.

For social skills and team-building goals, be a model and encourager. For structured fact- and skill-based, practice-and-review tasks, be a model, director, or coach. For higher-level conceptual and thinking tasks, be a model and facilitator.

Move around the room, and monitor the groups. Examples 1. 2. 3.

For social skills and team-building goals, watch for listening, turn taking, encouraging, and managing conflict. For structured fact- and skill-based, practice-and-review tasks, watch for questioning, giving multiple elaborated explanations, attention, and practice. For higher-level conceptual and thinking tasks, watch for questioning, explaining, elaborating, probing, divergent thinking, providing rationales, synthesizing, and using and connecting knowledge sources.

Start small and simple until you and the students know how to use cooperative methods. Examples 1. 2. 3.

For social skills and team-building goals, try one or two skills, such as listening and paraphrasing. For structured fact- and skill-based, practice-and-review tasks, try pairs of students quizzing each other. For higher-level conceptual and thinking tasks, try reciprocal questioning using pairs and just a few question stems.

For more information on cooperative learning, see http://www.co-operation.org. Source: Based on Implications of cognitive approaches to peer learning for teacher education, by A. Woolfolk Hoy and M. TschannenMoran, 1999. In A. O’Donnell and A. King (Eds.), Cognitive perspectives on peer learning. Lawrence Erlbaum.

Advanced students also may not benefit from cooperative learning when groups are mixed in ability. The pace often is too slow, the tasks too simple, and there is just too much repetition. In addition, students with exceptionally high ability often fall into the role of teacher or end up just doing the work quickly for the whole group. If you use

449

450 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning mixed-ability groups, the challenges are to use complex tasks that allow work at different levels and keep advanced students engaged without losing the rest of the group (Smith, 2006). Cooperative learning may be an excellent choice for students who are English learners (ELs), however. The Jigsaw cooperative structure is especially helpful because all students in the group, including the students who are ELs, have information that the group needs, so they also must talk, explain, and interact. In fact, the Jigsaw approach was developed in response to the need to create high interdependence in diverse groups. In many classrooms today, four to six or more languages are represented. Teachers can’t be expected to master every heritage language spoken by all of their students every year. In these classrooms, cooperative groups can help as students work together on academic tasks. Students who speak two languages can help translate and explain lessons to others in the group. Speaking in a smaller group may be less anxiety provoking for students who are learning another language, so the students who are ELs may get more language practice with feedback in these groups (Smith, 2006).

Module 32 Summary Collaboration and Cooperation (pp. 438–450) What are the differences between collaboration and cooperation? One view is that collaboration is a philosophy about how to relate to others—how to learn and work. Collaboration is a way of dealing with people that respects differences, shares authority, and builds on the knowledge that is distributed among other people. Cooperation, on the other hand, is a way of working together with others to attain a shared goal. Describe five elements that define true cooperative learning. Promotive interaction means that group members encourage and facilitate each other’s efforts. They usually interact face-to-face and close together, but they also could interact via digital media around the world. Group members experience positive interdependence; they need each other for support, explanations, and guidance. Even though they work together and help each other, members of the group must ultimately demonstrate learning on their own. They are held individually accountable for learning, often through individual tests or other assessments. If necessary, the collaborative skills important for effective group functioning, such as giving constructive feedback, reaching consensus, and involving every member, are taught and practiced before the groups tackle a learning task. Finally, members monitor group processes and relationships to make sure the group is working effectively and to learn about the dynamics of groups. What are the learning theory underpinnings of cooperative learning, and what can go wrong?  Learning can be enhanced in cooperative groups through rehearsal and elaboration (information processing theories), creation and resolution of disequilibrium (Piaget’s theory), or scaffolding of higher mental processes (Vygotsky’s theory). But if there is pressure in a group for conformity—perhaps because rewards are being misused or one student dominates the others— interactions can be unproductive and unreflective. Without feedback about accuracy, misconceptions might be reinforced, or the worst, not the best, ideas may be combined to construct a superficial or even incorrect understanding. Students who work in groups but arrive at wrong answers may be more confident that they are right. Also, the ideas of low-status students may be ignored or even ridiculed while the contributions of high-status students are accepted and reinforced, regardless of the merit of either set of ideas. How should tasks match design in cooperative learning? Teachers have a role before, during, and after cooperative learning. First, they must select an appropriate task. A relatively structured task works well with a structured technique; extrinsic rewards can enhance motivation, effort, and persistence under these conditions; roles, especially those that focus attention on the work to be accomplished, also may be productive. On the other hand, strategies that encourage extended and productive interactions are appropriate when the goal is to develop higher-order thinking and problem solving. The use of rewards may well divert the group away from the goal of in-depth cognitive processing. When the goal of peer learning is enhanced social skills

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

or increased intergroup understanding and appreciation of diversity, the assignment of specific roles and functions within the group might support communication. Rewards probably are not necessary and may actually get in the way because the goal is to build community, a sense of respect, and responsibility for team members. What are some possible strategies for cooperative learning? Strategies include reciprocal questioning, Jigsaw, and constructive/structured controversy, but there are many other cooperative structures. But cooperative learning is not for everyone. Sometimes students with learning disabilities and students with more advanced knowledge do not benefit from cooperative learning.

MODULE 33

Technology in Teaching and Learning

Learning Objective 10.5 Describe positive and negative influences of technologymediated instruction on how children and adolescents develop and learn.

Designing Interactive Digital Learning Environments It seems that computers, smartphones, iPads, tablets, digital readers, wikis, and interactive video games, along with Twitter, Google, WhatsApp, TikTok, Snapchat, Instagram, Facebook, Slack, and other digital tools and media, have changed life for everyone. Homes and schools are filled with digital media. Many of your students will have already had years of experience using digital tools to play and learn even from early ages. Their immersion in technology-enhanced games may make traditional school activities seem tame and boring by comparison (Common Sense Media, 2013; Connor-Zachocki et al., 2015; Graesser, 2013; Lenhart, 2015; Turkle, 2011). When COVID-19 hit, even the most routine social exchanges went digital, and the primary methods of teaching and learning did too. Stop & Think How many digital interactions do you have in a typical day? What makes those interactions important and meaningful in your life? What opportunities have your digital interactions made possible? What, if anything, is lost through digital communication? For many students, doing homework often involves exchanging messages with friends via text, watching online videos for help, searching the Internet for information, and downloading resources—all while streaming a favorite playlist. Your students are unlikely to have ever known a world without digital media, so they have been called digital natives, homo zappiens, the Net generation, iGenerations, or Google Generation (Kirschner & van Merriënboer, 2013). Students are at least as expert at using technology as their teachers, and most of this expertise was acquired outside school (Graesser, 2013). But teachers are increasingly integrating technology into their lesson plans, often using constructivist principles as a guide.

Technology and Learning Does technology use support academic learning? The answer is complex because technologies are vast and ever-changing, as are the environments in which they are used and the skills of those using them. Research methods also differ across studies. Consider several findings from recent reviews:

• Computers are more likely to increase achievement if they support the basic processes that lead to deep learning: active engagement, frequent interaction with feedback, authenticity and real-world connection, scaffolding support, worked examples and problem-solving opportunities, and productive group work (Belland et al., 2017; Graesser, 2020a; Jackson et al., 2006; Tamim et al., 2011).

451

452 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning • Being in a one-to-one laptop program (students who have laptops assigned to them) is associated with higher achievement in math, English, science, and writing and more student-centered and project-based instruction (Zheng et al., 2016). Students also show greater enthusiasm for using devices like laptops, tablets, and mobile devices (e.g., Mulet et al., 2019).

• Basic reading processes, such as word decoding or phonological awareness, or basic number sense can be facilitated by specialized software that can provide individual feedback, advance at the right pace for each student, and increase motivation. Well-designed programs also can improve listening and reading comprehension (Baroody et al., 2013; Potocki et al., 2013; Savage et al., 2013).

• Computer-supported collaborative learning—collaborative learning that takes place with the help of technological tools such as video conferencing, discussion boards, games, and chats—can increase learning in science, technology, engineering, and math (Jeong et al., 2019). Scaffolding techniques such as scripts can increase the effects on learning and result in better collaboration skills (Vogel et al., 2017). What can we conclude? We can have some faith in these findings but remember that every review has limitations and counterexamples in the literature. Ultimately, like any teaching tool, digital devices and supports can be effective if used well, but just having access to them will not automatically increase academic achievement.

Technology-Rich Environments With all the technology available today, interest in technology-rich learning environments is growing. These environments include virtual worlds, computer simulations that support problem-based learning, intelligent tutoring systems, augmented reality activities, educational games, video creation, wearable devices, and artificial intelligence (like Siri and Alexa)—to name just a few. Let’s consider several ways in which technology can be useful in schools. First, teachers can design technology-based activities for their classrooms, for remote learning environments, or for blended models using both in-class and remote environments. Second, students can interact with technologies in a variety of ways, such as by using a computer or tablet to complete assignments or by collaborating in a virtual environment with other students or teachers using interactive applications. Cloud-based applications, such as Google documents, allow users to simultaneously access, edit, and store files, as long as they have an Internet connection. Finally, administrators use technology to track teacher, class, and student information in school, district, or statewide systems. You could be involved with any or all of these uses of technology in your teaching. A golden rule for technology integration in any classroom is that you do not need to reinvent the wheel. Focus on identifying centers of expertise where existing resources are available to adapt and build on. The proliferation of phones, tablets, and other portable devices has led to new opportunities for (almost) anywhere, anytime learning. The learning “environment” has become more dynamic than ever. Mobile learning refers to “learning across multiple contexts, through social and content interactions, using personal electronic devices” (Crompton, 2013, p. 4). But one question teachers should care about is whether the use of mobile technology leads to deep, transformational learning. Helen Crompton and Diane Burke (2020) attempted to find out. They analyzed 186 studies published between 2014 and 2019 to understand the effects of mobile technologies on students’ learning in grades PreK–12. They categorized the studies along a continuum from least to most transformational for learning. Figure 10.5 provides examples of classroom activities they reviewed from least to most transformational. More than half of the studies found that mobile technologies significantly promoted deep learning. Some education scholars have pointed to positive, and even transformational, ways that teachers could harness the power of personalized learning through mobile technologies. But others are tentative in their enthusiasm. Because our devices enable learning to

MOBILE LEARNING.

Mobile learning Learning that takes place across contexts through the use of personal electronic devices.

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

453

Figure 10.5 Do Mobile Technologies Enhance Learning? Helen Crompton and Diane Burke (2020) classified 186 studies that involved mobile technology devices in PreK–12 settings based on the degree to which technology use enhanced learning. This figure shows the percentage of studies that were in each learning outcome category. Stars reflect a more transformational use of technology. Examples from the research reviewed are included. Learning was the same. Technology was a substitution.

Technology transformed and redefined learning. • Fifth-graders conducted chemistry experiments, videorecorded findings, created stories, remixed the content, and shared with others.

22%

Learning was significantly modified through technology. • Augmented reality enabled students to locate stars, study water cycles, examine aquatic animals, and study human anatomy. • Speech-generating device helped students with autism. • QR codes were used in outdoor learning environments.

8%

• Preschoolers read a book using an e-reader.

Learning was somewhat enhanced by technology.

38% 32%

• Special education students used a math app to drill and practice with feedback. • First-graders used a vocabularybuilding app to learn to read and write new words.

Source: Based on Mobile learning and pedagogical opportunities: A configurative systematic review of PreK–12 research using the SAMR framework, by H. Crompton and D. Burke, 2020, Computers and Education. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2020.103945. Graphic created by Ellen Usher.

occur in multiple contexts and learners to interact with other people and with content in novel ways, understanding best practices for how to use them will be complicated (Danish & Hmelo-Silver, 2020). Richard Mayer (2020) suggested that teachers make learning outcomes, not technology, their primary focus. Mayer noted that few rigorous scientific experiments have focused on the effects of mobile devices on learning. In addition, teachers and learners will need to know how to put their mobile tools to optimal use (Bernacki et al., 2020). Virtual learning environments (VLEs) are technology-based platforms that allow learning in a variety of ways. The most traditional VLE, and one you are likely familiar with, is a learning management system (LMS). Most LMSs, such as Blackboard, Canvas, and D2L, are large, complex, and costly. Most sites have spaces for class readings, discussion groups, class-built wikis, slide presentations, a calendar, and many other resources. The LMS has expanded our teaching and learning options. During the initial months of the COVID-19 pandemic, most teachers had to learn how to build their own virtual classrooms—a new way of presenting content, scaffolding instruction, and interacting with learners. Teachers use a variety of virtual platforms to support instruction and learning. Betty’s Brain is an example of a clever VLE developed by Vanderbilt and Stanford Universities (go to http://www.vanderbilt.edu and search for “Betty’s Brain” for more information). In this virtual environment, students are challenged to learn a science topic so well that they can “teach” the topic to a computer agent known as Betty. The platform provides links to resources for the students to use when learning and planning their instruction. (Remember the research on cooperative learning shows that the explainer learns more than the listener; O’Donnell, 2006.) Like all good teachers, the students who are working with Betty must keep track of how well she is learning by asking Betty to answer questions and take quizzes. The platform also includes an expert on the topic, Mr. Davis, who grades Betty’s work and mentors the student “teachers.” See Figure 10.6 for an example screen from Betty’s Brain. Many virtual learning environments become personal learning environments (PLE) by providing tools that support individualized learning in a variety of contexts and

VIRTUAL LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS.

Virtual learning environments (VLEs) Technology-based platforms that allow learning in a variety of ways.

Learning management system (LMS) Systems that deliver e-learning, provide tools and learning materials, keep records, administer assessments, and manage learning.

Personal learning environment (PLE) Virtual platform that provides tools that support individualized learning in a variety of contexts and situations.

454 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Figure 10.6 Betty’s Brain: A Virtual Learning Environment Betty’s Brain is a computer-based learning environment that uses learning-by-teaching to engage students in learning about science topics.

Source: Based on http://www.teachableagents.org/research/bettysbrain.php.

Personal learning network (PLN) Framework in which knowledge is constructed through online peer interactions.

Immersive virtual learning environment (IVLE) A simulation of a real-world environment that immerses students in tasks like those required in a professional practicum.

situations. Learners assume control of how and when their learning occurs. Learning can be asynchronous—taking place anytime and anywhere. Students can download an assignment at a coffee shop, read the material on the bus, and then post an analysis on the discussion board at 4:00 a.m. from their room. Complex PLEs include tools that assess learners’ knowledge and then adapt the next content to fit their unique needs. Tools that support personal learning include computer-based training modules, e-books, cognitive tutors, videos by experts, content-creation platforms, quizzes, and self-assessment tools. Many have been very successful. For example, the intelligent tutoring system Cognitive Tutors teaches mathematics in schools throughout the United States (Graesser, 2013). Math is one thing, but how about more ill-structured domains like writing? An intelligent tutor can help here, too. In one study with 10th-graders, Writing Pal improved essay writing, although the students found some aspects of the system annoying (Roscoe & McNamara, 2013). We might think of our own online interactions with peers as our personal learning network (PLN). Our PLNs can consist of both synchronous (real-time) and asynchronous interactions. In K–12 schools, PLNs can be used for instruction and also as a resource for professional development. Outside of school, PLNs might be based around our social networking tools such as Facebook, Twitter, Edutopia, and EdWeb. These tools allow learning to move outside the school, city, and even country as we learn with others with similar interests (and sometimes opposing views!) around the globe. You benefit from these networks too—many great teaching ideas and resources shared by other teachers are available for free through the Open Educational Resources (https://www.oercommons.org/). The most complex VLE is an immersive virtual learning environment (IVLE). The IVLE is a simulation of a real-world environment. The purpose of an IVLE is to learn in an interactive environment that simulates a real space or scenario, for example, by being eco explorers in the rainforest or reporters covering a story about an outbreak of food poisoning in a local school (Bagley & Shaffer, 2009; Gee, 2008; Gibson et al., 2006; Shaffer et al., 2009). Some IVLEs are designed to mimic tasks required in a professional practicum. For example, high school students took part in a virtual internship called PurpleState in which they served as political media strategists. The internship helped students collaborate with others to understand multiple viewpoints around controversial IMMERSIVE VIRTUAL LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS.

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

455

political issues affecting their local community and raised students’ self-efficacy for civic engagement (Chen & Stoddard, 2020). Internship activities involved designing a media campaign proposal for special interest groups for or against a particular issue, thus blending real-world engagement in a virtual scenario. These and similar immersive environments often include cognitive tutors, either through live mentors or computer-programmed agents who provide prompts or feedback after analyzing the student’s behaviors. A meta-analysis of diverse studies that included one type of IVLE—an augmented reality component (i.e., an enhancement of the visual field for learning using a heads-up visual display) has shown positive effects on students’ learning (Garzón & Acevedo, 2019). Other evidence has shown that IVLEs may increase students’ enjoyment and engagement but do not always result in learning gains (Makransky et al., 2019, 2020; Mayer, 2019). What about computer-based educational games? A quick glance at people’s gaming behavior outside school shows how engaging well-designed games can be. One estimate is that American students ages 8 to 18 spend an average of 13 hours a week playing video games—some more and some less, but some up to 30 hours a week. Often they are playing with others who are not in the same room or even the same time zone. Over 50% of adults also play video games—you may be one of them (Arena, 2015; Lenhart, 2015). Many researchers suggest that games provide a natural, engaging form of learning and that “combining games with educational objectives could not only trigger students’ learning motivation, but also provide them with interactive learning opportunities” (Sung & Hwang, 2013, p. 44). When designed well, interactive games can be useful conduits for learning because they engage cognitive skills such as problem solving, build social skills such as cooperation and communication, and enhance motivation through goal setting, intermittent reinforcement, and interest (Adachi & Willoughby, 2017; and see Cluster 12). Games might include a knowledge base developed by experts, a challenge to students in the form of a quest or role-play, and a final product such as creating a database, report, design, or problem solution. Games typically involve failure and setback, too, so they encourage students to persist by learning new skills from others and practicing. Much of game learning happens vicariously as gamers study the performances of experts. As you can see, many of these features could help learners construct (and co-construct) new knowledge and skills. For example, Minecraft is one of the most popular games that enables learners to build meaningful content. This flexible program enables players to create worlds of their own by arranging blocks or play inside prebuilt worlds (e.g., Ming Dynasty, Sustainability City, Financial Literacy World). Players can interact, and teachers can monitor their whereabouts in the game. Minecraft has been used in diverse ways to enhance curricular learning such as when students have built or examined Civil War sites (social studies), city plans (math), animal cells (science), and storyboards (literature) (Baek et al., 2020). Massive multi-player online games (MMOs) are interactive gaming environments constructed in virtual worlds in which a large number of people—sometimes millions— play simultaneously. Many MMOs are role-playing games (RPGs) in which the learner assumes a character role of an avatar—a new persona that they create or customize. Many of these games have affinity groups—online communities that share knowledge, strategies, role-play scenarios, game modifications, or fan fiction stories and novels based on the games. These groups provide practice in problem solving, communication, reading, and writing. In fact, students who don’t seem to be good readers in school can be very capable readers of even complex text when they are trying to improve their gaming skills— a goal that makes sense to them. The University of Wisconsin Casual Learning Lab is using the wildly popular World of Warcraft as part of an after-school learning program. See http://wowinschool.pbworks.com for some ideas (King, 2015). But even with the promise of digital games, there is no guarantee that students will learn from all educational games or transfer their learning to situations outside the game (Jabbar & Felicia, 2015; Mayer, 2019; Ownston, 2012; Roschelle, 2013). The effectiveness of games on learning depends on how well games are designed for learning (de Freitas, 2018). Pieter Wouters and his colleagues in the Netherlands (2013) analyzed 38 studies of serious

GAMES.

Massive multi-player online games (MMOs) Interactive gaming environments constructed in virtual worlds with many players who may assume character roles or avatars.

Affinity groups Online communities for videogame users where they can share knowledge, strategies, role-play scenarios, game modifications, or fan fiction stories and novels based on the games they are playing.

456 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning educational computer games conducted from 1990 to 2012. They concluded that games were more effective than traditional instruction in terms of learning and retention, but not in terms of motivation. Students participating in games learned more when the games were supplemented with other forms of instruction, when there were multiple sessions, and when the students worked in groups. In addition, a synthesis of research with K–12 students concluded that games designed to enhance learning, particularly games that included scaffolding that adapted the game to students’ individual needs and interests, were more effective than teaching approaches that did not include games (Clark et al., 2016). As a teacher, you may not have access to well-designed games that match or support your curriculum. How can you capitalize on students’ extensive gaming outside school? One suggestion is to think of games as ways of “tilling the soil,” getting students ready for learning by developing prior knowledge (Arena, 2015). For example, teachers have developed semester-long projects based on the game series Civilization. Another suggestion is to “gamify” certain elements of learning—“adding game elements in order to change existing learning processes” (Sailer & Homner, 2020, p. 78). Gaming elements that add fictional aspects (world, characters) and that involve students in interactive collaboration appear to improve learning (Sailer & Homner, 2020). Even though not all your students will play the same games, you still can develop lessons around the games they do play. For instance, a language arts teacher might assign a persuasive essay requiring gaming enthusiasts to convince the rest of the class to play their favorite game. This means that students write based on personal expertise with games. See Arena (2015) for more ideas.

Computational Thinking and Coding Computational thinking The thought processes involved in formulating problems so you can represent their solution steps and algorithms for computing.

Using technology for learning and for life has become so pervasive for all ages that some educators argue students should learn computational thinking, defined as “knowing how to use data, models, simulations, and algorithmic thinking to formulate and solve problems” (Malby et al., 2017, p. 160). Computational thinking requires thinking like a computer scientist. According to the national K–12 Computer Science Teachers Association (2017), computational thinking is a skill that students should begin to develop in elementary school (http://www.csteachers.org). Computational thinking includes programming and coding, machine learning, and artificial intelligence (AI). Four decades ago, Seymore Papert (1980, 1991, 1993) introduced LOGO programming and turtle graphics for children. Since then, interest in teaching students to code has grown. Some educators have argued that programming teaches students to think logically in all areas, but others have said that programming just helps students learn to program. Even so, there is ongoing interest in and advocacy for the value of coding as a way to foster computational thinking. Courses in coding, machine learning, and AI are growing in popularity in K–12 schools around the globe (Rich et al., 2019; Tikva & Tambouris, 2021; and see the K–12 Computer Science Framework: https://k12cs.org). Students can examine the algorithms by which machines are able to “learn” from data inputs and make user recommendations—this is how Netflix, Hulu, and YouTube offer us a menu of shows we might like (check out the 2020 film The Social Dilemma). Understanding cybersecurity, ethics, and equity is increasingly critical. These would be excellent topics for inquiry-based collaborative learning. Tools and games related to teaching younger children about computational thinking are also exploding. Simple programming tools available today include Scratch, Alice, GameMaker, Kodable Pro, Cargo-Bot, Kodu, Daisy the Dinosaur, and Greenfoot. There is even a version of Scratch (developed by MIT) called Scratch Jr. that will allow children from kindergarten through second grade to program (de Ruiter & Bers, 2021)! Many of these programming languages allow even very young students to build programs by snapping together images of blocks on the computer screen. The way the blocks snap together controls actions of different characters on the computer screen (Grover & Pea, 2013). Costumes, sounds, colors, and other effects can be added. Table 10.6 provides other resources for learning to code.

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Table 10.6 Resources for Teaching Coding ORGANIZATION

WEBSITE

Girls Who Code

https://girlswhocode.com

Code.org

https://code.org

mBlock

https://mblock.makeblock.com

Technology Education and Literacy in Schools

https://www.tealsk12.org

Made with Code from Google

https://www.madewithcode.com

Minecraft Education Edition

https://education.minecraft.net

Cognimates (AI platform for building games, robots, and AI models)

https://cognimates.me

Google AI Experiments

https://experiments.withgoogle.com/collection/ai

Coding in the Classroom, Edutopia

https://www.edutopia.org/topic/coding-classroom

Source: Based on Malby et al. (2017). Transforming learning with new technologies (3rd ed.). Pearson; Touretzky et al. (2019). Envisioning AI for K–12: What should every child know about AI? 33rd AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence, AAAI 2019, 9795–9799. https://doi.org/10.1609/aaai.v33i01.33019795

Media Use, Digital Citizenship, and Media Literacy With the acceleration of digital media comes a new concern with literacy—digital and media literacy. Today, to be literate—to be able to read, write, and communicate—children have to read and write in many media, not just printed words. Information abounds from news, films, videos, websites, social media, photographs, artwork, magazines, music, TV, billboards, and more. How do children read these messages? What media are appropriate for children? What is your responsibility as a teacher to promote responsible digital use and literacy? DEVELOPMENTALLY APPROPRIATE ACTIVITIES. Digital media are appealing, but are they appropriate for younger learners, such as preschool children? And how much screen time is appropriate for children and adolescents? These are hotly debated issues. In making decisions about using any technological program or tool with your students, ask these four questions (Bullard, 2017): (1) Is it the best tool for the job? (2) Will it produce added value to the activity? (3) Is the activity itself beneficial to the child? (4) Are the benefits worth the costs? Using these questions will help you identify developmentally appropriate ways to use technology with young children. As a general rule, computers should not be used to do solitary drill-and-practice activities. Software for young children should include simple spoken directions; the activities should be open-ended and encourage discovery, exploration, problem solving, understanding of cause and effect, and social interaction. Children should be able to remain in control of the activities through a variety of responses. Figure 10.7 provides a related checklist to help you evaluate whether certain software is appropriate for young children. There is another important consideration: Do the program’s multimedia features (e.g., embedded videos, music, added sounds, images) add to learning or take away from it? One danger is that programs will include attractive visuals or sound effects that actually interrupt and interfere with the development of important concepts. For example, do the sounds of a buzz saw and the thud of a falling tree in a Peter Rabbit storytelling program foster distractibility and interfere with understanding the story, plot, and characters? Maybe (Tsantis et al., 2003). Multimedia elements should focus on meaning and not just provide attractive “bells and whistles” that can add to extraneous cognitive load. One final consideration has to do with the amount of technology use. Screen time has increased among all age groups over the past decade. What might be the costs? Jean Twenge and her colleagues (2018) investigated the psychological well-being of over a million 8th-, 10th-, and 12th-graders from 1996 to 2016. They noticed a sharp decline in students’ well-being in 2012 in conjunction with similarly sharp increases in screen time (social media, Internet, texting) and decreases in activities not involving digital devices (e.g., sports/exercise, hanging out socially). Adolescents who were the happiest were

457

458 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Figure 10.7 Checklist for Selecting Appropriate Software for Young Children When screening software, consider the following criteria: ❑ Does the software have high educational or informational value? Is value added by using the software? Or would this information be better presented in a different format? ❑ Is the software developmentally appropriate for the children using it? According to Haugland (2005), only 20% of software is appropriate for children. Beware of software that is just an electronic work sheet page. The age limit, educational objectives, and educational philosophy should be clearly stated (Peterson, Verenikina, & Herrington, 2008). ❑ Is the software designed so that the child can use it independently (simple and clear directions, uses speech when appropriate, uses picture menus, and organized for intuitive use) (Prairie, 2005)? ❑ If providing simulations, they are realistic and real-world (Peterson, Verenikina, & Herrington, 2008). ❑ Is the child able to exercise control when using the software (sets the pace, can repeat a process, can stop and resume, chooses from multiple paths, and frequently saves)? ❑ Does the software encourage active learning (requires active participation and encourages exploration and further investigation, allows for trial and error)? ❑ Is the software exciting and interesting (utilizes many senses including sound, music, and voice; includes graphics and sounds that are motivating for young children; and is relevant to the group of children using the software)? ❑ Does the software scaffold children’s learning (provides increasing challenges and a variety of levels, provides nonthreatening feedback allowing children to know their progress, provides hints and instruction, and does not penalize for mistakes)? ❑ Is the software anti-bias, containing respectful images of diverse cultures, multiple languages, people of different ages, abilities, colors, and diverse family structures (NAEYC, 1996)? ❑ Does the software promote prosocial values (no violence or implicit violence is present such as “blowing it up” to get rid of mistakes) (Tsantis, Bewick, & Thouvenelle, 2003)? ❑ Is the software preselected to match curricular goals, and is the software tied closely to other curricular activities? Software should support or be supported by the curriculum. ❑ Does the software have high-quality graphics and sounds? Graphics and sound need to add to the quality rather than being distracting. ❑ Is the software accessible for children with special needs? ❑ Does the software provide delight, enchantment, and adventure such as opportunities for free exploration, finding hidden secrets, and elements of surprise (Plowman, McPake, & Stephen, 2012)? Source: Bullard, J. (2017). Creating environments for learning: Birth to age eight (3rd ed.). Pearson, p. 348. Reprinted and Electronically Reproduced by Permission of Pearson Education, Inc.

those who spent the least amount of time on their electronic devices. Other researchers have found that one to two hours of digital engagement per day is not harmful to the psychosocial functioning of children aged 6 months to 17 years old (Orben & Przybylski, 2019; Przybylski et al., 2020). However, excessive use (more than 5 hours per day) leads to problems, and social media use has a small but negative effect on well-being (Orben, 2020). As schools and societies become more screen based, setting limits for ourselves and learners is important. As the saying goes, variety is the spice of life. Teachers should keep this in mind by varying the opportunities students have to engage in activities on and off screen both in school and out. Interacting with so much media means that students will need to learn how to be good stewards of information, safe and ethical users of digital media, and critical consumers. This will include teaching students about privacy related to securing/

DIGITAL CITIZENSHIP.

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

sharing of personal information and others’ information, intellectual property rights and proper attribution of content, protection from bad actors (e.g., malware, phishing), and professionalism. Learners also need to know how to evaluate the veracity of what they find online. Project Zero at Harvard has developed a K–12 Digital Citizenship curriculum with these and many other ideas for supporting students’ digital well-being (James et al., 2019; and see https://www.commonsense.org/education). CRITICAL MEDIA LITERACY. Being “media literate” includes the ability to critically assess print and online materials. Project Look Sharp at Ithaca College is directed by Cynthia Scheibe, a developmental psychologist whose mission is to provide K–16 educators with materials, training, and support to integrate media literacy and critical thinking about media into their lessons. For example, during the 2016 U.S. presidential election, Project Look Sharp provided 10 different lesson plans that helped students critically evaluate the candidates’ campaign ads, videos, websites, articles, and news releases on topics such as racial justice, national security, voting rights, economic inequality, climate change, and immigration. Students used critical questions to guide them (e.g., Who made and sponsored this message? Who is the target audience? How accurate and credible is the information provided? What is left out?). Similar instructional materials were designed to help students evaluate media coverage related to the COVID-19 vaccine. Helping your students ask critical questions about media seems more important than ever. The Guidelines: Supporting the Development of Media Literacy, based on work by Scheibe and Rogow (2008), give specific ideas for supporting the development of media literacy in your students.

GUIDELINES Supporting the Development of Media Literacy Use media to practice general observation, critical thinking, analysis, perspective taking, and communication skills. Examples 1.

2. 3. 4.

Ask students to think critically about the information presented in advertising, “news” programs, and textbooks. Would different people interpret the messages in differing ways? Foster creativity and communication by having students produce their own media on a topic you are studying. Ask students to compare ways that information might be presented in a documentary, TV news report, advertisement, public service announcement, and so on. Give examples of how word selection, background music, camera angles, color, and so on, can be used to set a mood or bias a message.

Use media to stimulate interest in a new topic. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Have students work in small groups to read, analyze, and discuss a controversial magazine, newspaper, or online article about a topic. Ask students to do a media search for information about a topic. Use a short video, magazine illustration, blog entry, or brief article to stimulate discussion, encouraging students to express what they already know or their opinion about a topic.

Help students identify what they already know or believe about a topic based on popular media content. Help them identify erroneous beliefs. Examples 1. 2.

What do students “know” about space travel? What have they learned about biology from advertisements? (Continued)

459

460 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Use media as a standard pedagogical tool. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Encourage students to follow (and write about) current events, including tracking a single story across diverse media sources. Assign homework that makes use of different media. Have students express opinions or attempt to persuade using different media, including photographs, collages, videos, poems, songs, animated films.

Analyze the effects that media had on historical events. Examples 1. 2.

How were Native Americans portrayed in art and in films? What sources of information were available 50 years ago? 100 years ago?

Use video effectively. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Show short segments, not whole films or programs. Leave the lights on to encourage active viewing and discussion. Before viewing, let students know what they should be looking/listening for. Pause periodically during viewing to point out important information or ask questions.

For more ideas, see the Project Look Sharp website.

Blending and Flipping: Technology-Powered Pedagogy We will all probably look back on the COVID-19 pandemic as a turning point in how teachers delivered instruction. Nearly all classrooms were initially converted to remote delivery models, and many moved to blended learning formats in which learners engaged in both in-person and online formats for a portion of the time. But even before the pandemic, many teachers adopted pedagogical approaches that leveraged the power of technology to allow for more interaction and activity (Graham et al., 2017). One example of this is a flipped classroom design (Bergmann & Sams, 2012). Salman Khan’s 2011 TED talk “Let’s Use Video to Reinvent Education” called for teachers to “flip” the traditional model of instruction by using video to deliver lecture material and using class time for more interactive work (see https://khanacademy.org). In a flipped classroom, what has traditionally happened in classrooms—lessons, lectures, note taking, worksheets, direct teaching—is moved outside class to be completed independently online. Then what usually happens at home—homework, projects, practice—happens in class under the teacher’s supervision and support. It appears that teachers see the value in this flipped approach. In 2014, 78% of teachers in a national survey reported that they had flipped at least one lesson, and 45% said they flipped once or twice a week (Maloy et al., 2021). What does a flipped classroom look like? Figure 10.8 gives some basics. The flipped classroom and related approaches that blend online and in-person learning elements set up the conditions for learners to actively construct meaning. Teachers are no longer just providers of information. Students can learn core content at their own pace outside class. During class time, students interact with material and with each other. The teacher is a facilitator and scaffolder of learning. Groups and individuals can work on activities at their level, allowing teachers to differentiate instruction to students’ needs. For students who do not have access to powerful learning technologies at home, some class time should be devoted to online researching, learning apps or games, creating digital portfolios, and listening to or creating podcasts. Does flipping work? A description of findings from studies conducted between 2012 and 2018 revealed both strengths and weaknesses (Birgili et al., 2021). Students in flipped classrooms typically learned more, but most studies have been conducted in college classrooms (Shi et al., 2020). Another strength is that flipping instruction allows students to learn more at their own pace and to be more active during in-class learning. For example,

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Figure 10.8 Traditional and Flipped Classrooms Traditional Classroom

What happens during class

What happens outside of class

Flipped Classroom

Teacher-led instruction with whole groups, small groups, and individuals

Student-led activities with whole groups, small groups, and individuals

Students as learners

Students as teachers and learners

Teacher comes ready to teach by imparting information or directing activities

Students come prepared to learn by doing activities and adding to online information

Students do homework using paper worksheets and writing prompts

Students watch videos or pencasts or listen to podcasts of teacher presentations

Reading assignments come from paper textbooks

Reading assignments come from online textbooks or interactive web resources

Source: Maloy, R. W., Verock, R.-E., Edwards, S. A., & Trust, T. (2021). Transforming learning with new technologies (4th ed.). Pearson.

a fifth-grade math teacher observed more student-to-student talk after instruction was flipped (de Araujo et al., 2017). One challenge to this approach, however, has been getting students to be self-disciplined enough to engage deeply with the direct instructional materials when unsupervised or at home (Birgili et al., 2021). Some fear that students’ and teachers’ workloads are actually increasing by flipping instruction and that instructional quality is diminished.

Dilemmas of Constructivist Practice Years ago, Larry Cremin (1961) observed that progressive, innovative pedagogies require infinitely skilled teachers. Today, the same could be said about constructivist teaching, and particularly the kind of teaching that involves technology-mediated learning (Pulham & Graham, 2018). We have already seen that many varieties of constructivism and many practices flow from these different conceptions. We also know that teaching today often happens in a context of high-stakes testing and accountability. In these situations, teachers who use constructivist approaches face conceptual, pedagogical, technological, cultural, and political dilemmas as they implement constructivist practices. Likewise, meaningful learning requires scaffolding instruction in ways that support learners’ cognitive, behavioral, and affective engagement as individuals and as members of sociocultural groups (Borup et al., 2020). This naturally leads to some dilemmas and questions, some of which were outlined by Mark Windschitl (2002, p. 133):

• How can I honor students’ attempts to think for themselves while remaining faithful to accepted disciplinary ideas?

• How can I manage new kinds of discourse and collaborative work in the classroom? • How can I accommodate the worldviews of students from diverse backgrounds while at the same time transforming my own classroom culture?

• How do my own past images of what is proper and possible in a classroom prevent me from seeing the potential for a different kind of learning environment?

• Can I trust students to accept responsibility for their own learning? • How can I allow for student-driven, problem-based learning and meet specific state and local standards?

• How can I gain the support of administrators and parents for teaching in such a radically different and unfamiliar way? We do not know all the answers to these questions, but with evolving technologies and times, sociocultural constructivist practices that support meaningful learning certainly hold promise.

461

462 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

Module 33 Summary Designing Interactive Digital Learning Environments (pp. 451–461) What are some possible uses of technology in education? Technologies such as computers, mobile devices (iPads, tablets, smartphones), and interactive gaming systems are extremely popular among young people. In education, virtual learning environment (VLE) is a broad term that describes many ways of learning in virtual systems. There are different kinds of VLEs. The most traditional VLE is referred to as a learning management system (LMS). A personal learning environment (PLE) framework provides tools that support individualized learning in a variety of contexts and situations; the learners assume control of how and when their learning occurs, such as through online peer interactions. The most complex VLE is an immersive virtual learning environment (IVLE). The IVLE is a simulation of a real-world environment where students work alone or with others to solve problems, create projects, simulate the skills of experts, visit historical sites, tour world-class museums, or play games that teach and apply academic skills. Does technology enhance learning?  Technology by itself will not guarantee improvement in academic achievement; like any tool, technology must be used well by confident, competent teachers. Additional evidence from rigorous scientific studies is needed in K–12 settings. Some educators are suggesting that all students should learn computational thinking—thinking like a computer scientist—to formulate and solve problems that can be solved using the computational processes like those applied by computers. Many systems allow even very young students to create computer programs. Also, every student should learn to be a good digital citizen, which includes the ability to critically evaluate the digital media they consume. What are blended instruction and a flipped classroom? Blended instruction refers to a combination of online and in-person learning. One type of blended instruction is the flipped classroom. In a flipped classroom, what usually happens in class—lessons, lectures, note taking, worksheets, direct teaching— is moved outside class, and what usually happens at home—homework, projects, practice—happens in class under the teacher’s supervision and support. For students who do not have access to powerful learning technologies at home, some class time can be devoted to online research, learning games, wikis, blogs, creating digital portfolios, listening to or creating podcasts, or using learning apps. What are some dilemmas of constructivist practice? Teachers must learn how to design, facilitate, and scaffold instruction in ways that support learners’ cognitive, behavioral, and affective engagement— often while also integrating technologies to help. This can raise common dilemmas around the degree of control, classroom management, challenging prior assumptions about “best” practices, and reconciling the tension between student-supported learning and standards-driven instruction.

Cluster 10 Review Key Terms Advance organizer Affinity groups Appropriating Brainstorming Cognitive apprenticeship Collaboration Community of practice Complex learning environments Computational thinking Constructive/Structured controversy Constructivism/Constructivist approach Cooperation Cooperative learning Co-regulated learning Immersive virtual learning environment (IVLE) Inquiry learning Jigsaw classroom

Learning management system (LMS) Learning sciences Massive multi-player online games (MMOs) Mobile learning Multiple representations of content Personal learning environment (PLE) Personal learning network (PLN) Problem-based learning Radical constructivism Reciprocal questioning Scaffolding Situated learning Social negotiation Spiral curriculum Virtual learning environments (VLEs) Zone of proximal development

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

463

Connect and Extend to Licensure Multiple-Choice Questions 1.

2.

3.

Which one of the following activities would NOT be consistent with a constructivist environment? A.

Students are given frequent opportunities to engage in complex, meaningful, problem-based activities.

B.

Students work collaboratively and are given support to engage in task-oriented dialogue with one another.

C.

Teachers elicit students’ ideas and experiences in relationship to key topics, then fashion learning situations that assist students in elaborating on or restructuring their current knowledge.

D.

Teachers employ limited assessment strategies and give feedback on products rather than processes.

4.

In Mr. Lawrence’s classroom, students are engaged in learning the art of driving. They watch Mr. Lawrence model techniques, receive hints and feedback from him on their performance, and are encouraged to put into words the new skills they are practicing. This type of learning is best referred to as which one of the following? A.

Reciprocal teaching

B.

Cognitive apprenticeship

C.

Cooperative learning

D.

Schema building

Constructed-Response Questions Case To infuse her class with constructivist strategies, Brenda Rhodes planned several problem-based learning scenarios. One of the scenarios required students to find a solution for their city’s homeless population. Over the past few years, the number of homeless individuals and families had grown alarmingly quickly. Social service agencies, shelters, and

Group activities must be well planned. Students need to be prepared to work in groups, and teachers have to be explicit in stating their expectations. Which one of the following strategies is NOT an element that defines true cooperative learning? A.

Positive interdependence and individual accountability

B.

Group processing

C.

Competition

D.

Collaborative and social skills

Research demonstrates that constructive controversy can lead to greater learning, open-mindedness, seeing the perspectives of others, creativity, motivation, and engagement. Which one of the following is a setup for activities that engage students in constructive/structured controversies? A.

Students work in pairs within their four-person cooperative groups to research a particular argument.

B.

Each student is part of a group, and each group member is given part of the material to be learned by the whole group. Students become experts on their piece and then teach it to the others in their group.

C.

Students intuitively understand the design that helps them think deeply about what they read.

D.

A combination of academic learning with personal and social development for secondary and college students is created.

businesses in the central city were struggling to deal with the challenge. Brenda believed her students would find the topic interesting and that it met the criteria for problem-based learning. 5.

Does Brenda Rhodes’s activity of finding a solution for the city’s homeless population as a topic meet the requirements for problem-based learning? Explain your answer.

6.

Identify several types of scaffolding the students might use to help them solve their problem.

What Would They Do? Teachers’ Casebook: Necessity Is the Mother of Inventive Teaching At the beginning of this cluster, we asked you to think critically about how you would plan interactive, collaborative assignments for your students during remote or hybrid instruction. Now, as you consider these teachers’ responses to that question, we again suggest that you think critically. Do the following responses reflect (or contradict) any of the concepts discussed in this cluster? Do the suggestions and solutions described here make sense in light of what you have learned in this or other classes about best practices? How do these ideas align with or challenge your personal philosophy

of teaching? Take a look at how some practicing teachers responded to the situation described at the beginning of the cluster.

Chris Dillon • K–12th-Grade Music Teacher Model Laboratory School at Eastern Kentucky University, Richmond, KY The phrase “technology-enhanced instruction” is a misnomer— instruction is already technologically enhanced in today’s traditional classroom. Whether instruction is virtual, face-to-face, or a hybrid combination, the most critical feature of any pedagogically sound learning environment is the establishment

464 Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning of the student-teacher relationship. This relationship can be facilitated virtually in many different ways; however, I try to remember that the student on the other end of the screen is a human first, a student second. Establishing my role of “teacher” is important but establishing myself as a person who wants to build community and value my students as individuals is the first step to ensuring students feel as if they belong, regardless of the environment in which they’re learning. As we see classes and instruction move to hybrid or virtual learning spaces, we must adjust our assessments and assignments to fit our students’ needs. Because we are able to search for the answer to any question using the internet, assessments and assignments should be created in a way that makes students build on their skills, not regurgitate information. For example, I asked both my virtual and face-to-face students to use technology to create their own original electronic music compositions rather than having them define musical terminology or analyze a piece of music they’re not into. I provided them with examples of each step in the process, facilitated students creating original compositions in small groups, then had them create their own work using the skills they built over the course of the class. Regardless of subject area or method of delivery (virtual, hybrid, or face-to-face), classes should be conducted in a manner that allows for student choice, individualized attention (small group or one-on-one), meaningful and timely feedback, and opportunities for collaboration.

Donnie Piercey • 5th-Grade Teacher & 2021 Kentucky Teacher of the Year Stonewall Elementary School, Lexington, KY Because COVID has forced schools to address the issues that many of us had been screaming about for ages pre-pandemic (especially surrounding equity and access), the role of technology in the classroom has changed. As I reflect on what hybrid strategies or assignments I’ll carry on post-pandemic, the ones which place an emphasis on student creativity come to mind. Creativity isn’t something that can be contained within classroom walls, nor should it be restrained in any type of hybrid model of instruction. Keeping students engaged, especially when learning remotely, is not easy. Making sure students are on the correct webpage or using the proper resource is a new kind of classroom management strategy that we’re all trying to figure out how to accomplish every day. However, when my students are using technology to solve problems that they have a genuine interest in, those issues rarely come up. On larger projects, I encourage my students to self-choose problems that they want to solve. When students know they have autonomous ownership over their problem solving—especially when they’re working collaboratively with their classmates—the engagement is always high. I’ve learned that this holds true whether we’re in person or on the other side of a video call.

Coralie Hafner • 1st-Grade Teacher Olde Orchard Alternative Elementary, Columbus, OH Effective technology-enhanced instruction begins with having consistent and clear communication with students and

families. This can be offered by providing a home base for families to refer back to that houses all of the necessary information and resources, which would decrease their feeling overwhelmed and confused. Offer whole group and small group sessions that are tailored to specific needs. Have clear expectations for students while they’re online (independent work and small groups) creating daily schedules to limit time off task. Be intentional with how and what’s communicated, ensuring that each interaction and assignment is necessary and enhances learning. Through Google Classroom I would provide access to all assignments and activities that students are expected to complete. Google forms would be used for exit slips, assessments, and feedback. I would use Google Slides to create an interactive virtual classroom where students would navigate through a set of slides that have been linked to specific websites and learning tasks. Chrome Canvas is a virtual white board that can be linked to the Google Class Folder. I would have my students complete home-based projects, virtual showcases (Read alouds, Writer’s Workshop) through the use of Flipgrid. To enhance student independence and engagement, students would use and submit a Google Form checkoff list. Assignments would not only be provided through the interactive classroom as they would include the engagement of family members, virtual field trips, combining classrooms with other teachers, and the recording of personal interviews and experiences. Collaboration would be offered through small group sessions, peer sharing through virtual art and writing fairs, breakout groups, and responding to student videos. Creating clear communication norms while interacting online and providing intentional time for celebration and recognition throughout the sessions will increase cooperation and collective advocacy as well as enhance the learning environment and experience for all learners.

Lori Hall • 9th-Grade Math Teacher Monroe High School, Monroe, NC Teaching two sets of students, those physically in the room and those joining remotely, would definitely require a new set of strategies. Because this may be a reality for the upcoming school year, it has been on my mind all summer. No matter what, I am responsible for getting the content to my classes as equitably as possible. I am an advocate for cooperative learning and if you ever walked into my classroom, you would see students working together at tables and having mathematical discussions as well as problem solving. In a remote situation, I would continue assigning cooperative tasks to my students and pair those in person with those who are remote to create an opportunity for mathematical discourse between the two groups. This would provide insight into their perceptions and misconceptions. In order for this to work, the expectations for the assignment would need to be clear. This is not group work, where some students do all the work and everyone gets a grade. There will have to be different tasks assigned to different students; they would need to either record their conversations or invite me to join so that I could ensure that everyone is engaged in the conversation. Cooperative learning would allow accountability for each student and also provide

Cluster 10 Constructivism and Interactive Learning

the socialization that those remote students would be missing. Education continues to evolve and I must keep a growth mindset to keep up with the changes and meet the needs of my students.

Melissa Cho • 3rd- and 4th-Grade Teacher Royal Heights Elementary School, Surrey, British Columbia, Canada Overnight, the pandemic immensely shifted the way I planned and taught as an educator. Changing to an online platform forced me to be resourceful and find meaningful ways to connect, reach, and engage my learners with their school community through remote learning. There were bountiful amounts of resources available to me, but the difficulty was finding the right digital platforms that were efficient, effective and easy to navigate for myself, my learners and their families. Moving forward, when planning future curriculum and activities, it is important to consider the current teaching conditions when implementing activities so that learners are able to adapt to a routine with or without my physical presence. Utilizing technology within the classroom is important to keep current in the case that remote learning becomes a reality again. It is important to keep learning activities clear, simple, interactive and short. Using digital platforms for reading such as RAZ Kids and EPIC Books allowed students to have access to materials while at home, but using apps such as LOOM to record read alouds and other lessons also ensured that learners who did not have access to appropriate technology such as Wifi were able to participate and engage through offline applications. Hosting whole group and small group meetings during remote learning played a large part in maintaining student engagement during online learning. Classroom meetings helped learners remain connected to their peers and adults in their classroom community, even from afar. It is important to choose a platform that offered screen sharing, video, and audio as it aided in the facilitation of small and whole group meetings. When facilitating a whole class meeting, it is important to always begin with sharing. We always started the day with a sharing circle which helped keep learners curious and engaged with one another. I also found that hosting mini lesson groups online helped learners connect closely and collaborate with a variety of peers. Incorporating simple activities during remote learning and having learners bring them to small group meetings to share or build together was helpful in order to create collaboration. Having learners being responsible for bringing one small piece of an activity to a guided meeting helped keep them accountable during remote learning.

Kathryn Larsen • Assistant Principal, Grades 7–9 Pleasant Grove Junior High School, Pleasant Grove, UT Technology should be leveraged to increase student achievement. Simply using technology, such as a laptop, a Chromebook an iPad, or an LMS (learning management system) will not result in enhanced learning. Instead, technology should provide an opportunity for students to engage in

465

critical thinking and collaboration in ways that they could not do without the technology. For example, a book club that typically engaged students in a discussion with other students in the physical classroom could be expanded to a discussion on an LMS platform to include students in multiple classrooms or even students in multiple schools. This broadens student perspectives, while still requiring critical thinking, collaboration, and different forms of communication on the part of the students. Collaborative groups can be set up in many remote learning platforms. These collaborative groups can be teacher assigned or the students’ choice. Setting up collaborative groups will allow students to use district-provided technology and even their own phones if available to work with their peers. Many LMS platforms provide features such as discussion boards, the ability to assign groups of students to different assignment modules, and unique ways to post work. Face-toface and even online time at the beginning of the school year should be used to teach collaboration skills, as teachers cannot assume that students already know how to collaborate. During school dismissal and online learning, my students continued their work in book clubs. This is a format we worked through numerous times in class, so I set it up online in as similar a way as possible. Students chose the books they would like to read, I assigned different online modules to each book group, and they collaborated together to reach the goals that they set for themselves as they read and discussed the book. It is very difficult to engage students in an online platform when the teacher has not taken the time to build relationships with students. Instead of focusing on all the learning that may have been lost at the end of the last school year, work to build relationships with new students so that they feel unity and belonging in the classroom, whether that is a face-to-face classroom, a hybrid, or an online setting. When engaging in a hybrid or fully online setting, choose five or six assignment structures and explicitly teach students how to complete those assignments. If you have face-to-face time at the start of the year, use it wisely to go over what your class will look like during online instruction, so students feel prepared for that inevitability. Whether you start the year in person, in a hybrid model, or online, take the time to set both in-person and online class norms. Ensure that your students are aware of what the expectations are for the class in whatever format it may be offered. Remember that students do not automatically know expectations. These must be taught. Feedback and self-reflection are key to independence and engagement when learning remotely. Remote learning leads to feelings of isolation, and student engagement is likely to suffer when fostering a culture of belonging and autonomy becomes more difficult because of remote learning. In an online setting, it is imperative that teachers acknowledge the good work that students are doing independently and that they offer personal feedback daily. Teachers should keep as many of the feedback structures as possible the same in online learning as during face-to-face instruction. This is demanding and time consuming for the teacher, but students cannot engage and thrive when they are not receiving timely, specific, and kind feedback.

Cluster 11

Tobithe M. J. W., Age 10, Cameroon ICAF.org

Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

Teachers’ Casebook: Failure to Self-Regulate What Would You Do? You know that your students need to be organized and self-regulating to do well in both their current and their future classes. But many of the students just don’t seem to know how to take charge of their own learning. They have trouble completing larger projects—many wait until the last minute. They can’t organize their work or decide what is most important. Some aren’t keeping up with assignments. Their book bags are disaster areas—filled with long-overdue assignment sheets and class handouts from last semester crumbled in with permission slips for field trips. Others seem constantly distracted by their phones. You are concerned because they will need to be much more organized and on top of their work as they progress through their education. You have so much material to cover to meet district guidelines, but many of your students are drowning in the amount of work they already have. 466

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

Critical Thinking • Which organizational skills do students need to be successful in your subject or class? • What could you do to teach these skills while still covering the material that will be on the proficiency or achievement tests the students will have to take in the spring? • How would you help students develop an authentic sense of efficacy for guiding their own learning?

Overview and Objectives In the past four clusters, we analyzed different aspects of learning. We considered behavioral and information processing explanations of what and how people learn. We examined complex cognitive processes such as concept learning and problem solving. These explanations of learning focus on the individual and what is happening in that person’s “head.” Recent perspectives have called attention to two other aspects of learning that are critical: social and cultural factors. In the previous cluster, we examined psychological and social constructivism. In this cluster, we look at social cognitive theory—a current view of learning and motivation that discusses dynamic interactions among many of the behavioral, personal, and environmental (including social and cultural) factors involved in learning and motivation. Social cognitive theory has its roots in Bandura’s (1977, 1986) early criticisms of behavioral views of learning, as you read in Cluster 7. Social cognitive theory moved beyond behaviorism to focus on humans as self-directed agents who make choices and marshal resources to reach goals. Concepts such as selfefficacy, agency, and self-regulated learning are key in social cognitive theories. These concepts are important components of motivation as well, so this cluster provides a good path from learning to the discussion of motivation in the next cluster. We end the cluster with a look back at our tour through different models of instruction. Rather than debating the merits of each approach, we will consider the contributions of these different models of instruction, grounded in different theories of learning. Don’t feel that you must choose the “best” approach—there is no such thing. Even though theorists argue about which model is best, excellent teachers don’t debate. They apply all the approaches, using each one when appropriate. By the time you have completed this cluster, you should be able to: 11.1 Distinguish between social learning theory and social cognitive theory,

including an explanation of triadic reciprocal causality. 11.2 Discuss the roles of observation and modeling in learning, including

factors that support learning by observation.

467

468 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation 11.3 Define self-efficacy and agency, distinguish these concepts from self-

concept and self-esteem, explain the sources of self-efficacy, and discuss self-efficacy for teaching. 11.4 Describe important components of self-regulated learning. 11.5 Apply your knowledge to teach for self-efficacy and self-regulated

learning. 11.6 Explain the meaning and different applications of four basic theories of

learning.

OUTLINE Teachers’ Casebook—Failure to Self-Regulate: What Would You Do? Overview and Objectives MODULE 34: Social Cognitive Theory and Applications Social Cognitive Theory A Self-Directed Life: Albert Bandura Beyond Behaviorism Triadic Reciprocal Causality Modeling: Learning by Observing Others Elements of Observational Learning Observational Learning in Teaching Agency and Self-Efficacy Self-Efficacy, Self-Concept, and Self-Esteem Sources of Self-Efficacy Self-Efficacy in Learning and Teaching Teachers’ Sense of Efficacy MODULE 35: Self-Regulation in Learning and Teaching Self-Regulated Learning: Skill and Will How Does Self-Regulation Develop? A Social Cognitive Model of Self-Regulated Learning Reaching Every Student: Technology and Self-Regulation Self-Regulation of Emotions Teaching Toward Self-Efficacy and Self-Regulated Learning Complex Tasks Agency and Control Self-Management Self-Evaluation Collaboration Bringing It All Together: Theories of Learning Cluster 11 Review Key Terms • Connect and Extend to Licensure • Teachers’ Casebook—Failure to Self-Regulate: What Would They Do?

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

MODULE 34

Social Cognitive Theory and Applications

Learning Objective 11.1 Distinguish between social learning theory and social cognitive theory, including an explanation of triadic reciprocal causality. Learning Objective 11.2 Discuss the roles of observation and modeling in learning, including factors that support learning by observation. Learning Objective 11.3 Define self-efficacy and agency, distinguish these concepts from self-concept and self-esteem, explain the sources of self-efficacy, and discuss self-efficacy for teaching.

Social Cognitive Theory Most of what is known today as social cognitive theory is based on the work begun by Albert Bandura in the 1950s at Stanford University. Before we talk about the theory, let’s meet the man.

A Self-Directed Life: Albert Bandura Albert Bandura’s life story should be a movie. You could say he lived the American dream, except that he was from Canada. His parents were immigrants from Central Europe; they chose the rugged land of northern Alberta for their family farm. Bandura’s parents never went to school, but they valued education. His father taught himself to read in three languages, giving young Albert a great model of self-regulated learning—a concept that figures prominently in social cognitive theory today. While attending high school, Bandura worked many jobs, including a stint as a carpenter at a furniture factory and one as a road worker on the Alaska Highway in the Yukon. He finished his undergraduate degree at the University of British Columbia in 3 years, even though he had to cram all his classes into the morning to have time for his afternoon jobs. Because he needed a morning class to fill one time slot, he enrolled in introductory psychology and found his future profession (Bandura, 2007, p. 46). His next stop was to attend a graduate school program at the epicenter of psychological research in 1950—the University of Iowa. After earning his PhD (in 3 years again), Bandura joined the faculty at Stanford University in 1953—he was 28 years old. He held emeritus status there as the David Starr Jordan Professor of Social Science in Psychology and was working in his office on a new book until the day before he died at age 95 on July 26, 2021. In 2015, Bandura received one of Canada’s highest civilian honors (the Order of Canada), and in 2016, President Barack Obama bestowed upon him the U.S. National Medal of Science for his extraordinary contributions to psychological science. We dedicated this edition to Professor Bandura for his influence on our work and the field of educational psychology. When I (Ellen writing) read Bandura’s autobiography, I was struck by how much his theories reflected his life as a self-directed, self-regulating learner growing up in a challenging environment. Describing his experiences in his two-teacher high school, Bandura (2007) said: We had to take charge of our own learning. Self-directed learning was an essential means of academic self-development, not a theoretical abstraction. The paucity of educational resources turned out to be an enabling factor that has served me well rather than an insurmountable handicapping one. The content of courses is perishable, but self-regulatory skills have lasting functional value whatever the pursuit might be. (p. 45) In the next sections we will look at the key features of Albert Bandura’s work and social cognitive theory by considering four topics: moving beyond behaviorism, the concept of triadic reciprocal causality, the power of observational learning, and the key function of self-efficacy in the development of human agency.

469

470 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Modeling (II, B2) Teachers often use modeling to teach students new behaviors. Identify the characteristics that tend to make models effective in instructional contexts.

Social learning theory Theory that emphasizes learning through observation of others.

Modeling Changes in behavior, thinking, or emotions that happen through observing another person—a model.

Social cognitive theory Theory of learning and motivation that adds to social learning theory a consideration of cognitive factors such as beliefs, self-perceptions, and expectations.

Triadic reciprocal causality An explanation of human functioning as the interaction between personal, behavioral, and environmental influences.

Beyond Behaviorism Bandura believed basic behavioral principles were correct as far as they went but also too limited to explain complex human thinking and learning. In his autobiography, Bandura (2007) described the shortcomings of behaviorism and the need to put people in social context: I found this behavioristic theorizing discordant with the obvious social reality that much of what we learn is through the power of social modeling. I could not imagine a culture in which its language; mores; familial customs and practices; occupational competencies; and educational, religious, and political practices were gradually shaped in each new member by rewarding and punishing consequences of their trial-and-error performances. (p. 55) As you learned in Cluster 7, Bandura’s early social learning theory included enactive learning (learning through reinforcement and punishment of your own behaviors) and added observational learning, that is, learning through modeling and observing others. When people experience changes in their own behavior, thinking, or emotions as a result of observing another person (a model) being reinforced or punished for particular behaviors, then modeling has occurred. Over time, Bandura’s explanations of learning included more attention to cognitive (personal) factors such as expectations and beliefs in addition to the social influences of models (Bandura, 1986, 1997, 2016, 2018). In 1986, he renamed his perspective social cognitive theory. This perspective retains an emphasis on the role of other people serving as models and teachers (the social part of social cognitive theory) but also emphasizes the role of thinking, believing, expecting, anticipating, self-regulating, and making comparisons and judgments (the cognitive part). Social cognitive theory is a dynamic system that explains human adaptation, learning, and motivation. The theory addresses how people develop social, emotional, cognitive, and behavioral capabilities; how people regulate their own lives; and what motivates them (Bandura, 2007, 2019; Bandura & Locke, 2003). Many of the concepts from this cluster will help you understand motivation in Cluster 12.

Triadic Reciprocal Causality I claimed earlier that social cognitive theory describes a system. This system, called triadic reciprocal causality, is the dynamic interplay among three kinds of influences: personal, environmental, and behavioral, as shown in Figure 11.1. Personal factors (beliefs, expectations, cognitive abilities, motivation, attitudes, and knowledge), the physical and social environment (resources, consequences of actions, other people, models and teachers, and physical and virtual settings), and behavior (individual actions, choices, and verbal statements) all influence and are influenced by each other. Figure 11.1 shows the interaction of person, environment, and behavior in learning settings (Pajares & Usher, 2008). External factors such as models, instructional strategies, classroom environments, or teacher feedback (elements of the environment for students) can affect student personal factors such as goals, sense of efficacy for the task (described in the next section), attributions (beliefs about causes for success and failure), expectations, and self-regulatory processes such as planning, monitoring, and controlling distractions (Schunk & DiBenedetto, 2020). For example, teacher feedback can lead students to feel either more confident or more discouraged, and then the students adjust their goals accordingly. Environmental factors, such as rewards for turning in homework, and personal factors, such as setting challenging goals, can encourage useful behaviors like effort and persistence that lead to successful learning (Usher & Schunk, 2018). But these behaviors also reciprocally influence personal factors. For example, as students perform better through increased effort (behavior), their self-efficacy and interest (personal factors) are likely to increase. Finally, student behaviors also affect their environment. For example, if students do not persist or if they perform poorly on assignments, teachers may change instructional strategies or learning group assignments, thus changing the learning environment for the students.

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

471

Figure 11.1 A Social Cognitive View of Reciprocal Influences in Learning All three forces—personal, social/environmental, and behavioral—are in constant interaction. They influence and are influenced by each other.

• • • • • • • • • •

• • • • • • • • • • • • • • •

Behavioral Factors Academic achievement, school performance Selection of learning activities Choice of courses or major Physical activity Homework completion Effort and persistence Organizational skills Coping skills Interactions with classmates and teachers Withdrawal or engagement

Personal Factors Beliefs, goals, and expectations Thoughts and feelings Cognition and metacognition Memory Background knowledge Motivation Awareness of discrimination or prejudice Mental health Physical and cognitive attributes/deficits Personality Gender, ethnicity, race, age, cultural heritage Academic, social, mental, and verbal ability Curiosity and creativity Intelligence Religion

• • • • • • • • • • • • • •

Environmental Factors Teacher support and expectations Local, state, and national policies Societal and familial values and norms Classroom/school climate Learning structures (e.g., scaffolding, reinforcement) Social status and support Evaluative messages from others Online environment (e.g., social media) Physical environment (e.g., geography, climate) Psychological climate (e.g., hostile, welcoming) Peer group Exposure to diversity Curriculum and policies Economic resources

Think for a minute about the power of triadic reciprocal causality in classrooms. If personal factors, behaviors, and the environment are in constant interaction, then cycles of events are progressive and self-perpetuating. Suppose a student had difficulties in his previous school. The first day at his new school he is late to class because he got lost in the unfamiliar building. The student happens to have several visible tattoos and pierced body parts. He is anxious about his first day and hopes to do better at this new school, but the teacher’s initial reaction to his late entry and dramatic appearance is a bit hostile. The student feels insulted and responds in kind, so the teacher begins to form expectations about him and becomes more vigilant, less trusting. The student senses the distrust. He decides that this school will be just as worthless as his previous one and wonders why he should bother to try. The teacher sees the student’s disengagement and invests less effort in teaching him, and the cycle continues. These reciprocal effects are more than hypothetical. Using data gathering from over 22,000 teachers and their students in 16 countries, researchers showed that teachers change their teaching practices based on how capable they feel in their teaching skills. These teaching practices, in turn, affect students’ beliefs about their own capabilities to learn (Bonneville-Roussy et al., 2019). In other words, just as Bandura predicted, the beliefs and behaviors of one person

472 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation can create the environment for another! Performing well in school is related to what students believe and how motivated they are, but these beliefs are connected to whether students feel supported in their learning environment (Burns et al., 2018). Let’s look more closely at two key elements of social cognitive theory: observational learning and self-efficacy. As we examine each of these elements, we emphasize their implications for teaching.

Modeling: Learning by Observing Others What causes an individual to learn and perform modeled behaviors and skills? Several factors play a role. First, the developmental level of the observer makes a difference in learning. As children grow older, they can focus attention for longer periods of time, more effectively identify the important elements of a model’s behavior to observe, use memory strategies to retain information, and motivate themselves to practice, as you can see in Table 11.1. A second influence is the status of the model. Children are motivated to imitate the actions of others who seem competent, powerful, prestigious, and enthusiastic, so parents, teachers, older siblings, athletes, action heroes, rock stars, or film personalities may serve as models, depending on the age and interests of the child. Third, by watching others who are similar to us, we learn about what behaviors are appropriate, and we are able to identify the range of behaviors we probably could accomplish. Children and adolescents pay close attention to models they perceive to be similar to themselves in age, gender, or race. (When you watch your favorite show, do you pay closer attention to characters who seem similar to you?) All students, no matter what their cultural background, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, or gender, can benefit from seeing successful, capable models who look and sound like them. For instance, when people in developing countries watch television shows where relatable lead characters learn how to effectively deal with the social problems they are facing (e.g., women’s rights, HIV prevention, lack of educational access for girls), viewers’ beliefs about what is possible for them also change (Bandura, 2016, 2019). How?

Table 11.1 Factors That Affect Observational Learning CHARACTERISTIC

EFFECTS ON MODELING PROCESS

Developmental Status

Improvements with development include longer attention span and increased capacity to process information, use strategies, compare performances with memorial representations, and adopt intrinsic motivators.

Model Prestige and Competence

Observers pay greater attention to competent, high-status models. Consequences of modeled behaviors convey information about functional value. Observers attempt to learn actions they believe they will need to perform.

Vicarious Consequences

Consequences to models convey information about behavioral appropriateness and likely outcomes of actions. Valued consequences motivate observers. Similarity in attributes or competence signals appropriateness and heightens motivation.

Outcome Expectations

Observers are more likely to perform modeled actions they believe are appropriate, attainable, and will result in rewarding outcomes.

Goal Setting

Observers are likely to attend to models who demonstrate behaviors that help observers attain goals.

Values

Observers are more likely to attend to models who display behaviors that the observers believe are important and find satisfying.

Self-Efficacy

Observers attend to models when they believe they are capable of learning or performing the modeled behavior. Observation of similar models affects self-efficacy (“If they can do it, I can too”).

Source: From Schunk, D. H. (2020). Learning theories: An educational perspective (8th ed.), p. 139. Reprinted by permission of Pearson Education, Inc.

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

Look at Table 11.1 for help finding the answer: The factors relate to how people learn from models, which involves goals, consequences, and expectations. If observers expect that certain actions of models will lead to particular outcomes (e.g., specific practice regimens lead to improved athletic performance) and the observers value those outcomes or goals, then the observers will pay attention to the models and try to reproduce their behaviors. (This explains why my nephew loves watching surfing videos on YouTube!) Finally, observers are more likely to learn from models if the observers have a high level of self-efficacy— if they believe they are capable of doing the actions needed to reach the goals or at least capable of learning how to do them (Schunk et al., 2014). Of course, this works both ways. Watching another person perform a skill successfully can also increase the observers’ beliefs that they can do it, too. We’ll examine this more later.

Elements of Observational Learning Stop & Think Your interview for a position in the middle school is going well. Now you are asked, “Who are your ‘teacher’ role models? Do you see yourself doing things that teachers from your past have done? Are there teachers from films or books that you would like to emulate?” Through observational learning, we discover not only how to perform a behavior but also what will happen to us in specific situations if we perform it. Observation can be a very efficient learning process. The first time children hold hairbrushes, cups, or baseball bats, they usually brush, drink, or swing as well as they can, given their current muscle development and coordination. Through comparison with a model, we also can identify adjustments that may produce better outcomes. To learn new behaviors or refine current abilities, Bandura (1986) proposed that four elements of observational learning are essential: paying attention, retaining information or impressions, producing behaviors, and being motivated to repeat the behaviors. ATTENTION. To learn through observation, we have to pay attention. This includes both selective attention (paying attention to the correct cues and information) as well as sustained attention (maintaining focus). In teaching, you will have to ensure students’ attention to the critical features of the lesson by making clear presentations and highlighting important points. In demonstrating a skill (for example, threading a sewing machine, performing a dissection, or operating a lathe), you may need to have students look over your shoulder as you work. Seeing your hands from the same perspective as they see their own directs their attention to the right features of the situation and makes observational learning easier.

To imitate the behavior of a model, you have to remember each step. This involves mentally representing the model’s actions in some way, probably as verbal steps (“Hwa-Rang, the eighth form in Tae Kwon Do, is a palm-heel block, then a middle riding stance punch, then . . .”), as visual images, or both. Retention can be improved by mental rehearsal (imagining imitating the behavior) or by actual practice. Of course, the ability to watch a model via an online video enables us to offload some of the memory work to the replay function. Even so, practicing as we watch helps us remember the elements of the desired behavior, such as the sequence of steps. Ultimately, to be able to apply what we have learned, we must go through the retention phase of observational learning.

RETENTION.

Once we “know” how a behavior should look and remember the elements or steps, we still may not perform it smoothly without a great deal of practice, feedback, and coaching about subtle points. In the production phase, practice makes the behavior smoother and more expert. However, if a child does not have the physical or developmental skills needed to produce the behavior, even extensive practice and feedback may not be enough. The ideal conditions for practice toward expert production will typically involve some form of feedback that compares the learner’s performance to that of the model (e.g., specific teacher feedback, explicit coaching, reviewing video of the performance, deliberate practice of the weak parts) (Ericsson & Pool, 2016).

PRODUCTION.

473

474 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

Direct reinforcement Reinforcement given after successful completion of a task.

Vicarious reinforcement Increasing the chances that we will repeat a behavior by observing another person being reinforced for that behavior.

Self-reinforcement Controlling (selecting and administering) your own reinforcers.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Observational Learning (II, B2) Identify situations in which observational learning may be a wise approach, and describe the essential elements of effective observational learning.

MOTIVATION AND REINFORCEMENT. Social cognitive theory distinguishes between acquisition and performance. We may acquire a new skill or behavior through observation, but we may not perform that behavior until we have some motivation or incentive to do so. Reinforcement can play several roles in observational learning. If we anticipate being reinforced for imitating the actions of a model, we may be more motivated to pay attention, remember, and reproduce the behaviors. In addition, reinforcement is important in maintaining learning through persistence. Without some kind of reinforcing consequence, new behaviors will likely disappear (Schunk, 2020). For example, if an unpopular student adopted the dress of the “in” group but was ignored or ridiculed, it is unlikely that the imitation would continue. Similarly, learning new academic tasks is usually accompanied by some failure—and reinforcement for progressive gains helps learners maintain focus on their growth rather than on their current struggles. Bandura has identified three forms of reinforcement that can encourage observational learning. First, of course, the observer may reproduce the behaviors of the model and receive direct reinforcement, as when a gymnast successfully executes a front flip/roundoff combination and the coach/model says, “Excellent!” But the reinforcement need not be direct—it may be vicarious reinforcement. The observers may simply see others reinforced for a particular behavior and then increase their production of that behavior. For example, if you compliment two students on the attractive illustrations in their lab reports, several other students who observe your compliments may turn in illustrated lab reports next time. Most media ads hope for this kind of effect. People in commercials become deliriously happy when they drive a particular car or ingest a specific energy drink, and the viewer is supposed to do the same; the viewer’s behavior is reinforced vicariously by the actors’ obvious pleasure. Punishment can also be vicarious: You may slow down on a stretch of highway after seeing several people get speeding tickets there. A student may remove her comments on Instagram after seeing a friend teased for making similar posts. The final form of reinforcement is self-reinforcement, or controlling your own reinforcers—one aspect of self-regulation described later in this cluster. Self-reinforcers may be intrinsic (e.g., feelings of satisfaction at a job well done) or extrinsic (e.g., rewarding yourself with a special treat after accomplishing a goal). If one goal of education is to produce people who are capable of educating themselves, then students must learn to manage their own lives, set their own goals, and provide their own reinforcement. In adult life, rewards are sometimes vague, and goals often take a long time to reach. Think about how many small steps are required to complete an education and find your first job. In teaching, sometimes self-reinforcement is all that keeps you going in the face of difficult students and demanding parents. Life is filled with tasks that call for this sort of self-regulation (Rachlin, 2004). Social cognitive theory has some powerful implications for teaching. Let’s look more closely at using observational learning in teaching.

Observational Learning in Teaching Stop & Think How would you incorporate observational learning into your teaching? What are the skills, attitudes, and strategies that can be modeled in teaching your subject? We have explored how observational learning takes place and examined the factors that influence it. Let’s now take a look at five possible outcomes of observational learning as they occur in classrooms: directing attention, encouraging existing behaviors, changing inhibitions, teaching new behaviors and attitudes, and arousing emotions (Schunk, 2020). By observing others, we not only learn about actions but also notice the objects involved in the actions. For example, in a preschool class, when one child plays enthusiastically with a toy that has been ignored for days, many other children may want to have the toy, even if they play with it in different ways or simply carry it around. This happens, in part, because the children’s attention has been drawn to that particular toy. I can remember

DIRECTING ATTENTION.

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

trying to teach my middle school students difficult vocabulary words by providing definitions. Seeing their boredom, I one day decided to ask students to take turns writing a funny or grotesque sentence on the board that included the target word. Just seeing their peers stand in front of the class and read their clever sentences held the observers in rapt attention. FINE-TUNING ALREADY-LEARNED BEHAVIORS. All of us have had the experience of looking for cues from other people when we find ourselves in unfamiliar situations. Observing the behavior of others tells us which of our already-learned behaviors to use: the proper fork for eating the salad, when to leave a gathering, what kind of language is appropriate, and so on. Adopting the dress and grooming styles of TV or music idols is another example of this kind of effect. STRENGTHENING OR WEAKENING INHIBITIONS. If class members witness one student breaking a class rule and getting away with it, they may learn that undesirable consequences do not always follow rule breaking. If the rule breaker is a well-liked, high-status class leader, the effect of the modeling may be even more pronounced. This ripple effect (Kounin, 1970) can work to the teacher’s benefit. When the teacher deals effectively with a rule breaker, especially a class leader, the idea of breaking this rule may be inhibited for the other students viewing the interaction. This does not mean that teachers must reprimand each student who breaks a rule, but once a teacher has called for a particular action, following through is an important part of capitalizing on the ripple effect.

William James (1899/2001) said, “The teacher who meets with the most success is the teacher whose own ways are the most imitable” (p. 26). Modeling has long been used to teach dance, sports, and crafts, as well as skills in subjects such as food science, chemistry, welding, and language learning. Cognitive modeling can also be applied deliberately in the classroom to teach mental skills and to broaden horizons—to teach new ways of thinking, such as thinking through the steps in a complex math problem (Zimmerman, 2013). Teachers serve as models for a vast range of behaviors, from pronouncing vocabulary words to reacting to the seizure of a student with epilepsy, to being enthusiastic about learning. For example, a teacher might model critical thinking skills by thinking “out loud” about a student’s question. Or a teacher concerned about girls who seem to have stereotyped ideas about their career prospects might invite women with nontraditional jobs to speak to the class or expose students to exemplary models in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM; for example, see engineergirl.org). Teachers can demonstrate their love of reading or music or art or history by sharing their favorite books, films, artists, and so on. Studies indicate that modeling can be most effective when the teacher makes use of all the elements of observational learning—attention, retention, production, and especially reinforcement and practice. It is important to select appropriate models, however. For example, students who doubt their own abilities might be best paired with a peer who keeps trying and finally masters the material rather than someone who gets it right on the first try with little effort (Schunk, 2020).

TEACHING NEW BEHAVIORS.

AROUSING EMOTION. Finally, through observational learning, people may develop emotional reactions to situations they have never experienced personally. Hearing, watching, or reading about a situation are powerful forms of observation. This may be most obvious through fear-inducing observations. News reports of shark attacks have many of us anxious about swimming in the ocean. The tragedies of school shootings reported in the media prompt parents, teachers, and students alike to develop new concerns over safety in schools. Some terrible examples of modeling occur with hate group activities, “copycat killings,” or suicide clusters in schools. When frightening things happen to students who are similar in age or circumstances, your students may need to be given an opportunity to talk about their emotions. But not all observations lead to negative emotions. Watching or reading about the courageous acts of others who spend their lives advocating for human and civil rights can lead to emotional responses that promote social change. Indeed, diverse media have made it possible to observe distant or previously unimaginable realities. For example, youth participating in the Young Voices for the Planet program

Ripple effect “Contagious” spreading of behaviors through imitation.

475

476 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation have engaged in a social network video campaign featuring youth-led activities that help protect the environment and combat climate change (Bandura & Cherry, 2020). Seeing media portrayals of acts of kindness or heroism can arouse emotion that may prompt imitative behaviors or increased “faith” in oneself and humanity. The Guidelines: Using Observational Learning will give you some ideas about using observational learning in the classroom. One final thing you can do to promote healthy observational learning is to help your students select appropriate models. Bandura (2016) pointed out that the environments in which we live are not just externally imposed; we have some say in how we select and

GUIDELINES Using Observational Learning Model behaviors and attitudes you want your students to learn. Examples 1. 2.

3.

4. 5.

Show enthusiasm for the subject you teach. Be willing to demonstrate both the mental and the physical tasks you expect the students to perform. I once saw a teacher sit down in the sandbox while her 4-year-old students watched her demonstrate the difference between “playing with sand” and “throwing sand.” Model good problem solving—think out loud as you work through a difficult problem. A language arts teacher might stop and say, “Now let me see if I remember what happened so far” or “That was a hard sentence. I’m going to read it again.” Model coping skills, persistence, and effort. Engage with a difficult problem, even if you seem to reach a dead end. Try new strategies or take a break and return to the problem later. Model your expectations. I remember seeing my fellow teachers talking to one another during a school assembly while occasionally looking up to chastise students who were doing the same thing. Students recognize hypocrisy. Promote desired behaviors by engaging in them yourself.

Use peers, especially class leaders, as models. Examples 1. 2.

3.

In group work, pair students who do well with those who are having difficulties. Alternate this with pairings of peers at similar levels (see Cluster 10 for more suggestions). When choosing a model, alternate between coping models who work through problems slowly and correct their errors as they work and mastery models who solve problems quickly and without error. Ask students to demonstrate the difference between “whispering” and “silence—no talking.”

Make sure students see that positive behaviors lead to reinforcement for others. Examples 1. 2.

Point out the connections between positive behavior and positive consequences in stories. Be fair in giving reinforcement. The same rules for rewards should typically apply to both the students with problems and the students who do not cause trouble.

Enlist the help of class leaders in modeling behaviors for the entire class. Examples 1. 2.

Ask a well-liked student to be friendly to an isolated or fearful student. Let high-status students lead an activity when you need class cooperation or when students are likely to be reluctant at first. Popular students can model dialogues in foreign-language classes or be the first to tackle dissection procedures in biology.

For more information on observational learning, go to http://www.readwritethink.org and search for “modeling.”

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

477

create our environments—including our social ones. For example, high school students choose their classes, their peer groups, their extracurricular activities, and even their social media friends. Helping students reflect on how these social groups influence their behaviors might empower them to choose wisely.

Agency and Self-Efficacy Remember that social cognitive theory emphasizes the dynamic interaction of personal, behavioral, and environmental events in shaping human lives. Bandura (1997) placed particular emphasis on the personal factors. Because of people’s capacity for self-influence, we have a role in determining what we learn, with whom we interact, and how we act. Bandura (2016) called this human agency—“To be an agent is to exert intentional influence over one’s functioning and over the course of events by one’s actions” (p. 4). Agency involves the ability to make intentional choices and plans, design appropriate courses of action, and then motivate and regulate the execution of these plans and actions. This is the major difference between social cognitive theory and behaviorism: In social cognitive theory, we can change or ignore our environments, control our own behavior, support the actions of others, and take charge of our lives. Behaviorism removes the “personal” from the equation and focuses entirely on how the reinforcing (or punishing) environment predicts what we do. When we discuss self-regulation later in the cluster, you will see how students and teachers can become more agentic—more self-directing and in charge of their own learning and motivation. Central to our own agency are the beliefs we hold about our own capabilities, referred to as self-efficacy. Simply put, self-efficacy answers the question “Can I do this?” We ask ourselves this question about all sorts of situations—Can I do math? Can I keep my cool when my friend talks about me behind my back? Can I teach this group of students? Can I do 10 push-ups? Can I stop checking my phone 100 times a day for new messages? Bandura (1997) defined self-efficacy as “beliefs in one’s capabilities to organize and execute the courses of action required to produce given attainments” (p. 3). Learners’ self-efficacy is distinct from but related to what they expect the outcomes of their behavior will be (e.g. “Will I succeed or fail? Will I be liked or laughed at?” “Will I be more accepted by teachers in this new school?”). How students answer these questions depends on their beliefs about their personal competence or effectiveness in a given area. Researchers have shown that self-efficacy is essential for positive student outcomes across grade levels and content domains. Students with high self-efficacy exert greater levels of effort, persistence, and resilience when engaged in challenging tasks—as well as experience lower levels of stress and anxiety for the task (Klassen & Usher, 2010; Schunk & DiBenedetto, 2020). In studies of students going from middle through high school, students with a higher level of self-efficacy for having control over their academic outcomes had higher levels of confidence in their academic abilities, earned higher grades, were more engaged, and were more likely to graduate (Anderson et al., 2019; Caprara et al., 2005).

Self-Efficacy, Self-Concept, and Self-Esteem Most people assume that self-efficacy is the same as self-concept or self-esteem, but it isn’t. Although these terms refer to self-beliefs, they differ in important ways. Self-efficacy refers to a “context-specific assessment of competence to perform a specific task” (Pajares, 1997, p. 15). Self-concept, on the other hand, has historically referred to a more global self-perception. Self-concept is a judgment based on both external and internal comparisons, using other people or other aspects of the self as frames of reference (Marsh et al., 2019). It answers the questions “How good am I at this? How am I compared to others?” But self-efficacy focuses on your perceived capability to successfully accomplish a particular task—the question is “Can I do this?” What matters in self-efficacy judgments is whether you believe you can do it, not whether others would be successful. Also, self-efficacy beliefs are strong predictors of behavior, but self-concept has typically shown weaker predictive

Human agency The capacity to coordinate learning skills, motivation, and emotions to reach your goals.

Self-efficacy A sense of being able to deal effectively with a particular task. Beliefs about your personal capabilities in a particular situation.

478 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

Mastery experiences Students’ awareness of their own previous successful performance—the most powerful source of efficacy information.

Vicarious experiences Accomplishments that are modeled by someone else.

power, particularly when it comes to predicting how well students will do on a specific task (Anderman & Anderman, 2014; Bandura, 1997). Self-efficacy is context specific—that is, people’s beliefs about what they can do depend on the subject area or the task they are facing. For example, my sense of efficacy for singing is really low, but I feel confident in my ability to read a map and navigate (except in certain cities like Rome that are hopeless). Even young children have developed different efficacy beliefs for different tasks. One study found that by the first grade, students already differentiated among their sense of efficacy for reading, for writing, and for spelling (Wilson & Trainin, 2007). Self-efficacy is concerned with judgments of personal competence; self-esteem is concerned with judgments of self-worth (Usher, 2015). Self-esteem is determined in large part by the value we place on our performance in a domain (math, appearance, singing, soccer, etc.) and our concern over what others think about our competence (Harter & Whitesell, 2003). Self-esteem is not affected if we feel incompetent in areas we don’t really value. There is no consistent relationship between self-esteem and self-efficacy. It is possible to feel highly efficacious in one area and still not have a high level of self-esteem, or vice versa (Valentine et al., 2004). As I confessed earlier, I have very low self-efficacy for singing, but my self-esteem is not affected, probably because my life does not require singing. But if my self-efficacy for teaching a particular class (my teaching self-efficacy) started decreasing after several bad experiences, I know my self-esteem would suffer because I value teaching.

Sources of Self-Efficacy Social persuasion Evaluative messages that students receive from people around them that convey information about their capabilities.

How is it that we come to believe we are capable in one area and hopeless in another? Bandura (1997) identified four sources of self-efficacy: mastery experiences, vicarious experiences, social persuasion, and physiological and emotional arousal. Mastery experiences refer to how we interpret our own direct experiences—usually this is the most powerful source of efficacy information. Successes raise efficacy beliefs, while failures lower them (Usher, Ford, et al., 2019). Bandura (1997) also referred to these as “enactive” experiences, given that we do not necessarily interpret all of our performances as masterful. Sometimes we have little or no direct experience in a domain. We therefore base our self-efficacy judgments on other sources, such as the vicarious experiences we have through observing the performances of others. Watching a model not only teaches us new ways of doing things but also alters our beliefs about what we can do. When I was young, my close friend Jessica could play a fierce Mozart sonata. With my chin perched at the end of the piano, I watched her fingers glide across the keys and said to myself, “If she can play this, then I can, too.” When an observer feels similar to the model, the vicarious influence on self-efficacy is stronger (Gerber et al., 2018). In school, a peer’s good performance can enhance other students’ own sense of efficacy. When an esteemed model fails, on the other hand, our self-efficacy might decrease. There are some exceptions to this pattern, however. For example, watching someone struggle using poor skills might convince observers that they can succeed at the same task by implementing better skills. The popularity of skill-based television shows and online videos that model how to cook, redesign your kitchen, improve your golf swing, change the battery in your car, or do a new dance move demonstrates the influence of vicarious experiences. These are particularly effective when viewers can identify with the people in the show. In some circumstances, vicarious experiences can be a powerful source of self-efficacy. Seeing figures from history, social rights activists, and even fictional characters stand up for themselves or their cause can convince observers that they can too (Morgenroth et al., 2015). Models who counteract gender- and race-based stereotypes, so long as observers’ exposure to them is somewhat long term, can play an important role in the self-efficacy of people who are engaging in domains where they are underrepresented (e.g., women, people of color; Olsson & Martiny, 2018; Zeldin et al., 2008). A third source of self-efficacy, social persuasion, refers to the evaluative messages students receive from those around them. These messages take many forms, both

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

overt, like a “pep talk” or specific performance feedback, and covert, such as a teacher ’s tendency to avoid calling on certain students to answer difficult questions. Social persuasion alone may not always create enduring increases in self-efficacy, but a persuasive boost in self-efficacy can lead a student to make an effort, attempt new strategies, or try hard enough to succeed (Bandura, 1982). Social persuasion from peers or trusted experts such as teachers can counter occasional setbacks that might have instilled self-doubt and interrupted persistence. The potency of persuasion depends on the credibility, trustworthiness, expertise, and/or status of the persuader. For example, teachers’ evaluations of their students have a more powerful effect on students’ selfefficacy when students view their teacher as highly credible (Won et al., 2017). During skill development, people may be more inclined to pay attention to what others tell them about their abilities (Bandura, 1997). Social persuasion that highlights prior successes (mastery) on similar tasks, identifies short-term goals and accomplishments, or focuses on the importance of effort is more likely to boost self-efficacy and initiate successful performance (Bandura, 1997; Usher & Pajares, 2008). A final source of self-efficacy is how students interpret the level of physiological or emotional arousal that they experience before or during an activity. For instance, the thought of giving a public speech makes a middle school student’s palms sweat and her chest tighten. She interprets this as a sign that she is not capable of speaking. We read our emotions and bodies in ways that alter our perceptions of our capabilities (Usher & Pajares, 2009). For example, when asked what makes him feel less confident in math, a high school student in one of my studies said, “Math makes me just want to throw my freaking guts up” (Usher, Ford, et al., 2019, p. 46). Whether negative or positive, a strong physiological response can have a profound effect on self-efficacy. As you think about the task of teaching, are you anxious and worried (lower self-efficacy) or excited and “psyched” (higher self-efficacy)? At times you can help students reappraise or relabel their arousal—they are not anxious, they are psyched and ready! Table 11.2 summarizes the sources of self-efficacy. Some evidence suggests that students’ personal or cultural backgrounds may be related to how they interpret the sources of self-efficacy (Usher & Weidner, 2018). For instance, Butz and Usher (2015) asked 2,500 upper elementary and middle school students about the sources of their self-efficacy in reading and math. Girls were more likely than boys to list social sources of their self-efficacy (i.e., vicarious experience and social persuasion). Context also mattered. Many students reported different sources of self-efficacy in math and reading. In other circumstances, such as online statistics learning, students draw from all four of the sources listed in Table 11.2 when forming their self-efficacy (Huang et al., 2020). Other cultural differences can affect how self-efficacy develops. One study found that the math selfefficacy of adolescents in Korea and the Philippines was based more on social persuasion from peers and parents than on other sources (Ahn et al., 2016). Mindful instruction will include approaches that support learners’ self-efficacy in different ways.

Table 11.2 Sources of Self-Efficacy SOURCE

EXAMPLE

Mastery Experiences

Past successes and failures in similar situations, as perceived by the individual. To increase self-efficacy, the success must be attributed to the ability, effort, choices, and strategies of the individual—not to luck or extensive help from others.

Vicarious Experiences

Seeing other people like you succeed on a task or reach a goal that is similar to the one you face.

Social Persuasion

Encouragement, informational feedback, useful guidance from a trusted source, evaluation.

Physiological Arousal

Positive or negative arousal—excitement and a feeling of being “psyched” and ready (increases self-efficacy) or a sense of anxiety and foreboding (decreases self-efficacy).

479

Physiological or emotional arousal Physical and psychological reactions causing a person to feel alert, attentive, wide awake, excited, or tense.

480 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

Self-Efficacy in Learning and Teaching Stop & Think On a scale from 1 to 100, how confident are you that you will finish reading this cluster today? Let’s assume your sense of efficacy is around 90 out of 100 for completing this cluster. Higher self-efficacy leads to greater effort and persistence in the face of setbacks, so even if you are interrupted in your reading, you are likely to return to the task. I believe I can finish writing this section today, so I have resumed work on it after my computer crashed and I had to start over on several pages. Self-efficacy also influences motivation and performance through goal setting (Schunk & DiBenedetto, 2020). If we have a high sense of efficacy in a given area, we will set higher goals, be less afraid of failure, and find new strategies when old ones fail. If your sense of efficacy for reading this cluster is high, you are likely to set high goals for completing it—maybe you will take some notes, too. If your sense of efficacy is low, however, you may avoid the reading altogether or give up easily when problems arise or you are interrupted with a better offer (Usher & Schunk, 2018). Research indicates that performance in school is improved and self-efficacy is increased when students (a) adopt short-term goals so it is easier to judge progress; (b) are taught to use specific learning strategies such as outlining or summarizing that help them focus attention; and (c) are made aware of their own achievements, rather than simply praised for their engagement, because achievement signals increasing competence (Pajares, 2006). See the Guidelines: Supporting Self-Efficacy for ideas about how you can support students’ self-efficacy (and your own!). In schools, we are particularly interested in self-efficacy for learning mathematics, writing, history, language, science, health promotion, and other subjects, as well as self-efficacy for using learning strategies and for handling the many other challenges that classrooms present. Consider a few findings from research with students. Self-efficacy is related to school performance for students from elementary school through college (Honicke & Broadbent, 2016; Richardson et al., 2012). But self-efficacy has also been shown to predict adolescents’ life satisfaction (Burger & Samuel, 2017), use of deep processing learning strategies for college students (Prat-Sala & Redford, 2010), elementary and high school students’ writing self-regulation and performance (Zumbrunn et al., 2020), college major and career choice (Lent et al., 2017), and performance in college for older students (Elias & MacDonald, 2007). The value of self-efficacy seems to cut across sociocultural contexts. For example, self-efficacy is related to math/science goals and interests for Mexican American youth (Navarro et al., 2007), staying in school for Italian secondary students (Caprara et al., 2008), language performance for French elementary school students (Joët et al., 2011), reading fluency in Finnish primary students (Peura et al., 2019), computer and information literacy for adolescents from 13 countries (Hatlevik et al., 2018), and career selection for African American (Dickinson et al., 2017) and Asian American (Kantamneni et al., 2018) college students. Maybe you are thinking, sure, higher self-efficacy is related to higher achievement because students who have more ability have higher self-efficacy. But these relationships between self-efficacy and achievement hold even when we take ability into account. For example, when students with the same ability in math and reading are compared, the ones with higher self-efficacy perform better (Grigg et al., 2018; Usher, Li, et al., 2019). Beliefs really do matter! Self-efficacy encourages higher goals, persistence, and effort. People with high selfefficacy are better equipped to take charge of their own lives—they can act with agency. What is the most motivating level of efficacy? Should students be accurate, optimistic, or pessimistic in their estimations? Evidence indicates that a higher sense of self-efficacy supports motivation, even when the belief is a slight overestimation (Bandura, 1997). Children and adults who are optimistic about the future are more mentally and physically healthy, less depressed, and more motivated to achieve (Seligman, 2006). As we saw in Cluster 2, maintaining an optimistic self-view may also serve to buffer students from the negative effects of experiencing prejudice and discrimination. After examining almost

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

GUIDELINES Supporting Self-Efficacy Emphasize students’ progress in a particular area. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Return to earlier material students have learned, and show how much “easier” it is now. Encourage students to improve on projects as they learn more. Keep examples of particularly good work in portfolios as well as work that shows growth and improvement over time, and periodically have students review and reflect on their improvements.

Set learning goals for your students, and model a mastery orientation for them. Examples 1. 2. 3.

4. 5.

Guide students to set goals that focus on gaining skill, competency, or understanding. Recognize progress and improvement. Avoid nonspecific praise. Share examples of how you have developed your abilities in a given area, and provide other models of achievement who are similar to your students—no superhuman models whose accomplishments seem unattainable. Share stories about students from a variety of circumstances who succeeded in spite of physical, mental, social, or economic challenges. Don’t excuse failure because a student has problems outside school. Help the student succeed inside school.

Make specific suggestions for improvement, and revise grades when improvements are made. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Return work with specific comments noting what the students did right, what they did wrong, and why they might have made the mistakes. Experiment with peer editing or the use of peer examples, minimizing personal comparisons. Show students how their revised, higher grade reflects greater competence and raises their class average.

Stress connections between past efforts and past accomplishments. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Have individual goal-setting and goal-review conferences with students in which you ask students to reflect on how they solved difficult problems. Confront self-defeating, failure-avoiding strategies directly. Use evidence from the students’ past accomplishments to convey your belief in their capability for growth.

For more information on self-efficacy, go to the “P20 Motivation and Learning Lab” website.

140 studies of motivation, Sandra Graham found that many African American students reported strong self-concepts, resilience, and high expectations, even in the face of difficulties (Graham, 1996; Graham & Taylor, 2002). As you might expect, there are dangers in underestimating personal capabilities because then students are less likely to put forth effort and may give up more easily. But there are dangers in continually overestimating one’s efficacy as well. Students who think that they are better readers than they actually are may not be motivated to go back and repair misunderstandings as they read. They don’t discover that they didn’t really understand the material until it is too late (Pintrich & Zusho, 2002). Students may be especially prone to overestimate their capabilities when a task is new or unfamiliar. Helping students assess the demands of the task and to evaluate their self-efficacy accordingly is important for supporting students’ academic skills and development (Osterhage et al., 2019).

481

482 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

Teachers’ Sense of Efficacy

Teachers’ sense of efficacy Teachers’ beliefs about how capable they are of reaching even the most difficult students and helping them learn.

You saw in Cluster 1 that much of my own research (Anita writing here) has focused on teachers’ sense of efficacy, defined as teachers’ beliefs that they can reach even difficult students to help them learn. This confidence belief appears to be one of the few personal characteristics of teachers that predict student achievement and motivation (Burić & Kim, 2020; Fackler et al., 2021; Lazarides et al., 2018; Woolfolk Hoy et al., 2009). When teachers take responsibility for student success or failure (rather than assigning that responsibility to student ability or to external barriers), then the teachers are more “intentional” in their approaches to reaching their students and are more successful in meeting their learning needs (Putman et al., 2009). A review of 165 studies showed that teachers with higher self-efficacy report greater job satisfaction and commitment and lower feelings of burnout (Zee & Koomen, 2016; and see Klassen et al., 2011). On the other hand, teachers who reported high emotional exhaustion and low self-efficacy were more likely to use harsher disciplinary sanctions against their students (Eddy et al., 2020). Of course, the relationship between teacher self-efficacy and these variables is reciprocal—they affect each other. For example, when teachers have a higher sense of efficacy, they teach more effectively and their students learn more, and when students learn more, teacher self-efficacy grows (Holzberger et al., 2013). Most researchers agree that teachers’ sense of efficacy is multidimensional (Fackler et al., 2021; Tschannen-Moran et al., 1998). Indeed, teachers are responsible for many different skills—they must design and deliver lessons successfully, manage the classroom flow and student behavior, and promote a high level of student engagement. Teaching successfully through and beyond a pandemic requires an ability to navigate multiple technologies (Cheng et al., 2020). Teachers also must be able to reach students from different backgrounds and with unique learning needs. Remember that self-efficacy is task-specific; teachers might feel highly confident in one area but full of self-doubt in another. How does a sense of efficacy for teaching develop? Teachers generally rely on the same four sources described earlier when judging their capabilities, although the interplay of these sources may be more complex for teachers than for students (Morris et al., 2017; Yada et al., 2019). Building a flexible set of instructional tools and strong content knowledge helps teachers gain a sense of mastery for their craft. Socially transmitted information might be just as powerful as mastery experience when it comes to self-efficacy for teaching. After all, to find out whether we are effective as teachers, we need feedback from our students. Teaching is an inherently social act. Evaluations by students, parents, and administrators convey important information about how well we are doing. Vicarious experience is another fundamental source of teaching self-efficacy. Aspiring teachers might watch video playbacks of their own mock lessons or pay close attention to how experienced teachers manage their classes. In one study, preschool teachers who discussed video playback of their teaching with the support of an instructional coach reported higher self-efficacy than did teachers who did not (von Suchodoletz et al., 2018). Both self-modeling and social modeling can lead to changes in teaching self-efficacy. Working with a proficient model who is also supportive can boost a new teacher’s self-efficacy. Klassen and Durksen (2014) found that the more support Canadian preservice teachers received from their cooperating mentor teachers, the more their self-efficacy increased and their stress decreased during an 8-week practicum. Lastly, teachers also read their own emotional and physiological states related to teaching as a sign of their efficacy or inefficacy for the task. Anxious daydreaming about classroom catastrophes can undermine self-efficacy. Feeling “in the zone” while teaching can signal to teachers that they have what it takes to help students grow. What is the optimal self-efficacy level for a teacher to have? As with any kind of self-efficacy, there may be both benefits and dangers in overestimating capabilities. Optimistic teachers probably set higher goals, work harder, reteach when necessary, and persist in the face of problems. But when beginning teachers overrate their abilities, they don’t take

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

the steps necessary to improve their skills—until they are confronted with serious problems. So some benefits might follow from having doubts about your efficacy. As Wheatley (2002) pointed out, sometimes“ doubt motivates change” (p. 18). Let’s revisit Bandura’s (1997) powerful assertion that “people’s level of motivation, affective states, and actions are based more on what they believe than on what is objectively true” (p. 2, emphasis added). Both teachers’ and students’ beliefs are guides to what they will do and feel. One area in which self-efficacy plays an important role is in self-regulation. Unless we believe we can regulate the many demands of being human, we will likely spend our time reacting to life’s circumstances rather than proactively exercising our own agency. We turn to this issue next. How can you help your students (and yourself) to lead a self-directed, self-regulated life?

Module 34 Summary Social Cognitive Theory (pp. 469–472) Distinguish between social learning and social cognitive theories.  Social learning theory expanded behavioral views of reinforcement and punishment. In behavioral views, reinforcement and punishment directly affect behavior. In social learning theory, observing another person, a model, and being reinforced or punished can have similar effects on the observer’s behavior. Social cognitive theory expands social learning theory to include cognitive factors such as beliefs, expectations, and perceptions of self. Current social cognitive theory is a dynamic system that explains human adaptation, learning, and motivation. The theory addresses how people develop social, emotional, cognitive, and behavioral capabilities; how people regulate their own lives; and what motivates them. What is triadic reciprocal causality? Triadic reciprocal causality is the dynamic interplay among three kinds of influences: personal, environmental, and behavioral. Personal factors (beliefs, expectations, cognitive abilities, motivation, attitudes, and knowledge), the physical and social environment (resources, consequences of actions, other people, models and teachers, and physical settings), and behavior (individual actions, choices, and verbal statements) all influence and are influenced by each other.

Modeling: Learning by Observing Others (pp. 472–477) What is modeling?  Learning by observing others is a key element of social cognitive theory. Modeling is influenced by the developmental characteristics of the observer, the status and prestige of the model, the consequences of the model’s actions as seen by the observer, the observer’s expectations about performing the observed behaviors (will I be rewarded?), the links that the observers perceive between their goals and the models’ behaviors (will doing what the model does get me what I want?), and the observer’s self-efficacy (can I do it?). What kinds of outcomes can observational learning encourage? Observational learning can lead to five possible outcomes: directing attention, encouraging existing behaviors, changing inhibitions, teaching new behaviors and attitudes, and arousing emotions. By directing attention, we gain insight into how others do things and what objects are involved in their actions. Encouraging or fine-tuning existing behaviors can lead to the development of good habits or can make work more efficient. Observing others also has the capacity to cue us in to others’ attention, which can cause us to become more or less “self-conscious” about our behavior; when others are doing something, it’s easier for us to do the same. Young children in particular learn by watching and emulating others, but everyone can gain insight into how something is done well (or poorly) by observing someone else do it. Finally, observing can lead to the association of emotions with certain activities. If others are observed enjoying an activity, the observer may learn to enjoy the activity as well.

Agency and Self-Efficacy (pp. 477–483) What is self-efficacy, and how is it different from other self-schemas? Self-efficacy is distinct from other self-schemas in that it involves judgments of capabilities specific to a particular task. Self-concept is a more global construct that contains more general evaluative perceptions about

483

484 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation the self in comparison to others. Self-efficacy also differs from self-esteem. Self-efficacy is concerned with judgments of personal capabilities; self-esteem is concerned with judgments of self-worth. What are the sources of self-efficacy? Four sources of self-efficacy are direct experiences (of mastery or failure), vicarious experiences (performances that are modeled by someone else), social persuasion (evaluative judgments by others or specific performance feedback), and level of emotional or physiological arousal you experience as you face the task. How does self-efficacy affect motivation? Greater self-efficacy leads to greater effort, persistence in the face of setbacks, higher goals, and finding new strategies when old ones fail. If sense of efficacy is low, however, people may avoid a task altogether or give up easily when problems arise. What is teachers’ sense of efficacy? One of the few personal characteristics of teachers related to student achievement is a teacher’s sense of self-efficacy—the belief that he or she can reach even difficult students to help them learn. Teachers with a high sense of efficacy work harder, persist longer, and are less likely to experience burnout. Teachers’ sense of efficacy is higher in schools where the other teachers and administrators have high expectations for students and where teachers receive help from their principals in solving instructional and management problems. Self-efficacy grows from real success with students, so any experience or training that helps you succeed in the day-to-day tasks of teaching will give you a foundation for developing a sense of efficacy in your career. There may be some benefits to a lower sense of efficacy, if this encourages teachers to pursue professional development and improvement.

MODULE 35

Self-Regulation in Learning and Teaching

Learning Objective 11.4 Describe important components of self-regulated learning. Learning Objective 11.5 Apply your knowledge to teach for self-efficacy and self-regulated learning. Learning Objective 11.6 Explain the meaning and different applications of four basic theories of learning.

Self-regulation Process of activating and sustaining thoughts, behaviors, and emotions in order to reach goals.

Self-regulated learning A view of learning that emphasizes the role of both skill and will when analyzing learning tasks, setting goals, applying skills, maintaining motivation, monitoring progress, and making adjustments about how learning is carried out.

Self-Regulated Learning: Skill and Will As you may remember from the beginning of this cluster, Albert Bandura said his early education in a tiny school in Canada had given him a set of self-regulation skills that lasted a lifetime. He believed that a major goal of all education is to “equip students with the intellectual tools, self-beliefs, and self-regulatory capabilities to educate themselves throughout their lifetime” (Bandura, 1993, p. 136). Self-regulation is the process we use to activate and sustain our thoughts, attention, behaviors, and emotions in order to reach our goals (Perry & Rahim, 2011). When the goals involve learning, we talk about self-regulated learning. Self-regulated learners have the skill and will to learn—they are “metacognitive, motivated to learn, and strategic” (Perry & Rahim, 2011, p. 122). This means they have a combination of academic learning skills, self-awareness, self-control, and motivation for learning. Self-regulated learners are also able to adapt their behaviors to the particular conditions they face and to inhibit the desire to act when doing so would be inappropriate or disadvantageous. To continue learning independently throughout life, you must be self-regulated— what we sometimes refer to in conversations as a self-starter. But starting an activity is only part of being a good self-regulator. With so much information and distraction instantly at our fingertips on our digital devices, remaining deeply engaged in learning can be a challenge. How do you persist and stay focused on your goal as you browse all that

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

information and are distracted by texts, tweets, and fascinating videos of cute kittens? Or, as someone recently posted on my social media feed, “Do you control your technology, or does it control you?” Stop & Think Think about the class you are taking for which you are using this textbook. On a 7-point scale—from 1 = not at all true of me to 7 = very true of me— answer the following questions: 1. When I study for a test, I try to put together the information from class and from the book. 2. When I do homework, I try to remember what the teacher said in class so I can answer the questions correctly. 3. I’m certain I can understand the ideas taught in this class. 4. I ask myself questions to make sure I know the material I have been studying. 5. Even when study materials are dull and uninteresting, I keep working until I finish. By answering the questions in the Stop & Think, you have just responded to five items from the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ) (Midgley et al., 1998; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990). This questionnaire has been used in hundreds of studies to assess students’ self-regulated learning and motivation. How did you do? The first two questions assess your use of cognitive strategies, like those we discussed in Cluster 9. The third question assesses your sense of efficacy for this class. But the last two questions (4 and 5) assess specific self-regulation strategies involved in studying. Self-regulated learners transform their mental abilities, whatever they are, into academic skills and strategies. Students who are better at regulating their thoughts, feelings, motivation, and behavior in school tend to perform better academically (Dent & Koenka, 2016). In middle school and high school, strategy use and studying are central skills for academic success. In earlier grade levels, self-regulation of attention and emotion are particularly important for learning and achieving in school. In fact, regardless of their developmental level, students need to be able to understand the tasks they face, plan ahead for how they will perform those tasks, monitor their progress, and adjust their strategies as needed along the way.

How Does Self-Regulation Develop? The concept of self-regulated learning integrates much of what is known about effective learning and motivation. Three major factors influence the development of skill and will: knowledge, motivation, and self-discipline or volition. As children grow, so does their repertoire of self-regulatory skills and their ability to deploy those skills in different circumstances. To be self-regulated learners, students need knowledge about themselves, the subject, the task, strategies for learning, and the contexts in which they will apply their learning. “Expert” students know about themselves and can reflect on their own learning processes. This “metacognitive knowledge” (remember that metacognition refers to thinking about your own thinking) includes knowing your optimal learning approaches, what is easy and what is hard about learning certain material, coping strategies for handling difficult material, your interests and talents, and how to use your strengths (Efklides, 2011). Experts also know quite a bit about the subject being studied, and they can adapt their knowledge to meet new demands; the more they know, the easier it is to learn more (Alexander et al., 2009). They are more likely to understand that different learning tasks require different approaches and effort on their part. A simple memory task, for example, might require a mnemonic strategy (see Cluster 8), whereas a complex comprehension task might be approached through more effortful practices like concept maps of the key ideas or frequent self-quizzing

KNOWLEDGE.

485

486 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation (Carpenter et al., 2020; see Cluster 9). Also, these self-regulated learners know that learning is often difficult and knowledge is seldom absolute; there usually are different ways of looking at problems as well as different solutions (Greene et al., 2010; Winne, 2017). These expert students not only know what each task requires but also can apply the strategy needed. They can skim or read carefully. They can use memory strategies or reorganize the material. As they become more knowledgeable in a field, they apply many of these strategies automatically. In short, they have mastered a large, flexible repertoire of learning, help-seeking, and coping strategies (see Cluster 9). Finally, self-regulated learners think about the contexts in which they will apply their knowledge—when and where they will use their learning—so they can set motivating goals and connect their present efforts to their accomplishments (Winne, 2017). Clearly, helping students develop both content knowledge and self-knowledge will be important for helping students become self-regulated. Later in the cluster, we consider some suggestions for how to do this. Knowing about what is needed to accomplish your learning goals is important, but it may not be sufficient. Motivation also influences the degree to which students regulate their own learning. In Cluster 12, you will learn about many facets of motivation, but let’s consider a few that researchers have found to be related to self-regulated learning. Students who demonstrate greater individual interest in an activity tend to be better at regulating themselves in that activity (Renninger & Hidi, 2016; Thoman et al., 2017). My neighbor, a 10-year-old basketball enthusiast, can spend hours shooting hoops in her driveway, drawing elaborate defense plans for her team, and studying the moves of her role models. Activities that are valued, whether for their utility in getting us to the next level or simply for the enjoyment they bring, can lead us to deeper and more systematic engagement (Eccles et al., 2015). Motivational beliefs also influence self-regulation. For example, students who have higher self-efficacy for managing their learning tasks (i.e., self-efficacy for self-regulation) not only use more effective selfregulatory strategies but also perform better in school (Usher & Schunk, 2018). In addition, they believe their own intelligence and abilities are improvable (Job et al., 2015). Even if they are not intrinsically motivated by a particular task, they are serious about getting the intended benefit from it. Those who believe in their capabilities are better able to focus their attention and other cognitive and emotional resources for the task at hand. They know why they are studying, so their actions and choices feel more self-determined and less controlled by others (Zimmerman, 2011). Effective self-regulation also includes understanding how to motivate yourself and stay motivated during all phases of a task, a process called metamotivation (Sansone et al., 2019; Scholer et al., 2018). But knowledge and motivation are not always enough. Self-regulated learners need volition or self-discipline. As Lyn Corno once said, “Where motivation denotes commitment, volition denotes follow-through” (Corno, 1992, p. 72).

MOTIVATION.

I am one month behind in this project (this is Ellen writing). I have been up writing at 5:00 a.m. every day. Some days I start the day distracted with other tasks or barely awake, but I want to keep writing because the deadline for this cluster is very near (well, passed actually). I have knowledge and motivation, but to keep going I need a good dose of volition. Volition is an old-fashioned word for willpower. In more technical terms, volition involves an internally driven, intentional decision to act on behalf of valued goals (Haggard, 2019). Typically, students need volition to overcome the resistance they feel when they experience a conflict between two desired outcomes (Oettingen et al., 2015). For example, you might need an act of volition to decide between hanging out with friends or doing some reading that is required for a paper due in a few weeks. In this way, an act of volition is voluntary, that is, a free will behavior. This makes volitional acts distinct from acts that are automatic or habitual. Almost all students (and teachers!) have experienced the conflict that arises when shortterm non-academic goals seem more appealing than long-term academic goals. For example, when over 1,000 middle and high school students were asked to describe such dilemmas,

VOLITION.

Volition Willpower; an internally-driven, intentional decision to reach valued goals by applying selfregulated learning.

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

they mentioned the tension between meeting academic requirements and watching Netflix, socializing with friends, giving in to their emotions, or just daydreaming (Duckworth et al., 2016). What helps people act volitionally in the service of longer-term, valued goals? For one thing, people need to feel a sense of control over the tasks they face (Efklides, 2011). Experience sticking with tasks to reach goals helps people understand their own active agency in achieving success (Metcalfe & Greene, 2007). I can stick with this writing because I have done similar writing before and held the finished product in my hands. Self-regulated learners know how to protect themselves from distractions—by choosing situations that are more conducive to studying, for example, so they are not interrupted (Duckworth et al., 2019). And they know what to do when they are tempted to stop working and have (another) cup of coffee—the temptation I’m facing now—that, and a beautiful day that beckons me to go out and work in the yard. (Yard work always looks appealing when I face a tough writing job—doing laundry is a close second.) Volition is deliberate and effortful, but with practice it can become more automatic—a habit or a “work ethic” (Corno, 2011). William James knew this over 100 years ago. He told teachers that acts of will (i.e., voluntary attention and intentional decision making) make up a sliver of our daily lives and that habits make up most of what we do. Therefore, James urged teachers to seize every opportunity to help their students choose well, even if those choices were difficult. What is a volitional choice today, James said, becomes a habit tomorrow. A newly developed habit then serves as a “‘shortcut’ self-control strategy” (Fiorella, 2020, p. 607). A longitudinal study of first- through sixth-grade students showed that students whose work habits increased the most during middle childhood had higher high school achievement (Simpkins et al., 2020). The work habits children developed early in school were positively related to their educational attainment at age 26! You may have heard of two concepts related to volition: self-control and grit. Both concepts have received growing attention in recent years. Self-control has been defined as “the ability to alter cognition, emotion, impulsive tendencies, and behaviors in the service of social norms, personal standards, and goals” (Li et al., 2021, p. 74). Walter Mischel’s famous delay of gratification experiment in the late 1980s provides a glimpse into the importance of self-control (Mischel, 2014). He offered 4-year-old children an enticing marshmallow to eat and promised to give a second marshmallow to those who could postpone eating the first marshmallow until he returned from a 15-minute errand. (That’s a long time!) Most children gobbled up the first marshmallow before he returned, but others waited, employing various self-control strategies to help them resist the temptation. (You can view this experiment online in several funny YouTube videos.) Mischel and his colleagues then conducted follow-up studies with these children over a period of many years. They were shocked at what they found. Children who had exercised self-control by waiting for the second marshmallow had higher SAT scores, responded better to stress, had fewer health problems, possessed better social skills, and enjoyed a host of other benefits. Mischel’s study has been replicated with a more diverse sample of students, and the outcomes have been quite similar (Falk et al., 2020; Watts et al., 2018). Does this mean that some people are just naturally good at self-control and others are not? No, not exactly. Although some children are predisposed for greater self-control, environmental factors also affect children’s ability to exercise self-control. Everyone can learn new self-control strategies, and those with more strategies perform better in school (Duckworth et al., 2014, 2019). Volition is not always a momentary affair. Sometimes it takes years to accomplish a long-term goal (college degree, anyone?). What does it take to stay committed for the long haul? Researchers have recently claimed that it takes true grit—a personality trait characterized by “perseverance and passion to pursue long-term goals” (Duckworth et al., 2007, p. 1087). Angela Duckworth and her colleagues claim that students with more grit do better in school and in life (Duckworth, 2016; Duckworth et al., 2007). Over the past decade, grit has become quite a buzzword in education circles. Recent conferences and publications targeting K–12 and higher education personnel have paid a great deal of attention to the subject. Is grit really an important determinant of student success? The Point/Counterpoint looks at the evidence from diverse psychological perspectives.

487

Grit A personality trait characterized by passion and persistence to meet long-term goals.

488 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Are “Grittier” Students More Successful? Most of us would agree that those who passionately persist toward their goals tend to meet with more success. Grit might be one important aspect of the volition component of selfregulation, but does it warrant all the attention it is receiving? Some education researchers have cast doubt on grit as a new phenomenon, citing mixed evidence of its relationship to success. These scholars emphasize that other self-regulatory factors are just as (if not more) important for determining student success. Let’s look at these perspectives more closely.

Point Grit is essential for student success, and teachers should therefore promote it. The jacket of Paul Tough’s 2012 best-selling book How Children Succeed begins with the age-old question “Why do some children succeed while others fail?” His answer is in his subtitle: Grit, Curiosity, and the Hidden Power of Character. Tough argues that success in school requires more than outstanding intellectual abilities and talent. To end up at the top of one’s class requires not just talent but a repertoire of “soft” skills—sometimes called “noncognitive factors” (Farrington et al., 2012). For Tough, such skills may be the deciding factor in whether children can escape dire circumstances: “There is no antipoverty tool we can provide for disadvantaged young people that will be more valuable than the character strengths . . . [such as] conscientiousness, grit, resilience, perseverance, and optimism” (p. 189). Tough’s claims lean on the work of psychologist and former school teacher Angela Duckworth, who similarly wondered whether factors other than IQ or talent—things like effort, determination, self-control, persistence, and what she named grit—might actually be just as important in predicting success. To investigate this, Duckworth and her associates asked hundreds of young people to rate their grittiness by responding to items like “I finish whatever I begin,” and “I am diligent” (see http://www.angeladuckworth.com). They found that students with higher grit scores also had higher SAT scores and grades (Duckworth et al., 2007; Duckworth & Quinn, 2009; Park et al., 2018). Grittier kids placed higher in the National Spelling Bee competition (Duckworth & Quinn, 2009). Chicago high school juniors with more grit were more likely to graduate on time (Eskreis-Winkler et al., 2014). These findings often held true even when ability measures (IQ, past GPA) were included as predictors. Even novice teachers’ level of grit was found to predict their teaching effectiveness (Duckworth et al., 2009). How can grit predict success better than one’s previous success? Duckworth (2016) offers her theory: “When you consider individuals in identical circumstances, what each achieves depends on just two things, talent and effort . . . but effort factors into the calculations twice, not once” (p. 42). First, putting forth initial effort is necessary to build skills. Second, maintaining effort is needed to apply skills to be productive. In their respective 2016 books, Duckworth and Tough argued that even though grit is a personality trait, it can also be cultivated. Their books highlight stories from numerous paragons of grit and offer tips for how parents, teachers, and leaders can promote gritty individuals. Although both authors have cautioned readers that promoting students’ grit may not be an easy fix, their books flew off the shelves. The two are frequently invited keynote speakers at meetings for school leaders across the United States. Clearly, grit has found a solid fan base among educators. But grit also has its fair share of critics.

Counterpoint A focus on grit as the key to success is too narrow. Education policy makers and researchers have been wary about the phenomenon of grit as a key to success. Grit’s popularity has spawned contentious commentary in media outlets such as The Atlantic (“The Limitations of Teaching ‘Grit’ in the Classroom,” “Is Grit Overrated? The Downsides of Dogged, Single-Minded Persistence”), The New York Times (“Putting Grit in Its Place”), and EdWeek (“Is ‘Grit’ Racist?”). The debate has several prongs (for a summary, see Alfie Kohn’s

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

2014 article in the Washington Post “Sometimes It’s Better to Quit Than to Prove Your Grit”). First, some argue that to focus on grit, an individual-level variable, as the deciding factor in student success is to blame students for their own school (mis)fortunes and to let schools off the hook; this distracts from examining systemic inequalities that place a greater burden on some students than others (Gorski, 2016; Love, 2019). Furman University Professor Paul Thomas (2014) is among those who see the promotion of grit as veiled racism/classism: Part of the “grit” narrative includes the assumption that successful students and people (read “White”) are successful primarily because they work hard; they earn their success. The flip side of this “grit” narrative is that unsuccessful students and people (read “African American” and “Latino/a”) are unsuccessful because they simply do not try hard enough. Second, many have noted that grit appears to be a rebranding of well-researched variables such as conscientiousness, willpower, persistence, and resilience (Rimfeld et al., 2016). In fact, researchers have found little empirical distinction between new measures of grit and established measures, such as conscientiousness (Ponnock et al., 2020). Although Duckworth and her associates have tried to draw distinctions among some of these ideas, such as between grit and self-control (Duckworth & Gross, 2014), researchers have found considerable overlap between students’ responses to grit items and to other measures of self-regulation (Muenks et al., 2017). They question grit’s uniqueness. Third, regarding the link between grit and success, some evidence shows that this relationship is not as conclusive as Duckworth and colleagues’ earlier work suggested. Marcus Credé and his colleagues (2017) examined reports from 66,807 students and found that in some academic settings, grit is unrelated to students’ academic achievement. An investigation of Boston-area eighth-graders also revealed that grit was not significantly correlated with students’ achievement in math or language arts (West et al., 2016). Furthermore, when researchers have considered grit alongside other variables hypothesized to predict student performance, grit loses its predictive power (Muenks et al., 2017; Steinmayr et al., 2018). For example, middle school students’ math and reading self-efficacy, but not grit, predicted their school grades, standardized test scores, and teachers’ ratings of their math and reading competence (Usher, Li, et al., 2019). Perhaps students are unlikely to persist at something unless they believe they can be successful.

Beware of Either/Or The idea that success is within reach to anyone willing to show dedication and persistence strikes a powerful chord. Maybe most of us believe as William James (1907) did that “we are making use of only a small part of our possible mental and physical resources” (p. 3). We are intrigued by the hopeful idea that empowering (or debilitating) psychological factors can explain the different life outcomes of two people with similar ability. A quick look at Oscarnominated films reveals our fascination—we pay money to hear and watch stories of dogged perseverance in the face of great odds. They inspire us to consider our own higher potential. Ultimately, however, the research evidence does not support such a simple story. Social cognitive theory acknowledges that many factors, both internal and external to the individual, must be considered. Yes, self-regulation matters in how successful a learner will be. And a certain level of grit may be one important aspect of self-regulation. But a one-variable approach is insufficient—appealing as it might be. Focusing too much on grit might lead educators to overlook other problems that affect students’ self-regulation. As this cluster explains, to be self-regulated requires the engagement of cognitive, metacognitive, motivational, and volitional mechanisms, none of which casts the deciding vote on whether a student will ultimately succeed. In addition, we must consider the larger context in which learning takes place. Is what we are asking students to do worth their perseverance and effort in the first place? Does grit come at the expense of other aspects of a quality and “successful” education—such as one’s full humanity, physical and psychological health, compassion for others, autonomy, and lifelong curiosity (Love, 2019)? Once again, it’s not as simple as either/or.

489

490 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

Co-regulation A transitional phase during which students gradually appropriate self-regulated learning and skills through modeling, direct teaching, feedback, and coaching from teachers, parents, or peers.

Shared regulation Students and teachers working together to regulate each other through reminders, prompts, and other guidance.

DEVELOPMENTAL CHANGES IN SELF-REGULATION. How do children develop the knowledge, motivation, and volition for optimal functioning? The social cognitive theory perspective emphasizes the interaction of personal factors (e.g., neurobiological development, personal beliefs, cognition), environmental/social factors (e.g., basic needs support, cognitive engagement from family, peers, teachers), and behavioral factors. For example, behavioral self-regulation develops rapidly during early childhood and the preschool years, but personal and environmental factors are related to this development. Being a girl, having better language skills, and having a mother with a higher education level are factors associated with earlier development of behavioral self-regulation among preschool-aged children (Greene et al., 2010; Montroy et al., 2016). Self-regulation generally improves over time as learners’ neurobiological systems grow and adapt, but environmental stressors and social isolation can thwart development (Blair & Raver, 2015). Even so, early school experiences can help students get back on track. During early and middle childhood, many self-regulation skills are learned through observation and emulation (White & DiBenedetto, 2018). As learners become competent in applying the skills, they begin to internalize modeled strategies and demonstrate greater levels of self-control. They can eventually engage in independent, adaptive self-regulation in new situations. Self-regulation relies on cognitive development too, as explained in Clusters 4, 8, and 9. Three key executive functions that underlie successful self-regulation are inhibitory control (resisting impulsive urges), working memory (holding information in mind while performing other mental operations), and cognitive flexibility (seeing something from different perspectives). These executive functions are necessary for the self-regulation required to solve complex problems (Cole et al., 2019; Diamond, 2012). Evidence has shown that adolescents across the globe increase in their executive functioning skills and plateau toward their mid-20s. However, late adolescence is accompanied by a heightened period of sensation seeking and risk-taking, which may make it challenging for teens to make good decisions, as parents and teachers know well (Steinberg et al., 2018). How can teachers and parents promote better executive functioning among learners of all ages? Adele Diamond and Daphne Ling (2016, 2019) reviewed results from 179 research studies that tested various interventions. The studies showed that executive functions are impaired under conditions of stress, poor health, or emotional turmoil. Executive functions are enhanced when learners are relaxed and healthy and feel a greater sense of connectedness to others. Mindfulness practices with a movement component (e.g., tai chi) were most effective for improving students’ executive functioning skills. School programs that emphasized the development of social and emotional competencies were helpful, too. Executive functions are also sharpened when activities are challenging and when frequent opportunities for sustained practice are provided (Diamond, 2012). Two social processes support the development of self-regulation: co-regulation and shared regulation. Co-regulation is a transitional phase during which students gradually appropriate and internalize self-regulated learning skills through modeling, direct teaching, feedback, and coaching from teachers, parents, or peers. Shared regulation occurs when students and teachers work together to regulate each other through reminders, prompts, and other guidance (Hadwin et al., 2018). Now that you have learned about the components of self-regulation and how it develops, let’s examine a self-regulation model from the perspective of social cognitive theory. What does self-regulation in action look like?

A Social Cognitive Model of Self-Regulated Learning Albert Bandura may have gone from high school graduate to professor at Stanford in six years using his self-regulated learning knowledge and skills, but not all of your students will have the ability to be Banduras with established habits of cognition, motivation, and volition. In fact, many will be like Tracy, a high school student described here by self-regulation expert Barry Zimmerman: An important mid-term math exam is two weeks away, and [Tracy] had begun to study while listening to popular music “to relax her.” Tracy has not set any study

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

goals for herself—instead she simply tells herself to do as well as she can on the test. She uses no specific learning strategies for condensing and memorizing important material and does not plan out her study time, so she ends up cramming for a few hours before the test. She has only vague self-evaluative standards and cannot gauge her academic preparation accurately. Tracy attributes her learning difficulties to an inherent lack of mathematical ability and is very defensive about her poor study methods. However, she does not ask for help from others because she is afraid of “looking stupid,” or seek out supplementary materials from the library because she “already has too much to learn.” She finds studying to be anxiety-provoking, has little self-confidence in achieving success, and sees little intrinsic value in acquiring mathematical skill. (Zimmerman, 2002, p. 64) Clearly, Tracy is unlikely to do well on the test. What would help? For an answer, let’s consider Zimmerman’s (2013) model of self-regulated learning shown in Figure 11.2. According to Zimmerman, self-regulation involves a three-phase cycle—forethought, performance, and reflection. In Phase 1, the forethought phase, Tracy needs to set clear, reasonable goals and plan a few strategies for accomplishing those goals. And Tracy’s motivational beliefs make a difference at this point, too. If Tracy had a high self-efficacy for applying the strategies that she planned, if she believed that using those strategies would lead to math learning and success on the test, if she saw some connections between her own interests and the math learning, and if she were trying to master the material—not just look good or avoid looking bad—then she would be on the road to self-regulated learning (Usher & Schunk, 2018). After thinking about the task at hand, Tracy moves into Phase 2—the performance phase. Performing brings new challenges. Now Tracy must have a repertoire of self-control (volitional) and learning strategies. She might use imagery, mnemonics, attention focusing, and other techniques such as those described in Clusters 8 and 9 to remain engaged (Duckworth et al., 2014). She also will need to self-observe—monitor how things are going— so she can change strategies if needed. Actual recording of time spent, problems solved, or pages written may provide clues about when or how to make the best use of study time. Turning off the music might help, too. Finally, Tracy needs to move to Phase 3—the reflection phase—by looking back on her performance and reflecting on what happened. Here, her self-regulation involves an evaluation of her performance (Panadero et al., 2017). What worked and what didn’t? Why?

Figure 11.2 Zimmerman’s Three-Phase Model of Self-Regulated Learning

• Task analysis • Motivation • Planning and goal setting

Performance • Self-monitoring • Strategy use • Self-talk

• Attributions for success/failure • Self-reaction • Revision of strategies and beliefs

Reflection

Forethought

At all three phases: • Self-observation • Self-evaluation • Self-reaction

491

492 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation If she attributes successes to effort and good strategy use and avoids self-defeating actions and beliefs, she might increase her self-efficacy for doing a similar task in the future. She might also revise her goals for next time or give herself a reward for a job well done. If, on the other hand, Tracy reflects on her own weak efforts, she might pretend to not care or assume she is “no good at math.” Each phase in Zimmerman’s model flows into the next, and the cycle continues as students encounter new learning challenges. You might recall that the capacity for self-influence is a hallmark of social cognitive theory (Bandura, 2018). At all phases of the self-regulatory cycle, learners must observe and evaluate themselves. Most of the time, learners’ self-regulation depends on their own self-set or internalized standards. “If adequate self-standards are lacking, people exercise little self-directedness” (Bandura, 1986, p. 363). Part of your role as a teacher is helping learners internalize reasonable standards that will help them become good self-regulators. We have looked at just one theoretical model of self-regulated learning. There are actually several models of self-regulated learning, but all agree that the cognitive processes needed for self-regulated learning require effort (see Panadero, 2017, for a review of six models). Let’s now look at two classroom examples.

Reaching Every Student: Technology and Self-Regulation In Cluster  10, we saw some examples of how technology-rich environments permit students to engage more interactively as they explore complex concepts. Over the past decade, and particularly during the COVID-19 pandemic, many schools have turned to technology-mediated instruction through remote learning, one-to-one laptop initiatives, online course offerings, and hybrid teaching approaches (Zheng et al., 2016). Well-designed, technology-enhanced resources can also benefit students with disabilities. What are the skills needed for meaningful learning to take place in these environments? Without a doubt, students need metacognitive and self-regulatory skills so they won’t get lost in a sea of information—and potential distraction. They need to actively evaluate the credibility and trustworthiness of information they find online. They also need motivation to stay focused in cognitively demanding, and often socially isolated, online worlds (Mayer, 2014). When the concepts students are learning are challenging and complicated, the presentation of scaffolds and prompts in the online learning context can enhance students’ self-regulation (Azevedo et al., 2018; Moos, 2018). Including learning supports to reduce the cognitive burden on students’ processing is especially important for supporting self-regulated learning in technology-rich environments (see Universal Design for Learning guidelines at https://udlguidelines.cast.org and in Cluster 14). Does too much technology impair students’ self-regulation? This probably depends on which technologies are being used and which aspects of self-regulation are important. For example, researchers have been curious about whether taking notes by typing is better or worse for students’ learning than taking notes by handwriting them. One study found that students who used laptops to take notes wrote more but performed worse on a subsequent exam than students who took notes by hand (Mueller & Oppenheimer, 2014). When some students were asked to respond to phone text messages while taking notes for class, they performed even worse (Flanigan & Titsworth, 2020). This is why I turned my phone facedown while writing this cluster! Having to monitor multiple technological devices comes at a price—divided attention and more shallow engagement with content. Even being in close proximity to distracted people can disrupt the self-regulation process. When college students were sitting in a place where they could see peers who were multitasking on laptops, they performed worse on a test (Sana et al., 2013). Findings like these are among the reasons some teachers (even tech experts) have decided to place limits on their students’ use of digital devices in the classroom (Shirky, 2014). Not only must students navigate the content-relevant information at their fingertips, they also frequently end up navigating their social relationships in the same digital spaces. (How many times have you checked your phone or social media since you began reading this cluster?)

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

How could you provide self-regulation teaching or coaching for your students who are learning with technology? How might you model successful self-regulated learning strategies for your students while using multimedia in your classroom or during remote instruction? These questions are becoming increasingly critical for today’s teachers. As social cognitive theory would indicate, the digital environment can influence both the ways we think and how we behave.

Self-Regulation of Emotions School days can be filled with all kinds of emotions—some are helpful for learning, but some are not. Emotion regulation is critical for academic and personal development (Weissberg et al., 2015). Individuals who effectively interpret emotions (e.g., anxiety, anger, frustration, excitement) in themselves and others, identify goals that incorporate those emotional signals, and eventually regulate emotion and behavior in ways that maximize successful engagement in social situations are characterized as having high emotional intelligence, a concept you learned about in Cluster 5. Higher emotional intelligence leads to greater well-being and academic performance (Cassady & Boseck, 2008; MacCann et al., 2020). Undergirding emotional intelligence is the ability to effectively regulate one’s emotions. Let’s take a closer look at the importance of emotion regulation in both learning and teaching. Students need effective coping strategies to address the social and emotional aspects of a learning situation so that their emotional response becomes an asset, and not a barrier, to successfully achieving educational goals. These coping strategies rely on a broad range of skills. You learned about some of the core social and emotional skills and competencies involved in learning in Table 4.2 (back in Cluster 4). Those include self-awareness, self-management, social awareness, relationship skills, and responsible decision making (http://www.casel.org). Over the past decade, numerous school-based programs have been launched to help teachers and students cultivate skills for regulating their social and emotional environments. Learners who take part in school-based programs designed to help them develop better emotion regulation skills, prosocial behaviors, and social competence have greater mental health, positive social adjustment, and academic achievement in the short and long term (Domitrovich et al., 2017; Jones et al., 2019). Researchers at the Harvard Graduate School of Education have created a useful tool that compares the social emotional learning features of a variety of programs: http://exploresel.gse.harvard.edu/compare-frameworks. This tool can help you understand how different programs support the social and emotional self-regulation skills described previously and elsewhere in this cluster. Developing competence in social and emotional self-regulation is especially important at pivotal moments in brain development, such as during the early years, when students are learning how to learn in schools (Immordino-Yang et al., 2019). During the preschool years, emotion regulation becomes more integrated with structured learning. For example, Carlos Valiente and his colleagues (2010) followed almost 300 students through kindergarten to assess the relations between effortful self-control, emotionality, and academic achievement. They found that students’ anger, sadness, and shyness were negatively related to achievement and that self-control was positively related to achievement, particularly for students who showed lower levels of negative emotions. Another key period of development occurs during adolescence, when learners experience increased sensitivity to social cues and their changing bodies. Negative emotions, such as anxiety, shame, or guilt, can undermine learners’ attention and motivation (Ben-Eliyahu, 2019). Ruminating over negative experiences requires great cognitive and emotional energy, which distracts from academic learning. Clearly, helping students develop emotional self-regulation skills can set them on a good path for learning in school, increase their resilience, and support healthy relationships with their peers. After all, emotional, behavioral,

SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL LEARNING.

493

494 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation and cognitive self-regulation are nearly always intertwined. Difficulties in one area usually mean difficulties in another. The good news is that social and emotional competencies are malleable—they can be taught (Schonert-Reichl, 2019). How can teachers help their students better regulate their social and emotional lives? One primary way, says Daniel Goleman (2015), is by setting an example. “The teacher (and entire school staff, for that matter) ideally would model emotional and social competence for students” (p. 594). This is likely why many successful social and emotional learning programs are designed to work with teachers’ emotion regulation skills first. As Kimberly Schonert-Reichl (2017) noted, “Stress in the classroom is contagious—simply put, stressed-out teachers tend to have stressed-out students” (p. 137). Teachers therefore need to receive emotional and social regulation instruction as part of their own training because they are key influencers in their students’ academic and social environment (Valiente et al., 2020). Although some colleges of education still offer little in the way of social-emotional training, several outstanding programs have been designed to equip teachers with skills to help themselves and their students become better at regulating emotions. For example, the RULER program (Recognizing, Understanding, Labeling, Expressing, and Regulating emotions) aims first to help adults build their own emotional intelligence so that they can model emotion regulation skills and cultivate a supportive environment for their students (see a review of this and other programs by Jones et al., 2017). Fifth- and sixth-grade teachers who used the RULER approach had higher levels of connection and warmth with their students and were more supportive of student autonomy, a key factor related to motivation, as you will learn in Cluster 12 (Rivers et al., 2013). Another way to support students’ emotion regulation is through a teaching approach that is culturally inclusive and sustaining, as you learned about in Clusters 2 and 3 (Jagers et al., 2019). Relationships built on trust and respect help students activate positive emotions and down-regulate negative ones (more about this in Cluster 13). Opportunities for students to take part in culturally supportive communities (e.g., Black student union, LGBTQ+ alliances) can increase students’ feelings of safety and well-being while providing social and emotional support. Such programs may be particularly important during adolescence, as young people grapple with who they are as individuals and members of distinct social groups. Designing lessons that expose learners to diverse viewpoints and experiences also broadens learners’ understanding and empathy for each other and for people who may not be represented in your classroom. Above all, it is important to remember that well-intentioned social and emotional learning approaches that do not challenge interpersonal and structural inequalities are not serving their purpose. As Dena Simmons (2021) pointed out, an approach that fosters social and emotional well-being involves “taking a stand against the injustices we witness in our districts, schools, and classrooms [and] creating space for processing the collective traumas of the pandemic, racism, and other forms of oppression in our school communities.” The Guidelines: Encouraging Emotional Self-Regulation on the next page give some ideas of practices that promote emotion regulation. We mentioned that teachers are instrumental socializing agents for students’ emotion regulation, but they also need strategies for managing job-related stress. In the first years of teaching, high levels of stress lead to “burnout” for teachers who are not able to develop effective coping strategies to handle the many pressures that they face (Chang, 2009). The most commonly reported sources of teacher stress are student misbehaviors, interpersonal challenges, and work-related pressure—such as meeting personal or externally imposed standards (Cano-Garcia et al., 2005; Griffith et al., 1999). The COVID-19 pandemic multiplied the stressors that many teachers face, particularly when they had to carry out duties remotely and then adapt instruction to meet the needs of both in-person and remote learners. Many teachers returning to work faced considerable risks to their own health and the health of their families.

TEACHER STRESS AND SELF-REGULATION.

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

GUIDELINES Encouraging Emotional Self-Regulation Create a climate of trust in your classroom. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Avoid listening to “tattletale” stories about students. Follow through with fair consequences. Avoid unnecessary comparisons, and give students opportunities to improve their work.

Help students recognize and express their feelings. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Provide a vocabulary of emotions, and note descriptions of emotions in characters or stories. Be clear and descriptive about your own emotions. Encourage students to write in journals about their own feelings. Protect the privacy of these writings (see trust in the first part of the feature).

Help students recognize emotions in others, and develop empathy and compassion. Examples 1. 2.

For young children, “Look at Chandra’s face. How do you think she feels when you say those things?” For older students, use readings, analysis of diverse characters in literature, films, or role reversals to help them identify the emotions of others.

Provide strategies for coping with emotions. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Discuss or practice alternatives such as stopping to think how the other person feels, seeking help, or leaving the scene. Use centering practices such as self-talk, deep breathing, or mindful movement to diffuse or prevent emotional outbursts. Model strategies for students. Talk about how you handle anger, disappointment, or anxiety.

Help students recognize cultural differences in how people express emotion. Examples 1. 2.

Have students write about or discuss how they show emotions in their family. Teach students to “check it out.” Ask the other people how they are feeling.

For ideas about promoting emotional competence, see http://casel.org.

Teachers who are most effective at handling stressors in the classroom demonstrate both the high levels of teaching self-efficacy discussed earlier and a positive level of emotional self-regulation (Kim et al., 2019; Schonert-Reichl et al., 2017). Teachers are more likely to maintain a healthy professional life if they can manage professional stressors such as student disruptions and pressures from parents without becoming emotionally charged, keep things in perspective, and seek support from their peers (Collie et al., 2012; Jennings & Frank, 2015; Woolfolk Hoy, 2013). When teachers feel supported by their administrators, they report feeling less stress. For example, teachers who felt that their school leaders gave them autonomy in their work reported greater emotional “buoyancy”—that is, they felt more able to manage their stress (Collie, 2021). By contrast, when teachers felt pressured or controlled or that their autonomy was thwarted by restrictive policies, they reported feeling emotionally exhausted. Several approaches have been found to help reduce teachers’ stress. One promising practice is mindfulness training, which helps teachers focus attention and emotional resources on the present situation without judgment, recognizing and releasing unnecessary

495

496 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation expectations and biases, and developing greater compassion for self and others (Roeser et al., 2013). Another approach, the Cultivating Awareness and Resilience in Education (CARE) program, provides mindfulness-based professional development to K–12 educators. Teachers who were randomly assigned to receive the CARE training, which features emotional skill development, mindfulness-based stress reduction, and compassion training, showed significant gains in well-being, self-efficacy, and mindfulness, along with reduced burnout compared with teachers assigned to a control group (Jennings et al., 2013, 2017). The program also led to better emotional support of students. A meta-analysis of similar studies showed that engagement in mindfulness practices was associated with decreases in teacher stress, anxiety, depression, and burnout (Zarate et al., 2019).

Teaching Toward Self-Efficacy and Self-Regulated Learning We have considered the important role that self-efficacy and self-regulation play in a social cognitive view of teaching and learning. How might these ideas be applied in your classroom? Stop & Think How are you studying right now? What goals have you set for your reading today? What is your plan for learning, and what strategies are you using right now to learn? How did you learn those strategies? Fortunately, a growing body of research offers guidance about how to design tasks and structure classroom interactions to support students’ development of and engagement in self-regulated learning (Brown et al., 2014; Diamond & Ling, 2016; Duckworth et al., 2018; Perry, 1998; Stoeger & Ziegler, 2011; see Schunk & Greene, 2017). This research indicates that students develop academically effective forms of self-regulated learning and a sense of efficacy for learning when teachers involve them in complex, meaningful tasks that extend over long periods of time, much like the constructivist activities described in Cluster 10. Also, to develop self-regulated learning and self-efficacy for learning, students need to have some control or agency over their learning processes and products; they need to make choices about what to work on, where, and with whom. They also need to have some control over the difficulty of the task—how much to read or write, at what pace, and with what level of support. And, because self-management and self-evaluation are key to effective self-regulated learning and a sense of efficacy, teachers can help students develop self-regulated learning by involving them in setting criteria for evaluating their learning processes and products and then giving them opportunities to reflect on and make judgments about their progress using those standards. It helps to work in collaboration with peers and seek feedback from them. As you saw earlier, this has been called shared regulation. Throughout the entire process, teachers must co-regulate the task by “providing just enough and just in time information and support to facilitate students’ acquisition and application of” self-regulated learning (Perry & Rahim, 2011, p. 130). Let’s examine each of these research-based ways to support the development of self-efficacy and self-regulation.

Complex Tasks Inviting students to engage in complex, meaningful tasks that are properly scaffolded can help them develop self-regulatory skills and a sense of efficacy for long-term goals. The term complex refers to the design of tasks, not their level of difficulty. Teachers don’t want to assign students tasks that are too difficult and that lead to frustration. This is especially true when students have learning difficulties. In fact, research indicates that the most motivating and academically beneficial tasks for students are those that challenge but don’t overwhelm them (Eitel et al., 2020; Mayer, 2014). Remember what you learned in Cluster 8 about extraneous cognitive load.

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

From a design point of view, tasks are complex when they address multiple goals and involve large chunks of meaning—for example, projects and thematic units. Furthermore, complex tasks extend over long periods of time, engage students in a variety of cognitive and metacognitive processes, and allow for the production of a wide range of products (Perry et al., 2002). For example, a study of Egyptian pyramids might result in the production of written reports, maps, diagrams, skits, models, and even an Egyptian “museum.” Even more important, complex tasks provide students with information about their learning progress. These tasks require them to engage in deep, elaborative thinking, planning, and problem solving. In the process, students develop and refine their cognitive and metacognitive strategies. Furthermore, succeeding at such tasks increases students’ self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation (McCaslin & Good, 1996; Turner, 1997). Such projects often afford learners opportunities to cope with stressful situations, regulate emotions, and make adaptations to accomplish their goals (Matthews et al., 2002).

Agency and Control Teachers are in a prime position to help students develop a sense of their own agency. When students have choices (e.g., about what to produce, how to produce it, where to work, with whom to work), they are more likely to anticipate a successful outcome (increased self-efficacy) and consequently increase effort and persist when difficulties arise (Turner & Paris, 1995). Also, by involving students in making decisions, teachers invite them to take responsibility for learning by planning, setting goals, monitoring progress, and evaluating outcomes (Turner, 1997). Finally, when students perceive they have control over their learning activities, they maintain higher levels of motivation to complete the task, as predicted by numerous theories of motivation presented in Cluster 12 (Ryan & Deci, 2017). These are qualities of highly effective, self-regulating learners. Remember from Cluster 5 that, according to Sternberg (2004), one aspect of intelligence is choosing or adapting environments so that you can succeed. Bandura (2016) would agree—one way to be an agent is to select environments that work best for you by exercising self-control. Giving students choices creates opportunities for them to adjust the level of challenge that particular tasks present (e.g., they can choose easy or more challenging reading materials, determine the nature and amount of writing in a report, supplement writing with other expressions of learning). But what if students make poor academic choices? Highly effective teachers who are high in self-regulated learning carefully consider the choices they give to students. They make sure students have the knowledge and skills they need to operate independently and make good decisions (Perry & Rahim, 2011). For example, when students are learning new skills or routines, teachers can offer choices with constraints (e.g., students must write a minimum of four sentences/paragraphs/pages, but they can choose to write more; they must demonstrate their understanding of an animal’s habitat, food, and babies, but they can write, draw, or speak their knowledge). Highly effective teachers also teach and model good decision making. For example, when students are choosing partners, teachers can ask them to consider what they need from their partner (e.g., shared interest and commitment, perhaps knowledge or skills that they need to develop). When students are making choices about how best to use their time, these teachers ask, “What can you do when you’re finished? What can you do if you are waiting for my help?” Often, lists are generated and posted so students can refer to them while they work. Finally, highly effective teachers give students feedback about the choices they make and tailor the choices they give to suit the unique characteristics of particular learners. For example, they might encourage some students to select research topics for which resources are readily available and written at a level that is accessible to the learner. Alternatively, they might encourage some students to work collaboratively versus independently to ensure they have the support and shared regulation they need to be successful. It takes some practice to determine the extent to which scaffolds are needed to support learner agency and self-regulation (Vaughn, 2018).

497

498 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation Participatory communities—those in which learners have leadership and voice—also provide support for student agency. For example, youth participatory action research or opportunities for project-based learning can enhance a sense of personal and shared responsibility, both of which support self-regulation. When such projects are undertaken in the service of larger social or cultural goals, they become more personally and/or collectively valued (Jagers et al., 2019). Experimental evidence shows that when students are engaged with tasks that involve self-transcendent goals as opposed to self-focused goals, their academic self-regulation and motivation are enhanced (Yeager et al., 2014).

Self-Management

Self-instruction Talking oneself through the steps of a task.

Much of being a successful self-regulator involves being a good self-manager. To be sure, some psychologists rely on the ideas of behaviorism for modifying personal behavior through the use of reinforcement and punishment. A social cognitive approach extends this. For instance, Donald Meichenbaum (1977) had success teaching students who were having difficulty controlling their behavior to “talk themselves through” tasks. Meichenbaum called his method cognitive behavior modification. You may remember from Cluster 3 that there is a stage in cognitive development when young children seem to guide themselves through a task using private speech (Vygotsky, 1987a). They talk to themselves, often repeating the words of a parent or teacher. In cognitive behavior modification, students are taught directly how to use this self-instruction. Meichenbaum (1977) outlined the steps, which are similar to Bandura’s (1986) description of vicarious learning and self-modeling: 1. An adult model performs a task while talking through the steps out loud (cognitive modeling). 2. The child performs the same task under the direction of the model’s instructions (overt, external guidance). 3. The child performs the task while self-instructing aloud (overt, self-guidance). 4. The child whispers the instructions aloud while going through the task (faded, overt self-guidance). 5. The child performs the task while guiding activities via private speech (covert selfinstruction). (p. 32) Students can engage in four self-management skills to increase their learning: listening, planning, working, and checking (Manning & Payne, 1996). How might cognitive self-instruction help students develop these skills? One possibility is to use personal booklets or class posters that prompt students to “talk to themselves” about these skills. For instance, one fifth-grade class designed a set of prompts for each of the four skills and posted the prompts around the classroom. The prompts for listening included “Does this make sense?” “Am I getting this?” “I need to ask a question now before I forget.” “Pay attention!” “Can I do what he’s saying to do?” Planning prompts were “Do I have everything together?” “Do I have my friends tuned out for right now?” “Let me get organized first.” “What order will I do this in?” “I know this stuff!” Posters for these and the other two skills, working and checking, are shown in Figure 11.3. Part of the power of this process lies in getting students involved in thinking about and creating their own guides and prompts. Many active reading programs encourage students to ask, “What do good readers do?” and then use their answers as prompts. Having the discussion and posting the ideas makes students more self-aware and increases their sense of control over their own learning. Teachers can model self-directed talk for their students by “thinking aloud” during instruction (White, 2017). Today, entire school intervention programs are based on similar types of cognitive behavior modification as a form of self-management, which is one of the core skills also needed for effective social and emotional regulation. For example, the Coping Power Program includes training for both parents and their children, beginning in the last half of one academic year and continuing through the entire next school year. The training for

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

499

Figure 11.3 Posters to Remind Students to “Talk Themselves Through” Listening, Planning, Working, and Checking in School These four posters were designed by a fifth-grade class to help them remember to use self-instruction. Some of the reminders reflect the special world of these preadolescents.

Poster 1 While Listening: 1. Does this make sense? 2. Am I getting this? 3. I need to ask a question now before I forget. 4. Pay attention. 5. Can I do what he's saying to do?

Poster 3 While Working: 1. Am I working fast enough? 2. Stop staring at my girlfriend and get back to work. 3. How much time is left? 4. Do I need to stop and start over? 5. This is hard for me, but I can manage.

Poster 2

Poster 4

While Planning: 1. Do I have everything together? 2. Do I have my friends tuned out for right now? 3. Let me get organized first. 4. What order will I do this in? 5. I know this stuff!

While Checking: 1. Did I finish everything? 2. What do I need to recheck? 3. Am I proud of this work? 4. Did I write all the words? Count them. 5. I think I finished. I organized myself. Did I daydream too much?

Source: Manning, B. H. & Payne, B. D. Self-talk for teachers and students: Metacognitive strategies for personal and classroom use, © 1996. Reprinted by permission of Pearson Education, Inc.

students often focuses on emotion regulation skills, such as becoming aware of feelings (especially anger), learning to relax, redirecting the focus away from angry feelings, making coping self-statements, seeing the perspectives of others, developing social problem-solving skills, and dealing with peer pressure by practicing how to say no (Lochman & Wells, 2003). Another similar approach is Tools for Getting Along (Smith et al., 2016). Both programs have been effective in helping early adolescent students to “get along” with their classmates and teachers, particularly when they are upset or angry. In addition, in psychotherapy, tools based on cognitive behavior modification have proved to be some of the most effective ways of dealing with psychological problems such as depression.

Self-Evaluation Evaluation practices that support self-regulated learning are nonthreatening. They are embedded in ongoing activities, emphasize processes as well as products, focus on personal progress, and help students to interpret errors as opportunities for learning to occur. In these contexts, students enjoy and actually seek challenging tasks because the risk of participation—or perceived risk—is low (Paris & Ayres, 1994). Involving students in generating evaluation criteria and evaluating their own work also reduces the anxiety that often accompanies assessment by giving students a sense of control over the outcome. Students can judge their work in relation to a set of qualities both they and their teachers identify as “good” work. They can consider the effectiveness of their approaches to learning and alter their behaviors in ways that enhance it (Winne, 2011; Winne & Perry, 2000). Classrooms that are high in self-regulated learning have both formal and informal opportunities for students to evaluate their learning. For example, one student teacher asked fourth- and fifth-grade students to submit reflection journals describing the games they

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Self-Regulation (II, A1) Take a look at the Merton Ethel Harris Research and Training Centre (https://self-reg.ca) for tips to help students develop the goals, metacognitive skills, and self-regulatory practices that can support a lifelong devotion to learning.

500 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation designed with a partner or a small group of collaborators for a probability and statistics unit (Perry et al., 2004). Their journals explained their contribution to the group’s process and product and also described what they learned from participating. The student teacher took these reflections into account when she evaluated the games. More informally, teachers ask students, “What have you learned about yourself as a writer today?” “What do good researchers and writers do?” “What can we do that we couldn’t do before?” Questions like these, posed to individuals or embedded in class discussions, prompt students’ metacognition, motivation, and strategic action—the components of self-regulated learning (Panadero et al., 2017).

Collaboration Teamwork can facilitate self- and co-regulation. The Committee on Increasing High School Students’ Engagement and Motivation to Learn (2004) concluded that when students can put their heads together, they are more receptive to challenging assignments—the very kind of complex task that develops self-regulation. The most effective uses of cooperative/ collaborative relationships to support self-regulated learning are those that reflect a climate of community and shared problem solving (Perry & Drummond, 2002; Perry et al., 2002). In these contexts, teachers and students actually co-regulate one another’s learning and motivation, offering support, whether working alone, in pairs, or in small groups (McCaslin & Vriesema, 2018). This support is instrumental to individuals’ development and use of metacognition, intrinsic motivation, and strategic action (e.g., sharing ideas, comparing strategies for solving problems, identifying everyone’s area of expertise). Teachers who are skilled at supporting self-regulation spend time at the start of each school year teaching routines and establishing norms of participation (e.g., how to give constructive feedback, how to take turns and share tasks when working collaboratively, how to interpret and respond to peers’ suggestions). As you will see in Cluster 13, developing useful management and learning procedures and routines takes time at the beginning of the year, but it is time well spent. Once routines and patterns of interaction are established, students can focus on learning together, and teachers can attend to teaching academic skills and the curriculum. Cluster 10 provides some additional suggestions for technology-mediated collaborations.

Bringing It All Together: Theories of Learning How can we make sense of the diverse perspectives on learning we have explored for the last four clusters? We have considered behavioral, cognitive, constructivist (individual and social), and social cognitive explanations of what people learn and how they learn it. Table 11.3 on the next page presents a summary of these perspectives on learning. Rather than debating the merits of each approach in Table 11.3, consider their contributions to understanding learning and improving teaching. Don’t feel that you must choose the “best” approach—there is no such thing. Chemists, biologists, and nutritionists rely on different theories to explain and improve health. Different views of learning can be used together to create productive learning environments for the diverse students you will teach. Behavioral theory helps us understand the role of cues in setting the stage for behaviors and the role of consequences and practice in encouraging or discouraging particular behaviors. But much of humans’ lives and learning is more than behaviors. Language and higher-order thinking require complex information processing and memory—something the cognitive models help us understand. And what about the person as a creator and constructor of knowledge, not just a processor of information? Here, constructivist perspectives have much to offer. Social cognitive theory illustrates the powerful learning opportunities afforded through modeling and observational learning and highlights the important roles of agency and self-direction. Finally, life requires self-regulation, and we have a lot

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

501

to regulate—our thoughts, bodies, behaviors, environments, relationships, emotions, and more. Promoting effective self-regulation skills promotes greater success in learning regardless of the operational process of learning. We like to think of the four main learning theories in Table 11.3 as four pillars for teaching. Students must first understand and make sense of the material (constructivist); then they must remember what they have understood (cognitive—information processing); then they must practice and apply (behavioral) their new skills and understanding to make them more fluid and automatic—a permanent part of their repertoire. Finally, they must take charge of their own learning (social cognitive). Failure to attend to any part of the process results in lower-quality learning.

Table 11.3 Four Views of Learning There are variations within each of these views of learning and overlaps as well, especially in constructivist views. BEHAVIORAL

COGNITIVE

APPLIED BEHAVIORAL ANALYSIS B. F. SKINNER

INFORMATION PROCESSING

Knowledge

• Fixed body of knowledge to acquire • Stimulated from outside

Learning

CONSTRUCTIVIST

SOCIAL COGNITIVE

INDIVIDUAL

SOCIAL/SITUATED

JEAN PIAGET

LEV VYGOTSKY

SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY ALBERT BANDURA

• Fixed body of knowledge to acquire • Stimulated from outside • Prior knowledge influences how information is processed

• Changing body of knowledge, individually constructed in social world • Built on what learner brings

• Socially constructed knowledge • Built on what participants contribute, construct together

• Changing body of knowledge, constructed in interaction with others and the environment

• Acquisition of facts, skills, concepts • Occurs through drill, guided practice

• Acquisition of facts, skills, concepts, and strategies • Occurs through the effective application of strategies

• Active construction, restructuring prior knowledge • Occurs through multiple opportunities and diverse processes to connect to what is already known

• Collaborative construction of socially defined knowledge and values • Occurs through socially constructed opportunities

• Active construction of knowledge based on observation, interacting in the physical and social world, and developing agency— becoming more self-regulating

Teaching

• Transmission presentation (Telling)

• Transmission • Guide students toward more “accurate” and complete knowledge

• Challenge, guide thinking toward more complete understanding

• Co-construct knowledge with students

• Presenting models, demonstrating, supporting self-efficacy and self-regulation

Role of Teacher

• Manager, supervisor • Correct wrong answers

• Teach and model effective strategies • Correct misconceptions

• Facilitator, guide • Listen for student’s current conceptions, ideas, thinking

• Facilitator, guide • Co-participant • Co-construct different interpretation of knowledge; listen to socially constructed conceptions

• Model, facilitator, motivator • Model of self-regulated learning

Role of Peers

• Not usually considered

• Not necessary but can influence information processing

• Not necessary but can stimulate thinking, raise questions

• Ordinary and necessary part of process of knowledge construction

• Serve as models • Ordinary and necessary part of process of knowledge construction

Role of

• Passive recipient of information • Active listener, direction-follower

• Active processor of information, strategy user • Organizer and reorganizer of information • Rememberer

• Active construction (within mind) • Active thinker, explainer, interpreter, questioner

• Active co-construction with others and self • Active thinker, explainer, interpreter, questioner • Active social participator

• Active co-construction with others and self • Active thinker, explainer, interpreter, questioner • Active social participator

Student

J. ANDERSON

502 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

Module 35 Summary Self-Regulated Learning: Skill and Will (pp. 484–496) What factors are involved in self-regulated learning? One important goal of teaching is to prepare students for lifelong learning. To reach this goal, students must be self-regulated learners; they must have a combination of the knowledge, motivation to learn, and volition that provides the skill and will to learn independently and effectively. Knowledge includes an understanding of self, subject, task, learning strategy, and contexts for application. Motivation to learn provides the commitment and includes self-beliefs and interest. Volition is the follow-through that stays the course despite distraction. Personality characteristics such as grit, conscientiousness, and self-control are important here. What is the self-regulated learning cycle? Zimmerman’s model of self-regulated learning notes three phases: forethought (which includes setting goals, making plans, self-efficacy, and motivation); performance (which involves self-control and self-monitoring); and reflection (which includes self-evaluation and adaptations, leading to the forethought/planning phase again). What are some examples of teaching students to be more self-regulated?  Self-regulated learners engage in four types of activities: analyzing the task, setting goals and designing plans, engaging in learning, and adjusting their approach to learning. Teaching students to be more self-regulated might take the form of providing opportunities to identify and analyze the task at hand. Students should ask themselves: What is the task? What is an ideal outcome of the task? Students may also benefit from goal-setting practice; they may ask: What are my short-term goals? What are my long-term goals? Learning strategies such as identifying important details and developing a big picture of material is the next step in the process. Finally, students need to reflect on whether they were successful and devise strategies for overcoming shortcomings in their self-regulation process. They may ask themselves: Where was I successful? Where do I need to improve in order to meet my goals in the future? What are the skills involved in emotional self-regulation? Emotionally self-regulated individuals are aware of their own emotions and the feelings of others—realizing that inner emotions can differ from outward expressions. They can talk about and express emotions in ways that are appropriate for their cultural group. They can feel empathy for others in distress and also cope with their own distressing emotions—they can handle stress. Emotional self-regulators can also employ a variety of problem-solving and coping strategies to help them manage the personal and social emotional stimuli to promote optimal performance. Emotional self-regulation involves an understanding that relationships are defined in part by how emotions are communicated between people. All these skills come together to produce a capacity for emotional self-regulation, which is a central part of social and emotional learning. To effectively help students become better at social and emotional learning, teachers must also learn emotion regulation strategies that help mitigate their stress, such as mindfulness-based practices.

Teaching Toward Self-Efficacy and Self-Regulated Learning (pp. 496–500) How can teachers support the development of self-efficacy and self-regulated learning? Teachers should involve students in complex, meaningful tasks that extend over long periods of time. Teachers should provide students control over their learning processes and products allowing them to make choices as agents. They can help students manage themselves by strategies like cognitive behavior modification and self-talk. Teachers should involve students in setting criteria for evaluating their learning processes and products and then give them opportunities to make judgments about their progress using those standards. Finally, teachers should encourage students to work collaboratively with and seek feedback from peers.

Bringing It All Together: Theories of Learning (pp. 500–501) What is the value of the four different perspectives on learning? The behavioral, cognitive, constructivist, and social cognitive learning theories are four pillars for teaching. Students must first understand and make sense of the material (constructivist); then they must remember what they have understood (cognitive—information processing); then they must practice and apply (behavioral) their new skills and understanding to make them more fluid and automatic—a permanent part of their repertoire. Finally, they must take charge of their own learning (social cognitive). Failure to attend to any part of the process results in lower-quality learning.

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

503

Cluster 11 Review Key Terms Self-regulation Self-reinforcement Shared regulation Social cognitive theory Social learning theory Social persuasion Teachers’ sense of efficacy Triadic reciprocal causality Vicarious experiences Vicarious reinforcement Volition

Co-regulation Direct reinforcement Grit Human agency Mastery experiences Modeling Physiological or emotional arousal Resilience Ripple effect Self-efficacy Self-instruction Self-regulated learning

Connect and Extend to Licensure Multiple-Choice Questions 1.

2.

3.

4.

“I believe I will do well in this class,” Chris declared to her brother. “I received a perfect score on the verbal part of the SAT, and I have always excelled in my literature classes.” Chris is demonstrating which of the following?

Through observational learning, one learns how to perform a behavior and also what will happen in specific situations if one performs it. Observation can be a very efficient learning process. What four elements must be met in order to learn from observation? A.

Attention, retention, production, and motivation/ reinforcement

High levels of empathy

B.

Attention, cognition, belief, and value

C.

High self-efficacy in English

C.

D.

Low self-efficacy in English

Observation, desire, developmental capability, and suitability

D.

Observation, reinforcement

A.

High self-esteem

B.

Modeling is defined as changes in behavior, thinking, or emotions that happen through observing another person. Which theory and theorist are associated with learning through observation? A.

Behaviorist theory, Skinner

B.

Constructivist theory, Piaget

C.

Social cognitive theory, Bandura

D.

Sociocultural theory, Vygotsky

Miss Hutton turned around with a scowl on her face and addressed her second-graders. “What do you think you’re doing, Johnny? Did I tell you to get out of your seat? Don’t you ever let me catch you up without permission again.” The class sat silently with their eyes wide. When the lunch bell rang an hour later, not one of the children dared to move from their seats. This is an example of which one of the following? A.

Self-regulation

C.

Reciprocal causality

B.

Self-efficacy

D.

Vicarious learning

motivation,

abstraction,

and

Constructed-Response Questions Case “Marcus! Look how well you did on your spelling test!” Mr. Bonner smiled at Marcus, who beamed in response. “I knew when we started to chart your progress you would really do well. You have had three perfect scores on the last three tests. It just goes to show, you studied every night for a few minutes, and your grade has gone up, up, up! I’m looking at the chart we made at the beginning of the year, and I think it really helped.” “Mr. Bonner, I would like to do the same thing in math. I think if I practice every night and chart my progress, I’ll get perfect grades in math, too! I’ll make a chart and pick out some math games I can play at home to improve.” 5.

How is Mr. Bonner encouraging Marcus’s self-efficacy?

6.

How is Marcus’s response to Mr. Bonner an example of self-regulated learning?

504 Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

What Would They Do? Teachers’ Casebook: Failure to Self-Regulate At the beginning of this cluster, we asked you to think critically about how you would help your students develop organizational and self-regulation skills. Now, as you consider these teachers’ responses to that question, we again suggest that you think critically. Do the following responses reflect (or contradict) any of the concepts discussed in this cluster? Do the suggestions and solutions described here make sense in light of what you have learned in this or other classes about best practices? How do these ideas align with or challenge your personal philosophy of teaching?

Kyle Brown • 3rd-Grade Teacher Hjorth Road Elementary School, Surrey, British Columbia, Canada In my primary classroom, building and maintaining supports that encourage student autonomy is crucial to help organize time and complete tasks in a productive way. In literacy, for example, we complete a process known as Story Workshop. To do this process well, we begin the year by developing collective understanding about stories and use this knowledge to co-construct criteria, which is posted in the room and helps students to gradually become more independent in completing the basic components of a story. Once we have established this foundation, students begin tailoring their work to different goals depending on where they are with their literacy development. Furthermore, embedded into this classroom practice are possibilities for choice in writing materials, representation (e.g., oral retelling, drawing) and work space (e.g., independently, with a friend, with a group, with a teacher). These options, together with supports such as the classroom rubric, which visually represents our foundational story writing criteria, help students to work at their own pace and take ownership over their final product.

Miriam Miller • English Language Learning Teacher Educator University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada As educators, we want our students to develop a wide range of skills to feel empowered in their learning, to know the steps involved in a task, plan and make intentional choices on how to engage in the task, and reflect and iterate on their learning so that they might be successful. These are the skills I want my students to have so they might regulate their learning. If faced with a similar situation, I might share my observation with the class that getting started on a task seems to be a challenge. I would invite the students to think about why that might be and, together, we would problem solve what we might do. I would facilitate the conversation to address the steps of what it takes to get started, to stay on task and work toward completion. As a result, we might generate a series of questions the students can ask of themselves when they are about to start

some work (e.g., What is the task? What do I need to do to complete the task? What strategy will I use to do it? How is it going? What else should I try?). I would use the co-created questions (or steps) to create an anchor chart with visuals to support students. The next time we begin a learning activity as part of curricular exploration, we would review our anchor chart prior to starting and then refer to it as students experience a challenge (e.g., are students struggling because they don’t know what the job is [task interpretation] or is it because they’ve started doing the job but aren’t using an effective strategy to complete the task?). Students’ efficacy develops when they co-create the steps in the learning process and are empowered to actively regulate their learning.

Nicole Macpherson • K–12th-Grade District Learning Support Consultant Burnaby School District, British Columbia, Canada Though there are many unique and wonderful classroom profiles, it is becoming increasingly common to see classrooms in which students have a lack of organizational skills to begin and/or execute projects. For this group, I will focus on establishing a classroom culture that values routines, expectations, and student voice. Each class will begin with a review of our “schedule”—upcoming social and academic activities. This review makes things predictable for my students and empowers them to anticipate [and] then take control of their learning, which leads to success that builds a sense of self-efficacy and confidence. I will use their interests as a platform to engage them in material and routines we do each day. For this class I would explicitly teach key executive functioning skills, such as organizing ideas and planning and prioritizing tasks, to build a foundation for teaching curricular content. Once my students have systematic ways to organize their thoughts it will set them up to pick out key information and weave in curricular content. I like to use these same strategies in most subject areas and emphasize their importance through modeling, graphic organizers, and having students create self-monitoring checklists for each activity or class. To continue to support the development of self-efficacy, I will offer my students choice, provide specific positive feedback, encourage goal setting and offer opportunities for self-evaluation to motivate them to monitor their own learning. Encouraging my students to routinely reflect about what they are doing, how they are doing, and managing their own emotions around their learning expectations is important to their ability to become self-regulated learners.

Jane W. Campbell • 2nd-Grade Teacher John P. Faber Elementary School, Dunellen, NJ To begin the year, I teach several routines that help students to become more independent and successful. First they are introduced to a homework folder labeled with the classroom number and school name. Ownership is important, so they

Cluster 11 Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation

write their own names on the label, too. There are also designated sections for parent signatures, homework to be returned to school, and homework to be kept at home. Each day students put their things into the proper section. I check the students’ success by walking through the room and looking at their folders. As different students become proficient, they become student helpers to help spot check other students as well. Organizing the students takes time, but once the routine is established, most students can successfully complete the task. As the routine is practiced and established, the students become successful and more self-reliant. Everyone is happy: the students, the parents, and the teacher.

Carla S. Higgins • K–5th Grade Literacy Coordinator Legend Elementary School, Newark, OH I don’t make assumptions about my students’ organizational skills. Instead, I explicitly teach them skills that work for our class and support future organization such as using a structured folder for class paperwork, frequent checkpoints, and an assignment calendar or agenda. I include students in planning due dates by considering what it would take to complete each assignment. For longer assignments, I ask students to help create a reasonable timeline for completing steps of the project and offer frequent checks for completion of the steps. Finally, since we live in a culture where technology drives much of our communication, I set up a website or email reminder system to provide additional support for students and to communicate with parents to keep them involved.

Marie Hoffman Hurt • 8th-Grade Foreign-Language (German and French) Teacher Pickerington Local Schools, Pickerington, OH Part of being a good teacher is learning how to teach the “process” of learning alongside the required content material. In the grand scheme of teaching, content-specific learning is only a small percentage of what I teach—something I didn’t expect when I started my career in education. A large part of succeeding in life (and on achievement tests!), rather than just knowing how to conjugate a French verb, is knowing the habits, routines, and learning skills that students master while learning those French verbs. With this in mind, it is much easier to keep the task at hand in perspective. If a teacher focuses on fundamental strategies such as organization and planning, and inextricably links these strategies to the operation of the classroom, these concepts become second nature to the students. Students are better able to absorb and learn the

505

content-specific material because they have the tools necessary to do so.

Kelly L. Hoy • 5th-Grade Humanities Teacher Katherine Delmar Burke School, San Francisco, CA In an elementary school classroom, organizational skills are central to alleviating stress for students, teachers, and even parents. From the desk to the binder to the backpack, somehow students’ paperwork mysteriously disappears. There are ways to battle the infamous “black hole” book bag or desk. Teachers should take time at the end of each period to clearly state where the assignment should be placed, and each child can give a signal that his or her paper is in the correct place. For time-sensitive projects, having different dates in which notes, drafts, and final projects are due will help students learn time management. Students can check off that they have the correct materials in assignment logs and get a teacher’s initials. Periodic “book bag checks” can help students organize their book bags for homework.

Patricia A. Smith • High School Math Teacher Earl Warren High School, San Antonio, TX In my high school mathematics classes, I spend the first 2 months of the school year training my students in organizational skills. All of my students are given a schedule that outlines topics of discussion, assignment due dates, and quiz and test dates. I also give each of them a “scorecard” where they keep track of their own grades. This serves as a double check for me as the teacher and also provides a sense of ownership of earned grades to the students. All my students have a three-ring binder with a plastic cover–; their schedule fits inside the plastic cover. Early in the year, I start every class with a look at the schedule and question students on assignment due dates. In addition, I collect all assignments and tests in colored folders unique to each class section. When the students walk into my classroom and see their designated color of folder on my desk, they know that something is due. Most of my quizzes are the take-home variety. I place them on a table in the back of the room, and students are responsible for locating, completing, and returning them. In fact, I put them out several days in advance and do not accept late quizzes, thereby increasing student responsibility and organization. Graded papers are also processed in the same manner, thereby perpetuating the new and orderly system and disabling the old unorganized ways.

Cluster 12

AlexZi/Shutterstock

Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Teachers’ Casebook: Motivating Students When Resources Are Thin What Would You Do? It is July, and you have finally secured a teaching position for the fall. You have a number of ideas you hope will inspire learning in students. However, you are discovering that the teaching resources in the school where you will teach are slim to none; the only materials available are some aging texts and the workbooks that go with them. Every idea you have suggested for software, learning apps, simulation games, science project supplies, field trips, or other, more active teaching materials has been met with the same response: “There’s no money in the budget for that.” As you look over the texts and workbooks, you wonder how the students could be anything but bored by them. Also, you note that the texts look pretty high level for the grade level and student community. But the objectives in the workbooks are important. Besides, the district curriculum requires these units. Students will be tested on them in district-wide assessments next spring, so you want to do a good job of supporting students’ learning.

506

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Critical Thinking • How would you arouse student curiosity and interest about the topics and tasks in the workbooks? • How would you establish the value of learning this material? • How would you handle the difficulty level of the texts? • What do you need to know about motivation to solve these problems? • What do you need to know about your students to motivate them?

Overview and Objectives Most educators agree that motivating students is one of the critical tasks of teaching. To learn, students must be cognitively, emotionally, and behaviorally engaged in productive class activities. We begin with the question “What is motivation?” and examine many of the answers that have been proposed, including a discussion of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. You already know quite a bit that applies to motivation based on your understanding of behavioral, cognitive, social cognitive, and sociocultural theories of learning. Next, we consider more closely five broad factors or themes that frequently appear in discussions of motivation: needs and self-determination, goal orientations, expectancies and values, attributions and beliefs, and feelings such as interests, curiosity, flow, or anxiety. How do we put all this information together in teaching? How do we create environments, situations, and relationships that encourage motivation and engagement in learning? First, we consider how the influences on motivation come together to support motivation to learn. Then we discuss a number of strategies for developing a motivational climate in your classroom and provide a toolkit for helping your students stay motivated. By the time you have completed this cluster, you should be able to: 12.1 Define motivation and describe the differences between intrinsic and

extrinsic motivation. 12.2 Explain how learners’ needs—including the needs for competence,

autonomy, and relatedness—influence their motivation to learn. 12.3 Describe the different kinds of goal orientations and examine their

influences on motivation. 12.4 Discuss how students’ expectations for success, value of the task, and

perceptions of cost can influence motivation. 12.5 Discuss how students’ beliefs and attributions about their school

experiences and their ability mindsets can influence motivation.

507

508 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching 12.6 Describe the role of interests, curiosity, flow, emotions, and anxiety in

motivation. 12.7 Explain how teachers can influence and encourage students’

motivation to learn. We began our examination of motivation in the previous cluster when we explored students’ beliefs about their capabilities—their self-efficacy. We broaden our examination of motivation in this cluster given the influential and important role motivation plays in students’ social interactions and academic achievement in your classroom. Students with the same abilities and prior knowledge may perform quite differently, based on their motivation (Wentzel & Miele, 2016). So how does that work? Let’s start with a basic question: What is motivation?

OUTLINE Teachers’ Casebook—Motivating Students When Resources Are Thin: What Would You Do? Overview and Objectives MODULE 36: Motivation Basics and the Role of Needs What Is Motivation? Meeting Some Students Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation What You Already Know About Motivation Needs and Self-Determination Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs Self-Determination: Need for Competence, Autonomy, and Relatedness Needs: Lessons for Teachers MODULE 37: Goals, Values, and Ability Beliefs Goals and Goal Orientations Goal Setting: What Am I Trying to Do? Making Goals Work: Feedback, Framing, and Commitment Goal Orientations: Why Am I Trying to Do This? Social Goals Goals: Lessons for Teachers Expectancies, Values, and Costs Expectancy for Success Task Value Perceived Cost Expectancy, Value, and Cost: Lessons for Teachers Attributions and Beliefs About Ability Attributions in the Classroom Teacher Attributions Trigger Student Attributions Mindsets About Ability Ability Mindsets in the Classroom Mindsets About Failure Attributions and Mindsets: Lessons for Teachers MODULE 38: Interests, Curiosity, and Emotions How Do You Feel About Learning? Interest, Curiosity, and Emotions Tapping Interests Curiosity: Novelty and Complexity

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

509

Flow Emotions Anxiety in the Classroom Reaching Every Student: Coping with Negative Emotions Interest, Curiosity, and Emotions: Lessons for Teachers MODULE 39: Supporting Students' Motivation to Learn Strategies to Encourage Motivation to Learn Why Am I Learning This? Is It Valuable, Useful, Relevant? Will I Enjoy This? Can I Do It? Building Confidence and Positive Expectations What Do I Need to Do to Succeed? Providing Structures That Support Progress Do I Belong in This Classroom? Creating Inclusive Opportunities Cluster 12 Review Key Terms • Connect and Extend to Licensure • Teachers’ Casebook—Motivating Students When Resources Are Thin: What Would They Do?

MODULE 36

Motivation Basics and the Role of Needs

Learning Objective 12.1 Define motivation and describe the differences between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Learning Objective 12.2 Explain how learners’ needs—including the needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness—influence their motivation to learn.

What Is Motivation? Motivation is usually defined as the processes that initiate, direct, and sustain behavior. What are the underlying processes that help explain why students do what they do? We can think of motivation in terms of different components, such as needs, goals, and beliefs. We can also focus on the reasons why people do what they do—whether those are more internal or external. Knowing about the different components of motivation and the factors that affect motivation helps to explain why some students put forth more effort, persist longer, learn more, and score higher on tests (Lazowski & Hulleman, 2016). Psychologists study motivation as part of a process or sequence that takes place over time (Graham & Weiner, 2012). Motivational components, the middle section of Figure 12.1, include the needs, goals, values, and beliefs learners have. Consider the different motivational questions that help us evaluate our own motivation (e.g., Do I value this activity? Can I do it?). But it’s also important to consider what factors contributed to those particular motives (the left side of the figure). For example, knowing about a student’s past experience and social setting helps explain why they may have certain needs, goals, or beliefs. What are the behaviors and outcomes associated with motivation (the right side of the figure)? These include the choices students make, their engagement in their learning, and their academic performance. These are only some of the important outcomes of motivational processes. Notice at the bottom of the figure that students’ thoughts, feelings, and social circumstances can play a role at each stage. Throughout this cluster, we will examine many motivational sequences, not all of which follow this linear order. We all know how it feels to be motivated, to move energetically toward a goal, or to work hard, even if we are not thrilled by the task. But are some people just naturally more motivated than others? Some psychologists have explained motivation in terms of personal traits or individual characteristics. Certain people, so the theory goes, are interested in cars or art, so they spend hours in garages or galleries. However, other psychologists see motivation more as a state, a temporary event affected by context. If, for example, you are reading this paragraph because you have a test tomorrow, you are motivated (at least for now) by the situation. The motivation

Motivation An internal state that arouses, directs, and maintains behavior.

510 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Figure 12.1 Motivation as a Process We can think about motivation in terms of a process or an episode. Internal and external factors (left) can trigger a learner’s motivation (middle). Different motivational components guide what people will do next (right). Thoughts, feelings, and social dynamics can influence all stages of a motivational sequence. Factors That Affect Motivation • • • • •

Individual traits/characteristics Past experiences Cultural identity Environmental conditions Support from others

Outcomes of Motivation

Motivational Components (e.g., needs, values, goals, beliefs)

Why am I doing this? What do I need? What do I value? What are my goals? Am I able to do this? How do I feel about this?

• • • • •

Choice Self-regulation/procrastination Intensity of engagement Persistence Learning & performance

Cognitive, Metacognitive, Affective, & Social Processes Source: Graphic created by Ellen Usher.

we experience at any given time usually is some combination of internal factors and external situations, and these may be short-lived or more enduring (Deckers, 2018). You may be studying because you are a conscientious learner and because you are preparing for a test.

Meeting Some Students Many factors influence motivation and engagement in learning. To get a sense of the complexity of motivation, let’s step into a high school science classroom just after the teacher has given directions for a lab activity. The student profiles are adapted from Stipek (2002).

• Hopeless Geraldo won’t even start the assignment—as usual. He just keeps saying, “I don’t understand” or “This is too hard.” When he answers your questions correctly, he “guessed” and he “doesn’t really know.” Geraldo spends most of his time staring into space; he is falling further and further behind.

• Safe Sumet checks with you about every step—he wants to be perfect. You once gave him bonus points for doing an excellent color drawing of the apparatus, and now he produces a work of art for lab every time. But Sumet won’t risk getting a B. If it isn’t required or on the test, Sumet isn’t interested in doing the work.

• Satisfied Sophia, on the other hand, is interested in this project. In fact, she knows more than you do about it. Evidently she spends hours reading about chemistry and performing experiments. But her overall grade in your class is between B- and C because she never turns in homework. Sophia is satisfied with the C she can get on tests without even trying.

• Defensive Drake doesn’t have his lab manual—again—so he has to share with another student. Then he pretends to be working but spends most of his time making fun of the assignment or trying to get answers from other students when your back is turned. He is afraid to try because if he makes an effort and fails, he fears that everyone will think he is “dumb.”

• Anxious Aimee is a good student in most subjects, but she freezes on science tests and “forgets” everything she knows when she has to answer questions in class. Her parents are scientists and expect her to become one, too, but her prospects for this future look dim.

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

511

Stop & Think Each of these students presents a different motivational challenge connected to one or more motivation-related areas: (a) choices, (b) getting started, (c) engagement, (d) persistence, or (e) thoughts and feelings. Can you identify the problems? The answers are on page 512. Although each student you teach will have their own motivational challenges, you will need to figure out how to motivate and teach the entire class. In the next few pages, we will look more closely at the components of motivation so we can better understand students like these.

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation At some point, most of us probably wonder whether our behavior (or perhaps more often the behavior of those around us) is motivated by internal or by external forces. This isn’t always easy to determine! Most psychologists agree that it is impossible to know for sure what motivated a behavior just by looking at what someone does. The essential difference between internal and external motives is a person’s reason for acting—whether the locus of causality for the action (the location of the cause) is internal or external—inside or outside the person. Students who read or practice their backstroke or paint may be reading, swimming, or painting because they freely chose the activity based on personal interests (internal locus of causality/intrinsic motivation) or because someone or something else outside is influencing them (external locus of causality/extrinsic motivation; Reeve, 2018). A classic distinction is made between intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation. Intrinsic motivation is the natural human tendency to seek out and conquer challenges as we pursue personal interests and exercise our capabilities. When we are intrinsically motivated, we do not need incentives or punishments because the activity itself is satisfying and rewarding (Ryan & Deci, 2020). Satisfied Sophia studies chemistry outside school simply because she loves learning about chemistry; no one makes her do it. In contrast, when we do something to earn a grade, to avoid punishment, to please the teacher, or for some other reason that has very little to do with the task itself, we experience extrinsic motivation. We are not really interested in the activity for its own sake; we care only about what it will gain us. Safe Sumet works for the grade; he has little interest in the subject itself. Extrinsic motivation has been associated with negative emotions, poor academic achievement, and maladaptive learning strategies (Corpus et al., 2009). However, extrinsic motivation also has benefits if it provides incentives as students try new things, gives them an extra push to get started, or helps them persist to complete a mundane task. Beware of either/or! Sometimes students have no intention to act at all (e.g., “I don’t see why we should have physical education.”). Researchers have referred to this as amotivation—no engagement at all (Bartholomew et al., 2018). As you think about your own motivation, you probably realize that the dichotomy between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation is too either/or—too all-or-nothing. How can you think about motivation in a more nuanced way? One way is to think of our activities as falling along a continuum from externally determined (by environmental and social factors) to self-determined (through internal processes), as depicted in Figure 12.2 on the next page. For example, some students freely choose to work hard on activities that they don’t find particularly enjoyable because they know the activities are important in reaching a valued goal— such as spending hours studying educational psychology to become a good teacher. Those students are freely choosing to accept extrinsic policies, such as licensure requirements, and trying to get the most benefit from the requirements by working hard (Ryan & Deci, 2020). A second way to avoid either/or thinking is to recognize that the same behavior, at any particular time, might be motivated by both intrinsic and extrinsic factors (Vansteenkiste & Mouratidis, 2016). When I (Ellen writing) took part in a triathlon competition, I was motivated by the fitness benefits of my training as well as the internal and external recognition of my accomplishment at the finish line. (I wish I could motivate myself to train that way again!)

Locus of causality The location—internal or external—of the cause of behavior.

Intrinsic motivation Motivation associated with activities that are internally rewarding.

Extrinsic motivation Motivation created by external factors such as rewards and punishments.

Amotivation A complete lack of any intent to act—no engagement at all.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Promoting Intrinsic Motivation to Learn (I, C2, 3) For a set of practical tips, guidelines, and suggestions for boosting and maintaining motivation to learn, see the special issue of the journal Theory Into Practice published in May 2021 (edited by Bembenutty et al., 2021).

512 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Figure 12.2 The Extrinsic-Intrinsic Motivation Continuum We can think of our activities as falling along a continuum from fully determined by other people and the external environment (extrinsic motivation) to fully self-determined and internal (intrinsic motivation). Extrinsic Motivation Associated with receiving or avoiding reward or punishment; fulfilling obligations

Intrinsic Motivation

External

Somewhat External

Somewhat Internal

Internal

Aligned with personal values, interests; fully endorsed by the self

Locus of Causality Source: Graphic created by Ellen Usher.

Stop & Think Let’s see if you correctly categorized some of the motivation-related factors experienced by the students you met on page 511. They were (a) choices, (b) getting started, (c) engagement, (d) persistence, or (e) thoughts and feelings. • Hopeless Geraldo has trouble with getting started (b) and with a sense of despair (e); during the activity he feels defeated and helpless. • Safe Sumet makes good choices (a), gets started right away (b), and persists (d). But he is not really engaged and takes little pleasure in the work (c and e). • As long as she is following her own choices (a), Satisfied Sophia is prompt in getting started (b), engaged (c), persistent (d), and enjoys the task (e). • Defensive Drake makes poor choices (a), procrastinates (b), avoids engagement (c), and gives up easily (d) because he is so concerned about how others will judge him (e). • Anxious Aimee’s problems have to do with what she thinks and how she feels as she works (e). Her worry and anxiety may lead her to make poor choices (a) and procrastinate (b), which only makes her more anxious at test time. Take a moment to reflect on your own motivation in school. Did you find fractions inherently interesting? Was your curiosity piqued by irregular verbs? If teachers count on intrinsic motivation to energize all their students all of the time, they will be disappointed. There are situations in which incentives and external supports are necessary. Teachers must encourage and nurture intrinsic motivation while making sure that what fosters extrinsic motivation also supports learning (Anderman, 2021; Brophy, 2003). This may become more challenging in the upper grades because both intrinsic and extrinsic motivation decline as students move up in grade levels (Scherrer & Preckel, 2019). To combat these trends, teachers need to know about the factors that influence motivation—and there are many. Let’s begin by reviewing some of the factors you have already learned about.

What You Already Know About Motivation Motivation is a vast and complicated subject encompassing many theories and explanations. Some theories were developed through work with animals in laboratories. Others are based on research with humans who took part in studies that involved games or puzzles. The work done in clinical or industrial psychology inspired additional theories as well. Researchers also use surveys and interviews to understand students’ and teachers’ motivation. You already know a bit about motivation based on the theories of learning and cognition we discussed earlier in this book. Behaviorists tend to emphasize extrinsic motivation caused by incentives, rewards, and punishment. Cognitive views stress a person’s active search for meaning, understanding, and competence, along with the power of the individual’s attributions and interpretations. In social cognitive theory, self-efficacy and agency are

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

513

central factors in motivation. Self-efficacy is your belief that you can successfully carry out a particular task in a given situation. Sociocultural views emphasize engaged participation and maintaining your identity within a community. Even though you already have some good ideas about motivation, today there are many more specific explanations for motivation that have implications for teaching. Our examination of these explanations will be selective; otherwise we would never finish. Let’s start with some of your personal experiences with motivation. Stop & Think Why are you reading this cluster? Are you curious about motivation and interested in the topic? Is there a test in your near future? Do you need this course to earn a teaching license or to graduate? Maybe you believe that you will do well in this class, and that belief keeps you working. Maybe you just got caught up in the ideas and can’t put the book down. Perhaps it is some combination of these reasons. What motivates you to study motivation? To organize the many ideas about motivation in a way that is useful for teaching, we will examine five broad themes that are reflected in many contemporary explanations of motivation (Anderman, 2020; Hattie et al., 2020). These include a discussion of needs and self-determination; goals and goal orientations; expectancies, values, and costs; attributions and beliefs about ability; and interest, curiosity, and emotions. Could your answers to the Stop & Think questions be sorted into any of these facets of motivation? If you don’t know now, you will by the end of this cluster.

Needs and Self-Determination Early research in psychology viewed motivation in terms of trait-like needs. Three of the main needs studied extensively in this earlier work were the needs for achievement, power, and affiliation (Pintrich, 2003). Abraham Maslow’s influential theory emphasized a hierarchy that included all these needs and more.

Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs Maslow (1970) suggested that humans have a hierarchy of needs ranging from lower-level needs for survival and safety to higher-level needs for knowledge, understanding, and finally self-actualization (see Figure 12.3 on the next page). Self-actualization is Maslow’s term for self-fulfillment, the realization of personal potential—“being all that you can be.” Each of the lower-level needs must be fairly well satisfied before the higher-level needs become primary motives. Maslow (1968) called the four lower-level needs—for survival, safety, belonging, and esteem—deficiency needs. Until these needs are satisfied, people tend to focus their energies on relieving their deficiency in these areas and may suffer mentally and/or physically. Once their deficiency needs are reasonably well met, however, people become more motivated by three higher-level needs—cognitive needs, aesthetic needs, and the need for self-actualization. Maslow called these being needs, or growth needs. Meeting these higher needs does not diminish motivation because these needs can never be completely filled. Growing in your creativity, knowledge, appreciation of beauty, and full humanness is inherently rewarding and self-sustaining. For example, the more successful you are in your efforts to develop as a teacher, the harder you are likely to strive for even greater improvement. How does Maslow’s theory fit along the intrinsic-extrinsic motivation continuum? Consider the way Maslow (1968) described lower- and higher-order needs as motives: Just as all trees need sun, water, and foods from the environment, so do all people need safety, love, and status from their environment . . . [but] once satiated with these elementary, species-wide necessities, each tree and each person proceeds to develop in [their] own style. . . . In a very meaningful sense, development then becomes more determined from within rather than from without. (p. 39)

Hierarchy of needs Maslow’s model of seven levels of human needs, from basic physiological requirements to the need for self-actualization.

Self-actualization Fulfilling one’s potential.

Deficiency needs Maslow’s four lower-level needs, which must be satisfied first before higher-level needs can be addressed.

Being needs Maslow’s three higher-level needs, sometimes called growth needs.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Maslow (I, C1) Consider how problems with satisfying Maslow’s hierarchy of needs can affect student learning. Link these ideas to direct or vicarious experiences you might have had in school.

514 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Figure 12.3 Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs Maslow called the four lower-level needs—for survival, safety, belonging, and self-esteem— deficiency needs because when these needs are satisfied, the motivation to fulfill them decreases. He labeled the three higher-level needs—cognitive needs, aesthetic needs, and finally selfactualization—growth needs, or being needs, because when they are met, a person’s motivation does not cease.

SelfActualization Realizing your full potential. Becoming everything you are capable of becoming.

ds

ee

N th

ow Gr

Aesthetic needs Beauty—in art and nature— symmetry, balance, order, form. Cognitive needs Knowledge and understanding, curiosity, exploration, need for meaning and predictability. Esteem needs The esteem and respect of others and self-esteem and self-respect. A sense of competence.

s

ed

Love and belongingness

c

ien

.

fic De

e yN

Safety needs Protection from potentially dangerous objects or situations (e.g., the elements, physical illness). The threat is both physical and psychological (e.g., “fear of the unknown”). Importance of routine and familiarity. Physiological needs Food, drink, oxygen, temperature regulation, elimination, rest, activity, sex.

Source: Graphic created by Ellen Usher.

Several aspects of Maslow’s theory have been criticized. People do not always behave as the theory might predict. Most of us move back and forth among different types of needs and may even be motivated by many needs at the same time. Some people deny themselves safety or friendship to achieve knowledge or greater self-esteem. Another critique is that Maslow’s theory offers no clear pathway to assessing self-actualization except to describe it as occurring when you are choosing activities that are in full alignment with your truest self. Criticisms aside, Maslow’s theory does give us a way of looking at the whole student, whose physical, emotional, and intellectual needs are all interrelated. Needs affect how learners think, their short-term goals, and how they feel. When children are hungry, they will have trouble focusing on academic learning. A child whose feelings of safety and belonging are threatened by divorce may have little interest in learning how to divide fractions. If school is a fearful, unpredictable place where neither teachers nor students are emotionally safe, they are likely to be more concerned with security and less with learning or teaching. Belonging to a social group and maintaining self-esteem within that group are important to students. If doing what the teacher says conflicts with group rules, students may choose to ignore the teacher’s wishes or even defy the teacher. Self-determination theory is a more recent approach to motivation that focuses on human needs (Reeve, 2018; Ryan & Deci, 2017).

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

515

Self-Determination: Need for Competence, Autonomy, and Relatedness Self-determination theory suggests that humans share three fundamental, basic needs. We all need to feel competent and capable, to have a sense of autonomy and control over our lives, and to be connected to others in relationships. Notice that these are similar to early conceptions of basic needs for achievement (competence), power (autonomy and control), and affiliation (relatedness). The need for competence refers to our need to demonstrate ability or mastery over the tasks at hand. Satisfying this need results in a sense of accomplishment, promotes self-efficacy, and helps learners establish better learning goals for future tasks (Kim et al., 2010). The need for autonomy is central to self-determination because it refers to the need to be driven by our own wishes and choices rather than external rewards or pressures (Reeve, 2018; Ryan & Deci, 2020). People feel autonomous when they fully endorse what they are doing. Think about how often students (and teachers) lack a sense of autonomy in their everyday tasks. Pressure from external controls such as the rules, schedules, deadlines, and limits imposed by others often threaten the fulfilment of this need. Finally, the need for relatedness refers to the desire to belong and to establish close emotional bonds and attachments with others who care about us. How does the fulfilment of these three needs relate to students’ motivation? When students feel autonomous, competent, and related in school, their reasons for doing things will be more intrinsic. When students perceive their schools or teachers as controlling, unsupportive of their developing competence, or unwelcoming, they become more extrinsically motivated. Some psychologists have asked whether the needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness are universal across cultural contexts. For example, teaching and parenting practices in some cultures (e.g., east Asian countries) may be more controlling than in some Western cultures. However, different cultural norms may lead students to interpret such practices differently. For example, a practice that might seem strict and controlling to a student socialized in one context might seem like a sign of care to a student in another cultural context. Nevertheless, studies conducted in diverse cultural settings—including China, Singapore, Brazil, Nigeria, India, Israel, and Korea—have shown that excessively controlling practices thwart students’ need for autonomy, competence, and relatedness and undermine their intrinsic motivation to learn (Reeve et al., 2018). SELF-DETERMINATION IN THE CLASSROOM. According to self-determination theory, “students are fully capable of motivating and engaging themselves. They do not need educators to motivate them” (Reeve et al., 2018, p. 34). Does this mean that teachers do not affect students’ motivation? Quite the opposite. Teachers can do a great deal to set up the conditions for students’ needs to be met, as we will see throughout this cluster. First, let’s take a look at some findings from classroom research. Classroom environments that support student self-determination (i.e., need satisfaction) are associated with greater student interest and curiosity (even interest in homework assignments), sense of competence, creativity, conceptual learning, grades, school attendance and satisfaction, engagement, use of self-regulated learning strategies, psychological well-being, and preference for challenge. These relationships have been found in research of classrooms from first grade through graduate school (Hafen et al., 2012; Howard et al., 2017; Jang et al., 2012; Moller et al., 2006; Pulfrey et al., 2013; Reeve, 2018; Theis et al., 2020). Researchers have also suggested that classroom practices may affect learners’ biological responses. In one experiment, students were exposed to either a highly controlling teacher or an autonomy-supportive teacher. Those with the controlling teacher had higher salivary cortisol levels, which are a marker of stress (Reeve & Tseng, 2011). Need support may be especially helpful for students who have experienced hardship or deprivation. For example, among adolescent girls living in Malawi, need satisfaction was positively related to intrinsic motivation. The satisfaction of needs was more strongly related to the intrinsic motivation of girls experiencing more socioeconomic hardship (van Egmond et al., 2017).

Need for competence The desire to demonstrate ability or mastery over the tasks at hand.

Need for autonomy The desire to have our own wishes, rather than external rewards or pressures, determine our actions.

Need for relatedness The desire to belong and to establish close emotional bonds and attachments with others who care about us.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Self-Determination (I, C3) Understand how selfdetermination can boost or diminish motivation and describe practical steps that teachers can take to establish a sense of self-determination in students.

516 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching Experiences such as being praised or criticized, reminded of deadlines, assigned grades, given choices, or lectured about rules can influence students’ level of intrinsic motivation by affecting their sense of self-determination. According to self-determination theorists, classroom events have two aspects: controlling and informational. If an event is highly controlling—if it pressures students to act or feel a certain way—then students will experience less control, and their intrinsic motivation will decrease. If, on the other hand, the event provides information at the appropriate level and without coercion, students’ sense of competence and intrinsic motivation will typically increase. Here is an example of a more controlling communication:

NEED-SUPPORTIVE VERSUS NEED-THWARTING CLASSROOMS.

Your paper is due on Monday. Today, we are going to the school library. In the library, you will find information from books and Internet sites to use for your paper. Don’t waste your time; don’t goof off; make sure to get your work done. In the library, you may work by yourself or with a partner. (Reeve, 2009, p. 169) This teacher may believe that he is supporting autonomy because he offered a choice. Contrast his message with the following statement, which gives information about why the library visit is valuable: Your paper is due on Monday. As a way of helping you write a well-researched paper, we are going to where the information is—the school library. The reason we are going to the library is to find the information you need from books and Internet sites. While there, you may be tempted to goof off, but students in the past have found that a trip to the library was a crucial part of writing an excellent paper. To help you write your best possible paper, you may work in the way you wish—by yourself or with a partner. (Reeve, 2009, p. 169) Students who perceive their teachers as controlling feel less autonomous, competent, and related to others. They therefore report feeling less intrinsically motivated. For instance, researchers conducted a longitudinal study of high school students’ motivation in physical education. Students who were exposed to controlling coaches reported less intrinsic motivation, greater fear of failure, and avoidance of challenges (Bartholomew et al., 2018). On the other hand, when students feel that their teachers support their needs, they are more able and willing to handle challenges. For example, students from 43 high school science classes were asked to respond to daily survey questions about their motivation throughout a challenging six-week unit. Although all students expressed doubts about their competence as the science work became more challenging, those who perceived greater autonomy support from their teachers felt less uncertain and remained more engaged (Patall et al., 2018). Their teachers were more attuned to and accepting of the difficulties students were experiencing in science. Learning and work quality are also affected by whether the learning environment is supportive of students’ needs. Students in controlling environments tend to improve only on rote recall tasks. When students are pressured to perform, they often seek the quickest, easiest solution. Even though controlling styles of teaching are less effective, teachers are under pressure from administrators, accountability requirements, and cultural expectations to be “in charge,” and parents expect good class “discipline.” In addition, students can seem passive and unengaged or even defiant. Some teachers equate control with efficient classroom structure or feel more comfortable with a controlling style (Reeve, 2018). Assuming you are willing to resist those pressures, how can you support students’ need for autonomy and competence? One answer is to focus on establishing an informational structure in your classroom that guides and clarifies rather than demands or domineers (Aelterman et al., 2019). An obvious first step is to limit your controlling language (e.g., must, ought, have to, should). A second step is to make sure the information you provide highlights students’ growing competence by emphasizing gains made through persistence and practice and by encouraging student reflection, for example, on portfolio entries or work samples. The Guidelines: Supporting SelfDetermination and Autonomy give some more ideas. As you will see, often what supports one need, like autonomy, also supports another, like relatedness.

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

GUIDELINES Supporting Self-Determination and Autonomy Allow and encourage students to make choices. Examples 1.

2. 3.

Design several different ways to meet a learning objective (e.g., a paper, a compilation of interviews, a test, a news broadcast), and let students choose one. Encourage them to explain the reasons for their choice. Appoint student committees to make suggestions about streamlining procedures such as caring for class pets or distributing equipment. Allow students to choose work partners as long as they focus on the task.

Help students plan actions to accomplish self-selected goals. Examples 1.

2.

Experiment with goal cards. Students list their short- and long-term goals and then record three or four specific actions that will move them toward the goals. Goal cards are personal—like credit cards. Encourage middle and high school students to set goals in each subject area, record them in a goal book or on a goal-setting or notes app, and check progress toward the goals on a regular basis.

Hold students accountable for the consequences of their choices. Examples 1.

2.

If students choose to work with friends and do not finish a project because too much time was spent socializing, grade the project as it deserves, and help the students see the connection between lost time and poor performance. When students choose a topic that captures their imagination, discuss the connections between their investment in the work and the quality of the products that follow.

Provide rationales for limits, rules, and constraints. Examples 1. 2.

Explain reasons for rules. Provide a rationale for why a learning activity is worthy of time, attention, and effort.

Acknowledge and accept negative emotions. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Communicate that it is okay (and normal) to feel bored waiting for a turn, for example. Communicate that sometimes important learning involves frustration, confusion, and weariness. Acknowledge students’ perspectives: “Yes, this problem is difficult,” or “I can understand why you might feel that way.”

Use noncontrolling, positive feedback. Examples 1. 2. 3.

See poor performance or behavior as a problem to be solved, not a target of criticism. Avoid controlling language: “should,” “must,” “have to.” Provide unexpected, spontaneous, and genuine praise.

Display patience. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Take the time to listen, observe, and understand what the student is trying to do. Avoid verbal (“hurry up”) and nonverbal (clapping, snapping, tapping) signs of impatience. Provide appropriate wait time for a student to think and respond.

For more information on self-determination theory, see http://selfdeterminationtheory.org. Source: Adapted from 150 Ways to Increase Intrinsic Motivation in the Classroom, by James P. Raffini. Copyright © 1996, by Pearson Education, “Autonomy-Supportive Teaching: What It Is, How to Do It,” by Johnmarshall Reeve, 2016 and Understanding Motivation and Emotion (7th ed.), by Johnmarshall Reeve. Copyright © 2018 by Wiley.

517

518 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching Think about your best teachers over the years. What were the qualities that made them great? You probably remember teachers who cared and forged emotional connections with you. Students who feel a sense of connection and relatedness to teachers, parents, and peers are more emotionally engaged in school and more intrinsically motivated (Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Guay et al., 2021; Lawson & Lawson, 2013). Unfortunately, some students feel little sense of membership in their schools or classes. DeLeon Gray (2017) examined high school students’ perceptions of “standing out while fitting in”—their belief that they could show up authentically as individuals while also being welcomed as members of their class. Students of color and students who qualified for free or reduced-price school meals were overrepresented among students who felt that their need to fit in was “unfulfilled” or “somewhat fulfilled.” When students felt a strong degree of fitting in, they reported greater intrinsic value for learning and more positive academic emotions. The motivational benefits of feeling a sense of belonging at school have been reported in a recent meta-analysis. Teacher support was the strongest factor predicting student belonging (Allen et al., 2018). All students need caring teachers, but students who are at risk of school difficulties have an even greater need for this kind of teacher. Positive relationships with teachers increase the likelihood that students will succeed in high school and go on to college (Thompson, 2008; Woolfolk Hoy & Weinstein, 2006). In addition, emotional and physical problems—ranging from eating disorders to suicide—are more common among individuals who feel excluded or lack social relationships (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). As you learned in Cluster 2, your students’ social identities are tied to the social groups that they belong to. This means that their individual needs are also associated with the need for their social groups to feel respected by others (Kachanoff et al., 2020). Organizations and school structures that recognize and support students’ individual and group identities play an important role in meeting students’ need for relatedness and, in turn, their intrinsic motivation. At the end of this cluster, we provide some more specific ways that you can increase students’ sense of belonging in your classroom. SUPPORTIVE RELATIONSHIPS AS MOTIVATORS.

Needs: Lessons for Teachers From infancy to old age, people want to be competent, connected, and personally invested in their pursuits. Students are more likely to participate in activities that help them grow more competent and less likely to engage in activities that hold the possibility of failure. This means that your students need choices and appropriately challenging tasks—not too easy, but not impossible either (Patall & Hooper, 2017). They also benefit from watching their competence grow, perhaps through self-monitoring systems or portfolios. To be connected, students need to feel that people in school care about them and that others can be trusted to help them learn. And don’t forget: Supporting students’ needs also means attending to your own needs. Teachers who had greater self-compassion (i.e., non-judgmental attitude toward themselves, acceptance of their own shortcomings) reported greater need satisfaction and used more autonomy-supportive strategies with their students (Moè & Katz, 2020). What else matters in motivation? Many theories include goals as key elements.

Module 36 Summary What Is Motivation? (pp. 509–513) Define motivation. Motivation is the processes that initiate, direct, and sustain behavior. The study of motivation focuses on how and why people initiate actions directed toward specific goals, how long it takes them to get started in the activity, how intensively they are involved in the activity, how persistent they are in their attempts to reach these goals, and what they are thinking and feeling along the way. What is the difference between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation? Intrinsic motivation is the natural tendency to seek out and conquer challenges as we pursue personal interests and exercise capabilities—it is motivation to do something when we don’t have to. Extrinsic motivation

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

is based on factors not related to the activity itself. We are not really interested in the activity for its own sake; we care only about what it will gain us. How does locus of causality apply to motivation? The essential difference between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation is the person’s reason for acting, that is, whether the locus of causality for the action is inside or outside the person. If the locus is internal, the motivation is intrinsic; if the locus is external, the motivation is extrinsic. Most motivation has elements of both. In fact, intrinsic and extrinsic motivation may be two separate tendencies—both can operate at the same time in a given situation. What do you already know about motivation? Behaviorists tend to emphasize extrinsic motivation caused by incentives, rewards, and punishment. Cognitive views stress a person’s active search for meaning, understanding, and competence and the power of the individual’s attributions and interpretations. In social cognitive theory, self-efficacy and agency are central factors in motivation. Self-efficacy is your belief that you can execute particular behaviors at a particular level in a given situation. Sociocultural views emphasize engaged participation and identity within a community. What are six current explanations of motivation? To organize the many ideas about motivation in a way that is useful for teaching, we examined six broad areas or approaches. Most contemporary explanations of motivation include a discussion of needs and self-determination, goals, expectancies and values, attributions, self-beliefs, and finally, the emotional “hot” side of motivation— interests, curiosity, and anxiety.

Needs and Self-Determination (pp. 513–518) Distinguish between deficiency needs and growth needs in Maslow’s theory. Maslow called four lower-level needs deficiency needs: survival, safety, belonging, and self-esteem. When these needs are satisfied, the motivation for fulfilling them decreases. He labeled the three higher-level needs growth or being needs: cognitive needs, aesthetic needs, and self-actualization. When they are met, a person’s motivation increases to seek further fulfillment. What are the basic needs that affect motivation, and how does self-determination affect motivation? Self-determination theory suggests that motivation is affected by the need for competence, autonomy, and relatedness. When these core needs are met, students experience self-determination and are intrinsically motivated. They are more interested in their work, have a greater sense of self-esteem, and learn more. When teachers communicate with students by providing information rather than seeking to control them, students’ intrinsic motivation is supported. In addition, teachers must acknowledge the students’ perspective, offer choices, provide rationales for limits, and treat poor performance as a problem to be solved rather than a target for criticism.

MODULE 37

Goals,Values, and Ability Beliefs

Learning Objective 12.3 Describe the different kinds of goal orientations and examine their influences on motivation. Learning Objective 12.4 Discuss how students’ expectations for success, value of the task, and perceptions of cost can influence motivation. Learning Objective 12.5 Discuss how students’ beliefs and attributions about school experiences and their ability mindsets can influence motivation.

Goals and Goal Orientations When students strive to read a book or make a 4.0 GPA, they are involved in goal-directed behavior. In pursuing goals, students are generally aware of some current condition (I haven’t even opened my book), some ideal condition (I will read and understand every page), and the discrepancy between the two (I should read and take notes on this reading

519

520 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching today). Goals motivate people to act to reduce the discrepancy between “where they are” and “where they want to be.” Goal setting is usually effective for me. In addition to the routine tasks, such as eating lunch, which will happen without much attention, I often set goals for each day. For example, today I intend to finish this section, walk at least 8,000 steps, grade my students’ response papers, and make a quiche for dinner (I know—not too exciting). Having decided to do these things, I will feel uncomfortable if I don’t complete the list.

Goal Setting: What Am I Trying to Do? According to Locke and Latham (2002), there are four main reasons why goal setting improves performance. Goals: 1. Direct attention to the task at hand and away from distractions. Every time my mind wanders from this reading, my goal of finishing the section helps snap me back. 2. Energize effort. The more challenging the goal (to a point), the greater the effort. 3. Increase persistence. When we have a clear goal, we are less likely to give up until we reach the goal: Hard goals demand effort, and tight deadlines lead to faster work. 4. Promote the development of new knowledge and strategies when old strategies fall short. For example, if your goal is making an A and you don’t reach that goal on your first quiz, you might try a new study approach for the next quiz, such as explaining the key points to a friend. Goals that are specific, elaborated, moderately difficult, and proximal (likely to be reached in the near future) tend to enhance motivation and persistence (Anderman, 2021; Schunk et al., 2014). Specific, elaborated goals provide clear standards for judging performance. They are much more effective than “do your best” goals (Locke & Latham, 2015). If goals are specific and performance falls short, we keep going. For example, Ralph Ferretti and his colleagues (2009) gave fourth- and sixth-grade students either a general goal for writing a persuasive essay (“write a letter to a teacher about whether students should be given more out-of-class assignments . . .”) or the general goal accompanied by specific subgoals such as:

• You need to say very clearly what your opinion or viewpoint is. • You need to think of two or more reasons to back up your opinion. • You need to explain why those reasons are good reasons for your opinion. (p. 580) Students both with and without learning disabilities wrote more persuasive essays when they were given specific subgoals. In another study, first-year college students assigned to take part in an intervention that involved writing about their personal goals performed better academically than students in a control condition (Schippers et al., 2020). The more students reflected on their personal goals and the more specific their goals were, the better they performed academically. Goals that are moderate in difficulty provide a challenge, but not an unreasonable one. Finally, goals that can be reached fairly soon are not likely to be pushed aside by more immediate concerns. Groups such as Alcoholics Anonymous show they are aware of the motivating value of short-term goals when they encourage their members to stop drinking “one day at a time.” Also, breaking a long-term assignment into short-term steps is a way to take advantage of the motivating power of proximal goals. You can probably see that setting attainable goals goes hand in hand with self-efficacy—students’ beliefs in their capabilities to perform tasks. In fact, the relationship is reciprocal (Locke & Latham, 2019). Students with higher self-efficacy in an area tend to set higher goals for themselves, and setting and accomplishing manageable goals raises their self-efficacy (Schunk & DiBenedetto, 2020). Teachers can enhance motivation by supporting both goals and beliefs.

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Making Goals Work: Feedback, Framing, and Commitment Setting specific and attainable goals is helpful, but three additional factors affect how likely it is that we will realize our goals. The first is feedback. To be motivated by a discrepancy between “where you are” and “where you want to be,” you must have an accurate sense of both your current status and how far you have to go. Feedback that shows us our progress is most effective. Like many others, for instance, I wear a personal feedback device. I keep my smartwatch on everywhere I go because it lets me know how far I’ve walked and how many more steps I need to reach my goal. When feedback is designed to highlight progress (e.g., “You are 25% of the way to accomplishing your goal!”) rather than deficits (“You still have 75% of your goal left to complete!”), self-efficacy and perseverance are enhanced (Bandura, 1997). Third-grade students who were given progress feedback on their writing along with a goal-setting exercise improved significantly more in their writing fluency compared with students who only had a goal (Koenig et al., 2016). The second factor affecting motivation to pursue a goal is goal framing. Relational goal structures—whether goals are framed as cooperative, competitive, or individualistic— affect motivation. Table 12.1 shows how these three goal structures differ and what their possible effects are on motivation-related outcomes. When goals are framed in competitive terms, students’ performance and peer relationships are worse than when goals are shared by members of the same group (Roseth et al., 2008). Cooperative learning structures, as you saw in Cluster 10, can facilitate motivation by promoting a sense of belonging, collective efficacy, and shared investment in learning. Cooperation helps students set attainable goals

Table 12.1 Goal Framing Through Different Relational Goal Structures Each goal structure is associated with a different relationship between the individual and the group. This relationship influences motivation to reach the goal. GOAL FRAMING

DEFINITION

EXAMPLES

POSSIBLE EFFECTS

COOPERATIVE

Students believe their goal is attainable only if other students will also reach the goal.

• • • • • •

Team sports Relay race Quilting bee A class garden A symphony A play

• Social inclusion • Positive peer relationships • Better grades • Fewer behavioral problems • Greater task value, interest • Collective efficacy beliefs • Social trust

COMPETITIVE

Students believe they will reach their goal if and only if other students do not reach the goal.

• • • • • •

Singles tennis match Speech contest 100-yard dash School valedictorian Beauty pageant College scholarships

• Extrinsic motivation • Social exclusion • Negative intergroup dynamics • Stress, negative affect • Marginalization of some • Performance goals • Comparative self-evaluations

INDIVIDUALISTIC

Students believe that their own attempt to reach a goal is not related to other students’ attempts to reach the goal.

• Fitness goals • Learning a new language • Making honor roll • Enjoying a museum • Losing/gaining weight • Stopping smoking

Sources: Johnson & Johnson (1999); Roseth et al., (2008); Van Ryzin & Roseth (2018)

• • • • • •

Intrinsic motivation Interest Value Mastery experience Intrapersonal growth Disconnectedness from others

521

522 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching and negotiate when learning tasks are complex. When the goal of a learning task is framed in a way that connects it to students’ intrinsic needs (i.e., to become more competent, self-directed, and connected with others), then students process information more deeply and persist longer to gain a conceptual (not superficial) understanding (Boden et al., 2020). On the other hand, linking the activity to the extrinsic goals of meeting someone else’s standards promotes rote learning—not deep understanding or persistence (Vansteenkiste et al., 2006). The third factor is goal commitment. Commitment matters: The relationship between higher goals and better performance is strongest when people are committed to their goals (Locke & Latham, 2002). If students reject goals set by others or refuse to set their own goals, then their motivation will suffer. So, rather than establishing the goals for the students directly, teachers can promote higher goal acceptance if the students are involved in setting goals and make an active commitment to the goal—for instance, by writing goals down and checking them off as they reach them. Involving students in this process also supports their need for autonomy. Sharing goals with a respected friend or teacher may also increase goal commitment (Klein et al., 2020).

Goal Orientations: Why Am I Trying to Do This? Another motivational aspect of goals has to do with the reasons why people want to accomplish their goals or aims. Why, for instance, does my friend want to run an ultra-marathon? Why does another friend post constantly to Instagram? Why do some of my students seek out harder projects to work on while others choose to work on easier ones? Stop & Think On a scale from 1 (Strongly Agree) to 5 (Strongly Disagree), rate your level of agreement with these statements: The main reason I do my work for this class is . . .

Goal orientations Patterns of beliefs about goals related to achievement in school.

Mastery goal A personal intention to improve abilities and learn, no matter how performance suffers.

To learn more about the topic

To keep my GPA high

To know more than the others

To show how smart I am

To become a better teacher

To avoid performing poorly

To avoid looking incompetent

To outperform others in the class

Goals are specific targets. But the preceding statements reflect your goal orientation. Goal orientations are the reasons we pursue goals and the standards we use to evaluate progress toward those goals. For example, your target might be to make an A in this course. Are you doing so to master educational psychology—to learn all about it—or to perform—to look good in the eyes of your friends and family? Although there are many reasons why people pursue different goals, most researchers have focused on two main goal orientations— mastery and performance (Urdan & Kaplan, 2020). A mastery goal orientation focuses on individual development and learning. A performance goal orientation focuses on a demonstration of competence to others or outperforming others. In the Stop & Think exercise you just completed, can you tell which goal orientations are reflected in the different answers? Most of the questions were adapted from popular achievement goal orientation measures (Hulleman et al., 2010). The point of a mastery goal (also called a task goal or learning goal) is to improve, to learn, no matter how awkward you appear. When students set mastery goals, they are more invested, especially if they feel they have choices and a sense of autonomy (Benita et al., 2014; Michou et al., 2016). Students with mastery goals tend to seek challenges, persist when they encounter difficulties, and feel better about their work (Goetz et al., 2016; Rolland, 2012). They focus on the task at hand and are not worried about how their performance “measures up” in comparison to that of others in the class. We often say that people who are mastery goal oriented “get lost in their work.” In addition, they are more likely to seek appropriate help, use deeper cognitive processing strategies, apply better study strategies, and generally approach academic tasks with confidence (Anderman & Patrick, 2012; Senko et al., 2011).

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Students with performance goals (also called ability goals or ego goals) care about demonstrating their ability to others. They may be focused on getting good test scores and grades, or they may be more concerned with winning and beating other students. Students whose goal is outperforming others may do things to look smart, but sometimes they act in ways that actually interfere with learning. For example, they may cheat or use shortcuts to get finished, work hard only on graded assignments, be upset and hide papers with low grades, choose tasks that are easy, avoid collaborating with other students, and be very uncomfortable with assignments that have unclear evaluation criteria (Anderman & Anderman, 2014; Senko et al., 2011). Competitive structures can encourage performance goals. Some of my (Ellen’s) fifth-grade students wanted to earn the schoolwide award for “reading the most books,” but this performance goal led them to select short and less challenging books to read. Performance goals sound pretty dysfunctional, don’t they? Earlier research indicated that performance goals generally were detrimental to learning, but like extrinsic motivation, a performance goal orientation may not be all bad, all of the time. In fact, some research indicates that both mastery and performance goals are associated with using active learning strategies and high self-efficacy (Midgley et al., 2001). Under certain learning conditions, such as when trying to outperform others, pursuing performance goals has been related to higher achievement (Senko, 2016, 2019). And, as is the case with intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, students can, and often do, pursue mastery and performance goals at the same time (Schwinger et al., 2016). For example, you may want to really understand educational psychology but also get a top grade in your class (Anderman & Patrick, 2012). To account for these findings, educational psychologists have examined another dimension of students’ goal orientations: approach or avoidance. Students may be motivated to either approach mastery (i.e., to learn as much as possible) or avoid misunderstanding (i.e., out of fear of not achieving mastery). They may likewise be motivated to demonstrate their competence to others—a performance approach orientation, or they might be highly motivated to avoid looking incompetent—a performance avoidance orientation. Table 12.2 shows examples and the effects of each kind of goal orientation in this 2 × 2 framework. Where do you see the most problems with each orientation? Do you agree that the real problems are with avoidance? Students who fear misunderstanding (mastery-avoidance) may be perfectionists—focused on getting it exactly right or afraid they will never live up to WAIT—ARE PERFORMANCE GOALS ALWAYS BAD?

Table 12.2 Goal Orientations Students may have either an approach or an avoidance focus for mastery and performance goal orientations. They may hold several orientations at once. GOAL ORIENTATION

APPROACH FOCUS

AVOIDANCE FOCUS

Mastery

Focus: Mastering the task, learning, understanding Standards Used: Self-improvement, progress, deep understanding

Focus: Avoiding misunderstanding; fear of not living up to potential Standards Used: Past performance, perfectionism, imagined best or past personal best (e.g., I must avoid being worse than I was before)

Performance

Focus: Being superior, winning, being the best, seeming competent to others Standards Used: Normative— getting the highest grade, beating the competition, being number one

Focus: Avoiding demonstration of low ability, avoiding losing, avoiding being last, not looking incompetent Standards Used: Normative—not being the worst/slowest, not getting the lowest grade

Source: Adapted from Schunk, D. H., Meece, J., & Pintrich, P. R. (2014). Motivation in education: Theory, research, and applications (4th ed.). Pearson Education, Inc. and Urdan, T. C., & Kaplan, A. (2020). The origins, evolution, and future direction of achievement goal theory. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61.

523

Performance goal A personal intention to seem competent or perform well in the eyes of others.

524 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching their potential. Students who try to avoid looking incompetent (performance-avoidance) may adopt defensive, failure-avoiding strategies like Defensive Drake, described earlier— they might cheat, pretend not to care, or make a show of “not really trying” so they have an excuse for failure (Harackiewicz & Linnenbrink, 2005; Linnenbrink-Garcia & Patall, 2016). Research in both Eastern and Western cultures has demonstrated that failure-avoiding strategies are associated with student helplessness, truancy, disengagement from school, negative emotions, and lower academic achievement (De Castella et al., 2013; Huang, 2016). Two final cautions—performance-approach goals can turn into performance-avoidance goals if students are not successful in looking smart or winning. The path might even lead from performance approach (trying to win) to performance avoidance (saving face and trying not to look incompetent) to learned helplessness (I give up!). So teachers are wise to avoid trying to motivate using competition and social comparisons (Brophy, 2005). In addition, performance-approach and performance-avoidance goals tend to be moderately correlated, so students may pursue both types of performance goals at once (Linnenbrink-Garcia et al., 2012). Some students don’t want to learn, look smart, or avoid looking like they are less capable. They just want to finish fast or escape work altogether, perhaps because they expect to fail or maybe because they just are not interested (Schunk et al., 2014). John Nicholls called these students work-avoidant learners—they feel successful when they don’t have to try hard, when the work is easy, or when they can “goof off” (Nicholls & Miller, 1984). To escape work, these students may say the assignment is too hard or too long, disrupt class, make excuses for not working, or cheat. Of course, if you avoid work using these strategies, you don’t learn much and probably don’t enjoy school either (King & McInerney, 2014).

WORK-AVOIDANCE GOALS.

Work-avoidant learners Students who don’t want to learn or to look smart but just want to avoid work.

How might the learning context in your school or classroom affect students’ goal orientation? Goals are constructed as part of the reciprocal interactions of person, environment, and behavior described by social cognitive theory (Urdan & Kaplan, 2020). For example, in a highly competitive classroom environment, students might be more likely to adopt performance goals. In contrast, in a supportive, learner-centered classroom, even a student with a lower sense of self-efficacy might be encouraged to aim for higher mastery goals. Furthermore, when students work collaboratively to promote each other’s mutual success, their mastery goals are better predictors of their success (Levontin & Bardi, 2018). Researchers have assessed the way students and teachers perceive their classroom goal structure—the goals that they think are emphasized in that class (Murayama & Elliot, 2009). Lisa Bardach and her colleagues (2020) conducted a meta-analysis of 68 studies of classroom goal structures. They found that students’ personal goal orientations were aligned with the goal structures their teachers used in class. When teachers established a positive motivational climate that promoted learning and mastery, students had significantly higher mastery goals. These and similar results suggest that challenge, support, and a focus on learning (not on “looking good” or avoiding poor performance) seem to create a positive classroom environment in the short term that supports learners’ academic success in the long term (Ramos et al., 2021). Researchers have recognized that the motivation and meaning behind students’ goals may also be affected by their cultural context (Zusho & Clayton, 2011). For example, Minhye Lee and Mimi Bong (2016) asked several hundred Korean middle schoolers to list and then rank by order of importance the reasons why they studied. Overall, students listed reasons having to do with social status (the desire to attain a better social standing at school or in life). However, when describing their present class-specific goals, students were more likely to describe mastery goals. The authors concluded that “the type of achievement goal students pursue depends heavily on the dominant culture in the achievement setting” (p. 290).

CONTEXTUAL INFLUENCES ON GOAL ORIENTATION.

Social goals A wide variety of needs and motives to be connected to others or part of a group.

Social Goals Another category of goals becomes more important as students move from childhood to adolescence—social goals. Social goals refer to the reasons why students interact (or refrain from interacting) with others. This may include the goal to make friends, become liked by others, exert control over others, seek help, or develop prosocial behaviors (Jones, 2020).

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

525

Classrooms and schools are social environments, and therefore it makes sense that students’ academic and social goals would be connected. You learned in previous clusters that students develop their social and emotional competencies in concert with their academic and cognitive skills. As a primary socializing space, schools provide opportunities for nonacademic goal pursuits such as athletics, dating, and “hanging out,” which can compete with schoolwork. Some social goals are related to learning. For example, selecting friends who are mastery-oriented in school leads students to adopt similar goals (Juvonen & Knifsend, 2016). Sometimes social goals impede learning. For instance, adolescents’ goal of maintaining friendly relations can get in the way of learning when cooperative learning group members don’t challenge wrong answers or misconceptions because they are afraid of hurting each other’s feelings (Tschannen-Moran & Woolfolk Hoy, 2000). On the other hand, the goal of bringing honor to your family or team by working hard or being part of a peer group that values academics can also support learning (Pintrich, 2003; Rogat & Linnenbrink-Garcia, 2019; Ryan, 2001; Zusho & Clayton, 2011). A desire to be socially included is associated with students’ emotional well-being and selfesteem. In one study, students who sought out social relationships were more likely to report positive emotional conditions such as joy, whereas students who avoided relationships reported higher levels of fear, shame, and sadness (Shim et al., 2013). We have talked about goals in separate categories, but students can and do pursue several, often competing goals at once (Darnon et al., 2010; Vansteenkiste & Mouratidis, 2016). Students have to coordinate their goals so they can make decisions about what to do and how to act. What if interest in doing homework is overtaken by interest in a video game or a romantic text message exchange? What if students do not see a connection between achievement in school and success in life, particularly because discrimination and inequity present barriers to their success? They are not likely to set academic mastery as a goal. The need for peer relationships is basic and strong for most people, but what if social and academic goals are incompatible because peer groups do not value school? Sometimes, “fitting in” with the peer group means not achieving in school—and succeeding in the peer group is more important (Gray, 2014).

Goals: Lessons for Teachers Students are more likely to work toward goals that are clear, specific, reasonable, moderately challenging, and attainable within a relatively short period of time. If teachers focus on student performance, high grades, and competition, they may encourage students to set performance avoidance goals. This could undermine the students’ ability to get deeply engaged in the task and set them on a path toward alienation from learning in school and learned helplessness (Anderman, 2021; Brophy, 2005). Students may not yet be experts at setting their own goals or keeping these goals in mind, so encouragement and coaching are necessary. Feedback should compare students to themselves, not to others in the class— other-comparisons encourage students to avoid looking bad instead of striving to learn. If you use any reward or incentive systems, be sure the goal you set is to learn and improve in some area, not just to perform well or look smart. And be sure the goal is not too difficult. Students, like adults, are unlikely to stick with tasks or respond well to teachers who make them feel insecure or incompetent, which leads us to our next topic—the power of expectations in motivation.

Expectancies, Values, and Costs Many influential explanations of motivation can be characterized as expectancy × value theories. This means that motivation is seen as the product of two main forces: the individual’s expectation of reaching a goal and the individual’s value of that goal. In other words, the important questions are “If I try hard, will I succeed?” and “If I succeed, will the outcome be valuable or rewarding to me?” A related piece of the value part of the equation is the perceived cost of engaging in an activity: “How much will doing this cost me?” Notice that expectancy, value, and cost are personal interpretations—maybe I really won’t succeed,

Expectancy × value theories Explanations of motivation that emphasize individuals’ expectations for success combined with their valuing of the goal.

526 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching or perhaps the success won’t be rewarding—but perception is more powerful than reality in influencing motivation (Eccles & Wigfield, 2020). Motivation is a product of expectancy and value because if either factor is low (or if cost is too high), then there is little motivation to work toward a goal. For example, if I believe I have a good chance of making the basketball team (high expectation) and if making the team is very important to me (high value), then my motivation should be strong. But if either factor is low (I believe I don’t have a chance of making the team, or I couldn’t care less about playing basketball), then my motivation will be low, too. Let’s look at these motivational components more closely.

Expectancy for Success Expectancy for success Students’ beliefs about how well they will perform on an upcoming task.

Self-efficacy A sense of being able to deal effectively with a particular task. Beliefs about your personal capabilities in a particular situation.

Value An individual’s belief about the extent to which a task or assignment is generally useful, enjoyable, or otherwise important.

Importance or attainment value The importance of doing well on a task; how success on the task meets personal or identity-based needs.

Interest or intrinsic value The enjoyment a person gets from a task.

Utility value The usefulness of a task for meeting your goals.

Students usually have an idea about how well they will do on their school assignments. This belief is a student’s expectancy for success (How well will I do?). We talked quite a bit about expectations in Cluster 11 when we discussed social cognitive theory and self-efficacy. When thinking about whether they will be successful, students likely consider their beliefs about their personal capabilities (self-efficacy; Can I do this task?) and their more general academic self-concept (Am I a good student?). If you think expectancies and ability beliefs have a lot in common, you are right. Expectations for success, self-efficacy, and academic self-concept are correlated (Marsh et al., 2019). However, they are distinct. For example, a student may have high self-efficacy for solving equations, but when she learns that her algebra teacher tends to grade some students more favorably, she lowers her expectancy for success (Usher, 2015). Despite the student’s strong self-efficacy, her teacher’s bias leads her to lower her expectation for success and, likely, her overall motivation in math. Students’ expectancies for success and their ability beliefs are strong predictors of how well they will perform in school (Wigfield et al., 2016). Students’ beliefs about their abilities in one subject area may differ from their beliefs in another subject area, leading to different educational pathways over time. For example, one study showed that between Grades 1 and 12, some students’ beliefs about their math and language arts abilities became increasingly divergent and were related to their educational and career choices (Gaspard et al., 2020). Students whose ability beliefs remained more stable (i.e., did not decline throughout their schooling) were more likely to take advanced classes and have higher aspirations. Expectations and ability beliefs predict achievement, but what about the value side of the equation?

Task Value Value is an individual’s belief about the extent to which a task or assignment is generally useful, enjoyable, or otherwise important. Perceptions of value predict the choices students make about participating in a task or activity in the first place, such as whether to work hard on an assignment, enroll in advanced science classes, or join the track team (Wigfield & Eccles, 2020). We can think of task value as having four characteristics: importance, interest, utility, and cost (Eccles & Wigfield, 2020; Hulleman & Barron, 2016). Importance or attainment value is the personal significance of doing well on the task; this is closely tied to the needs and identity of the individual (e.g., to be well liked, athletic, masculine). For instance, if someone has a strong need to appear smart and believes that a high grade on a test proves you are smart, then the test has high attainment value for that person. A second component is interest or intrinsic value. This is simply the enjoyment we get from the activity itself. Some people like the experience of learning. Others enjoy the feeling of hard physical effort or the challenge of solving puzzles. Later in the cluster we will explore interest in greater depth. Tasks also can have utility value; that is, they are useful for achieving a short-term or long-term goal. You can see from our discussion of task value that personal and environmental influences on motivation interact constantly. Often the degree to which we value a task depends on how we have been socialized or our desire to maintain consistency with the norms of our social group. Sometimes social or environmental factors can help us see the value of a task. Personal factors, such as our needs, beliefs, and goals, also affect what we value. Jamaal Matthews (2018) used a mixed method design to study this complexity. He recognized that

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

527

many urban Black and Latino adolescents face cultural stigma that might have lowered their perceptions about the value of math. He surveyed, observed, and talked with students to understand their perceptions and found that students of teachers who consistently demonstrated the relevance of math concepts to everyday life (e.g., calculating the slope that a skateboard travels downhill) reported valuing math more. Students’ experiences pointed to the ways that race, stigma, and context were related to expectancies and values in math.

Perceived Cost For all the benefits our chosen activities bring, there are typically costs, too. Jacqueline Eccles and Allan Wigfield (2020) have therefore argued that a more complete understanding of motivation considers costs as a fourth component of how a task is valued. How much energy/effort will be required (effort cost)? What could I be doing instead (loss of valued alternatives)? What are the risks if I fail (emotional cost)? What will others think of me (social cost)? How much energy have I expended already (sunk cost)? Is the cost worth the possible benefit? Acknowledging the cost reminds us that motivation involves not only what “pulls” us toward an activity but also what “pushes” us away because sometimes “the price is not worth it.” Like values, perceived costs may be influenced by the sociocultural environment (Tonks et al., 2018). Researchers investigating Korean middle school students’ motivation in math found that the more costly students thought math was, the more anxious they felt, the more they procrastinated, and the less organized they were in math (Jiang et al., 2020). Perceived cost is also related to instructional design and delivery. For example, a complex or poorly structured lesson might impose greater extraneous cognitive load on students, which then makes them feel that learning the material is too costly (Feldon et al., 2019).

Expectancy, Value, and Cost: Lessons for Teachers The strength of our motivation in a particular situation is determined by our expectation that we can succeed, the value of that success to us, and the cost of pursuing the goal (Eccles & Wigfield, 2020). From first grade through graduate school, expectancies, values, and costs predict a number of important outcomes such as choice of activities and courses, persistence, achievement, dropping out of a major, pursuing an advanced degree, and career choices (especially for women). It appears that students must first believe that they can succeed before they will value a task, but it takes both expecting to succeed and valuing a task to maintain a high level of performance and persistence. Also, perceptions of costs— particularly related to how much effort might be required—affect students’ performance in science, engineering, mathematics, or technology (STEM) fields and their decisions about whether to pursue STEM careers (Linnenbrink-Garcia & Patall, 2016; Perez et al., 2019). To help students believe that their own success is within their reach, teachers can do all the things described in Cluster 11 to encourage self-efficacy. But they should also make a powerful case for the value of their subject and help students persist in the face of difficulty, even when great effort is needed to succeed.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Attribution Theory (I, C1)

Attributions and Beliefs About Ability One well-known explanation of motivation begins with the assumption that humans try to understand the causes of their successes and failures. Particularly when something unexpected happens, we all ask, “Why?” Students might ask themselves, “Why did I flunk my midterm?” or “Why did I do so well this grading period?” or “Why did my teacher say that to me?” They may attribute their successes and failures to any number of factors, such as ability, effort, mood, knowledge, luck, help, interest, clarity of instructions, the interference of others, or unfair policies. We also try to explain other people’s behaviors, successes, or failures—that others are smart or lucky or work hard, for example. Attribution theories of motivation describe how the individuals’ explanations, justifications, and excuses influence their motivation and emotion (Weiner, 2018).

Go to the Encyclopedia of Psychology. Do a web search for “attribution theory” to learn more about using principles derived from this theory to boost intrinsic motivation to learn.

Attribution theories Descriptions of how individuals’ explanations, justifications, and excuses influence their motivation and emotion.

528 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching Bernard Weiner and Sandra Graham are two of the educational psychologists responsible for relating attribution theory to school learning (Graham, 2020; Graham & Weiner, 2012; Weiner, 1986, 2018). They argue that most of the attributed causes for successes or failures can be characterized in terms of three dimensions: 1. Locus (location of the cause—internal or external to the person). For example, attributing a great piano performance to your musical talent or hard work is an internal attribution. Explaining that the performance is based on coaching from a great teacher is an external attribution. 2. Stability (whether the cause of the event is the same across time and in different situations). For example, most people view talent as stable but luck as unstable because luck changes over time. 3. Controllability (whether the person can control the cause). For example, your personal effort is controllable, but perhaps your innate musical talent is not. Every cause for success or failure can be categorized along these three dimensions. For instance, luck is external (locus), unstable (stability), and uncontrollable (controllability). In attribution theory, ability is usually considered stable and uncontrollable, but as we will soon see, intelligence can be viewed as unstable and controllable. The locus and controllability dimensions are closely related to Ryan and Deci’s (2020) concept of locus of causality (see Figure 12.2 on page 512). Graham and Weiner have argued that these three attributional dimensions have important implications for people’s motivation because they affect what people expect, what they value, and how they feel. The stability dimension, for example, seems to be closely related to expectations about the future. If students attribute their failure to stable factors such as the difficulty of the subject or an unfair teacher, they will expect to keep failing in that subject or with that teacher. But if they attribute the outcome to unstable factors such as mood or luck, they can hope for better outcomes next time. The internal/external locus dimension seems to be closely related to feelings of self-esteem. If success or failure is attributed to internal factors, success will lead to pride and increased motivation, whereas failure will diminish self-esteem. The controllability dimension is related to achievement emotions. If we feel responsible for our failures, we may feel guilt; if we feel responsible for successes, we may feel pride. Failing at a task we cannot control can lead to shame or anger (Graham, 2020; Weiner, 2018).

Attributions in the Classroom

Learned helplessness The expectation, based on previous experiences with a lack of control, that all of one’s efforts will lead to failure.

Students with a strong sense of self-efficacy (see Cluster 11) for a given task (“I can do this math task well”) and those who hold a mastery goal orientation (“The reason I am doing this is to learn and improve”) tend to attribute their failures to lack of effort (“I should have double-checked my work”), misunderstanding directions, or just not studying enough (Grassinger & Dresel, 2018). These are internal, controllable attributions. As a consequence, students with high self-efficacy usually focus on strategies for succeeding next time. This response often leads to achievement, pride, and a greater feeling of control. The greatest motivational problems arise when students attribute failures to stable, uncontrollable causes. People with a low sense of self-efficacy (“I cannot do math”) tend to attribute their failures to lack of ability (“I’m just dumb”). These tendencies are apparent across age levels, cultural groups, and academic topics (Hsieh & Kang, 2010). Such students may seem resigned to failure, depressed, and/or helpless—what we generally call “unmotivated” (Weiner, 2000, 2010). This can create a downward spiral of failure and concealment. When people come to expect that the events and outcomes in their lives are mostly uncontrollable, they can develop learned helplessness (Seligman, 1975). To understand the power of learned helplessness, consider this classic experiment (Hiroto & Seligman, 1975): Subjects received either solvable or unsolvable puzzles. In the next phase of the

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

experiment, all subjects were given a series of solvable puzzles. The subjects who struggled with unsolvable puzzles in the first phase of the experiment usually solved significantly fewer puzzles in the second phase. They had learned that they could not control the outcome, so why even try? Learned helplessness can lead to motivational, cognitive, and affective challenges. Students who feel hopeless, like Hopeless Geraldo described earlier, expect to fail—why should they even try? So motivation suffers. Because they are pessimistic about learning, these students miss opportunities to practice and improve skills and abilities and then might develop cognitive deficits. They often suffer from affective problems such as depression, anxiety, and listlessness (Alloy & Seligman, 1979). How can teachers help their students make more healthy attributions? It’s important to first understand how the attributions teachers make may be linked to those that their students make.

Teacher Attributions Trigger Student Attributions When a teacher attributes student failure to forces beyond the student’s control, the teacher tends to respond with sympathy and avoid giving punishments. If, however, the teacher attributes the failures to a controllable factor such as lack of effort, the teacher’s response is more likely to be irritation or anger, and reprimands may follow. These tendencies seem to be consistent across time and cultures (Weiner, 1986, 2000, 2011). What do students make of these reactions from their teachers? Sandra Graham (2020) gives some surprising answers. Evidence indicates that when teachers respond to students’ mistakes with pity, praise for a “good try,” or unsolicited offers to help, students are more likely to attribute their own failure to an uncontrollable cause—usually lack of ability. Does this mean that teachers should be critical and withhold help? Of course not! But it is a reminder that unintended messages may undermine students’ motivation. Graham and other researchers have shown that ethnic minority students could be the victims of well-meaning pity from White teachers—a form of implicit bias. For example, in one experiment, researchers asked White teachers to give feedback on a student’s poorly written essay (Harber et al., 2012). Only the student’s race was changed across conditions. Teachers’ feedback on essays written by Black and Latino students was more positive than on those written by White students. The findings were replicated with undergraduate students as essay raters (Harber et al., 2019). This research suggests that White teachers might “ease up” on requirements or provide more positive feedback to ethnic minority students so that they will “experience success.” But an indirect communication may accompany the pity, praise, and extra help: “You don’t have the ability to do this, so I will overlook your errors.” This kind of feedback, even if motivated by a desire to protect students’ self-efficacy or feelings, can be a subtle form of racism. Teachers can also positively impact students’ attributions, with benefits to both achievement and motivation. One way is through attributional retraining. Students who are taught to reframe their thinking about what caused their challenges to occur more adaptively (e.g., by attributing the cause to unstable and controllable factors) feel more optimistic that they can improve their performance (Perry & Hamm, 2017). For example, helping lower-performing students attribute achievement to effort instead of ability has improved their course grades and test performance (Hulleman & Barron, 2016). Guiding adaptive attributions is particularly important in contexts where students have been historically devalued or stigmatized—where they have been sent the message that they “should not” be able to do well (e.g., “Girls aren’t good at math.”). The Guidelines: Encouraging a Healthy Mindset on page 533 discuss more ways to foster adaptive attributions. Thus far, we have talked about needs, goals, expectancies, values, costs, and attributions, but there is another factor that must be considered in explaining motivation—the underlying “theory” people hold about abilities. Let’s start with a basic question: What do students believe about intelligence?

529

530 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Mindsets About Ability Stop & Think Rate these statements taken from Dweck (2000) on a scale from 1 (Strongly Agree) to 6 (Strongly Disagree). You have a certain amount of intelligence, and you really can’t do much to change it. You can learn new things, but you can’t really change your basic intelligence. No matter who you are, you can change your intelligence a lot. No matter how much intelligence you have, you can always change it quite a bit.

Fixed mindset A personally held belief that abilities are stable, uncontrollable, set traits.

Growth mindset A personally held belief that abilities are unstable, controllable, and improvable.

Some of the most powerful beliefs affecting motivation in school are beliefs about intelligence and other abilities (Dweck, 2006; Gunderson et al., 2013; Headden & McKay, 2015; Romero et al., 2014; Yeager & Dweck, 2020). These beliefs range on a continuum from a fixed mindset that assumes that abilities are stable, uncontrollable, set traits to a growth mindset that suggests that abilities are unstable, controllable, and improvable. Carol Dweck has noted that students with a growth mindset believe their talents and abilities can be increased with good teaching or coaching, practice, effort, and persistence. The bottom line is that everyone can get smarter by working hard (Morehead, 2012). But not everyone has such a mindset about abilities. In fact, Dweck (2006) has found that about 40% of students hold a fixed mindset and 40% a growth mindset—the rest are in between. Look back at your answers to the Stop & Think questions—how would you characterize your mindset about intelligence? Throughout the early elementary grades, most students have a growth mindset—they believe that effort is the same as intelligence. Smart people try hard, and trying hard makes you smart (Dweck, 2000; Stipek, 2002). At around age 11 or 12, children can differentiate among effort, ability, and performance. At about this time, they come to believe that someone who succeeds without working at all must be really smart and that someone who works hard all the time must not be (Muenks & Miele, 2017). This is when mindsets begin to influence motivation. Students who hold a fixed mindset tend to set performance-avoidance goals to avoid looking bad in the eyes of others. They are more likely to cheat to and seek situations where they can look smart to protect their self-esteem. Like Safe Sumet, they keep doing those things they can do well without expending too much effort or risking failure because either one—working hard or failing—signifies (to them) a low level of ability that cannot be changed. To work hard but still fail would be devastating. In contrast, holding a growth mindset is associated with greater motivation and learning. Students with growth mindsets tend to set moderately difficult goals, the kind we have seen are the most motivating. Believing that you can improve your ability helps you focus on the processes of problem solving and applying good strategies instead of on the products of test scores and grades (Anderman, 2021; Xu et al., 2020). In other words, effort and ability are both important for mastery (Muenks & Miele, 2017).

Ability Mindsets in the Classroom Researchers have found considerable variation in the effects of fixed and growth mindsets on students’ motivation, achievement, and mental health (Yeager & Dweck, 2020). This is because the meaning and impact of students’ mindsets can depend on personal and environmental factors (as you saw in Cluster 11). The most robust finding is that students who experience academic challenges or setbacks are more negatively affected by holding a fixed mindset (Yeager et al., 2019). However, other evidence has shown that in some contexts and cultures, such as China, having a fixed mindset is not associated with students’ achievement (Li & Bates, 2019). Most mindset research has asked students about their beliefs about intelligence. What about students’ mindset about abilities in different areas like math, reading, dance, art, or language learning? Less research has focused on

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

531

these. For example, Elizabeth Gunderson and her colleagues (2017) found that students’ math ability mindsets, but not their reading and writing mindsets, were related to their motivation and achievement. Teachers have personal theories about ability, too, and these can be conveyed to students in strange ways. In fact, Dweck (2006) described a very powerful experience she had in sixth grade. Her teacher seated students around the room in order of their IQ scores and would not let the “lower IQ” students carry the flag, clean the erasers, or take notes to the principal—wow, what a message that effort doesn’t matter! I (this is Anita) had an elementary school music teacher who not only graded us on our singing but also seated us based on our singing ability—best in front and worst in the back. I was in the back row for three years and hated that class. I was also convinced I could never improve my singing until I had a teacher in high school who encouraged growth. Teachers who hold fixed mindsets about their students are quicker to form judgments and slower to modify their opinions when confronted with contradictory evidence (Stipek, 2002). Teachers who hold growth mindsets, in contrast, tend to set mastery goals and seek situations in which students can improve their skills because improvement means getting smarter. Failure is not devastating; it simply indicates that more work is needed. Ability is not threatened. Mindsets become the motivational climate of the classroom. Just as a healthy soil yields healthier plants, efforts to increase students’ growth mindsets were more successful when students’ teachers also held growth mindsets (Yeager et al., 2022). Peers’ mindsets also matter. One intriguing study from the Philippines found evidence that high school students’ mindsets are “contagious” to their peers. The mindsets of a student’s classmates measured earlier in the year predicted the student’s mindset later in the year (King, 2020).

Mindsets About Failure Students and teachers have different responses to failure and setbacks during learning. I (Ellen writing) once taught an aspiring teacher who wrote her entire philosophy of learning on the premise that failure would not be allowed in her classroom. Failure, she believed, would be bruising to her students’ self-esteem and therefore should be avoided at all costs. Unfortunately, such failure-avoiding strategies generally lead to more problems. Let’s look at three approaches to failure in terms of the attributions and mindsets we have learned about. The first approach to failure, as we just saw, might be called failure avoidance. From an attributional perspective, to avoid failure might mean maintaining a sense of control and stability. But students who take this approach often stick to what they know or avoid taking risks, like Safe Sumet, whom you met at the start of this cluster. Even when they are successful, if students constantly fear failure, they may have trouble developing a solid sense of self-efficacy. Their learning will suffer, and they will likely not have strong positive feelings about school. The second approach to failure is to take a more proactive (if counterintuitive) posture to avoid failing by setting up external conditions that could be blamed if failure occurs. The aim is to protect one’s self-esteem. Students who take this stance might claim not to care about their performance. They may adopt self-defeating strategies such as feeble efforts or setting very low or ridiculously high goals. Just before a test, a student might say, “I didn’t study at all!” Then any grade above failing is a success. Procrastination is another example. Low grades do not imply low ability if the student can claim, “I did okay considering I didn’t start the term paper until last night.” All these are self-handicapping strategies because the students are setting up roadblocks to their own achievement to protect their self-esteem or sense of competence (Graham, 2020). They allow the individual to redirect the blame to an external cause. Very little learning is going on. Self-handicapping appears to be more damaging for achievement with elementary- and middle school–age students (Schwinger et al., 2014; Urdan, 2004). Yet a third approach is to welcome and even encourage failure during learning. Students who are not fearful of failure set moderately difficult goals, take risks, and cope

Self-handicapping Students’ engagement in behavior that blocks their own success in order to avoid testing their true ability.

532 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching with failure constructively. They learn fast, have more self-confidence and energy, welcome concrete feedback (as long as it does not threaten them), and are eager to learn “the rules of the game” so that they can succeed. This also means that they seek help when they need it. All of these factors make for persistent, successful learning (Covington & Mueller, 2001). Remember that the orientation students have to failure and setbacks is often modeled for them by parents and teachers. For example, Kyla Haimovitz and Carol Dweck (2016) found that fourth- and fifth-grade children whose parents held the belief that failure is debilitating (e.g., “The effects of failure are negative and should be avoided”) were more likely to hold a view that intelligence is fixed. If students come to believe that failing means they are incompetent, they are likely to adopt many self-handicapping, self-defeating strategies. And teachers who stress performance, grades, and competition encourage self-handicapping without realizing they are doing so (Anderman & Anderman, 2014). Just telling students to “try harder” is not particularly effective. Students need real evidence that effort will pay off, that setting a higher goal will not lead to failure, that they can improve, and that abilities can be changed. They need authentic mastery experiences and a classroom culture that reinforces the notion that through challenge comes growth (Haimovitz & Dweck, 2017).

Attributions and Mindsets: Lessons for Teachers If students believe they lack the ability to understand higher mathematics, they will probably act on this belief even if their actual abilities are well above average. These students are likely to have little motivation to tackle trigonometry or calculus because they expect to do poorly in these areas. How can teachers support a growth mindset (besides, of course, not seating your students in order of IQ test scores)? Dweck (2013) suggests the following:

• Teach students about the brain and how new connections are formed whenever you deal with difficulties and challenges and learn, retrieve, and apply knowledge. Learning changes the brain, and struggling is good! One growth mindset curriculum puts it this way: “Everyone knows that when you lift weights, your muscles get bigger and you get stronger. But most people don’t know that when they practice and learn new things, parts of their brain change and get larger, a lot like the muscles do” (Headden & McKay, 2015).

• Present yourself as a learning coach and resource, not a judge of students’ abilities.

• Give feedback focused on the learning process and strategies. Praise good strategies and effort rather than right answers (Park et al., 2016).

• Don’t avoid criticisms, but make them constructive and focused on improvement. • Don’t comfort students by saying, “It’s okay—maybe you are not just a math person.” Instead say, “You just haven’t mastered this strategy yet, but you can.” As you consider applying the concept of mindsets, beware of what Dweck recently described as “false growth mindset.” Teachers who hold false growth mindsets may claim they believe in growth just because they know they should, but they do so without truly embracing or even understanding the concept. Many students see right through this. And those who don’t see through it might receive the opposite message. For example, just praising students for working hard, even when the outcome is a failure, can lead to what Jere Brophy (1981) called “praise as a consolation prize” (p. 18). The students think they are getting praised because they really don’t have the ability to do any better, so the (unintended) consequence is to reinforce a fixed mindset. Rather than praising simple effort, tie the praise to the process that leads to learning, such as a student’s application of good strategies.

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

GUIDELINES Encouraging a Healthy Mindset Emphasize that abilities are not set but are always improvable. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Share examples of how you have improved your knowledge and skills, for example, in writing, at a sport, or doing a craft. Tell about your own failures that became successes when you tried new strategies or got the right help. Save first drafts and finished products from students in previous classes to show how much the students improved with effort and support.

Teach directly about the difference between learning goals and performance goals. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Encourage students to set a small-step goal for one subject. Recognize improvements often, with private authentic praise that focuses on the processes and strategies that led to success. Use personal best goals, not between-student competition.

Make the classroom a place where failure is just diagnostic—failure tells what needs to be improved, what you haven’t mastered yet. Examples 1.

2. 3.

If a student gives a wrong answer in class, say, “I bet others would give that answer, too. Let’s examine why that is not the best answer. This gives us a chance to dig deeper—excellent!” Encourage revising, improving, polishing, and redoing with an emphasis on improvement. Show students connections between their revised work and a higher grade, but emphasize their growing competence.

Encourage help seeking and help giving. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Teach students how to ask explicit questions about what they do not understand. Recognize students who are helpful. Train students to assume helping roles by assisting each other with ongoing needs (e.g., as technology guides or progress checkers).

When it comes to mindsets about ability, we once again need to beware of either/or. As Dweck has noted, “Everyone is a mixture of fixed and growth mindsets. You could have a predominant growth mindset in an area but there can still be things that trigger you into a fixed mindset trait” (Gross-Loh, 2016, p. 5). The real challenge for teachers and students is to identify what triggers a fixed view of ability, work on reframing this over time, and finally be able to stay in a growth mindset in a given area for longer periods. The Guidelines: Encouraging a Healthy Mindset on the next page provide more ideas. What else do we know about motivation? Feelings matter.

Module 37 Summary Goals and Goal Orientations (pp. 519–525) What kinds of goals are the most motivating? Goals increase motivation if they are specific, moderately difficult, and attainable in the near future. How students relate to their goals in the classroom is influenced by how goals are framed. Goal structures can be competitive,

533

534 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching individualistic, or cooperative. Cooperative goal structures can encourage motivation and increase learning, especially for low-achieving students. Describe mastery, performance, work-avoidant, and social goals. A mastery goal is the intention to gain knowledge and master skills, leading students to seek challenges and persist when they encounter difficulties. A performance goal is the intention to get good grades or to appear smarter or more capable than others, leading students to be preoccupied with themselves and how they appear (ego-involved learners). Students can approach or avoid these two kinds of goals—the problems are greatest with avoidance. Another kind of avoidance is evident with work-avoidant learners, who simply want to find the easiest way to handle the situation. Students with social goals can be supported or hindered in their learning, depending on the specific goal (i.e., have fun with friends or bring honor to the family). What makes goal setting effective in the classroom? For goal setting to be effective in the classroom, students need accurate feedback about their progress toward goals, and they must commit to the goals set. Generally, students are more willing to adopt goals that seem realistic, reasonably difficult, meaningful, and validated by activities connecting them to their intrinsic interests.

Expectancives, Values, and Cost (pp. 525–527) What are expectancy × value theories? Expectancy × value theories suggest that motivation to reach a goal is the product of our expectations for success and the value of the goal to us. If either is low, our motivation will also be low. Values have to be considered in relation to the cost of pursuing a given activity. So the strength of our motivation in a particular situation is determined by our expectation that we can succeed, the value of that success to us, and the cost of pursuing the goal. What are different task values? Tasks can have attainment, intrinsic, or utility value for students. Attainment value is the importance to the student of succeeding. Intrinsic value is the enjoyment the student gets from the task. Utility value is determined by how useful the task is for reaching our short-term or long-term goals.

Attributions and Beliefs About Ability (pp. 527–533) What are the three dimensions of attributions? According to Weiner and Graham, most of the attributed causes for successes or failures can be characterized in terms of three dimensions: locus (location of the cause internal or external to the person), stability (whether the cause stays the same or can change), and controllability (whether the person can control the cause). The greatest motivational problems arise when students attribute failures to stable, uncontrollable causes. These students may seem resigned to failure, depressed, helpless—what we generlly call “unmotivated.” How do mindsets and beliefs about ability affect motivation? When people hold a fixed mindset about ability—that is, they believe that ability is fixed—they tend to set performance goals and strive to protect themselves from failure. When they have a growth mindset and believe ability is improvable, however, they tend to set mastery goals and handle failure constructively. What is learned helplessness, and what deficits does it cause? When people come to believe that the events and outcomes in their lives are mostly uncontrollable, they have developed learned helplessness, which is associated with three types of deficits: motivational, cognitive, and affective. Students who feel helpless will be unmotivated and reluctant to attempt work. They miss opportunities to practice and improve skills and abilities, so they develop cognitive deficits and they often suffer from affective problems such as depression, anxiety, and listlessness. How do attitudes about failure influence motivation? Mastery-oriented students tend to value achievement and see ability as improvable, so they focus on mastery goals, take risks, and cope with failure constructively. A low sense of self-worth seems to be linked with the failure-avoiding strategies intended to protect the individual from the consequences of failure. One such strategy is self-handicapping, in which students undermine their own success to avoid testing their true ability. These strategies may seem to help in the short term but are damaging to motivation and self-esteem in the long run.

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

MODULE 38

Interests, Curiosity, and Emotions

Learning Objective 12.6 Describe the roles of interests, curiosity, flow, emotions, and anxiety in motivation.

How Do You Feel About Learning? Interest, Curiosity, and Emotions Do you remember starting school? Were you curious about what might be in store, excited about your new world, interested and challenged? Many children are. But a common concern of parents and teachers is that curiosity and excitement are replaced over time by a sense of drudgery and disinterest. School becomes a job you have to do—a workplace where the work is not that interesting. In fact, interest in school decreases from elementary to high school, with boys showing greater declines than girls. Even students in the internationally top-rated high schools in Finland found school less enjoyable and valuable in their lives as they moved from ninth to eleventh grade (Wang et al., 2015). The transition to middle school is particularly linked to a decline in interest. These declines are troubling because interest is related to students’ attention, grades, reading achievement, desire to set challenging goals and solve difficult problems, and depth of learning (Linnenbrink-Garcia & Patall, 2016; Renninger & Bachrach, 2015).

Tapping Interests Stop & Think As part of your interview for a job in a large high school, the principal asks, “How would you get students interested in learning? In your teaching, how would you tap their interests?” Interests can be situational (sparked by momentary environmental conditions) or individual (relatively enduring desire to engage one’s attention). Situational interests are shorter-lived aspects of the activity, text, or materials that catch and keep the student’s attention. Individual interests are the more long-lasting aspects of the person, such as an enduring tendency to enjoy subjects such as history or mathematics or activities such as music, gaming, or fashion. Students with individual interests in learning seek new information and have more positive attitudes toward schooling. Both individual and situational interests are related to learning. And interests increase when students feel competent, so even if students are not initially attracted to a subject or activity, they may develop interests as they experience success. Ann Renninger and Suzanne Hidi (2016) have described a four-phase model of interest development.

TWO KINDS OF INTERESTS.

Situational Interest is Triggered → Situational Interest is Maintained → → Individual Interest Emerges → Well-Developed Individual Interest is Established Consider the interests of Julia, a graduating senior in college described by Hidi and Renninger (2006). As Julia waits nervously in the dentist’s office, flipping through a magazine, her attention is drawn (situational interest is triggered) to an article about a man who left his engineering job to become a facilitator in legal conflict resolution. When she is called to the dentist’s chair, she is still reading the article, so she marks her place and returns to finish reading after her appointment (situational interest is maintained). She takes notes and, over the next weeks, searches the Internet, visits the library, and meets with her advisor to get more information about this career option (emerging individual interest). Four years later, Julia is enjoying her job as a facilitator as she handles more and more arbitration cases for a law firm (well-developed, enduring individual interest).

535

536 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching In the early stages of this four-phase model, emotions play a big role—feelings of excitement, pleasure, fun, and curiosity. Situational interest may be triggered by positive feelings, as when Julia started reading. Curiosity followed and helped Julia stay engaged as she learned more about becoming a facilitator. As Julia added knowledge to her curiosity and positive feelings, her personal interest emerged, and the cycle of positive feelings, curiosity, and knowledge continued to build enduring interest. CATCHING AND HOLDING INTERESTS. Whenever possible, it helps to connect academic con-

tent to students’ enduring individual interests. But given that the content you will teach is determined by state standards in most classrooms today, it will be difficult to tailor every lesson to each student’s interests. You will have to rely more on triggering and maintaining situational interest. Here, the challenge is to not only catch but also hold students’ interest (Pintrich, 2003). For example, Mathew Mitchell (1993) found that using computers, groups, and puzzles caught students’ interest in secondary mathematics classes, but the interests did not hold. Lessons that held the students’ interest over time included math activities that were related to real-life problems and active participation in laboratory experiments and projects. Challenge, choices, novelty, fantasy, working with others, hands-on activities and experiments, encouraging students’ explanations, instructional conversations, assuming the role of an expert, personal relevance, utility value, and participating in a group project also can trigger and support interest (Renninger & Bachrach, 2015; Renninger & Hidi, 2016; Tröbst et al., 2016). Culturally relevant materials cultivate individual interest by connecting to students’ lives. For example, Christopher Emdin and his colleagues (2016) implemented a hiphop-themed science program for urban high school students who were predominantly African American or Latino and from low-income communities. Students were tasked with crafting science-based raps or spoken word poems. The semester culminated in a school-wide rap battle held at a local university. This activity enhanced both situational and individual interest in science. It also enabled students to interleave their social, emotional, and academic lives. Initiatives like using hip-hop to enhance motivation and learning in science are a form of personalized education, an approach that has been shown to increase situational and individual interest (Reber et al., 2018). How can you provide personalized learning experiences for your students? One way is to incorporate activities that connect to students’ individual interests outside the classroom (e.g., music, games, sports). A second way is to provide choice in what students learn or how they demonstrate their learning. A third way is to invite students to actively connect the lesson to their own lives (e.g., How does this topic relate to you or to the people or things you care about?). In short, learning that has personal meaning transcends the walls of the classroom, which can enhance and sustain students’ interest (Bergin, 2016). Remember that what works for one student or group of students might not work for others. For example, an approach that is useful for triggering interest for students who have low initial enthusiasm may not work for students who were already interested in the subject. In addition, complex materials can be more interesting than simple materials, as long as students have a growth mindset and believe they can effectively cope with the complexity (Sylvia et al., 2009). When tasks are difficult and students have lower expectations for succeeding, you can support interest by setting goals that emphasize learning. With these mastery-approach goals, difficulties and mistakes are just part of learning and building your brain (Tanaka & Murayama, 2014). Your own interests and enthusiasm can also go a long way in promoting students’ interest (Lazarides et al., 2019). There are other cautions to consider in supporting students’ interests, as you can see in the Point/Counterpoint.

Curiosity: Novelty and Complexity Curiosity and interest are related. A student who is interested may display the very same behaviors as a student who is curious. However, curiosity is distinct from interest in some important ways (Grossnickle, 2016; Shin & Kim, 2019). Curiosity arises when students’ attention is focused on resolving uncertainty to fill a gap in their knowledge (Jirout et al., 2018), whereas interest supposes that students have an ongoing desire to engage with a topic.

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Does Making Learning Fun Make for Good Learning? When many beginning teachers are asked about how to motivate students, they often mention making learning fun. But is it necessary for learning to be fun?

Point Teachers should make learning fun. When we searched “making learning fun” on Google, we found almost 2 billion hits (including a website by that very name for early childhood educators—http://makinglearningfun.com). Clearly, there is interest in making learning fun. Research shows that passages in texts that are more interesting are remembered better (Schunk et al., 2014). For example, in an international study of 15-year-olds, enjoyment of reading proved to be a strong predictor of reading achievement, for both individuals and countries, in the United States and also in 12 of the highest-performing countries—Korea, Singapore, Japan, Hong Kong, Finland, Canada, New Zealand, Australia, Germany, the United Kingdom, China, and the Netherlands (Lee, 2014). Games and simulations can make learning more fun, too. When my (Anita’s) daughter was in the eighth grade, all the students in her grade spent 3 days playing a game her teachers had designed called ULTRA. Students were divided into groups and formed their own “countries.” Each country had to choose a name, symbol, national flower, and bird. They wrote and sang a national anthem and elected government officials. The teachers allocated different resources to the countries. To get all the materials needed for the completion of assigned projects, the countries had to establish trade with one another. There was a monetary system and a stock market. Students had to work with their fellow citizens to complete cooperative learning assignments. Some countries “cheated” in their trades with other nations, and this allowed debate about international relations, trust, and war. Liz says she had fun—but she also learned how to work in a group without the teacher’s supervision and gained a deeper understanding of world economics and international conflicts.

Counterpoint Fun can get in the way of learning. As far back as the early 1900s, educators warned about the dangers of focusing on fun in learning. None other than John Dewey, who wrote extensively about the role of interest in learning, cautioned that you can’t make boring lessons interesting by mixing in fun like you can make bad chili good by adding some tasty, spicy hot sauce. Dewey wrote, “When things have to be made interesting, it is because interest itself is wanting. Moreover, the phrase itself is a misnomer. The thing, the object, is no more interesting than it was before” (Dewey, 1913, pp. 11–12). There is a good deal of research indicating that adding interest by incorporating fascinating but irrelevant details actually gets in the way of learning the important information. These “seductive details,” as they have been called, divert the readers’ attention from the less interesting main ideas, especially if the material to be learned is complex and makes great demands on working memory (Park et al., 2015; Reber, 2016). For example, Shannon Harp and Richard Mayer (1998) used high school science texts with emotional interest and seductive details about swimmers and golfers who were injured by lightning, but these “fun” details failed to improve learning. Adding interesting but unrelated pictures to a social studies reading assignment also led to decreased learning for middle school students in China (Wang & Adesope, 2014). In all these examples, the seductive details may have disrupted students’ attempts to follow the logic of the explanations and thus interfered with their comprehending the text. Harp and Mayer concluded, “The best way to help students enjoy a passage is to help them understand it” (p. 100).

Beware of Either/Or Of course we want our classes to be engaging, interesting, even fun—but the focus through it all should be on learning. Even if the work is tough and at times repetitious, students need to learn to persevere. Working hard is a part of life. Working hard together can be fun.

537

538 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching Curiosity can be important, particularly at early phases of interest development (Hidi & Renninger, 2019). When my (Ellen here) students and I were settling in for class last fall, one student, a music education major, entered a couple of minutes late carrying a large, oddly shaped instrument case. I immediately decided to use it as a teaching prop. “Wow, Seth. That case is huge! What do you all think is inside Seth’s case?” Just like that, our collective curiosity was engaged (I myself did not know). We spent some time discussing the instrument’s features, piecing together our hypotheses based on our collective background knowledge. I can’t remember a more engaged moment for my class during the entire fall semester. As it happened, that day we were covering a topic that you learned about in Cluster 3—Piaget’s concept of disequilibrium and its implications for teaching. Perfect timing—we had just entered a state of cognitive disequilibrium to name and classify this mystery instrument. This “mismatch” between what we knew and didn’t know—a gap not too big but not too small—was motivating to resolve. (By the way, before showing us the instrument, Seth told us that it was a euphonium, which for some of us led to even more disequilibrium and schema reconstruction!) The Guidelines: Building on Students' Interests and Curiosity offer some strategies that you might find useful in your classroom. Notice how some of these strategies came in handy during my impromptu lesson involving Seth’s musical instrument. Your students will similarly need some base of knowledge before they can recognize gaps in that knowledge that lead to curiosity (Ainley, 2019). Asking students to make guesses and then providing feedback can be helpful. Also, proper handling of mistakes can stimulate curiosity by pointing to missing knowledge. Finally, the more we learn about a topic, the

GUIDELINES Building on Students’ Interests and Curiosity Relate content objectives to student experiences. Examples 1.

2. 3.

With a teacher in another school, establish correspondence pals across the classes. Through writing letters, students exchange personal experiences, photos, drawings, and written work and ask and answer questions (“Have you learned cursive writing yet?” “What are you doing in math now?” “What are you reading?”). Letters can be mailed in a bundle or sent via email. Identify and invite community experts for different assignments or tasks. Who knows how to do graphic design? How to cook? How to navigate the stock market? Have a “Switch Day” when students exchange roles with a school staff member or support person. Students must research the role by interviewing their staff member, prepare for the job, dress the part for the day they take over, and then evaluate their success after the switch.

Identify student interests, hobbies, and extracurricular activities that can be incorporated into class lessons and discussions. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Have students design and conduct interviews to learn about each other’s interests. Build a class library with books that connect to students’ interests, hobbies, and cultures. Allow choices (stories in language arts or projects in science) based on students’ interests.

Support instruction with humor, personal experiences, and anecdotes that show the human side of the content. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Share your own hobbies, interests, and favorites. Tell students there will be a surprise visitor; then dress up as the author of a story and tell about “yourself” and your writing. Use creative writing activities to trigger interest, such as crafting catchy story hooks, creating shocking (e.g., silly, grotesque) sentences and diagramming their parts of speech, or writing to an admired person who has passed away.

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

539

Use original source material with interesting content or for the purpose of critical analysis. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Archival letters, diaries, or photographs from a local library. Darwin’s notes in biology. Headlines from different perspectives related to events throughout history. Depictions of different social norms in television or print media.

Create surprise and curiosity. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Have students predict what will happen in an experiment, and then show them whether they were right or wrong. Provide quotes from history, and ask students to guess who said it. Use high-novelty reading materials with elements such as active/emotional verbs (clinging versus walking), unfamiliar characters (orangutan versus fox), unusual adjectives (hairy versus brown), and surprising endings (Beike & Zentall, 2012).

For more information on students’ interests and motivation, see http://www.readwritethink.org and search for “interests.”

more curious we may become about that subject. As Maslow (1970) predicted, fulfilling the need to know increases, not decreases, the need to know more. Curiosity has been associated with greater motivation, exploration, memory, learning, and well-being (Jirout et al., 2018).

Flow Have you ever been “in the zone” or “lost in thought”? You may have been experiencing flow—a mental state in which you are fully immersed in a task with deep concentration and focused attention. When individuals are in flow, they face a highly challenging task with matching high levels of the needed skills—they are stretched but not broken. Individuals in flow experience greater enjoyment in the task, continue working without prompting, and tend to generate higher-quality, more creative products (Abuhamdeh & Csikszentmihalyi, 2012). Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi (pronounced “Me-high Cheek-sent-me-high”) first identified flow in studies of artists, chess players, mountain climbers, musicians, and children at play, but then he examined flow during the activities that take most of our time—work and school. He found that flow was less common in school and more common at work, where people have clear goals and immediate feedback and can apply their skills. The lowest levels of flow for adults occurred on the weekends when there were no particular goals or structured activities except watching television or scrolling through the web (Beard, 2015; Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). What would it look like for students and teachers to find flow in schools? David Shernoff and his colleagues (2014) noted that optimal learning environments that support flow have many of the features mentioned already in this cluster, such as activities that provide a high degree of challenge but are intrinsically satisfying and personally meaningful. In addition, activities that connect the head (intellect) and the heart (affect) through both work-like and play-like structures are optimally engaging. For example, a K–12 school in Indianapolis built part of the curriculum around each student’s list of personally intriguing learning goals for the year. The curriculum also included lessons focused on big ideas or themes for all students such as “Working in Harmony.” In an interview, Csikszentmihalyi pointed to the way the broader theme engaged students: In mathematics you learn the harmony of numbers, and the beauty of being able to manipulate numbers so they always end up in the right way in the equations, and in music you learn the harmony of sounds. In social studies you learn

Flow A mental state in which you are fully immersed in a challenging task that is accompanied by high levels of concentration and involvement.

540 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching about the history of wars and peace, especially peace and how we learn to work together in Sociology. (Beard, 2015, pp. 356–357) This school had a good balance of differentiation (developing individual interests) and integration (focusing on shared interests). Of course, if students lack the challenge or skill elements required for flow, they likely experience other emotions, such as apathy or boredom. It’s time to take a closer look at the role of emotions in learning.

Emotions How do you feel about learning? Excited, bored, curious, fearful? Today, researchers emphasize that learning is not just about the cold cognition of reasoning and problem solving. Learning also is influenced by emotion and mood, so hot cognition plays a role in learning as well (Bohn-Gettler & Rapp, 2011). One educational psychologist has argued that “emotions are the experiential glue of learning environments in the 21st century” (Graesser, 2020a). Part of this growing emphasis comes from the expanding research from neuroscience about the role of emotion in all aspects of learning.

Emotions Three interrelated factors— physiological responses, behaviors, and feelings—that produce an affective response to a situation.

EMOTION AND THE BODY. Although particular regions of the brain have been touted as the “seat” of our emotional lives, the entire body is involved in generating our emotions (Barrett, 2017; Immordino-Yang et al., 2019). How we sleep, what we eat, and whether our physiological needs are met all affect how we assess and deal with the challenges each day brings. Emotions are complex and not the same thing as feelings. For example, two people might feel stimulated and aroused thinking about riding a huge roller coaster, but one person gets a thrilling feeling of anticipation, while another feels panic and dread. Thus, emotion, in this case about roller coasters, is a complex phenomenon arising from the back-and-forth interplay of bodily responses (arousal, rapid heart beat, heightened blood pressure, etc.), cognitive predictions (“It will be a blast” versus “I’m going to die”), and conscious feelings (anticipation and excitement versus fear and dread) (Gluck et al., 2020). So human emotions are the outcome of physiological responses triggered by the brain, combined with interpretations of the situation and other information such as the predictions our brain makes from our past experiences and the context around us (Barrett, 2017). For instance, hearing startling sounds during an action movie might cause a brief emotional reaction, but hearing the same sounds in the middle of the night as you are walking through a dark alley could lead to stronger and more lasting emotional reactions. It should be clear from this description that some of the factors creating emotions are not really under our conscious control—something to remember when working with students in emotionally charged moments. A student’s split-second and uncontrollable emotional judgment—“I sucked at this before and I am doomed to fail again”—can sabotage learning before it starts. However, building self-efficacy and a growth mindset can help students re-label that feeling of dread as a feeling of challenge and excitement (Crum et al., 2020). The same is true for teachers. Split-second emotional decisions about a student who “is just trying to make me look bad” can destroy chances for a good relationship. How could you reappraise that feeling? These difficult-to-control aspects of our brains affect learning, attention, memory, and decision making (Scalise & Felde, 2017). Humans are more likely to pay attention to, learn about, and remember events, images, and readings that provoke emotional responses. Indeed, humans are wired to keep themselves alive and healthy by assessing rewards and threats from their environment. Whether classrooms feel emotionally safe or threatening can affect learners’ ability to think and remember (cognition) and their sense of autonomy, social connectedness, and perceived competence (aspects of intrinsic motivation). At a physiological level, emotions can affect learning by changing brain dopamine levels and the speed of neural firings that influence long-term memory, and by directing attention toward one aspect of the situation (Pekrun et al., 2006). Sometimes, emotions interfere with learning by taking up attention or working memory space that could be used for learning. In teaching, we are concerned about a particular kind of emotions—those related to achievement in school. How can we use these findings to support learning in school? Let’s look at one model of achievement emotions.

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

In the past, with the exception of anxiety, emotions generally were overlooked in research on learning and motivation (Pekrun & Linnenbrink-Garcia, 2015). But as you just read, research in psychology and neuroscience has shown that emotions are both causes and consequences of learning and motivational processes. Reinhard Pekrun (2017) has provided a taxonomy of achievement emotions—the emotions related to academic activities (e.g., studying) or performances (e.g., successes or failures).

ACHIEVEMENT EMOTIONS.

Stop & Think As you read this cluster and prepare for the class for which you are using this text, how are you feeling? What is the focus of your emotion—is it this reading activity, something that happened in class, or something that is coming up? Is your feeling generally positive or negative? How would you describe what is happening in your body? Jot down a few phrases that describe your emotional state. Pekrun’s (2018) three-dimensional classification of achievement emotions might be useful for understanding your emotions right now (see Figure 12.4). Let’s start with the object focus of emotions. Students have emotions related to the learning activities they are doing, but they can also respond emotionally to their achievement outcomes, which can be anticipated in the future or having occurred in the past. The second dimension involves the emotional valence—positive/pleasant or negative/unpleasant. The third dimension is the activation involved in the emotional experience. Activation involves both psychological and physical reactions—changes in brain wave patterns, blood pressure, heart rate, and breathing rate. Some emotions activate our physiological systems; others, such as boredom, deactivate it. Let’s imagine a typical classroom scenario, in which you, the learner, get a sinking feeling when the teacher announces an upcoming test. The object of focus is a prospective outcome (the test), the emotional valence is negative, and the response is activating, as felt by the tension in your body. On the other hand, remembering that you earned an A on the last test might bring a sense of relief. How would you use Pekrun’s taxonomy to describe your emotions right now? When positive achievement emotions are activated, students are more interested, have greater intrinsic motivation, are better at self-regulation, and learn more (Pekrun, 2017). Positive emotions that are deactivating, such as feeling relaxed, might help reinforce learning or foster creativity. When negative achievement emotions are activated, such as anxiety or

Figure 12.4 Pekrun’s Taxonomy of Achievement Emotions Pekrun’s three dimensions of achievement emotions include the object focus of emotions (emotions during an activity or related to a future or past activity), emotional valence (positive or negative), and activation level (whether the emotion activates or deactivates the physiological system). Emotions During Activities

Positive/ Pleasant

Negative/ Unpleasant

Emotions Related to Future Outcomes

Emotions Related to Past Outcomes

Enjoying (Activating)

Hopeful, Excited (Activating)

Proud, Grateful (Activating)

Relaxed (Deactivating)

Imagining Relief (Deactivating)

Relieved, Content (Deactivating)

Frustrated, Angry (Activating)

Anxiety (Activating)

Ashamed, Angry (Activating)

Bored (Deactivating)

Hopelessness (Deactivating)

Sad, Disappointed (Deactivating)

Source: Graphic created by Ellen Usher.

541

542 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching shame, some students become less interested in their academic work. Other students, particularly when they are highly invested in their schoolwork, become more extrinsically motivated and set performance goals to demonstrate their competence or avoid failure. Deactivating negative emotions tend to reduce students’ motivation. For example, boredom, which we feel when we are unable to pay attention or to find meaning in activities, leads to inferior learning (Westgate, 2020). Poor learning leads to being more bored as the student falls further (and hopelessly) behind (Pekrun et al., 2017). Boredom is the opposite of curiosity, which involves paying attention and making meaning. Boredom can be a big problem in school and home-based learning because it is associated with lack of intrinsic motivation, weak effort, shallow processing of information, and poor self-regulated learning (Tze et al., 2016). Strong negative or unpleasant emotions tend to get in the way of learning, whether they are activating or deactivating. EMOTIONS IN THE CLASSROOM. Schools can be a place full of new possibilities, opportunities for diversity, curiosity, and self-discovery. On the other hand, schools can be a stressful place, characterized by peer conflict, constant evaluation by others, and pressure to achieve. What is the emotional landscape of classrooms in the United States? Over 20,000 high school students were asked to provide up to three words in response to the question “How do you feel at school?” (Moeller et al., 2020). Negative emotions were most prevalent. Nearly 40% of students said “tired,” 29% “stressed,” 26% “bored,” and 12% “anxious.” Of those expressing positive emotions, 23% said they felt “happy” and 5% said they were “excited.” This pattern would likely have been different in the younger grades. These reports were made before the pandemic. Learning remotely during the pandemic was enjoyable for some and frustrating for others. The return to in-person instruction brought strong positive and negative emotional responses from students and teachers, but on average, anxiety and stress appeared to be higher for both groups (De France et al., 2021; Steiner & Woo, 2021). How can you increase positive achievement emotions and decrease negative emotions in the subject(s) you teach? Students are more likely to experience negative emotions if they believe they (a) have little control over the learning activities and (b) don’t value the activities. Matching challenge to the students’ skill levels and giving choices can increase students’ sense of control and flow. In addition, efforts to build interest and show the value of the activities also help foster positive emotions. Researchers tracked primary school students’ sense of control and achievement values in math across three time points. As students felt more control over their math learning, they reported greater enjoyment and less boredom, but only when they also perceived math as valuable (Putwain et al., 2018). Several points are important to keep in mind as you think about achievement emotions. First, remember that achievement emotions are domain specific. The fact that students enjoy and feel proud of their work in math does not mean they will enjoy science or history (Goetz et al., 2008; Pekrun et al., 2010, 2014). Second, some level of emotion activation can be desirable in school—even stress. Generally speaking, higher levels of arousal are helpful on simple tasks such as sorting laundry, but lower levels of arousal are better for complex tasks such as taking the SAT or GRE. Third, emotions are contagious. Well, not exactly. But neuroscientists have shown that when social groups interact, group members’ emotions become highly correlated (Saxbe et al., 2020). The emotional climate of the classroom matters. High school students whose teachers showed greater warmth and communion reported greater enjoyment and less anxiety for learning (Mainhard et al., 2018). In addition, teachers who enjoy their subjects tend to be more enthusiastic and encourage student enjoyment, so make sure that you are teaching from your own interests and passions as much as possible (Brophy, 2008; Frenzel et al., 2009; Long & Woolfolk Hoy, 2006). Sometimes, despite our best efforts, students still seem to experience a high degree of anxiety when learning. Let’s take a closer look at anxiety in the classroom.

Anxiety General uneasiness, a feeling of tension.

Anxiety in the Classroom One of the most frequently studied academic emotions is anxiety. At one time or another, everyone has experienced anxiety, or a general uneasiness, feeling of self-doubt, and sense of tension. Recent work on “academic anxieties,” which is a broad term that encompasses

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

experiences of anxiety in educational settings, has demonstrated that many forms of anxiety—test anxiety, math anxiety, science anxiety, public speaking anxiety—can lead to patterns of beliefs and behaviors that hamper performance and promote disengagement in learning (Cassady, 2010; Hart et al., 2016; Ramirez et al., 2018). For example, math anxiety impairs math learning even among children as young as 6 years old, which can affect students’ later progression in school (Tomasetto et al., 2021). Anxiety can be both a cause and an effect of school failure—students do poorly because they are anxious, and their poor performance increases their anxiety, creating a vicious cycle for the learner. Academic anxiety has both trait and state components. So your students may have a personal level of anxiety they bring to different learning situations, and the situation itself may trigger perceptions of threat or self-doubt that add to their basic trait level of anxiety (Covington, 1992; Zeidner, 2014). Anxiety seems to have both cognitive and affective components. The cognitive side includes worry and negative thoughts—thinking about how bad it would be to fail and worrying that you will, for example. The affective side involves physiological and emotional reactions such as sweaty palms, upset stomach, racing heartbeat, or fear (Jain & Dowson, 2009; Schunk et al., 2014). Whenever there are pressures to perform, severe consequences for failure, and competitive comparisons among students, anxiety may increase. Research with school-age children shows a relationship between the quality of sleep (how quickly and how well they sleep) and anxiety. Better-quality sleep is associated with positive arousal or an “eagerness” to learn. Poor-quality sleep, on the other hand, is related to debilitating anxiety and decreased school performance. You may have discovered these relationships for yourself in your own school career (Meijer & van den Wittenboer, 2004). Contemporary orientations to academic anxiety suggest that anxiety affects the beliefs and behaviors of learners in three phases of the learning–testing cycle: preparation, performance, and reflection. During the preparation phase (classroom instruction, studying, test preparation), learners with anxiety tend to have difficulty effectively focusing attention on the relevant material, employing quality study tactics, and maintaining a positive self-worth toward the learning event. Instead of concentrating, they keep noticing the tight feelings in their chest, thinking, “I’m so tense, I’ll never understand this stuff!” From the beginning, anxious students may miss much of the information they are supposed to learn because their thoughts are focused on their own worries. Regardless of whether the learner is unskilled at studying, avoiding the content due to uneasiness caused by the anxiety, or merely distracted by thoughts of the consequences of failing, the material to be learned is clearly compromised (Zeidner & Matthews, 2011). But the problems do not end here. In the performance phase, anxiety blocks retrieval of what was (often poorly) learned (Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1992). Finally, in the reflection phase, learners with anxiety build attributions for failure that further impede their future performance by developing beliefs that they are simply incapable of succeeding at the task, determining that they have no control over the situation, and setting ineffective goals for future situations.

Reaching Every Student: Coping with Negative Emotions Many students, including those with learning disabilities or emotional disorders or those identified as gifted or advanced, may experience negative emotions such as anxiety in school. The emotion regulation strategies you learned about in Cluster 11 will help students manage this stress. For example, when experiencing anxiety, students can use problem-focused, self-regulating strategies like planning a study schedule, borrowing good notes, or finding a protected place to study. Or they could use emotion-focused strategies by attempting to reduce the activation of negative feelings, for example, by using relaxation exercises or describing the feelings to a friend. Of course, avoidance strategies, like going out for pizza or suddenly launching an all-out desk-cleaning attack (can’t study until you get organized!) might alleviate anxiety in the short term but can increase anxiety in the long term. Different strategies are helpful at different times and for different people and situations (Zeidner & Matthews, 2011). What else can teachers do? First, they can help anxious learners become more effective at recognizing the source of their anxious feelings. Helping students reinterpret their

543

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Test Anxiety (I, C3) Test Taking and Anxiety Go to http://www.psu.edu or http:// www.osu.edu (or most college websites) and search for “test taking skills” to find tips and insights into addressing the problems associated with test anxiety. (And the tips might be useful for doing well on the PRAXIS II® exam!)

544 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching body’s sensations can slowly rewire the brain to manage stressful situations more adaptively (Jamieson et al., 2016). As Lisa Feldman Barrett (2020) explains, A hammering heartbeat is not necessarily anxiety. Research shows that students can learn to experience their physical sensations not as anxiety but as energized determination, and when they do, they perform better on tests. That determination seeds their brains to predict differently in the future so they can get their butterflies flying in formation. (pp. 78–79) The Guidelines: Coping with Test Anxiety give more strategies for helping students cope with this common source of anxiety. Similarly, teachers can help students adopt attributional styles that allow them to feel a greater sense of control over their learning and performance. So, rather than developing a failure-accepting view, students can learn to identify situations where they have been successful and recognize that with support and effort, they can achieve better outcomes. Second, teachers can help highly anxious students set realistic goals because these individuals often have difficulty making wise choices. They tend to select either extremely difficult or extremely easy tasks. In the first case, they are likely to fail, which will increase their sense of hopelessness and anxiety about school. In the second case, they will probably succeed on the easy tasks, but they will miss the sense of satisfaction that could encourage greater effort and ease their fears about schoolwork. Goal cards, progress charts, or goal-planning journals may help here (Jain & Dowson, 2009). Third, teachers can support improved performance by teaching students more effective methods for learning and studying. Research on anxious learners indicates that they tend to

GUIDELINES Coping With Test Anxiety Make sure all instructions are clear. Uncertainty can lead to anxiety. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Write test instructions on the board or on the test itself instead of giving them orally. Check with students to make sure they understand. Ask several students how they would do the first question, exercise, or sample question on a test. Correct any misconceptions. If you are using a new format or starting a new type of task, give students examples or models to show how it is done.

Avoid unnecessary time pressures. Examples 1. 2.

Give occasional take-home tests. Make sure all students can complete classroom tests within the period given.

Remove some of the pressures from major tests and exams. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Teach test-taking skills; give practice tests; provide study guides. Provide numerous assessment opportunities to avoid weighing grades heavily on one test. Teach for mastery, allowing opportunities for students to correct their errors for some credit. Use variety in test items because some students have difficulty with particular formats.

Develop alternatives to written tests. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Try oral, open-book, or group tests. Include options within tests on which students can demonstrate their understanding (e.g., answer one of several essay prompts). Have students do projects, organize portfolios of their work, make oral presentations, or create a finished product.

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Teach students self-regulation strategies. Examples 1. 2.

3.

Before the test: Encourage students to see the test as an important and challenging task that they have the capabilities to prepare for. During the test: Remind students that the test is one of many opportunities to show what they know. Encourage task focus—pick out the main idea in the question, slow down, stay relaxed. Help students reframe their bodily response in ways that energize them. After the test: Think back on what went well and what could be improved. Focus on controllable attributions—study strategies, effort, careful reading of questions, relaxation strategies.

For more information about academic anxiety, search the websites at most colleges and universities. For example, go to the website of The Ohio State University, and search for “test anxiety.” Klee et al. (2021) also provide a set of strategies for helping high school students cope with anxiety in math.

spend more time studying, but the methods they adopt tend to be repetitive and low quality (Cassady, 2004). As teachers help students build both the cognitive and emotional skills necessary to overcome anxiety, the students should begin to observe the steady gains in performance and ideally internalize the strategies that have helped them be more successful. Finally, teachers can limit the environmental triggers of anxiety in their classrooms. They can examine their underlying biases to reduce the presence of stereotype threat messages in their classrooms, promote mastery-oriented classroom goal structures, and provide positive role models who show interest in the content. They can also frame assignments as feasible (rather than start off with statements such as “This is really hard stuff”). Also, when teachers are “stressed out” about accountability and statewide testing, they can transmit this anxiety to their students. The more teachers are visibly distressed or continually emphasize “how important this test is,” the more students might view testing as a threatening condition, prompting negative emotions and activating test anxiety.

Interest, Curiosity, and Emotions: Lessons for Teachers Make efforts to keep the level of emotion activation right for the task at hand. If students are sleepy, energize them by introducing variety, piquing their curiosity, surprising them, or giving them a brief chance to be physically active. Learn about their personal interests, and incorporate these into lessons and assignments. If negative emotions like anxiety seem too great, follow the previous suggestions. Support learners’ self-efficacy following the strategies you learned about in this cluster and in Cluster 11. When students perceive this kind of support from their teachers, they enjoy learning even more (Goetz et al., 2020). We have shared a lot of information in this cluster about motivation and emotion. How can we put it all together to help you and your fellow teachers create environments, situations, and relationships that encourage motivation in the classroom? We address these questions next.

Module 38 Summary How Do You Feel About Learning? Interest, Curiosity, and Emotions (pp. 535–545) How do interests and emotions affect learning? Learning and information processing are influenced by emotion. Students are more likely to pay attention to, learn from, and remember events, images, and readings that provoke emotional responses or that are related to their personal interests. However, there are cautions in responding to students’ interests. “Seductive details,” interesting bits of information that are not central to the learning, can hinder learning. How does curiosity affect learning, and what can teachers do to stimulate curiosity in their subject area? Curiosity can be a powerful motivational tool that captures and maintains students’ attention in school. Teachers can foster curiosity by tapping into students’ interests, illustrating

545

546 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching connections between course material and applications that may be interesting to students, and allowing students to find these connections for themselves. An example might include asking students to identify the simple machines at work in a skateboard or roller coaster. What is flow? Flow is a mental state in which you are fully immersed in a challenging task that is accompanied by high levels of concentration and focused attention. When individuals are in flow, they face this highly challenging task with matching high levels of the needed skills—they are stretched but not broken. Individuals in flow experience greater enjoyment in the task, continue working without prompting, and tend to generate higher-quality, more creative products. What are the dimensions of achievement emotions in learning? According to Pekrun’s taxonomy of achievement emotions, students experience emotions on three dimensions. The object focus dimension refers to whether the emotions are related to the activity, or to the outcomes of the activity, whether in the future or in the past. The valence dimension refers to whether the emotions are positive or negative. The activation dimension refers to physiological arousal of the emotional event. Anger and anxiety are examples of activating emotions; relief or hopelessness are deactivating. How does anxiety interfere with learning? Anxiety can be the cause or the result of poor performance; it can interfere with attention to, learning of, and retrieval of information. Many anxious students need help developing effective test-taking and study skills.

MODULE 39

Supporting Students' Motivation to Learn

Learning Objective 12.7 Explain how teachers can influence and encourage students’ motivation to learn.

Strategies to Encourage Motivation to Learn Motivation to learn The tendency to find academic activities meaningful and worthwhile and to try to benefit from them.

Teachers are concerned about developing a particular kind of motivation in their students— the motivation to learn, defined as valuing academic activities and trying persistently to get benefit from them (Brophy, 1988, 2008). Motivation to learn involves more than wanting or intending to learn. It includes the quality of the student’s mental efforts. For example, reading this text 11 times may indicate persistence, but motivation to learn implies that you are engaging more thoughtful, active study strategies, such as summarizing, elaborating the basic ideas, outlining in your own words, drawing graphs of the key relationships, and so on. It would be wonderful if all our students came to us filled with the motivation to learn, but they don’t. As teachers, we have two major goals. The immediate goal is to get students productively engaged with the work of the class; in other words, catch their interest and create a state of motivation to learn. Engagement actually increases motivation, which leads to more learning, and learning leads to greater motivation, and on and on (Reeve & Lee, 2014). A longer-term goal is to develop in our students a more enduring form of motivation based on their individual interests and an underlying sense of personal agency. This will mean they are able to educate themselves for the rest of their lives. How can you accomplish these goals in your classroom? You already have the knowledge needed, based on what we have discussed about students’ fundamental needs; autonomy and choice; clear, specific, mastery goals; expectations for success and growth mindsets; valuing learning tasks; self-beliefs about efficacy and controllable causes of achievement; and emotions that support learning. Table 12.3 shows how each of these components contributes to students’ motivation to learn. With this knowledge in mind, what are some strategies that you can use to increase motivation to learn? It might be helpful to think of your support in response to five motivational questions students often ask: Why am I learning this? Do I like school and find it important, useful, and relevant? Will I enjoy this? Can I do what it takes to succeed? Do I belong in this classroom? (Committee on Increasing High School Students’ Engagement and Motivation to Learn, 2004; Eccles & Wigfield, 1985; Turner et al., 2014).

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

547

Table 12.3 The Building Blocks of Students’ Motivation to Learn Motivation to learn is built on the following components. Certain characteristics related to each component increase motivation, whereas others lower it. MOTIVATIONAL COMPONENT

CHARACTERISTICS THAT ENHANCE MOTIVATION TO LEARN

CHARACTERISTICS THAT DIMINISH MOTIVATION TO LEARN

Source

Intrinsic: Personal factors such as needs, interests, curiosity, enjoyment

Extrinsic: Environmental factors such as rewards, social pressure, punishment

Needs and Self-Determination

Support self-actualization and self-determination: Fundamental needs (safety, security, belonging, competence) are met. Autonomy and appropriate choice are supported.

Undermine self-actualization and selfdetermination: Classroom is unsafe, basic needs are unmet (hungry, fearful); few opportunities for choice; little meaningful social connection.

Goals

Clear, specific mastery and performanceapproach goals: Tendency to choose moderately difficult and challenging goals; personal satisfaction in meeting challenges and improving; concerned with mastering the task; little fear of failure

Mastery and performance avoidance goals: Tendency to choose very easy or very difficult vague goals; fear of failing and looking incompetent

Expectancies, Values, and Costs

Positive: High expectancy for success. Tasks have meaning, relevance, importance; costs of learning are reasonable.

Negative: Low expectancy for success. Tasks have no clear relevance and seem trivial or of little use; costs of learning are high.

Self-Efficacy and Self-Concept

High perceived competence: Clear evidence of personal mastery, successful models to emulate, growth mindset, adaptive strategies, supportive feedback, positive emotions, joy, challenge

Low perceived competence: Few successes, fear of failure. Self-handicapping strategies, learned helplessness, goals too easy or too hard, fixed mindset, depression, hopelessness

Attributions

Stable, controllable: Successes and failures attributed to effort, persistence, applying correct strategies, studying right (or wrong) material, growth mindset

Unstable, uncontrollable: Successes and failures attributed to luck, fixed ability, difficulty of task, unfair teacher

Mindset About Ability

Growth mindset: Belief in ability to develop through learning and sustained effort in challenging tasks. Effort brings growth and change.

Fixed mindset: Belief that abilities are limited and cannot change with effort. Learned helplessness. Effort indicates low ability.

Interests and Emotions

Positive: Curiosity piqued, tasks linked to personal and situational interests, low anxiety and pressure for performance, skill level aligns with level of challenge

Negative: Inability to pay attention, tasks have little meaning. High stress, frustration. Tasks are too easy or too hard compared to skill level. Lack of sustained interest and little curiosity.

Why Am I Learning This? Is It Valuable, Useful, Relevant? Jere Brophy (2008) reminded teachers that learning is valuable: “Powerful ideas expand and enrich the quality of students’ subjective lives” (p. 140). These ideas give us lenses for viewing the world, tools for making decisions, and frames for appreciating the beauty in words and images. An entire issue of Theory Into Practice is devoted to Jere’s ideas about engaging students in the “why” of learning (Turner et al., 2011). One way to demonstrate value and build appreciation is with authentic tasks. If you ask students to memorize definitions they will never use, to learn the material only because it is on the test, or to repeat work they already understand, then there can be little motivation to learn. But if the tasks are connected to current and future real-life problems, students are more likely to see the genuine utility value of the work and are also more likely to find the tasks meaningful and interesting (Pugh & Phillips, 2011). An authentic task relates to students’ lives and requires them to use the tools of the discipline they are studying (say, biology or history) to solve a problem. Solving the authentic problem immerses the students in the culture of the discipline and connects it with their lives (Belland et al., 2013). For example, a physics teacher might use skateboarding as a basis for problems and examples, knowing that skateboarding is an authentic task for many of her students.

CREATING AUTHENTIC TASKS.

Sometimes activities may lack intrinsically interesting qualities despite their relevance to students’ academic skill development INCREASING PERCEIVED RELEVANCE AND UTILITY.

Authentic task Tasks that have some connection to real-life problems the students will face outside the classroom.

548 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching or life outside school. In such cases, helping students see the usefulness or “instrumental” value of tasks is necessary. 1. When these connections are not obvious, you should explain the connections to your students or, even better, support their autonomy by asking them to choose a way to explain how the material will be important in their lives (Rosenzweig et al., 2019). 2. In some situations, teachers can provide incentives and rewards for learning (see Cluster 7). Remember, though, that giving rewards when students are already interested in the activity may undermine intrinsic motivation (Hidi, 2016). 3. Use ill-structured, real-world problems in teaching. Connect problems in school to real problems in life and the broader society, such as buying your first car, making decisions about mobile phone plans, or writing a persuasive letter to a lawmaker. 4. Invite students to think about how what they are learning is connected to their self-interests and communities. Ask students to show how a task might be personally beneficial or meaningful to others in the community (Priniski et al., 2019). Provide opportunities to share learning with community members.

Will I Enjoy This? We want students to see the value of the tasks they perform. We want them to work to learn, not just try to get the grade or get finished. But we also want them to enjoy the process—to be curious and engaged as they make choices and invest effort. Let’s examine several strategies for encouraging students’ intrinsic value and interest (Brophy, 1988). ENHANCING CURIOSITY, INTEREST, AND INTRINSIC VALUE.

1. Tie class activities to student interests in sports, music, current events, pets, common problems or conflicts with family and friends, social movements, fads, television and movie personalities, or other significant features of their lives (Reber et al., 2018). 2. Arouse curiosity. Point out puzzling discrepancies between students’ beliefs and the facts. For example, Stipek (2002) described a teacher who asked her fifth-grade class if there were “people” on some of the other planets. When the students said yes, the teacher asked if people needed oxygen to breathe. Because the students had just learned this fact, they responded yes. Then the teacher told them that there is no oxygen in the atmosphere of the other planets. This surprising discrepancy between what the children knew about oxygen and what they believed about life on other planets led to a rousing discussion of the atmospheres of other planets. 3. Make the learning task fun. Many lessons can be taught through appropriately used but still fun simulations or games (see the Point/Counterpoint on page 537). 4. Make use of novelty and familiarity. Don’t overuse a few teaching approaches or motivational strategies. We all need some variety. Varying the goal structures of tasks (cooperative, competitive, individualistic) can help. When the material being covered in class is abstract or unfamiliar to students, try connecting it to something they know and understand. For example, talk about the size of a large area, such as the Acropolis in Athens, in terms of football fields. SUPPORTING AUTONOMY. Children and adolescents spend literally thousands of hours in schools where other people decide what will happen. Yet we know that self-determination and a sense of internal locus of causality are critical to maintaining intrinsic motivation and engagement (Jang et al., 2010; Reeve, 2018). To support students’ autonomy, consider providing a range of selections and allowing students to pick an option that is important and relevant to them (Katz & Assor, 2007). But beware of giving too many choices. Like totally unguided discovery or aimless discussions, unstructured or unguided choices can be counterproductive for learning (Patall & Hooper, 2019). The alternative is bounded choice—giving students a range of options that set valuable tasks for them but also allowing them to follow personal interests. Students can have input about work partners, seating arrangements, how to demonstrate their knowledge, or what to read.

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Can I Do It? Building Confidence and Positive Expectations We want students to have confidence in their ability so they will approach learning with energy, enthusiasm, and an expectation for success. But no amount of encouragement or “cheerleading” will substitute for real accomplishment. To ensure genuine progress: 1. Begin work at the students’ level, and move in small steps. One possibility is to have very easy and very difficult questions on every test and assignment so all students are both successful and challenged. When grades are required, make sure all the students in class have a chance to make at least a C if they work hard. 2. Make sure students’ learning goals are clear, specific, and possible to reach in the near future. Break long-term projects into subgoals. If possible, give students a range of goals at different levels of difficulty, and let them choose. 3. Emphasize self-comparison, not comparison with others. Give specific feedback and corrections. Tell students what they are doing right as well as what is wrong and why it is wrong. Periodically, give students a question or problem that was once hard for them but now seems easy. Point out how much progress they have made. 4. Communicate to students that academic ability is improvable and specific to the task at hand. In other words, the fact that a student has trouble in algebra doesn’t necessarily mean that geometry will be difficult. Don’t undermine your efforts to stress improvement by displaying only the papers that earned the highest marks on the bulletin board. 5. Model good problem solving, especially when you have to try several approaches. Students need to see that learning is not smooth and error free, even for the teacher.

What Do I Need to Do to Succeed? Providing Structures That Support Progress We want students to believe that success will come when they apply good learning strategies instead of believing that their only option is to use self-defeating, failure-avoiding, face-saving strategies. When things get difficult, we want them to stay focused on the task and not “freeze” from fear of failure (Klee et al., 2021). Classroom structures can help students make steady progress. 1. Give students frequent opportunities to respond through questions and answers, short assignments, or demonstrations of skills and correct problems quickly. You don’t want students to practice errors too long. 2. When possible, have students create a finished product. They will be more persistent and focused on the task when the end is in sight. For example, I (Anita) often begin a house-painting project thinking I will work for just an hour and then find myself still painting hours later because I want to see the finished product. 3. Avoid heavy emphasis on grades and competition. An emphasis on grades forces students to focus on performance, not learning. Anxious students are especially hard hit by highly competitive evaluation. 4. Reduce the task risk without oversimplifying it. When tasks are risky (failure is likely and the consequences of failing are grave), student motivation suffers. For difficult, complex, or ambiguous tasks, provide students with plenty of time, support, resources, and help and the chance to revise or improve work. 5. Model motivation to learn for your students. Talk about your interest in the subject and how you deal with difficult learning tasks (Xu et al., 2012). 6. Teach the particular learning strategies that students will need to master the material being studied. Show students how to learn and remember so they won’t fall back on self-defeating strategies or rote memory. 7. Be aware of situations and communications that increase anxiety; teach students strategies for coping with anxiety in learning and testing.

549

550 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Do I Belong in This Classroom? Creating Inclusive Opportunities We want students to feel as though they belong in school—that their teachers and classmates care about them and can be trusted. Feeling accepted and respected by others goes a long way in enhancing motivation and well-being. Remember that students differ in terms of language, culture, economic privilege, personality, knowledge, experience, and social identity. Their needs, goals, interests, beliefs, and emotions will also develop differently according to their sociocultural contexts (Nolen, 2020). Teachers support students’ motivation by understanding that students’ motives do not work in isolation from their lived experiences. People’s lives, like their motivation, involve complex systems of meaning. Consider this reflection by one of my (Ellen’s) former students, Saira Matthews, about her high school English teacher. As an adolescent, I found the best learning environment for me to be my AP American Literature class. While every other English class I had taken prior to this 11th grade class had pre-selected materials, Mrs. Smith offered a lot more fluidity in our choice of readings. It was the first time I was able to read literature by or about Black people that had nothing to do with slavery. Mrs. Smith gave us guidelines for our papers and projects, but what we read was completely up to us. Not only did this class open my eyes to writers such as Toni Morrison and Maya Angelou, it made me feel really competent as a reader. It also inspired me to start writing more for pleasure rather than just to turn in assignments. Seeing myself in literature and being able to discuss nuances of African American culture beyond slavery was crucial in building my confidence as a writer, which is now an important aspect of me as a graduate student. Notice that Mrs. Smith’s openness to support Saira’s autonomy as a reader led Saira to choose books by Black authors whose experiences resonated with her own identity. This raised Saira’s interest in reading, she felt more competent, and she felt culturally connected in her class. These shifts in her values and beliefs led to a change in her goal orientation toward writing. This is an example of how opportunity structures can support students’ motivation and belonging in school (Gray et al., 2018). Let’s take a look at several research-based strategies for creating belonging-supportive learning environments outlined by Kaleen Healey and Chloe Stroman (2021, pp. 1–2). As you will see, many touch on what was happening in Mrs. Smith’s classroom. 1. Respect each student’s identity along multiple dimensions so that every student feels understood and known as a person and thinker in the environment. (You can find specific strategies for how to do this in Cluster 2.) 2. Affirm each student’s capacity to succeed in the environment by combining high expectations with the feedback and support needed to meet them. 3. Recognize each student’s agency and contributions to the classroom, institution, community, and society. 4. Challenge exclusion and promote inclusion by paying attention to the practices, policies, and norms that may send signals to students about whether they belong. For example, you can work to increase access to learning environments that are accelerated and eliminate practices that inequitably exclude students (e.g., disciplinary sanctions). 5. Use instructional resources and pedagogy that counter negative stereotypes and create cultural continuity via instructional activities that draw on students’ experiences, use collaboration and exploration, and are relevant for communal goals. The Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships offer some strategies for outreach to families that could enhance students’ motivation by connecting learning activities to their lived experiences outside of school. 6. Attend to system-level policies and practices to communicate respect and support for every student, ensure the equitable use of resources and access to information, and engage students in system-level decision making. Creating a supportive environment is not only motivating, it is fundamental to good teaching. That’s why we have devoted a large part of Cluster 13 to it.

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

GUIDELINES Family and Community Partnerships Supporting the Sociocultural Foundations of Motivation Understand family goals for children. Examples 1. 2.

In an informal setting, around coffee or snacks, meet with families individually or in small groups to listen to their goals for their children. Send out questionnaires electronically or via response cards sent home with students asking what skills the families believe their children most need to work on. Pick one goal for each child, and develop a plan for working toward the goal both inside and outside school. Share the plan with the families, and ask for feedback.

Identify student and family interests that can be related to goals. Examples 1. 2.

Ask a member of the family to share a skill or hobby. Identify “family favorites”—favorite foods, music, vacations, sports, activities, hymns, movies, games, snacks, recipes, memories. Tie class lessons to interests.

Give families a way to track progress toward goals. Examples 1. 2.

Provide simple “progress charts” or goal cards that can be posted on the refrigerator. Ask for parents’ or caregivers’ feedback (and mean it) about your effectiveness in helping their children.

Work with families to build confidence and positive expectations. Examples 1. 2.

Avoid comparing one child in a family to another during conferences and discussions with family members. Ask family members to highlight strong points of homework assignments. They might attach a note to assignments describing the three best aspects of the work and one element that could be improved.

Make families partners in showing the value of learning. Examples 1. 2.

Invite family members to the class to demonstrate how they use mathematics or writing in their work. Involve parents or caregivers in identifying skills and knowledge that could be applied at home and prove helpful to the family right now, for example, keeping records on home expenditures, writing letters to service providers, or researching vacation destinations.

Provide resources that build skill and will for families. Examples 1. 2.

Give family members simple strategies for helping their children improve study skills. Involve older students in a “homework hotline” network for helping younger students.

Have frequent celebrations of learning. Examples 1.

2.

Invite families to a “museum” at the end of a unit on dinosaurs. Students create the museum in the auditorium, library, or cafeteria. After visiting the museum, families go to the classroom to examine their child’s portfolio for the unit. Place mini-exhibits of student work at local grocery stores, libraries, or community centers.

551

552 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Module 39 Summary Strategies to Encourage Motivation to Learn (pp. 546–551) Define motivation to learn. Teachers are interested in a particular kind of motivation—student motivation to learn. Student motivation to learn involves taking academic work seriously, trying to get the most from it, and applying appropriate learning strategies in the process. How can teachers support autonomy and authenticity? To be optimally motivating, the tasks that teachers ask students to perform should be authentic and should support students’ self-determination. Connecting to students’ interests and providing choice enhance both authenticity and autonomy. Like totally unguided discovery or aimless discussions, unstructured or unbounded choices can be counterproductive for learning. Teachers can support autonomy by providing bounded choices—a range of options that set out valuable tasks for them but also allow them to follow personal interests. The balance must be just right so that students are not bewildered by too much choice or bored by too little room to explore. How can teachers support value, interest, and positive emotions? When students encounter tasks that are related to their interests, stimulate their curiosity, or are connected to real-life situations, they are more likely to be motivated to learn. Tasks can have attainment, intrinsic, or utility value for students. Attainment value is the importance to the student of succeeding. Intrinsic value is the enjoyment the student gets from the task. Utility value is determined by how much the task contributes to reaching short-term or long-term goals. How can teachers support self-efficacy and expectancies for success? Setting goals that are manageable and helping students see their progress toward their goals can support self-efficacy and expectancies. Goals that are clear, specific, and short term are most motivating. Emphasizing selfcomparison rather than peer comparison helps to build a sense of mastery. Teachers can also model problem-solving strategies and persistence. What structures are most helpful for maintaining students’ engagement and progress? Frequent and diverse assessments help students demonstrate their skill development in ways that engage their interest and reduce boredom. Anxiety can be reduced by focusing goals on learning rather than performance and lowering the risks involved in tasks. What can teachers do to help students feel like they belong? Belonging and relatedness are central to motivation. Assignments that show respect and support for students’ identity development, affirm students’ ability to succeed, value students’ personal contributions, are culturally relevant, and are anti-racist are part of a supportive and welcoming learning environment.

Cluster 12 Review Key Terms Amotivation Anxiety Attribution theories Authentic task Being needs Deficiency needs Emotions Expectancy for success Expectancy × value theories Extrinsic motivation Fixed mindset Flow Goal orientations Growth mindset Hierarchy of needs Importance or attainment value Incentive Interest or intrinsic value

Intrinsic motivation Learned helplessness Locus of causality Mastery goal Motivation Motivation to learn Need for autonomy Need for competence Need for relatedness Performance goal Self-actualization Self-efficacy Self-handicapping Social goals Utility value Value Work-avoidant learners

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

553

Connect and Extend to Licensure Multiple-Choice Questions 1.

2.

3.

4.

Miss Johnson would like her students to be motivated to do their work without bribing them with treats or promises of extra recess time. Which one of the following is the type of motivation she should encourage in her students? A.

Extrinsic

B.

Intrinsic

C.

Locus of control

D.

Relatedness

Why should educators concern themselves with Abraham Maslow’s hierarchy of needs? A.

The stages in students’ development might determine their ability to be successful in certain subjects.

B.

Social and emotional growth can impact students in their ability to cooperate with their peers.

C.

Deficiencies in students’ lives can impact their ability to succeed academically.

D.

Parenting styles determine whether or not students succeed academically.

Which of the following is true regarding extrinsic motivation? A.

Extrinsic motivation should be avoided at all costs because it undermines a student’s intrinsic desire.

B.

Extrinsic motivation is not associated with grades and incentives.

C.

Extrinsic motivation may be necessary to initially encourage students to engage in certain activities.

D.

Extrinsic motivation is more desirable than intrinsic motivation in the classroom because educators have increased control.

Constructed-Response Questions Case

A.

Autonomy and competence

Stephanie Wilson had been educated in “old school methods.” Her teachers insisted on straight rows of seated students who did not talk during lectures or complain about assignments. While Stephanie had been successful in this model, not all of her past classmates had flourished in such a rigid environment. As a new teacher, she wanted a more student-friendly environment. She envisioned a classroom where students were stimulated by the activities and worked collaboratively. “I want my students to look forward to coming to school. I want them to be agents in the learning process, not just passive recipients of my curriculum.” She imagined designing learning situations in which her students could all achieve. Step-by-step, they could all learn! As her students would progress, she would see when they got off the track and manage to remediate before they started to do poorly. In this way, Stephanie thought, none of her students would be failures.

B.

Autonomy and relatedness

5.

C.

Relatedness and competence

Explain why Stephanie’s plan to provide early remediation when students are struggling is a good idea.

D.

Autonomy, relatedness, and competence

6.

How can Stephanie support self-determination and autonomy in her classroom?

Teachers who select all content for their students and insist on students accomplishing their assignments on their own neglect which of the following aspects of self-determination?

What Would They Do? Teachers’ Casebook: Motivating Students When Resources Are Thin At the beginning of this cluster, we asked you to think critically about how you would motivate students when school resources are thin. Now, as you consider these teachers’ responses to that question, we again suggest that you think critically. Do the following responses reflect (or contradict) any of the concepts discussed in this cluster? Do the suggestions and solutions described here make sense in light of what you have learned in this or other classes about best practices? How do these ideas align with or challenge your personal philosophy of teaching?

Maria Soisson • 6th-Grade Math Teacher

In the case presented, the teacher is given seemingly dry and boring material. While the year may be outlined by the required units, this is a great opportunity to be creative with the standards! I would use the workbooks as a guide for the order in which to teach the standards as well as a potential practice set. From there, the way I present the material in class will evolve as I get to know my students and their interests. I will take what I learn about them and find ways to connect the standards to the topics about which they are passionate. Building connections with students creates a sense of mutual respect and obligation which, in turn, will make both me and my students work harder for each other.

Vanessa Sauer • 2nd- and 3rd-Grade Teacher

Walnut Springs Middle School, Westerville, OH

Royal Heights Elementary School, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

The question of motivation is one that educators work to solve on a daily basis. Secondary teachers have specialized in a subject and want to share their excitement and curiosity with students.

Textbooks, especially those that look outdated, scream both “overwhelming” and “boring.” I would say use the textbooks and limited resources available as guidelines for your big ideas.

554 Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching Pull out what the big ideas are and learning objectives for the year. Focus on a variety of teaching strategies in order to develop students’ understanding, such as using pictures for brainstorming or gathering prior knowledge, bringing snippets of text to focus in on and have students respond to, or integrating cross curricular subject areas such as drama and music as assessment for learning. Use games to make it fun and light. Evoke students’ imaginations and personal lived experiences, making the content relevant to their lives through story. Sharing your own stories and modelling vulnerability builds community and trust. When teaching is approached through a lens of curiosity and wonder, the learning has no boundaries and sets a natural tone of inquiry-based learning. Students and teachers learn throughout the process from and with each other. Show that you are learning from what students have to say. Have fun with the freedom and autonomy you have as a teacher to be creative and think outside of the box. If you find it fun and interesting, they will too! Don’t forget to collaborate with other teachers. Your colleagues are professional educators and from them there is a lot to be learned. Oh right, and there’s also the Internet.

be able to pull helpful pieces of information and ideas from them. Remember that you do not need to reinvent the wheel and to use materials available to you. I have always used and skimmed textbooks for my own knowledge to learn the content included. I will pull the important aspects and use them as my key teachings. If the content is too difficult for learners, I will try to find anchor books (picture books) to help hook my learners and engage them in the topic at hand. With learning goals, it is important to always plan student centered activities and have their best interest in mind. Engaging the five senses is important and utilizing information to teach key learning goals can be done through discussion, hands on activities, role play and independent reflections. Having a variety of activities planned that target a learning goal will help meet the needs of all individual learners’ preferences. Providing learners with choice is also an important aspect of in class and remote learning. It is important to understand and know your learners on an individual and whole group level in order to plan activities that engage all learners. Knowing your learners’ strengths and challenges is necessary in order to help set them up for success.

Ashley Richardson • Middle School English/Language Arts Teacher

Melodi Sterner • 6th- and 7th-Grade Teacher

Lansing Charter Academy, Lansing, MI

Riverwatch Middle School, Suwanee, GA

I would look at the years the materials were published and would consider not using them if they were more than 25 years old. There may be some ideas and skills that are relevant, but we have a myriad of resources that have been made available to teachers, and I would want to use current texts to get my students to the levels they need to be at to be successful global citizens. I would keep the learning objectives and their order and find supplemental texts that support the objectives that will not be as intimidating. I would still keep the difficult texts to use for mini-lessons on annotating and scanning for main ideas. The school’s culture and the history of student success is one of the first observations I make when starting at a new school. Some schools celebrate their scholars, and they are driven by their competition with each other to be the best. Other schools may consider the scholars “over-achievers” or “nerds,” whereas others prioritize sports over academics. I would need to assess students’ personal levels of accountability when given difficult tasks and easier tasks. On the first day of school, I have students write a letter to introduce themselves to me. I can assess their writing style and their work ethic if the letter is thoughtful and has length. It’s also minimal risk in getting wrong answers to just talk about yourself, so I usually get a 90% return rate. That way, it is only a few students who didn’t turn the assignment, and I can pinpoint their history of completing work and participating in class. I use instruction to drive my discipline and motivation.

Learning in the classroom does not happen because of materials and textbooks. Learning also does not NOT happen because of the lack thereof. This was one of the first things I had to learn right out of college as I embarked on my first teaching assignment. What makes learning in the classroom happen is relationships. The first task is to embark on learning your community and learning your students. Asking questions like “What are they interested in these days? What is important to them?” Talking with all the students, every day, about the things they are interested in, and using their names, is the first step in having any learning happen in the room. With limited resources, you get creative as a teacher. Build your own presentations. Start the lessons each day with something that they can relate to, talk about, feel successful with, then ease that into the day’s lesson. Students want to be told the truth. Telling them WHY they are learning what they are learning and WHAT may be difficult goes a long way. Having conversations about learning makes a difference with kids. If the resources that you are given are not on level with the students, talk about that. Ask them what they may need to be successful. Involve the students in the learning process but be real about it. I have found that motivation in learning simply starts with treating each student as a real person and giving that individual the opportunity to make the decisions about learning. This encourages a growth mindset. When students feel safe to learn, they learn. Have the students help create better ways to get the information. Help generate ideas such as “We should underline words that we don’t know as we hit them in our texts. Then work together to find the meaning.” Giving students the power to overcome adversity gives them wings in their learning. Kids will fight and overcome any obstacle with you, if you just form relationships first.

Melissa Cho • 3rd- and 4th-Grade Teacher Royal Heights Elementary School, Surrey, British Columbia, Canada In education, it is important to be resourceful and utilize materials available to you. Regardless of the quality do not be quick to discard them until you have reviewed them, as you may

Cluster 12 Motivation in Learning and Teaching

Marianne Robinson • 4th- through 7th-Grade Teacher Meadowbrook Elementary School & Scott Cree Middle School, Coquitlam, British Columbia, Canada In terms of the texts and workbooks, I would let the content of the material guide the focus of the unit to align with district curriculum requirements. To find updated resources, look for Open Education Resources (OERs) via your district cloud tools webpage (or equivalent) that contain the same topics as the aging texts. Collaborating with colleagues is another way to get some ideas to help inspire your students’ connection to their work. I would get student curiosity and interest piqued about the content in the texts by designing a great hook as an entry point to prime their brains for ideas related to the topics. It could be a PowerPoint I have designed (which would include subtitles for any audio or video and alt text for images), bringing in a physical artifact, doing a demonstration or having a guest speaker come in. I would favor a more emergent approach to the topics that allows students to interface freely to exchange prior knowledge and perhaps some pre-existing interest in the material. To do this, students would be invited to draw connections to the topics via brainstorming sessions where they can use the visual learning strategy of mind mapping, or another strategy that works for them, to make connections between topics and themselves. I would try a project-based learning approach so they have more agency to explore the topics and demonstrate their learning. It’s valuable to include, either beforehand or along their learning journey, targeted teaching around skill sets I identify to help students successfully digest material for their projects. This could include applying, analyzing, evaluating, creating, or anything unique to them as learners. The more accessible content is for students with different learning styles, experiences and abilities, the more intrinsically motivated students will be to complete meaningful work.

Pam Gaskill • 2nd-Grade Teacher Riverside Elementary School, Dublin, OH Teaching is inherently creative. Use your time and creativity this summer to acquaint yourself with the required objectives, and think about ways in which you can make them meaningful and relevant to your students. Explore other available resources in the community, such as libraries, speakers’ bureaus, and resource centers. Plan to incorporate a variety of activities such as videos, group work, field trips, projects, and speakers so that your students will remain interested and involved. Use materials that your students have access to from home—books, videos, artifacts, Internet printouts. It is amazing how cooperative parents can be when asked to help in specified ways. You might even make use of the old workbook pages, not in the traditional way, but for cooperative work. You can facilitate student success by pairing weaker readers with more competent readers to discuss and complete the worksheets. Stress that

555

everyone needs to work together to learn the material. Active participation and engagement with the materials will help your students to construct their own meanings more effectively.

Michael Yasis • 5th-Grade Teacher L. H. Tanglen Elementary School, Minnetonka, MN Most learning is acquired through active learning and participation. Therefore, the workbooks that focus on drill and practice, if given as the primary source of learning, most likely would bore the students. I would approach this situation by first engaging the students in a discussion to assess their prior knowledge. I would then challenge and extend their understanding of the concepts through guided discovery, building on similar examples from the “boring” workbooks. While they work on the concepts independently in their workbooks, their confidence and self-esteem will increase.

Kelly Mcelroy Bonin • High School Counselor Klein Oak High School, Spring, TX Simply being excited to be working with the third graders and showing interest and enthusiasm for the subject matter should arouse the students’ interest and encourage them to learn. How many times have you heard it said, “Mrs. Energy was the best teacher I ever had. She took the most boring, difficult subject and made it fun and interesting.” I have heard this so many times both as a student and as a teacher, and it proves my point. Just the fact that the teacher is excited about the material shows the students that this is important information that they need, plus they are curious about the material when they respect and like their teacher. If I felt like the difficulty level of the textbooks was too great, I would have to break the lessons down into smaller increments and use different techniques—discussion, re-teaching, group projects, and so on—to enrich the students and adapt to their level of learning. When your students are motivated, they can accomplish anything—it doesn’t matter what materials are available to them, what the difficulty level of the textbook is. Kids will be motivated when their teacher truly cares about them, is passionate about the material, and makes school interesting.

Danielle Hartman • 2nd-Grade Teacher Claymont Elementary School, Ballwin, MO First of all, don’t get discouraged. You don’t need a textbook to be a successful teacher. Look over the district’s curriculum guides, and see what the objectives are for each unit you will be teaching. Once you know the objectives, get creative. Keeping the students motivated and interested in learning is essential. By giving them choice and using a variety of teaching methods, you will allow them to stay actively engaged in their learning. You will be amazed at what the students will come up with when they are given choices.

Cluster 13

Emma W., Age 5, California, USA

Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Teachers’ Casebook: Bullies and Victims What Would You Do? Two boys are terrorizing one of your students. These boys are larger, stronger, and older than the boy in your class, who is small for his age and gender atypical. There are racial overtones as well—the bullies are White, and the victim is Black. Unfortunately, the bullies are fairly popular, in part because they are successful athletes. There are incidents on the bus before and after school, in the gym, in the hallways, and at lunch—including intimidation, extortion of lunch money, tripping, shoving, and verbal taunts—“fag” and assorted racial epithets are favorite chants. You do not have the two bullies in any of your classes, but your student has started to miss school routinely, and when he is in class, the quality of his work is declining.

Critical Thinking • How would you handle this situation? • Who should be involved? • What would you do about the verbal homophobic and racial insults? • What would you do if the bullies were in your classes? • What would you do if the bullies and victim were girls?

Overview and Objectives 556

“Maintaining a positive and organized classroom setting free from disruption is critical to providing an instructional environment conducive to teaching

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

and learning. Yet it is by no means an easy task” (Skiba et al., 2016, p. 120). This cluster looks at the ways that teachers create supportive social and physical environments for learning by examining classroom organization and management—two of the main concerns of teachers, particularly beginning teachers, and a significant cause of burnout if teachers feel ineffective (Aloe et al., 2014). The very nature of classes, teaching, and students makes good organization a critical ingredient of success, and we will investigate why this is true. Successful teachers create more time for learning, involve more students, form supportive relationships with students, and help students become self-managing. A positive learning environment must be established and maintained throughout the year. One of the best ways to accomplish this is by working to prevent problems from occurring at all. But when problems arise—as they always do—an appropriate response is important. What will you do when students challenge you openly in class, when one student asks your advice on a difficult personal problem, or when another withdraws from all participation? We will examine the ways that teachers can communicate effectively with their students in these and many other situations. By the time you have completed this cluster, you should be able to: 13.1 Relate academic learning time and student cooperation to creating and

maintaining a classroom climate conducive to academic achievement and socio-emotional well-being. 13.2 Summarize the research on the contributions to classroom management

of rules, procedures, consequences, and the design of the physical space. Give special attention to establishing your management system during the first weeks of class, including the differences in requirements between in-person and remote learning. 13.3 Discuss how to maintain a positive learning environment by

encouraging student engagement, preventing problems, and developing caring, respectful relationships with your students. 13.4 Identify strategies (especially penalties and alternatives, restorative

justice, and student self-discipline) for preventing and addressing student misbehaviors, including bullying. 13.5 Characterize successful teacher–student and student–student

communication through such approaches as empathetic listening, I-messages, and problem solving. 13.6 Explain the need for and approaches to culturally relevant classroom

management.

OUTLINE Teachers’ Casebook—Bullies and Victims: What Would You Do? Overview and Objectives MODULE 40: Creating Positive Learning Environments The What and Why of Supportive Classroom Organization The Basic Task: Gain Their Cooperation The Basic Goals: Access, Time, Relationships, and Self-Management

557

558 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments Creating a Positive Learning Environment Relevant Research Results Routines and Rules Required: In-Person Learning Routines and Rules Required: Remote Learning Planning Spaces for Learning Getting Started: The First Weeks of Class MODULE 41: What Now? Maintaining a Positive Context for Learning Maintaining a Good Environment for Learning Encouraging Engagement Prevention Is the Best Medicine Caring Relationships: Connections with School MODULE 42: Positive Response to Problems, Communication, and Connection Dealing with Discipline Problems Stopping Problems Quickly If You Impose Penalties What About School Suspensions and Zero Tolerance? Reaching Every Student: Restorative Justice Teacher-Imposed Penalties Versus Student Responsibility Bullying and Cyberbullying Special Challenges with High School Students The Need for Communication Message Sent—Message Received Empathetic Listening When Listening Is Not Enough: I-Messages, Assertive Discipline, and Problem Solving Diversity: Culturally Responsive Management Cluster 13 Review Key Terms • Connect and Extend to Licensure • Teachers’ Casebook—Bullies and Victims: What Would They Do?

MODULE 40

Creating Positive Learning Environments

Learning Objective 13.1 Relate academic learning time and student cooperation to creating and maintaining a classroom climate conducive to academic achievement and socio-emotional well-being. Learning Objective 13.2 Summarize the research on the contributions to classroom management of rules, procedures, consequences, and the design of the physical space; give special attention to establishing your management system during the first weeks of class, including the differences in requirements between in-person and remote learning.

The What and Why of Supportive Classroom Organization As we discuss classroom organization in this cluster, I (Anita) will be making a distinction between in-person learning and remote learning. As I noted in Cluster 1, expanding your knowledge and skill to include successfully managing remote learning will prepare you for whatever the future throws your way. Let’s start with what you believe.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Stop & Think What do you believe about classroom organization and management? On a 5-point scale from Strongly disagree (1) to Strongly agree (5), how would you respond to these items? 1. Pupils can be trusted to work together without supervision. 2. Being friendly with pupils often leads them to become too familiar. 3. Teachers should consider revision of their teaching methods if these methods are criticized by their pupils. 4. Pupils often misbehave in order to make the teacher look bad. 5. It is often necessary to remind pupils that their status in school differs from that of teachers. Items 2, 4, and 5 in the Stop & Think challenge are custodial items. If you tended to agree with these items, you probably are more teacher-centered in your philosophy of management and interested in maintaining order, rules, and structure in your classes. If you tended to agree more with items 1 and 3, you may tend to be more humanistic in your philosophy and are more optimistic about students’ abilities to become responsible and self-regulated learners. You just took five items from the PCI (Pupil Control Ideology) scale. It was developed by my husband, Wayne Hoy, and his colleagues over 50 years ago and is still used widely today (Willower et al., 1967). If you want to take the full survey, go to https://www.waynekhoy .com/pupil-control. In study after study, classroom organization stands out as the variable with the largest impact on student achievement and social competence (Marzano & Marzano, 2003; Murray et al., 2018). Knowledge of and skill in classroom management are marks of expertise in teaching, and stress and exhaustion from managerial difficulties are precursors to teacher burnout (Emmer & Stough, 2001; Hong, 2012). What is it about classrooms that makes organization so critical? Classrooms are particular kinds of environments. They have distinctive features that influence their inhabitants no matter what the teacher believes about education (Doyle, 2006). Classrooms are multidimensional. They are crowded with people, tasks, and time pressures. Many individuals—all with differing goals, preferences, and abilities—must share resources, use and reuse materials without losing them, move into and out of the room, and so on. Events occur simultaneously—everything happens at once, and the pace is fast. Teachers have literally hundreds of exchanges with students during a single day. In this rapid-fire existence, events are unpredictable. Even when plans are carefully made, a lesson can still be interrupted by a technology glitch or a loud, angry discussion right outside the classroom. Because classrooms are public, the way the teacher handles these unexpected intrusions is seen and judged by everyone. Students are always noticing if the teacher is being “fair.” Is there favoritism? What happens when a rule is broken? Finally, classrooms have histories. The meaning of a particular teacher’s or student’s actions depends in part on what has happened before. The 15th time a student arrives late requires a different teacher response compared with the first late arrival. In addition, the history of the first few weeks of school affects life in the class for the rest of the year.

The Basic Task: Gain Their Cooperation The basic organizational task for teachers is to achieve order and harmony by gaining and maintaining student cooperation in class activities (Doyle, 2006). Given the multidimensional, simultaneous, fast-paced, unpredictable, public, and historical nature of classrooms, this is quite a challenge. Gaining student cooperation means planning activities, having materials ready, making appropriate behavioral and academic demands on students, giving clear signals, accomplishing transitions smoothly, foreseeing problems and stopping them before they start, selecting and sequencing activities so that flow and interest are sustained, maintaining positive, encouraging relationships with students based on mutual respect—and

559

560 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments much more. Also, different activities require different skills. For example, a new or complicated activity may be a greater threat to classroom order than a familiar or simple activity. Obviously, gaining the cooperation of kindergartners is not the same task as gaining the cooperation of high school seniors. During kindergarten and the first few years of elementary school, direct teaching of classroom rules and procedures is important. For children in the middle elementary years, many classroom routines have become relatively automatic, but new procedures for a particular activity may have to be taught directly, and the entire system still needs monitoring and maintenance. Toward the end of elementary school, some students begin to test and defy authority. The challenges at this stage are to deal productively with these disruptions and to motivate students who are becoming less concerned about teachers’ opinions and more interested in their social lives. By the end of high school, the challenges are to handle the curriculum, fit academic material to students’ interests and abilities, and help students become more self-managing so they are ready for life after high school (Emmer & Evertson, 2017; Evertson & Emmer, 2017).

The Basic Goals: Access, Time, Relationships, and Self-Management Stop & Think You are interviewing for a job in a great district—it is known for innovation. The assistant principal looks at you for a moment and then asks, “What is your philosophy of classroom management?” How would you answer? One of my students was asked the Stop & Think question about her classroom management philosophy during a job interview. She told me she got the job and was grateful she had paid attention to this cluster! Classroom management is an overarching name for a whole range of “strategies teachers use on a daily basis to build a positive classroom environment that is structured, engaging, and productive, and encourages student learning and growth” (Gaias et al., 2019, p. 124). Classroom management is more than just order—order for its own sake is an empty, unethical goal. What, then, is the point of working so hard to manage classrooms? There are at least four reasons, and here they are. Each classroom activity has its own rules for participation. Sometimes these rules are clearly stated by the teacher, but often they are implicit and unstated. For example, in a reading group, students may have to raise their hands to make a comment, but in a morning meeting in the same class, they may simply have to catch the teacher’s eye. Teacher and students may not even be aware that they are following different rules for different activities. These rules, or participation structures, define who can talk, what they can talk about, when and to whom they can talk, and how long they can talk. Because the participation structures that some students use at home do not match those for school, these students may not participate as successfully in classroom activities (Cazden, 2001; Charles & Cole, 2019). What can we conclude? To reach the first goal of good classroom organization—giving all students access to learning—you must make sure everyone knows how to participate in class activities. The key is awareness. What are your rules and expectations? Are they understandable, given your students’ cultural backgrounds and home experiences? What unspoken rules or values may be operating? Are you clearly signaling appropriate ways to participate? Some students, particularly those with behavioral and emotional challenges, may require direct teaching and practicing of important participation behaviors (Emmer & Stough, 2001).

ACCESS TO LEARNING.

Classroom management Techniques used to maintain a healthy learning environment, relatively free of behavior problems.

Participation structures The formal and informal rules for how to take part in a given activity.

I once used a stopwatch to time the commercials during a TV quiz show. I was amazed to find that half of the program was devoted to commercials. Seems like football games are even worse! If you used a similar approach in classrooms, timing all the different activities throughout the day, you might be surprised by how little actual teaching takes place. Almost every study examining time and learning has found a significant relationship between time spent on content and student learning. Yet many minutes each day are lost through interruptions, disruptions, late starts, and rough transitions (C. S. Weinstein & Novodvorsky, 2015).

MORE TIME FOR LEARNING.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

A school year seems like a long time, right? Let’s say a typical high school class is mandated by the state to meet 126 hours per school year (180 days times 42 minutes a day). When we consider student absences and school interruptions such as assembly programs, those 126 hours are more like 119 hours available for learning for the typical student. But in every class, elementary or secondary, there are interruptions, clerical tasks, lunch, changing classrooms (or recess in elementary school), collecting and distributing materials, and dealing with behavior problems, so the time available for teaching is typically decreased by about 20%. Now we are left with 96 hours of actual instructional time. Good classroom organization can take back some of those hours for teaching so that more instructional time is available. But simply making more time for instruction will not automatically lead to learning. To be valuable, time must be used effectively. Basically, students will learn what they think about deeply, practice, and apply. Time spent actively involved in specific learning tasks often is called engaged time or sometimes time on task—we can estimate this to be about 80% of the instructional time. Again, however, engaged time doesn’t guarantee learning. Students may be struggling with material that is too difficult, or they may be using the wrong learning strategies. When students are working with a high rate of success—really learning and understanding—we call the time spent academic learning time; again, we can estimate this to be about 80% of the time they are engaged. Now we are down to 62 hours. Figure 13.1 shows how the 126-plus hours of time mandated for a high school year in most states can become only about 62 hours of quality academic learning time for a typical student. So the third goal of class management is to increase academic learning time by keeping all students actively engaged in worthwhile, appropriate learning activities. Engagement in the early grades predicts achievement in later grades and even the decision to drop out of high school (Fredricks et al., 2004). MANAGEMENT MEANS RELATIONSHIPS. All students should feel emotionally and physically safe in the classroom. Beyond that, they also should experience respect and caring. When students feel caring and support from their teachers and their peers, they are more

Figure 13.1 Who Knows Where The Time Goes? The 126-plus hours per year of instruction in a class mandated by most states can represent only 60 to 70 hours of quality academic learning time. 150

125

126

119 96

Hours

100

77

75

62

50

25

0

Total Time (Hours mandated by the state)

Attended Time ( Total time minus days absent)

Actual Engaged Academic Time (Actual Time academic (Attended time minus time minus recess, lunch, time students transitions, are “off task”) etc.)

Academic Learning Time (Engaged time on meaningful, appropriate tasks)

Source: From Middle and secondary classroom management (5th ed.), by C. S. Weinstein and I. Novodvorsky. McGraw-Hill. Copyright © 2015 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, p. 182. Adapted with permission of The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.

561

Engaged time Time spent actively engaged in the learning task at hand. Also referred to as time on task.

Time on task Time spent actively engaged in the learning task at hand. Also referred to as engaged time.

Academic learning time Time when students are actually succeeding at the learning task.

562 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments likely to cooperate with classroom activities. Cooperation leads to learning, learning to a sense of self-efficacy, and self-efficacy to more cooperation. The opposite is true as well. When students sense that their teachers and peers don’t care about them, they feel no particular desire to cooperate—why cooperate with people who don’t like or respect you? The research on teacher–student relationships is clear. When teachers focus on developing high-quality, trusting relationships, students are less defiant, more willing to seek appropriate help, less likely to be absent or to be held back in school, and more likely to achieve (Chen et al., 2019; Dennie et al., 2019; Meyer & L. Anderman, in press).

Self-management Management of your own behavior and acceptance of responsibility for your own actions. Also the use of behavioral learning principles to change your own behavior.

MANAGEMENT FOR SELF-MANAGEMENT. The final goal of any organizational system is to help students become better able to manage themselves. If teachers focus on student compliance, they will spend much of the teaching/learning time monitoring and correcting. Students come to perceive the purpose of school as just following rules, not constructing deep understanding of academic knowledge. And complex learning structures such as cooperative or problem-based learning require student self-management. Compliance with rules is not enough to make these learning structures work (McCaslin & Good, 1998). The movement from demanding obedience to teaching self-regulation and self-control is a major shift in discussions of classroom management (Evertson & Weinstein, 2006). Students learn self-control by making choices and dealing with the consequences, setting goals and priorities, managing time, collaborating to learn, mediating disputes, making peace, and developing trusting relations with trustworthy teachers and classmates (Bear, 2005). Encouraging self-management requires extra time, but teaching students how to take responsibility is an investment well worth the effort. Nancy Perry and Rebecca Collie (2011) compared a preservice preparation program that instructed student teachers about how to coach their students to be self-regulated learners with other programs that did not emphasize self-regulation. The student teachers who developed self-regulation knowledge and skills were more confident, less stressed, and more engaged during their student teaching compared to other prospective teachers who did not learn how to help their students to become self-regulated. This makes sense—if you teach your students to manage their own behavior and learning, you should have fewer management problems, less stress, and more time to teach, which would support your growing sense of teacher efficacy. In another study of ethnic minority students in the Netherlands, researchers concluded that teaching emotional self-regulation skills such as anger management early in the school year led to better relationships and less conflict with the teachers by the end of the year (de Jong et al., 2018). These relationships probably are reciprocal, that is, teachers develop closer relationships with students who can manage their emotions, and students have less trouble managing their emotions when they feel close to and supported by teachers.

Creating a Positive Learning Environment As you think about creating a positive learning environment, review what you already know from this book (retrieval practice is great for learning, right?). You know, for example, that problems are prevented when individual variations such as those discussed in Clusters 2 through 6 are taken into account in instructional planning. Sometimes students become disruptive because the work assigned is too difficult. And students who are bored by easy lessons may find more exciting activities to fill their time. In one sense, teachers prevent discipline problems whenever they make an effort to motivate students. A student engaged in learning is usually not involved in a clash with the teacher or other students at the same time. All plans for motivating students are steps toward preventing problems. And what do we know about learning? We know students need clear goals and examples, practice and review, active participation, and the chance to form connections. Learning how to live productively in classrooms is no exception.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

563

Relevant Research Results What else can teachers do? For several years, educational psychologists at the University of Texas at Austin investigated classroom management quite thoroughly (Emmer & Evertson, 2017; Emmer & Gerwels, 2006; Evertson & Emmer, 2017). Their general approach was to study a large number of classrooms, making frequent observations during the first weeks of school and less frequent visits later in the year. After several months, the researchers noted dramatic differences. Some classes had very few organizational or behavioral problems, whereas others had many. The most and least effective teachers were identified on the basis of the quality of their students’ classroom behavior and achievement later in the year. Next, the researchers looked at their observation records of the first weeks of class to see how the effective teachers got started, and they made other comparisons between the teachers who ultimately had harmonious, high-achieving classes and those whose classes were fraught with problems. On the basis of these comparisons, the researchers developed a number of principles. They then taught these principles to a new group of teachers, and the results were impressive. Teachers who applied the principles had fewer problems, their students spent more time learning and less time disrupting, and achievement was higher. The findings of these studies formed the basis for two books on classroom management (Emmer & Evertson, 2017; Evertson & Emmer, 2017). Many of the ideas in the following pages are from these books. (Personal aside—Ed Emmer was my dissertation supervisor at the University of Texas.)

Routines and Rules Required: In-Person Learning Stop & Think What are the three or four most important rules you will have for your in-person classroom? In this section we focus on organizing traditional in-person classrooms. At the elementary school level, teachers must lead 20 to 30 students of varying abilities through many different activities each day. Without efficient rules and procedures, a great deal of time is wasted dealing with the same questions and issues over and over. “Emily tripped me!” “I left my homework on the bus.” At the secondary school level, teachers must meet daily with more than 100 students who use dozens of materials and often change rooms. Secondary school students are also more likely to challenge teachers’ authority. The effective managers studied by Emmer, Evertson, and their colleagues had planned procedures and rules for coping with all these situations. These procedures and rules were clear and concrete, stated in positive terms whenever possible (what to do instead of what not to do), observable and not vague, taught and practiced, and reviewed when needed (Skiba et al., 2016). ROUTINES AND PROCEDURES. How will materials and assignments be distributed and collected? Under what conditions can students leave the room? How will grades be determined? What are the special routines for handling equipment and supplies in science, art, or vocational classes? Procedures and routines describe how activities are accomplished in classrooms, but they are seldom written down; they are simply the ways of getting things done in class. Carol Weinstein and her colleagues (Romano & Weinstein, 2019; Weinstein & Novodvorsky, 2015) suggest that teachers establish routines to cover the following areas for in-person instruction:

1. Administrative routines, such as taking attendance 2. Student movement, such as entering and leaving or going to the bathroom 3. Housekeeping, such as watering plants or storing personal items 4. Lesson-running routines, such as how to collect assignments or return homework 5. Interactions between teacher and student, such as how to get the teacher’s attention when help is needed 6. Talk among students, such as giving help or socializing You might use these six areas as a framework for planning your class routines. The Guidelines: Establishing Class Routines for In-Person Learning on the next page should help you as you plan.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Procedures and Routines (I, C4) Efficient procedures and routines reduce confusion and opportunities for misbehavior, and they save time that can be devoted to learning tasks. Identify frequent activities or classroom events that would benefit from wellstructured procedures or routines. Explain principles for establishing procedures and routines so that students are likely to observe them.

Procedures/routines Prescribed steps for an activity.

564 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

GUIDELINES Establishing Class Routines for In-Person Learning Determine procedures for student upkeep of desks, classroom equipment, and other facilities. Examples 1. 2.

3.

Set aside a cleanup time each day or once a week in self-contained classes. Demonstrate and have students practice how to push chairs under the desk, take and return materials stored on shelves, silence or remove phones, use the sink or water fountain, assemble lab equipment, and so on. Put a rotating monitor in charge of equipment or materials.

Decide how students will be expected to enter and leave the room. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Have a procedure for students to follow as soon as they enter the room. Some teachers have a standard assignment (“Have your homework out and be checking it over”). Inform students under what conditions they can leave the room, and make sure they understand when they need to ask for permission to do so. Tell students how they should gain admission to the room if they are late. Set up a policy about class dismissal. Many teachers require students to be in their seats and quiet before they can leave at the end of class. The teacher, not the bell, dismisses class.

Establish signals for getting students’ attention and teach them to your students. Examples 1.

2. 3.

In the classroom, flick the lights on and off, sound a chord on a piano or recorder, sound a bell like the “ring bell for service” at a sales counter, move to the podium and stare silently at the class, use a phrase like “Eyes, please,” or move to the front of the class. In the halls, raise a hand, clap once, or use some other signal to indicate “Stop.” On the playground, raise a hand or whistle to indicate “Line up.”

Set routines for student participation in class. Examples 1. 2. 3.

4.

Decide whether you will have students raise their hands for permission to speak or simply require that they wait until the speaker has finished. Determine a signal to indicate that you want everyone to respond at once. Some teachers raise a cupped hand to their ear. Others preface the question with “Everyone.” Make sure you are clear about differences in procedures for different activities: reading group, learning center, discussion, teacher presentation, seatwork, peer learning group, library, and so forth. Establish how many students at a time can be at the teacher’s desk, learning center, sink, bookshelves, class computers, reading corner, or bathroom.

Determine how you will communicate, collect, and return assignments. Examples 1.

2.

Establish a place for listing assignments. Some teachers reserve a particular corner of the board for assignments. Others write assignments in colored chalk. For younger students, it may be better to prepare assignment sheets or folders, color-coding them for the math workbook, reading packet, and science kit. When all students have home access to the Internet, teachers can post assignments online on a class calendar and include alerts or reminders for students who are not keeping up. Be clear about how and where assignments should be collected. Some teachers collect assignments in a box or bin; others have a student collect work while they introduce the next activity; some collect online.

For ideas about involving students in developing rules and procedures, see https://www. edutopia.org/article/simple-ways-promote-student-voice-classroom.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

565

Unlike routines, rules, which specify expected and forbidden actions in the class, are often written down and posted. They are the dos and don’ts of classroom life. In establishing rules, you should consider what kind of atmosphere you want to create. What student behaviors will help you teach effectively? What limits do the students need to guide their behavior? The rules you set should be consistent with school rules and also in keeping with principles of learning. For example, we know from the research on small-group learning that students benefit when they explain work to peers. They learn as they teach. A rule that forbids students to help each other may be inconsistent with good learning principles. Or a rule that says, “No erasures when writing” may make students focus more on preventing mistakes than on communicating clearly in their writing (Emmer & Stough, 2001; Romano & Weinstein, 2019). Rules should be positive and observable (raise your hand to be recognized). Having a few general rules that cover many specifics is better than listing all the dos and don’ts. But, if specific actions are forbidden, such as leaving the campus, then a rule should make this clear (Emmer & Gerwels, 2006).

RULES.

RULES FOR IN-PERSON TEACHING IN ELEMENTARY SCHOOL. Evertson and Emmer (2017) give four examples of general rules for elementary school classes:

1. Respect and be polite to all people. Give clear explanations of what you mean by “polite,” including not hitting, fighting, or teasing. Examples of polite behavior include waiting your turn, saying “please” and “thank you,” and not calling names. This applies to behavior toward adults (including substitute teachers) and peers. 2. Be prompt and prepared. This rule highlights the importance of the academic work in the class. Being prompt includes the beginning of the day as well as transitions between activities. Being prepared means the having the right materials and the right mental attitude for success. 3. Listen quietly while others are speaking. This applies to the teacher and other students, in both large-class lessons and small-group discussions. 4. Obey all school rules. This reminds students that all school rules apply in your classroom. Then students cannot claim, for example, that they thought it was OK to chew gum or listen to music on their smartphone in your class, even though these are against school rules, “because you never made a rule against it for us.” Whatever the rule, students need to be taught the behaviors that the rule includes and excludes. Examples, practice, and discussion will be needed before learning is complete. As you’ve seen, different activities often require different rules. This can be confusing for elementary students until they have thoroughly learned all the rules. To prevent confusion, you might consider making signs that list the key rules for each activity. Then, before the activity, you can post the appropriate sign as a reminder. This provides clear and consistent cues about participation structures so all students, not just the “well behaved,” know what is expected. Of course, you’ll need to explain and discuss these rules before the signs can have their full effect. RULES FOR IN-PERSON TEACHING IN SECONDARY SCHOOL.

Emmer and Evertson (2017)

suggest five examples of rules for secondary students: 1. Be prompt and prepared. Prompt means being in class on time but also moving quickly into and out of group work or other tasks. Being prepared means having the right materials (the type of pen, pencil, paper, laptop or tablet, notebook, texts, and so on) and the right attitude for learning. 2. Respect and be polite to all people. This covers fighting, verbal abuse, and general troublemaking, but emphasize as well positive examples of respect and kindness. All people includes the teacher and substitute teachers. 3. Listen and stay seated while someone else is speaking. This applies when the teacher or other students are talking.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Rules (I, C4) Fair, consistently enforced rules can have a positive effect on motivation to learn by promoting a safe and warm classroom environment. Describe how to establish and maintain effective rules. Keep in mind age-related concerns.

Rules Statements specifying expected and forbidden behaviors; dos and don’ts.

566 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments 4. Respect other people’s property. This means property belonging to the school, the teacher, or other students. Take care of others’ property, ask permission to borrow, and return borrowed items in good shape. 5. Obey all school rules. As with the elementary class rules, this covers many behaviors and situations so you do not have to repeat every school rule for your class. This reminds students that the school rules apply in your room, too, and gives you the chance to talk about which school rules are particularly important in your class (no cell phones, no texting . . .). It also reminds the students that you will be monitoring them inside and outside your class. Make sure you know all the school rules. Some secondary students are very adept at convincing teachers that their misbehavior “really isn’t against the rules.” These rules are more than ways to maintain order. In their study of 34 middle school classrooms, Lindsay Matsumura and her colleagues (2008) found that having explicit rules about respecting others in the classroom predicted the number of students who participated in class discussion, so it seems clear that respect is a gateway to student engagement and class dialogue that supports learning.

Natural/logical consequences Instead of punishing, have students redo, repair, or in some way face the consequences that naturally flow from their actions.

CONSEQUENCES. As soon as you decide on your rules and procedures for in-person learning, you must consider what you will do when a student breaks a rule or does not follow a procedure. It is too late to make this decision after the rule has been broken. For many infractions, the logical consequence is going back to “do it right.” Students who run in the hall may have to return to where they started and walk properly. Incomplete papers can be redone. Materials left out should be put back. You can use natural or logical consequences to support social and emotional development by doing the following (Charles & Cole, 2019; M. J. Elias & Schwab, 2006):

• Separate the deed from the doer in your response. The problem is the behavior, not the student.

• Emphasize to students that they have the power to choose their actions and so avoid losing control.

• Encourage student reflection, self-evaluation, and problem solving. Avoid teacher lecturing.

• Help students identify and give a rationale for what they could do differently next time in a similar situation. The main point here is that decisions about consequences (positive and negative) must be made early on so students know before they follow or break a rule or procedure what this will mean for them. I encourage my student teachers to get a copy of the school rules and their cooperating teacher’s rules and then plan their own. Sometimes, consequences are complicated, as you will see when we discuss penalties later in this cluster. WHO SETS THE RULES AND CONSEQUENCES? In Cluster 1, I described Ken, an expert teacher who worked with his students to establish a students’ “Bill of Rights” instead of defining rules. These “rights” cover most situations that might require a “rule” and help the students move toward the goal of becoming self-managing. In a recent class, the Bill of Rights included the rights to be treated politely, have a 2-minute break between working periods, make choices about the day’s schedule, and not have people take your things, among several others. If you are going to involve students in setting rules or creating a constitution, you may need to wait until you have established a sense of community in your classroom. Before students can contribute meaningfully to the class rules, they need to trust the teacher and the situation (M. J. Elias & Schwab, 2006). Developing rights and responsibilities rather than rules makes an important point to students. “Teaching children that something is wrong because there is a rule against it is not the same as teaching them that there is a rule against it because it is wrong, and helping

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Table 13.1 Laws to Protect Our Rights 1. Follow directions the first time. 2. Speak nicely, be courteous, and respect other people, their feelings, and their things. Follow the Bill of Rights. 3. Laugh at the right time for the right time. 4. Respect others’ right to learn. Do not distract others. Don’t be nosy. Don’t yell. Remember to get quiet at countdown. 5. Talk at the right times with the right tone of voice and volume. 6. Make sure transitions and movements are calm, quiet, careful, and elegant. 7. Follow all classroom and school procedures, like: bathroom; lunch and recess; morning; dismissal; etc. Source: From Elementary classroom management (7th ed.), p. 104, by M. E. Romano and C. S. Weinstein. McGraw-Hill. Copyright © 2019 by The McGraw-Hill Companies. Adapted with permission of The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.

them to understand why this is so” (C. S. Weinstein, 1999, p. 154). Students should understand that the rules are developed so that everyone can work and learn together. I might add that when Ken had some very difficult classes, he and his students established some “laws” that protect students’ rights, as you can see in Table 13.1.

Routines and Rules Required: Remote Learning When Emily Pulham and Charles Graham (2018) reviewed studies of the skills needed for hybrid teaching compared to the skills needed for online-only instruction, they found that management played a big role in both approaches. They also found that few preservice teachers were taught relevant skills for organizing and managing hybrid or remote learning. Some of the important competencies are setting expectations, managing time, monitoring students’ behavior, designing appropriate discipline strategies, and handling communication and collaboration. Even if students are not in the room, the managerial challenges are still the same—gain their cooperation, create community and an engaging learning environment, develop routines so students know what is expected, and build relationships. As Heather Wolpert-Gawron (2017) noted, “. . . many online educators don’t realize that the best practices in traditional environments should not be discarded simply because the participants are interacting digitally from various locations. They still need to be managed as a cohesive group of learners” (p. 1). So what are experienced teachers learning about organizing and managing remote instruction? The Guidelines: Establishing Routines and Rules for Remote Learning will give you ideas. Some suggestions are based in part on Healy (2020), Hoy (2020), Lisciandrello (2020), Schwartz (2020), and Wolpert-Gawron (2017).

GUIDELINES Establishing Routines and Rules for Remote Learning In the first online session, give a tour of your virtual class environment. Examples 1. 2.

3. 4.

Demonstrate where online your students can find class meeting times, teacher office hours, assignments, and due dates. Determine the class norms and expectations for office hours, videoconferencing, and discussions (see next two guidelines for details). Post these online and show students where to find them. Record this virtual tour of the class, and post it so students can go back and review. Give an extra-credit quiz on the information provided so students will pay attention and take it seriously—watch the reviewing of your virtual tour take off! (Continued)

567

568 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Give students these tips about how to use videoconferencing effectively. Examples 1. 2.

3. 4.

5.

Begin every day by restarting your computer and turning off all notifications and alerts. Make sure your device is prepared for videoconferencing. Close any browser windows and unneeded applications to ensure your computer’s processing power is able to stream and receive video. Put your iPad/Chromebook on a table or flat surface—avoid moving your device around unless you are instructed to. Make sure you are using the built-In microphone (internal microphone) and built-in output (internal speakers); then mute your microphone BEFORE you join—this limits background noise and visual distractions for all participants. Your teacher may also take control of the microphone feature for the class. If you are having technical trouble, send a chat message to your teacher.

Establish student norms and expectations for office hours and videoconferencing: dos and don’ts. Examples 1. 2. 3.

4. 5. 6.

Do be on time and prepared. Have all the materials you will need. Do dress like you are in the classroom—no pajamas or T-shirts with offensive messages. Do set up your space for serious work. Remember, everyone can see your space and background. Turn off distractions such as TVs or music. Locate in a quiet area and/or use headphones to reduce background noise. Don’t chew gum or eat (except with accommodations, just like in-person school)—no crumbs or ketchup on your chin. Don’t send messages or have side conversations. This is still school, so no sharing of Minecraft strategies in private chats. Sit so you can be seen.

Establish norms and expectations for video discussions. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Don’t interrupt the speaker, but know how to request a turn. Use constructive criticism only. Be respectful and kind. Share the time—give others a chance to speak. Build on their ideas. Ask questions. Do not record the meeting—in some states that could be illegal.

Make special efforts to create community and build relationships, even though it is at a distance. Examples 1.

2.

3.

4. 5. 6.

Write a “Welcome Letter” to all your students before school starts, assuring them you will work together to navigate any new remote learning territory. See Figure 13.2 for the letter our daughter wrote to her fifth-grade students. Find ways for students to learn more about each other—share information about personal interests, activities, and concerns. Find commonalities in their life experiences. If everyone has Internet access, make a brief introductory video of yourself and have students do the same—post the videos in an “Our Class” folder. Create connections with small-group and breakout sessions, but don’t group randomly. Decide on criteria for forming groups—interests? Goals? Similar or different skills to contribute to group projects? Have students create a contract for small-group work—how will they treat each other, and how will they get the work done? Tell students they should use their real names—no changing their Zoom ID or Google profile to a nickname or anything but the name used in school. Create a spreadsheet with all the ways to contact each student and the student’s caregiver—email, phone, text.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

569

Be present and available for students. Examples 1. 2.

3. 4. 5.

Set up a virtual office and times for students to be in touch with questions and concerns. Some teachers use Google Hangout or Skype. Be wise about contacting students. Send messages to the whole class when everyone needs to know and to individual working groups just for their concerns, and occasionally email or call an individual student when a private message is needed (use this sparingly). Participate in discussion threads. If a discussion is dying down, share your thoughts. Take more time than usual to check in with students. How are they feeling and coping? Make boundaries on your time clear. When are you not available?

Teach about plagiarism. It is easy to cheat online. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Cite sources. Use your own words or indicate when you are quoting. Online plagiarism is easy to detect—let students know you are checking. Discuss examples of plagiarism.

Time management can be a big problem—address it directly. Examples 1. 2.

Establish deadlines and interim deadlines for assignments. Set a class calendar and automatic reminders if possible.

For more information, see https://www.edutopia.org/article/simple-ways-promote-studentvoice-classroom.

Welcome to fifth grade humanities! My name is Ms. Hoy and I wanted to take a moment and tell you how excited I am to have you in class. We will be starting the year virtually and I have many fun lessons planned for the beginning of the year and beyond. I am eager to hear about all the good books you have read and your summer antics with your family. We will take this distance learning journey one day at a time. I took the liberty of putting together a small humanities folder for each of you. No need to read anything or do anything in this folder yet—I will explain the activities and materials on the first day of class. Bring a pencil, spiral notebook, and any markers or colored pencils/crayons that you might use. In our distance learning schedule, you will have some humanities sessions on Zoom and these classes are referred to as synchronous. There are also days where you will be working independently and we refer to these classes as asynchronous (The little “a” at the beginning of synchronous changes the whole meaning of the word.) Do not fret—we will go over how to navigate your schedule. The schedule for the first partial week is a little different so that you can meet all of your teachers in the first three days. We will spend the first few weeks of school getting to know each other as people and as learners. Our discussions will center around identity and what it means to be an ally. We will begin writing stories together, playing games, and choosing new books. Until we meet in “Zoomland” enjoy your time with your family and get some rest for the first official day of upper school on September 2, 2020. Go fifth grade! The bitmoji to the left is me closing my eyes and imagining our time together. I look forward to seeing you soon. Ms. Hoy Source: Kelly L. Hoy, September 2020. Used with permission.

Figure 13.2 Welcome to Remote Learning Sending something similar to this informative and reassuring letter should ease the stress of your students as they face a new school situation (K. Hoy, 2020). Even without an unexpected switch to different pathways for teaching and learning, every new year is stressful for most students. Make sure they feel welcome, whether in person or online, as you address their major concerns first.

570 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Planning Spaces for Learning Stop & Think Think back over all the rooms in all the schools you have attended. Which ones stand out as inviting or exciting? Which ones were cold and empty? Did one teacher have a design that let students do different things in various parts of the room?

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Classroom Space (I, C4) The physical organization of a class has an effect on student behavior and learning. Describe how the physical layout of classrooms can affect the learning environment. Apply principles of classroom organization to enhance learning and minimize disruption.

Spaces for learning should invite and support the activities you plan for your classes, and they should respect the inhabitants of the space. This respect begins at the door for young children by helping them identify their classroom. One school that has won awards for its architecture paints each classroom door a different bright color so young children can find their “home” (Herbert, 1998). Once inside, spaces can be created that invite quiet reading, group collaboration, focused lectures, discussion and debate, or independent research. If students are to use materials, they should be able to reach them. Students with disabilities should feel welcome by being able to easily navigate in the space. Of course, teachers have to design spaces for health and safety today, too—so more space may be needed between students if social distancing is required. In terms of classroom arrangement, there are two basic ways of organizing space: personal territories and interest areas. A personal territory is your own (usually assigned) seat. Years of research indicate that a front seat location seems to increase participation and learning for students who are predisposed to speak in class, but a seat in the back will make it more difficult to participate and easier to sit back and daydream (Blume et al., 2019). To “spread the action around,” Romano and Weinstein (2019) suggest that teachers move around the room when possible, establish eye contact with and direct questions to students seated far away, and vary the seating so the same students are not always consigned to the back. Horizontal rows share many of the advantages of the traditional row and column arrangements. Both are useful for independent seatwork and teacher, student, or media presentations; they encourage students to focus on the presenter and simplify housekeeping. Horizontal rows also permit students to work more easily in pairs. However, this is a poor arrangement for large-group discussion. Clusters of four or circle arrangements are best for student interaction. Clusters permit students to talk, help one another, share materials, and work on group tasks. Circles are especially useful for discussions but still allow for independent seatwork. Both arrangements, however, are poor for whole-group presentations and may make class management more difficult. The fishbowl or stack special formation, where students sit close together near the focus of attention (the back row may even be standing), should be used only for short periods of time because it is not comfortable and can lead to discipline problems. On the other hand, the fishbowl can create a feeling of group cohesion and is helpful when the teacher wants students to watch a demonstration, brainstorm on a class problem, or see a small visual aid.

PERSONAL TERRITORIES AND SEATING ARRANGEMENTS.

INTEREST AREAS. The design of interest areas can influence the way the areas are used by

students. Years ago, for example, my friend Carol Weinstein (1977) worked with a teacher to make changes in interest areas that helped the teacher meet her objectives of having more girls involved in the science center and having all students experiment more with a variety of manipulative materials. In a second study, changes in a library corner led to more involvement in literature activities throughout the class (Morrow & Weinstein, 1986). Personal territories and interest areas are not mutually exclusive; many teachers use a design that combines these types of organization. Individual students’ desks—their territories—are placed in the center, with interest areas in the back or around the periphery of the room. This allows the flexibility needed for both large- and small-group activities. No matter how you arrange your classroom, keep the Guidelines: Designing Learning Spaces in mind.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

GUIDELINES Designing Learning Spaces Note the fixed features, and plan accordingly. Examples 1. 2.

Remember that the media center, computers, and charging stations for tablets need electrical outlets. Keep art supplies near the sink, small-group work by a whiteboard.

Create easy access to materials and a well-organized place to store them. Examples 1. 2.

Make sure materials are easy to reach and visible to students. Have enough shelves so that materials need not be stacked.

Provide students with clean, convenient surfaces for studying. Examples 1. 2.

Put bookshelves next to the reading area, games by the game table. Prevent fights by avoiding crowded workspaces.

Avoid dead spaces and “racetracks.” Examples 1. 2.

Don’t have all the interest areas around the outside of the room, leaving a large dead space in the middle. Avoid placing a few items of furniture right in the middle of this large space, creating a “racetrack” around the furniture.

Arrange things so you can see your students and they can see all instructional presentations. Examples 1. 2.

Make sure you can see over partitions. Design seating so that students can see instruction without moving their chairs or desks.

Make sure work areas are private and quiet. Examples 1. 2.

Make sure there are no tables or work areas in the middle of traffic lanes; a person should not have to pass through one area to get to another. Keep noisy activities as far as possible from quiet ones. Increase the feeling of privacy by placing partitions, such as bookcases or pegboards, between areas or within large areas.

Provide choices and flexibility. Examples 1. 2.

Establish private cubicles for individual work, open tables for group work, and cushions on the floor for whole-class meetings. Give students a place to keep their personal belongings. This is especially important if students don’t have personal desks.

Be sensitive to the symbolic communication of spatial features. Examples 1. 2. 3.

In a history class, in all the posters and pictures on the wall, are only white male historical characters displayed? Do students see their culture, language, and symbols represented in visuals and decorations? Are the shelves, workspaces, computers, and so on, easily accessible to students with disabilities? (Continued)

571

572 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Try new arrangements; then evaluate and improve. Examples 1. 2.

Have a “2-week arrangement”; then evaluate. Enlist the aid of your students. They have to spend many hours in the room too, and designing a classroom can be a very challenging educational experience.

For more ideas on classroom design, go to http:// www.edutopia.org and search for “classroom designs.”

Getting Started: The First Weeks of Class Determining a room design, rules, and procedures are the first steps toward having a well-organized class, but how do effective teachers gain students’ cooperation in those early critical days and weeks? One study carefully analyzed the first weeks’ activities of effective and ineffective elementary teachers, and found striking differences (Emmer & Evertson, 2017; Evertson & Emmer, 2017). In the effective teachers’ classrooms, the very first day was well organized. Nametags were ready. There was something interesting for each child to do right away. Materials were set up. The teachers had planned carefully to avoid any last-minute tasks that might take them away from their students. These teachers dealt with the children’s pressing concerns first. “Where do I put my things?” “How do I pronounce my teacher’s name?” “Can I whisper to my neighbor?” “Where is the bathroom?” The effective teachers were explicit about their expectations. They had a workable, easily understood set of rules and taught the students the most important rules right away. They taught the rules like any other subject—with lots of explanation, examples, and practice. Throughout the first weeks, the effective managers continued to spend quite a bit of time teaching rules and procedures. Some used guided practice to teach procedures; others used rewards to shape behavior. Most taught students to respond to a bell or some other signal to gain their attention. These teachers worked with the class as a whole on enjoyable academic activities. They did not rush to get students into small groups or to start them in readers. This whole-class work gave the teachers a better opportunity to continue monitoring all students’ learning of the rules and procedures. Misbehavior was stopped quickly and firmly but not harshly. In the poorly managed classrooms, the first weeks were quite different. Rules were not workable; they were either too vague or very complicated. For example, one teacher made a rule that students should “be in the right place at the right time.” Students were not told what this meant, so their behavior could not be guided by the rule. Neither positive nor negative behaviors had clear, consistent consequences. After students broke a rule, ineffective managers gave a vague criticism, such as “Some of my children are too noisy,” or issued a warning but did not follow through with the threatened consequence. In the poorly managed classes, procedures for accomplishing routine tasks varied from day to day and were never taught or practiced. Instead of dealing with these obvious needs, ineffective managers spent time on procedures that could have waited. For example, one teacher had the class practice for a fire drill the first day but left unexplained other procedures that would be needed every day. Students wandered around the classroom aimlessly and had to ask each other what they should be doing. Often the students talked to one another because they had nothing productive to do. Ineffective teachers frequently left the room. Many became absorbed in paperwork or in helping just one student. They had not made plans for how to deal with late-arriving students or interruptions. One ineffective manager tried to teach students to respond to a bell as a signal for attention but later let the students ignore it. All in all, the first weeks in these classrooms were disorganized and filled with surprises for teachers and students alike.

EFFECTIVE MANAGERS FOR ELEMENTARY STUDENTS.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

What about getting started in a secondary school class? It appears that many of the differences between effective and ineffective elementary school teachers are the same at the secondary level. Again, effective managers focus on establishing rules, procedures, and expectations on the first day of class. These standards for academic work and class behavior are clearly communicated to students and consistently enforced during the first weeks of class. Student behavior is closely monitored, and infractions of the rules are dealt with quickly. In classes with less prepared students, work cycles are shorter; students are not required to spend long, unbroken periods on one type of activity. Instead, during each period, they are moved smoothly through several different tasks. In general, effective teachers carefully follow each student’s progress, so students cannot avoid work without facing consequences (Emmer & Evertson, 2017). With all this close monitoring and consistent enforcement of the rules, you may wonder if effective secondary teachers have to be grim and humorless. Not necessarily. As any experienced teacher can tell you, there is much more to smile about when the class is cooperative. For more ideas about getting started on the first day of class, see the helpful book by Harry and Rosemary Wong, The First Days of School: How to Be an Effective Teacher (5th ed., 2018).

EFFECTIVE MANAGERS FOR SECONDARY STUDENTS.

Module 40 Summary The What and Why of Supportive Classroom Organization (pp. 558–562) What are the challenges of classroom management? As you learn to teach, you should be aware of your philosophy of classroom management. Are you more teacher centered and structured or more student centered and humanistic? Classrooms are challenging because they are multidimensional, full of simultaneous activities, fast-paced and immediate, unpredictable, public, and affected by the history of students’ and teachers’ actions. A teacher must juggle all these elements every day. Productive classroom activity requires students’ cooperation. Maintaining cooperation is different for each age group. Young students are learning how to “go to school” and need to learn the general procedures of school. Older students need to learn the specifics required for working in different subjects. Working with adolescents requires teachers to understand the power of the adolescent peer group. What are the goals of effective classroom organization and management? The goals of effective classroom organization and management are to make ample time for learning; improve the quality of time used by keeping students actively engaged; make sure participation structures are clear, straightforward, and consistently signaled; develop positive relationships with students; and encourage student self-management, self-control, and responsibility.

Creating a Positive Learning Environment (pp. 562–573) Distinguish between rules and procedures. Rules are the specific dos and don’ts of classroom life. They usually are written and posted. Procedures cover administrative tasks, student movement, housekeeping, and routines for accomplishing lessons, interactions between students and teachers, and interactions among students. Rules can be written in terms of rights, and students may benefit from participating in establishing these rules. Consequences should be established for following and breaking the rules and procedures so that the teacher and the students know what will happen. Rules, procedures, and consequences must fit the situation—in-person versus remote learning. Distinguish between personal territories and interest-area spatial arrangements. There are two basic kinds of spatial organization: territorial (the traditional classroom arrangement) and functional (dividing space into interest or work areas). Flexibility is often the key. Access to materials, convenience, privacy when needed, ease of supervision, symbolic messages communicated, and a willingness to reevaluate plans are important considerations in the teacher’s choice of physical arrangements.

573

574 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments Contrast the first school week of effective and ineffective classroom managers. Effective classroom managers spend the first days of class teaching a workable, easily understood set of rules and procedures by using lots of explanation, examples, and practice. Students were occupied with organized, enjoyable activities, and they learned to function cooperatively in the group. Quick, firm, clear, and consistent responses to infractions of the rules characterized effective teachers. The teachers had planned carefully to avoid any last-minute tasks that might have taken them away from their students. These teachers dealt with the children’s pressing concerns first. In contrast, for ineffective managers, procedures for accomplishing routine tasks varied from day to day and were never taught or practiced. Students talked to one another because they had nothing productive to do. Ineffective teachers frequently left the room. Many became absorbed in paperwork or in helping just one student. They had not made plans for how to deal with typical problems such as late-arriving students or interruptions.

MODULE 41

What Now? Maintaining a Positive Context for Learning

Learning Objective 13.3 Discuss how to maintain a positive learning environment by encouraging student engagement, preventing problems, and developing caring, respectful relationships with your students.

Maintaining a Good Environment for Learning A good start is just that—a beginning. Effective teachers build on this beginning. They maintain their management system by preventing problems and keeping students engaged in productive learning activities. We have discussed several ways to accomplish these goals. In Cluster 12, on motivation, for example, we considered stimulating curiosity, relating lessons to student interests, establishing learning goals instead of performance goals, and having positive expectations. What else can teachers do?

Encouraging Engagement Stop & Think What activities keep you completely engaged—the time just seems to disappear? What is it about those activities that keeps you focused?

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Promoting Student Engagement (I, C4) A principle of educational psychology is that the more students are cognitively engaged in an activity, the more they are likely to learn. What tactics can teachers employ to maximize their students’ cognitive engagement during learning tasks?

In general, as teacher supervision increases, students’ engaged time also increases. Obviously, this has implications for remote learning when students are entirely on their own. This does not mean that teachers should eliminate independent work for students. It simply means that this type of activity usually requires careful planning and monitoring. Activities with clear steps are likely to be more absorbing because one step leads naturally to the next. When students have all the materials they need to complete a task, they tend to stay involved. And, as you now know, students will be more engaged if they are involved in authentic tasks—activities that have connections to real life. Also, activities are more engaging when the level of challenge is higher (Emmer & Gerwels, 2006). Of course, teachers can’t supervise every student all the time or rely on challenge to keep students motivated. In their study of elementary and secondary teachers, Evertson, Emmer, and their colleagues found that effective class managers at all levels had well-planned systems for encouraging students to manage their own work (Emmer & Evertson, 2017; Evertson & Emmer, 2017). The Guidelines: Keeping Students Engaged on the next page are based on their findings.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

GUIDELINES Keeping Students Engaged Make basic work requirements clear. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Specify and post the routine work requirements for headings, paper size, pen or pencil use, and neatness. Establish and explain rules about late or incomplete work and absences. If a pattern of incomplete work begins to develop, deal with it early; speak with parents if necessary. Make due dates reasonable, and stick to them unless the student has a very good excuse for lateness.

Communicate the specifics of assignments. Examples 1.

2. 3.

4.

With younger students, have a routine procedure for giving assignments, such as writing them on the board in the same place each day. With older students, assignments may be dictated, posted online, or given in a syllabus. Remind students of upcoming assignments. With complicated assignments, give students a sheet describing what to do, what resources are available, due dates, and so on. Older students should also be told your grading criteria. Demonstrate how to do the assignment, do the first few questions together, or provide a sample worksheet.

Monitor work in progress. Examples 1.

2.

When you give an assignment in class, make sure each student gets started correctly. If you check only students who raise their hands for help, you will miss those who think they know what to do but don’t really understand, those who are too shy to ask for help, and those who don’t plan to do the work at all. Check progress periodically. In discussions, make sure everyone has a chance to respond.

Give frequent academic feedback. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Elementary students should get papers back the day after they are handed in. Good work can be displayed in the classroom (but make sure it is not from the same students over and over), and graded papers sent home to parents each week. Students of all ages can keep records of grades, projects completed, and extra credits earned. For older students, break up long-term assignments into several phases, giving feedback at each point.

For more ideas, go to http://www.edutopia.org and search for “keeping students engaged.”

Prevention Is the Best Medicine The ideal way to manage problems, of course, is to prevent them in the first place—this certainly will make more time for learning. In a classic study, Jacob Kounin (1970) examined classroom management by comparing effective teachers, whose classes were relatively free of problems, with ineffective teachers, whose classes were continually plagued by chaos and disruption. Observing both groups in action, Kounin was surprised to find the teachers were NOT very different in the way they handled discipline once problems arose. The difference was that the successful managers were much better at preventing problems. First, these teachers captured students’ attention and maintained engagement

575

576 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments in work that was at appropriate levels for all students. Second, these effective classroom managers were especially skilled in four specific kinds of prevention: “withitness,” overlapping activities, group focusing, and movement management. More recent research confirms that expert classroom managers focus on prevention (Jones & Jones, 2016; Stahnke & Blömeke, 2021). Withitness According to Kounin, awareness of everything happening in a classroom.

Overlapping Supervising several activities at once.

Group focus The ability to keep as many students as possible involved in activities.

Movement management Keeping lessons and the group moving at an appropriate (and flexible) pace, with smooth transitions and variety.

Withitness means communicating to students that you are aware of everything that is happening in the classroom. “With-it” teachers seem to have eyes in the back of their heads. They avoid becoming absorbed by distractions or interacting with only a few students because this encourages the rest of the class to wander. These teachers are always scanning the room, making eye contact with individual students, so the students know they are being monitored (Charles & Cole, 2019; Romano & Weinstein, 2019). These teachers prevent minor disruptions from becoming major. They also know who instigated the problem, and they make sure they deal with the right people. In other words, they don’t make what Kounin called timing errors (waiting too long before intervening) or target errors (blaming the wrong student and letting the real perpetrators escape responsibility for their behavior). If two problems occur at the same time, effective managers deal with the more serious one first. For example, a teacher who tells two students to stop whispering but ignores even a brief shoving match by the bookshelves communicates a lack of awareness. Students begin to believe they can get away with almost anything if they are clever. WITHITNESS.

Overlapping means keeping track of and supervising several activities at the same time. For example, a teacher may have to check the work of an individual and at the same time keep a small group working by saying, “Right, go on,” and stop an incident in another group with a quick “look” or reminder (Charles, 2011; Charles & Cole, 2019). Maintaining a group focus means keeping as many students as possible involved in appropriate class activities and avoiding narrowing in on just one or two students. Problems arise when teachers have students work one at a time while the rest of the class waits and watches. All students should have something to do during a lesson. For example, the teacher might ask everyone to write the answer to a question, and then call on individuals to respond while the other students compare their answers. Choral responses might be required while the teacher moves around the room to make sure everyone is participating. Group focus can be especially challenging during remote learning. How will you ensure that everyone has something productive to do, even as you interact with one student or student group? OVERLAPPING AND GROUP FOCUS.

Movement management means keeping lessons and the group moving at an appropriate (and flexible) pace, with smooth transitions and variety. The effective teacher avoids abrupt transitions, such as announcing a new activity before gaining the students’ attention or starting a new activity in the middle of something else. When transitions are abrupt, one-third of the class will be doing the new activity, many will be working on the old lesson, several will be asking other students what to do, some will be taking the opportunity to have a little fun, and most will be confused. Another transition problem Kounin noted is the slowdown, or taking too much time to start a new activity. Sometimes teachers give too many directions at once. Clear, well-timed directions are especially important for remote teaching because it is difficult to “read” student confusion online. MOVEMENT MANAGEMENT.

But what about the students? What can they do? When students lack social and emotional skills such as being able to share materials, read the intentions of others, or handle frustration, classroom problems often follow. So all efforts to teach social and emotional self-regulation are steps toward preventing management problems. Over the short term, educators can teach and model these skills and then

STUDENT SOCIAL SKILLS AS PREVENTION.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

give students feedback and practice using them in a variety of settings. Over the long term, teachers can help change attitudes that value aggression over cooperation and compromise (M. J. Elias & Schwab, 2006). Figure 13.3 is a lesson outline that can be used to target and improve specific social skills for an individual or small group. You can see that this is an example of good teaching. There is a clear goal defined by specific and observable targets, modeling, practice with the teacher and then (very important) practice in the natural setting like the gym or lunchroom where the problem usually occurs, and finally some kind of recognition or reinforcement (Jones & Jones, 2016).

Figure 13.3 A Social Skills Lesson Design This design can be used with individuals or groups to identify and practice targeted social skills. Describe the inappropriate behavior ______________________________________ _____________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ Rationale for a new behavior ____________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ Describe the appropriate behavior ________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ Skill components of the new behavior ______________________________________ 1. __________________________________________________________________ 2. __________________________________________________________________ 3. __________________________________________________________________ 4. __________________________________________________________________ 5. __________________________________________________________________ 6. __________________________________________________________________ Model demonstration example __________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ Student practice example ______________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ Natural setting practice (if different than initial practice) ____________________ ____________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ Independent practice assignment ________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ Individual or group reinforcement strategy ________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ Source: Jones, V. & Jones, L. Comprehensive classroom management: Creating communities of support and solving problems, Update, Loose-Leaf Version, 11th ed., p. 372. ©2016. Reprinted and Electronically Reproduced by Permission of Pearson Education, Inc., New York, NY.

577

578 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Caring Relationships: Connections with School All efforts directed toward building positive relationships with students and creating a classroom community are steps toward preventing management problems. Results of many studies confirm that the affective quality of teacher–student relationships, that is, whether teachers and students feel close or in conflict, is important for students’ classroom engagement, academic achievement, and social-emotional development (Meyer & L. Anderman, in press). One powerful example is a study that followed 1,364 students from kindergarten through ninth grade. The students who experienced positive relationships with their teacher for the first 6 years in school had higher GPAs, enrolled in more advanced science classes, and set higher educational aspirations. They also had stronger social skills as well as higher math and English self-concepts. As the researchers noted, “. . . even though teachers are transient figures in students’ lives, the strength and influence of these relationships can (and do) have lasting consequences” (Ansari et al., 2020, p. 9). In a study of engagement in math (Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2015), students reported working harder, enjoying math more, and sharing ideas and materials with each other more when teachers were warm, caring, and responsive to student needs. The students also reported greater cognitive, social, and emotional engagement in math learning when the teachers made learning goals clear, used proactive/preventative management strategies, and orchestrated smooth transitions (more evidence for Kounin’s management strategies). Positive teacher relationships are particularly important at the transition to middle school for students who face academic risks (Hughes & Cao, 2018). In a study that followed over 1,200 students from fifth and sixth grades, Stefania Sette and her colleagues (2020) found that students who were more liked by their teacher were more likely to be included by their peers and this inclusion was associated with greater success in school. Finally, in a study of 58 high schools, when adults actively connected with students through informal greetings, questions, and conversations outside classrooms in cafeterias, hallways, and stairwells, there were fewer negative student behaviors (Cash et al., 2019). What else supports strong relationships? Students respect teachers who maintain their authority without being rigid or harsh, are fair and honest with them, demonstrate emotional support and caring, make sure students understand the material, ask if something is wrong when they seem upset, and use creative instructional practices to “make learning fun.” Students also value teachers who show academic and personal caring by acting like real people (not just as teachers), sharing responsibility, minimizing the use of external controls, including everyone, searching for students’ strengths, communicating effectively, and showing an interest in their students’ lives and pursuits. Finally, students like and respect teachers who show respect for their cultural identity (Meyer & L. Anderman, in press; Turner et al., 2014; Wentzel, 2002; Woolfolk Hoy & Weinstein, 2006). How can you develop positive relationships with your students? The EMR model, (which stands for Establish-Maintain-Restore), is a research-based possibility. When middle school teachers were trained to use EMR, their students were less disruptive and more engaged in academic tasks. The approach was especially helpful for students who had poor relationships with their teachers before the EMR intervention (Duong et al., 2019). In fact, when Laurie Kincade and her colleagues (2020) analyzed almost 800 studies that evaluated programs to improve teacher–student relationships, EMR was one of the two programs with the largest effects. Here are the highlights of the model:

TEACHER CONNECTIONS.

EMR model A research-based strategy for developing positive relationships with students. EMR stands for Establish-Maintain-Restore.

• Establish—the goal is to encourage student belonging, trust, and connections by “banking time,” that is, by having individual warm interactions with students that are validating. The interactions are person to person, not academic or directive. Find some common interests. Learn about the student as a person.

• Maintain—once established, you have to work to protect the positive relationship by striving for a 5-to-1 ratio of positive-to-negative interactions. If you find yourself criticizing or correcting more than about one time in five interactions, strive to be more positive.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

• Restore—it is difficult to avoid conflicts or misunderstandings. When they occur, it is your responsibility as a teacher to make things right again by separating the deed from the doer, working to solve the problem together (more about this later in the cluster), validating the student’s feelings, not holding a grudge, and generally being the adult in the situation. There is more to the EMR approach than the preceding bullets. Go to http://www. edutopia.org and search for “Establish-Maintain-Restore.” Students who believe “their teachers are caring, empathic, and fair and help resolve personal problems” are more likely to feel a greater sense of belonging (our next topic) than those students who perceive a negative relationship with their teachers (Allen et al., 2018, p. 25). Stop & Think Think back to your middle school days. On a 4-point scale from 1 (Disagree) to 4 (Agree), how would you have rated these three items? 1. The adults at my school like me as much as they like other students. 2. There is an adult at my school who cares about me. 3. I enjoy going to school. How do you want your future students to answer these questions? SCHOOL CONNECTIONS AND BELONGING. The three questions in the Stop & Think are from the Milwaukee Youth Belonginess Scale, developed to measure students’ sense of connection to their family, peers, and school. In the Stop & Think, you rated the items that focused on connections to school (Slaten et al., 2019). Students who feel connected with school are happier, more engaged in schoolwork, less likely to be absent, more likely to graduate, and more self-disciplined; have higher self-efficacy; and are less likely to be involved in dangerous behaviors such as substance abuse, violence, and early sexual activity (Allen et al., 2018; Prati et al., 2018; Waters & Cross, 2010). If students perceive their schools are competitive places where they are treated differently based on race, gender, or ethnicity, then they are more likely to act out or withdraw altogether. But when they feel that they have choices, that the emphasis is on personal improvement and not comparisons, and that they are respected and supported by teachers, students are more likely to bond with schools (Osterman, 2000; Schachner et al., 2019). One way of supporting belonging in school is by connecting with students’ families and home lives (Gage et al., 2016). For example, students in China describe their teachers as high on caring. This may be because Chinese teachers spend quite a bit of time in students’ homes, learning about their home life, and offering help outside school. These teachers show respect for the families and cultures of their students by their willingness to visit and to help (Jia et al., 2009; Suldo et al., 2009). CREATING COMMUNITIES OF CARE FOR ADOLESCENTS. The transition to high school is a particularly important time to maintain caring teacher–student relationships. Students have more teachers and fewer close relationships with teachers, just at a time when emotional, social, and academic stresses are increasing. Feeling a sense of belonging is important for all students, but particularly for students who may, because of language, race, gender, or poverty, feel disconnected from the basically White middle-class culture of most schools (Gray et al., 2018). In one study that followed 572 students from ninth grade through high school, Cari Gillen-O’Neel and Andrew Fuligni (2013) found that girls’ sense of belonging in school was higher than boys’ in ninth grade, but over the high school years, this connection declined for girls but not for boys. One possible reason is that boys participate more often than girls in extracurricular activities such as sports; these activities connect them to the school. So encouraging girls to participate in school activities, including sports, may build a sense of belonging for them. In addition, having positive relationships with students appears to support teachers’ sense of

579

580 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments well-being. When we remember the basic human need for relatedness—the feeling that others care about you described in Cluster 12—we can understand why caring relationships in school would support both students’ sense of belonging and teachers’ well-being (Spilt et al., 2011). Check & Connect, developed at the University of Minnesota, is one program that has been successful in developing caring relationships with disengaged, marginalized adolescents, although it can be used with students of all ages. A goal of Check & Connect is “to foster school completion with academic and social competence” (http://checkandconnect .umn.edu). The program works by developing a relationship between a trained mentor and students. The mentor checks on the students, monitoring their attendance, behavior, and grades, and connects with students by offering individualized interventions in partnership with other school staff, families, and community resources. Results of research on Check & Connect show that the program improves student engagement and achievement (Anderson et al., 2004; Skiba et al., 2016). When Randall Sampson, one of my former graduate students, became the assistant principal of a large, diverse high school in Columbus, Ohio, he initiated something similar to Check & Connect. He personally monitored those students who had completed basic math classes with grades good enough to move to advanced placement classes but who had not signed up for the classes. He contacted the students’ parents, talked to the students, and signed them up for the advanced classes. Then he checked in on them every week to be sure they were engaged in class. His interventions were very successful, leading to a 600% increase in Black students enrolled in AP classes and a 35% increase in AP exam scores. His initiatives kept some potentially disengaged students on track for college admission when the students and their families had not considered college a possibility. Randall founded Liberty Leadership Development (http://www.LibertyLD.com) to bring these kinds of positive interventions to more schools such as Wilson Preparatory Academy in North Carolina (http://www.wilsonpreparatoryacademy.org). But beware. Just having a mentor will not necessarily be helpful for a student unless the relationship is strong, as Randall’s was with the students he supported. Having a poor-quality relationship with a mentor can actually be detrimental (Lyons & McQuillin, 2019). See the Guidelines: Creating Caring Relationships for more ideas, many taken from Jones and Jones (2016) and M. Marshall (2013).

GUIDELINES Creating Caring Relationships Get to know students as individuals. Examples 1. 2.

3. 4.

5.

Eat lunch with a different group of students every day; work with a club, extracurricular activity, or sports group; attend student activities. When learning is remote, make sure every student has contact with at least one adult from the school each day. Enlist teachers, librarians, reading specialists, counselors, and teacher’s aides. If students do not have Internet, call on the phone. Show your interest in your students as individuals. Schedule individual conferences with students. If you are teaching remotely, have virtual office hours and make sure you meet with the students who seem to disappear or don’t seek your help. As we noted earlier, if you are starting the year with remote learning and your students have video access, do an introductory video of yourself and ask students to do the same—have students pronounce their names so all can learn.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Communicate your respect for students’ abilities. Examples 1.

2. 3. 4.

“Model respect for diversity—by expressing admiration for a student’s bilingual ability, by commenting enthusiastically about the number of different languages that are represented in the class, and by including examples and content from a variety of cultures” (C. S. Weinstein et al., 2003, p. 272). Comment to students privately and positively about your observations of their performances in extracurricular activities. Encourage students to use personal interests as a subject for their writing. Greet students at the door to the class every day.

Keep communications authentic but professional. Examples 1.

2. 3.

4. 5.

Send brief personal notes to students acknowledging a good job performed on assignments, their hard work and persistence, a birthday, or concern about absences. Include a get-well card along with homework sent to a student who is ill. Share some stories from your own life as examples of excitement about a subject, making mistakes (and learning from them), and persistence and overcoming difficulties. Do not friend students on social media, and be very careful about your language and picture postings in all electronic communications—many things can be misinterpreted. Create school-related email accounts that are different from your personal accounts. Check with your school policy about sharing personal information such as religion, sexual orientation, or political views. If you are meeting alone with a student, do so in an area visible to others—sadly, teachers today must protect against having their positive relationships with students misinterpreted by others or by the student.

Seek student input and respect it, but don’t take it too personally. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Consider a suggestion box or community meetings for younger students. Listen to student concerns and complaints without getting defensive. Ask for suggestions, but also share your rationales for assignments and grades. Ask students directly for anonymous feedback about whether they feel respected and cared for in your class. Use simple questionnaires that don’t reveal the identity of the student through handwriting. Consider asking students to rate the three items found in the previous Stop & Think exercise.

Module 41 Summary Maintaining a Good Environment for Learning (pp. 574–581) How can teachers encourage engagement? In general, as teacher supervision increases, students’ engaged time also increases. When the task provides continuous cues for the student about what to do next, involvement will be greater. Activities with clear steps are likely to be more absorbing because one step leads naturally to the next. Making work requirements clear and specific, providing needed materials, and monitoring activities all add to engagement. Explain the factors identified by Kounin that prevent management problems in the classroom. To create a positive environment and prevent problems, teachers must take individual differences into account, maintain student motivation, and reinforce positive behavior. Successful problem preventers are skilled in four areas described by Kounin: “withitness,” overlapping, group focusing, and movement management. When penalties have to be imposed, teachers should impose them calmly and privately. In addition to applying Kounin’s ideas, teachers can prevent problems by establishing a caring classroom community and teaching students to use social skills and emotional self-regulation skills.

581

582 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments How do teachers help students form connections with schools, and why are these connections important? The affective quality of teacher–student relationships, that is, whether teachers and students feel close or in conflict, affects students’ classroom engagement, academic achievement, and social-emotional development. To get started on building connections, teachers should make expectations for both academic work and student behaviors clear. Respect for students’ needs and rights should be at the center of class procedures. Students know that their teachers care about them when teachers try to make classes interesting, are fair and honest with them, make sure they understand the materials, and have ways to cope with students’ concerns and troubles. The EMR model (Establish-Maintain-Restore) and Check & Connect are two examples of successful programs.

MODULE 42

Positive Response to Problems, Communication, and Connection

Learning Objective 13.4 Identify strategies (especially penalties and alternatives, restorative justice, and student self-discipline) for preventing and addressing student misbehaviors, including bullying. Learning Objective 13.5 Characterize successful teacher–student and student–student communication through such approaches as empathetic listening, I-messages, and problem solving. Learning Objective 13.6 Explain the need for and approaches to culturally relevant classroom management.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Student Misbehavior (I, C4) Even the most well-managed classroom will have instances of student misbehavior. Explain the principles for dealing with common student misbehaviors. What strategies can teachers employ to deal fairly and effectively with those problems?

Dealing with Discipline Problems Before we even discuss dealing with discipline problems, remember that every school has policies and procedures for handling behavior problems, especially more serious issues. Make sure you know all these procedures and requirements before you develop your management plan. Also keep in mind that being an effective manager does not mean publicly correcting every minor infraction of the rules. This kind of public attention may actually reinforce the misbehavior, as we saw in Cluster 7. The key is being aware of what is happening and knowing what is important so you can prevent problems.

Stopping Problems Quickly It is critical that you have many effective ways to de-escalate rather than escalate student behavior problems. As problems escalate, students may need to save face in front of their peers by challenging or defying the teacher. This can lead to excluding students from class, sending them to detention, or suspension—all outcomes that place students, especially students of color, at greater risk of negative or dangerous outcomes such as disengaging from school or getting caught up in the juvenile justice system and the school-to-prison pipeline (Hwang, 2018; Skiba et al., 2016). It may help you to be more effective at de-escalating problems if you approach students’ social/behavioral problems more like you would academic problems—as an opportunity to learn a better way. For example, generally when students make an academic mistake we assume that they were trying to get it right and did not make the mistake on purpose. They just need smaller steps, clearer explanations, better examples, models, or practice. But when the problem is behavioral, we often assume the student is not trying or that the error was intentional. We follow with criticisms, punishments, or exclusion from the class. Successful teachers tend to see their role in improving behavior as interpersonal and instructional— helping their students learn and practice better decisions and actions. Your attributions

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

about the problem—whether it was a mistake or intentional—matter (Jones & Jones, 2016; Stahnke & Blömeke, 2021). Most students comply quickly when the teacher redirects behavior or gives a desist (a “stop doing that”) instruction. But some students are the targets of more than their share of desists. Emmer and Evertson (2017) and Levin and Nolan (2000) suggest eight simple ways to stop misbehavior quickly, moving from least to most intrusive:

• Make eye contact with or move closer to the offender. Other nonverbal signals, such as pointing to the work students are supposed to be doing, might be helpful. Make sure the student actually stops the inappropriate behavior and gets back to work. If you do not, students will learn to ignore your signals.

• Try verbal hints such as “name-dropping” (simply insert the student’s name into the lecture), asking the student a question, or making a humorous (not sarcastic) comment such as “I must be hallucinating. I haven’t called on anyone, and yet I am hearing voices!”

• Ask students if they are aware of the negative effects of their actions, or send an I-message, described later in the cluster.

• Involve the whole class in a brief interactive activity such as “Think-Pair-Share.” Especially if more than one student is “drifting,” this activity can reengage the drifters.

• If they are not performing a class procedure correctly, remind the students of the procedure, and have them follow it correctly. You may need to quietly collect a toy, comb, cell phone, or note that is competing with the learning activities while privately informing the students that their possessions will be returned after class. You also could put a sticky note on the student’s desk inviting the student to talk with you after the lesson is over.

• In a calm, unhostile way, ask the student to state the correct rule or procedure and then to follow it. Glasser (1969) proposes three questions: “What are you doing? Is it against the rules? What should you be doing?”

• Tell the student in a clear, assertive, and unhostile way to stop the misbehavior. (Later in the cluster, we will discuss assertive messages to students in more detail.) If students “talk back,” simply and calmly repeat your statement.

• Offer a choice. For example, when a student continued to call out answers no matter what the teacher tried, the teacher said, “John, you have a choice. Stop calling out answers immediately and begin raising your hand to answer or move your seat to the back of the room and you and I will have a private discussion later. You decide” (Levin & Nolan, 2000, p. 177).

If You Impose Penalties Many teachers prefer the use of logical consequences, described earlier, as opposed to penalties. For example, if one student has harmed another, you can require the offending student to make an “apology of action,” which includes a verbal apology plus somehow repairing the damage done. This helps offenders develop empathy and social perspective taking as they think about what would be an appropriate “repair” (M. J. Elias & Schwab, 2006). If you must impose penalties, again, start with the least intrusive and move on only when you have to. In their case studies of four expert elementary school teachers, Romano and Weinstein (2019) found that the teachers’ penalties fell into seven categories, as shown in Table 13.2.

583

584 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Table 13.2 Seven Categories of Penalties for Students 1. Expressions of disappointment. If students like and respect their teacher, then a serious, sorrowful expression of disappointment may cause students to stop and think about their behavior. 2. Loss of privileges. Students can lose free time. If they have not completed homework, for example, they can be required to do it during a free period or recess. 3. Time-out: Exclusion from the group. Students who distract their peers or fail to cooperate can be separated from the group until they are ready to cooperate. Some teachers give a student a pass for 10 to 15 minutes. The student must go to another class or study hall, where the other students and teachers ignore the offending student for that time. 4. Written reflections on the problem. Students can write in journals, write essays about what they did and how it affected others, or write letters of apology—if this is appropriate. Another possibility is to ask students to describe objectively what they did; then the teacher and the student can sign and date this statement. These records are available if parents or administrators need evidence of the students’ behavior. 5. Visits to the principal’s office. Expert teachers tend to use this penalty rarely, but they do use it when the situation warrants. Some schools require students to be sent to the office for certain offenses, such as fighting. If you tell a student to go to the office and the student refuses, you might call the office saying the student has been sent. Then the student has the choice of either going to the office or facing the principal’s penalty for “disappearing” on the way. 6. Detentions. Detentions can be very brief meetings after school, during a free period, or at lunch. The main purpose is to talk about what has happened. 7. Contacting parents. If problems become a repeated pattern, most teachers contact the student’s family. This is done to seek support for helping the student, not to blame the parents or punish the student. Source: From Elementary classroom management (7th ed.), pp. 300–301, by M. E. Romano and C. S. Weinstein. McGraw-Hill. Copyright © 2019 by The McGraw-Hill Companies. Adapted with permission of The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.

There is a caution about penalties. As Carolyn Orange (2005) notes, “Effective, caring teachers would not use low achievement status, grades, or the like as a means of discipline. This strategy is unfair and ineffective. It only serves to alienate the student” (p. 76). If you must impose penalties, the Guidelines: Imposing Penalties give ideas about how to do it. Some of these examples are based on ideas from expert teachers described by Weinstein and Novodvorsky (2015) and Romano and Weinstein (2019).

GUIDELINES Imposing Penalties Delay the discussion of the situation until you and the students involved are calmer and more objective. Examples 1. 2.

Say calmly to a student, “Sit there and think about what happened. I’ll talk to you in a few minutes” or “I don’t like what I just saw. Talk to me during your free period today.” Say, “I’m really angry about what just happened. Everybody take out journals; we are going to write about this.” After a few minutes of writing, the class can discuss the incident.

Impose penalties privately. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Make arrangements with students privately. Stand firm in enforcing arrangements. Resist the temptation to “remind” students in public that they are not keeping their side of the bargain. Move close to a student who must be disciplined and speak so that only the student can hear.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

After imposing a penalty, reestablish a positive relationship with the student immediately. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Send the student on an errand, or ask him or her for help. Compliment the student’s work, or give a symbolic “pat on the back” when the student’s behavior warrants. Look hard for such an opportunity. For 2 minutes each day for 10 days in a row, have a personal conversation with the student about something of interest to him or her—sports, games, films—make an effort to know what those interests are. This investment in time can pay off by regaining learning time for the student and the entire class.

Set up a graded list of penalties that will fit many occasions. Example 1.

For not turning in homework: (a) receive reminder; (b) receive warning; (c) hand homework in before close of school day; (d) stay after school to finish work; (e) participate in a teacher–student–parent conference to develop an action plan.

Always teach problem-solving strategies along with penalties to help students learn what to do next time (M. J. Elias & Schwab, 2006). Examples 1.

2.

Use Problem Diaries, where students record what they were feeling, identify the problem and their goal, and then think of other possible ways to solve the problem and achieve the goal. Try Keep Calm 5–2–5: At the first physical signs of anger, students say to themselves, “Stop. Keep Calm,” and then take several slow breaths, counting to 5 breathing in, 2 holding breath, and 5 breathing out.

What About School Suspensions and Zero Tolerance? There is quite a bit of discussion today about school suspensions and zero-tolerance penalties for rule breaking in the schools. Is this a good idea? The Point/Counterpoint: Is Zero Tolerance a Good Idea? looks at both sides.

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Is Zero Tolerance a Good Idea? With the very visible violence in schools today, some districts have instituted “zero-tolerance” policies for rule breaking. This usually means expelling or suspending students the first time they break a rule. It also can mean punishing minor rule infractions every time, hoping to deter major infractions in the future. Do zero-tolerance policies make sense?

Point Zero tolerance is necessary for now. The arguments for zero tolerance focus on school safety, fairness, preparation for the future, and the responsibilities of schools and teachers to protect the students and themselves. Educators argue that zero-tolerance policies set clear expectations and allow for fast responses to misbehavior. These approaches keep students safer and create a less chaotic learning environment. Zero tolerance for all reduces favoritism because everyone must be treated the same way, but as we will see later, this does not always happen. Also, advocates of zero tolerance claim that these policies prepare students for the real world where, for example, police don’t care why you were speeding or your boss may not care why you were late (Morin, 2020; Sasser, 2017). Of course, many of the incidents reported in the news seem like overreactions to childhood pranks or, worse, to (Continued)

585

586 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

innocent mistakes or lapses of memory. But how do school officials separate the innocent from the dangerous? For example, it has been widely reported that Andy Williams (the boy who killed two classmates in Santee, California) assured his friends before the shootings that he was only joking about “pulling a Columbine.” Schools also claim that having zero-tolerance policies has cut down on the number of drugs in schools because having exceptions can lead to having no clear rules at all. Finally, parents are in favor of these policies (Sasser, 2017).

Counterpoint Zero tolerance makes zero common sense. An Internet search using the keywords “zero tolerance” and “schools” will locate a wealth of information about the policy—much of it against. For example, what sense does this story make? The newspaper St. Petersburg Times described a 10-year-old girl whose mother packed a plastic knife in the girl’s lunchbox so she could cut her apple. The girl dutifully handed the plastic knife over to her teacher and was expelled from school for possession of a weapon. In another case, a teenage boy was also expelled. He broke the “no cell phone use” rule for talking to his mother, a soldier stationed in Iraq. He had not spoken to her for a month (Hyder & Hussain, 2015). Even though supporters of zero tolerance say that these policies create fairness because punishments must be consistent, often athletes and good students are forgiven and Black adolescents are punished more often and more harshly for even minor infractions (Amemiya et al., 2020; Curwin, 2015). In addition, punishment and zero-tolerance policies have not been very successful in preventing major infractions including bullying, even though they are used often (Amemiya et al., 2020; Rigby, 2012; Welsh & Little, 2018). The American Psychological Association’s Zero Tolerance Task Force (2008) reached the following conclusions: • Schools are not any safer or more effective in disciplining students now than before they instituted zero tolerance. • The higher rates of suspension caused by zero tolerance have not led to less racial bias in disciplining students. • Zero-tolerance policies can actually lead to increases in bad behavior that then lead to higher dropout rates. In addition, zero-tolerance policies can discourage students from informing teachers when the students learn that a classmate is “planning to do something dangerous.” The zero-tolerance rules get in the way of trusting relationships between teachers and students (Syvertsen et al., 2009).

Beware of Either/Or Surely we can ask adults to use good judgment in applying rules in dangerous situations but to not feel trapped by the rules when student actions are not intended to harm and are not dangerous. Whole-school policies that are less punitive and more preventative and proactive and that improve the climate of the school are good alternatives to zero tolerance. Restorative justice is one option.

Reaching Every Student: Restorative Justice Restorative justice A system of dealing with student misconduct that focuses on building, nurturing, and repairing relationships while giving a voice to victims, offenders, and the community.

Many schools around the world and in the United States and Canada are seeking alternatives to punitive, rigid zero-tolerance systems. One possibility is restorative justice—an approach that teaches lifelong lessons. In the school where you teach, you might encounter a program called restorative interventions, restorative practices, restorative measures, restorative approaches, restorative discipline, or restorative justice—look for “restorative” (Gonzalez et al., 2018). In restorative justice, a problem behavior is harmful not because it “breaks a rule” but rather because the negative action adversely affects members of the classroom and school

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

community (Fronius et al., 2016). Restorative justice focuses on building, nurturing, and repairing relationships while giving a voice to victims, offenders, and the community. The goal is to reduce reliance on traditional punishments and police involvement, repair the harm to victims, but still hold misbehaving students accountable while supporting them and reintegrating them into the community. Vincent Anfara Jr. and his colleagues (2013) describe seven principles that guide restorative justice:

Meeting needs: As in functional behavioral assessment (Cluster 7), the assumption is that behaviors, even negative behaviors, have goals. Students are trying to meet some need, even if their way of reaching the goal or meeting the need is a problem. See Table 13.4 later in this cluster for some reasons for bullying. Responsibility and accountability with support and compassion: Students take responsibility for the harm they caused in a supportive community. Making things right: Harm is defined not as “breaking a rule” but as hurting members of the community. The appropriate response is to make things right. Conflict is a learning opportunity: Everyone in the school community can learn to handle conflict more productively. Students become better problem solvers and teachers learn better ways of relating to their students. Building healthy communities: Beyond responding to misbehavior, restorative justice builds respectful communities where relationships are strengthened. Restoring relationships: Even in the strongest communities, conflict happens. Rather than punish, the goal is to repair and restore. Addressing power imbalances: Restorative justice requires schools to examine whether their practices are causing harm to students and perpetuating injustice. There are different ways to implement restorative justice at the school or classroom levels, including victim–offender mediation, conferencing to resolve conflict, or peacemaking circles (Parker & Bickmore, 2020; Zehr et al., 2015). For example, in a victim–offender mediation, the participants might meet with a facilitator (often the teacher) and sometimes members of their family. The victim and the offender express their views and describe their experiences, with monitoring by the facilitator to keep discussions productive. When the process works well, the participants express remorse for hurting each other, forgive the perceived transgressions, and reconcile their conflict. The outcome of restorative justice usually is an agreement that includes how to reestablish cooperation and participation in the classroom community—perhaps an apology of action, restitution, and a plan for dealing positively with possible future conflicts. See Christen Dillon’s description in the Casebook responses on page 603–604. Restorative justice emphasizes student responsibility, as does the next approach, Discipline Without Stress—a goal for every teacher!

Teacher-Imposed Penalties Versus Student Responsibility Marvin Marshall (2013) believes that even though classroom management is the responsibility of the teacher, discipline is really the responsibility of the student. Classroom management is about how things are done in the classroom and involves procedures, routines, and structures—the teachers’ responsibility. Discipline is about how people behave and involves self-control and emotional self-regulation—the student’s responsibility. Students must discipline themselves to be self-regulated learners and ultimately productive, successful, happy adults. A focus on obedience and teacher-imposed penalties often results in resistance, resentment, cheating, and even defiance, but a focus on responsibility creates a classroom community and a culture of learning. Of course, anyone who has worked with children or adolescents knows that self-discipline is not automatic—it must be taught and practiced like any other skill. Marshall describes strategies for achieving these goals that focus on (a) communicating in positive terms and using “when–then” contingencies (“When you finish your work, then

587

588 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments you can listen to music on your phone”); (b) offering choices and eliciting consequences from students (“What shall we do about . . .?”); and (c) encouraging reflection and selfevaluation. One approach that incorporates these three principles is to teach students a hierarchy of behaviors using explanations and examples. The hierarchy is:

• Level A: Anarchy—Aimless, chaotic. • Level B: Bossing/Bullying—Breaking laws and making your own standards; obeying only when the enforcer has more power or authority.

• Level C: Cooperation/Conformity—Complies with expectations, conforms to peer influence.

• Level D: Democracy—Self-disciplined, initiative, responsibility for your own actions. The behaviors at levels C and D may look the same on the outside, but the difference is the motivation. For example, if a student picks up a piece of trash on the floor because the teacher asks (external motivation), the level is C (cooperation), but if the student picks up the trash without being asked (internal motivation), the level is D (democracy and self-discipline). When a student does not act at least at level C or D, the teacher asks, “What level was that behavior?” If the student does not move to at least level C and cooperate, the teacher might ask him or her to self-reflect and write an essay that addresses three questions: What did I do? What can I do to prevent it from happening again? What will I do now? Figure 13.4 summarizes Marshall’s model. His book Discipline Without Stress ® Punishments or Rewards: How Teachers and Parents Promote Responsibility & Learning (M. Marshall, 2013) has many other strategies to encourage student self-discipline.

Bullying and Cyberbullying Bullying is not just aggression or fighting. Bullying is intentional and repeated harmful behavior toward a victim. The goal is to achieve social dominance through intense, systematic abuse of physical or social power (Kaufman, et al., 2020). Cyberbullying adds the possibility to attack anonymously, so being more physically or socially powerful may be less of a factor. Also, with cyberbullying, a vast public audience can be involved, whereas other forms of bullying often have fewer witnesses. The line between good-natured exchanges and hostile teasing may seem thin, but a rule of thumb is that teasing someone who is less powerful or less popular or using any racial, ethnic, impairment, or religious slur should not be tolerated. Bullying can begin as early as preschool, generally peaks during middle school, and then declines a bit by the end of high school—which means bullying could be a problem in any grade you teach. Both bullies and victims are at risk for long-term academic, psychological, and behavioral problems, including drug use (Hymel & Swearer, 2015; Patton et al., 2013; Thomas et al., 2015; Ttofi et al., 2016). There are many ways to be a bully, as you can see in Table 13.3 on page 590. For students in grades 4 through 12, about 31% report being physically bullied, 51% verbally bullied, 37% socially bullied, and 12% cyberbullied, but some studies have found as many as 80% of students report being bullied in some way. Rates of physical bullying appear to be going down, but rates of cyberbullying are going up (Graham, 2016; Hymel & Swearer, 2015). VICTIMS. Studies from both Europe and the United States indicate that about 12% of boys

and 6% of girls are chronic victims of bullying—the constant targets of physical or verbal attacks from ages 8 to 16, the longest time period measured so far (Hymel & Swearer, 2015). One kind of victim tends to have low self-esteem and to feel anxious, lonely, insecure, and unhappy. They often are prone to crying and withdrawal; in general, when attacked, they won’t defend themselves. These victims may believe that they are rejected because they have flaws that they cannot change or control—no wonder they are depressed and helpless! In addition, students who are different from the larger peer group are more likely to be bullied, including students who are obese or small, unpopular, or members of ethnic or

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Figure 13.4 The Discipline Without Stress® Teaching Model Here are the key concepts in Marshall’s (2013) model.

I

CLASSROOM MANAGEMENT vs. DISCIPLINE The key to effective classroom management is teaching and practicing procedures. This is the teacher’s responsibility. Discipline, on the other hand, has to do with behavior and is the student’s responsibility.

II

THREE PRINCIPLES TO PRACTICE POSITIVITY

CHOICE

REFLECTION

Teachers practice changing negatives into positives. “No running” becomes “We walk in the hallways.” “Stop talking” becomes “This is quiet time.”

Choice response thinking is taught—as well as impulse control—so students are not victims of their own impulses.

Since a person can only control another person temporarily and because no one can actually change another person, asking REFLECTIVE questions is the most effective approach to actuate change in others.

III

THE RAISE RESPONSIBILITY SYSTEM (RRSystem) TEACHING THE HIERARCHY (Teaching)

CHECKING FOR UNDERSTANDING (Asking)

GUIDED CHOICES (Eliciting)

The hierarchy engenders a desire to behave responsibly and a desire to put forth effort to learn. Students differentiate between internal and external motivation—and learn to rise above inappropriate peer influence.

Students reflect on the LEVEL of chosen behavior. This approach SEPARATES THE PERSON FROM THE BEHAVIOR, thereby negating the usual tendency to defend one’s actions. It is this natural tendency towards self-defense that leads to confrontations.

If disruptions continue, a consequence or procedure is ELICITED to redirect the inappropriate behavior. This approach is in contrast to the usual coercive approach of having a consequence IMPOSED.

IV

USING THE SYSTEM TO INCREASE MOTIVATION & LEARNING Using the hierarchy BEFORE a lesson or activity and AFTER a lesson or activity increases motivation, improves learning, and raises academic achievement.

Source: Reprinted with permission from Marshall, M. (2013). Discipline Without Stress® punishments or rewards: How teachers and parents promote responsibility & learning (2nd ed.). Piper Press.

language minorities, as well as students who are gifted, have disabilities, or are lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, and questioning (LGBTQ). Unfortunately, many victims go on to become bullies themselves, creating a continuing supply of victimizers and victims (Graham, 2016; J. S. Hong & Garbarino, 2012; Walters & Espelage, 2018). Prolonged bullying has negative effects on the academic achievement, social relationships, and mental health of the victims. Children who have been chronic victims through

589

590 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Table 13.3 What Does Bullying Look and Sound Like? TYPE OF BULLYING

DESCRIPTION

BEHAVIORS THAT COUNT AS BULLYING

Physical Bullying

Any unwanted physical contact in which one participant exerts power or force over another

Hitting, pinching, punching, kicking, shoving; withholding/stealing/destroying property

Verbal Bullying

Any comment considered offensive or threatening to the victim

Hurtful teasing, name-calling, criticizing, humiliating, taunting, threatening, making derogatory comments about any aspect of the individual (see also identity and sexual bullying in this table)

Social/Relational Bullying

Intentional manipulation of people’s social lives, friendships, or reputation

Leaving people out on purpose, spreading rumors, convincing others not to be friends with someone, damaging friendships or reputations, setting someone up to look foolish

Cyberbullying

Harm by posting on Discord, Yubo, Snapchat, Twitter, Instagram, Tumblr, Monkey, WhatsApp, Houseparty, Kik, Amino, Whisper, YouNow, Reddit, BroadcastMe, Chat, Burn Note, Yik Yak, MeetMe, GroupMe, TikTok, Live.me, Omegle, email

Spreading rumors, ghosting, subtweeting, griefing, creating fake accounts in someone else’s name, hate speech; inflicting all verbal bullying possibilities listed above and sending to many individuals 24/7; texting embarrassing/compromising pictures or videos; see http://www.commonsensemedia.org and search for “cyberbullying” for more information

Identity Bullying

Leaving people out or treating them badly because of their racial or ethnic background, religion, or disability

Calling racist names; telling jokes that mock race, religion or disability; making negative comments about race, religion, religious beliefs, or disability; taunting; intentionally making someone feel uncomfortable

Sexual Bullying

Leaving individuals out, treating them badly, or making them feel uncomfortable because of their sex

Making sexist comments or jokes, touching, pinching, grabbing in a sexual way; making crude comments about someone’s sexual behavior or orientation; spreading sexual rumors

Source: Based on http://www.prevnet.ca/bullying/types.

elementary and middle school are more depressed and more likely to attempt suicide as young adults. Signs that students, like the boy in the opening case, are being bullied include anxiety, depression, headaches, and stomachaches as well as frequent tardiness and absence from school. In fact, about 160,000 children avoid school every day and thousands more drop out of school altogether because they are always afraid (Bradshaw et al., 2013; Holland et al., 2019; Juvonen & Schacter, 2020). Students who were victims are more likely than bullies to kill or injure others in schools (Reinke & Herman, 2002a, 2002b). In the past years, we have seen tragic consequences when bullied students turned guns on their tormentors in schools in the United States and in Europe. Having friends can help students cope with bullying. Also, being in an ethnically diverse school can lessen feelings of vulnerability, perhaps because power is divided and shared among the groups (Graham, 2016). Ken Rigby (2012) examined the research and concluded that students bully for four main reasons. Rigby suggests that to effectively combat bullying, schools and teachers need to address the underlying motivation, not just the bullying behavior. See Table 13.4 for the reasons and possible actions. WHY DO STUDENTS BULLY?

WHAT CAN TEACHERS DO? BULLYING AND TEASING. A longitudinal study that followed a representative sample of first- through sixth-grade students for 2 years found that aggressive children whose teachers taught them conflict management strategies were moved away from a life path of aggression and violence (Aber et al., 2003). But when teachers are silent about aggression and teasing, students may “hear” the teacher’s agreement with the insult (C. S. Weinstein & Novodvorsky, 2015). So what should you do? Respond immediately when you see an incident—don’t ignore it. Students often think teachers won’t help and so don’t bother to report bullying.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Table 13.4 Reasons for Bullying and Possible School Responses REASONS FOR BULLYING

POSSIBLE SCHOOL AND TEACHER ACTIONS

Bullies feel annoyed or insulted or have some grievance against the victim, so they feel justified in lashing out. There may or may not be a reasonable basis for the grievance felt.

Help students read the intentions of others more accurately. Use role-plays, readings, and drama to develop the capacity to “walk in someone else’s shoes.” Try conflict resolution or peer mediation.

They simply enjoy putting the victim under pressure, especially if bystanders seem to find the whole situation “fun.” The bullies claim it is innocent—“no big deal.”

Stress with students that it is not fun unless the target of the aggression is genuinely laughing, too. Develop empathy through literature activities and class community building such as circle time and shared concerns; focus on what bystanders can and should do to stop bullying.

The bully believes the aggression against the victim will gain or maintain acceptance for himself or herself in a valued group. The goals are power and social dominance.

In lessons and in relations with students, emphasize making moral judgments, thinking for yourself, and resisting conformity to group pressures. Also, sensitive discussion of prejudice and homophobia can help students resist pressures from groups to harm others based on their race, ethnicity, gender identity, or language.

The bully wants something from the victim and is willing to inflict harm to get it, and/or the bully is basically sadistic—hurting other people feels good.

Restorative justice practices and community conferences may help the bullies feel genuine remorse. For older students, if the acts are criminal, there are legal sanctions.

But if teachers take an active role and if students believe their teachers will separate those involved in bullying or contact parents and the principal, they are more likely to report bullying. Check your biases and be active. Bullying of all kinds is not “harmless” or “just part of growing up.” Use the incident as a teachable moment to talk about difficult topics and discuss why bystanders often do nothing. Finally, model an appreciation of differences and diversity (Cortes & Kochenderfer-Ladd, 2014; Graham, 2016). Also, consequences for the bullies should be clear, be stated in advance, escalate if the bullying continues, include a reflection on the situation (talking to the teacher or writing in a journal), and be meant not to humiliate the bully but rather to teach better ways of dealing with the reasons for bullying listed in Table 13.4 (Ansary et al., 2015). Remember functional behavioral assessment from Cluster 7. What does the student get out of the bullying behavior, and what is a better way to achieve that goal? Finally, in classrooms where there are positive teacher–student relationships, students show more sympathy for the victims of bullying—another good reason to establish strong emotional bonds with your students (Waasdorp et al., 2019). One classroom approach that has proved helpful in decreasing bullying is prevention through cooperative learning. Because people tend to seek friends who share their attitudes and behavior, bullies tend to hang together and reinforce each other’s actions. Also, victims often are socially isolated students with few friends. Cooperative activities that create positive interdependence to reach mutual goals can support forming new friendships and including all students. When Mark Van Ryzin and Cary Roseth (2018) used cooperative learning activities in a randomized experimental/control group design with 1,460 seventh-grade students, they found significantly less bullying and victimization among students who participated in cooperative groups. Some well-researched school-wide bullying prevention programs have been successful. For example, the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program had good results in a large-scale study following 120 schools (grades 3–11) over 2 years (Limber et al., 2018). Unfortunately, however, the results are mixed on the effectiveness of many school-wide bullying prevention programs. Administrators often prefer to adopt anti-bullying programs that they heard about from colleagues rather than determine if there was real scientific evidence that the

591

592 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments programs work—and many don’t (Ansary et al., 2015; Graham, 2016; Swearer et al., 2010). And even in schools that reduce bullying, if a few students are still bullied, those victims may feel worse because they believe the school is safe so it must be their fault—something is wrong with them. They become more isolated than ever and may need extra support or counseling to realize that the bullies are seeking power—it is the bullies’ fault (Juvonen & Schacter, 2020). One general approach that can be effective is involving parents. Teachers and schools can reach out to parents to work together on preventing and responding to incidents of bullying. Parents should know about school policies and how they can support school anti-bullying efforts. For this to work, parents and schools need to trust each other so they can communicate effectively (Lindstrom Johnson et al., 2019). About 95% of adolescents have access to a smartphone and thus a way to bully or be bullied. Up to 70% of adolescents experience cyberbullying over the course of a year (Morin et al., 2018; Yang et al., 2020). For example, when 16-year-old Denise broke up with her boyfriend, he sought revenge by posting her email address and cell phone number on websites and blogs devoted to sex. For months, she got embarrassing and frightening phone calls and messages (Strom & Strom, 2005). Now bullies have new ways to torment victims using email, cell phones, text messaging, all the social media sites listed in Table 13.3, web blogs, and online voting booths. (More possibilities have probably appeared since I wrote this paragraph.) This kind of bullying is difficult to combat because the perpetrators can hide but damage can be long term. Table 13.5 has some ideas for dealing with cyberbullying.

CYBERBULLYING.

Table 13.5 Ideas for Dealing with Cyberbullying • Develop an explicit policy for acceptable in-school use of the Internet, and include it in the school handbook (or your class rules). The policy should spell out what constitutes cyberbullying and list consequences. • Make sure that children and young people are aware that bullying will be dealt with seriously. • Ensure that parents/guardians who express cyberbullying concerns are taken seriously. • Explain to students that they – Should never share or give out personal information, PINs, passwords, phone numbers, and so on. – Should not delete messages; they do not have to read them, but they should show them to an adult they trust. Messages can be used to take action against cyberbullies. – Should not open a message from someone they don’t know. – Should never reply to the message or a weblink embedded in a message. – Probably can block the sender’s message if they are being bullied through cell phones, email, Facebook, Twitter, Snapchat, WhatsApp, instant messaging, and so on. – Can forward the messages to their Internet service provider. – Should tell an adult. – Should show the message to the police if it contains physical threats. – Should speak out against cyberbullying. – Should never send messages when they are angry. – Should never send messages they wouldn’t want others to see. • Focus some class projects on cyberbullying. For example, students in one school posted on a “Wall of Shame” cruel comments that others in the school had posted on Facebook (without identifying information). The same could be done for Twitter or other social network sites. • Make parents aware of the fact that all of the major Internet service providers offer some form of parental controls. • Encourage parents to keep computers in a public room in the house. • Invite members of the local police department to come to school to speak with parents and students about proper Internet use. • Make sure ethics are included in any computer instruction given at your school. Source: From Middle and secondary classroom management (5th ed.), by C. S. Weinstein and I. Novodvorsky. McGraw-Hill. Copyright © 2015 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, p. 182. Adapted with permission of the McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Special Challenges with High School Students Quite a few secondary students never complete their schoolwork. Because students at this age have many assignments and teachers have many students, both teachers and students may lose track of what has and has not been turned in. It often helps to teach students how to use a daily planner—paper or electronic. In addition, teachers must keep accurate records. The most important thing is to enforce the established consequences for incomplete work. Do not pass a student because you know he or she is “bright enough” to pass. Make it clear to these students that the choice is theirs: They can do the work and pass, or they can refuse to do the work and face the consequences. You might also ask, in a private moment, if there is anything interfering with the student’s ability to get the work done. There is also the challenge of students who continually break the same rules, always forgetting materials, for example, or getting into fights. What should you do? Seat these students away from others who might be influenced by them. If rules are broken, be consistent in applying established consequences. Do not accept promises that they will do better next time. Teach the students how to monitor their own behavior; some of the self-regulation techniques described in Cluster 11 should be helpful. Finally, remain friendly with the students. Try to catch them in a good moment so you can talk to them about something other than their rule breaking. A defiant, hostile student can pose serious problems. If there is an outburst, try to get out of the situation as soon as possible; everyone loses in a public power struggle. One possibility is to give the student a chance to save face and cool down by saying, “It’s your choice to cooperate or not. You can take a minute to think about it.” If the student complies, the two of you can talk later about controlling the outbursts. If the student refuses to cooperate, you can tell him or her to wait in the hall until you get the class started on work and then step outside for a private talk. If the student refuses to leave, send another class member for the assistant principal. Again, follow through. If the student complies before help arrives, do not let him or her off the hook. If outbursts occur frequently, you might have a conference with the counselor, family members, or other teachers. If the problem is an irreconcilable clash of personalities, the student should be transferred to another teacher. It sometimes is useful to keep records of these incidents by logging the student’s name, words and actions, date, time, place, and teacher’s response. These records may help identify patterns and can prove useful in meetings with administrators, families, or special services personnel (Burden, 1995). Some teachers have students sign each entry to verify the incidents. Fighting or destruction of property is a difficult and potentially dangerous problem. The first step is to send for help and get the names of participants and witnesses. Then remove any students who may have gathered to watch; an audience will only make things worse. Do not try to break up a fight without help. Make sure the school office is aware of the incident; usually the school has a policy for dealing with these situations. What else can you do? The Guidelines: Handling Potentially Explosive Situations are based on Weinstein and Novodvorsky (2015).

GUIDELINES Handling Potentially Explosive Situations Move slowly and deliberately toward the problem situation. Examples 1. 2.

Walk slowly; then be as still as possible. Establish eye-level position. (Continued)

593

594 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Be respectful. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Keep a reasonable distance. Do not crowd the student. Do not get “in the student’s face.” Speak respectfully. Use the student’s name. Avoid pointing or gesturing.

Be brief. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Avoid long-winded statements or nagging. Stay with the agenda. Stay focused on the problem at hand. Do not get sidetracked. Deal with less severe problems later.

Avoid power struggles. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Speak privately if possible; don’t threaten. Do not get drawn into “I won’t, you will” arguments. Don’t make threats or raise your voice.

Inform the student of the expected behavior and the negative consequence as a choice or decision for the student to make. Then withdraw from the student and allow some time for the student to decide. Examples 1.

2.

“Michael, you need to return to your desk, or I will have to send for the principal. You have a few seconds to decide.” The teacher then moves away, perhaps attending to other students. If Michael does not choose the appropriate behavior, deliver the negative consequences. (“You are choosing to have me call the principal.”) Follow through with the consequence.

Source: Based on material in Middle and secondary classroom management: Lessons from research and practice (5th ed.), by C. S. Weinstein & I. Novodvorsky. Published by McGraw-Hill. Copyright © 2015 by McGraw-Hill. Adapted with permission from the McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.

The Need for Communication Put Yourself in Their Place A student says to you, “That book you assigned is really dumb—I’m not reading it!” What do you say? Communication between teacher and students is essential when problems arise. Communication is more than “teacher talks—student listens.” It is more than the words exchanged between individuals. We communicate in many ways. Our actions, movements, voice tone, facial expressions, and many other nonverbal behaviors send messages to our students. Many times, the messages we intend to send are not the messages our students receive (Jones & Jones, 2016).

Message Sent—Message Received Teacher: Jacob, where is your homework? Jacob: I left it in my dad’s car this morning. Teacher: Again? You will have to bring me a note tomorrow from your father saying that you actually did the homework. No grade without the note. Message Jacob receives: I can’t trust you. I need proof you did the work. ***

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

595

Teacher: Sit at every other desk. Put all your things under your desk. Hannah and Gabriela, you are sitting too close together. One of you move! Message Hannah and Gabriela receive: I expect you two to cheat on this test. *** A new student comes to Ms. Lincoln’s kindergarten. The child is messy and unwashed. Ms. Lincoln puts her hand lightly on the girl’s shoulder and speaks: Ms. Lincoln: I’m glad you are here. (Her muscles tense, and she leans away from the child.) Message student receives: I don’t like you. I think you are bad. A student may respond with hostility if she or he feels insulted by the teacher but may not be able to say exactly where the feeling of being insulted came from. Perhaps it was in the teacher’s tone of voice or lack of eye contact. But the teacher senses the student’s hostility and feels attacked for no reason. The first principle of communication is that people respond to what they think was said or meant, not necessarily to the speaker’s intended message or actual words. Students in my classes have told me about one instructor who encourages accurate communication by using the paraphrase rule. Before any participant, including the teacher, is allowed to respond to any other participant in a class discussion, he or she must summarize what the previous speaker said. If the summary is wrong, indicating the speaker was misunderstood, the speaker must explain again. The respondent then tries again to paraphrase. The process continues until the speaker agrees that the listener has heard the intended message. Paraphrasing is more than a classroom exercise. It can be the first step in communicating with students. Before teachers can deal appropriately with any student problem, they must know what the real problem is. A student who says, “This book is really dumb! Why did we have to read it?” may really be saying, “The book was too difficult for me. I couldn’t read it, and I feel dumb.”

Paraphrase rule Policy whereby listeners must accurately summarize what a speaker has said before being allowed to respond.

Empathetic Listening Let’s keep going with the situation in which the student found the reading assignment “dumb.” How might a teacher handle this positively? Student: This book is really dumb! Why did we have to read it? Teacher: You’re pretty upset. This seemed like a worthless assignment to you. [Teacher paraphrases the student’s statement, trying to hear the emotions as well as the words.] Student: Yeah! Well, I guess it was worthless. I mean, I don’t know if it was. I couldn’t exactly read it. Teacher: It was a struggle to read, and that bothers you. Student: Sure, I felt really dumb. I know I can write a good report, but not with a book this tough. Teacher: I think I can give you some hints that will make the book easier to understand. Can you see me after school today?

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Teacher–Student Communication (III, A) A well-managed classroom requires a bidirectional line of communication between the teacher and students. Describe the various communication styles that teachers employ when interacting with students, and explain how those styles affect student behavior.

Student: OK. Here the teacher used empathetic listening to allow the student to find a solution. (As you can see, this approach relies heavily on paraphrasing.) By trying to hear the student and by avoiding the tendency to jump in too quickly with advice, solutions, criticisms, reprimands, or interrogations, the teacher keeps the communication lines open. Here are a few unhelpful responses the teacher might have made:

• I chose the book because it is the best example of the author’s style in our library. (The teacher justifies the choice; this prevents the student from admitting that this “important” assignment is too difficult.)

Empathetic listening Hearing the intent and emotions behind what another says and reflecting them back by paraphrasing.

596 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments • Did you really read it? I bet you didn’t do the work. (The teacher accuses; the student hears, “The teacher doesn’t trust me!” and must either defend herself or himself or accept the teacher’s view.)

• Your job is to read the book, not ask me why. I know what’s best. (The teacher pulls rank, and the student hears, “You can’t possibly decide what is good for you!” The student can rebel or passively accept the teacher’s judgment.) Empathetic, active listening is more than a parroting of the student’s words; it should capture the emotions, intent, and meaning for the student behind the words. When you are just getting started, try this simple formula for active listening: “You feel . . . because . . .” (Nemec et al., 2017). This may seem awkward at first, but when students realize they really have been heard and not evaluated negatively for what they have said or felt, they begin to trust the teacher and to talk more openly. Sometimes the true problem surfaces later in the conversation. Empathetic, active listening has proved useful beyond teaching in counseling, business management, parenting, friendships, and many other situations where trusting relationships are important (Jonsdottir & Fridriksdottir, 2020).

When Listening Is Not Enough: I-Messages, Assertive Discipline, and Problem Solving Let’s assume a student is doing something that actively interferes with teaching. The teacher decides the student must stop. Confrontation, not listening, is required. Jones and Jones (2016) describe an approach first developed by Thomas Gordon—sending an I-message to intervene and change a student’s behavior. Basically, this means telling a student in a straightforward, assertive, and nonjudgmental way what she or he is doing, how it affects you as a teacher, and how you feel about it. The student is then free to change voluntarily and often does so. Here are two I-messages:

I-MESSAGES.

I-message Clear, nonaccusatory statement of how something is affecting you.

If you leave your book bags in the aisles, I might trip and hurt myself. When you all call out, I can’t concentrate on each answer, and I’m frustrated. Learning to use I-messages can help your students handle conflict more productively, too. For example, Esme Codell, an excellent first-year teacher, taught her fifth-graders a simple four-step process and posted the steps on a bulletin board: “1. Tell person what you didn’t like. 2. Tell person how it made you feel. 3. Tell person what you want in the future. 4. Person responds with what they can do. Congratulations! You are a Confident Conflict Conqueror!” (Codell, 2001, p. 23). Lee and Marlene Canter (1992; Canter, 1996) suggest other approaches called assertive discipline when student behaviors have to be changed. Many teachers are ineffective with students because they are either wishy-washy and passive or hostile and aggressive (Charles & Cole, 2019). Instead of telling the student directly what to do, teachers use a passive response style— they ask the student to try or to think about the appropriate action. The passive teacher might comment on the problem behavior without actually telling the child what to do differently: “Why are you doing that? Don’t you know the rules?” or “Sam, are you disturbing the class?” Or teachers may give the students “one more chance” every time. Finally, teachers may ignore behavior that should receive a response, or they may wait too long before responding. A hostile response style involves different mistakes. Teachers may make “you” statements that condemn the student without stating clearly what the student should be doing: “You should be ashamed of the way you’re behaving!” or “You never listen!” or “You are acting like a baby!” Teachers may also threaten students angrily but follow through too seldom, perhaps because the threats are too vague—“You’ll be very sorry you did that when I get

ASSERTIVE DISCIPLINE.

Assertive discipline Clear, firm, nonhostile response style.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

through with you!”—or too severe. For example, a teacher tells a student in a physical education class that he will have to “sit on the bench for 3 weeks.” A few days later, the team is short one member, and the teacher lets the student play, never returning him to the bench to complete the 3-week sentence. Often a teacher who has been passive becomes hostile and explodes when students persist in misbehaving. In contrast with both the passive and hostile styles, an assertive response communicates to the students that you care too much about them and the process of learning to allow inappropriate behavior to persist. Assertive teachers clearly state what they expect. To be most effective, the teachers often look into a student’s eyes when speaking and address the student by name. Assertive teachers’ voices are calm, firm, and confident. They are not sidetracked by accusations such as “You just don’t understand!” or “You don’t like me!” Assertive teachers do not get into a debate about the fairness of the rules. They expect changes, not promises or apologies. More recent versions of assertive discipline focus on teaching students how to behave responsibly and working to establish mutual respect and trust (Charles & Cole, 2019). CONFRONTATIONS AND NEGOTIATIONS. If I-messages or assertive responses fail and a student persists in misbehaving, teacher and student are in a conflict. Several pitfalls now loom. The two individuals become less able to perceive each other’s behavior accurately. Research has shown that the angrier you are with another person, the more you see the other as the villain and yourself as an innocent victim. Because you feel the other person is in the wrong and he or she feels just as strongly that the conflict is all your fault, very little mutual trust is possible. A cooperative solution to the problem is almost impossible. In fact, by the time the discussion has gone on a few minutes, the original problem can be lost in a sea of charges, countercharges, and self-defense. There are three methods of resolving a conflict between a teacher and a student. One is for the teacher to impose a solution. This may be necessary during an emergency, as when a defiant student refuses to go to the hall to discuss a public outburst, but it is not a good solution for most conflicts. The second method is for the teacher to give in to the student’s demands. You might be convinced by a particularly compelling student argument, but again, this should be used sparingly. Problems arise when either the teacher or the student gives in completely. There is a third possibility called problem solving that allows both teacher and student to have input into a solution. Many versions of problem-solving strategies can work. Jones and Jones (2016) suggest one that is grounded in positive relationships between the teacher and the students and also encourages student responsibility and accountability. Figure 13.5 on the next page shows the basic steps. Many of the conflicts in classrooms can be important learning experiences for all concerned.

Diversity: Culturally Responsive Management In response to concerns about increased violence in urban schools and the ongoing crisis of school shootings everywhere, many districts adopted harsh punishments and zero-tolerance policies—immediate suspension or expulsion for even minor misconduct. We discussed the problems with zero tolerance earlier in this cluster. One major concern is that these harsh codes are enforced in ways that reinforce racial inequality. Schools with higher concentrations of Black and poor students tend to have harsher discipline policies than schools with more White, Asian, or middle- and upper-income students. Black and Latino/a Americans, especially boys, are punished more often and more harshly than other students. Black students are two to three times more likely to get an office referral or be suspended compared with White students, and Pacific Islander students are more likely to be disciplined than other Asian American or White students. These students lose time from learning as they

597

598 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Figure 13.5 A Problem-Solving Model Here is a general set of steps to guide problem solving about student behaviors.

Step 1:

Establish a warm, personal relationship with the student. (Develop a “positive relationship bank account” with the student.)

Step 2:

Deal with the present behavior. “What happened?” (Develop a time line/functional assessment.) “What did you do?” (Help students take responsibility for their role in the problem. Help them develop an internal locus of control.)

Step 3:

Make a value judgment. “Is it helping you?” (Help students consider their own behavior and underlying assumptions.) “Is it helping others?” (Enhance student’s social cognition.) “Is it against a rule/does it violate a compelling state interest?” (Help students understand their own and others’ rights and responsibilities within the community.)

Step 4:

Work out a plan. (Social skills training) “What do you need me to do?” (Empowerment /functional assessment) “What do you need other students to do?” (Empowerment /functional assessment)

Step 5:

Make a commitment. “Are you going to do this?” (Enhance student’s accountability/responsibility.)

Step 6:

Follow up. “I’ll check later and see how the plan has worked.” (Supportive/caring environment)

Step 7:

No put-downs, but do not accept excuses. “If the plan didn’t work, let’s analyze why and develop a new plan.” (High expectations and persistence in working with students)

Source: Jones, Vern; Jones, Louise, Comprehensive classroom management: Creating communities of support and solving problems, Update, Loose-Leaf Version, 11th ed., p. 324. ©2017. Reprinted and Electronically Reproduced by Permission of Pearson Education, Inc.

spend more hours in detention or suspension (Fronius et al., 2016; Gregory et al., 2017; IspaLanda, 2018; Larson et al., 2019; Nguyen et al., 2019). Why? The notion that Black and Latino/a students are punished more because they commit more serious offenses is not supported by the data (Huang, 2020). Instead, these students are punished more severely for minor offenses such as rudeness or defiance—words and actions that are interpreted by teachers as meriting severe punishment. One explanation is a lack of cultural synchronization between teachers and students and possible implicit

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

(unconscious) biases of teachers and administrators (Rudd, 2014). Black students may be disciplined for behaviors that were never intended to be disruptive or disrespectful. Unfortunately, when administrators learn about racial discrepancies in their schools, they seldom make changes in disciplinary actions (McIntosh et al., 2020). Culturally responsive classroom management has five dimensions: (a) understanding and addressing your own beliefs, biases, values, and stereotypes that are grounded in your own ethnic culture; (b) developing a knowledge of your students’ cultural backgrounds; (c) understanding the broader social, economic, and political context of classroom teaching—schools often reflect and reinforce the discriminatory practices of the larger society; (d) an ability and willingness to use culturally appropriate management strategies; and (e) a commitment to building caring classrooms (Sikba et al., 2016; Weinstein et al., 2004). High-quality teacher–student relationships are central to culturally responsive classroom management. Positive behavioral supports (PBS—see Demetrick Tensley’s Casebook response on page 000) that emphasize reinforcement for positive behaviors have been successful, too, which makes sense because PBS focuses on positive, not negative, behavior and should encourage good teacher–student relationships (Long et al., 2019). Teachers of color are more likely to use culturally responsive strategies, so if you are not a member of this group, you may have to work at learning and applying these strategies. Specifically, you can work on developing ways of incorporating students’ perspectives into your teaching and management (Gaias et al., 2019). Culturally responsive classroom management is simply a part of the larger concept of culturally relevant teaching. Geneva Gay (2006) sums it up:

599

Culturally responsive classroom management Taking cultural meanings and styles into account when developing management plans and responding to students.

If the classroom is a comfortable, caring, embracing, affirming, engaging, and facilitative place for students then discipline is not likely to be much of an issue. It follows then that both classroom management and school achievement can be improved for students from different ethnic, racial, social, and linguistic backgrounds by ensuring that curriculum and instruction are culturally relevant and personally meaningful for them. I once asked a gifted educator in an urban New Jersey high school which teachers are most effective with the really tough students. He said there are two kinds: teachers who can’t be intimidated or fooled and expect their students to learn and teachers who really care about the students. When I asked, “Which kind are you?” he answered, “Both!” He is an example of a “warm demander,” a teacher who seems to be most effective with students placed at risk (Irvine & Armento, 2001; Irvine & Fraser, 1998). Warm demanders “expect a great deal of their students, convince them of their own brilliance, and help them to reach their potential in a disciplined and structured environment” (Delpit, 2012, p. 77). They are “strong yet compassionate, authoritative yet loving, firm yet respectful” (Weinstein et al., 2004, p. 34). Sometimes these warm demanders appear harsh to outside observers. Carla Monroe and Jennifer Obidah (2002) studied Ms. Simpson, a Black teacher working with her eighth-grade science class. She describes herself as having high expectations for academics and behavior in her classes—so much so that she believed her students perceived her as “mean.” Yet she often used humor and dialect to communicate her expectations, as in the following exchange: Ms. Simpson [addressing the class]: If you know you’re going to act the fool just come to me and say, “I’m going to act the fool at the pep rally,” so I can go ahead and send you to wherever you need to go. [Class laughs.] Ms. Simpson: I’m real serious. If you know you’re having a bad day, you don’t want anybody touching you, you don’t want nobody saying nothing to you, somebody bump into you you’re going to snap—you need to come up to me and say, “I’m going to snap and I can’t go to the pep rally.” [The students start to call out various comments.]

Warm demanders Teachers who are especially effective with Black students; they show both high expectations and great caring for their students.

600 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments Ms. Simpson: Now, I just want to say I expect you to have the best behavior because you’re the most mature students in the building . . . don’t make me stop the pep rally and ask the eighth graders to leave. Edward: We’ll have silent lunch won’t we? [Class laughs.] Ms. Simpson: You don’t want to dream about what you’re going to have. [Class laughs.] OK, 15 minutes for warm-ups. [The students begin their warm-up assignment.] As we have seen throughout this book, families are important partners in education. This statement applies to classroom management as well. But researchers have seldom asked what Black and Latino/a parents want for their children by way of management. One study did examine this question and found that the parents saw school discipline as more than rules and punishments. For these parents, discipline was not an end in itself, but instead a way to ensure safety, order, self-discipline, self-advocacy, and academic rigor (Golann et al., 2019). When parents and teachers share the same expectations and support each other, they can create a more positive classroom environment and more time for learning. The Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships—Classroom Organization and provide ideas for working with families and the community. You can find more ideas through the Harvard Family Research Project (http://hfrp.org). We have looked at quite a few perspectives on classroom management. Clearly, there is no one-size-fits-all strategy for creating supportive social and physical spaces for learning. The American Psychological Association has a wealth of research-based resources for teachers on classroom management. See http://www.apa.org and search for “classroom management” to find videos, modules, and other possibilities. Also visit http://www.edutopia. org and search for “classroom management.”

GUIDELINES Family and Community Partnerships Classroom Organization and Management Make sure families know the expectations and rules of your class and school. Examples 1. 2. 3.

4.

At a Family Fun Night, have your students do skits showing the rules—how to follow them and what breaking them “looks like” and “sounds like.” Make a poster for the refrigerator at home that describes, in a light way, the most important rules and expectations. For older students, give families a list of due dates for the major assignments, along with tips about how to encourage quality work by pacing the effort—preventing last-minute panic. Some schools require family members to sign a paper indicating they are aware of the due dates. Communicate in appropriate ways—use the family’s first language when possible. Tailor messages to the reading level of the home.

Make families partners in recognizing good citizenship. Examples 1. 2.

Send positive notes home when students, especially students who have had trouble with classroom management, work well in the classroom. Give ideas for ways any family, even those with few economic resources, can celebrate accomplishment—a favorite food; the chance to choose a game to play; a comment to a special person such as an aunt, grandparent, or minister; the chance to read to a younger sibling.

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

Identify talents in the community to help build a learning environment in your class. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Have students write letters to carpet and furniture stores asking for donations of remnants to carpet a reading corner. Find family members who can build shelves or room dividers, paint, sew, laminate manipulatives, write stories, repot plants, or network computers. Contact businesses for donations of computers, printers, or other equipment.

Seek cooperation from families when behavior problems arise. Examples 1. 2.

Talk to families over the phone or in their home. Keep good records about the problem behavior. Listen to family members, and solve problems with them.

Module 42 Summary Dealing with Discipline Problems (pp. 582–594) Describe eight levels of intervention in misbehavior. Teachers can first make eye contact with the student or use other nonverbal signals and then try verbal hints such as simply inserting the student’s name into the lecture. Next, the teacher asks if the offender is aware of the negative effects of the actions and then reminds the student of the procedure and has her or him follow it correctly. Especially if a few students are “drifting away,” you can involve the whole class in a brief interactive activity such as Think-Pair-Share. If this does not work, the teacher can ask the student to state the correct rule or procedure and then follow it and then tell the student in a clear, assertive, and nonhostile way to stop the misbehavior. If this fails, too, the teacher can offer a choice—stop the behavior or meet privately to work out the consequences. If you must impose penalties, consider the seven categories used carefully by experienced teachers: express disappointment, remove privileges, time-out, reflecting on the problem, visit to the principal, detention, and contacting parents. All penalties must be applied with care and, ultimately, with kindness. What are some alternatives to harsh penalties and zero-tolerance policies? Beware of inflexible zero-tolerance policies. They often increase racial injustice. Restorative justice programs are a good alternative, especially if these programs involve the entire school and community. Restorative justice focuses on building, nurturing, and repairing relationships while giving a voice to victims, offenders, and the community. The goal is to reduce reliance on traditional punishments and police involvement but still hold misbehaving students accountable. Another alternative to teacher-imposed penalties is Marshall’s Discipline Without Stress Teaching Model, which emphasizes student responsibility and autonomy. What can teachers do about bullying, teasing, and cyberbullying? Prolonged bullying has negative effects on the academic achievement, social relationships, and mental health of the victims and the victimizers. Teachers often underestimate the amount of peer conflict and bullying, including cyberbullying, that happens in schools. Bullying involves both an imbalance of power among students and repeated attempts at harm and may take place in a variety of settings—including those in which students are not face-to-face with one another at school. Teachers can think of bullying as a form of violence and approach strategies for overcoming bullying as they would strategies to overcome other violent acts. For example, prevention of bullying can take the form of developing a respectful classroom community and discussing conflict. Also, ask what the bullies are getting out of their behavior and how they can achieve those goals in more positive ways.

601

602 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments What are some challenges in secondary classrooms? Teachers working in secondary schools should be prepared to handle students who don’t complete schoolwork, repeatedly break the same rule, or openly defy teachers. These students may also be experiencing new and powerful stressors. As a result, secondary students may benefit if teachers provide opportunities or point out resources for these students to seek out help and support. Teachers might also find consultation with guidance counselors and parents or caregivers helpful.

The Need for Communication (pp. 594–597) What is meant by “empathetic listening”? Communication between teacher and student is essential when problems arise. All interactions between people, even silence or neglect, communicate some meaning. Empathetic, active listening can be a helpful response when students bring problems to teachers. Teachers must reflect back to the students what they hear them saying. This reflection is more than a parroting of words; it should capture the emotions, intent, and meaning behind them. Distinguish among passive, hostile, and assertive response styles. The passive response style can take several forms. Instead of telling the student directly what to do, the teacher simply comments on the behavior, asks the student to think about the appropriate action, or threatens but never follows through. In a hostile response style, teachers may make “you” statements that condemn the student without stating clearly what the student should be doing. An assertive response communicates to the students that the teacher cares too much about them and the process of learning to allow inappropriate behavior to persist. Assertive teachers clearly state what they expect. If communication attempts fail, teachers and students can try problem-solving strategies and encourage student responsibility and accountability.

Diversity: Culturally Responsive Management (pp. 597–601) What is culturally responsive management, and why is it needed? Black and Latino/a Americans, especially boys, are punished more often and more harshly than other students, but they do not commit more serious offenses. Instead, these students are punished more severely for minor offenses such as rudeness or defiance—words and actions that are interpreted by teachers as meriting severe punishment. One explanation is a lack of cultural synchronization between teachers and students. High-quality teacher–student relationships are central to culturally responsive management. Positive behavioral supports (PBS; Cluster 7) that emphasize reinforcement for positive behaviors have been successful, too. Lisa Delpit suggests that warm demanders, who expect a great deal of their students, convince them of their own brilliance, and help them to reach their potential in a disciplined and structured environment, are helpful for students of color.

Cluster 13 Review Key Terms Academic learning time Assertive discipline Classroom management Culturally responsive classroom management Empathetic listening EMR model Engaged time Group focus I-message Movement management Natural/logical consequences

Overlapping Paraphrase rule Participation structures Procedures/routines Restorative justice Rules Self-management Time on task Warm demanders Withitness

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

603

Connect and Extend to Licensure Multiple-Choice Questions 1.

2.

What is the aim of classroom management? A.

To keep an orderly classroom

B.

To establish the primacy of the teacher

C.

To sustain a quiet and disciplined environment

D.

To maintain a positive, productive learning environment

Which of the following is NOT a benefit of teaching students to be self-regulated? A.

3.

4.

Students demonstrate the ability to fulfill their own needs without interfering with the rights and needs of others.

C.

Teachers should exhibit withitness and overlapping in their activities.

D.

Teachers must understand and practice movement management.

Which of the following techniques is recommended for approaching and disciplining a student who may be prone to explosive behavior? A.

Move swiftly and get as close to the misbehaving student as possible.

B.

Ensure that there are several witnesses to the confrontation.

C.

Be respectful and brief.

D.

Use a loud voice to establish power.

B.

Teachers have fewer management problems, less stress, and more time to teach.

C.

Students require increased teacher attention; therefore, they learn more.

Case

D.

Although it requires extra time initially, it leads to greater teacher self-efficacy.

It happened every day. Ginny Harding had to reprimand two boys in her class continually. Instead of feeling like a coach and mentor, Ginny started to feel like a nag. It wore on both boys and also on her. The boys were not malicious, they were just third-graders being third-graders. She remembered hearing an adage that one should not continue to do the same thing and expect different results. Over the next weekend, Ginny decided to develop a more effective manner of handling this latest challenge.

Mr. Ruiz was constantly plagued by students disrupting his English class. Determined to finally gain control, he resorted to after-school detention, dropping letter grades, and belittling his students. When his evaluation by the principal occurred at the end of the term, he received low scores on his classroom management skills. His principal, Dr. Simon, provided feedback based on research. Which one of the following would NOT be consistent with ideal ways to deal with Mr. Ruiz’s problems? A.

Teachers should begin the school year with severe consequences so students understand that the teacher controls the classroom.

B.

Teachers should aim to prevent classroom problems before they occur.

Constructed-Response Questions

5.

List several simple ways in which Ginny Harding can quickly stop the boys from misbehaving.

6.

Routines and procedures can also reduce the incidence of misbehavior by assisting students in smooth transitions from one activity to another. List several classroom operations and activities that should have an established routine or procedure.

What Would They Do? Teachers’ Casebook: Bullies and Victims At the beginning of this cluster, we asked you to think critically about how to respond to problems with bullies at school. Now, as you consider these teachers’ responses to that question, we again suggest that you think critically. Do the following responses reflect (or contradict) any of the concepts discussed in this cluster? Do the suggestions and solutions described here make sense in light of what you have learned in this or other classes about best practices? How do these ideas align with or challenge your personal philosophy of teaching?

Christen Dillon • K–12th-Grade Music Teacher Model Laboratory School at Eastern Kentucky University, Richmond, KY Classroom teachers typically have colleagues who are better equipped to handle this situation from a professional standpoint. Thankfully, we as teachers are able to rely on professional learning communities for support and help with students. After informing my administration, the next step in this process would be to let my student know I am eager to support and I care, without addressing the situation directly; sexuality and gender are so personal this could cause undue stress if your relationship with the student isn’t established.

604 Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments A comment as simple as “I’ve noticed your work has been declining” is enough to show your student you know something is wrong and that you are interested in helping, but does not acknowledge the situation and allows the student to make the choice to share with you if desired. Restorative justice is a practice I use in my classrooms to promote healthy student-to-student and student-to-teacher interactions. It is my job to explicitly teach students interpersonal skills needed to interact with people who look and act “differently.” Using the principles of restorative justice in this scenario would allow the bullies, bullied student, teacher, parents, and administration to come to an agreement that allows the bullies to make restitution to their victim in a way that is meaningful, rather than just punishing the bullies and moving on. This restitution could take the form of the bullies completing community service hours at their local pride center or helping to create a program at the school that teaches others about differences between gender and sexuality. It’s important to remember that representation matters—students need to see people like them in their lessons and in their schools. It is our job as teachers to make sure that all students feel accepted, regardless of our own feelings about matters such as religion and sexuality.

Demetrick Tensley • 6th–8th-Grade Business Education and Computer Science Teacher Seneca Middle School, Seneca, SC The described scenario is intense and multifaceted. As a high school teacher, I would approach it layer by layer. This situation requires a team approach. To initiate this team effort, I would cogently communicate all relevant information in writing to administration, guidance, and the school resource officer. I would also request a meeting to discuss the situation. Some schools have seen factors like gender, athletic ability, or personal connections skew disciplinary processes. By having all appropriate individuals in the loop from the onset, these pressures may be reduced and decision making more in line with policy and procedure. Intimidation and bullying could intensify as the “popular” and “successful” athletes are investigated. I would therefore implore the team to act summarily to notify the family of the student and provide appropriate arrangements for this student’s instruction. Swift action would be needed as retaliatory acts toward this student would likely be detrimental to their wellbeing. These incidents are alleged to have happened on and off school property and involve extortion of money, assault, gender discrimination, and xenophobia. To pursue these claims, a comprehensive investigation must be initiated. Witness statements and surveillance footage will be key. After review of the body of evidence, discipline in line with the district’s code of conduct and possibly legal action can be levied. I would follow up with a guided discussion on bullying in my classroom. This would be an additional measure that alone

would not be sufficient. These incidents do not occur in isolation. I would press the team to conduct a schoolwide SWOT (Strengths, Weaknesses, Opportunities, Threats) analysis to determine factors that led to this extensive bullying. From there, a Positive Behavioral Interventions and Supports (PBIS) program could be implemented to cultivate a more inclusive learning environment.

Mari Ann Banks, PH.D. • Chief Diversity Officer City Schools of Decatur, Decatur, GA I am the Chief Diversity Officer in an urban school district. If consulted about this situation, although several issues must be addressed, I would encourage the teacher to begin with reaching out to the bullied student immediately. The absenteeism and declining grades are of serious concern and they could possibly be indicating thoughts of self-harm. The student needs to know that someone sees him and cares about what he is experiencing. This can also be a time where the teacher can collect more data on the bullying incidents from this student’s perspective. Simultaneously, I would suggest increasing supervision in high-risk areas such as the hallways, locker rooms, and lunchroom to prevent a repeat of the bullying behavior. Given my position, however, I would like to specifically speak to the gender- and race-related aspects of the bullying. There’s a strong correlation between bias and bullying. Targets of children who bully are often from groups who are marginalized because of various cultural characteristics about which others hold prejudiced assumptions (such as race, immigration status, sexual orientation, etc.). For example, GLSEN’s National School Climate Survey indicates that verbal and physical harassment as well as physical assault have only increased for students who identify as GLBTQ since 2015. Race-based harassment and violence in U.S. schools has increased as well. Therefore, there is little surprise that students have chosen to target someone they perceive as atypical. Because this behavior is so prevalent, teachers cannot solve it on an individual level. It must be addressed systemically. Schools must create a learning environment that is inhospitable to those who would bully.

Miriam Miller • Teacher Educator and Intermediate Teacher for English Language Learners University of British Columbia, Vancouver BC, CanadaPreparing Academic Leaders Academy, Maple Heights, OH First, I want to acknowledge how complex this situation is; there are many layers in this bullying situation, including the power imbalance related to physical development, social capital, race and gender identity, and even crime. We know that bullying is a relational problem, so my immediate response is to wonder what could have happened to prevent the situation before it escalated. As an educator, I want to create positive peer relationships and a classroom climate where prosocial, upstander behavior is the norm and bullying is unacceptable. To promote positive relationships and

Cluster 13 Creating Supportive Learning Environments

605

prevent bullying, I would use restorative practices, social and emotional learning (SEL), and instructional design (e.g., cooperative learning/positive interdependence) in my classroom.

empathy for their victim and, hopefully, initiate a change in their behaviors.

In this case, where the situation has escalated beyond prevention, intervention is required. Regardless of gender identity, I would immediately involve the school principal and counselor. I would want the student who has been bullied to feel the adults at school care about his safety and are actively doing what they can to ensure he is protected. I would encourage the student to continue to report any instance of bullying. If the family is willing, I would include the family in these conversations. With support from the counselor, I would work with the student to build social skills (e.g., learning to be assertive through role-playing) and positive self-concept and mental health (e.g., encouraging the student to find things they are good at doing). Separately, the principal and counselor would address the students who were bullying (their families would also be involved).

Keith J. Boyle • 9th–12th-Grade English Teacher

To address the hateful comments, I would hold a community circle to discuss homophobia and racism. I would encourage my students to problem-solve what to do if they heard homophobic/racist comments. I would invite my students to refer to our co-created classroom agreements outlining our norms for behavior and reflect on what we can do to promote inclusion and care. I would do this regardless of which students were in my class. For the students who were bullying, I would work with the principal to create a formative (restorative) discipline plan, so that the consequences are opportunities to teach and learn, rather than punitive discipline that might only perpetuate the problem.

Jolita Harper • 3rd-Grade Teacher Preparing Academic Leaders Academy, Maple Heights, OH I believe that the entire learning community has a clear role in preventing acts of intimidation between students, and that this is best accomplished with clear communication between all parties. Care should be taken to spread awareness between colleagues as to the nature of the situation. Classroom teachers who are alert to these instances of bullying are then able to provide an additional presence in situations, such as in hallways and the lunchroom, where this is likely to take place. Further, communication between individual classroom teachers and the victim of this bullying is essential. I would make certain to provide a sensitive ear to this student’s plight as we work together to formulate alternatives toward improving the situation. Finally, in the event that the two bullying students were in my classes, I would communicate with them in such a way as to make clear the effect of their actions on others in an effort to promote

Dunellen High School, Dunellen, NJ Errant behavior throughout the middle school may be indicative of future behavioral problems and, as many things in life, the more this misbehavior is allowed to exist, the longer it will have a chance to thrive. In this case of a child being continually bullied by two other children (gender having no bearing in this situation), the knowledge of this wrongdoing must not be ignored or isolated. I would interview both the victim and the bullies, separately, to glean as much information as possible. If this were a singular incident, I would attempt to handle it myself via contact with the pertinent parents. However, if this were a recurring problem, the administration must be made aware. Any administrator will acknowledge that to be left in the dark about a serious situation within the environs of his or her responsibility is precarious. The appropriate guidance counselor should also be involved. The gravity of abusive behavior toward fellow students must be emphasized to the offenders. Significant punitive action is integral to send a message to the entire community that their school is indeed a haven in which one can feel the uninhibited freedom to learn.

Kelley Crockett • Professor and Former Elementary School Teacher San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, and Fort Worth, TX Bullying cannot be tolerated. No school, no teacher, no administrator can afford a climate in which abusive behavior is allowed to germinate. Any incident of victimization must be immediately documented and submitted to the principal. As well, I would schedule a conference that same day with the school counselor for my student to both allow another avenue of documentation and reinforce support that the problem is being aggressively addressed. How I handle the next step depends on the administration in place, but the important issue to remember is that there is a next step. The teacher must follow up with the student. Within 48 hours I would privately ask my student if there have been any further incidents. If he hesitates or acknowledges continued harassment I would direct him to write it down, and I would document any questions I had asked him and his responses. I would then include his statement and my own in another report for both the principal and the counselor. As teachers, we hold the front line. To the children in our care, we represent one of the first relationships with authority and civilized society. We can do no less than lend our voice and action to the betterment of our world.

Cluster 14

Savannah, Age 4

Teaching Every Student

Teachers’ Casebook: Reaching and Teaching Every Student What Would You Do? You have started a new job in a high school in your hometown. The community is very diverse. In the classes you will be teaching, you are likely to find a wide range of reading levels, family incomes, learning strengths, and learning challenges. You know that two of your students are practically ready for college, while several struggle to read and write at grade level. Also, you know that reading English academic texts will be a challenge for some of your students, even though they converse in English with little trouble.

Critical Thinking • How would you differentiate instruction to meet the diverse needs of students? • Do different philosophies of teaching provide different answers to this question? • How will you assess learning if you have successfully differentiated instruction? • How could you incorporate technology into your teaching to reach all your students? 606

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Overview and Objectives Much of this text has been about learning and learners. In fact, we have spent 13 clusters discussing development, individual and cultural differences, learning, and motivation. Why? The ultimate goal is so you can become a great teacher. Are there particular characteristics that distinguish effective teachers from ineffective ones? Research on whole-class teaching points to the importance of several factors that we will explore, including how teachers’ beliefs about their students’ abilities—teacher expectations—might influence student learning and teacher–student relationships. What else do we know about teaching? Some researchers call for ambitious instruction that creates challenging but also engaging classes filled with complex tasks and high-quality discussions where students base their statements on evidence. We will look at this conception of teaching. Teachers also are designers—they create learning environments (Warr & Mishra, 2021). In the process, they set goals for their students, develop teaching strategies and activities, and assess to see if goals have been met. We look at how teachers plan, including how to use state standards, taxonomies of learning objectives, themes, and essential questions as a basis for planning. With this foundation of knowing how to set goals and make plans, as well as an understanding of the characteristics of effective teachers, we move to a consideration of some general teacher-centered strategies: direct instruction, independent work, homework, questioning, recitation, group discussion, and Quality Talk. We then pull goals and strategies together by exploring the Understanding by Design model. In the final section of this cluster, we will focus on how to match teaching to the needs and abilities of students through differentiated instruction, flexible grouping, and universal design for learning. By the time you have completed this cluster, you should be able to: 14.1 Identify the methods used to study teaching as well as the

characteristics of effective teachers and effective classroom climates, including the role of teacher expectations and the goal of ambitious teaching. 14.2 Describe the planning process, including the possibilities of Lesson

Study, and how to develop Learning Objectives that are consistent with the standards in your state using either Bloom’s taxonomy or Webb’s Depth of Knowledge levels. 14.3 Discuss the appropriate uses of direct instruction, independent work,

homework, questioning (particularly authentic questions), feedback, dialogue, and group discussion and explain how to use Understanding by Design to integrate outcomes, evidence for reaching those outcomes, and teaching strategies. 14.4 Define differentiated instruction and apply this approach to teaching

a diverse group of students, including how to apply the guidelines of universal design for learning in your teaching.

607

608 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

OUTLINE Teachers’ Casebook—Reaching and Teaching Every Student What Would You Do? Overview and Objectives MODULE 43: Planning for Effective Teaching Research on Teaching Characteristics of Effective Teachers Knowledge for Teaching Teacher Expectations The Goal: Ambitious Teaching The First Step: Planning Research on Planning Learning Goals Flexible and Creative Plans—Using Taxonomies The Cognitive Demands of Learning Objectives: Webb’s Depth of Knowledge Planning from a Constructivist Perspective MODULE 44: Teaching Every Student Teaching Approaches Research on Teaching Strategies Explicit Teaching and Direct Instruction Independent Work and Homework Questioning, Discussion, Dialogue, and Feedback Lessons for Teachers: Fitting Teaching to Your Goals Putting It All Together: Understanding by Design Differentiated Instruction Within-Class and Flexible Grouping Differentiated Instruction in Inclusive Classrooms Reaching Every Student: Differentiation with Universal Design for Learning Cluster 14 Review Key Terms • Connect and Extend to Licensure • Teachers’ Casebook—Reaching and Teaching Every Student: What Would They Do?

MODULE 43

Planning for Effective Teaching

Learning Objective 14.1 Identify the methods used to study teaching as well as the characteristics of effective teachers and effective classroom climates, including the role of teacher expectations and the goal of ambitious teaching. Learning Objective 14.2 Describe the planning process, including the possibilities of Lesson Study, and how to develop Learning Objectives that are consistent with the standards in your state using either Bloom’s taxonomy or Webb’s Depth of Knowledge levels.

Research on Teaching How would you identify the keys to successful teaching? You might ask experienced teachers, students, principals, or college professors of education to list the characteristics of good teachers. Or you could do intensive case studies of a few classrooms over a long period. You might observe classes, rate different teachers on certain characteristics, and then see which characteristics were associated with teachers whose students either achieved the most or were the most motivated to learn. (To do this, of course, you would

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

have to decide how to assess achievement and motivation.) You could identify teachers whose students, year after year, learned more than students working with other teachers; then you could observe the more successful teachers and note what they do. You might also train teachers to apply several different strategies to teach the same lesson and then determine which strategy led to the greatest student learning. You could video teachers and then ask them to view the videos and report what they were thinking about as they taught and what influenced their decisions while teaching; this process is called stimulated recall. You might study transcripts of classroom dialogue to learn what helped students understand the material. You might use the relationships identified between teaching and learning as the basis for developing teaching approaches and then test these approaches in design experiments, or by collaborating with teachers in participatory research to improve their practice. Since the 1970s, all of these approaches and more have been used to investigate teaching (see Floden, 2001; Good, 2014; Gröschner et al., 2013 for summaries of almost 50 years of research). Let’s examine some of the specific knowledge gained from these projects, keeping in mind that much of the research was conducted in K–8 classrooms, often during mathematics lessons, and that student performance on standardized tests was generally used as the indicator of good teaching.

Characteristics of Effective Teachers Stop & Think Think about the most effective teacher you ever had—the one who sparked your interest and expanded your knowledge. What were the characteristics of that person? What made that teacher so effective? Some of the earliest research on effective teaching identified three teacher characteristics related to learning: clarity, warmth, and knowledge. Recent research has focused on knowledge, so we will spend some extra time on that characteristic. CLARITY AND ORGANIZATION. When Barak Rosenshine and Norma Furst (1973) reviewed about 50 studies of teaching, they concluded that clarity was the most promising teacher behavior for future research on effective teaching. Teachers who provide clear presentations and explanations tend to have students who learn more and who rate their teachers more positively (Comadena et al., 2007; Hines et al., 1985). The clearer and less vague the teacher’s explanations and instructions were, the more the students learned. Vague explanations were filled with “somes” (somehow, some way, some people, sometime, someone, something . . .) and “you knows” (Evertson & Emmer, 2017).

Research has found that ratings of teachers’ enthusiasm are correlated with student achievement gains (Keller et al., 2013, 2016) and are also related to student interest in the subject (Keller et al., 2014). Also, teachers’ positive emotions while working (e.g., feeling inspired, alert, attentive, and interested and not nervous, ashamed, or irritable), their job satisfaction, and their self-efficacy are all related to experiencing enthusiasm (Buric & Moè, 2020). It is possible that when teachers are enthusiastic, they capture and hold student attention, and also enthusiastic teachers model engagement and interest in learning. Student attention, interest, and engagement lead to learning. Of course, it is easier to be an enthusiastic teacher when your students are learning (Keller et al., 2013). Warmth, friendliness, and understanding seem to be the teacher traits most strongly associated with students’ liking the teacher and the class in general (Hamann et al., 2000; K. Madsen, 2003). Also, in studies of the emotional climate of the classroom, researchers consistently find that students learn more in classes where teacher–student relationships are warm, caring, nurturing, and congenial; the teacher takes student needs and perspectives into account; and teachers are not harsh or sarcastic. It is likely that the link between the positive emotional climate and student learning is student engagement (Meyer & Anderman, in press; Reyes et al., 2012; Tennant et al., 2014). What about another important teacher characteristic—knowledge?

ENTHUSIASM AND WARMTH.

609

610 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Knowledge for Teaching Expert teachers Experienced, effective teachers who have developed solutions for classroom problems. Their knowledge of the teaching process and content is extensive and well organized.

As you saw in Clusters 8 and 9, knowledge is the defining characteristic of expertise. Expert teachers have elaborate systems of knowledge for understanding problems in teaching. For example, when a beginning teacher is faced with students’ wrong answers on math or history tests, all of these answers may seem about the same—wrong. But for an expert teacher, wrong answers are part of a rich system of knowledge that could include: 1. How to recognize several types of wrong answers, 2. The misunderstanding or lack of information behind each kind of mistake, 3. The best way to reteach and correct the misunderstanding, 4. Materials and activities that have worked in the past, and 5. Several ways to test whether the reteaching was successful.

Pedagogical content knowledge (PCK) Teacher knowledge that combines mastery of academic content with knowing how to teach the content and how to match instruction to student differences.

Reflective Thoughtful and inventive. Reflective teachers think back over situations to analyze what they did and why and to consider how they might improve learning for their students.

This unique kind of teacher knowledge that combines mastery of academic content with knowing how to teach the content and how to match instruction to student differences is called pedagogical content knowledge (PCK). This knowledge is very complex and specific to the situation (e.g., first-period physics), topic (e g., the concept of “force”), students (advanced? struggling? learning English as an additional language?), and even the individual teacher. These teachers are reflective practitioners, constantly trying to understand and improve their work with students (Hogan et al., 2003). Do teachers who know more about their subject have a more positive impact on their students? It depends on the subject. When H. C. Hill, Rowan, and Ball (2005) tested U.S. first- and third-grade teachers’ specific knowledge of the math concepts that they actually teach and their understanding of how to teach those concepts, they found that teachers with greater content and pedagogical content knowledge had students who learned more mathematics. In a study by Lui and Bonner (2016), math conceptual knowledge was more important in lesson planning than beliefs and theories about teaching and learning for both preservice and in-service elementary teachers. Studies in German high schools have found that math teachers with more pedagogical content knowledge have students who are more cognitively engaged and more supported in learning, and this higher-quality instruction predicts higher student math achievement (Baumert et al., 2010). When we look at teachers’ knowledge of facts and concepts in other subjects besides math, as measured by test scores and college grades, the relationship to student learning is unclear and may be indirect. Teachers who know more may make clearer presentations and recognize student difficulties more easily. They are ready for any student questions and do not have to be evasive or vague in their answers. Thus, knowledge helps teachers be clearer, more organized, and more responsive to student questions (Aloe & Becker, 2009). Pedagogical content knowledge is a foundation for effective teaching, but the real-time thinking and decision making based on that knowledge has been called pedagogical reasoning or instructional reasoning. This is “the thinking that underpins informed professional practice” (Loughran, 2019, p. 4). The thing about teaching is that there are hundreds of large and small decisions every day—which student to call on next, how long to spend on a topic, whether to answer a question or turn it back on the student, for example. Pedagogical reasoning is the union of thought and action. This reasoning requires diagnosing what specific students need in specific situations and doing that quickly, all the while keeping track of what is happening “behind your back.” There is another line of important research on teacher characteristics—the examination of teacher expectations.

Teacher Expectations Marvin Marshall (2013) tells the story of a teacher who was delighted when she saw the listing for her new class. “Wow, have I got a bright class this year! Look at these amazing IQs—116, 118, 122, 124. . . .” The teacher designed a host of challenging activities, set high expectations for her students, and communicated her confidence in them to excel. They did. Only much later did the teacher discover that the numbers beside the students’ names were their locker numbers!

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Can expectations make a difference? Over 50 years ago, a study by Robert Rosenthal and Lenore Jacobson (1968) captured the attention of the national media in a way that few studies by psychologists have since then. Debate about the meaning of the results continues (De Boer et al., 2010; Jussim, 2013; Jussim et al., 2009; Snow, 1995). What did Rosenthal and Jacobson say that caused such a stir? They randomly chose several students in a number of elementary school classrooms, and then told the teachers that these students probably would make significant intellectual gains during the year. That group of students did indeed make larger gains than normal that year. The researchers presented data suggesting the existence of a Pygmalion effect or self-fulfilling prophecy in the classroom. A selffulfilling prophecy is a groundless expectation that leads to behaviors that then make the original expectation come true (Merton, 1948). An example is a false belief that a bank is failing; this leads to a rush of patrons withdrawing money, which then causes the bank to fail as expected. Recent research has demonstrated that teacher expectation effects are real and matter for students (Gentrup et al., 2020; Johnston et al., 2019; Rubie-Davies et al., 2020; Van den Broeck et al., 2020). Let’s look deeper.

611

Pygmalion effect Exceptional progress by a student as a result of high teacher expectations for that student; named for the mythological king Pygmalion, who made a statue and then caused it to be brought to life.

Self-fulfilling prophecy A groundless expectation that is confirmed because it has been expected.

Stop & Think When you thought about the most effective teacher you ever had, was one of the characteristics that the teacher believed in you or demanded the best from you? How did the teacher communicate that belief? TWO KINDS OF EXPECTATION EFFECTS. Two kinds of expectation effects can occur in classrooms. In the self-fulfilling prophecy just described, the teacher’s beliefs about the students’ abilities have no basis in fact, but student behavior comes to match the initially inaccurate expectation. The second kind of expectation effect occurs when teachers are fairly accurate in their initial reading of students’ abilities and respond to students appropriately. The problems arise when students show some improvement but teachers do not alter their expectations to take account of the improvement. This is called a sustaining expectation effect because the teacher’s unchanging expectation sustains the student’s achievement at the expected level. The chance to raise expectations, provide more appropriate teaching, and thus encourage greater student achievement is lost. In practice, self-fulfilling prophecy effects seem to be stronger in the early grades, and sustaining effects are more likely in the later grades (Kuklinski & Weinstein, 2001).

Research in the United States and around the world documents many possible sources of teachers’ expectations, including intelligence test scores (especially if they are not interpreted appropriately), gender, notes from previous teachers, the medical or psychological reports in students’ permanent files, prior knowledge about older brothers and sisters, appearance (higher expectations for attractive students), previous achievement, SES, race and ethnicity, immigrant status, language, parents’ incarceration, diagnoses of learning challenges, being overweight, and the actual behaviors of the student (Johnston et al., 2019; Sneyers et al., 2020; Van Matre et al., 2000). Even the student’s after-school activities can be a source of expectations. Teachers tend to hold higher expectations for students who participate in extracurricular activities than for students who do nothing after school. And recent studies show that some teachers may hold class-level expectations; that is, they have higher or lower expectations for all the students in a particular class (Pitten Cate & Glock, 2018; Rubie-Davies, 2010; Rubie-Davies et al., 2020). Some students are more likely than others to be the recipients of sustaining expectations. For example, withdrawn children provide little information about themselves, so teachers may sustain their expectations about these children simply for lack of new input (Jones & Gerig, 1994). Self-fulfilling prophecy effects tend to be stronger for students from lower-SES families and for Black students (De Boer et al., 2010). In a synthesis of over 50 studies, Harriet Tenenbaum and Martin Ruck (2007) found that teachers held higher expectations for and directed more positive questions and encouragement toward European American students compared with Black and Latino/a students. The highest expectations were reserved for Asian American students. SOURCES OF EXPECTATIONS.

Sustaining expectation effect Student performance is maintained at a certain level because teachers don’t recognize improvements.

612 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student Expectations and beliefs focus attention and organize memory, so teachers may pay attention to and remember the information that fits their initial expectations (Fiske, 1993). Even when student performance does not fit expectations, the teacher may rationalize and attribute the performance to external causes beyond the student’s control. For example, a teacher may assume that the low-ability student who did well on a test must have cheated and that the high-ability student who failed must have been upset that day. In both cases, behavior that seems out of character is dismissed. It may take many instances of supposedly uncharacteristic behavior to change the teacher’s beliefs about a particular student’s abilities. Thus, expectations often remain in the face of contradictory evidence (Brophy, 1998). DO TEACHERS’ EXPECTATIONS REALLY AFFECT STUDENTS’ ACHIEVEMENT? The answer to the

question “Do teachers’ expectations really affect students’ achievement?” is more complicated than it might seem. There are two ways to investigate the issue. One is to give teachers unfounded expectations about their students and note if these baseless expectations have any effects. The other approach is to identify the naturally occurring expectations of teachers and study the effects of these expectations. The answer to the question of whether teacher expectations affect student learning depends in part on which approach is taken to study the question. The original Rosenthal and Jacobson experiment used the first approach—giving teachers groundless expectations and noting the effects. A careful analysis of the results revealed that even though first- through sixth-grade students participated in the study, the self-fulfilling prophecy effects could be traced to dramatic changes in just five students in first and second grades. Unfounded expectations likely have only a small effect on student IQ scores (the outcome measure used by Rosenthal and Jacobson) and only in the early years of a new school setting—in the first years of elementary school and then again in the first years of middle school (Raudenbush, 1984). But what about the second approach—naturally occurring expectations? Research shows that teachers do indeed form beliefs about students’ capabilities. Many of these beliefs are accurate assessments based on the best available data and are corrected as new information is collected (Jussim & Harber, 2005). But inaccuracies can make a difference. For example, a study by Christine Rubie-Davies and her colleagues (2020) followed 31 teachers (grades 2 to 7) for a year. The teachers were chosen because their reading achievement expectations for their whole class were either higher or lower than their students’ actual reading levels at the beginning of the year—so the expectations were inaccurate in one direction or the other. By the end of the year, students of the highexpectation teachers had more positive beliefs about themselves and their teachers and also achieved at higher levels compared to the students in the low-expectation teachers’ classes. In a longitudinal study by Nicole Sorhagen (2013), teachers’ over- and underestimations of students’ math and language abilities in first grade predicted the students’ math, reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and verbal reasoning standardized test scores at age 15, and the impact was greater for students from lower-income families. Another study in high schools found that students were more likely to complete college when their 10th-grade teachers held higher expectations for them. The teachers in the study had positive biases on average, but less so when a White teacher was describing a Black student (Papageorge et al., 2020). If teachers decide that some students are less able and if the teachers lack effective strategies for working with lower-achieving students, then students may experience a double threat—low expectations and inadequate teaching (Good & Brophy, 2008). Perhaps the underestimation of the abilities of children in poverty is one factor contributing to the opportunity gap for those students. As you become a teacher, you “need to understand the importance of teacher expectations in predicting both academic and socio-psychological outcomes, particularly for disadvantaged students”(Rubie-Davies et al., 2020, p. 1196).

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Table 14.1 Teacher Expectations and Instruction Here are some ways that teachers differentiate their instruction and communications toward students based on the teachers’ expectations about student abilities. INTERACTIONS AND RELATIONSHIPS

EXPECTATIONS

INSTRUCTION

High

-More challenging lessons -More opportunities for choice -More likely to be assigned to high or gifted group

-More opportunities and time to respond to teacher questions -More opportunity for follow-up questions—tell me more -More stay-with behavior—teacher provides clues or rephrases the questions -More teacher praise and positive feedback about performance -Greater teacher warmth, smiles, affective support -Greater encouragement to pursue higher education

Low

-Less challenging lessons, more drill and practice -Fewer opportunities for choice -More likely to be assigned to low groups

-Fewer opportunities and less time to respond to teacher questions -Less opportunity for follow-up questions -More give-up behavior—teacher provides the answer or calls on another student to do so -More teacher criticism for wrong answers, less praise for correct answers -Less corrective feedback for wrong answers -Fewer teacher smiles, less personal warmth

The power of the expectation effect depends on the age of the students (generally speaking, younger students are more susceptible) and on how differently a teacher treats high- versus low-expectation students (Kuklinski & Weinstein, 2001). Teachers may use different instructional strategies and also have different interactions and relationships with students based on expectations, as shown in Table 14.1 (Gentrup et al., 2020; Good, 2014). In terms of instruction, when students who are ready for more challenging work are not given the opportunity to try it because teachers believe they cannot handle it, a sustaining expectation effect is likely. In terms of interactions and relationships, the effects can be huge, as the expectation differences build year after year with many teachers (Trouilloud et al., 2006). Of course, not all teachers form inappropriate expectations or act on their expectations in unconstructive ways (Babad et al., 1982). The Guidelines: Avoiding the Negative Effects of Teacher Expectations may help you avoid some of these problems. But avoiding the problem might be more difficult than it seems. In general, low-expectation students also tend to be the most disruptive students. (Of course, low expectations can reinforce their desire to disrupt or misbehave.) Teachers may call on these students less, wait a shorter time for their answers, and give them less praise for right answers, partly to avoid the wrong, careless, or silly answers that can cause disruptions, delays, and digressions. The challenge is to deal with these very real threats to classroom management without communicating low expectations to some students or fostering their own low expectations of themselves. And sometimes, low expectations become part of the culture of the school—beliefs shared by teachers and administrators alike (Rubie-Davies et al., 2020; Weinstein et al., 1995). LESSONS FOR TEACHERS: COMMUNICATING APPROPRIATE EXPECTATIONS.

613

614 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

GUIDELINES Avoiding the Negative Effects of Teacher Expectations Use information about students from tests, cumulative folders, parent communications, and other teachers very carefully. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Avoid reading cumulative folders early in the year. Be critical and objective about the reports you hear from other teachers. Be flexible in your expectations—a student’s label or your judgment might be wrong. Don’t let communications from parents shape your expectations for their children—get to know the student.

Be flexible in your use of grouping strategies. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Review work of students often, and experiment with new groupings. Use different groups for different subjects. Use mixed-ability groups in cooperative exercises.

Provide both challenge and support. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Don’t say, “This is easy, I know you can do it.” Offer a wide range of problems, and encourage all students to try a few of the harder ones for extra credit. Find something positive about these attempts. Make sure your high expectations come with academic and emotional support for students’ struggles. “Holding high standards without providing a warm environment is merely harsh. A warm environment without high standards lacks backbone” (Jussim, 2013).

Be especially careful about how you respond to low-achieving students during class discussions. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Give them prompts, cues, and time to answer. Give ample praise for good answers. Call on low achievers as often as you do high achievers.

Use materials that show a wide range of ethnic groups. Examples 1. 2.

Check readers and library books. Is there ethnic diversity? Ask students to research and create their own materials based on community or family sources.

Make sure that your teaching does not reflect racial, ethnic, or sexual stereotypes or prejudice. Examples 1. 2.

Use a checking system to be sure you call on and include all students. Monitor the content of the tasks you assign. Do boys get the “hard” math problems to work at the board? Do you avoid having students with limited English give oral presentations?

Be fair in evaluation and disciplinary procedures. Examples 1. 2.

Make sure equal offenses receive equal punishment. Find out from students in an anonymous questionnaire whether you seem to be favoring certain individuals. Try to grade student work without knowing the identity of the student. Ask another teacher to give you a “second opinion” from time to time.

Communicate to all students that you believe they can learn—and mean it. Examples 1. 2.

Return papers that do not meet standards with specific suggestions for improvements. If students do not have the answers immediately, wait, probe, and then help them think through an answer.

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

615

Involve all students in learning tasks and in privileges. Examples 1. 2.

Use some system to make sure you give each student practice in reading, speaking, and answering questions. Keep track of who gets to do what job. Are some students always on the list, whereas others seldom make it?

Monitor your nonverbal behavior. Examples 1.

Do you lean away or stand farther away from some students? Do some students get smiles when they approach your desk, whereas others get only frowns?

2.

Does your tone of voice vary with different students?

For more information, see https://www.ascd.org/el/articles/are-you-communicating-highexpectations.

The Goal: Ambitious Teaching High expectations for students are a critical ingredient in ambitious teaching. Ambitious teaching (or ambitious instruction) is a relatively recent term describing an approach to teaching grounded in challenging goals, complex but engaging tasks, and high-quality discussions building on students’ ideas. The term is used most often to describe mathematics teaching, but all teachers can strive for ambitious instruction if they:

• Have deep knowledge, especially of the big ideas in their field. • Know their students well, including the lives the students live daily outside class and how they see the world.

• Know how to create teaching and learning spaces to connect the big ideas of the subject matter with the lives of their students.

• Hold high expectations that students’ capabilities to learn far exceed what most people expect.

• Work together with students on rich academic content, authentic problems, puzzling natural events, and intriguing questions.

• Press students for critical thinking and explanations based on evidence (Grant & Gradwell, 2010; Reich, 2017). In general, goals and expectations are ambitious. Teachers develop the deep-thinking processes their students need to take on intellectually demanding tasks and push their students to elaborate, clarify, support, and justify their ideas and problem solutions (Anagnostopoulos et al., 2020). If this reminds you of the robust learning we discussed in Cluster 9, you are right—A+ for remembering! For more information, see http://ambitiousscience teaching.org. Ambitious teaching requires careful planning—our next topic.

The First Step: Planning Stop & Think Greta Morine-Dershimer (2006) asks which of the following are true about teacher planning: Time is of the essence.

A little planning goes a long way.

Plans are made to be broken.

You can do it yourself.

Don’t look back.

One size fits all.

Ambitious teaching An approach to teaching grounded in challenging goals, complex but engaging tasks, and high-quality discussions building on students’ ideas.

616 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Research on Planning

Lesson Study As a group, teachers develop, test, improve, and retest lessons until they are satisfied with the final version.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Instructional Objectives (II, B1) Describe the key elements of behavioral and instructional objectives. Be able to write each type of objective for a content area that you expect to teach.

When you thought about the “What Would You Do?” challenge at the beginning of this cluster, you were planning. Planning influences what students will learn because planning transforms the available time and curriculum materials into activities, assignments, and tasks for students—time is of the essence in planning. When a teacher decides to devote 7 hours to language arts and 15 minutes to science in a given week, the students in that class will learn more language than science. Planning done at the beginning of the year is particularly important because many routines and patterns, such as time allocations, are established early (Weinstein & Novodvorsky, 2015). So a little planning does go a long way in terms of what will be taught and what will be learned. Second, teachers engage in several levels of planning—by the year, term, unit, week, and day. All the levels must be coordinated. As you gain experience in teaching, it will be easier to coordinate these levels of planning and incorporate the state and district curriculum standards as well. Third, plans reduce—but do not eliminate—uncertainty in teaching. Planning must allow flexibility. So plans are not made to be broken—but sometimes they need to be bent a bit. To plan creatively and flexibly, teachers need to have wide-ranging knowledge about students, their interests, their abilities, and the subjects being taught—ambitious teaching again. The plans of beginning teachers sometimes don’t work because they lack knowledge about the students or the subject—they can’t estimate how long it will take students to complete an activity, for example, or they stumble when asked for an explanation or a different example (Calderhead, 1996). In planning, you can do it yourself—but collaboration is better. Working with other teachers and sharing ideas is one of the best experiences in teaching. Some educators think that a collaborative approach to planning used in Japan called kenshu or “mastery through study” is one reason why Japanese students do so well on international tests. In the United States and many other countries, kenshu is called Lesson Study (Wood, 2020). In Lesson Study, small groups of teachers complete cycles of setting goals for learning, examining relevant research and curriculum possibilities, and designing a model lesson. Next, one group member teaches the lesson while other teachers observe or gather data, maybe even interviewing students. The lesson is videorecorded so all members can review the video, analyze student responses, and improve the lesson further. Other teachers try the revised lesson, and more improvements follow. At the end of the school year, all the study groups may publish the results of their work, so Lesson Study is a form of professional development as well as curriculum development—both teaching and the curriculum get better and better (Lee & Tan, 2020). Information about “Lesson Study” and many examples of lesson plans are available on the Internet. But even great science lesson plans taken from a terrific website must be adapted to your situation. Some of the adaptation comes before you teach and some comes after. In fact, much of what experienced teachers know about planning comes from looking back—reflecting—on what worked and what didn’t, so DO look back on your plans and grow professionally in the process. Collaborative reflection and revising lessons are major components of the kenshu or Lesson Study approach to planning. Finally, there is no one model for effective planning. One size does NOT fit all in planning. Planning is a creative problem-solving process for experienced teachers; they know how to complete many lessons and can teach segments of lessons effectively, so for familiar lessons, they don’t necessarily continue to follow the detailed lesson-planning models they learned during their teacher preparation programs, even if those detailed plans helped in the beginning. No matter how you plan, you must have a learning goal in mind. We turn to this next.

Learning Goals It is difficult to get somewhere if you don’t know where you are going. Similarly, it is difficult to plan a unit or lesson if you don’t know the point—if you don’t have a clear target. “Learning targets define academic success—they state what we want students to know and be able to do” (Chappuis & Stiggins, 2017, p. 42). Over the years, targets for student learning have been called goals and objectives, instructional objectives, Learning Objectives,

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

content standards, behavioral objectives, educational objectives, grade-level indicators, performance expectations, lesson objectives, competencies, curricular aims, benchmarks, and many other labels. Be sure to learn what language and labels are used in your school and district and what the labels actually mean (Popham, 2020). AN EXAMPLE OF STATE-LEVEL GOALS: THE COMMON CORE. We hear quite a bit today about grand visions and goals for education such as “promote student achievement and preparation for global competitiveness by fostering educational excellence and ensuring equal access” (U.S. Department of Education, 2021). But very general goals are meaningless as potential guidelines for your instruction. States may turn these grand goals into standards or indicators. No Child Left Behind mandated that states adopt their own content standards in mathematics and reading, so all 50 states did. But there were problems. For example, there were redundancies across grades or missing steps between grades. Also, there was great variation among the various states’ standards in terms of rigor and difficulty. Most of these standards focused on basic proficiency, not the kinds of capabilities needed for 21st-century careers (Petrilli, 2020). To address these problems, beginning in 2009, the Council of Chief State School Officers (CCSSO) and the National Governors Association Center for Best Practices (NGA Center) led an effort to define consistent national standards for each grade, K–12, in two broad areas: (a) English language arts and literacy in history/ social studies, science, and technical subjects and (b) mathematics. Table 14.2 gives a few examples of Common Core State Standards in these areas. State adoption of the Common Core State Standards is a moving target. Initially, 45 states, the District of Columbia, and four territories adopted the standards, and one more state, Minnesota, adopted just the English/language arts standards. Since then, four states have withdraw, and at least 12 states are in the process of withdrawing. Many states that are still using the Common Core State Standards have renamed them or altered them slightly. To see what your state is doing, go to your state’s Department of Education web page and search for “standards.”

Table 14.2 A Few Examples of the Common Core State Standards for Grades 6 and 11–12 in Literature, Writing, and Mathematics SUBJECT AND SKILL

GRADE 6

GRADES 11 AND 12

Reading Literature: Key Ideas

Cite textual evidence to support analysis of what the text says explicitly as well as inferences drawn from the text.

Cite strong and thorough textual evidence to support analysis of what the text says explicitly as well as inferences drawn from the text, including determining where the text leaves matters uncertain.

Conduct short research projects to answer a question, drawing on several sources and refocusing the inquiry when appropriate.

Conduct short as well as more sustained research projects to answer a question (including a self-generated question) or solve a problem; narrow or broaden the inquiry when appropriate; synthesize multiple sources on the subject, demonstrating understanding of the subject under investigation.

Find the area of right triangles, other triangles, special quadrilaterals, and polygons by composing into rectangles or decomposing into triangles and other shapes; apply these techniques in the context of solving real-world and mathematical problems.

Prove theorems about triangles. Theorems include: a line parallel to one side of a triangle divides the other two proportionally, and conversely; the Pythagorean Theorem proved using triangle similarity.

and Details

Writing: Research to Build and Present Knowledge

Mathematics: Geometry

617

618 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student There is continuing debate about the value of the Common Core. Those who favor the standards note that we need higher, more consistent standards because the United States routinely performs near the bottom in international achievement tests. Students graduating under the older, basic skills standards were not really prepared for college and careers. In response to the Common Core standards, teaching materials, textbooks, and assessments are improving and becoming more aligned with the standards—we are testing what we teach. If we drop this effort altogether, we will lose the progress we have made (Calfee et al., 2014; Petrilli, 2020). Criticisms of the Common Core include that the standards limit teachers’ creativity, don’t allow states and local districts to add their own content to the curriculum, and lead to too much testing. In addition, the standardized tests students are required to take often still are not aligned with the Common Core standards, so students are taught one thing and tested on another (Polikoff, 2015). As a political issue, there certainly is objection to the Common Core. In a survey of attitudes about the Common Core, Morgan Polikoff and his colleagues (2016) found that Republicans were 90% more likely than Democrats to oppose the standards. But the troubling bottom line is that there is little evidence that simply adopting the Common Core standards has raised student achievement, and even in studies that find some gain, the improvement is small, perhaps in part because the standards were implemented inconsistently (Loveless, 2020; Polikoff, 2020). Beware of Either/Or. As a teacher you should be familiar with the standards in your state— they may be very close to the Common Core. These standards impact what is taught (curriculum materials, textbooks, course planning) and what is assessed in every grade. But remember, the standards are goals—they do not tell you how to teach, just what outcomes to aim for. Whatever the term—objectives, outcomes, targets, standards, indicators, or other labels—having a clear learning goal helps teachers avoid what Grant Wiggins and Jay McTighe (2006) call the “twin sins” of instructional design—activityfocused teaching (lots of hands-on, interesting activities—but no goal) and coverage-focused teaching (a forced march through the textbook—but no goal). In either case, learning can be lost if the teacher is not clear about why students are doing the activities or reading the texts. Frame your learning objectives broadly enough so they help organize your instruction but specifically enough to serve as a guide to your assessment; then test what you teach. CLASSROOMS GOALS FOR LEARNING.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Taxonomies of Educational Objectives (II, B1) Taxonomies influence every aspect of instruction from textbook design to lesson planning. List the major objectives of each of the taxonomies, and describe the focus of each objective. Be able to incorporate these objectives into instructional objectives that you design.

Taxonomy Classification system.

Flexible and Creative Plans—Using Taxonomies Stop & Think

Think about your assignments for one of your classes.

What kind of thinking is involved in doing the assignments: • Remembering facts and terms? • Understanding key ideas? • Applying information to solve problems? • Analyzing a situation, task, or problem? • Making evaluations or giving opinions? • Creating or designing something new? In the 1950s, a group of experts in educational evaluation led by Benjamin Bloom set out to improve college and university examinations. The impact of their work has affected education at all levels around the world (Anderson & Sosniak, 1994). Bloom and his colleagues developed a taxonomy, or classification system, of educational outcomes divided into three domains: cognitive, affective, and psychomotor. In real life, of course, behaviors from these three domains occur simultaneously. While students are answering essay questions, they are writing or using a keyboard (psychomotor) and remembering or reasoning (cognitive) and also might have some emotional response to the task (affective), even if the major domain involved is cognitive (Popham, 2020).

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Bloom’s taxonomy of the thinking domain, or cognitive domain, was considered one of the most significant educational writings of the 20th century (Anderson & Sosniak, 1994). The six basic outcomes are knowledge, comprehension, application, analysis, synthesis, and evaluation (Bloom et al., 1956). It is common to consider these outcomes as a hierarchy, with each skill building on those below, but this is not entirely accurate, especially in subjects such as mathematics. Still, you will hear many references to lower-level and higher-level outcomes or objectives, with knowledge, comprehension, and application considered lower level and the other categories considered higher level. As a rough way of thinking about learning targets, this can be helpful. The taxonomy can also be useful in planning assessments because different procedures are appropriate for outcomes at the various levels, as you will see in Cluster 15. In 2001, a group of educational researchers published the first major revision of the cognitive taxonomy, and this is the one we use today (Anderson & Krathwohl, 2001).

THE COGNITIVE DOMAIN.

619

Cognitive domain In Bloom’s taxonomy, memory and reasoning objectives.

1. Remembering: Remembering or recognizing something without necessarily understanding, using, or changing it. 2. Understanding: Understanding the material being communicated without necessarily relating it to anything else. 3. Applying: Using a general concept to solve a particular problem. 4. Analyzing: Breaking something down into its parts. 5. Evaluating: Judging the value of materials or methods as they might be applied in a particular situation. 6. Creating: Creating something new by combining different ideas. The 2001 revision of Bloom’s taxonomy added a new dimension—to recognize that cognitive processes must process something, you have to remember or understand or apply some form of knowledge. If you look at Table 14.3, you will see the result. We now have the six processes of remembering, understanding, applying, analyzing, evaluating, and creating acting on four kinds of knowledge—factual, conceptual, procedural, and metacognitive. Consider how this revised taxonomy might suggest learning targets for a social studies/language arts class. Here’s an example that targets analyzing conceptual knowledge: After reading a historical account of the battle of the Alamo, students will be able to explain the author’s point of view or bias. And here’s an outcome for evaluating metacognitive knowledge: Students will reflect on and describe their strategies for identifying the biases of the author. Go to https://tips.uark.edu and search for “Bloom’s taxonomy” for more explanations and examples.

Table 14.3 A Revised Taxonomy in the Cognitive Domain The revised taxonomy includes cognitive processes operating on different kinds of knowledge. The verbs in the chart are examples of what might be used to create objectives. THE COGNITIVE PROCESS DIMENSION THE KNOWLEDGE

1. REMEMBER

2. UNDERSTAND

3. APPLY

4. ANALYZE

5. EVALUATE

6. CREATE

A. Factual Knowledge

list

summarize

classify

order

rank

combine

B. Conceptual Knowledge

describe

interpret

experiment

explain

assess

plan

C. Procedural Knowledge

tabulate

predict

calculate

differentiate

conclude

compose

D. Metacognitive Knowledge

appropriate use

execute

select strategy

change strategy

reflect

invent strategy

DIMENSION

Source: Anderson\Krathwohl\Airasian\Cruikshank\Mayer\Pintrich\Raths\Wittrock. A taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing: A revision of Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives, Abridged Edition, 1st edition, © 2001. Reprinted by permission of Pearson Education, Inc. NJ.

620 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student Affective domain Objectives focusing on attitudes and feelings.

The Learning Objectives in the affective domain, or domain of emotional response, have not yet been revised from the original version. These outcomes run from least to most committed (Krathwohl et al., 1964). The affective domain has five basic Learning Objectives:

THE AFFECTIVE DOMAIN.

1. Receiving: Being aware of or attending to something in the environment. 2. Responding: Showing some new behavior as a result of experience. 3. Valuing: Showing some definite involvement or commitment. 4. Organization: Integrating a new value into your general set of values, giving it some ranking among your general priorities. 5. Characterization by value: Acting consistently with the new value. For example, an outcome for a nutrition class at the valuing level (showing involvement or commitment) might be stated: “After completing the unit on food contents and labeling, at least 50% of the class will commit to the junk food boycott project by giving up fast food for a month.” Psychomotor domain Physical ability and coordination objectives.

THE PSYCHOMOTOR DOMAIN. James Cangelosi (1990) provided a useful way to think about outcomes in the psychomotor domain, or realm of physical ability, as either (a) voluntary muscle capabilities that require endurance, strength, flexibility, agility, or speed or (b) the ability to perform a specific skill.

Here are two psychomotor outcomes: Four minutes after completing a 1-mile run in 8 minutes or under, your heart rate will be below 120. Use a computer trackpad effectively to “drag and drop” files.

The Cognitive Demands of Learning Objectives: Webb’s Depth of Knowledge Webb’s Depth of Knowledge (DOK) A taxonomy system similar to Bloom’s cognitive domain in that it moves from lower- to higher-level thinking; often is used in planning curriculum and assessments around the Common Core State Standards.

Norman Webb (2002) devised a classification system for Learning Objectives based on the cognitive demands—the complexity of the thinking involved. Complexity is not the same as difficulty. Remembering a whole page of dialogue may be difficult but not complex—you just memorize the exact words without in-depth thinking. Webb’s Depth of Knowledge (DOK) system is similar to Bloom’s cognitive domain taxonomy in that it moves from lower- to higher-level thinking. The system often is used in planning curriculum and assessments around the Common Core State Standards (Hess, 2013). Table 14.4 shows the four levels of Webb’s DOK and some examples. The Guidelines: Using Learning Objectives should help you if you use goals for every lesson or even for just a few assignments.

Planning from a Constructivist Perspective Stop & Think Think about the same course assignments you analyzed in the previous Stop & Think. What are the big ideas that run through all those assignments? What other ways could you learn about those ideas besides the assignments?

Constructivist approach See constructivism.

Traditionally, it has been the teacher’s responsibility to do most of the planning for instruction, but new ways of planning are emerging. In constructivist approaches, planning is shared and negotiated. The teacher and students together make decisions about content, activities, and approaches. Rather than having specific student behaviors and skills as outcomes, the teacher has overarching goals—big ideas, essential questions, or themes—that guide planning (Borich, 2011). These goals are understandings or abilities that the teacher returns to again and again. Since the 1990s, teaching with themes and integrated content has been a major element in planning and designing lessons

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

621

Table 14.4 Webb’s Depth of Knowledge Levels DOK LEVELS

DESCRIPTION

EXAMPLE VERBS

EXAMPLE ACTIVITIES

Recall and

Working with facts, terms, simple procedures, or formulas; no transformation of information; no “figuring it out.” You know it or you don’t.

Calculate, report, describe, tell, list, draw, define, memorize, match, retell, measure, find, recall

Describe in your own words; make a chart or timeline; spellcheck a paragraph; look up words in a dictionary; follow steps; underline new words in a sentence; perform basic calculations; brainstorm ideas for a class project

Skill/Concept

Demonstrating conceptual understanding by explaining or making a model; converting from one form of information to another; explaining why or how

Infer, estimate, interpret, distinguish, predict, compare-contrast

Multi-step calculations; explain the meaning of a concept; make a working model; design a portfolio for your work

Strategic Thinking

Using analysis and evaluation to solve real-world problems; supporting ideas with evidence; integrating knowledge from several subject matter areas to create a solution or design

Plan, differentiate, cite evidence, refute, generalize, analyze, develop

Create a database; design a website; write a short story; write and record a podcast

Strategic thinking and problem solving over time; complex planning; adjusting plans to fit new circumstances; synthesis and reflection

Design and conduct, synthesize, collaborate, self-evaluate and reflect

Create and perform a work for an audience; research a topic and write a report; develop and test multiple hypotheses and draw conclusions

Reproduce

and Reasoning

Extended Thinking

Source: Based on Hess, K. (2013). A guide for using Webb’s Depth of Knowledge with Common Core State Standards. The Common Core Institute; Popham, W. J. (2020). Classroom assessment: What teachers need to know (9th ed.). Pearson; Petit, M., & Hess, K. (2006). Applying Webb’s Depth of Knowledge and NAEP levels of complexity in mathematics. Marge Petit & Karin K. Hess, Center for Assessment. Permission to reproduce is given when authorship is fully cited [email protected]

GUIDELINES Using Learning Objectives Avoid “word magic”—phrases that sound noble and important but say very little, such as “Students will become deep thinkers.” Examples 1. 2.

Keep the focus on specific changes that will take place in the students’ knowledge and skills. Ask students to explain the meaning of the learning targets. If they can’t give specific examples of what you mean, the targets are not communicating your intentions to your students.

Suit the activities to the target. Examples 1. 2. 3.

If the goal is the memorization of vocabulary, give the students memory aids and practice exercises. If the goal is the ability to develop well-thought-out positions, consider position papers, debates, projects, or mock trials. If you want students to become better writers, give many opportunities for writing and rewriting.

Make sure your tests are related to your targets. Examples 1. 2.

Write learning targets and rough drafts for tests at the same time. Revise these drafts of tests as the units unfold and targets change. Weight the tests according to the importance of the various targets and the time spent on each.

622 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Table 14.5 Some Themes and Essential Questions for Integrated Planning for Middle and High School Students THEMES

ESSENTIAL QUESTIONS

Courage

How are acts of courage revealed in our community?

Diversity and Variation

Why is it important to understand different cultures?

Survival

How can we deal with the diminishing resources on earth?

Groups and Institutions

Why do people live in communities?

Communities of the Future

What is the future of big cities?

Communication/Language

Who is entitled to own the airways?

Human Rights and Responsibilities

What are human beings’ responsibilities about climate?

Identity/Coming of Age

How do people choose friends?

Interdependence

In what ways do animals, including humans, and plants depend on each other?

Change and Conservation

Why do people recycle or not recycle?

Probability and Prediction

How can probability be used in real life to predict the future?

Source: Based on Clarke, J. H. & Agne, R. M. (1997). Curriculum development: Interdisciplinary high school teaching. Pearson; and Thompson, G. (1991). Teaching through themes. Scholastic.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Planning Thematic Units (II, A2) Thematic learning units that integrate two or more content areas have become common in modern classrooms. Describe the principles involved in designing these activities, and explain how student learning can be assessed.

and units from kindergarten (Roskos & Neuman, 1998) through high school (Clarke & Agne, 1997). In Cluster 12, you saw an example of a whole school that used the theme of “working in harmony” as a big idea for planning across grades K through 12 (Beard, 2015). Some other topics for integrating themes with younger children are people, friendship, communications, habitats, communities, and patterns. Possibilities for older students are given in Table 14.5. For more ideas about “essential questions,” search that term on Google. Let’s assume you have some clear, valuable learning targets. You still need to decide what’s happening on Monday. You need to design teaching that is appropriate for your goals.

Module 43 Summary Research on Teaching (pp. 608–615) What methods have been used to study teaching? For years, researchers have tried to unravel the mystery of effective teaching using classroom observation, case studies, interviews, experimentation with different methods, stimulated recall (teachers view videotapes and explain their teaching), analysis of lesson transcripts, and other approaches to study teaching in real classrooms. What are the general characteristics of good teaching? A variety of teacher qualities are related to good teaching. Research suggests teachers who receive proper training and certification have more successful students. Although it is important, teacher knowledge of a subject is not sufficient for effective teaching. Thorough knowledge does lead to greater clarity and better organization, which are both tied to good teaching. Teachers who provide clear presentations and explanations tend to have students who learn more and who rate their teachers more positively. Teacher warmth, friendliness, and understanding seem to be the traits most strongly related to positive student attitudes about the teacher and the course in general. What do expert teachers know? It takes time and experience to become an expert teacher. These teachers have a rich store of well-organized knowledge about the many specific situations of teaching. This knowledge is very complex and specific to the situation, topic, students, and even

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

the individual teacher. Within the particular situation and topic, expert teachers have clear goals and take individual differences into account when planning for their students. Expert teachers also know how to be reflective practitioners—how to use their experience as a way to grow and improve in their teaching. What are some sources of teacher expectations? Sources include language or immigrant status, gender, notes from previous teachers, medical or psychological reports found in cumulative folders, ethnic background, prior knowledge about older brothers and sisters, physical characteristics, previous achievement or IQ scores, SES, parents’ status or behaviors, and the actual behaviors of the student. What are the two kinds of expectation effects, and how do they happen? The first is the self-fulfilling prophecy, in which the teacher’s beliefs about the students’ abilities have no basis in fact, but student behavior comes to match the initially inaccurate expectation. The second is a sustaining expectation effect, in which teachers are fairly accurate in their initial reading of students’ abilities and respond to students appropriately, but they do not alter their expectations to take account of any improvement. When this happens, the teacher’s unchanging expectation can sustain the student’s achievement at the expected level. In practice, sustaining effects are more common than self-fulfilling prophecy effects. What are the different avenues for communicating teacher expectations? Some teachers tend to treat students differently, depending on their own views of how well the students are likely to do. Differences in treatment toward low-expectation students may include setting less challenging lessons, focusing on lower-level learning, giving fewer choices, providing inconsistent feedback, and communicating less respect and trust. Students may behave accordingly, fulfilling teachers’ predictions or staying at an expected level of achievement.

The First Step: Planning (pp. 615–622) What are the levels of planning, and how do they affect teaching? Teachers engage in several levels of planning—by the year, term, unit, week, and day. All the levels must be coordinated. The plan determines how time and materials will be turned into activities for students. There is no single model of planning, but all plans should allow for flexibility. Planning is a creative problem-solving process for experienced teachers. It is more informal—“in their heads.” What is the Common Core? To address problems with inconsistency, lack of rigor, and redundancies in many state-level standards, beginning in 2009, the Council of Chief State School Officers (CCSSO) and the National Governors Association Center for Best Practices (NGA Center) led an effort to define consistent national standards for each grade, K–12, in two broad areas: (a) English language arts and literacy in history/social studies, science, and technical subjects and (b) mathematics. Today adoption of the Common Core is a moving target. Make sure you learn about the standards in your state. What is a learning target, and what are the three taxonomies of educational outcomes? Learning targets tell what students should know and be able to do—what successful learning would look like. Bloom and others have developed taxonomies categorizing basic objectives in the cognitive, affective, and psychomotor domains. A taxonomy encourages systematic thinking about relevant targets and ways to evaluate them. Six outcomes objectives are listed in the cognitive domain: remembering, understanding, applying, analyzing, evaluating, and creating, acting on four kinds of knowledge: factual, conceptual, procedural, and metacognitive. Outcomes in the affective domain run from least committed to most committed. Outcomes in the psychomotor domain generally move from basic perceptions and reflex actions to skilled, creative movements. A more concise way to think about Learning Objectives is to target knowledge, reasoning, performance skills, products, or dispositions. Describe constructivist planning. Planning is shared and negotiated in constructivist approaches. Rather than having specific student behaviors as objectives, the teacher has overarching goals or “big ideas” that guide planning. Integrated content and teaching with themes or essential questions are often part of the planning. Assessment of learning is ongoing and mutually shared by teacher and students.

623

624 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

MODULE 44

Teaching Every Student

Learning Objective 14.3 Discuss the appropriate uses of direct instruction, independent work, homework, questioning (particularly authentic questions), feedback, dialogue, and group discussion and explain how to use Understanding by Design to integrate Learning Objectives, evidence for reaching those outcomes, and teaching strategies. Learning Objective 14.4 Define differentiated instruction and apply this approach to teaching a diverse group of students, including how to apply the guidelines of universal design for learning in your teaching.

Teaching Approaches For many people, “teaching” means an instructor explaining material to students; lecture is a classic form, but there are many other possibilities. In this section, you will learn some basic formats for putting teaching plans into action. Of course, different methods may be more or less appropriate, depending on the subject, the students’ prior knowledge, and the particular learning goals. We begin with strategies for teaching explicit facts and concepts.

Research on Teaching Strategies As you saw earlier, there was an explosion of research on teaching in the 1970s and 1980s. This early work identified a number of general teaching strategies that were associated with student learning (Emmer et al., 1980; Good, Biddle, & Brophy, 1975; Good & Grouws, 1975; Rosenshine & Stevens, 1986). The results still stand strong today. These enduring effective general strategies include:

• Communication of clear learning goals, • Effective use of time so students have ample opportunities to learn material at the right difficulty level,

• Proactive and caring classroom management, • Clear explanations with examples/nonexamples, • Teaching that focuses on meaning and also provides guided and independent practice,

• Frequent checks for student understanding with immediate effective feedback, and • A curriculum aligned with the learning goals and the assessments used to measure those goals. The results of the early research on teaching identified a model that was related to improved student learning as assessed by standardized tests—direct instruction. Direct instruction/Explicit teaching Systematic instruction for mastery of basic skills, facts, and information.

Active teaching Teaching characterized by high levels of teacher explanation, demonstration, and interaction with students.

Explicit Teaching and Direct Instruction Barak Rosenshine and Robert Stevens (1986) called this approach direct instruction or explicit teaching. Tom Good (1983a) used the term active teaching to describe a similar approach. Researchers identified the elements of direct instruction by comparing teachers whose students learned more than expected (based on entering knowledge) with teachers whose students performed at an expected or average level. Because the focus was on traditional forms of teaching in U.S. classrooms, the research could not identify successful innovations. Effectiveness was usually defined as average improvement in standardized test scores for a whole class or school, so the results hold for large groups, but not necessarily for every student in the group. Even when the average achievement of a group improves, the achievement of some individuals may decline (Good, 1996; Shuell, 1996).

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Given these conditions, you can see that direct instruction applies best to the teaching of basic skills—clearly structured knowledge and essential skills, such as science facts, mathematics computations, reading vocabulary, and grammar rules (Rosenshine & Stevens, 1986). These skills involve tasks that are relatively unambiguous, so they can be taught step-by-step and evaluated by standardized tests. How would a teacher turn these themes into actions? Rosenshine and his colleagues (Rosenshine, 1988; Rosenshine & Stevens, 1986) identified six teaching functions based on the research on effective instruction. These could serve as a checklist or framework for teaching basic skills.

ROSENSHINE’S SIX TEACHING FUNCTIONS.

1. Review and check the previous day’s work. Reteach if students misunderstood or made errors.

625

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Teacher-Centered Instruction (II, A3) Teacher-centered instruction is often thought of as the “traditional” approach to instruction. In what situations is this instructional format most effective? What are the basic steps involved in carrying out this form of instruction?

2. Present new material. Make the purpose clear, teach in small steps, and provide many examples and nonexamples of the ideas and concepts you are teaching.

Basic skills

3. Provide guided practice. Question students, give practice problems, and listen for misconceptions and misunderstandings. Reteach if necessary. Continue guided practice until students answer about 80% of the questions correctly.

Clearly structured knowledge that is needed for later learning and that can be taught step-by-step.

4. Give feedback and correctives based on student answers. Reteach if necessary. (Remember, quality feedback is an element of most models of effective teaching.) 5. Provide independent practice. Let students apply the new learning on their own, in cooperative groups or as homework. The success rate during independent practice should be about 95%. The point is for the students to practice until the skills become overlearned and automatic—until the students are confident. Hold students accountable for the work they do—check it. 6. Review weekly and monthly to consolidate learning. Include some review items as homework. Test often, and reteach material missed on the tests. These six functions are not steps to be followed in a particular order, but all of them are elements of effective instruction. For example, feedback, review, and reteaching should occur whenever necessary and should match the abilities of the students. Also, keep in mind the age and prior knowledge of your students. For younger or the less prepared students, use more and shorter cycles of presentation, guided practice, feedback, and correctives. WHY DOES DIRECT INSTRUCTION WORK? Well-organized presentations with clear explanations

and reviews can all help students construct understandings. For example, reviews activate prior knowledge, so the student is ready to understand. Brief, clear presentations and guided practice manage students’ cognitive load and avoid taxing their working memories. Numerous examples and nonexamples that highlight similarities and differences give many pathways and associations for building networks of connected concepts. Guided practice can also give the teacher a snapshot of the students’ thinking as well as their misconceptions, so these can be addressed directly as misconceptions rather than simply as “wrong answers.” Teacher explanation is useful for communicating a large amount of material to many students in a short period of time, introducing a new topic, giving background information, or motivating students to learn more on their own. Teacher presentations are therefore most appropriate at the lower levels of the taxonomies described earlier: for remembering, understanding, applying, receiving, responding, and valuing. Every subject, even college English or chemistry, requires some direct instruction. Students may need some direct instruction in how to use various manipulative materials so they can actually learn from (not just play with) the materials. Students working in cooperative groups may need guidance, modeling, and practice in how to ask questions and give explanations. And to solve difficult problems, students may need some direct instruction in possible problem-solving strategies (Arends, 2014). EVALUATING DIRECT INSTRUCTION. Direct instruction, particularly when it involves extended teacher presentations or lectures, has some disadvantages. You may find that some students have trouble listening for more than a few minutes at a time and that they simply

626 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Table 14.6 Active Learning and Teacher Presentations Here are some ideas for keeping students cognitively engaged during teacher presentations. They can be adapted for many ages.

Scripted cooperation Learning strategy in which two students take turns summarizing material and critiquing the summaries.

Write an Answer: Pose a question, ask everyone to write a brief answer, and then call on students to share what they wrote.

Voting: Pose two alternative explanations; ask how many agree with each (may be a good idea to ask the students to close their eyes and vote so they won’t be swayed by the votes of others).

I used to think , but now I know : After a lesson, ask students to fill in the blanks and then share their results with the person beside them.

Choral Response: Have the whole class restate in unison important facts and ideas, such as “In a right triangle, a2 + b2 = c2.”

Think-Pair-Share: Pose a question, have students think of an answer on their own and then consult with a neighbor to improve the answer, and then ask volunteers to share their ideas.

One-Minute-Write: After a section of the lesson, students write for 1 minute to summarize the key points or raise a question about what is not clear to them.

tune you out. Teacher presentations can put the students in a passive position by doing much of the cognitive work for them; this may prevent students from asking or even thinking of questions (Freiberg & Driscoll, 2005). Critics also claim that direct instruction is based on the wrong theory of learning. Teachers break material into small segments, present each segment clearly, and reinforce or correct, thus transmitting accurate understandings from teacher to student. The student is viewed as an “empty vessel” waiting to be filled with knowledge rather than an active constructor of knowledge (Berg & Clough, 1991; Driscoll, 2005). Unfortunately, for younger and less prepared learners, without teacher guidance, the understandings that students construct can be incomplete and misleading (Sweller et al., 2007). But done well, direct instruction and explanation can help all students learn actively, not passively (Leinhardt, 2001). Scripted cooperation is one way of incorporating active learning into lectures. Several times during the presentation, the teacher asks students to work in pairs. One person is the summarizer, and the other critiques the summary; then they switch roles for the next summary/critique. This gives students a chance to check their understanding, organize their thinking, and translate ideas into their own words. Other possibilities are described in Table 14.6. Deanna Kuhn (2007) said it well: As for direct instruction, of course it has a place. Each young student does not need to reinvent knowledge from the ground up. The challenge is to formulate what we want direct instruction to be. In doing so, it is well to keep in mind that it is students who construct meaning from such instruction and decide what it is that they will learn. (p. 112) See the Guidelines: Effective Direct Instruction for more ideas about teaching effectively.

GUIDELINES Effective Direct Instruction Use a number of examples. Examples 1. 2.

In mathematics class, ask students to point out all the examples of right angles that they can find in the room. In teaching about islands and peninsulas, use maps, slides, models, or postcards.

Organize your lessons carefully. Examples 1. 2.

Provide objectives that help students focus on the purpose of the lesson. Begin lessons by writing a brief outline on the board, or work on an outline with the class as part of the lesson.

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

3. 4.

If possible, break the presentation into clear steps or stages. Review periodically.

Anticipate and plan for difficult parts in the lesson. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Plan a clear introduction to the lesson that tells students what they are going to learn and how they will learn it. Do the exercises and anticipate student problems—consult the teachers’ manual for ideas. Have definitions ready for new terms, and prepare several relevant examples for concepts. Think of analogies that will make ideas easier to understand. Organize the lesson in a logical sequence; include checkpoints that incorporate oral or written questions or problems to make sure the students are following the explanations.

Strive for clear explanations. Examples 1.

2. 3. 4. 5. 6.

Avoid vague words and ambiguous phrases: Steer clear of “the somes”—something, someone, sometime, somehow; “the not verys”—not very much, not very well, not very hard, not very often; and other unspecific fillers, such as most, not all, sort of, and so on, of course, as you know, I guess, in fact, or whatever, and more or less. Use specific (and, if possible, colorful) names instead of it, them, and thing. Refrain from using pet phrases such as you know, like, and Okay? Record one of your lessons to check yourself for clarity. Give explanations at several levels so all students, not just the brightest, will understand. Focus on one idea at a time, and avoid digressions.

Make clear connections by using explanatory links such as because, if–then, or therefore. Examples 1. 2.

“The North had an advantage in the Civil War because its economy was based on manufacturing.” Explanatory links are also helpful in labeling visual material such as graphs, concept maps, or illustrations.

Signal transitions from one major topic to another with phrases. Examples 1. 2.

Use wording like “The next area,” “Now we will turn to,” and “The second step is.” Outline topics, listing key points, drawing concept maps on the board, or using an overhead projector.

Communicate an enthusiasm for your subject and the day’s lesson. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Tell students why the lesson is important. Have a better reason than “This will be on the test” or “You will need to know it next year.” Emphasize the value of the learning itself. Be sure to make eye contact with the students. Vary your pace and volume in speaking. Use silence for emphasis.

Independent Work and Homework You may have heard the term seatwork, but the more current name is independent work—you know students can work many other places besides their “seats” because today they may not have assigned seats in class. INDEPENDENT WORK. Independent classroom-desk work is often overused. For example, a summary of research from 1975 to 2000 found that students with learning disabilities, who

627

628 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student often have trouble improving without teacher guidance, were spending about 40% of their time on independent work (Vaughn et al., 2002). Independent work should follow up a lesson and give students supervised practice with quick feedback. It should not be the main mode of instruction. Unfortunately, many workbook pages and worksheets do little to support the learning of important goals. Before you make an assignment, ask yourself, “Does doing this work help students learn anything that matters?” Students should be able to see the connection between the independent work and the lesson. Tell them why they are doing the work. The goals should be clear, all the materials that might be needed should be provided, and the work should be moderately difficult—doable but neither too easy nor too hard. When independent work is too easy, students are not cognitively engaged. When the work is too difficult, students often resort to guessing or copying someone else’s answers just to finish. There are several alternatives to workbooks and worksheets, such as reading silently and reading aloud to a partner; writing for a “real” audience; writing letters or journals; transcribing conversations and punctuating them properly; making up problems; working on long-term projects and reports; solving brainteasers and puzzles; and computer activities (Romano, & Weinstein, 2019). One of my favorites is creating a group story. Two students begin a story on the computer. Then two more add a paragraph. The story grows with each new pair’s addition. The students are reading and writing, editing and improving. With so many different authors, each writer may spark the creative thinking of other contributors. Any independent work requires careful monitoring. Being available to students doing the work is more effective than offering students help before they ask for it. Short, frequent contacts are best (Brophy & Good, 1986). Sometimes you may be working with a small group while other students do independent work. In these situations, it is especially important for students to know what to do if they need help. One expert teacher described by Romano and Weinstein (2019) taught students a rule, “Ask three, then me.” Students have to consult three classmates before seeking help from the teacher. This teacher also spends time early in the year showing students how to help each other—how to ask questions and how to explain. Stop & Think Think back to your elementary and high school days. Do you remember any homework assignments? What sticks in your mind about those assignments? Like so many methods in education, homework has moved in and out of favor. In the early 1900s, homework was viewed as an important path to mental discipline, but by the 1940s, it was criticized as too much drill and low-level learning. Then, in the 1950s, homework was rediscovered as a way to catch up with the Soviet Union in science and mathematics, only to be seen as putting too much pressure on students during the more laid-back 1960s. By the 1980s, homework was in again as a way to improve the standing of American children compared with students around the world (Cooper & Valentine, 2001). As our first-grade granddaughter worked on her spelling, reading, and math homework (she had assignments every day), I wondered again about the value of homework. No need to guess—educators have been studying the effects of homework for over 75 years (Cooper, 2004, 2007; Cooper et al., 2006; Epstein & Van Voorhis, 2012; Fitzmaurice et al., 2020; Flunger et al., 2015; Kalenkoski & Pabilonia, 2014; Trautwein, 2007). Most of the studies involved math and reading or English homework but not social studies, science, or other subjects. What have they shown? The Point/Counterpoint: Is Homework Valuable? gives some answers.

HOMEWORK.

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Is Homework Valuable? Were all those hours you spent doing your own homework or supervising the work of others really worth it? It depends. . . .

Point Often homework is not valuable. David Berliner and Gene Glass (2014) said it bluntly: “Let the dog eat it. Homework does not boost achievement” (p. 113). No matter how interesting an activity is, students will eventually get bored with it—so why give them work both in and out of school? They will simply grow weary of learning. And important opportunities are lost for community involvement or leisure activities that would create well-rounded citizens. When parents help with homework, they can do more harm than good—sometimes confusing their children or teaching them incorrectly. And students from poorer families often must work at jobs after school, so they miss doing the homework; then the learning discrepancy between the rich and poor grows even greater. Besides, the research is inconsistent about the effects of homework. For example, one study found that in-class work was better than homework in helping elementary students learn (Cooper & Valentine, 2001). Harris Cooper and his colleagues (2006; Harris Cooper, 2007) reviewed many studies of homework and concluded that there is little relationship between homework and learning for young students, but the relationship between homework and achievement grows progressively stronger for older students. Young students may be too distracted by elements of their home environment (toys, TV, games, siblings, etc.) and lack the metacognitive skills to focus their attention in the midst of distractions or manage their time. As a consequence of these concerns, many school districts are reviewing their homework policies to make sure homework is more meaningful and give families more time for other activities (Pinsker, 2019).

Counterpoint There is a place for homework. Most research examines the relationship between amount of time spent on homework (as reported by students or parents) and achievement in terms of grades or tests. Evidence indicates that students in high school who do more homework (and watch less television after school) have higher grades, even when other factors such as gender, grade level, ethnicity, socioeconomic status (SES), and amount of adult supervision are taken into consideration (Cooper et al., 2006; Cooper & Valentine, 2001; Cooper, Valentine et al., 1999). High school girls spend about 1 hour more per week on homework than boys, even taking into account ability, parents’ help, outside activities such as jobs, and type of coursework. One suggestion is that this additional time spent studying is a factor causing the gender gap that favors girls in college admission and completion—the girls know how to study (Gershenson & Holt, 2015). Consistent with all these findings, the National Education Association suggests a “10-minute rule,” which means 10 minutes of homework per night starting in the first grade and increasing by 10 minutes each grade, so a 12th-grader would be assigned about 120 minutes of homework per night (Walker, 2017). But here is the possible problem with just thinking of homework in terms of time: Not every student takes the same time to do the same homework. Another approach is to focus on effort instead of time. Students’ self-reported effort on homework is consistently and positively related to student achievement (Trautwein et al., 2009). “High homework effort means that a student does his or her best to solve the tasks assigned. There need not be a close relationship between effort and time on homework: A student putting as much effort as possible into a homework assignment might finish in 5 min or still be working after an hour” (Trautwein & Lüdtke, 2007, p. 432). (Continued)

629

630 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Barbara Flunger and her colleagues (2015) identified 5 types of learners in eighth grade based on their homework effort (persistence and compliance) and time spent. Fast learners invested good effort to complete homework quickly. High-effort learners invested great effort and more time in homework, while average learners invested some effort and little time. Struggling learners had trouble investing effort but spent high levels of time on homework. Then there were minimalists, who invested little effort or time. As you might predict, the fast learners and high-effort learners did significantly better over the year in terms of grades and test scores. Also, it is not surprising that students who are interested in their homework and believe it is valuable for their learning are more engaged in the work, and this engagement is associated with higher achievement (Rodríguez et al., 2019).

Beware of Either/Or Even with the changes in attitudes toward homework over the years, the homework load for middle and high school students has not changed much since the mid-1980s. Upper elementary students seem to have increased homework, however (Loveless, 2014). The real question is not about assigning homework versus not assigning homework, but rather assigning the right kind of homework to the right learners. Students are more likely to put in effort if they see the homework as interesting, valuable, reasonably challenging, and not anxiety provoking— this could require some differentiated homework assignments (Dettmers et al., 2010). So the challenge is to get students to put their best efforts into appropriate homework and to not assign homework that is low quality. Time spent struggling is not time well spent.

To benefit from homework, students must understand the assignment. It may help to do the first few questions as a class to clear up any misconceptions. This is especially important for students who may have no one at home to consult if they have problems with the assignment. A second way to keep students involved is to hold them accountable for completing the work correctly, not just for filling in the page. This means the work should be checked, the students given a chance to correct the errors or revise work, and the results counted toward the class grade. Expert teachers often have ways of correcting homework quickly during the first minutes of class by having students check each other’s or their own work. If students get stuck on homework, they need help at home, someone who can scaffold their work without just “giving the answer” (Pressley, 1995). But many families don’t know how to help (Hoover-Dempsey et al., 2001). The Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships—Homework provide ideas for helping families deal with homework. But no matter what, be prepared for some parent complaints. In a study of parents’ attitudes toward homework, 15% wanted less and 25% wanted more, but thankfully, 60% felt the amount of homework assigned was just right (Loveless, 2014).

GUIDELINES Family and Community Partnerships Homework Make sure families know what students are expected to learn. Examples 1.

2.

At the beginning of a unit, send home a list of the main objectives, examples of major assignments, key due dates, a homework “calendar,” and a list of free resources available at libraries or on the Internet. Provide a clear, concise description of your homework policy, including how homework is counted toward class grades, as well as consequences for late, forgotten, or missing homework.

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

631

Help families find a comfortable and helpful role in their child’s homework. Examples 1. 2.

Have some homework assignments that are fun and involve the whole family—puzzles, family albums, watching a television program together and doing a “review.” In conferences, ask families how you could help them to support their child in completing and learning from homework. Checklists? Background reading? Websites? Explanations of study skills?

Encourage parents to help structure the home for homework without becoming controlling and punitive or intrusive (Dumont et al., 2014). Examples 1.

2. 3. 4.

Remind families that “helping with homework” means encouraging, listening, monitoring, praising, discussing, brainstorming—not punishing, harassing, threatening, helping when help isn’t needed, or doing the work for their child. Encourage families to set aside a quiet time and place for everyone in the family to study. Make this time a regular part of the daily routine. Encourage parents to focus on modeling and supporting time management and persistence skills in doing homework: homework before play, friends, TV, games, or screen time. Make sure the needed materials are all available so the child won’t interrupt homework to find a pen or ruler.

Solicit and use suggestions from families about homework. Examples 1. 2.

Find out what responsibilities the child has at home—how much time is available for homework. Periodically, have a “homework hotline” for call-in questions and suggestions.

If no one is at home to help with homework, set up other support systems. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Assign study buddies who can be available over the phone. If students have computers, provide lists of Internet helplines. Locate free help in public libraries, and make these resources known.

Take advantage of family and community “funds of knowledge” to connect homework with life in the community and life in the community with lessons in school (Moll et al., 1992). Examples 1. 2.

Create a class lesson about how family members use math and reading in sewing and in house construction (Epstein & Van Voorhis, 2001). Design interactive homework projects that families do together to evaluate needed products for their home, for example, deciding on the best buy on shampoo or paper towels.

Questioning, Discussion, Dialogue, and Feedback Teachers pose questions, students answer. This form of teaching, sometimes called recitation, has been with us for many years (Romano & Weinstein, 2019). The pattern from the teacher’s point of view consists of IRF: Initiation (teacher asks questions), Response (student answers), and Follow-up (praising, correcting, probing, or expanding) (Burbules & Bruce, 2001; Gillies, 2016). These steps are repeated over and over. As you will see later, there are other ways of thinking about discussion, but for now let’s consider the heart of recitation—the initiation, or questioning, phase. Good questions are important for many kinds of quality teaching. Effective, authentic questions may be among the most powerful teaching tools to keep students cognitively engaged. Questions play several roles in cognition. They can help students rehearse information for effective recall. They can work to identify gaps in students’ knowledge, provoke curiosity, and nurture long-term interest. They can initiate cognitive conflict and promote the disequilibrium that results in a changed knowledge structure.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Questioning (III, C) Effective questioning skills are among the most valuable skills a teacher can possess—and among the most difficult to develop. For guidance on asking effective questions in the classroom, read Question Types (https://teaching.uchicago.edu/ resources/teaching-strategies/ asking-effective-questions/).

632 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student They can serve as cues, tips, or reminders as an expert guides a novice in a learning experience. Students as well as teachers should learn to question effectively. I tell my students that the first step in doing a good research project is asking a good question. For now, we will focus on teachers’ questions. Many of the beginning teachers I work with are surprised to discover how valuable good questions can be and how difficult they are to create. Stop & Think Think back to your most recent class. What kinds of questions did your professor ask? What sort of thinking was required to answer the questions: remembering, understanding, applying, analyzing, evaluating, or creating? How long did the professor wait for an answer?

Convergent questions Questions with only one right answer—usually factual questions or rote knowledge questions.

Divergent questions Questions that have no single correct answer.

Some educators have estimated the typical teacher asks between 30 and 120 questions an hour, or about 1,500,000 questions over a teaching career (Sadker & Sadker, 2006). What are these questions like? Many can be categorized in terms of Bloom’s taxonomy in the cognitive domain. Table 14.7 offers examples of questions at the different taxonomic levels. Another way to categorize questioning is in terms of convergent questions (only one right answer) or divergent questions (many possible answers). Questions about concrete facts are convergent: “Who ruled England in 1540?” “Who wrote the original Peter Pan?” Questions dealing with opinions or hypotheses are divergent: “In this story, which character is most like you and why?” “In 100 years, which of the past five presidents will be most admired and why?”

KINDS OF QUESTIONS.

Table 14.7 Classroom Questions for the Cognitive Domain Questions can be posed that encourage thinking at every level of Bloom’s taxonomy in the cognitive domain. Of course, the thinking required depends on what has gone before in the discussion. CATEGORY

TYPE OF THINKING EXPECTED

EXAMPLES

Remembering

Recalling or recognizing information as learned without really using or changing it

-List the capitals of. . . . -What are the six parts . . .? -Which strategy does the text say you should use here . . .?

Understanding

Demonstrating understanding of the materials without necessarily relating it to anything else

-Summarize in your own words. . . . -What does . . . . mean in this sentence? -Predict the next step . . . .

Applying

Using information to solve a problem with a single correct answer

-Classify these plants. . . . -Calculate the area of. . . . -Select the best strategy for . . . .

Analyzing

Breaking something down into parts; identifying reasons and motives; making inferences based on specific data; analyzing conclusions to see if supported by evidence

-What was the first breakthrough in . . .? The second? -Explain why Washington, D.C., was chosen. . . . -Which of the following are facts, and which are opinions . . .? -Based on your experiment, what is the chemical . . .?

Evaluating

Judging the merits of materials or methods as they might be applied in a particular situation, offering opinions, applying standards

-Rank the top 10 U.S. senators in terms of effectiveness in. . . . -Which painting do you believe to be better? Why? -Which study strategy is the best for you in . . .?

Creating

Creating something new; original thinking; original plan, proposal, design, or story

-What’s a good name for . . .? -How could we combine those two ideas? -How could we raise money for . . .? -What would the United States be like if Germany had won . . .?

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

No matter what kinds of questions you ask, be sure that some are authentic questions, meaning you as the teacher don’t already know the answer. Examples are “Have we seen this issue before?” “How is what we measure affected by the tools we use?” “In what we just read, how do we separate fact from opinion?” Authentic questions serve several functions (Kelly et al., 2018). They give some authority to students to shape the flow of discussion and connect the class topics to their own lives, so the students are more invested and engaged. Authentic questions also set expectations that everyone will get involved, not just the students who usually have the “right answers.” Finally, authentic questions make you think critically and analyze. Authentic questions open the door for deep explanations. As we have seen before, there is strong evidence that questions requiring deep explanations help students improve academic performance and enhance authentic understanding. What is a deep explanation?

ASKING AUTHENTIC QUESTIONS.

Examples of deep explanations include those that inquire about causes and consequences of historical events, motivations of people involved in historical events, scientific evidence for particular theories, and logical justifications for the steps of a mathematical proof. Examples of the types of questions that prompt deep explanations are why, why-not, how, what-if, how does X compare to Y, and what is the evidence for X? These questions and explanations occur during classroom instruction, class discussion, and during independent study. (Pashler et al., 2007, p. 29) Different patterns seem to be better for certain types of students. The best pattern for younger students and for lower-ability students of all ages is simple questions that allow a high percentage of correct answers, ample encouragement, help when the student does not have the correct answer, and praise. For high-ability students, the successful pattern includes harder questions at both higher and lower levels and more critical feedback (Berliner, 1987; Good, 1988). Whatever their age or ability, however, all students should have some experience with thought-provoking, deep authentic questions and, if necessary, help in learning how to answer them. To master critical thinking and problem-solving skills, students must have time to think about their answers. But classic research shows that teachers wait an average of only 1 second for students to answer (Rowe, 1974). When teachers learn to pose a question and then wait at least 3 to 5 seconds before calling on a student to answer, students tend to give longer answers; more students are likely to participate, ask questions, and volunteer appropriate answers; student comments involving analysis, synthesis, inference, and speculation tend to increase; and the students generally appear more confident in their answers (Sadker & Sadker, 2006). This seems like a simple improvement in teaching, but 5 seconds of silence is not that easy to handle. It takes practice. You might try asking students to jot down ideas or even discuss the question with another student and formulate an answer together. This makes the wait more comfortable and gives students a chance to think. Of course, if it is clear that students are lost or don’t understand the question, waiting longer will not help. When your question is met with blank stares, rephrase the question or ask if anyone can clarify it. However, some evidence shows that extending wait times does not affect learning in university classes, so with advanced high school students, you might conduct your own evaluation of wait time (Duell, 1994; Ingram & Elliott, 2016). A word about selecting students to answer questions. If you call only on volunteers, then you may get the wrong idea about how well students understand the material. Also, the same people volunteer over and over again. Many expert teachers have some systematic way of making sure that they call on everyone: They pull names from a jar or check names off a list as each student speaks (Romano & Weinstein, 2019; Weinstein & Novodvorsky, 2015). Another possibility is to put each student’s name on an index card and then shuffle the cards and go through the deck as you call on people. You can use the card to make notes about the quality of students’ answers or any extra help they seem to need.

FITTING THE QUESTIONS TO THE STUDENTS—AND WAITING.

633

Authentic questions Questions that the teacher does not have a predetermined answer for.

634 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student What do you do after the student answers? The most common response, occurring about 50% of the time in most classrooms, is simple acceptance—“Okay” or “Uh-huh” (Sadker & Sadker, 2006). But there are better reactions, depending on whether the student’s answer is correct, partially correct, or wrong. If the answer is quick, firm, and correct, simply accept the answer or ask another question. If the answer is correct but hesitant, give the student feedback about why the answer is correct: “That’s right, Chris, the Senate is part of the legislative branch of government because the Senate. . . .” This allows you to explain the material again. If this student is unsure, others may be confused as well. If the answer is partially or completely wrong but the student has made an honest attempt, you should probe for more information, give clues, simplify the question, review the previous steps, or reteach the material. If the student’s wrong answer is silly or careless, however, it is better simply to correct the answer and go on (Good, 1988; Rosenshine & Stevens, 1986). Simply put, feedback is information about how the student’s current performance relates to the learning goal—essential information for constructing understanding (Tan et al., 2020; van den Berg et al., 2014). John Hattie and Helen Timperley (2007) reviewed several decades of research on feedback and developed a model to guide teachers. The model proposes three feedback questions: “Where am I going?” “How am I going?” and “Where to next?” The first question is about goals and goal clarity. The second is about progress— movement toward goals. The third question is about moving forward to improve understandings when goals are not met yet or to build on attained goals. The Hattie and Timperley model also considers the focus of the feedback on four levels: task, process, self-regulation, and self-feedback. Here are some examples (p. 90):

RESPONDING TO STUDENT ANSWERS.

Task Feedback: “You need to include more about the Treaty of Versailles.” Process Feedback: “This page may make more sense if you use the strategies we talked about earlier.” Self-Regulation Feedback: “You already know the key features of the opening of an argument. Check to see whether you have incorporated them in your first paragraph.” Self-Feedback: Less effective: “That’s an intelligent response, well done.” More effective: “You must have thought deeply about that response, well done.” Hattie and Timperley argue that feedback about process and self-regulation is the most powerful because it helps students move toward deep understanding, mastery, and self-direction in learning. Feedback about self (usually praise) is common in classes but is not effective unless the praise provides information about how effort, persistence, or self-regulation moved the student forward, as in “You are terrific—you stuck with this and revised again, and now this essay makes a powerful argument.” Finally, your feedback as a teacher can include respectful inquiry to explore the student’s answer with them (Tan et al., 2020). This means that feedback is accompanied by asking questions to better understand the student’s thinking, being open to follow where the answers lead, and attentive listening—giving students time to answer, looking at them directly, calling them by name. This two-way feedback interaction process models for students how to be more metacognitive—how to think about their own thinking. Sounds like good advice in general.

Group discussion Conversation in which the teacher does not have the dominant role; students pose and answer their own questions.

GROUP DISCUSSION AND QUALITY TALK. Classrooms “are the training grounds for citizenship, and discussions afford students opportunities not only to hear diverse viewpoints, but also to substantiate their claims with evidence” (Reisman, 2015, p. 1). During a group discussion a teacher may pose questions, listen to student answers, react, and probe for more information, but in a true group dialogue, the teacher does not have a dominant role. Students ask questions, answer each other’s questions, and respond to each other’s answers. These kinds of student-centered dialogues are relatively rare, however. One study of 64 middle schools found that only 1.6 minutes per 60-minute class were devoted to these

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

discussions (Applebee et al., 2003). But no matter what kind of class discussions you have, remember the value of authentic questions that reveal students’ thinking so you and other students can understand and build on their ideas. “Ambitious teachers engage students in collaborative negotiation of ideas motivated by authentic questions. . . . Such instruction requires teachers to be adept at using students’ ideas as fuel to power classroom activity” (Kavanaugh et al., 2020). Group discussions have many advantages. Motivation and engagement can be higher. Students learn to express themselves clearly, justify opinions, and tolerate different views. Group discussion also gives students a chance to ask for clarification and the opportunity to examine their own thinking, follow personal interests, and assume responsibility by taking leadership roles in the group. Discussions are also useful when students are trying to understand difficult concepts that go against common sense. By thinking together, challenging each other, and suggesting and evaluating possible explanations, students are more likely to reach a genuine understanding (Wu et al., 2013). Of course, there are disadvantages. Class discussions are quite unpredictable and may easily digress into exchanges of ignorance. You may have to do a good deal of preparation to ensure that participants have enough background knowledge for the discussion. Some members of the group may have great difficulty participating and may become anxious if forced to speak. And large groups are often unwieldy. In many cases, a few students will dominate the discussion while the others daydream. At times teachers have to intervene to keep students focused on the goal, connected to the actual learning materials being discussed, and listening to each other (Arends, 2014; Freiberg & Driscoll, 2005; Reisman, 2015). Are discussions effective learning tools? In a major review of research conducted from 1964 to 2003 on the value of discussing texts for improving student comprehension, Karen Murphy and her colleagues (2009) reached some surprising conclusions. They examined a wide range of discussion formats including Instructional Conversations, Junior Great Books Shared Inquiry, Questioning the Author, Literature Circles, Book Club, and Grand Conversation—to name just a few. They found that many of these approaches were very successful in increasing student talk, limiting teacher talk, and promoting students’ literal interpretations of the texts they discussed. But getting students to talk more did not necessarily promote their critical thinking, reasoning, or argumentation skills. The researchers concluded, “Simply putting students into groups and encouraging them to talk is not enough to enhance comprehension and learning; it is but a step in the process” (p. 760). What happens before, during, and after the discussions are the keys. Based on the findings from their research, Murphy and her colleagues developed an approach to discussions called Quality Talk to get the most benefit from discussing texts and readings (Murphy et al., 2016, 2018). “Quality Talk (QT) is a multifaceted approach toward classroom discussions designed to increase students’ high-level comprehension by encouraging students to think and talk about, around, and with the text” (Murphy et al., 2018, p. 1120). Students meet in small groups (four to six members) to discuss the reading. The teacher selects the text and topic for discussion, but the students have control over taking turns in the discussion and over their own interpretations—the teacher is not looking for one right answer. Table 14.8 on the next page describes what happens before, during, and after the small-group discussions. The Guidelines: Productive Group Discussions on the next page give some more ideas for facilitating a productive group discussion, whether they are whole class or small group.

Lessons for Teachers: Fitting Teaching to Your Goals In the midst of all our discussions about methods, we have to keep in mind that the first questions should be: What should students learn? and What is worth knowing today? Then we can match methods to goals.

635

Quality Talk An approach to classroom discussions designed to increase students’ high-level comprehension by encouraging students to think and talk about, around, and with the text.

636 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Table 14.8 Quality Talk in Small-Group Discussion: Before, During, and After Just putting students in discussion groups does not guarantee learning. You have to prepare and have a plan before, during, and after. TIME

TEACHER

STUDENTS

Before the small-group discussion

Provides mini lessons explicitly teaching how to ask and respond to authentic questions in critical and analytic ways and how to make evidence-based arguments and claims

-Read text -Complete activities in their journal to be sure they understand the key features (e.g., main idea) -Craft some authentic questions about the text

During the small-group discussion

-Facilitates discussion by encouraging students to search the text for arguments, assumptions, beliefs, themes, etc. -Models critical and analytic thinking -Challenges and prompts students to provide evidence, make personal connections -Gradually turns over more responsibility to the group as they improve

-Encourage each other to talk about personal connections to the text -Retrieve and revisit basic information from the text -Gradually take over control of turn-taking and the authority to make and challenge individual interpretations of the text

After the small-group discussion

-Gives feedback about the discussions, possible improvements in the process for next time, challenges any misconceptions -Checks for understanding using appropriate assessments and activities

-Complete post-discussion activities in their journals that require critical thinking and analysis -Set goals for the next group discussion

Source: Based on Murphy, P. K., Firetto, C. M., Wei, L., Li, M., & Croninger, R. M. V. (2016). What REALLY works: Optimizing classroom discussions to promote comprehension and critical-analytic thinking. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 27–35; Murphy, P. K., Greene, J. A., Firetto, C. M., Hendrick, B. D., Li, M., Montalbano, C., & Wei, L. (2018). Quality talk: Developing students’ discourse to promote high-level comprehension. American Educational Research Journal, 55, 1113–1160.

GUIDELINES Productive Group Discussions Make sure students are prepared to discuss. Examples 1.

2.

“Tomorrow we will discuss the first chapter in your novel. Bring a list with one thing that wasn’t clear to you, one question that will make others think really deeply, and one connection to your life.” “Tonight write a one paragraph summary of Chapter 3 for tomorrow’s discussion. Tomorrow before the discussion you will swap paragraphs and share feedback.”

Invite shy children to participate. Examples 1. 2.

“What’s your opinion, Jazmin?” or “Does anyone have another opinion?” Don’t wait until there is a deadly silence to ask shy students to reply. Most people, even those who are confident, hate to break a silence.

Direct student comments and questions back to another student. Examples 1. 2. 3.

“That’s an unusual idea, Kolter. Rosa, what do you think of Kolter’s idea?” “That’s an important question, Mei. Alejandro, do you have any thoughts about how you’d answer that?” Encourage students to look at and talk to one another rather than wait for your opinion.

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

637

Make sure that you understand what a student has said. If you are unsure, other students may be unsure as well. Examples 1. 2.

Ask a second student to summarize what the first student said; then the first student can try again to explain if the summary is incorrect. “Karen, I think you’re saying. . . . Is that right, or have I misunderstood?”

Probe for more information, and ask students to elaborate and defend their positions. Examples 1. 2. 3.

“That’s a strong statement. Do you have any evidence to back it up?” “Did you consider any other alternatives?” “Tell us how you reached that conclusion. What steps did you go through?”

Bring the discussion back to the subject. Examples 1. 2.

“Let’s see, we were discussing . . . and Jada made one suggestion. Does anyone have a different idea?” “Before we continue, let me try to summarize what has happened thus far.”

Give time for thought before asking for responses. Example 1.

“How would your life be different if television had never been invented? Jot down your ideas on paper, and we will share reactions in a minute.” After a minute: “Hiromi, will you tell us what you wrote?”

When a student finishes speaking, look around the room to judge reactions. Examples 1. 2.

If other students look puzzled, ask them to describe why they are confused. If students are nodding assent, ask them to give an example of what was just said.

There is no one best way to teach. Different goals and student needs require different teaching methods at different times. Direct instruction often leads to better performance on achievement tests, whereas the open, informal methods such as discovery learning or inquiry approaches discussed in Cluster 10 are associated with better performance on tests of creativity, abstract thinking, and problem solving. In addition, the open methods are better for improving attitudes toward school and for stimulating curiosity, cooperation among students, and lower absence rates (Borich, 2011; Walberg, 1990). According to these conclusions, when the goals of teaching involve problem solving, creativity, understanding, and mastering processes, many approaches besides direct instruction should be effective. Every student may require direct, explicit teaching for some learning goals some of the time, but all students also need to experience more open, constructivist, student-centered teaching as well (Zhang et al., 2021).

Putting It All Together: Understanding by Design We have covered quite a bit of territory here, from objectives to teaching strategies. Grant Wiggins and Jay McTighe’s (2006) Understanding by Design (UbD) pulls it all together— expectations for high-level critical thinking, goals, evidence for learning, and teaching approaches. The focus is on deep understanding, which is characterized by the ability to (a) explain, (b) interpret, (c) apply, (d) have perspective, (e) empathize, and (f) have self-knowledge about a topic. The big idea behind UbD is backward design. Teachers first identify the important end results for students—the key understandings and big ideas that are the goals of instruction. To focus on understanding (not just fun activities or covering the text), teachers write essential questions—questions that go to the heart of the ideas and push thinking deeper: “What is the greatest problem of the democratic system?” “Who is entitled to own the airways?” “What makes a mathematical argument convincing?” Next the teacher identifies what evidence would demonstrate deep understanding (performance

Understanding by Design (UbD) A system of lesson and unit planning that starts with key objectives for understandings and then moves backward to design assessments and learning activities.

638 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student tasks, quizzes, informal assessments?). Then and only then do the teachers design the learning plan—the instruction—they design backward from the end results to the teaching plan. This idea of going from clear goals to teaching plans is at the heart of every approach to using standards and specific targets in teaching. Wiggins and McTighe provide a template to guide backward design planning. You can see many examples of these templates online. Do a web search for “Understanding by Design template.” The process for completing a plan using backward design for a unit is shown in Figure 14.1. The teacher/designer moved backward from a core mathematics

Figure 14.1 Planning by Design The planning process for a lesson on the Pythagorean Theorem the teacher/designer planned backward from the core standards. Step 1—Designing the Goal Apply the Pythagorean Theorem to determine unknown side lengths in right triangles in real-world and mathematical problems in two and three dimensions. Common Core State Standards in Mathematics, Grade 8.G.B.7

Key Understandings 1. The area formed by the square on top of the hypotenuse of a right triangle is equal to the total area of the two squares formed on the tops of the other two sides. 2. There are multiple ways to prove the Pythagorean Theorem.

Essential Questions 1. What makes a mathematical argument of the Pythagorean Theorem convincing?

3.….

3.….

2. Are there any real world uses of the Pythagorean Theorem?

What Will the Student Know? 1. What is a hypotenuse of a right triangle? 2. What is the length of any side of a right triangle, given the two other sides? 3. ….. What Will the Student Be Able to Do? 1. Draw a graphic illustration that demonstrates the validity of the Pythagorean Theorem. 2. ……

Step 2—Designing the Assessment

Authentic/Real-World Assessments 1. Can you calculate the height of a flagpole based on its shadow—how? 2. You have an old media cabinet with an opening that is 34” by 34”. You want a new flat screen TV that is at least a 42” diagonal. Assume the ratio of a new TV height to width is 3/5. Will it fit? Why? 3. Given that the distance between successive bases in baseball is 90 feet, what is the distance a throw has to travel from third base and first base? 4…..

Traditional Assessments 1. Questions on homework 2. Self-questions developed based on chapter, along with answers and justifications. 3. Unit test 4…..

Step 3—Designing the Learning 1. In groups, investigate the area of squares, triangles, and rectangles around the classroom—compare your group’s areas with the areas calculated by other groups for the same objects. 2. Using cardboard pieces, scissors, a ruler, and pencil, prove the Pythagorean Theorem. 3. Modules 6 & 7 in text

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

639

standard by identifying key understandings and essential questions based on the standard, then planning assessments including traditional tests as well as textbook assignments and performance tasks that include real-life applications, and finally creating learning experiences that will support understanding. Can you see how many levels of Bloom’s taxonomy are represented here? So far, we have talked about approaches to teaching—targets, strategies, and learning plans. But in today’s diverse classrooms, one size does not fit all. Teachers have to fit their instruction to the needs and abilities of their students—they have to differentiate instruction.

Differentiated Instruction The idea of differentiated instruction—adapting teaching to the abilities and needs of each learner—is an ancient one. To prove it, Lyn Corno (2008, p. 161) quotes these words of Quintilian from the fifth century b.c.: Some students are slack and need to be encouraged; others work better when given a freer rein. Some respond best when there is some threat or fear; others are paralyzed by it. Some apply themselves to the task over time, and learn best; others learn best by concentration and focus in a single burst of energy. Obviously, Quintilian appreciated the need for fitting instruction to the student. With the increasingly diverse students in today’s classrooms, differentiating instruction is more important than ever. Kelly Puzio and his colleagues note that teachers who successfully differentiate “focus on clear conceptual goals; consider a wide variety of assessment data; carefully plan lessons and units considering students’ needs, preferences, and strengths; and flexibly adapt the curriculum and instruction to suit his or her students” (Puzio et al., 2020, p. 460). Reviewing 20 years of work on differentiation in literacy classes, these researchers concluded that when teachers are supported in differentiating, their students have significantly higher literacy achievement—writing is especially improved. Teachers can plan for student differences in advance by designing different levels of support and scaffolding for the tasks, texts, and tools that students will encounter—called designed differentiation. But even with good planning, some differentiation happens on the fly to adapt during teaching—called interactional differentiation. There are three targets for designed differentiation, content, process, and product. Here are a few examples (Puzio et al., 2020): Content: Different types of texts (books, films, maps, graphic novels); modified texts (the teacher pre-highlights key passages); more or less difficult material

Differentiated instruction A flexible approach to teaching that matches content, process, and product based on student differences in readiness, interests, and learning needs. Takes into account students’ abilities, prior knowledge, and challenges so that instruction matches not only the subject being taught but also students’ needs.

Process: Task differentiation (write a one-paragraph summary versus a one-page critique); different tools (text to speech software, dictation, sentence starters, concept mapping software) Product: Different compositions (write a new ending to the story, do a character analysis, design a web page); different performances (present a tableau, retell the story with added imagery) Many approaches to differentiation rely on appropriate groupings.

Within-Class and Flexible Grouping It is not unusual to have 3- to 5-year ability differences in any given classroom (Castle et al., 2005). But even if you decided to simply forge ahead (against Quintilian’s advice) and teach the same material in the same way to your entire class, you would not be alone. Differences in students’ prior knowledge are a major challenge for teachers, especially in subjects that build on previous knowledge and skills such as math and science. One answer has been ability grouping, but that also poses a number of problems.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® The Teacher’s Role in StudentCentered Instruction (II, A3) The teacher’s role in student-centered instruction is significantly different from that in teacher-centered instruction.

640 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student Students in many classes and schools are grouped by ability, even though there is no definitive evidence that this within-class ability grouping is superior to other approaches (Becker et al., 2014). In a random sample of primary grade teachers in the United States, 63% reported using within-class ability groups for reading. Students in lower-ability groups were less likely to be asked critical comprehension questions and were given fewer opportunities to make choices about what to read (Chorzempa & Graham, 2006). For schools with students from lower SES families, grouping often means that these students are segregated into lower-ability tracks. According to Paul George (2005):

THE PROBLEMS WITH ABILITY GROUPING.

Within-class ability grouping System of grouping in which students in a class are divided into two or three groups based on ability in an attempt to accommodate student differences.

In my 3 decades of experience with this issue, when homogenous grouping is the primary strategy for organizing students in schools with significant racial and ethnic diversity in the population, the result is almost always deep, and often starkly obvious, division of students on the basis of race, ethnicity, and social class. (p. 187) Thoughtfully constructed and well-taught ability groups in math and reading can be effective, but the point of any grouping strategy should be to provide appropriate challenge and support—that is, to reach children within their zone of proximal development, that area where students can learn and develop (Vygotsky, 1997). In other words, to be effective, different groups require appropriate adjustments in content, process, and product for the groups (Kulik & Kulik, 1997). Flexible grouping is one possible answer. Flexible grouping Grouping and regrouping students based on learning needs.

In flexible grouping, students are grouped and regrouped based on their learning needs. Assessment is continuous so that students are always working within their zone of proximal development. Arrangements might include small groups, partners, individuals, and even the whole class—depending on which grouping best supports each student’s learning of the particular academic content. Flexible grouping approaches include high-level instruction and high expectations for all students, regardless of their group placement (Corno, 2008). One 5-year longitudinal study of flexible grouping in a high-needs urban elementary school found 10% to 57% increases in students who reached mastery level, depending on the subject area and grade level. Teachers received training and support in the assessment, grouping, and teaching strategies needed, and by the end of the study, 95% of the teachers were using flexible grouping. The teachers in the study believed that some of the gains came because students were more focused on learning and more confident (Castle et al., 2005). Do a web search for “flexible grouping” to find many ideas and examples. As we have seen repeatedly throughout this text, working at a challenging level but one you can master with effort and support is more likely to encourage learning and motivation. If you ever decide to use flexible grouping in your class, the Guidelines: Using Flexible Grouping should make the approach more effective (Arends, 2014; Good & Brophy, 2008). FLEXIBLE GROUPING.

GUIDELINES Using Flexible Grouping Form and re-form groups based on accurate diagnosis of students’ current performance in the subject being taught. Examples 1. 2.

Use scores on the most recent reading assessments to establish reading groups and rely on current math performance to form math groups. Assess continuously. Change group placement frequently when students’ achievement changes.

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

641

Make sure different groups get appropriately different instruction, not just the same material. Make sure teachers, methods, and pace are adjusted to fit the needs of the group. Examples 1. 2. 3.

4. 5.

Vary more than pace; fit teaching to students’ interests and knowledge. Assign all groups research reports, but make some written, and others oral or PowerPoint® presentations. Organize and teach groups so that low-achieving students get appropriate extra instruction—not just the same material again. Make lower-achieving groups smaller so students get extra attention. Make sure all work is meaningful and respectful—no worksheets for lower-ability groups while the higher-ability groups do experiments and projects. Try alternatives. For example, DeWayne Mason and Tom Good (1993) found that supplementing whole-class instruction in math with remediation and enrichment for students when they needed it worked better than dividing the class into two ability groups and teaching these groups separately.

Discourage comparisons between groups, and encourage students to develop a whole-class spirit. Examples 1. 2.

Don’t seat groups together outside the context of their reading or math group. Avoid naming ability groups—save the names for mixed-ability or whole-class teams.

Group by ability for one or at the most, two subjects. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Make sure that many lessons and projects mix members from the groups. Experiment with learning strategies that stress cooperation (described in Cluster 10). Keep the number of groups small (two or three at most) so that you can provide as much direct teaching as possible. Leaving students alone for too long leads to less learning.

Stop & Think When you think about teaching in an inclusive classroom, what are your concerns? Do you have enough training? Will you get the support you need from school administrators or specialists? Will working with the students with disabilities take time away from your other responsibilities?

Differentiated Instruction in Inclusive Classrooms The Stop & Think questions are common ones, and sometimes such concerns are justified. But effective teaching for students with disabilities, and all students for that matter, does not require a unique set of skills. It is a combination of good teaching practices and sensitivity to all your students. All your students need to learn the academic material, and they need to be full participants in the day-to-day life of the classroom. Assistive technologies can help. The Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA) requires that all students eligible for special education services be considered for assistive technology. Assistive technology is any product, piece of equipment, or system that is used to increase, maintain, or improve the functional capabilities of individuals with disabilities (Goldman et al., 2006). For students who require small steps and many repetitions to learn a new concept, computers are the perfect patient tutors, repeating steps and lessons as many times as necessary. A well-designed computer instructional program is engaging and interactive— two important qualities for students who have problems paying attention or a history of

ASSISTIVE TECHNOLOGY.

Assistive technology Devices, systems, and services that support and improve the capabilities of individuals with disabilities.

642 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student failure that has eroded motivation. For example, a math or spelling program might use images, sounds, and gamelike features Here are a few websites to start your search. to maintain the attention of a student with an attention-deficit disorder. Interactive digital media programs teach hearing peoAT RESOURCES ple how to use sign language. Many programs do not involve For information about types and training in AT, go these websites: sound, so students with hearing impairments can get the full • Assistive Technology Industry Association: benefit from the lessons. Students who have trouble reading can https://www.atia.org/home/at-resources/what-is-at/ use programs that will “speak” a word for them if they touch • Edutopia (http://www.edutopia.org; click “topics”; click “all topics”; click “Assistive Technology”) the unknown word. With this immediate access to help, these • International Society for Technology in Education (http://www students are much more likely to get the reading practice they .ISTE.org • The Iris Center (https://Iris.peabody.vanderbilt.edu/module/at) need to prevent falling further and further behind. Other de• Microsoft Company (http://www.microsoft.com; search vices actually convert printed pages and typed texts to spoken ”assistive technology”) words for students who are blind or others who benefit from • National Center on Accessible Educational Materials (http:// hearing information. For the student with a learning disability aem.cast.org) • Types of assistive technologies: https://www.nichd.nih.gov/ whose writing is not legible, word processors produce perfect health/topics/rehabtech/conditioninfo/device penmanship so the ideas can finally get on paper. Once the Source: Based on Friend, M. & Bursuck, W. D. (2019). Including students with special ideas are recorded, students can reorganize and improve their needs: A practical guide for classroom teachers (8th ed.).Hoboken, NJ: Pearson, p. 170. writing without the agony of rewriting by hand (Hallahan et al., 2019). Figure 14.2 is a list of resources for assistive technology. Things are changing so fast, see if your school has a specialist to provide support about assistive technologies that might help your students.

Figure 14.2 Resources for Assistive Technologies

Reaching Every Student: Differentiation with Universal Design for Learning

Universal design Considering the needs of all users in the design of new tools, learning programs, or websites.

Universal design for learning (UDL) Strategies and structures to make learning accessible to as many students as possible in inclusive settings.

With these tremendous advances in technology have come new barriers, however. Many computers have graphic interfaces. Manipulating the programs requires precise mouse or track pad movements, as you may remember when you first learned to point and click. These maneuvers are often difficult for students with motor problems or visual impairments. And the information available on the Internet often is unusable for students with visual problems. Researchers are working on the problem—trying to devise ways for people to access the information nonvisually. In architecture, the concept of universal design addresses these concerns by considering the needs of all users in the design of new buildings, tools, learning programs, websites, and so on. The same principle of design for everyone in education is called unive rsal design for learning (UDL). The principles of UDL are to provide multiple ways to engage with the learning process, multiple ways to represent or present the learning materials, and multiple ways for students to act and express their understanding. The Center for Applied Special Technology (CAST) has been the leader in developing the field of universal design for learning. Their Guidelines for Universal Design for Learning are used around the world. Table 14.9 is a summary of those guidelines. You can find an interactive version at https://udlguidelines.cast.org (CAST, 2018; Hallahan et al., 2019; Pisha & Coyne, 2001). You can see how these guidelines pull together many of the topics in this book.

• Motivation and the “Why” of learning, including interest, effort, autonomy support, mastery goals, and self-regulation (Clusters 11 and 12).

• Representations and “What” is learning, including verbal and visual representations, languages, symbols, big ideas, patterns (Clusters 6, 8, and 9).

• Expressing learning and the “How” of learning, including different ways of responding, multiple tools, goal setting, learning strategies, monitoring and managing learning (Clusters 5, 7, 13, and 14).

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Table 14.9 Guidelines for Universal Design for Learning (UDL) CAST

The Universal Design for Learning Guidelines

Provide multiple means of

Provide multiple means of

Provide multiple means of

Engagement

Representation

Action & Expression

Build

Access

Affective Networks The “WHY” of Learning

Internalize

Until learning has no limits

Recognition Networks The “WHAT” of Learning

Strategic Networks The “HOW” of Learning

Provide options for

Provide options for

Provide options for

Recruiting Interest

Perception

Physical Action

• Optimize individual choice and autonomy • Optimize relevance, value, and authenticity • Minimize threats and distractions

• Offer ways of customizing the display of information • Offer alternatives for auditory information • Offer alternatives for visual information

• Vary the methods for response and navigation • Optimize access to tools and assistive technologies

Provide options for

Provide options for

Provide options for

Sustaining Effort & Persistence

Language & Symbols

Expression & Communication

• Heighten salience of goals and objectives • Vary demands and resources to optimize challenge • Foster collaboration and community • Increase mastery-oriented feedback

• Clarify vocabulary and symbols • Clarify syntax and structure • Support decoding of text, mathematical

• Use multiple media for communication • Use multiple tools for construction and composition • Build fluencies with graduated levels of support for

notation, and symbols • Promote understanding across languages • illustrate through multiple media

practice and performance

Provide options for

Provide options for

Provide options for

Self-Regulation

Comprehension

Executive Functions

Promote expectations and beliefs that optimize motivation • Facilitate personal coping skills and strategies • Develop self-assessment and reflection

• Activate or supply background knowledge • Highlight patterns, critical features, big ideas,

• Guide appropriate goal-setting • Support planning and strategy development • Facilitate managing information and resources • Enhance capacity for monitoring progress



and relationships

• Guide information processing and visualization • Maximize transfer and generalization

Source: CAST (2018). Universal Design for Learning Guidelines version 2.2. Retrieved from http://udlguidelines.cast.org.

Module 44 Summary Teaching Approaches (pp. 624–639) How should teachers use direct instruction and homework? Direct instruction is appropriate for teaching basic skills and explicit knowledge. It includes the teaching functions of review/ overview, presentation, guided practice, feedback and correctives (with reteaching if necessary), independent practice, and periodic reviews. The younger or less able the students, the shorter the presentation should be, with more cycles of practice and feedback. The “10-minute rule” for homework means 10 minutes of homework per night starting in the first grade and increasing by 10 minutes each grade, so a 12th-grader would be assigned about 120 minutes. Distinguish between convergent and divergent questions and describe deep questions. Convergent questions have only one right answer. Divergent questions have many possible answers. Deep questions (such as why, why not, how, what if, how does X compare to Y, and what is the evidence for?) require thoughtful and well-reasoned explanations—students have to think for themselves. The best pattern for younger students and for lower-ability students of all ages is simple questions that allow a high percentage of correct answers, ample encouragement, help when the student does not have the correct answer, and praise. For high-ability students, the successful pattern includes harder questions at both higher and lower levels and more critical feedback. Whatever their age or ability, all students should have some experience with deep, thought-provoking questions and, if necessary, help in learning how to answer them.

643

644 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student How can wait time affect student learning? When teachers pose a question and then learn to wait at least 3 to 5 seconds before calling on a student to answer, students tend to give longer answers; more students are likely to participate, ask questions, and volunteer appropriate answers; student comments involving analysis, synthesis, inference, and speculation tend to increase; and the students generally appear more confident in their answers. What are the uses and disadvantages of group discussion? Group discussion helps students participate directly, express themselves clearly, justify opinions, and tolerate different views. Group discussion also gives students a chance to ask for clarification, examine their own thinking, follow personal interests, and assume responsibility by taking leadership roles in the group. So group discussions help students evaluate ideas and synthesize personal viewpoints. However, discussions are quite unpredictable and may easily digress into exchanges of ignorance. Quality Talk is one way to increase students’ high-level comprehension of texts and support personal engagement with the text. To use this approach, teachers have to plan carefully for what will happen before, during, and after the small-group discussions at the heart of the strategy. How can you match teaching to your goals? Different goals and student needs require different teaching methods. Direct instruction often leads to better performance on achievement tests, but the open, informal methods such as discovery learning or inquiry approaches are associated with better performance on tests of creativity, abstract thinking, and problem solving. In addition, the open methods are better for improving attitudes toward school and for stimulating curiosity, cooperation among students, and lower absence rates. How can you use Understanding by Design to plan quality instruction? The focus of UbD is on deep understanding, which is characterized by the ability to (a) explain, (b) interpret, (c) apply, (d) have perspective, (e) empathize, and (f) have self-knowledge about a topic. The big idea behind UbD is backward design. Teachers first identify the important end results for students—the key understandings and big ideas that are the goals of instruction. To focus on understanding (not just fun activities or covering the text), teachers write essential questions—questions that go to the heart of the ideas and push thinking deeper. The UbD template guides planning based on these considerations.

Differentiated Instruction (pp. 639–643) What characterizes differentiated instruction? A flexible approach to teaching that matches content, process, and product based on student differences in readiness, interests, and learning needs. Takes into account students’ abilities, prior knowledge, and challenges so that instruction matches not only the subject being taught but also students’ needs. What are the problems with ability grouping? Academic ability groupings can have disadvantages and advantages for students and teachers. Students in higher-ability groups may benefit, but students in lower-ability groups are less likely to be asked critical comprehension questions and are given fewer opportunities to make choices about readings and assignments. For schools with students from lower SES families, grouping often means that these students are segregated even in their own classes, so ability grouping can create segregation within diverse schools. What are the alternatives available for grouping in classes, including flexible grouping? In flexible grouping, students are grouped and regrouped based on their learning needs. Assessment is continuous so that students are always working within their zone of proximal development. Arrangements might include small groups, partners, individuals, and even the whole class— depending on which grouping best supports each student’s learning of the particular academic content. Within-class ability grouping, if handled sensitively and flexibly, can have positive effects, but alternatives such as cooperative learning may be better. What characterizes effective teaching for students with disabilities? Effective teaching for students with disabilities does not require a unique set of skills. It is a combination of good teaching practices and sensitivity to all students. Students with disabilities need to learn the academic material, and they need to be full participants in the day-to-day life of the classroom.

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

645

To accomplish the first goal, students with learning disabilities benefit from using extended practice distributed over days and weeks and from advanced organizers such as focusing students on what they already know or stating clear learning targets. Universal design for learning (UDL) is a possibility for accomplishing the second goal of fully including students in the day-to-day life of the classroom. The principles of UDL are to provide multiple ways to engage with the learning process, multiple ways to represent or present the learning materials, and multiple ways for students to act and express their understanding.

Cluster 14 Review Key Terms Ambitious teaching Active teaching Affective domain Assistive technology Authentic questions Basic skills Cognitive domain Constructivist approach Convergent questions Differentiated instruction Direct instruction/Explicit teaching Divergent questions Expert teachers Flexible grouping Group discussion

Lesson Study Pedagogical content knowledge (PCK) Psychomotor domain Pygmalion effect Quality Talk Reflective Scripted cooperation Self-fulfilling prophecy Sustaining expectation effect Taxonomy Understanding by Design (UbD) Universal design Universal design for learning (UDL) Webb’s Depth of Knowledge (DOK) Within-class ability grouping

Connect and Extend to Licensure Multiple-Choice Questions 1.

2.

Direct instruction is best used when teachers do which one of the following?

D. 3.

Require a parent signature to ensure collaboration with family.

Homework has long been a staple of education. For students to gain the most from their homework experience, ALL BUT which one of the following suggestions should be followed?

Ellen Baker knew that her new job in a middle school would require that she understand differentiated instruction. Using this strategy, her students would be more apt to progress and master the concepts they needed to succeed throughout their school years. One of the techniques she decided to use involved grouping students by their learning needs. She would group students who had scored poorly on their fractions tests with similar students to remediate and develop that skill. She likened this type of differentiated instruction to having students continually work in their zone of proximal development. The type of strategy Ellen Baker wants to use is referred to as which one of the following?

A.

Establish that students understand the assignment.

A.

Flexible grouping

B.

Hold students accountable for completing the work correctly.

B.

Jigsaw

C.

C.

Collaborative group work

Check students’ work and allow for corrections and revisions.

D.

Peer tutoring

A.

Teach basic skills

B.

Have their students explore numerous pathways to solve a mathematics problem

C.

Encourage their students to refine their creativity in art

D.

Assign critical thinking exercises

646 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student 4.

Teachers sometimes make determinations about their students’ abilities based on little evidence. When teachers expect their students will not do well, their words and actions can make that expectation come true regardless of the validity. This effect is referred to as which one of the following? A.

Self-fulfilling prophecy

B.

The zone of proximal development

C.

Professional license

D.

Supportive withdrawal

Constructed-Response Questions Case Although Casey Yost had done well in her college classes, she was having a difficult time with her student teaching.

Her mentor teacher continually scolded her for not correctly writing her objectives and rushing through lessons. Casey did not understand how she could both make her lessons clear and manage to cover the material necessary for the students’ upcoming standardized tests. “Casey, if your students don’t understand the material, it won’t make any difference if you cover the material or not. Let’s review one of your objectives for the upcoming lesson. ‘Students will understand fractions.’ This objective is too general. How can you measure if your students ‘understand’? You need to select words that correspond to specific actions that you can observe or measure. Let’s try to develop a few objectives that are more specific.” 5.

In what ways can Casey Yost avoid writing objectives that are too general?

6.

List several strategies that Casey can employ during instruction to teach her students more effectively.

What Would They Do? Teachers’ Casebook: Reaching and Teaching Every Student At the beginning of this cluster, we asked you to think critically about how to deal with the wide range of reading levels, family incomes, learning strengths, and learning challenges in your classroom. Now, as you consider these expert teachers’ responses to the case, we again suggest that you think critically. Do the following responses reflect (or contradict) any of the concepts discussed in this cluster? Do the suggestions and solutions described here make sense in light of what you have learned in this or other classes about best practices? How do these ideas align with or challenge your personal philosophy of teaching?

Amy Pochodylo • 9th–12th-Grade Science Teacher Buckeye Valley High School, Delaware, OH When differentiating instruction, I always start by thinking about the learning targets being addressed and the ways that students could show mastery of those targets. Whenever possible, I like to also differentiate assessment, so that it is accessible in a variety of ways to students as well. A great framework that I have found for differentiation is the layered classroom (https://www.help4teachers.com/how.htm). To help differentiate for different reading levels, I’ve used interactive reading guides (where students work with a partner and discuss the reading in a structured activity as they go), leveled texts (great technology tools like Newsela help with this), and offered independent reading guides to help students determine what information they should be looking for in a text. I also try to provide access to content in multiple formats, such as by offering a reading, a video, and an opportunity for small-group instruction whenever possible.

I also differentiate the formative assessment opportunities. I build in independent work days where students can revisit topics and self-assess their knowledge, choosing to complete options such as edPuzzle video questions, online lab simulations, or other activities. In order to make sure that students are meeting learning goals, I also build in whole-class activities, such as hands-on labs, so that I can assess student knowledge and address common misconceptions with the whole class. Periodic whole-class activities give us a common experience to draw on as well when it is time for assessment. When it is time for a summative assessment, I often include choice for students in open-ended questions, so they can decide which questions to answer to best demonstrate their knowledge. It can also be useful to allow them to answer in a variety of ways, including written responses, typed responses, or verbal responses (technology tools like Flipgrid make it easy for students to record their responses).

Anne Liners • 9th–12th-Grade English/Language Arts Student Teacher South High School, Minneapolis, MN A constructivist approach to teaching is incredibly helpful for accommodating diverse learners. Students who are able to make their own meaning from the course content will naturally bring the material into their own Zone of Proximal Development. For example, assigning a research inquiry project might mean that some students find peer-reviewed sources from academic databases while others struggle through articles written at a fourth-grade reading level. As long as both of those students—and everyone in between—are challenging themselves, practicing research skills along with reading skills,

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

and learning new content, then everybody wins. Assessments should look for this progress. Most importantly, teachers must be intentional not to create a hierarchy within the diversity. Even in my English classes, I can’t assume that better reader means smarter kid. A student with a learning disability that obstructs their math abilities might be a brilliant saxophone player or babysitter or chef. And students who struggle with English are often made to feel ‘dumb’ next to their native-speaker classmates, but they have another whole language worth of knowledge. To accommodate all students, a teacher must draw out these strengths. A diverse classroom offers the opportunity for an exchange of the wide range of knowledge that all of these kids bring to school—not just the high achieving ones. Students in diverse schools don’t just learn the content, they learn the languages, cultures, struggles, and skills of their classmates. Planning opportunities for students to guide how and what they learn will help teachers take advantage of the diverse brilliance of their classes.

647

personalized learning, communicate with their classmates and families, and assess and evaluate their learning. All students have access to the adaptive learning abilities of this technology and use this to control the challenge of an assignment. Assessment occurs on a daily basis. I am constantly observing students to understand them as learners. We run through each stage of an informal inquiry process to inform private discussions I have with students to assess their learning. I collect student work samples, give exit tasks, play games, and perform various other methods of formative assessment to share with students in order to prompt them to talk about what and how they have learned, and how far they have come in their learning. Together we use these discussions to revise or create new learning goals and develop and act upon hunches and ideas about supports they need to achieve their goals. I find it very rare that students empowered with their own learning need coaching to accurately assess their work. Often, they have much deeper insights into their learning than I ever could.

Melissa Roy • 10th-Grade Reading/English Teacher Lisa White • 2nd-/3rd-Grade Combined and 3rd-/4th-Grade Combined Elementary Teacher Moody Elementary School, Port Moody, British Columbia, Canada I very highly personalize instruction to meet the diverse needs of students. Primarily, I build a classroom environment purposefully around the practices known to promote self-regulated learning (SRL). In addition, I attend to practices that support social-emotional learning (SEL), executive functioning, and self-determination. I am proud that this allows for students of all abilities and needs to be part of our rich and respectful learning community. A foundation of SEL is essential in creating a calm and caring learning environment. My students and I build a common language around SEL, executive function, and self-determination knowledge, skills, and concepts, which are reviewed regularly and as needed throughout the year. I use opportunities I have to coach students through the curriculum, or a productive struggle, to teach them how to learn in various contexts. I focus on teaching the skills, strategies, and mindsets that we all need in order to be successful humans. Students are given control over challenges as they choose what and how they will learn, where and when they will learn it, across the curriculum with the guidelines we build together. We work as a community and as individuals. Students are encouraged to reflect on their own successes and struggles, then receive feedback and support from their peers. Students have learned to coach each other through struggles and celebrate one another’s successes. Technology has become an essential tool in our classroom. Students use technology to advance and reflect on their

Lake Brantley High School, Altamonte Springs, FL As a constructivist, I believe education should be something more akin to what Yeats described as “lighting a fire” rather than “filling a pail.” In my English class I adopted the reading and writing workshop approach in order to ignite students’ love for both and meet them at their level of need. Students love it! They mostly read self-selected texts based on their own interests and abilities and demonstrate mastery of state standards in a variety of ways, including both formal and informal assessments such as book talks or research projects on a topic of interest related to the text. Likewise, students are given choice of mode and medium of writing. I use direct instruction to teach a series of mini lessons on writing techniques, which students are expected to implement as they write. Every piece of writing will not be assessed; instead, students are encouraged to self-assess pieces (with guidance from me and peers) and select their best work for publication and grading. Students are required to write a certain number of finished pieces and read a certain number of texts by the end of the semester according to goals we set together, with each student’s goals being unique to her or his needs. And though goals are individual, I understand that learning is an inherently social process, so I build in daily opportunities for students to collaboratively problem solve and give and receive feedback on their work as often as needed. In our school we are fortunate to have plentiful technology available to support differentiation and we utilize it daily! Using our online learning management system, I design modules that include opportunities for enrichment and remediation, and also use this platform to facilitate online discussions and collaborations between students.

648 Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student Tiffinay Al-Lateef • Kindergarten–1st-Grade Teacher KIPP Columbus Primary, Columbus, OH To meet the diverse needs of my students, I would differentiate instruction in my classroom. To address the wide range of reading I would provide skill appropriate, culturally relevant texts. To build and bridge gaps with basic reading tools, I would pre-teach and build on vocabulary as much as possible for students. Yes, different philosophies of teaching would have a direct effect on what and how information is being taught to students. While trying to reach every student in my classroom, I would build and focus on individualized assessments for each of my students instead of using mostly standardized testing. Prior to testing I would research the interest of my students, to see what is important to them. This way I would incorporate relevant interest of the students in assessments and my lessons. Technology is present now more than ever before. I would use smart white boards, Chromebook, iPads music, and cell phones. Due to the current pandemic, technologies such as Zoom and Google Classroom help me to reach all my students virtually.

Lou De Lauro • 5th-Grade Language Arts Teacher John P. Faber School, Dunellen, NJ In your hometown you probably know a lot of people. To be successful you are going to have to use the town to help you. If you plan properly, you should be able to secure one guest a week for the entire school year. The kids will love meeting new people each week and reading with them. But you need more than a guest a week to visit your classroom. So ask the businesses in town. Maybe a business can run a fundraiser so you can purchase alternative texts for your students. Maybe the local library can introduce you to their biggest donor who might donate texts to you. Maybe you can apply for a grant with the local educational foundation to get new materials. But you need more help. You are a teacher; you were probably a strong student who connected with your former teachers. Visit any teachers that are still teaching, and get their advice on what to do. What has worked in the past may work well now, too. Devote many hours after school to your students. Small-group instruction will help these kids. Get the two students who are practically ready for college small stipends donated by a local business so they stay after school and help you with your challenging students. I think that if you fully take advantage of your home court, this is one situation in which you can easily prevail.

Marie Hoffman Hurt • 8th-Grade Foreign Language (German and French) Teacher Pickerington Local Schools, Pickerington, OH To start, I would encourage a teacher to look beyond the general classifications of “white, working, middle class, and English speaking.” Even in a class full of students who fit this demographic, there is an array of individuals. Each student learns differently and has different interests. A good teacher will recognize this and challenge students as people, not as groups. Do your best to layer as much as you can throughout your lessons. Give students choices. Use what resources you have—in this case particularly resources for students who are learning English as a second language. Even praising students’ individual characteristics and accomplishments outside the classroom sets the tone. Finally, keep in mind that you are only one person and can only give your best. Don’t overwork yourself and burn out— you are no good to your students or your family if you are wiped out.

M. Denise Lutz • High School Technology Coordinator Grandview Heights High School, Columbus, OH Studies have shown that student success is directly related to teacher effectiveness. In today’s diverse classrooms a teacher must develop effective classroom pedagogy that incorporates effective instructional strategies, uses effective classroom management strategies, and designs effective classroom curriculum to meet the needs of all learners. It is necessary to communicate learning goals for all students, track individual progress, and celebrate successes. Under the guidance of the teacher, students should learn to work collaboratively in small groups and as a cohesive class encouraging and helping one another to be successful. A teacher who establishes and maintains classroom rules and procedures while acknowledging students who do and do not follow these rules and procedures fosters this kind of environment. Consistency, trust, and authenticity will help to advance the development of effective relationships between the teacher, the home, and among class members. Effective classroom curriculum always begins with the end in mind. The teacher should have a clear picture of what mastery of content would look like for each of his or her students. Understanding the big idea and defining essential questions will guide the collection of activities and lessons that will move each student in the direction of success. The direction of success will remain the same for all students, but lessons and activities may present different paths for individuals to traverse. Today’s teacher must work from day one to get to know each individual and to establish a culture of collaboration among the group.

Cluster 14 Teaching Every Student

Patricia A. Smith • High School Math Teacher Earl Warren High School, San Antonio, TX Because this new teacher is a product of the same school system, it will be imperative to begin classroom instruction with absolutely no preconceived opinions toward any particular student. Likewise, a diverse population requires the teacher to resolve student situations discreetly and judiciously, and not publicly. The initial goals of the teacher would be to facilitate student work in a cooperative manner and engender teacher trust. Planning and organizing icebreaker exercises the first few days of the school year could prove extremely profitable. With a wide range of reading levels, small groups would work to the teacher’s advantage. I would not suggest grouping students according to reading level at all times, but would opt to appoint a recognized student leader to orchestrate daily oral recitations. Moreover, I would select reading materials suited

649

to all students and keep the assignments brief to avoid overwhelming struggling readers. The student leader could also design questions to gauge comprehension and give the group a follow-up spelling test. Initially, the spelling test would be composed of 5 to 10 simple words that students could either print or write. Subsequently, as the students gain confidence and experience success, the readings could be assigned as homework and the students would be required to write a short paragraph answering a reading comprehension question. If the teacher remains well organized, the instructional time allotted for small-group interaction should not extend over 15 minutes in a single class period. Thus, the teacher would not forfeit traditional grammar lessons for the entire class but would still provide limited individualized instruction. I would also supplement SAT reading and English practice for all college-bound students.

Cluster 15

Nino B., Age 10, Georgia ICAF.org

Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Teachers’ Casebook: Giving Meaningful Grades What Would You Do? Your school requires that you give letter grades to your class. You can use any method you want, as long as an A, B, C, D, or F appears for each of the subject areas on every student’s report card, every grading period. Some teachers are using worksheets, quizzes, homework, and tests. Others are assigning group work and portfolios. A few teachers are individualizing standards by grading on progress and effort more than on final achievement. Some are trying contract approaches and experimenting with longer-term projects, while others are relying almost completely on daily class work. Two teachers who use group work are considering giving credit toward grades for being a “good group member” or competitive bonus points for the top-scoring group. Others are planning to use improvement points for class rewards, but not for grades. Your only experience with grading was using written comments and a mastery approach that rated the students as making satisfactory or unsatisfactory progress toward particular learning targets. You want a system that is reliable, fair, and manageable but also 650

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

encourages learning, not just performance. And you want a system that gives the students feedback they can use to prepare for the state proficiency tests.

Critical Thinking • What would you choose as your major graded assignments and projects? • Would you include credit for behaviors such as group participation or effort? • How would you put all the elements together to determine a grade for every student for every marking period? • How would you justify your system to the principal and to the students’ families, especially when the teachers in your school are using so many different criteria? • What do you think of the wide range of criteria being used by different teachers—is this fair to students? • How will these issues affect the grade levels you will teach?

Overview and Objectives As you read this cluster, you will examine assessment, testing, and grading, focusing not only on the effects they are likely to have on students but also on practical ways to develop better methods for testing and grading. Teachers who are experts in assessment have developed ways to assign, collect, evaluate, organize, and return student work and then use the results of assessments to plan future instruction. They make it look easy, but as you learned in Cluster 13, routines have to be taught, explained, and practiced to work smoothly. The same holds for assessment and grading routines (Fives & Barnes, 2020). If you are feeling a bit challenged and unsure of your skills in these different areas, you are not alone. Even experienced teachers feel inadequately prepared for this important task (Ayalon & Wilkie, 2020). The goal of this cluster is to get you ready! I (Anita in this cluster) begin with a consideration of the basic concepts in assessment, including reliability, validity, and absence of bias. Next, we examine the many types of tests teachers prepare each year and approaches to assessment that don’t rely on traditional testing. Then, we explore the effects grades are likely to have on students and the very important topic of communicating with students and families. How will you justify the grades you give? Finally, because standardized tests are so important today, we spend some time looking at testing, the meaning of test scores, alternatives to traditional testing, and the role of tests in teacher evaluation. By the time you have completed this cluster, you should be able to: 15.1 Describe the basics of assessment, including types of assessments

(standardized tests, classroom assessments, and measurements as well as formative, interim, and summative assessments), and explain how reliability, validity, and absence of bias are used to understand and judge assessments.

651

652 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing 15.2 Describe two kinds of test interpretations (norm-referenced and

criterion-referenced), how to use selected-response and constructed-response/essay testing appropriately in teaching, and the advantages as well as criticisms of traditional testing. 15.3 Explain how to use formative assessment to improve instruction and

describe ways to design and evaluate authentic assessments, including portfolios, performances, and the development of rubrics. 15.4 Describe the effects of grading on students and the types of strategies

teachers can use to communicate to parents about grades. 15.5 Explain how to interpret common standardized test scores (percentile

rank, stanine, grade-equivalent, scale score) as well as current issues and criticisms concerning accountability and teacher evaluation, high-stakes assessment, growth versus proficiency tests, and value-added approaches.

OUTLINE Teachers’ Casebook—Giving Meaningful Grades: What Would You Do? Overview and Objectives MODULE 45: Key Concepts in Assessment and Testing Basics of Assessment Measurement and Assessment Assessing the Assessments: Reliability and Validity Classroom Assessment: Testing Interpreting Any Test Score Using the Tests from Textbooks Selected-Response Testing Constructed Responses: Essay Testing Assessing Traditional Testing MODULE 46: Formative Assessment and Grading Formative and Authentic Classroom Assessments Informal Assessments Authentic Assessments: Performances and Portfolios Evaluating Portfolios and Performances Assessing Complex Thinking Classroom Assessment: Lessons for Teachers Grading Norm-Referenced Versus Criterion-Referenced Grading Effects of Grading on Students Grades and Motivation Beyond Grading: Communicating with Families MODULE 47: Standardized Testing and Teacher Evaluation Standardized Testing Types of Scores Interpreting Standardized Test Reports Accountability and High-Stakes Testing New Directions: PARCC and SBAC Using High-Stakes Testing Well: Lessons for Teachers

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

653

Reaching Every Student: Helping Students with Disabilities Prepare for High-Stakes Tests Teacher Accountability and Evaluation Cluster 15 Review Key Terms • Connect and Extend to Licensure • Teachers’ Casebook—Giving Meaningful Grades: What Would They Do?

MODULE 45

Key Concepts in Assessment and Testing

Learning Objective 15.1 Describe the basics of assessment including types of assessments (standardized tests, classroom assessments, and measurements, as well as formative, interim, and summative assessments) and explain how reliability, validity, and absence of bias are used to understand and judge assessments. Learning Objective 15.2 Describe two kinds of test interpretations (norm-referenced and criterion-referenced), how to use selected-response and constructed-response/essay testing appropriately in teaching, and the advantages as well as criticisms of traditional testing.

Basics of Assessment Would it surprise you to learn that published tests, such as college entrance exams, are creations of the 20th century? In the early to mid-1900s, college entrance was generally based on grades, essays, and interviews. From your own experience, you know that testing has come a long way since then—too far, say some critics. Published tests today are called standardized tests because they are administered, scored, reported, and interpreted in a standard manner— same directions, time limits, and scoring for all (Popham, 2020). The schools where you teach probably will use standardized tests, especially to meet the growing demands for accountability. In most schools, however, teachers do not have much say in selecting these tests. Classroom assessments, on the other hand, are created or selected by teachers. Classroom assessments can take many different forms—unit tests, essays, portfolios, group projects, performances, oral presentations, videos, designs and plans, journals, websites, wikis, or products such as artwork or clothing—the list is long. Assessments are critical because teaching involves making many kinds of judgments—decisions based on values: “Is this software or book appropriate for my students?” “Should Olivia get a B− or a C+ on the project?” This cluster is about the judgments involved in all forms of assessment and grading, with an emphasis on classroom assessments—the kind you will create and interpret. Before we look at either classroom or standardized assessments, let’s examine some key distinctions that apply to both, beginning with the difference between measurement and assessment.

Standardized tests Tests given, usually nationwide, under uniform conditions and scored according to uniform procedures.

Classroom assessments Classroom assessments are selected and created by teachers and can take many different forms—unit tests, essays, portfolios, projects, performances, oral presentations, and so on.

Measurement and Assessment Measurement is quantitative—the description of an event or characteristic using numbers. Measurement tells how much, how often, or how well by providing scores, ranks, or ratings. Instead of saying, “Noah doesn’t seem to understand addition,” a teacher might say, “Noah answered only 2 of the 15 problems correctly in his addition homework.” Measurement also allows a teacher to compare one student’s performance on a particular task with either a specific standard or the performances of other students on the same task. Not all teachers’ decisions involve measurement. But measurement, when properly done, can provide unbiased data for decisions. In fact, data-based decision making is very important in today’s schools. Increasingly, measurement specialists are using the term assessment to describe the process of gathering information from a range of sources about students’ learning. Assessment

Measurement An evaluation expressed in quantitative (number) terms.

Assessment Procedures used to obtain information about student performance.

654 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing consists of many ways—quantitative and qualitative—to sample and observe students’ skills, knowledge, and abilities, so assessment is the broader term that includes measurement along with many other techniques (Brookhart & Nitko, 2019). Assessments can be formal, such as planned unit tests, or informal, such as observing who emerges as a leader during group work. Assessments can be designed by classroom teachers or by local, state, or national agencies. And today, assessments can go well beyond paper-and-pencil exercises to include judgments based on students’ performances, portfolios, projects, or products (Popham, 2020). There are three general uses or purposes for assessment, depending on where you are in the instructional cycle: formative, interim, and summative assessment (Northwest Education Association, 2015). Formative assessment is a process that occurs before or during instruction. The purposes of formative assessment are to guide the teacher in planning and adjusting instruction and to provide feedback to help students improve learning. In other words, formative assessment helps form instruction by providing feedback that supports the efforts of both teachers and students (Brookhart, 2018; Dixson & Worrell, 2016). Students often take a formative assessment before instruction, a pretest that helps the teacher determine what students already know. Teachers sometimes assess during instruction to see what areas of weakness remain so they can direct teaching toward the problem areas. These formative assessments are not graded, so students who tend to be very anxious about “real” tests may find this low-pressure practice in test taking especially helpful. Also, the feedback from formative tests can help students become better judges of quality and more self-regulated in their learning (Clark, 2012). Interim (growth or benchmark) assessments occur at regular intervals during the school year to determine student progress and growth in an objective way. These assessments might be used to differentiate instruction, determine if special services are appropriate, assess progress in Response to Intervention (RtI) programs (see Cluster 5), or evaluate whether a particular curriculum is working as hoped (NWEA, 2015). Some standardized test packages now have interim assessments to help teachers determine whether their students are making good progress and if they will be ready for the standardized test when it comes (Dixson & Worrell, 2016). For an example of interim assessments used by about 12 states, see the Smarter Balanced Assessment System at https://www.smarterbalanced.org. Summative assessment occurs at the end of instruction. Its purpose is to measure the students’ level of proficiency—have they learned the prescribed content and skills? Summative assessment, therefore, provides a summary of students’ accomplishments. The final exam is a classic example. Standardized tests such as the SAT or GRE are also summative— they assess what you have learned. Some educators say formative is assessment for learning and summative is assessment of learning (Chappuis & Stiggins, 2020). The distinctions among formative, interim, and summative assessments, however, are really based on how the results are used. If the purpose of the assessment is to improve your teaching and help students guide their own learning, then the evaluation is formative. If the purpose is to track growth and progress over time, the assessment is interim. And if the purpose is to evaluate final achievement (and help report a course grade), the assessment is summative (Dixson & Worrell, 2016). In fact, the same assessment could be used as a formative evaluation at the beginning of the unit, as an interim assessment during the unit, and as a summative evaluation at the end. Because the formative uses of assessment are really the most important in teaching, we will spend more time on them later. For now let’s consider how to judge any assessment.

FORMATIVE, INTERIM, AND SUMMATIVE ASSESSMENT.

Formative assessment Ungraded testing used before or during instruction to aid in planning and diagnosis.

Pretest Formative test for assessing students’ knowledge, readiness, and abilities before instruction begins.

Interim (growth or benchmark) assessments Assessments that occur at regular intervals during the school year to determine student progress and growth in an objective way.

Summative assessment Testing that follows instruction and assesses achievement.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Types of Assessment (II, C1, 4) Understand the purposes of formative and summative assessment. Explain how teachers and students can make effective use of the information generated by each type of test.

Assessing the Assessments: Reliability and Validity One of the most common problems with the use of assessments, especially tests, is misinterpretation of results. No assessment provides a perfect picture of a person’s abilities; the results from a test provide only one small sample of behavior. Three factors are important in developing good assessments and interpreting results: reliability, validity, and absence of bias. These terms are applied most often to test scores, even though the underlying considerations apply to all assessments.

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Scores are reliable if a test gives a consistent, precise, and stable “reading” of a person’s ability from one occasion to the next, assuming the person’s ability remains the same. A reliable thermometer works in a similar manner, giving you a reading of 100°C each time you measure the temperature of boiling water. So, when you see “reliability,” think consistency and precision of measurement. There are several ways to compute reliability, but all the possibilities give numbers between 0.0 and 1.0, like a correlation coefficient. Above .90 is considered very reliable; .80 to .90 is good, and below .80 is not very good reliability for standardized tests such as the SAT or ACT—most of these have a test-retest reliability of .80 to .90 based on about 3 months between testings (Brookhart & Nitko, 2019). Generally speaking, longer tests are more reliable than shorter ones.

655

RELIABILITY OF TEST SCORES.

Errors in Test Scores. All tests are imperfect estimators of the qualities or skills they are trying to measure. There are sources of error related to the student such as mood, motivation, test-taking skills, or even cheating. Sometimes the errors are in your favor, and you score higher than your knowledge might warrant; sometimes the errors go against you. There are also sources of error related to the test itself: the directions are unclear, the reading level is too high, the items are ambiguous, or the time limits are wrong. The score each student receives always includes some amount of error. How can scores be more precise and error-free? As you might guess, this returns us to the question of reliability. The more reliable the test scores are, the less error there will be in the score actually obtained. On standardized tests, the developers take this into consideration and make estimations of how much the students’ scores would probably vary if they were tested repeatedly. This estimation is called the standard error of measurement. Thus, a reliable test can also be defined as one with a small standard error of measurement. Confidence Interval. Never base your opinion of a student’s ability or achievement on the exact score the student obtains. For standardized tests, many test companies now report scores using a confidence interval, or “standard error band,” that encloses the student’s actual score. This makes use of the standard error of measurement and allows a teacher to consider the range of scores that might include a student’s true score—the score the student would get if the measurement were completely accurate and error-free. Assume, for example, that two students in your class take a standardized achievement test in Spanish. The standard error of measurement for this test is 5. One student receives a score of 79 and the other a score of 85. At first glance, these scores seem quite different. But when you consider the standard error bands around the scores, not just the scores alone, you see that the bands overlap. The first student’s true score might be anywhere between 74 and 84 (that is, the actual score of 79 plus or minus the standard error of 5). The second student’s true score might be anywhere between 80 and 90. Both students could have the same true score of 80, 81, 82, 83, or 84 because the score bands overlap at those numbers. It is crucial to keep in mind the idea of standard error bands when selecting students for special programs. No child should be rejected simply because the score obtained missed the cutoff by 1 or 2 points. The student’s true score might well be above the cutoff point. See Figure 15.5 later in this cluster for a test report that includes these score bands. You can apply the same reasoning to political surveys—they generally report a margin of error (usually in very small print at the bottom of the TV screen). If a particular survey in Michigan, say, had one candidate at 47% and the other at 53%, the results would be a “statistical tie” (the language you hear on television) if the poll had a margin of error of ±4%. That is because either candidate’s true percentage could be in the overlapping band of 49%, 50%, or 51%. Differences between candidates’ ratings are truly different and reliable only when the score bands do not overlap. If test scores are sufficiently reliable, the next question is whether the scores are valid or, more accurately, whether the judgments and decisions based on the test scores are valid. To have validity, the decisions and inferences based on the test must be supported VALIDITY.

Reliability Consistency of test results.

Standard error of measurement Hypothetical estimate of variation in scores if testing were repeated.

Confidence interval Range of scores within which an individual’s true score is likely to fall.

True score The score the student would get if the measurement were completely accurate and error-free.

Validity Degree to which a test measures what it is intended to measure.

656 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing by evidence. This means that validity is judged in relation to a particular use or purpose— that is, in relation to the actual decision being made and the evidence for that decision. A particular test might be valid for one purpose but not for another (Haertel, 2018; Popham, 2020). Different kinds of evidence support a particular judgment. If the purpose of a test is to measure the skills covered in a course, then we want content-related evidence of validity, so we should see test questions on all the important course topics and not on extraneous information. Have you ever taken a test that dealt only with a few ideas from one lecture or just a few pages of the textbook? Then decisions based on that test (like your grade) certainly lacked content-related evidence of validity. Some tests are designed to predict outcomes. The SATs, for example, are intended to predict performance in college. If SAT scores correlate with academic performance in college as measured by the criterion of, say, grade-point average in the first year, then we have criterion-related evidence of validity for the use of the SAT in admissions decisions. Most standardized tests are designed to measure some psychological characteristic or “construct” such as reasoning ability, reading comprehension, achievement motivation, intelligence, creativity, and so on. It is a bit more difficult to gather construct-related evidence of validity, yet this is a very important requirement—probably the most important. Construct-related evidence of validity is gathered over many years. It is indicated by correlations with other established tests and logical patterns of scores. For example, older children can answer more questions on intelligence tests than younger children can. This fits with our construct of intelligence. If the average 5-year-old answered as many questions correctly on a test as the average 13-year-old, we would doubt that the test really measured intelligence. Today, many psychologists suggest that gathering content- and criterion-related evidence are simply ways of determining if the test actually measures the construct it was designed to measure. A test must be reliable in order to be valid. For example, if an intelligence test yields different results each time it is given to the same child over the course of a few months, then, by definition, it is not reliable. Certainly, it couldn’t be a valid measure of intelligence because intelligence is assumed to be fairly stable, at least over a short period of time. However, reliability will not guarantee validity. If that intelligence test gave the same score every time for a particular child but didn’t predict school achievement, speed of learning, or other characteristics associated with intelligence, then performance on the test would not be a true indicator of intelligence. The test would be reliable—but invalid. Reliability and validity are issues with all assessments, not just standardized tests but classroom assessments as well. The third important criterion for judging assessments is absence of bias. Assessment bias “refers to qualities of an assessment instrument that offend or unfairly penalize a group of students because of their gender, race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, religion, or other such group-defining characteristic” (Popham, 2020, p. 128). Biases are aspects of the assessment such as content, language, or examples that might distort the performance of a group—either for better or for worse. Offensiveness occurs when a particular group might be insulted by the content of the assessment. Offended, angry students may not perform at their best. What about unfairly penalizing test takers based on ethnic, racial, gender, religious, or social class biases? Many people believe that tests can unfairly penalize some groups who may not have an equal opportunity to show what they know on the test because the language of the test and the tester may be different from the languages of the students. Also, tests may reflect unfair penalization because different groups have had different opportunities to learn the material tested. The questions asked tend to center on experiences and facts more familiar to students from the dominant culture than to students from minority groups. Consider this test item for fourth-graders described by Popham (2020, p. 375):

ABSENCE OF BIAS.

Assessment bias Qualities of an assessment instrument that offend or unfairly penalize a group of students because of the students’ gender, socioeconomic status, race, ethnicity, and so on.

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

657

My uncle’s field is computer programming. Look at the sentences below. In which sentence does the word field mean the same as it does in the boxed sentence above? A. The softball pitcher knew how to field her position. B. They prepared the field by spraying and plowing it. C. I know the field I plan to enter when I finish college. D. The doctor used a wall chart to examine my field of vision. Items like this are included on most standardized and textbook tests. But not all family members describe their work as a field of employment. What if your parents worked at a grocery store or a car repair shop? Are these fields? Life outside class has prepared some students, but not others, for this item. The meaning of “field” is part of your cultural knowledge. When you think about it, how can you separate background, context, and culture from cognition? Every student’s learning is embedded in his or her culture, and every test question emerges from some kind of cultural knowledge, so be sensitive to the kinds of questions you ask. And the purpose of the assessment can actually affect evaluators’ judgments. For example, when participants in a study were told the purpose was to evaluate the student’s work to give grades based on the number of mistakes, the evaluators found more mistakes if they thought the student was from a lower-SES family. If the evaluators were told the purpose was to give feedback to guide learning, the gap in finding mistakes for high- and low-SES students was reduced (Autin et al., 2019). With this background in the basic concepts of formative, interim, and summative assessments and attention to reliability, validity, and absence of bias, we are ready to enter the classroom.

Classroom Assessment: Testing Stop & Think Think back to your most recent test. What was the format? Did you feel that the test results were an accurate reflection of your knowledge or skills? Have you ever had to design a test? What makes a good, fair test? When most people think of assessments in a classroom, they usually think of testing. As you will see shortly, although teachers today have many other options, testing is still a significant activity in most classrooms. In this section, we will examine how to interpret test scores, evaluate the tests that accompany standard curriculum materials, and write your own test questions.

Interpreting Any Test Score Before we even look at different kinds of tests, there is an important consideration. The outcome of any test has no meaning by itself; we must make some kind of comparison in order to interpret test results. There are two basic types of comparisons: In the first, a test score is compared to the scores obtained by other people who have taken the same test. This is called a norm-referenced comparison. The second type is criterion-referenced. Here, the score is compared to a fixed standard or minimum passing score. The same test can be interpreted in either a norm-referenced or criterion-referenced way, depending on the type of comparison made.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Traditional Assessment (II C1, 2, 4) Selected-response and essay tests continue to have important roles in effective assessment and evaluation programs. Describe the appropriate uses of these types of tests. Identify the advantages and limitations of each.

658 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing Norm-referenced testing Testing in which scores are compared with the average performance of others.

Norm group Large sample of students serving as a comparison group for scoring tests.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Criterion-/Norm-Referenced Tests (II, C5) The ERIC Digest Norm- and Criterion-Referenced Testing (https://eric. ed.gov/?id=ED410316) describes the purposes, content, and issues related to criterion- and normreferenced tests. Giving accurate feedback to parents is part of a teacher’s job. When talking with a parent about a child’s abilities, do you think the use of norm-referenced or criterion-referenced test results is more desirable?

NORM-REFERENCED TEST INTERPRETATIONS. In norm-referenced testing, the people who have taken the test provide the norms for determining the meaning of a given individual’s score or grade. You can think of a norm as being the typical level of performance for a particular group. By comparing the individual’s raw score (the actual number correct) to the norm, we can determine if that raw score is above, below, or around the average for that group. There are at least four types of norm groups (comparison groups) in education— the class or school itself, the school district, national samples, and international samples. Students in national norm groups used for large-scale assessment programs are tested one year, and then the scores for that group serve as comparisons or norms every year for several years until the test is revised, or re-normed. The norm groups are selected so that all socioeconomic status (SES) levels are included in the sample. Because students from high-SES backgrounds tend to do better on many standardized tests, a high-SES school district will almost always have higher scores compared to the national norm group that includes a wide range of SES levels. Norm-referenced tests cover many general outcomes. They are especially appropriate when only the top few candidates can be admitted to a program. However, norm-referenced measurement has its limitations. The results of a norm-referenced test do not tell you whether students are ready to move on to more advanced material. For instance, knowing that two students are in the top 3% of the class on a test of algebraic concepts will not tell you if they are ready to move on to advanced math; everyone in the class may have a limited understanding of the algebraic concepts, and no one may be ready to move on. Norm-referenced tests also are not particularly appropriate for measuring affective and psychomotor outcomes. To measure individuals’ psychomotor learning, you need to compare their performance to a clear description of standards. In the affective area, attitudes and values are personal; comparisons among individuals are not really appropriate. For example, how could we measure an “average” level of artistic preferences? Finally, norm-referenced tests tend to encourage competition and comparison of scores. Some students compete to be the best. Others, realizing that being the best is impossible, may compete to be the worst. Both goals have their casualties.

When test scores are compared not to the scores of others but to a given criterion or standard of performance, this is called criterion-referenced testing. To decide who should be allowed to drive a car, it is important to determine just what standard of performance works for selecting safe drivers. It does not matter how your test results compare with the results of others. If your performance on the test was in the top 10% but you consistently ran through red lights, you would not be a good candidate for receiving a license, even though your score was high. Criterion-referenced tests measure the mastery of a very specific learning target. For example, a criterion-referenced test would be useful in measuring the students’ ability to add three-digit numbers. A test could be designed with 20 different problems, and the standard for mastery could be set at 17 correct out of 20. (The standard is often somewhat arbitrary and may be based on such things as the teacher’s experience.) If two students receive scores of 7 and 11, it does not matter that one student did better than the other because neither met the standard of 17. Both need more help with addition. The results of a criterion-referenced test should tell the teacher exactly what the students can and cannot do, at least under certain conditions; thus, these are the right kinds of tests for formative and interim assessments. When teaching basic skills, comparison to a preset standard is often more important than comparison to the performance of others. It is not very comforting to know, as a parent, that your child is better in reading than most of the students in her class if none of the students is reading at grade level. Sometimes standards for meeting the criterion must be set at 100% correct. You would not like to have your appendix removed by a surgeon who left surgical instruments inside the body only 10% of the time.

CRITERION-REFERENCED TEST INTERPRETATIONS.

Criterion-referenced testing Testing in which scores are compared to a set performance standard.

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

659

Criterion-referenced tests are not appropriate for every situation. Many subjects cannot be broken down into a set of specific targets. And, although standards are important in criterion-referenced testing, they can often be arbitrary, as you have already seen. When deciding whether a student has mastered the addition of three-digit numbers comes down to the difference between 16 or 17 correct answers, it seems difficult to justify one particular standard over another. Finally, at times, it is valuable to know how the students in your class compare to other students at their grade level both locally and nationally. You can see that each type of test is well suited for certain situations, but each also has its limitations.

Using the Tests from Textbooks Most elementary and secondary school texts today come complete with supplemental materials such as teaching manuals and ready-made tests. Using these tests can save time, but is this good teaching practice? The answer depends on your learning targets, the way you teach the material, and the quality of the tests provided. If the textbook test:

• Is of high quality, • Covers all your learning goals, • Matches your testing plan, • Asks student to know and do what they were taught, and • Fits the instruction you actually provided for your students, then it may be the right test to use. Check the reading level of the items provided and be prepared to revise and improve them to meet the needs of your class (McMillan, 2019; Russel & Airasian, 2012). What if no tests are available for the material you want to cover or the tests provided in your teachers’ manuals are not appropriate for your students? Then it’s time for you to create your own tests. We will consider the two major kinds of traditional tests—selectedresponse and constructed response/essay.

Selected-Response Testing Multiple-choice questions, matching exercises, true/false statements, and short-answer or fill-in items are all types of selected-response testing. The scoring of these types of items is relatively straightforward because the answers are more clear-cut than essay answers. These types of items are what many of us envision when we think of “traditional” testing (Chappuis & Stiggins, 2020). How should you decide which item format is best for a particular test? Use the one that provides the most direct measure of the Learning Objective you intended for your students. In other words, if you want to see how well students can write a letter, have them write a letter, don’t ask multiple-choice questions about letters. But if many different item formats will work equally well, then use multiple-choice questions because they are easier to score fairly and can cover many topics. Switch to other formats if writing good multiple-choice items for the material is not possible or appropriate. For example, if related concepts such as terms and definitions need to be linked, then a matching item is a better format than multiple-choice. If it is difficult to come up with several wrong answers for a multiple-choice item, try a true/false question instead. Alternatively, ask the student to supply a short answer that completes a statement (fill in the blank). Variety in testing can lower students’ anxiety because the entire grade does not depend on one type of question that a particular student may find difficult. Table 15.1 on the next page gives guidance for using true/false, matching, and fill-in-the-blank items. We will look closely at the multiple-choice format because it is the most versatile—and the most difficult to use well.

Selected-response testing A form of testing in which students choose the correct response from a set of possible responses provided by the teacher or the test developer instead of creating their own response. Multiple-choice and true-false tests are common examples of selected response testing.

660 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Table 15.1 Guidance for Using True/False, Matching, and Fill-in-the-Blank Items Here is a checklist for your test preparation. GUIDELINE FOR TRUE/FALSE ITEMS            Write the item as a simple declarative sentence.            Make items entirely true or entirely false as stated—no complex “idea salads” with some truth and some falsehood. GUIDELINES FOR MATCHING ITEMS                                            

Provide clear directions for making the match. Keep the list of items to be matched short—no more than 10. Include only homogeneous items—don’t mix names, dates, events, and so on. Keep the wording of the response options short and grammatically the same. Don’t let grammar be a clue.            Provide more response options than items to be matched so students can’t guess the last match because only one answer is left. GUIDELINES FOR FILL-IN ITEMS            Ask a question. Don’t just make a statement with a blank in it—more than one answer might work in a statement.            Provide one blank per item—ask one question, get one answer, and then move to the next question.            Don’t make the length of the blank a clue either.            Put the blank toward the end.

Source: Adapted from Chappuis, J., & Stiggins, R. J. (2020). An introduction to student-involved assessment for learning (3rd ed.)., pp. 156–157. Pearson. Adapted with permission. Reprinted and Electronically Reproduced by Permission of Pearson Education, Inc., New York, NY.

Even though your education professors may reject the use of multiple-choice tests, about 50% of public school teachers endorse these tests (Banks, 2012), so you should know how to use them well. Giving students experience answering multiple-choice tests (as long as this format fits your learning targets) may help prepare the students for state achievement testing that relies on multiple-choice items (Chappuis & Stiggins, 2020). Carefully written multiple-choice items can assess higher-order outcomes if you ask students to apply or analyze a concept, but crafting these items is not easy (Waugh & Gronlund, 2013). For example, Mark Smith (2017) asked 12th-grade students to “think aloud” as they answered multiple-choice items from a U.S. history test on the National Assessment of Educational Progress—a well-known standardized test. The items were intended to measure historical thinking, but the results of the think-alouds indicated that the items really assessed factual knowledge, reading comprehension, and test-taking skills.

USING MULTIPLE-CHOICE TESTS.

Stem The question part of a multiplechoice item.

Distractors Wrong answers offered as choices in a multiple-choice item.

WRITING MULTIPLE-CHOICE QUESTIONS. Some students jokingly refer to multiple-choice tests as “multiple-guess” tests—a sign that these tests are often poorly designed. Your goal in writing test items is to design them so that they measure student achievement, not test-taking and guessing skills, and this can be a challenge. The stem of a multiple-choice item is the part that asks the question or poses the problem. The choices that follow are called alternatives. The wrong answers are called distractors because their purpose is to distract students who have only a partial understanding of the material. If there were no good distractors, students with only a vague understanding would have no difficulty in finding the right answer. The Guidelines: Writing Multiple-Choice Test Items should help you write good stems and alternatives.

Constructed Responses: Essay Testing The best way to measure some learning targets is to ask students to create answers on their own; essay questions are one way to accomplish this. When you want to assess mastery of larger chucks of knowledge (like asking students how atoms combine to form other substances) or evaluate reasoning (give a rationale for an experiment), then a constructed response is a good choice (Chappuis & Stiggins, 2020). The most difficult part of

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

GUIDELINES Writing Multiple-Choice Test Items Make the stem clear and simple, and present only a single problem. Ask a complete question with no unessential details. Example Poor There are several different kinds of standard or derived scores. An IQ score is especially useful because

Better An advantage of an IQ score is

State the problem in the stem in positive terms. Negative language is confusing. If you must use words such as not, no, all but, or except, underline them or type them in all capitals. Example Poor Which of the following is not a standard score?

Better Which of the following is NOT a standard score?

Do not expect students to make extremely fine discriminations among answer choices. Example The percentage of area in a normal curve falling between +1 and −1 standard deviations is about:

Poor a. b. c. d.

66%. 67%. 68%. 69%.

Better e. f. g. h.

14%. 34%. 68%. 95%.

Make sure each alternative answer fits the grammatical form of the stem so that no answers are obviously wrong. Example Poor The Stanford-Binet test yields an a. IQ score. b. reading level. c. vocational preference. d. mechanical aptitude.

Better The Stanford-Binet is a test of a. intelligence. b. reading level. c. vocational preference. d. mechanical aptitude. (Continued)

661

662 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Word response options as briefly as possible, and make sure they are grammatically the same (Chappuis & Stiggins, 2020, p. 155). Example Poor Why did colonists migrate to the United States? a. To escape heavy taxation by their native governments. b. Religion. c. They sought the adventure of living among Native Americans in the new land. d. There was the promise of great wealth in the New World.

Better Why did colonists migrate to the United States? a. To escape taxation b. For religious freedom c. For adventure d. To become rich

Avoid including two distractors that have the same meaning. If only one answer can be right, and if two answers are the same, then these two must both be wrong. This narrows down the choices considerably.

Avoid using categorical words such as always, all, only, or never unless they can appear consistently in all the alternatives. Most smart test takers know that categorical answers are usually wrong.

Avoid using the exact wording found in the textbook. Poor students may recognize the answers without knowing what they mean.

Avoid overuse of all of the above and none of the above. Such choices may be helpful to students who are simply guessing. In addition, using all of the above may trick a quick student who sees that the first alternative is correct and does not read on to discover that the others are correct, too.

Avoid obvious patterns on a test. They aid students who are guessing. The position of the correct answer should be varied, as should its length. Make sure there is only one correct or best answer. If more than one answer might be correct, make sure the stem clearly asks for the “best” answer. Base distractors on common errors and misconceptions (Gierl et al., 2017).

essay testing is judging the quality of the answers, but writing good, clear questions is not particularly easy, either. CONSTRUCTING ESSAY TESTS. Because answering essay questions takes time, true essay tests cover less material than selected-response tests. Thus, for efficiency, essay tests should be limited to the assessment of important, complex Learning Objectives. A good essay question includes (a) a clear and specific context for the question, (b) a statement of what students should describe or explain, and (c) guidelines for what should be covered in the answer. Here is an example:

We have been studying the importance of the carbon cycle and how it works [context]. Based on your understanding of the carbon cycle, explain (a) why we

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

need to know about it, and (b) how carbon moves from one place to another [what to describe or explain]. Be sure to include the following [guidelines]:

• Why it is important to understand the carbon cycle (5 points) • The four major reservoirs where carbon is stored (4 points) • At least six ways that carbon gets transferred from one place to another (6 points) (Chappuis & Stiggins, 2020, pp. 187–188) Students need ample time to answer a question like this. If more than one essay is assigned in the same class period, you may want to suggest time limits for each question. Remember, however, that time pressure increases anxiety and may prevent accurate assessment of some students. Whatever your approach, don’t try to make up for the limited amount of material an essay test can cover by including a large number of questions. Rather than including more than two or three essay questions in a single class period, it would be better to plan on more frequent testing. Combining an essay question with a number of selected-response items is one way to prevent the problem of limited sampling of course material (Waugh & Gronlund, 2013). When possible, a good first step in grading essays is to construct a set of scoring criteria or a rubric (more on this later) and share it with students. After all, students can succeed only if they know what success means—what does success look like (Chappuis & Stiggins, 2020)? Here is an example of a question and criteria from TenBrink (2003, p. 326).

EVALUATING ESSAYS.

Question: Defend or refute the following statement: Civil wars are necessary to the growth of a developing country. Cite reasons for your argument and use examples from history to help substantiate your claim. Scoring Rubric: All answers, regardless of the position taken, should include (a) a clear statement of the position, (b) at least five logical reasons, (c) at least four examples from history that clearly substantiate the reasons given. Once you have set your expectations for answers, you can assign points to the various parts of the essay. You might also give points for the organization of the answer and the internal consistency of the essay. You can then assign grades such as 1 to 5 or A, B, C, D, and F and sort the papers into piles by grade. As a final step, skim the papers in each pile to see if they are comparable in quality. These techniques will help ensure fairness and reliability in grading. When grading essay tests that contain several questions, it makes sense to grade all responses to one question before moving on to the next. This helps prevent the quality of a student’s answer to one question from influencing your reaction to the student’s other answers. After you finish reading and scoring the first question, shuffle the papers so that no students end up having all their questions graded first (e.g., when you may be taking more time to give feedback or are applying stricter standards) or last (when you may be tired of writing feedback or more lax in your standards). You may achieve greater objectivity if you ask students to put their names on the back of the paper so that grading is anonymous.

Assessing Traditional Testing Your stand on traditional testing is part of your philosophy of teaching. First consider the positives of testing—right answers are important. Even though schooling is about learning to think and solve problems, it is also about knowledge. Students must have something to think about—facts, ideas, concepts, principles, theories, explanations, arguments, images, opinions. One reason that American students, compared with students in many other developed countries, lack essential knowledge may be because American schools emphasize

663

664 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing process—critical thinking, self-esteem, problem solving—more than content. To teach more about content, teachers will need to assess content learning. Well-designed traditional tests can evaluate students’ knowledge effectively and efficiently and thus provide feedback for students and information for teacher planning (Popham, 2020). But there are strong criticisms as well. Do traditional tests really tell us what students can do with that knowledge out in the real world? True understanding cannot be measured by tests that ask students to use skills and knowledge out of context. Let’s look at a few alternative approaches to classroom assessment.

Module 45 Summary Basics of Assessment (pp. 653–657) Distinguish between measurement and assessment. Measurement is the description of an event or characteristic using numbers. Assessment includes measurement but is broader because it includes all kinds of ways to sample and observe students’ skills, knowledge, and abilities. Distinguish among formative, interim, and summative assessment. In the classroom, formative (ungraded, diagnostic assessment) helps form and support learning by providing feedback to both the students and the teacher. Interim (growth) assessments occur at regular intervals during the school year to determine student progress in an objective way—to see if the students are moving toward proficiency. Summative assessment occurs at the end of instruction and summarizes students’ accomplishments in order to report on achievement. What is test reliability? Some tests are more reliable than others; that is, they yield more stable and consistent estimates. Care must be taken in the interpretation of test results. Each test is only a sample of a student’s performance on a given day. The score is only an estimate of a student’s hypothetical true score. The standard error of measurement takes into account the possibility for error and is one index of test reliability. What is test validity? The most important consideration about a test is the validity of the decisions and judgments that are based on the test results. Evidence of validity can be related to content, criterion, or construct. Construct-related evidence for validity is the broadest category and encompasses the other two categories of content and criterion. Tests must be reliable to be valid, but reliability does not guarantee validity. What is absence of bias? Tests must be free of assessment bias. Bias occurs when tests include material that offends or unfairly penalizes a group of students because of the students’ gender, SES, race, religion, or ethnicity. Culture-fair tests have not proved to solve the problem of assessment bias.

Classroom Assessment: Testing (pp. 657–664) Distinguish between norm-referenced and criterion-referenced tests.  In norm-referenced tests, a student’s performance is compared to the average performance of others. In criterionreferenced tests, scores are compared to a preestablished standard. Norm-referenced tests cover a wide range of general learning goals. However, results of norm-referenced tests do not tell whether students are ready for advanced material, and they are not appropriate for affective and psychomotor learning goals. Criterion-referenced tests measure the mastery of very specific goals. How can testing support learning? Learning is supported by frequent testing using cumulative questions that ask students to apply and integrate knowledge. With the goals of assessment in mind, teachers are in a better position to design their own tests or evaluate the tests provided by textbook publishers. Describe two kinds of traditional testing.  Two traditional formats for testing are the selected-response test and the constructed-response/essay test. Selected-response tests, which can include multiple-choice, true/false, fill-in, and matching items, should be written with specific guidelines in mind. Writing and scoring essay questions requires careful planning, in addition to criteria to discourage bias in scoring.

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

MODULE 46

665

Formative Assessment and Grading

Learning Objective 15.3 Explain how to use formative assessment to improve instruction and describe ways to design and evaluate authentic assessments, including portfolios, exhibitions, performances, and the development of rubrics. Learning Objective 15.4 Describe the effects of grading on students and the types of strategies teachers can use to communicate to parents about grades.

Formative and Authentic Classroom Assessments Formative assessments, as you saw earlier, are assessments for learning. They help form and support instruction. Formative assessments may be as informal and “on the fly” as the teacher noticing many students in Spanish class are having trouble with one particular verb construction in conversations and homework to as formal as quizzes embedded in the curriculum that are just like tests but don’t count toward a grade. As emphasis on formative assessment has increased around the world, achievement is increasing as well (Chappuis & Stiggins, 2020; Decristan et al., 2015). Early on in a unit, assessments should be formative (provide feedback but not count toward a grade), saving the actual graded assessments for later in the unit when all students have had the chance to learn the material. One approach is informal assessments.

Informal Assessments Informal assessments are ungraded assessments that gather information from multiple sources to help teachers make decisions (Banks, 2012). Two examples of informal assessment are exit tickets and journals. EXIT TICKETS. The exit ticket is a simple assessment and can even be done “on the fly” as you notice students struggling. After a lesson, the teacher poses a question or problem. Then each student answers on a separate sheet of paper that becomes her or his ticket to leave the class. Before the next class, the teacher reviews the tickets and creates small discussion groups made up of at least one student with a solid understanding of the material mixed in with students who are struggling. After reviewing and reteaching the concepts that seem to have been the most misunderstood, the teacher tells the students to discuss the exit ticket question in their groups and identifies the students who have a solid understanding as the “topic leaders” (Dixson & Worrell, 2016). Other exit ticket assessments include a 2-minute fast write about the key ideas in the lesson or a description of the “muddiest point,” the idea that was most confusing or unclear. Students don’t even have to put their names on these exit tickets, but a quick read will tell the teacher where reteaching and explanation are needed the next day. JOURNALS. Individual and group journals are flexible and widely used informal assessments. Teachers may consult the journals to learn students’ concerns and interests so instruction can be more authentic and engaging. But often journals focus on academic learning, usually through responses to prompts. Banks (2012, p. 113) describes one high school physics teacher who asked his students to respond to these three questions in their journals:

1. How can you determine the coefficient of friction if you know only the angle of the inclined plane? 2. Compare and contrast magnetic, electronic, and gravitational fields. 3. If you were to describe the physical concept of sound to your best friend, what music would you use to demonstrate this concept?

Informal assessments Ungraded (formative) assessments that gather information from multiple sources to help teachers make decisions.

666 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing When he read the students’ journals, the teacher realized that many of the students’ basic assumptions about friction, acceleration, and velocity came from personal experiences and not from scientific reasoning. His approach to teaching had to change to reach the students. The teacher never would have known to make the changes in his instruction without reading the journals, which served in this case as a formative assessment. There are many other kinds of informal assessments. Every time teachers ask questions or watch students perform skills, the teachers are conducting informal assessments. INVOLVING STUDENTS IN ASSESSMENTS. One way to provide feedback and develop students’ sense of efficacy for learning is to involve the students in the formative assessment process. Students can keep track of their own progress and assess their improvement. Here are other ideas, some taken from Chappuis and Stiggins (2020). Students might:

• Learn about the criteria for judging work by examining and discussing with a peer examples of good, average, and poor products or performances. Then pick a poor example and revise to improve it.

• Describe to the teacher or a peer (orally or in writing) the way they approached an Authentic assessments Assessment procedures that test skills and abilities as they would be applied in real-life situations.

assignment, the problems they encountered, the options they considered, and the final result.

• Analyze their strengths and weaknesses before starting a project and then discuss with the teacher or peers how they will use their strengths and overcome their weaknesses as they work on the project.

• In pairs, make up questions that might be on the test, explain why those are good questions, and then answer them together.

• Look back at earlier work and analyze how they have grown by describing “I used to think . . . but now I know. . . .” After doing a few of these analyses, summarize using a frame such as: What did I know before I started? What did I learn? What do I want to learn next?

• Before a major test, do a free write on these prompts: “What exactly will be on the test?” “What kinds of questions will be asked (multiple-choice, essay, etc.)?” “How well will I do?” “What do I need to study to make sure I am ready?” Students also can provide themselves with formative feedback by completing the quiz analysis sheet in Figure 15.1 on the next page. After an ungraded quiz, students analyze their mistakes and make plans for improving. This approach makes good use of the quiz—especially because many students just toss the papers without learning anything from the outcome.

Authentic Assessments: Performances and Portfolios Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Authentic Tests (II, C1, 2, 4) The emphasis on student-centered learning has been accompanied by an emphasis on authentic tests. Understand the purpose, value, and advantages of these forms of assessment. Describe their characteristics and the potential problems with their use.

Authentic assessments ask students to apply skills and abilities as they would in real life. For example, they might use fractions to enlarge or reduce recipes. If our instructional goals for students include the abilities to write, speak, listen, create, think critically, do research, solve problems, or apply knowledge, then our tests should ask students to write, speak, listen, create, think, research, solve, and apply. How can this happen? Many educators suggest we look to the arts and sports for analogies to solve this problem. If we think of the “test” as being the recital, science fair project, game, mock court trial, or other performance, then teaching to the test is just fine. All coaches, artists, and musicians gladly “teach” to these “tests” because performing well on these tests is the whole point of instruction. Authentic assessment asks students to perform. The performances may be thinking performances, physical performances, creative

PERFORMANCES.

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

667

Figure 15.1 Reviewing and Analyzing a Quiz or Assignment A form like this can help students analyze a quiz or assignment to serve as formative assessment. After identifying mistakes as “fixable” or “I don’t get it,” the student makes plans to relearn. Reviewing My Results Assignment:

Name:

Date:

Please look at your corrected test and mark whether each problem is right or wrong. Then look at the problems you got wrong and decide if you made a mistake you can fix without help. If you did, mark the “Fixable Mistake” column. For all the remaining problems you got wrong, mark the “Don’t Get it” column.

Problem

Learning Target

Right

Wrong

Fixable Mistake

Don’t Get it

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Analyzing My Results I AM GOOD AT THESE! Learning targets I got right: I AM PRETTY GOOD AT THESE, BUT NEED TO DO A LITTLE REVIEW Learning targets I got wrong because of a fixable mistake: What I can do to keep this from happening again: I NEED TO KEEP LEARNING THESE Learning targets I got wrong and I’m not sure what to do to correct them: What I can do to get better at them: Source: Chappuis, J., & Stiggins, R. J. (2020). An introduction to student-involved assessment for learning (3rd ed.). Pearson, p. 167. Reprinted and Electronically Reproduced by Permission of Pearson Education, Inc., New York, NY.

performances, or other forms. So performance assessment is any form of assessment that requires students to carry out an activity or produce a product to demonstrate learning (Russell & Airasian, 2012). It may seem odd to talk about thinking as a performance, but there are many parallels. Serious thinking is risky because real-life problems are not well defined. Often, the outcomes of our thinking are public; others evaluate our ideas. Like a dancer auditioning for

Performance assessments Any form of assessment that requires students to carry out an activity or produce a product in order to demonstrate learning.

668 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Portfolio Assessment (II, C1, 2) For a discussion of the advantages, limitations, design, and implementation of portfolio programs, go to https://www. teach-nology.com/litined/ assessment/alternative/portfolios/.

Portfolio A collection of the student’s work in an area, showing growth, self-reflection, and achievement.

a Broadway show, we must cope with the consequences of being evaluated. Like a potter looking at a lump of clay, a student facing a difficult problem must experiment, observe, redo, imagine, and test solutions; apply both basic skills and inventive techniques; make interpretations; decide how to communicate results to the intended audience; and often accept criticism and improve the initial solution (Clark, 2012; Eisner, 1999). The concern with authentic assessment has led to the development of several approaches based on the goal of performance in context. For example, instead of asking students, “If you bought a toy for 69 cents and gave the clerk a dollar, how much change would you get back?” have students work in pairs with real money to role-play making different purchases or set up a mock class store and have students make purchases and give change (Kim & Sensale Yazdian, 2014; Popham, 2020; Waugh & Gronlund, 2013). The Center for Technology in Learning of SRI International, a nonprofit science research institute, also provides an online resource bank of performance-based assessments linked to the National Science Education Standards. The resource is called PALS (Performance Assessment Links in Science). Go to the website http://pals.sri.com; search for the performance tasks for kindergarten through 12th grade. You can select tasks by standard and grade level. PORTFOLIOS. For years, photographers, artists, models, and architects have had portfolios to display their skills and show to prospective employers. A portfolio is a systematic collection of work, often including work in progress, revisions, student self-analyses, and reflections on what the student has learned. Written work or artistic pieces are common contents of portfolios, but student portfolios might also include letters to the portfolio readers describing each entry and its importance, graphs, diagrams, pictures or digital slideshows, PowerPoint presentations, recordings of the students reading their work, unedited and final drafts of persuasive essays or poems, lists of books read, annotated website addresses, peer comments, videorecordings, laboratory reports, and computer programs—anything that demonstrates learning in the area being taught and assessed (Popham, 2020). There is a distinction between process portfolios and final, or “best-work,” portfolios. The distinction is similar to the difference between formative and summative evaluation. Process portfolios document learning and show progress. Best-work portfolios showcase final accomplishments (Johnson & Johnson, 2002). Table 15.2 on the next page shows some examples of portfolios for both individuals and groups. The Guidelines: Creating Portfolios on the next page give some ideas for using portfolios in your teaching.

Evaluating Portfolios and Performances Checklists, rating scales, and scoring rubrics are helpful when you assess performances because assessments of performances and portfolios are criterion referenced, not norm referenced. In other words, the students’ products and performances are compared to established public standards, not ranked in relation to other students’ work. SCORING RUBRICS. A checklist or rating scale gives specific feedback about elements of a

performance. Scoring rubrics are rules that are used to determine the quality of a student performance, often on a 4-point scale from “excellent” (4) to “inadequate” (1) or on a scale that assigns points to each category—10 points for excellent, 6 for good, and so on (Mabry, 1999). For example, a rubric describing excellent delegation of responsibility in a group research project might be: Each student in the group can clearly explain what information the group needs, what information s/he is responsible for locating, and when the information is needed. Scoring rubrics Rules that are used to determine the quality of a student’s performance.

This rubric was generated using Rubistar, an online service for educators that allows you to select a subject area and category and then create a rubric. To get the preceding rubric, I chose the subject of writing—“group planning and research project”—and the category of “delegation of responsibility.”

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Table 15.2 Process and Best-Works Portfolios for Individuals and Groups Here are a few examples of how to use portfolios in different subjects. THE PROCESS PORTFOLIO SUBJECT AREA

INDIVIDUAL STUDENT

COOPERATIVE GROUP

Science

Documentation (running records or logs) of using the scientific method to solve a series of laboratory problems

Documentation (observation checklists) of using the scientific method to solve a series of laboratory problems

Mathematics

Documentation of mathematical reasoning through double-column mathematical problem solving (computations on the left side and running commentary explaining thought processes on the right side)

Documentation of complex problem solving and use of higher-level strategies

Language Arts

Evolution of compositions from early notes through outlines, research notes, response to others’ editing, and final draft

Rubrics and procedures developed to ensure highquality peer editing

SUBJECT AREA

INDIVIDUAL STUDENT

COOPERATIVE GROUP

Language Arts

The best compositions in a variety of styles—expository, humor/ satire, creative (poetry, drama, short story), journalistic (reporting, editorial columnist, reviewer), and advertising copy

The best dramatic production, video project, TV broadcast, newspaper, advertising display

Social Studies

The best historical research paper, opinion essay on a historical issue, commentary on current event, original historical theory, review of a historical biography, account of participation in academic controversy

The best community survey, paper resulting from academic controversy, oral history compilation, multidimensional analysis of a historical event, press corps interview with a historical figure

Fine Arts

The best creative products such as drawings, paintings, sculptures, pottery, poems, thespian performances

The best creative products such as murals, plays written and performed, inventions thought of and built

THE BEST-WORKS PORTFOLIO

Source: Based on D. W. Johnson and R. T. Johnson (2002), Meaningful assessment: A manageable and cooperative process. Pearson Education, Inc.

GUIDELINES Creating Portfolios Involve students in selecting the pieces that will make up their portfolios. Examples 1.

2.

During the unit or semester, ask each student to select work that fits certain criteria, such as “my most difficult problem,” “my best work,” “my most improved work,” or “three approaches to.” For their final submissions, ask students to select pieces that best show how much they have learned.

Make sure the portfolios include information that shows student self-reflection and self-criticism. Examples 1.

Ask students to include a rationale for their selections. (Continued)

669

670 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

2. 3. 4.

Have each student write a “guide” to his or her portfolio, explaining how strengths and weaknesses are reflected in the work included. Include self- and peer critiques, indicating specifically what is good and what might be improved. Model self-criticism of your own productions.

Make sure the portfolios reflect the students’ activities in learning. Examples 1. 2.

Include a representative selection of projects, writings, drawings, and so forth. Ask students to relate the goals of learning to the contents of their portfolios.

Be aware that portfolios can serve different functions at different times of the year. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Early in the year, it might hold unfinished work or “problem pieces.” At the end of the year, it should contain only what the student is willing to make public. Throughout the year, the portfolios may be the basis for family night conferences. Students can lead the discussion and explain to their family what they have learned by talking them through their portfolios.

Be certain that portfolios demonstrate students’ growth. Examples 1. 2.

Ask students to make a “history” of their progress along certain dimensions and to illustrate points in their growth with specific works. Ask students to include descriptions of activities outside class that reflect the growth illustrated in the portfolio.

Teach students how to create and use portfolios. Examples 1. 2.

Keep models of very well done portfolios as examples, but stress that each portfolio is an individual statement. Examine your students’ portfolios frequently, especially early in the year when they are just getting used to the idea. Give constructive feedback that provides explanations and information about better strategies and problem-solving approaches.

James Popham (2020) emphasizes that rubrics should be neither so specific that they apply to only one task nor so general that they give no useful guidance. For example:

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Scoring Rubrics (II, C3) Kathy Schrock’s Guide for Educators (http://www. schrockguide.net/assessmentand-rubrics.html) provides information about every aspect of the use of scoring rubrics in the classroom as well as an extensive collection of rubrics that can be used or adapted by teachers.

• Write a 400-word essay about home fire escape plans that includes the rationale provided by the firefighter who visited our class and the six elements of a good plan. (Too specific—describes only one task and does not address the more general skill of writing essays.)

• Essays will be judged poor, fair, good, or excellent. (Too general—provides no more information than grading essays as D, C, B, or A.) The Guidelines: Developing a Rubric on the next page give more ideas; some are taken from Johnson and Johnson (2002), Guskey (2020), and Popham (2020). It is often helpful to have students join in the development of rating scales and scoring rubrics. When students participate, they are challenged to decide what quality work looks or sounds like in a particular area. They know in advance what is expected. As students gain practice in designing and applying scoring rubrics, their work and their learning often improve. RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY. Because the teacher’s personal judgment plays such a central role in evaluating performances, reliability is a concern. One teacher’s “excellent” could be another teacher’s “adequate.” Research shows that when raters are experienced and scoring rubrics are well developed and refined, reliability may improve (Herman & Winters, 1994; LeMahieu et al., 1993).

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

GUIDELINES Developing a Rubric 1.

2.

3. 4.

5. 6. 7. 8.

Make sure the skill to be assessed is important and can be taught. It takes time to develop good rubrics, so make sure the skill being assessed is worth everyone’s time and that the skill can be improved with instruction and practice. Look at models. Show students examples of good and not-so-good work based on composites of work not linked to individual students. Identify the characteristics that make the good ones good and the bad ones bad. List criteria. Use the discussion of models to begin a list of what counts in quality work. Articulate gradations of quality. Describe the best and worst levels of quality; then fill in the middle levels based on your knowledge of common problems and the discussion of not-so-good work. Practice on models. Have students use the rubrics to evaluate the models you gave them in Step 2. Use self- and peer assessment. Give students their task. As they work, stop them occasionally for self- and peer assessment. Revise. Always give students time to revise their work based on the feedback they get in Step 6. Use teacher assessment. In your grading, be sure to use the same rubric students used to assess their work.

Note: Step 3 may be necessary only when you are asking students to engage in a task with which they are unfamiliar. Steps 5 and 6 are useful but time-consuming; you can do these on your own, especially when you’ve been using rubrics for a while. A class experienced in rubric-based assessment can streamline the process so that it begins with listing criteria, after which the teacher writes out the gradations of quality, checks them with the students, makes revisions, and then uses the rubric for self-, peer, and teacher assessment. For a great explanation of using rubrics, see Mertler (2001), available at https://scholarworks. umass.edu/pare/vol7/iss1/25/. This online article includes several links that allow you to create and customize rubrics for your class, including http://rubistar.4teachers.org.

In terms of validity, some evidence shows that students who are classified as “master” writers on the basis of portfolio assessment are judged less capable using standard writing assessments. Which form of assessment is the best reflection of enduring qualities? It is hard to say. In addition, when rubrics are developed to assess specific tasks, the results of applying them may not predict performance on anything except very similar tasks, so we do not know whether a student’s performance on a specific task is a valid indicator for the larger area of study (Haertel, 1999; McMillan, 2004). DIVERSITY AND BIAS IN PERFORMANCE ASSESSMENT. Equity is an issue in all assessment and no less so with performances and portfolios. With a public performance, there could be bias effects based on a student’s appearance and speech or the student’s access to expensive audio, video, or graphic tools. Performance assessments have the same potential as other tests to discriminate unfairly against students who are not wealthy or who are culturally different. And the extensive group work, peer editing, and out-of-class time devoted to portfolios means that some students may have access to greater networks of support, technology, and outright help. Many students in your classes will come from families that have sophisticated tech devices and publishing capabilities. Others may have little support from home. These differences can be sources of bias and inequity, especially in portfolios and performances.

Assessing Complex Thinking In order to develop complex thinking ability, we have to be able to assess it. The good news is that assessing complex thinking and higher-order outcomes actually can help students

671

672 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing master and retain core facts as well (Jensen et al., 2014). So you don’t have to choose between testing facts and testing complex thinking. Assessments of complex thinking don’t have to be tests. Carol Lee and Susan Goldman (2015) have developed assessments of complex thinking in literacy that involve authentic reading and writing. For example, to assess middle and high school students’ abilities to cite evidence in making claims about symbolism in the text, the students had to (a) put events in order from the reading to show they comprehended the plot, (b) use a graphic guide to organize claims and evidence they used to answer the question, and (c) write an essay in which they compared and contrasted two stories in relation to the generalizations of the author and the structure of the text. The two stories had the theme of coming of age and incorporated symbolism as a rhetorical tool. Students’ essays were judged with a rubric that evaluated the quality of the claims, whether the evidence cited was supported by the texts, the ability to organize the essay around similarities and differences, and the general clarity of the writing. You can see that assessing complex thinking is demanding for both the students and the teacher!

Classroom Assessment: Lessons for Teachers One major message in this cluster is the importance of correctly matching the type of assessment tools used with the target—the knowledge being assessed. Look at Table 15.3, which summarizes the possibilities and limitations of aligning different assessment tools with their targets. The second lesson is, as you saw in the previous cluster and several others, feedback is critical for learning. The word “feedback” appears 46 times in this cluster and countless times throughout the book. Providing feedback to students is a key professional skill for

Table 15.3 Aligning Different Assessment Tools with Their Targets Different Learning Objectives require different assessment methods. ASSESSMENT METHOD TARGET TO BE

PERFORMANCE

PERSONAL

ASSESSED

SELECTED-RESPONSE

ESSAY

ASSESSMENT

COMMUNICATION

Knowledge Mastery

Multiple-choice, true/ false, matching, and fill-in can sample mastery of elements of knowledge.

Essay exercises can tap understanding of relationships among elements of knowledge

Not a good choice for this target—three other options (selected-response, essay, personal communication) preferred

Can ask questions, evaluate answers, and infer mastery—but a time-consuming option

Reasoning

Can assess understanding of basic patterns of reasoning

Written descriptions of complex problem solutions can provide a window into reasoning proficiency

Can watch students solve some problems and infer about reasoning proficiency

Can ask student to “think aloud” or can ask follow-up questions to probe reasoning

Skills

Can assess mastery of the prerequisites of skillful performance—but cannot tap the skill itself

Can assess mastery of the prerequisites of skillful performance—but cannot tap the skill itself

Can observe and evaluate skills as they are being performed

Strong match when skill is oral communication proficiency; also can assess mastery of knowledge prerequisite to skillful performance

Ability to Create

Can assess mastery of knowledge prerequisite to the ability to create quality products—but cannot assess the quality of products themselves

Can assess mastery of knowledge prerequisite to the ability to create quality products—but cannot assess the quality of products themselves

A strong match can assess: (a) proficiency in carrying out steps in product development and (b) attributes of the product itself

Can probe procedural knowledge and knowledge of attributes of quality products—but not product quality

Proficiency

Products

Source: From “Where Is Our Assessment Future and How Can We Get There?” by R. J. Stiggins. In R. W. Lissitz, W. D. Schafer (Eds.), Meaningful Assessment: A Manageable and Cooperative Process. Published by Allyn & Bacon. Copyright © 2002 by Pearson Education. Adapted by permission of the publisher.

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

673

teachers and one of the most powerful influences on student learning (Hattie, 2009; Schuldt, 2019). What makes feedback valuable? Don’t just provide right answers. Give explanations about why the right answer is correct and the wrong answer isn’t. In learning concepts, give additional correct examples of the concept. Discuss better learning strategies. To make sure students understand and value the feedback, have a conversation with the students about the feedback—don’t just write or say the feedback and assume it was heard. You might even encourage the students to write the feedback down and review it later. Encourage students to identify three strengths, three areas for improvement, and an action plan to use the feedback to get better. Finally, give feedback in a way that communicates that the students are getting better and are worth the effort to improve—support their self-efficacy for learning and improving (Finn et al., 2018; van der Kleij, 2019). In sum, Lorien Schuldt (2019) notes that feedback should:

• Focus on the task and the student’s progress toward the goal, not on the student as a person or on comparisons with other students.

• Provide information about what to do next—how to close the gap between “where I am” and “where I’m going.”

• Be constructive and highlight strengths as well as paths to improvement. • Be timely, that is, come at a time when students can act on the guidance to improve. • Foster student agency and ownership of the work. No matter how you assess students, ultimately you will assign grades. We turn to that job next.

Grading Gia, a 10th-year visual arts teacher, said: “I don’t think any teacher likes grading. It’s time consuming. Assessing the work isn’t much fun. Developing good lessons and teaching well are a lot more fun” (Olsen & Buchanan, 2019, p. 2004). The teachers I know would agree. Grading is one of those school practices that has hardly changed over the years. Teachers often grade the way they do because that’s the way it has always been done (Guskey, 2020). So why grade? Although some people claim that the purpose of grading is to sort students or motivate them to work, the best reason (maybe the only legitimate reason) is to communicate to the students and their families how the students are progressing toward important learning goals. In fact, Tom Guskey, a world-renowned expert on grading, notes two essential consequences of grades that must be changed; 1. We must help students and their families understand that grades reflect not who students are as learners but where they are in their learning journey—and where is always temporary. 2. We must never use grades to sort, select, or rank students (p. 14). Simply put, grades should answer the question students always ask, “How am I doing?” The challenge is to combine a vast amount of information about an individual into a symbol or other indicator that communicates “how you’re doing” (Hübner et al., 2020). First, the teacher must make a major decision: Should a student’s grade reflect his or her status in comparison with the rest of the class, or should the grade reflect the amount of material learned and how well it has been learned? In other words, should grading be norm referenced or criterion referenced?

Norm-Referenced Versus Criterion-Referenced Grading In norm-referenced grading, the major influence on a grade is the student’s standing in comparison with others who also took the course. If a student studies very hard and almost everyone else does too, the student may receive a disappointing grade, perhaps a C

Norm-referenced grading In determining grades, assessing students’ achievement in relation to one another.

674 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing Grading on the curve Norm-referenced grading that compares students’ performance to an average level.

Criterion-referenced grading Assessment of each student’s mastery of course objectives.

or D. One common type of norm-referenced grading is called grading on the curve. How you feel about this approach probably depends on where your grades generally fall along that “curve,” but this approach certainly violates Guskey’s (2020) caution about using grades to sort, select, or rank students. When you think about it, if the curve arbitrarily limits the number of good grades that can be given, then, in the game of grading, most students will be losers and motivation will suffer (Guskey & Bailey, 2001; Haladyna, 2002; Kohn, 1996). Tom Guskey (2011) pointed out the fallacy of grading on the curve: “The normal bell-shaped curve describes the distribution of randomly occurring events when nothing intervenes” (p. 17). But teaching is intervening—we want to teach in such a way that all students learn. Finally, grading on the curve really does not communicate what the student knows or can do, which is the main reason for grading in the first place. For this purpose, we need criterion-referenced grading. In criterion-referenced grading, the grade represents a list of accomplishments. If clear learning targets have been set for the course, the grade may represent a certain number of targets reached satisfactorily. When a criterion-referenced system is used, criteria for each grade generally are spelled out in advance. It is then up to the student to earn the grade she or he wants to receive. Theoretically, in this system, all students can achieve an A if they reach the criteria. Criterion-referenced grading has the advantage of relating judgments about a student to the achievement of clearly defined instructional goals or standards. Some school districts have developed reporting systems in which report cards list learning targets along with judgments about the student’s attainment of each target. Reporting is done at the end of each unit of instruction. The elementary school report card shown in Figure 15.2 demonstrates the relationship between assessment and the goals of the unit. Most schools have a specified grading system, so we won’t spend time here on the many possible systems, except to say that if your school is trying to improve its grading systems, from teacher gradebooks to report cards to permanent records and transcripts, the best resource is the carefully researched and clearly written book Get Set, Go: Creating Successful Grading and Reporting Systems (2020) by Thomas Guskey. Let’s consider an important question about any grading system: What are the effects of grades on students?

Effects of Grading on Students How do you feel about grades? Stop & Think Think back on your report cards and grades over the years. Did you ever receive a grade that was lower than you expected? How did you feel about yourself, the teacher, the subject, and school in general as a result of the lower grade? What could the teacher have done to help you understand and profit from the experience? When we think of grades, we often think of competition. Highly competitive classes may be particularly hard on anxious students, students who lack self-confidence, students who are less prepared, and students who are learning English. Although high standards do tend to be generally related to increased academic learning, it is clear that teachers must strike a balance between high standards and a reasonable chance to succeed. High standards without a way to reach them is simply cruel punishment. So should low grades and failure be avoided in school? The situation is not that simple. THE VALUE OF FAILING? Humans generally avoid making errors. “However, a growing body of research in cognitive and educational psychology has established that errors in low-stakes contexts enhance learning when they are followed by corrective feedback” (Wong & Lim, 2019). Feedback—there is that word again. One way to encourage critical thinking and support development of robust knowledge is to ask students questions that push their understanding and even draw out mistakes or errors and then

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Figure 15.2 A Criterion-Referenced Report Card This is one example of a criterion-referenced report card. Other forms are possible, but all criterion-referenced reports indicate student progress toward specific goals.

Source: Guskey, T. R., Swan, G. M., & Jung, L. A. (2011). Grades that mean something. Phi Delta Kappan, 93(2), p. 53. Used with permission.

provide clear feedback with reteaching and the chance to improve (Guskey, 2020). For this to work, the class must be a safe place that supports taking risks and making errors so error correction is possible and valued. And the level of difficulty must be just right—not so hard that students just give up. Also, teachers can describe common errors and explain why they are wrong and why the right answers are right (Wong & Lim, 2019). Failure also can support persistence and motivation, especially if teachers help the students see connections between hard work and improvement. Efforts to protect students from failure and guarantee success may be counterproductive, especially if students come to feel entitled to high grades without exerting much effort—a bad life lesson (Tomlinson, 2005b). RETENTION IN GRADE. What about the effect of failing an entire grade—that is, of being “held back”? Retained children are more likely to be male, members of minority groups, living in poverty, younger, and less likely to have participated in early childhood programs (Beebe-Frankenberger et al., 2004; G. Hong & Raudenbush, 2005). Is retention a good policy? See the Point/Counterpoint to examine the issue.

675

676 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

POINT/COUNTERPOINT: Should Children Be Held Back? In 2017, about 1,082,000 students (kindergarten through grade 12) were retained in grade (Digest of Educational Statistics, 2020). For the last 100 years, parents and educators have debated about the value of retention versus social promotion (passing students on to the next grade with their peers). What are the arguments and the evidence?

Point Yes, retention just makes sense. The traditional arguments for retention include that classes will be more similar in achievement level if the low-achieving students are “held back,” so teaching will be easier for the teacher and can be at a higher level for the students in the class. Also, learning with younger classmates may be more developmentally appropriate for the lower-achieving students and could motivate them to work harder (Hong & Raudenbush, 2005). One study in Florida found that students retained in third grade based on reading proficiency test scores had to take fewer remedial courses in high school and were just as likely to graduate as students not retained (Schwerdt et al., 2015, 2017). Critics of the study noted, however, that retained students all received intensive extra services intended to raise reading achievement, so those additional supports may have made the difference and not the actual repeating of third grade (Robinson-Cimpian, 2015). Retention in kindergarten for children considered “not ready” for first grade is a common practice. Compared with students who are relatively younger (January to August birthdays), students who are relatively older (born September to November) have higher achievement in school on average (Cobley et al., 2009). In fact, some parents hold their son or daughter back to give the child an edge over peers in each grade thereafter or because the child was born late in the year—a practice sometimes called “academic red-shirting” (Wallace, 2014). About 4% to 5.5% of children delay entry to kindergarten. The most likely to be red-shirted are White, male, and high-SES children. Schools that serve those populations have higher rates of school delay (Bassok & Reardon, 2013). The results on academic redshirting are mixed. Some studies have found benefits for students who have been held back by their parents, but other studies have found no benefits

Counterpoint No, retention is not effective. Even though a few studies support the value of retention (e.g., Marsh, 2016; Schwerdt et al., 2015, 2017), almost a century of research indicates that it is not helpful and may even be harmful. Most research finds that grade retention is associated with poor long-term outcomes such as dropping out of school, higher arrest rates, fewer job opportunities, and lower self-esteem (Andrews, 2014; Jimerson et al., 2002; Jimerson & Ferguson, 2007; Shepard & Smith, 1989). Lucy Barnard-Brak (2008) studied a national sample of 986 children who had been identified as having learning disabilities and concluded that “delayed kindergarten entrance was not associated with better academic achievement for children with learning disabilities across time” (p. 50). Even though G. Hong and Raudenbush (2005) acknowledged the arguments for retention (see the Point section), their extensive study that followed almost 12,000 kindergarten students through the end of first grade found just the opposite. The researchers compared retained and promoted students from schools that practice retention as well as promoted students from schools that practice social promotion. They found no evidence that retention improved either reading or mathematics achievement. In addition, retention did not seem to improve instruction in the first grade by making the class more similar in academic ability. After 1 year, the retained students were an average of 1 year behind, and evidence indicated that these children would have done better if promoted. Another study that followed retained and promoted students for 4 years found

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

some short-term advantages for retained students in social and behavioral skills, followed by long-term problems and vulnerabilities. The authors suggest that the “strugglesucceed-struggle” pattern may undermine academic motivation for retained students and interfere with peer relations (Demanet & Van Houtte, 2016; Wu et al., 2010). Jan Hughes and her colleagues (2018) followed students for years who were retained in grades 1 through 5 and found that being held back led to a significant increase in dropping out of school, especially for Black and Hispanic girls. And the negative impact may not be limited to the retained student. In one study of over 79,000 students, seventh-graders who simply attended school with many peers who were old for their grade (because they had been retained or “red-shirted”) were more likely to get in trouble at school or be suspended (Muschkin et al., 2014).

Beware of Either/Or Using Research for Children. No matter what, children who are having trouble should get help, whether they are promoted or retained. However, just covering the same material again in the same way won’t solve the children’s academic or social problems. The best approach may be to promote the children along with their peers but to give them special remediation during the summer or over the next year (Demanet & Van Houtte, 2016). In addition, because the inability to focus attention and self-regulate is an important aspect of readiness to learn (Blair, 2002), help should also focus on improving these skills as well. An even better approach would be to prevent the problems before they occur by providing extra resources in the early years.

Grades and Motivation The assessments you give should support students’ motivation to learn—not their motivation to work for a good grade. But is there really a difference between working for a grade and working to learn? The answer depends in part on how a grade is determined. If you test only at a simple but detailed level of knowledge, you may force students to choose between complex learning and a good grade. But when a grade reflects meaningful learning and learning is supported by formative assessments with useful feedback, working for a grade and working to learn become the same thing. Today there is increasing discussion of grading for learning, not just grading of learning—the same distinction made between formative and summative assessment. If teachers grade in the best interests of their students and talk to students about how to interpret and learn from the grades as feedback, then grades might support motivation to learn (McMillan, 2019). Low grades by themselves generally do not encourage greater efforts, however. Students receiving low grades are more likely to withdraw, blame others, decide that the work is “dumb,” or feel responsible for the low grade but helpless to make improvements. They give up on themselves or on school. In fact, low grades at the beginning of the year may set up a downward spiral: lower grades, less engagement in schoolwork, lower grades, less engagement, and on and on (Poorthuis et al., 2015; Tomlinson, 2005b). Rather than assigning a failing grade, you might consider the work incomplete and give students support in revising or improving it. Maintain high standards and give students a chance to reach them. Again, formative assessment and quality feedback are key (Guskey, 2011, 2018; Polio & Hochbein, 2015). The Guidelines: Using Any Grading System give ideas for fair and reasonable use of any grading system.

Beyond Grading: Communicating with Families No number or letter grade conveys the totality of a student’s experience in a class or course. Students, families, and teachers sometimes become too focused on the end point—the grade. But communicating with families should involve more than just sending home grades. There are a number of ways to be in touch with and report to families. Many teachers I know have a

677

678 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

GUIDELINES Using Any Grading System Explain your grading policies to students early in the course and remind them of the policies regularly. Examples 1. 2.

Give older students a handout describing the assignments, tests, grading criteria, and schedule. Explain to younger students in a low-pressure manner how their work will be evaluated.

Base grades on clearly specified, reasonable standards. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Specify standards by developing a rubric with students. Show anonymous examples of poor, good, and excellent work from previous classes. Discuss workload and grading standards with more experienced teachers. Give a few formative tests to get a sense of your students’ abilities before you give a graded test. Take tests yourself first to gauge the difficulty of the test and to estimate the time your students will need.

Base your grades on as much objective evidence as possible. Examples 1. 2.

Plan in advance how and when you will test. Keep a portfolio of student work. This may be useful in student or parent conferences.

Be sure students understand test directions. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Outline the directions on the board. Ask several students to explain the directions. Go over a sample question first.

Correct, return, and discuss tests as soon as possible. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Have students who wrote good answers read their responses for the class; make sure they are not the same students each time. Discuss why wrong answers, especially popular wrong choices, are incorrect. As soon as students finish a test, give them the answers to questions and the page numbers where answers are discussed in the text.

As a rule, do not change a grade. Examples 1. 2.

Make sure you can defend the grade in the first place. DO change any clerical or calculation errors.

Guard against bias in grading. Examples 1. 2.

Ask students to put their names on the backs of their papers. Use a point system or model papers when grading essays.

Keep pupils informed of their standing in the class. Examples 1. 2.

Write the distribution of scores on the board after tests. Schedule periodic conferences to go over work from previous weeks.

Give students the benefit of the doubt. All measurement techniques involve error. Examples 1. 2.

Unless there is a very good reason not to, give the higher grade in borderline cases. If a large number of students miss the same question in the same way, revise the question for the future and consider throwing it out for that test.

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Avoid reserving high grades and high praise for answers that conform to your ideas or to those in the textbook. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Give extra points for correct and creative answers. Withhold your opinions until all sides of an issue have been explored. Reinforce students for disagreeing in a rational, productive manner. Give partial credit for partially correct answers.

Make sure each student has a reasonable chance to succeed, especially at the beginning of a new task. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Pretest students to make sure they have prerequisite abilities. When appropriate, provide opportunities for students to take a retest to raise their grades, but make sure the retest is as difficult as the original. Consider failing efforts as “incomplete,” and encourage students to revise and improve. Base grades more on work at the end of the unit; give ungraded work in the beginning of the unit.

Balance written and oral feedback. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Consider giving short, lively written comments with younger students and more extensive written comments with older students. When the grade on a paper is lower than the student might have expected, be sure the reason for the lower grade is clear. Tailor comments to the individual student’s performance; avoid writing the same phrases over and over. Note specific errors, possible reasons for errors, ideas for improvement, and work done well. Students often don’t read or misinterpret written feedback. Periodically have individual conferences and give constructive feedback—strengths and areas for improvement.

Make grades as meaningful as possible. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Tie grades to the mastery of important learning targets. Give ungraded assignments to encourage exploration. Experiment with performances and portfolios.

Base grades on more than just one criterion. Examples 1. 2.

Use essay questions as well as multiple-choice items on a test. Grade oral reports and class participation.

Source: General conferencing guidelines adapted from Problems in middle and high school teaching: A handbook for student teachers and beginning teachers (pp. 182–187), by A. M. Drayer, 1979. Allyn & Bacon. Copyright © 1979 by Allyn & Bacon. Adapted by permission of the author and publisher.

beginning-of-the-year newsletter or student handbook that communicates homework, behavior, and grading policies to families. Other options described by Guskey and Bailey (2001) are:

• Notes attached to report cards • “Happy notes” sent home describing a good moment or accomplishment • Phone calls, especially “Good News” calls • School open houses • Student-led conferences • Portfolios or exhibits of student work • Homework hotlines

679

680 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing • School or class web pages • Home visits Conferences with parents or caregivers are often expected of teachers in elementary school and can be equally important in middle and high school. You already have some ideas about productive conferences in the Family and Community Partnership Guidelines in Cluster 5 (pp. 211–212), in the section on student-led conferences in Cluster 6 (pp. 280–281), and coming up in the Family and Community Partnerships Guidelines about explaining test results on page 687. In addition, the more skilled teachers are at communicating, the more effective they will be at conducting these conferences. Listening and problem-solving skills such as those discussed in Cluster 13 can be particularly important. When you are working with families or students who are angry or upset, make sure you really hear their concerns, not just their words. The atmosphere should be friendly and unrushed. Any observations about the student should be as factual as possible, based on observation or information from assignments. Information gained from a student or a parent/caregiver should be kept confidential. One kind of information that will interest parents is their child’s standardized test scores. In the next section we look at these tests.

Module 46 Summary Formative and Authentic Classroom Assessments (pp. 665–673) How can teachers use formative assessment? Formative assessments may be informal and “on the fly,” such as exit tickets or journals, or formal assessments, such as quizzes embedded in the curriculum. Whatever the form, students can participate in the formative assessment by analyzing their work and making plans for improvement. What is authentic assessment? Critics of traditional testing believe that teachers should use authentic tests and other authentic assessment procedures. Authentic assessment requires students to perform tasks and solve problems that are similar to the real-life performances that will be expected of them outside of school. Describe portfolios. Portfolios are examples of authentic assessment. A portfolio is a collection of a student’s work, sometimes chosen to represent growth or improvement or sometimes featuring “best work.” Portfolios may be produced by individuals or cooperative groups. What are the issues of reliability, validity, and equity with portfolios and performance assessment? Using authentic assessments does not guarantee reliability, validity, and equity (absence of bias). Using rubrics is one way to make assessment more reliable and valid. But the results from assessment based on rubrics may not predict performance on related tasks. Also, rater bias based on the appearance, speech, or behavior of students or a lack of resources may place students from minority groups at a disadvantage in performance assessments or projects. Why assess complex thinking? Assessing complex thinking and higher-order outcomes actually can help students master and retain core facts as well. In other words, testing for complex understanding improves learning at all the levels of Bloom’s taxonomy, from remembering factual knowledge to analyzing and creating high-level knowledge. There are approaches in each subject that assess students’ abilities to analyze, apply, evaluate, and create.

Grading (pp. 673–680) Describe two kinds of grading. Grading can be either norm referenced or criterion referenced. One popular norm-referenced system is grading on the curve, based on a ranking of students in relation to the average performance level. This is not recommended. Criterion-referenced report cards usually indicate how well the individual student has met each of several learning goals. How can failure support learning? Students need experience in coping with failure, so standards must be high enough to encourage effort. Occasional failure can be positive if appropriate feedback is provided. Making errors in low-stakes contexts can enhance learning if students get quality corrective feedback. Students who never learn how to cope with failure may give up quickly when their first efforts are unsuccessful.

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

681

Which is better, “social promotion” or being “held back”? Simply retaining or promoting a student who is having difficulty will not guarantee that the student will learn. Unless the student is very young or emotionally immature compared to others in the class, the best approach may be to promote, but provide extra support such as tutoring or summer school sessions. Differentiated instruction could prevent problems. Can grades promote learning and motivation? Learning is increased by written or oral feedback that includes specific comments on errors or faulty strategies, but that balances this criticism with suggestions about how to improve—along with comments on the positive aspects of the work. Grades can encourage students’ motivation to learn if they are tied to meaningful learning. How can communications with families support learning? Not every communication from the teacher needs to be tied to a grade. Communication with students and families can be important in helping a teacher understand students and present effective instruction by creating a consistent learning environment. Students and families have a legal right to see all the information in the students’ records, so the contents of files must be appropriate, accurate, and supported by evidence.

MODULE 47

Standardized Testing and Teacher Evaluation

Learning Objective 15.5 Explain how to interpret common standardized test scores (percentile rank, stanine, grade-equivalent, scale score) as well as current issues and criticisms concerning accountability and teacher evaluation, high-stakes assessment, growth versus proficiency tests, and value-added approaches.

Standardized Testing For as long as I can remember, educators and policymakers have been concerned about the test performance of American students. Today, politicians point to the Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) (Mullis et al., 2020) showing that the United States is still behind many other developed countries in math and science test scores. In response, there has been more testing at every grade, so you must be knowledgeable. Understanding what standardized test scores really mean and how they can be used (or misused) is a good start.

Types of Scores Put Yourself in Their Place At your first parent conference, a mother and father are concerned about their child’s percentile rank of 86. They say that they expect their child to “get close to 100%. We know she should be able to do that because her grade-equivalent score is half a year above her grade!” Do they understand the meaning of these scores? To understand the scores from tests, you need to know some basics about different types of scores and what they tell you, but first you need to know some (easy) statistics. You have probably had a great deal of experience with means. A mean is simply the arithmetical average of a group of scores. To calculate the mean, you add the scores and divide the total by the number of scores in the distribution. The mean offers one way of measuring central tendency, the score that is typical or representative of the whole group of scores. But very high or very low scores affect the mean, so when there are a few very high or low scores, the median may be better than the mean to represent central tendency. The median is the middle score in a ranked list of scores, the point at which half the scores are larger and half are smaller. The mode is the score that occurs most often.

MEASUREMENTS OF CENTRAL TENDENCY AND STANDARD DEVIATION.

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Concepts of Standardized Testing (II, C5) Be able to define norm groups, measures of central tendency, standard deviation, normal distribution, reliability, and validity and to explain their roles in standardized tests.

Mean Arithmetical average.

Central tendency Typical score for a group of scores.

Median Middle score in a group of scores.

Mode Most frequently occurring score.

682 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Standard deviation Measure of how widely scores vary from the mean.

Variability Degree of difference or deviation from mean.

Range Distance between the highest and the lowest scores in a group.

Normal distribution The most commonly occurring distribution, in which scores are distributed evenly around the mean.

The measure of central tendency gives a score that is representative of the group of scores, but it does not tell you anything about how the scores are distributed. Two groups of scores may both have a mean of 50 but be alike in no other way. One group might contain the scores 50, 45, 55, 55, 45, 50, 50; the other group might contain the scores 100, 0, 50, 90, 10, 50, 50. In both cases, the mean, median, and mode are all 50, but the distributions are quite different. The standard deviation is a measure of how widely the scores vary from the mean. The larger the standard deviation, the more spread out the scores are in the distribution. The smaller the standard deviation, the more the scores are clustered around the mean. For example, in the distribution 50, 45, 55, 55, 45, 50, 50, the standard deviation is much smaller than in the distribution 100, 0, 50, 90, 10, 50, 50. Another way of saying this is that distributions with very small standard deviations have less variability in the scores. Knowing the mean and the standard deviation of a group of scores gives you a better picture of the meaning of an individual score. For example, suppose you received a score of 78 on a test. You would be very pleased with the score if the mean of the test were 70 and the standard deviation were 4. In this case, your score would be 2 standard deviations above the mean, a score well above average. Consider the difference if the mean of the test had remained at 70 but the standard deviation had been 20. In the second case, your score of 78 would be less than 1 standard deviation from the mean. You would be much closer to the middle of the group, with a score above average but not high. Knowing the standard deviation tells you much more than simply knowing the range of scores. No matter how the majority scored on the tests, one or two students may do very well or very poorly and thus make the range very large. THE NORMAL DISTRIBUTION. Standard deviations are very useful in understanding test results.

They are especially helpful if the results of the tests form a normal distribution. You may have encountered the normal distribution before. It is the bell-shaped curve, the most famous frequency distribution because it describes many naturally occurring physical and social phenomena. Many scores fall in the middle, giving the curve its bell appearance. You find fewer and fewer scores as you look out toward the end points, or tails, of the distribution. The normal distribution has been thoroughly analyzed by statisticians. The mean of a normal distribution is also its midpoint. Half the scores are above the mean, and half are below it. In a normal distribution, the mean, median, and mode are all the same point. Another convenient property of the normal distribution is that the percentage of scores falling within each area of the curve is known, as you can see in Figure 15.3.

Figure 15.3 The Normal Distribution The normal distribution, or bell-shaped curve, has certain predictable characteristics. For example, 68% of the scores are clustered within 1 standard deviation below to 1 standard deviation above the mean.

34%

2%

34%

2%

14%

.1

14%

–2 SD

–1 SD

0

+1 SD

+2 SD

1

10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90

99

Percentile Ranks

99.9

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

A person scoring within 1 standard deviation of the mean obviously has company. Many scores pile up here. In fact, 68% of all scores are located in the area from 1 standard deviation below to 1 standard deviation above the mean. About 16% of the scores are higher than 1 standard deviation above the mean. Of this higher group, only 2% are higher than 2 standard deviations above the mean. Similarly, only about 16% of the scores are less than 1 standard deviation below the mean, and of that group only about 2% are lower than 2 standard deviations below the mean. At 2 standard deviations from the mean in either direction, the scorer has left the pack. The SAT college entrance exam is one example of a normal distribution. The mean of the Math section is about 500 with a standard deviation of 100, though in 2020, the actual mean was 523 and the standard deviation was 117 (College Board, 2020). So, because only 16% of the scores are higher than 1 standard deviation above the mean in a normal distribution, only about 16% of the people who took the Math test scored above 640 (523 + 117 = 640). Now we are ready to look at different kinds of test scores. Ranking is the basis for one very useful kind of score reported on standardized tests, a percentile rank score. In percentile ranking, each student’s raw score (actual number correct) is compared with the raw scores of the students in the norm group (comparison group). The percentile rank shows the percentage of students in the norm group that scored below a particular raw score. If a student’s score were better than threequarters of the students in the norm group, the student would score in the 75th percentile or have a percentile rank of 75. You can see that this does not mean that the student had a raw score of 75 correct answers or even that the student answered 75% of the questions correctly. Rather, the 75 refers to the percentage of people in the norm group whose scores on the test were below this student’s score. A percentile rank of 50 means that a student has scored better than 50% of the norm group and has achieved an average score. There is one caution in interpreting percentile scores. Differences in percentile ranks do not mean the same thing in terms of raw score points in the middle of the scale as they do at the fringes. For example, the difference between the 50th and 60th percentile might be just 2 raw points, whereas the difference on the same test between the 90th and 99th percentile could be about 10 points. So a few answers right or wrong can make a bigger difference in percentile scores if you are near the middle.

PERCENTILE RANK SCORES.

Grade-equivalent scores are generally obtained from separate norm groups for each grade level. The average of the scores of all the 10th-graders in the norm group defines the 10th-grade–equivalent score. Suppose the raw-score average of the 10th-grade norm group is 38. Any student who attains a raw score of 38 on that test will be assigned a grade-equivalent score of 10th grade. Grade-equivalent scores are generally listed in numbers such as 8.3, 4.5, 7.6, 11.5, and so on. The whole number gives the grade. The decimals stand for tenths of a year, but they are usually interpreted as months. Suppose a student with the grade-equivalent score of 10 is a seventh-grader. Should this student be promoted immediately? Probably not. Different forms of tests are used at different grade levels, so the seventh-grader may not have had to answer items that would be given to 10th-graders. The high score may represent superior mastery of material at the seventh-grade level rather than a capacity for doing advanced work. Even though an average 10th-grader could do as well as our seventh-grader on this particular test, the 10th-grader would certainly know much more than this seventh-grade test covered. Also, grade-equivalent score units do not mean the same thing at every grade level. For example, a second-grader reading at the first-grade level would have more trouble in school than an 11th-grader who reads at the 10th-grade level. Because grade-equivalent scores are misleading and are often misinterpreted, especially by parents, most educators and psychologists strongly believe they should not be used at all. Several other forms of reporting are more appropriate.

GRADE-EQUIVALENT SCORES.

As you may remember, one problem with percentile ranks is the difficulty in making comparisons among ranks. A discrepancy of a certain number of raw-score

STANDARD SCORES.

Percentile rank Percentage of those in the norming sample who scored below an individual’s score.

Grade-equivalent score Measure of grade level based on comparison with norming samples from each grade.

683

684 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Standard scores Scores based on the standard deviation.

z score Standard score indicating the number of standard deviations above or below the mean that a particular score falls.

T score Standard score with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10.

points has a different meaning at different places on the scale. With standard scores, on the other hand, a difference of 10 points is the same everywhere on the scale. Standard scores are based on the standard deviation. A very common standard score is called the z score. A z score tells how many standard deviations above or below the average a raw score is. In the example described earlier, in which you were fortunate enough to get a 78 on a test where the mean was 70 and the standard deviation was 4, your z score would be +2, or 2 standard deviations above the mean. If a person were to score 64 on this test, the score would be 1.5 standard deviations below the mean, and the z score would be −1.5. A z score of 0 would be no standard deviations above the mean—in other words, right on the mean. Measurements similar to z scores are used when you take a bone density test. Your score will compare your bone density to that of a healthy 30-year-old. If your score is below −1, you are moving toward osteoporosis. Below −2, you are there. Because it is often inconvenient to use negative numbers, other standard scores have been devised to eliminate this difficulty. The T score has a mean of 50 and uses a standard deviation of 10. Thus, a T score of 50 indicates average performance. If you multiply the z score by 10 (which eliminates the decimal) and add 50 (which gets rid of the negative number), you get the equivalent T score as the answer. The person whose z score was −1.5 would have a T score of 35. First multiply the z score by 10: −1.5 − 10 = −15 Then add 50: −15 + 50 = 35

Stanine scores Whole-number scores from 1 to 9, each representing a wide range of raw scores.

Before we leave this discussion of types of scores, we should look at one other widely used method. Stanine scores (the name comes from “standard nine”) are standard scores. There are only nine possible scores on the stanine scale, the whole numbers 1 through 9. The mean is 5, and the standard deviation is 2. Stanine scores provide a method of considering a student’s rank because each of the nine scores includes a specific range of percentile scores in the normal distribution. For example, a stanine score of 1 is assigned to the bottom 4% of scores in a distribution. A stanine of 2 is assigned to the next 7%. Of course, some raw scores in this 7% range are better than others, but they all get a stanine score of 2. Each stanine score represents a wide range of raw scores. This has the advantage of encouraging teachers and parents to view a student’s score in more general terms instead of making fine distinctions based on a few points. Figure 15.4 compares the four types of standard scores we have considered, showing how each would fall on a normal distribution curve.

Figure 15.4 Three Types of Standard Scores on a Normal Distribution Curve Using this figure, you can translate one type of standard into another.

–3 SD Standard deviation z scores –3 T scores 20 SAT scores 200 Stanine scores

–2 SD

–1 SD

0

+1 SD

+2 SD

+3 SD

–2 30

–1 40

(Mean) 0 50

+1 60

+2 70

+3 80

300

400

500

600

700

800

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Interpreting Standardized Test Reports Stop & Think Look at the test printout in Figure 15.5. What are this student’s strengths and weaknesses? How do you know? What specific information can teachers expect from achievement test results? Test publishers usually provide individual profiles for each student, showing scores on each subtest. Figure 15.5 is an example of a Student Report for Sally, a fourth-grader, on the Stanford Achievement Test, 10th Edition. Note that the Student Report has three sections. The first (About This Student’s Performance) is a brief narrative explanation that may include a Lexile Measure™ (see http:// lexile.com), which is computed from the Reading Comprehension score and helps teachers identify Sally’s reading level in order to select appropriate texts. The second section (Subtests and Totals) attempts to paint a picture of the student’s achievement in Reading, Mathematics, Language, Spelling, Science, Social Science, Listening, and Thinking Skills. That section also includes total scores on the battery of tests and scores on the Otis-Lennon School Ability test—a kind of group IQ or scholastic aptitude test. Some of the subtests are further divided into more specific assessments. For example, Reading is broken down into Word Study Skills, Reading Vocabulary, and Reading Comprehension. Next to each subtest are several different ways of reporting Sally’s score. The school decides which scores are reported based on a list of possible reporting formats. This school chose the following types of scores:

Number Correct: Under the second column is the number of items that Sally answered correctly for that subtest (the total number of items on the subtest is in the first column, Number Possible). Scaled Score: This is the basic score used to derive all the other scores, sometimes called a growth score because it describes growth in achievement that typically occurs as students move through the grades. For example, the average score for third-graders might be 585, whereas the average score for 10th-graders might be 714 on tests with possible scores that range from 0 to 1,000 across the entire K–12 grades. Often, the difficulty of items is included in calculating scale scores (Popham, 2020). National PR-S (National Percentile Rank and Stanine): This score tells us where Sally stands in relation to students at her grade level across the country in terms of percentile rank (percent with a lower score) and stanine, so a score of 59-5 is a percentile of 59 and a stanine of 5. National NCE (Normal Curve Equivalent): This is a standard score derived from the percentile rank, with a range of 1 to 99, a mean of 50, and a standard deviation of 21. Grade Equivalent: This indicates that Sally’s scaled score is the same as an average student in the indicated grade and month of school. Beware of the problems with grade-equivalent scores described earlier. AAC (Achievement/Ability Comparison) Range: The ACC score compares Sally’s achievement on each subtest to a norm group of other students who have her same ability as measured by the Otis-Lennon School Ability test. The ACC range categorizes Sally’s ACC score as HIGH, MIDDLE, or LOW. You can see that Sally is in the middle on most of the subtests, so her achievement is in the middle compared to students with abilities similar to hers. National Grade Percentile Bands: This is the range of national percentile scores in which Sally’s true score is likely to fall. You may remember from our discussion of true scores that this range, or confidence interval, is determined by adding and subtracting the standard error of the test from Sally’s actual score. Chances are high that Sally’s true score is within this range. Bands that do not overlap indicate likely differences in achievement. The bottom of Figure 15.5 (clusters) breaks Sally’s subtests down into even more specific skills. For each skill we see the number of questions possible to answer (NP), the number

685

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Interpreting Achievement Tests (II, C4) Accurate information from the teacher is essential for students’ academic progress. See Cluster 7 for a discussion of how to use praise effectively. These guidelines apply to written feedback as well.

Figure 15.5 A Typical Score Report

Source: Sample Stanford Student Report in score report sampler: Guide teaching and learning toward high academic standards for the Stanford Achievement Test Series, 10th Edition (Stanford 10). Copyright © 2009 by NCS Pearson, Inc. Reproduced with permission. All rights reserved.

A sample test score report, with no actual test data used.

686 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Sally attempted (NA), and the number she got correct (NC). The check marks beside the skills indicate if she is average, above average, or below average in each. Notice that some skills are assessed with only a few (3 to 8) questions. Remember that fewer items mean less reliability. As a teacher you probably will be expected to explain test results to your students and their families. Make sure you know the meaning of all the types of scores your school reports—raw scores, percentiles and percentile bands, scale scores, Lexiles, grade equivalents, stanines, or any others that appear on test reports. With high school students, it is also useful to tell both the students and their parents that only 25% of academic success in college is associated with a student’s scores on the SAT or ACT. The other 75% is due to motivation, hard work, study habits, interests, and other factors under the student’s control (Popham, 2020). High school grades are better predictors than admission test scores of on-time college graduation, partly because grades provide information about student self-regulation and motivation (Galla et al., 2019). This can be critically important information for families who are afraid their children “aren’t meant for college.” The Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships Conferences and Explaining Test Results give some other tips.

DISCUSSING TEST RESULTS WITH FAMILIES.

GUIDELINES Family and Community Partnerships Conferences and Explaining Test Results General Conferencing Guidelines Decide on a few clear goals for the conference. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Gather information about the student to help in your instruction. Explain grades or test results. Let parents know what is coming during the next unit or marking period. Solicit help from parents. Make suggestions for use at home.

Begin and end with a positive statement. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

“Jacob is a natural leader.” “Eve really enjoys the science center.” “Yesim is really supportive when other students are upset.” “Ashanti’s sense of humor keeps the class positive.”

Listen actively. Examples 1. 2.

Accept the emotions of parents or caregivers. Don’t try to talk them out of what they feel. “You seem to feel frustrated when Lee doesn’t do his homework.”

Respect family members’ time and their concerns about their child—establish a partnership. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Speak plainly and briefly, and avoid jargon. Be tactful, but don’t avoid talking about tough issues. Ask families to follow through on class goals at home: “Ask Leona for her homework checklist, and help her keep it up to date. I will do the same at school.”

Learn from the family members. Examples 1.

Send home a brief questionnaire to be returned before the conference so you can prepare: What are the parents’ concerns and questions? (Continued)

687

688 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

2.

What are the students’ interests and strengths as revealed in hobbies or extracurricular activities?

Follow up and follow through. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Send a brief note thanking the family members for attending. Share student successes through notes or email messages. Keep families informed before problems develop.

Explaining and Using Test Results In nontechnical terms, explain the meaning of each type of score on the test report and explain why tests are not “perfect.” Examples 1.

2. 3. 4.

If the test is norm referenced, know what the comparison group was (national? state? local district?). Explain that the child’s score shows how he or she performed in relation to the other students in the comparison group. If the test is criterion referenced, explain that the scores show how well their child performed specific tasks such as word problems or reading comprehension. Encourage parents to think of the score not as a single point, but as a range or band that includes the score. Ignore small differences between scores.

For norm-referenced tests, use percentile scores. They are the easiest to understand. Examples 1.

2.

Percentile scores tell what percentage of students in the comparison group made the same score or lower. Higher percentiles are better, and 99 is as high as you can get; 50 is average. Percentile scores do not tell the “percent correct,” so scores that would be bad on a classroom test (say, 65% to 75% or so) are above average—even good—as percentile scores.

Avoid using grade-equivalent scores. Examples 1.

2.

If parents want to focus on the “grade level” of their child, tell them that high gradeequivalent scores reflect a thorough understanding of the current grade level and NOT the capacity to do higher grade-level work. Tell parents that the same grade-equivalent score has different meanings in different subjects—reading versus mathematics, for example.

Source: Based on ideas from The successful classroom: Management strategies for regular and special education teachers, by D. P. Fromberg & M. Driscoll. Published by Teachers College, Columbia University; Scholastic. (2011). Teacher tips for successful parent-teacher conferences, retrieved from https://www.scholastic.com/teachers/articles/teaching-content/teacher-tips-successful-parent-teacher-conferences/; and T. E. Eissenberg & Lawrence M. Rudner (1988). Explaining test results to parents. Retrieved from https://scholarworks.umass.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi? article=1001&context=pare

Accountability and High-Stakes Testing Stop & Think How has standardized testing affected your life so far? What opportunities have been opened or closed to you based on test scores? Was the process fair? Every day, decisions about individuals are based on the results of tests. Should Darius be issued a driver’s license? How many and which students from the eighth grade would benefit from an accelerated program in science? Who needs extra tutoring? Test scores may affect “admission” to first grade, promotion from one grade to the next, high school graduation, access to special programs, teacher licensure and tenure, and school funding.

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing MAKING DECISIONS. When making decisions about individuals based on test results, it is important to distinguish between the quality of the test itself and the way the test is used. Who will be tested? What are the consequences of choosing one test over another for a particular purpose with a given group? What is the effect of the testing on the students? How will the test scores of minority-group students be interpreted? What do we really mean by intelligence, achievement, growth, and proficiency? Do the tests we use to measure these constructs capture what we intended to assess? How will test results be integrated with other information about the individual to make judgments? Answering these questions requires ethical choices based on values as well as accurate information about what tests can and cannot tell us. Keep these values issues in mind as we examine testing uses and decisions. Because the decisions affected by test scores are so critical, many educators call this process high-stakes testing. One of the high-stakes uses for test results is to hold teachers, schools, and administrators accountable for student performance. For example, teacher bonuses might be tied to their students’ achievement, or schools’ funding may be affected by testing results. But, as James Pellegrino (2014) noted, accountability should never be the only purpose of testing: “The problem is that other purposes of assessment, such as providing instructionally relevant feedback to teachers and students, get lost when the sole goal of states is to use them to obtain an estimate of how much students have learned in the course of a year” (p. 1). We need whole systems of assessments that give teachers and students tools for instruction and learning (Guskey, 2020).

The teachers I work with are frustrated that test results often come too late in the year to help them plan instruction or remediation for their current students. This is one reason formative and interim assessments are becoming more popular— the teachers get the information in time to modify and improve instruction. Teachers also are troubled by the amount of time that testing takes—both to prepare for the tests and to give them. They complain that the tests cover material that their curriculum does not include. What are other problems with high-stakes testing?

WHAT DO TEACHERS THINK?

Studies have found that in some states, 80% of the elementary schools spend about 20% of their instructional time preparing for the end-of-grade tests (Abrams & Madaus, 2003). Studies of the actual high-stakes tests in action show testing narrows the curriculum. What is tested is studied, what is not tested is ignored (Haertel, 2018). For example, using the Texas Assessment of Academic Skills has led to curriculum changes that overemphasize what is tested and neglect other areas. In addition, it seems that the test of mathematics is also a test of reading. Students with poor reading ability have trouble with the math test, especially if their first language is not English. Another problem—a study of 8 million fourth- and eighth-grade students taking state assessments in 47 states found that the test format affects the scores of girls compared with boys. Males generally do better on multiple-choice items and females on essay items, so in states that rely more on multiple-choice items, there may be a gender gap favoring boys, and the opposite will be true for essay items. The researchers found that about 25% of the variation in gender gaps among the 47 states was due to test item format (Reardon et al., 2018). Finally, tests have become too important—an end in themselves. As one of the most prominent measurement experts has said: “Testing is no longer being employed merely to measure the effects of policy interventions—rather, it has itself become the intervention . . .” (Haertel, 2018, p. 204). Some uses of standardized tests just are not appropriate, as illustrated in Table 15.4.

689

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Standardized Testing: Major Issues (II, C5) Since their inception, there have been controversies regarding the use of standardized tests in the schools. Familiarize yourself with the major issues that underlie these controversies. Explain the positions of the different camps in these controversies.

High-stakes testing Standardized tests whose results have powerful influences when used by school administrators, other officials, or employers to make decisions.

Accountable Making teachers and schools responsible for student learning, usually by monitoring learning with high-stakes tests.

DOCUMENTED PROBLEMS WITH HIGH-STAKES TESTING.

New Directions: PARCC and SBAC In the early 2010s, the federal government gave millions of dollars to two large consortia of states, the Partnership for Assessment of Readiness for College and Careers (PARCC) and the Smarter Balanced Assessment Consortium (SBAC), to develop high-quality assessments

Connect and Extend to PRAXIS II® Alternatives to Standardized Testing (II, C1) For an overview of the major forms of authentic testing, go to Teachervision.com and search for “authentic assessment.”

690 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Table 15.4 Inappropriate Uses for High-Stakes Test Results Beware of some uses for standardized test results. Tests were not designed for these purposes. Pass/Fail Decisions

To deny students graduation from any grade, there must be strong evidence that the test used is valid, reliable, and free of bias. Some tests have been challenged in the courts and found to meet these standards, but not all tests are good enough to make pass/fail decisions.

State-to-State Comparisons

You cannot really compare states using standardized test scores. States do not have the same curricula, tests, resources, or challenges. If comparisons are made, they usually tell us what we already know—some states have more funding for schools and more families with higher incomes or higher education levels.

Evaluation of Teachers or Schools

Many influences on test scores—family and community resources—are outside the control of teachers and schools. Often students move from school to school, so many students taking a test in the spring may have been attending that school only for a few weeks.

Identifying Where to Buy a House

Generally speaking, the schools with the highest test scores are in the neighborhoods where families have the highest levels of education and income. They may not be the “best schools” in terms of teaching, programs, academic growth for their students, or leadership, but they are the schools lucky enough to have the “right” students.

Source: From Haladyna, T. H. (2002). Essentials of standardized achievement testing: Validity & accountability. Pearson Education, Inc. Adapted by permission.

aligned to the Common Core State Standards. The assessments were meant to measure complex thinking using a range of testing approaches including performance assessment. Since then, some states have formed smaller consortia or are working independently to develop their own assessments that match the standards in their states (Popham, 2020). Most of these assessments are online—no more paper-and-pencil tests—so feedback can be faster and can help inform instruction. Stay tuned for changes in your state. But no matter how fancy the computer-based testing is, the same questions still apply to these new tests: Are they valid? Are they reliable? Do they measure what you taught? Do they measure important outcomes that match the challenges of life today? Do they give you information to improve your teaching (Pellegrino, 2014)?

Using High-Stakes Testing Well: Lessons for Teachers Overreliance on test scores can be harmful to student learning, but test scores can provide some information if used wisely. After all, good tests should assess what students know, and knowledge is a major goal of schooling (Goldhaber & Özek, 2019). Of course, the tests used must be reliable, valid for the purposes used, and free of bias. In addition, the testing program must: 1. Match the content standards of the district—this is a vital part of validity. 2. Be part of the larger assessment plan. No individual test provides all the necessary information about student achievement. It is critical that schools avoid making pass/ fail decisions based on a single test. 3. Test complex thinking, not just skills and factual knowledge. 4. Provide alternate assessment strategies for students with identifiable disabilities. 5. Provide opportunities for retesting when the stakes are high. 6. Include all students in the testing but also provide informative reports of the results that make the students’ situations clear if they have special challenges or circumstances such as disabilities. 7. Provide appropriate remediation when students fail. 8. Make sure all students taking the test have adequate opportunities to learn the material being tested. 9. Take into account the first language of the students. Students who have difficulty reading or writing in English will not perform well on tests that require English proficiency. 10. Use test results for children, not against them (Haladyna, 2002).

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Reaching Every Student: Helping Students with Disabilities Prepare for High-Stakes Tests Erik Carter and his colleagues (2005) tested a procedure for preparing students with learning disabilities, mild intellectual disabilities, and language impairments for a highstakes state test. The students were ages 15 to 19; over half were Black males, and all had individualized education programs (IEPs—see Cluster 5) to guide their education. None had passed the state-required achievement tests. Over six class periods, an instructor taught the students strategies such as filling in bubbles on answer sheets completely, sorting problems by difficulty and doing the easy ones first, using rounding to estimate answers in math, identifying exactly what the question is asking by underlining key words and phrases, and employing strategies for eliminating alternatives that have redundant information or extreme qualifiers. The good news is that after completing the preparation program, students improved their scores significantly on the tests. But the bad news is that the increases were not large enough to bring most of the students to the passing level. The authors recommend that preparation for testing should occur much earlier for students with disabilities. At an average age of 16, the students in this study already were discouraged (Carter et al., 2005). The Guidelines: Preparing Yourself and Your Students for Testing should help you and all your students prepare for high-stakes testing.

GUIDELINES Preparing Yourself and Your Students for Testing Advice for Teachers Make sure the test actually covers the content of the unit of study. Examples 1. 2. 3.

Compare test questions to course learning goals. Make sure that there is good overlap. Check to see if the test is long enough to cover all important topics. Find out if there are any complications your students experience with the test, such as not enough time, too difficult a level of reading, and so on. If there are issues, discuss these problems with appropriate school personnel.

Make sure students know how to use all the test materials. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Several days before the testing, do a few practice questions with a similar format. Demonstrate the use of the answer sheets, especially computer-scored answer sheets. Check with new students, shy students, slower students, and students who have difficulty reading to make sure they understand the questions. Make sure students know if and when guessing is appropriate.

Follow instructions for administering the test exactly. Examples 1. 2.

Practice giving the test before you actually use it. Follow the time limits exactly.

Make students as comfortable as possible during testing. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Do not create anxiety by making the test seem like the most important event of the year. Help the class relax before beginning the test, perhaps by telling a joke or having everyone take a few deep breaths. Don’t be tense yourself! Make sure the room is quiet. Discourage cheating by monitoring the room. Don’t become absorbed in your own paperwork. (Continued)

691

692 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

Advice for Students Use the night before the test effectively. Examples 1. 2.

Study the night before the exam, ending with a final look at a summary of the key points, concepts, and relationships. Get a good night’s sleep. If you know you generally have trouble sleeping the night before an exam, try getting extra sleep on several previous nights.

Set the situation so you can concentrate on the test. Examples 1. 2.

Give yourself plenty of time to eat and get to the exam room. Don’t sit near a friend. It may make concentration difficult. If your friend leaves early, you may be tempted to do so, too.

Make sure you know what the test is asking. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4.

Read the directions carefully. If you are unsure, ask the instructor or proctor for clarification. Read each question carefully to spot tricky words, such as not, except, and all of the following but one. On an essay test, read every question first, so you know the size of the job ahead of you and can make informed decisions about how much time to spend on each question. On a multiple-choice test, read every alternative, even if an early one seems right.

Use time effectively. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Begin working right away and move as rapidly as possible while your energy is high. Do the easy questions first. Don’t get stuck on one question. If you are stumped, mark the question so you can return to it easily later, and go on to questions you can answer more quickly. On a multiple-choice test, if you know you will not have time to finish, fill in all the remaining questions with the same letter if there is no penalty for guessing. If you are running out of time on an essay test, do not leave any questions blank. Briefly outline a few key points to show the instructor you knew the answer but needed more time.

Know when to guess on multiple-choice or true/false tests. Examples 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Always guess when only right answers are scored. Always guess when you can eliminate some of the alternatives. Don’t guess if there is a penalty for guessing, unless you can confidently eliminate at least one alternative. Are correct answers always longer? shorter? in the middle? more likely to be one letter? more often true than false? Does the grammar give the right answer away or eliminate any alternatives?

Check your work. Examples 1. 2.

Even if you can’t stand to look at the test another minute, reread each question to make sure you answered the way you intended. If you are using a machine-scored answer sheet, check occasionally to be sure the number of the question you are answering corresponds to the number of the answer on the sheet.

On essay tests, answer as directly as possible. Examples 1. 2.

Avoid flowery introductions. Answer the question in the first sentence and then elaborate. Don’t save your best ideas for last. Give them early in the answer.

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

3.

693

Unless the instructor requires complete sentences, consider listing points, arguments, and so on by number in your answer. It will help you organize your thoughts and concentrate on the important aspects of the answer.

Learn from the testing experience. Examples 1. 2.

Pay attention when the teacher reviews the answers. You can learn from your mistakes, and the same question may reappear in a later test. Notice if you are having trouble with a particular kind of item; adjust your study approach next time to handle this type of item better.

Teacher Accountability and Evaluation One application of standardized tests worries educators—these less-than-perfect tests are sometimes used to evaluate schools and even individual teachers. But it is not fair to evaluate teachers based on test scores for students who began the year with them far below grade level or who were only in the class a few months (Anagnostopoulos et al., 2021). These real concerns have led to new ideas about test scores and testing. Even here, there are strong cautions—read on. What would you say about a teacher whose students began the year reading at the third-grade level and ended the year reading at the fifth-grade level? Sounds like a great year of growth in reading, right? But what if the students were sixth-graders? If we judge that teacher only by her students’ achievement at the end of the year, we might think the teacher failed—students ending the sixth grade and still reading at the fifth-grade level! No teaching awards for her! But actually the teacher was very effective (assuming she had the students all year). She added value to their learning—2 years’ worth, in fact. The idea of value-added modeling (VAM) is to assess actual growth compared to some baseline of expected average growth. If students can be expected to grow one grade level but they grow two, that is above expected growth. Value-added measures use statistical procedures to determine what students could be expected to learn based on student data from previous years and maybe other relevant information. If the actual student achievement is greater than predicted, then the estimate of the teacher’s or school’s effect is positive (value is added). If students score as predicted, then the effect is zero, and if they score lower than predicted, then the effect is negative. You can imagine that to make good judgments with a value-added approach, the tests used must be valid, reliable, and aligned with the curriculum. In addition, there must be room at the top and the bottom of the test scores to capture a full range of achievement. Most current standardized tests, however, measure only grade-level standards, so they are not good at assessing growth for students who perform well above or below the grade level. To assess growth, teachers should have the students for most of the year. Also, the smaller the focus (on just one class instead of a whole school), the more uncertain is the estimate of effects, so the same teacher might seem to have larger effects in some years and smaller effects in other years. But here are the two biggest concerns. First, to assess teacher effects on learning, students should be assessed as they enter a teacher’s class and when they leave. But that would mean giving standardized tests twice a year and too much time is taken up already with testing. In practice, usually student scores from the previous spring testing are compared to the current-year testing, but that means the summer learning loss that many low-income students experience can affect scores, and the teacher had nothing to do with summer learning loss (Atteberry & Mangan, 2020). Second, to really be able to attribute student gains on test scores to their teacher, the students and teachers would have to be

VALUE-ADDED MODELS.

Value-added modeling (VAM) Approaches that use statistical analyses to indicate the average test score gain for students, adjusted for their student characteristics such as prior level of achievement.

694 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing assigned randomly to classes—that never happens. So even with very good, well-chosen tests, other factors involved in who gets into which class with which teacher may still be contributing to test score gains or losses. For example, consider a beloved math teacher in the school. Parents of the best math students push to get their children in this teacher’s class. Because the students are already scoring at the top of the yearly achievement tests, there is little room for growth, and the teacher’s value-added contribution could seem low. Or the scores of other students might not “grow” because the school was in bad physical condition, the instructional materials were poor quality and dated, or the students worked after school and never had time for homework—all outside the teacher’s control (Popham, 2020). In an extensive review of value-added research, Kimberlee Everson (2017) noted that no one has suggested a “framework or methodology for untangling a teacher’s contribution from confounding influences, such as access to classroom materials, specialist support, or tutoring” (p. 61). The researchers who have developed value-added modeling are very concerned about misuses, especially in teacher evaluation (Tobiason, 2019). To be valuable, the value-added scores must be based on several years of data from a large number of students. These measures should only be used to identify strengths and weaknesses in the school or the curriculum and to guide professional development, but not to evaluate individual teachers (American Educational Research Association Council, 2015; Blazar et al., 2016). Still, you may be in a school that uses value-added measures, so it makes sense to learn about them. In her extensive review of VAM research, Kimberlee Everson (2017) concluded that we cannot use value-added measures or any procedures to evaluate teachers until we answer some fundamental questions about what is good teaching—a question we have raised throughout this book. Everson asks, is a good teacher one who helps students reach only academic goals or nonacademic goals as well? Does a good teacher bring all students to a set standard, regardless of their background, or does the teacher strive to take every student as far as she or he can go? Who is the right comparison person for a teacher? In Cluster 1, we described different approaches to defining good teaching. Making sure the way we evaluate teachers matches our conception of good teaching is really a question about validity in assessing teachers. In terms of reliability, in an extensive test of the New Mexico evaluation system as applied to about 21,000 teachers from 2015 to 2016, Sy Doan and colleagues (2019) found that 40% of the teachers they studied would have gotten a different composite score if they were reevaluated in the same year. The greater the weight of value-added information in the composite scores, the less consistent the estimates of scores at reevaluation. SO HOW SHOULD WE EVALUATE TEACHERS?

In 2009, the Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation launched the Measures of Effective Teaching (MET) Project, a research partnership between 3,000 teachers and research teams at dozens of institutions. The foundation tackled teacher evaluation because research shows that teachers matter; they matter more than technology or funding or school facilities. In pursuing the goal, the project members made a key assumption: teaching is complex; multiple measures will be needed to capture effective teaching and provide useful feedback for personnel decisions and professional development. In addition to using student achievement gains on state tests, the MET researchers examined many established and newer measures of effectiveness. The final report of the project (MET Project, 2013) identified the following three measures that, if used together, are a valid and reliable way of assessing teaching that leads to student learning:

A BROADER APPROACH: MEASURES OF TEACHER EFFECTIVENESS.

1. Student gains on state tests. 2. Surveys of student perceptions of their teachers based on the Tripod Student Perception Survey developed by Ron Ferguson at Harvard University (Ferguson, 2008). This survey asks students to agree or disagree with statements such as “My teacher takes time to help us remember what we learn” (for K–2 students); “In class we learn to correct our mistakes” (upper elementary students); and “In this class, my teacher accepts

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

nothing less than our full effort” (secondary students) (from Cambridge Education, Tripod Project, Student Survey System). 3. Classroom observations from the Danielson (2013) Framework for Teaching. These look like good choices. Both Tripod scores from students and Danielson Framework scores from observers predict student academic growth on achievement tests (Sandilos et al., 2019). But remember, teaching is complex. To capture effective teaching, these measures have to be used accurately and together. Also, as you look at teacher observation in general—the same person teaching the same material to different students can be rated as more or less effective by an observer, depending on the students (Fauth et al., 2020). As experienced teachers know, students who are more interested and motivated make teachers look better. Also, there is evidence that male teachers in general and teachers in classrooms with high concentrations of Black, Latino, male, and low-performing students receive significantly lower class-observation ratings. Class composition may bias ratings (Campbell & Ronfeldt, 2018). No system is perfect. The most important system for you is the one that your school will use to evaluate your teaching—find out what it is.

Module 47 Summary Standardized Testing (pp. 681–695) What are mean, median, mode, and standard deviation? The mean (arithmetical average), median (middle score), and mode (most common score) are all measures of central tendency. The standard deviation reveals how scores spread out around the mean. A normal distribution is a frequency distribution represented as a bell-shaped curve. Many scores cluster in the middle; the farther from the midpoint, the fewer the scores. Describe different kinds of scores. There are several basic types of standardized test scores: percentile rankings, which indicate the percentage of others who scored at or below an individual’s score; grade-equivalent scores, which indicate how closely a student’s performance matches average scores for a given grade; and standard scores, which are based on the standard deviation. T and z scores are both common standard scores. A stanine score is a standard score that incorporates elements of percentile rankings. What are some current issues in testing? Controversy over standardized testing has focused on the role of tests and interpretation of results, the problems with accountability based on test scores, and the ways testing restricts the curriculum. If the test matches important learning targets of the curriculum, is given to students who actually studied the curriculum for a reasonable period of time, is free of bias, fits the students’ language capabilities, and was administered properly, then test results provide some information about the effectiveness of the school. Expert teachers see both the advantages of and problems with standardized testing, but about 50% believe such tests do more harm than good. Teachers should use results to improve instruction, not to stereotype students or justify lowered expectations. PARCC and SBAC, two large consortia of states, are developing a new set of K–12 assessments in English and math anchored in what it takes to be ready for college and careers and consistent with the Common Core State Standards. Can students become better test takers? How?  Performance on standardized tests can be improved if students gain experience with this type of testing and are given training in study skills and problem solving. Many students can profit from direct instruction about how to prepare for and take tests. Involving students in designing these test preparation programs can be helpful. Students with learning challenges may benefit from intensive and ongoing preparation for taking tests, particularly if the test-taking strategies are tied to specific problems and content learned and tested. What are some current directions in teacher evaluation?  Concerns about the problems with high-stakes tests, the unfairness in evaluating teachers based on test scores of students who began the year far below grade level, and the variability in curricula in different school districts and states have led to new ideas about test scores and testing. Value-added modeling indicates

695

696 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing the average test score gain for students, adjusted for their characteristics such as prior level of achievement (where they started the year). Experts agree that value-added scores alone cannot be used to evaluate schools or teachers. The Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation launched the Measures of Effective Teaching (MET) Project, a research partnership between 3,000 teachers and research teams at dozens of institutions that has identified a three-part system for evaluating good teaching that includes gains on state achievement tests, student perceptions of teachers, and classroom observations using the Danielson Framework for Teaching. Teacher observation, however, is subject to bias based on the behavior of the students or even class composition.

Cluster 15 Review Key Terms Accountable Assessment Assessment bias Authentic assessments Central tendency Classroom assessments Confidence interval Criterion-referenced grading Criterion-referenced testing Distractors Formative assessment Grade-equivalent score Grading on the curve High-stakes testing Informal assessments Interim (growth or benchmark) assessments

Standard error of measurement Standard scores Standardized tests Stanine scores Stem Summative assessment T score True score Validity Value-added modeling (VAM) Variability z score

Mean Measurement Median Mode Norm group Norm-referenced grading Norm-referenced testing Normal distribution Percentile rank Performance assessments Portfolio Pretest Range Reliability Scoring rubrics Selected-response testing Standard deviation

Connect and Extend to Licensure Multiple-Choice Questions 1.

2.

“Your daughter’s scores are above the average nationally. The average score would be a 50.” What type of scoring is being used in the yearly tests Mr. Taylor’s class must take?

Which of the following assessment methods provides feedback that is nonevaluative, occurs before or during instruction, and guides teachers in planning and improving instruction? A.

Summative

B.

Criterion referenced

C.

Formative

D.

Norm referenced

The yearly standardized test in Mr. Taylor’s class is given in the spring. By the summer, Mr. Taylor’s students received their scores in the mail. Many of the parents were upset and contacted Mr. Taylor regarding their children’s “low scores.” “Mr. Taylor, I don’t understand how my daughter could be in honors classes and have scores on her standardized test in the 70s?”

3.

A.

Criterion referenced

B.

Norm referenced

C.

Raw scores

D.

Authentic scoring

Maria had proven to be a very good student in Mr. Rhodes’s class despite having just moved to the United States from another country. So it came as a surprise when Maria performed poorly on her tests. When Mr. Rhodes consulted another teacher who also had Maria as a student, he learned that his test questions may have had something to do with it. His colleague explained that Maria came from a relatively primitive area that does not have television. Her exposure to

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

concepts that draw on a wide variety of cultural experiences was limited. This indicated to Mr. Rhodes that his tests unfairly penalized Maria. This type of assessment that unfairly penalizes a student for his or her lack of resources or cultural knowledge demonstrates which of the following?

4.

A.

Attribution bias

B.

Assessment bias

C.

Reliability bias

D.

Validity bias

Which one of the following types of assessments would be most beneficial for assessing a student’s ability to debate? A.

Formative assessment

B.

Portfolio assessment

C.

Summative assessment

D.

Performance assessment

Constructed-Response Questions Case “How will you grade our oral book reports, Miss Wren?”

697

“I am going to grade them with each of you! We will develop what is called a rubric. It is a list of items on which you should focus while preparing and presenting your book report.” “How will we be involved?” “You will help me make the rubric. What are some of the things you think should be included in your oral report?” “I think we should have to have audience participation, or else it gets too boring!” “Good idea, Terry. What else?” The class continued to add to the rubric until there were six aspects on which to focus. “Class, you will also assist me in grading your performance by using the rubric. By making the rubric and using it to grade yourself, you should do very well.” “There are no surprises that way!” Lisa shouted. 5.

In addition to listing the criteria for what constitutes quality work and having students self-assess, list some additional guidelines for Miss Wren to remember when developing a rubric.

6.

Grades, a form of extrinsic reinforcement, can be a source of celebration for Miss Wren’s students or a punishment. To make the most of grades and increase her students’ chances for success, what should Miss Wren keep in mind when grading her students?

What Would They Do? Teachers’ Casebook: Giving Meaningful Grades At the beginning of this cluster, we asked you to think critically about what kind of grading system you will use. Now, as you consider these teachers’ responses to that question, we again suggest that you think critically. Do the following responses reflect (or contradict) any of the concepts discussed in this cluster? Do the suggestions and solutions described here make sense in light of what you have learned in this or other classes about best practices? How do these ideas align with or challenge your personal philosophy of teaching?

Monika Stahlstrom • 3rd–7th-Grade Teacher East Kensington Outdoor Learning School, Surrey, British Columbia, CA Consulting curriculum documents and performance standards is a key first step towards successful assessment. The teacher needs to have a clear understanding of what excellence looks like in order to know what questions to ask and feedback to give to promote learning and growth. At the beginning of the school year, communicating the grading language and definitions to parents and learners is essential. Clarity in how you interpret progress in relation

to learning standards, no matter the method, is important to ensure shared understandings. In this scenario, you are mandated to use letter grades, so make clear the definitions of these letters early and often. From here, take time to teach students mastery language. In this case, I would use mastery language with letter grades equated for reporting periods only as I have found letter grades to hinder the process of learning; letter grades tend to become the goal rather than mastering the learning goal itself. With students, practice assessing learning with mastery language using something familiar, like learning how to ride a bike or toddlers learning how to walk. This helps give a reference that is more easily understood than first trying self-assessment with writing a good paragraph or effective communication in math. Then, when planning for assessment, determine which curricular skills, content, understanding and core competencies to focus on and name throughout the unit of study. It is important to give students time to engage with the learning goals by co-creating criteria that demonstrate a skill at a standard. Using examples and allowing students to sort and determine for themselves what works and why allows them to take ownership of the learning goal and understand it from the beginning.

698 Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing Instructionally, students are intentionally reminded of the goals and the criteria decided upon consistently through visuals, rubrics with the students’ exact language, reflections, and in self and peer assessments. Students use single-point rubrics for ease and efficiency at the end of lessons while multi-point mastery rubrics are used for clarity and as a means to build understanding of goals and next steps in learning. Throughout the unit, descriptive feedback is provided during lessons or in response to assignments that ask questions and is specific about learning progress in relation to standards. In the end, students would either complete a project or task that requires application of learned skills and knowledge or choose a strong piece of evidence to showcase growth. This evidence can be chosen from the variety of times they were asked to use the skills and knowledge to best demonstrate where they are in their learning now, how they know, and where they will go to next. This process works in all subject areas and grades. The assessment in the end, the letter grade equated from the mastery language, is easily justified to parents, administrators, and students as it is clear from the beginning what that grade standard looks like as they have been central to the learning process. We also have multiple pieces of evidence to draw upon to prove the grade itself if it were ever unclear. From start to finish, this process of assessment ends up becoming the means to deep learning and reflection for all.

Maria Avila • 5th-Grade Math and Science Teacher Stone Lakes Elementary School, Orlando, FL I have worked at multiple schools, one of which allowed us the freedom and flexibility to grade as we wished, provided we could justify our system to parents. Although this was great for the teachers, it wasn’t so great for the students or parents, as this system replicated the scenario above. It led to parents requesting specific teachers because they were “easy graders.” Ultimately, having a standard grading approach across a grade level or school would be the best-case scenario. I feel my current school does just that. Teachers have the freedom and flexibility to grade as we wish, however we must come up with a general system across a grade level. Our grade level currently assigns a weighted average of 40% on tests, 35% on quizzes and 25% on class work. This is not ideal for me as I struggle with assigning so much weight on tests, however it is one I can live with. It provides the flexibility for me to weight projects as a test grade if I choose to do so. The major graded assignments and projects that I assign tend to have students demonstrate proficiency with a concept or standard. As a math and science teacher, I provide students with an error analysis that they have to justify. They examine the error and provide a justification as to where the error was made and why. I then grade this using a rubric. In fact, most of the open-ended assignments I grade have

a rubric that I have discussed, reviewed, and modified with the students. When it comes to group projects, I have the students self-assess their work and I note their participation in the social/ emotional component of their report card. In my 24 years of teaching, grading continues to be an ever-evolving issue—there just isn’t a perfect method, but we can strive for fairness.

Katie Churchill • 3rd-Grade Teacher Oriole Parke Elementary School, Chicago, IL I use a combination of assessment tools to evaluate my students. Using a rubric that students and parents alike are familiar with provides an easy-to-follow-and-understand grading system. The rubric needs to remain in a focal area in the classroom as a constant reminder to the students of what their expectations are. By differentiating instruction consistently to cover all learning styles and modalities, the students hopefully become more involved and invested in their own learning and, as a result, produce better quality work and exceed expectations. Several factors play a part in obtaining a particular letter grade. The letter grade is earned through a combination of group work, completing objectives, and following the rubric guidelines for quality work.

Madya Ayala • High School Preparatory Teacher Eugenio Garza Lagüera, Campus Garza Sada, Monterrey, N. L. Mexico I think it is important to assess a cross-section of student work. First, portfolios can be a useful way to gather various types of work throughout the year. Using a portfolio, a teacher can then attach a letter grade to student progress and achievement. It is important to grade children not only on progress, but also on their understanding of material. I use meaningful, written assessments to test for retention and understanding of my students’ knowledge. Finally, I grade various projects and experiments so that the students who are better project-based learners will be graded fairly. I also like the idea of using a rubric system to grade students on writing or projects. Under a rubric system, a teacher allocates a certain number of points to each content area. It is then easy to attach a letter grade based on the number of points received.

Katie Piel • Kindergarten–6th-Grade Teacher West Park School, Moscow, ID Students should be given the latitude to express achievement in different ways like group projects, daily class work, tests, and individual projects. All students are held accountable for demonstrating their own learning. With each teacher grading on a different standard, the teachers must also take on the responsibility of collaborating with their peers. Communicating

Cluster 15 Classroom Assessment, Grading, and Standardized Testing

to other teachers the skills a student can be expected to bring with him or her to the next level is crucial.

Allan Osborne • Assistant Principal Snug Harbor Community School, Quincy, MA Any grading system should consider a student’s progress and effort. Grading systems also should be individualized to account for a student’s unique strengths and weaknesses. Thus, a student with a learning disability should not be held to the same expectations as a gifted student. The most critical aspect of any successful grading system is that it is fair. Fairness dictates that students and their parents be given

699

information in advance about class requirements and expectations, along with a description of grading criteria. A system that is fair can be easily justified. It is also important to keep accurate and detailed records of student progress. In addition to recording grades on tests, quizzes, and projects, anecdotal records describing a student’s typical performance should be kept. These records can be valuable if a report card grade is questioned. Although group assignments can be an important learning experience, I would be reluctant to place too much emphasis on a group project grade. As we all know, each member of the group does not participate equally, and thus, a group grade does not reflect the contribution of each individual member.

This page intentionally left blank

Licensure Appendix Part 1 Licensure Examination Study Guide You probably will have to take a licensure examination in order to become a teacher in your state. In over 40 states and jurisdictions, the Praxis II® test is the required licensure examination. This section highlights the concepts from each cluster that may be on your licensure test.

Cluster 1 Developing relationships with professionals: Until you become a teacher, it will be difficult to establish a working relationship with other practitioners. However, you might find this site beneficial: K–12 Practitioners’ Circle (nces.ed.gov/practitioners/teachers.asp). Keeping current with educational issues: Education Week (http://www.edweek.org) will keep you up-to-date about innovations in teaching, policy initiatives, and changes in public laws related to education. These issues are often highly complex. The use of critical thinking skills is essential when making judgments about the information you will encounter in this type of publication.

Cluster 2 Recognize the influences that socioeconomic status, race, ethnicity, gender, sexual orientation, and community values may have on: ●

Student learning preferences



Academic achievement



Attitudes, self-esteem, and expectations for success



Opportunities for quality educational experiences



Teachers’ attention to students

Devise strategies that: ●

Increase awareness of stereotypes and biases



Eliminate racist, sexist, and other discriminatory teaching practices



Promote culturally-relevant teaching



Promote positive school–home relationships

Cluster 3 Understand how the brain works: ●

Brain imaging techniques



Neurogenesis and brain development



Plasticity



Brain-based educational strategies

For theories of cognitive development, you should understand: ●

Basic assumptions of Piagetian and Vygotskian theories



How students build their unique knowledge bases

A-1

A-2 Licensure Appendix ●

How students acquire skills



Important terms and concepts related to each theory



The key steps, mechanisms, or milestones related to each theory



The limitations of each theory

Cluster 4 Understand the major concepts and progressions related to: ●

Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological model of development



Erikson’s theory of psychosocial development



Piaget’s and Kohlberg’s perspectives on moral development



Gilligan’s theory of caring



The distinction between moral judgments and social conventions



Haidt’s social intuitionist model

Design or choose strategies that: ● ●



Support optimal social and emotional development of students Help students cope with major life transitions and challenges to safety as well as physical and mental health Help students build a sense of self-concept, self-esteem, and self-identity (including racial identity)

Recognize signs or behaviors that indicate sexual abuse or child abuse.

Cluster 5 Explain the effects of legislation on public education: ●

Americans with Disabilities Act



Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act



Section 504



Individualized education programs



Inclusion and least restrictive environment

Understand views of intelligence and describe its measurement: ●

Types of intelligence tests and their uses



Multiple intelligences



Interpreting intelligence scores

Explain how creativity is defined and assessed. Describe and differentiate among learning styles and preferences. Accommodate the needs of students with exceptionalities: ●

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder



Visual and speech difficulties



Differences in mental abilities



Learning disabilities



Intellectual disabilities



Autism spectrum disorders

Licensure Appendix

Cluster 6 For the development of language, you should understand: ●

Diversity in language development



The major accomplishments of language development of school-age children



The relationship between language and literacy



Basic steps that teachers can take to enhance literacy among their students



Strategies that support English acquisition in non-English–speaking students

Cluster 7 Understand the basic assumptions and contributions of these behaviorists: ●

Pavlov



Skinner

Determine appropriate behavioral techniques to: ●

Foster appropriate classroom conduct

Understand basic processes of operant conditioning and their roles in learning, including: ●

Antecedents and consequences



Types of reinforcement and reinforcement schedules



Punishment



Shaping

Cluster 8 Understand how memory and recall are affected by: ●

The limitations, capacities, and capabilities of the various structures of human memory (e.g., memory stores)



The manner in which humans process information



Prior knowledge of a topic



Executive control processes

Explain how students and teachers can enhance learning through the use of: ●

Elaboration and mnemonic devices



Organized presentations



Meaningful learning and instructional activities

Cluster 9 Focus on each of these major topics: ●

Metacognitive knowledge and learning



Learning strategies ●

Basic principles of teaching these strategies



Cognitive processes involved in various strategies



Appropriate uses of different strategies

A-3

A-4 Licensure Appendix ●

Problem solving ●

General problem-solving strategies/heuristics and algorithms



The value of problem representation



Factors that impede problem solving



Critical thinking



Transfer of learning ●

Types of transfer and promoting transfer

Cluster 10 Explain the advantages and appropriate uses of major student-centered approaches to learning and instruction: ●

Inquiry learning



Problem-based learning



Cognitive apprenticeships



Cooperative learning

Understand important concepts related to student-centered models of instruction: ●

Situated learning



Complex learning environments



Authentic tasks



Multiple representations of content



Piaget and Vygotsky: theories of constructivism

Recognize how technology influences learning both positively and negatively.

Cluster 11 Focus on these major topics: ●

Bandura’s social cognitive theory



Modeling and observational learning



Learner agency



Self-efficacy



Teachers’ sense of efficacy



Self-regulated learning



Helping students monitor and regulate learning



Self-regulation of emotions

Cluster 12 Describe the theoretical foundations of the major approaches to motivation. ●



Identify and define important terms related to motivation including needs, selfdetermination, goals, expectancies, values, costs, attributions, beliefs about ability, interest, curiosity, and emotions. Use your knowledge of motivation to: ●

Identify situations and conditions that can enhance or diminish student motivation to learn



Design strategies to support individual and group work in the classroom



Implement practices that help students become self-motivated

Licensure Appendix

Cluster 13 Understand principles of classroom management that promote positive relationships by: ●

Establishing daily procedures and routines



Developing student–teacher relationships and parent–teacher relationships



Responding effectively to minor student misbehavior



Implementing reasonable rules, procedures, penalties, and rewards



Keeping students actively engaged in purposeful learning

Diagnose problems and prevent or reduce inappropriate behaviors by: ●

Communicating effectively with students and parents



Addressing misbehaviors in the least intrusive way possible



Confronting disruptive behaviors in an effective, efficient manner

Cluster 14 Develop plans for instruction and consider: ●

The role of objectives in instruction



Writing learning targets



The use of educational taxonomies to identify effective objectives



The role of independent practice (i.e., independent work/seatwork and homework)



Direct instruction ●

Basic assumptions



Appropriate uses/principles of implementation

Understand the basic principles of teacher-centered and student-centered forms of instruction, including: ●

Appropriate uses and limitations



The role of the teacher



Effective questioning techniques



Whole-group discussions



Recitation



Thematic/interdisciplinary instruction



Differentiated instruction

Cluster 15 Describe the characteristics and purposes of major types of tests: ●

Criterion-referenced and norm-referenced tests



Formative, interim, and summative tests

Explain the major issues related to concerns about standardized testing, including: ●

High-stakes testing



Bias in testing



Test-taking programs

A-5

A-6 Licensure Appendix Understand major concepts related to classroom assessment and grading: ●

Formative and summative assessment



Reliability and validity



Criterion-referenced and norm-referenced grading

Describe the characteristics, uses, and limitations of major assessment techniques, including: ●

Multiple-choice items



Essays



Portfolios



Exhibitions (performances)

Design a scoring rubric for an authentic learning task that possesses: ●

Validity



Reliability



Generalizability



Equity

Standardized testing ●

Interpreting different types of scores



Criticisms of standardized testing

Part 2 Correlating Text Content to the PRAXIS II® Principles of Learning and Teaching Tests and InTASC Model Core Teaching Standards Each state in the country has its own set of licensure requirements that new teachers must meet in order to work in the classroom. An increasing number of states are basing their requirements on standards developed by InTASC (Interstate Teacher Assessment and Support Consortium). These standards are based on 10 principles and foundations of effective teaching practice that InTASC has identified as essential for optimal student learning. Many states assess new teachers’ knowledge of those principles through the use of tests from the Praxis II® series published by the Educational Testing Service. Within the Praxis II® series are three Principles of Learning and Teaching (PLT) tests, one each for grades K–6, 5–9, and 7–12. Each PLT test assesses students’ knowledge of educational psychology and its application in the classroom. The following table is designed to help you study for your PLT test and meet the Essential Knowledge indicators for each of InTASC’s 10 principles of effective teaching. The left-hand column of the table lists the topics assessed in a PLT test. The right-hand column contains InTASC’s Knowledge standards. In the center column, you will find the clusters and sections in this textbook that correspond to the PLT tests and InTASC standards.

A-7

A-8 Licensure Appendix PRAXIS II® TOPICS I.

Students as Learners

A.

Student Development and the Learning Process

1.

Theoretical foundations about how learning occurs: how students construct knowledge, acquire skills, and develop habits of mind



Examples of important theorists:



WOOLFOLK AND USHER TEXT CONNECTIONS

Clusters 6–12 (entire clusters)



Jean Piaget

Cluster 3/Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development Cluster 10/Cognitive and Social Constructivism



Lev Vygotsky

Cluster 3/Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Perspective Cluster 10/Cognitive and Social Constructivism



Howard Gardner

Cluster 5/Another View: Gardner’s Multiple Intelligences



Robert Sternberg

Cluster 5/Another View: Sternberg’s Successful Intelligence



Albert Bandura

Cluster 7/Beyond Behaviorism: Bandura’s Challenge and Observational Learning Cluster 11/Social Cognitive Theory



Urie Bronfenbrenner

Cluster 4/Bronfenbrenner: The Social Context for Development

Important terms that relate to learning theory: ●

Adaptation

Cluster 3/Basic Tendencies in Thinking



Conservation

Cluster 3/Four Stages of Cognitive Development



Constructivism

Cluster 3/The Role of Learning and Development; Implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories for Teachers Cluster 10/Constructivism and Interactive Learning (entire cluster)



Equilibration

Cluster 3/Basic Tendencies in Thinking



Co-constructed process

Cluster 3/The Social Sources of Individual Thinking



Private speech

Cluster 3/The Role of Language and Private Speech



Scaffolding

Cluster 3/Private Speech and the Zone; Vygotsky: What Can We Learn?; Guidelines: Applying Vygotsky’s Ideas in Teaching Cluster 10/ Facilitating Deep Learning in a Constructivist Classroom; Common Elements of Learner-Centered Teaching



Zone of proximal development

Cluster 3/The Zone of Proximal Development; Reaching Every Student: Teaching in the “Magic Middle” Cluster 10/Social Constructivism



Learning

Cluster 2/Diversity in Learning Cluster 3/Cognitive Development Cluster 7/Understanding Learning; Behavioral Views of Learning Cluster 8/Cognitive Views of Learning Cluster 9/Metacognition; Learning Strategies Cluster 10/Learning in Groups Cluster 10/Learning through Cooperation Cluster 10/Constructivist Views of Learning

INTASC ESSENTIAL KNOWLEDGE INDICATORS

1(d) The teacher understands how learning occurs—how learners construct knowledge, acquire skills, and develop disciplined thinking processes—and knows how to use instructional strategies that promote student learning.

Licensure Appendix

PRAXIS II® TOPICS

WOOLFOLK AND USHER TEXT CONNECTIONS

A-9

INTASC ESSENTIAL KNOWLEDGE INDICATORS

Cluster 11/Social Cognitive Theory Cluster 11/Modeling: Learning by Observing Others Cluster 11/Self-Regulated Learning: Skill and Will Cluster 11/Bringing It All Together: Theories of Learning Cluster 12/Motivation in Learning and Teaching (entire cluster) ●

Knowledge

Cluster 3/The Social Sources of Individual Thinking; Activity and Constructing Knowledge Cluster 8/The Importance of Knowledge in Cognition; Teaching for Long-Lasting Knowledge: Basic Principles and Applications Cluster 10/How Is Knowledge Constructed?; Knowledge: Situated or General?



Memory

Cluster 8/Cognitive Views of Memory; Long-Term Memory



Schemas

Cluster 8/Schemas



Transfer

Cluster 9/Teaching for Transfer

2.

Human development in the physical, social, emotional, moral, and cognitive domains



Contributions of important theorists:



1(e) The teacher understands that each learner’s cognitive, linguistic, social, emotional, and physical development influence learning and knows how to make instructional decisions that build on learners’ strengths and needs



Jean Piaget

Cluster 3/Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development Cluster 10/Cognitive and Social Constructivism



Lev Vygotsky

Cluster 3/Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Perspective Cluster 10/Cognitive and Social Constructivism



Albert Bandura

Cluster 7/Beyond Behaviorism: Bandura’s Challenge and Observational Learning Cluster 11/Social Cognitive Theory



Erik Erikson

Cluster 4/Erikson: Stages of Psychosocial Development



Lawrence Kohlberg

Cluster 4/Kohlberg’s Theories of Moral Development



Carol Gilligan

Cluster 4/Criticisms of Kohlberg’s Theory



Jonathan Haidt

Cluster 4/Beyond Reasoning: Haidt’s Social Intuitionist Model of Moral Psychology

Major progressions in each developmental domain and the ranges of individual variation within each domain

Cluster 2/Ethnic and Racial Identity; Gender Identity Cluster 3/Four Stages of Cognitive Development; Information Processing, Neo-Piagetian, and Neuroscience Views of Cognitive Development; Some Limitations of Piaget’s Theory; The Social Sources of Individual Thinking Cluster 3/Four Stages of Cognitive Development; Information Processing, Neo-Piagetian, and Neuroscience Views of Cognitive Development; Some Limitations of Piaget’s Theory; The Social Sources of Individual Thinking Cluster 4/Physical and Motor Development; Identity and Self-Concept; Understanding Others and Moral Development Cluster 6/The Development of Language

A-10 Licensure Appendix PRAXIS II® TOPICS

WOOLFOLK AND USHER TEXT CONNECTIONS



Impact of students’ physical, social, emotional, moral, and cognitive development on their learning and ways to address these factors when making decisions

Cluster 2/Creating Culturally Welcoming Classrooms; Guidelines: Avoiding Gender Bias in Teaching; Guidelines: Culturally Relevant Teaching Cluster 3/Four Stages of Cognitive Development; Some Limitations of Piaget’s Theory; Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships; Guidelines: Teaching the Concrete-Operational Child; Guidelines: Helping Students to Use Formal Operations; The Social Sources of Individual Thinking; Assisted Learning; The Zone of Proximal Development Cluster 4/Physical Development; Erikson: Stages of Psychosocial Development; Identity and Self-Concept; Understanding Others and Moral Development; Personal/Social Development: Lessons for Teachers Cluster 6/The Development of Language Cluster 10/Guidelines: Supporting the Development of Media Literacy Cluster 11/Teaching Toward Self-Efficacy and Self-Regulated Learning; Guidelines: Encouraging Emotional Self-Regulation Cluster 13/Maintaining a Good Environment for Learning



How development in one domain, such as physical, may affect performance in another domain, such as social

Cluster 3/General Principles of Development; The Brain and Cognitive Development; Influences on Development Cluster 4/Physical Development

B.

Students as Diverse Learners

1.

Differences in the ways students learn and perform

Cluster 5/Learner Differences and Learning Needs Cluster 2/Culture and Diversity



Learning styles and preferences

Cluster 5/Learning Styles/Preferences Cluster 2/Diversity in Learning



Multiple intelligences

Cluster 5/Another View: Gardner’s Multiple Intelligences





Performance modes Concrete-operational thinking Visual and aural learners

Cluster 3/Later Elementary to the Middle School Years: The Concrete-Operational Stage; Guidelines: Teaching the Concrete-Operational Child Cluster 5/Learning Styles: Proceed with Caution; Students with Learning Challenges Cluster 6/Generation 1.5: Learners in Two Worlds



Race and ethnicity

Cluster 2/Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Discrimination; Ethnicity and Race in Teaching and Learning; Creating Culturally Welcoming Classrooms



Gender differences

Cluster 2/Gender and Sexual Orientation in Teaching and Learning; Guidelines: Avoiding Gender Bias in Teaching Cluster 4/Sex Differences in Self-Concept of Academic Competence Cluster 5/Sex Differences in Intelligence and Achievement



Cultural expectations and styles

Cluster 2/Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity (entire cluster) Cluster 6/Teaching Immigrant Students and English Learners Cluster 6/Classrooms Today Cluster 14/Teacher Expectations



INTASC ESSENTIAL KNOWLEDGE INDICATORS

2(g) The teacher understands and identifies differences in approaches to learning and performance and knows how to design instruction that uses each learner’s strengths to promote growth.

Licensure Appendix

PRAXIS II® TOPICS ●

2.

3.

4.

5.

English Language Learners

Areas of exceptionality in students’ learning

WOOLFOLK AND USHER TEXT CONNECTIONS

Cluster 5/Learner Differences and Learning Needs

Special physical or sensory challenges

Cluster 5/Students with Communication Impairments; Students with Health and Sensory Impairments



Learning disabilities

Cluster 5/Students with Specific Learning Disabilities; Individualized Education Program; Section 504 Protections Cluster 6/Special Challenges: Students Who Are English Learners with Unique Learning Needs and Gifts



Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

Cluster 5/Students with Hyperactivity and Attention Disorders



Intellectual disabilities

Cluster 5/Students with Intellectual Disabilities; Guidelines: Teaching Students with Intellectual Disabilities



Autism spectrum disorder

Cluster 5/Autism Spectrum Disorders



Gifts and talents

Cluster 5/Students Who Are Gifted and Talented Cluster 5/Individual Differences and the Law



Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA), Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA); Section 504 Protections for Students

Cluster 5/IDEA; Section 504 Protections



Inclusion, mainstreaming, and least restrictive environment

Cluster 5/Individual Differences and the Law; Least Restrictive Environment Cluster 14/Reaching Every Student: Differentiation with Universal Design for Learning

Approaches for accommodating various learning styles and intelligences

Cluster 5/Learner Differences and Learning Needs— Focus on: Creativity in the Classroom; Learning Styles/ Preferences; Individual Differences and the Law; Teaching Students with Gifts and Talents



Differentiated instruction

Cluster 14/Differentiated Instruction; Assistive Technology; Reaching Every Student: Differentiation with Universal Design for Learning



Alternative assessment

Cluster 15/Formative and Authentic Classroom Assessments



Testing modifications

Cluster 15/Reaching Every Student: Helping Students with Disabilities Prepare for High-Stakes Tests

Process of second-language acquisition and strategies to support the learning of students

INTASC ESSENTIAL KNOWLEDGE INDICATORS

Cluster 6/Diversity in Language Development; Teaching Immigrant Students and English Learners; Special Challenges: Students Who Are English Learners with Unique Learning Needs and Gifts Cluster 2/Creating Culturally Welcoming Classrooms



Legislation and institutional responsibilities relating to exceptional students

A-11

Cluster 6/Basic Communication and Academic Language; Dialect Differences in the Classroom; Teaching English Learners; Teaching Immigrant Students and English Learners; What Is the Best Way to Teach Students Who Are ELs?; Bilingual Education and English Learners

2(h) The teacher understands students with exceptional needs, including those associated with disabilities and giftedness, and knows how to use strategies and resources to address these needs.

9(j) The teacher understands laws related to learners’ rights and teacher responsibilities (e.g., for educational equity, appropriate education for learners with disabilities, confidentiality, privacy, appropriate treatment of learners, reporting in situations related to possible child abuse).

2(g) The teacher understands and identifies differences in approaches to learning and performance and knows how to design instruction that uses each learner’s strengths to promote growth.

2(i) The teacher knows about secondlanguage-acquisition processes and knows how to incorporate instructional strategies and resources to support language acquisition.

A-12 Licensure Appendix PRAXIS II® TOPICS

WOOLFOLK AND USHER TEXT CONNECTIONS

6.

Cluster 2/ Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity (entire cluster) Cluster 5/Learner Differences and Learning Needs

Understanding of influences of individual experiences, talents, and prior learning, as well as language, culture, family, and community values on students’ learning ●

Multicultural backgrounds

Cluster 6/Teaching Immigrant Students and English Learners Cluster 2/Ethnic and Racial Differences in School Achievement; Economic and Social Class Differences; Education Is Cultural; Culturally Relevant Pedagogy; Creating Culturally Welcoming Classrooms; Diversity in Learning; Lessons for Teachers: Teaching Every Student (entire cluster)

INTASC ESSENTIAL KNOWLEDGE INDICATORS

2(j) The teacher understands that learners bring assets for learning based on their individual experiences, abilities, talents, prior learning, and peer and social group interactions, as well as language, culture, family, and community values. 2(k) The teacher knows how to access information about the values of diverse cultures and communities and how to incorporate learners’ experiences, cultures, and community resources into instruction.



Age-appropriate knowledge and behaviors

Cluster 3/Four Stages of Cognitive Development; Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Perspective; Implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories for Teachers Cluster 4/The Self, Social, and Moral Development— Focus on: The Preschool Years: Trust, Autonomy, and Initiative; The Elementary and Middle School Years: Industry Versus Inferiority; Adolescence: The Search for Identity; Understanding Others and Moral Development



The student culture at the school

Cluster 2/Peer Influences and Resistance Cultures Cluster 4/Bronfenbrenner: The Social Context for Development; Peers Cluster 12/Learned Helplessness



Family backgrounds

Cluster 4/Families Cluster 2/Poverty and Social Inequality; Poverty and Academic Outcomes Cluster 4/Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships Cluster 12/Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships



Linguistic patterns and differences

Cluster 5/Students with Communication Impairments Cluster 6/The Development of Language; Dialects; What Is Involved in Being Bilingual?



Cognitive patterns and differences

Cluster 5/Learning Styles/Preferences Cluster 8/Individual Differences in Working Memory; Long-Term Memory Cluster 9/Individual Differences in Metacognition



Social and emotional issues

Cluster 4/Understanding Others and Moral Development Cluster 5/Students with Emotional or Behavioral Disorders Cluster 6/Affective and Emotional/Social Considerations Cluster 11/Self-Regulation of Emotions

1(g) The teacher understands the role of language and culture in learning and knows how to modify instruction to make language comprehensible and instruction relevant, accessible, and challenging.

Licensure Appendix

PRAXIS II® TOPICS C.

Student Motivation and the Learning Environment

1.

Theoretical foundations about human motivation and behavior



Important terms that relate to motivation and behavior

WOOLFOLK AND USHER TEXT CONNECTIONS

Cluster 12/Motivation in Learning and Teaching (entire cluster)

A-13

INTASC ESSENTIAL KNOWLEDGE INDICATORS

3(i) The teacher understands the relationship between motivation and engagement and knows how to design learning experiences using strategies that build learner self-direction and ownership of learning.

Cluster 7/Operant Conditioning: Trying New Responses; Reinforcement Schedules Cluster 11/Social Cognitive Views of Learning and Motivation Cluster 12/Motivation in Learning and Teaching Cluster 14/Teacher Expectations

2.

How knowledge of human emotion and behavior should influence strategies for organizing and supporting individual and group work in the classroom

Cluster 10/Collaboration and Cooperation; Guidelines: Using Cooperative Learning; Designing Interactive Digital Learning Environments Cluster 11/Reaching Every Student: Technology and Self-Regulation; Guidelines: Encouraging Emotional Self-Regulation; Teaching Toward Self-Efficacy and Self-Regulated Learning Cluster 12/Anxiety in the Classroom; Reaching Every Student: Coping with Negative Emotions; Guidelines: Coping with Anxiety; Guidelines: Supporting SelfDetermination and Autonomy; Strategies to Encourage Motivation to Learn Cluster 14/Teacher Expectations

3(j) The teacher knows how to help learners work productively and cooperatively with each other to achieve learning goals.

3.

Factors and situations that are likely to promote or diminish students’ motivation to learning, and how to help students to become self-motivated

Cluster 11/Guidelines: Encouraging Emotional SelfRegulation; Teaching Toward Self-Efficacy and SelfRegulated Learning Cluster 12/Needs: Lessons for Teachers; Goals: Lessons for Teachers; Interest, Curiosity, and Emotions: Lessons for Teachers; Supporting Autonomy

3(i) The teacher understands the relationship between motivation and engagement and knows how to design learning experiences using strategies that build learner self-direction and ownership of learning.

4.

Principles of effective management and strategies to promote positive relationships, cooperation, and purposeful learning

Cluster 7/Methods for Encouraging Behaviors; Guidelines: Applying Operant Conditioning: Encouraging Positive Behaviors; Handling Undesirable Behavior; Reaching Every Student: Positive Behavior Supports; Current Applications: Functional Behavioral Assessment, Positive Behavior Supports, and Self-Management Cluster 10/Collaboration and Cooperation; Guidelines: Using Cooperative Learning Cluster 13/The What and Why of Supportive Classroom Organization; Caring Relationships: Connections with Schools; The Need for Communication; Diversity: Culturally Responsive Management

3(k) The teacher knows how to collaborate with learners to establish and monitor elements of a safe and productive learning environment including norms, expectations, routines, and organizational structures.



Establishing daily procedures and routines

Cluster 13/Routines and Rules Required: In-Person Learning; Routines and Rules Required: Remote Learning; Guidelines: Establishing Class Routines for In-Person Learning; Guidelines: Establishing Routines and Rules for Remote Learning



Establishing classroom rules, punishments, and rewards

Cluster 13/Prevention Is the Best Medicine; Reaching Every Student: Restorative Justice Cluster 14/Teacher Expectations



Giving timely feedback

Cluster 7/Guidelines: Applying Operant Conditioning: Using Praise Appropriately Cluster 13/The Need for Communication Cluster 14/Responding to Student Answers Cluster 15/Effects of Grading on Students



Maintaining accurate records

Cluster 15/Authentic Assessments: Performances and Portfolios; Guidelines: Creating Portfolios; Evaluating Portfolios and Performances

A-14 Licensure Appendix PRAXIS II® TOPICS ●

Communicating with parents and caregivers

Clusters 3–9, 12–15/Guidelines: Family and Community Partnerships (one in each cluster)



Using objective behavior descriptions

Cluster 7/Putting It All Together: Applied Behavior Analysis



Responding to student misbehavior

Cluster 7/Contingency Contracts, Token Reinforcement, and Group Consequences; Handling Undesirable Behavior; Guidelines: Applying Operant Conditioning: Using Punishment Cluster 13/Dealing with Discipline Problems; Special Challenges with High School Students; Guidelines: Imposing Penalties; Assertive Discipline; Confrontations and Negotiations; Guidelines: Handling Potentially Explosive Situations



Arranging classroom space

Cluster 10/Designing Interactive Digital Learning Environments Cluster 13/Planning Spaces for Learning; Guidelines: Designing Learning Spaces



Pacing and the structure of the lesson

Cluster 13/Encouraging Engagement; Guidelines: Keeping Students Engaged; Withitness; Overlapping and Group Focus; Movement Management Cluster 14/Clarity and Organization; Guidelines: Effective Direct Instruction

II.

Instruction and Assessment

A.

Instructional Strategies

1.

Major cognitive processes associated with student learning Critical thinking Creative thinking

Cluster 9/Critical Thinking and Argumentation Cluster 5/Creativity: What It Is and Why It Matters



Inductive and deductive thinking

Cluster 3/High School and College: Formal Operations



Problem structuring and problem solving

Cluster 9/Problem Solving; Guidelines: Applying Problem Solving



Invention

Cluster 5/Creativity: What It Is and Why It Matters



Memorization and recall

Cluster 8/Cognitive Views of Memory

● ●

2.

WOOLFOLK AND USHER TEXT CONNECTIONS

Major categories, advantages, and appropriate uses of instructional strategies

INTASC ESSENTIAL KNOWLEDGE INDICATORS

8(j) The teacher understands the cognitive processes associated with various kinds of learning (e.g., critical and creative thinking, problem framing and problem solving, invention, memorization, and recall) and how these processes can be stimulated.

8(k) The teacher knows how to apply a range of developmentally, culturally, and linguistically appropriate instructional strategies to achieve learning goals.



Cooperative learning

Cluster 10/Collaboration and Cooperation; Guidelines: Using Cooperative Learning



Direct instruction (often referred to as teachercentered instruction)

Cluster 14/Explicit Teaching and Direct Instruction



Discovery learning

Cluster 10/Inquiry-Based Learning; Problem-Based Learning



Whole-group discussion

Cluster 14/Group Discussion and Quality Talk; Guidelines: Productive Group Discussions



Independent study

Cluster 9/Learning Strategies; Guidelines: Becoming an Expert Student Cluster 11/Agency and Self-Efficacy; Self-Regulated Learning: Skill and Will Cluster 14/Independent Work and Homework

Licensure Appendix

PRAXIS II® TOPICS

3.

4.



Interdisciplinary instruction (sometimes referred to as thematic instruction)

Cluster 10/Inquiry-Based Learning; Problem-Based Learning Cluster 14/Planning from a Constructivist Perspective



Questioning

Cluster 14/Questioning, Discussion, Dialogue, and Feedback

Principles, techniques, and methods associated with major instructional strategies

Direct instruction (often referred to as teachercentered instruction)

Cluster 14/Explicit Teaching and Direct Instruction; Questioning, Discussion, Dialogue, and Feedback; Rosenshine’s Six Teaching Functions



Student-centered models

Cluster 10/Cognitive and Social Constructivism

Methods for enhancing student learning through the use of a variety of resources and materials Planning Instruction

1.

Techniques for planning instruction to meet curriculum goals, including the incorporation of learning theory, subject matter, curriculum development, and student development ●







● ●



Cluster 10/Designing Interactive Digital Learning Environments Cluster 14/Assistive Technology; Reaching Every Student: Differentiation with Universal Design for Learning

5(j) The teacher understands how current interdisciplinary themes (e.g., civic literacy, health literacy, global awareness) connect to the core subjects and knows how to weave those themes into meaningful learning experiences.

8(n) The teacher knows how to use a wide variety of resources, including human and technological, to engage students in learning. 7(i) The teacher understands learning theory, human development, cultural diversity, and individual differences and how these impact ongoing planning.

National and state learning standards State and local curriculum frameworks State and local curriculum guides

Cluster 14/An Example of State-Level Goals: The Common Core Cluster 15/Accountability and High-Stakes Testing

Scope and sequence in specific disciplines Units and lessons

Cluster 14/The First Step: Planning; Classroom Goals for Learning; Planning from a Constructivist Perspective

Behavioral objectives: affective, cognitive, and psychomotor Learner objectives and outcomes

Cluster 14/Learning Goals; Flexible and Creative Plans—Using Taxonomies; Guidelines: Using Learning Objectives

Techniques for creating effective bridges between curriculum goals and students’ experiences ● Modeling

INTASC ESSENTIAL KNOWLEDGE INDICATORS

8(k) The teacher knows how to apply a range of developmentally, culturally, and linguistically appropriate instructional strategies to achieve learning goals.



B.

2.

WOOLFOLK AND USHER TEXT CONNECTIONS

A-15

Cluster 7/Beyond Behaviorism: Bandura’s Challenge and Observational Learning

Cluster 11/Modeling: Learning By Observing Others



Guided practice

Cluster 3/Assisted Learning Cluster 10/Facilitating Deep Learning in a Constructivist Classroom; Cognitive Apprenticeships; Reciprocal Questioning Cluster 14/Rosenshine’s Six Teaching Functions



Independent practice, including homework

Cluster 11/Agency and Self-Efficacy; Self-Regulated Learning: Skill and Will Cluster 14/Independent Work and Homework



Transitions

Cluster 13/Overlapping and Group Focus; Movement Management; Guidelines: Keeping Students Engaged

7(g) The teacher understands content and content standards and how these are organized in the curriculum.

7(j) The teacher understands the strengths and needs of individual learners and how to plan instruction that is responsive to these strengths and needs.

A-16 Licensure Appendix PRAXIS II® TOPICS

WOOLFOLK AND USHER TEXT CONNECTIONS



Activating students’ prior knowledge

Cluster 8/The Importance of Knowledge in Cognition; Long-Term Memory Cluster 9/Defining Goals and Representing the Problem Cluster 10/Scaffolding



Anticipating preconceptions

Cluster 9/Factors That Hinder Problem Solving



Encouraging exploration and problem solving

Cluster 3/Implications of Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories for Teachers Cluster 9/Problem Solving Cluster 11/Agency and Self-Efficacy



Building new skills on those previously acquired

Cluster 3/Basic Tendencies in Thinking Cluster 5/Students with Intellectual Disabilities; Guidelines: Teaching Students with Intellectual Disabilities Cluster 8/Long-Term Memory; Constructing Declarative Knowledge: Making Meaningful Connections; Development of Procedural Knowledge Cluster 10/Scaffolding Cluster 11/Observational Learning in Teaching

INTASC ESSENTIAL KNOWLEDGE INDICATORS

C.

Assessment Strategies

1.

Types of assessments

Cluster 15/Norm-Referenced Versus CriterionReferenced Grading; Formative and Authentic Classroom Assessments; Classroom Assessment: Testing

6(j) The teacher understands the differences between formative and summative applications of assessment and knows how and when to use each.

2.

Characteristics of assessments

Cluster 15/Basics of Assessment; Assessing the Assessments: Reliability and Validity; Writing MultipleChoice Questions; Constructed Responses: Essay Testing; Reliability, Validity, Generalizability; Guidelines: Developing a Rubric

6(k) The teacher understands the range of types and multiple purposes of assessment and how to design, adapt, or select appropriate assessments to address specific learning goals and individual differences, and to minimize sources of bias.

3.

Scoring assessments

Cluster 15/Using Multiple-Choice Tests; Evaluating Essays; Evaluating Portfolios and Performances; Guidelines: Developing a Rubric

4.

Uses of assessments

Cluster 15/Norm-Referenced Versus Criterion-Referenced Grading; Accountability and High-Stakes Testing; Formative and Authentic Classroom Assessments; Classroom Assessment: Testing

5.

Understanding of measurement theory and assessmentrelated issues

Cluster 15/Norm-Referenced Versus Criterion-Referenced Grading

6(1) The teacher knows how to analyze assessment data to understand patterns and gaps in learning, to guide planning and instruction, and to provide meaningful feedback to all learners.

III. Communication Techniques A.

Basic, effective verbal and nonverbal communication techniques

Cluster 13/The Need for Communication; Prevention Is the Best Medicine

8(m) The teacher understands how multiple forms of communication (oral, written, nonverbal, digital, visual) convey ideas, foster self-expression, and build relationships.

B.

Effect of cultural and gender differences on communications in the classroom

Cluster 6/Dialect Differences in the Classroom Cluster 2/Sociolinguistics; Gender in Teaching and Learning; Creating Culturally Welcoming Classrooms

3(1) The teacher understands how learner diversity can affect communication and knows how to communicate effectively in differing environments.

C.

Types of questions that can stimulate discussion in different ways for different purposes

Cluster 10/Facilitating Deep Learning in a Constructivist Classroom Cluster 14/Questioning, Discussion, Dialogue, and Feedback

Licensure Appendix

A-17

INTASC ESSENTIAL KNOWLEDGE INDICATORS

PRAXIS II® TOPICS

WOOLFOLK AND USHER TEXT CONNECTIONS

1.

Probing for learner understanding

Cluster 10/Facilitating Through Asking and Answering Deep Questions; Guidelines: Facilitating Deep Questioning Cluster 14/Questioning, Discussion, Dialogue, and Feedback

2.

Helping students articulate their ideas and thinking processes

Cluster 9/Guidelines: Applying Problem Solving Cluster 14/Group Discussion and Quality Talk; Guidelines: Productive Group Discussions

5(n) The teacher understands communication modes and skills as vehicles for learning (e.g., information gathering and processing) across disciplines as well as vehicles for expressing learning.

3.

Promoting risk taking and problem solving

Cluster 9/Problem Solving

8(j) The teacher understands the cognitive processes associated with various kinds of learning (e.g., critical and creative thinking, problem framing and problem solving, invention, memorization, and recall) and how these processes can be stimulated.

4.

Facilitating factual recall

Cluster 14/Questioning, Discussion, Dialogue, and Feedback

5.

Encouraging convergent and divergent thinking

Cluster 5/Assessing Creativity Cluster 14/Kinds of Questions

6.

Stimulating curiosity

Cluster 12/Tapping Interests; Guidelines: Building on Students’ Interests and Curiosity; Curiosity: Novelty and Complexity

7.

Helping students to question

Cluster 10/Facilitating Through Asking and Answering Deep Questions; Guidelines: Facilitating Deep Questioning Cluster 14/Fitting the Questions to the Students—and Waiting

IV. Profession and Community A.

The Reflective Practitioner

1.

Types of resources available for professional development and learning

Clusters 1–15/Teachers’ Casebook (opening and closing sections of each cluster)

Professional literature Colleagues 2.

Ability to read and understand articles about current views, ideas, and debates regarding best teaching practices

Cluster 1/Using Research to Understand and Improve Learning Clusters 1–15/Point/Counterpoint (one in each cluster)

3.

Why personal reflection on teaching practices is critical, and approaches that can be used to achieve this

Clusters 1–15/Point/Counterpoint (one in each cluster)

9(g) The teacher understands and knows how to use a variety of self-assessment and problem-solving strategies to analyze and reflect on his/her practice and to plan for adaptations/adjustments.

B.

The Larger Community

1.

Role of the school as a resource to the larger community

Cluster 2/Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity Cluster 13/Caring Relationships: Connections with School

10(1) The teacher understands schools as organizations within historical, cultural, political, and social contexts and knows how to work with others across the system to support learners.

2.

Factors in the students’ environment outside of school (family circumstances, community environments, health, and economic conditions) that may influence students’ life and learning

Cluster 2/Economic and Social Class Differences Cluster 4/Families; Peers Cluster 5/Students with Learning Challenges

10(m) The teacher understands that alignment of family, school, and community spheres of influence enhances student learning and that discontinuity in these spheres of influence interferes with learning.

A-18 Licensure Appendix PRAXIS II® TOPICS

WOOLFOLK AND USHER TEXT CONNECTIONS

INTASC ESSENTIAL KNOWLEDGE INDICATORS

3.

Basic strategies for involving parents/guardians and leaders in the community in the educational process

Cluster 2/Who Are You? Who Are Your Students? Culture and Diversity Clusters 3–9, 12–15/Family and Community Partnerships (one in each cluster)

10(n) The teacher knows how to work with other adults and has developed skills in collaborative interaction appropriate for both face-to-face and virtual contexts.

4.

Major laws related to students’ rights and teacher responsibilities

Cluster 5/Individual Differences and the Law; The Rights of Students and Families Cluster 13/The Need for Communication

9(j) The teacher understands laws related to learners’ rights and teacher responsibilities (e.g., for educational equity, appropriate education for learners with disabilities, confidentiality, privacy, appropriate treatment of learners, reporting in situations related to possible child abuse).

Equal education Appropriate education for handicapped children ● Confidentiality and privacy ● Appropriate treatment of students Reporting situations related to possible child abuse ● ●

Cluster 4/Teachers and Child Abuse Cluster 4/Mandated Reporting

Note: The InTASC (Interstate Teacher Assessment and Support Consortium) standards were developed by the Council of Chief State School Officers and member states. Copies may be downloaded from the council’s website at http://www.ccsso.org. Source: Council of Chief State School Officers. (2013, April). Interstate Teacher Assessment and Support Consortium InTASC Model Core Teaching Standards and Learning Progressions for Teachers 1.0: A Resource for Ongoing Teacher Development. Washington, DC: Author.

Glossary Absence seizure A seizure involving only a small part of the brain and that causes a child to lose contact briefly with ongoing events—short lapses of consciousness. Academic language The entire range of language used in elementary, secondary, and university-level schools, including words, concepts, strategies, and processes from academic subjects. Academic learning time Accommodation

Time when students are actually succeeding at the learning task.

Altering existing schemes or creating new ones in response to new information.

Accountable Making teachers and schools responsible for student learning, usually by monitoring learning with high-stakes tests. Acronym Technique for remembering by using the first letter of each word in a phrase to form a new, memorable word. Action research Systematic observations or tests of methods conducted by teachers or schools to improve teaching and learning for their students. Active teaching Teaching characterized by high levels of teacher explanation, demonstration, and interaction with students. Adaptation

Adjustment to the environment.

Adolescent egocentrism

Assumption that everyone else shares one’s thoughts, feelings, and concerns.

Advance organizer A statement or tool that introduces and summarizes concepts to help students organize the information they will learn about. Affective domain

Objectives focusing on attitudes and feelings.

Affinity groups Online communities for video-game users where they can share knowledge, strategies, role-play scenarios, game modifications, or fan fiction stories and novels based on the games they are playing. Algorithm

Step-by-step procedure for solving a problem; prescription for solutions.

Ambitious teaching An approach to teaching grounded in challenging goals, complex but engaging tasks, and high-quality discussions building on students’ ideas. Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990 (ADA) Federal legislation prohibiting discrimination against individuals with disabilities in employment, transportation, public access, local government, and telecommunications. Amotivation A complete lack of any intent to act—no engagement at all. Analogical thinking the one at hand. Anorexia nervosa

Heuristic in which one limits the search for solutions to situations that are similar to

Eating disorder characterized by very limited food intake.

Antecedents Events that precede an action. Anxiety General uneasiness, a feeling of tension. Applied behavior analysis behavior.

The application of behavioral learning principles to understand and change

Appropriating Being able to internalize or take for yourself knowledge and skills developed in interaction with others or with cultural tools. Argumentation

The process of debating a claim with someone else.

Articulation disorders omission of sounds. Assertive discipline

Any of a variety of pronunciation difficulties, such as the substitution, distortion, or Clear, firm, nonhostile response style.

Assessment Procedures used to obtain information about student performance. Assessment bias Qualities of an assessment instrument that offend or unfairly penalize a group of students because of the students’ gender, socioeconomic status, race, ethnicity, and so on. Assimilation

Fitting new information into existing schemes.

Assisted learning Providing strategic help in the initial stages of learning, gradually diminishing that help as students gain independence. Assistive technology Devices, systems, and services that support and improve the capabilities of individuals with disabilities.

G-1

G-2 Glossary Attachment Attention

Forming an emotional bond with another person, initially a parent or family member. Focus on a stimulus.

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Current term for disruptive behavior disorders marked by overactivity, excessive difficulty sustaining attention, or impulsiveness. Attribution theories Descriptions of how individuals’ explanations, justifications, and excuses influence their motivation and emotion. Authentic assessments real-life situations. Authentic questions Authentic task classroom.

Assessment procedures that test skills and abilities as they would be applied in

Questions that the teachers do not have a predetermined answer for.

Tasks that have some connection to real-life problems the students will face outside the

Autism spectrum disorders Set of developmental disabilities significantly affecting verbal and nonverbal communication and social interaction, generally evident before age 3, and ranging from mild to major. Automated basic skills

Skills that are applied without conscious thought.

Automaticity The ability to thoroughly perform learned tasks without much mental effort. The result of learning to perform a behavior or thinking process so thoroughly that the performance is automatic and does not require effort. Autonomy

Independence.

Availability heuristic Judging the likelihood of an event based on what is available in your memory, assuming those easily remembered events are common. Balanced bilingualism

Adding a second language capability without losing your heritage language.

Basic skills Clearly structured knowledge that is needed for later learning and that can be taught step-by-step. Behavioral learning theories Explanations of learning that focus on external events as the cause of changes in observable behaviors. Being needs

Maslow’s three higher-level needs, sometimes called growth needs.

Belief perseverance Bias

The tendency to hold on to beliefs, even in the face of contradictory evidence.

A prejudicial preference or action.

Bilingual

Speaking two languages and dealing appropriately with the two different cultures.

Binge eating disorder peanut butter.

Uncontrolled eating of large quantities of food, such as a whole cake or entire jar of

Bioecological model Bronfenbrenner’s theory describing the nested social and cultural contexts that shape development. Every person develops within a microsystem inside a mesosystem that is embedded in an exosystem—all of which are a part of the macrosystem of the culture. All development occurs in and is influenced by the time period—the chronosystem. Blended families

Parents, children, and stepchildren merged into families through remarriages.

Bottom-up processing Perceiving based on noticing separate defining features and assembling them into a recognizable pattern. Brainstorming

Generating ideas without stopping to evaluate them.

Bulimia nervosa Eating disorder characterized by overeating and then getting rid of the food by self-induced vomiting or laxatives. CAPS

A strategy that can be used in reading literature: Characters, Aim of story, Problem, Solution.

Case study

Intensive study of one individual or one situation.

Central executive The part of working memory that is responsible for monitoring and directing attention and other mental resources. Central tendency Cerebral palsy

Typical score for a group of scores.

Condition involving a range of motor or coordination difficulties due to brain damage.

Chain mnemonics

Memory strategies that associate one element in a series with the next element.

Chunking Grouping individual bits of data into meaningful, larger units. Classical conditioning Classification

Association of automatic responses with new stimuli.

Grouping objects into categories.

Classroom assessments Classroom assessments are selected and created by teachers and can take many different forms—unit tests, essays, portfolios, projects, performances, oral presentations, and so on. Classroom management behavior problems.

Techniques used to maintain a healthy learning environment, relatively free of

Glossary Cmaps Tools for concept mapping developed by the Institute for Human and Machine Cognition that are connected to many knowledge maps and other resources on the Internet. Coactions Joint actions of individual biology and the environment—each shapes and influences the other. Co-constructed process A social process in which people interact and negotiate (usually verbally) to create an understanding or to solve a problem. The final product is shaped by all participants. Code switching

Moving between two speech forms.

Cognitive apprenticeship A relationship in which a less experienced learner acquires knowledge and skills under the guidance of an expert. Cognitive development Gradual, orderly changes by which mental processes become more complex and sophisticated. Cognitive domain In Bloom’s taxonomy, memory and reasoning objectives. Cognitive load The volume of cognitive resources necessary to complete a task. Cognitive science brain.

The interdisciplinary study of thinking, language, artificial intelligence (AI), and the

Cognitive view of learning A general approach that views learning as an active mental process of acquiring, remembering, and using knowledge. Collaboration

A philosophy about how to relate to others—how to learn and work together.

Collective monologue Form of speech in which children in a group talk but do not really interact or communicate. Commitment In Marcia’s theory of identity statuses, individuals’ choices concerning political and religious beliefs, for example, usually as a consequence of exploring the options. Community of practice Compensation dimension.

Social situation or context in which ideas are judged useful or true.

The principle that changes in one dimension can be offset by changes in another

Complex learning environments real life.

Problems and learning situations that mimic the ill-structured nature of

Computational thinking The thought processes involved in formulating problems so you can represent their solution steps and algorithms for computing. Computerized axial tomography (CAT) A technique that uses X-ray technology to provide enhanced, three-dimensional images of the part of the body scanned. Concept A category used to group similar events, ideas, objects, or people. Concept map A drawing that charts the relationships among ideas. Concrete operations Mental tasks tied to concrete objects and situations. Conditioned response (CR) Learned response to a previously neutral stimulus. Conditioned stimulus (CS) conditioning. Confidence interval Confirmation bias ing evidence.

Stimulus that evokes an emotional or physiological response after

Range of scores within which an individual’s true score is likely to fall. Seeking information that confirms our choices and beliefs while ignoring disconfirm-

Consequences Events that follow an action. Conservation appearance.

Principle that some characteristics of an object remain the same despite changes in its

Constructive/Structured controversy controversy.

Students work in cooperative groups to research a particular

Constructivism/Constructivist approach View that emphasizes the active role of the learner in building understanding and making sense of information. Context Internal and external circumstances and situations that interact with the individual’s thoughts, feelings, and actions to shape development and learning. The physical or emotional backdrop associated with an event. Contiguity Association of two events because of repeated pairing. Contingency contract A contract between the teacher and a student specifying what the student must do to earn a particular reward or privilege. Continuous reinforcement schedule Convergent questions edge questions.

Presenting a reinforcer after every appropriate response.

Questions with only one right answer—usually factual questions or rote knowl-

G-3

G-4 Glossary Convergent thinking

Narrowing possibilities to a single answer.

Cooperation Way of working with others to attain a shared goal. Cooperative learning Situations in which elaboration, interpretation, explanation, and argumentation are integral to the activity of the group and where learning is supported by other individuals. Co-regulated learning Process of sharing the regulation of learning, behaviors, motivation, and emotions among group members. Co-regulation A transitional phase during which students gradually appropriate self-regulated learning and skills through modeling, direct teaching, feedback, and coaching from teachers, parents, or peers. Correlations Creativity

Statistical descriptions of how closely two variables are related.

Imaginative, original thinking or problem solving.

Criterion-referenced grading Criterion-referenced testing Critical periods

Assessment of each student’s mastery of course objectives. Testing in which scores are compared to a set performance standard.

If learning doesn’t happen during these periods, it never will.

Critical thinking Evaluating conclusions by logically and systematically examining the problem, the evidence, and the solution. Crystallized intelligence

Ability to apply culturally approved problem-solving methods.

Cueing Providing a stimulus that “sets up” a desired behavior. Cultural deficit model A model that explains the school achievement problems of ethnic minority students by assuming that their cultures are inadequate and does not prepare the students to succeed in school. Cultural tools The real tools (computers, scales, etc.) and symbol systems (numbers, language, graphs) that allow people in a society to communicate, think, solve problems, and create knowledge. Culturally relevant pedagogy Excellent teaching that supports students’ cultural identities by promoting academic success, developing/maintaining cultural competence, and developing a critical consciousness to challenge the status quo. Culturally responsive classroom management Taking cultural meanings and styles into account when developing management plans and responding to students. Culture The knowledge, values, attitudes, and traditions that guide the behavior of a group of people and allow them to solve the problems of living in their environment. Cyber aggression Using email, Twitter, Facebook, Instagram, or other social media to spread rumors, make threats, or otherwise terrorize peers. Decay

The weakening and fading of memories with the passage of time.

Decentering

Focusing on more than one aspect at a time.

Declarative knowledge

Verbal information; facts; “knowing that” something is the case.

Deficiency needs Maslow’s four lower-level needs, which must be satisfied first before higher-level needs can be addressed. Defining attribute

Qualities that connect members of a group to a specific concept.

Deliberate practice Becoming expert in a skill that has clear criteria by focusing on the discrepancies between your current skill level and expert performance and working persistently to improve your performance, usually with guidance and feedback from an expert coach. Descriptive studies Studies that collect detailed information about specific situations, often using observation, surveys, interviews, recordings, or a combination of these methods. Design-based research Practitioners identify research questions based on problems of practice; then researchers gather and analyze the data to address those problems. Desirable difficulty The more effort that is required to remember something, the better you will learn and the stronger the memory will be—as long as the efforts are successful. Development Orderly, adaptive changes we go through between conception and death; these developmental changes remain for a reasonably long period of time. Developmental crisis

A specific conflict whose resolution prepares the way for the next stage.

Deviation IQ Score based on a statistical comparison of an individual’s performance with the average performance of others in that age group. Dialect

Any variety of a language spoken by a particular group.

Differentiated instruction A flexible approach to teaching that matches content, process, and product based on student differences in readiness, interests, and learning needs. Takes into account students’ abilities, prior knowledge, and challenges so that instruction matches not only the subject being taught but also students’ needs.

Glossary Direct instruction/Explicit teaching information. Direct reinforcement

Systematic instruction for mastery of basic skills, facts, and

Reinforcement given after successful completion of a task.

Disability The inability to do something specific such as walk or hear. Discrimination

Treating unfairly or acting unfairly toward particular categories of people.

Disequilibrium In Piaget’s theory, the “out-of-balance” state that occurs when an individual realizes that his or her current ways of thinking are not working to solve a problem or understand a situation. Distractors Wrong answers offered as choices in a multiple-choice item. Distributed learning/practice

Practice in brief periods with rest intervals.

Distributive justice Beliefs about how to divide materials or privileges fairly among members of a group; follows a sequence of development from equality to merit to benevolence. Divergent questions

Questions that have no single correct answer.

Divergent thinking Coming up with many possible solutions. Domain-specific knowledge one specific topic. Domain-specific strategies

Information that is useful in a particular situation or that applies mainly to Consciously applied skills to reach goals in a particular subject or problem.

Dual coding theory Suggests that information is stored in long-term memory as either visual images or verbal units, or both. Educational psychology The discipline concerned with teaching and learning processes; applies the methods and theories of psychology and has its own as well. Effective instruction delivery (EID) Instructions that are concise, clear, and specific, and that communicate an expected result. Statements work better than questions. Egocentric Assuming that others experience the world the way you do. Elaboration Adding and extending meaning by connecting new information to existing knowledge. Elaborative rehearsal Keeping information in working memory by associating it with something else you already know. Electroencephalograph (EEG) A technique that measures electrical patterns in the brain created by neuron movements using electrodes attached to the scalp. Embodied cognition Theory stating that cognitive processes develop from real-time, goal-directed interactions between humans and their environment. Emergent literacy The skills and knowledge, usually developed in the preschool years, that are the foundation for the development of reading and writing. Emotional Intelligence (EI) The abilities to monitor your own and other people’s feelings, to discriminate among different feelings, and to use this information to guide your own thinking, decisions, and actions. Emotional and behavioral disorders Behaviors or emotions that deviate so much from the norm that they interfere with the child’s own growth and development and/or the lives of others—inappropriate behaviors, unhappiness or depression, fears and anxieties, and trouble with relationships. Emotions Three interrelated factors—physiological responses, behaviors, and feelings—that produce an affective response to a situation. Empathetic listening by paraphrasing. Empirical

Hearing the intent and emotions behind what another says and reflecting them back

Based on systematically collected data.

Enactive learning

Learning by doing and experiencing the consequences of your actions.

EMR model A research-based strategy for developing positive relationships with students. EMR stands for Establish-Maintain-Restore. Engaged time Time spent actively engaged in the learning task at hand. Also referred to as time on task. English as a second language (ESL) The classes devoted to teaching English to students who are English learners. English learners (ELs) English.

Students who are learning English and whose primary or heritage language is not

Epilepsy Disorder marked by seizures and caused by abnormal electrical discharges in the brain. Episodic buffer The process that brings together and integrates information from the phonological loop, visuo-spatial sketchpad, and long-term memory under the supervision of the central executive. Episodic memory Long-term memory for information tied to a particular time and place, especially memory of the events in a person’s life.

G-5

G-6 Glossary Equilibration Search for mental balance between cognitive schemes and information from the environment. Ethnic-racial identity The beliefs, feelings, significance, and meaning people have about their ethnicity or race. Ethnicity

A cultural heritage shared by a group of people.

Ethnography A descriptive approach to research that focuses on life within a group and tries to understand the meaning of events to the people involved. Event-related potential (ERP) scalp.

Measurements that assess electrical activity of the brain through the skull or

Exceptional students Students who fall outside the typical range of development and require special educational services to reach their full potential. Executive control processes Processes such as selective attention, rehearsal, elaboration, and organization that influence encoding, storage, and retrieval of information in memory. Executive functioning All those processes that we use to organize, coordinate, and perform goal-directed, intentional actions, including focusing attention, inhibiting impulsive responses, making and changing plans, and using memory to hold and manipulate information. Exemplar

An actual memory of a specific object.

Expectancy for success Students’ beliefs about how well they will perform on an upcoming task. Expectancy × value theories Explanations of motivation that emphasize individuals’ expectations for success combined with their valuing of the goal. Experimentation

Research method in which variables are manipulated and the effects recorded.

Expert teachers Experienced, effective teachers who have developed solutions for classroom problems. Their knowledge of the teaching process and content is extensive and well organized. Explicit memory

Long-term memories that involve deliberate or conscious recall.

Exploration In Marcia’s theory of identity statuses, the process by which adolescents consider and try out alternative beliefs, values, and behaviors in an effort to determine which will give them the most satisfaction. Expressive vocabulary All the different words a person uses in speaking or writing. Extended families Different family members—grandparents, aunts, uncles, cousins, and so on—living in the same household or at least in daily contact with the children in the family. Extinction

The disappearance of a learned response.

Extraneous cognitive load Extrinsic motivation Fixed mindset

The resources required to process stimuli irrelevant to the task.

Motivation created by external factors such as rewards and punishments.

A personally held belief that abilities are stable, uncontrollable, set traits.

Flashbulb memories Flexible grouping

Clear, vivid memories of emotionally important events in your life.

Grouping and regrouping students based on learning needs.

Flow A mental state in which you are fully immersed in a challenging task that is accompanied by high levels of concentration and involvement. Fluency disorder A language disorder characterized by disruptions in the production of speech sounds, commonly called stuttering. Fluid intelligence

Mental efficiency; nonverbal abilities grounded in brain development.

Flynn effect Because of better health, smaller families, increased complexity in the environment, and more and better schooling, IQ test scores are steadily rising. Focal onset seizure A seizure originating in just one area of the brain lasting only a minute or two. The person may experience a sudden feeling of joy, sadness, anger, or nausea, or sensations such as taste, smell, or movement in one part of the body. Formal operations

Mental tasks involving abstract thinking and coordination of several variables.

Formative assessment Ungraded testing used before or during instruction to aid in planning and diagnosis. Free, appropriate public education (FAPE) for all students no matter their needs.

Public funding to support appropriate educational programs

Functional behavioral assessment (FBA) Procedures used to obtain information about antecedents, behaviors, and consequences to determine the reason or function of the behavior. Functional fixedness

Inability to use objects or tools in a new way.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) An MRI is an imaging technique that uses a magnetic field along with radio waves and a computer to create detailed pictures of the inside of the body. A functional MRI uses an MRI to measure the tiny changes that take place in the brain during brain activity. Funds of knowledge Knowledge that families and community members have acquired in many areas of work, home, and religious life that can become the basis for teaching.

Glossary Gender biases Different views of men, women, or gender-nonconforming people often favoring one group over the other. Gender identity nonbinary).

The sense of self along the female-to-male continuum or as neither female nor male (i.e.,

Gender roles The behaviors and characteristics that the culture stereotypically associates with being a man or a woman. Gender schemas Organized cognitive structures that include gender-related information that influences how children think and behave. Genderlects

Different ways of talking for men and women.

General intelligence (g) A general factor in cognitive ability that is related in varying degrees to performance on all mental tests. General knowledge many situations.

Information that is useful in many different kinds of tasks; information that applies to

Generalized onset seizure

A seizure involving a large portion of the brain.

Generation 1.5 Students whose characteristics, educational experiences, and language fluencies are somewhere in between those of students born in the United States and those of students who are recent immigrants. Generativity

Sense of concern for future generations.

Gestalt German for pattern or configuration. Gestalt theorists hold that people organize their perceptions into coherent wholes. Gifted and talented many domains.

Students who demonstrate outstanding aptitudes and competencies in one or more of

Glial cells The white matter of the brain. These cells greatly outnumber neurons and appear to have many functions such as fighting infections, controlling blood flow and communication among neurons, and providing the myelin coating around axon fibers. Goal orientations

Patterns of beliefs about goals related to achievement in school.

Goal-directed actions

Deliberate actions toward a goal.

Good behavior game Arrangement where a class is divided into teams and each team receives demerit points for breaking agreed-upon rules of good behavior. Grade-equivalent score Measure of grade level based on comparison with norming samples from each grade. Grading on the curve Norm-referenced grading that compares students’ performance to an average level. Grit

A personality trait characterized by passion and persistence to meet long-term goals.

Group consequences Rewards or punishments given to a class as a whole for adhering to or violating rules of conduct. Group discussion Conversation in which the teacher does not have the dominant role; students pose and answer their own questions. Group focus The ability to keep as many students as possible involved in activities. Growth mindset

A personally held belief that abilities are unstable, controllable, and improvable.

Handicap A disadvantage in a particular situation, sometimes caused by a disability. Heritage language The language spoken in the student’s home or by members of the student’s family. Heuristic General strategy used in attempting to solve problems. Hierarchy of needs Maslow’s model of seven levels of human needs, from basic physiological requirements to the need for self-actualization. High-stakes testing Standardized tests whose results have powerful influences when used by school administrators, other officials, or employers to make decisions. Hostile aggression Human agency

Bold, direct action that is intended to hurt someone else; unprovoked attack.

The capacity to coordinate learning skills, motivation, and emotions to reach your goals.

Hypothesis/hypotheses ous research.

A prediction of what will happen in a research study based on theory and previ-

Hypothetico-deductive reasoning A formal-operations problem-solving strategy in which an individual begins by identifying all the factors that might affect a problem and then deduces and systematically evaluates specific solutions. “I” message

Clear, nonaccusatory statement of how something is affecting you.

Identity Principle that an individual or object remains the same over time. The complex answer to the question: “Who am I?” Identity achievement

Strong sense of commitment to life choices after free consideration of alternatives.

G-7

G-8 Glossary Identity diffusion

Uncenteredness; confusion about who you are and what you want.

Identify-first reference Using terms such as “autistic” or “deaf” to describe a person—some people prefer this reference because they “claim” the disability as their own and value it as a part of who they are. It is a source of pride to be part of that culture. Identity foreclosure Images

Acceptance of parental life choices without considering options.

Representations based on the physical attributes—the appearance—of information.

Immersive virtual learning environment (IVLE) A simulation of a real-world environment that immerses students in tasks like those required in a professional practicum. Immigrants

People who voluntarily leave their country to become permanent residents in a new place.

Impairment

Any loss or abnormality in a physical or psychological structure or function.

Implicit memory Knowledge that we are not conscious of recalling but that influences our behavior or thought without our awareness. Importance or attainment value The importance of doing well on a task; how success on the task meets personal or identity-based needs. Incentive

An object or event that encourages or discourages behavior.

Inclusion

The integration of all students, including those with severe disabilities, into regular classes.

Individualized education program (IEP) Annually revised program for an exceptional student that details the present achievement level, goals, and strategies and is drawn up by teachers, parents, specialists, and (if possible) the student. Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA) Latest amendment of PL 94–142; guarantees a free public education to all children regardless of disability. Industry

Eagerness to engage in productive work.

Informal assessments Ungraded (formative) assessments that gather information from multiple sources to help teachers make decisions. Information processing Initiative

The human mind’s activity of taking in, storing, and using information.

Willingness to begin new activities and explore new directions.

Inquiry learning Approach in which the teacher presents a puzzling situation or question and students solve the problem by gathering data and testing their conclusions. Insight In problem solving, the sudden realization of a solution. In the triarchic theory of intelligence, the ability to deal effectively with novel situations. Instrumental aggression Strong actions aimed at claiming an object, place, or privilege—not intended to harm but may lead to harm. Integration Integrity

Fitting the child with special needs into existing class structures.

Sense of self-acceptance and fulfillment.

Intellectual disabilities before age 18. Intelligence

Significantly below-average intellectual and adaptive social behavior, evident

Ability or abilities to acquire and use knowledge for solving problems and adapting to the world.

Intelligence quotient (IQ)

Score comparing mental and chronological ages.

Interest or intrinsic value

The enjoyment a person gets from a task.

Interference The process that occurs when remembering certain information is hampered by the presence of other information. Interim (growth or benchmark) assessments Assessments that occur at regular intervals during the school year to determine student progress and growth in an objective way. Interleaved practice Mixing up practice by, for example, tossing from 2 and 4 feet before being tested at 3 feet, solving different types of problems, or practicing different vocabulary words. Intermittent reinforcement schedule Internalize

Presenting a reinforcer after some but not all responses.

Process whereby children adopt external standards as their own.

Intersectionality Our overlapping, intersecting social identities (gender, sexual orientation, class, ethnicity, religion, age, etc.) that shape each and every one of us in unique ways. Interval schedule Intimacy

Length of time between reinforcers. Reinforcement based on time between responses.

Forming close, enduring relationships with others.

Intrinsic cognitive load Intrinsic motivation

The resources required by the task itself, regardless of other stimuli.

Motivation associated with activities that are internally rewarding.

Jason Flatt Act Legislation passed in 20 states that requires educators to complete in-service training about awareness and prevention of death by suicide.

Glossary Jigsaw classroom A learning process in which each student is part of a group and each group member is given part of the material to be learned by the whole group. Students become “expert” on their piece and then teach it to the others in their group. Keyword method

System of associating new words or concepts with similar-sounding cue words and images.

KWL A strategy to guide reading and inquiry: Before—What do I already know? What do I want to know? After—What have I learned? Lateralization

The specialization of the two hemispheres (sides) of the brain cortex.

Learned helplessness The expectation, based on previous experiences with a lack of control, that all of one’s efforts will lead to failure. Learning Process through which experience causes permanent change in knowledge or behavior. Learning management system (LMS) Systems that deliver e-learning, provide tools and learning materials, keep records, administer assessments, and manage learning. Learning preferences Preferred ways of studying and learning, such as using pictures instead of text, working with other people versus working alone, learning in structured or in unstructured situations, and so on. Learning sciences An interdisciplinary science of learning, based on research in psychology, education, computer science, philosophy, sociology, anthropology, neuroscience, and other fields that study learning. Learning strategies Learning styles

A special kind of procedural knowledge—knowing how to approach learning tasks.

Characteristic approaches to learning and studying.

Least restrictive environment (LRE) to the greatest extent possible.

Educating each child with peers in the general education classroom

Legally blind Seeing at 20 feet what a person with normal vision can see at 200 feet and/or having severely restricted peripheral vision. Lesson study As a group, teachers develop, test, improve, and retest lessons until they are satisfied with the final version. Levels of processing theory

Theory that recall of information is based on how deeply it is processed.

Limited English proficient (LEP) A term also used for students who are learning English when their primary or heritage language is not English—not the preferred term because of the negative connotations. Use English learner (EL) instead. Loci method Technique of associating items with specific places. Locus of causality The location—internal or external—of the cause of behavior. Long-term memory

Permanent store of knowledge.

Low vision Vision limited to close objects. Mainstreaming

Teaching children with disabilities in regular classes for part or all of their school days.

Maintenance rehearsal

Keeping information in working memory by repeating it to yourself.

Mandated Reporting By law, teachers and other professionals who have frequent contact with children must report suspected child abuse and neglect. Massed practice

Practice for a single extended period.

Massive multi-player online games (MMOs) Interactive gaming environments constructed in virtual worlds with many players who may assume character roles or avatars. Mastery experiences Students’ awareness of their own previous successful performance—the most powerful source of efficacy information. Mastery goal A personal intention to improve abilities and learn, no matter how performance suffers. Maturation Mean

Genetically programmed, naturally occurring changes over time.

Arithmetical average.

Means-ends analysis

Heuristic in which a goal is divided into subgoals.

Measurement An evaluation expressed in quantitative (number) terms. Median Middle score in a group of scores. Melting pot A metaphor for the absorption and assimilation of immigrants into the mainstream of society so that ethnic differences vanish. Menarche The first menstrual period in girls. Mental age

In intelligence testing, a performance that represents average abilities for an age group.

Meta-analysis An integration and summary of many individual studies to synthesize the outcomes into one result that characterizes the findings from the studies. Metacognition Knowledge about your own thinking processes and how to control them along with the skills to regulate your thinking.

G-9

G-10 Glossary Metalinguistic awareness

Understanding your own use of language.

Mirror systems Areas of the brain that fire both during perception of an action by someone else and when performing the action. Mnemonics

Techniques for remembering; the art of memory.

Mobile learning Mode

Learning that takes place across contexts through the use of personal electronic devices.

Most frequently occurring score.

Modeling

Changes in behavior, thinking, or emotions that happen through observing another person—a model.

Monolingual

Speaking only one language.

Moral dilemma Moral realism

Situations in which no choice is clearly and indisputably right. Stage of development wherein children see rules as absolute.

Moral reasoning

The thinking process involved in judgments about questions of right and wrong.

Morality of cooperation can change them. Moratorium Motivation

Stage of development wherein children realize that people make rules and people

Identity crisis; suspension of choices because of struggle. An internal state that arouses, directs, and maintains behavior.

Motivation to learn The tendency to find academic activities meaningful and worthwhile and to try to benefit from them. Movement management Keeping lessons and the group moving at an appropriate (and flexible) pace, with smooth transitions and variety. Multicultural education Education that promotes equity in the schooling of all students and challenges racism and other forms of discrimination. Multiple representations of content Considering problems using various analogies, examples, and metaphors. Myelination The process by which neural fibers are coated with a fatty sheath called myelin that makes message transfer more efficient. Natural/logical consequences Instead of punishing, have students redo, repair, or in some way face the consequences that naturally flow from their actions. Near-infrared optical tomography (NIR-OT) A technique that uses an optical fiber to transmit nearinfrared light through the scalp and into the brain. Some of the light is reflected back, indicating blood flow and oxygenation in the blood, which reveals brain activity. Need for autonomy The desire to have our own wishes, rather than external rewards or pressures, determine our actions. Need for competence

The desire to demonstrate ability or mastery over the tasks at hand.

Need for relatedness The desire to belong and to establish close emotional bonds and attachments with others who care about us. Negative correlation A relationship between two variables in which a high value on one is associated with a low value on the other. Example: height and distance from top of head to the ceiling. Negative reinforcement Strengthening behavior by removing an aversive stimulus when the behavior occurs. Neo-Piagetian theories More recent theories that integrate findings about attention, memory, and strategy use with Piaget’s insights about children’s thinking and the construction of knowledge. Neurogenesis Neurons

The production of new neurons.

Nerve cells that store and transfer information.

Neutral stimulus Nigrescence

Stimulus not connected to a response.

The process of developing a Black racial identity.

Nondiscriminatory evaluations A basic principle of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act specifying schools must determine each student’s disability and educational needs using tools and procedures that are culturally responsive. Norm group

Large sample of students serving as a comparison group for scoring tests.

Norm-referenced grading In determining grades, assessing students’ achievement in relation to one another. Norm-referenced testing Normal distribution around the mean. Object permanence

Testing in which scores are compared with the average performance of others.

The most commonly occurring distribution, in which scores are distributed evenly The understanding that objects have a separate, permanent existence.

Observational learning

Learning by observation and imitation of others—vicarious learning.

Glossary Operant conditioning Learning in which voluntary behavior is strengthened or weakened by consequences or antecedents. Operants

Voluntary (and generally goal-directed) behaviors emitted by a person or an animal.

Operations Actions an individual carries out by thinking them through instead of literally performing the actions. Organization Ongoing process of arranging information and experiences into mental systems or categories. Ordered and logical network of relations. Overlapping Supervising several activities at once. Overlearning Practicing a skill past the point of mastery. Overregularize To apply a rule of syntax or grammar in situations when the rule does not apply, for example, saying, “The bike was braked.” Overt aggression

A form of hostile aggression that involves physical attack.

Paraphrase rule Policy whereby listeners must accurately summarize what a speaker has said before being allowed to respond. Parenting styles

The ways of interacting with and disciplining children.

PARS A four-step reading strategy: Preview the material to identify main ideas; Ask questions related to the main idea; Read the chapter to find answers to your questions; Summarize the main ideas in your own words. Part learning

Breaking a list of items into shorter lists.

Participant observation A method for conducting descriptive research in which the researcher becomes a participant in the situation in order to better understand life in that group. Participants/subjects

People or animals studied.

Participation structures

The formal and informal rules for how to take part in a given activity.

Pedagogical content knowledge (PCK) Teacher knowledge that combines mastery of academic content with knowing how to teach the content and how to match instruction to student differences. Peer cultures Groups of children or adolescents with their own rules and norms, particularly about such things as dress, appearance, music, language, social values, and behavior. Percentile rank Percentage of those in the norming sample who scored below an individual’s score. Perception Interpretation of sensory information. Performance assessments Any form of assessment that requires students to carry out an activity or produce a product in order to demonstrate learning. Performance goal

A personal intention to seem competent or perform well in the eyes of others.

Personal development Changes in personality that take place as one grows. Personal learning environment (PLE) Virtual platform that provides tools that support individualized learning in a variety of contexts and situations. Personal learning network (PLN) interactions.

Framework in which knowledge is constructed through online peer

Person-first reference Language that places the emphasis on the individual and not the disability by speaking of “an individual with a specific learning disability” or any other condition. Perspective-taking ability

Understanding that others have different feelings and experiences.

Phonological loop Part of working memory. A speech- and sound-related system for holding and rehearsing (refreshing) words and sounds in short-term memory for about 1.5 to 2 seconds. Physical development

Changes in body structure and function over time.

Physiological or emotional arousal Physical and psychological reactions causing a person to feel alert, attentive, wide awake, excited, or tense. Plasticity The brain’s tendency to remain somewhat adaptable or flexible. Portfolio

A collection of the student’s work in an area, showing growth, self-reflection, and achievement.

Positive behavior supports (PBS) Interventions designed to replace problem behaviors with new actions that serve the same purpose for the student. Positive correlation A relationship between two variables in which the two increase or decrease together. Example: calorie intake and weight gain. Positive practice overcorrection Positive reinforcement

Practicing correct responses immediately after errors.

Strengthening behavior by presenting a desired stimulus after the behavior.

Positron emission tomography (PET) A method of localizing and measuring brain activity using computer-assisted motion pictures of the brain.

G-11

G-12 Glossary Pragmatics culture.

The rules for when and how to use language to be an effective communicator in a particular

Precorrection A tool for positive behavior support that involves identifying the context for a student’s misbehavior, clearly specifying the alternative expected behavior, modifying the situation to make the problem behavior less likely, and then rehearsing the expected positive behaviors in the new context and providing powerful reinforcers. Prejudice

Prejudgment or irrational generalization about an entire category of people.

Premack principle Principle stating that a more preferred activity can serve as a reinforcer for a less preferred activity. Preoperational

The stage before a child masters logical mental operations.

Presentation punishment Decreasing the chances that a behavior will occur again by presenting an aversive stimulus following the behavior; also called Type I punishment. Pretest

Formative test for assessing students’ knowledge, readiness, and abilities before instruction begins.

Priming Principle

Activating a concept in memory or the spread of activation from one concept to another. Established relationship between factors.

Private speech Children’s self-talk, which guides their thinking and actions. Eventually, these verbalizations are internalized as silent inner speech. Problem

Any situation in which you are trying to reach some goal and must find a means to do so.

Problem solving

Creating new solutions for problems.

Problem-based learning Students are confronted with a problem that launches their inquiry as they collaborate to find solutions and learn valuable information and skills in the process. Procedural knowledge Procedural memory

Knowledge that is demonstrated when we perform a task; “knowing how.”

Long-term memory for how to do things.

Procedures/Routines

Prescribed steps for an activity.

Production deficiency Students learn problem-solving strategies, but do not apply them when they could or should. Productions The contents of procedural memory; rules about what actions to take, given certain conditions. Propositional network Set of interconnected concepts and relationships in which long-term knowledge is held. Prototype

A best example or best representative of a category.

Psychomotor domain Physical ability and coordination objectives. Psychosocial Puberty

Describing the relationship of the individual’s emotional needs to the social environment.

The physiological changes during adolescence that lead to the ability to reproduce.

Punishment

Process that weakens or suppresses behavior.

Pygmalion effect Exceptional progress by a student as a result of high teacher expectations for that student; named for the mythological king Pygmalion, who made a statue and then caused it to be brought to life. Qualitative research Exploratory research that attempts to understand the meaning of events to the participants involved using such methods as case studies, interviews, ethnography, participant observation, and other approaches that focus on a few people in depth. Quality Talk An approach to classroom discussions designed to increase students’ high-level comprehension by encouraging students to think and talk about, around, and with the text. Quantitative research Research that studies many participants in a more formal and controlled way using objective measures such as experimentation, statistical analyses, tests, and structured observations. Quasi-experimental studies Studies that fit most of the criteria for true experiments, with the important exception that the participants are not assigned to groups at random. Instead, existing groups such as classes or schools participate in the experiments. Race

A socially constructed category based on appearances and ancestry.

Racial and ethnic pride

A positive self-concept about your racial or ethnic heritage.

Radical constructivism right or wrong.

Knowledge is assumed to be the individual’s construction; it cannot be judged

Random Without any definite pattern; following no rule. Range Distance between the highest and the lowest scores in a group. Ratio schedule

Reinforcement based on the number of responses between reinforcers.

Receptive vocabulary The words a person can understand in spoken or written language. Reciprocal questioning

Students work in pairs or triads to ask and answer questions about lesson material.

Glossary Reconstruction

Re-creating information by using memories, expectations, logic, and existing knowledge.

Reflective Thoughtful and inventive. Reflective teachers think back over situations to analyze what they did and why, and to consider how they might improve learning for their students. Refugees A special group of immigrants who also relocate voluntarily but who are fleeing their home country because living there is not safe for them. Reinforcement Reinforcer

Use of consequences to strengthen behavior.

Any event that follows a behavior and increases the chances that the behavior will occur again.

Relational aggression A form of hostile aggression that involves verbal attacks and other actions meant to harm social relationships. Reliability Consistency of test results. Removal punishment Decreasing the chances that a behavior will occur again by removing a pleasant stimulus following the behavior; also called Type II punishment. Representativeness heuristic Judging the likelihood of an event based on how well the events match your prototypes—what you think is representative of the category. Reprimands Criticisms for misbehavior; rebukes. Resilience The ability to adapt successfully in spite of difficult circumstances and threats to development. Resistance culture majority culture.

Group values and beliefs about refusing to adopt the behaviors and attitudes of the

Respondents Responses (generally automatic or involuntary) elicited by specific stimuli. Response Observable reaction to a stimulus. Response cost Punishment by loss of reinforcers. Response set Rigidity; the tendency to respond in the most familiar way. Response to intervention (RtI) A process to make sure students get appropriate research-based instruction and support as soon as possible and that teachers are systematic in documenting the interventions they have tried with these students so they can use this information in planning instruction. Restorative justice An alternative to punishment that focuses on building, nurturing, and repairing relationships while giving a voice to victims, offenders, and the community. Restructuring

Conceiving of a problem in a new or different way.

Retrieval Process of searching for and finding information in long-term memory. Retrieval practice/Testing effect Practicing by retrieving information from memory instead of rereading or restudying—more effective because retrieval seems to help memories consolidate in the brain and strengthens the neural pathways so the knowledge is easier to find later. Reversibility A characteristic of Piagetian logical operations—the ability to think through a series of steps and then mentally reverse the steps and return to the starting point; also called reversible thinking. Reversible thinking Thinking backward, from the end to the beginning. Ripple effect

“Contagious” spreading of behaviors through imitation.

Rote memorization Remembering information by repetition without necessarily understanding the meaning of the information. Rules Statements specifying expected and forbidden behaviors; dos and don’ts. Scaffolding The practice of providing students with support for meaning making and independent thinking. Schema-driven problem solving Recognizing a problem as a “disguised” version of an old problem for which one already has a solution. Schemas (singular, schema)

In cognitive theory, basic structures for organizing information; concepts.

Schemes Mental systems or categories of perception and experience. Scoring rubrics

Rules that are used to determine the quality of a student’s performance.

Script Schema, or expected plan, for the sequence of steps in a common event such as buying groceries or ordering pizza. Scripted cooperation Learning strategy in which two students take turns summarizing material and critiquing the summaries. Section 504 A part of civil rights law that prevents discrimination against people with disabilities in programs that receive federal funds, such as public schools. Selected-response testing A form of testing in which students choose the correct response from a set of possible responses provided by the teacher or the test developer instead of creating their own response. Multiple-choice and true-false tests are common examples of selected-response testing. Self-actualization

Fulfilling one’s potential.

G-13

G-14 Glossary Self-concept Individuals’ knowledge and beliefs about themselves—their ideas, feelings, attitudes, and expectations. Self-efficacy A sense of being able to deal effectively with a particular task. Beliefs about your personal capabilities in a particular situation. Self-esteem

The value each of us places on our own characteristics, abilities, and behaviors.

Self-fulfilling prophecy

A groundless expectation that is confirmed because it has been expected.

Self-handicapping Students’ engagement in behavior that blocks their own success in order to avoid testing their true ability. Self-instruction

Talking oneself through the steps of a task.

Self-management Management of your own behavior and acceptance of responsibility for your own actions. Also the use of behavioral learning principles to change your own behavior. Self-regulated learning A view of learning that emphasizes the role of both skill and will when analyzing learning tasks, setting goals, applying skills, maintaining motivation, monitoring progress, and making adjustments about how learning is carried out. Self-regulation Process of activating and sustaining thoughts, behaviors, and emotions in order to reach goals. Self-regulatory knowledge Knowing how to manage your learning, or knowing how and when to use your declarative and procedural knowledge. Self-reinforcement

Controlling (selecting and administering) your own reinforcers.

Semantic memory Semilingual

Memory for meaning.

A lack of proficiency in any language; speaking one or more languages inadequately.

Semiotic function The ability to use symbols—language, pictures, signs, or gestures—to represent actions or objects mentally. Sensitive periods Times when a person is especially ready to learn certain things or is responsive to certain experiences. Sensorimotor

Involving the senses and motor activity.

Sensory memory

System that holds sensory information very briefly.

Serial-position effect The tendency to remember the beginning and the end, but not the middle of a list. Seriation

Arranging objects in sequential order according to one aspect, such as size, weight, or volume.

Sexual orientation A multidimensional construct that refers to people’s sexual identity, sexual attraction, and/or sexual behavior. Shaping

Reinforcing each small step of progress toward a desired goal or behavior.

Shared regulation Students and teachers working together to regulate each other through reminders, prompts, and other guidance. Sheltered instruction Approach to teaching that improves English language skills while teaching content to students who are English learners by putting the words and concepts of the content into context to make the content more understandable. Sheltered Instruction Observation Protocol (SIOP®) of sheltered instruction is present for a teacher. Short-term memory

An observational system to ensure that each element

Component of memory system that holds information for about 20 seconds.

Situated learning The idea that skills and knowledge are tied to the situation in which they were learned and that they are difficult to apply in new settings. Social and emotional learning (SEL) The process of integrating thinking, emotion, and behavior into teaching and learning so that adults and children develop skills to be aware of themselves and others, learn to manage their own and others’ emotions and behavior, make responsible decisions, and build positive relationships. Social cognitive theory Theory of learning and motivation that adds to social learning theory a consideration of cognitive factors such as beliefs, self-perceptions, and expectations. Social conventions Social development Social goals

Agreed-upon rules and ways of doing things in a particular situation. Changes over time in the ways we relate to others.

A wide variety of needs and motives to be connected to others or part of a group.

Social isolation

Removal of a disruptive student for 5 to 10 minutes.

Social learning theory

Theory that emphasizes learning through observation of others.

Social negotiation Aspect of learning process that relies on collaboration with others and respect for different perspectives. Social persuasion Evaluative messages that students receive from people around them that convey information about their capabilities.

Glossary Sociocultural theory Emphasizes role in development of cooperative dialogues between children and more knowledgeable members of society. Children learn the culture of their community (ways of thinking and behaving) through these interactions. Socioeconomic status (SES) prestige.

Relative standing in a society based on income, power, background, and

Sociolinguistics The study of the formal and informal rules for how, when, about what, to whom, and how long to speak in conversations within cultural groups. Source monitoring Deciding if a memory is real and accurate by remembering where you encountered particular information—your own experience, a dream, TV, or someone else’s experience. Spasticity Overly tight or tense muscles, characteristic of some forms of cerebral palsy. Specific learning disability Problem with acquisition and use of language; may show up as difficulty with reading, writing, reasoning, or math. Speech impairment Spermarche

Inability to produce sounds effectively for speaking.

The first sperm ejaculation for boys.

Spiral curriculum Bruner’s design for teaching that introduces the fundamental structure of all subjects early in the school years and then revisits the subjects in more and more complex forms over time. Spreading activation Retrieval of pieces of information based on their relatedness to one another. Remembering one bit of information activates (stimulates) recall of associated information. Standard deviation

Measure of how widely scores vary from the mean.

Standard error of measurement Standard scores

Hypothetical estimate of variation in scores if testing were repeated.

Scores based on the standard deviation.

Standardized tests Tests given, usually nationwide, under uniform conditions and scored according to uniform procedures. Stanine scores Whole-number scores from 1 to 9, each representing a wide range of raw scores. Statistically significant Stem

Not likely to be a chance occurrence.

The question part of a multiple-choice item.

Stereotype category.

A widely held, often oversimplified schema that organizes knowledge or perceptions about a

Stereotype threat The emotional and cognitive burdens that can result from heightened awareness that your performance in an academic situation might confirm a stereotype that others hold about you. Stimulus Event that activates behavior. Stimulus control Capacity for the presence or absence of antecedents to cause behaviors. Story grammar

Typical structure or organization for a category of stories.

Structured English immersion (SEI) An environment that teaches English rapidly by maximizing instruction in English and using English at a level appropriate to the abilities of the students in the class who are English learners. Successive approximations up a complex behavior. Summative assessment

Reinforcing small steps to reach a goal; the small component steps that make

Testing that follows instruction and assesses achievement.

Sustaining expectation effect recognize improvements.

Student performance is maintained at a certain level because teachers don’t

Synapses The tiny space between neurons—chemical messages are sent across these gaps. Synaptic plasticity

See plasticity.

Syntax The order of words in phrases or sentences. T score Standard score with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. Task analysis System for breaking down a task hierarchically into basic skills and subskills. Taxonomy

Classification system.

Teachers’ sense of efficacy Teachers’ beliefs about how capable they are of reaching even the most difficult students and helping them learn. Theory

Integrated statement of principles that attempts to explain a phenomenon and make predictions.

Theory of mind An understanding that other people are people too, with their own minds, thoughts, feelings, beliefs, desires, and perceptions. Theory of multiple intelligences (MI) In Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences, an individual has eight separate abilities: logical-mathematical, linguistic, musical, spatial, bodily-kinesthetic, interpersonal, intrapersonal, and naturalist.

G-15

G-16 Glossary Theory-based An explanation for concept formation that suggests our classifications are based on ideas about the world that we create to make sense of things. Time on task

Time spent actively engaged in the learning task at hand. Also referred to as engaged time.

Time out Technically, the removal of all reinforcement. In practice, isolation of a student from the rest of the class for a brief time. Token reinforcement system System in which tokens earned for academic work and positive classroom behavior can be exchanged for some desired reward. Tonic-clonic seizure:

A generalized seizure that includes uncontrolled movements, twitching, or spasms.

Top-down processing Making sense of information by using context and what we already know about the situation; sometimes called conceptually driven perception. Transfer Influence of previously learned material on new material; the productive (not reproductive) uses of cognitive tools and motivations. Transition programming Gradual preparation of students with special needs to move from high school into further education or training, employment, or community involvement. Trauma-informed educators Teachers who recognize the students’ history of trauma; understand how trauma can affect the students’ emotions, behavior, and learning; and design learning environments that address the needs of the students while avoiding retraumatizing them. Triadic reciprocal causality An explanation of human functioning as the interaction between personal, behavioral, and environmental influences. Triarchic theory of successful intelligence A three-part description of the mental abilities (thinking processes, coping with new experiences, and adapting to context) that lead to more or less intelligent behavior. True score

The score the student would get if the measurement were completely accurate and error-free.

Unconditioned response (UR)

Naturally occurring emotional or physiological response.

Unconditioned stimulus (US) response.

Stimulus that automatically produces an emotional or physiological

Understanding by Design (UbD) A system of lesson and unit planning that starts with key objectives for understandings and then moves backward to design assessments and learning activities. Universal design websites.

Considering the needs of all users in the design of new tools, learning programs, or

Universal design for learning (UDL) Strategies and structures to make learning accessible to as many students as possible in inclusive settings. Utility value

The usefulness of a task for meeting your goals.

Validity Degree to which a test measures what it is intended to measure. To be valid, the decisions and inferences based on the test must be supported by evidence. Value An individual’s belief about the extent to which a task or assignment is generally useful, enjoyable, or otherwise important. Value-added modeling (VAM) Approaches that use statistical analyses to indicate the average test score gain for students, adjusted for their student characteristics such as prior level of achievement. Variability

Degree of difference or deviation from mean.

Verbalization

Putting your problem-solving plan and its logic into words.

Vicarious experiences

Accomplishments that are modeled by someone else.

Vicarious reinforcement Increasing the chances that we will repeat a behavior by observing another person being reinforced for that behavior. Virtual learning environments (VLEs) ways. Visuo-spatial sketchpad information. Voicing problems

Technology-based platforms that allow learning in a variety of

Part of working memory. A holding system for visual, tactile, and spatial

Inappropriate pitch, quality, loudness, or intonation.

Volition Willpower; an internally driven, intentional decision to reach valued goals by applying selfregulated learning. Warm demanders Teachers who are especially effective with Black students; they show both high expectations and great caring for their students. Webb’s Depth of Knowledge (DOK) A taxonomy system similar to Bloom’s cognitive domain in that it moves from lower- to higher-level thinking; often is used in planning curriculum and assessments around the Common Core State Standards.

Glossary What Works Clearinghouse A service of the U.S. Institute of Education Sciences that reviews the existing research on different programs, products, practices, and policies in education in order to provide educators with the information they need to make evidence-based decisions. Within-class ability grouping System of grouping in which students in a class are divided into two or three groups based on ability in an attempt to accommodate student differences. Withitness According to Kounin, awareness of everything happening in a classroom. Work-avoidant learners

Students who don’t want to learn or to look smart, but just want to avoid work.

Working memory The brain system that provides temporary holding and processing of information to accomplish complex cognitive tasks such as language comprehension, learning, and reasoning; the information that you are focusing on at a given moment. Working-backward strategy problem.

Heuristic in which you start with the goal and move backward to solve the

z score Standard score indicating the number of standard deviations above or below the mean that a particular raw score falls. Zero reject A basic principle of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act specifying that no student with a disability, no matter what kind or how severe, can be denied a free public education. Zone of proximal development (ZPD) Developmental phase at which a child can master a task if given appropriate help and support from a more capable person.

G-17

This page intentionally left blank

References Aamodt, S., & Wang, S. (2008). Welcome to your brain: Why you lose your car keys but never forget how to drive and other puzzles of everyday life. Bloomsbury. Aber, J. L., Brown, J. L., & Jones, S. M. (2003). Developmental trajectories toward violence in middle childhood: Course, demographic differences, and response to school-based intervention. Developmental Psychology, 39, 324–348. Aboud, F. E., Tredoux, C., Tropp, L. R., Brown, C. S., Niens, U., Noor, N. M., & the Una Global Evaluation Group. (2012). Interventions to reduce prejudice and enhance inclusion and respect for ethnic differences in early childhood: A systematic review. Developmental Review, 32, 307–336. Abrami, P. C., Bernard, R. M., Borokhovski, E.,Waddington, D. I., Wade, C. A., & Persson, T. (2015). Strategies for teaching students to think critically: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 85, 275–314. Abrams, L. M., & Madaus, G. F. (2003). The lessons of high stakes testing. Educational Leadership, 61(32), 31–35. Abuhamdeh, S., & Csikzentmihalyi, M. (2012). Attentional involvement and intrinsic motivation. Motivation & Emotion, 36(3), 257–267. Ackerman, B. P., Brown, E. D., & Izard, C. E. (2004). The relations between contextual risk, earned income, and the school adjustment of children from economically disadvantaged families. Developmental Psychology, 40, 204–216. Adachi, P. J. C., & Willoughby, T. (2017). The link between playing video games and positive youth outcomes. Child Development Perspectives, 11(3), 202–206. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12232 ADL. (2019). Beyond the binary: Discussing transgender and gender non-conforming identity in K–12 schools. https://www.adl.org/media/4768/download Aelterman, N., Vansteenkiste, M., Haerens, L., Soenens, B., Fontaine, J. R. J., & Reeve, J. (2019). Toward an integrative and fine-grained insight in motivating and demotivating teaching styles: The merits of a circumplex approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(3), 497–521. https://doi.org/10.1037 /edu0000293 Agarwal, P. K. (2019). Retrieval practice & Bloom’s taxonomy: Do students need fact knowledge before higher order learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(2), 189–209. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000282 Agarwal, P. K. (2020). Retrieval practice: A powerful tool for lasting learning. Educational Leadership, 20(8), 76–81. Agarwal, P. K., Bain, P. M., & Chamberlain, R. W. (2012). The value of applied research: Retrieval practice improves classroom learning and recommendations from a teacher, a principal, and a scientist. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 437–448. Agulló, J. A., & Herrero, E. C. (2019). Bilingual education research: A bibliometric study. Estudios de Lingüística Inglesa Aaplicada, 235–270. Ahn, H. S., Usher, E. L., Butz, A. R., & Bong, M. (2016). Cultural differences in the understanding of modelling and feedback as sources of self-efficacy information. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 112–136. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12093 Ahtone, T. (2017). How “Rez Accents” strengthen Native American identity. Yes! Magazine. https://www.yesmagazine.org/democracy/2017/03/06/how-rez-accents-strengthen-native-identity/ Ainley, M. (2019). Curiosity and interest: Emergence and divergence. Educational Psychology Review, 31(4), 789–806. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09495-z Akhtar, S., Justice, L. V., Morrison, C. M., & Conway, M. A. (2018). Fictional first memories. Psychological Science, 29(10), 1612–1619. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618778831 Alarcon, G. M., & Edwards, J. M. (2013). Ability and motivation: Assessing individual factors that contribute to university retention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 129–137. Alberto, P. A., & Troutman, A. C. (2017). Applied behavior analysis for teachers (interactive 9th ed.). Pearson. Albrecht, D. (2021). The journey from traditional parent involvement to an alliance for empowerment: A paradigm shift. Theory Into Practice, 60, 7–17. Aldrup, K., Klusmann, U., Lüdtke, O., Göllner, R., & Trautwein, U. (2018). Social support and classroom management are related to secondary students’ general school adjustment: A multilevel structural equation model using student and teacher ratings. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 1066 –1083. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000256. Alexander, P. A. (2018). Past as prologue: Educational psychology’s legacy and progeny. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 147–162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000200 Alexander, P. A., Kulikowich, J. M., & Schulze, S. K. (1994). How subject-matter knowledge affects recall and interest. American Educational Research Journal, 31, 313–337. Alexander, P. A., Schallert, D. L., & Reynolds, R. E. (2009). What is learning anyway? A topographical perspective considered. Educational Psychologist, 44, 176–192. Alexander, P. A., & Winne, P. H. (2006). Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed.). Erlbaum. Alferink, L. A., & Farmer-Dougan, V. (2010). Brain-(not) based education: Dangers of misunderstanding and misapplication of neuroscience research. Exceptionality, 18, 42–52. Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2011). Does discovery-based instruction enhance learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 1–18.

R-1

R-2 References Allen, J., Gregory, A., Mikami, A., Lun, J., Hamre, B., & Pianta, R. (2013). Observations of effective teacher-student interactions in secondary school classrooms: Predicting student achievement with the Classroom Assessment Scoring System—Secondary. School Psychology Review, 42, 76–98. Allen, K., Kern, M. L., Vella-Brodrick, D., Hattie, J., & Waters, L. (2018). What schools need to know about fostering school belonging: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 30(1), 1–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-016-9389-8 Allington, R. L., & McGill-Frazen, A. (2003). The impact of summer setback on the reading achievement gap. Phi Delta Kappan, 85(1), 68–75. Allington, R. L., & McGill-Frazen, A. (2008). Got books? Educational Leadership, 65(7), 20–23. Alloway, T. P. (Ed.). (2018). Working memory and clinical developmental disorders: Theories, debates, and interventions. Routledge. Alloway, T. P., Banner, G. E., & Smith, P. (2010). Working memory and cognitive styles in adolescents’ attainment. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 567–581. Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2006). Verbal and visuo-spatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77, 1698–1716. Alloy, L. B., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1979). On the cognitive component of learned helplessness and depression. The Journal of Learning and Motivation, 13, 219–276. Al-Namlah, A. S., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2012). Self-regulatory private speech relates to children’s recall and organization of autobiographical memories. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(3), 441–446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.02.005 Aloe, A. M., Amo, L. C., & Shanahan, M. E. (2014). Classroom management self-efficacy and burnout: A multivariate meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 101–126. Aloe, A. M., & Becker, B. J. (2009). Teacher verbal ability and school outcomes: Where is the evidence? Educational Researcher, 38, 612–624. Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Westview Press. Amabile, T. M. (2001). Beyond talent: John Irving and the passionate craft of creativity. American Psychologist, 56, 333–336. Ambrose, D., & Sternberg, R. (Eds.). (2012). How dogmatic beliefs harm creativity and higher-level thinking. Taylor and Francis. Amemiya, J., Mortenson, E., & Wang, M. T. (2020). Minor infractions are not minor: School infractions for minor misconduct may increase adolescents' defiant behavior and contribute to racial disparities in school discipline. The American Psychologist, 75(1), 23–36. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000475 American Educational Research Association Council. (2015). AERA statement on use of value-added models (VAM) for the evaluation of educators and educator preparation programs. Author. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.) DSM-V. Author. American Psychological Association. (2015). Top 20 principles from psychology for Pre-K–12 teaching and learning. American Psychological Association: Coalition for Psychology in Schools and Education. Author. American Psychological Association. (2019). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (7th edition). APA. American Psychological Association, Presidential Task Force on Educational Disparities. (2012). Ethnic and racial disparities in education: Psychology’s contributions to understanding and reducing disparities. http://www.apa.org/ed/resources/racial-disparities.aspx American Psychological Association, Zero Tolerance Task Force. (2008). Are Zero Tolerance policies effective in the schools? An evidentiary review and recommendations. American Psychologist, 63, 852–862. doi: 10.1037/0003–066X.63.9.852 Anagnostopoulos, D., Cavanna, J., & Charles-Harris, S. (2020). Managing to teach ambitiously in the first year? The Elementary School Journal, 120(4). https://doi.org/10.1086/708660 Anagnostopoulos, D., Wilson, S. M., & Charles-Harris, S. (2021). Contesting quality teaching: Teachers’ pragmatic agency and the debate about teacher evaluation. Teaching and Teacher Education, 98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2020.103246 Anderman, E. (2011). Educational psychology in the twenty-first century: Challenges for our community. Educational Psychologist, 46, 185–196. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2011.587724 Anderman, E. M. (2015). Students cheat for good grades. Why not make the classroom about learning and not testing? The Conversation. Anderman, E. M. (2020). Achievement motivation theory: Balancing precision and utility. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101864 Anderman, E. M. (2021). Sparking student motivation: The power of teachers to rekindle a love of learning. Corwin. Anderman, E. M., & Anderman, L. H. (2014). Motivating children and adolescents in schools. Pearson. Anderman, E. M., Cupp, P. K., & Lane, D. (2009). Impulsivity and academic cheating. Journal of Experimental Education, 78, 135–150. Anderman, E. M., & Midgley, C. (2004). Changes in self-reported academic cheating across the transition from middle school to high school. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 499–517. Anderman, E. M., & Patrick, H. (2012). Achievement goal theory, conceptualization of ability/intelligence, and classroom climate. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), The handbook of research on student engagement. Springer Science. Anderson, A. R., Christenson, S. L., Sinclair, M. F., & Lehr, C. A. (2004). Check & connect: The importance of relationships for promoting engagement with school. Journal of School Psychology, 42, 95–113.

References Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, Al, Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., Rothstein, H. R., & Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in eastern and western countries: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 151–173. Anderson, C. W., Holland, J. D., & Palincsar, A. S. (1997). Canonical and sociocultural approaches to research and reform in science education: The story of Juan and his group. The Elementary School Journal, 97, 359–384. Anderson, J. R. (2020). Cognitive psychology and its implications (9th ed.). Worth Publishers. Anderson, J. R., Reder, L. M., & Simon, H. A. (1996). Situated learning and education. Educational Researcher, 25(4), 5–11. Anderson, L. W., & Krathwohl, D. R. (Eds.). (2001). A taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing: A revision of Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives. Longman. Anderson, L. W., & Sosniak, L. A. (Eds.). (1994). Bloom’s taxonomy: A forty-year retrospective. Ninety-third yearbook for the National Society for the Study of Education: Part II. University of Chicago Press. Anderson, M. D. (2015, December). The resurgence of single-sex education. The Atlantic. http://www.theatlantic .com/education/archive/2015/12/the-resurgence-of-single-sex-education/421560/ Anderson, R. C., Graham, M., Kennedy, P., Nelson, N., Stoolmiller, M., Baker, S. K., & Fien, H. (2019). Student agency at the crux: Mitigating disengagement in middle and high school. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 56, 205–217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.12.005 Anderson, R. C., Nguyen-Jahiel, K., McNurlen, B., Archodidou, A., Kim, S.-Y., Reznitskaya, A., Tillmanns, M., & Gilbert, L. (2001). The snowball phenomenon: Spread of ways of talking and ways of thinking across groups of children. Cognition and Instruction, 19, 1–46. Anderson, S. R. (2010). How many languages are there in the world? Linguistic Society of America. Andrä, C., Mathias, B., Schwager, A., Macedonia, M., & von Kriegstein, K. (2020). Learning foreign language vocabulary with gestures and pictures enhances vocabulary memory for several months post-learning in eight-year-old school children. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 815–850. https://doi.org/10.1007 /s10648-020-09527-z Andrew, M. (2014). The scarring effects of primary-grade retention? A study of cumulative advantage in the educational career. Social Forces, 93, 653–685. Anfara Jr., V. A., Evans, K. R., & Lester, N. L. (2013) Restorative justice in education: What we know so far, Middle School Journal, 44, 57–63. https://doi.org/10.1080/00940771.2013.11461873 Anmarkrud, Ø., Andresen, A., & Bråten, I. (2019). Cognitive load and working memory in multimedia learning: Conceptual and measurement issues. Educational Psychologist, 54(2), 61–83. https://doi.org /10.1080/00461520.2018.1554484 Annenberg Foundation. (2017). Annenberg learner. Neuroscience and the learner:Making connections [An online course for K–12 teachers]. https://www.learner.org/courses/neuroscience/ Ansari, A., Hofkens, T. L., & Pianta, R. C. (2020). Teacher-student relationships across the first seven years for education and adolescent outcomes. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 71, 101200. Ansary, N. S., Elias, M. J., Greene, M. B., & Green, S. (2015). Guidance for schools selecting antibullying approaches: Translating evidence-based strategies to contemporary implementation realities. Educational Researcher, 44, 27–36. Anyon, J. (2012). Schools and poverty, Teachers College Record, 114, 1–3. ID Number: 16696. Anzures, G., Kelly, D. J., Pascalis, O., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Viviés, X. D., & Lee, K. (2013). Own- and other-race face identity recognition in children: The effects of pose and feature composition. Developmental Psychology. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0033166 APA. (2019, May 30). Belief in learning styles myth may be detrimental. ScienceDaily. Retrieved February 17, 2021, from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/05/190530101133.htm Appel, M., & Kronberger, N. (2012). Stereotypes and the achievement gap: Stereotype threat prior to test taking. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 609–635. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9200-4 Applebee, A. N., Langer, J. A., Nystrand, M., & Gamoran, A. (2003). Discussion-based approaches to developing understanding: Classroom instruction and student performance in middle and high school English. American Educational Research Journal, 40, 685–730. https://doi.org /10.3102/00028312040003685 Archer, S. L., & Waterman, A. S. (1990). Varieties of identity diffusions and foreclosures: An exploration of the subcategories of the identity statuses. Journal of Adolescent Research, 5, 96–111. Arena, D. (2015). Videogames as tillers of the soil. Theory Into Practice, 54, 94–100. Arends, R. I. (2012). Learning to teach (9th ed.). McGraw-Hill. Arends, R. I. (2014). Learning to teach (10th ed.). McGraw-Hill. Arends, R. I., & Kilcher, A. (2010). Teaching for student learning: Becoming an accomplished teacher. Routledge. Arens, A. K., Yeung, A. S., Craven, R. G., & Hasselhorn, M. (2011, August 22). The twofold multidimensionality of academic self-concept: Domain specificity and separation between competence and affect components. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 970–981. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025047 Armstrong, K. (2018, July/August). Convention coverage of Daniel Willingham’s presentation: Preparing teachers for the unexpected. Observer Magazine. https://www.psychologicalscience.org/observer /preparing-teachers-for-the-unexpected Arnett, J. J. (2015). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens through the twenties (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press. Arnold, M. L. (2000). Stage, sequence, and sequels: Changing conceptions of morality, post-Kohlberg. Educational Psychology Review, 12, 365–383.

R-3

R-4 References Aronson, E. (2000). Nobody left to hate: Teaching compassion after Columbine. Worth. Aronson, J. (2002). Stereotype threat: Contending and coping with unnerving expectations. In J. Aronson & D. Cordova (Eds.), Improving education: Classic and contemporary lessons from psychology (pp. 279–301). Academic Press. Artelt, C., & Schneider, W. (2015). Cross-country generalizability of the role of metacognitive knowledge in students’ strategy use and reading competence. Teachers College Record, 117(1). Artiles, A. J. (2019). Fourteenth Annual Brown Lecture in Education Research: Reenvisioning equity research: Disability identification disparities as a case in point. Educational Researcher, 48(6), 325–335. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X19871949 Ashman, G., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2020). Problem-solving or explicit instruction: Which should go first when element interactivity is high? Educational Psychology Review, 32, 229–247. Assouline, S. G., Colangelo, N., VanTassel-Baska, J., & Lupkowski-Shoplik, A. (2015). A nation empowered: Evidence trumps the excuses holding back America’s brightest students (Vol. 2). University of Iowa, Connie Belin & Jacqueline N. Blank International Center for Gifted Education and Talent Development. Asterhan, C. S. C., & Babichenko, M. (2015). The social dimension of learning through argumentation: Effects of human presence and discourse style. Journal of Educational Psycology, 107, 740–755. Asterhan, C. S. C., & Schwarz, B. B. (2016). Argumentation for learning: Well-trodden paths and unexplored territories, Educational Psychologist, 51, 164–187. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2016.1155458 Asterhan, C. S. C., Schwarz, B. B., & Cohen-Eliyan, N. (2014). Outcome feedback during collaborative learning: Contingencies between feedback and dyad composition. Learning and Instruction, 34, 1–10. Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. Spence & J. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). Academic Press. Atteberry, A., & Mangan, D. (2020). The sensitivity of teacher value-added scores to the use of fall or spring test scores. Educational Researcher, 49(5), 335–349. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X20922993 Au, T. K., Knightly, L. M., Jun, S., & Oh, J. S. (2002). Overhearing a language during childhood. Psychological Science, 13, 238–243. Autin, F., Batruch, A., & Butera, F. (2019). The function of selection of assessment leads evaluators to artificially create the social class achievement gap. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(4), 717–735. https:// doi.org/10.1037/edu0000307 Auxier, B., Anderson, M., Perrin, A., & Turner, E. (2020). Parenting children in the age of screens. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2020/07/28/parenting-children-in-the-age-of-screens/ Avramidis, E., Bayliss, P., & Burden, R. (2000). Student teachers’ attitudes toward the inclusion of children with special education needs in the ordinary school. Teaching and Teacher Education, 16, 277–293. Ayalon, M., & Wilkie, K. J. (2020). Developing assessment literacy through approximations of practice: Exploring secondary mathematics pre-service teachers developing criteria for a rich quadratics task. Teaching and Teacher Education, 89, 103011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2019.103011 Azevedo, R., Taub, M., & Mudrick, N. V. (2018). Understanding and reasoning about real-time cognitive, affective, and metacognitive processes to foster self-regulation with advanced learning technologies. In D. H. Schunk, & J. A. Greene (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (2nd ed., pp. 254–270). Routledge. Babad, E. Y., Inbar, J., & Rosenthal, R. (1982). Pygmalion, Galatea, and the Golem: Investigations of biased and unbiased teachers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 459–474. Baddeley, A. D. (2001). Is working memory still working? American Psychologist, 56, 851–864. Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford University Press. Baddeley, A. D. (2019).Working memories: Postmen, divers and the cognitive revolution. Routledge. Baddeley, A. D., Eysenck, M. W., & Anderson, M. C. (2015).Memory (2nd ed.). Psychology Press. Bae, C. L., Therriault, D. J., & Redifer, J. L. (2019). Investigating the testing effect: Retrieval as a characteristic of effective study strategies. Learning and Instruction, 60, 206–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .learninstruc.2017.12.008 Baek, Y., Min, E., & Yun, S. (2020). Mining educational implications of Minecraft. Computers in the Schools, 37(1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/07380569.2020.1719802 Bagley, E., & Shaffer, D. W. (2009). When people get in the way: Promoting civic thinking through epistemic gameplay. International Journal of Gaming and Computer-Mediated Simulations, 1, 36–52. Bahrick, H. P. (2000). Long-term maintenance of knowledge. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 347–362). Oxford University Press. Bailey, U. L., Lorch, E. P., Milich, R., & Charnigo, R. (2009). Developmental changes in attention and comprehension among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Child Development, 80, 1842–1855. Bailey, J. M., Vasey, P. L., Diamond, L. M., Breedlove, S. M., Vilain, E., & Epprecht, M. (2016). Sexual orientation, controversy, and science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17(2), 45–101. https://doi. org/10.1177/1529100616637616 Baker, K. (1998). Structured English immersion breakthrough in teaching limited-English-proficient students. Phi Delta Kappan, 80(3), 199–204. http://pdkintl.org/kappan/kbak9811.htm Balass, M., Nelson, J. R., & Perfetti, C. A. (2010). Word learning: An ERP investigation of word experience effects on recognition and word processing. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 126–140. Baldwin, J. M. (1895). Mental development in the child and the race: Methods and processes. Macmillan. Bandura, A. (1965). Influence of models’ reinforcement contingencies on the acquisition of imitative responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 589–595. Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanisms in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122–147. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Prentice-Hall.

References Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and functioning. Educational Psychologist, 28, 117–148. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. Freeman. Bandura, A. (2007). Albert Bandura. In L. Gardner & W. M. Runyan (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. IX, pp. 43–75). American Psychological Association. Bandura, A. (2016). Moral disengagement: How people do harm and live with themselves. Worth. Bandura, A. (2018). Toward a psychology of human agency: Pathways and reflections. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 130–136. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617699280 Bandura, A. (2019). Applying theory for human betterment. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(1), 12–15. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618815165 Bandura, A., & Cherry, L. (2020). Enlisting the power of youth for climate change. American Psychologist, 75(7), 945–951. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000512 Bandura, A., & Locke, E. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 87–99. Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1963). Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 3–11. Banks, J. A. (2014). An introduction to multicultural education (5th ed.). Pearson. Banks, J. A. (2015). Cultural diversity and education: Foundations, curriculum, and teaching (6th ed.). Routledge. Banks, J. A. (2019). An introduction to multicultural education (6th ed.). Pearson. Banks, J. A., & Banks, C. A. M. (Eds.). (2016). Multicultural education: Issues and perspectives (9th ed.). Wiley. Banks, S. R. (2012). Classroom assessment: Issues and practice (2nd ed.). Pearson. Bardach, L., Oczlon, S., Pietschnig, J., & Lüftenegger, M. (2020). Has achievement goal theory been right? A meta-analysis of the relation between goal structures and personal achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(6), 1197–1220. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000419 Barkley, R. A. (Ed.). (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed., pp. 547–588). Guilford. Barnard-Brak, L. (2008). Academic red-shirting among children with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities: A Contemporary Journal, 6, 43–54. Barnett, M. S., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 612–637. Baroody, A. J., Eiland, M. D., Purpura, D. J., & Reid, E. E. (2013). Can computer-assisted discovery learning foster first graders’ fluency with the most basic addition combinations? American Educational Research Journal, 50, 533–573. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831212473349 Barrett, L. F. (2017). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. Barrett, L. F. (2020). Seven and a half lessons about the brain. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. Barros, E., Silver, J., & Stein, R. E. K. (2009). School recess and group classroom behavior. Pediatrics, 123, 431–436. Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., Mouratidis, A., Katartzi, E., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., & Vlachopoulos, S. (2018). Beware of your teaching style: A school-year long investigation of controlling teaching and student motivational experiences. Learning and Instruction, 53, 50–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.07.006 Barzilai, S., & Eilam, B. (2018). Learners’ epistemic criteria and strategies for evaluating scientific visual representations. Learning and Instruction, 58, 137–147. Barzilai, S., & Zohar, A. (2014). Reconsidering personal epistemology as metacognition: A multifaceted approach to the analysis of epistemic thinking, Educational Psychologist, 49, 13–35. https://doi.org/10.1080 /00461520.2013.863265 Barzilai, S., Zohar, A. R., & Mor-Hagani, S. (2018). Promoting integration of multiple texts: A review of instructional approaches and practices. Educational Psychology Review, 30, 973–999. https://doi.org /10.1007/s10648-018-9436-8 Bassok, D., & Reardon, S. F. (2013). “Academic Redshirting” in kindergarten: Prevalence, patterns, and implications. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 35(3), 283–297. https://doi.org /10.3102/0162373713482764 Batalova, J., Hanna, M., & Levesque, C. (2021, February). Frequently requested statistics on immigrants and immigration in the United States. Immigration Policy Institute. https://www.migrationpolicy.org/article /frequently-requested-statistics-immigrants-and-immigration-united-states Bauer, L., Broady, K., Edelberg, W., & O’Donnell, J. (2020, September). Ten facts about COVID-19 and the U.S. economy. The Hamilton Project. The Brookings Institution. https://www.brookings.edu/research /ten-facts-about-covid-19-and-the-u-s-economy/ Bauer, P. J. (2006). Event memory. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Cognition, perception, and language (6th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 373–425). Wiley. Baumeister, R. F. (2005). Self-concept, self-esteem, and identity. In V. Derlega, B. Winstead, & W. Jones (Eds.), Personality: Contemporary theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 246–280). Wadsworth. Baumeister, R. F., & Bargh, J. A. (2014). Conscious and unconscious: Toward an integrative understanding of human mental life and action. In J. W. Sherman, B. Gawronski, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories of the social mind (pp. 35–49). Guilford Press. Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. L., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4, 1–44. Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. Baumert, J., Kunter, M., Blum, W., Brunner, M., Voss, T., Jordan, A., Klusmann, U., Krauss, S., Neubrand, M., & Tsai, Y.-M. (2010). Teachers’ mathematical knowledge, cognitive activation in the classroom, and student progress. American Educational Research Journal, 47(1), 133–180.

R-5

R-6 References Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology, 4, 1–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0030372 Baumrind, D. (1996). The discipline controversy revisited. Family Relations, 45, 405–414. Baumrind, D. (2005). Patterns of parental authority and adolescent authority. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 108, 61–69. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.128 Baumrind, D., Larzelere, R. E., & Owens, E. B. (2010). Effects of preschool parents’ power assertive patterns and practices on adolescent development. Parenting: Science and Practice, 10, 157–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15295190903290790 Bayliss, D. M., Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., Gunn, D., & Leigh, E. (2005). Mapping the developmental constraints on working memory span performance. Developmental Psychology, 41, 579–597. Beane, J. A. (1991). Sorting out the self-esteem controversy. Educational Leadership, 49(1), 25–30. Bear, G. G. (2005). Developing self-discipline and preventing and correcting misbehavior (with Cavalier, A. R., & Manning, M. A.). Allyn & Bacon. Beard, K. S. (2015). Theoretically speaking: An interview with Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi on Flow Theory development and its usefulness in addressing contemporary challenges in education. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 353–364. Beauchamp, C., & Beauchamp, M. H. (2013). Boundary as bridge: An analysis of the educational neuroscience literature from a boundary perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 25, 47–67. Becker, M., Neumann, M., Tetzner, J., Böse, S., Knoppick, H., Maaz, K., Baumert, J., & Lehmann, R. (2014). Is early ability grouping good for high-achieving students’ psychosocial development? Effects of the transition into academically selective schools. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 555–568. Beebe-Frankenberger, M., Bocian, K. L., MacMillan, D. L., & Gresham, F. M. (2004). Sorting second grade students with academic deficiencies: Characteristics differentiating those retained in grade from those promoted to third grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 204–215. Beike, S. M., & Zentall, S. S. (2012). “The snake raised its head”: Content novelty alters the reading performance of students at risk for reading disabilities and ADHD. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 529–540. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027216 Beilock, S. L., Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., & Levine, S. C. (2010). Female teachers’ math anxiety affects girls’ math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(5), 1860–3. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0910967107 Belland, B. R. (2011). Distributed cognition as a lens to understand the effects of scaffolds: The role of transfer of responsibility. Educational Psychology Review, 23, 577–600. Belland, B. R. (2014). Scaffolding: Definition, current debates, and future directions. In J. M. Spector, M. D. Merrill, J. Elen, & M. J. Bishop (Eds.), Handbook of research on educational communications and technology (4th ed., pp. 505–518). Springer. Belland, B. R. (2017). Instructional scaffolding in STEM education: Strategies and efficacy evidence. Springer OPEN. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-02565-0 Belland, B. R., Kim, C.-M., & Hannafin, M. J. (2013). A framework for designing scaffolds that improve motivation and cognition. Educational Psychologist, 48, 243–270. Belland, B. R., Walker, A. E., Kim, N. J., & Lefler, M. (2017). Synthesizing results from empirical research on computer-based scaffolding in STEM Education: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 87(2), 309–344. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654316670999 Bembenutty, H., Schunk, D. H., & DiBenedetto, M. K. (2021). Applications of motivation research to practice. Theory Into Practice. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/00405841.2021.1929000 Ben-Eliyahu, A. (2019). Academic emotional learning: A critical component of self-regulated learning in the emotional learning cycle. Educational Psychologist, 54(2), 84–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2019. 1582345 Benita, M., Roth, G., & Deci, E. L. (2014). When are mastery goals more adaptive? It depends on experiences of autonomy support and autonomy. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 258–267. Benner, A. D., & Graham, S. (2013). The antecedents and consequences of racial/ethnic discrimination during adolescence: Does the source of discrimination matter? Developmental Psychology, 49(8), 1602–1613. Benner, A. D., & Mistry, R. S. (2020). Child development during the COVID-19 pandemic through a life course theory lens. Child Development Perspectives, 14(4), 236–243. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12387 Bennett, C. I. (2011). Comprehensive multicultural education: Theory and practice (7th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Bereiter, C. (1995). A dispositional view of transfer. In A. McKeough, J. Lupart, & A. Marini (Eds.), Teaching for mastery: Fostering generalization in learning (pp. 21–34). Erlbaum. Bereiter, C. (1997). Situated cognition and how I overcome it. In D. Kirshner & J. A. Whitson (Eds.), Situated cognition: Social, semiotic, and psychological perspectives (pp. 281–300). Erlbaum. Berg, C. A., & Clough, M. (1991). Hunter lesson design: The wrong one for science teaching. Educational Leadership, 48(4), 73–78. Berger, K. S. (2015). The developing person through childhood and adolescence (10th ed.). Worth. Berger, K. S. (2018). The developing person: Childhood through adolescence (11th ed.). Worth Publishers. Berger, K. S. (2020). The developing person through the lifespan (11th ed.). Worth Publishers. Bergin, D. A. (2016). Social influences on interest. Educational Psychologist, 51(1), 7–22. https://doi.org/10.1080 /00461520.2015.1133306 Bergmann, J., & Sams, A. (2012). Flip your classroom: Reach every student in every class every day. International Society for Technology in Education. Berk, L. E. (2019). Exploring child and adolescent development. Pearson Berk, L. E., & Spuhl, S. T. (1995). Maternal interaction, private speech, and task performance in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 145–169.

References Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150–177. Berliner, D. C. (1987). But do they understand? In V. Richardson-Koehler (Ed.), Educators’ handbook: A research perspective (pp. 259–293). Longman. Berliner, D. C. (2002). Educational research: The hardest science of all. Educational Researcher, 31(8), 18–20. Berliner, D. C. (2006). Educational psychology: Searching for essence throughout a century of influence. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 3–27). Erlbaum. Berliner, D. C. (2013). Inequality, poverty, and the socialization of America's youth for the responsibilities of citizenship. Theory Into Practice, 52, 203–209. Berliner, D. C., & Glass, G. V. (2014). Myths and lies that threaten America’s public schools: The real crisis in education. Teachers College Press. Bernacki, M. L., Crompton, H., & Greene, J. A. (2020). Towards convergence of mobile and psychological theories of learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101828 Bernacki, M. L., Vosicka, L., & Utz, J. C. (2020). Can a brief, digital skill training intervention help undergraduates “learn to learn” and improve their STEM achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(4), 765–781. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000405 Berry, R. Q., III. (2005). Voices of success: Descriptive portraits of two successful African American male middle school mathematics students. Journal of African American Studies, 8(4), 46–62. Berthold, K., & Renkl, A. (2009). Instructional aids to support a conceptual understanding of multiple representations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 70–87. Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive functioning. Child Development, 81, 1641–1660. Bialystok, E. (2018). Bilingual education for young children: Review of the effects and consequences. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 21, 666–679. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2016 .1203859 Bichler, S., Schwaighofer, M., Stadler, M., Bühner, M., Greiff, S., & Fischer, F. (2020). How working memory capacity and shifting matter for learning with worked examples—A replication study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(7), 1320–1337. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000433 Bigler, R. S., Hayes, A. R., & Liben, L. S. (2014). Analysis and evaluation of the rationales for single-sex schooling. In L. S. Liben, R. S. Bigler, & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior: The role of gender in educational contexts and outcomes (pp. 225–260). Elsevier. Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 162–166. https:// doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00496.x Birgili, B., Seggie, F. N., & Oğuz, E. (2021). The trends and outcomes of flipped learning research between 2012 and 2018: A descriptive content analysis. Journal of Computers in Education, 4. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s40692-021-00183-y Bishop, R., Ladwig, J., & Berryman, M. (2014). The centrality of relationships for pedagogy: The Whanaungatanga thesis. American Educational Research Journal, 51, 184–214. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0002831213510019 Bisra, K., Liu, Q., Nesbit, J. C., Salimi, F., & Winne, P. H. (2018). Inducing self-explanation: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 30, 703–725. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-018-9434-x Bjorklund, D. E. (2012). Children’s thinking (5th ed.). Wadsworth Cengage Learning. Bjorklund, D. F. (2018). A metatheory for cognitive development (or “Piaget is dead” revisited). Child Development, 89, 2288–2302. https://doi.org/10.1111/CDEV.13019 Bjorklund, D. F., Brown, R. D., & Bjorklund, B. R. (2002). Children’s eyewitness memory: Changing reports and changing representations. In P. Graf & N. Ohta (Eds.), Lifespan memory development (pp. 101–126). MIT Press. Blachman, B. A., Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Murray, M. S., Munger, K. A., & Vaughn, M. G. (2014). Intensive reading remediation in grade 2 or 3: Are there effects a decade later? Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 46–57. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033663 Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57, 111–127. Blair, C. (2006). How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence? A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognition. Main article with commentaries. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 109–160. Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological approach. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 711–731. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev -psych-010814-015221 Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2016). Poverty, stress, and brain development: New directions for prevention and intervention. Academic Pediatrics, 16, S30–S36. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.acap.2016.01.010 Blazar, D., Litke, E., & Barmore, J. (2016). What does it mean to be ranked a “high” or “low” value-added teacher? Observing differences in instructional quality across districts. American Educational Research Journal, 53, 324–359. Blazier, C. (2017). Building the future: Children and the sustainable development goals in rich countries. UNICEF Innocenti Report Card 14. https://www.unicef-irc.org/publications/890-building-the-future-children -and-the-sustainable-development-goals-in-rich-countries.html Bloom, B. S. (1982). The role of gifts and markers in the development of talent. Exceptional Children, 48, 510–522. Bloom, B. S., Engelhart, M. D., Frost, E. J., Hill, W. H., & Krathwohl, D. R. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives. Handbook I: Cognitive domain. David McKay.

R-7

R-8 References Bloom, B. S., Sosniak, L. A., Sloane, K. D., Kalinowski, A. G., Gustin, W. C., & Monsaas, J. A. (1985). Developing talent in young people. Ballantine Books. Bloom, P. (2002). How children learn the meanings of words. MIT Press. Blume, F., Göllner, R., Moeller, K., Dresler, T., Ehlis, A.-C., & Gawrilow, C. (2019). Do students learn better when seated close to the teacher? A virtual classroom study considering individual levels of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity. Learning and Instruction, 61, 138–147. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.learninstruc.2018.10.004 Blunt, J. R., & Karpicke, J. D. (2014). Learning with retrieval-based concept mapping. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 849–858. Bobbitt Nolen, S. (2020). Challenging research norms in educational psychology. Educational Psychologist, 55, 267–272. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2020.1810043 Boden, K. K., Zepeda, C. D., & Nokes-Malach, T. J. (2020). Achievement goals and conceptual learning: An examination of teacher talk. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(6), 1221–1242. http://dx.doi .org/10.1037/edu0000421 Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2012). Tools of the mind: Vygotskian approach to early childhood education. In J. L. Rooparine & J. Jones, Approaches to early childhood education (6th ed., pp. 241–260). Pearson. Bohn-Gettler, C. M., & Rapp, D. N. (2011). Depending on my mood: Mood-driven influences on text comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 562–577. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023458 Boivin, M., Brendgen, M., Viraro, F., Dionne, G., Girard, A., Perusse, D., & Tremblay, R. E. (2013). Strong genetic contribution to peer relationship difficulties at school entry: Findings from a longitudinal twin study. Child Development, 84, 1098–1114. Bokosmaty, S., Sweller, J., & Kalyuga, S. (2015). Learning geometry problem solving by studying worked examples: Effects of learner guidance and expertise. American Educational Research Journal, 52, 307–333. Bonneville-Roussy, A., Bouffard, T., Palikara, O., & Vezeau, C. (2019). The role of cultural values in teacher and student self-efficacy: Evidence from 16 nations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 59. https://doi .org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101798 Borich, G. D. (2011). Effective teaching methods: Research-based practice (7th ed.). Pearson. Borko, H. (1993). Ms. Daniels: Strengths and limitations in a novice's teaching. In A. E. Woolfolk (Ed.), Readings and cases in educational psychology (pp. 265–271). Allyn & Bacon. Borko, H., & Putnam, R. (1996). Learning to teach. In D. Berliner & R. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 673–708). Macmillan. Borman, G. D., & Overman, L. T. (2004). Academic resilience in mathematics among poor and minority students. The Elementary School Journal, 104, 177–195. Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S, & Wolke, D. (2013). Systems and cascades in cognitive development and academic achievement. Child Development, 84, 154–162. Borrero, N. E., & Yeh, C. J. (2010). Ecological English language learning among ethnic minority youth. Educational Researcher, 39, 571–581. Borst, G., Poirel, N., Pineau, A., Cassotti, M., & Houdé, O. (2013). Inhibitory control efficiency in a Piaget-like class-inclusion task in school-age children and adults: A developmental negative priming study. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1366–1374. Borup, J., Graham, C. R., West, R. E., Archambault, L., & Spring, K. J. (2020). Academic Communities of Engagement: An expansive lens for examining support structures in blended and online learning. Educational Technology Research and Development, 68(2), 807–832. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-020-09744-x Bos, C. S., & Reyes, E. I. (1996). Conversations with a Latina teacher about education for language-minority students with special needs. The Elementary School Journal, 96, 344–351. Boston Children’s Hospital. (2021). Suicide in teens and children: Symptoms & causes. https://www .childrenshospital.org/conditions-and-treatments/conditions/s/suicide-and-teens/symptoms-and -causes Bowers, J. S. (2016a). Psychology, not educational neuroscience, is the way forward for improving educational outcomes for all children: Reply to Gabrieli (2016) and Howard-Jones et al. (2016). Psychological Review, 123, 628–635. Bowers, J. S. (2016b). The practical and principled problems with educational neuroscience. Psychological Review, 123, 600–612. Boyce, W. T., Levitt, P., Martinez, F. D., McEwen, B. S., & Shonkoff, J. P. (2021). Genes, environments, and time: The biology of adversity and resilience. Pediatrics, 147(2), e20201651. https://doi.org/10.1542 /peds.2020-1651 Boyle, J. R. (2010a). Note-taking skills of middle school students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 530–540. Boyle, J. R. (2010b). Strategic note-taking for middle school students with learning disabilities in science classes. Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 33, 93–109. Boyle, J. R., & Weishaar, M. (2001). The effects of a strategic note-taking technique on the comprehension and long term recall of lecture information for high school students with LD. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 16, 125–133. Brackett, M. A., Bailey, C. S., Hoffmann, J. D., & Simmons, D. N. (2019). RULER: A theory-driven, systemic approach to social, emotional, and academic learning. Educational Psychologist, 54, 144–161. https://doi. org/10.1080/00461520.2019.1614447 Bradshaw, C. P., Waasdorp, T. E., & Leaf, P. J. (2015). Examining variation in the impact of school-wide positive behavioral interventions and supports: Findings from a randomized controlled effectiveness trial. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 546–557.

References Bradshaw, C. P., Waasdorp, T. E., & O’Brennan, L. M. (2013). A latent class approach to examining forms of peer victimization. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 839–849. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032091 Bradshaw, C. P., Zmuda, J. H., Kellam, S. G., & Ialongo, N. S. (2009). Longitudinal impact of two universal preventive interventions in first grade on educational outcomes in high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 926–937. Brainerd, C. J. (2003). Jean Piaget, learning research, and American education. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Educational psychology: A century of contributions (pp. 251–287). Erlbaum. Branch, A. J. (2020). Promoting ethnic identity development while teaching subject matter content: A model of ethnic identity exploration in education. Teaching and Teacher Education, 87, 102918. https://doi .org/10.1016/j.tate.2019.102918 Brannon, L. (2002). Gender: Psychological perspectives (3rd ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L., & Cocking, R. R. (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school. National Academy Press. Bransford, J. D., & Schwartz, D. (1999). Rethinking transfer: A simple proposal with multiple implications. In A. Iran-Nejad & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Review of research in education (Vol. 24, pp. 61–100). American Educational Research Association. Brantlinger, E. (2004). Who wins and who loses? Social class and students’ identities. In M. Sadowski (Ed.), Adolescents at school: Perspectives on youth, identity, and education (pp. 107–126). Harvard University Press. Branum-Martin, L., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., & Mehta, P. D. (2010). Contextual effects of bilingual programs on beginning reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 341–355. Bray, I., Slater, A., Lewis-Smith, H., Bird, E., & Sabey, A. (2019). Promoting positive body image and tackling overweight/obesity in children and adolescents: A combined health psychology and public health approach. Preventive Medicine, 116, 219–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2018.08.011 Bredekamp, S. (2017). Effective practices in early childhood education: Building a foundation (3rd ed.). Pearson. Brice, A. E., & Brice, R. G. (2009). Language development: Monolingual and bilingual acquisitions. Allyn & Bacon. Brichinall, L., Spendlove, D., & Buck, R. (2019). In the moment: Does mindfulness hold the key to improving the resilience and wellbeing of pre-service teachers? Teaching and Teacher Education, 86, 1–8. Briesch, A. M., & Chafouleas, S. M. (2009). Review and analysis of literature on self-management interventions to promote appropriate classroom behaviors (1988–2008). School Psychology Quarterly, 24, 106–118. Brix, N., Ernst, A., Braskhøj Lauridsen, L. L., Parner, E., Støvring, H., Olsen, J., Henriksen, T. B., & RamlauHansen, C. H. (2018). Timing of puberty in boys and girls: A population-based study. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 33, 70–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/ppe.12507 Broderick, P. C., & Blewitt, P. (2015). The life span: Human development for helping professions. Pearson. Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., Tremblay, R. E., Bates, J. E., Brame, B., Dodge, K., Fergusson, D., Horwood, J. L., Loeber, R., Laird, R., Lynam, D. R., Moffitt, T. E., Pettit, G. S., & Vitaro, F. (2003). Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six site, cross-national study. Developmental Psychology, 39, 222–245. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989). Ecological systems theory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development (Vol. 6, pp. 187–249). JAI Press. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 793–827). Wiley. Brookhart, S. M. (2018). Summative and formative feedback. In A. A. Lipnevich & J. K. Smith (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of instructional feedback (pp. 52–78). Cambridge University Press. Brookhart, S. M., & Nitko, T. J. (2019). Educational assessment of students (8th ed.). Pearson. Brooks-Gunn, J. (1988). Antecedents and consequences of variations in girls’ maturational timing. In M. D. Levin & E. R. McAnarney (Eds.), Early adolescent transitions (pp. 101–121). Lexington Books. Brophy, J. E. (1981). Teacher praise: A functional analysis. Review of Educational Research, 51, 5–21. Brophy, J. E. (1988). On motivating students. In D. Berliner & B. Rosenshine (Eds.), Talks to teachers (pp. 201–245). Random House. Brophy, J. E. (1998). Motivating students to learn. McGraw-Hill. Brophy, J. E. (2003). An interview with Jere Brophy by B. Gaedke & M. Shaughnessy. Educational Psychology Review, 15, 199–211. Brophy, J. E. (2005). Goal theorists should move on from performance goals. Educational Psychologist, 40, 167–176. Brophy, J. E. (2008). Developing students’ appreciation for what is taught in school. Educational Psychologist, 43, 132–141. Brophy, J. E., & Good, T. (1986). Teacher behavior and student achievement. In M. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (3rd ed., pp. 328–375). Macmillan. Brown, A. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation, and other more mysterious mechanisms. In F. Weinert & R. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation, and understanding (pp. 65–116). Erlbaum. Brown, A. L., & Palincsar, A. S. (2018). Guided, cooperative learning and individual knowledge acquisition. In L. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 393–451). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315044408-13 Brown, B. B. (2004). Adolescents’ relationships with peers. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed., pp. 363–394). Wiley. Brown, C. S. (2014). Parenting beyond pink and blue: How to raise your kids free of gender stereotypes. Ten Speed Press. Brown, C. S. (2017). Discrimination in childhood and adolescence: A developmental intergroup approach. Routledge. Brown, C. S., & Stone, E. (2016). Gender stereotypes and discrimination: How sexism impacts development. In S. S. Horn, M. D. Ruck, & L. S. Liben (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior: Vol. 50. Equity and justice in developmental science: Theoretical and methodological issues (pp. 105–133). Elsevier.

R-9

R-10 References Brown, J., Jones, S. M., LaRusso, M. D., & Aber, J. L (2010). Improving classroom quality: Teacher influences and experimental impacts of the 4Rs Program. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 153–167. Brown, P. C., Roedinger, H. L., III, & McDaniel, M. A. (2014). Make it stick: The science of successful learning. The Belknap Press. Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Hembrooke, H. (2002). The nature of children's true and false narratives. Developmental Review, 22(3), 520–554. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0273-2297(02)00006-0 Bruer, J. T. (1997). Education and the brain: A bridge too far. Educational Researcher, 26, 4–16. http://dx.doi .org/10.3102/0013189X026008004 Bruer, J. T. (1999). In search of … brain-based education. Phi Delta Kappan, 80, 648–657. Bruer, J. T. (2002). Avoiding the pediatrician’s error: How neuroscientists can help educators (and themselves). Nature Neuroscience, 5, 1031–1033. Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Norton. Bruner, J. S. (1973). Beyond the information given: Studies in the psychology of knowing. Norton. Bruning, R. H., Schraw, G. J., & Norby, M. M. (2011). Cognitive psychology and instruction (5th ed.). Pearson. Brunner, M., Keller, U., Dierendinck, C., Reichert, M., Ugen, S., Fischbach, A., & Martin, R. (2010). The structure of academic self-concepts revisited: The nested Marsh/Shavelson model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 964–981. Buck Institute for Education. (2019). Gold standard PBL: Essential project design elements. Buck Institute for Education. https://my.pblworks.org/system/files/documents/Gold_Standard_PBL_Essential_v2019.pdf Buckner, J. C. (2012). Education research on homeless and housed children living in poverty: Comments on Masten, Fantuzzo, Herbers, and Voight. Educational Researcher, 41, 403–407. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0013189X12466588 Budiman, A. (2020, August). Key findings about U.S. immigrants. Pew Research Center. https://www .pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/08/20/key-findings-about-u-s-immigrants/ Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (1994). Exploring the “planning fallacy”: Why people underestimate their task completion times. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 366–381. Buhs, E. S., Ladd, G. W., & Herald, S. L. (2006). Peer exclusion and victimization: Processes that mediate the relation between peer group rejection and children’s classroom engagement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 1–13. Bui, D. C., Myerson, J., & Hale, S. (2013). Note-taking with computers: Exploring alternative strategies for improved recall. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 299–309. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030367 Bullard, J. (2017). Creating environments for learning: Birth to age eight (3rd ed.). Pearson. Burbules, N. C., & Bruce, B. C. (2001). Theory and research on teaching as dialogue. In V. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (4th ed., pp. 1102–1121). American Educational Research Association. Burden, P. R. (1995). Classroom management and discipline: Methods to facilitate cooperation and instruction. Longman. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2020). Learn more, earn more: Education leads to higher wages, lower unemployment. https://www.bls.gov/careeroutlook/2020/data-on-display/education-pays.htm Burger, K., & Samuel, R. (2017). The role of perceived stress and self-efficacy in young people’s life satisfaction: A longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46(1), 78–90. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-016 -0608-x Burić, I., & Kim, L. E. (2020). Teacher self-efficacy, instructional quality, and student motivational beliefs: An analysis using multilevel structural equation modeling. Learning and Instruction, 66, 101302. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.101302 Buric, I., & Moè, A. (2020). What makes teachers enthusiastic: The interplay of positive affect, self-effcacy and job satisfaction. Teaching and Teachers Education, 89, 103008. Burns, E. C., Martin, A. J., & Collie, R. J. (2018). Adaptability, personal best (PB) goals setting, and gains in students’ academic outcomes: A longitudinal examination from a social cognitive perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 53, 57–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.02.001 Butcher, K. R. (2006). Learning from text with diagrams: Promoting mental model development and inference generation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 182–197. Butler, D. L., Schnellert, L., & Perry, N. E. (2017). Developing self-regulated learners. Pearson. Butz, A. R., & Usher, E. L. (2015). Salient sources of self-efficacy in reading and mathematics. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 42, 49–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.04.001 Byers-Heinlein, K., & Lew-Williams, C. (2013). Bilingualism in the early years: What the science says. Learning Landscapes, 7, 95–112. https://doi.org/10.36510/learnland.v7i1.632. Byrnes, J. P. (1996). Cognitive development and learning in instructional contexts. Allyn & Bacon. Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (2006). The making of developmental psychology. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed.), Vol. 1: Theoretical models of human development (pp. 89–165). Wiley. Cajete, G. A. (2020). Indigenous science, climate change, and indigenous community building: A framework of foundational perspectives for indigenous community resilience and revitalization. Sustainability, 12, 9569. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12229569 Çakıroğlu, J., Aydın, Y. C., & Woolfolk Hoy, A. (2012). Science teaching efficacy beliefs. In B. Frazer, K. Tobin, & C. McRobbie (Eds.), Second international handbook of science education (pp. 449–462). Springer. Calderhead, J. (1996). Teacher: Beliefs and knowledge. In D. Berliner & R. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 709–725). Macmillan. Calfee, R., Wilson, K. A., Flannery, B., & Kapinus, B. A. (2014). Formative assessment for the Common Core literacy standards. Teachers College Record, 116(11), 1–32. http://www.tcrecord.org ID Number: 17649. Callaghan, T., Moll, H., Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., Liszkowski, U., Behne, T., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Early social cognition in three cultural contexts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76(2), 1–142.

References Callahan, C. M., Moon, T. R., Oh, S., Azano, A. P., & Hailey, E. P. (2015). What works in gifted education: Documenting the effects of an integrated curricular/instructional model for gifted students. American Educational Research Journal, 52, 137–167. Callahan, M. (2018, July). “Autistic person” or “person with autism”: Is there a right way to identify people? News@Northeastern. https://news.northeastern.edu/2018/07/12/unpacking-the-debate-over-person -first-vs-identity-first-language-in-the-autism-community/ Cameron, J., & Pierce, W. D. (1996). The debate about rewards and intrinsic motivation: Protests and accusations do not alter the results. Review of Educational Research, 66, 39–52. Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2003). A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science, 27, 843–873. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2706_2 Campbell, S. L., & Ronfeldt, M. (2018). Observational evaluation of teachers: Measuring more than we bargained for? American Educational Research Journal, 55(6), 1233–1267. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0002831218776216 Cangelosi, J. S. (1990). Designing tests for evaluating student achievement. Longman. Cano-Garcia, F. J., Padilla-Munoz, E. M., & Carrasco-Ortiz, M. A. (2005). Personality and contextual variables in teacher burnout. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 929–940. Canter, L. (1996). First the rapport—then the rules. Learning, 24(5), 121. Canter, L., & Canter, M. (1992). Lee Canter’s Assertive Discipline: Positive behavior management for today’s classroom. Lee Canter and Associates. Cantrell, S. C., Almasi, J. F., Carter, J. S., Rintamaa, M., & Madden, A. (2010). The impact of a strategy-based intervention on the comprehension and strategy use of struggling adolescent readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 257–280. Capa, Y. (2005). Novice teachers’ sense of efficacy. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH. Caprara, G. V., Fida, R., Vecchione, M., Del Bove, G., Vecchio, G. M., Barbaranelli, C., & Bandura, A. (2008). Longitudinal analysis of the role of perceived self-efficacy for self-regulated learning in academic continuance and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 525–534. Caprara, G. V., Pastorelli, C., Regalia, C., Scabini, E., & Bandura, A. (2005). Impact of adolescents’ filial selfefficacy on quality of family functioning and satisfaction. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15, 71–97. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2005.00087.x Carbonneau, K. J., Marley, S. C., & Selig, J. P. (2013). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of teaching mathematics with concrete manipulatives. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 380–400. Cardoza, K. (2019, April 9). How schools are responding to migrant children. Education Week. https://www .edweek.org/leadership/how-schools-are-responding-to-migrant-children/2019/04 Carlson, S. M., Koenig, M. A., & Harms, M. B. (2013). Theory of mind. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4, 391–402. Carney, R. N., & Levin, J. R. (2000). Mnemonic instruction, with a focus on transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 783–790. Carney, R. N., & Levin, J. R. (2002). Pictorial illustrations still improve students’ learning from text. Educational Psychology Review, 14, 5–26. Carpenter, S. K. (2012). Testing enhances the transfer of learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 279–283. Carpenter, S. K., Cepeda, N. J., Rohrer, D., Kang, S. H. K., & Pashler, H. (2012). Using spacing to enhance diverse forms of learning: Review of recent research and implications for instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 369–378. Carpenter, S. K., Endres, T., & Hui, L. (2020). Students’ use of retrieval in self-regulated learning: Implications for monitoring and regulating effortful learning experiences. Educational Psychology Review, 32(4), 1029–1054. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09562-w Carroll, J. B. (2005). The three-stratum theory of cognitive abilities. In D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (2nd ed., pp. 69–76). Guilford Press. Carter, E. W., Wehby, J., Hughes, C., Johnson, S. M., Plank, D. R., Barton-Arwood, S. M., & Lunsford, L. B. (2005). Preparing adolescents with high-incidence disabilities for high-stakes testing with strategy instruction. Preventing School Failure, 49(2), 55–62. Case, R. (1985). A developmentally-based approach to the problem of instructional design. In R. Glaser, S. Chipman, & J. Segal (Eds.), Teaching thinking skills (Vol. 2, pp. 545–562). Erlbaum. Case, R. (1992). The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge. Erlbaum. Case, R. (1998). The development of conceptual structures. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 745–800). Wiley. CASEL. (2020). About CASEL. https://casel.org/about–2/ Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28, 62–77. Cash, A. H., Debnam, K. J., Waasdorp, T. E., Wahl, M., & Bradshaw, C. P. (2019). Adult and student interactions in nonclassroom settings. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(1), 104–117. https://doi.org/10.1037 /edu0000275 Cassady, J. C. (2004). The influence of cognitive test anxiety across the learning-testing cycle. Learning and Instruction, 14(6), 569–592. Cassady, J. C. (2010). Anxiety in schools: The causes, consequences, and solutions for academic anxieties. Peter Lang.

R-11

R-12 References Cassady, J. C., & Boseck, J. J. (2008). Educational psychology and emotional intelligence: Toward a functional model for emotional information processing. In J. C. Cassady & M. A. Eissa (Eds.), Emotional intelligence: Perspectives on educational and positive psychology (pp. 3–24). Peter Lang. CAST. (2018). Universal Design for Learning Guidelines version 2.2. http://udlguidelines.cast.org Castejon, J. L., Perez, A. M., & Gilar, R. (2010). Confirmatory factor analysis of Project Spectrum activities. A second-order g factor or multiple intelligences? Intelligence, 38, 481–496. Castellano, J. A., & Diaz, E. I. (Eds.). (2002). Reaching new horizons. Gifted and talented education for culturally and linguistically diverse students. Allyn & Bacon. Castle, S., Deniz, C. B., & Tortora, M. (2005). Flexible grouping and student learning in a high-needs school. Education and Urban Society, 37, 139–150. Catrysse, L., Gijbels, D., & Donche, V. (2018). It is not only about the depth of processing: What if eye am not interested in the text? Learning and Instruction, 58, 284–294. Caughy, M. O., O’Campo, P. J., Randolph, S. M., & Nickerson, K. (2002). The influence of racial socialization practices on the cognitive and behavioral competence of African American preschoolers. Child Development, 73, 1611–1625. Causadias, J. M. (2020). What is culture? Systems of people, places, and practices. Applied Developmental Science, 24(4), 310–322. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2020.1789360 Caye, A., Swanson, J. M., Coghill, D., & Rohde, L. A. (2019). Treatment strategies for ADHD: An evidencebased guide to select optimal treatment. Molecular Psychiatry, 24(3), 390–408. https://doi.org/10.1038 /s41380-018-0116-3 Cazden, C. (2001). Classroom discourse: The language of teaching and learning (2nd ed.). Heinemann. CDC. (2019). Distracted driving. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. https://www.cdc.gov /motorvehiclesafety/distracted_driving/index.html Center for Promise. (2015). Don’t quit on me: What young people who left school say about the power of relationships. America’s Promise Alliance. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2020). Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2019. MMWR, 69(Suppl-1), 1–88. https://www.cdc.gov/healthyyouth/data/yrbs/pdf/2019 /su6901-H.pdf Chafouleas, S. M., Johnson, A. H., Overstreet, S., & Santos, N. M. (2016). Toward a blueprint for trauma-informed service delivery in schools. School Mental Health, 8, 144–162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-015-9166-8 Chance, P. (1991). Backtalk: A gross injustice. Phi Delta Kappan, 72, 803. Chance, P. (1992). The rewards of learning. Phi Delta Kappan, 73, 200–207. Chance, P. (1993). Sticking up for rewards. Phi Delta Kappan, 74, 787–790. Chang, M.-L. (2009). An appraisal perspective of teacher burnout: Examining the emotional work of teachers. Educational Psychology Review, 21, 193–218. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-009-9106-y Chappuis, J., & Stiggins, R. (2020). Classroom assessment for student learning: Doing it right—doing it well (3rd ed.). Pearson. Chappuis, J., & Stiggins, R. J. (2017). An introduction to student-involved assessment for learning. Pearson. Charles, C. M. (2011). Building classroom discipline (10th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Charles, C. M., & Cole, K. M. (2019). Building classroom management: Methods and models (12th ed.). Pearson. Charles, E. P., Baker, C. M., Hartman, K., Easton, B. P., & Kreuzberger, C. (2013). Motion capture controls negate the violent video-game effect. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 2519–2523. Chawla, D. S. (2019, July 3). Large study supports discarding the term “high-functioning autism.” Spectrum. https://www.spectrumnews.org/news/large-study-supports-discarding-term-high-functioning-autism/ Chen, C. H., & Yang, Y. C. (2019). Revisiting the effects of project-based learning on students’ academic achievement: A meta-analysis investigating moderators. Educational Research Review, 26, 71–81. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2018.11.001 Chen, F., & Fleer, M. (2016). A cultural-historical reading of how play is used in families as a tool for supporting children’s emotional development in everyday life. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 24(2), 305–319. https://doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2016.1143268 Chen, J. A., & Stoddard, J. D. (2020). A virtual internship to prepare high school students for civic and political action. Educational Technology Research and Development, 68, 3449–3470. https://doi.org/10.1007 /s11423-020-09847-5 Chen, M., Zee, M., Koomen, H. M. K., & Roorda, D. L. (2019). Understanding cross-cultural differences in affective teacher-student relationships: A comparison between Dutch and Chinese primary school teachers and students. Journal of School Psychology,76, 89–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2019.07.011 Chen, Z., Mo, L., & Honomichl, R. (2004). Having the memory of an elephant: Long-term retrieval and the use of analogues in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 415–433. Cheng, S.-L., Lu, L., Xie, K., & Vongkulluksn, V. W. (2020). Understanding teacher technology integration from expectancy-value perspectives. Teaching and Teacher Education, 91, 103062. https://doi .org/10.1016/j.tate.2020.103062 Cherniss, C., Extein, M., Goleman, D., & Weissberg, R. P. (2006). Emotional intelligence: What does the research really indicate? Educational Psychologist, 41, 239–245. https://doi.org/10.1207 /s15326985ep4104_4 Cheung, A. C. K., & Slavin, R. E. (2012). Effective reading programs for Spanish-dominant English language learners (ELLs) in the elementary grades: A synthesis of research. Review of Educational Research, 82, 351–395. Chi, M. T. H. (1978). Knowledge structures and memory development. In R. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? (pp. 73–96). Erlbaum.

References Chi, M. T. H., & VanLehn, K. A. (2012). Seeing deep structure from the interactions of surface features. Educational Psychologist, 47, 177–188. Chi, M. T. H., & Wylie, R. (2014). The ICAP Framework: Linking cognitive engagement to active learning outcomes. Educational Psychologist, 49, 219–243. Child Trends Databank. (2019). Children in poverty. https://www.childtrends.org/?indicators=children-in -poverty Children’s Defense Fund. (2019). 2017–2018 National Survey of Children’s Health. 2019. “Indicator 6.13: Adverse Childhood Experiences.” Data Resource Center for Child & Adolescent Health. https://www .childhealthdata.org/browse/survey/ Children’s Defense Fund. (2020). The state of America’s children 2020. https://www.childrensdefense.org /the-state-of-americas-children–2020/ Children’s Defense Fund. (2020a). The state of America’s children: Child poverty. Author. https://www .childrensdefense.org/policy/resources/soac-2020-child-poverty/ Children’s Defense Fund. (2020b). The state of America’s children: Child welfare. Author. https://www .childrensdefense.org/policy/resources/soac-2020-child-welfare-tables/ Children’s Defense Fund. (2020c). The state of America’s children: Moments in America. Author. https://www .childrensdefense.org/policy/resources/soac-2020-moments/ Chin, M. J., Quinn, D. M., Dhaliwal, T. K., & Lovison, V. S. (2020). Bias in the air: A nationwide exploration of teachers’ implicit racial attitudes, aggregate bias, and student outcomes. Educational Researcher, 49(8), 566–578. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X20937240 Chiu, M. M., & Chow, B. W.-Y. (2015). Classmate characteristics and student achievement in 33 countries: Classmates’ past achievement, family socioeconomic status, educational resources, and attitudes toward reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 152–169. Cho, B., Woodward, L., Li, D., & Barlow, W. (2017). Examining adolescents’ strategic processing during online reading with a question-generating task. American Educational Research Journal, 54, 691–724. Choi, H., & Lee, H. S. (2020). Knowing is not half the battle: The role of actual test experience in the forward testing effect. Educational Psychology Review, 32(3), 765–789. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09518-0 Choi, H.-H., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (2015). Effects of the physical environment on cognitive load and learning: Towards a new model of cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 244. Chorzempa, B. F., & Graham, S. (2006). Primary-grade teachers’ use of within-class ability grouping in reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 529–541. Christopher, J. C., Wendt, D. C., Marecek, J., & Goodman, D. M. (2014). Critical cultural awareness: Contributions to a globalizing psychology. American Psychologist, 69, 645–655. Clark, D. A., & Svinicki, M. (2015). The effect of retrieval on post-task enjoyment of studying. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 51–67. Clark, D. B., Tanner-Smith, E. E., & Killingsworth, S. S. (2016). Digital games, design, and learning: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 86(1), 79–122. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0034654315582065 Clark, I. (2012). Formative assessment: Assessment is for self-regulated learning. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 205–249. Clark, J. M., & Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory and education. Educational Psychology Review, 3, 149–210. Clark, K. (2009). The case for Structured English Immersion. Educational Leadership, 66(7), 42–46. Clarke, J. H., & Agne, R. M. (1997). Curriculum development; Interdisciplinary high school teaching. Allyn & Bacon. Cleminshaw, C. L., DuPaul, G. J., Kipperman, K. L., Evans, S. W., & Owens, J. S. (2020). Social deficits in high school students with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the role of emotion dysregulation. School Psychology, 35(4), 233–242. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000392 Cobb, P., & Bowers, J. (1999). Cognitive and situated learning: Perspectives in theory and practice. Educational Researcher, 28(2), 4–15. Cobley, S., McKenna, J., Baker, J., & Wattie, N. (2009). How pervasive are relative age effects in secondary school education? Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 520–528. Codell, E. R. (2001). Educating Esme: Diary of a teacher’s first year. Algonquin Books. Coffield, F. J., Moseley, D. V., Hall, E., & Ecclestone, K. (2004). Learning styles and pedagogy in post–16 learning: A systematic and critical review. Learning and Skills Research Centre/University of Newcastle upon Tyne. Cohen, A. B. (2009). Many forms of culture. American Psychologist, 64, 194–204. Cohen, A. B. (2010). Just how many different forms of culture are there? American Psychologist, 65, 59–61. Cohen, E. G. (1994). Designing group work (2nd ed.). Teachers College Press. Cohen, E. G., & Lotan, R. A. (2014). Designing groupwork: Strategies for the heterogeneous classroom (3rd ed.). Teachers College Press. Cokley, K., McClain, S., Jones, M., & Johnson, S. (2011). A preliminary investigation of academic disidentification, racial identity, and academic achievement among African American adolescents. The High School Journal, 95, 54–68. Cole, M. (1998). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Harvard University Press. Cole, P. M., Ram, N., & English, M. S. (2019). Toward a unifying model of self-regulation: A developmental approach. Child Development Perspectives, 13(2), 91–96. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12316 Colledge, E., Bishop, D. V. M., Koeppen-Schomerus, G., Price, T. S., Happe, F., Eley, T., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2002). The structure of language abilities at 4 years: A twin study. Developmental Psychology, 38, 749–757. College Board. (2020). SAT suite of assessments annual report. College Board. https://reports.collegeboard .org/pdf/2020-total-group-sat-suite-assessments-annual-report.pdf

R-13

R-14 References Collie, R. J. (2021). COVID-19 and teachers’ somatic burden, stress, and emotional exhaustion: Examining the role of principal leadership and workplace buoyancy. AERA Open, 7. https://doi.org /10.1177/2332858420986187 Collie, R. J., Shapka, J. D., & Perry, N. E. (2012). School climate and social–emotional learning: Predicting teacher stress, job satisfaction, and teaching efficacy. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 1189–1204. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029356 Collins, A. (2006). Cognitive apprenticeship. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 47–77). The Cambridge University Press. Collins, A., Brown, J. S., & Newman, S. E. (1989). Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching the crafts of reading, writing, and mathematics. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Galser (pp. 453–494). Erlbaum. Collins, C., & Ehrenhalt, J. (2018). Best practice for serving LGBTQ students. Learning for Justice. https://www .learningforjustice.org/magazine/publications/best-practices-for-serving-lgbtq-students Collins, W. A., & Steinberg, L. (2006). Adolescent development in interpersonal context. In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Series Eds.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 1003–1067). Wiley. Comadena, M. E., Hunt, S. K., & Simonds, C. J. (2007). The effects of teacher clarity, nonverbal immediacy, and caring on student motivation, affective and cognitive learning. Communication Research Reports, 24, 241–248. Committee on Increasing High School Students’ Engagement and Motivation to Learn. (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering high school students’ motivation to learn. The National Academies Press. Common Sense Media. (2013a). Media and violence: An analysis of current research. Author. Common Sense Media. (2013b). Zero to eight: Children’s media use in America 2013. https://www .commonsensemedia.org/zero-to-eight-2013-infographic Common Sense Media. (2017). The Common Sense Census: Media use by kids age zero to eight 2017. https://www .commonsensemedia.org/research/the-common-sense-census-media-use-by-kids-age-zero-to-eight-2017 Common Sense Media. (2019). The Common Sense Census: Media use by tweens and teens, 2019. https://www .commonsensemedia.org/research/the-common-sense-census-media-use-by-tweens-and-teens-2019 Connor, C. M., Phillips, B. M., Kaschak, M., Apel, K., Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Crowe, E. C., Thomas-Tate, S., Cooper Johnson, L., & Lonigan, C. J. (2014). Comprehension tools for teachers: Reading for understanding from prekindergarten through fourth grade. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 379–401. Connor-Zachocki, J., Husbye, N. E., & Gee, J. P. (2015). Digital media and learning. Theory Into Practice, 54, 83–177. Cook, J. L., & Cook, G. (2014). The world of children (3rd ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Cooper, C. E., Crosnoe, R., Suizzo, M.-A., & Pituch, K. A. (2010). Poverty, race, and parental involvement during the transition to elementary school. Journal of Family Issues, 31, 859–883. Cooper, H. (2007). The battle over homework: Common ground for administrators, teachers, and parents. Corwin Press. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781483329420 Cooper, H. M. (2004). Special issue: Homework. Theory Into Practice, 43(3). Cooper, H. M., Robinson, J. C., & Patall, E. A. (2006). Does homework improve academic achievement? A synthesis of research, 1987–2003. Review of Educational Research, 76, 1–62. Cooper, H. M., & Valentine, J. C. (Eds.). (2001, Summer). Special issue: Homework. Educational Psychologist, 36(3). Cooper, H. M., Valentine, J. C., Nye, B., & Kindsay, J. J. (1999). Relationships between five after-school activities and academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 369–378. Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2020). Applied behavior analysis (3rd ed.). Pearson. Coplan, R. J., Prakash, K., O’Neil, K., & Armer, M. (2004). Do you “want” to play? Distinguishing between conflicted shyness and social disinterest in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 40, 244–258. Copple, C., & Bredekamp, S. (2008). Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs serving children from birth through age 8 (3rd ed.). National Association for the Education of Young Children. Corbin, C. M., Alamos, P., Lowenstein, A. E., Downer, J. T., & Brown, J. L. (2019). The role of teacher-student relationships in predicting teachers’ personal accomplishment and emotional exhaustion. Journal of School Psychology, 77, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2019.10.001 Corno, L. (1992). Encouraging students to take responsibility for learning and performance. Elementary School Journal, 93, 69–85. Corno, L. (2008). On teaching adaptively. Educational Psychologist, 43, 161–173. Corno, L. (2011). Studying self-regulation habits. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of selfregulation of learning and performance (pp. 361–375). Routledge. Corpus, J. H., McClintic-Gilbert, M. S., & Hayenga, A. O. (2009). Within-year changes in children’s intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations: Contextual predictors and academic outcomes. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 154–166. Cortes, K. I., & Kochenderfer-Ladd, B. (2014). To tell or not to tell: What influences children’s decisions to report bullying to their teachers? School Psychology Quarterly, 29, 336–348. Costello, M. B. (2016). The Trump Effect: The impact of the presidential campaign on our nation’s schools. Southern Poverty Law Center. https://www.splcenter.org/sites/default/files/splc_the_trump_effect.pdf Cothran, D. J., & Ennis, C. D. (2000). Building bridges to student engagement: Communicating respect and care for students in urban high school. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 33(2), 106–117. Covington, M. V. (1992). Making the grade: A self-worth perspective on motivation and school reform. Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Covington, M. V., & Mueller, K. J. (2001). Intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation: An approach/avoidance reformulation. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 157–176.

References Cowan, N. (2008). What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? In Sossin, W. S., Lacaille, J. C., Castellucci, V. F., & Belleville, S. (Eds.), Progress in brain research (Vol. 169, pp. 323–338), Elsevier. Cowan, N. (2014). Working memory underpins cognitive development, learning, and education. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 197–223. Crabbe, R., Pivnick, L. K., Bates, J., Gordon, R. A., & Crosnoe, R. (2019). Contemporary college students’ reflections on their high school peer crowds. Journal of Adolescent Research, 34(5), 563–596. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0743558418809537 Craik, F. I. M. (2020). Remembering: An activity of mind and brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 1–24. Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684. Credé, M., Tynan, M. C., & Harms, P. D. (2017). Much ado about grit: A meta-analytic synthesis of the grit literature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(3), 492–511. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000102 Creese, A. (2009). Building on young people’s linguistic and cultural continuity: Complementary schools in the United Kingdom. Theory Into Practice, 48, 267–273. Cremin, L. A. (1961). The transformation of the school: Progressivism in American Education, 1876–1957. Knopf. Creswell, J. W. (2015). Planning, conducting, and evaluating quantitative and qualitative research (5th ed.). Pearson. Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). Reaping the benefits of pursuing self-esteem without the costs. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 392–414. Crompton, H. (2013). A historical overview of mobile learning: Toward learner-centered education. In Z. L. Berge & L. Y. Muilenburg (Eds.), Handbook of mobile learning (pp. 3–14). Routledge. Crompton, H., & Burke, D. (2020). Mobile learning and pedagogical opportunities: A configurative systematic review of PreK–12 research using the SAMR framework. Computers and Education, 156, 103945. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2020.103945 Crooks, C. V., Hoover, S., & Smith, A. C. G. (2020). Feasibility trial of the school-based STRONG intervention to promote resilience among newcomer youth. Psychology in the Schools, 57, 1815–1829. Crosnoe, R., Morrison, F., Burchinal, M., Pianta, R., Keating, D., Friedman, S. L., & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2010). Instruction, teacher–student relations, and math achievement trajectories in elementary school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 407–417. Cross, T., & Polk, L. (2018). Burn bright, not out: Tips for managing online teaching. Journal of Educators Online, 15(3). https://doi.org/10.9743/jeo.2018.15.3.1 Cross, W. E. (1991). Shades of black: Diversity in African-American identity. Temple University Press. Cross, W. E., Jr., & Cross, T. B. (2007). Theory, research, and models. In S. M. Quintana & C. McKown (Eds.), Race, racism and developing child (pp. 154–181). Wiley. Cross, W. E., Jr., Grant, B. O., & Ana V. (2012). Black identity and well-being: Untangling race and ethnicity. In J. M. Sullivan & A. M. Esmail (Eds.), African American identity: Racial and cultural dimensions of the black experience (pp. 125–146). Lexington Books. Crum, A. J., Jamieson, J. P., & Akinola, M. (2020). Optimizing stress: An integrated intervention for regulating stress responses. Emotion, 20(1), 120–125. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000670 Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety. Experiencing flow in work and play. Jossey-Bass. Cucina, J. M., Peyton, S. T., Su, C., & Byle, K. A. (2016). Role of mental abilities and mental tests in explaining high-school grades. Intelligence, 54, 90–104. Curwin, R. (2015, September). The real meaning of zero-tolerance. https://www.edutopia.org/blog /real-meaning-of-zero-tolerance-richard-curwin Cusick, P. A. (2014). The logic of the U.S. educational system and teaching. Theory Into Practice, 53, 176–182. https://doi.org/10.1080/00405841.2014.916568 Cutuli, J. J., Desjardins, C. D., Herbers, J. E., Long, J. D., Heistad, D., Chan, C.-K., Hinz, E., & Masten, A. S. (2013). Academic achievement trajectories of homeless and highly mobile students: Resilience in the context of chronic and acute risk. Child Development, 84, 841–857. Dadds, M. R., & Tully, L. A. (2019). What is it to discipline a child: What should it be? A reanalysis of time-out from the perspective of child mental health, attachment, and trauma. American Psychologist, 74, 794–808. Dagenais, C., Lysenko, L., Abrami, P., Bernard, R., Ramde, J., & Janosz, M. (2012). Use of research-based information by school practitioners and determinants of use: A review of empirical research. Evidence & Policy, 8(3), 285–309. https://doi.org/10.1332/174426412X654031 Damon, W. (1994). Fair distribution and sharing: The development of positive justice. In B. Puka (Ed.), Fundamental research in moral development. Moral development: A compendium, Vol. 2 (pp. 189–254). Garland. Danielson, C. (2013). The Framework for Teaching evaluation instrument: 2013 edition. The Danielson Group. Danish, J., & Hmelo-Silver, C. E. (2020). On activities and affordances for mobile learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101829 Dargue, N., & Sweller, N. (2020). Learning stories through gesture: Gesture’s effects on child and adult narrative comprehension. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 249–276. Darnon, C., Dompnier, B., Gillieron, O., & Butera, F. (2010). The interplay of mastery and performance goals in social comparison: A multiple-goal perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 212–222. DaSilva Iddings, A. C. (2009). Bridging home and school literacy practices: Empowering families of recent immigrant children. Theory Into Practice, 48, 304–311. Davidson, E., Reback, R., Rockoff, J., & Schwartz, H. L. (2015). Fifty ways to leave a child behind: Idiosyncrasies and discrepancies in states’ implementation of NCLB. Educational Researcher, 44, 347–358. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0013189X15601426

R-15

R-16 References Davis, H. A. (2003). Conceptualizing the role and influence of student–teacher relationships on children’s social and cognitive development, Educational Psychologist, 38, 207–234. de Araujo, Z., Otten, S., & Birisci, S. (2017). Mathematics teachers’ motivations for, conceptions of, and experiences with flipped instruction. Teaching and Teacher Education, 62, 60–70. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.tate.2016.11.006 De Boer, H., Bosker, R. J., & van der Werf, M. P. C. (2010). Sustainability of teacher expectation bias effects on long-term student performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 168–179. De Bruin, A. B. H., Roelle, J., & Baars, M., EFG-MRE (2020). Synthesizing cognitive load and self-regulation theory: A theoretical framework and research agenda. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 903–915. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09576-4 De Bruin, C. L., Deppeler, J. M., Moore, D. W., & Diamond, N. T. (2013). Public school–based interventions for adolescents and young adults with an autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 83, 521–550. De Castella, K., Byrne, D., & Covington, M. (2013). Unmotivated or motivated to fail? A cross-cultural study of achievement motivation, fear of failure, and student disengagement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(3), 861–880. De France, K., Hancock, G. R., Stack, D. M., Serbin, L. A., & Hollenstein, T. (2021). The mental health implications of COVID-19 for adolescents: Follow-up of a four-wave longitudinal study during the pandemic. American Psychologist. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000838 de Freitas, S. (2018). Are games effective learning tools? A review of educational games. Educational Technology & Society, 21(2), 74–84. De George, G. (2008). Is it language or is it special needs? Appropriately diagnosing English language learners having achievement difficulties. In L. S. Verplaetse & N. Migliacci (Eds.), Inclusive pedagogy for English language learners: A handbook of research-informed practices (pp. 277–303). Erlbaum. de Groot, A. D. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. Mouton Publishers. de Jong, E. M., Koomen, H. M. Y., Jellesma, F. C., & Roorda, D. L. (2018). Teacher and child perceptions of relationship quality and ethnic minority children's behavioral adjustment in upper elementary school: A cross-lagged approach. Journal of School Psychology, 70, 27–43. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.jsp.2018.06.003 de Ruiter, L. E., & Bers, M. U. (2021). The Coding Stages Assessment: Development and validation of an instrument for assessing young children's proficiency in the ScratchJr programming language. Computer Science Education. https://doi.org/10.1080/08993408.2021.1956216 Dearing, E., Kreider, H., Simpkins, S., & Weiss, H. B. (2006). Family involvement in school and low-income children’s literacy: Longitudinal associations between and within families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 653–664. Deary, I. J. (2012). Intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 453–482. Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668. Deckers, L. (2018). Motivation: Biological, psychological, and environmental (5th ed.) Routledge. Decristan, J., Klieme, E., Kunter, M., Hochweber, J., Büttner, G., Fauth, B., Hondrich, A. L., Rieser, S., Hertel, S., & Hardy, I. (2015). Embedded formative assessment and classroom process quality: How do they interact in promoting science understanding? American Educational Research Journal, 52, 1133–1159. DeCuir-Gunby, J. T. (2009). A review of the racial identity development of African American adolescents: The role of education. Review of Educational Research, 79(1), 103–124. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0034654308325897 Defend Innocence. (2021). 7 ways teachers con help prevent child sexual abuse. https://defendinnocence.org /child-sexual-abuse-risk-reduction/proactive-parenting/build-support/7-ways-teachers-can-help -prevent-child-sexual-abuse/ Del Toro, J., & Wang, M. (2020). School cultural socialization and academic performance: Examining ethnicracial identity development as a mediator among African American adolescents. Child Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13467 Delpit, L. (1995). Other people’s children: Cultural conflict in the classroom. New York Press. Delpit, L. (2003). Educators as “Seed People”: Growing a new future. Educational Researcher, 7(32), 14–21. Delpit, L. (2012). “Multiplication is for White people”: Raising expectations for other people’s children. The New Press. Demanet, J., & Van Houtte, M. (2016). Are flunkers social outcasts? A multilevel study of grade retention effects on same-grade friendships. American Educational Research Journal, 53, 745–780. Demetriou, A., Christou, C., Spanoudis, G., & Platsidou, M. (2002). The development of mental processing: Efficiency, working memory and thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(1). Demuth, K. (1990). Subject, topic, and Sesotho passive. Journal of Child Language, 17, 67–84. Dennie, D., Acharya, P., Greer, D., & Bryant, C. (2019). The impact of teacher-student relationships and classroom engagement on student growth percentiles of 7th and 8th grade students. Psychology in the Schools, 56, 765–780. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.22238 Dent, A. L., & Koenka, A. C. (2016). The relation between self-regulated learning and academic achievement across childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 28(3), 425–474. http:// doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9320-8 Denton, C. A., Tolar, T. D., Fletcher, J. M., Barth, A. E., Vaughn, S., & Francis, D. J. (2013). Effects of tier 3 intervention for students with persistent reading difficulties and characteristics of inadequate responders. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 633–648. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032581

References DeRose, L. M., Shiyko, M. P., Foster, H., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2011). Associations between menarcheal timing and behavioral developmental trajectories for girls from age 6 to age 15. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1329–1342. Derry, S. J. (1992). Beyond symbolic processing: Expanding horizons for educational psychology. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 413–419. Desautel, D. (2009). Becoming a thinking thinker: Metacognition, self-reflection, and classroom practice. Teachers College Record, 111, 1997–2020. ID Number: 15504. http://www.tcrecord.org DeSoto, K. A., & Roediger, H. (2019). Remembering the Presidents. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 138–144. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721418815685 Dettmers, S., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Kunter, M., & Baumert, J. (2010). Homework works if homework quality is high: Using multilevel modeling to predict the development of achievement in mathematics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 467–482. DeWalt, P. S. (2013). Discourse on African American/Black Identity: Engaging the expanded nigrescence theory with a diasporic consciousness. SpringerPlus, 2, 233. https://doi.org/10.1186/2193-1801-2-233 Dewey, J. (1913). Interest and effort in education. Houghton-Mifflin Diament, M. (2019, February). Inclusion increasingly the norm for students with disabilities. DiversityScoop. https://www.disabilityscoop.com/2019/02/19/inclusion-increasingly-norm/26067/ Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 335–341. http://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412453722 Diamond, A., & Ling, D. S. (2016). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 34–48. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005 Diamond, A., & Ling, D. S. (2019). Review of the evidence on, and fundamental questions about, efforts to improve executive functions, including working memory. In J. M. Novick, M. F. Bunting, M. R. Dougherty, & R. W. Engle (Eds.), Cognitive and working memory training: Perspectives from psychology, neuroscience, and human development. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780199974467.003.0008 Diamond, L. M. (2020). Gender fluidity and nonbinary gender identities among children and adolescents. Child Development Perspectives, 14(2), 110–115. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12366 Diaz-Rico, L. T. (2018). Crosscultural, language, and academic development handbook, the: A complete k–12 reference guide (6th ed.). Pearson. dickey, l. m., & Budge, S. L. (2020). Suicide and the transgender experience: A public health crisis. American Psychologist, 75(3), 380–390. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000619 Dickinson, J., Abrams, M. D., & Tokar, D. M. (2017). An examination of the applicability of social cognitive career theory for African American college students. Journal of Career Assessment, 25(1), 75–92. https:// doi.org/10.1177/1069072716658648 Diemer, M. A., Marchand, A. D., & Mistry, R. S. (2020). Charting how wealth shapes educational pathways from childhood to early adulthood: A developmental process model. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49(5), 1073–1091. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01162-4 Diemer, M. A., Mistry, R. S., Wadsworth, M. E., López, I., & Reimers, F. (2013). Best practices in conceptualizing and measuring social class in psychological research. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 13(1), 77–113. https://doi.org/10.1111/asap.12001 Digest of Educational Statistics. (2020). Number and percentage of elementary and secondary school students retained in grade, by sex, race/ethnicity, and grade level: 1994 through 2017. https://nces.ed.gov /programs/digest/d18/tables/dt18_225.90.asp?referer=raceindica.asp DiGiacomo, D., & Penuel, W. R. (2018). Organizing learning environments for relational equity with new digital media. In P. Resta & T. Laferrière (Eds.), International handbook of information technology in primary and secondary education (2nd ed., pp. 1047–1061). Springer International. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-53803-7_75-1 Dillon, K. P., & Bushman, B. (2017). Effects of exposure to gun violence in movies on children’s interest in real guns. JAMA Pediatrics, 171, 1057–1062. Dingfelder, S. F. (2005). Closing the gap for Latino patients. Monitor on Psychology, 36(1), 58–61. Dirk, J., & Schmiedek, F. (2016). Fluctuations in elementary school children’s working memory performance in the school context. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 722–739. Dixon, L. Q., Zhao, J., Shin, J.-Y., Wu, S., Su, J.-H., Burgess-Brigham, R., Gezer, M. U., & Snow, C. (2012). What we know about second language: Acquisition: A synthesis from four perspectives. Review of Educational Research, 82, 5–60. Dixson, D. D., & Worrell, F. C. (2016). Formative and summative assessment in the classroom. Theory Into Practice, 55(2), 153–159. https://doi.org/10.1080/00405841.2016.1148989 Doan, S., Schweig, J. D., & Mihaly, K. (2019). The consistency of composite ratings of teacher effectiveness: Evidence from New Mexico. American Educational Research Journal, 56(6), 2116–2146. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0002831219841369 Dobber, M., Zwart, R., Tanis, M., & van Oers, B. (2017). Literature review: The role of the teacher in inquiry-based education. Educational Research Review, 22(November), 194–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2017.09.002 Dodge, K. A. (2011). Context matters in child and family policy. Child Development, 82, 433–442. Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocial model of the development of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 39, 349–371. Doggett, A. M. (2004). ADHD and drug therapy: Is it still a valid treatment? Child Health Care, 8, 69–81. Dohn, N. B. (2016). Explaining the significance of participationist approaches for understanding students’ knowledge acquisition. Educational Psychologist, 51, 188–209.

R-17

R-18 References Doidge, N. (2015, February 6). Our amazing plastic brains. The Wall Street Journal. https://www.wsj.com/ articles/our-amazingly-plastic-brains-1423262095 Dombo, E. A., & Sabatino, C. A. (2019). Trauma care in schools: Creating safe environments for students with adverse childhood experiences. American Educator. https://www.aft.org/ae/summer2019/dombo_sabatino Domitrovich, C. E., Durlak, J., Staley, K. C., & Weissberg, R. P. (2017). Social-emotional competence: An essential factor for promoting positive adjustment and reducing risk and school children. Child Development, 88(2), 408–416. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12739 Domitrovich, C. E., Li, Y., Mathis, E., & Greenberg, M. T. (2019). Individual and organizational factors associated with teacher self-reported implementation of the PATHS curriculum. Journal of School Psychology, 76, 168–185. Donaldson, J., & Allen-Handy, A. (2020). The nature and power of conceptualizations of learning. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 545–570. Dougherty, M. R., & Robey, A. (2018). Neuroscience and education: A bridge astray? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 401–406. Douglas, K. M., & Albro, E. R. (2014). The progress and promise of the Reading for Understanding Research Initiative. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 341–355. Doyle, W. (2006). Ecological approaches to classroom management. In C. Evertson & C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook for classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues. Erlbaum. Drayer, A. M. (1979). Problems in middle and high school teaching: A handbook for student teachers and beginning teachers. Allyn & Bacon. Driscoll, M. P. (2005). Psychology of learning for instruction (3rd ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Dubarry, M., & Alves de Lima, D. (2003). Notes on Generation 1.5. http://faculty.deanza.edu/alvesdelimadiana /stories/storyReader$438 Dubinsky, J. M., Guzey, S. S., Schwartz, M. S., Roehrig, G., MacNabb, C., Schmied, A., & Cooper, J. L. (2019). Contributions of neuroscience knowledge to teachers and their practice. The Neuroscientist. https:// doi.org/10.1177/1073858419835447 Dubinsky, J. M., Roehrig, G., & Varma, S. (2013). Infusing neuroscience into teacher professional development. Educational Researcher, 42, 317–329. Duckworth, A. L. (2016). Grit: The power of passion and perseverance. Scribner. Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Self-control in school-age children. Educational Psychologist, 49(3), 199–217. http://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2014.926225 Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., & Gross, J. J. (2016). Situational strategies for self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(1), 35–55. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615623247 Duckworth, A. L., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Self-control and grit: Related but separable determinants of success. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(5), 319–325. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414541462 Duckworth, A. L., Milkman, K. L., & Laibson, D. (2018). Beyond willpower: Strategies for reducing failures of self-control. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 19(3), 102–129. https:// doi.org/10.1177/1529100618821893 Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D., & Kelly, D. R. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 1087–1101. Duckworth, A. L., & Quinn, P. D. (2009). Development and validation of the Short Grit Scale (GritS). Journal of Personality Assessment, 91, 166–174. Duckworth, A. L., Quinn, P. D., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2009). Positive predictors of teacher effectiveness. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(6), 540–547. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760903157232 Duckworth, A. L., Quinn, P. D., & Tsukayama, E. (2012). What No Child Left Behind leaves behind: The roles of IQ and self-control in predicting standardized achievement test scores and report card grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 439–451. Duckworth, A. L., Taxer, J. L., Eskreis-Winkler, L., Galla, B. M., & Gross, J. J. (2019). Self-control and academic achievement. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 373–399. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev -psych-010418-103230 Duell, O. K. (1994). Extended wait time and university student achievement. American Educational Research Journal, 31, 397–414. Dufrene, B. A., Doggett, R. A., Henington, C., & Watson, T. S. (2007). Functional assessment and intervention for disruptive classroom behaviors in preschool and Head Start classrooms. Journal of Behavioral Education, 16, 368–388. Duke, N. K., Halvorsen, A. L., Strachan, S. L., Kim, J., & Konstantopoulos, S. (2021). Putting PjBL to the test: The impact of project-based learning on second graders’ social studies and literacy learning and motivation in low-SES school settings. American Educational Research Journal, 58(1), 160–200. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0002831220929638 Dumont, H., Trautwein, U., Nagy, G., & Nagengast, B. (2014). Quality of parental homework involvement: Predictors and reciprocal relations with academic functioning in the reading domain. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 144–161. Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). Family poverty, welfare reform, and child development. Child Development, 71, 188–196. Duncan, G. J., Magnuson, K., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2017). Moving beyond correlations in assessing the consequences of poverty. Annual Review of Psychology, 68(1), 413–434. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev -psych-010416-044224 Duncker, K. (1945). On solving problems. Psychological Monographs, 58(5, Whole No. 270). Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and

References educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14(1), 4–58. https:// doi.org/10.1177/152910061245326 Dunn, D. S., & Andrews, E. E. (2015). Person-first and identity-first language: Developing psychologists’ cultural competence using disability language. American Psychologist, 70, 255–264. Dunn, R., Dunn, K., & Price, G. E. (1989). Learning Styles Inventory (LSI): An inventory for identification of how individuals in grades 3 through 12 prefer to learn. Price Systems. Duong, M. T., Pullmann, M. D., Buntain-Ricklefs, J., Lee, K., Benjamin, K. S., Nguyen, L., & Cook, C. R. (2019). Brief teacher training improves student behavior and student-teacher relationships in middle school. School Psychology, 34, 212–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000296 Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of enhanced students’ social emotional learning: A metaanalysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Development, 82, 405–432. Dusenbury, L., & Falco, M. (1995). Eleven components of effective drug abuse prevention curricula. Journal of School Health, 65, 420–425. Dux, P. E., Tombu, M. N., Harrison, S., Rogers, B. P., Tong, F., & Marois, R. (2009). Training improves multitasking performance by increasing the speed of information processing in human prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 16, 127–138. Dweck, C. S. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Routledge Press. Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. Random House. Dweck, C. S. (2013). Mindsets: How to motivate students (and yourself). Educational Horizons, 91(2), 16–21. Ebbinghaus, H. (1964). Memory (H. A. Ruger & C. E. Bussenius, Trans.). Dover. (Original work published 1885). Eberhard, D. M., Simons, G. F., & Fennig, C. D. (Eds.). (2020). Ethnologue: Languages of the world (23rd ed.). SIL International. Online: http://www.ethnologue.com. Ebersbach, M. (2009). Achieving a new dimension: Children integrate three stimulus dimensions in volume estimations. Developmental Psychology, 45, 877–883. Eccles, J., & Wigfield, A. (1985). Teacher expectations and student motivation. In J. Dusek (Ed.), Teacher expectancies (pp. 185–226). Erlbaum. Eccles, J., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 1017–1095). Wiley. Eccles, J. S., Fredricks, J. A., & Baay, P. (2015). Expectancies, values, identities, and self-regulation. In G. Oettingen & P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Self-regulation in adolescence (pp. 30–56). Cambridge University Press. Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2020). From expectancy-value theory to situated expectancy-value theory: A developmental, social cognitive, and sociocultural perspective on motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101859 Echevarría, J., & Graves, A. (2015). Sheltered content instruction: Teaching English learners with diverse abilities (5th ed.). Pearson. Echevarría, J., Vogt, M., & Short, D. J. (2018). Making content comprehensible for elementary English learners: The SIOP® Model (5th ed.). Pearson. Echeverria-Estrada, C., & Batalova, J. (2020). Chinese immigrants in the United States. Migration Policy Institute. https://www.migrationpolicy.org/article/chinese-immigrants-united-states-2018 Eddy, C. L., Huang, F. L., Cohen, D. R., Baker, K. M., Edwards, K. D., Herman, K. C., & Reinke, W. M. (2020). Does teacher emotional exhaustion and efficacy predict student discipline sanctions? School Psychology Review, 49(3), 239–255. https://doi.org/10.1080/2372966X.2020.1733340 Edwards, S. (2017). Play-based learning and intentional teaching: Forever different? Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 42, 4–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.23965/AJEC.42.2.01 Edwards, S., Mantilla, A., Grieshaber, S., Nuttall, J., & Wood, E. (2020). Converged play characteristics for early childhood education: Multi-modal, global-local, and traditional-digital. Oxford Review of Education, 46(5), 637–660. https://doi.org/10.1080/03054985.2020.1750358 Efklides, A. (2011). Interactions of metacognition with motivation and affect in self-regulated learning: The MASRL model. Educational Psychologist, 46, 6–25. Ehrenfeld, T. (2011). Reflections on mirror neurons. Observer: Association for Psychological Science, 24(3), 11–13. Ehrlich, K. B. (2020). How does the social world shape health across the lifespan? Insights and new directions. American Psychologist, 75(9), 1231–1241. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000757 Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 701–778). Wiley. Eisenberg, R., Pierce, W. D., & Cameron, J. (1999). Effects of rewards on intrinsic motivation— Negative, neutral, and positive: Comment on Deci, Koestner, and Ryan (1999). Psychological Bulletin, 125, 677–691. Eisner, E. W. (1999). The uses and limits of performance assessments. Phi Delta Kappan, 80, 658–660. Eissenberg, T. E., & Rudner, L. M. (1988). Explaining test results to parents. http://pareonline.net/getvn .asp?v=1&n=1 Eitel, A., Endres, T., & Renkl, A. (2020). Self-management as a bridge between cognitive load and selfregulated learning: The illustrative case of seductive details. Educational Psychology Review, 32(4), 1073–1087. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09559-5

R-19

R-20 References Eitel, A., & Scheiter, K. (2015). Picture or text first? Explaining sequence effects when learning with pictures and text. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 153–180. El Yaafouri, L. (2019). Identifying and supporting gifted ELLS. Edutopia. https://www.edutopia.org/Article /identifying-and-supporting-gifted-ells Elder, L., & Paul, R. (2012). Critical thinking: Tools for taking charge of your learning and your life (3rd ed.). PrenticeHall. Elias, M. J., & Schwab, Y. (2006). From compliance to responsibility: Social and emotional learning and classroom management. In C. Evertson & C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook for classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues. Erlbaum. Elias, S. M., & MacDonald, S. (2007). Using past performance, proxy efficacy, and academic self-efficacy to predict college performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 2518–2531. Elkind, D. (1981). Obituary—Jean Piaget (1896–1980). American Psychologist, 36, 911–913. Elkind, D. (1985). Egocentrism redux. Developmental Review, 5, 127–134. Ellemers, N. (2018). Gender stereotypes. Annual Review of Psychology, 69(1), 275–298. https:// doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011719 Ellington, A. J. (2003). A meta-analysis of the effects of calculators on students’ achievement and attitude levels in precollege mathematics classes. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 34, 433–463. Ellington, A. J. (2013). The impact of calculators on student achievement in the K–12 mathematics classroom. In J. Hattie & E. M. Anderman (Eds.), International guide to student achievement (pp. 303–306). Routledge. Emdin, C. (2016). For white folks who teach in the hood … and the rest of y’all too: Reality pedagogy and urban education. Beacon Press. Emdin, C., Adjapong, E., & Levy, I. (2016). Hip-hop based interventions as pedagogy/therapy in STEM: A model for urban science education. Journal for Multicultural Education, 10(3), 307–321. https:// doi.org/10.1108/JME-03-2016-0023 Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 148–168. Emmer, E. T., & Evertson, C. M. (2017). Classroom management for middle and high school teachers (10th ed.). Pearson. Emmer, E. T., Evertson, C. M., & Anderson, L. M. (1980). Effective classroom management at the beginning of the school year. Elementary School Journal, 80, 219–231. Emmer, E. T., & Gerwels, M. C. (2006). Classroom management in middle school and high school classrooms. In C. Evertson & C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook for classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues. Erlbaum. Emmer, E. T., & Stough, L. M. (2001). Classroom management: A critical part of educational psychology with implications for teacher education. Educational Psychologist, 36, 103–112. Engel de Abreu, P. M. J., & Gathercole, S. (2012). Executive and phonological processes in second language acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 974–986. Engle, R. W. (2001). What is working memory capacity? In H. Roediger, J. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. Suprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 297–314). American Psychological Association. Epilepsy Foundation. (2017). Types of seizures. https://www.epilepsy.com/learn/types-seizures Epstein, J. L., & Van Voorhis, F. L. (2001). More than minutes: Teachers’ roles in designing homework. Educational Psychologist, 36, 181–193. Epstein, J. L., & Van Voorhis, F. L. (2012). The changing debate: From assigning homework to designing homework. In S. Suggate & E. Reese (Eds.), Contemporary debates in child development and education (pp. 263–273). Routledge. https://www.routledge.com/Contemporary-Debates-in-Childhood-Education-and -Development-1st-Edition/Suggate-Reese/p/book/9780415614900 Erdelyi, M. H. (2010). The ups and downs of memory. American Psychologist, 65, 623–633. Erickson, W., Lee, C., & von Schrader, S. (2013). Disability statistics from the 2011 American Community Survey (ACS). Cornell University Employment and Disability Institute (EDI). https://www.disabilitystatistics .org/StatusReports/2011-PDF/2011-StatusReport_US.pdf Ericsson, A. (2011, August). Deliberate practice and the future of education and professional training. Keynote address at the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, University of Exeter, UK. Ericsson, A., & Pool, R. (2016). Peak: Secrets from the new science of expertise. Houghton Mifflin. Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1999). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. In S. Ceci & W. Williams (Eds.), The nature-nurture debate: The essential readings Series: Essential readings in developmental psychology (pp. 200–256). Blackwell. Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). Norton. Erikson, E. H. (1980). Identity and the life cycle (2nd ed.). Norton. Erin’s Law (2021). What is Erin’s Law? http://www.erinslaw.org/erins-law/ Eskreis-Winkler, L., & Fishbach, A. (2019). Not learning from failure—The greatest failure of all. Psychological Science, 30, 1733–1744. Eskreis-Winkler, L., Shulman, E. P., Beal, S. A., & Duckworth, A. L. (2014). The grit effect: Predicting retention in the military, the workplace, school and marriage. Frontiers in Psychology: Personality Science and Individual Differences, 5(36), 1–12. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00036 Espinosa, L. M. (2008). Challenging common myths about young English language learners. Foundation for Child Development. http://fcd-us.org/sites/default/files/MythsOfTeachingELLsEspinosa.pdf Ethnologue. (2020). How many languages are there in the world? https://www.ethnologue.com/guides /how-many-languages

References Ettekal, I., & Ladd, G. W. (2015). Costs and benefits of children’s physical and relational aggression trajectories on peer rejection, acceptance, and friendships: Variations by aggression subtypes, gender, and age. Developmental Psychology, 51, 1756–1770. Evans, G. W. (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. American Psychologist, 59, 77–92. Evans, G. W., & Kim, P. (2013). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 43–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12013 Everson, K. C. (2017). Value-Added Modeling and educational accountability: Are we answering the real questions? Review of Educational Research, 87(1), 35–70. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654316637199 Evertson, C. M., & Emmer, E. T. (2017). Classroom management for elementary school teachers (10th ed.). Pearson. Evertson, C. M., & Weinstein, C. S. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues. Erlbaum. Eysenck, M. W. (2012). Fundamentals of cognition (2nd ed.). Taylor and Francis. Fabes, R. A., Martin, C. L., & Hanish, L. D. (2019). Gender integration and the promotion of inclusive classroom climates. Educational Psychologist, 54(4), 271–285. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2019.1631826 Fabiano, G. A., Pelham, W. E., Coles, E. K., Gnagy, E. M., Chronis-Tuscano, A., & O’Connor, B. C. (2009). A meta-analysis of behavioral treatments for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 129–140. Facione, P. A. (2011). Think critically. Pearson. Fackler, S., Malmberg, L. E., & Sammons, P. (2021). An international perspective on teacher self-efficacy: Personal, structural and environmental factors. Teaching and Teacher Education, 99. https://doi .org/10.1016/j.tate.2020.103255 Falk, A., Kosse, F., & Pinger, P. (2020). Re-revisiting the Marshmallow Test: A direct comparison of studies by Shoda, Mischel, and Peake (1990) and Watts, Duncan, and Quan (2018). Psychological Science, 31(1), 100–104. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619861720 Fantuzzo, J., Davis, G., & Ginsburg, M. (1995). Effects of parent involvement in isolation or in combination with peer tutoring on student self-concept and mathematics achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 272–281. Farrington, C., Roderick, M., Allensworth, E., Nagaoka, J., Keyes, T. S., Johnson, D., & Beechum, N. O. (2012). Teaching adolescents to become learners: The role of noncognitive factors in shaping school performance. University of Chicago Consortium on Chicago School Research. Fauth, B., Wagner, W., Bertram, C., Göllner, R., Roloff, J., Lüdtke, O., Polikoff, M. S., Klusmann, U., & Trautwein, U. (2020). Don’t blame the teacher? The need to account for classroom characteristics in evaluations of teaching quality. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(6), 1284–1302. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000416 Fazio, L. K., & Marsh, E. J. (2019). Retrieval-based learning in children. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(2), 111–116. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721418806673 Feldman, R. S. (2004). Child development (3rd ed.). Prentice-Hall. Feldon, D. F., Callan, G., Juth, S., & Jeong, S. (2019). Cognitive load as motivational cost. Educational Psychology Review, 31(2), 319–337. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09464-6 Fenesi, B., Sana, F., Kim, J. A., & Shore, D. I. (2015). Reconceptualizing working memory in educational research. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 333–351. Fenton, D. F. (2007). The implications of research on expertise for curriculum and pedagogy. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 91–110. Ferguson, C. J. (2015a, October 13). The Blog: Do angry birds make for angry children? http://www .huffingtonpost.com/christopher-j-ferguson/do-angry-birds-make-for-angry-children_b_8276610.html Ferguson, C. J. (2015b). Do angry birds make for angry children? A meta-analysis of video game influences on children’s and adolescents’ aggression, mental health, prosocial behavior, and academic performance. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 646–666. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615592234 Ferguson, C. J., Barr, H., Figueroa, G., Foley, K., Gallimore, A., LaQuea, R., Merritt, A., Miller, S., NguyenPham, H., Spanogle, C., Stevens, J., Trigani, B., & Garza, A. (2015). Digital poison? Three studies examining the influence of violent video games on youth. Computers in Human Behavior, 50, 399–410. Ferguson, R. F. (2008). The TRIPOD Project framework. Harvard University. Fernandes, M. A., Wammes, J. D., & Meade, M. E. (2018). The surprisingly powerful influence of drawing on memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 302–308. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0963721418755385 Fernet, C., Guay, F., Senécal, C., & Austin, S. (2012). Predicting intraindividual changes in teacher burnout: The role of perceived school environment and motivational factors. Teaching and Teacher Education, 28, 514–525. Ferrer, E., & McArdle, J. J. (2004). An experimental analysis of dynamic hypotheses about cognitive abilities and achievement from childhood to early adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 40, 935–952. Ferretti, R. P., Lewis, W. E., & Andrews-Weckerly, S. (2009). Do goals affect the structure of students’ argumentative writing strategies? Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 577–589. Finkel, D., Reynolds, C. A., McArdle, J. J., Gatz, M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2003). Latent growth curve analyses of accelerating decline in cognitive abilities in adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 39, 535–550. Finley, T. (2017). Making sure your praise is effective. Edutopia. https://www.edutopia.org/article/makingsure-your-praise-effective Finn, B., Thomas, R., & Rawson, K. (2018). Learning more from feedback: Elaborating feedback with examples enhances concept learning. Learning and Instruction, 54, 104–113. Finnilä, K., Mahlberga, N., Santtilaa, P., & Niemib, P. (2003). Validity of a test of children's suggestibility for predicting responses to two interview situations differing in degree of suggestiveness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85, 32–49.

R-21

R-22 References Fiore, S. M., Graesser, A., & Greiff, S. (2018). Collaborative problem-solving education for the twenty-firstcentury workforce. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(6), 367–369. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0363-y Fiorella, L. (2020). The science of habit and its implications for student learning and well-being. Educational Psychology Review, 32(3), 603–625. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09525-1 Fiorella, L., & Kuhlmann, S. (2020). Creating drawings enhances learning by teaching. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(4), 811–822. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000392 Fiorella, L., & Mayer, R. E. (2016). Eight ways to promote generative learning. Educational Psychology Review, 28(4), 717–741. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9348-9 Fiorella, L., & Zhang, Q. (2018). Drawing boundary conditions for learning by drawing. Educational Psychology Review, 30, 1115–1137. Firth, E., Elbich, D. B., Christensen, A. P., Rosenberg, M. D., Chen, Q., Kane, M. J., Silvia, P. J., Seli, P., & Beaty, R. E. (2021). Intelligence and creativity share a common cognitive and neural basis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General,150, 609–632. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000958 Fischer, K. W. (2009). Mind, brain, and education: Building a scientific groundwork for learning and teaching. Mind, Brain, and Education, 3, 2–16. Fiske, S. T. (1993). Social cognition and social perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 155–194. Fiske, S. T. (2018). Stereotype content: Warmth and competence endure. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(2), 67–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417738825 Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Brooks Cole. Fitzgerald, M. S., & Palincsar, A. S. (2019). Teaching practices that support student sensemaking across grades and disciplines: A conceptual review. Review of Research in Education, 43(1), 227–248. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0091732X18821115 Fitzmaurice, H., Flynn, M., & Hanafin, J. (2020). Primary teachers' homework practices: Identity, expectations, policies and cultural values. Issues in Educational Research, 30(3), 897–919. http://www.iier.org.au /iier30/fitzmaurice.pdf Fives, H., Hamman, D. & Olivárez, A. (2007). Does burnout begin with student-teaching? Analyzing efficacy, burnout, and support during the student-teaching semester. Teaching and Teaching Education. 23, 916–934. Fives, H., & Barnes, N. (2020). Navigating the complex cognitive task of classroom assessment. Teaching and Teacher Education, 92, 103063. Flanigan, A. E., & Titsworth, S. (2020). The impact of digital distraction on lecture note taking and student learning. Instructional Science, 48(5), 495–524. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-020-09517-2 Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1995). Young children’s knowledge about thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60(1) (Serial No. 243). Flavell, J. H., Miller, P. H., & Miller, S. A. (2002). Cognitive development (4th ed.). Prentice-Hall. Fleith, D. (2000). Teacher and student perceptions of creativity in the classroom environment. Roeper Review, 22, 148–153. Fletcher, A., Bonell, C., & Hargreaves, J. (2008). School effects on young people’s drug use: A systematic review of intervention and observational studies. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42, 209–220. Floden, R. E. (2001). Research on effects of teaching: A continuing model for research on teaching. In V. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (4th ed., pp. 3–16). American Educational Research Association. Flom, R., & Pick, A. D. (2003). Verbal encouragement and joint attention in 18-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 121–134. Florit, E., & Cain, K. (2011). The simple view of reading: Is it valid for different types of alphabetic orthographies? Educational Psychology Review, 23, 553–576. Flower, A., McKenna, J. W., Bunuan, R. L., Muething, C. S., & Vega, Jr., R. (2014). Effects of the good behavior game on challenging behaviors in school settings. Review of Educational Research, 84, 546–571. FluencyCORP. (2020, April). American English dialects. https://fluencycorp.com/american-english-dialects/ Flunger, B., Trautwein, U., Nagengast, B., Lüdtke, O., Niggli, A., & Schnyder, I. (2015). The Janus-faced nature of time spent on homework: Using latent profile analyses to predict academic achievement over a school year. Learning and Instruction, 39, 97–106. Flynn, J. R. (2012). Are we getting smarter? Rising IQ in the twenty-first century. Cambridge University Press. Foerde, K., Knowlton, B. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science, 103, 11778–11783. Fogarty, M., Oslund, E., Simmons, D., Davis, J., Simmons, L., Anderson, L., Clemens, N., & Roberts, G. (2014). Examining the effectiveness of a multicomponent reading comprehension intervention in middle schools: A focus on treatment fidelity. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 425–449. Fong, C. J., Patall, E., Vasquez, A. C., & Stautberg, S. (2019). A meta-analysis if negative feedback on intrinsic motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 121–162. Francis, D. J., Lesaux, N., & August, D. (2006). Language of instruction. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language Minority Children and Youth (pp. 365–413). Erlbaum. Frankenhuis, W. E., & Nettle, D. (2020). The strengths of people in poverty. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(1), 16–21. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419881154 Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, 59–109. Freeman, S. (2011). Top 10 myths about the brain, How Stuff Works. http://health.howstuffworks.com /human-body/systems/nervous-system/10-brain-myths.htm Freiberg, H. J., & Driscoll, A. (2005). Universal teaching strategies (4th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Freiberg, J. (2006). Research-based programs for preventing and solving discipline problems. In C. Evertson & C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook for classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues. Erlbaum.

References Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., Lüdtke, O., Pekrun, R., & Sutton, R. E. (2009). Emotional transmission in the classroom: Exploring the relationship between teacher and student enjoyment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 705–716. Frey, N., & Fisher, D. (2010). Reading and the brain: What early childhood educators need to know. Early Childhood Education Journal, 38, 103–110. Frey, W. H. (2018). The US will become “minority white” in 2045, Census projects. Brookings. https://www.brookings .edu/blog/the-avenue/2018/03/14/the-us-will-become-minority-white-in-2045-census-projects/ Friedman-Weieneth, J. L., Harvey, E. A., Youngswirth, S. D., & Goldstein, L. H. (2007). The relation between 3-year-old children’s skills and their hyperactivity, inattention, and aggression. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 671–681. Friend, M. (2018). Special education: Contemporary perspectives for school professionals (5th ed.). Pearson. Friend, M., & Bursuck, W. D. (2012). Including students with special needs: A practical guide for classroom teachers (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon/Pearson. Friend, M., & Bursuck, W. D. (2019). Including students with special needs: A practical guide for classroom teachers (8th ed.). Pearson. Frith, E., Elbich, D. B., Christensen, A. P., Rosenberg, M. D., Chen, Q., Kane, M. J., Silvia, P. J., Seli, P., & Beaty, R. E. (2020). Intelligence and creativity share a common cognitive and neural basis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000958. Frith, E., Elbich, D. B., Christensen, A. P., Rosenberg, M. D., Chen, Q., Kane, M. J., Silvia, P. J., Seli, P., & Beaty, R. E. (2021). Intelligence and creativity share a common cognitive and neural basis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(4), 609–632. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000958 Fromberg, D. P., & Driscoll, M. (2011). The successful classroom: Management strategies for regular and special education teachers. Scholastic. Fronius, T., Persson, H., Guckenburg, S., Hurley, N., & Petrosino, A. (2016, February). Restorative Justice in U.S. schools: A research review. WestEd Justice and Prevention Research Center. http://jprc.wested .org/wp-content/uploads/2016/02/RJ_Literature-Review_20160217.pdf Fuchs, L. S., Powell, S. R., Cirino, P. T., Schumacher, R. F., Marrin, S., Hamlett, C. L., Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., & Changas, P. C. (2014). Does calculation or word-problem instruction provide a stronger route to prealgebraic knowledge? Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 990–1006. Fuligni, A. J., & Tsai, K. M. (2015). Developmental flexibility in the age of globalization: Autonomy and identity development among adolescents. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 411–431. Fuller, B., Kim, Y., Galindo, C., Bathia, S., Bridges, M., Duncan, G. J., & García Valdivia, I. (2019). Worsening school segregation for Latino children? Educational Researcher, 48(7), 407–420. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0013189X19860814 Fuller-Thomson, E., & Dalton, A. (2011, January 5). Suicidal ideation among individuals whose parents have divorced: Findings from a representative Canadian community survey. Psychiatry Research. http://www .ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21251718 Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a factor in children’s academic engagement and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(11), 148–161. Furtak, E. M., Seidel, T., Iverson, H., & Briggs, D. C. (2012). Experimental and quasi-experimental studies of inquiry-based science teaching: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 82, 300–329. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0034654312457206 Fyfe, E. R., McNeil, N. M., & Borjas, S. (2015). Benefits of “concreteness fading” for children’s mathematics understanding. Learning and Instruction, 35, 104–120. Fyfe, E. R., Rittle-Johnson, B., & DeCaro, M. S. (2012). The effects of feedback during exploratory mathematics problem solving: Prior knowledge matters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 1094–1108. https:// doi.org/10.1037/a0028389 Gage, N. A., Karson, A., Sugai, G., & Chafouleas, S. M. (2016). Student perceptions of school climate as predictors of office discipline referrals. American Educational Research Journal, 53, 492–515. Gagné, E. D. (1985). The cognitive psychology of school learning. Little, Brown. Gagné, E. D., Yekovich, C. W., & Yekovich, F. R. (1993). The cognitive psychology of school learning (2nd ed.). Harper-Collins. Gaias, L. M., Lindstrom Johnson, S., Bottiani, J. H., Debnam, K. J., & Bradshaw, C. P. (2019). Examining teachers' classroom management profiles: Incorporating a focus on culturally responsive practice. Journal of School Psychology, 76, 124–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2019.07.017 Galla, B., Shulman, E., Plummer, B. D., Gardner, M., Hutt, S., Goyer, J. P., D'Mello, S. K., Finn, A. S., & Duckworth, A. (2019). Why high school grades are better predictors of on-time college graduation than are admissions test scores: The roles of self-regulation and cognitive ability. American Educational Research Journal, 56, 2077–2115. Galliher, R. V., McLean, K. C., & Syed, M. (2017). An integrated developmental model for studying identity content in context. Developmental Psychology, 53(11), 2011–2022. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000299 Gallimore, R., & Goldenberg, C. (2001). Analyzing cultural models and settings to connect minority achievement and school improvement research. Educational Psychologist, 36, 45–56. Galton, M., Hargreaves, L., & Pell, T. (2009). Group work and whole-class teaching with 11–14-year-olds compared. Cambridge Journal of Education, 39, 119–140. Gándara, P. (2018). Immigrant students: Our kids, our future. Learning Policy Institute. https:// learningpolicyinstitute.org/blog/immigrant-students-our-kids-our-future? Ganis, G., Thompson, W. L., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2004). Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: An fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 226–241. Garbarino, J., & deLara, E. (2002). And words can hurt forever: How to protect adolescents from bullying, harassment, and emotional violence. Free Press.

R-23

R-24 References Garcia, E. E. (1992). “Hispanic” children: Theoretical, empirical, and related policy issues. Educational Psychology Review, 4, 69–94. Garcia, E. E. (2002). Student cultural diversity: Understanding the meaning and meeting the challenge. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. Garcia, S. B., & Tyler, B.-J. (2010). Meeting the needs of English language learners with learning disabilities in the general curriculum. Theory Into Practice, 49, 113–120. Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. Basic Books. Gardner, H. (1998). Reflections on multiple intelligences: Myths and messages. In A. Woolfolk (Ed.), Readings in educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 61–67). Allyn & Bacon. Gardner, H. (1999). Are there additional intelligences? The case for the naturalist, spiritual, and existential intelligences. In J. Kane (Ed.), Educational information and transformation (pp. 25–40). Prentice-Hall. Gardner, H. (2003, April 21). Multiple intelligence after twenty years. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, IL. Gardner, H. (2009). Birth and the spreading of a meme. In J.-Q. Chen, S. Moran, & H. Gardner (Eds.), Multiple intelligences around the world (pp. 3–16). Wiley. Gardner, H. (2011). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences (4th ed.). Basic Books. Gardner, H., & Hatch, T. (1989), Multiple intelligencs go to school. Educational Researcher, 18(8), 4–10. Gargiulo, R. M., & Bouck, E. (2021). Special education in contemporary society: An introduction to exceptionality (7th ed.). Sage Publishing. Garlick, D. (2002). Understanding the nature of the general factor of intelligence: The role of individual differences in neural plasticity as an explanatory mechanism. Psychological Review, 109, 116–136. Garner, P. W., & Spears, F. M. (2000). Emotion regulation in low-income preschool children. Social Development, 9, 246–264. Garnets, L. (2002). Sexual orientations in perspective. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 8, 115–129. Garrison, J. (1995). Deweyan pragmatism and the epistemology of contemporary social constructivism. American Educational Research Journal, 32, 716–741. Garzón, J., & Acevedo, J. (2019). Meta-analysis of the impact of Augmented Reality on students’ learning gains. Educational Research Review, 27, 244–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2019.04.001 Gaskill, P. (Ed.). (2013). Special issue: Fifty years of Theory Into Practice: Learning from the past, looking to the future. Theory Into Practice, 52, 1–150. Gaspard, H., Lauermann, F., Rose, N., Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2020). Cross-domain trajectories of students’ ability self-concepts and intrinsic values in math and language arts. Child Development, 91(5), 1800–1818. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13343 Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190. Gay, G. (2006). Connections between classroom management and culturally responsive teaching. In C. Evertson & C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook for classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues. Erlbaum. Gay, G. (2010). Culturally responsive teaching: Theory, research, and practice (2nd ed.). Teachers College Press. Geary, D. C. (1995). Sexual selection and sex differences in spatial cognition. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 289–303. Gee, J. P. (2008). Learning and games. In K. Salen (Ed.), The ecology of games: Connecting youth, games, and learning (pp. 21–40). The MIT Press, The John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Series on Digital Media and Learning. https://doi.org/10.1162/dma1.9780262693646.021 Gehlbach, H. (2004). A new perspective on perspective taking: A multidimensional approach to conceptualizing an aptitude. Educational Psychology Review, 16, 207–234. Geier, R., Blumenfeld, P., Marx, R., Krajcik, J., Fishman, B., Soloway, E., & Clay-Chambers, J. (2008). Standardized test outcomes for students engaged in inquiry based science curriculum in the context of urban reform. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 45, 922–939. Gelman, R. (2000). The epigenesis of mathematical thinking. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 27–37. Gelman, R., & Cordes, S. A. (2001). Counting in animals and humans. In E. Dupoux (Ed.), Essay in honor of Jacques Mehler. MIT Press. Gentner, D., Loewenstein, J., & Thompson, L. (2003). Learning and transfer: A general role for analogical encoding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 393–408. Gentrup, S., Lorenz, G., Kristen, C., & Kogan, I. (2020). Self-fulfilling prophecies in the classroom: Teacher expectations, teacher feedback and student achievement. Learning and Instruction, 66, 101296. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.101296 Genzlinger, N. (2018, March 26). Linda Brown, symbol of landmark desegregation case, dies at 75. New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2018/03/26/obituaries/linda-brown-symbol-of-landmark -desegregation-case-dies.html George, P. S. (2005). A rationale for differentiated instruction in the regular classroom. Theory Into Practice, 44, 185–193. Gerber, J. P., Wheeler, L., & Suls, J. (2018). A social comparison theory meta-analysis 60+ years on. Psychological Bulletin, 144(2), 177–197. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000127 Gershenson, S., & Holt, S. B. (2015). Gender gaps in high school students’ homework time. Educational Researcher, 44, 432–441. Gershenson, S., Holt, S. B., & Papageorge, N. (2016). Who believes in me? The effect of student-teacher demographic match on teacher expectations. Economics of Education Review, 52, 209–224. Gershoff, E. T., Goodman, G. S., Miller-Perrin, C., Holden, G. W., Jackson, Y., & Kazdin, A. E. (2019). There is still no evidence that physical punishment is effective or beneficial: Reply to Larzelere, Gunnoe, Ferguson, and Roberts (2019) and Rohner and Melendez-Rhodes (2019). American Psychologist, 74, 503–505. https:// doi.org/10.1037/amp0000474

References Gersten, R. (1996). Literacy instruction for language-minority students: The transition years. The Elementary School Journal, 96, 217–220. Gersten, R., Baker, S. K., Shanahan, T., Linan-Thompson, S., Collins, P., & Scarcella, R. (2007). Effective literacy and English language instruction for English learners in the elementary grades. IES Practice Guide. What Works Clearinghouse. Gestwicki, C. (2017). Developmentally appropriate practice: Curriculum and development in early education (6th ed.). Cengage Learning. Gibson, D., Aldrich, C., & Prensky, M. (Eds.). (2006). Games and simulations in online learning: Research and development frameworks. Information Science Publishing. Gierl, M. J., Bulut, O., Guo, Q., & Zhang, X. (2017). Developing, analyzing, and using distractors for multiple-choice tests in education: A comprehensive review. Review of Educational Research, 87(6), 1082–1116. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654317726529 Gillen-O’Neel, C., & Fuligni, A. (2013). A longitudinal study of school belonging and academic motivation across high school. Child Development, 84, 678–692. Gillet, J. W., Temple, C., & Crawford. A. (2017), Understanding reading problems (9th ed.). Pearson. Gilliam, W. S., Maupin, A. N., Reyes, C. R., Accavitti, M., & Shic, F. (2016). Do early educators’ implicit biases regarding sex and race relate to behavior expectations and recommendations of preschool expulsions and suspensions? (A research study brief). Yale University Child Study Center. http://ziglercenter.yale.edu/ Gillies, R., & Boyle, M. (2011). Teachers’ reflections of cooperative learning (CL): A two-year follow-up. Teaching Education, 22, 63–78. Gillies, R. M. (2003). The behaviors, interactions, and perceptions of junior high school students during small-group learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 15–22. Gillies, R. M. (2004). The effects of cooperative learning on junior high school students during small group learning. Learning and Instruction, 14, 197–213. Gillies, R. M. (2014). Cooperative learning: Developments in research. International Journal of Educational Psychology, 3(2), 125–140. Gillies, R. M. (2016). Dialogic interactions in the cooperative classroom. International Journal of Educational Research, 76, 178e189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2015.02.009 Gillies, R. M., & Rafter, M. (2020). Using visual, embodied, and language representations to teach the 5E instructional model of inquiry science. Teaching and Teacher Education, 87, 102951. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2019.10295 Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development. Harvard University Press. Gilligan, C., & Attanucci, J. (1988). Two moral orientations: Gender differences and similarities. MerrillPalmer Quarterly, 34, 223–237. Giluk, T. L., & Postlethwaite, B. E. (2015). Big Five personality and academic dishonesty: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Individual Differences, 72, 59–67. Ginsburg, K. R. (2007). The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child bonds. Pediatrics, 119, 182–191. Gladwell, M. (2008). Outliers: The study of success. Little, Brown, and Company. Glasser, W. (1969). Schools without failure. Harper & Row. Glăveanu, V. P. (2018). Educating which creativity? Thinking Skills and Creativity 27, 25–32. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.11.006 Glăveanu, V. P., Hanson, M. H., Baer, J., Barbot, B., Clapp, E. P., Corazza, G., Hennessey, B., Kaufman, J., Lebuda, I., Lubart, T., Montuori, A., Ness, I. J., Plucker, J., Reiter-Palmon, R., Sierra, Z., Simonton, D. K., Neves-Pereira, M. S., & Sternberg, R. (2020). Advancing creativity theory and research: A socio-cultural manifesto. Journal of Creative Behavior, 54, 741–745. Glazewski, K. D., & Ertmer, P. A. (2020). Fostering complex problem solving for diverse learners: Engaging an ethos of intentionality toward equitable access. Educational Technology Research and Development, 68(2), 679–702. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-020-09762-9 Gleitman, H., Fridlund, A. J., & Reisberg, D. (1999). Psychology (5th ed.). Norton. GLSEN. (2019). Safe Space kit: A guide to supporting lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender and queer students in your school. https://www.glsen.org/sites/default/files/2019-11/GLSEN%20English%20SafeSpace%20 Book%20Text%20Updated%202019.pdf Gluck, M. A., Mercado, E., & Myers, C. E. (2020). Learning and memory: From brain to behavior (4th ed.). MacMillan. Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: On land and underwater. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 325–331. Goetz, T., Cronjaeger, H., Frenzel, A. C., Ludtke, O., & Hall, N. (2010). Academic self-concept and emotion relations: Domain specificity and age effects. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 44–58. Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Hall, N. C., & Pekrun, R. (2008). Antecedents of academic emotions: Testing the internal/external frame of reference model for academic enjoyment. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 9–33. Goetz, T., Keller, M. M., Lüdtke, O., Nett, U. E., & Lipnevich, A. A. (2020). The dynamics of real-time classroom emotions: Appraisals mediate the relation between students’ perceptions of teaching and their emotions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(6), 1243–1260. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000415 Goetz, T., Sticca, F., Pekrun, R., Murayama, K., & Elliot, A. J. (2016). Intraindividual relations between achievement goals and discrete achievement emotions: An experience sampling approach. Learning and Instruction, 41, 115–125. Golann, J. W., Debs, M., & Weiss, A. L. (2019). “To Be Strict on Your Own”: Black and Latinx Parents Evaluate Discipline in Urban Choice Schools. American Educational Research Journal, 56(5), 1896–1929. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0002831219831972

R-25

R-26 References Goldenberg, C. (1996). The education of language-minority students: Where are we, and where do we need to go? The Elementary School Journal, 96, 353–361. Goldenberg, C., Tolar, T. D., Reese, L., Francis, D. J., Bazán, A. R., & Mejia-Arauz, R. (2014). How important is teaching phonemic awareness to children learning to read in Spanish? American Educational Research Journal, 51, 604–633. Goldhaber, D., & Özek, U. (2019). How much should we rely on student test achievement as a measure of success? Educational Researcher, 48, 479–483. Goldhaber, D., Quince, V., & Theobald, R. (2018). Has it always been this way? Tracing the evolution of teacher quality gaps in U.S. public schools. American Educational Research Journal, 55(1), 171–201. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0002831217733445 Goldman, S. R., Lawless, K., Pellegrino, J. W., & Plants, R. (2006). Technology for teaching and learning with understanding. In J. Cooper (Ed.), Classroom teaching skills (8th ed., pp. 104–150). Houghton-Mifflin. Goldstein, L. S., & Lake, V. E. (2000). “Love, love, and more love for children”: Exploring preservice teachers’ understandings of caring. Teaching and Teacher Education, 16, 861–872. Goldstone, R. L., & Day, S. B. (2012). Introduction to “New Conceptualizations of Transfer of Learning.” Educational Psychologist, 47, 149–152. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2012.695710 Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence: Why it can matter more than IQ. Bantam Books. Goleman, D. (2015). The future of SEL. In J. A. Durlak, C. E. Domitrovich, R. P. Weissberg, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Handbook of social and emotional learning: Research and practice (pp. 593–596). Guilford Press. Golombok, S., Rust, J., Zervoulis, K., Croudace, T., Golding, J., & Hines, M. (2008). Developmental trajectories of sex-typed behavior in boys and girls: A longitudinal general population study of children aged 2.5–8 years. Child Development, 79, 1583–1593. Gonzales, N., Moll, L. C., Floyd-Tenery, M., Rivera, A., Rendon, P., Gonzales, R., & Amanti, C. (1993). Teacher research on funds of knowledge: Learning from households. The Georgetown University National Center for Research on Cultural Diversity and Second Language Learning. http://www.ncela.gwu.edu/pubs/ncrcdsll/epr6.htm Gonzalez, A. L. (2010, June). Hispanics in the US: A new generation. BBC News: US and Canada. http://www .bbc.co.uk/news/10209099 González, J. M. (2011). Words were all we had. In M. de la Luz Reyes (Ed.), Words were all we had: Becoming biliterate against the odds (pp. 26–35). Teachers College Press. Gonzalez, N., Moll, L. C., & Amanti, C. (2005). Funds of knowledge: Theorizing practices in households and classrooms. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. González, T., Sattler, H., & Buth, A. J. (2018). New directions in whole- school restorative justice implementation. Conflict Resolution Quarterly, 36, 207–220. https://doi.org/10.1002/crq.21236 Gonzalez, V. (1999). Language and cognitive development in second language learning: Educational implications for children and adults. Allyn & Bacon. Gonzalez, V., Brusca-Vega, R., & Yawkey, T. (1997). Assessment and instruction of culturally diverse students with or at-risk of learning problems: From research to practice. Allyn & Bacon. Good, T. L. (1983). Classroom research: A decade of progress. Educational Psychologist, 18, 127–144. Good, T. L. (1988). Teacher expectations. In D. Berliner & B. Rosenshine (Eds.), Talks to teachers (pp. 159–200). Random House. Good, T. L. (1996). Teaching effects and teacher evaluation. In J. Sikula (Ed.), Handbook of research on teacher education (pp. 617–665). Macmillan. Good, T. L. (2014). What do we know about how teachers influence student performance on standardized tests: And why do we know so little about other student outcomes? Teachers College Record, 116(1). http:// www.tcrecord.org ID Number: 17289 Good, T. L., Biddle, B., & Brophy, J. (1975). Teachers make a difference. Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. E. (2008). Looking in classrooms (10th ed.). Longman. Good, T. L., & Grouws, D. (1975). Teacher rapport: Some stability data. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67(2), 179–182. Goodrich, J. M., Lonigan, C. J., & Farver, J. M. (2013). Do early literacy skills in children’s first language promote development of skills in their second language? An experimental evaluation of transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 414–426. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031780 Gorrel, S., & Murray, S. B. (2019). Eating disorders in males. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 28(4), 461–651. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2019.05.012 Gorski, P. (2013). Reaching and teaching students in poverty: Strategies for erasing the opportunity gap. Teachers College Press. Gorski, P. C. (2016). Poverty and the ideological imperative: A call to unhook from deficit and grit ideology and to strive for structural ideology in teacher education. Journal of Education for Teaching, 42(4), 378–386. https://doi.org/10.1080/02607476.2016.1215546 Goswami, U. (2004). Neuroscience, education, and special education. British Journal of Special Education, 31, 175–183. Gottlieb, G., Wahlsten, D., & Lickliter, R. (2006). The significance of biology for human development: A developmental psychobiological systems view. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 1: Theoretical models of human development, pp. 210–257). Wiley. Goupil, L., & Kouider, S. (2019). Developing a reflective mind: From core metacognition to explicit self-reflection. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 403–408. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0963721419848672 Graesser, A. C. (2013). Evolution of advanced learning technologies in the 21st Century. Theory Into Practice, 52, 93–101. Graesser, A. C. (2020a). Emotions are the experiential glue of learning environments in the 21st century. Learning and Instruction, 70, 101212.

References Graesser, A. C. (2020b). Learning science principles and technologies with agents that promote deep learning. In R. Feldman (Ed.), Learning science: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 2–33). McGraw Hill. Graesser, A. C., Fiore, S. M., Greiff, S., Andrews-Todd, J., Foltz, P. W., & Hesse, F. W. (2018). Advancing the science of collaborative problem solving. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 19(2), 59–92. https:// doi.org/10.1177/1529100618808244 Graham, C. R., Borup, J., Pulham, E., & Larsen, R. (2017). K–12 blended teaching readiness: Phase 1-instrument development. Michigan Virtual University. https://mvlri.org/research/publications/k12-blended -teaching-readiness-instrument-development/ Graham, L. J., White, S. L. J., Cologon, K., & Pianta, R. C. (2020). Do teachers’ years of experience make a difference in the quality of teaching? Teaching and Teacher Education, 96, 103–190. Graham, S. (1996). How causal beliefs influence the academic and social motivation of African-American children. In G. G. Brannigan (Ed.), The enlightened educator: Research adventures in the schools (pp. 111–126). McGraw-Hill. Graham, S. (2016). Victims of bullying in schools. Theory Into Practice, 55, 136–144. Graham, S. (2018). Race/ethnicity and social adjustment of adolescents: How (not if) school diversity matters. Educational Psychologist, 53, 64–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2018.1428805 Graham, S. (2020). An attributional theory of motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101861 Graham, S., & Taylor, A. (2002). Ethnicity, gender, and the development of achievement values. In A. Wigfield & J. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 121–146). Academic Press. Graham, S., & Weiner, B. (2012). Motivation: Past, present, and future. In K. R. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA Educational Psychology Handbook: Vol. 1: Theories, constructs, and critical issues (pp. 367–397). American Psychological Association. Grant, S. G., & Gradwell, J. M. (Eds.). (2010). Teaching history with big ideas: Cases of ambitious teachers. Rowman & Littlefield Education. Grassinger, R., & Dresel, M. (2018). Who learns from errors on a class test? Antecedents and profiles of adaptive reactions to errors in a failure situation. Learning and Individual Differences, 53, 61–68. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.11.009 Gray, D. L. (2014). Understanding STEM-focused high school students’ perceptions of task importance: The role of “standing out” and “fitting in” in mathematics class. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39, 29–41. Gray, D. L. L. (2017). Is psychological membership in the classroom a function of standing out while fitting in? Implications for achievement motivation and emotions. Journal of School Psychology, 61, 103–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2017.02.001 Gray, D. L. L., Hope, E. C., & Matthews, J. S. (2018). Black and belonging at school: A case for interpersonal, instructional, and institutional opportunity structures. Educational Psychologist, 53(2), 97–113. https:// doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2017.1421466 Gray, P. (2011). Psychology (6th ed.). Worth. Gredler, M. E. (2005). Learning and instruction: Theory into practice (5th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Gredler, M. E. (2009a). Hiding in plain sight: The stages of mastery/self-regulation in Vygotsky’s culturalhistorical theory. Educational Psychologist, 44, 1–19. Gredler, M. E. (2009b). Learning and instruction: Theory into practice (6th ed.). Merrill. Gredler, M. E. (2012). Understanding Vygotsky for the classroom: Is it too late? Educational Psychology Review, 24, 113–131. Green, M., & Piel, J. A. (2010). Theories of human development: A comparative approach (2nd ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Greene, J. A., Muis, K. R., & Pieschl, S. (2010). The role of epistemic beliefs in students’ self-regulated learning with computer-based learning environments: Conceptual and methodological issues. Educational Psychologist, 45, 245–257. Greeno, J. G., Collins, A. M., & Resnick, L. B. (1996). Cognition and learning. In D. Berliner & R. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 15–46). Macmillan. Greenwald, A. G., & Lai, C. K. (2020). Implicit social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 71(1), 419–445. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050837 Greenwald, A. G., Oakes, M. A., & Hoffman, H. G. (2003). Targets of discrimination: Effects of race on responses to weapons holders. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 399–405. Gregory, A., Ruzek, E., Hafen, C. A., Mikami, A. Y., Allen, J. P., & Pianta, R. C. (2017). My Teaching PartnerSecondary: A video-based coaching model. Theory Into Practice, 56(1), 38–45. Gregory, A., Skiba, R. J., & Mediratta, K. (2017). Eliminating disparities in school discipline: A framework for intervention. Review of Research in Education, 41(1), 253–278. https://doi. org/10.3102/0091732X17690499 Greiff, S., Wüstenberg, S., Molnár, G., Fischer, A., Funke, J., & Csapó, B. (2013). Complex problem solving in educational contexts—something beyond g: Concept, assessment, measurement invariance, and construct validity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 364–379. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031856 Grewal, D., & Salovey, P. (2005). Feeling smart: The science of emotional intelligence: A new idea in psychology has matured and shows promise of explaining how attending to emotions can help us in everyday life. American Scientist, 93, 330–339. http://www.jstor.org/stable/27858608 Griffith, J., Steptoe, A., & Cropley, M. (1999). An investigation of coping strategies associated with job stress in teachers. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 517–531. Grigg, S., Perera, H. N., McIlveen, P., & Svetleff, Z. (2018). Relations among math self-efficacy, interest, intentions, and achievement: A social cognitive perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 53, 73–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.01.007

R-27

R-28 References Grigorenko, E. L. (2019). Creativity: A challenge for contemporary education. Comparative Education, 55, 116–132. https://doi.org/10.1080/03050068.2018.1541665 Grigorenko, E. L., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, L. S., Wagner, R. K., Willcutt, E. G., & Fletcher, J. M. (2020). Understanding, educating, and supporting children with specific learning disabilities: 50 years of science and practice. The American Psychologist, 75(1), 37–51. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000452 Grigorenko, E. L., Jarvin, L., Diffley, R., III, Goodyear, J., Shanahan, E. J., & Sternberg, R. J. (2009). Are SATs and GPA enough? A theory-based approach to predicting academic success in secondary school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 964–981. Gröschner, A., Seidel, T., & Shavelson, R. S. (2013). Methods for studying teacher and teaching effectiveness. In J. Hattie & E. Anderman (Eds.), International guide to student achievement. Routledge. Gross-Loh, C. (2016, December). How praise became a consultation prize. The Atlantic. Grossman, P., Dean, C. G. P., Kavanagh, S. S., & Herrmann, Z. (2019). Preparing teachers for project-based teaching. Phi Delta Kappan, 100(7), 43–48. Grossnickle, E. M. (2016). Disentangling curiosity: Dimensionality, definitions, and distinctions from interest in educational contexts. Educational Psychology Review, 28(1), 23–60. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-014-9294-y Grover, S., & Pea, R. (2013). Computational thinking in K–12: A review of the state of the field. Educational Researcher, 42, 38–43. Guay, F., Morin, A. J. S., Litalien, D., Howard, J. L., & Gilbert, W. (2021). Trajectories of self-determined motivation during the secondary school: A growth mixture analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(2), 390–410. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000482 Guglielmi, R. S. (2008). Native language proficiency, English literacy, academic achievement, and occupational attainment in limited-English-proficient students: A latent growth modeling perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 322–342. Guglielmi, R. S. (2012, March 5). Math and science achievement in English language learners: Multivariate latent growth modeling of predictors, mediators, and moderators. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 580–602. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027378 Gunderson, E. A., Gripshover, S. J., Romero, C., Dweck, C. S., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2013). Parent praise to 1- to 3-year-olds predicts children’s motivational frameworks 5 years later. Child Development, 84(5), 1526–1541. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12064 Gunderson, E. A., Hamdan, N., Sorhagen, N. S., & D’Esterre, A. P. (2017). Who needs innate ability to succeed in math and literacy? Academic-domain-specific theories of intelligence about peers versus adults. Developmental Psychology, 53(6), 1188–1205. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000282 Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). The role of parents and teachers in the development of gender-related math attitudes. Sex Roles, 66(3–4), 153–166. Gunn, K. C. M., & Delafield-Butt, J. T. (2015). Teaching children with autism spectrum disorder with restricted interests: A review of evidence for best practice. Review of Educational Research, 85, 1–23. Guo, D., McTigue, E., Matthews, S., & Zimmer, W. K. (2020). The impact of visual displays on learning across the disciplines: A systematic review. Educational Psychology Review, 1–30. Guo, J.-P., Pang, M. F., Yang, L.-Y., & Ding, Y. (2012). Learning from comparing multiple examples: On the dilemma of “similar” or “different.” Educational Psychology Review, 24, 251–269. Gupta, N., Wilcox, S. M., & Mullins, N. (2021). Transgender students and Tilte IX: Biden administration signals shift. The National Law Review, XI(29). https://www.natlawreview.com/article/transgender-students -and-title-ix-biden-administration-signals-shift Gurian, M., & Stevens, K. (2005). The minds of boys: Saving our sons from falling behind in school and life. Jossey-Bass. Gurian, M., Stevens, K., & Daniels, P. (2009). Single-sex classrooms are succeeding. Educational Horizons, 87, 234–245. Guskey, T. R. (2011). Five obstacles to grading reform. Educational Leadership, 69(3), 17–21. Guskey, T. R. (2018). Feedback, correctives, and the use of pre-assessments. In A. A. Lipnevich & J. K. Smith (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of instructional feedback (pp. 432–450). Cambridge University Press. Guskey, T. R. (2020). Get set, go! Creating successful grading and reporting systems. Solution Tree Press. Guskey, T. R., & Bailey, J. M. (2001). Developing grading and reporting systems for student learning. Corwin Press. Guskey, T. R., & Brookhart, S. M. (2019). Introduction. In T. Guskey & S. Brookhart (Eds.), What we know about grading: What works, what doesn't, and what's next (pp. 1–11). Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Guskey, T. R., Swan, G. M., & Jung, L. A. (2011). Grades that mean something. Phi Delta Kappan, 93(2), 52–57. Gutiérrez, K. D., Cortes, K., Cortez, A., DiGiacomo, D., Higgs, J., Johnson, P., Ramón Lizárraga, J., Mendoza, E., Tien, J., & Vakil, S. (2017). Replacing representation with imagination: Finding ingenuity in everyday practices. Review of Research in Education, 41(1), 30–60. https://doi.org/10.3102/0091732X16687523 Hadwin, A. F., Järvelä, S., & Miller, M. (2018). Self-regulation, co-regulation and shared regulation in collaborative learning environments. In D. Schunk & J. Greene (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (2nd ed., pp. 83–106). Routledge. Haertel, E. H. (1999). Performance assessment and educational reform. Phi Delta Kappan, 80, 662–666. Haertel, E. H. (2018). Tests, test scores, and constructs, Educational Psychologist, 53, 203–216. https:// doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2018.1476868 Hafen, C. A., Allen, J. P., Mikami, A. Y., Gregory, A., Hamre, B., & Pianta, R. C. (2012). The pivotal role of adolescent autonomy in secondary school classrooms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41, 245–255. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-011-9739-2

References Hagborg, W. J. (1993). Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale and Harter’s Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents: A concurrent validity study. Psychology in Schools, 30, 132–136. Hagemans, M. G., van der Meij, H., & de Jong, T. (2013). The effects of a concept map-based support tool on simulation-based inquiry learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029433 Haggard, P. (2019). The neurocognitive bases of human volition. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, pp. 9–28. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103348 Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Pantheon Books. Haidt, J. (2013). Moral psychology for the twenty-first century. Journal of Moral Education, 42, 281–297. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2013.817327 Haimovitz, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2016). Parents’ views of failure predict children’s fixed and growth intelligence mind-sets. Psychological Science, 27(6), 859–869. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0956797616639727 Haimovitz, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2017). The origins of children’s growth and fixed mindsets: New research and a new proposal. Child Development, 88(6), 1849–1859. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12955 Hakuta, K. (1986). Mirror of language: The debate on bilingualism. Basic Books. Hakuta, K., & Garcia, E. E. (1989). Bilingualism and education. American Psychologist, 44, 374–379. Haladyna, T. H. (2002). Essentials of standardized achievement testing: Validity and accountability. Allyn & Bacon. Hales, C. M., Carroll, M. D., Fryar, C. D., & Ogden, C. L. (2017). Prevalence of obesity among adults and youth: United States, 2015–2016. NCHS Data Brief No.288, 1–8. Haley, M. H., & Austin, T. Y. (2014). Content-based second language teaching and learning: An interactive approach (2nd ed.). Pearson. Halim, M. L., Ruble, D., Tamis-LeMonda, C., & Shrout, P. E. (2013). Rigidity in gender-typed behaviors in early childhood: A longitudinal study of ethnic minority children. Child Development, 84, 1269–1284. Hallahan, D. P., Kauffman, J. M., & Pullen, P. C. (2019). Exceptional children: An introduction to spectral education (14th ed.). Pearson. Halpern, D. F. (2012). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). Psychology Press. Halpern, D. F., Eliot, L., Bigler, R. S., Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L. D., Hyde, J. S., Liben, L., & Martin, C. L. (2011). The pseudoscience of single-sex schooling. Science, 333, 1706–1707. Hamann, D. L., Baker, D. S., McAllister, P. A., & Bauer, W. I. (2000). Factors affecting university music students’ perceptions of lesson quality and teaching effectiveness. Journal of Research in Music Education, 48, 102–113. Hamers, J. F., & Blanc, M. H. A. (2000). Bilinguality and bilingualism (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press. Hamilton, J. (2009). Multitasking teens may be muddling their brains. http://www.npr.org/templates /story/story.php?storyId=95524385 Hamman-Ortiz, L., & Palmer, D. (2020). Identity and two-way bilingual education: Considering student perspectives: Introduction to the special issue. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2020.1819096 Hammer, C. S., Farkas, G., & Maczuga, S. (2010). The language and literacy development of Head Start children: A study using the family and child experiences survey database. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 70–83. Hammer, C. S., Lawrence, F. R., & Miccio, A. W. (2007). Bilingual children’s language abilities and reading outcomes in Head Start and kindergarten. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 237–248. Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher–child relationships and the trajectory of children’s school outcomes through eighth grade. Child Development, 72, 625–638. Hand, V., & Gresalfi, M. (2015). The joint accomplishment of identity. Educational Psychologist, 50(3), 190–203. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2015.1075401 Hannah-Jones, N. (2015, July 31). The problem we all live with—part one. [Audio podcast]. In Chicago Public Media (Producer) This American Life (Episode 562). http://www.thisamericanlife.org/radio-archives /episode/562/the-problem-we-all-live-with Hannah-Jones, N. (2016, June 9). Choosing a school for my daughter in a segregated city. New York Times. http://www.nytimes.com/2016/06/12/magazine/choosing-a-school-for-my-daughter-in-a-segregated -city.html?_r=1 Hanushek, E. A., Rivkin, S. G., & Kain, J. J. (2005). Teachers, schools and academic achievement. Econometrica, 73, 417–458. Hapgood, S., Magnusson, S. J., & Palincsar, A. S. (2004). Teacher, text, and experience: A case of young children’s scientific inquiry. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13, 455–505. Harackiewicz, J. M., & Linnenbrink, E. A. (2005). Multiple achievement goals and multiple pathways for learning: The agenda and impact of Paul R. Pintrich. Educational Psychologist, 40, 75–84. Harber, K. D., Gorman, J. L., Gengaro, F. P., Butisingh, S., Tsang, W., & Ouellette, R. (2012). Students’ race and teachers’ social support affect the positive feedback bias in public schools. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 1149–1161. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028110 Harber, K. D., Reeves, S., Gorman, J. L., Williams, C. H., Malin, J., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2019). The conflicted language of interracial feedback. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(7), 1220–1242. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000326 Hardin, C. J. (2008). Effective classroom management: Models and strategies for today’s classrooms (2nd ed.). Merrill/Prentice-Hall.

R-29

R-30 References Hardman, M., Winston, E., & Drew, C. J. (2017). Human exceptionality: School, community, and family (12th ed.). Cengage Learning. Harklau, L., Losey, K. M., & Siegal, M. (Eds.). (1999). Generation 1.5 Meet college composition: Issues in the teaching of writing to U.S.-educated learners of ESL. Erlbaum. Harp, S. F., & Mayer, R. E. (1998). How seductive details do their damage: A theory of cognitive interest in science learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 414–434. Harper, K. (2017). 5 things to know about racial and ethnic disparities in special education. Child Trends. https://www.childtrends.org/publications/5-things-to-know-about-racial-and-ethnic-disparities-in -special-education Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do: Parents matter less than you think and peers matter more. Free Press. Harrison, J. R., Bunford, N., Evans, S. W., & Owens, J. S. (2013). Educational accommodations for students with behavioral challenges: A systematic review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 83, 551–597. Hart, S. A., Logan, J. A. R., Thompson, L., Kovas, Y., McLoughlin, G., & Setrill, S.A. (2016). A latent profile analysis of math achievement, numerosity, and math anxiety in twins. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 181–193. Hart, S. C., DiPerna, J. C., Lei, P.-W., & Cheng, W. (2020). Nothing lost, something gained? Impact of a universal social-emotional learning program on future state test performance. Educational Researcher, 49(1), 5–19. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X19898721 Harter, S. (2003). The development of self-representation during childhood and adolescence. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 610–642). Guilford. Harter, S. (2006). The self. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), Social, emotional and personality development (6th ed., pp. 646–718). Wiley. Harter, S. (2012). The construction of the self: Developmental and sociocultural foundations. Guilford Press. Harter, S., & Whitesell, N. R. (2003). Beyond the debate: Why some adolescents report stable self-worth over time and situation, whereas others report changes in self-worth. Journal of Personality, 71, 1027–1058. Hartley, B. L., & Sutton, R. M. (2013). A stereotype threat account of boys’ academic underachievement. Child Development, 84, 1716–1733. Hartshore, J. K., & Ullman, M. T. (2006). Why girls say “holded” more than boys. Developmental Science, 9, 21–32. Hatlevik, O. E., Throndsen, I., Loi, M., & Gudmundsdottir, G. B. (2018). Students’ ICT self-efficacy and computer and information literacy: Determinants and relationships. Computers and Education, 118, 107–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2017.11.011 Hattie, J., Hodis, F. A., & Kang, S. H. K. (2020). Theories of motivation: Integration and ways forward. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101865 Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007). The power of feedback. Review of Educational Research, 77, 81–112. Hattie, J. A. C. (2009). Visible learning: A synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievement. Routledge. Haugland, S. (2005). Selecting or upgrading software and websites in the classroom. Early Childhood Education Journal, 32, 5329–5340. Hayes, S. C., Rosenfarb, I., Wulfert, E., Munt, E. D., Korn, Z., & Zettle, R. D. (1985). Self-reinforcement effects: An artifact of social standard setting? Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 201–214. Headden, S., & McKay, S. (2015). Motivation matters: How new research can help teachers boost student engagement. Carnegie Foundation for the Advancement of Teaching. Healey, K., & Stroman, C. (2021). Structures for belonging: A synthesis of research on belonging-supportive learning environments. Student Experience Research Network. http://studentexperiencenetwork.org/wp-content /uploads/2021/03/Structures-for-Belonging.pdf Healy, N. (2020). Keys to success in distance learning. https://www.edutopia.org/article/keys-success -distance-learning Hearn, S., Saulnier, G., Strayer, J., Glenham, M., Koopman, R., & Marcia, J. E. (2012). Between integrity and despair: Toward construct validation of Erikson’s eighth stage. Journal of Adult Development, 19, 1–20. Heath, S. B. (1989). Oral and literate traditions among black Americans living in poverty. American Psychologist, 44, 367–373. Hecht, S. A., & Vagi, K. J. (2010). Sources of group and individual differences in emerging fraction skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 843–859. Helms, J. E. (1995). An update of Helms’s White and People of Color racial identity models. In J. G. Ponterotto, J. M. Casas, L. A. Suzuki, & C. M. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural counseling (pp. 181–198). Sage. Helms, J. E. (2014). A review of White racial identity theory: The sociopolitical implications of studying White racial identity in psychology. In S. Cooper & K. Ratele (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th International Congress of Psychology (pp. 12–27). London and Psychology Press. Helms, J. E. (2020). A race is a nice thing to have: A guide to being a White person or understanding the White persons in your life (3rd ed.). Cognella. Hennessey, B. A. (2015). Reward, task motivation, creativity and teaching: Towards a cross-cultural examination. Teachers College Record, 117, http://www.tcrecord.org ID Number: 18099 Hennessey, M. N., Higley, K., & Chesnut, S. R. (2012). Persuasive pedagogy: A new paradigm for mathematics education. Review of Educational Research, 24, 187–204. Henrich, C. C., Brookmeyer, K. A., Shrier, L. A., & Shahar, G. (2006). Supportive relationships and sexual risk behavior in adolescence: An ecological/transactional approach. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31, 286–297.

References Henriksen, D., & Mishra, P. (2015). We teach who we are: Creativity in the lives and practices of accomplished teachers. Teachers College Record, 117, 1–46. Henry, B. (2011, May 2). Personal communication. Herbers, J. E., Cutuli, J. J., Keane, J. N., & Leonard, J. A. (2020). Childhood homelessness, resilience, and adolescent mental health: A prospective, person-centered approach. Psychology in the Schools, 57(12), 1830–1844. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.22331 Herbert, E. A. (1998). Design matters: How school environment affects children. Educational Leadership, 56(1), 69–71. Herman, J., & Winters, L. (1994). Portfolio research: A slim collection. Educational Leadership, 52(2), 48–55. Herman, M. (2004). Forced to choose: Some determinants of racial identification in multi-racial adolescents. Child Development, 75, 730–748. Hermkes, R., Mach, H., & Minnameier, G. (2018). Interaction-based coding scaffolding processes. Learning and Instruction, 54, 147–155. Herrell, A. L., & Jordan, M. (2016). 50 Strategies for teaching English language learners (5th ed.). Pearson. Herron, A. (2013, December 10). Single-sex classrooms: Educators, students say the change makes a difference. Winston-Salem Journal. http://www.journalnow.com/news/local/article_f39deeb6–613c-11e3-be5c -0019bb30f31a.html Hess, K. (2013). A guide for using Webb’s Depth of Knowledge with Common Core State Standards. The Common Core Institute. https://education.ohio.gov/getattachment/Topics/Teaching/Educator-Evaluation -System/How-to-Design-and-Select-Quality-Assessments/Webbs-DOK-Flip-Chart.pdf.aspx Hetherington, E. M. (2006). The influence of conflict, marital problem solving and parenting on children’s adjustment in nondivorced, divorced and remarried families. In A. Clarke-Stewart & J. Dunn (Eds.), Families count: Effects on child and adolescent development (pp. 203–237). Cambridge University Press. Hidi, S. (2016). Revisiting the role of rewards in motivation and learning: Implications of neuroscientific research. Educational Psychology Review, 28(1), 61–93. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9307-5 Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111–127. Hidi, S. E., & Renninger, K. A. (2019). Interest development and its relation to curiosity: Needed neuroscientific research. Educational Psychology Review, 31(4), 833–852. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09491-3 Hilgard, E. R. (1996). History of educational psychology. In R. Calfee & D. Berliner (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 990–1004). Macmillan. Hill, E. L., & Khanem, F. (2009). The development of hand preference in children: The effect of task demands and links with manual dexterity. Brain and Cognition, 71, 99–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. bandc.2009.04.006 Hill, H. C., Rowan, B., & Ball, D. L. (2005). Effects of teachers’ mathematics knowledge for teaching on student achievement. American Educational Research Journal, 42, 371–406. Hinckley, D. (2014, March 5). Average American watches 5 hours of TV per day, report shows. New York Daily News. http://www.nydailynews.com/life-style/average-american-watches-5-hours-tv-day -article-1.1711954 Hines, C. V., Cruickshank, D. R., & Kennedy, J. J. (1985). Teacher clarity and its relation to student achievement and satisfaction. American Educational Research Journal, 22, 87–99. Hines, M. (2004). Brain gender. Oxford University Press. Hinton, C., Miyamoto, K., & Della-Chiesa, B. (2008). Brain research, learning and emotions: Implications for education research, policy and practice. European Journal of Education, 43, 87–103. Hiroto, D. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Generality of learned helplessness in man. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 311–327. Hirsch, E. D., Jr. (1996). The schools we need: Why we don’t have them. Doubleday. Hirvikoski, T., Waaler, E., Alfredsson, J., Pihlgren, C., Holmström, A., Johnson, A., Rück, J., Wiwe, C. Bothén, P., & Nordström, A. L. (2011). Reduced ADHD symptoms in adults with ADHD after structured skills training group: Results from a randomized controlled trial. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 49, 175–185. Hmelo-Silver, C. E. (2003, April). The constructivist teacher: Facilitating problem-based learning. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association annual meeting, Chicago, IL. Hmelo-Silver, C. E. (2004). Problem-based learning: What and how do students learn? Educational Psychology Review, 16, 235–266. Hmelo-Silver, C. E., Ravit, G. D., & Chinn, C. A. (2007). Scaffolding and achievement in problem-based and inquiry learning: A response to Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark (2006). Educational Psychologist, 42, 99–107. Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development. Cambridge University Press. Hogan, T., Rabinowitz, M., & Craven, J. A., III. (2003). Representation in teaching: Inferences from research of expert and novice teachers. Educational Psychologist, 38, 235–247. Holland, A. A., Stavinoha, P. L., Swearer, S. M., Solesbee, C., Patel, S., & Klesse, L. J. (2019). Rate and frequency of bullying victimization in school-age children with neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1). School Psychology, 34, 687–694. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000333 Holzberger, D., Philipp, A., & Kunter, M. (2013). How teachers’ self-efficacy is related to instructional quality: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 774–786. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032198 Hong, G., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2005). Effects of kindergarten retention policy on children’s cognitive growth in reading and mathematics. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 27, 205–224. Hong, J. S., & Garbarino, J. (2012). Risk and protective factors for homophobic bullying in schools: An application of the social–ecological framework. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 271–285. Hong, J. Y. (2012). Why do some beginning teachers leave the school, and others stay? Understanding teacher resilience through psychological lenses. Teachers and Teaching: Theory and Practice, 18, 417–440.

R-31

R-32 References Honicke, T., & Broadbent, J. (2016). The influence of academic self-efficacy on academic performance: A systematic review. Educational Research Review, 17, 63–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2015.11.002 Hoogerheide, V., Loyens, S. M. M., & van Gog, T. (2014). Effects of creating video-based modeling examples on learning and transfer. Learning and Instruction, 33, 108–119. Hoogerheide, V., Renkl, A., Fiorella, L., Paas, F., & van Gog, T. (2019). Enhancing example-based learning: Teaching on video increases arousal and improves problem-solving performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(1), 45–56. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000272 Hoogerheide, V., Visee, J., Lachner, A., & van Gog, T. (2019). Generating an instructional video as homework activity is both effective and enjoyable. Learning and Instruction, 64, 101226. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.learninstruc.2019.101226 Hoogman, M., van Rooij, D., Klein, M., Boedhoe, P., Ilioska, I., Li, T., Patel, Y., Postema, M. C., Zhang-James, Y., Anagnostou, E., Arango, C., Auzias, G., Banaschewski, T., Bau, C., Behrmann, M., Bellgrove, M. A., Brandeis, D., Brem, S., Busatto, G. F., Calderoni, S., …, Franke, B. (2020). Consortium neuroscience of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder: The ENIGMA Adventure. Human Brain Mapping, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25029 hooks, b. (1994). Teaching to transgress: Education as the practice of freedom. Routledge. Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., Battiato, A. C., Walker, J. M. T., Reed, R. P., DeJong, J. M., & Jones, K. P. (2001). Parental involvement in homework. Educational Psychologist, 36, 195–209. Hopkins, E. J., Dore, R. A., & Lillard, A. S. (2015). Do children learn from pretense? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 1–18. dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.004 Hopkins, M., Thompson, R. K., Linquanti, R., Hakuta, K., & August, D. (2013). Fully accounting for English learner performance: A key issue in ESEA reauthorization. Educational Researcher, 42(2), 101–108. Horn, J. L., & McArdle, J. J. (2007). Understanding human intelligence since Spearman. In R. Cudeck & R. C. MacCallum (Eds.), Factor analysis at 100: Historical developments and future directions (pp. 205–247). Erlbaum. Horn, S. S. (2019). Sexual orientation and gender identity-based prejudice. Child Development Perspectives, 13(1), 21–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12311 Horovitz, B. (2002, April 22). Gen Y: A tough crowd to sell. USA Today, pp. B1–2. Horowitz, J. M., Igielnik, R., & Kochhar, R. (2020a, January 9). Most Americans say there is too much economic inequality in the U.S., but fewer than half call it a top priority. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewsocialtrends .org/wp-content/uploads/sites/3/2020/01/PSDT_01.09.20_economic-inequailty_FULL.pdf Horowitz, J. M., Parker, K., Brown, A., & Cox, K. (2020b, October 6). Amid national reckoning, Americans divided on whether increased focus on race will lead to major policy change. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewsocialtrends .org/wp-content/uploads/sites/3/2020/10/PSDT_10.06.20_race.update.fullreport-1.correx.pdf Horst, E. A. (2011). Reexamining gender issues in Erikson’s Stages of Identity and Intimacy. Journal of Counseling and Development, 73, 271–278. Howard, J. (2019). Securing the future of play in early childhood education: Journeying with children toward the essence of play to evidence its function and value. In D. Whitebread, V. Grau, K. Kumpulainen, M. M. McClelland, N. Perry, & D. Pino-Pasternak (Eds.), The Sage handbook of developmental psychology and early childhood education (pp. 201–222). Sage. Howard, J. L., Gagné, M., & Bureau, J. S. (2017). Testing a continuum structure of self-determined motivation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 143(12), 1346–1377. Howard-Jones, P. (2014). Neuroscience and education: A Review of educational interventions and approaches informed by neuroscience. Education Endowment Foundation. Howard-Jones, P. A. (2014). Neuroscience and education: Myths and messages. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(12), 817e824. Howard-Jones, P. A., Varma, S., Ansari, D., Butterworth, B., De Smedt, B., Goswami, U., & Thomas, M. S. C. (2016). The principles and practices of educational neuroscience: Comment on Bowers (2016). Psychological Review, 123, 620–627. Howe, C., Hennessy, S., Mercer, N., Vrikki, M., & Wheatley, L. (2019). Teacher–student dialogue during classroom teaching: Does it really impact on student outcomes? Journal of the Learning Sciences, 28(4–5), 462–512. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508406.2019.1573730 Hoy, K. L. (2020, September 21). Norms and Expectations for Video Conferencing Advisory Meetings and Classes. Kelly Hoy. 5th Grade Classes. Personal communication. Hoy, W. K., & Adams, C. M. (2016). Quantitative research in education (2nd ed.). Sage. Hsieh, P. P., & Kang, H. (2010). Attribution and self-efficacy and their interrelationship in the Korean EFL context. Language Learning, 60(3), 606–627. Hu, F.-T., Ginns, P., & Bobis, J. (2015). Getting the point: Tracing worked examples enhances learning. Learning and Instruction, 35, 85–93. Huang, C. (2016). Achievement goals and self-efficacy: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 19, 119–137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2016.07.002 Huang, F. L. (2020). Prior problem behaviors do not account for the racial suspension gap. Educational Researcher, 49, 493–502. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X20932474 Huang, X., Mayer, R. E., & Usher, E. L. (2020). Better together: Effects of four self-efficacy-building strategies on online statistical learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 63. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.cedpsych.2020.101924 Huber, C. R., & Kuncel, N. R. (2016). Does college teach critical thinking? A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 86, 431–468. Hübner, N., Wagner, W., Hochweber, J., Neumann, M., & Nagengast, B. (2020). Comparing apples and oranges: Curricular intensification reforms can change the meaning of students’ grades! Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(1), 204–220. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000351

References Hudley, C., & Novak, A. (2007). Environmental influences, the developing brain, and aggressive behavior. Theory Into Practice, 46, 121–129. Huesmann, L. R., Moise-Titus, J., Podolski, C. P., & Eron, L. D. (2003). Longitudinal relations between children’s exposure to TV violence and their aggressive and violent behavior in young adulthood: 1977–1992. Developmental Psychology, 39, 201–221. Hughes, J. N., & Cao, Q. (2018). Trajectories of teacher-student warmth and conflict at the transition to middle school: Effects on academic engagement and achievement. Journal of School Psychology, 67, 148–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2017.10.003 Hughes, J. N., West, S. G., Kim, H., & Bauer, S. S. (2018). Effect of early grade retention on school completion: A prospective study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(7), 974–991. https://doi.org/10.1037 /edu0000243 Huguet, P., & Régner, I. (2007). Stereotype threat among schoolgirls in quasi-ordinary classroom circumstances. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 345–360. Huguley, J. P., Wang, M. T., Vasquez, A. C., & Guo, J. (2019). Parental ethnic-racial socialization practices and the construction of children of color’s ethnic-racial identity: A research synthesis and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 145(5), 437–458. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000187 Hulleman, C. S., & Barron, K. E. (2016). Motivation interventions in education: Bridging theory, research, and practice. In L. Corno & E. M. Anderman (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (3rd ed., pp. 160–171). American Psychological Association. Hulleman, C. S., Schrager, S. M., Bodmann, S. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). A meta-analytic review of achievement goal measures: Different labels for the same constructs or different constructs with similar labels? Psychological Bulletin, 136(3), 422–449. Hung, D. W. L. (1999). Activity, apprenticeship, and epistemological appropriation: Implications from the writings of Michael Polanyi. Educational Psychologist, 34, 193–205. Hung, W., Dolmans, D. H. J. M., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2019). A review to identify key perspectives in PBL meta-analyses and reviews: Trends, gaps and future research directions. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 24(5), 943–957. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-019-09945-x Hunt, J. McV. (1961). Intelligence and experience. Ronald. Hunt, N., & Marshall, K. (2013). Exceptional children and youth: An introduction to special education (5th ed.). Cengage. Hurry, J., Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Pretzlik, U., Parker, M., Curno, C., & Midgley, L. (2005). Transforming research on morphology into teacher practice. Research Papers in Education, 20(2), 187–206. Hushman, C., & Marley, S. C. (2015). Guided instruction improves elementary student learning and selfefficacy in science. Journal of Educational Research, 108, 371–381. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2014 .899958 Hussar, W., Zhang, J., Hein, S., Wang, K., Roberts, A., Cui, A., Smith, M., Mann, F. B., Barmer, A., & Dilig, R. (2020, May). The condition of education 2020 (NCES 2020–144). U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2020/2020144.pdf Hutchinson, N. L. (2009). Inclusion of exceptional learners in Canadian classrooms: A practical handbook for teachers (3rd ed.). Prentice Hall. Hwang, N. (2018). Suspensions and achievement: Varying links by type, frequency, and subgroup.Educational Researcher, 47(6), 363–374. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X18779579 Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60, 581–592. Hyde, J. S., Bigler, R. S., Joel, D., Tate, C. C., & van Anders, S. M. (2019). The future of sex and gender in psychology: Five challenges to the gender binary. American Psychologist, 74(2), 171–193. https:// doi.org/10.1037/amp0000307 Hyde, L. W., Gard, A. M., Tomlinson, R. C., Burt, S. A., Mitchell, C., & Monk, C. S. (2020). An ecological approach to understanding the developing brain: Examples linking poverty, parenting, neighborhoods, and the brain. American Psychologist, 75(9), 1245–1259. Hyder, N., & Hussain, M. (2015, July 20). Zero tolerance policies and bullying in the classroom. Association of Teacher Educators. https://www.aaeteachers.org/index.php/blog/1504-zero-tolerance-policies-and-bullying-in-the -classroom Hymel, S., & Swearer, S. M. (2015). Four decades of research on school bullying. American Psychologist, 70, 293–299. IES. (2018). Improving mathematical problem solving in grades 4 through 8: Instructional tips based on the educator’s practice guide. Institute for Educational Science. What Works Clearinghouse. https://ies.ed.gov/ncee /wwc/PracticeGuide/16 Immordino-Yang, M. H., Darling-Hammond, L., & Krone, C. R. (2019). Nurturing nature: How brain development is inherently social and emotional, and what this means for education. Educational Psychologist, 54(3), 185–204. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2019.1633924 Ingram, J., & Elliott, V. (2016). A critical analysis of the role of wait time in classroom interactions and the effects on student and teacher interactional behaviours. Cambridge Journal of Education, 46, 37–53. Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence: An essay on the construction of formal operational structures. Basic Books. Inns, A. I., & Slavin, R. E. (2018). Cooperative learning: Theoretical foundations and relevance across cultures. In G. A. D. Liem & D. M. McInerney (Eds.), Big theories revisited 2 (pp. 237–266). Information Age. Iordanou, K., Kuhn, D., Matos, F., Shi, Y., & Hemberger, L. (2019). Learning by arguing. Learning and Instruction, 63, 101207. Irewole-Ojo, F. O., Senbanjo, I. O., Oduwole, A. O., & Njokanma, O. F. (2018). Age of pubertal events among school girls in Lagos, Nigeria. Journal of Pediatric Endocrinology and Metabolism, 31(3), 313–321. https:// doi.org/10.1515/jpem-2017-0344

R-33

R-34 References Irvine, J. J., & Armento, B. J. (2001). Culturally responsive teaching: Lesson planning for elementary and middle grades. McGraw-Hill. Irvine, J. J., & Fraser, J. W. (1998, May). Warm demanders. Education Week. http://www.edweek.org/ew /ewstory.cfm?slug=35irvine.h17&keywords=Irvine Isaacson, W. (2007). Einstein: His life and universe. Simon and Schuster. Ispa-Landa, S. (2018). Persistently harsh punishments amid efforts to reform: Using tools from social psychology to counteract racial bias in school disciplinary decisions. Educational Researcher, 47(6), 384–390. Ito, M., Arum, R., Conley, D., Guttiérez, K., Kirshner, B., Livingstone, S., Michalchik, V., Penuel, W., Peppler, K., Pinkard, N., Rhodes, J., Salen Tekinbaş, K., Schor, J., Sefton-Green, J., & Watkins, S. C. (2020). The Connected Learning Research Network: Reflections of a decade of engaged scholarship. The Connected Learning Alliance. https://clalliance.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/02/CLRN_Report.pdf Jabbar, A. I. A., & Felicia, P. (2015). Gameplay engagement and learning in game-based learning: A systematic review. Review of Educational Research, 85, 740–779. Jackson, L. A., von Eye, A., Biocca, F. A., Barbatsis, G., Zhao, Y., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (2006). Does home Internet use influence the academic performance of low-income children? Developmental Psychology, 42, 429–435. Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in children’s selfcompetence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Development, 73, 509–527. Jacobson, M., Goldwater, M., Markauskaite, L., Lai, P., Kapur, M., Roberts, G., & Hilton, C. (2020). Schema abstraction with productive failure and analogical comparison: Learning designs for far across domain transfer. Learning and Instruction, 65, 101222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.101222 Jaeger, A. J., Velazquez, M. N., Dawdanow, A., & Shipley, T. F. (2018). Sketching and summarizing to reduce memory for seductive details in science text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(7), 899–916. https:// doi.org/10.1037/edu0000254 Jaffee, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2000). Gender differences in moral orientation. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 703–726. Jagers, R. J., Rivas-Drake, D., & Williams, B. (2019). Transformative social and emotional Learning (SEL): Toward SEL in service of educational equity and excellence. Educational Psychologist, 54(3), 162–184. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2019.1623032 Jain, S., & Dowson, M. (2009). Mathematics anxiety as a function of multidimensional self-regulation and self-efficacy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 240–249. James, C., Weinstein, E., & Mendoza, K. (2019). Teaching digital citizens in today's world: Research and insights behind the Common Sense K–12 Digital Citizenship Curriculum. Common Sense Media. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 2). Holt. James, W. (1899/2001). Talks to teachers on psychology: And to students on some of life’s ideals. Dover. James, W. (1907). The energies of men. The Philosophical Review, 16(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.2307/2177575 Jamieson, J. P., Brett J. P., Greenwood, E. J., & Altose, A. J. (2016). Reappraising stress arousal improves performance and reduces evaluation anxiety in classroom exam situations. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(6), 579–587. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616644656 Jang, H., Kim, E. J., & Reeve, J. (2012). Longitudinal test of self-determination theory’s motivation mediation model in a naturally occurring classroom context. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 1175–1188. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028089 Jang, H., Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (2010). Engaging students in learning activities: It is not autonomy support or structure but autonomy support and structure. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 588–600. Janika, A., & Forcier, M. (2016). Transgender and gender nonconforming youth: Psychosocial and medical considerations. Rhode Island Medical Journal, 31–34. Jarrett, R. (1995). Growing up poor: The family experiences of socially mobile youth in low-income African American neighborhoods. Journal of Adolescent Research, 10, 111–135. Jaswal, V. K., & Markman, E. M. (2001). Learning proper and common names in inferential versus ostensive contexts. Child Development, 72, 787–802. Jennings, P. A. (2019). Teaching in a trauma-sensitive classroom: What educators can do to support students. American Educator. https://www.aft.org/ae/summer2019/jennings Jennings, P. A., Brown, J. L., Frank, J. L., Doyle, S., Oh, Y., Davis, R., Rasheed, D., DeWeese, A., DeMauro, A. A., Cham, H., & Greenberg, M. T. (2017). Impacts of the CARE for teachers program on teachers’ social and emotional competence and classroom interactions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(7), 1010–1028. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000187 Jennings, P. A., & Frank, J. L. (2015). Inservice preparation for educators. In J. A. Durlak, C. E. Domitrovich, R. P. Weissberg, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Handbook of social and emotional learning: Research and practice (pp. 422–437). The Guilford Press. Jennings, P. A., Frank, J. L., Snowberg, K. E., Coccia, M. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (2013). Improving classroom learning environments by cultivating awareness and resilience in education (CARE): Results of a randomized controlled trial. School Psychology Quarterly, 28(4), 374–390. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000035 Jensen, E. (2009). Teaching with poverty in mind: What being poor does to kids’ brains and what schools can do about it. Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Jensen, E. (2013). Engaging students with poverty in mind: Practical strategies for raising achievement. Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Jensen, E. (2016). Poor students, rich teaching: Mindsets for change. Solution Tree Press. Jensen, J. L., McDaniel, M. A., Woodard, S. M., & Kummer, T. A. (2014). Teaching to the test … or testing to teach: Exams requiring higher order thinking skills encourage greater conceptual understanding. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 307–329.

References Jensen, L. A., Arnett, J. J., Feldman, S. S., & Cauffman, E. (2002). It’s wrong but everybody does it: Academic dishonesty among high school and college students. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 209–228. Jeong, H., Hmelo-Silver, C. E., & Jo, K. (2019). Ten years of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning: A meta-analysis of CSCL in STEM education during 2005–2014. Educational Research Review, 28, 100284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2019.100284 Jerome, E. M., Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2009). Teacher–child relationships from kindergarten to sixth grade: Early childhood predictors of teacher-perceived conflict and closeness. Social Development, 18(4), 915–945. Jerrim, J., Oliver, M., & Sims, S. (2019). The relationship between inquiry-based teaching and students’ achievement. New evidence from a longitudinal PISA study in England. Learning and Instruction, 61, 35–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.12.004 Jia, Y., Way, N., Ling, G., Yoshikawa, H., Chen, X., & Hughes, D. (2009). The influence of student perceptions of school climate on socioemotional and academic adjustment: A comparison of Chinese and American adolescents. Child Development, 80, 1514–1530. Jiang, Y., Ekono, M., & Skinner, C. (2016). Basic facts about low-income children: Children under 18 years, 2014. National Center for Children in Poverty, Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University. Jiang, Y., Kim, S. - il, & Bong, M. (2020). The role of cost in adolescent students’ maladaptive academic outcomes. Journal of School Psychology, 83, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2020.08.004 Jimerson, S. R., Anderson, G. E., & Whipple, A. D. (2002). Winning the battle and losing the war: Examining the relation between grade retention and dropping out of high school. Psychology in the Schools, 39, 441–457. Jimerson, S. R., & Ferguson, P. (2007). A longitudinal study of grade retention: Academic and behavioral outcomes of retained students through adolescence. School Psychology Quarterly, 22, 314–339. Jirout, J. J., Vitiello, V. E., & Zumbrunn, S. K. (2018). Curiosity in schools. In G. Gordon (Ed.), The new science of curiosity (pp. 243–266). Nova. Jitendra, A. K., Star, J. R., Starosta, K., Leh J. M., Sood, S., Caskie, G., Hughes, C. L., & Mack, T. R. (2009). Improving seventh grade students’ learning of ratio and proportion: The role of schema-based instruction. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 250–264. Job, V., Walton, G. M., Bernecker, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2015). Implicit theories about willpower predict selfregulation and grades in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(4), 637–647. http:// doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000014 Joët, G., Usher, E. L., & Bressoux, P. (2011). Sources of self-efficacy: An investigation of elementary school students in France. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 649–663. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024048 Johnson, D. W. (2015). Constructive controversy: Theory, research, practice. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316105818 Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2002). Meaningful assessment: A meaningful and cooperative process. Allyn & Bacon. Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2009a). An educational psychology success story: Social interdependence theory and cooperative learning. Educational Researcher, 38, 365–379. Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2009b). Energizing learning: The instructional power of conflict. Educational Researcher, 38(1), 37–51. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X08330540 Johnson, R. C. (2015). Long-run impacts of school desegregation and school quality on adult attainments. National Bureau of Economic Research (NBER) working paper #16664. Summary in The NBER Digest (May 2011). http://socrates.berkeley.edu/~ruckerj/johnson_schooldesegregation_NBERw16664.pdf Johnson, R. C. (2019). Children of the dream: Why school integration works. Basic Books. Johnson, S. (2008, January 14). A childhood in poverty informs her teaching. USA Today, p. 7D. John-Steiner, V., & Mahn, H. (1996). Sociocultural approaches to learning and development: A Vygotskian framework. Educational Psychologist, 31, 191–206. Johnston, O., Wildy, H., & Shand, J. (2019). A decade of teacher expectations research 2008–2018: Historical foundations, new developments, and future pathways. Australian Journal of Education, 63(1), 44–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004944118824420 Jolly, J. L. (2008). Lew Terman: Genetic study of genius—Elementary school students. Gifted Child Today, 31, 27–33. Jones, M. G., & Gerig, T. M. (1994). Silent sixth-grade students: Characteristics, achievement, and teacher expectations. Elementary School Journal, 95, 169–182. Jones, M. H. (Ed.). (2020). Social goals in the classroom: Findings on student motivation and peer relations. Routledge. Jones, M. L., & Galliher, R. V. (2015). Daily racial microaggressions and ethnic identification among native American young adults. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037 /a0037537 Jones, S., Bailey, R., Brush, K., & Kahn, J. (2017). Kernels of practice for SEL: Low-cost, low-burden strategies. Harvard School of Education. https://www.wallacefoundation.org/knowledge-center/documents /kernels-of-practice-for-sel.pdf Jones, S., McGarrah, M. W., & Kahn, J. (2019). Social and emotional learning: A principled science of human development in context. Educational Psychologist, 54, 129–143. Jones, S. M., Brush, K., Bailey, R., Brio-Meisels, G., MyIntyre, J., Kahn, J., Nelson, B., & Stickle, B. (2017). Navigating SEL from the inside out: Looking inside across 21 leading SEL programs: A practical resource for schools and OST providers. Harvard Graduate School of Education and The Wallace Foundation. https://www .wallacefoundation.org/knowledge-center/Documents/Navigating-Social-and-Emotional-Learning -from-the-Inside-Out.pdf

R-35

R-36 References Jones, S. M., & Dindia, K. (2004). A Meta-Analytic Perspective on Sex Equity in the Classroom. Review of Educational Research, 74(4), 443–471. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543074004443 Jones, S. M., McGarrah, M. W., & Kahn, J. (2019). Social and emotional learning: A principled science of human development in context. Educational Psychologist, 54(3), 129–143. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520 .2019.1625776 Jones, V., & Jones, L. (2016). Comprehensive classroom management: Creating communities for support and classroom problems (11th ed.). Pearson. Jonsdottir, I. J., & Fridriksdottir, K. (2020) Active listening: Is it the forgotten dimension in managerial communication? International Journal of Listening, 34, 178–188. https://doi.org/10.1080/10904018.2019.1 613156 Judd, T. (2013). Making sense of multitasking: Key behaviours. Computers and Education, 63, 358–367. Jurbergs, N., Palcic, J., & Kelly, M. L. (2007). School-home notes with and without response cost: Increasing attention and academic performance in low-income children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly, 22, 358–379. Jussim, L. (2013). Teachers’ expectations. In J. Hattie & E. Anderman (Eds.), International guide to student achievement. Routledge. Jussim, L., & Harber, K. (2005). Teacher expectations and self-fulfilling prophecies: Knowns and unknowns; resolved and unresolved controversies. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 131–135. Jussim, L., Robustelli, S., & Cain, T. (2009). Teacher expectations and self-fulfilling prophecies. In A. Wigfield & K. Wentzel (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 349–380). Erlbaum. Juvonen, J., & Knifsend, C. (2016). School-based peer relationships and achievement motivation. In K. Wenzel & D. Miele (Eds.), Handbook of motivation in school (2nd ed., pp. 231–250). Routledge. Juvonen, J., & Schacter, H. L. (2020). When low rates of bullying increase risks for those who are bullied: The safe school paradox. Jama Pediatrics, 174(4), 317–317. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.5888 K–12 Computer Science Teachers Association. (2017). CS standards. https://www.csteachers.org/page /standards Kachanoff, F. J., Wohl, M. J. A., Koestner, R., & Taylor, D. M. (2020). Them, us, and I: How group contexts influence basic psychological needs. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(1), 47–54. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0963721419884318 Kaendler, C., Wiedmann, M., Rummel, N., & Spada, H. (2015). Teacher competencies for the implementation of collaborative learning in the classroom: A framework and research review. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 505–536. Kagan, J., & Herschkowitz, N. (2005). A young mind in a growing brain. Erlbaum. Kagan, S. (1994). Cooperative learning. Kagan Cooperative Learning. Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Strauss and Giroux. Kalenkoski, C. M., & Pabilonia, S. W. (2014). Does high school homework increase academic achievement? (IZA Discussion Paper No.8142). Institute for the Study of Labor (IZA). Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory: How many types of load does it really need? Educational Psychology Review, 23, 1–19. Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Tuovinen, J., & Sweller, J. (2001). When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 579–588. Kalyuga S., & Sweller, J. (2018). Cognitive load and expertise reversal. In K. Ericsson, R. Hoffman, A. Kozbelt, & M. Williams (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 793–811). Cambridge University Press. Kana’iaupuni, S. M., Ledward, B., & Malone, N. (2017). Mohala i ka wai. Cultural advantage as a framework for Indigenous culture-based education and student outcomes. American Educational Research Journal, 54(1_suppl):311S–339S. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831216664779 Kanazawa, S. (2010). Evolutionary psychology and intelligence research. American Psychologist, 65(4), 279–289. Kantamneni, N., McCain, M. R. C., Shada, N., Hellwege, M. A., & Tate, J. (2018). Contextual factors in the career development of prospective first-generation college students: An application of social cognitive career theory. Journal of Career Assessment, 26(1), 183–196. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072716680048 Kaplan, J. S. (1991). Beyond behavior modification (2nd ed.). Pro-Ed. Karpicke, J. J., & Grimaldi, P. J. (2012). Retrieval-based learning: A perspective for enhancing meaningful learning. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 401–418. Karwowski, M., Jankowska, D. M., Brzeski, A., Czerwonka, M., Gajda, A., Lebuda, I., & Beghetto, R. A. (2020). Delving into creativity and learning. Creativity Research Journal, 32, 4–16. https://doi.org/10.1080 /10400419.2020.1712165 Katz, I., & Assor, A. (2007). When choice motivates and when it does not. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 429–442. Katz, S. R. (1999). Teaching in tensions: Latino immigrant youth, their teachers, and the structures of schooling. Teachers College Record, 100(4), 809–840. Katzir, T., & Paré-Blagoev, J. (2006). Applying cognitive neuroscience research to education: The case of literacy. Educational Psychologist, 4, 53–74. Kaufman, T. M., Huitsing, G., & Veenstra, R. (2020). Refining victims’ self-reports on bullying: Assessing frequency, intensity, power imbalance, and goal-directedness. Social Development, 29, 375–390. Kavanagh, S., Conrad, J., & Dagogo-Jack, S. (2020). From rote to reasoned: Examining the role of pedagogical reasoning in practice-based teacher education. Teaching and Teacher Education, 89. 102991. Kazdin, A. E. (2001). Behavior modification in applied settings (6th ed.). Wadsworth.

References Kazdin, A. E. (2008). The Kazdin method for parenting the defiant child. Houghton-Mifflin. Keefe, J. W., & Monk, J. S. (1986). Learning style profile examiner’s manual. National Association of Secondary School Principals. Keller, M., Goetz, T., Becker, E. S., Morger, V., & Hensley, L. (2014). Feeling and showing: A new conceptualization of dispositional teacher enthusiasm and its relation to students’ interest. Learning and Instruction, 33, 29–38. Keller, M., Neumann, K., & Fischer, H. E. (2013). Teacher enthusiasm and student learning. In J. Hattie & E. Anderman (Eds.), International guide to student achievement. Routledge. Keller, M. M., Woolfolk Hoy, A., Goetz, T., & Frenzel, A. C. (2016). Teacher enthusiasm: Reviewing and redefining a complex construct. Educational Psychology Review, 28, 743–769. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1064 Kelly, J. B. (2020). Changing perspectives on children’s adjustment following divorce: A view from the United States. Childhood, 10(2), 237–254. Kelly, S., Olney, A. M., Donnelly, P., Nystrand, M., & D’Mello, S. K. (2018). Automatically measuring question authenticity in real-world classrooms. Educational Researcher, 47(7), 451–464. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0013189X18785613 Kelly, Y., Zilanawala, A., Sacker, A., Hiatt, R., & Viner, R. (2017). Early puberty in 11-year-old girls: Millennium cohort study findings. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 102(3), 232–237. https://doi.org/10.1136 /archdischild-2016-310475 Kemp, C., & Carter, M. (2006). The contribution of academic skills to the successful inclusion of children with disabilities. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 18, 123–146. Kempert, S., Saalbach, H., & Hardy, I. (2011). Cognitive benefits and costs of bilingualism in elementary school students: The case of mathematical word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 547–561. Kicken, W., Brand-Gruwel, S., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Slot, W. (2009). Design and evaluation of a development portfolio: How to improve students’ self-directed learning skills. Instructional Science, 37, 453–473. Killewald, A., Pfeffer, F. T., & Schachner, J. N. (2017). Wealth inequality and accumulation. Annual Review of Sociology, 43, 379–404. Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932. Kim, J., Schallert, D. L., & Kim, M. (2010). An integrative cultural view of achievement motivation: Parental and classroom predictors of children’s goal orientations when learning mathematics in Korea. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 418–437. Kim, J. S., & Guryan, J. (2010). The efficacy of a voluntary summer book reading intervention for low-income Latino children from language minority families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 20–31. Kim, J. S., & Quinn, D. M. (2013). The effects of summer reading on low-income children’s literacy achievement from kindergarten to grade 8: A meta-analysis of classroom and home interventions. Review of Educational Research, 83, 386–431. Kim, S. Y., Wang, Y., Orozco-Lapray, D., Shen, Y., & Murtuza, M. (2013). Does “Tiger Parenting” exist? Parenting profiles of Chinese Americans and adolescent developmental outcomes. Asian American Journal of Psychology, 4, 7–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030612. Kim, L. E., Jörg, V., & Klassen, R. M. (2019). A meta-analysis of the effects of teacher personality on teacher effectiveness and burnout. Educational Psychology Review, 31(1), 163–195. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648 -018-9458-2 Kim, Y., & Sensale Yazdian, L. (2014). Portfolio assessment and quality teaching, Theory Into Practice, 53, 220–227. https://doi.org/10.1080/00405841.2014.916965 Kincade, L., Cook, C., & Goerdt, A. (2020). Meta-analysis and common practice elements of universal approaches to improving student-teacher relationships. Review of Educational Research, 90(5), 710–748. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654320946836 King, A. (1990). Enhancing peer interaction and learning in the classroom through reciprocal questioning. American Educational Research Journal, 27, 664–687. King, A. (1994). Guiding knowledge construction in the classroom: Effects of teaching children how to question and how to explain. American Educational Research Journal, 31, 338–368. King, A. (2002). Structuring peer interactions to promote high-level cognitive processing. Theory Into Practice, 41, 31–39. King, E. M. (2015). Designing after-school learning using the Massively Multiplayer Online role-play game. Theory Into Practice, 54, 128–135. King, R. B. (2020). Mindsets are contagious: The social contagion of implicit theories of intelligence among classmates. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(2), 349–363. https://doi.org/10.1111 /bjep.12285 King, R. B., & McInerney, D. M. (2014). The work avoidance goal construct: Examining its structure, antecedents, and consequences. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39, 42–58. Kirk, S. A., Gallagher, J. J., Anastasiow, N. J., & Coleman, M. R. (2006). Educating exceptional children (11th ed.). Houghton Mifflin. Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1 Kirschner, P. A., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2013). “Do learners really know best?” Urban legends in education. Educational Psychologist, 48, 169–183. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2013.804395 Kittleman, A., Strickland-Cohen, M. K., Pinkelman, S. E., & McIntosh, K. (2020). Variables contributing to abandonment and readoption of SWPBIS. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 22(2), 67–77. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098300719888748.

R-37

R-38 References Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). Equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychological Science, 15, 661–667. Klass, P. (2011, October). Hearing bilingual: How babies sort out language. New York Times. Retrieved July 26, 2013, from https://www.nytimes com/2011/10/11/health/views/11klass.html?_r=0 Klassen, R. M., & Chiu, M. M. (2010). Effects on teachers’ self-efficacy and job satisfaction: Teacher gender, years of experience, and job stress. Journal of Educational Psychology, 10, 741–756. doi: 10.1037/a0019237. Klassen, R. M., & Durksen, T. L. (2014). Weekly self-efficacy and work stress during the teaching practicum: A mixed methods study. Learning and Instruction, 33, 158–169. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.learninstruc.2014.05.003 Klassen, R. M., Tze, V. M. C., Betts, S. M., & Gordon, K. A. (2011). Teacher efficacy research 1998–2009: Signs of progress or unfulfilled promise? Educational Psychology Review. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9141-8 Klassen, R. M., & Usher, E. L. (2010). Self-efficacy in educational settings: Recent research and emerging directions. In T. C. Urdan & S. A. Karabenick (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: Vol. 16A. The decade ahead: Theoretical perspectives on motivation and achievement (pp. 1–33). Emerald Publishing Group. Klee, H. L., Miller, A. D., & Buehl, M. M. (2021). Strategies for alleviating students’ math anxiety: Controlvalue theory in practice. Theory Into Practice. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/ 10.1080 /00405841.2021.1932157 Klein, D., Mok, K., Chen, J.-K., & Watkins, K. E. (2014). Age of language learning shapes brain structure: A cortical thickness study of bilingual and monolingual individuals. Brain and Language, 131, 20–24. Klein, H. J., Lount, R. B., Jr., Park, H. M., & Linford, B. J. (2020). When goals are known: The effects of audience relative status on goal commitment and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 105(4), 372–389. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000441 Klein, S., Lee, J., McKinsey, P., & Archer, C. (2014). Identifying U.S. K–12 public schools with deliberate sex segregation. Feminist Majority Foundation, Education Equality Program. http://feminist.org/education/pdfs /IdentifyingSexSegregation12-12-14.pdf Klein, S. B. (2015). Learning: Principles and applications (7th ed.). Sage. Klein, S. S., & Harris, A. H. (2007). A users guide to the Legacy Cycle. Journal of Education and Human Development, 1. Kleinfeld, J. (2005). Culture fuels boys’ learning problems. Alaska Daily News, p. B6. Klinger, J., & Orosco, M. J. (2010). This issue: Response to intervention. Theory Into Practice, 49, 247–249. Klopack, E. T., Sutton, T. E., Simons, R. L., & Simons, L. G. (2020). Disentangling the effects of boys’ pubertal timing: The importance of social context. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49, 1393–1405. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01141-9 Knapp, M. S., & Woolverton, S. (2003). Social class and schooling. In J. A. Banks & C. A. Banks (Eds.), Handbook of research on multicultural education. Jossey-Bass. Kobayashi, K. (2005). What limits the encoding benefit of note-taking? A meta-analytic examination. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30, 242–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2004.10.001 Koch, I., Poljac, E., Müller, H., & Kiesel, A. (2018). Cognitive structure, flexibility, and plasticity in human multitasking—An integrative review of dual-task and task-switching research. Psychological Bulletin, 144(6), 557–583. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000144 Koenig, E. A., Eckert, T. L., & Hier, B. O. (2016). Using performance feedback and goal setting to improve elementary students’ writing fluency: A randomized controlled trial. School Psychology Review, 45(3), 275–295. https://doi.org/10.17105/SPR45-3.275-295 Kogachi, K., & Graham, S. (2020). Numerical minority status in middle school and racial/ethnic segregation in academic classes. Child Development, 91(6), 2083–2102. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13408 Kohlberg, L. (1963). The development of children’s orientations toward moral order: Sequence in the development of moral thought. Vita Humana, 6, 11–33. Kohlberg, L. (1975). The cognitive-developmental approach to moral education. Phi Delta Kappan, 56, 670–677. Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development. Harper & Row. Kohn, A. (1993). Rewards versus learning: A response to Paul Chance. Phi Delta Kappan, 74, 783–787. Kohn, A. (1996). By all available means: Cameron and Pierce’s defense of extrinsic motivators. Review of Educational Research, 66, 1–4. Kohn, A. (2005). Unconditional teaching. Educational Leadership, 62, 12–17. Kohn, A. (2014, April 4). Sometimes it’s better to quit than to prove your grit. Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/opinions/sometimes-its-better-to-quit-than-to-prove-your -grit/2014/04/04/24075a84-b8f8-11e3-96ae-f2c36d2b1245_story.html Kokko, K., & Pulkkinen, L. (2000). Aggression in childhood and long-term unemployment in adulthood: A cycle of maladaptation and some protective factors. Developmental Psychology, 36, 463–472. Korf, R. (1999). Heuristic search. In R. Wilson & F. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. 372–373). MIT Press. Koriat, A., Goldsmith, M., & Pansky, A. (2000). Toward a psychology of memory accuracy. In S. Fiske (Ed.), Annual review of psychology (pp. 481–537). Annual Reviews. Korman, H. T. N., O’Keefe, B., & Repka, M. (2020, October 21). Missing in the margins: Estimating the scale of the COVID-19 attendance crisis. Bellwether Education Partners. https://bellwethereducation.org /publication/missing-margins-estimating-scale-covid-19-attendance-crisis Korte, G. (2015, December 11). The Every Student Succeeds Act vs. No Child Left Behind: What’s changed? USA Today. http://www.usatoday.com/story/news/politics/2015/12/10/every-student-succeeds-act -vs-no-child-left-behind-whats-changed/77088780/ Kosciw, J. G., Clark, C. M., Truong, N. L., & Zongrone, A. D. (2020). The 2019 National School Climate Survey: The experiences of lesbian, gay, bisexual and transgender youth in our nation's schools. Gay, Lesbian and Straight Education Network (GLSEN).

References Koslouski, J. B., & Stark, K. (2021). Promoting learning for students experiencing adversity and trauma: The everyday, yet profound, actions of teachers. The Elementary School Journal. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1086/712606 Kosslyn, S. M., & Koenig, O. (1992). Wet mind: The new cognitive neuroscience. Free Press. Kounin, J. S. (1970). Discipline and group management in classrooms. Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Koury, A. S., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2014). School readiness of children from immigrant families: Contributions of region of origin, home, and childcare. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 268–288. Kozulin, A. (2003). Psychological tools and mediated learning. In A. Kouzlin, B. Gindis, V. Ageyev, & S. M. Miller (Eds.), Vygotsky’s educational theory in cultural context (pp. 15–38). Cambridge University Press. Kozulin, A., Gindis, B., Ageyev, V., & Miller, S. M. (Eds.). (2003). Vygotsky’s educational theory in cultural context. Cambridge University Press. Kratchovil, C. J. (2009). Current pharmacotherapy for ADHD. 2nd International Congress on ADHD. From Childhood to Adult Disease. May 21–24, 2009, Vienna, Austria. Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 1, 61. Krathwohl, D. R., Bloom, B. S., & Masia, B. B. (1964). Taxonomy of educational objectives. Handbook II: Affective domain. David McKay. Kratzig, G. P., & Arbuthnott, K. D. (2006). Perceptual learning style and learning proficiency: A test of the hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 238–246. Kraus, M. W., Park, J. W., & Tan, J. J. X. (2017). Signs of social class: The experience of economic inequality in everyday life. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(3), 422–435. https:// doi.org/10.1177/1745691616673192 Kretschmann, J., Vock, M., & Lüdtke, O. (2014). Acceleration in elementary school: Using propensity score matching to estimate the effects on academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 1080–1095. Kroger, J. (2000). Identity development: Adolescence through adulthood. Sage. Kroger, J., & Marcia, J. E. (2011). The identity statuses: Origins, meanings, and interpretations. In S. J. Schwartz, K. Luyckx, & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and research (pp. 31–53). Springer Science+Business Media. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-7988-9_2 Kroger, J., & Marcia, J. E. (2013). The identity statuses: Origins, meanings, and interpretations. In S. J. Schwartz, K. Luyckx, & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and research (pp. 31–53). New York, NY: Springer Science+Business Media. Kroger, J., Martinussen, M., & Marcia, J. E. (2010). Identity status change during adolescence and young adulthood: A meta-analysis. Journal of Adolescence, 33, 683–698. Kronholz, J. (2011). Challenging the gifted: Nuclear chemistry and Sartre draw the best and brightest to Reno. Education Next, 11(2), 1–8. http://educationnext.org/challenging-the-gifted/ Kuhfeld, M., Soland, J., Tarasawa, B., Johnson, A., Ruzek, E., & Liu, J. (2020). Projecting the potential impact of covid-19 school closures on academic achievement. Educational Researcher, 49(8), 549–565. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0013189X20965918 Kuhn, D. (2007). Is direct instruction an answer to the right question? Educational Psychologist, 42, 109–113. Kuhn, D. (2015). Thinking together and alone. Educational Researcher, 44(1), 46–53. Kuhn, D., & Dean, D. (2004). Connecting scientific reasoning with casual inference. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 261–288. Kuhn, D., & Franklin, S. (2006). The second decade: What develops (and how). In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Cognition, perception, and language (6th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 953–993). Wiley. Kuhn, D., Goh, W., Iordanou, K., & Shaenfield, D. (2008). Arguing on the computer: A microgenetic study of developing argument skills in a computer-supported environment. Child Development, 79, 1310–1328. Kuklinski, M. R., & Weinstein, R. S. (2001). Classroom and developmental differences in a path model of teacher expectancy effects. Child Development, 72, 1554–1578. Kulik, J. A., & Kulik, C. C. (1997). Ability grouping. In N. Colangelo & G. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of gifted education (2nd ed., pp. 230–242). Allyn & Bacon. Kyndt, E., Raes, E., Lismont, B., Timmers, F., Dochy, F., & Cascallar, E. (2014). A meta-analysis of the effects of face-to-face cooperative learning: Do recent studies falsify or verify earlier findings? Educational Research Review, 10, 133–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2013.02.002 LaBrot, Z. C., Kupzyk, S., Strong-Bak, W., Pasqua, J. L., & Mahon, J. (2020). Examination of group-based behavioral skills training for parents of children with intellectual and neurodevelopmental disorders. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 42. https://doi.org/10.1080/07317107.2020.1738715 LaBrot, Z. C., Radley, K. C., Dart, E., Moore, J., & Cavell, H. J. (2018). A component analysis of behavioral skills training for effective instruction delivery. Journal of Family Psychotherapy, 29(2), 122–141. https:// doi.org/10.1080/08975353.2017.1368813 Lachner, A., Backfisch, I., Hoogerheide, V., van Gog, T., & Renkl, A. (2020). Timing matters! Explaining between study phases enhances students’ learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(4), 841–853. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000396 Ladd, G. W., Kochenderfer-Ladd, B., Ettekal, I., Cortes, K., Sechler, C., & Visconti, K. J. (2014b). The 4R-SUCCESS program: Promoting children’s social and scholastic skills in dyadic classroom activities. Group Dynamics and Organizational Consulting, 45, 25–44. Ladd, G. W., Kochenderfer-Ladd, B., Visconti, K. J., Ettekal, I., Sechler, C. M., & Cortes, K. I. (2014a). Gradeschool children’s social collaborative skills: Links with partner preference and achievement. American Educational Research Journal, 51, 152–183. Ladson-Billings, G. (1990). Like lightning in a bottle: Attempting to capture the pedagogical excellence of successful teachers of Black students. Qualitative Studies in Education, 3, 335–344.

R-39

R-40 References Ladson-Billings, G. (1992). Culturally relevant teaching: The key to making multicultural education work. In C. A. Grant (Ed.), Research and multicultural education (pp. 106–121). Falmer Press. Ladson-Billings, G. (1994). The dream keepers. Jossey-Bass. Ladson-Billings, G. (1995). But that is just good teaching! The case for culturally relevant pedagogy. Theory Into Practice, 34, 161–165. Ladson-Billings, G. (2004). Landing on the wrong note: The price we paid for Brown. Educational Researcher, 33(7), 3–13. Ladson-Billings, G. (2006). From the achievement gap to the education debt: Understanding achievement in U.S. schools. Educational Researcher, 35(7), 3–12. Ladson-Billings, G. (2009). The dreamkeepers: Successful teachers of African American children. Jossey-Bass. Lahat, A., Helwig, C. C., & Zelazo, P. D. (2013). An event-related potential study of adolescents’ and young adults’ judgments of moral and social conventional violations. Child Development, 84, 955–969. Lamb, M. E., & Lewis, C. (2005). The role of parent-child relationships in child development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (5th ed., pp. 429–468). Erlbaum. Landrum, T. J., & Kauffman, J. M. (2006). Behavioral approaches to classroom management. In C. M. Evertson & C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook of classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues. Erlbaum. Langan-Fox, J., Waycott, J. L., & Albert, K. (2000). Linear and graphic organizers: Properties and processing. International Journal of Cognitive Ergonomics, 4(1), 19–34. LaPier, R. (2018, October). How the loss of Native American languages affects our understanding of the natural world. The Conversation. https://theconversation.com/how-the-loss-of-native-american-languages -affects-our-understanding-of-the-natural-world–103984 Larson, K. E., Bottiani, J. H., Pas, E. T., Kush, J. M., & Bradshaw, C. P. (2019). A multilevel analysis of racial discipline disproportionality: A focus on student perceptions of academic engagement and disciplinary environment. Journal of School Psychology, 77, 152–167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2019.09.003 Lather, P. (2004). Scientific research in education: A critical perspective. Journal of Curriculum and Supervision, 20, 14–30. Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics, and culture in everyday life. Cambridge University Press. Lave, J. (1997). The culture of acquisition and the practice of understanding. In D. Kirshner & J. A. Whitson (Eds.), Situated cognition: Social, semiotic, and psychological perspectives (pp. 17–35). Erlbaum. Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge University Press. Lawlor, J., Conneely, C., Oldham, E., Marshall, K., & Tangney, B. (2018). Bridge21: Teamwork, technology and learning. A pragmatic model for effective twenty-first-century team-based learning. Technology, Pedagogy and Education, 27(2), 211–232. https://doi.org/10.1080/1475939X.2017.1405066 Lawson, M. A., & Lawson, H. A. (2013). New conceptual framework for student engagement research, policy, and practice. Review of Educational Research, 83, 432–479. Lazarides, R., Buchholz, J., & Rubach, C. (2018). Teacher enthusiasm and self-efficacy, student-perceived mastery goal orientation, and student motivation in mathematics classrooms. Teaching and Teacher Education, 69, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2017.08.017 Lazarides, R., Gaspard, H., & Dicke, A. L. (2019). Dynamics of classroom motivation: Teacher enthusiasm and the development of math interest and teacher support. Learning and Instruction, 60, 126–137. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.01.012 Lazonder, A. W., & Harmsen, R. (2016). Meta-analysis of inquiry-based learning: Effects of guidance. Review of Educational Research, 86(3), 681–718. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654315627366 Lazowski, R. A., & Hulleman, C. S. (2016). Motivation interventions in education: A meta-analytic review. Review of Educational Research, 86, 602–640. Leaper, C. (2002). Parenting girls and boys. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting, Vol. 1: Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 127–152). Erlbaum. Leaper, C., & Brown, C. S. (2014). Sexism in schools. In L. S. Liben, R. S. Bigler, & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior: The role of gender in educational contexts and outcomes (pp. 189–224). Elsevier. Leaper, C., & Brown, C. S. (2018). Sexism in childhood and adolescence: Recent trends and advances in research. Child Development Perspectives, 12(1), 10–15. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12247 Leaper, C., & Smith, T. S. (2004). A meta-analytic review of gender variations in children’s language use: Talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive speech. Developmental Psychology, 40, 993–1027. Lee, C. D. (2016). Examining conceptions of how people learn over the decades through AERA Presidential Addresses: Diversity and equity as persistent conundrums. Educational Researcher, 45, 73–82. Lee, C. D., & Goldman, S. R. (2015). Assessing literary reasoning: Text and task complexities. Theory Into Practice, 54, 213–277. Lee, H. S., Fincham, J. M., & Anderson, J. R. (2015). Learning from examples versus verbal directions in mathematical problem solving. Mind, Brain, and Education, 9(4), 232–245. Lee, H. S., & Ha, H. (2019). Metacognitive judgments of prior material facilitate the learning of new material: The forward effect of metacognitive judgments in inductive learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(7), 1189–1201. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000339 Lee, J. (2014). Universal factors of student achievement in high-performing Eastern and Western countries. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 364–374. Lee, J., & Bean, F. D. (2010). The diversity paradox: Immigration and the color line in 21st century America. Russell Sage Foundation. Lee, J., & Shute, V. J. (2010). Personal and social-contextual factors in K–12 academic performance: An integrative perspective on student learning. Educational Psychologist, 45, 185–202.

References Lee, J., & Zhou, M. (2015). The Asian American achievement paradox. Russell Sage Foundation. Lee, K., Ng, E. L., & Ng, S. F. (2009). The contributions of working memory and executive functioning to problem representation and solution generation in algebraic word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 373–387. Lee, L. H. J., & Tan, S. C. (2020). Teacher learning in Lesson Study: Affordances, disturbances, contradictions, and implications. Teaching and Teacher Education, 89, 102986. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2019.102986 Lee, M., & Bong, M. (2016). In their own words: Reasons underlying the achievement striving of students in schools. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(2), 274–294. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000048 Lee, S., & Waters, S. F. (2021). Asians and Asian Americans’ experiences of racial discrimination during the COVID-19 pandemic: Impacts on health outcomes and the buffering role of social support. Stigma and Health. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/sah0000275 Lee, S. J., Park, E., & Wong, J.-H. S. (2017). Racialization, schooling, and becoming American: Asian American experiences. Educational Studies, 53(5), 492–510. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131946.2016.1258360 Lee, S. J., Wong, N.-W. A., & Alvarez, A. N. (2008). The model minority and the perpetual foreigner: Stereotypes of Asian Americans. In N. Tewari & A. Alvarez (Eds.), Asian American psychology: Current perspectives (pp. 69–84). CRC Press. Leinhardt, G. (2001). Instructional explanations: A commonplace for teaching and location for contrasts. In V. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (4th ed., pp. 333–357). American Educational Research Association. LeMahieu, P., Gitomer, D. H., & Eresh, J. T. (1993). Portfolios in large-scale assessment: Difficult but not impossible. Unpublished manuscript, University of Delaware. Lemelson, R. (2003). Obsessive-compulsive disorder in Bali. Transcultural Psychiatry, 40, 377–408. Lenhart, A. (2015). Teens, social media & technology: Overview 2015. Pew Research Center. https:// www.pewresearch.org/internet/2015/04/09/teens-social-media-technology-2015/ Lent, R. W., Ireland, G. W., Penn, L. T., Morris, T. R., & Sappington, R. (2017). Sources of self-efficacy and outcome expectations for career exploration and decision-making: A test of the social cognitive model of career self-management. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 99, 107–117. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.jvb.2017.01.002 Leong, D. J., & Bodrova, E. (2012). Assessing and scaffolding make-believe play. Young Children, 67, 28–32. Lepper, M. R., Keavney, M., & Drake, M. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic reward: A commentary on Cameron and Pierce’s meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 66, 5–32. Leppink, J., Paas, F., van Gog, T., van der Vleuten, C. P. M., & van Merriënboer, J. J. C. (2014). Effects of pairs of problems and examples on task performance and different types of cognitive load. Learning and Instruction, 30, 32–42. Lerner, R. M., Theokas, C., & Bobek, D. L. (2005). Concepts and theories of human development: Historical and contemporary dimensions. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (5th ed., pp. 3–43). Erlbaum. Leung, A. K.-Y., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2010). Multicultural experience, idea receptiveness, and creativity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 723–741. Levin, J. R., & Nolan, J. F. (2000). Principles of classroom management: A professional decision-making model. Allyn & Bacon. Levitin, D. J. (2015, January 15). Why the modern world is bad for your brain. The Guardian: Neuroscience. https://www.theguardian.com/science/2015/jan/18/modern-world-bad-for-brain-daniel-j-levitin -organized-mind-information-overload Levontin, L., & Bardi, A. (2018). Pro-social goals in achievement situations: Amity goal orientation enhances the positive effects of mastery goal orientation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(8), 1258–1269. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218765745 Lewno-Dumdie, B., & Hajovsky, D. B. (2020). Do global ability composites differentially predict academic achievement across gender? Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 38(6), 706–723. Lhamon, C. E., & Gupta, V. (2016, May 13). Dear colleague letter on transgender students. U.S. Department of Justice, Civil Rights Division, U.S. Department of Education, Office for Civil Rights. Li, J.-B., Bi, S.-S., Willems, Y. E., & Finkenauer, C. (2021). The association between school discipline and self-control from preschoolers to high school students: A three-level meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 91(1), 73–111. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654320979160 Li, Y., & Bates, T. C. (2019). You can’t change your basic ability, but you work at things, and that’s how we get hard things done: Testing the role of growth mindset on response to setbacks, educational attainment, and cognitive ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(9), 1640–1655. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000669 Liebtag, E., & Haugen, C. (2015, May 14). Shortage of dual-language teachers: Filling the gap. Education Week. http://blogs.edweek.org/edweek/global_learning/2015/05/shortage_of_dual_language _teachers_filling_the_gap.html Lillard, A. S., Lerner, M. D., Hopkins, E. J., Dore, R. A., Smith, E. D., & Palmquist, C. M. (2013). The impact of pretend play on children’s development: A review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 39, 1–34. https:// doi.org/10.1037/a0029321 Lillemyr, O. F., Søbstad, F., Marder, K., & Flowerday, T. (2011). A multicultural perspective on play and learning in primary school. International Journal of Early Childhood, 43, 43–65. Limber, S. P., Olweus, D., Wang, W., Masiello, M., & Breivik, K. (2018). Evaluation of the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program: A large scale study of U. S. students in grades 3-11. Journal of School Psychology, 69, 56–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2018.04.004

R-41

R-42 References Lin, T.-J., Jadallah, M., Anderson, R. C., Baker, A. R., Nguyen-Jahiel, K., Kim, I.-H., Kuo, L.-J., Miller, B. W., Dong, T., & Wu, X. (2015). Less is more: Teachers’ influence during peer collaboration. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 609–629. Lindsay, P. H., & Norman, D. A. (1977). Human information processing: An introduction to psychology (2nd ed.). Academic Press. Lindstrom Johnson, S., Waasdorp, T. E., Gaias, L. M., & Bradshaw, C. P. (2019). Parental responses to bullying: Understanding the role of school policies and practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(3), 475–487. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000295 Linn, M. C., & Eylon, B. S. (2006). Science education: Integrating views of learning and instruction. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 511–544). Erlbaum. Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Middleton, M. J., Ciani, K. D., Easter, M. A., O’Keefe, P. A., & Zusho, A. (2012). The strength of the relation between performance-approach and performance-avoidance goal orientations: Theoretical, methodological, and instructional implications. Educational Psychologist, 47, 281–301. https:// doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2012.722515 Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., & Patall, E. A. (2016). Motivation. In L. Corno & E. M. Anderman (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (3rd ed., pp. 91–103.). Routledge. Lisciandrello, J. (2020). Online classroom management: Five tips for teachers in transition. https:// roomtodiscover.com/online-classroom-management/ Lisette, W., Loyens, S. M. M., van Gog, T., Derous, E., & Schmidt, H. G. (2014). Is there a role for direct instruction in problem-based learning? Comparing student-constructed versus integrated model answers. Learning and Instruction, 34, 22–31. Liu, S., Wang, Y., & Nuttall, A. K. (2020). Cross-race and cross-ethnic friendships and psychological wellbeing trajectories among Asian American adolescents: Variations by school context. Developmental Psychology, 56(11), 2121–2136. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001109 Liu, W. M., Ali, S. R., Soleck, G., Hopps, J., Dunston, K., & Pickett, T., Jr. (2004). Using social class in counseling psychology research. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 3–18. LoBue, V. (2018). Violent media and aggressive behavior in children. Psychology Today. https://www .psychologytoday.com/us/blog/the-baby-scientist/201801/violent-media-and-aggressive-behavior-inchildren Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (2003). The Coping Power program for preadolescent aggressive boys and their parents: Effects at the one-year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 571–578. Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705–717. Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2015). Breaking the rules: A historical overview of goal-setting theory. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Advances in motivation science (pp. 99–126). https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.adms.2015.05.001 Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2019). The development of goal setting theory: A half century retrospective. Motivation Science, 5(2), 93–105. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000127 Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2007). Knowledge about the mind: Links between theory of mind and later metamemory. Child Development, 78, 148–167. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00990.x Loftus, E. F., & Greenspan, R. L. (2017). If I'm certain, is it true? Accuracy and confidence in eyewitness memory. Psychological Science in the Public Interest: A Journal of the American Psychological Society, 18(1), 1–2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1529100617699241 Lombardi, D., Bailey, J. M., Bickel, E. S., & Burrell, S. (2018). Scaffolding scientific thinking: Students’ evaluations and judgments during Earth science knowledge construction. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 54, 184–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.06.008 London, K., & Ceci, S. J. (2012). Competence, credibility, and reliability of children’s forensic reports: Introduction to special issue on child witness research. Developmental Review, 32, 161–164. London, R. (2019a). Rethinking recess, creating safe and inclusive playtime for all children in school. Harvard Education Press. London, R. (2019b). The right to play: Eliminating the opportunity gap in elementary school recess. Phi Delta Kappan, 101(3), 48–52. https://doi.org/10.1177/0031721719885921 Long, A., Miller, F. G., & Upright, J. J. (2019). Classroom management for ethnic-racial minority students: A meta-analysis of single-case design studies. School Psychology Quarterly: The Official Journal of the Division of School Psychology, American Psychological Association, 34(1), 1–13. Long, J. F., & Woolfolk Hoy, A. (2006). Interested instructors: A composite portrait of individual differences and effectiveness. Teaching and Teacher Education, 22, 303–314. Lonigan, C. J., Farver, J. M., Nakamoto, J., & Eppe, S. (2013). Developmental trajectories of preschool early literacy skills: A comparison of language-minority and monolingual-English children. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1943–1957. Lonigan, C. J., & Phillips, B. M. (2016). Response to instruction in preschool: Results of two randomized studies with children at significant risk of reading difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 114–129. Lorch, R. F., Lorch, E. P., Ritchey, K., McGovern, L., & Coleman, D. (2001). Effects of headings on text summarization. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 171–191. Lorde, A. (1982, February). Learning from the 60s. [Speech transcript.] Howard University. https://www .blackpast.org/african-american-history/1982-audre-lorde-learning-60s/ Loughran, J. (2019). Pedagogical reasoning: The foundation of the professional knowledge of teaching. Teachers and Teaching, 25, 523–535. https://doi.org/10.1080/13540602.2019.1633294 Love, B. L. (2019). We want to do more than survive: Abolitionist teaching and the pursuit of educational freedom. Beacon Press.

References Loveless, T. (2014). The 2014 Brown Center Report on American education: How well are American students learning? (The Brookings Institution, 2014, Vol. 3, No. 2). Loveless, T. (2020, Spring). Common Core has not worked. Forum: Education Next, 20(2). https://www .educationnext.org/common-core-has-not-worked-forum-decade-on-has-common-core-failed/ Loyens, S. M. M., Jones, S. H., Mikkers, J., & van Gog, T. (2015). Problem-based learning as a facilitator of conceptual change. Learning and Instruction, 38, 34–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.03.002 Lucariello, L. M., Nastasi, B. K., Dwyer, C., Skiba, R., DeMarie, D., & Anderman, E. M. (2016). Top 20 psychological principles for PK–12 education, Theory Into Practice, 55, 86–93. https://doi.org/10.1080/00405841.2 016.1152107 Luckner, A. E., & Pianta, R. C. (2011). Teacher student interactions in fifth grade classrooms: Relations with children’s peer behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 32, 257–266. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2011.02.010 Lui, A. M., & Bonner, S. M. (2016). Preservice and inservice teachers’ knowledge, beliefs, and instructional planning in primary school mathematics. Teaching and Teacher Education, 56, 1–13. https://doi.org /10.1016/j.tate.2016.01.015 Luke, N., & Banerjee, R. (2013). Differentiated associations between childhood maltreatment experiences and social understanding: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Developmental Review, 33, 1–28. Luna, B., Paulsen, D. J., Padmanabhan, A., & Geier, C. (2013). The teenage brain: Cognitive control and motivation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 94–100. Lyle, K. B., Bego, C. R., Hopkins, R. F., Hieb, J. L., & Ralston, P. A. S. (2020). How the amount and spacing of retrieval practice affect the short- and long-term retention of mathematics knowledge. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 277–295. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09489-x Lyons, M. D., & McQuillin, S. D. (2019). Risks and rewards of school-based mentoring relationships: A reanalysis of the student mentoring program evaluation. School Psychology Quarterly, 34(1), 76–85. https:// doi.org/10.1037/spq0000265 Mabry, L. (1999). Writing to the rubrics: Lingering effects of traditional standardized testing on direct writing assessment. Phi Delta Kappan, 80, 673–679. MacCann, C., Jiang, Y., Brown, L. E. R., Double, K. S., Bucich, M., & Minbashian, A. (2020). Emotional intelligence predicts academic performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(2), 150–186. https:// doi.org/10.1037/bul0000219 Macdonald, K., Germine, L., Anderson, A., Christodoulou, J., & Mcgrath, L. M. (2017). Dispelling the myth: Training in education or neuroscience decreases but does not eliminate beliefs in neuromyths. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01314 Mace, F. C., Belfiore, P. J., & Hutchinson, J. M. (2001). Operant theory and research on self-regulation. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed.). Erlbaum. Macionis, J. J. (2003). Sociology (9th ed.). Prentice-Hall. Macionis, J. J. (2019). Sociology (17th ed.). Pearson. Maddux, W. W., & Galinsky, A. D. (2009). Cultural borders and mental barriers: Living in and adapting to foreign cultures facilitates creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1047–1061. Madsen, C. H., Becker, W. C., Thomas, D. R., Koser, L., & Plager, E. (1968). An analysis of the reinforcing function of “sit down” commands. In R. K. Parker (Ed.), Readings in educational psychology. Allyn & Bacon. Madsen, K. (2003). The effect of accuracy of instruction, teacher delivery, and student attentiveness on musicians’ evaluation of teacher effectiveness. Journal of Research in Music Education, 51, 38–51. Maehler, C., & Schuchardt, K. (2016). Working memory in children with specific learning disorders and/or attention deficits. Learning and Individual Differences, 49, 341–347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.05.007 Magnusson, S. J., & Palincsar, A. S. (1995). The learning environment as a site of science reform. Theory Into Practice, 34, 43–50. Mainhard, T., Oudman, S., Hornstra, L., Bosker, R. J., & Goetz, T. (2018). Student emotions in class: The relative importance of teachers and their interpersonal relations with students. Learning and Instruction, 53, 109–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.07.011 Makransky, G., Andreasen, N. K., Baceviciute, S., & Mayer, R. E. (2020). Immersive virtual reality increases liking but not learning with a science simulation and generative learning strategies promote learning in immersive virtual reality. Journal of Educational Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000473 Makransky, G., Terkildsen, T. S., & Mayer, R. E. (2019). Adding immersive virtual reality to a science lab simulation causes more presence but less learning. Learning and Instruction, 60, 225–236. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.12.007 Malby, R. W., Verock, R.-E., Edwards, S. A., & Woolf, B. P. (2017). Transforming learning with new technologies (3rd ed.). Pearson. Maloy, R. W., Verock, R.-E., Edwards, S. A., & Trust, T. (2021). Transforming learning with new technologies (4th ed.). Pearson. Manfra, M. M. (2019). Action research and systematic, intentional change in teaching practice. Review of Research in Education, 43, 163–196. Manning, B. H., & Payne, B. D. (1996). Self-talk for teachers and students: Metacognitive strategies for personal and classroom use. Allyn & Bacon. Mao, Y., White, T., Sadler, P. M., & Sonnert, G. (2017). The association of precollege use of calculators with student performance in college calculus. Educational Studies Mathematics, 94, 69–83. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s10649-016-9714-7

R-43

R-44 References Marcia, J. E. (1967). Ego identity statuses: Relationship to change and self-esteem, “general maladjustment,” and authoritarianism. Journal of Personality, 35, 118–133. Marcia, J. E. (1994). The empirical study of ego identity. In H. Bosma, T. Graafsma, H. Grotebanc, & D. DeLivita (Eds.), The identity and development. Sage. Marcia, J. E. (2007). Theory and measure: The identity status interview. In M. Watzlawik & A. Born (Eds.), Capturing identity: Quantitative and qualitative methods (pp. 1–15). University Press of America. Marinova-Todd, S., Marshall, D., & Snow, C. (2000). Three misconceptions about age and L2 learning. TESOL Quarterly, 34(1), 9–34. Markman, E. M. (1992). Constraints on word learning: Speculations about their nature, origins, and domain specificity. In M. Gunnar & M. Maratsos (Eds.), Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 59–101). Erlbaum. Markowitz, A. J. (2018). Changes in school engagement as a function of No Child Left Behind: A comparative interrupted time series analysis. American Educational Research Journal, 55(4), 721–760. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0002831218755668 Marks, A. K., Patton, F., & Coll, C. G. (2011). Being bicultural: A mixed-methods study of adolescents’ implicitly and explicitly measured multiethnic identities. Developmental Psychology, 47, 270–288. Marley, S. C., & Carbonneau, K. J. (2014). Theoretical perspectives and empirical evidence relevant to classroom instruction with manipulatives. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 1–7. Marsh, H, W. (1990). The structure of academic self-concept: The Marsh/Shavelson Model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 623–636. Marsh, H. W. (2016). Cross-cultural generalizability of year in school effects: Negative effects of acceleration and positive effects of retention on academic self-concept. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 256–273. Marsh, H. W., Abduljabbar, A. S., Parker, P. D., Morin, A. J., Abdelfattah, F., Nagengast, B., & Abu-Hilal, M. A. (2015). The Big-Fish-Little-Pond Effect: Generalizability of social comparison processes over two age cohorts from Western, Asian, and Middle Eastern Islamic countries. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 258–271. Marsh, H. W., & Craven, R. (2002). The pivotal role of frames of reference in academic self-concept formation: The Big Fish Little Pond Effect. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Adolescence and education (Vol. II, pp. 83–123). Information Age. Marsh, H. W., Craven, R. G., & Martin, A. (2006a). What is the nature of self-esteem: Unidimensional and multidimensional perspectives. In M. Kernis (Ed.), Self-esteem: Issues and answers (pp. 16–25). Psychology Press. Marsh, H. W., Kuyper, H., Morin, A. J. S., Parker, P. D., & Seaton, M. (2014). Big-fish-little-pond social comparison and local dominance effects: Integrating new statistical models, methodology, design, theory and substantive implications. Learning and Instruction, 33, 50–66. Marsh, H. W., & O’Mara, A. (2008). Reciprocal effects between academic self-concept, self-esteem, achievement, and attainment over seven adolescent years: Unidimensional and multidimensional perspectives of self-concept. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 542–552. Marsh, H. W., Parker, P. D., Guo, J., Pekrun, R., & Basarkod, G. (2020). Psychological comparison processes and self-concept in relation to five distinct frame-of-reference effects: Pan-human cross-cultural generalizability over 68 countries. European Journal of Personality, 34(2), 180–202. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2232 Marsh, H. W., Pekrun, R., Parker, P. D., Murayama, K., Guo, J., Dicke, T., & Arens, A. K. (2019). The murky distinction between self-concept and self-efficacy: Beware of lurking jingle-jangle fallacies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(2), 331–353. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000281 Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Köller, O., & Baumert, J. (2006b). Integration of multidimensional self-concept and core personality constructs: Construct validation and relations to well-being and achievement. Journal of Personality, 74, 403–456. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00380.x Marsh, H. W., Xu, M., & Martin, A. J. (2012). Self-concept: A synergy of theory, method, and application. In K. R. Harris, S. Graham, & T. C. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook, Vol. 1: Theories, constructs, and critical issues (pp. 427–458). American Psychological Association. Marshall, H. H. (Ed.). (1992). Redefining student learning: Roots of educational change. Ablex. Marshall, H. H. (1996). Implications of differentiating and understanding constructivist approaches. Journal of Educational Psychology, 31, 235–240. Marshall, M. (2013). Discipline without Stress® punishments or rewards: How teachers and parents promote responsibility & learning (2nd ed.). Piper Press. Martella, A. M., Klahr, D., & Li, W. (2020). The relative effectiveness of different active learning implementations in teaching elementary school students how to design simple experiments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(8), 1582–1596. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000449 Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., & Hanish, L. D. (2014). Gendered-peer relationships in educational contexts. In L. S. Liben, R. S. Bigler, & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior: The role of gender in educational contexts and outcomes (pp. 151–187). Elsevier. Martin, J. (2006). Social cultural perspectives in educational psychology. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 595–614). Erlbaum. Marvin, K. L., Rapp, J. T., Stenske, M. T., Rojas, N. R., Swanson, G. J., & Bartlett, S. M. (2010). Response repetition as an error-correction procedure for sight-word reading: A replication and extension. Behavioral Interventions, 25, 109–127. Marzano, R. J., & Marzano, J. S. (2003, September). The key to classroom management. Educational Leadership, 61(1), 6–13. Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (2005). Constructivist theories. In B. Hopkins (Ed.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of child development. Cambridge University Press.

References Masland, L. C., & Lease, M. A. (2013). Effects of achievement motivation, social identity, and peer group norms on academic conformity. Social Psychology of Education, 16, 661–681. Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.). Van Nostrand. Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.). Harper and Row. Mason, D. A., & Good, T. L. (1993). Effects of two-group and whole-class teaching on regrouped elementary students’ mathematics achievement. American Educational Research Journal, 30, 328–360. Mason, L. (2007). Introduction: Bridging the cognitive and sociocultural approaches in research on conceptual change: Is it possible? Educational Psychologist, 42, 1–7. Master, A., Cheryan, S., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2016). Computing whether she belongs: Stereotypes undermine girls’ interest and sense of belonging in computer science. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 424–437. Matson, J. L., Matson, M. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2007). Social-skills treatments for children with autism spectrum disorders. Behavior Modification, 31, 682–707. Matsumura, L. C., Slater, S. C., & Crosson, A. (2008). Classroom climate, rigorous instruction and curriculum, and students’ interactions in urban middle schools. The Elementary School Journal, 108, 293–312. Matthews, G., Zeidner, M., & Roberts, R. D. (2002). Emotional intelligence: Science and myth. MIT Press. Matthews, J. S. (2018). When am I ever going to use this in the real world? Cognitive flexibility and urban adolescents’ negotiation of the value of mathematics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(5), 726–746. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000242 Matthews, J. S., Kizzie, K. T., Rowley, S. J., & Cortina, K. (2010). African Americans and boys: Understanding the literacy gap, tracing academic trajectories, and evaluating the role of learning-related skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 757–771. Mavilidi, M. F., & Zhong, L. (2019). Exploring the development and research focus of cognitive load theory, as described by its founders: interviewing John Sweller, Fred Paas, and Jeroen van Merriënboer. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 255–259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09463-7 May, H., Gray, A., Sirinides, P., Goldsworthy, H., Armijo, M., Sam, C., Gillespie, J. N., & Tognatta, N. (2015). Year one results from the multisite randomized evaluation of the i3 scale-up of Reading Recovery. American Educational Research Journal, 52, 547–581. May, S. (2017). Bilingual education: What the research tells us. In G. Ofelia, A. M. Y. Lin, & S. May (Eds.), Bilingual and multilingual education (pp. 81–100). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-02258-1_4 Mayer, R. E. (1984). Twenty-five years of research on advance organizers. Instructional Science, 8, 133–169. Mayer, R. E. (2001). Multimedia learning. Cambridge University Press. Mayer, R. E. (2005). Cognitive theory of multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 31–48). Cambridge University Press. Mayer, R. E. (2008). Learning and instruction (2nd ed.). Merrill/Prentice-Hall. Mayer, R. E. (2011). Applying the science of learning. Pearson. Mayer, R. E. (2012). Information processing. In K. R. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook: Vol. 1. Theories, constructs, and critical issues (pp. 85-99). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13273-004 Mayer, R. E. (2014). Principles for multimedia learning with Richard E. Mayer. http://hilt.harvard.edu /blog/principles-multimedia-learning-richard-e-mayer Mayer, R. E. (2017). How can brain research inform academic learning and instruction? Educational Psychology Review, 29, 835–846. Mayer, R. E. (2019). Computer games in education. Annual Review of Psychology, 70(1), 531–549. https:// doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102744 Mayer, R. E. (2020). Where is the learning in mobile technologies for learning? In Contemporary Educational Psychology, 60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101824 Mayer, R. E., & Wittrock, M. C. (2006). Problem solving. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 287–303). Erlbaum. McCafferty, S. G. (2004). Introduction. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 14(1), 1–6. McCaslin, M., & Good, T. (1996). The informal curriculum. In D. Berliner & R. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 622–670). Macmillan. McCaslin, M., & Good, T. L. (1998). Moving beyond management as sheer compliance: Helping students to develop goal coordination strategies. Educational Horizons, 76, 169–176. McCaslin, M., & Hickey, D. T. (2001). Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: A Vygotskian view. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 227–252). Erlbaum. McCaslin, M., & Vriesema, C. C. (2018). Co-regulation: A model for classroom research in a Vygotskian perspective. In G. A. D. Liem & D. M. McInerney (Eds.), Big theories revisited 2 (pp. 319–352). Information Age. McCormick, M. P., & O’Connor, E. E. (2015). Teacher–child relationship quality and academic achievement in elementary school: Does gender matter? Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 502–516. McCrudden, M. T., Gwen Marchand, G., Schutz, P. (2019). Mixed methods in educational psychology inquiry. Contemporary Psychology Review, 57, 1–8. McDermott, K. B., Zerr, C. L. (2019). Individual differences in learning efficiency. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 607–613. McDevitt, T. M., & Ormrod, J. E. (2020). Child development and education (7th ed.). Pearson. McDougall, P., & Vallancourt, T. (2015). Long-term outcomes of peer vicitimization in childhood and adolescence. American Psychologist, 70, 300–310. McGoey, K. E., & DuPaul, G. J. (2000). Token reinforcement and response cost procedures: Reducing disruptive behavior of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly, 15, 330–343.

R-45

R-46 References McHugh, J. R., & Barlow, D. H. (2010). The dissemination and implementation of evidence-based psychological treatments: A review of current efforts. American Psychologist, 65(2), 73–84. https://doi.org/10.1037 /a0018121 McIntosh, K., Smolkowski, K., Gion, C. M., Witherspoon, L., Bastable, E., & Girvan, E. J. (2020). Awareness is not enough: A double-blind randomized controlled trial of the effects of providing discipline disproportionality data reports to school administrators. Educational Researcher, 49, 533–537. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0013189X20939937 McIntosh, P. (1989, July/August). White privilege: Unpacking the invisible knapsack. Peace and Freedom Magazine, 10–12. http://nationalseedproject.org/white-privilege-unpacking-the-invisible-knapsack McIntosh, P. (2009). White people facing race: Uncovering the myths that keep racism in place. Commissioned by The Saint Paul Foundation and the Seeking Educational Equity and Diversity (SEED) Project. McKellar, S. E., Marchand, A. D., Diemer, M. A., Malanchuk, O., & Eccles, J. S. (2019). Threats and supports to female students’ math beliefs and achievement. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 29(2), 449–465. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12384 McKinley, J. C. (2011, January 24). Shot in the head, but getting back on his feet and on with his life. New York Times, A-16. http://www.nytimes.com/2011/01/24/us/24rehab.html?scp=7&sq=Houston%20 rehabilitation&st=cse McLeod, S. A. (2010). Kolb—learning styles. http://www.simplypsychology.org/learning-kolb.html McMillan, J. H. (2004). Classroom assessment: Principles and practice for effective instruction (3rd ed.). Allyn & Bacon. McMillan, J. H. (2019). Surveys of teachers’ grading practices and perceptions. In T. Guskey & S. Brookhart (Eds.) What we know about grading: What works, what doesn’t, and what’s next (pp. 84-112). Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Meens, D. E., & Howe, K. R. (2015). NCLB and its wake: Bad news for democracy. Teachers College Record, 117(6), pp. 1–44. http://www.tcrecord.org ID Number: 17878 Meichenbaum, D. (1977). Cognitive-behavior modifications: An integrative approach. Plenum Press. Meijer, A. M., & van den Wittenboer, G. L. H. (2004). The joint contribution of sleep, intelligence and motivation to school performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 95–106. Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49, 270–291. Melnick, S. A., & Meister, D. G. (2008). A comparison of beginning and experienced teacher concerns. Education Research Quarterly, 31(3), 39–56. Melrose, S. (2013). Facilitating constructivist learning environments using mind maps and concept maps as advance organizers. JPACTe Journal for the Practical Application of Constructivist Theory in Education, 7(1). http://www.jpacte.org/uploads/9/0/0/6/9006355/2013-1-melrose.pdf Mendle, J., & Ferrero, J. (2012). Detrimental psychological outcomes associated with pubertal timing in adolescent boys. Developmental Review, 32, 49–66. Menon, V. (2016). Working memory in children's math learning and its disruption in dyscalculia. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 125–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.014 Mercer, N. (2013). The social brain, language, and goal-directed collective thinking: A social conception of cognition and its implications for understanding how we think, teach, and learn. Educational Psychologist, 48, 148–168. Merritt, J., Lee, M. Y., Rillero, P., & Kinach, B. M. (2017). Problem-based learning in K–8 mathematics and science education: A literature review. Interdisciplinary Journal of Problem-Based Learning, 11(2), 5–17. https:// doi.org/10.7771/1541-5015.1674 Mertler, C. A. (2001). Designing scoring rubrics for your classroom. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 7(25). http://PAREonline.net/getvn.asp?v=7&n=25. Mertler, C. A., & Charles, C. M. (2005). Introduction to educational research (5th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Merton, R. K. (1948). The self-fulfilling prophecy. Antioch Review, 8, 193–210. Messano, F., Childress, S., & Atwood, J. (2020, July). How to meet students’ social-emotional and academic needs when schools reopen. Insight Brief: New Schools Venture Fund. https://medium.com/newschools-venture -fund/new-report-how-to-meet-students-social-emotional-and-academic-needs-when-schools-reopen -818708430ea3 Messina, J. P., & Surprenant, C. W. (2015). Situationism and the neglect of negative moral education. Ethical Theory and Moral Practices, 18, 835–849. MET Project. (2013, January). Ensuring fair and reliable measures of teaching effectiveness: Culminating findings from the MET Projects’ three-year study. Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation. Metcalfe, J., & Greene, M. J. (2007). Metacognition of agency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 184–199. Metzler, C. W., Biglan, A., Rusby, J. C., & Sprague, J. R. (2001). Evaluation of a comprehensive behavior management program to improve school-wide positive behavior support. Education and Treatment of Children, 24(4), 448–470. Meyer, D. K., & Anderman, L. H. (in press). The relevance of educational psychology research in teacher education. In P. Schutz & K. Muis Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (4th ed.). American Psychological Association, Division 15. Michou, A., Matos, L., Gargurevich, R., Gumus, B., & Herrera, D. (2016). Building on the enriched hierarchical model of achievement motivation: Autonomous and controlling reasons underlying mastery goals. Psychologica Belgica, 56(3), 269–287.

References Mickelson, R. A., Bottia, M. C., & Lambert, R. (2013). Effects of school racial composition on K–12 mathematics outcomes: A metaregression analysis. Review of Educational Research, 83, 121–158. Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. (2001). Performance-approach goals: Good for what, for whom, under what circumstances, and at what cost? Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 77–86. Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., Middleton, M., Maehr, M. L., Urdan, T., Anderman, L. H., Anderman, E. M., & Roeser, R. (1998). The development and validation of scales assessing students’ achievement goal orientations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 113–131. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97. Miller, G. A. (2005). Tips for getting children’s attention. Early Childhood Today, 19. Miller, N., & Harrington, H. J. (1993). Social categorization and intergroup acceptance: Principles for the development and design of cooperative learning teams. In R. Hertz-Lasarowitz & N. Miller (Eds.), Interaction in cooperative groups: The theoretical anatomy of group learning (pp. 203–227). Cambridge University Press. Miller, P. H. (2016). Theories of developmental psychology (6th ed.). Worth. Miller, R. B. (1962). Analysis and specification of behavior for training. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Training research and education: Science edition. Wiley. Miller, S. A. (2005). Tips for getting children’s attention. Early Childhood Today. https://www.scholastic.com /teachers/articles/teaching-content/teacher-tips-getting-childrens-attention/ Miller-Cotto, D., & Byrnes, J. P. (2020). What’s the best way to characterize the relationship between working memory and achievement? An initial examination of competing theories. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112, 1074–1084. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000395 Milner, H. R., IV. (2003). Teacher reflection and race in cultural contexts: History, meaning, and methods in teaching. Theory Into Practice, 42(3), 173–180. Milner, H. R., IV. (2010). Start where you are but don’t stay there: Understanding diversity, opportunity gaps, and teaching in today’s schools. Harvard Education Press. Milner, H. R., IV. (2013). Rethinking achievement gap talk in urban education. Urban Education, 48, 3–8. Milner, H. R., IV. (2015). Rac(e)ing to class: Confronting poverty and race in schools and classrooms. Harvard Education Press. Miranda, T. Z. (2008). Bilingual education for all students: Still standing after all these years. In L. S. Verplaetse & N. Migliacci (Eds.), Inclusive pedagogy for English language learners: A handbook of researchinformed practices (pp. 257–275). Erlbaum. Mischel, W. (2014). The Marshmellow Test: Why self-control is the engine of success. Little, Brown and Company. Mitchell, C. (2020, February). The nation’s English-learner population has surged: 3 things to know. Education Week. https://www.edweek.org/leadership/the-nations-english-learner-population-has -surged-3-things-to-know/2020/02 Mitchell, M. (1993). Situational interest: Its multifaceted structure in the secondary school mathematics classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 424–436. Mitchison, D., Mond, J., Bussey, K., Griffiths, S., Trompeter, N., Lonergan, A., Pike, M. K., Murray, S. B., & Hay, P. (2020). DSM-5 full syndrome, other specified, and unspecified eating disorders in Australian adolescents: Prevalence and clinical significance. Psychological Medicine, 50(6), 981–990. https:// doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000898 Moè, A., & Katz, I. (2020). Self-compassionate teachers are more autonomy supportive and structuring whereas self-derogating teachers are more controlling and chaotic: The mediating role of need satisfaction and burnout. Teaching and Teacher Education, 96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2020.103173 Moeller, J., Brackett, M. A., Ivcevic, Z., & White, A. E. (2020). High school students’ feelings: Discoveries from a large national survey and an experience sampling study. Learning and Instruction, 66, 101301. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.101301 Moll, L. (2014). L. S. Vygotsky and education. Routledge. Moll, L. C., Amanti, C., Neff, D., & Gonzalez, N. (1992). Funds of knowledge for teaching: Using a qualitative approach to connect homes and classrooms. Theory Into Practice, 31, 132–141. Moller, A. C., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). Choice and ego-depletion: The moderating role of autonomy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(8), 1024–1036. Möller, J., & Pohlmann, B. (2010). Achievement differences and self-concept differences: Stronger associations for above or below average students? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 435–450. Monroe, C. R., & Obidah, J. E. (2002, April). The impact of cultural synchronization on a teacher’s perceptions of disruption: A case study of an African American middle school classroom. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA. Montroy, J. J., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., McClelland, M. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2016). The development of self-regulation across early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 52(11), 1744–1762. https:// doi.org/10.1037/dev0000159 Montrul, S. (2010). Dominant language transfer in adult second language learners and heritage speakers. Second Language Research, 26, 293–327. Moos, D. C. (2018). Emerging classroom technology: Using self-regulation principles as a guide for effective implementation. In D. H. Schunk, & J. A. Greene (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (2nd ed., pp. 243–253). Routledge. Morehead, J. (2012). Stanford University’s Carol Dweck on the growth mindset and education. https:// onedublin.org/2012/06/19/stanford-universitys-carol-dweck-on-the-growth-mindset-and-education/

R-47

R-48 References Morehead, K., Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Blasiman, R., & Hollis, R. B. (2019). Note-taking habits of 21st century college students: Implications for student learning, memory, and achievement. Memory, 27, 807–819. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2019.1569694 Morehead, K., Rhodes, M. G., & DeLozier, S. (2016). Instructor and student knowledge of study strategies. Memory, 24, 257–271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.1001992 Morgan, H. (2009). What every teacher needs to know to teach Native American students. Multicultural Education, 16(4), 10–12. Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Hillemeir, M. M., Mattison, R., Maczuga, S., Li, H., & Cook, M. (2015). Minorities are disproportionately underrepresented in special education: Longitudinal evidence across five disability conditions. Educational Researcher, 44, 278–292. Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Hillemeier, M. M., Pin, W. H., & Maczuga, S. (2019). Kindergarten children’s executive functions predict their second-grade academic achievement and behavior. Child Development, 90, 1802–1816. Morgenroth, T., Ryan, M. K., & Peters, K. (2015). The motivational theory of role modeling: How role models influence role aspirants’ goals. Review of General Psychology, 19(4), 465–483. https://doi.org/10.1037 /gpr0000059 Morin, A. (2020, July). Pros and cons of zero-tolerance policies in schools. VeryWellFamily. https://www .very wellfamily.com/the-pros-and-cons-of-zero-tolerance-policies-1094916 Morin, H. K., Bradshaw, C. P., & Kush, J. M. (2018). Adjustment outcomes of victims of cyberbullying: The role of personal and contextual factors. Journal of School Psychology, 70, 74–88. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.jsp.2018.07.002 Morin, V. A., & Miller, S. P. (1998). Teaching multiplication to middle school students with mental retardation. Education & Treatment of Children, 21, 22–36. Morine-Dershimer, G. (2006). Instructional planning. In J. Cooper (Ed.), Classroom teaching skills (7th ed., pp. 20–54). Houghton-Mifflin. Morris, D. B., Usher, E. L., & Chen, J. A. (2017). Reconceptualizing the sources of teaching self-efficacy: A critical review of emerging literature. Educational Psychology Review, 29, 95–833. https://doi.org/10.1007 /s10648-016-9378-y Morris, E. W., & Perry, B. L. (2016). The punishment gap: School suspension and racial disparities in achievement. Social Problems, 63(1), 68–86. Morrow, L. M., & Weinstein, C. (1986). Encouraging voluntary reading: The impact of a literature program on children’s use of library centers. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 330–346. Morton, M. H., Dworsky, A., Matjasko, J. L., Curry, S. R., Schlueter, D., Chávez, R., & Farrell, A. F. (2018). Prevalence and correlates of youth homelessness in the United States. Journal of Adolescent Health, 62(1), 14–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.10.006 Moshman, D. (1982). Exogenous, endogenous, and dialectical constructivism. Developmental Review, 2, 371–384. Moshman, D. (1997). Pluralist rational constructivism. Issues in Education, 3, 229–234. Moshman, D. (2021). Reasoning, argumentation, and deliberative democracy. Routledge. Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 33–52. Mueller, P. A., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2014). The pen is mightier than the keyboard: Advantages of longhand over laptop note taking. Psychological Science, 25, 1159–1168. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614524581 Muenks, K., & Miele, D. B. (2017). Students’ thinking about effort and ability: The role of developmental, contextual, and individual difference factors. Review of Educational Research, 87(4), 707–735. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0034654316689328 Muenks, K., Wigfield, A., Yang, J. S., & O’Neal, C. R. (2017). How true is grit? Assessing its relations to high school and college students’ personality characteristics, self-regulation, engagement, and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109, 599–620. Muhonen, H., Rasku-Puttonen, H., Pakarinen, E., Poikkeus, A. M., & Lerkkanen, M. K. (2016). Scaffolding through dialogic teaching in early school classrooms. Teaching and Teacher Education, 55, 143–154. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2016.01.007 Muldner, K., Lam, R., & Chi, M. T. H. (2014). Comparing learning from observing and from human tutoring. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 69–85. Mulet, J., van de Leemput, C., & Amadieu, F. (2019). A critical literature review of perceptions of tablets for learning in primary and secondary schools. Educational Psychology Review, 31(3), 631–662. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09478-0 Mullins, T. S., Campbell, E. M., & Hogeveen, J. (2020). Neighborhood deprivation shapes motivational-neurocircuit recruitment in children. Psychological Science, 31(7), 881–889. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0956797620929299 Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Foy, P., Kelly, D. L., & Fishbein, B. (2020). TIMSS 2019 International Results in Mathematics and Science. Boston College, TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center. https:// timssandpirls.bc.edu/timss2019/international-results/ Mun, R. U., Langley, S. D., Ware, S., Gubbins, E. J., Siegle, D., Callahan, C. M., McCoach, D. B., & Hamilton, R. (2016). Effective practices for identifying and serving English learners in gifted education: A systematic review of the literature. National Center for Gifted Education, University of Connecticut. https://ncrge.uconn.edu /wp-content/uploads/sites/982/2016/01/NCRGE_EL_Lit-Review.pdf Murayama, K., & Elliot, A. J. (2009). The joint influence of personal achievement goals and classroom goal structures on achievement-relevant outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 432–447. Murdock, S. G., O’Neill, R. E., & Cunningham, E. (2005). A comparison of results and acceptability of functional behavioral assessment procedures with a group of middle school students with emotional/behavioral disorders (E/BD). Journal of Behavioral Education, 14, 5–18.

References Murdock, T. A., & Anderman, E. M. (2006). Motivational perspectives on student cheating: Toward an integrated model of academic dishonesty. Educational Psychologist, 42, 129–145. Murdock, T. B., Hale, N. M., & Weber, M. J. (2001). Predictors of cheating among early adolescents: Academic and social motivations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 96–115. Murphey, D., & Sacks, V. (2019). Supporting students with adverse childhood experiences: How educators and schools can help. American Educator. https://www.aft.org/ae/summer2019/murphey_sacks Murphy, N. A., Carbone, P. S., & the Council on Children with Disabilities. (2008). Promoting the participation of children with disabilities in sports, recreation, and physical activities. Pediatrics, 12(15), 1057–1061. Murphy, P. K., & Benton, S. L. (2010). The new frontier of educational neuropsychology: Unknown opportunities and unfulfilled hopes. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 153–155. Murphy, P. K., Firetto, C. M., Wei, L., Li, M., & Croninger, R. M. V. (2016). What REALLY works: Optimizing classroom discussions to promote comprehension and critical-analytic thinking. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 27–35. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215624215 Murphy, P. K., Greene, J. A., Firetto, C. M., Hendrick, B. D., Li, M., Montalbano, C., & Wei, L. (2018). Quality talk: Developing students’ discourse to promote high-level comprehension. American Educational Research Journal, 55, 1113–1160. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831218771303 Murphy, P. K., Greene, J. A., Firetto, C. M., Li, M., Lobczowski, N. G., Duke, R. F., Wei, L., & Croninger, R. M. V. (2017). Exploring the influence of homogeneous versus heterogeneous grouping on students’ text-based discussions and comprehension. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51, 336–355. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.09.003 Murphy, P. K., Rowe, M. L., Ramani, G., & Silverman, R. (2014). Promoting critical-analytic thinking in children and adolescents at home and in school. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 561–578. Murphy, P. K., Wilkinson, I. A. G., Soter, A. O., Hennessey, M. N., & Alexander, J. F. (2009). Examining the effects of classroom discussion on students’ comprehension of text: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 740–764. Murray, D. W., Rabiner, D. L., Kuhn, L., Pan, Y., & Sabet, R. F. (2018). Investigating teacher and student effects of the Incredible Years Classroom Management Program in early elementary school. Journal of School Psychology, 67, 119–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2017.10.004 Muschkin, C. G., Glennie, E., & Beck, A. N. (2014). Peer contexts: Do old for grade and retained peers influence student behavior in middle school? Teachers College Record, 116(4). http://www.tcrecord.org ID Number: 17405 Myers, D. G. (2005). Exploring psychology (6th ed. in modules). Worth. Myers, D. G., & DeWall, C. N. (2018). Psychology (12th ed.). Worth Publishers. Nadler, J. T., & Clark, M. H. (2011). Stereotype threat: A meta-analysis comparing African Americans to Hispanic Americans. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 41, 872–890. NAEA. (2019). A conversation with Howard Gardner. Art Education, 72, 55–58. https://doi.org/10.1080 /00043125.2019.1603514 NAEP. (2018). Nation’s Report Card: Technology and Engineering Literacy (TEL). https://www.nationsreportcard. gov/tel_2018_highlights/ Nagata, J. M., Ganson, K. T., & Murray, S. B. (2020). Eating disorders in adolescent boys and young men: An update. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 32(4), 476–481. https://doi.org/10.1097/MOP.0000000000000911 NAGC. (2019). What is giftedness? National Association for Gifted Children. https://www.nagc.org /resources-publications/resources/what-giftedness Nagle, R. (2019). The U.S. has spent more erasing Native languages than saving them. High Country News. https://www.hcn.org/issues/51.21-22/indigenous-affairs-the-u-s-has-spent-more-money-erasing-native -languages-than-saving-them# Nakajima, N., Jung, H., Pradhan, M., Hasan, A., Kinnell, A., & Brinkman, S. (2020). Gender gaps in cognitive and social-emotional skills in early primary grades: Evidence from rural Indonesia. Developmental Science, 23(5). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12931 Nancekivell, S. E., Shah, P., & Gelman, S. A. (2020). Maybe they’re born with it, or maybe it’s experience: Toward a deeper understanding of the learning style myth. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112, 221–235. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000366 National Alliance of Black School Educators. (2002). Addressing over-representations of African American students in special education: The prereferral intervention process. Council for Exceptional Education. National Assessment of Educational Progress. (2020). National Report Card. U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics. https://www.nationsreportcard .gov/mathematics/nation/groups/?grade=4 National Center for Education Statistics. (2020, May). Racial/ethnic enrollment in public schools. https://nces .ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator_cge.asp National Center for Homeless Education. (2021). National overview. http://profiles.nche.seiservices.com /ConsolidatedStateProfile.aspx National Center on Family Homelessness. (2014). America’s youngest outcasts: A report card on child homelessness. American Institutes for Research. National Commission on Teaching and America’s Future. (2003). No dream denied: A pledge to America’s children. Author. National Immigration Forum. (2020, November). Fact Sheet: U.S. refugee resettlement. https://immigrationforum .org/article/fact-sheet-u-s-refugee-resettlement/ National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Early Child Care Research Network. (2005a). Child care and child development. Guilford Press.

R-49

R-50 References National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Early Child Care Research Network. (2005b). Pathways to reading: The role of oral language in the transition to reading. Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 428–442. National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2021). Monitoring the Future Study: Trends in prevalence of various drugs. https://www.drugabuse.gov/drug-topics/trends-statistics/monitoring-future/monitoring-future -study-trends-in-prevalence-various-drugs National Science Foundation. (2020). Doctoral recipients from U.S. universities 2019. Special Report NSF 21–308. https://ncses.nsf.gov/pubs/nsf21308/assets/report/nsf21308-report.pdf National Science Foundation. (2019). Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in science and engineering: 2019. National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics. Special Report NSF 19–304. https://ncses .nsf.gov/pubs/nsf19304/ Navarro, R. L., Flores, L. Y., & Worthington, R. L. (2007). Mexican American middle school students’ goal intentions in mathematics and science: A test of social cognitive career theory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 320–335. Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, A., Boykin, W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., Halpern, D. F., Loehlin, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., & Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77–101. Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111, 486–511. Nemec, P. B., Spagnolo, A. C., & Soydan, A. S. (2017). Can you hear me now? Teaching listening skills. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 40(4), 415–417. https://doi.org/10.1037/prj0000287 Neumeister, K. L. S., & Cramond, B. (2004). E. Paul Torrance (1915–2003). American Psychologist, 59, 179. Newsom, J. S. (Producer & Director). (2011). Miss representation [Motion picture]. Girls' Club Entertainment. Newton, P. M., & Salvi, A. (2020). How common is belief in the learning styles neuromyth, and does it matter? A pragmatic systematic review. Frontiers in Education, 5, 602451. https://doi.org/10.3389 /feduc.2020.602451 Nguyen, B. M. D., Noguera, P., Adkins, N., & Teranishi, R. T. (2019). Ethnic discipline gap: Unseen dimensions of racial disproportionality in school discipline. American Educational Research Journal, 56(5), 1973– 2003. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831219833919 Nguyen, U. S., Smith, S., & Granja, M. R. (2020, October). Young children in deep poverty: Racial/ethnic disparities and child well-being compared to other income groups. National Center for Children in Poverty. https:// www.nccp.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/10/Deep-Poverty-Report-11.11.20_Final.pdf Nicholls, J. G., & Miller, A. (1984). Conceptions of ability and achievement motivation. In R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education. Vol. 1: Student Motivation (pp. 39–73). Academic Press. NIDA. (2020, June 10). Prevention principles. https://www.drugabuse.gov/publications/preventing-drug -use-among-children-adolescents/prevention-principles NIDDK. (2021). Diabetes. National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases. https://www .niddk.nih.gov/health-information/diabetes Nieto, S., & Bode, P. (2012). Affirming diversity: The sociopolitical context of multicultural education (6th ed.). Pearson. NIMH. (2021). National Institute of Mental Health. Suicide. https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/statistics /suicide.shtml Nisbett, R. E. (2009). Intelligence and how to get it: Why schools and cultures count. W. W. Norton. Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2012). Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67, 130–159. Noddings, N. (1995). Teaching themes of care. Phi Delta Kappan, 76, 675–679. Noddings, N. (2013). Standardized curriculum and loss of creativity, Theory Into Practice, 52, 210–215. https://doi.org/10.1080/00405841.2013.804315 Noe-Bustamante, L., Mora, L., & Lopez, M. H. (2020, August 11). About one-in-four U.S. Hispanics have heard of Latinx, but just 3% use it. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/hispanic/wp-content /uploads/sites/5/2020/08/PHGMD_2020.08.11_Latinx_FINAL.pdf Noguera, P. (2005). The racial achievement gap: How can we assume an equity of outcomes. In L. Johnson, M. E. Finn, & R. Lewis (Eds.), Urban education with an attitude. SUNY Press. Nokes, J. D., Dole, J. A., & Hacker, D. J. (2007). Teaching high school students to use heuristics while reading historical texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 492–504. Nokes-Malach, T. J., & Mestre, J. P. (2013). Toward a model of transfer as sense-making. Educational Psychologist, 48, 184–207. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2013.807556 Nolen, S. B. (2020). A situative turn in the conversation on motivation theories. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61, 101866. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101866 Norbert, F. (2005). Research findings on early first language attrition: Implications for the discussion of critical periods in language acquisition. Language Learning, 55(3), 491–531. Northwest Education Association. (2015). The case for growth: Why measure student learning? NWEA. Novack, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). Learning from gesture: How our hands change our minds. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 405–412. Nucci, L., & Ilten-Gee, R. (2018). Moral education. In M. D Waggoner & N. C. Walker (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of religion and American education (pp. 117–133). Oxford University Press. Nucci, L. P. (2001). Education in the moral domain. Cambridge Press. Nucci, L. P. (2009). Nice is not enough: Facilitating moral development. Pearson. Nussbaum, E. M. (2011). Argumentation, dialogue theory, and probability modeling: Alternative frameworks for argumentation research in education. Educational Psychologist, 46, 84–106. https://doi.org /10.1080/00461520.2011.558816

References Nussbaum, E. M. (2021). Critical integrative argumentation: Toward complexity in students’ thinking. Educational Psychologist, 56(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2020.1845173 Nussbaum, M. (2020). Critical integrative argumentation: Toward complexity in students’ thinking. Educational Psychologist. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2020.1845173 Nyberg, L., & Pudas, S. (2019). Successful memory aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 219–243. Nylund, D. (2000). Treating Huckleberry Finn: A new narrative approach to working with kids diagnosed ADD/ ADHD. Jossey-Bass. O’Connor, C. (1997). Dispositions toward (collective) struggle and educational resilience in the inner city: A case analysis of six African American high school students. American Educational Research Journal, 34, 593–629. O’Doherty, J. P., Cockburn, J., & Pauli, W. M. (2017). Learning, reward, and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 73–100. O’Donnell, A. M. (Ed.). (2002, Winter). Promoting thinking through peer learning. Theory Into Practice, 41(1). O’Donnell, A. M. (2006). The role of peers and group learning. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 781–802). Erlbaum. O’Donnell, A. M., & O’Kelly, J. (1994). Learning from peers: Beyond the rhetoric of positive results. Educational Psychology Review, 6, 321–350. O’Leary, K. D., & O’Leary, S. (Eds.). (1977). Classroom management: The successful use of behavior modification (2nd ed.). Pergamon. O’Leary, S. (1995). Parental discipline mistakes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 11–13. O’Mara, A. J., Marsh, H. W., Craven, R. G., & Debus, R. L. (2006). Do self-concept interventions make a difference? A synergistic blend of construct validation and meta-analysis. Educational Psychologist, 41, 181–206. O'Neill, J. C., Marraccini, M. E., Bledsoe, S. E., Knotek, S. E., & Tabori, A. V. (2020). Suicide postvention practices in schools: School psychologists’ experiences, training, and knowledge. School Psychology, 35(1), 61–71. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000331 O’Reilly, T., Wang, Z., & Sabatini, J. (2019). How much knowledge is too little? When a lack of knowledge becomes a barrier to comprehension. Psychological Science, 30, 1344–1351. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0956797619862276 Oakes, J. (2005). Keeping track. Yale University Press. Oakes, J. (2017). Public scholarship: Education research for a diverse democracy. Educational Researcher, 47, 91–104. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X17746402 Odegard, T. N., & Toglia, M. P. (2013). Children as eyewitnesses: Historical background, and factors affecting children's eyewitness testimony. In R. E. Holliday & T. A. Marche (Eds.), Child forensic psychology: Victim and eyewitness memory (p. 95–118). Palgrave Macmillan. OECD. (2015). The ABC of gender equality in education: Aptitude, behaviour, confidence. OECD Publishing. OECD. (2017). PISA 2015 results (Volume V): Collaborative problem solving. Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264285521-en OECD. (2020). Girls’ and boys’ performance in PISA. PISA 2018 Results (Volume II): Where all students can succeed. OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/f56f8c26-en Oettingen, G., Schrage, J., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2015). Volition. In L. Corno & E. H. Anderman (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (3rd ed., pp. 104–117). Taylor & Francis. Ogbu, J. U. (1987). Variability in minority school performance: A problem in search of an explanation. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 18, 312–334. Ogbu, J. U. (1997). Understanding the school performance of urban blacks: Some essential background knowledge. In H. Walberg, O. Reyes, & R. P. Weissberg (Eds.), Children and youth: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 190–240). Ablex. Ohio Department of Education. (2014). Myths about second language learning. Ohio Department of Education. http://education.ohio.gov/Topics/Other-Resources/Limited-English-Proficiency/Research/Myths -about-Second-Language-Learning Okagaki, L. (2001). Triarchic model of minority children’s school achievement. Educational Psychologist, 36, 9–20. Okagaki, L. (2006). Ethnicity, learning. In P. Alexander & P. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 615–634). Erlbaum. Okkinga, M., van Steensel, R., van Gelderen, A. J. S., van Schooten, E., Sleegers, P. J. C., & Arends, L. R. (2018). Effectiveness of reading-strategy interventions in whole classrooms: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 30(4), 1215–1239. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-018-9445-7 O’Leary, A. P., & Sloutsky V. M. (2019). Components of metacognition can function independently across development. Developmental Psychology, 55, 315–328. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000645 Olsen, B., & Buchanan, R. (2019). An investigation of teachers encouraged to reform grading practices in secondary schools. American Educational Research Journal, 56(5), 2004–2039. https://doi.org /10.3102/0002831219841349 Olson, D. R. (2004). The triumph of hope over experience in the search for “what works”: A response to Slavin. Educational Researcher, 33(1), 24–26. Olsson, M., & Martiny, S. E. (2018). Does exposure to counterstereotypical role models influence girls’ and women’s gender stereotypes and career choices? A review of social psychological research. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02264 Orange, C. (2005). 44 smart strategies for avoiding classroom mistakes. Corwin Press. Orben, A. (2020). Teenagers, screens and social media: A narrative review of reviews and key studies. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 55(4), 407–414. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-019-01825-4

R-51

R-52 References Orben, A., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). Screens, teens, and psychological well-being: Evidence from three time-use-diary studies. Psychological Science, 30(5), 682–696. https://doi.org/10.1177 /0956797619830329 Orfield, G., Ee, J., Frankenberg, E., & Siegel-Hawley, G. (2016). Brown at 62: School segregation by race, poverty, and state. Civil Rights Project / Proyecto Derechos Civiles, UCLA: Author. https://civilrightsproject .ucla.edu Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2007). Understanding the brain: The birth of a learning science. Author. Orlando, L., & Machado, A. (1996). In defense of Piaget’s theory: A reply to 10 common criticisms. Psychological Review, 103, 143–164. Ormrod, J. E. (2020). Human learning (8th ed.). Pearson. ORR. (2020). History. Office of Refugee Resettlement, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. https://www.acf.hhs.gov/orr/about/history Orr, A., Baum, J., Brown, J., Gill, E., Kahn, E., & Salem, A. (2015). Schools in transition: A guide for supporting transgender students in K–12 schools. American Civil Liberties Union, Gender Spectrum, Human Rights Campaign, National Center for Lesbian Rights, and National Education Association. https://www.aclu .org/sites/default/files/field_document/schools_in_transition_6.3.16.pdf Osborne, J. F., Borko, H., Fishman, E., Gomez Zaccarelli, F., Berson, E., Busch, K. C., Reigh, E., & Tseng, A. (2019). Impacts of a practice-based professional development program on elementary teachers’ facilitation of and student engagement with scientific argumentation. American Educational Research Journal, 56(4), 1067–1112. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831218812059 Osborne, J. W., & Jones, B. D. (2011). Identification with academics and motivation to achieve in school: How the structure of the self influences academic outcomes. Educational Psychology Review, 23, 131–158. Osenk, I., Williamson, P., & Wade, T. D. (2020). Does perfectionism or pursuit of excellence contribute to successful learning? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Assessment, 32(10), 972–983. https:// doi.org/10.1037/pas0000942 Ostarek, M., & Huettig, F. (2019). Six challenges for embodiment research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(6), 593–599. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419866441 Osterhage, J., Usher, E. L., Douin, T. A., & Bailey, W. M. (2019). Opportunities for self-evaluation increase student calibration in an introductory biology course. CBE-Life Sciences Education, 18(2), 1–10. https:// doi.org/10.1187/cbe.18-10-0202 Osterman, K. F. (2000). Students’ need for belonging in the school community. Review of Educational Research, 70, 323–367. Ostrov, J. M., & Godleski, S. A. (2010). Toward an integrated gender-linked model of aggression subtypes in early and middle childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 233–242. Otto, B. (2010). Language development in early childhood (5th ed.). Merrill. Ovando, C. J., & Collier, V. P. (1998). Bilingual and ESL classrooms: Teaching in multicultural contexts (2nd ed.). McGraw-Hill. Owens, R. D., Jr. (2020). Language development (10th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Pearson. Ownston, R. D. (2012). Computer games and the quest to find their affordances for learning. Educational Researcher, 41(3), 105–106. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X12439231 Pahlke, E., Hyde, J. S., & Allison, C. M. (2014). The effects of single-sex compared with coeducational schooling on students’ performance and attitudes: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140(4), 1042–1072. https://doi-org/10.1037/a0035740 Pai, H.-H., Sears, D. A., & Maeda, Y. (2015). Effects of small-group learning on transfer: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 79–102. Paik, E. S., & Schraw, G. (2013). Learning with animation and illusions of understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 278–289. Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford University Press. Paivio, A. (2006). Mind and its evolution; a dual coding theoretical interpretation. Erlbaum. Pajares, F. (1997). Current directions in self-efficacy research. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 1–49). JAI Press. Pajares, F. (2000, April). Seeking a culturally attentive educational psychology. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA. http://www.uky .edu/~eushe2/Pajares/AERA2000Discussant.html Pajares, F. (2003). William James: Our father who begot us. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Educational psychology: A century of contributions (pp. 41–64). Erlbaum. Pajares, F. (2006). Self-efficacy during childhood and adolescence: Implications for teachers and parents. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Adolescence and education, Vol. 5: Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 339–367). Information Age Publishing. Pajares, F. (2008). Self-efficacy information. https://www.uky.edu/~eushe2/Bandura/banconversion.html Pajares, F., & Schunk, D. H. (2001). Self-beliefs and school success: Self-efficacy, self-concept, and school achievement. In R. Riding & S. Rayner (Eds.), Perception (pp. 239–266). Ablex. Pajares, F., & Schunk, D. H. (2002). Self and self-belief in psychology and education: An historical perspective. In J. Aronson & D. Cordova (Eds.), Psychology of education: Personal and interpersonal forces (pp. 1–19). Academic Press. Pajares, F., & Usher, E. L. (2008). Self-efficacy, motivation, and achievement in school from the perspective of reciprocal determinism. In M. Maehr, T. C. Urdan, & S. Karabenick (Eds.), Advances in

References motivation and achievement. Vol. 15: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 391–423). Emerald Group Publishing Limited. Palardy, G. (2013). High school socioeconomic segregation and student attainment. American Educational Research Journal, 50, 714–754. Palincsar, A. S. (1998). Social constructivist perspectives on teaching and learning. In J. T. Spence, J. M. Darley, & D. J. Foss (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology (pp. 345–375). Annual Reviews. Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 117–175. Palincsar, A. S., Fitzgerald, M., Marcum, M., & Sherwood, C. (2017). Examining the work of “scaffolding” in theory and practice: A case study of 6th graders and their teacher interacting with one another, an ambitious science curriculum, and mobile devices. International Journal of Educational Research, 90, 191–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.IJER.2017.11.006 Palincsar, A. S., & Herrenkohl, L. R. (2002). Designing collaborative learning contexts. Theory Into Practice, 61, 26–32. Palincsar, A. S., Magnusson, S. J., Collins, K. M., & Cutter, J. (2001). Promoting deep understanding of science in students with disabilities in inclusion classrooms. Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 24(1), 15–32. Pan, S. C., Tajran, J., Lovelett, J., Osuna, J., & Rickard, T. C. (2019). Does interleaved practice enhance foreign language learning? The effects of training schedule on Spanish verb conjugation skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(7), 1172–1188. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000336 Panadero, E. (2017). A review of self-regulated learning: Six models and four directions for research. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017 00422 Panadero, E., Jonsson, A., & Botella, J. (2017). Effects of self-assessment on self-regulated learning and selfefficacy: Four meta-analyses. Educational Research Review, 22, 74–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.edurev.2017.08.004 Panitz, T. (1996). A definition of collaborative vs cooperative learning. http://www.londonmet.ac.uk /deliberations/collaborative-learning/panitz-paper.cfm Papageorge, N. W., Gershenson, S., & Kyungmin, K. (2020). Teacher expectations matter. Review of Economics and Statistics, 102, 234–251. Papert, S. (1980). Mindstorms: Children, computers, and powerful ideas. Basic Books. Papert, S. (1991). Situating constructionism. In I. Harel & S. Papert (Eds.), Constructionism (pp. 1–11). Ablex. Papert, S. (1993). The children’s machine: Rethinking school in the age of the computer. Basic Books. Paris, D. (2012). Culturally sustaining pedagogy: A needed change in stance, terminology, and practice. Educational Researcher, 41(3), 93–97. Paris, S. G., & Ayres, L. R. (1994). Becoming reflective students and teachers: With portfolios and authentic assessment. American Psychological Association. Park, B., Flowerday, T., & Brünken, R. (2015). Cognitive and affective effects of seductive details in multimedia learning. Computers and Human Behavior, 44, 267–278. Park, D., Gunderson, E. A., Tsukayama, E., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2016). Young children’s motivational frameworks and math achievement: Relation to teacher-reported instructional practices, but not teacher theory of intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(3), 300–313. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000064 Park, D., Yu, A., Baelen, R. N., Tsukayama, E., & Duckworth, A. L. (2018). Fostering grit: Perceived school goal-structure predicts growth in grit and grades. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 55, 120–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.09.007 Park, G., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2013). When less is more: Effects of grade skipping on adult STEM productivity among mathematically precocious adolescents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 176–198. Parke, R. D., & Buriel, R. (2006). Socialization in the family: Ethnic and ecological perspectives. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed., Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development, pp. 553–618). Wiley. Parker, C., & Bickmore, K. (2020). Classroom peace circles: Teachers’ professional learning and implementation of restorative dialogue. Teaching and Teacher Education, 95, 103–129. Parker, K., Morin, R., Horowitz, J. M., Lopez, M. H., & Rohal, M. (2015, June 11). Multiracial in America: Proud, diverse and growing in numbers. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewsocialtrends.org /wp-content/uploads/sites/3/2015/06/2015-06-11_multiracial-in-america_final-updated.pdf Pas, E. T., Ryoo, J. H., Musci, R. J., & Bradshaw, C. P. (2019). A state-wide quasi-experimental effectiveness study of the scale-up of school-wide Positive Behavioral Interventions and Supports. Journal of School Psychology, 73, 41–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2019.03.001 Pashler, H., Bain, P., Bottge, B., Graesser, A., Koedinger, K., McDaniel, M., & Metcalfe, J. (2007). Organizing instruction and study to improve student learning (NCER 2007–2004). National Center for Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Pashler, H., McDaniel, M., Rohrer, D., & Bjork, R. (2009). Learning styles: Concepts and evidence. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9, 105–119. Patall, E. A., & Hooper, S. Y. (2017). The role of choice in understanding adolescent autonomy and academic functioning. In B. Soenens, S. Van Petegem, & M. Vansteenkiste (Eds.), Autonomy in adolescent development: Towards conceptual clarity (pp. 145–167). Routledge. Patall, E. A., & Hooper, S. Y. (2019). The promise and peril of choosing for motivation and learning. In K. Renninger & S. Hidi (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of motivation and learning (pp. 238–262). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316823279.012 Patall, E. A., Hooper, S., Vasquez, A. C., Pituch, K. A., & Steingut, R. R. (2018). Science class is too hard: Perceived difficulty, disengagement, and the role of teacher autonomy support from a daily diary perspective. Learning and Instruction, 58, 220–231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.07.004

R-53

R-54 References Patton, D. U., Hong, J. S., Williams, A. B., & Allen-Meares, P. (2013). A review of research on school bullying among African American youth: An ecological systems analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 25, 245–260. Pauk, W., & Owens, R. J. Q. (2014). How to study in college (11th ed.). Cengage—Wadsworth. Paul, R., & Elder, L. (2014). Critical thinking: Concepts and tools (7th ed.). Foundation for Critical Thinking. Paul, R., & Elder, L. (2020). The miniature guide to critical thinking concepts and tools (8th ed.). Rowman & Littlefield. Paulos, L. (2007). Multitasking madness. Scholastic Choices, 23(1), 10–13. PBS. (2017, March 15). White pride. (S1E3) [Video.] In What I hear when you say. PBS. https://www.pbs.org /whatihear/web-series/white-pride/ Pearl, R., Leung, M. C., Acker, R. V., Farmer, T. W., & Rodkin, P. C. (2007). Fourth- and fifth-grade teachers’ awareness of their classrooms’ social networks. The Elementary School Journal, 108, 25–39. Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Linguistics, 18, 41–58. Pearson, J., & Keogh, R. (2019). Redefining visual working memory: A cognitive-strategy, brainregion approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(3), 266–273. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0963721419835210 Pedaste, M., Mäeots, M., Siiman, L. A., De Jong, T., Van Riesen, S. A., Kamp, E. T., Manoli, C. C., Zacharia, Z. C., & Tsourlidaki, E. (2015). Phases of inquiry-based learning: Definitions and the inquiry cycle. Educational Research Review, 14, 47–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2015.02.00 Pekrun, R. (2017). Emotion and achievement during adolescence. Child Development Perspectives, 11(3), 215– 221. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12237 Pekrun, R. (2018). Control-value theory: A social-cognitive approach to achievement emotions. In G. A. D. Liem & D. M. McInerney (Eds.), Big theories revisited 2 (pp. 165–190). Information Age. Pekrun, R., Cusack, A., Murayama, K., Elliot, A. J., & Thomas, K. (2014). The power of anticipated feedback: Effects on students’ achievement goals and achievement emotions. Learning and Instruction, 29, 115–124. Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2006). Achievement goals and discrete achievement emotions: A theoretical model and prospective test. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 583–597. Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupinisky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in achievement settings: Exploring control–value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 531–549. Pekrun, R., Lichtenfeld, S., Marsh, H. W., Murayama, K., & Goetz, T. (2017). Achievement emotions and academic performance: Longitudinal models of reciprocal effects. Child Development, 88(5), 1653–1670. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12704 Pekrun, R., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (Eds.). (2015). International handbook of emotions in education. Taylor & Francis. Pellegrino, J. R. (2014). Assessment in the service of teaching and learning: Changes in practice enabled by recommended changes in policy. Teachers College Record, 116(11), 1–10. http://www.tcrecord.org ID Number: 17653 Pellis, S. (2006). The effects of orbital frontal cortex damage on the modulation of defensive responses by rats in playful and nonplayful social contexts. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 72–84. Peregoy, S. F., & Boyle, O. F. (2017). Reading, writing, and learning in ESL: A resource book teaching K–12 English learners. Pearson. Perez, C. (2015, June 29). U.S. has more Spanish speakers than Spain. New York Post. http://nypost .com/2015/06/29/us-has-more-spanish-speakers-than-spain/ Perez, T., Dai, T., Kaplan, A., Cromley, J. G., Brooks, W. D., White, A. C., Mara, K. R., & Balsai, M. J. (2019). Interrelations among expectancies, task values, and perceived costs in undergraduate biology achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 72, 26–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2019.04.001 Perkins, D. N., & Salomon, G. (2012). Knowledge to go: A motivational and dispositional view of transfer. Educational Psychologist, 47, 248–258. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2012.693354 Perner, J. (2000). Memory and theory of mind. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 297–312). Oxford University Press. Perry, N. E. (1998). Young children’s self-regulated learning and contexts that support it. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 715–729. Perry, N. E., & Collie, R. J. (2011, April). School climate and social and emotional learning: Predictors of early career teacher well-being and efficacy. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA. Perry, N. E., & Drummond, L. (2002). Helping young students become self-regulated researchers and writers. The Reading Teacher, 56, 298–310. Perry, N. E., Phillips, L., & Dowler, J. (2004). Examining features of tasks and their potential to promote self-regulated learning. Teachers College Record, 106, 1854–1878. Perry, N. E., & Rahim, A. (2011). Studying self-regulated learning in classrooms. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (pp. 122–136). Routledge. Perry, N. E., VandeKamp, K. O., & Mercer, L. K. (2000, April). Investigating teacher–student interactions that foster self-regulated learning. In N. E. Perry (Chair), Symposium conducted at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA. Perry, N. E., VandeKamp, K. O., Mercer, L. K., & Nordby, C. J. (2002). Investigating teacher-student interactions that foster self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 37, 5–15.

References Perry, R., & Hamm, J. (2017). An attribution perspective on competence and motivation. In A. Elliot, C. Dweck, & D. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (2nd ed., pp. 61–84). Guilford Press. Peterson, C. (2012). Children’s autobiographical memories across the years: Forensic implications of childhood amnesia and eyewitness memory for stressful events. Developmental Review, 31, 287–306. Peterson, C., & Wellman, H. (2019). Longitudinal Theory of Mind (ToM) development from preschool to adolescence with and without tom delay. Child Development, 90, 1917–1934. https://doi.org/10.1111 /cdev.13064 Peterson, G. W., Cobas, J. A., Bush, J. R., Supple, A., & Wilson, S. M. (2004). Parent-youth relationships and the self-esteem of Chinese adolescents: Collectivism versus individualism. Marriage & Family Review, 36, 173–200. Petit, M., & Hess, K. (2006). Applying Webb’s Depth of Knowledge and NAEP levels of complexity in mathematics. Center for Assessment. https://www.peoriapublicschools.org/cms/lib/IL01001530 /Centricity/Domain/28/DOK%20Math.pdf Petitto, L. A. (2009). New discoveries from the bilingual brain and mind across the life span: Implications for education. Brain, Mind, and Education, 3, 185–197. Petitto, L. A., & Kovelman, I. (2003). The bilingual paradox: How signing-speaking bilingual children help us resolve bilingual issues and teach us about the brain’s mechanisms underlying all language acquisition. Language Learning, 8(3), 5–18. Petrilli, M. J. (2020, Spring). Stay the course on national standards. Forum: Education Next, 20(2). https:// www.educationnext.org/stay-course-on-national-standards-forum-decade-on-has-common-core-failed/ Peura, P. I., Viholainen, H. J. K., Aro, T. I., Räikkönen, E. M., Usher, E. L., Sorvo, R. M. A., Klassen, R. M., & Aro, M. T. (2019). Specificity of reading self-efficacy among primary school children. Journal of Experimental Education, 87(3), 496–516. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2018.1527279 Pew Research Center. (2016, May 11). America’s shrinking middle class: A close look at changes within metropolitan areas. http://www.pewsocialtrends.org/files/2016/05/Middle-Class-Metro-Areas-FINAL.pdf Pfiffner, L., Barkley, R. A., & DuPaul, G. J. (2006). Treatment of ADHD in school settings. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed., pp. 547–588). Guilford. Phillips, D. C. (1997). How, why, what, when, and where: Perspectives on constructivism and education. Issues in Education, 3(2), 151–194. Phippen, J. W. (2015, July 5). The racial imbalance of special education. The Atlantic Monthly. https:// www.theatlantic.com/education/archive/2015/07/the-racial-imbalances-of-special-education/397775/ Phye, G. D. (1992). Strategic transfer: A tool for academic problem solving. Educational Psychology Review, 4, 393–421. Phye, G. D. (2001). Problem-solving instruction and problem-solving transfer: The correspondence issue. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 571–578. Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child (M. Cook, Trans.). Basic Books. Piaget, J. (1962). Comments on Vygotsky’s critical remarks concerning “The language and thought of the child” and “Judgment and reasoning in the child.” MIT Press. Piaget, J. (1963). Origins of intelligence in children. Norton. Piaget, J. (1964). Development and learning. In R. Ripple & V. Rockcastle (Eds.), Piaget rediscovered (pp. 7–20). Cornell University Press. Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. Free Press. Piaget, J. (1966). Response to Brian Sutton-Smith. Psychological Review, 73(1), 111–112. https:// doi.org/10.1037/h0022600 Piaget, J. (1969). Science of education and the psychology of the child. Viking. Piaget, J. (1970a). Piaget’s theory. In P. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 703–732). Wiley. Piaget, J. (1970b). The science of education and the psychology of the child. Orion Press. Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and knowledge. Edinburgh Press. Piaget, J. (1974). Understanding causality (D. Miles and M. Miles, Trans.). Norton. Piaget, J. (1985). The equilibrium of cognitive structures: The central problem of intellectual development (T. Brown & K. L. Thampy, Trans.). University of Chicago Press. Piaget, J. (1995). Sociological studies. Routledge. (Original work published 1965.) Pianta, R. C., Belsky, J., Vandergrift, N., Houts, R., & Morrison, F. J. (2008a). Classroom effects on children’s achievement trajectories in elementary school. American Educational Research Journal, 45, 365–397. Pianta, R. C., LaParo, K. M., & Hamre, B. K. (2008b). Classroom assessment scoring system manual: Pre-K. Brookes. Piff, P. K., Kraus, M. W., & Keltner, D. (2018). Unpacking the inequality paradox: The psychological roots of inequality and social class. In J. M. Olson (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 57 (pp. 53–124). https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2017.10.002 Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. Penguin. Pinker, S. (2017, January 5). What you just forgot may be “sleeping.” Wall Street Journal. http://www.wsj .com/articles/what-you-just-forgot-may-be-sleeping–1483633227 Pinquart, M., & Kauser, R. (2018). Do the associations of parenting styles with behavior problems and academic achievement vary by culture? Results from a meta-analysis. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 24(1), 75–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000149 Pinsker, J. (2019, March 28). The cult of homework. The Atlantic. https://www.theatlantic.com/education /archive/2019/03/homework-research-how-much/585889/

R-55

R-56 References Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspective on the role of student motivation in learning and teaching. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 667–686. Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33–40. Pintrich, P. R., & Zusho, A. (2002). The development of academic self-regulation: The role of cognitive and motivational factors. In A. Wigfield & J. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 249–284). Academic Press. Pisha, B., & Coyne, P. (2001). Smart for the start: The promise of universal design for learning. Remedial and Special Education, 22, 197–203. Pit-ten Cate, I. M., & Glock, S. (2018) Teacher expectations concerning students with immigrant backgrounds or special educational needs. Educational Research and Evaluation, 24(3–5), 277–294. https://doi.org/10.1080 /13803611.2018.1550839 Plant, E. A., & Peruche, B. M. (2005). The consequences of race for police officers’ responses to criminal suspects. Psychological Science, 16, 180–183. Plavnick, J. B., & Ferreri, S. J. (2013). Single-case experimental designs in educational research: A methodology for causal analyses in teaching and learning. Educational Psychology Review, 25, 549–569. Plomin, R. (2018). Blueprints: How DNA makes us who we are. MIT Press. Plomin, R., & von Stumm, S. (2018). The new genetics of intelligence. Nature Reviews Genetics, 19, 148–159. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg.2017.104 Plucker, J. A., Beghetto, R. A., & Dow, G. T. (2004). Why isn’t creativity more important to educational psychologists? Potential pitfalls and future directions in creativity research. Educational Psychology, 39(2), 83–96. Polikoff, M. S. (2015). How well aligned are textbooks to the Common Core Standards in mathematics? American Educational Research Journal, 52, 1185–1211. Polikoff, M. S. (2020). Common standards are not enough. Forum: Education Next, 20(2). https://www .educationnext.org/common-standards-arent-enough-forum-decade-on-has-common-core-failed/ Polikoff, M. S., Hardaway, T., Marsh, J. A., & Plank, D. N. (2016). Who is opposed to Common Core and why? Educational Researcher, 45(4), 263–266. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X16651087 Polio, M., & Hochbein, C. (2015). The association between standards-based grading and standardized test scores in a high school reform model. Teachers College Record, 117. 110302. http://www.tcrecord.org ID Number: 18111 Ponnock, A., Muenks, K., Morell, M., Seung Yang, J., Gladstone, J. R., & Wigfield, A. (2020). Grit and conscientiousness: Another jangle fallacy. Journal of Research in Personality, 89, 104021. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.jrp.2020.104021 Poorthuis, A. M. G., Juvonen, J., Thomaes, S., Denissen, J. J. A., Orobio de Castro, B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2015). Do grades shape students’ school engagement? The psychological consequences of report card grades at the beginning of secondary school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 842–854. Popham, W. J. (2020). Classroom assessment: What teachers need to know (9th ed.). Pearson. Popov, V., Marevic, I., Rummel, J., & Reder, L. M. (2019). Forgetting is a feature, not a bug: Intentionally forgetting some things helps us remember others by freeing up working memory resources. Psychological Science, 30, 1303–1317. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619859531 Portwood, S. G., Lawler, M. J., & Roberts, M. C. (2021). Special Issue: Adverse childhood experiences: Translating research into action. American Psychologist, 76, 181–390. Posada, G., Jacobs, A., Richmond, M., Carbonell, O. A., Alzate, G., Bustamante, M. R., & Quiceno, J. (2002). Maternal care giving and infant security in two cultures. Developmental Psychology, 38, 67–78. Posner, M. I. (1973). Cognition: An introduction. Scott, Foresman. Poteat, V. P., Scheer, J. R., & Mereish, E. H. (2014). Factors affecting academic achievement among sexual minority and gender-variant youth. In L. S. Liben, R. S. Bigler, & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior: The role of gender in educational contexts and outcomes (pp. 261–301). Elsevier. Potocki, A., Ecalle, J., & Magnan, A. (2013). Effect of computer-assisted comprehension training in lessskilled comprehenders in second grade: A one-year follow-up study. Computers and Education, 63, 131–140. Prati, G., Albanesi, C., & Cicognani, E. (2018). The relationship between sense of community in the school and students’ aggressive behavior: A multilevel analysis. School Psychology Quarterly, 33(4), 512–516. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000260 Prat-Sala, M., & Redford, P. (2010). The interplay between motivation, self-efficacy, and approaches to studying. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 283–305. Prawat, R. S. (1996). Constructivism, modern and postmodern. Issues in Education, 3, 215–226. Preckel, T., Goetz, T., & Frenzel, A. (2010). Ability grouping of gifted students: Effects on academic selfconcept and boredom. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 451–472. Premack, D. (1965). Reinforcement theory. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 13, pp. 123–180). University of Nebraska Press. Pressley, M. (1995). More about the development of self-regulation: Complex, long-term, and thoroughly social. Educational Psychologist, 30, 207–212. Pressley, M. (1996, August). Getting beyond whole language: Elementary reading instruction that makes sense in light of recent psychological research. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto. Pressley, M., & Harris, K. A. (2006). Cognitive strategies instruction: From basic research to classroom instruction. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 265– 286). Erlbaum.

References Pressley, M., & Woloshyn, V. (2000). Cognitive strategy instruction that really improves children’s academic performance (2nd ed.). Brookline Books. Price, L. F. (2005). The biology of risk taking. Educational Leadership, 62(7), 22–27. Price, W. F., & Crapo, R. H. (2002). Cross-cultural perspectives in introductory psychology (4th ed.). Wadsworth. Priniski, S. J., Rosenzweig, E. Q., Canning, E. A., Hecht, C. A., Tibbetts, Y., Hyde, J. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2019). The benefits of combining value for the self and others in utility-value interventions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(8), 1478–1497. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000343 Prinz, A., Golke, S., & Wittwer, J. (2020). To what extent do situation-model-approach interventions improve relative metacomprehension accuracy? Meta-analytic insights. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 917–949. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09558–6 Przybylski, A. K., Orben, A., & Weinstein, N. (2020). How much is too much? Examining the relationship between digital screen engagement and psychosocial functioning in a confirmatory cohort study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(9), 1080–1088. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.jaac.2019.06.017 Pugh, K. J., & Phillips, M. M. (2011). Helping students develop an appreciation for school content. Theory Into Practice, 50, 285–292. Pulfrey, C., Darnon, C., & Butera, F. (2013). Autonomy and task performance: Explaining the impact of grades on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 39–57. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029376 Pulham, E., & Graham, C. R. (2018). Comparing K–12 online and blended teaching competencies: A literature review. Distance Education, 39(3), 411–432, https://doi.org/10.1080/01587919.2018.1476840 Puncochar, J., & Fox, P. W. (2004). Confidence in individual and group decision-making: When “Two Heads” are worse than one. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 582–591. Puntambekar, S., & Hubscher, R. (2005). Tools for scaffolding students in a complex learning environment: What have we gained and what have we missed? Educational Psychologist, 40, 1–12. Putman, M., Smith, L. L., & Cassady, J. C. (2009). Promoting change through professional development: The place of teacher intentionality. Literacy Research & Instruction, 48, 207–220. Putwain, D. W., Pekrun, R., Nicholson, L. J., Symes, W., Becker, S., & Marsh, H. W. (2018). Control-value appraisals, enjoyment, and boredom in mathematics: A longitudinal latent interaction analysis. American Educational Research Journal, 55(6), 1339–1368. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831218786689 Puzio, K., Colby, G. T., & Algeo-Nichols, D. (2020). Differentiated literacy instruction: Boondoggle or best practice? Review of Educational Research, 90(4), 459–498. https://doi .org/10.3102/0034654320933536 Pyke, A. A., & LeFevre, J. A. (2011). Calculator use need not undermine direct-access ability: The roles of retrieval, calculation, and calculator use in the acquisition of arithmetic facts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 607–616. Quin, D. (2017). Longitudinal and contextual associations between teacher–student relationships and student engagement: A systematic review. Review of Educational Research, 87(2), 345–387. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0034654316669434 Rachlin, H. (2004). The science of self-control. Harvard University Press. Radford, J., Bosanquent, P., Webster, R., & Blatchford, P. (2015). Scaffolding learning for independence: Clarifying teacher and teaching assistant roles for children with special educational needs. Learning and Instruction, 35, 1–10. Radley, K. C., & Dart, E. H. (2016). Antecedent strategies to promote children’s and adolescents’ compliance with adult requests: A review of the literature. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 19(1), 39–54. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567–015–0197–3 Radvansky, G. A., & Ashcraft, M. (2018). Cognition (7th ed.). Pearson. Rai, M. K., Loschky, L. C., & Harris, R. J. (2015). The effects of stress on reading: A comparison of first-language versus intermediate second-language reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 348–363. RAINN. (2021). Children and teens: Statistics. Rape, Abuse, and Incest National Network. https://www.rainn .org/statistics/children-and-teens Ramirez, G., Shaw, S. T., & Maloney, E. A. (2018). Math anxiety: Past research, promising interventions, and a new interpretation framework. Educational Psychologist, 53(3), 145–164. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520 .2018.1447384 Ramirez, J. D., Yuen, S. D., & Ramey, D. R. (1991). Final report: Longitudinal study of structured immersion strategy, early-exit, and late-exit transitional bilingual education programs for language-minority children. Aguirre International. Ramos, A., De Fraine, B., & Verschueren, K. (2021). Learning goal orientation in high-ability and averageability students: Developmental trajectories, contextual predictors, and long-term educational outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(2), 370–389. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000476 Rau, M. A. (2020). Comparing multiple theories about learning with physical and virtual representations: Conflicting or complementary effects? Educational Psychology Review, 32, 297–325. Raudenbush, S. (1984). Magnitude of teacher expectancy effects on pupil IQ as a function of the credibility of expectancy induction: A synthesis of findings from 18 experiments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 85–97. Raudenbush, S. W. (2009). The Brown legacy and the O’Connor challenge: Transforming schools in the images of children’s potential. Educational Researcher, 38, 169–180. Raudsepp, E., & Haugh, G. P. (1977). Creative growth games. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Reardon, S. F. (2011). The widening academic achievement gap between the rich and the poor: New evidence and possible explanations. In R. Murnane & G. Duncan (Eds.), Whither opportunity? Rising inequality and

R-57

R-58 References the uncertain life chances of low-income children. Russell Sage Foundation Press. See more at http://cepa .stanford.edu/content/widening-academic-achievement-gap-between-rich-and-poor-new-evidence-and -possible#sthash.gUtBj6hG.dpuf Reardon, S. F., Kalogrides, D., Fahle, E. M., Podolsky, A., & Zárate, R. C. (2018). The relationship between test item format and gender achievement gaps on math and ELA tests in fourth and eighth grades. Educational Researcher, 47, 284–294. Reber, R. (2016). Seductive details in teaching. Psychology Today. https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog /critical-feeling/201609/seductive-details-in-teaching Reber, R., Canning, E. A., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2018). Personalized education to increase interest. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(6), 449–454. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721418793140 Reddy, S. (2014a, August 25). More schools consider later start times for teenagers. Wall Street Journal. http:// online.wsj.com/articles/more-schools-consider-later-start-times-for-teenagers-1409009295#printMode Reddy, S. (2014b, October 13). Teen researchers defend media multitasking. Wall Street Journal. http://www .wsj.com/articles/teen-researchers-defend-media-multitasking–1413220118 Reddy, S. (2016, May 24). Can adults live with a distinct kind of ADHD? Wall Street Journal. http://www.wsj .com/articles/can-adults-get-a-different-kind-of-adhd-1464023009?tesla=y Reder, L. M. (1996). Different research programs on metacognition: Are the boundaries imaginary? Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 383–390. Reeve, J. (1996). Motivating others: Nurturing inner motivational resources. Allyn & Bacon. Reeve, J. (2009). Why teachers adopt a controlling motivating style toward students and how they can become more autonomy supportive. Educational Psychologist, 44, 159–175. Reeve, J. (2018). Understanding motivation and emotion (7th ed.). John Wiley and Sons. Reeve, J., & Lee, W. (2014). Students’ classroom engagement produces longitudinal changes in classroom motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 527–540. Reeve, J., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2018). Sociocultural influences on student motivation as viewed through the lens of self-determination theory. In G. A. D. Liem & D. M. McInerney (Eds.), Big theories revisited 2 (pp. 15–40). Information Age. Reeve, J., & Tseng, C.-M. (2011). Cortisol reactivity to a teacher’s motivating style: The biology of being controlled versus supporting autonomy. Motivation and Emotion, 35(1), 63–74. Reich, J. (2017, January 23). In pursuit of ambitious instruction: Leading for deeper learning. Education Week. https://www.edweek.org/education/opinion-in-pursuit-of-ambitious-instruction-leading-for-deeper -learning/2017/01 Reid, J. M., & Byrd, P. (1998). Grammar in the composition classroom. Heinle & Heinle Publisher. Reimann, P., & Chi, M. T. H. (1989). Human expertise. In K. J. Gilhooly (Ed.), Human and machine problem solving (pp. 161–191). Plenum Press. Reinke, W. M., & Herman, K. C. (2002a). A research agenda for school violence prevention. American Psychologist, 57, 796–797. Reinke, W. M., & Herman, K. C. (2002b). Creating school environments that deter antisocial behaviors in youth. Psychology in the Schools, 39, 549–560. Reis, S. M., Kaplan, S. N., Tomlinson, C. A., Westberg, K. L., Callahan, C. M., & Cooper, C. R. (2002). Equal does not mean identical. In L. Abbeduto (Ed.), Taking sides: Clashing on controversial issues in educational psychology (pp. 31–35). McGraw-Hill/Duskin. Reis, S. M., & Renzulli, J. S. (2004). Current research on the social and emotional development of gifted and talented students: Good news and future possibilities. Psychology in the Schools, 41(1), 119–130. https:// doi.org/10.1002/pits.10144 Reis, S. M., & Renzulli, J. S. (2009). Myth 1: The gifted and talented constitute one single homogeneous group and giftedness is a way of being that stays in the person over time and experiences. Gifted Child Quarterly, 53(4), 233–235. Reis, S. M., & Renzulli, J. S. (2010). Is there still a need for gifted education? An examination of current research. Learning & Individual Differences, 20, 308–317. Reisman, A. (2015). Entering the historical problem space: Whole-class text-based discussion in history class. Teachers College Record, 117, 1–44. http://www.tcrecord.org ID Number: 17783 Reljic, G., Ferring, D., & Martin, R. (2015). A meta-analysis on the effectiveness of bilingual programs in Europe. Review of Educational Research, 85, 92–128. Renkl, A. (2011). Instruction based on examples. In R. E. Mayer, & P. A. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of research on learning and instruction (pp. 272–295). Routledge. Renninger, K. A., & Bachrach, J. E. (2015). Studying triggers for interest and engagement using observational methods. Educational Psychologist, 50, 58–69. Renninger, K. A., & Hidi, S. (2016). The power of interest for motivation and learning. Routledge. Renzulli, J. S. (2011). Theories, actions, and change: An academic journey in search of finding and developing high potential in young people. Gifted Child Quarterly, 55, 305–308. Renzulli, J. S., & Reis, S. M. (2003). The schoolwide enrichment model: Developing creative and productive giftedness. In N. Colangelo & G. A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of gifted education (pp. 184–203). Allyn & Bacon. Rey, G. D. (2012). A review of research and a meta-analysis of the seductive detail effect. Educational Research Review, 7, 216–237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2012.05.003 Reyes, M. R., Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., White, M., & Salovey, P. (2012, March 5). Classroom emotional climate, student engagement, and academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 700–712. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027268 Reynolds, C. R., & Shaywitz, S. E. (2009). Response to Intervention: Ready or not? Or, from wait-to-fail to watch-them-fail. School Psychology Quarterly, 24, 130–145.

References Rhodes, M., & Baron, A. (2019). The development of social categorization. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 1(1), 359–386. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084824 Rice, D., & Muller, C. (2013). Equity or marginalization? The high school course-taking of students labeled with a learning disability. American Educational Research Journal, 50, 656–682. Rice, F. P., & Dolgin, K. G. (2002). The adolescent: Development, relationships, and culture (10th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Rich, P. J., Browning, S. F., Perkins, M., Shoop, T., Yoshikawa, E., & Belikov, O. M. (2019). Coding in K-8: International trends in teaching elementary/primary computing. TechTrends, 63, 311–329. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11528-018-0295-4 Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 353–387. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0026838 Richey, J. E., & Nokes-Malach, T. J. (2015). Comparing four instructional techniques for promoting robust knowledge. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 181–218. Rideout, V. (2017). The Common Sense census: Media use by kids age zero to eight. Common Sense Media. Riedesel, C. A., & Schwartz, J. E. (1999). Essentials of elementary mathematics (2nd ed.). Pearson Education. Rigby, K. (2012). Bullying in schools: Addressing desires, not only behaviours. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 339–348. Rigg, K. (2019). School-based drug prevention programs. Psychology Today. https://www.psychologytoday .com/us/blog/drugs-and-the-people-who-use-them/201909/school-based-drug-prevention-programs Riggle, E. D. B., & Rostosky, S. S. (2012). A positive view of LGBTQ: Embracing identity and cultivating well-being. Rowman & Littlefield. Rimfeld, K., Kovas, Y., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2016). True grit and genetics: Predicting academic achievement from personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 780–789. http://doi.org/10.1037 /pspp0000089 Rimm, S. B., Siegle, D. B., & Davis, G. A. (2018). Education of the gifted and talented (7th ed.). Pearson. Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Baroody, A. E., Larsen, R. A. A., Curby, T. W., & Abry, T. (2015). To what extent do teacher–student interaction quality and student gender contribute to fifth graders’ engagement in mathematics learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 170–185. Ritchey, K. (2015). Black identity development. The Vermont Connection, 35, Article 12. : http://scholarworks. uvm.edu/tvc/vol35/iss1/12 Ritchie, S. J. (2016). Intelligence: All that matters. Hodder & Stoughton. Ritchie, S. J., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2018). How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychological Science, 29, 1358–1369. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2007). Does comparing solution methods facilitate conceptual and procedural knowledge? An experimental study on learning to solve equations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 561–574. Rivas-Drake, D., Seaton, E. K., Markstrom, C., Quintana, S., Syed, M., Lee, R. M., Schwartz, S. J., UmañaTaylor, A. J., French, S., Yip, T., Cross, W. E., Knight, G. P., & Sellers, R. M. (2014). Ethnic and racial identity in adolescence: Implications for psychosocial, academic, and health outcomes. Child Development, 85(1), 40–57. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12200 Rivas-Drake, D., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Schaefer, D. R., & Medina, M. (2017). Ethnic-racial identity and friendships in early adolescence. Child Development, 88(3), 710–724. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12790 Rivers, S. E., Brackett, M. A., Reyes, M. R., Elbertson, N. A., & Salovey, P. (2013). Improving the social and emotional climate of classrooms: A clustered randomized controlled trial testing the RULER approach. Prevention Science, 14(1), 77–87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-012-0305–2 Rivkin, S. G., Hanushek, E. A., & Kain, J. F. (2001). Teachers, schools, and academic achievement. Amherst College. Robbins, S. B., Lauver, K., Davis, H. L., Davis, D., Langley, R., & Carlstrom, A. (2004). Psychosocial and study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 261–288. Roberge, M. M. (2002). California’s Generation 1.5 immigrants: What experiences, characterisitcs, and needs do they bring to our English classes? The CATESOL Journal, 14(1), 107–129. Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., Davies, I. R. L., & Shapiro, L. R. (2004). The development of color categories in two languages: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 554–571. Robinson, D. H. (1998). Graphic organizers as aids to test learning. Reading Research and Instruction, 37, 85–105. Robinson, D. H., & Kiewra, K. A. (1995). Visual argument: Graphic outlines are superior to outlines in improving learning from text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 455–467. Robinson, J. P., & Espelage, D. L. (2012). Bullying explains only part of LGBTQ–heterosexual risk disparities: Implications for policy and practice. Educational Researcher, 41, 309–319. Robinson-Cimpian, J. P. (2015). Review of “The Effects of Test-Based Retention on Student Outcomes Over Time: Regression Discontinuity Evidence from Florida.” National Education Policy Center. https://nepc.colorado. edu/thinktank/review-NBER-retention Robinson-Cimpian, J. P., Lubienski, S. T., Ganley, C. M., & Copur-Gencturk, Y. (2014). Teachers’ perceptions of students’ mathematics proficiency may exacerbate early gender gaps in achievement. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1262–1281. Rodríguez, S., Piñeiro, I., Regueiro, B., & Estévez, I. (2019). Intrinsic motivation and perceived utility as predictors of student homework engagement. Revista de Psicodidáctica, 25, 93–99. Roeser, R. W., Peck, S. C., & Nasir, N. S. (2006). Self and identity processes in school motivation, learning, and achievement. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 391–424). Erlbaum.

R-59

R-60 References Roeser, R. W., Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Jha, A., Cullen, M., Wallace, L., Wilensky, R., Oberle, E., Thomson, K., Taylor, C., & Harrison, J. (2013). Mindfulness training and reductions in teacher stress and burnout: Results from two randomized, waitlist-control field trials. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 787–804. Roessger, K. M., Daley, B. J., & Hafez, D. A. (2018). Effects of concept mapping using practice, feedback, and relational framing. Learning and Instruction, 54, 11–21. Rogat, T. K., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2019). Demonstrating competence within one’s group or in relation to other groups: A person-oriented approach to studying achievement goals in small groups. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 59, 101781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101781 Rogers, L. O., Rosario, R. J., Padilla, D., & Foo, C. (2021). “[I]t’s hard because it’s the cops that are killing us for stupid stuff”: Racial identity in the sociopolitical context of Black Lives Matter. Developmental Psychology, 57(1), 87–101. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001130 Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. Oxford University Press. Rogoff, B. (1995). Observing sociocultural activity on three planes: Participatory appropriation, guided participation, and apprenticeship. In J. Wertsch, P. del Rio, & A. Alverez (Eds.), Sociocultural studies of mind (pp. 139–164). Cambridge University Press. Rogoff, B. (1998). Cognition as a collaborative process. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2 (5th ed., pp. 679–744). Wiley. Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford University Press. Rogoff, B. (2016). Culture and participation: A paradigm shift. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 182–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.12.002 Rogoff, B., Callanan, M., Gutiérrez, K. D., & Erickson, F. (2016). The organization of informal learning. Review of Research in Education, 40(1), 356–401. https://doi.org/10.3102/0091732X16680994 Rogoff, B., & Morelli, G. (1989). Perspectives on children’s development from cultural psychology. American Psychologist, 44, 343–348. Rogowsky, B. A., Calhoun, B. M., & Tallal, P. (2015). Matching learning style to instructional method: Effects on comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 64–78. Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R. F., & Hartwig, M. K. (2020). The scarcity of interleaved practice in mathematics textbooks. Educational Psychology Review, 32(3), 873–883. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09516-2 Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R. F., Hartwig, M. K., & Cheung, C.-N. (2020). A randomized controlled trial of interleaved mathematics practice. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(1), 40–52. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000367 Roid, G. H. (2003). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales, Fifth Edition. Riverside. Rojas, R., & Iglesias, A. (2013). The language growth of Spanish-speaking English language learners. Child Development, 84, 630–646. Rolland, R. G. (2012). Synthesizing the evidence on classroom goal structures in middle and secondary schools: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Review of Educational Research, 82, 396–435. Romano, M., & Weinstein, C. S. (2019). Elementary classroom management: Lessons from research and practice (7th ed.). McGraw-Hill. Romero, C., Master, A., Paunesku, D., Dweck, C. S., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Academic and emotional functioning in middle school: The role of implicit theories. Emotion, 14, 227–234. http://dx.doi. org/10.1037/a0035490 Rosch, E. H. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In T. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 111–144). Academic Press. Roschelle, J. (2013). Special issue on CSCL: Discussion. Educational Psychologist, 48, 67–70. https://doi.org /10.1080/00461520.2012.749445 Roscoe, R. D., & McNamara, D. S. (2013). Writing Pal: Feasibility of an intelligent writing strategy tutor in the high school classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 1010–1025. Rose, N. S., LaRocque, J., Riggall, A. C., Gosseries, O., Starrett, M. J., Meyering, E. E., & Postle, B. R. (2016). Reactivation of latent working memories with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Science, 354, 1136–1139. Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. Basic Books. Rosenberg, M. S., Westling, D. L., & McLeskey, J. (2011). Special education for today’s teachers: An introduction. Allyn & Bacon/Pearson. Rosenshine, B. (1988). Explicit teaching. In D. Berliner & B. Rosenshine (Eds.), Talks to teachers (pp. 75–92). Random House. Rosenshine, B., & Furst, N. (1973). The use of direct observation to study teaching. In R. Travers (Ed.), Second handbook of research on teaching. Rand McNally. Rosenshine, B., & Meister, C. (1992, April). The uses of scaffolds for teaching less structured academic tasks. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA. Rosenshine, B., & Stevens, R. (1986). Teaching functions. In M. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (3rd ed., pp. 376–391). Macmillan. Rosenthal, L. (2016). Incorporating intersectionality into psychology: An opportunity to promote social justice and equity. American Psychologist, 71, 474–485. Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom. Holt, Rinehart, Winston. Rosenzweig, E. Q., Harackiewicz, J. M., Priniski, S. J., Hecht, C. A., Canning, E. A., Tibbetts, Y., & Hyde, J. S. (2019). Choose your own intervention: Using choice to enhance the effectiveness of a utility-value intervention. Motivation Science, 5(3), 269–276. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000113 Roseth, C. J., Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (2008). The relationship between interpersonal relationships and achievement within cooperative, competitive, and individualistic conditions: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 223–246. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.223

References Roseth, C. J., Lee, Y. K., & Saltarelli, W. A. (2019). Reconsidering Jigsaw social psychology: Longitudinal effects on social interdependence, sociocognitive conflict regulation, motivation, and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(1), 149–169. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000257 Roseth, C. J., Saltarelli, A. J., & Glass, C. R. (2011). Effects of face-to-face and computer-mediated constructive controversy on social interdependence, motivation, and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 804–820. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024213 Roskos, K., & Neuman, S. B. (1998). Play as an opportunity for literacy. In O. N. Saracho & B. Spodek (Eds.), Multiple perspectives on play in early childhood education (pp. 100–115). State University of New York Press. Rowe, M. B. (1974). Wait-time and rewards as instructional variables: Their influence on language, logic, and fate control. Part 1: Wait-time. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 11, 81–94. Rowe, M. L. (2018). Understanding socioeconomic differences in parents’ speech to children. Child Development Perspectives, 12(2), 122–127. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12271 Rozek, C. S., Hyde, J. S., Svoboda, R. C., Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2015). Gender differences in the effects of a utility-value intervention to help parents motivate adolescents in mathematics and science. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 195–206. Rubie-Davies, C. M. (2010). Teacher expectations and perceptions of student attributes: Is there a relationship? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 121–135. Rubie-Davies, C. M., Meissel, K., Alansari, M., Watson, P., Flint, A., & McDonald, L. (2020). Achievement and belief outcomes of students with high and low expectation teachers. Social Psychology of Education, 23, 1173–1201. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-020-09574-y Rubin, K. H., Bowker, J. C., McDonald, C. L., & Menzer, M. M. (2012). Peer relationships in childhood. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 2242–2275). Oxford University Press. Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R., Chen, X., Buskirk, A. A., & Wojslawowicz, J. C. (2005). Peer relationships in childhood. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (pp. 469–512). Erlbaum. Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., Chen, X., Bowker, J. C., McDonald, K. L., & Heverly-Fitt, S. (2015). Peer relationships. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (7th ed.) Psychology Press. Rubinsten, O., & Henik, A. (2006). Double dissociations of functions in developmental dyslexia and dyscalculia. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 854–867. Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender development. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 858–932). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Rucinski, C. L., Brown, J. L., & Downer, J. T. (2018). Teacher–child relationships, classroom climate, and children’s social-emotional and academic development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 992–1004. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000240 Ruck, M. D., Mistry, R. S., & Flanagan, C. A. (2019). Children’s and adolescents’ understanding and experiences of economic inequality: An introduction to the special section. Developmental Psychology, 55(3), 449–456. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000694 Rudd, T. (2014). Racial disproportionality in school discipline. Kirwin Institute, The Ohio State University. http://kirwaninstitute.osu.edu/racial-disproportionality-in-school-discipline-implicit-bias-is-heavily -implicated/ Rudolph, K. D., Lambert, S. F., Clark, A. G., & Kurlakowsky, K. D. (2001). Negotiating the transition to middle school: The role of self-regulatory processes. Child Development, 72, 926–946. Rummel, N., Levin, J. R., & Woodward, M. M. (2003). Do pictorial mnemonic text-learning aids give students something worth writing about? Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 327–334. Rundle, S., & Dunn, R. (2010). Learning styles: Online learning style assessments and community. http://www .learningstyles.net Russell, M. K., & Airasian, P. W. (2012). Classroom assessment: Concepts and applications (7th ed.). McGraw-Hill. Ryan, A. (2001). The peer group as a context for development of young adolescents’ motivation and achievement. Child Development, 72, 1135–1150. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1996). When paradigms clash: Comments on Cameron and Pierce’s claim that rewards do not undermine intrinsic motivation. Review of Educational Research, 66, 33–38. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. Guilford Press. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2020). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation from a self-determination theory perspective: Definitions, theory, practices, and future directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61, 101860. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101860 Saavedra, A. R., Liu, Y., Haderlein, S. K., Rapaport, A., Garland, M., Hoepfner, D., Morgan, K. L., & Hu, A. (2021, February 22). Knowledge in Action efficacy study over two years. USC Dornsife Center for Economic and Social Research. https://cesr.usc.edu/sites/default/files/Knowledge%20in%20Action%20Efficacy%20 Study_18feb2021_final.pdf Sabzalian, L. (2019). Indigenous children’s survivance in public schools. Routledge. Sackett, P. R., Kuncel, N. R., Arneson, J. J., Cooper, S. R., & Waters, S. D. (2009). Does socioeconomic status explain the relationship between admissions tests and post-secondary academic performance? Psychological Bulletin, 135, 1–22. Sadker, M., & Sadker, D. (2006). Questioning skills. In J. Cooper (Ed.), Classroom teaching skills (8th ed., pp. 104–150). Houghton-Mifflin.

R-61

R-62 References Said-Metwaly, S., Fernández-Castilla, B., Kyndt, E, Van den Noortgate, W., & Barbot, B. (2020). Does the fourth-grade slump in creativity actually exist? A meta-analysis of the development of divergent thinking in school-age children and adolescents. Educational Psychology Review, 33, 275–298. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09547-9 Sailer, M., & Homner, L. (2020). The gamification of learning: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 32(1), 77–112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09498-w Salchegger, S. (2016). Selective school systems and academic self-concept: How explicit and implicit school-level tracking relate to the big-fish–little-pond effect across cultures. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 405–423. Salomon, G., & Perkins, D. N. (1989). Rocky roads to transfer: Re-thinking mechanisms of a neglected phenomenon. Educational Psychologist, 24, 113–142. Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 9, 185–211. SAMHSA. (2014). SAMHSA’s concept of trauma and guidance for a trauma-informed approach (HHS Publication No. [SMA] 14–4884). Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. https://ncsacw .samhsa.gov/userfiles/files/SAMHSA_Trauma.pdf Sana, F., Weston, T., & Cepeda, N. J. (2013). Laptop multitasking hinders classroom learning for both users and nearby peers. Computers and Education, 62, 24–31. http://doi.org/10.1016 /j.compedu.2012.10.003 Sanders, W. L., & Rivers, J. C. (1996). Cumulative and residual effects of teachers on student academic achievement. University of Tennessee Value-Added Research and Assessment Center. Sandilos, L. E., Sims, W. A., Norwalk, K. E., & Reddy, L. A. (2019). Converging on quality: Examining multiple measures of teaching effectiveness. Journal of School Psychology, 74, 10–28. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.jsp.2019.05.004 Sanger Study Center. (2021). Study smart, not hard. University of Texas at Austin. https://ugs.utexas.edu /slc/study Sansone, C., Geerling, D. M., Thoman, D. B., & Smith, J. L. (2019). Self-regulation of motivation: A renewable resource for learning. In A. Renninger & S. Hidi (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and learning (pp. 87–110). Cambridge. Santiago, C., Raviv, T., & Jaycox, L. H. (2018). Creating healing school communities: School-based interventions for students exposed to trauma. American Psychological Association. https:// doi.org/10.1037/0000072-000 Sasser, N. (2017, September). Positive effects of the zero-tolerance policy used in schools. The Classroom. https://www.theclassroom.com/positive-effects-zero-tolerance-policy-used-schools-17208.html Sattler, J. M. (2020). Assessment of children: Cognitive foundations and applications (6th ed. Revised). Jerome M. Sattler Publishing Inc. Sattler, J. M., & Hoge, R. D. (2006). Assessment of children: Behavioral, social, and clinical foundations. Sattler. Savage, R., Abrami, P. C., Piquette, N., Wood, E., Deleveaux, G., Sanghera-Sidhu, S., & Burgos, G. (2013). A (Pan-Canadian) cluster randomized control effectiveness trial of the ABRACADABRA Web-based literacy program. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 310–328. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031025 Savin-Williams, R. C. (2016). Sexual orientation: Categories or continuum? Commentary on Bailey et al. (2016). Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17(2), 37–44. https:// doi.org/10.1177/1529100616637618 Savin-Williams, R. C., & Cohen, K. M. (2015). Gay, lesbian, and bisexual youth. In J. D. Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (2nd ed., Vol. 9, pp. 649–655). Elsevier. Savin-Williams, R. C., & Vrangalova, Z. (2013). Mostly heterosexual as a distinct sexual orientation group: A systematic review of the empirical evidence. Developmental Review, 33, 58–88. Sawyer, K. (2015). A call to action: The challenges of creative teaching and learning. Teachers College Record, 117, 1–34. Sawyer, R. K. (2006). Introduction: The new science of learning. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 1–16). Cambridge. Sax, L. (2005). Why gender matters. Doubleday. Saxbe, D. E., Beckes, L., Stoycos, S. A., & Coan, J. A. (2020). Social allostasis and social allostatic load: A new model for research in social dynamics, stress, and health. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(2), 469–482. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619876528 Saxe, G. B. (1999). Source of concepts: A cross cultural-developmental perspective. In E. K. Scholnick, K. Nelson, S. A. Gelman, & P. H. Miller (Eds.), Conceptual development: Piaget’s legacy (pp. 253–267). Erlbaum. Scalise, K., & Felde, M. (2017). Why neuroscience matters in the classroom: Principles of brain-based instructional design for teachers. Pearson. Scanlan, M. (2015). Paradigm shift happens. Teachers College Record. http://www.tcrecordorg ID Number: 18137 Schachner, M. K., Schwarzenthal, M., van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Noack, P. (2019). How all students can belong and achieve: Effects of the cultural diversity climate amongst students of immigrant and nonimmigrant background in Germany. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(4), 703–716. https://doi.org/10.1037 /edu0000303 Schacter, D. L., Gilbert, D. T., Nock, M. K., & Wegner, D. M. (2020). Psychology (5th ed.). Worth Publishers. Schalke, D., Brunner, M., Geiser, C., Preckel, F., Keller, U., Spengler, M., & Martin, R. (2013). Stability and change in intelligence from age 12 to age 52: Results from the Luxembourg MAGRIP Study. Developmental Psychology, 109, 1529–1543. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030623 Scheibe, C., & Rogow, F. (2008). 12 basic principles for incorporating media literacy and critical thinking into any curriculum (3rd ed.). Project Look Sharp—Ithaca College.

References Scherer, R., Siddiq, F., & Sánchez Viveros, B. (2019). The cognitive benefits of learning computer programming: A meta-analysis of transfer effects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(5), 764–792. https:// doi.org/10.1037/edu0000314 Scherrer, V., & Preckel, F. (2019). Development of motivational variables and self-esteem during the school career: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Review of Educational Research, 89(2), 211–258. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0034654318819127 Schippers, M. C., Morisano, D., Locke, E. A., Scheepers, A. W. A., Latham, G. P., & de Jong, E. M. (2020). Writing about personal goals and plans regardless of goal type boosts academic performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 60, 101823. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101823 Schmidgall, S. P., Eitel, A., & Scheiter, K. (2019). Why do learners who draw perform well? Investigating the role of visualization, generation and externalization in learner-generated drawing. Learning and Instruction, 60, 138–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.01.006 Schmidt, E. M., Hoffman, J. A., Mulé, C., & Briesch, A. (2021). Effects of a teacher training program to promote physically active play among preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of School Psychology, 85, 57–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2021.01.003 Schmidt, H. G., van der Molen, H. T., te Winkel, W. W. R., & Wijnen, W. H. F. W. (2009). Constructivist, problem-based learning does work: A meta-analysis of curriculum comparisons involving a single medical school. Educational Psychologist, 44(4), 227–249. Schneider, W., & Bjorklund, D. F. (1992). Expertise, aptitude, and strategic remembering. Child Development, 63, 416–473. Schnotz, W., & Wagner, I. (2018). Construction and elaboration of mental models through strategic conjoint processing of text and pictures. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(6), 850–863. https://doi.org/10.1037 /edu0000246 Schoenfeld, A. H. (2011). How we think: The theory of goal-oriented decision making and its educational applications. Routledge. Scholer, A. A., Miele, D. B., Murayama, K., & Fujita, K. (2018). New directions in self-regulation: The role of metamotivational beliefs. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(6), 437–442. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0963721418790549 Schonert-Reichl, K. A. (2019). Advancements in the landscape of social and emotional learning and emerging topics on the horizon. Educational Psychologist, 54, 222–232. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2019 .1633925 Schonert-Reichl, K. A., & Hymel, S. (2007). Educating the heart as well as the mind: Social and emotional learning for school and life success. Education Canada, 20–25. Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Kitil, M. J., & Hanson-Peterson, J. (2017). To reach the students, teach the teachers: A national scan of teacher preparation and social and emotional learning. A report prepared for the Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning (CASEL). University of British Columbia. Schoor, C., Narciss, S., & Körndle, H. (2015). Regulation during cooperative and collaborative learning: A theory-based review of terms and concepts. Educational Psychologist, 50, 97–119. Schraw, G. (2006). Knowledge: Structures and processes. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 825–847). Erlbaum. Schroeder, N. L., Nesbit, J. C., Anguiano, C. J., & Adesope, O. O. (2018). Studying and constructing concept maps: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 30, 431–455. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-0179403-9 Schuldt, L.C. (2019). Feedback in action: Examining teachers’ oral feedback to elementary writers. Teaching and Teacher Education, 83, 64–76. Schunk, D. H. (2020). Learning theories: An educational perspective (8th ed.). Allyn & Bacon/Pearson. Schunk, D. H., & DiBenedetto, M. K. (2020). Motivation and social cognitive theory. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101832 Schunk, D. H., & Greene, J. A. (Eds.). (2017). Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance. Routledge. Schunk, D. H., Meece, J. L., & Pintrich, P. R. (2014). Motivation in education: Theory, research, and applications (4th ed.). Pearson. Schwab, J. J. (1973). The Practical 3: Translation into curriculum. School Review, 81, 501–522. Schwartz, B., Wasserman, E. A., & Robbins, S. J. (2002). Psychology of learning and behavior (5th ed.). Norton. Schwartz, M. S., Hinesley, V., Chang, Z., & Dubinsky, J. M. (2019). Neuroscience knowledge enriches pedagogical choices. Teaching and Teacher Education, 83, 87–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2019.04.002 Schwartz, S. (2020, August 5). Classroom routines must change. Here's what teaching looks like under covid19. Education Week. https://www.edweek.org/ew/articles/2020/08/06/classroom-routines-have-to -change-heres-what.html Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1992). Advances in anxiety theory: A cognitive process approach. In K. A. Hagtvet & T. B. Johnsen (Eds.), Advances in test anxiety research (Vol. 7, pp. 2–31). Swetts & Zeitlinger. Schwerdt, G., West, M. R., & Winters, M. A. (2015). The effects of test-based retention on student outcomes over time: Regression discontinuity evidence from Florida. National Bureau of Educational Research. Schwerdt, G., West, M. R., & Winters, M. A. (2017). The effects of test-based retention on student outcomes over time: Regression discontinuity evidence from Florida. Journal of Public Economics, 152, 154–169. Schwinger, M., Steinmayr, R., & Spinath, B. (2016). Achievement goal profiles in elementary school: Antecedents, consequences, and longitudinal trajectories. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 46, 164–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.05.006 Schwinger, M., Wirthwein, L., Lemmer, G., & Steinmayr, R. (2014). Academic self-handicapping and achievement: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 744–761.

R-63

R-64 References Scruggs, T. E., & Mastropieri, M, A. (2013). Individual differences and learning challenges, Theory Into Practice, 52, 63–72. Scudellari, M. (2015). Myths that will not go away. Nature, 528, 322–325. Seaton, E. K., Gee, G. C., Neblett, E., & Spanierman, L. (2018). New directions for racial discrimination research as inspired by the integrative model. American Psychologist, 73(6), 768–780. https:// doi.org/10.1037/amp0000315 Seaton, E. K., Quintana, S., Verkuyten, M., & Gee, G. C. (2017). Peers, policies, and place: The relation between context and ethnic/racial identity. Child Development, 88(3), 683–692. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12787 Seitz, S. (2018). Disordered eating as a precursor to eating disorders. National Eating Disorders Association. https://www.nationaleatingdisorders.org/blog/disordered-eating-precursor-eating-disorders Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. Freeman. Seligman, M. E. P. (2006). Learned optimism: How to change your mind and your life (2nd ed.). Pocket Books. Semega, J., Kollar, M., Shrider, E. Z., & Creamer, J. F. (2020, September). Income and poverty in the United States: 2019 (Report No. P60-270). U.S. Census Bureau. https://www.census.gov/content/dam/Census /library/publications/2020/demo/p60-270.pdf Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skills: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 445–460. Senko, C. (2016). Achievement goal theory: A story of early promises, eventual discords, and future possibilities. In K. Wentzel, & D. Miele (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school, Vol. 2 (pp. 75–95). Taylor & Francis. Senko, C. (2019). When do mastery and performance goals facilitate academic achievement? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 59, 101795. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101795 Senko, C., Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2011). Achievement goal theory at the crossroads: Old controversies, current challenges, and new directions, Educational Psychologist, 46, 26–47. https://doi.org /10.1080/00461520.2011.538646 Serpell, R. (1993). Interface between sociocultural and psychological aspects of cognition. In E. Forman, N. Minick, & C. A. Stone (Eds.), Contexts for learning: Sociocultural dynamics in children’s development (pp. 357–368). Oxford University Press. Sette, S., Gasser, L., & Grütter, J. (2020). Links between teachers’ liking of students, peer inclusion, and students’ academic achievement: A two-wave longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49(3), 747–756. http://doi.org//10.1007/s10964-019-01048-5 Seufert, T. (2020). Building bridges between self-regulation and cognitive load—an invitation for a broad and differentiated attempt. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 1151–1162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09574–6 Shaffer, D. W., Hatfield, D., Svarovsky, G. N., Nash, P., Nulty, A., Bagley, E., Frank, K., Rupp, A. A., & Mislevy, R. J. (2009). Epistemic network analysis: A prototype for 21st century assessment of learning. International Journal of Learning Media, 1(2), 33–53. Shayer, M. (2003). Not just Piaget; not just Vygotsky, and certainly not Vygotsky as alternative to Piaget. Learning and Instruction, 13, 465–485. Shearer, C. B. (2020). Multiple intelligences in gifted and talented education: Lessons learned from neuroscience after 35 years. Roeper Review, 42(1), 49–63. Shechtman, Z., & Yaman, M. A. (2012). SEL as a component of a literature class to improve relationships, behavior, motivation, and content knowledge. American Educational Research Journal, 49, 546–567. Sheets, R. H. (2005). Diversity pedagogy: Examining the role of culture in the teaching-learning process. Allyn & Bacon. Shen, H. (2016, May). Why women earn less: Just two factors explain post-PhD pay gap. Nature. https:// doi.org/10.1038/nature.2016.19950 Shepard, L. A., & Smith, M. L. (1989). Academic and emotional effects of kindergarten retention. In L. Shepard & M. Smith (Eds.), Flunking grades: Research and policies on retention (pp. 79–107). Falmer Press. Shernoff, D. J., Abdi, B., Anderson, B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2014). Flow in schools revisited: Cultivating engaged learners and optimal learning. In M. J. Furlong, R. Gilman, & E. S. Huebner (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology in schools (2nd ed., pp. 211–226). Routledge. Shi, Y., Ma, Y., MacLeod, J., & Yang, H. H. (2020). College students’ cognitive learning outcomes in flipped classroom instruction: A meta-analysis of the empirical literature. Journal of Computers in Education, 7(1), 79–103. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40692-019-00142-8 Shim, S. S., Wang, C., & Cassady, J. C. (2013). Emotional well-being: The role of social achievement goals and self-esteem. Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 840–845. Shin, D. D., & Kim, S.-I. (2019). Homo curious: Curious or interested? Educational Psychology Review, 31(4), 853–874. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09497-x Shirky, C. (2014, September 25). Why a leading professor of new media banned technology use in class. The Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/answer-sheet/wp/2014/09/25/why-a -leading-professor-of-new-media-just-banned-technology-use-in-class/?utm_term=.70b811731095 Shores, K., Kim, H. E., & Still, M. (2020). Categorical inequality in Black and White: Linking disproportionality across multiple educational outcomes. American Educational Research Journal, 57(5), 2089–2131. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0002831219900128 Shuell, T. J. (1996). Teaching and learning in a classroom context. In D. Berliner & R. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 726–764). Macmillan. Siddle Walker, V. (2001). African American teaching in the South: 1940–1960. Review of Educational Research, 38, 751–779. Sidney, P. G., & Thompson, C. A. (2019). Implicit analogies in learning: Supporting transfer by warming up. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(6), 619–625. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419870801

References Siegler, R. S. (2000). The rebirth of children’s learning. Child Development, 71, 26–35. Siegler, R. S. (2004). Turning memory development inside out. Developmental Review, 24, 469–475. Siegler, R. S., Saffran, J., Eisenberg, N., & Gershoff, E. (2020). How children develop (6th ed.). Worth. Silvén, M. (2001). Attention in very young infants predicts learning of first words. Infant Behavior & Development, 24, 229–237. Silverman, S. K. (2008, April, 11). Personal communication, Columbus, Ohio. Simmons, D. (2019). Why we can’t afford to whitewash social-emotional learning. ASCD Education Update, 61, 4, 1–5. Simmons, D. (2021). Why SEL alone isn’t enough. Educational Leadership, 78(6), 30–34. Simon, D. P., & Chase, W. G. (1973). Skill in chess. American Scientist, 61, 394–403. Simon, T. (2010). Rewards and challenges of cognitive neuroscience studies of persons with intellectual and developmental disabilities. Special issue for the American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 115, 79–82. https://doi.org/10.1352/1944–7558–115.2.79 Simonton, D. K. (2000). Creativity: Cognitive, personal, developmental, and social aspects. American Psychologist, 55, 151–158. Simpkins, S. D., Tulagan, N., Lee, G., Ma, T. L., Zarrett, N., & Vandell, D. L. (2020). Children’s developing work habits from middle childhood to early adolescence: Cascading effects for academic outcomes in adolescence and adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 56(12), 2281–2292. https:// doi.org/10.1037/dev0001113 Singley, K., & Anderson, J. R. (1989). The transfer of cognitive skill. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Sirin, S. R. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research, 75, 417–453. Skiba, R., Ormiston, H., Martinez, S., & Cummings, J. (2016). Teaching the social curriculum: Classroom management as behavioral instruction, Theory Into Practice, 55, 120–128. Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review, 57, 193–216. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. Macmillan. Skinner, B. F. (1989). The origins of cognitive thought. American Psychologist, 44, 13–18. Slama, R. B. (2012). A longitudinal analysis of academic English proficiency outcomes for adolescent English language learners in the United States. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 265–285. https:// doi.org/10.1037/a0025861 Slaten, C. D., Rose, C. A., Bonifay, W., & Ferguson, J. K. (2019). The Milwaukee Youth Belongingness Scale (MYBS): Development and validation of the scale utilizing item response theory. School Psychology, 34(3), 296–306. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000299 Slater, L. (2002, February 3). The trouble with self-esteem. The New York Times Magazine, pp. 44–47. Slavin, R. E. (1995). Cooperative learning (2nd ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Slavin, R. E. (2017). Instruction based on cooperative learning. In R. E. Mayer, & P. A. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of research on learning and instruction (2nd ed., pp. 387–404). Routledge. Slavin, R. E. (2020). How evidence-based reform will transform research and practice in education. Educational Psychologist, 55(1), 21–31. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2019.1611432 Sleek, S. (2015). Point A to point B: Charting the brain’s navigation system. Observer, 28(7), 20–23. Smetana, J. G. (2000). Middle-class African American adolescents’ and parents’ conceptions of parental authority and parenting practices: A longitudinal investigation. Child Development, 71, 1672–1686. Smetana, J. G. (2017). Current research on parenting styles, dimensions, and beliefs. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 19–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.012 Smith, D. C., Tyler, N. C., & Smith, S. (2014). Introduction to special education: New horizons. Pearson. Smith, D. D. (2006). Introduction to special education: Teaching in an age of opportunity (5th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Smith, D. G., Xiao, L., & Bechara, A. (2012). Decision making in children and adolescents: Impaired Iowa Gambling Task performance in early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1180–1187. https:// doi.org/10.1037/a0026342 Smith, E. E., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2007). Cognitive psychology: Mind and brain. Pearson/Prentice-Hall. Smith, F. (1975). Comprehension and learning: A conceptual framework for teachers. Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Smith, M. D. (2017). Cognitive validity: Can multiple-choice items tap historical thinking processes? American Educational Research Journal, 54(6), 1256–1287. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831217717949 Smith, S. M., Glenberg, A., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Environmental context and human memory. Memory and Cognition, 6, 342–353. Smith, S. W., Daunic, A. P., Aydin, B., Van Loan, C. L., Barber, B. R., & Taylor, G. G. (2016). Effect of Tools for Getting Along on student risk for emotional and behavioral problems in upper elementary classrooms: A replication study. School Psychology Review, 45(1), 73–92. https://doi.org/10.17105/SPR45-1.73-92 Sneyers, E., Vanhoof, J., & Mahieu, P. (2020). Bias in primary school teachers’ expectations of students? A study of general and specific bias towards SES, ethnicity and gender, Studia Paedagogica, 25(2), 71–96. https://doi.org/0.5817/SP2020-2-4 Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language, 4, l–22. Snow, C. E. (1993). Families as social contexts for literacy development. In C. Daiute (Ed.), New directions for child development (No. 61, pp. 11–24). Jossey-Bass. Snow, R. E. (1995). Pygmalion and intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 169–171. Snyder, K. E., Malin, J. L., Dent, A. L., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2014). The message matters: The role of implicit beliefs about giftedness and failure experiences in academic self-handicapping. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 230–241.

R-65

R-66 References Soemer, A., & Schwan, S. (2012). Visual mnemonics for language learning: Static pictures versus animated morphs. Educational Psychology, 104, 565–579. Solomon, D., Watson, M. S., & Battistich, V. A. (2001). Teaching and schooling effects on moral/prosocial development. In V. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (4th ed., pp. 566–603). American Educational Research Association. Son, L. K., & Simon, D. A. (2012). Distributed learning: Data, metacognition, and educational implications. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 379–399. Soodak, L. C., & McCarthy, M. R. (2006). Classroom management in inclusive settings. In C. M. Evertson & C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook of classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues. Erlbaum. Sorhagen, N. S. (2013). Early teacher expectations disproportionately affect poor children’s high school performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 465–477. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031754 Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man: Their nature and measurement. Macmillan. Spencer, S. J., Logel, C., & Davies, P. G. (2016). Stereotype threat. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 415–437. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-073115-103235 Spilt, J. L., Koomen, H. M. Y., & Thijs, J. T. (2011). Teacher wellbeing: The importance of teacher–student relationships. Educational Psychology Review, 23, 257–277. Sprenger, M. (2005). Inside Amy’s brain. Educational Leadership, 62(7), 28–32. Sprenger, M. (2010). Brain-based teaching in the digital age. Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. SRCD. (2009). Young Hispanic children: Boosting opportunities for learning. Society for Research in Child Development: Social Policy Report Briefs, 23(2), 1–2. Srna, S., Schrift, R. Y., & Zauberman, G. (2018). The illusion of multitasking and its positive effect on performance. Psychological Science, 29, 1942–1955. Stahl, S. A. (2002). Different strokes for different folks? In L. Abbeduto (Ed.), Taking sides: Clashing on controversial issues in educational psychology (pp. 98–107). Hill/Duskin. Stahnke, R., & Blömeke, S. (2021). Novice and expert teachers’ situation-specific skills regarding classroom management: What do they perceive, interpret and suggest? Teaching and Teacher Education, 98, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2020.103243. Starck, J. G., Riddle, T., Sinclair, S., & Warikoo, N. (2020). Teachers are people too: Examining the racial bias of teachers compared to other American adults. Educational Researcher, 49(4), 273–284. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0013189X20912758 Starko, A. J. (2014). Creativity in the classroom: Schools of curious delight (5th ed.). Routledge. Stattin, H., Kerr, M, & Skoog, T. (2011). Early pubertal timing and girls’ problem behavior: Integrating two hypotheses. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1271–1287. Steenbergen-Hu, S., Makel, M. C., & Olszewski-Kubilius, P. (2016). What one hundred years of research says about the effects of ability grouping and acceleration on K–12 students’ academic achievement: Findings of two second-order meta-analyses. Review of Educational Research, 86(4), 849–899. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0034654316675417 Steinberg, L. (1998). Standards outside the classroom. In D. Ravitch (Ed.), Brookings papers on educational policy (pp. 319–358). Brookings Institute. Steinberg, L. (2005). Adolescence (7th ed.). McGraw-Hill. Steinberg, L. (2014). Adolescence (10th ed.). McGraw-Hill. Steinberg, L., Icenogle, G., Shulman, E. P., Breiner, K., Chein, J., Bacchini, D., Chang, L., Chaudhary, N., Giunta, L. Di, Dodge, K. A., Fanti, K. A., Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Oburu, P., Pastorelli, C., Skinner, A. T., Sorbring, E., Tapanya, S., Tirado, L. M. U., & Takash, H. M. S. (2018). Around the world, adolescence is a time of heightened sensation seeking and immature self-regulation. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12532. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12532 Steiner, E. D., & Woo, A. (2021). Job-related stress threatens the teacher supply: Key findings from the 2021 State of the U.S. Teacher Survey. RAND Corporation. https://www.rand.org/pubs/research_reports/RRA1108-1 .html Steinmayr, R., Weidinger, A. F., & Wigfield, A. (2018). Does students’ grit predict their school achievement above and beyond their personality, motivation, and engagement? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 53, 106–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.02.004 Stephens, J., & Wangaard, D. B. (2016). The achieving with integrity seminar: An integrative approach to promoting moral development in secondary school classrooms. International Journal for Educational Integrity, 12, 1–16. Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge University Press. Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Culture and intelligence. American Psychologist, 59, 325–338. Sternberg, R. J. (2014). Robert Sternberg: Successful https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ow05B4bjGWQ Sternberg, R. J. (2018). Context-sensitive cognitive and educational testing. Educational Psychology Review, 30, 857–884. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-017-9428-0 Sternberg, R. J. (2018). Successful intelligence in theory, research, and practice. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of human intelligence (pp. 308–321). Cambridge University Press. https:// doi.org/10.1017/9781316817049.020 Sternberg, R. J. (2020). Intelligence. In Oxford research encyclopedia of education, Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190264093.013.872 Sternberg, R. J., & Davidson, J. (1982, June). The mind of the puzzler. Psychology Today, 37–44. Sternberg, R. J., & Sternberg, K. (2012). Cognitive psychology (6th ed.). Wadsworth.

References Stevenson, H. C. (2014). Promoting racial literacy in schools: Differences that make a difference. Teachers College Press. Stiggins, R. J. (2002). Where is our assessment future and how can we get there? In R. W. Lissitz & W. D. Schafer (Eds.), Meaningful assessment: A manageable and cooperative process. Allyn & Bacon. Stinson, D. W. (2006). African American male adolescents, schooling (and mathematics): Deficiency, rejection, and achievement. Review of Educational Research, 76(4), 477–506. https://doi .org/10.3102/00346543076004477 Stipek, D. J. (2002). Motivation to learn: Integrating theory and practice (4th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Stockard, J., Wood, T. W., Coughlin, C., & Rasplica Khoury, C. (2018). The effectiveness of direct instruction curricula: A meta-analysis of a half century of research. Review of Educational Research, 88(4), 479–507. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654317751919 Stoeger, H., & Ziegler, A. (2011). Self-regulatory training through elementary-school students’ homework completion. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (pp. 87–101). Routledge. Stone, E., Brown, C. S., & Jewell, J. A. (2015). The sexualized girl: The development of a within-gender stereotype among children. Child Development, 86, 1604–1622. Strauss, V. (2015, December 7). The successor to No Child Left Behind has, it turns out, big problems of its own. The Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/answer-sheet/wp/2015/12/07 /the...to-no-child-left-behind-has-it-turns-out-big-problems-of-its-own/ Strom, P. S., & Strom, R. D. (2005). Cyberbullying by adolescents: A preliminary assessment. The Educational Forum, 70(1), 21–36. Südkamp, A., Kaiser, J., & Möller, J. (2012). Accuracy of teachers’ judgments of students’ academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 743–762. Suldo, S. M., Friedrich, A. A., White, T., Farmer, J., Minch, D., & Michalowski, J. (2009). Teacher support and adolescents’ subjective well-being: A mixed-methods investigation. School Psychology Review, 38, 67–85. Sullivan, J. R., & Castro-Villarreal, F. (2013). Special education policy, response to intervention and the socialization of youth. Theory Into Practice, 52, 180–189. Sullivan, M. A., & O’Leary, S. G. (1990). Maintenance following reward and cost token programs. Behavior Therapy, 21, 139–149. Sun, Y., Tao, F., Su, P., Mai, J., Shi, H., Han, Y., Wang, H., Lou, X., Han, J., & Liu, J. (2012). National estimates of the pubertal milestons among urban and rural Chinese girls. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51, 279–284 Sung, H-.Y., & Hwang, G.-J. (2013). A collaborative game-based learning approach to improving students’ learning performance in science courses. Computers and Education, 63, 43–51. Swansea University. (2021, January 6). Ineffective “learning styles” theory persists in education. ScienceDaily. https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2021/01/210106111959.htm Swanson, H. L. (1990). The influence of metacognitive knowledge and aptitude on problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 306–314. Swanson, T. C. (2005). Providing structure for children with learning and behavior problems. Intervention in School and Clinic, 40, 182–187. Swearer, S. M., Espelage, D. L., Vaillancourt, T., & Hymel, S. (2010). What can be done about school bullying? Linking research to educational practice. Educational Researcher, 39, 38–47. Sweller, J., Kirschner, P. A., & Clark, R. E. (2007). Why minimally guided teaching techniques do not work: A reply to commentaries. Educational Psychologist, 42, 115–121. Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (2019). Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 261–292. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09465-5 Sylvia, P. J., Henson, R. A., & Templin, J. L. (2009). Are the sources of interest the same for everyone? Using multilevel mixture models to explore individual differences in appraisal structures. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 1389–1406. Synodi, E. (2010). Play in the kindergarten: The case of Norway, Sweden, New Zealand and Japan. International Journal of Early Years Education, 18, 185–200. Syvertsen, A. K., Flanagan, C. A., & Stout, M. (2009). Code of silence: Students’ perceptions of school climate and willingness to intervene in a peer’s dangerous plan. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 219–232. Talbot, M. (2002, February 24). Girls just want to be mean. The New York Times Magazine, pp. 24–291. Tamim, R. M., Bernard, R. M., Borokhovski, E., Abrami, P. C., & Schmid, R. F. (2011). What forty years of research says about the impact of technology on learning: A second-order meta-analysis and validation study. Review of Educational Research, 81, 4–28. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654310393361 Tan, F. D. H., Whipp, P. R., Gagné, M., & Van Quaquebeke, N. (2020). Expert teacher perceptions of two-way feedback interaction. Teaching and Teacher Education, 87, ARTN 102930 Tanaka, A., & Murayama, K. (2014). Within-person analyses of situational interest and boredom: Interactions between task-specific perceptions and achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(4), 1122–1134. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036659 Tang, M., Ginns, P., & Jacobson, M. J. (2019). Tracing enhances recall and transfer of knowledge of the Water Cycle. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 439–455. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09466-4 Tang, Y., Zhang, W., Chen, K., Feng, S., Ji, Y., Shen, J., Reiman, E. M., & Liu, Y. (2006). Arithmetic processing in the brain shaped by culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 103, 10775–10780. Taylor, M., McLean, L., Bryce, C. I., Abry, T., & Granger, K. L. (2019). The influence of multiple life stressors during teacher training on burnout and career optimism in the first year of teaching. Teaching and Teacher Education, 86, 1–11.

R-67

R-68 References Taylor, R. D., Oberle, E., Durlak, J. A., & Weissberg, R. P. (2017). Promoting positive youth development through school-based social and emotional learning interventions: A meta-analysis of follow-up effects. Child Development, 88(4), 1156–1171. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12864 TenBrink, T. D. (2003). Assessment. In J. Cooper (Ed.), Classroom teaching skills (7th ed., pp. 311–353). Houghton-Mifflin. Tenenbaum, H. R., & Ruck, M. D. (2007). Are teachers’ expectations different for racial minority than for European American students? A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(2), 253–273. Tenenbaum, H. R., Winstone, N. E., Leman, P. J., & Avery, R. E. (2020). How effective is peer interaction in facilitating learning? A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(7), 1303–1319. https:// doi.org/10.1037/edu0000436 Tennant, J. E., Demaray, M. K., Malecki, C. K., Terry, M. N., Clary, M., & Elzinga, N. (2014). Students’ ratings of teacher support and academic and social–emotional well-being. School Psychology Quarterly, 30, 494–512. Terman, L. M., Baldwin, B. T., & Bronson, E. (1925). Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children. In L. M. Terman (Ed.), Genetic studies of genius (Vol. 1). Stanford University Press. Terman, L. M., & Oden, M. H. (1947). The gifted child grows up. In L. M. Terman (Ed.), Genetic studies of genius (Vol. 4). Stanford University Press. Terman, L. M., & Oden, M. H. (1959). The gifted group in mid-life. In L. M. Terman (Ed.), Genetic studies of genius (Vol. 5). Stanford University Press. Tharp, R. G. (1989). Psychocultural variables and constants: Effects on teaching and learning in schools. American Psychologist, 44, 349–359. Tharp, R. G., & Gallimore, R. (1988). Rousing minds to life: Teaching, learning, and schooling in social context. Cambridge University Press. Theis, D., Sauerwein, M., & Fischer, N. (2020). Perceived quality of instruction: The relationship among indicators of students’ basic needs, mastery goals, and academic achievement. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(S1), 176–192. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12313 Theodore, L. A., Bray, M. A., Kehle, T. J., & Jenson, W. R. (2001). Randomization of group contingencies and reinforcers to reduce classroom disruptive behavior. Journal of School Psychology, 39, 267–277. Thoman, D. B., Sansone, C., & Geerling, D. (2017). The dynamic nature of interest: Embedding interest within self-regulation. In P. A. O'Keefe & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), The science of interest (pp. 27–47). Springer International Publishing AG. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-55509-6_2 Thoman, D. B., Smith, J. L., Brown, E. R., Chase, J., & Lee, J. Y. K. (2013). Beyond performance: A motivational experiences model of stereotype threat. Educational Psychology Review, 25, 211–243. Thomas, H. J., Connor, J. P., & Scott, J. G. (2015). Integrating traditional bullying and cyberbullying: Challenges of definition and measurement in adolescents––a review. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 135–152. Thomas, K. T., & Thomas, J. R. (2008). Principles of motor development for elementary school physical education. The Elementary School Journal, 108, 181–195. Thomas, M. S. C. (2019). Response to Dougherty and Robey (2018) on neuroscience and education: Enough bridge metaphors—Interdisciplinary research offers the best hope for progress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 337–340. Thomas, P. L. (2014, January 30). The “grit” narrative, “grit” research, and codes that blind [Blog post]. https:// radicalscholarship.wordpress.com/2014/01/30/the-grit-narrative-grit-research-and-codes-that-blind/ Thompson, G. (2008). Beneath the apathy. Educational Leadership, 65(6), 50–54. Thompson, R. A., & Raikes, H. A. (2003). Toward the next quarter-century: Conceptual and methodological challenges for attachment theory. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 691–718. Tierney, W. G., & Ward, J. D. (2017). Coming out and leaving home: A policy and research agenda for LGBT homeless students. Educational Researcher, 46(9), 498–507. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X17733964 Tingstrom, D. H., Sterling-Turner, H. E., & Wilczynski, S. M. (2006). The Good Behavior Game: 1962–2002. Behavior Modification, 30, 225–253. Tikva, C., & Tambouris, E. (2021). Mapping computational thinking through programming in K-12 education: A conceptual model based on a systematic literature review. Computers & Education, 162, 104083. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2020.104083 Tjosvold, D., Druckman, D., Johnson, R. T., Smith, K. A., & Roseth, C. (2020). Valuing cooperation and constructive controversy: A tribute to David W. Johnson. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 13(4), 343–362. https://doi.org/10.1111/ncmr.12145 Tobias, S. (2010). Generative learning theory, paradigm shifts, and constructivism in educational psychology: A tribute to Merl Wittrock. Educational Psychologist, 45, 51–54. Tobiason, G. (2019). Talking our way around expert caution: A rhetorical analysis of VAM. Educational Researcher, 48, 19–30. Tobler, N., & Stratton, H. (1997). Effectiveness of school-based drug prevention programs: A metaanalysis of the research. Journal of Primary Prevention, 18, 71–128. Tomasello, M. (2006). Acquiring linguistic constructions. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, language, and perception (6th ed., pp. 255–298). Wiley. Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552. Tomasetto, C., Morsanyi, K., Guardabassi, V., & O’Connor, P. A. (2021). Math anxiety interferes with learning novel mathematics contents in early elementary school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(2), 315–329. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000602 Tomlinson, C. A. (2005b). Grading and differentiation: Paradox or good practice? Theory Into Practice, 44, 262–269.

References Tomlinson, C. A. (2017). How to differentiate instruction in academically diverse classrooms (3rd ed.). Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Tomsaik, M. J., Napolitano, C. M., & Moser, U. (2019). Trajectories of academic performance across compulsory schooling and thriving in young adulthood. Child Development, 90, e745–e762. Tonks, S. M., Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2018). Expectancy-value theory in cross-cultural perspective: What have we learned in the last 15 years? In G. A. D. Liem, & D. M. McInerney (Eds.), Big theories revisited 2 (pp. 91–116). Information Age Publishing. Toomey, R. B., & Russell, S. T. (2016). The role of sexual orientation in school victimization: A meta-analysis. Youth & Society, 48(2), 176–201. https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118X13483778 Topping, K., Dekhinet, R., & Zeedyk, S. (2011). Hindrances for parents in enhancing child language. Educational Psychology Review, 23, 413–455. Torrance, E. P. (1972). Predictive validity of the Torrance tests of creative thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior, 6, 236–262. Torrance, E. P. (1986). Teaching creative and gifted learners. In M. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (3rd ed., pp. 630–647). Macmillan. Torrance, E. P. (2000). A graphic assessment of the creativity of an eight-year-old. In The Torrance test of creative thinking. Scholastic Testing Service. Torrance, E. P. (2008). The Torrance tests of creative thinking: Norms-technical manual. Scholastic Testing Service. Torrance, E. P., & Safter, H. T. (1986). Are children becoming more creative? Journal of Creative Behavior, 20, 1–13. Tough, P. (2012). How children succeed: Grit, curiosity, and the hidden power of character. Houghton Mifflin. Touretzky, D., Gardner-McCune, C., Martin, F., & Seehorn, D. (2019). Envisioning AI for K–12: What should every child know about AI? 33rd AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence, AAAI 2019, 9795–9799. https:// doi.org/10.1609/aaai.v33i01.33019795 Tourva, A., Spanoudis, G., & Demetriou, A. (2016). Cognitive correlates of developing intelligence: The contribution of working memory, processing speed and attention. Intelligence, 54, 136–146. Träff, U., Olsson, L., Skagerlund, K., & Östergren, R. (2020). Kindergarten domain-specific and domaingeneral cognitive precursors of hierarchical mathematical development: A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112, 93–109. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000369 Trautwein, U. (2007). The homework–achievement relation reconsidered: Differentiating homework time, homework frequency, and homework effort. Learning and Instruction, 17, 372–388. Trautwein, U., & Lüdtke, O. (2007). Students’ self-reported effort and time on homework in six school subjects: Between-students differences and within-student variation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 232–234. Trautwein, U., Schnyder, I., Niggli, A., Neuman, M., & Lüdtke, O. (2009). Chameleon effects in homework research: The homework–achievement association depends on the measures used and the level of analysis chosen. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 77–88. Tricot, A., & Sweller, J. (2014). Domain-specific knowledge and why teaching generic skills does not work. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 265–283. Tröbst, S., Kleickmann, T., Lange-Schubert, K., Rothkopf, A., & Möller, K. (2016). Instruction and students’ declining interest in science: An analysis of German fourth- and sixth-grade classrooms. American Educational Research Journal, 53, 162–193. Trouilloud, D., Sarrazin, P., Bressoux, P., & Bois, J. (2006). Relation between teachers’ early expectations and students’ later perceived competence in physical education classes: Autonomy-supportive climate as a moderator. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 75–86. Tsantis, L. A., Bewick, C. J., & Thouvenelle, S. (2003, November). Examining some common myths about computer use in the early years [Electronic Version]. Beyond the Journal: Young Children on the Web, pp. 1–9. http://journal.naeyc.org/btj/200311/CommonTechnoMyths.pdf Tschannen-Moran, M., & Woolfolk Hoy, A. (2000). Collaborative learning: A memorable model. The Teacher Educator, 36, 148–165. Tschannen-Moran, M., Woolfolk Hoy, A., & Hoy, W. K. (1998). Teacher efficacy: Its meaning and measure. Review of Educational Research, 68, 202–248. Ttofi, M. M., Farrington, D. P., Losel, F., Crago, R. V., & Theodorakis, N. (2016). School bullying and drug use later in life: A meta-analytic investigation. School Psychology Quarterly, 31(1), 8–27. Turkle, S. (2011). Alone together: Why we expect more from technology and less from ourselves. Basic Books. Turnbull, A., Turnbull, H. R., Wehmeyer, M. L., & Shjogren, K. A. (2020). Exceptional lives (9th ed.). Pearson. Turner, J. C. (1997). Starting right: Strategies for engaging young literacy learners. In J. T. Guthrie & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Reading engagement: Motivating readers through 21st century America integrated instruction (pp. 183–204). International Reading Association. Turner, J. C., Christensen, A., Kackar-Cam, H. Z., Trucano, M., & Fulmer, S. M. (2014). Enhancing students’ engagement: Report of a 3-year intervention with middle school teachers. American Educational Research Journal, 51, 1195–1226. Turner, J. C., & Paris, S. G. (1995). How literacy tasks influence students’ motivation for literacy. The Reading Teacher, 48, 662–673. Turner, J. C., Patrick, H., & Meyer, D. (2011). Engaging students in learning: A special issue dedicated to Jere Brophy. Theory Into Practice, 50. Twenge, J. M., Martin, G. N., & Campbell, W. K. (2018). Decreases in psychological well-being among American adolescents after 2012 and links to screen time during the rise of smartphone technology. Emotion, 18(6), 765–780. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000403

R-69

R-70 References Tyler, K. M., Uqdah, A. L., Dillihunt, M. L., Beatty-Hazelbaker, R., Conner, T., Gadson, N., Henchy, A., Hughes, T., Mulder, S., Owens, E., Roan-Belle, C., Smith, L., & Stevens, R. (2008). Cultural discontinuity: Toward a quantitative investigation of a major hypothesis in education. Educational Researcher, 37(5), 280–297. Tyson, K. (2011). Integration interrupted: Tracking, black students, and acting white after Brown. Oxford University Press. Tyson, K. (2013). Tracking segregation, and the opportunity gap. In P. L. Carter & K. G. Welner (Eds.), Closing the opportunity gap: What America must do to give every child an even chance (pp. 169–180). Oxford Scholarship Online. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199982981.003.0012 Tze, V. M. C., Daniels, L. M., & Klassen, R. M. (2016). Evaluating the relationship between boredom and academic outcomes: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 28(1), 119–144. https://doi.org/10.1007 /s10648-015-9301-y U.S. Department of Agriculture. (2016). Food and Nutrition Service overview. http://www.fns.usda.gov/pd /overview U.S. Department of Education. (2004). 26th Annual report to Congress on the implementation of the Individuals with Disabilities Act, 2005. Office of Special Education and Rehabilitative Services. U.S. Department of Education. (2016). 2013–2014 Civil rights data collection: A first look. http://www2.ed.gov /about/offices/list/ocr/docs/2013-14-first-look.pdf U.S. Department of Education. (2017). IDEA Regulations, Sec. 300.8 c 4, https://sites.ed.gov/idea/regs/b /a/300.8/c/4 U.S. Department of Education. (2019). National Assessment of Educational Progress Report Card: Mathematics. https://www.nationsreportcard.gov/highlights/mathematics/2019/ U.S. Department of Education. (2019). Office of Elementary and Secondary Education, Consolidated State Performance Report, 2013–14. Digest of Education Statistics 2019, table 219.46. U.S. Department of Education. (2020). OSEP Fast Facts: Children identified with autism. https://sites.ed.gov /idea/osep-fast-facts-children-with-autism–20/ U.S. Department of Education. (2021). Overview and mission statement. https://www2.ed.gov/about/landing. jhtml U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Administration on Children, Youth and Families, Children’s Bureau. (2020). Mandatory reporters of child abuse and neglect. https://www.childwelfare.gov/pubPDFs/manda.pdf U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Administration on Children, Youth and Families, Children’s Bureau. (2021). Child maltreatment 2019. https://www.acf.hhs. gov/cb/report/child-maltreatment–2019 Udell, W. (2007). Enhancing adolescent girls’ argument skills in reasoning about personal and non-personal decisions. Cognitive Development, 22, 341–352. Uline, C. L., & Johnson, J. F. (2005). Closing the achievement gap: What will it take? Theory Into Practice, 44(1), 1–3. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15430421tip4401_1 Umaña- Taylor, A. J., Quintana, S. M., Lee, R. M., Cross, W. E., Rivas- Drake, D., Schwartz, S. J., Syed, M., Yip, T., Seaton, E., & Ethnic and Racial Identity in the 21st Century Study Group. (2014). Ethnic and racial identity during adolescence and into young adulthood: An integrated conceptualization. Child Development, 85(1), 21–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12196 Umbreit, J. (1995). Functional analysis of disruptive behavior in an inclusive classroom. Journal of Early Intervention, 20(1), 18–29. UNHCR. (2021). Convention and protocol relating to the status of refugees. The UN Refugee Agency: USA. https://www.unhcr.org/3b66c2aa10.html University of Lynchburg. (2021). Study tips. https://www.lynchburg.edu/academics/tutoring-academic -support/top-10-study-skills/ Unkelbach, C., & Högden, F. (2019). Why does George Clooney make coffee sexy? The case for attribute conditioning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 540–546. https://doi .org/10.1177/0963721419859354 Urban Institute. (2019, March 14). Part of us: A data-driven look at children of immigrants. Urban Institute. https://www.urban.org/features/part-us-data-driven-look-children-immigrants Urdan, T. (2004). Predictors of academic self-handicapping and achievement: Examining achievement goals, classroom goal structures, and culture. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 251–264. Urdan, T., & Kaplan, A. (2020). The origins, evolution, and future directions of achievement goal theory. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101862 Usher, E. L. (2015). Personal capability beliefs. In L. Corno & E. H. Anderman (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (3rd ed., pp. 146–159). Taylor & Francis. Usher, E. L., Ford, C. J., Li, C. R., & Weidner, B. L. (2019). Sources of math and science self-efficacy in rural Appalachia: A convergent mixed methods design. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 57, 32–53. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.10.003 Usher, E. L., Li, C. R., Butz, A. R., & Rojas, J. P. (2019). Grit and self-efficacy: Are both essential for children’s academic success? Journal of Educational Psychology, 111, 877–902. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000324 Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2008). Sources of self-efficacy in school: Critical review of the literature and future directions. Review of Educational Research, 78, 751–796. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654308321456 Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2009). Sources of self-efficacy in mathematics: A validation study. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 89–101. Usher, E. L., & Schunk, D. H. (2018). A social cognitive theoretical perspective of self-regulation. In D. H. Schunk & J. A. Greene (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (2nd ed., pp. 19–35). Routledge.

References Usher, E. L., & Weidner, B. L. (2018). Sociocultural influences on self-efficacy development. In G. A. D. Liem & D. M. McInerney (Eds.), Big theories revisited 2 (pp. 141–164). Information Age. Vacca, R. T., Vacca, J. A. L., & Mraz, M. (2014), Content area reading: Literacy and learning across the curriculum (11th ed.). Pearson. Valentine, J. C., DuBois, D. L., & Cooper, H. (2004). The relations between self-beliefs and academic achievement: A systematic review. Educational Psychologist, 39, 111–133. Valenzuela, A. (1999). Subtractive schooling: U.S.–Mexican youth and the politics of caring. SUNY Press. Valiente, C., Lemery-Chalfant, K., & Swanson, J. (2010). Prediction of kindergartners’ academic achievement from their effortful control and emotionality: Evidence for direct and moderated relations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 550–560. Valiente, C., Swanson, J., DeLay, D., Fraser, A. M., & Parker, J. H. (2020). Emotion-related socialization in the classroom: Considering the roles of teachers, peers, and the classroom context. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 578–594. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000863 Van de Pol, J., van Loon, M., van Gog, T., Braumann, S., & de Bruin, A. (2020). Mapping and drawing to improve students’ and teachers’ monitoring and regulation of students' learning from text: Current findings and future directions.Educational Psychology Review, 32, 951–977. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648 -020-09560–y van de Pol, J., Volman, M., & Beishuizen, J. (2010). Scaffolding in teacher–student interaction: A decade of research. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 271–296. Van de Walle, J. A., Karp, K. S., & Bay-Williams, J. M. (2010). Elementary and middle school mathematics: Teaching developmentally (7th ed.). Pearson. van den Berg, L., Ros, A., & Beijaard, D. (2014). Improving teacher feedback during active learning: Effects of a professional development program. American Educational Research Journal, 51, 772–809. van den Bergh, L., Denessen, E., Hornstra, L., Voeten, M., & Holland, R. W. (2010). The implicit prejudices attitudes of teachers: Relations to teacher expectation and achievement gap. American Educational Research Journal, 47, 497–527. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831209353594 Van den Broeck, L. V., Demanet, J., & Houtte, M. V. (2020). The forgotten role of teachers in students’ educational aspirations. School composition effects and the buffering capacity of teachers’ expectations culture. Teaching and Teacher Education, 90, 103015. van der Kleij, F. S. (2019). Comparison of teacher and student perceptions of formative assessment feedback practices and association with individual student characteristics. Teaching and Teacher Education, 85, 175–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2019.06.010 van der Mass, H. L. J., Dolan, C. V., Grasman, R. P. P. P., Wicherts, J. M., Huizenga, H. M., & Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2006). A dynamic model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review, 113, 842–861. van der Meij, J. (2012). Draw your physics homework? Art as a path to understanding in physics teaching. American Educational Research Journal, 49, 356–407. http://doi.org/10.3102/0002831211435521 Van Der Veer, R. (2007). Vygotsky in context: 1900–1935. In H. Daniels, M. Cole, & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), The Cambridge companion to Vygotsky (pp. 21–49). Cambridge University Press. van Egmond, M. C., Navarrete Berges, A., Omarshah, T., & Benton, J. (2017). The role of intrinsic motivation and the satisfaction of basic psychological needs under conditions of severe resource scarcity. Psychological Science, 28(6), 822–828. van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Sweller, J. (2010). Cognitive load theory: Advances in research on worked examples, animations, and cognitive load measurement. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 375–378. van Leeuwen, A., & Janssen, J. (2019). A systematic review of teacher guidance during collaborative learning in primary and secondary education. Educational Research Review, 27, 71–89. https://doi.org/10.1016 /j.edurev.2019.02.001 Van Matre, J. C., Valentine, J. C., & Cooper, H. (2000). Effect of students’ after-school activities on teachers’ academic expectations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 167–183. Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Kirschner, P. A. (2013). Ten steps to complex learning (2nd ed.). Routledge. Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17, 147–177. Van Meter, P. (2001). Drawing construction as a strategy for learning from text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 129–140. Van Ryzin, M. J., & Roseth, C. J. (2018). Cooperative learning in middle school: A means to improve peer relations and reduce victimization, bullying, and related outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 1192–1201. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000265 Vandell, D. L. (2004). Early child care: The known and the unknown. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 387–414. Vandermass-Peler, M. (2002). Cultural variations in parental support of children’s play. In W. J. Lonner, D. L. Dinnel, S. A. Hayes, & D. N. Sattler (Eds.), Online readings in psychology and culture (Unit 11, Chapter 3; Vol. 2007). Center for Cross-Cultural Research, Western Washington University. Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Intrinsic versus extrinsic goal contents in self-determination theory: Another look at the quality of academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 41, 19–31. Vansteenkiste, M., & Mouratidis, A. (2016). Emerging trends and future directions for the field of motivation psychology: A special issue in honor of Prof. Dr. Willy Lens. Psychologica Belgica, 56(3), 118–142. https:// doi.org/10.5334/pb.354 Vantieghem, W., Vermeersch, H., & Van Houtte, M. (2014). Transcending the gender dichotomy in educational gender gap research: The association between gender identity and academic self-efficacy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39, 369–378.

R-71

R-72 References Varma, S., McCandliss, B. D., & Schwartz, D. L. (2008). Scientific and pragmatic challenges for bridging education and neuroscience. Educational Researcher, 37, 140–152. Vasquez, J. A. (1990). Teaching to the distinctive traits of minority students. The Clearing House, 63, 299–304. Vaughn, M. (2018). Making sense of student agency in the early grades. Phi Delta Kappan, 99(7), 62–66. Vaughn, S. R., & Bos, C. S., & Schumm, J, S. (2014). Teaching students who are exceptional, divers, and at risk in the general education classroom (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson. Vaughn, S., Levy, S., Coleman, M., & Bos, C. S. (2002). Reading instruction for students with LD and EBD: A synthesis of observation studies. Journal of Special Education, 36(1), 2–13. Verkuyten, M. (2016). Further conceptualizing ethnic and racial identity research: The social identity approach and its dynamic model. Child Development, 87(6), 1796–1812. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12555 Verplaetse, L. S., & Migliacci, N. (2008). Inclusive pedagogy: An introduction. In L. S. Verplaetse & N. Migliacci (Eds.), Inclusive pedagogy for English language learners: A handbook of research-informed practices (pp. 3–13). Erlbaum. Vidal-Abarca, E., Mañá, A, & Gil, L. (2010). Individual differences for self-regulating task-oriented reading activities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 817–826. Villegas, L. (2020). Ever-present but unexpected: Native American English learners. New America. https:// www.newamerica.org/education policy/edcentral/ever-present-unexpected-native-american-english -learners/ Visser, B. A., Ashton, M. C., & Vernon, P. A. (2006). Beyond g: Putting multiple intelligences theory to the test. Intelligence, 34, 487–502. Vogel, F., Wecker, C., Kollar, I., & Fischer, F. (2017). Socio-cognitive scaffolding with computer-supported collaboration scripts: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 29(3), 477–511. https://doi.org/10.1007 /s10648-016-9361-7 Vogt, M., & Echevarría, J. (2022). 99 ideas and activities for teaching English learners with SIOP® (2nd ed.). Pearson Von Suchodoletz, A., Jamil, F. M., Larsen, R. A. A. A., & Hamre, B. K. (2018). Personal and contextual factors associated with growth in preschool teachers’ self-efficacy beliefs during a longitudinal professional development study. Teaching and Teacher Education, 75, 278–289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2018.07.009 Vossoughi, S., Davis, N. R., Jackson, A., Echevarria, R., Muñoz, A., & Escudé, M. (2021). Beyond the binary of adult versus child centered learning: Pedagogies of joint activity in the context of making. Cognition and Instruction, 39(3), 211–241. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2020.1860052 Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher mental process. Harvard University Press. Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. MIT Press. Vygotsky, L. S. (1987a). The genetic roots of thinking and speech. In R. W. Rieber & A. S. Carton (Eds.), Problems of general psychology, Vol. 1. Collected works (pp. 101–120). Plenum. (Work originally published 1934). Vygotsky, L. S. (1987b). Problems of general psychology. Plenum. Vygotsky, L. S. (1987c). Thought and word. In R. W. Rieber & A. S. Carton (Eds.), Collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 1. Problems of general psychology (pp. 243–285). Plenum. (Work originally published 1934). Vygotsky, L. S. (1993). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 2 (J. Knox & C. Stevens, Trans.). Plenum. Vygotsky, L. S. (1997). Educational psychology (R. Silverman, Trans.). St. Lucie. Vygotsky, L. S. (1998). The problem of age. In R. W. Rieber (Ed.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 5 (pp. 187–205). (M. J. Hall, Trans.). Plenum. Vygotsky, L. S. (2012). Thought and language. MIT Press. Waasdorp, T. E., Monopoli, W. J., Horowitz-Johnson, Z., & Leff, S. (2019). Peer sympathy for bullied youth: Individual and classroom considerations. School Psychology Review, 48, 193–206. Waits, B. K., & Demana, F. (2000). Calculators in mathematics teaching and learning: Past, present, future. In M. J. Burke & F. R. Curcio (Eds.), Learning mathematics for a new century: NCTM 2000 Yearbook (pp. 51–66). National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Walberg, H. J. (1990). Productive teaching and instruction: Assessing the knowledge base. Phi Delta Kappan, 72, 470–478. Walberg, H. J., & Blast, J. L. (2014). Rewards: How to use rewards to help children learn and why teachers don’t use them well. The Heartland Institute. Walker, J. E., Shea, T. M., & Bauer, A. M. (2004). Behavior management: A practical approach for educators. Merrill/Prentice Hall. Walker, T. (2017, January). If elementary schools say no to homework, what takes its place? NEA Today. NEA. http://neatoday.org/2017/01/26/no-homework-policy/ Wallace, J. B. (2014, September 12). Should children be held back for kindergarten? Wall Street Journal. https://www.wsj.com/articles/should-children-be-held-back-for-kindergarten-1410536168 Walls, M. L., & Whitbeck, L. B. (2011). Maturation, peer context, and indigenous girls’ early-onset substance use. Journal of Early Adolescence, 31(3), 415–442. Walqui, A. (2008). The development of teacher expertise to work with adolescent English learners: A model and a few priorities. In L. S. Verplaetse & N. Migliacci (Eds.), Inclusive pedagogy for English language learners: A handbook of research-informed practices (pp. 103–125). Erlbaum. Walsh, M. (2015, October 6). CNN’s “#Being13” is a worthy look at the digital lives of young adolescents. Education Week. http://blogs.edweek.org/edweek/education_and_the_media/2015/10/cnns_being_13 _is_a_worthy_look_at_the_digital_lives_of_young_adolescents.html Walters, G. D., & Espelage, D. L. (2018). From victim to victimizer: Hostility, anger, and depression as mediators of the bullying victimization-bullying perpetration association. Journal of School Psychology, 68, 73–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2017.12.003

References Wang, M.-T., Chow, A., Hofkens, T., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2015). The trajectories of student emotional engagement and school burnout with academic and psychological development: Findings from Finnish adolescents. Learning and Instruction, 36, 57–65. Wang, W. (2020, November). The U.S. divorce rate has hit a 50 year low. Institute for Family Studies. Retrieved February 28, 2020, from https://ifstudies.org/blog/the-us-divorce-rate-has-hit-a-50-year-low Wang, Z., & Adesope, O. O. (2014). Effects of seductive details on multimedia learning. Journal of Studies in Education, 4, 32–44. Wanzek, J., Vaughn, S., Scammacca, N. K., Metz, K., Murray, C. S., Roberts, G., & Danielson, L. (2013). Extensive reading interventions for students with reading difficulties after grade 3. Review of Educational Research, 83, 163–195. Ward, L. M. (2004). Wading through the stereotypes: Positive and negative associations between media use and Black adolescents’ conception of self. Developmental Psychology, 40, 284–294. Ward, L. M., & Grower, P. (2020). Media and the development of gender role stereotypes. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 2(1), 177–199. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-051120-010630 Warr, M., & Mishra, P. (2021). Integrating the discourse on teachers and design: An analysis of ten years of scholarship. Teaching and Teacher Education, 99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2020.103274 Waterhouse, L. (2006). Multiple intelligences, the Mozart effect, and emotional intelligence: A critical review. Educational Psychologist, 41, 207–225. Waters, S., & Cross, D. (2010). Measuring students’ connectedness to school, teachers, and family: Validation of three scales. School Psychology Quarterly, 25, 164–177. Watson, J. B. (1919). Psychology from the standpoint of a behaviorist. Lippincott. Watson, J. M., & Strayer, D. L. (2010). Supertaskers: Profiles in extraordinary multitasking ability. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 17, 479–485. Watts, T. W., Duncan, G. J., & Quan, H. (2018). Revisiting the Marshmallow Test: A conceptual replication investigating links between early delay of gratification and later outcomes. Psychological Science, 29(7), 1159–1177. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618761661 Waugh, C. K., & Gronlund, N. E. (2013). Assessment of student achievement (10th ed.). Pearson. Waxman, S. R., & Lidz, J. L. (2006). Early word learning. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (6th ed., pp. 299–335). Wiley. Webb, N. L. (2002). Alignment study in language arts, mathematics, science, and social studies of state standards and assessment for four states. Council of Chief State School Officers. Webb, N. M., Farivar, S. H., & Mastergeorge, A. M. (2002). Productive helping in cooperative groups. Theory Into Practice, 41, 13–20. Webb, N. M., & Mastergeorge, A. M. (2003). The development of students’ helping behavior and learning in peer-directed small groups. Cognition and Instruction, 21, 361–428. Wechsler, D. (2014). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (5th ed.). NCS Pearson. Wei, L., Murphy, P. K., & Firetto, C. M. (2018). How can teachers facilitate productive small-group talk? The Elementary School Journal, 118(4), 578–609. Weil, E. (2008, March 2). Should boys and girls be taught separately? The New York Times Magazine, pp. 33–451. Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. Springer-Verlag. Weiner, B. (2000). Interpersonal and intrapersonal theories of motivation from an attributional perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 12, 1–14. Weiner, B. (2010). The development of an attribution-based theory of motivation: A history of ideas. Educational Psychologist, 45, 28–36. Weiner, B. (2011). An attribution theory of motivation. In A. W. Kruglanski, P. A. M. Van Lange, & E. T. Higgins Eds.). Handbook of theories in social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 135–155). Sage Publications. Weiner, B. (2018). The legacy of an attributional approach to motivation and emotion: A no-crisis zone. Motivation Science, 4, 4–14. Weinstein, C. S. (1977). Modifying student behavior in an open classroom through changes in the physical design. American Educational Research Journal, 14, 249–262. Weinstein, C. S. (1999). Reflections on best practices and promising programs: Beyond assertive classroom discipline. In H. J. Freiberg (Ed.), Beyond behaviorism: Changing the classroom management paradigm (pp. 147–163). Allyn & Bacon. Weinstein, C. S., Curran, M., & Tomlinson-Clarke, S. (2003). Culturally responsive classroom management: Awareness into action. Theory Into Practice, 42, 269–276. Weinstein, C. S., & Novodvorsky, I. (2015). Middle and secondary classroom management: Lessons from research and practice (5th ed.). McGraw-Hill. Weinstein, C. S., & Romano, M. E. (2015). Elementary classroom management (6th ed.). McGraw-Hill. Weinstein, C. S., & Romano, M. E. (2019). Elementary classroom management (7th ed.). McGraw-Hill. Weinstein, C. S., Tomlinson-Clarke, S., & Curran, M. (2004). Toward a conception of culturally responsive classroom management. Journal of Teacher Education, 55, 25–38. Weinstein, R. S., Madison, S. M., & Kuklinski, M. R. (1995). Raising expectations in schools: Obstacles and opportunities for change. American Educational Research Journal, 32, 121–159. Weissberg, R. P., Durlak, J. A., Domitrovich, C. E., & Gullotta, T. P. (2015). Social and emotional learning: Past, present, and future. In J. A. Durlak, C. E. Domitrovich, R. P. Durlak, J. A., Domitrovich, C. E., Weissberg, R. P., & Gullotta, T. P. (Eds.), Handbook of social and emotional learning: Research and practice (pp. 3–19). Guilford.

R-73

R-74 References Wells, A. E., Hunnikin, L. M., Ash, D. P., & Van Goozen, S. H. M. (2020). Children with behavioral problems misinterpret the emotions and intentions of others. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 48, 213–222. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-019-00594-7 Welsh, J. A., Nix, R. L., Blair, C., Bierman, K. L., & Nelson, K. E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 43–53. Welsh, R. O., & Little, S. (2018). The school discipline dilemma: A comprehensive review of disparities and alternative approaches. Review of Educational Research, 88, 752–794. https://doi .org/10.3102/0034654318791582 Wenger, I., Schulze, C., Lundström, U., & Prellwitz, M. (2020). Children’s perceptions of playing on inclusive playgrounds: A qualitative study. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1080 /11038128.2020.1810768 Wentzel, K. R. (2002). Are effective teachers like good parents? Teaching styles and student adjustment in early adolescence. Child Development, 73, 287–301. Wentzel, K. R. (2019). Introduction to the special issue on social and emotional learning. Educational Psychologist, 54, 127–-128. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2019.1637739 Wentzel, K. R., Barry, C. M., & Caldwell, K. A. (2004). Friendships in middle school: Influences on motivation and school adjustment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 195–203. Wentzel, K. R., & Miele, D. B. (2016). (Eds.). Handbook of motivation at school (2nd ed.). Routledge. Werker, J. F. (2018). Perceptual beginnings to language acquisition. [Target article with peer commentaries]. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(4), 703–728. Werker, J. F., & Ward, L. (2018). Critical periods and early experience. In P. Tortell & M. Turin (Eds.), Memory (pp. 121–128). Peter Wall Institute for Advanced Studies, University of British Columbia. Werts, M. G., Culatta, A., & Tompkins, J. R. (2007). Fundamentals of special education: What every teacher should know (3rd ed.). Pearson/Allyn & Bacon-Merrill. Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Harvard University Press. Wertsch, J. V. (2007). Mediation. In H. Daniels, M. Cole, & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), The Cambridge companion to Vygotsky (pp. 178–192). Cambridge University Press. West, M. R., Kraft, M. A., Finn, A. S., Martin, R. E., Duckworth, A. L., Gabrieli, C. F. O., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2016). Promise and paradox: Measuring students’ non-cognitive skills and the impact of schooling. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 38(1), 148–170. https://doi.org/10.3102/0162373715597298 Westgate, E. C. (2020). Why boredom is interesting. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(1), 33–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419884309 Westling, E., Andrews, J. A., Hampson, S. E., & Peterson, M. (2008). Pubertal timing and substance use: The effects of gender, parental monitoring and deviant peers. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42, 555–563. Wetzel, N., Scharf, F., & Widmann, A. (2019). Can't ignore-distraction by task-irrelevant sounds in early and middle childhood. Child Development, 90, 819–830. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13109 Wheatley, K. F. (2002). The potential benefits of teacher efficacy doubts for educational reform. Teaching and Teacher Education, 18, 5–22. White, M. C. (2017). Cognitive modeling and self-regulation of learning in instructional settings. Teachers College Record, 199(13), 1–26. White, M. C., & DiBenedetto, M. K. (2018). Self-regulation: An integral part of standards-based education. In D. H. Schunk & J. A. Greene (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (2nd ed., pp. 208–222). Routledge. Whitehead, A. N. (1929). The aims of education. Macmillan. Whitehurst, G. J., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Payne, A. C., Crone, D. A., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). Outcomes of an emergent literacy program in Headstart. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 542–555. Wigfield, A., Byrnes, J. P., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Development during early and middle adolescence. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 87–113). Erlbaum. Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2020). Thirty-five years of research on students’ subjective task values and motivation: A look back and a look forward. In A. Elliot (Ed.), Advances in motivation science: Vol. 7 (pp. 162–193). Elsevier. Wigfield, A., Tonks, S. M., & Klauda, S. L. (2016). Expectancy-value theory. In K. R. Wentzel & D. B. Miele (Eds.). Handbook of motivation at school (2nd ed., pp. 55–74). Routledge. Wiggins, G., & McTighe, J. (2006). Understanding by design (2nd ed.). Pearson. Wiley, J., Jaeger, A. J., & Taylor, A. R. (2018). When analogies harm: The effects of analogies on metacomprehension. Learning and Instruction, 55, 113–123. Wilkerson, I. (2020). Caste: The origins of our discontents. Random House. Williams, C. D., Byrd, C. M., Quintana, S. M., Anicama, C., Kiang, L., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Calzada, E. J., Gautier, M. P., Ejesi, K., Tuitt, N. R., Martinez-Fuentes, S., White, L., Marks, A., Rogers, L. O., & Whitesell, N. (2020). A lifespan model of ethnic-racial identity. Research in Human Development, 17(2–3), 99–129. https://doi.org/10.1080/15427609.2020.1831882 Willingham, D. T. (2004). Reframing the mind. Education Next, 4(3), 19–24. Willingham, D. T. (2006). Ask the cognitive scientist: How praise can motivate—or stifle. American Educator, Winter 2005–2006. https://www.aft.org/ae/winter2005-2006/willingham Willingham, D. T. (2009). Why don’t students like school? Jossey-Bass. Willingham, D. T., Hughes, E. M., & Dobolyi, D. G. (2015). The scientific status of learning styles theories. Teaching Psychology, 42, 266–271. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628315589505 Willower, D. J., Eidell, T. L., & Hoy, W. K. (1967). The school and pupil control. The Pennsylvania State University.

References Wilson, M., & Trainin, G. (2007). First-grade students’ motivation and achievement for reading, writing, and spelling. Reading Psychology, 28, 257–282. Windschitl, M. (2002). Framing constructivism in practice as the negotiation of dilemmas: An analysis of the conceptual, pedagogical, cultural, and political challenges facing teachers. Review of Educational Research, 72, 131–175. Winett, R. A., & Winkler, R. C. (1972). Current behavior modification in the classroom: Be still, be quiet, be docile. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 499–504. Wink, J., & Putney, L. (2002). A vision of Vygotsky. Allyn & Bacon. Winne, P. H. (2001). Self-regulated learning viewed from models of information processing. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 153–189). Erlbaum. Winne, P. H. (2011). A cognitive and metacognitive analysis of self-regulated learning. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (pp. 15–32). Routledge. Winne, P. H. (2017). Cognition and metacognition within self-regulated learning. In D. H. Schunk & J. A. Greene (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (2nd ed., pp. 36–48). Routledge. Winne, P. H., & Perry, N. E. (2000). Measuring self-regulated learning. In P. Pintrich, M. Boekaerts, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 531–566). Academic Press. Winner, E. (2000). The origins and ends of giftedness. American Psychologist, 55, 159–169. Winner, E. (2003). Musical giftedness. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 4, 1, 2–5. Winsler, A., & Naglieri, J. A. (2003). Overt and covert verbal problem-solving strategies: Developmental trends in use, awareness, and relations with task performance in children age 5 to 17. Child Development, 74, 659–678. Wittrock, M. C. (1982, March). Educational implications of recent research on learning and memory. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association. Wittwer, J., & Renkl, A. (2010). How effective are instructional explanations in example-based learning? A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 393–409. Wolf, M., Barzillai, M., Gottwald, S., Miller, L., Spencer, K., Norton, E., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. (2009). The RAVE-O intervention: Connecting neuroscience to the classroom. Mind, Brain, and Education, 3, 84–93. Wolfe, P. (2010). Brain matters: Translating research into classroom practice (2nd ed.). Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Wolff, M., Wells, B., Ventura-DiPersia, C., Renson, A., & Grov, C. (2017). Measuring sexual orientation: A review and critique of US data collection efforts and Implications for health policy. The Journal of Sex Research, 54, 507–531. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2016.1255872 Wolpert-Gawron, H. (2017, November). Extending classroom management online. Edutopia. https://www .edutopia.org/article/extending-classroom-management-online Won, S., Lee, S. Y., & Bong, M. (2017). Social persuasions by teachers as a source of student self-efficacy: The moderating role of perceived teacher credibility. Psychology in the Schools, 54(5), 532–547. https:// doi.org/10.1002/pits.22009 Wong, H. K., & Wong, R. T. (2018). The first days of school: How to be an effective teacher (5th ed.). Harry K. Wong Publications, Inc. Wong, K. F., & Xiao, Y. (2010). Diversity and difference: Identity issues of Chinese heritage language learners from dialect backgrounds. Heritage Language Journal, 7, 153–187. Wong, L. (1987). Reaction to research findings: Is the feeling of obviousness warranted? Dissertation Abstracts International, 48/12, 3709B. University Microfilms #DA 8801059. Wong, L. (2015). Essential study skills (8th ed.). Cengage. Wong, S. S., & Lim, S. W. (2019). Prevention–Permission–Promotion: A review of approaches to errors in learning. Educational Psychologist, 54, 1–19. Wood, D., Bruner, J., & Ross, S. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 181–191. Wood, K. (2020). The path of teachers’ learning through lesson and learning studies. International Journal for Lesson & Learning Studies, 9(2), 93–99. https://doi.org/ 10.1108/IJLLS-12-2019–0083 Woodward, A., & Needham, A. (Eds.). (2009). Learning and the infant mind. Oxford University Press. Woodward, J., Beckmann, S., Driscoll, M., Franke, M., Herzig, P., Jitendra, A., Koedinger, K. R., & Ogbuehi, P. (2018). Improving mathematical problem solving in grades 4 through 8: A practice guide: Revised (NCEE 2012–4055). National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/Docs/PracticeGuide/MPS_PG_043012.pdf/. Woolfolk, A. E. (1993). Readings and cases in educational psychology. Allyn & Bacon. Woolfolk, A. E., & Perry, N. E. (2015). Child and adolescent development (2nd ed.). Pearson. Woolfolk Hoy, A. (2013). A reflection on the place of emotion in teaching and teacher education. In M. Newberry, A. Gallant, & P. Riley (Eds.), Advances in research in education: Emotion and school: International perspectives on the function, process and products of the ‘“other” curriculum (pp. 255–270). Emerald. Woolfolk Hoy, A. (in press). Gaining efficacy with experience: From teacher education to classroom practice. In T. Good & M. McCaslin (Eds.), Routledge encyclopedia of education: Educational psychology. Routledge/ Taylor and Francis. Woolfolk Hoy, A., & Burke-Spero, R. (2005). Changes in teacher efficacy during the early years of teaching: A comparison of four measures. Teaching and Teacher Education, 21, 343–356. Woolfolk Hoy, A., Demerath, P., & Pape, S. (2002). Teaching adolescents: Engaging developing selves. In T. Urdan & F. Pajares (Eds.), Adolescence and education (Vol. I, pp. 119–169). Information Age Publishing. Woolfolk Hoy, A., & Hoy, W. K. (2020). Instructional leadership: A research-based guide to learning in schools (5th ed.). Pearson/Allyn & Bacon. Woolfolk Hoy, A., Hoy, W. K., & Davis, H. (2009). Teachers’ self-efficacy beliefs. In K. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation in school (pp. 627–654). Erlbaum.

R-75

R-76 References Woolfolk Hoy, A., & Tschannen-Moran, M. (1999). Implications of cognitive approaches to peer learning for teacher education. In A. O’Donnell & A. King (Eds.), Cognitive perspectives on peer learning (pp. 257–284). Erlbaum. Woolfolk Hoy, A., & Weinstein, C. S. (2006). Students’ and teachers’ perspectives about classroom management. In C. Evertson & C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook for classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues (pp. 181–220). Erlbaum. Wouters, P., van Nimwegen, C., van Oostendrop, H., & van der Spek, E. D. (2013). A meta-analysis of the cognitive and motivational effects of serious games. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 249–265. Wu, S. P. W., & Rau, M. A. (2018). Effectiveness and efficiency of adding drawing prompts to an interactive educational technology when learning with visual representations. Learning and Instruction, 55, 93–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.09.010 Wu, W., West, S. G., & Hughes, J. N. (2010). Effect of grade retention in first grade on psychosocial outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 135–152. Wu, X., Anderson, R. C., Nguyen-Jahiel, K., & Miller, B. (2013). Enhancing motivation and engagement through collaborative discussion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 622–632. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032792 Wu, Y., & Jobson, L. (2019). Maternal reminiscing and child autobiographical memory elaboration: A metaanalytic review. Developmental Psychology, 55, 2505–2521. Xie, H., Mayer, R. E., Wang, F., & Zhou, Z. (2019). Coordinating visual and auditory cueing in multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(2), 235–255. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000285 Xu, J., Coats, L. T., & Davidson, M. L. (2012). Promoting student interest in science: The perspectives of exemplary African American teachers. American Educational Research Journal, 49, 124–154. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0002831211426200 Xu, K. M., Koorn, P., de Koning, B., Skuballa, I. T., Lin, L., Henderikx, M., Marsh, H. W., Sweller, J., & Paas, F. (2020). A growth mindset lowers perceived cognitive load and improves learning: Integrating motivation to cognitive load. Journal of Educational Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000631 Yada, A., Tolvanen, A., Malinen, O. P., Imai-Matsumura, K., Shimada, H., Koike, R., & Savolainen, H. (2019). Teachers’ self-efficacy and the sources of efficacy: A cross-cultural investigation in Japan and Finland. Teaching and Teacher Education, 81, 13–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2019.01.014 Yale Center for Emotional Intelligence. (2021). https://www.ycei.org/our-history Yang, C., Sharkey, J. D., Reed, L. A., & Dowdy, E. (2020). Cyberbullying victimization and student engagement among adolescents: Does school climate matter? School Psychology, 35(2), 158–169. https:// doi.org/10.1037/spq0000353 Yang, L., Shuai, L., Du, Q., et al. (2009). Atomoxetine and executive functioning in Chinese ADHD children. From Childhood to Adult Disease. May 21–24, 2009, Vienna, Austria. Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 1, 135. Yap, A. C. (2016, October). A new generation of all-girls schools. The Atlantic. http://www.theatlantic.com/ education/archive/2016/10/a-new-generation-of-all-girls-schools/504044/ Yeager, D. S., Carroll, J. M., Buontempo, J., Cimpian, A., Woody, S., Crosnoe, R., Muller, C., Murray, J., Mhatre, P., Kersting, N., Hulleman, C., Kudyn, M., Murphy, M., Duckworth, A., Walton, G. M., & Dweck, C. S. (2022). Teacher mindsets help explain where a growth mindset intervention does and doesn’t work. Psychological Science, 33(1), 18–32. doi:10.1177/09567976211028984 Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2020). What can be learned from growth mindset controversies? The American Psychologist, 75(9), 1269–1284. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000794 Yeager, D. S., Hanselman, P., Walton, G. M., Murray, J. S., Crosnoe, R., Muller, C., Tipton, E., Schneider, B., Hulleman, C. S., Hinojosa, C. P., Paunesku, D., Romero, C., Flint, K., Roberts, A., Trott, J., Iachan, R., Buontempo, J., Yang, S. M., Carvalho, C. M., … Dweck, C. S. (2019). A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement. Nature, 573(7774), 364–369. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-019 -1466-y Yeager, D. S., Henderson, M. D., Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., D’Mello, S., Spitzer, B. J., & Duckworth, A. L. (2014). Boring but important: A self-transcendent purpose for learning fosters academic self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 559–580. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037637 Yell, M. L. (1990). The use of corporal punishment, suspension, expulsion, and timeout with behaviorally disordered students in public schools: Legal considerations. Behavioral Disorders, 15, 100–109. Yi, V., Mac, J., Na, V. S., Venturanza, R. J., Museus, S. D., Buenavista, T. L., & Pendakur, S. L. (2020). Toward an anti-imperialistic critical race analysis of the model minority myth. Review of Educational Research, 90(4), 542–579. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654320933532 Yip, T., Wang, Y., Mootoo, C., & Mirpuri, S. (2019). Moderating the association between discrimination and adjustment: A meta-analysis of ethnic/racial identity. Developmental Psychology, 55(6), 1274–1298. https:// doi.org/10.1037/dev0000708 Yont, K. M., Snow, C. E., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2003). The role of context in mother-child interactions: An analysis of communicative intents expressed during toy play and book reading with 12-month-olds. Journal of Pragmatics, 35, 436–454. Yough, M. (2010, August). An intervention: Teaching candidates’ beliefs and linguistic minority students. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association Annual Convention, San Diego, CA. Young, J. R. (2021, February 17). Citing racism and “years of bullying,” Dena Simmons resigns from Yale Center for Emotional Intelligence. EdSurge. https://www.edsurge.com/news/2021-02-17-citing-racism-and-years -of-bullying-dena-simmons-resigns-from-yale-center-for-emotional-intelligence Yue, C. L., Strom, B. C., Kornell, N., & Bjork, E. L. (2015). Highlighting and its relation to distributed study and students’ metacognitive beliefs. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 69–78.

References Zablotsky, B., Black, L. I., Maenner, M. J., Schieve, L. A., Danielson, M. L., Bitsko, R. H., Blumberg, S. J., Kogan, M. D., & Boyle, C. A. (2019). Prevalence and trends of developmental disabilities among children in the US: 2009–2017. Pediatrics, 144(4), e20190811. Zacks, J. M. (2020). Event perception and memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 165–191. Zamary, A., & Rawson, K. A. (2018). Are provided examples or faded examples more effective for declarative concept learning? Educational Psychology Review, 30, 1167–1197. Zarate, K., Maggin, D. M., & Passmore, A. (2019). Meta-analysis of mindfulness training on teacher wellbeing. Psychology in the Schools, 56(10), 1700–1715. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.22308 Zee, M., & Koomen, H. M. Y. (2016). Teacher self-efficacy and its effects on classroom processes, student academic adjustment, and teacher well-being: A synthesis of 40 years of research. Review of Educational Research, 86(4), 981–1015. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654315626801 Zehr, H., Amstutz, L. S., MacRae, A., & Parnis, K. (2015). The big book of restorative justice: Four classic justice & peacebuilding books in one volume. Good Books. Zeidner, M. (2014). Anxiety in education. In R. Pekrun & L. Linnenbrink (Eds.), International handbook of emotions and education (pp. 265–288). Taylor & Francis/Routledge. Zeidner, M., & Matthews, G. (2011). Anxiety101. Springer. Zeldin, A. L., Britner, S. L., & Pajares, F. (2008). A comparative study of the self-efficacy beliefs of successful men and women in mathematics, science, and technology careers. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 45, 1036–1058. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.20131 Zepeda, C. D., Hlutkowsky, C. O., Partika, A. C., & Nokes-Malach, T. J. (2019). Identifying teachers’ supports of metacognition through classroom talk and its relation to growth in conceptual learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(3), 522–541.https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000300 Zhang, L., Basham, J., Carter, R. A., & Zhang, J. (2021). Exploring factors associated with the implementation of student-centered instructional practices in U.S. classrooms. Teaching and Teacher Education, 99(4). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2020.103273 Zhang, X., Anderson, R. C., Morris, J., Miller, B., Nguyen-Jahiel, K. T., Lin, T. J., Zhang, J., Jadallah, M., Scott, T., Sun, J., Latawiec, B., Ma, S., Grabow, K., & Hsu, J. Y. L. (2016). Improving children’s competence as decision makers: Contrasting effects of collaborative interaction and direct instruction. American Educational Research Journal, 53(1), 194–223. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831215618663 Zheng, B., Warschauer, M., Lin, C.-H., & Chang, C. (2016). Learning in one-to-one laptop environments: A meta-analysis and research synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 86(4), 1052–1084. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0034654316628645 Zhou, Z., Peverly, S. T., Beohm, A. E., & Chongde, L. (2001). American and Chinese children’s understanding of distance, time, and speed interrelations. Cognitive Development, 15, 215–240. Zhu, W., Boiarskaia, E. A., Welk, G. J., & Meredith, M. D. (2010). Physical education and school contextual factors relating to students’ achievement and cross-grade differences in aerobic fitness and obesity. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 81, S53–S64. Zimmerman, B. (2011). Motivational sources and outcomes of self-regulated learning and performance. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (pp. 49–64). Routledge. Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Becoming a self-regulated learner. Theory Into Practice, 41, 64–70. Zimmerman, B. J. (2013). From cognitive modeling to self-regulation: A social cognitive career path. Educational Psychologist, 48(3), 135–147. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2013.794676 Zimmerman, B. J., & Schunk, D. H. (Eds.). (2001). Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed.). Erlbaum. Zirkel, S., & Pollack, T. M. (2016). “Just let the worst students go”: A critical case analysis of public discourse about race, merit, and worth. American Educational Research Journal, 53(6), 1522–1555. https:// doi.org/10.3102/0002831216676568 Zu, T., Hutson, J., Loschky, L. C., & Rebello, N. S. (2020). Using eye movements to measure intrinsic, extraneous, and germane load in a multimedia learning environment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112, 1338–1352. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000441 Zumbrunn, S., Broda, M., Varier, D., & Conklin, S. (2020). Examining the multidimensional role of selfefficacy for writing on student writing self-regulation and grades in elementary and high school. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(3), 580–603. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12315 Zusho, A., & Clayton, K. (2011). Culturalizing achievement goal theory and research. Educational Psychologist, 46, 239–260. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2011.614526

R-77

This page intentionally left blank

Name Index Aamodt, S., 97 Abdelfattah, F., 169 Abdi, B., 539 Abduljabbar, A. S., 169 Aber, J. L., 151, 590 Aboud, F. E., 44, 45 Abraham, C., 480 Abrami, P. C., 25, 403, 452 Abrams, L. M., 689 Abrams, M. D., 480 Abry, T., 7, 578 Abuhamdeh, S., 539 Abu-Hilal, M. A., 169 Accavitti, M., 64 Acevedo, J., 455 Acharya, P., 562 Acker, R. V., 150 Ackerman, B. P., 50 Adachi, P. J. C., 455 Adams, C. M., 26 Adesope, O. O., 384, 537 Adjapong, E., 536 Adkins, N., 598 ADL, 65 Aelterman, N., 516 Agarwal, P. K., 361, 362, 364 Ageyev, V., 117, 118, 122 Agne, R. M., 622 Agullo, J. A., 256 Ahn, H. S., 479 Ahtone, T., 258 Ainley, M., 538 Airasian, P. W., 659, 667 Akhtar, S., 354 Akinola, M., 540 Al Otaiba, S., 253, 254 Alamos, P., 11 Alansari, M., 611–613 Alarcon, G. M., 197 Albanesi, C., 579 Albert, K., 429 Alberto, P. A., 297, 299, 301–302, 308, 311, 314 Albrecht, D., 278 Albro, E. R., 253, 254 Aldrich, C., 454 Aldrich, N. J., 434, 435 Aldrup, K., 11 Alexander, J. F., 635 Alexander, P. A., 18, 358, 485 Alferink, L. A., 95, 98 Alfieri, L., 434, 435 Alfredsson, J., 221 Algeo-Nichols, D., 639 Ali, S. R., 47 Allen, J. P., 13, 15, 515, 598 Allen, K., 518, 579 Allen-Meares, P., 588

Allensworth, E., 488 Allington, R. L., 53 Allison, C. M., 71 Alloway, T. P., 216, 346 Alloy, L. B., 529 Almasi, J. F., 380 Aloe, A. M., 557, 610 Altose, A. J., 544 Alvarez, A. N., 259 Alves de Lima, D., 269 Alzate, G., 159 Amabile, T. M., 201, 203 Amadieu, F., 452 Amanti, C., 126, 279, 631 Ambridge, B., 346 Ambrose, D., 203 Amemiya, J., 586 American Educational Research Association Council, 694 American Psychiatric Association, 219, 233, 234 American Psychological Association (APA), 43, 57, 65, 207, 208, 331 American Psychological Association, Presidential Task Force on Educational Disparities, 63 American Psychological Association, Zero Tolerance Task Force, 586 Amo, L. C., 557 Amstutz, L. S., 587 Ana, V., 58 Anagnostopoulos, D., 615, 693 Anagnostou, E., 215 Anastasiow, N. J., 448 Anderman, E. M., 18, 478, 523, 532 Anderman, L. H., 478, 523, 532, 562, 578, 609 Anderson, A., 19 Anderson, A. R., 580 Anderson, B., 539 Anderson, C. A., 151 Anderson, C. W., 440 Anderson, G. E., 675 Anderson, J. R., 93–95, 190, 194, 330, 333, 335, 344, 349–350, 356–357, 365, 366, 389, 396, 399, 400 Anderson, L., 254 Anderson, L. M., 624 Anderson, L. W., 618, 619 Anderson, M., 163 Anderson, M. C., 340, 343, 347, 349 Anderson, M. D., 70

Anderson, R. C., 118, 442, 477, 635 Anderson, S. R., 249, 258 Andrä, C., 350 Andreasen, N. K., 455 Andresen, A., 342 Andrews, M., 676 Andrews, E. E., 189 Andrews, J. A., 137 Andrews-Todd, J., 437 Andrews-Weckerly, S., 520 Anfara, V. A. Jr., 587 Angell, A. L., 253 Anguiano, C. J., 384 Anicama, C., 59 Anmarkrud, Ø., 342 Annenberg Foundation, 99 Ansari, A., 11, 578 Ansari, D., 99, 101 Ansary, N. S., 591, 592 Anyon, J., 61, 264 Anzures, G., 45 Apel, K., 253, 254 Appel, M., 46 Applebee, A. N., 635 Arango, C., 215 Arbuthnott, K. D., 207 Archambault, L., 461 Archer, C., 70, 257 Archer, S. L., 163 Archodidou, A., 118 Arena, D., 455, 456 Arends, L. R., 386 Arends, R. I., 433, 625, 635, 640 Arens, A. K., 165, 477, 526 Armento, B. J., 81, 599 Armer, M., 150 Armstrong, K., 15 Arneson, J. J., 50 Arnett, J. J., 162, 177 Arnold, M. L., 176 Aro, M. T., 480 Aro, T. I., 480 Aronson, E., 438, 443, 446–447 Aronson, J., 45, 196 Artelt, C., 375 Artiles, A. J., 70 Arum, R., 424 Ash, D. P., 151 Ashcraft, M., 333, 340, 345, 349, 351, 352, 356, 361, 394 Ashman, G., 398 Ashton, M. C., 192 Assor, A., 548 Assouline, S. G., 19 Asterhan, C. S. C., 406, 407, 440 Atkinson, R. C., 333 Attanucci, J., 172

Atteberry, A., 693 Atwood, J., 154 Au, T. K., 257 August, D., 8, 272 Austin, S., 7 Austin, T. Y., 280 Autin, F., 657 Auxier, B., 163 Auzias, G., 215 Avery, R. E., 408, 443 Avramidis, E., 210, 223 Ayalon, M., 651 Aydin, B., 499 Aydın, Y. C., 7 Ayres, L. R., 499 Azano, A. P., 236 Azevedo, R., 492 Baay, P., 486 Babad, E. Y., 613 Babichenko, M., 407 Bacchini, D., 490 Baceviciute, S., 455 Bachrach, J. E., 535, 536 Backfisch, I., 393 Baddeley, A. D., 216, 340–343, 347, 349, 361 Bae, C. L., 362 Baek, Y., 455 Baelen, R. N., 64, 488 Baer, J., 202 Bagley, E., 454 Bahrick, H. P., 357 Bailey, C. S., 8, 9 Bailey, J. M., 429, 674, 679 Bailey, R., 9, 494 Bailey, U. L., 221 Bailey, W. M., 481 Bain, P. M., 352, 364, 385, 429, 430, 633 Baker, C. M., 152 Baker, D. S., 609 Baker, J., 676 Baker, K., 270 Baker, K. M., 482 Baker, S. K., 272, 477 Balass, M., 100 Baldwin, B. T., 237 Baldwin, J. M., 90 Ball, D. L., 610 Balsai, M. J., 527 Banaschewski, T., 215 Bandura, A., 151, 168, 319, 320, 423, 467, 469, 470, 472, 473, 476–480, 483, 492, 497, 498, 521 Banerjee, R., 155 Banks, C. A. M., 37 Banks, J. A., 37, 75, 81, 267

N-1

N-2 Name Index Banks, S. R., 660, 665 Banner, G. E., 346 Barbaranelli, C., 480 Barbatsis, G., 451 Barber, B. R., 499 Barbot, B., 202 Bardach, L., 524 Bardi, A., 524 Bargh, J. A., 63 Barkley, R. A., 216, 221 Barlow, D. H., 25 Barlow, W., 406 Barmer, A., 61 Barmore, J., 694 Barnard-Brak, L., 676 Barnes, N., 651 Barnett, M. S., 408 Baron, A., 45 Baroody, A. E., 578 Baroody, A. J., 435, 452 Barr, H., 152 Barrett, L. F., 43, 540, 544 Barron, K. E., 526, 529 Barros, E., 138 Barry, C. M., 149 Barth, A. E., 214 Bartholomew, K. J., 511, 516 Bartlett, S. M., 311 Barton-Arwood, S. M., 691 Barzilai, S., 375, 413 Barzillai, M., 100 Basarkod, G., 165, 166 Basham, J., 637 Bassok, D., 676 Bastable, E., 599 Batalova, J., 259, 268 Bates, J., 148 Bates, J. E., 150 Bates, T. C., 530 Bathia, S., 62 Batruch, A., 657 Battiato, A. C., 630 Battistich, V. A., 442 Bau, C., 215 Bauer, A. M., 312 Bauer, L., 49 Bauer, P. J., 346 Bauer, S. S., 677 Bauer, W. I., 609 Baum, J., 72, 73 Baumeister, R. F., 63, 158, 169, 518 Baumert, J., 167, 169, 610, 630, 640 Baumrind, D., 144, 145 Bayliss, D. M., 216 Bayliss, P., 210, 223 Bay-Williams, J. M., 391 Bazán, A. R., 254 Beal, S. A., 488 Bean, F. D., 56 Beane, J. A., 168 Bear, G. G., 562 Beard, K. S., 539–540, 622

Beatty-Hazelbaker, R., 78, 79 Beaty, R. E., 201, 204 Beauchamp, C., 99 Beauchamp, M. H., 99 Bechara, A., 96 Beck, A. N., 677 Becker, B. J., 610 Becker, E. S., 609 Becker, M., 640 Becker, S., 542 Becker, W. C., 22 Beckes, L., 542 Beckmann, S., 22, 390, 392 Beebe-Frankenberger, M., 675 Beechum, N. O., 488 Beghetto, R. A., 203 Bego, C. R., 385 Behne, T., 138 Behrmann, M., 215 Beijaard, D., 634 Beike, S. M., 539 Beilock, S. L., 68, 70, 532 Beishuizen, J., 428 Belfiore, P. J., 319 Belland, B. R., 389, 399, 402, 427, 428, 432, 433, 435, 451, 547 Bellgrove, M. A., 215 Belsky, J., 12, 13 Bembenutty, H., 511 Benbow, C. P., 19, 240 Ben-Eliyahu, A., 493 Benita, M., 522 Benjamin, K. S., 578 Benner, A. D., 46, 49, 63 Bennett, C. I., 52, 78 Benton, J., 515 Benton, S. L., 101 Beohm, A. E., 114 Bereiter, C., 409, 424 Berenbaum, S. A., 65 Berg, C. A., 626 Berger, K. S., 117, 121, 125, 135, 138, 145, 171, 257, 258 Bergin, D. A., 536 Bergmann, J., 460 Berk, L. E., 94, 106, 114, 115, 120, 121, 135, 137, 196 Berko, J., 251 Berliner, D. C., 18, 25, 26, 51, 629, 633 Bernacki, M. L., 387, 453 Bernard, R. M., 25, 451 Bernecker, K., 486 Berry, R. Q. III., 61 Berryman, M., 153 Berson, E., 407 Berthold, K., 361 Bertram, C., 695 Best, J. R., 115 Betts, S. M., 482 Bewick, C. J., 457, 458 Bi, S.-S., 487 Bialystok, E., 257, 261, 271 Bichler, S., 392, 393

Bickel, E. S., 429 Bickmore, K., 587 Biddle, B., 624 Bierman, K. L., 347 Biglan, A., 318 Bigler, R. S., 65, 66, 70, 197 Biocca, F. A., 451 Bird, E., 139–141 Birgili, B., 460, 461 Birisci, S., 461 Bishop, D. V. M., 249 Bishop, R., 153 Bisra, K., 393 Bitsko, R. H., 6 Bjork, E. L., 382 Bjork, R. A., 19, 207, 361 Bjorklund, B. R., 355 Bjorklund, D. F., 114, 115, 122, 331, 355 Blachman, B. A., 254 Black, L. I., 6 Blair, C., 51, 190, 196, 347, 490, 677 Blanc, M. H. A., 258 Blasiman, R., 382 Blast, J. L., 322, 323 Blatchford, P., 428 Blazar, D., 694 Blazier, C., 6 Bledsoe, S. E., 227 Blewitt, P., 93, 94 Blömeke, S., 576, 583 Bloom, B. S., 23, 237, 619 Bloom, P., 250, 259 Blum, W., 610 Blumberg, S. J., 6 Blume, F., 570 Blumenfeld, P. C., 435, 561 Blunt, J. R., 384 Bobbit Nolen, S., 24 Bobek, D. L., 143 Bobis, J., 394 Bocian, K. L., 675 Bode, P., 75 Boden, K. K., 522 Bodmann, S. M., 522 Bodrova, E., 122, 125 Boedhoe, P., 215 Bohn-Gettler, C. M., 540 Boiarskaia, E. A., 139 Bois, J., 613 Boivin, M., 150 Bokosmaty, S., 389, 393 Bond, R., 480 Bonell, C., 228 Bong, M., 479, 479, 524, 527 Bonifay, W., 579 Bonner, S. M., 610 Bonneville-Roussy, A., 471 Boodoo, G., 197 Borich, G. D., 620, 637 Borjas, S., 428 Borko, H., 15, 407 Borman, G. D., 52

Bornstein, M. H., 197 Borokhovski, E., 451 Borrero, N. E., 258, 259 Borst, G., 115 Borup, J., 460, 461 Bos, C. S., 81, 628 Bosanquent, P., 428 Böse, S., 640 Boseck, J. J., 493 Bosker, R. J., 542, 611 Boston Children’s Hospital, 227 Botella, J., 491, 500 Bottge, B., 352, 364, 385, 429, 430, 633 Bottia, M. C., 62 Bottiani, J. H., 560, 598, 599 Bouchard, A., 197 Bouck, E., 219, 220, 231, 233, 234, 239–240 Bouffard, T., 471 Bowers, J. S., 98, 423 Bowker, J. C., 148–149 Bowles, R. P., 490 Boyce, W. T., 52 Boykin, W., 197 Boyle, C. A., 6 Boyle, J. R., 382 Boyle, M., 440, 442, 443 Boyle, O. F., 258, 262, 272–274 Brackett, M. A., 8, 9, 494, 542, 609 Bradshaw, C. P., 310, 315, 318, 560, 578, 590, 592, 598, 599 Brainerd, C. J., 114, 122 Brame, B., 150 Branch, A. J., 58 Brandeis, D., 215 Brand-Gruwel, S., 20 Brannon, L., 66, 68 Bransford, J. D., 100, 331, 409 Brantlinger, E., 49 Branum-Martin, L., 272 Braskhøj Lauridsen, L. L., 136 Bråten, I., 342 Bray, I., 139–141 Bray, M. A., 310 Bredekamp, S., 123 Breiner, K., 490 Breivik, K., 591 Brem, S., 215 Brendgen, M., 150 Bressoux, P., 480, 613 Brett, J. P., 544 Brice, A. E., 263 Brice, R. G., 263 Brichinall, L., 15 Bridges, M., 62 Briesch, A. M., 234, 318 Briggs, D. C., 431, 433 Brinkman, S., 197 Brio-Meisels, G., 9, 494 Britner, S. L., 478 Brix, N., 136 Broadbent, J., 480

Name Index Broady, K., 49 Broda, M., 480 Broderick, P. C., 93, 94 Brody, N., 197 Broidy, L. M., 150 Bronfenbrenner, U., 143 Bronson, E., 237 Brookhart, S. M., 654, 655 Brookmeyer, K. A., 148 Brooks, P. J., 434, 435 Brooks, W. D., 527 Brooks-Gunn, J., 53, 137, 137 Brophy, J. E., 304, 512, 524, 525, 532, 542, 546, 547, 548, 612, 624, 628, 640 Brown, A. L., 63, 100, 331, 381 Brown, B. B., 148 Brown, C. S., 46, 59, 63, 66–68, 73 Brown, E. D., 50 Brown, E. R., 46 Brown, J., 15, 72, 73 Brown, J. L., 11, 151, 496, 590 Brown, J. S., 366 Brown, L. E. R., 199, 200, 493 Brown, P. C., 101, 208, 330, 345, 359, 361, 362, 380, 496 Brown, R. D., 355 Bruce, B. C., 631 Bruck, M., 354 Bruer, J. T., 98 Bruner, J. S., 124, 351, 421, 428 Bruning, R. H., 338, 344, 359, 360, 375, 390, 399, 321, 423 Brunken, R., 537 Brunner, M., 165, 166, 190, 610 Brusca-Vega, R., 189 Brush, K., 9, 494 Bryant, C., 562 Bryant, P., 22 Bryce, C. I., 7 Buchanan, R., 673 Buchholz, J., 482 Bucich, M., 199, 200, 493 Buck, R., 15 Buck Institute for Education, 436 Buckner, J. C., 54 Budge, S. L., 73 Budiman, A., 267 Buehl, M. M., 545, 549 Buehler, R., 398 Buenavista, T. L., 44 Bühner, M., 392, 393 Buhs, E. S., 150 Bui, D. C., 382 Bullard, J., 457 Bulut, O., 662 Bunford, N., 223 Buntain-Ricklefs, J., 578 Bunuan, R. L., 310 Buontempo, J., 530, 531 Burbules, N. C., 631 Burchinal, M., 13, 153

Burden, P. R., 593 Burden, R., 210, 223 Bureau, J. S., 515 Bureau of Labor Statistics, 197 Burger, K., 480 Burgess-Brigham, R., 268 Burgos, G., 452 Burić, I., 482, 609 Buriel, R., 145, 167 Burke, D., 452 Burke-Spero, R., 7 Burns, E. C., 472 Burrell, S., 429 Bursuck, W. D., 210–214, 218, 222, 223, 231, 232, 238, 303, 316–318, 385, 642 Burt, S. A., 52 Busatto, G. F., 215 Busch, K. C., 407 Bush, J. R., 167 Bushman, B. J., 151 Buskirk, A. A., 148 Bussey, K., 140 Bustamante, M. R., 159 Butcher, K. R., 350, 360 Butera, F., 515, 525, 657 Buth, A. J., 586 Butisingh, S., 81, 529 Butler, D. L., 318, 319 Butterworth, B., 99, 101 Büttner, G., 665 Butz, A. R., 24, 479 Byers-Heinlein, K., 257 Byle, K. A., 196 Byrd, C. M., 59 Byrd, P., 269 Byrne, D., 524 Byrnes, J. P., 96, 136, 158, 162, 357, 381 Cain, K., 253, 254 Cain, T., 611 Cairns, B. D., 89 Cairns, R. B., 89 Cajete, G. A., 78 Çakıroğlu, J., 7 Calderhead, J., 616 Calderoni, S., 215 Caldwell, K. A., 149 Calfee, R., 618 Calhoun, B. M., 207 Callaghan, T., 138 Callahan, C. M., 236, 236, 282 Callahan, M., 189 Callan, G., 527 Callanan, M., 436 Calzada, E. J., 59 Cameron, J., 322 Cameron-Faulkner, T., 250 Campbell, E. M., 299 Campbell, J. D., 169 Campbell, S. L., 695 Campbell, W. K., 457 Cangelosi, J. S., 620 Canning, E. A., 536, 548

Cano-Garcia, F. J., 494 Canter, L., 596 Canter, M., 596 Cantrell, S. C., 380 Cao, Q., 578 Capa, Y., 8 Caprara, G. V., 477, 480 Carbone, P. S., 139 Carbonell, O. A., 159 Carbonneau, K. J., 123, 426 Cardoza, K., 276 Carlson, S. M., 171 Carlstrom, A., 379 Carney, R. N., 364 Carpenter, S. K., 364, 408, 410, 486 Carrasco-Ortiz, M. A., 494 Carroll, J. B., 190, 195 Carroll, J. M., 531 Carroll, M. D., 139 Carter, E. W., 691 Carter, J. S., 380 Carter, M., 210 Carter, R. A., 637 Carvalho, C. M., 530 Cascallar, E., 442 Case, R., 115, 123 Casey, B. J., 96 Cash, A. H., 578 Caskie, G., 390 Cassady, J. C., 482, 493, 525, 543, 545 Cassotti, M., 115 Castejon, J. L., 192 Castellano, J. A., 283 Castle, S., 639, 640 Castro-Villarreal, F., 214 Catrysse, L., 345 Cauffman, E., 177 Caughy, M. O., 165 Causadias, J. M., 37 Cavanna, J., 615 Cavell, H. J., 301 Caye, A., 219, 220 Cazden, C., 560 Ceci, S. J., 197, 354, 408 Center for Applied Special Technology (CAST), 642, 643 Center for Promise, 55 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), 72, 337 Cepeda, N. J., 364, 492 Chafouleas, S. M., 225, 318, 579 Cham, H., 496 Chamberlain, R. W., 364 Chan, C.-K., 50, 54 Chance, P., 320, 322–323 Chandler, P., 391 Chang, C., 452, 492 Chang, L., 490 Chang, M.-L., 494 Chang, Z., 331 Changas, P. C., 391

N-3

Chappuis, J., 616, 654, 659, 660, 662–667 Charles, C. M., 27, 560, 566, 576, 596, 597 Charles, E. P., 152 Charles-Harris, S., 615 Charness, N., 400 Charnigo, R., 221 Chase, J., 46 Chase, W. G., 399 Chaudhary, N., 490 Chávez, R., 54 Chawla, D. S., 234 Chein, J., 490 Chen, C. H., 433 Chen, F., 138 Chen, J. A., 455, 482 Chen, J.-K., 70, 257 Chen, K., 97 Chen, M., 562 Chen, Q., 201, 204 Chen, X., 148, 579 Chen, Z., 396 Cheng, S.-L., 482 Cheng, W., 9 Cherniss, C., 197–199 Cherry, L., 476 Cheryan, S., 67 Chesnut, S. R., 422, 427 Cheung, A. C. K., 271 Cheung, C.-N., 362 Chi, M. T. H., 391, 399, 409, 410, 421 Child Trends Databank, 6 Children’s Defense Fund, 6, 49, 155 Childress, S., 154 Chin, M. J., 63 Chinn, C. A., 433, 435 Chiu, C.-Y., 204 Chiu, M. M., 143, 149 Cho, B., 406 Choi, H.-H., 343 Chongde, L., 114 Chorzempa, B. F., 640 Chow, A., 535 Chow, B. W.-Y., 143, 149 Christensen, A., 547, 578 Christensen, A. P., 201, 204 Christenson, S. L., 580 Christodoulou, J., 19, 97 Christopher, J. C., 78, 79 Christou, C., 346 Chronis-Tuscano, A., 221 Ciani, K. D., 524 Cicognani, E., 579 Cimpian, A., 531 Cirino, P. T., 391 Clapp, E. P., 202 Clark, A. G., 162 Clark, C. M., 73 Clark, D. A., 385 Clark, D. B., 456 Clark, I., 654

N-4 Name Index Clark, J. M., 350 Clark, K., 270, 271 Clark, M. H., 46 Clark, R. E., 434, 626 Clarke, J. H., 622 Clarke-Stewart, K. A., 13, 153 Clary, M., 609 Clay-Chambers, J., 435 Clayton, K., 524, 525 Clemens, N., 254 Cleminshaw, C. L., 219, 221 Clough, M., 626 Coan, J. A., 542 Coats, L. T., 549 Cobas, J. A., 167 Cobb, P., 423 Cobley, S., 676 Coccia, M. A., 496 Cockburn, J., 294 Cocking, R. R., 100, 331 Codell, E. R., 15, 596 Coffield, F. J., 207 Coghill, D., 219, 220 Cohen, A. B., 37 Cohen, D. R., 482 Cohen, E. G., 439, 442, 443 Cohen, K. M., 72 Cohen-Eliyan, N., 440 Cokley, K., 52 Colangelo, N., 19 Colby, G. T., 639 Cole, K. M., 560, 566, 576, 596, 597 Cole, M., 423 Cole, P. M., 490 Coleman, D., 380 Coleman, M., 628 Coleman, M. R., 448 Coles, E. K., 221 Coll, C. G., 165 Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning (CASEL), 9, 154 Colledge, E., 249 College Board, 683 Collie, R. J., 472, 495, 562 Collier, V. P., 271 Collins, A., 366, 436 Collins, A. M., 424 Collins, C., 73 Collins, K. M., 431 Collins, P., 272 Collins, W. A., 148, 149 Cologon, K., 15 Comadena, M. E., 609 Committee on Increasing High School Students’ Engagement and Motivation to Learn, 500, 546 Common Sense Media, 7, 151, 152, 451 Compton, D. L., 216, 218, 391, 434

Conklin, S., 480 Conley, D., 424 Conneely, C., 439 Conner, T., 78, 79 Connor, C. M., 253, 254 Connor, J. P., 588 Connor-Zachocki, J., 451 Conrad, J., 635 Conway, M. A., 354 Cook, C. R., 578 Cook, G., 59, 94, 148, 165 Cook, J. L., 59, 94, 148, 165 Cook, M., 189 Cooper, C. E., 53 Cooper, C. R., 236 Cooper, H. M., 478, 611, 628, 629 Cooper, J. O., 291, 297, 302, 303 Cooper, S. R., 50 Cooper Johnson, L., 253, 254 Coplan, R. J., 148–150 Copple, C., 123 Copur-Gencturk, Y., 68 Corazza, G., 202 Corbin, C. M., 11 Cordes, S. A., 114 Corno, L., 486, 487, 639, 640 Corpus, J. H., 511 Cortes, K., 425, 441 Cortes, K. I., 441, 591 Cortez, A., 425 Cortina, K., 59, 61 Costello, M. B., 45 Cothran, D. J., 153 Coughlin, C., 434 Council on Children with Disabilities, 139 Covington, M. V., 524, 532, 543 Cowan, N., 347 Cox, K., 63 Coyne, P., 642 Crabbe, R., 148 Crago, R. V., 588 Craik, F. I. M., 345, 359 Cramond, B., 202 Crapo, R. H., 249 Craven, J. A. III., 610 Craven, R. G., 165–167 Crawford. A., 377 Creamer, J. F., 49 Crede, M., 489 Creese, A., 259 Cremin, L. A., 461 Creswell, J. W., 23 Crocker, J., 169 Cromley, J. G., 527 Crompton, H., 387, 452, 453 Crone, D. A., 253 Croninger, R. M. V., 445, 635, 636 Cronjaeger, H., 167 Crooks, C. V., 276 Cropley, M., 494 Crosnoe, R., 13, 53, 148, 153, 530, 531

Cross, D., 579 Cross, T., 4 Cross, T. B., 58 Cross, W. E. Jr., 58 Crosson, A., 566 Croudace, T., 66 Crowe, E. C., 253, 254 Cruickshank, D. R., 609 Crum, A. J., 540 Csapó, B., 389 Csikszentmihalyi, M., 539 Cucina, J. M., 196 Cui, A., 61 Culatta, A., 119, 240 Cullen, M., 496 Cummings, J., 557, 563, 580, 582 Cunningham, E., 316 Cupp, P. K., 177 Curby, T. W., 578 Curno, C., 22 Curran, M., 581, 599 Curry, S. R., 54 Curwin, R., 586 Cusack, A., 542 Cusick, P. A., 8 Cutter, J., 431 Cutuli, J. J., 50, 54 Dadds, M. R., 312 Dagenais, C., 25 Dagogo-Jack, S., 635 Dai, T., 527 Dale, P. S., 489 Daley, B. J., 384 Dalton, A., 146 Damon, W., 173 Daniels, L. M., 541, 542 Daniels, P., 71 Danielson, C., 12, 13, 695 Danielson, L., 254 Danielson, M. L., 6 Danish, J., 453 Dargue, N., 350 Darling-Hammond, L., 198, 493, 540 Darnon, C., 515, 525 Dart, E. H., 301 DaSilva Iddings, A. C., 280 Daunic, A. P., 499 Davidoff, J., 249 Davidson, E., 8 Davidson, J., 390 Davidson, M. L., 549 Davies, I. R. L., 249 Davies, P. G., 45, 46, 58, 59 Davis, D., 379 Davis, G., 146 Davis, G. A., 236, 240 Davis, H. A., 153, 482 Davis, H. L., 379 Davis, J., 254 Davis, N. R., 427 Davis, R., 496 Dawdanow, A., 380 Day, S. B., 393, 408

de Araujo, Z., 461 De Boer, H., 611 De Bruin, C. L., 234 De Castella, K., 524 De Fraine, B., 524 De France, K., 542 de Freitas, S., 455 De George, G., 281 de Groot, A. D., 399 De Groot, E. V., 485 de Jong, E. M., 11, 562 de Jong, T., 383, 384, 431 de Koning, B., 530 de Ruiter, L. E., 456 De Smedt, B., 99, 101 Dean, C. G. P., 436 Dean, D., 407 Dearing, E., 254, 279 Deary, I. J., 190 Debnam, K. J., 560, 578, 599 Debs, M., 600 Debus, R. L., 167 DeCaro, M. S., 435 Deci, E. L., 322, 497, 511, 514, 515, 522, 548 Deckers, L., 510 Decristan, J., 665 DeCuir-Gunby, J. T., 59 Dedrick, R. F., 362 Defend Innocence., 155 DeJong, J. M., 630 Dekhinet, R., 257 Del Bove, G., 480 Del Toro, J., 61 Delafield-Butt, J. T., 234 deLara, E., 151 DeLay, D., 494 Deleveaux, G., 452 Della-Chiesa, B., 93, 97, 100, 101 DeLozier, S., 380 Delpit, L., 76, 79, 81, 113, 255, 264, 599 Demana, F., 119 Demanet, J., 611, 677 Demaray, M. K., 609 DeMarie, D., 331 DeMauro, A. A., 496 Demerath, P., 52 Demetriou, A., 190, 194, 216, 346 Demuth, K., 252 Denessen, E., 63 Denissen, J. J. A., 677 Deniz, C. B., 639, 640 Dennie, D., 562 Dent, A. L., 236, 485 Denton, C. A., 214 Deppeler, J. M., 234 DeRose, L. M., 137 Derous, E., 432, 435 Derry, S. J., 422 Desautel, D., 376 Desjardins, C. D., 50, 54 DeSoto, K. A., 364

Name Index D’Esterre, A. P., 531 Dettmers, S., 630 DeWall, C. N., 397 DeWalt, P. S., 58 DeWeese, A., 496 Dewey, J., 421, 537 Dhaliwal, T. K., 63 Diament, M., 209 Diamond, A., 490, 496 Diamond, L. M., 65, 72 Diamond, N. T., 234 Diaz, E. I., 283 Diaz-Rico, L. T., 257, 260, 271–272 DiBenedetto, M. K., 470, 477, 480, 490, 511, 520 Dicke, A. L., 536 Dicke, T., 477, 526 Dickens, W., 196 dickey, l. m., 73 Dickinson, J., 480 Diemer, M. A., 49, 51, 70 Dierendinck, C., 165, 166 Diffley, R. III., 197 Digest of Educational Statistics, 676 DiGiacomo, D., 425, 437 Dilig, R., 61 Dillihunt, M. L., 78, 79 Dillon, K. P., 151 Dindia, K., 68 Ding, Y., 393 Dingfelder, S. F., 77 Dionne, G., 150 DiPerna, J. C., 9 Dirk, J., 347 Dixon, L. Q., 268 Dixson, D. D., 654, 665 D’Mello, S., 498 D’Mello, S. K., 633, 687 Doan, S., 694 Dobber, M., 431 Dobolyi, D. G., 19 Dochy, F., 442 Dodge, K. A., 143, 150, 171, 490 Doggett, A. M., 221 Doggett, R. A., 317 Dohn, N. B., 421–424 Doidge, N., 93 Dolan, C. V., 190 Dole, J. A., 413 Dolgin, K. G., 227 Dolmans, D. H. J. M., 433, 434 Dombo, E. A., 226 Domitrovich, C. E., 9, 493 Dompnier, B., 525 Donche, V., 345 Donnelly, P., 633 Dore, R. A., 138 Double, K. S., 199, 200, 493 Dougherty, M. R., 98 Douglas, K. M., 253, 254 Douin, T. A., 481 Dow, G. T., 203

Dowdy, E., 592 Dowler, J., 500 Downer, J. T., 11 Dowson, M., 543, 544 Doyle, S., 496 Doyle, W., 559 Drake, M., 322 Drayer, A. M., 679 Dresel, M., 528 Dresler, T., 570 Drew, C. J., 187, 219 Driscoll, A., 626, 635 Driscoll, M., 22, 390, 392, 688 Driscoll, M. P., 101, 410, 424, 425, 626 Druckman, D., 447 Drummond, L., 500 Du, Q., 220 Dubarry, M., 269 Dubinsky, J. M., 93, 97, 99, 101, 331 DuBois, D. L., 478 Duckworth, A., 531, 687 Duckworth, A. L., 64, 197, 487–489, 491, 496, 498 Duell, O. K., 633 Dufrene, B. A., 317 Duke, N. K., 435 Duke, R. F., 445 Dumont, H., 631 Duncan, G. J., 51, 53, 62, 487 Duncker, K., 397 Dunlosky, J., 382, 385 Dunn, D. S., 189 Dunn, K., 207 Dunn, R., 207 Dunston, K., 47 Duong, M. T., 578 DuPaul, G. J., 219, 221, 303 Durksen, T. L., 482 Durlak, J. A., 9, 142, 493 Dusenbury, L., 228 Dux, P. E., 338 Dweck, C. S., 303, 486, 530–532 Dworsky, A., 54 Dwyer, C., 331 Dymnicki, A. B., 142 Easter, M. A., 524 Easton, B. P., 152 Ebbinghaus, H., 364 Eberhard, D. M., 249, 258 Ebersbach, M., 114 Ecalle, J., 452 Eccles, J. S., 70, 96, 136, 158, 162, 167, 486, 526, 527, 546 Ecclestone, K., 207 Echevarría, J., 261, 262, 268, 274, 276–279 Echevarria, R., 427 Echeverria-Estrada, C., 259 Eckert, T. L., 521 Eddy, C. L., 482 Edelberg, W., 49 Edwards, J. M., 197

Edwards, K. D., 482 Edwards, S., 137 Edwards, S. A., 456, 457, 461 Ee, J., 62 Efklides, A., 485, 487 Ehlis, A.-C., 570 Ehrenfeld, T., 331 Ehrenhalt, J., 73 Ehrlich, K. B., 51 Eidell, T. L., 559 Eilam, B., 406 Eiland, M. D., 435, 452 Eisenberg, N., 93, 94, 103, 104, 105, 112–117, 119, 120, 172, 176, 346 Eisenberg, R., 322 Eisner, E. W., 668 Eissenberg, T. E., 688 Eitel, A., 350, 385, 496 Ejesi, K., 59 Ekono, M., 49 El Yaafouri, L., 283 Elbertson, N. A., 494 Elbich, D. B., 201, 204 Elder, L., 403, 404 Eley, T., 249 Elias, M. J., 566, 577, 583, 585, 591, 592 Elias, S. M., 480 Eliot, L., 71 Elkind, D., 112 Ellemers, N., 66 Ellington, A. J., 119 Elliot, A. J., 522, 524, 540, 542 Elliott, V., 633 Elzinga, N., 609 Emdin, C., 76, 79, 81, 536 Emerson, M. J., 120 Emmer, E. T., 559, 560, 563, 565, 572, 573, 574, 583, 609, 624 Endres, T., 486, 496 Engel de Abreu, P. M. J., 375 Engelhart, M. D., 619 Engle, R. W., 347 English, M. S., 490 Ennis, C. D., 153 Epilepsy Foundation, 231 Eppe, S., 253 Epstein, J. L., 628, 631 Epstein, J. N., 253 Erdelyi, M. H., 357 Eresh, J. T., 670 Erickson, F., 436 Erickson, W., 232 Ericsson, A., 237, 365, 366, 400, 402, 473 Ericsson, K. A., 400 Erikson, E. H., 159, 169 Erin’s Law, 155 Ernst, A., 136 Eron, L. D., 131 Ertmer, P. A., 436 Escudé, M., 427

N-5

Eskreis-Winkler, L., 313, 487, 488 Espelage, D. L., 72, 73, 589, 592 Espinosa, L. M., 260 Estévez, I., 630 Ethnic and Racial Identity in the 21st Century Study Group, 57 Ethnologue., 259 Ettekal, I., 150, 151, 441, 442 Evans, G. W., 51–52, 54 Evans, K. R., 587 Evans, S. W., 219, 221, 223 Everson, K. C., 694 Evertson, C. M., 560, 562, 563, 565, 572, 573, 583, 609 Extein, M., 197–199 Eylon, B. S., 421, 436 Eysenck, M. W., 340, 343, 347–351 Fabes, R. A., 65, 71, 72, 176 Fabiano, G. A., 221 Facione, P. A., 405 Fackler, S., 482 Fahle, E. M., 689 Falco, M., 228 Falk, A., 487 Fanti, K. A., 490 Fantuzzo, J., 146 Farivar, S. H., 444 Farkas, G., 115, 189, 254 Farmer, J., 579 Farmer, T. W., 150 Farmer-Dougan, V., 95, 98 Farrell, A. F., 54 Farrington, C., 488 Farrington, D. P., 588 Farver, J. M., 253, 271 Fauth, B., 665, 695 Fazio, L. K., 362, 385 Felde, M., 93, 96, 99, 101, 540 Feldman, R. S., 135 Feldman, S. S., 177 Feldon, D. F., 527 Felicia, P., 455 Fenesi, B., 347 Feng, S., 97 Fennig, C. D., 249, 258 Fenton, D. F., 391 Ferguson, C. J., 152 Ferguson, J. K., 579 Ferguson, P., 676 Ferguson, R. F., 694 Fernandes, M. A., 385 Fernandez, S. C., 257 Fernet, C., 7 Ferrer, E., 190 Ferreri, S. J., 22 Ferrero, J., 136, 137 Ferretti, R. P., 520 Ferring, D., 271 Fida, R., 480 Fien, H., 477 Figueroa, G., 152

N-6 Name Index Finkel, D., 190 Finkenauer, C., 487 Finley, T., 304 Finn, A. S., 489, 687 Finn, B., 673 Finnilä, K., 354 Fiore, S. M., 438, 442 Fiorella, L., 385, 393, 444, 487 Firetto, C. M., 445, 635, 636 Fischbach, A., 165, 166 Fischel, J. E., 253 Fischer, A., 389 Fischer, F., 392, 393, 452 Fischer, H. E., 609 Fischer, K. W., 96–100, 113, 115 Fischer, N., 515 Fishbein, B., 681 Fisher, D., 100 Fishman, B., 435 Fishman, E., 407 Fiske, S. T., 44, 612 Fitts, P. M., 365 Fitzgerald, H. E., 451 Fitzgerald, M. S., 428, 429 Fitzmaurice, H., 629 Fives, H., 651 Fivush, R., 356 Flanagan, C. A., 52, 586 Flanigan, A. E., 492 Flannery, B., 618 Flavell, E. R., 376 Flavell, J. H., 108, 252, 376 Fleer, M., 138 Fleith, D., 204 Fletcher, A., 228 Fletcher, J. M., 214, 216, 218, 254, 434 Flint, A., 611–613 Flint, K., 530 Floden, R. E., 609 Flom, R., 250 Flores, L. Y., 480 Florit, E., 253, 254 Flower, A., 310 Flowerday, T., 138, 537 Floyd-Tenery, M., 279 Flunger, B., 628, 630 Flynn, J. R., 196 Flynn, M., 628 Foerde, K., 337 Fogarty, M., 254 Foley, K., 152 Foltz, P. W., 437 Fong, C. J., 313 Fontaine, J. R. J., 516 Foo, C., 59 Foorman, B. R., 272 Forcier, M., 137 Ford, C. J., 478, 479, 489 Foster, H., 137 Fox, P. W., 440 Foy, P., 681 Francis, D. J., 214, 254, 272 Frank, J. L., 495, 496

Frank, K., 454 Franke, B., 215 Franke, M., 22, 390, 392 Frankenberg, E., 62 Frankenhuis, W. E., 53 Franklin, S., 111 Fraser, A. M., 494 Fraser, J. W., 599 Fredricks, J. A., 486, 561 Freeman, S., 97 Freiberg, H. J., 626, 635 Freiberg, J., 317 French, S., 57 Frenzel, A. C., 167, 542, 609 Frey, N., 100 Frey, W. H., 6 Fridlund, A. J., 204 Fridriksdottir, K., 596 Friedman, S. L., 13, 153 Friedman-Weieneth, J. L., 219 Friedrich, A. A., 579 Friend, M., 187, 189, 210–212, 216–219, 222, 223, 227, 230–232, 234, 238, 239, 250, 303, 316–318, 385, 642 Frith, E., 201, 204 Fromberg, D. P., 688 Fronius, T., 587, 598 Frost, E. J., 619 Fryar, C. D., 139 Fuchs, D., 391 Fuchs, L. S., 391 Fujita, K., 486 Fuligni, A. J., 58, 579 Fuller, B., 62 Fuller-Thomson, E., 146 Fulmer, S. M., 546, 579 Funke, J., 389 Furrer, C., 518 Furst, N., 609 Furtak, E. M., 431, 433 Fyfe, E. R., 428, 435 Gabrieli, C. F. O., 489 Gabrieli, J. D. E., 489 Gadson, N., 78, 79 Gage, N. A., 579 Gagné, E. D., 356, 365, 366 Gagné, M., 515, 634 Gaias, L. M., 560, 592, 599 Galindo, C., 62 Galinsky, A. D., 204 Galla, B. M., 487, 687 Gallagher, J. J., 448 Galliher, R. V., 57 Gallimore, A., 152 Gallimore, R., 118, 267 Galton, M., 442 Galvan, A., 96 Gamoran, A., 635 Gándara, P., 268 Ganis, G., 330 Ganley, C. M., 68 Ganson, K. T., 140 Garbarino, J., 151, 589

Garcia, E. E., 77, 271 Garcia, S. B., 268, 281 García Valdivia, I., 62 Gard, A. M., 52 Gardner, H., 39, 190–192, 236 Gardner, M., 687 Gardner-McCune, C., 457 Gargiulo, R. M., 219, 220, 231, 233, 234, 239–240 Gargurevich, R., 522 Garland, M., 435 Garlick, D., 194, 198 Garner, P. W., 145 Garnets, L., 72 Garrison, J., 422 Garza, A., 152 Garzón, J., 455 Gaskill, P., 24 Gaspard, H., 526, 536 Gasser, L., 578 Gathercole, S. E., 346, 375 Gatz, M., 190 Gautier, M. P., 59 Gawrilow, C., 570 Gay, G., 78, 81, 599 Gay, Lesbian and Straight Education Network (GLSEN), 73, 74 Geary, D. C., 422 Gee, G. C., 57 Gee, J. P., 451, 454 Geerling, D. M., 486 Gehlbach, H., 171 Geier, C., 96 Geier, R., 435 Geiser, C., 190 Gelman, R., 114 Gendler, T. S., 487, 491 Gengaro, F. P., 81, 529 Gentner, D., 396 Gentrup, S., 611, 613 Genzlinger, N., 62 George, P. S., 640 Gerber, J. P., 478 Gerig, T. M., 611 Germine, L., 19, 97 Gershenson, S., 64, 612, 629 Gershoff, E. T., 93, 94, 103, 104, 105, 112–117, 119, 120, 172, 313, 346 Gersten, R., 262, 271, 272 Gerwels, M. C., 563, 565, 574 Gestwicki, C., 123 Getz, S., 96 Gezer, M. U., 268 Gibson, D., 454 Gierl, M. J., 662 Gijbels, D., 345 Gil, L., 376 Gilar, R., 192 Gilbert, D. T., 338, 357 Gilbert, L., 118 Gilbert, W., 518 Gill, E., 72, 73

Gillen-O’Neel, C., 579 Gillet, J. W., 377 Gilliam, W. S., 64 Gillieron, O., 525 Gillies, R. M., 394, 439, 440, 442, 443, 444, 631 Gilligan, C., 172 Giluk, T. L., 177 Gindis, B., 117, 118, 122 Ginns, P., 394 Ginsburg, K. R., 139 Ginsburg, M., 146 Gion, C. M., 599 Girard, A., 150 Girvan, E. J., 599 Gitomer, D. H., 670 Giunta, L. Di., 490 Gladstone, J. R., 489 Gladwell, M., 400 Glass, C. R., 447 Glass, G. V., 629 Glasser, W., 583 Glăveanu, V. P., 202, 204 Glazewski, K. D., 436 Gleitman, H., 204 Glenberg, A., 361 Glenham, M., 164 Glennie, E., 677 Glock, S., 611 Gluck, M. A., 91, 94, 294, 296, 299, 330, 331, 354, 540 Gnagy, E. M., 221 Godden, D. R., 361 Godleski, S. A., 151 Goerdt, A., 578 Goetz, T., 167, 522, 541, 542, 545, 609 Goh, W., 407 Golann, J. W., 600 Goldenberg, C., 254, 267, 271 Goldhaber, D., 54, 690 Golding, J., 66 Goldin-Meadow, S., 394, 530 Goldman, S. R., 641, 672 Goldsmith, M., 357 Goldstein, L. H., 219 Goldstein, L. S., 172 Goldstone, R. L., 393, 408 Goldwater, M., 408 Goleman, D., 197–199, 494 Göllner, R., 11, 570, 695 Gollwitzer, P. M., 486 Golombok, S., 66 Gomez Zaccarelli, F., 407 Gonzales, N., 279 Gonzales, R., 279 Gonzalez, A. L., 269 González, J. M., 255 Gonzalez, N., 126, 279, 631 González, T., 586 Gonzalez, V., 189, 258 Good, T. L., 440, 497, 562, 609, 612, 613, 624, 628, 633, 634, 640, 641

Name Index Goodman, D. M., 78, 79 Goodman, G. S., 313 Goodrich, J. M., 271 Goodyear, J., 197, 239 Gordon, K. A., 482 Gordon, R. A., 148 Gorman, J. L., 81, 529 Gorrel, S., 140 Gorski, P. C., 48, 51, 53, 55, 489 Gosseries, O., 345 Goswami, U., 99, 101, 216 Gottlieb, G., 89 Gottwald, S., 100 Goupil, L., 376 Goyer, J. P., 687 Grabow, K., 442 Gradwell, J. M., 615 Graesser, A. C., 352, 364, 385, 429, 430, 437, 442, 451, 454, 540, 633 Graham, C. R., 4, 460, 461, 567 Graham, L. J., 15 Graham, M., 477 Graham, S., 46, 62, 63, 481, 509, 528, 529, 531, 588–592, 640 Granger, K. L., 7 Granja, M. R., 54 Grant, B. O., 58 Grant, S. G., 615 Grasman, R. P. P. P., 190 Grassinger, R., 528 Graves, A., 268, 278 Gray, D. L. L., 61, 518, 525, 550, 579 Gray, P., 341, 342 Gredler, M. E., 117, 119, 120, 122–124, 376 Green, F. L., 376 Green, M., 103 Green, S., 591, 592 Greenberg, M. T., 9, 496 Greene, J. A., 387, 445, 453, 486, 490, 496, 635, 636 Greene, M. B., 591, 592 Greene, M. J., 487 Greeno, J. G., 424 Greenspan, R. L., 354 Greenwald, A. G., 44, 63 Greenwood, E. J., 544 Greer, D., 562 Gregory, A., 13, 15, 221, 515, 598 Greiff, S., 389, 392, 393, 437, 442 Gresalfi, M., 436 Gresham, F. M., 675 Grewal, D., 199 Grieshaber, S., 137 Griffin, D., 398 Griffith, J., 494 Griffiths, S., 140 Grigg, S., 480 Grigorenko, E. L., 197, 202, 203, 216, 218, 239, 434 Grimaldi, P. J., 364

Gripshover, S. J., 530 Gronlund, N. E., 660, 663, 668 Gröschner, A., 609 Gross, J. J., 487, 489, 491, 530 Gross-Loh, C., 533 Grossman, P., 436 Grossnickle, E. M., 536 Grouws, D., 624 Grover, S., 456 Grower, P., 66, 68 Grütter, J., 578 Guardabassi, V., 543 Guay, F., 7, 518 Gubbins, E. J., 282 Guckenburg, S., 587, 598 Gudmundsdottir, G. B., 480 Guglielmi, R. S., 271 Gullotta, T. P., 493 Gumus, B., 522 Gunderson, E. A., 68, 70, 530, 531, 532 Gunn, D., 216 Gunn, K. C. M., 234 Guo, D., 344 Guo, J., 59, 166, 477, 526 Guo, J.-P., 393 Guo, Q., 662 Gupta, N., 73 Gupta, V., 65, 73 Gurian, M., 70, 71 Guryan, J., 53 Guskey, T. R., 670, 673–675, 677, 679, 689 Gustin, W. C., 23 Gutiérrez, K. D., 425, 436 Gwen Marchand, G., 24 Ha, H., 378 Hacker, D. J., 413 Haderlein, S. K., 435 Hadwin, A. F., 441, 490 Haerens, L., 516 Haertel, E. H., 656, 671, 689 Hafen, C. A., 13, 15, 515, 598 Hafez, D. A., 384 Hagborg, W. J., 168 Hagemans, M. G., 383, 384 Haggard, P., 486 Hahn, C.-S, 197 Haidt, J., 171, 175 Hailey, E. P., 236 Haimovitz, K., 532 Hajovsky, D. B., 197 Hakuta, K., 8, 271 Haladyna, T. H., 674, 690 Hale, N. M., 162 Hale, S., 382 Hales, C. M., 139 Haley, M. H., 280 Halim, M. L., 66 Hall, E., 207 Hall, N. C., 167, 542 Hallahan, D. P., 6, 216–220, 222, 229, 230, 232–234, 376, 386, 642

Halpern, D. F., 71, 196, 197 Halvorsen, A. L., 435 Hamann, D. L., 609 Hamdan, N., 531 Hamers, J. F., 258 Hamilton, J., 338 Hamilton, R., 282 Hamlett, C. L., 391 Hamm, J., 529 Hamman-Ortiz, L., 274 Hammer, C. S., 254, 255 Hampson, S. E., 137 Hamre, B. K., 11–13, 482, 515 Han, J., 136 Han, Y., 136 Hanafin, J., 628 Hand, V., 436 Hanish, L. D., 65, 71, 72 Hanna, M., 268 Hannafin, M. J., 427, 432, 433, 547 Hannah-Jones, N., 62 Hanselman, P., 530 Hanson, M. H., 202 Hanson-Peterson, J., 495 Hanushek, E. A., 11 Hapgood, S., 431 Happe, F., 249 Harackiewicz, J. M., 67, 522–524, 536, 548 Harber, K. D., 81, 529, 612 Hardaway, T., 618 Hardin, C. J., 314 Hardman, M., 187, 219 Hardy, I., 257, 665 Hargreaves, J., 228 Hargreaves, L., 442 Harklau, L., 269 Harms, M. B., 171 Harms, P. D., 489 Harmsen, R., 431 Harp, S. F., 537 Harper, K., 223 Harrington, H. J., 443 Harris, A. H., 433 Harris, J. R., 148 Harris, K. A., 375, 379 Harris, R. J., 274 Harrison, J., 496 Harrison, J. R., 223 Harrison, S., 338 Hart, S. A., 543 Hart, S. C., 9 Harter, S., 137, 158, 165, 166, 478 Hartley, B. L., 46, 70 Hartman, K., 152 Hartshore, J. K., 252 Hartwig, M. K., 362 Harvey, E. A., 219 Hasan, A., 197 Hasselhorn, M., 165 Hatch, T., 192 Hatfield, D., 454

N-7

Hatlevik, O. E., 480 Hattie, J., 513, 518, 579 Hattie, J. A. C., 634, 673 Haugen, C., 274 Haugh, G. P., 397 Hay, P., 140 Hayenga, A. O., 511 Hayes, A. R., 70 Hayes, S. C., 318 Headden, S., 530, 532 Healey, K., 550 Healy, N., 567 Hearn, S., 164 Heath, S. B., 252 Hecht, C. A., 548 Hecht, S. A., 332 Heistad, D., 50, 54 Hellwege, M. A., 480 Helms, J. E., 58, 59 Helwig, C. C., 173 Hemberger, L., 408 Hembrooke, H., 354 Henderikx, M., 530 Henderson, M. D., 498 Hendrick, B. D., 635, 636 Henik, A., 217 Henington, C., 317 Hennessey, B., 202 Hennessey, B. A., 323 Hennessey, M. N., 422, 427, 635 Hennessy, S., 425 Henrich, C. C., 148 Henriksen, D., 201, 202, 205 Henriksen, T. B., 136 Henry, B., 68 Hensley, L., 609 Henson, R. A., 536 Herald, S. L., 150 Herbers, J. E., 50, 54 Herbert, E. A., 570 Herman, J., 670 Herman, K. C., 482, 590 Herman, M., 58 Hermkes, R., 121 Heron, T. E., 291, 297, 299, 302, 303 Herrell, A. L., 260 Herrenkohl, L. R., 445 Herrera, D., 522 Herrero, E. C., 256 Herrmann, Z., 436 Herron, A., 70 Herschkowitz, N., 143 Hertel, S., 665 Herzig, P., 22, 390, 392 Hess, K., 620 Hesse, F. W., 437 Hetherington, E. M., 146 Heverly-Fitt, S., 148, 149 Heward, W. L., 291, 297, 299, 303 Hiatt, R., 136 Hickey, D. T., 421 Hidi, S. E., 486, 535, 538, 548

N-8 Name Index Hieb, J. L., 385 Hier, B. O., 521 Higgs, J., 425 Higley, K., 422, 427 Hilgard, E. R., 18 Hill, E. L., 135 Hill, H. C., 610 Hill, W. H., 619 Hillemeier, M. M., 115, 189 Hilton, C., 408 Hinckley, D., 151 Hines, C. V., 609 Hines, M., 66 Hinesley, V., 331 Hinojosa, C. P., 530 Hinton, C., 93, 97, 101 Hinz, E., 50, 54 Hiroto, D. S., 528 Hirsch, E. D. Jr., 405 Hirvikoski, T., 221 Hlutkowsky, C. O., 375, 376, 378, 379 Hmelo-Silver, C. E., 433, 435, 452, 453 Hochbein, C., 677 Hochweber, J., 665, 673 Hodis, F. A., 513 Hoepfner, D., 435 Hoffman, H. G., 63 Hoffman, J. A., 234 Hoffman, M. L., 176 Hoffmann, J. D., 8, 9 Hofkens, T. L., 11, 535, 578 Hogan, T., 610 Högden, F., 295 Hoge, R. D., 203, 204 Hogeveen, J., 299 Holden, G. W., 313 Holland, A. A., 590 Holland, J. D., 440 Holland, R. W., 63 Hollis, R. B., 382 Holmes, J. D., 97 Holmström, A., 221 Holt, S. B., 64, 629 Holzberger, D., 482 Homner, L., 456 Hondrich, A. L., 665 Hong, G., 675, 676 Hong, J. S., 588, 589 Hong, J. Y., 559 Honicke, T., 480 Honomichl, R., 396 Hoogerheide, V., 393, 410, 444 Hoogman, M., 215 hooks, b., 76 Hooper, S. Y., 516, 518, 548 Hoover, S., 276 Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., 630 Hope, E. C., 61, 550, 579 Hopkins, E. J., 138 Hopkins, M., 8 Hopkins, R. F., 385 Hopps, J., 47

Horn, J. L., 190 Horn, S. S., 73 Hornstra, L., 63, 542 Horovitz, B., 295 Horowitz, J. M., 48, 57, 63 Horowitz-Johnson, Z., 591 Horst, E. A., 164 Hoselton, R., 315 Houdé, O., 115 Houts, R., 12, 13 Houtte, M. V., 611 Howard, J., 138 Howard, J. L., 515, 518 Howard-Jones, P. A., 99, 101 Howe, C., 425 Howe, K. R., 8 Hoy, K. L., 567, 569 Hoy, W. K., 27, 28, 482, 559 Hsieh, P. P., 528 Hsu, J. Y. L., 442 Hu, A., 435 Hu, F.-T., 394 Huang, C., 524 Huang, F. L., 482, 598 Huang, X., 479 Huber, C. R., 403–405 Hübner, N., 673 Hubscher, R., 124 Hudley, C., 52 Huesmann, L. R., 151 Huettig, F., 394 Hughes, C., 691 Hughes, D., 579 Hughes, E. M., 19 Hughes, J. N., 578, 677 Huguet, P., 46 Huguley, J. P., 59 Hui, L., 486 Huitsing, G., 588 Huizenga, H. M., 190 Hulleman, C. S., 67, 509, 522, 523, 526, 529–531 Hulme, C., 216 Hung, D. W. L., 436 Hung, W., 433, 434 Hunnikin, L. M., 151 Hunt, J. McV., 123, 125 Hunt, N., 232, 233 Hunt, S. K., 609 Hurley, N., 587, 598 Hurry, J., 22 Husbye, N. E., 451 Hushman, C., 123 Hussain, M., 586 Hussar, W., 61 Hutchinson, J. M., 319 Hutchinson, N. L., 220 Hutt, S., 687 Hwang, G.-J., 455 Hwang, N., 582 Hyde, J. S., 65, 67, 71, 172, 197, 548 Hyde, L. W., 52 Hyder, N., 586

Hymel, S., 153, 154, 588, 592 Iachan, R., 530 Ialongo, N. S., 310 Icenogle, G., 490 Igielnik, R., 48 Iglesias, A., 257 Ihori, N., 151 Ilioska, I., 215 Ilten-Gee, R., 173 Imai-Matsumura, K., 482 Immordino-Yang, M. H., 198, 493, 540 Inbar, J., 613 Ingram, J., 633 Inhelder, B., 111 Inns, A. I., 442, 445 Institute of Education Sciences (IES), 398 Iordanou, K., 407, 408 Ireland, G. W., 480 Irewole-Ojo, F. O., 136 Irvine, J. J., 81, 599 Isaacson, W., 350 Ispa-Landa, S., 598 Ito, M., 424 Ivcevic, Z., 542 Iverson, H., 431, 433 Izard, C. E., 50 Jabbar, A. I. A., 455 Jackson, A., 427 Jackson, L. A., 451 Jackson, Y., 313 Jacobs, A., 159 Jacobs, J. E., 167 Jacobson, L., 611 Jacobson, M. J., 394, 408 Jadallah, M., 442 Jaeger, A. J., 380, 396 Jaffee, S., 172 Jagers, R. J., 494, 498 Jain, S., 543, 544 James, C., 459 James, W., 18, 72, 168, 475, 489 Jamieson, J. P., 540, 544 Jamil, F. M., 482 Jang, H., 515, 548 Janika, A., 137 Janosz, M., 25 Janssen, J., 440 Jarrett, R., 145 Jarrold, C., 216 Järvelä, S., 441, 490 Jarvin, L., 197, 239 Jaswal, V. K., 250 Jaycox, L. H., 10 Jellesma, F. C., 11, 562 Jennings, P. A., 224, 495, 496 Jensen, E., 51, 52, 54, 55 Jensen, J. L., 672 Jensen, L. A., 177 Jenson, W. R., 310 Jeong, H., 452 Jeong, S., 527 Jerome, E. M., 13

Jerrim, J., 433, 434 Jerusalem, M., 543 Jewell, J. A., 67 Jha, A., 496 Ji, Y., 97 Jia, Y., 579 Jiang, Y., 49, 199, 200, 493, 527 Jimerson, S. R., 676 Jirout, J. J., 536, 539 Jitendra, A. K., 22, 390, 392 Jo, K., 452 Job, V., 486 Jobson, L., 354 Joel, D., 65, 71, 197 Joët, G., 480 Johnson, A., 221 Johnson, A. H., 225 Johnson, D. W., 440, 441, 442, 447, 448, 488, 521, 668–670 Johnson, J. F., 59 Johnson, P., 427 Johnson, R. C., 62, 63, 447 Johnson, R. T., 440, 441, 442, 447, 448, 521, 668–670 Johnson, S., 52, 300 Johnson, S. M., 691 John-Steiner, V., 117 Johnston, O., 611 Jolly, J. L., 237 Jones, B. D., 169 Jones, K. P., 630 Jones, L., 576, 577, 580, 583, 594, 596, 597 Jones, M., 52 Jones, M. G., 611 Jones, M. H., 524 Jones, M. L., 57 Jones, S. H., 433 Jones, S. M., 9, 15, 68, 151, 493, 494, 590 Jones, V., 576, 577, 580, 583, 594, 596, 597 Jonsdottir, I. J., 596 Jonsson, A., 491, 492, 500 Jordan, A., 610 Jordan, M., 260 Jörg, V., 495 Judd, T., 338 Jun, S., 257 Jung, H., 197 Jung, L. A., 675 Jurbergs, N., 309 Jussim, L., 611, 612, 614 Justice, L. V., 354 Juth, S., 527 Juvonen, J., 525, 590, 592, 677 K–12 Computer Science Teachers Association, 456 Kachanoff, F. J., 518 Kackar-Cam, H. Z., 546, 578 Kaendler, C., 443 Kagan, J., 143 Kagan, S., 443–446 Kahn, E., 72, 73

Name Index Kahn, J., 9, 493, 494 Kahneman, D., 38, 63 Kain, J. F., 11 Kain, J. J., 11 Kaiser, J., 238 Kalenkoski, C. M., 628 Kalinowski, A. G., 23 Kalogrides, D., 689 Kalyuga, S., 389, 391–393, 398, 434 Kamp, E. T., 431 Kana’iaupuni, S. M., 77 Kanazawa, S., 189, 190 Kane, M. J., 201, 204 Kang, H., 528 Kang, S. H. K., 364, 513 Kantamneni, N., 480 Kapinus, B. A., 618 Kaplan, A., 485, 522–524, 527 Kaplan, J. S., 312 Kaplan, S. N., 236 Kapur, M., 408 Karp, K. S., 391 Karpicke, J. D., 384 Karpicke, J. J., 364 Karson, A., 579 Kaschak, M., 253, 254 Katartzi, E., 511, 516 Katz, I., 518, 548 Katz, S. R., 153 Katzir, T., 98 Kauffman, J. M., 6, 216–220, 222, 229, 230, 232–234, 311, 314, 321, 376, 386, 642 Kaufman, J., 202 Kaufman, T. M., 588 Kauser, R., 145 Kavanaugh, S. S., 436, 635 Kazdin, A. E., 296, 302, 304, 308, 312–313 Keane, J. N., 54 Keating, D., 13, 153 Keavney, M., 322 Keefe, J. W., 207 Kehle, T. J., 310 Kellam, S. G., 310 Keller, M. M., 167, 545, 609 Keller, U., 166, 165 Kelly, D. J., 45 Kelly, D. L., 681 Kelly, D. R., 487, 488 Kelly, J. B., 146 Kelly, M. L., 309 Kelly, S., 633 Kelly, Y., 136 Keltner, D., 52 Kemp, C., 210 Kempert, S., 257 Kennedy, J. J., 609 Kennedy, P., 477 Keogh, R., 342 Kern, M. L., 518, 579 Kerr, M., 137 Kersting, N., 531

Keyes, T. S., 488 Khanem, F., 135 Kiang, L., 59 Kicken, W., 20 Kiesel, A., 338 Kiewra, K. A., 429 Kilcher, A., 433 Killewald, A., 49 Killingsworth, M. A., 339 Killingsworth, S. S., 456 Kim, C.-M., 427, 432, 433, 547 Kim, E. J., 515 Kim, H., 677 Kim, H. E., 61, 62 Kim, J., 435, 515 Kim, J. A., 347 Kim, J. S., 53 Kim, L. E., 482 Kim, M., 515 Kim, N. J., 451 Kim, P., 51–52 Kim, S. il, 527 Kim, S.-I., 536 Kim, S.-Y., 118 Kim, Y., 61, 668 Kim, Y.-S., 253, 254 Kinach, B. M., 434 Kincade, L., 578 Kindsay, J. J., 629 King, A., 444, 446 King, E. M., 455 King, R. B., 524, 531 Kinnell, A., 197 Kipperman, K. L., 219, 221 Kirk, S. A., 448 Kirschner, P. A., 20, 428, 434, 451, 626 Kirshner, B., 424 Kitil, M. J., 495 Kittleman, A., 315, 317 Kizzie, K. T., 59, 61 Klahr, D., 434, 435 Klass, P., 250 Klassen, R. M., 7, 477, 480, 482, 495, 542 Klauda, S. L., 526 Klee, H. L., 545, 549 Kleickmann, T., 536 Klein, D., 70, 257 Klein, H. J., 522 Klein, M., 215 Klein, S., 70, 257 Klein, S. B., 291, 294, 298 Klein, S. S., 433 Kleinfeld, J., 70 Klesse, L. J., 590 Klieme, E., 665 Klinger, J., 213 Klopack, E. T., 137 Klusmann, U., 11, 610, 695 Knapp, M. S., 54 Knifsend, C., 525 Knight, G. P., 57 Knightly, L. M., 257

Knoppick, H., 640 Knotek, S. E., 227 Knowlton, B. J., 337 Kobayashi, K., 382 Koch, I., 338 Kochenderfer-Ladd, B., 441, 442, 591 Kochhar, R., 48 Koedinger, K., 352, 364, 385, 429, 430, 633 Koedinger, K. R., 22, 390, 392 Koenig, E. A., 521 Koenig, M. A., 171 Koenig, O., 350 Koenka, A. C., 485 Koeppen-Schomerus, G., 249 Koestner, R., 322, 518 Kogachi, K., 62 Kogan, I., 611, 613 Kogan, M. D., 6 Kohlberg, L., 171 Kohn, A., 314, 322, 323, 488–489, 674 Koike, R., 482 Kokko, K., 150 Kollar, I., 452 Kollar, M., 49 Köller, O., 167, 169 Konstantopoulos, S., 435 Koomen, H. M. K., 562 Koomen, H. M. Y., 11, 482, 562, 580 Koopman, R., 164 Koorn, P., 530 Korf, R., 395 Koriat, A., 357 Korman, H. T. N., 54 Korn, Z., 318 Körndle, H., 423 Kornell, N., 382 Korte, G., 8 Kosciw, J. G., 73 Koser, L., 22 Koslouski, J. B., 10, 224, 226 Kosse, F., 487 Kosslyn, S. M., 330, 350, 351 Kouider, S., 376 Kounin, J. S., 475, 575 Koury, A. S., 269 Kovas, Y., 489, 543 Kovelman, I., 258, 259 Kozulin, A., 117, 118, 122 Kraft, M. A., 489 Krajcik, J., 435 Kratchovil, C. J., 220 Krathwohl, D. R., 619, 620 Kratzig, G. P., 207 Kraus, M. W., 49, 52 Krauss, S., 610 Kreider, H., 254, 278 Kretschmann, J., 19 Kreuzberger, C., 152 Kristen, C., 611, 613 Kroger, J., 162, 163

N-9

Kronberger, N., 46 Krone, C. R., 198, 493, 540 Kronholz, J., 236, 237 Krueger, J. L., 169 Kruger, A. C., 124 Kudyn, M., 531 Kuhfeld, M., 7 Kuhlmann, S., 385 Kuhn, D., 111, 407, 408, 439, 440, 626 Kuhn, L., 559 Kuklinski, M. R., 611, 613 Kulik, C. C., 640 Kulik, J. A., 640 Kulikowich, J. M., 358 Kummer, T. A., 672 Kuncel, N. R., 50, 403–405 Kunter, M., 482, 610, 630, 665 Kupzyk, S., 301 Kurlakowsky, K. D., 162 Kush, J. M., 592, 598 Kuyper, H., 166 Kyndt, E., 442 Kyungmin, K., 612 LaBrot, Z. C., 301 Lachner, A., 393, 444 Ladd, G. W., 150, 151, 441, 442 Ladson-Billings, G., 61, 62, 63, 76, 81, 82 Ladwig, J., 153 Lahat, A., 173 Lai, C. K., 44 Lai, P., 408 Laibson, D., 496 Lake, V. E., 172 Lamb, M. E., 145 Lambert, R., 62, 63 Lambert, S. F., 162 Landrum, T. J., 311, 314, 321 Lane, D., 177 Langan-Fox, J., 429 Langer, J. A., 635 Lange-Schubert, K., 536 Langley, R., 379 Langley, S. D., 282 Lansford, J. E., 490 Lanza, S., 167 LaParo, K. M., 13 LaPier, 252 LaQuea, R., 152 LaRocque, J., 345 Larsen, R., 460, 482, 578 Larson, K. E., 598 LaRusso, M. D., 15 Larzelere, R. E., 144 Latawiec, B., 442 Latham, G. P., 306, 318, 520, 522 Lather, P., 25 Lauermann, F., 526 Lauver, K., 379 Lave, J., 409, 421, 424, 436 Lawler, M. J., 10 Lawless, K., 641 Lawlor, J., 439

N-10 Name Index Lawrence, F. R., 255 Lawson, H. A., 518 Lawson, M. A., 518 Lazarides, R., 482, 536 Lazonder, A. W., 431 Lazowski, R. A., 509 Leaf, P. J., 318 Leaper, C., 65, 66, 67, 264 Leary, M. R., 518 Lease, M. A., 149 Lebuda, I., 202 Ledward, B., 77 Lee, C., 232 Lee, C. D., 330, 354, 672 Lee, G., 487 Lee, H. S., 378, 394 Lee, J., 3, 27, 29, 44, 56, 70, 537 Lee, J. Y. K., 46 Lee, K., 45, 390, 578 Lee, L. H. J., 616 Lee, M., 524 Lee, M. Y., 434 Lee, R. M., 56 Lee, S., 45 Lee, S. J., 44, 259 Lee, S. Y., 479 Lee, W., 546 Lee, Y. K., 443, 447 LeFevre, J. A., 119, 253 Leff, S., 591 Lefler, M., 451 Leh J. M., 390 Lehmann, R., 640 Lehr, C. A., 580 Lei, P.-W., 9 Leigh, E., 216 Leinhardt, G., 626 LeMahieu, P., 670 Leman, P. J., 408, 443, 444 Lemelson, R., 39 Lemery-Chalfant, K., 493 Lemmer, G., 531 Lenhart, A., 451, 455 Lens, W., 522 Lent, R. W., 480 Leonard, J. A., 54 Leong, D. J., 122 Lepper, M. R., 322 Leppink, J., 343 Lerkkanen, M. K., 427 Lerner, M. D., 138 Lerner, R. M., 143 Lesaux, N., 272 Lester, N. L., 587 Leung, A. K.-Y., 204 Leung, M. C., 150 Levesque, C., 268 Levin, J. R., 363, 364, 583 Levine, S. C., 68, 70, 530, 532 Levitin, D. J., 337 Levitt, P., 52 Levontin, L., 524 Levy, I., 536 Levy, S., 628

Lewedeg, V., 257 Lewis, C., 145 Lewis, W. E., 520 Lewis-Smith, H., 139–141 Lewno-Dumdie, B., 197 Lew-Williams, C., 257 Lhamon, C. E., 65, 73 Li, C. R., 24, 479 Li, D., 406 Li, H., 189 Li, J.-B., 487 Li, M., 445, 635, 636 Li, T., 215 Li, W., 435 Li, Y., 9, 530 Liben, L. S., 66, 70 Lichtenfeld, S., 541 Lickliter, R., 89 Lidz, J. L., 250 Liebtag, E., 274 Lieven, E., 250 Lillard, A. S., 138 Lillemyr, O. F., 138 Lim, S. W., 674, 675 Limber, S. P., 591 Lin, C.-H., 452, 492 Lin, L., 530 Lin, T. J., 442 Lin, T.-Z., 428 Linan-Thompson, S., 272 Lindsay, P. H., 334 Lindstrom Johnson, S., 560, 592, 599 Linford, B. J., 522 Ling, D. S., 490, 496 Ling, G., 579 Linn, M. C., 421, 436 Linnenbrink, E. A., 524 Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., 236, 524, 525, 527, 535, 541 Linquanti, R., 8 Lipnevich, A. A., 545 Lisciandrello, J., 567 Lisette, W., 432, 435 Lismont, B., 442 Liszkowski, U., 138 Litalien, D., 518 Litke, E., 694 Little, S., 586 Liu, J., 7, 136 Liu, Q., 393 Liu, S., 62 Liu, W. M., 47 Liu, Y., 97, 435 Livingstone, S., 424 Lobczowski, N. G., 445 LoBue, V., 151 Lochman, J. E., 499 Locke, E. A., 306, 318, 470, 520, 522 Lockhart, R. S., 345 Lockl, K., 376 Loewenstein, J., 396 Loftus, E. F., 354

Logan, J. A. R., 543 Logel, C., 45, 46, 58, 59 Loi, M., 480 Lombardi, D., 429 London, K., 354 London, R., 138–139 Lonergan, A., 140 Long, A., 599 Long, J. D., 50, 54 Long, J. F., 542 Lonigan, C. J., 214, 253, 254, 271 López, I., 49 Lopez, M. H., 43, 57 Lorch, E. P., 221, 380 Lorch, R. F., 380 Lorde, A., 42 Lorenz, G., 611, 613 Loschky, L. C., 274 Losel, F., 588 Losey, K. M., 269 Lotan, R. A., 439 Lou, X., 136 Loughran, J., 610 Lount, R. B. Jr., 522 Love, B. L., 76, 77, 79, 489 Loveless, T., 618, 630 Lovelett, J., 362 Lovison, V. S., 63 Lowenstein, A. E., 11 Loyens, S. M. M., 410, 432, 433, 435 Lu, L., 482 Lubart, T., 202 Lubienski, S. T., 68 Lubinski, D., 17, 240 Lucariello, L. M., 331 Luckner, A. E., 13 Lüdtke, O., 11, 19, 167–169, 542, 545, 628–630, 695 Luftenegger, M., 524 Lui, A. M., 610 Luke, N., 155 Lun, J., 13 Luna, B., 96 Lundström, U., 139 Lunsford, L. B., 691 Lupkowski-Shoplik, A., 19 Lyle, K. B., 385 Lyons, M. D., 580 Lysenko, L., 25 Ma, S., 442 Ma, T. L., 487 Ma, Y., 460 Maaz, K., 640 Mabry, L., 668 Mac, J., 44 MacCann, C., 199, 200, 493 Macdonald, K., 19, 97 MacDonald, S., 480 Mace, F. C., 319 Macedonia, M., 350 Mach, H., 121 Machado, A., 105 Macionis, J. J., 47–48, 56

Mack, T. R., 390 MacLeod, J., 460 MacMillan, D. L., 675 MacRae, A., 587 Maczuga, S., 115, 189, 254 Madaus, G. F., 689 Madden, A., 380 Maddux, W. W., 204 Madison, S. M., 613 Madsen, C. H., 22 Madsen, K., 609 Maeda, Y., 410 Maehler, C., 216 Maehr, M. L., 485 Maenner, M. J., 6 Mäeots, M., 431 Maggin, D. M., 496 Magnan, A., 452 Magnuson, K., 51 Magnusson, S. J., 431, 432 Mahieu, P., 611 Mahlberga, N., 354 Mahn, H., 117 Mahon, J., 301 Mai, J., 136 Maier, M. A., 540 Mainhard, T., 542 Makel, M. C., 445 Makransky, G., 455 Malanchuk, O., 70 Malby, R. W., 456, 457 Malecki, C. K., 609 Malin, J., 529 Malin, J. L., 236 Malinen, O. P., 482 Malmberg, L. E., 482 Malone, N., 77 Malone, P. S., 490 Maloney, E. A., 543 Maloy, R. W., 460, 461 Mañá, A., 376 Manfra, M. M., 25 Mangan, D., 693 Mann, F. B., 61 Manning, B. H., 498, 499 Manoli, C. C., 431 Mantilla, A., 137 Mao, Y., 119 Mara, K. R., 527 Marchand, A. D., 51, 70 Marcia, J. E., 162, 165 Marcum, M., 428 Marder, K., 138 Marecek, J., 78, 79 Marevic, I., 346 Marinova-Todd, S., 257 Markauskaite, L., 408 Markman, E. M., 250 Markowitz, A. J., 8 Marks, A., 59 Marks, A. K., 165 Markstrom, C., 57 Marley, S. C., 123, 426 Marois, R., 338

Name Index Marraccini, M. E., 227 Marrin, S., 391 Marsh, E. J., 362, 385 Marsh, H. W., 165–169, 477, 526, 530, 541, 542 Marsh, J. A., 618 Marshall, D., 257 Marshall, H. H., 422, 425 Marshall, K., 232, 233 Marshall, M., 233 Martella, A. M., 435 Martin, A. J., 165–167 Martin, C. L., 65, 66, 71, 72 Martin, F., 457 Martin, G. N., 457 Martin, J., 421, 422 Martin, M. O., 681 Martin, R., 165, 166, 190, 271, 272 Martin, R. E., 489 Martinez, F. D., 52 Martinez, S., 557, 563, 580, 582 Martinez-Fuentes, S., 59 Martinussen, M., 162 Martiny, S. E., 478 Marvin, K. L., 311 Marx, R., 435 Marzano, J. S., 559 Marzano, R. J., 559 Mascolo, M. F., 113 Masia, B. B., 620 Masiello, M., 591 Masland, L. C., 149 Maslow, A. H., 513, 539 Mason, D. A., 641 Mason, L., 422, 424 Masten, A. S., 50, 54 Master, A., 67, 530 Mastergeorge, A. M., 444 Mastropieri, M. A., 210, 213, 214 Mathias, B., 350 Mathis, E., 9 Matjasko, J. L., 54 Matos, F., 408 Matos, L., 522 Matson, J. L., 234 Matson, M. L., 234 Matsumura, L. C., 566 Matthews, G., 497, 543 Matthews, J. S., 59, 61, 526 Matthews, M. D., 487, 488 Matthews, S., 344 Mattison, R., 189 Maupin, A. N., 64 Mavilidi, M. F., 344 May, S., 271 Mayer, J., 199 Mayer, R. E., 291, 330, 344, 360, 404, 422, 429, 444, 453, 455, 479, 492, 496, 537 McAllister, P. A., 609 McArdle, J. J., 190 McCafferty, S. G., 120 McCain, M. R. C., 480

McCandliss, B. D., 97 McCarthy, M. R., 317 McCaslin, M., 421, 427, 440, 442, 497, 500, 562 McClain, S., 52 McClelland, M. M., 490 McClintic-Gilbert, M. S., 511 McCoach, D. B., 282 McCormick, M. P., 153 McCrudden, M. T., 24 McDaniel, M., 19, 207, 352, 364, 385, 429, 430, 633 McDaniel, M. A., 101, 208, 330, 345, 359, 361, 362, 380, 496, 672 McDermott, K. B., 358 McDevitt, T. M., 95, 96 McDonald, C. L., 148, 149 McDonald, K. L., 148, 149 McDonald, L., 611–613 McDougall, P., 150 McEwen, B. S., 52 McGarrah, M. W., 9, 493 McGill-Frazen, A., 53 McGoey, K. E., 303 McGovern, L., 380 Mcgrath, L. M., 19, 97 McHugh, J. R., 25 McIlveen, P., 480 McInerney, D. M., 524 McIntosh, K., 315599 McIntosh, P., 58, 78, 79 McKay, S., 530, 532 McKellar, S. E., 70 McKenna, J., 676 McKenna, J. W., 310 McKinley, J. C., 100 McKinsey, P., 70, 257 McLean, K. C., 57 McLean, L., 7 McLeod, S. A., 207 McLeskey, J., 222, 223 McLoughlin, G., 543 McMillan, J. H., 659, 671, 677 McNamara, D. S., 454 McNeil, N. M., 428 McNurlen, B., 118 McQuillin, S. D., 580 McTighe, J., 618, 638 McTigue, E., 344 Meade, M. E., 385 Medina, M., 57 Mediratta, K., 15, 598 Meece, J. L., 168, 306, 318, 473, 520, 523, 524, 537, 543 Meens, D. E., 8 Mehta, P. D., 272 Meichenbaum, D., 498 Meijer, A. M., 543 Meissel, K., 611–613 Meister, C., 125 Meister, D. G., 15 Mejia-Arauz, R., 254 Melby-Lervåg, M., 216

Melnick, S. A., 15 Melrose, S., 429 Meltzoff, A. N., 67 Mendle, J., 136, 137 Mendoza, E., 425 Mendoza, K., 459 Menon, V., 216 Menzer, M. M., 148, 149 Mercado, E., 91, 94, 294, 296, 299, 330, 331, 354, 540 Mercer, L. K., 376, 497, 500 Mercer, N., 117, 119, 425 Meredith, M. D., 139 Mereish, E. H., 73 Merritt, A., 152 Merritt, J., 434 Mertler, C. A., 27, 671 Merton, R. K., 611 Messano, F., 154 Messina, J. P., 171, 172 Mestre, J. P., 390 MET Project, 694 Metcalfe, J., 352, 364, 385, 429, 430, 587, 633 Metz, K., 254 Metzler, C. W., 318 Meyer, D. K., 547, 562, 578 Meyering, E. E., 345 Mhatre, P., 530 Miccio, A. W., 255 Michalchik, V., 424 Michalowski, J., 579 Michou, A., 522 Mickelson, R. A., 62, 63 Middleton, M. J., 485, 523, 524 Midgley, C., 177, 485, 523 Midgley, L., 22 Miele, D. B., 489, 508, 530 Migliacci, N., 261 Mihaly, K., 694 Mikami, A. Y., 13, 15, 515, 598 Mikkers, J., 433 Milich, R., 221 Milkman, K. L., 496 Miller, A., 524 Miller, A. D., 545, 549 Miller, B., 442, 635 Miller, F. G., 599 Miller, G. A., 339, 340 Miller, L., 100 Miller, M., 441, 490 Miller, N., 443 Miller, P. H., 89, 90, 103, 104, 108, 113–115, 118, 124, 144, 158, 159, 162, 164, 252, 422 Miller, R. B., 306 Miller, S., 152 Miller, S. A., 108, 252, 339 Miller, S. M., 117, 118, 122 Miller, S. P., 429 Miller-Cotto, D., 357 Miller-Perrin, C., 313 Milner, H. R. IV., 51, 52, 55, 57, 58, 61, 76, 153

N-11

Min, E., 455 Minbashian, A., 199, 200, 493 Minch, D., 579 Minnameier, G., 121 Miranda, T. Z., 261, 271 Mirpuri, S., 64 Mischel, W., 487 Mishra, P., 201, 202, 205, 607 Mislevy, R. J., 454 Mistry, R. S., 49, 51, 52 Mitchell, C., 52, 268 Mitchell, M., 536 Mitchison, D., 140 Miyake, A., 120 Miyamoto, K., 93, 97, 100, 101 Mo, L., 396 Moè, A., 518, 609 Moeller, J., 542 Moeller, K., 570 Moise-Titus, J., 151 Mok, K., 70, 257 Moll, H., 138 Moll, L. C., 117, 126, 279, 631 Moller, A. C., 515 Moller, J., 167, 238 Moller, K., 536 Molnár, G., 389 Mond, J., 140 Monk, C. S., 52 Monk, J. S., 207 Monopoli, W. J., 591 Monroe, C. R., 599 Monsaas, J. A., 23 Montalbano, C., 635, 636 Montroy, J. J., 490 Montrul, S., 258 Montuori, A., 202 Moon, T. R., 236 Moore, D. W., 234 Moore, J., 301 Moos, D. C., 492 Mootoo, C., 64 Mora, L., 43, 5777 Morehead, J., 530 Morehead, K., 380, 382 Morell, M., 489 Morelli, G., 114 Morgan, H., 78 Morgan, K. L., 435 Morgan, P. L., 115 Morgenroth, T., 478 Morger, V., 609 Mor-Hagani, S., 413 Morin, A., 585 Morin, A. J. S., 166, 169, 518 Morin, H. K., 592 Morin, R., 57 Morin, V. A., 429 Morine-Dershimer, G., 615 Morisano, D., 520 Morris, D. B., 482 Morris, E. W., 63 Morris, J., 442 Morris, P. A., 143

N-12 Name Index Morris, R., 100 Morris, T. R., 480 Morrison, C. M., 354 Morrison, F. J., 12, 13, 153, 490 Morrow, L. M., 570 Morsanyi, K., 543 Mortenson, E., 586 Morton, M. H., 54 Moseley, D. V., 207 Moser, U., 10 Moshman, D., 422, 423 Mouratidis, A., 511, 516, 525 Mraz, M., 377 Mudrick, N. V., 492 Mueller, C. M., 303 Mueller, K. J., 532 Mueller, P. A., 492 Muenks, K., 489, 530 Muething, C. S., 310 Muhonen, H., 427 Muis, K. R., 486, 490 Mulé, C., 234 Mulet, J., 452 Muller, C., 187, 530, 531 Müller, H., 338 Mullins, N., 73 Mullins, T. S., 299 Mullis, I. V. S., 681 Mun, R. U., 282 Munger, K. A., 254 Muñoz, A., 427 Munt, E. D., 318 Murayama, K., 524, 526, 536, 541, 542 Murdock, S. G., 316 Murdock, T. A., 177 Murdock, T. B., 161 Murphey, D., 226 Murphy, M., 531 Murphy, N. A., 139 Murphy, P. K., 101, 405, 430, 445, 635, 636 Murray, C. S., 254 Murray, D. W., 559 Murray, J. S., 530, 531 Murray, M. S., 254 Murray, S. B., 140 Muschkin, C. G., 677 Musci, R. J., 315, 317 Museus, S. D., 44 Myers, C. E., 91, 94, 294, 296, 299, 330, 331, 354, 540 Myers, D. G., 56, 397 Myerson, J., 382 MyIntyre, J., 9, 494 Na, V. S., 44 Nadler, J. T., 46 NAEA, 190, 204 Nagaoka, J., 488 Nagata, J. M., 140 Nagengast, B., 169, 628–631, 673 Nagin, D. S., 150 Nagle, R., 258 Naglieri, J. A., 120

Nagy, G., 631 Nakajima, N., 197 Nakamoto, J., 253 Napolitano, C. M., 10 Narciss, S., 423 Nash, P., 454 Nasir, N. S., 145, 146, 155, 158 Nastasi, B. K., 331 Nathan, M. J., 385 National Alliance of Black School Educators, 189 National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP), 197 National Association for Gifted Children (NAGC), 236, 237 National Center for Education Statistics, 55, 56 National Center for Homeless Education, 54 National Center on Family Homelessness, 54 National Commission on Teaching and America’s Future, 5 National Immigration Forum, 267 National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Early Child Care Research Network, 253 National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA), 228 National Science Foundation, 64 National Science Foundation, National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics, 64 Navarrete Berges, A., 515 Navarro, R. L., 480 Neblett, E., 57 Needham, A., 114, 122 Neff, D., 126, 279, 631 Neisser, U., 197 Nelson, B., 9, 494 Nelson, J. R., 100 Nelson, K. E., 347, 356 Nelson, N., 477 Nemec, P. B., 596 Nesbit, J. C., 384, 393 Ness, I. J., 202 Nett, U. E., 545 Nettle, D., 53 Neubrand, M., 610 Neuman, M., 629 Neuman, S. B., 622 Neumann, K., 609 Neumann, M., 640, 673 Neumeister, K. L. S., 202 Neves-Pereira, M. S., 202 Newman, S. E., 366 Newsom, J. S., 68

Newton, P. M., 207 Ng, E. L., 390 Ng, S. F., 390 Nguyen, B. M. D., 598 Nguyen, L., 578 Nguyen, U. S., 54 Nguyen-Jahiel, K. T., 118, 442, 635 Nguyen-Pham, H., 152 Nicholls, J. G., 524 Nicholson, L. J., 542 Nickerson, K., 165 NIDDK, 232 Niemib, P., 354 Niens, U., 44, 45 Nieto, S., 75 Nigam, M., 434 Niggli, A., 628, 630 NIMH, 227 Nisbett, R. E., 38, 196 Nitko, T. J., 654, 655 Nix, R. L., 347 Njokanma, O. F., 136 Noack, P., 579 Nock, M. K., 357 Noddings, N., 172, 203 Noe-Bustamante, L., 43, 57 Noguera, P., 153, 598 Nokes, J. D., 404, 413 Nokes-Malach, T. J., 375, 376, 378, 379, 390, 402, 412, 522 Nolan, J. F., 583 Nolen, S. B., 550 Noor, N. M., 44, 45 Norbert, F., 258 Norby, M. M., 338, 344, 359, 360, 375, 390, 399, 421, 423 Nordby, C. J., 376, 497, 500 Nordström, A. L., 221 Norman, D. A., 334 Northwest Education Association (NWEA), 654 Norton, E., 100 Norwalk, K. E., 11, 695 Novack, M., 394 Novak, A., 52 Novodvorsky, I., 560, 561, 563, 584, 590, 592–594, 616, 633 Ntoumanis, N., 511, 516 Nucci, L. P., 145, 173–175 Nulty, A., 454 Nunes, T., 22 Nussbaum, E. M., 407, 425, 430 Nussbaum, M., 408 Nuttall, A. K., 62 Nuttall, J., 137 Nyberg, L., 358 Nye, B., 629 Nylund, D., 219, 221, 222 Nystrand, M., 633, 635 Oakes, J., 20, 54 Oakes, M. A., 63 Oberle, E., 9, 496 Obidah, J. E., 599

O’Brennan, L. M., 590 Oburu, P., 490 O’Campo, P. J., 165 O’Connor, B. C., 221 O’Connor, C., 53 O’Connor, E. E., 153 O’Connor, P. A., 543 Oczlon, S., 524 Odegard, T. N., 355 Oden, M. H., 237 O’Doherty, J. P., 294 O’Donnell, A. M., 439, 440, 442, 444, 453 O’Donnell, J., 49 Oduwole, A. O., 136 Oettingen, G., 486 Ogbu, J. U., 52 Ogbuehi, P., 22, 390, 392 Ogden, C. L., 139 Oğuz, E., 460–461 Oh, J. S., 257 Oh, S., 236 Oh, Y., 496 Ohio Department of Education, 260 Okagaki, L., 53, 77 O’Keefe, B., 54 O’Keefe, P. A., 524 O’Kelly, J., 439 Okkinga, M., 386 Oldham, E., 439 O’Leary, A. P., 376 O’Leary, K. D., 303 O’Leary, S., 303, 311 O’Leary, S. G., 303 Oliver, M., 433, 434 Oller, D. K., 257 Olney, A. M., 633 Olsen, B., 673 Olsen, J., 136 Olson, D. R., 25 Olsson, L., 332 Olsson, M., 478 Olszewski-Kubilius, P., 445 Olweus, D., 591 O’Mara, A., 167 O’Mara, A. J., 167 Omarshah, T., 515 O’Neal, C. R., 487 O’Neil, K., 150 O’Neill, J. C., 227 O’Neill, R. E., 316 Oppenheimer, D. M., 492 Orange, C., 584 Orben, A., 458 O’Reilly, T., 353 Orfield, G., 62 Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD), 70, 96, 442 Orlando, L., 105 Ormiston, H., 557, 563, 580, 582 Ormrod, J. E., 95, 96, 330, 331, 381

Name Index Orobio de Castro, B., 677 Orosco, M. J., 213 ORR, 267 Orr, A., 72, 73 Osborne, J. F., 407 Osborne, J. W., 169 Osenk, I., 169 Osgood, D. W., 167 Oslund, E., 254 Ostarek, M., 394 Östergren, R., 332 Osterhage, J., 481 Osterman, K. F., 579 Ostrov, J. M., 151 Osuna, J., 362 Otten, S., 461 Otto, B., 250 Oudman, S., 542 Ouellette, R., 81, 529 Ovando, C. J., 271 Overman, L. T., 52 Overstreet, S., 225 Owens, E. B., 144 Owens, J. S., 219, 221, 223 Owens, R. D., Jr., 223, 249, 250, 251, 263, 264 Owens, R. J. Q., 383 Ownston, R. D., 455 Özek, U., 690 Paas, F., 342–344, 444, 530 Pabilonia, S. W., 628 Padilla, D., 59 Padilla-Munoz, E. M., 494 Padmanabhan, A., 96 Pahlke, E., 71 Pai, H.-H., 410 Paik, E. S., 208 Paivio, A., 350, 385 Pajares, F., 18, 35, 165, 168, 320, 470, 477, 479, 480 Pakarinen, E., 427 Palardy, G., 52, 81 Palcic, J., 309 Palikara, O., 471 Palincsar, A. S., 381, 421, 422, 428, 429, 431, 432, 440, 443, 445 Palmer, D., 274 Palmquist, C. M., 138 Pan, S. C., 362 Pan, Y., 559 Panadero, E., 491, 492, 500 Pang, M. F., 393 Panitz, T., 439 Pansky, A., 357 Papageorge, N. W., 64, 612 Pape, S., 52 Papert, S., 456 Pare-Blagoev, J., 98 Paris, A. H., 561 Paris, D., 78 Paris, S. G., 497, 499 Park, B., 537 Park, D., 64, 488, 532 Park, E., 44

Park, G., 19, 240 Park, H. M., 522 Park, J. W., 49 Park, L. E., 169 Parke, R. D., 145, 167 Parker, C., 587 Parker, J. H., 494 Parker, K., 57 Parker, M., 22 Parker, P. D., 166, 169 Parner, E., 136 Parnis, K., 587 Partika, A. C., 379 Pas, E. T., 315, 317, 598 Pascalis, O., 45 Pashler, H., 19, 207, 352, 364, 385, 429, 430, 633 Pasqua, J. L., 301 Passmore, A., 496 Pastorelli, C., 477, 490 Patall, E. A., 313, 516, 518, 524, 527, 535, 548, 628, 629 Patel, S., 590 Patel, Y., 215 Patrick, H., 522, 523, 547 Patton, D. U., 588 Patton, F., 165 Pauk, W., 383 Paul, R., 322, 403, 404 Pauli, W. M., 294 Paulos, L., 338 Paulsen, D. J., 96 Paunesku, D., 498, 530 Payne, A. C., 253 Payne, B. D., 498, 499 PBS, 59 Pea, R., 456 Pearl, R., 150 Pearson, B. Z., 257 Pearson, J., 342 Peck, S. C., 145, 146, 155, 158 Pedaste, M., 431 Pedersen, N. L., 190 Pekrun, R., 165, 166, 362, 385, 477, 522, 526, 540–542, 547 Pelham, W. E., 221 Pell, T., 442 Pellegrino, J. R., 690 Pellegrino, J. W., 641 Pellis, S., 138 Pendakur, S. L., 44 Penn, L. T., 480 Pennebaker, J. W., 529 Penuel, W., 424 Penuel, W. R., 437 Peppler, K., 424 Peregoy, S. F., 258, 262, 272–274 Perera, H. N., 480 Perez, A. M., 192 Perez, C., 259 Perez, T., 527 Perfetti, C. A., 100 Perkins, D. N., 409, 429 Perner, J., 375

Perrin, A., 163 Perry, B. L., 63 Perry, N. E., 63, 67, 109, 136, 138, 139, 149, 158, 171, 176, 237, 238, 253, 318, 376, 484, 495–497, 499, 500 Perry, R. P., 529, 542 Persson, H., 587, 598 Peruche, B. M., 63 Perusse, D., 150 Peters, K., 478 Peterson, C., 171, 355 Peterson, G. W., 167 Peterson, M., 137 Petit, M., 621 Petitto, L. A., 257–259 Petrilli, M. J., 617, 618 Petrosino, A., 587, 598 Pettit, G. S., 171 Peura, P. I., 480 Peverly, S. T., 114 Pew Research Center, 57 Peyton, S. T., 196 Pfeffer, F. T., 49 Pfiffner, L., 221 Philipp, A., 482 Phillips, B. M., 214, 253, 254 Phillips, D. C., 421, 422 Phillips, L., 500 Phillips, M. M., 547 Phippen, J. W., 223 Phye, G. D., 410, 411 Piaget, J., 22, 87, 102–105, 107–109, 111–114, 117, 118, 120–125, 127, 138, 158, 159, 174, 359, 406, 421, 422, 437, 440, 501 Pianta, R. C., 11–13, 153, 578 Pick, A. D., 250 Pickering, S. J., 346 Pickett, T. Jr., 47 Piel, J. A., 103, 158 Pierce, W. D., 322 Pieschl, S., 486, 490 Pietschnig, J., 524 Piff, P. K., 52 Pihlgren, C., 221 Pike, M. K., 140 Pin, W. H., 115 Pineau, A., 115 Piñeiro, I., 630 Pinger, P., 487 Pinkard, N., 424 Pinker, S., 98, 345 Pinquart. M., 145 Pinsker, J., 629 Pintrich, P. R., 481, 485, 513, 520, 523–525, 536, 537, 543 Piquette, N., 452 Pisha, B., 642 Pitten Cate, I. M., 611 Pituch, K. A., 53, 516 Pivnick, L. K., 148 Plager, E., 22

N-13

Plank, D. N., 618 Plank, D. R., 691 Plant, E. A., 63 Plants, R., 641 Platsidou, M., 346 Plavnick, J. B., 22 Plomin, R., 198 Plucker, J., 202 Plucker, J. A., 203 Plummer, B. D., 687 Podolski, C. P., 151 Podolsky, A., 689 Pohlmann, B., 167 Poikkeus, A. M., 427 Poirel, N., 115 Poldrack, R. A., 337 Polikoff, M. S., 618, 695 Polio, M., 677 Poljac, E., 338 Polk, L., 4 Pollack, T. M., 61 Ponnock, A., 489 Pool, R., 237, 366, 400, 402, 473 Poorthuis, A. M. G., 677 Popham, W. J., 617, 618, 621, 653, 654, 656, 664, 668, 670, 687, 690, 694 Popov, V., 346 Portwood, S. G., 10 Posada, G., 159 Posner, M. I., 365, 391 Postema, M. C., 215 Postle, B. R., 345 Postlethwaite, B. E., 177 Poteat, V. P., 73 Potocki, A., 452 Powell, S. R., 391 Pradhan, M., 197 Prakash, K., 150 Prati, G., 579 Prat-Sala, M., 480 Prawat, R. S., 422 Preckel, F., 190, 512 Preckel, T., 166 Prellwitz, M., 139 Premack, D., 305 Prensky, M., 454 Pressley, M., 253, 375, 379, 387, 630 Pretzlik, U., 22 Price, G. E., 207 Price, L. F., 96 Price, T. S., 249 Price, W. F., 249 Priniski, S. J., 548 Przybylski, A. K., 458 Pudas, S., 358 Pugh, K. J., 547 Pulfrey, C., 515 Pulham, E., 4, 460, 461 Pulkkinen, L., 150 Pullen, P. C., 66, 216–220, 222, 229, 230, 232–234, 376, 386, 642

N-14 Name Index Pullmann, M. D., 578 Puncochar, J., 440 Puntambekar, S., 124 Purpura, D. J., 435, 452 Putman, M., 482 Putnam, R., 15 Putney, L., 117, 124, 125 Putwain, D. W., 542 Puzio, K., 639 Pyke, A. A., 119 Quan, H., 487 Quiceno, J., 159 Quin, D., 11 Quince, V., 54 Quinn, D. M., 53, 63 Quinn, P. C., 45 Quinn, P. D., 197, 488 Quintana, S. M., 57, 59 Rabiner, D. L., 559 Rabinowitz, M., 610 Rachlin, H., 474 Radford, J., 428 Radley, K. C., 301 Radvansky, G. A., 333, 340, 345, 349, 351, 352, 356, 361, 394 Raes, E., 442 Rafter, M., 394 Rahim, A., 484, 496, 497 Rai, M. K., 274 Raijmakers, M. E. J., 190 Raikes, H. A., 145 Raikkonen, E. M., 480 RAINN, 155 Rakoczy, H., 138 Ralston, P. A. S., 385 Ram, N., 490 Ramani, G., 405 Ramde, J., 25 Ramey, D. R., 270 Ramirez, G., 68, 70, 543 Ramirez, J. D., 270 Ramlau-Hansen, C. H., 136 Ramon Lizarraga, J., 425 Ramos, A., 524 Randolph, S. M., 165 Rapaport, A., 435 Rapp, D. N., 540 Rapp, J. T., 311 Rasheed, D., 496 Rasku-Puttonen, H., 427 Rasplica Khoury, C., 434 Ratner, H. H., 124 Rau, M. A., 384, 385 Raudenbush, S. W., 59, 62, 612, 675, 676 Raudsepp, E., 397 Raver, C. C., 51, 490 Ravit, G. D., 433, 435 Raviv, T., 10 Rawson, K. A., 352, 382, 385, 673 Reardon, S. F., 51, 59, 676, 689 Reback, R., 8 Reber, R., 536, 537, 548

Reddy, L. A., 11, 695 Reddy, S., 96, 220, 338 Reder, L. M., 346, 375, 424 Redford, P., 480 Redifer, J. L., 362 Reed, L. A., 592 Reed, R. P., 630 Reese, L., 254 Reeve, J., 318, 511, 514–517, 546, 548 Regalia, C., 477 Regner, I., 46 Regueiro, B., 630 Reich, J., 615 Reichert, M., 165, 166 Reid, E. E., 435, 452 Reid, J. M., 269 Reigh, E., 407 Reiman, E. M., 97 Reimann, P., 391 Reimers, F., 49 Reinke, W. M., 482, 590 Reis, S. M., 236, 239, 240 Reisberg, D., 204 Reisman, A., 634, 635 Reiter-Palmon, R., 202 Reljic, G., 271, 272 Rendon, P., 279 Renkl, A., 361, 393, 444, 496 Renninger, K. A., 486, 535, 536, 538 Renzulli, J. S., 236, 239, 240 Repka, M., 54 Resnick, L. B., 424 Reeves, S., 529 Rey, G. D., 380 Reyes, C. R., 64 Reyes, E. I., 81 Reyes, M. R., 609 Reynolds, C. A., 190 Reynolds, C. R., 213 Reynolds, R. E., 485 Reznitskaya, A., 118 Rhodes, J., 424 Rhodes, M., 45 Rhodes, M. G., 380 Rice, D., 187 Rice, F. P., 227 Richardson, M., 480 Richey, J. E., 401, 412 Richmond, M., 159 Rickard, T. C., 362 Riddle, T., 63 Rideout, V., 6 Riedesel, C. A., 392 Rieser, S., 665 Rigby, K., 586, 590 Riggall, A. C., 345 Riggle, E. D. B., 73 Rillero, P., 434 Rimfeld, K., 489 Rimm, S. B., 236, 240 Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., 578 Rintamaa, M., 380

Ritchey, K., 58, 380 Ritchie, S. J., 10, 196 Rittle-Johnson, B., 393, 435 Rivas-Drake, D., 57, 494, 498 Rivera, A., 279 Rivers, J. C., 11 Rivers, S. E., 494, 609 Rivet, T. T., 234 Rivkin, S. G., 11 Robbins, S. B., 379 Robbins, S. J., 298 Roberge, M. M., 269 Roberson, D., 249 Roberts, A., 61, 530 Roberts, G., 254, 408 Roberts, M. C., 10 Roberts, R. D., 497 Robey, A., 98 Robinson, D. H., 429 Robinson, J. C., 628 Robinson, J. P., 72, 73 Robinson-Cimpian, J. P., 68, 676 Robustelli, S., 611 Rockoff, J., 8 Roderick, M., 488 Rodkin, P. C., 150 Rodriguez, S., 630 Roediger, H. L. III, 364 Roehrig, G., 93, 97, 99, 101 Roeser, R. W., 145, 146, 155, 158, 485, 496 Roessger, K. M., 384 Rogat, T. K., 525 Rogers, B. P., 338 Rogers, L. O., 59 Rogoff, B., 114, 117, 124, 421, 436 Rogow, F., 459 Rogowsky, B. A., 207 Rohal, M., 57 Rohde, L. A., 219, 220 Rohrer, D., 19, 207, 362, 364 Roid, G. H., 195 Rojas, J. P., 480, 489 Rojas, N. R., 311 Rojas, R., 257 Rolland, R. G., 522 Roloff, J., 695 Romano, M. E., 5, 563, 565, 570, 576, 583, 584, 628, 631, 633 Romero, C., 530 Ronfeldt, M., 695 Roorda, D. L., 11, 562 Ros, A., 634 Rosario, R. J., 59 Rosch, E. H., 351 Roschelle, J., 455 Roscoe, R. D., 454 Rose, C. A., 579 Rose, N., 526 Rose, N. S., 345 Rosenberg, M., 168 Rosenberg, M. D., 201, 204 Rosenberg, M. S., 222, 223

Rosenfarb, I., 318 Rosenshine, B., 125, 609, 624, 625, 634 Rosenthal, L., 42 Rosenthal, R., 611, 612 Rosenzweig, E. Q., 548 Roseth, C. J., 442, 443, 447, 521, 591 Roskos, K., 622 Ross, M., 398 Ross, S., 124, 428 Rostosky, S. S., 73 Roth, G., 522 Rothkopf, A., 536 Rowan, B., 610 Rowe, M. B., 633 Rowe, M. L., 53, 405 Rowley, S. J., 59, 61 Rozek, C. S., 67 Rubach, C., 482 Rubie-Davies, C. M., 611–613 Rubin, K. H., 148–149, 239 Rubinsten, O., 217 Ruble, D. N., 65 Rucinski, C. L., 11 Ruck, M. D., 52, 62 Rudd, T., 599 Rudner, L. M., 688 Rudolph, K. D., 162 Rummel, J., 346 Rummel, N., 363, 443 Rundle, S., 207 Rupp, A. A., 454 Rusby, J. C., 318 Russell, M. K., 667 Russell, S. T., 73 Rust, J., 66 Ruzek, E., 7, 15, 598 Ryan, A., 149, 525 Ryan, M. K., 478 Ryan, R. M., 322, 497, 511, 514, 515, 528 Ryoo, J. H., 315, 317 Saalbach, H., 257 Saavedra, A. R., 435 Sabatini, J., 353 Sabatino, C. A., 226 Sabet, R. F., 559 Sabey, A., 139–141 Sabzalian, L., 78 Sacker, A., 136 Sackett, P. R., 50 Sacks, V., 226 Sadker, D., 632–634 Sadker, M., 632–634 Sadler, P. M., 119 Saffran, J., 93, 94, 103, 104, 105, 112–115, 117, 119, 120, 172, 346 Safter, H. T., 202 Sailer, M., 456 Sakamoto, A., 151 Salchegger, S., 166 Saleem, M., 151

Name Index Salem, A., 72, 73 Salen Tekinbş, K., 424 Salimi, F., 393 Salmela-Aro, K., 535 Salomon, G., 409, 429 Salovey, P., 199, 494, 609 Saltarelli, A. J., 443, 447 Saltarelli, W. A., 447 Salvi, A., 207 SAMHSA., 224, 225 Sammons, P., 482 Sams, A., 460 Samuel, R., 480 Sana, F., 347, 492 Sanchez Viveros, B., 409 Sanders, W. L., 11 Sandilos, L. E., 11, 695 Sanger Study Center, 401 Sanghera-Sidhu, S., 452 Sansone, C., 486 Santiago, C., 10 Santos, N. M., 225 Santtilaa, P., 354 Sappington, R., 480 Sarrazin, P., 613 Sasser, N., 585, 586 Sattler, H., 586 Sattler, J. M., 192, 195, 203, 204 Sauerwein, M., 515 Saulnier, G., 164 Savage, R., 452 Savin-Williams, R. C., 72 Savolainen, H., 482 Sawyer, K., 202–204 Sawyer, R. K., 375, 425–426 Sax, L., 70 Saxbe, D. E., 542 Saxe, G. B., 114 Scabini, E., 477 Scalise, K., 90, 93, 96, 99, 101, 540 Scammacca, N. K., 254 Scanlan, M., 26 Scarcella, R., 272 Schachner, J. N., 49 Schachner, M. K., 579 Schacter, D. L., 357 Schacter, H. L., 590, 592 Schaefer, D. R., 57 Schalke, D., 190 Schallert, D. L., 485, 515 Scharf, F., 335 Schatschneider, C., 254 Scheepers, A. W. A., 520 Scheer, J. R., 73 Scheibe, C., 459 Scheiter, K., 350, 385 Schellinger, K. B., 142 Scherer, R., 409 Scherrer, V., 512 Schiefele, U., 167 Schieve, L. A., 6 Schippers, M. C., 520 Schlueter, D., 54

Schmid, R. F., 451 Schmidgall, S. P., 385 Schmidt, E. M., 234 Schmidt, H. G., 432, 434, 435 Schmiedek, F., 347 Schneider, B., 530 Schneider, W., 331, 375, 376 Schnellert, L., 318 Schnotz, W., 350 Schnyder, I., 628, 629, 630 Schoenfeld, A. H., 395, 399 Scholer, A. A., 486 Schonert-Reichl, K. A., 9, 153, 154, 494–496 Schoor, C., 423 Schor, J., 424 Schrage, J., 486 Schrager, S. M., 522 Schraw, G., 208, 332, 356, 395 Schraw, G. J., 344, 423 Schrift, R. Y., 338 Schroeder, N. L., 384 Schuchardt, K., 216 Schuldt, L. C., 673 Schulze, C., 139 Schulze, S. K., 358 Schumacher, R. F., 391 Schunk, D. H., 90, 91, 93, 94, 97, 101, 122, 124, 125, 165, 168, 291, 306, 318, 320, 333–336, 352, 363, 375, 376, 379, 386, 387, 400, 410, 421–424, 470, 472–475, 477, 480, 486, 491, 496, 520, 523, 524, 537, 543 Schutz, P., 24 Schwab, J. J., 26 Schwab, Y., 566, 577, 583, 585 Schwager, A., 350 Schwaighofer, M., 392, 393 Schwan, S., 363 Schwartz, B., 298 Schwartz, D. L., 97, 409 Schwartz, H. L., 8 Schwartz, J. E., 392 Schwartz, M. S., 298 Schwartz, S., 567 Schwartz, S. J., 57 Schwarz, B. B., 406, 407, 440 Schwarzenthal, M., 579 Schwarzer, R., 543 Schweig, J. D., 694 Schwerdt, G., 676 Schwinger, M., 523, 531 Scott, J. G., 588 Scott, T., 442 Scruggs, T. E., 210, 213, 214 Scudellari, M., 19 Sears, D. A., 410 Seaton, E. K., 57 Seaton, M., 166 Sechler, C. M., 441 Seehorn, D., 457 Sefton-Green, J., 424 Seggie, F. N., 460–461

Seidel, T., 431, 433, 609 Seitz, S., 140 Seli, P., 201, 204 Selig, J. P., 123, 426 Seligman, M. E. P., 480, 488, 528, 529 Sellers, R. M., 57 Semega, J., 49 Senbanjo, I. O., 136 Senecal, C., 7 Senechal, M., 253 Senko, C., 522, 523 Sensale Yazdian, L., 668 Serpell, R., 423 Setrill, S. A., 543 Sette, S., 578 Seung Yang, J., 489 Shada, N., 480 Shaenfield, D., 407 Shaffer, D. W., 454 Shah, P., 208 Shahar, G., 148 Shanahan, E. J., 197, 239 Shanahan, M. E., 557 Shanahan, T., 272 Shand, J., 611 Shapiro, L. R., 249 Shapka, J. D., 495 Sharkey, J. D., 592 Shavelson, R. S., 609 Shaw, S. T., 543 Shayer, M., 112 Shaywitz, S. E., 213 Shea, T. M., 312 Shearer, C. B., 190–193 Shechtman, Z., 176 Sheets, R. H., 38, 78 Shen, H., 64 Shen, J., 97 Shepard, L. A., 676 Shernoff, D. J., 539 Sherwood, C., 428 Shi, H., 136 Shi, Y., 408, 460 Shibuya, Al, 151 Shic, F., 64 Shiffrin, R. M., 333 Shim, S. S., 525 Shimada, H., 482 Shin, D. D., 536 Shin, J.-Y., 268 Shipley, T. F., 380 Shirky, C., 492 Shiyko, M. P., 137 Shjogren, K. A., 187, 189, 209, 219, 231 Shonkoff, J. P., 52 Shore, D. I., 347 Shores, K., 61, 62 Short, D. J., 274 Shrider, E. Z., 49 Shrier, L. A., 148 Shrout, P. E., 66 Shuai, L., 220

N-15

Shuell, T. J., 624 Shulman, E. P., 488, 490, 687 Shute, V. J., 3, 27, 29 Siddiq, F., 409 Siddle Walker, V., 76 Sidney, P. G., 408, 410 Siegal, M., 269 Siegel-Hawley, G., 62 Siegle, D., 282 Siegle, D. B., 236, 240 Siegler, R. S., 93, 94, 103, 104, 105, 112–115, 117, 119, 120, 122, 172, 346 Sierra, Z., 202 Siiman, L. A., 431 Silven, M., 250 Silver, J., 138 Silverman, R., 405 Silverman, S. K., 356 Silvia, P. J., 201, 204 Simmons, D., 10, 154, 200, 254, 494 Simmons, D. N., 8, 9 Simmons, L., 254 Simon, D. A., 364, 380, 386 Simon, D. P., 399 Simon, H. A., 366, 424 Simon, T., 229 Simonds, C. J., 609 Simons, G. F., 249, 258 Simons, L. G., 137 Simons, R. L., 137 Simonton, D. K., 202, 204 Simpkins, S. D., 254, 278, 487 Sims, S., 433, 434 Sims, W. A., 11, 695 Sinclair, M. F., 580 Sinclair, S., 63 Sirin, S. R., 48, 50 Skagerlund, K., 332 Skiba, R. J., 15, 331, 557, 563, 580, 582, 598 Skibbe, L. E., 490 Skinner, A. T., 490 Skinner, B. F., 296, 297 Skinner, C., 49 Skinner, E., 518 Skoog, T., 137 Skuballa, I. T., 530 Slama, R. B., 268 Slaten, C. D., 579 Slater, A., 139–141 Slater, A. M., 45 Slater, L., 168, 169 Slater, S. C., 566 Slavin, R. E., 22, 25,271, 442, 443, 445, 447 Sleegers, P. J. C., 386 Sleek, S., 331 Sloane, K. D., 23 Slot, W., 20 Sloutsky, V. M., 376 Smetana, J. G., 145 Smith, A. C. G., 276

N-16 Name Index Smith, D. C., 220 Smith, D. D., 448, 450 Smith, D. G., 96 Smith, E. D., 138 Smith, E. E., 351 Smith, F., 362 Smith, J. L., 46 Smith, K. A., 447 Smith, L. L., 482 Smith, M., 61 Smith, M. D., 660 Smith, M. L., 676 Smith, P., 347 Smith, S., 53, 220 Smith, S. M., 361 Smith, S. W., 499 Smith, T. S., 264 Smolkowski, K., 599 Sneyers, E., 611 Snow, C. E., 250, 253, 257, 268 Snow, R. E., 611 Snowberg, K. E., 496 Snyder, K. E., 236 Søbstad, F., 138 Soemer, A., 363 Soenens, B., 516 Soland, J., 7 Soleck, G., 47 Solesbee, C., 590 Solomon, D., 442 Soloway, E., 435 Son, L. K., 364, 380, 386 Sonnert, G., 119 Sood, S., 390 Soodak, L. C., 317 Sorbring, E., 490 Sorhagen, N. S., 531, 612 Sorvo, R. M. A., 480 Sosniak, L. A., 618, 619 Soter, A. O., 635 Soydan, A. S., 596 Spada, H., 443 Spagnolo, A. C., 596 Spanierman, L., 57 Spanogle, C., 152 Spanoudis, G., 190, 194, 216, 346 Spearman, C., 190 Spears, F. M., 145 Spencer, K., 100 Spencer, S. J., 45, 46, 58, 59 Spendlove, D., 15 Spengler, M., 190 Spilt, J. L., 580 Spinath, B., 523 Spitzer, B. J., 498 Sprague, J. R., 318 Sprenger, M., 93, 96, 101 Spring, K. J., 461 Spuhl, S. T., 120 SRCD, 255 Srna, S., 338 Stadler, M., 392, 393 Stahl, S. A., 207

Stahnke, R., 576, 583 Staley, K. C., 493 Star, J. R., 390, 395 Starck, J. G., 63 Stark, K., 10, 224, 226 Starko, A. J., 203, 206 Starosta, K., 390 Starrett, M. J., 343 Stattin, H., 137 Stautberg, S., 313 Stavinoha, P. L., 590 Steenbergen-Hu, S., 445 Stein, R. E. K., 138 Steinberg, L., 59, 137, 148, 149, 165, 490 Steingut, R. R., 516 Steiner, E. D., 542 Steinmayr, R., 489, 523, 531 Stenske, M. T., 311 Stephens, J., 176, 178 Steptoe, A., 494 Sterling-Turner, H. E., 310 Sternberg, K., 337, 350, 352–354 Sternberg, R. J., 193, 194, 197, 202, 203, 204, 236, 337, 350, 352–354, 390, 497 Stevens, J., 152 Stevens, K., 70 Stevens, R., 78–79, 624, 625, 634 Stevenson, H. C., 76 Sticca, F., 522 Stickle, B., 9, 494 Stiggins, R. J., 616, 654, 659, 660, 662–667 Still, M., 61, 62 Stinson, D. W., 53, 70, 267 Stipek, D. J., 510, 530, 531, 548 Stockard, J., 434 Stoddard, J. D., 455 Stoeger, H., 496 Stone, E., 65, 67, 73 Stoolmiller, M., 477 Stough, L. M., 559, 560, 565 Stout, M., 586 Støvring, H., 136 Stoycos, S. A., 542 Strachan, S. L., 435 Stratton, H., 228 Strauss, V., 8 Strayer, D. L., 338 Strayer, J., 164 Strom, B. C., 382 Strom, P. S., 592 Strom, R. D., 592 Stroman, C., 550 Strong-Bak, W., 301 Stupinisky, R. H., 542 Su, C., 196 Su, J.-H., 268 Su, P., 136 Sudkamp, A., 238 Sugai, G., 579 Suizzo, M.-A., 53 Suldo, S. M., 579

Sullivan, J. R., 214 Sullivan, M. A., 303 Suls, J., 478 Sun, J., 442 Sun, Y., 136 Sung, H-.Y., 455 Supple, A., 167 Surprenant, C. W., 171, 172 Sutton, R. E., 542 Sutton, R. M., 46, 70 Sutton, T. E., 137 Svarovsky, G. N., 454 Svetleff, Z., 480 Svinicki, M., 385 Svoboda, R. C., 67 Swan, G. M., 675 Swansea University, 207 Swanson, G. J., 311 Swanson, H. L., 376 Swanson, J., 493 Swanson, J. M., 219, 220 Swanson, T. C., 224 Swearer, S. M., 588, 590, 592 Sweller, J., 342–343, 350, 389, 391–393, 398, 434, 530, 626 Sweller, N., 350 Swing, E. L., 151 Syed, M., 57 Sylvia, P. J., 536 Symes, W., 542 Synodi, E., 138 Syvertsen, A. K., 586 Tabori, A. V., 227 Tajran, J., 362 Takash, H. M. S., 490 Talbot, M., 148 Tallal, P., 207 Tamim, R. M., 451 Tamis-LeMonda, C., 66 Tan, F. D. H., 616, 634 Tan, J. J. X., 49 Tan, S. C., 616 Tanaka, A., 536 Tang, M., 394 Tang, Y., 97 Tangney, B., 439 Tanis, M., 431 Tanner-Smith, E. E., 456 Tao, F., 136 Tapanya, S., 490 Tarasawa, B., 7 Tate, C. C., 65, 71, 197 Tate, J., 480 Taub, M., 492 Taxer, J. L., 487 Taylor, A., 481 Taylor, A. R., 396 Taylor, C., 496 Taylor, D. M., 518 Taylor, G. G., 499 Taylor, M., 7 Taylor, R. D., 9, 142 Te Winkel, W. W. R., 434 Temple, C., 377

Templin, J. L., 536 TenBrink, T. D., 663 Tenenbaum, H. R., 62, 408, 443, 444, 611 Tennant, J. E., 609 Teranishi, R. T., 598 Terkildsen, T. S., 455 Terman, L. M., 237 Terry, M. N., 609 Tetzner, J., 640 Tharp, R. G., 77, 78, 118 Theis, D., 515 Theobald, R., 54 Theodorakis, N., 588 Theodore, L. A., 310 Theokas, C., 143 Therriault, D. J., 362 Thijs, J. T., 580 Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., 511, 516 Thomaes, S., 677 Thoman, D. B., 46, 486 Thomas, D. R., 22 Thomas, H. J., 588 Thomas, J. R., 135, 136, 139 Thomas, K., 542 Thomas, K. T., 135, 136, 139 Thomas, M. S. C., 99, 101 Thomas, P. L., 489 Thomas, R., 673 Thomas-Tate, S., 253, 254 Thompson, C. A., 408, 410 Thompson, G., 518, 622 Thompson, L., 396, 543 Thompson, R. A., 145 Thompson, R. K., 8 Thompson, W. L., 330 Thomson, K., 496 Thouvenelle, S., 457, 458 Throndsen, I., 480 Tibbetts, Y., 548 Tien, J., 425 Tierney, W. G., 42, 54 Tikva, C., 456 Tillmanns, M., 118 Timmers, F., 442 Timperley, H., 634 Tingstrom, D. H., 310 Tipton, E., 530 Tirado, L. M. U., 490 Titsworth, S., 492 Tjosvold, D., 447 Tobias, S., 434 Tobiason, G., 694 Tobler, N., 228 Toglia, M. P., 355 Tokar, D. M., 480 Tolar, T. D., 214, 254 Tolvanen, A., 482 Tomasello, M., 124, 138, 249, 250 Tomasetto, C., 543 Tombu, M. N., 338 Tomlinson, C. A., 236, 675, 677 Tomlinson, R. C., 52

Name Index Tomlinson-Clarke, S., 581, 599 Tompkins, J. R., 240 Tomsaik, M. J., 10 Tong, F., 338 Tonks, S. M., 526, 527 Toomey, R. B., 73 Topping, K., 257 Torrance, E. P., 202, 240 Tortora, M., 639, 640 Tough, P., 488 Touretzky, D., 457 Tourva, A., 190, 194, 216 Traff, U., 332 Trainin, G., 478 Trautwein, U., 11, 167, 169, 628–630, 695 Tredoux, C., 44, 45 Tremblay, R. E., 150 Tricot, A., 343, 389 Trigani, B., 152 Trobst, S., 536 Trompeter, N., 140 Tropp, L. R., 44, 45 Trott, J., 530 Trouilloud, D., 613 Troutman, A. C., 297, 299, 301–302, 308, 311, 314 Trucano, M., 546, 578 Truong, N. L., 73 Trust, T., 461 Tsai, K. M., 58 Tsai, Y.-M., 610 Tsang, W., 81, 529 Tsantis, L. A., 457, 458 Tschannen-Moran, M., 445, 449, 482, 525 Tseng, A., 407 Tseng, C.-M., 515 Tsourlidaki, E., 431 Tsukayama, E., 64, 197, 488, 532 Ttofi, M. M., 588 Tucker-Drob, E. M., 10 Tuitt, N. R., 59 Tulagan, N., 487 Tully, L. A., 312 Tuovinen, J., 391 Turkheimer, E., 196 Turkle, S., 163, 451 Turnbull, A., 187, 189, 209, 219, 231 Turnbull, H. R., 187, 189, 209, 219, 231 Turner, E., 163 Turner, J. C., 497, 546–547, 578 Twenge, J. M., 457 Tyler, B.-J., 268, 281 Tyler, K. M., 78 Tyler, N. C., 220 Tynan, M. C., 489 Tyson, K., 62, 64 Tze, V. M. C., 7, 542 Udell, W., 407 Ugen, S., 165, 166 Uline, C. L., 59

Ullman, M. T., 252 Umana-Taylor, A. J., 57–59 Umbreit, J., 317 Una Global Evaluation Group, 44, 45 UNHCR, 267 University of Lynchburg, 401 Unkelbach, C., 295 Upright, J. J., 599 Uqdah, A. L., 78 Urban Institute, 6 Urbina, S., 197 Urdan, T., 485, 522, 524, 531 U.S. Department of Agriculture, 49 U. S. Department of Education, 61, 64, 189, 229, 233, 281, 282, 617 U.S. Department of Health, 6, 155 Usher, E. L., 24, 470, 477–480, 486, 489, 491, 526 Utz, J. C., 387, 453 Vacca, J. A. L., 377 Vacca, R. T., 377 Vagi, K. J., 332 Vaillancourt, T., 592 Vakil, S., 425 Valentine, J. C., 478, 611, 628, 629 Valenzuela, A., 153 Valiente, C., 493, 494 Vallancourt, T., 150 van Aken, M. A. G., 677 van Anders, S. M., 65, 71, 197 van de Leemput, C., 452 van de Pol, J., 428 van de Vijver, F. J. R., 579 Van de Walle, J. A., 391 van den Bergh, L., 63 Van den Broeck, L. V., 611 van den Wittenboer, G. L. H., 543 van der Kleij, F. S., 673 van der Mass, H. L. J., 190 van der Meij, H., 383 van der Meij, J., 383 van der Molen, H. T., 434 van der Spek, E. D., 455 Van Der Veer, R., 117 van der Vleuten, C. P. M., 343 van der Werf, M. P. C., 611 van Egmond, M. C., 515 van Gelderen, A. J. S., 386 van Gog, T., 343, 344, 393, 410, 432, 433, 435, 444 Van Goozen, S. H. M., 151 Van Houtte, M., 65, 677 van Leeuwen, A., 440 Van Loan, C. L., 499 Van Matre, J. C., 611 van Merriënboer, J. J. G., 20, 342, 343, 428, 433, 434, 451 Van Meter, P., 383

van Nimwegen, C., 455 van Oers, B., 431 van Oostendrop, H., 455 Van Quaquebeke, N., 634 Van Riesen, S. A., 431 van Rooij, D., 215 Van Ryzin, M. J., 521, 591 van Schooten, E., 386 van Steensel, R., 386 Van Voorhis, F. L., 628, 631 VandeKamp, K. O., 376, 497, 499 Vandell, D. L., 53, 487 Vandergrift, N., 13 Vandermass-Peler, M., 138 Vanhoof, J., 611 VanLehn, K. A., 409, 410 Vansteenkiste, M., 511, 516, 522, 525 VanTassel-Baska, J., 19 Vantieghem, W., 65 Varier, D., 480 Varma, S., 97, 99, 101 Vasquez, A. C., 59, 313, 516 Vasquez, J. A., 77 Vaughn, M., 497 Vaughn, M. G., 254 Vaughn, S., 214, 254, 628 Vecchio, G. M., 480 Vecchione, M., 480 Veenstra, R., 588 Vega, R. Jr., 310 Velazquez, M. N., 380 Vella-Brodrick, D., 518, 579 Venturanza, R. J., 44 Verkuyten, M., 57 Vermeersch, H., 65 Vernon, P. A., 192 Vernon-Feagans, L., 250 Verock, R.-E., 461 Verplaetse, L. S., 261 Verschueren, K., 524 Vezeau, C., 471 Vidal-Abarca, E., 376 Viholainen, H. J. K., 480 Villegas, L., 268 Viner, R., 136 Viraro, F., 150 Visconti, K. J., 441 Visee, J., 393, 444 Visser, B. A., 192 Vitaro, F., 150 Vitiello, V. E., 536, 539 Vivies, X. D., 45 Vlachopoulos, S., 511, 516 Vock, M., 19 Voeten, M., 63 Vogel, F., 452 Vogt, M., 261, 262, 274, 276, 277, 279 Vohs, K. D., 169 Volman, M., 428 von Eye, A., 451 von Kriegstein, K., 350

N-17

von Schrader, S., 232 von Stumm, S., 198 Von Suchodoletz, A., 482 Vongkulluksn, V. W., 482 Vosicka, L., 387, 453 Voss, T., 610 Vossoughi, S., 427 Votruba-Drzal, E., 51, 269 Vrangalova, Z., 72 Vriesema, C. C., 427, 440, 442, 500 Vrikki, M., 425 Vygotsky, L. S., 117–121, 498, 640 Waaler, E., 221 Waasdorp, T. E., 318, 578, 590–592 Wade, T. D., 169 Wadsworth, M. E., 49 Wagner, I., 350 Wagner, R. K., 216, 218, 434 Wagner, W., 673 Wahl, M., 578 Wahlsten, D., 89 Waits, B. K., 119 Walberg, H. J., 322, 323, 637 Walker, A. E., 451 Walker, J. E., 312 Walker, J. M. T., 630 Walker, T., 629 Wallace, J. B., 676 Wallace, L., 496 Walls, M. L., 137 Walqui, A., 261, 282 Walsh, M., 163 Walters, G. D., 589 Walton, G. M., 486, 498, 530, 531 Wammes, J. D., 385 Wang, C., 525 Wang, F., 360 Wang, H., 136 Wang, K., 61 Wang, M.-T., 59, 61, 535, 586 Wang, S., 97 Wang, W., 61, 146, 591 Wang, Y., 62, 64 Wang, Z., 353, 537 Wangaard, D. B., 176, 178 Wanzek, J., 254 Ward, J. D., 42, 54 Ward, L. M., 45, 66, 68, 250 Ware, S., 282 Warikoo, N., 63 Warneken, F., 138 Warr, M., 607 Warschauer, M., 452, 492 Wasserman, E. A., 298 Waterhouse, L., 190, 191 Waterman, A. S., 163 Waters, L., 518, 579 Waters, S., 579 Waters, S. D., 50 Waters, S. F., 45

N-18 Name Index Watkins, K. E., 70, 257 Watkins, S. C., 424 Watson, J. B., 291 Watson, J. M., 338 Watson, M. S., 442 Watson, P., 611–613 Watson, T. S., 317 Wattie, N., 676 Watts, T. W., 487 Waugh, C. K., 660, 663, 668 Waxman, S. R., 250 Way, N., 579 Waycott, J. L., 429 Wearing, H., 346 Webb, N. L., 620 Webb, N. M., 444 Weber, M. J., 162 Webster, R., 428 Wechsler, D., 195 Wecker, C., 452 Wegner, D. M., 357 Wehby, J., 691 Wehmeyer, M. L., 187, 189, 209, 219, 231 Wei, L., 430, 445, 635, 636 Weidinger, A. F., 489 Weidner, B. L., 479 Weil, E., 70 Weiner, B., 509, 527, 528, 529, 534 Weinstein, C. S., 5, 153, 518, 560–563, 565, 567, 570, 576, 578, 581, 583, 584, 590, 592–594, 599, 611, 613, 616, 628, 631, 633 Weinstein, E., 459 Weinstein, N., 458 Weinstein, R. S., 611, 613 Weishaar, M., 382 Weiss, A. L., 600 Weiss, H. B., 254, 278 Weissberg, R. P., 142, 197–199, 493 Welk, G. J., 139 Wellman, H., 171 Wells, A. E., 151 Wells, K. C., 499 Welsh, J. A., 347 Welsh, R. O., 586 Wendt, D. C., 78, 79 Wenger, E., 409, 424, 436 Wenger, I., 139 Wentzel, K. R., 9, 149, 508, 578 Werker, J. F., 250, 257 Werts, M. G., 240 Wertsch, J. V., 119 West, M. R., 489, 676 West, R. E., 461 West, S. G., 677 Westberg, K. L., 236 Westgate, E. C., 542 Westling, D. L., 222, 223 Westling, E., 137

Weston, T., 492 Wetzel, N., 335 Wheatley, K. F., 483 Wheatley, L., 425 Wheeler, L., 478 Whipp, P. R., 634 Whipple, A. D., 676 Whitbeck, L. B., 137 White, A. C., 527 White, A. E., 542 White, L., 59 White, M., 609 White, M. C., 490, 498 White, S. L. J., 15 White, T., 119, 579 Whitehead, A. N., 432 Whitehurst, G. J., 253 Whitesell, N., 59 Whitesell, N. R., 478 Wicherts, J. M., 190 Widmann, A., 335 Wiedmann, M., 443 Wigfield, A., 96, 136, 158, 162, 167, 489, 526, 527, 546 Wiggins, G., 618, 637, 638 Wijnen, W. H. F. W., 434 Wilcox, S. M., 73 Wilczynski, S. M., 310 Wildy, H., 611 Wilensky, R., 496 Wiley, J., 396 Wilkerson, I., 49 Wilkie, K. J., 651 Wilkinson, I. A. G., 635 Willcutt, E. G., 216, 218, 434 Willems, Y. E., 487 Williams, A. B., 586 Williams, B., 494, 498 Williams, C. D., 59 Williams, C. H., 529 Williamson, P., 169 Willingham, D. T., 19, 192, 304, 333, 338, 359, 362, 385 Willoughby, T., 455 Willower, D. J., 559 Wilson, K. A., 618 Wilson, M., 478 Wilson, S. M., 167, 693 Windschitl, M., 421, 422, 423, 461 Winett, R. A., 292 Wink, J., 117, 124, 125 Winkler, R. C., 292 Winne, P. H., 18, 387, 393, 486, 499 Winner, E., 204, 237 Winsler, A., 120 Winston, E., 187, 219 Winstone, N. E., 408, 443 Winters, L., 670 Winters, M. A., 676 Wirthwein, L., 531 Witherspoon, L., 599

Wittrock, M. C., 329 Wittwer, J., 393 Wohl, M. J. A., 518 Wojslawowicz, J. C., 148 Wolf, M., 100 Wolfe, P., 99 Wolke, D., 197 Woloshyn, V., 387 Wolpert-Gawron, H., 567 Won, S., 479 Wong, H. K., 573 Wong, J.-H. S., 44 Wong, K. F., 258 Wong, L., 20, 401 Wong, N.-W. A., 259 Wong, R. T., 573 Wong, S. S., 674 Wood, D., 124, 428 Wood, E., 137, 452 Wood, K., 616 Wood, T. W., 434 Woodard, S. M., 672 Woodward, A., 114, 122 Woodward, J., 22, 390, 392 Woodward, L., 406 Woodward, M. M., 363 Woody, S., 531 Woolf, B. P., 456, 457 Woolfolk Hoy, A., 7, 27, 28, 52, 153, 445, 449, 482, 495, 518, 525, 542, 578, 609 Woolverton, S., 54 Worrell, F. C., 654, 665 Worthington, R. L., 480 Wouters, P., 455 Wu, S., 268 Wu, S. P. W., 385 Wu, W., 677 Wu, X., 635 Wu, Y., 354 Wulfert, E., 318 Wustenberg, S., 389 Wylie, R., 421 Xiao, L., 96 Xiao, Y., 258 Xie, H., 360 Xie, K., 482 Xu, J., 549 Xu, K. M., 530 Xu, M., 166, 167 Yada, A., 482 Yale Center for Emotional Intelligence, 198, 199 Yaman, M. A., 176 Yang, C., 592 Yang, H. H., 460 Yang, J. S., 489 Yang, L., 220 Yang, L.-Y., 393 Yang, S. M., 530 Yang, Y. C., 433 Yap, A. C., 70 Yawkey, T., 189

Yeager, D. S., 498, 530, 531 Yeh, C. J., 258, 259 Yekovich, C. W., 365, 366 Yekovich, F. R., 365, 366 Yell, M. L., 224 Yeung, A. S., 165 Yi, V., 44 Yip, T., 57, 64 Yont, K. M., 250 Yoshikawa, H., 579 Yough, M., 274 Young, J. R., 200 Youngswirth, S. D., 219 Yu, A., 64, 488 Yue, C. L., 382 Yuen, S. D., 270 Yun, S., 455 Zablotsky, B., 6 Zacharia, Z. C., 431 Zacks, J. M., 354 Zamary, A., 352 Zarate, K., 496 Zárate, R. C., 689 Zarrett, N., 487 Zauberman, G., 338 Zee, M., 7, 482, 562 Zeedyk, S., 257 Zehr, H., 587 Zeidner, M., 497, 543 Zelazo, P. D., 173 Zeldin, A. L., 478 Zentall, S. S., 539 Zepeda, C. D., 375, 376, 378, 379, 522 Zerr, C. L., 358 Zervoulis, K., 66 Zettle, R. D., 318 Zhang, J., 637 Zhang, L., 637 Zhang, Q., 385 Zhang, W., 97 Zhang, X., 662 Zhang-James, Y., 215 Zhao, J., 268 Zhao, Y., 451 Zheng, B., 452, 492 Zhong, L., 344 Zhou, M., 44 Zhou, Z., 114, 360 Zhu, W., 139 Ziegler, A., 496 Zilanawala, A., 136 Zimmer, W. K., 344 Zimmerman, B. J., 386, 475, 486, 490, 491 Zirkel, S., 61 Zmuda, J. H., 310 Zohar, A. R., 375, 413 Zongrone, A. D., 73 Zumbrunn, S., 480 Zumbrunn, S. K., 536, 539 Zusho, A., 481, 524, 525 Zwart, R., 431

Subject Index ABAB experimental designs, 22, 24 Ability beliefs about, 477, 526 goals, 523 mindsets about, 530 mindsets in classroom, 530–531 Abolitionist teacher, 77 Absence seizures, 231 Abstract examples/ representations of concepts, 6 cognitive development, 103 cognitive views of learning, 351, 367 culture and diversity, 76 teaching of, 86, 129–130 Abstractions, 86, 115, 129 mindful, 409 Abuse, child, 155–158 Academic caring, 153 Academic language, 260–262, 268 Academic learning time, 561–562 Academic self-concept, 165–167 Acceleration of students, 19, 240 Acceptance, identitydevelopment models, 72 Accommodation, 105, 212, 213 Accountability and high-stakes testing, 688–689 Accountability for teachers, 689, 693–695 Achievement emotions, 541–542 ethnic and racial differences in, 59–61 motivation to learn and, 514 self-concept and, 165–167 student personal and socialcontextual factors, 28–29 teacher expectations and, 612 tests, 685–688 Achieving with Integrity (AwI) seminar, 178 Acquisition phase, learning strategies, 410–411 Acronyms, 363 Action research, 25 Actions, cognitive development and, 115 Active retrieval, 385 Active teaching, 624–625 Activity, Piaget’s theory, 104, 122–124 Adaptation, 104–105 Additive bilingualism, 258

Adolescent egocentrism, 112 Adolescents. See also High school-aged children biracial or multiracial, 57 brain development, 95–96 communities of care, creating, 579–580 in ethnic-racial identity, 57 formal operational stage (Piaget), 111–113 peers, influence of, 148–52 physical development, 136 psychosocial development (Erikson), 160–161 relationships with teachers, 152–154 self-concept, development of, 166–167 sleep and, 96 sociocultural theory (Vygotsky), 120–122 special challenges with, 593 Advance organizers, 429 Aesthetic needs, 513–514 Affective considerations, English learners, 274–278 Affective domain, 13–15, 620 Affinity groups, 455 African American Secretary of Defense, 45 African Americans. See also Diversity, Ethnic and Racial Identity culturally responsive classroom management, 597–600 discrimination, legacy of, 62–64 diversity in learning, 77–79 ethnic and racial identity, 56–64 literacy development, 61 racial inequality, 62 stereotype threat, 45–46 Agency, 477–483. See also Human agency Aggression. See also Bullying peers, influence of, 150 types of, 150–152 Algorithms, 395 Allergies, 232 Ambitious teaching, 615 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (AAIDD), 229 American Educational Research Association, 35

American Psychological Association, 18, 43 American Psychologist, 10 American Sign Language (ASL), 233, 252, 259 Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA), 213 Amotivation, 511 Amygdala, 91 Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis (ALS), 187 Analogical thinking, 396, 402 Analytical intelligence, 200, 204 Analyzing questions, 618–619, 632 Anorexia nervosa, 140 Antecedent–behavior– consequence (A–B–C), 24, 296, 316 Antecedents, 296, 300–301, 316 Anxiety, 529, 541–545 Applied behavior analysis, 291, 302–314, 501 Applying questions, 618–619, 632 Apprenticeships, cognitive, 436–437 Appropriating, 422 Appropriation, cognitive development and, 118, 119 Argument stratagem, 118 Argumentation, 406–408 Aristotle, 293 Arousal, 478, 479 Articulation disorders, 222 Artificial intelligence (AI), 456 Asexual people, 72 Asian Americans. See also Diversity diversity in learning, 77–79 graduation rates, 61 model minority students, 44 stereotype threat, 45–46 Asperger syndrome, 234 Assertive discipline, 596 Assessment, 647. See also Classroom assessments accountability and highstakes testing, 688–689 aligning tools with targets, 672 authentic assessments, 666–668 classroom assessment, overview of, 657–664 communication about, 680, 687–688 complex thinking, 671–672

of creativity, 201–202 essay testing, 660–663 frequent testing, 360, 361, 410, 663. See also Testing effect grading, 673–680 informal assessment, 665–666 interim (growth) assessment, 654, 657, 658, 689 involving students in, 666 learning outcomes and assessment method, 672 of learning styles, 207 measurement and, 653–654 overview, 653 performances and portfolios, 666–668 reliability and validity, 654–657 reports, standardized testing, 685–688 retrieval practice/testing effect, 361–362 score types, 681–684 standardized testing, 660, 681–687 stereotype threat, effects on test performance, 45–46 testing effect, 361–362, 381 from textbooks, 659 types of, 652–653 Assessment bias, 656–657 Assimilation, 105, 115 Assisted learning, 124–125 Assistive technology, 641–642 Asthma, 232 Attachment, 145–146, 159 Attainment value, 526 Attention, 535–545 cognitive learning, 498 complex cognitive processes, 390, 399 executive control process, 375 executive functioning, development of, 115 learning strategies, 379–381, 386 memory and, 335 motivation and, 546–551 multitasking and, 337–338 note taking, 382 observational learning and, 472–477 self-regulation of, 485 teaching and, 338–339 triarchic theory of successful intelligence, 193

S-1

S-2 Subject Index Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) overview of, 219–222 physical activity and, 138–139 Attention disorders, 215–216, 219–222 Attribution theories, 527–530 Attributions and mindsets, 532 Audience, imaginary, 112 Auditory complex, brain, 95, 216 Authentic assessments, 666–668 Authentic instruction, for developing critical thinking skills, 403 Authentic questions, 633 Authentic tasks, 547 complex learning environments and, 425 Authoritarian parents, 144 Autism spectrum disorders, 210, 213, 233–236 Automated basic skills, 365 Automaticity, 193, 199, 337 Autonomy, 159 motivation and, 514–518 need for, 515–518 support of, 548 Availability heuristic, 397 Aversive situations and stimulants, 297, 311 Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, 194 Axons, 93–94 Balanced bilingualism, 258, 268 Bandura, Albert, 469, 484, 490, 501 Basal ganglia, 91 Basic skills, 625 automated, 365 Behavior modification, 291. See also Applied behavior analysis Behavioral approaches, motivation, 512–513 Behavioral disorders, 223–229 Behavioral domain, 15 Behavioral learning theories applied behavior analysis, overview, 302–314 challenges, cautions, and criticisms, 320–324 vs. cognitive view of learning, 330 contiguity and classical conditioning, 293–295, 300 contingency contracts, 306 defined, 291 functional behavioral assessments (FBA), 316–317 group consequences, 310

lessons for teachers, 321 operant conditioning, 296–301, 304–305, 307, 313–314, 318, 319–320 positive behavior, encouraging, 303–306 positive behavior supports (PBS), 317–318 self-management, 318–320 summary chart, 483 Teachers’ Casebook, 288, 325–327 token reinforcement systems, 308–309 undesirable behaviors, response to, 310–314 Behavioral objectives, 616 Being needs, 513–514 Belief perseverance, 398 Beliefs, 470, 477 Belonging needs, 513–514 Belonging-supportive learning environments, 550 Benevolence, 173 Best-works portfolio, 668–669 Betty’s Brain, 453 Bias assessment, 656–657 confirmation, 398 defined, 44 gender differences and intelligence, 204–207 gender roles and, 66–72 performance assessment and, 671 stereotypes, prejudice, discrimination, 43–46 Bias-free language, 43 Biculturalism, 165, 259 Bilingual education and English learners, 270–274 Bilingualism, 255–260. See also English learners (ELs) balanced, 265, 268 benefits of, 257–258 definition, 256 gifted and talented students and, 282 heritage language, 258–259 limited bilingual students, 268 loss of language, 258–259 myths and misconceptions about, 259–260 needs and gifts, 281–283 research on bilingual education, 270–274 two-way immersion, 272–273 Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation, 694 Bill of Rights, classroom, 566–567 Binet, Alfred, 194 Binge eating disorder, 140

Bioecological model of development (Bronfenbrenner), 142–156 Biologically primary abilities, 393 Biologically secondary skill, 393 Biology, effect on development, 89 Bisexuals, 65, 72 Black Lives Matter, 34, 37, 59, 447 Black racial identity, 58 Blended families, 144 Blending classroom, 460–461 Bloom, Benjamin, 619, 632 Blue Lives Matter, 34 Bodily-kinesthetic intelligence, 191–192 Body image, 140–141 Body movement, cerebral cortex, 95 Body sensation, cerebral cortex, 95 Bottom-up processing, 335 Boys. See Males Brain adolescent development of, 95–96 behavioral learning theory, neuroscience of, 298–299 bilingualism and, 257 cerebral cortex, functional areas, 95 cognitive development and, 91–102 cognitive learning and, 330–331 common myths, 98–99 developing brain, 91–96, 198 emotion and, 540–543 forgetting, memory and, 357 imaging techniques, 91–92 instruction affecting development, 97 learning and teaching implications, 95–101 myelination of, 190 neuroscience of learning challenges, 215–216 observational learning, 394 plasticity, 93, 97 reading, learning and, 99–101 regions of, 91 retrieval practice/testing effect, 385 stem, 91 stress hormones, effect on, 52 Brainstorming, 377, 570 Bronfenbrenner, Urie, 142–156 bioecological model of development, 142–144 Brown, Linda, 62 Brown v. Board of Education of Topeka (1954), 62

Bulimia nervosa, 140 Bullying, 588–592 cyberbullying. See Cyber aggression (bullying) Teachers’ Casebook, 556, 603–605 types of, 181, 590 zero tolerance, 585–586 Burnout, 482, 494, 496 Canvas, 453 CAPS, 386 CAPTCHAs (Completely Automated Public Turing test to tell Computers and Humans Apart), 335 Career decisions, practical intelligence and, 201 Caring relationships, 578–581 Case studies, 22, 23 Caste, 49. See also Social class. CAT (computerized axial tomography) scans, 91, 92 Catastrophe theory, 114 CBITS (Cognitive Behavioral Intervention for Trauma in Schools), 276 Centering, 107 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 72, 137 Central executive, 340–341 Central tendency, 682–683 Cerebellum, 91, 216 Cerebral cortex, 95 Cerebral palsy, 230 Cerebrum, 91 Chain mnemonics, 363 Change, discontinuous, 90, 113–114 Cheating, 176–178 Check & Connect program, 580 Checklists, software appropriate for young children, 458 Child abuse, 155–156 Child-directed speech (CDS), 250 Childhood obesity, 139 Children. See also Adolescents; Infants brain development, 95–96 peers, influence of, 148–152 preoperational stage of development (Piaget), 107–108 theory of cognitive development (Piaget), 103–115 Chinese learners brain activity research, 97 cognitive ability, emphasis on, 114 Choice, support of, 516 Chunk and chew, 277 Chunking, 342

Subject Index Cisgender, 65 Clarity, 605 Class, social, 46–54 CLASS (Classroom Assessment Scoring System) model, 13–15 Class inclusion, 122 Classical conditioning, 294–295, 350 Classification, 109, 110 Classroom assessments, 653–673 authentic, 666–668 complex thinking, 671–672 essay testing, 660–663 formative, 665–666 lessons for teachers, 672–673 selected-response testing, 659–660 testing, 657–664 from textbooks, 659 Classroom environment authentic tasks and, 425 complex learning environments, 425 constructivism and interactive learning, 418–465 culturally welcoming classrooms, creation of, 75–82 digital media and learning, 451–462 encouraging engagement, 574–575 Framework for Teaching, 12–13 goals in social context, 516 learning spaces, planning for, 572–573 motivation, strategies for, 546–551 quick response to problems, 582–583 routines and rules, 563–567 self-determination, support for, 515 student anxiety, reducing, 542–543 triadic reciprocal causality, 470–472 Classroom goal structure, 524 Classroom organization, 15 Classroom organization and management, 552–595 basic goals, 560–562 bullying. See Bullying caring relationships, 578–581 communication, need for, 594–597 culturally responsive management, 597–601 first weeks of class, 572–573

for in-person learning, 563–564 for remote learning, 567–569 handling classroom disruptions, 310–314 high school students, special challenges with, 593–594 learning spaces, planning for, 570–572 problem prevention, 575–577 research on, 563 routines and rules, 563–567` zero tolerance policies, debate about, 585–586 Clinical interviews, 22–23 Cliques, 148 Cloud-based applications, 452 Cmaps, 383 Co-constructed processes, 117 Co-regulation of learning, 441, 490, 496 Coactions, 89 Code switching, 264 Coding, computational thinking and, 456–457 Cognitive apprenticeships, 436–437 Cognitive approaches, motivation, 512, 524 Cognitive-centered learning styles, 207–208 Cognitive constructivism, 421–422 Cognitive development, 88–89. See also Sociocultural theory (Vygotsky); Theory of cognitive development (Piaget) brain and, 91–102 cultural tools and, 118–119 culture and, 114 developing brain, 91–102 influences on, 104 learning and, 122 Piaget’s theories, 103–115, 122–127 regions of brain, 91 stages of, 105–113 Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory, 116–122, 124–125 Cognitive disabilities. See Intellectual disabilities Cognitive domain, 13–14, 619, 632 Cognitive function, brain regions, 91 Cognitive impairment. See Intellectual disabilities Cognitive learning theory, summary chart, 502 Cognitive load, 343–346, 392 Cognitive monitoring, 381 Cognitive needs, 513–514

Cognitive processes critical thinking and argumentation, 403–408 learning strategies, 379–387 metacognition, 375–379 neuroscience and, 215–216 problem solving, 388–402 transfer of learning, 408–411 Cognitive science, 330 Cognitive view of learning, 330 vs. behavioral view, 330 cognitive load and retaining information, 343–346 constructing declarative knowledge, 359–362 explicit memory, 349–355 implicit memory, 349, 355–356 information processing system, 333 knowledge, role of, 331–332 long-term memory, 349–358 procedural knowledge, development of, 365–367 working memory, 340–347 Cold cognition, 198 Collaboration, 438, 496, 500 cooperation and, 438–450 Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning (CASEL), 9, 154 Collaborative learning, 124, 439 computer-supported, 452 Collective monologue, 120 College-aged students, formal operational stage (Piaget), 111–113 Commitment, 162 Common Core Standards, 617–618 Common sense, questions of in research, 18–20 Communication, by teachers of appropriate expectations, 613–615 classroom organization and management, 594–597 of grades and progress, 677 Communication language, 260–262 Communication skills, students autism spectrum disorders, 233–234 diversity in learning, 77 Community of practice, 424 Community partnerships, 108, 145–146, 211, 280, 319, 360, 411, 550–551, 600, 630, 687–688 Comparative organizers, 429 Compensation, 109 Competence, need for, 515–518 Competitive goal framing, 521 Complementary methods, research, 23

S-3

Complex learning environments, 425 Complex tasks, self-regulation and, 496–497 Complex thinking, assessing, 671–672 Comprehension, 217 learning strategies for comprehending, 381 Computational thinking, 456–457 Computerized axial tomography (CAT) scans, 91, 92 Computers, use in learning, 451–461 Concept, explicit memory and, 349–355 Concept maps, 384–385 Conceptually-driven knowledge, 335 Concrete examples/ representations of concepts cognitive development, 101, 102, 106, 108, 110–111, 114 cognitive views of learning, 367 complex cognitive processes, 380 learning differences and needs, 262 Concrete-operational stage (Piaget), 106, 108–109 Concrete operations, defined, 108 Conditional knowledge, 378 Conditioned response (CR), 294 Conditioned stimulus (CS), 294 Conferences student-led, English learners and, 280–281 tips for, 211–212, 677–680, 687–688 Confidence interval, 655 Confirmation bias, 398 Confrontations, 597 Connected knowledge, 412 Connections with school, 578–581 Consequences, 296. See also Discipline classroom rules violations, 566–567 communication, need for, 594–597 logical, 566, 578 quick response to problems, 582–583 Conservation, 107, 109 Construct-related evidence of validity, 656 Constructive controversies, 447–448

S-4 Subject Index Constructivism, 418–465 cognitive apprenticeships, 436–437 cognitive constructivism, 421–422 collaboration and cooperation, 438–423 cooperative learning, 440–442. See also Groups defined, 421 designing learning environments and, 427–437 digital media, learning and, 451–461 dilemmas of, 461 inquiry-based learning, 431–436 instructional planning and, 620–621 learner-centered teaching, 424–427 learning, constructivist views of, 421–423 learning theories, summary chart, 502 social constructivism, 422–423 Constructivist approach. See Constructivism Constructivist practice, dilemmas of, 461 Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23 Content-related evidence of validity, 656 Context, 142, 361 Contiguity, 293–295 Contingency contracts, 306 Contingency support, 428 Continuous change, 90, 113 Continuous reinforcement schedule, 299 Control, attribution theories and, 528–529 Control and agency, teachers sharing with students, 497–498 Controlling communication, motivation and, 516 Controversial children, 149 Controversies, constructive/ structured, 447–448 Convergent questions, 632 Convergent thinking, 203 Cooperation, 438 among students and teacher, 559–562 Cooperative goal framing, 521 Cooperative learning. See also Groups collaboration and, 438–450 designing tasks, 442–444 effects of, 442

preparing students for, 444 students’ role in, 445 teacher’s role in, 442 techniques, 446–448 within-class and flexible grouping, 639–641 Coping Power Program, 498 Coronavirus pandemic, 10, 45. See also COVID-19 pandemic Corpus callosum, 91 Correlation studies, 20, 24 Costs, expectancy, and value, in motivation, 525–527 Coverage-focused teaching, 618 COVID-19 pandemic, 4, 7, 172, 227 digital learning, 451 lack of Internet connection, 53–54 social and economic inequality, 49 stress for teacher on, 494 students, mental health issues, 228 technology, 492 vaccine, 459 virtual learning environments (VLEs), 453 Creating questions, 618–620, 632 Creative intelligence, 204 Creativity, 200–207 Creativity-relevant process, 203 Criterion-referenced grading, 673–680 Criterion-referenced testing, 657–659 Criterion-related evidence of validity, 656 Critical periods in development, 90 second-language learning, 257 Critical thinking, 403–406 online sources, 405–406 teaching, 403–405 Cross-sectional studies, 23 Crowds, peer groups, 148 Crystallized intelligence, 190 Cueing, 301 Cultivating Awareness and Resilience in Education (CARE) program, 496 Cultural context crystallized intelligence, 190 pragmatics, 252 Cultural database, 37, 39 Cultural deficit model, 267 Cultural groups, 42–43 Cultural Identity Wheel, 42 Cultural tools, 118–119 Culturally relevant pedagogy, 75–77, 80

Culturally responsive classroom management, 597–601 Culture academic language proficiency, 260–262 brain plasticity and, 97 caring relationships and school connections, 578 cognitive development and, 114–115 culturally welcoming classrooms, creation of, 75–82 Deaf community, 233 definition, 37 dialect differences, 263–264 discontinuity, 79 diversity in learning, 77–79 economic and social class differences, 47–55 and educational history, 37–40 ethnicity and race, defining, 56–57 example student profiles, 41 funds of knowledge, 279 gender roles and bias, 66–72 iceberg model, 38–39 identity, ethnic and racial, 56–64 immigrant students, teaching of, 266–269 intersections and terminology, 42–43 language development, 249–256 language diversity and emergent literacy, 254–255 lessons for teachers, 79–82 moral development theory, 171–173 moral reasoning, differences in, 175–176 multicultural education, overview, 75–82 parenting and, 145 peer cultures, 148 play, differences in, 138 racial inequality, legacy of, 62–64 school achievement, ethnic and racial differences, 59–61 self-concept and, 166–167 sociocultural theory (Vygotsky), 116–122, 422–423 stereotypes, prejudice, discrimination, 43–46 stereotype threat, 45–46 terminology, 42–43

values and learning preferences, 77–78 Curiosity, motivation and, 536–539 Curiosity value, 548 Curriculum compacting, 240 Curriculum examples, Tools of the Mind, 125 Cyber aggression (bullying), 150, 588, 592 dealing with, 592 described, 588 Cyberbullying, 592 Danielson’s Framework for Teaching, 12–13 Data-driven processing, 335 Death by suicide, 227 Decay in memory, 345 Decentering, 107 Declarative knowledge, 332, 359–362, 365, 375 Declarative memory, 349–355 Decoding skills, 254 Deductive skills, 111 Deep knowledge, 412 Deep poverty, 54 Deep questions cognitive development, 113 development, 103, 114 facilitating through asking and answering, 429–431 teaching every student, 632, 634 Defend Innocence project, 155 Deficiency needs, 513–514 Defining attribute, 351 Delayed gratification, 487 Deliberate practice, 400 cognitive development, 353 cognitive views of learning, 365, 367 complex cognitive processes, 400 social cognitive views of learning and motivation, 473, 487 teaching every student, 593 Deliberative argumentation, 407 Deliberative-collaborative argumentation, 407–408 Dendrites, 93 Depth of processing theory, 345 Descriptions, combining verbal and graphic descriptions, 350 Descriptive studies, 20 Design-based research, 26 Design experiments, 609 Designed differentiation, 639 Designing class spaces, 570–572 Desirable difficulty, 359, 361

Subject Index Development, 88. See also Bioecological model of development (Bronfenbrenner); Theory of cognitive development (Piaget) appropriate computer activities, 457–458 argumentation skills, 407 constructivist view of learning, 418–462 definition of, 88–90 general principles of, 90 instruction affecting brain development, 97 language developmental milestones, 249–253 metacognition and, 376–379 moral development, 170–178 observational learning and, 472, 473 physical development, 134–141 psychosocial development (Erikson), 158–165 self-concept, 165 self-regulation and, 488–489 theories, 89–90 working memory, 345–348 Developmental crisis, 159 Developmental disability. See Intellectual disabilities; Physical disabilities Developmentally appropriate practice, 123 Deviation IQ, 195 Dewey, John, 16, 421, 431, 439, 537 Diabetes, 232 Dialects, 263–264 Dialogue for developing critical thinking skills, 403 as teaching method, 631–635 Differential reinforcement, 302 Differentiated instruction, 639–643 Differentiation, 540 Digital age, cognitive development and, 119 Digital citizenship, digital literacy, media literacy, 457–460 Digital learning environments, 451–461 Direct instruction, 218, 624–627, 643 Direct reinforcement, 474 Directives instruction manner, 379 Disabilities defined, 187 intellectual. See Intellectual disabilities

learning. See Learning disabilities; Specific learning disabilities (SLD) physical. See Physical disabilities Disability Etiquette, 187–188 Discipline assertive discipline, 596 beliefs about, 557–559 bullying and cyberbullying, 588 communication, need for, 594–597 consequences to rule violations, 566 culturally responsive classroom management, 597–601 discipline without stress® teaching model, 589 high school students, special challenges with, 593 problem prevention, 575–577 quick response to problems, 582–583 zero tolerance policies, 585–586 Discipline without Stress Punishments or Rewards, 588, 589 Discontinuous change, 113 Discrimination, 46 gender identity and sexual orientation, 73–74 racism and, 63–64 Discussion, as teaching method, 631–635 Disequilibrium, 105 Disordered eating, 139–141 Disputative argumentation, 407 Disruptive behavior, ignoring of, 303, 311, 317. See also Classroom organization and management Distractors, multiple choice questions, 660 Distributed learning/practice cognitive development, 99, 103 cognitive view of learning, 360, 364 complex cognitive processes, 386 teaching every student, 644 Distributive justice, 173 Divergent questions, 632 Divergent thinking, 203 Diversity assessment bias, 656–657, 671 caring relationships and school connections, 579 creativity and, 204 culturally responsive classroom management, 597–601

culturally welcoming classrooms, creation of, 75–83 economic and social class differences, 47–54 educational, overview, 37–38 emergent literacy and language, 253–254 ethnicity and race, 56–63 example student profiles, 41 gender identity and sexual orientation, 64–73 gender roles and bias, 66–72 identity, ethnic and racial, 56–64 immigrant students, teaching of, 266–281 in learning, 76–79 indigenous communities, 57–58, 77–78, 249, 436 intelligence, differences in, 197–198 interpreting cultural differences, 37–43 in learning, 77–79 moral reasoning, 175–176 multicultural education, overview, 75 parenting styles, 144 racial inequality, legacy of, 62–64 school achievement, ethnic and racial differences, 59–61 stereotype threat, 45–46 Division of Adolescent and School Health, 72 Divorce, 146 Domain general frame, 378 Domain-relevant skills, 203 Domain-specific knowledge cognitive views of learning, 332 vs. coherent knowledge, 412 complex cognitive processes, 389, 392, 395 Point/Counterpoint, 433 Domain-specific strategies, 365 Dominican Americans, 58 Dopamine receptors, 190, 299 Dow, Whitney, 59 Down Syndrome, 229 Drawing strategies, 384–385 Drug use and abuse, 227–228 Dual coding theory, 350 Dual-language development, 256–259. See also Bilingualism; English learners (ELs) Dyslexia, 215 E-journals, 277 Ear learners, 269

S-5

Eating disorders, See Disordered eating Economic factors. See Socioeconomic status (SES) Education is cultural, 37–38 current trends in, 4–11 Education Week, 12 Educational autobiography, 39–40 Educational debt, achievement gap and, 61 Educational history, cultural and, 39–43 Educational Psychologist, 9 Educational psychology, 16–18, 24–25 research in, 18–26 theories in, 26–27 EEG (electroencephalograph), 91, 92 Effective instruction delivery (EID), 301 Effective practice, 359 Efficacy, sense of, 482 Ego goals, 523 Egocentric, defined, 107 Egocentric speech, 120 Egocentrism, adolescent, 112 Elaborated goals, 520 Elaboration, 359–360 as executive control process, 375 Elaborative rehearsal, 345 Electroencephalograph (EEG), 91, 92 Elementary aged children classroom rules for, 565 concrete-operational stage (Piaget), 107–109 first weeks of class, 572–573 in-person teaching, rules for, 565 physical development, 135 preoperational stage of development (Piaget), 106–107 psychosocial development (Erikson), 158–165 relationships with teachers, 153 self-concept, development of, 165 sociocultural theory (Vygotsky), 120 teacher–student interactions in classroom, 14–15 Embodied cognition, 393–394 Emergent literacy, 253–256 Emotion-focused strategies, 543 Emotional arousal, 479 Emotional disorders, 187, 223–229

S-6 Subject Index Emotional disturbance, Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA), 223 Emotional intelligence (EI), 198–200 Emotional learning, 493–494 Emotional support, 13–14 Emotions, 540–542. See also Motivation achievement emotions, 541–542 adolescent brain development, 95–96 amygdala, 91 anxiety, 529, 541–546 brain regions for, 94–95 brain research and, 98–99 in classroom, 542 emotional intelligence, 493 English learners, 276, 266 negative, 543–545 observational learning and, 475–476 peer popularity and rejection, 149 physical development, 136 relatedness, need for, 515–518 self-concept and, 165–167 self-regulation of emotions, 493–496 social goals, 524–525 teacher burnout, 482, 494, 496 Empathetic listening, 595–596 Empirical, defined, 25 EMR model, 578–579 Enactive learning, 320, 470 Encounter stage of nigrescence, 58 Encouraging positive behaviors, 306, 307 Enculturation, 424 Engaged time, 561, 574 Engagement, encouragement of, 574 Engineering, 67. See also STEM (science, technology, engineering and mathematics) fields English as a second language (ESL), 270. See also English learners (ELs) English for Speakers of Other Languages (ESOL), 267, 270. See also English learners (ELs) English language learners (ELLs), 283. See also English learners (ELs) English learners (ELs), 268–269 bilingual education and, 270–274 cooperative learning and, 449 giftedness, recognizing, 282

immigrant students, 266–269 learners with disabilities, 281–282 standardized testing and, 8 structured English immersion (SEI), 270 teaching approaches, 270 trends in education, 6–7 Enrichment, 240 Enthusiasm, teacher, 609 Environment, classroom. See Classroom environment; Classroom organization and management Environment, influence of. See also Bioecological model of development (Bronfenbrenner) adaptations to, 104 effect on development, 89 intelligence, 135 practical intelligence, 193 social intuitionist model, moral development, 175–176 student achievement, 51–52 Epilepsy, 231 Episodic buffer, 340, 342 Episodic memory, 354 Equilibration, 105 Equity pedagogy, 75 Erikson, Erik, 158–165 Erin’s Law, 155 ERP (event-related potential), 91, 92 ESL (English as second language), 416 ESSA (Every Student Succeeds Act), 8, 12, 22 Essay testing, 660–663 Esteem needs, 514 Ethic of care, 172 Ethical issues, behavioral learning theory, 292 Ethnic-racial identity, 57 development, 58–59 multidimensional and flexible, 57 Ethnic minority groups, 57, 59, 63, 81 Ethnicity assessment bias, 656–657, 671 caring relationships and school connections, 578 culturally relevant pedagogy, 75–77 culturally responsive classroom management, 594–601 culturally welcoming classrooms, creation of, 75–82 defining, 56–57 diversity in learning, 77–79

identity, 57–59 immigrant students and English learners, teaching of, 266–269 intelligence, differences in, 197–198 moral reasoning, 171 motivation and, 532 parenting styles, 144 poverty rates, 49 pride, racial and ethnic, 165 racial inequality, legacy of, 62–64 school achievement and, 59–61 self-concept, ideas of, 165 stereotype threat, 45–46 trends in education, 6–7 Ethnography, 23 Etiquette, disabilities and, 187–188 Evaluating online resources, 406 Evaluating questions, 619, 632 Evaluating skills, metacognition and, 277, 378 Event-related potential (ERP), 91, 92 Every Student Succeeds Act (ESSA), 8, 12, 20 Evidence-based practices, 22 Examples. See also Abstract examples/representations of concepts; Concrete examples/representations of concepts; Worked examples problem solving and, 392–393 Exceptional students, 187 Executive control processes, 375–379 Executive functioning, 115 Executive processes, triarchic theory of intelligence, 193 Exemplar, 351 Exercise, 138–139 Exit tickets, 665 Expectancies, values, and costs, 525–527 Expectancy for success, 526 Expectancy × value theories, 525 Expectations, 610–615 academic self-concept and, 52 for students, 193, 470–471 triadic reciprocal causality, 470–471 Experience-dependent neural changes, 94 Experience-expectant neural changes, 93 Experimentation, 21, 23 Expert knowledge, 399–402 Expert reversal effect, 392

Expert teachers, 610 Explicit memory, 349–355 Explicit teaching, 624–627, 637 Exploration, 162 Expository organizers, 429 Expressive vocabulary, 251–252 Extended families, 144 External comparisons, selfconcept development, 166 Extinction, 300 Extraneous cognitive load, 343 Extrinsic motivation, 511–512 Facilitation constructivist classrooms, deep learning, 427–431 deep questions, asking and answering, 429–431 Fading process with scaffolding, 428 Failing, value of, 674–675 Failure avoidance, 531 False growth mindset, 532 Families bioecological model of development, 142–146 classroom management, communication about, 594–597 conferences with, 211, 680, 687, 688 connecting with families, 146 English language learners, working with, 278–280 preoperational children, caring for, 108 Family and community partnerships, motivation to learn, 108, 145, 211, 282, 319, 360, 411, 551, 600–601, 630, 687–688 Feedback, 11, 672–673, 674–675, 694 cognitive development, 102, 124 complex cognitive processes, 386, 412 constructivism, 423, 433, 435, 436, 441, 442, 450, 451, 458 culture and diversity, 68, 69, 81, 310 development and, 164, 169 goals and, 521–522 helpful feedback, 634 language development, 256, 260, 262, 274 learning differences and needs, 218 managing learning environments, 575, 577, 581 motivation in learning and teaching, 517, 531, 539, 548, 550 praise-and-ignore approach, 303

Subject Index responding to student answers, 634 social cognitive views, 470, 474, 479, 496, 497, 500 student attributions and, 529 teaching every student, 613, 624–625, 631, 634–635 triadic reciprocal causality, 470–472 Feeling different/uncertain, identity-development models, 72 Females ADHD diagnoses, 219 assessment bias, 656–657 gender and intelligence, 135 gender roles and bias, 66–72 genderlects, 264 gifted students, identifying, 237 moral development, 172 self-concept, 165 stereotype threat, 45–46 Field experiments, 21 Fill-in-the-blank items, 659–660 Fine-motor skills, 135 Finger spelling, 233 Fixed-interval reinforcement schedule, 299 Fixed mindset, 167, 530 Fixed-ratio reinforcement schedule, 299 Flashbulb memories, 354 Flatt, Clark, 227 Flatt, Jason, 227 Flexibility in thinking, creativity, 202 Flexible grouping, 639–641 Flexible knowledge, 414 Flipped classroom, 460 Flow, 539–540 Floyd, George, 63 Fluency disorder, 222 Fluid intelligence, 190 Flynn effect, 195–196 fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging), 91, 92 Focal onset seizures, 231 Focus, executive functioning, 115 Focusing on one dimension, preoperational thinking, 107 Forgetting, memory and, 345, 357 Formal operational stage of development, 106, 111–112 Formative assessment, 654, 657–658, 665–673, 677, 678, 689 constructivism, 436 Formative testing, 272 Framework for Teaching, 12–13 Framing, 379

Free, appropriate public education (FAPE), 209 Frequent testing, 663. See also Testing effect cognitive views of learning, 360 complex cognitive processes, 410 retrieval practice, 361–362 Freud, Sigmund, 158 Friendships, 148–149 Frontal lobe, brain, 91, 95, 216 Functional behavioral assessments (FBA), 316–317 Functional fixedness, 397 Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), 91, 92 Funds of knowledge, 279 g (general intelligence), 190 g (mental energy), 190 Games, 455 Gardner, Howard, 190–192, 204 Gates Foundation, 694 Gay students, 72–74 Gender ADHD diagnoses, 219 assessment bias, 656–657 expression, 65 gifted students, identifying, 237 intelligence, differences in, 197–198 moral development, 172 substance use disorder, 227–228 Gender biases, 67–72 Gender identity, 65, 73–74 Gender roles, 66–67 Gender schemas, 66–67 Genderlects, 264 General intelligence (g), 190 General knowledge, 332 General learning disability. See Intellectual disabilities; Learning disabilities; Specific learning disabilities (SLD) Generalized onset seizures, 231 Generalized research findings, 24 Generation 1.5, 269–281 Generativity, 164 Genetics effect on development, 89, 104 intelligence and, 198 Germane cognitive load, 343 Gestalt, 335–336 Gifted and talented students cooperative learning, use of, 448–450 English learners and, 281, 284 identification of, 238–239, 282

overview, 236–240 racial and ethnic discrimination and, 64 research on, 236–238 teaching, 240 Girls. See Females Glial cells, 94 Global self-esteem, 165, 168 GLSEN Safe Space Kit, 74 Goal-directed actions, 106 Goal-directed attention, 335 Goal orientations, 519–525 contextual influences on, 524 performance, 523–524 Goals, 486, 519–525 achievement emotions, 540 cooperative vs. competitive, 521–522 framing, 521 goal-setting, observational learning, 472, 481, 492 learning, 616–618 self-management and goalsetting, 318–319 setting, 520 structures, 521 work avoidance, 524 Good behavior game, 309 Google Apps, 451 Google documents, 452 Grade-equivalent scores, 683 Grade retention, 674–677 Grading, 673–680 on a curve, 673–674 effects on studentsmotivation, 674 guidelines for, 678–679 norm-referenced vs. criterionreferenced, 673–674 Gradual change in human development, 90 Grammar skills, developmental milestones, 252 Gray matter, 92 Grit, 487–89 Gross-motor skills, 135 Group consequences, 309 Group discussion, 634–637 Group focus in classroom management, 576 Group work, goals of, 439 Groups assigning roles in cooperative groups, 445–446 collaboration and cooperation, 438–442 composition, 444 designing cooperative learning tasks, 442–444 learning in, 439–440 misuses of, 439–440 preparing students for, 444–445 setting up, 444–446

S-7

within-class and flexible grouping, 639–641 Growth assessment, 664, 689 Growth mindset, 530–532 Guided participation, 436 Guidelines adolescents, positive body images, 141 aggression, dealing with, 152 anxiety, coping with test, 544 attention, gaining and maintaining, 339 becoming expert students, 400–401 caring relationships, 580–581 children of divorce, helping, 146 class routines for in-person learning, 564 classical conditioning, application of, 295 classroom organization and management, 600–601 concrete-operational stage, teaching tips, 110–111 conferences, productive, 211–212 conferences, tips for, 687–688 connecting with families, 146 cooperative learning, 449 creativity, applying and encouraging, 205–206 culturally relevant teaching, 80 deep questions, facilitating through asking and answering, 430 direct instruction, 626–627 disciplining students with emotional problems, 225 emotional self-regulation, 495 English learners, support for, 278 explosive situations, handling of, 593–594 eyewitness testimony, interviewing students and, 355 family and community partnerships, 280, 594–595 flexible grouping, 640–641 formal operational stage of development, 113 gender bias, avoiding, 69 grading systems, 678–679 group discussions, 636–637 healthy mindset, encouraging, 533 helping students understand and remember, 366–367 homework, 630–631 identity formation, supporting, 164

S-8 Subject Index Guidelines (Continued) initiative and industry, encouraging, 160 intellectual disabilities, students with, 29 interest and curiosity, building on, 538–539 IQ scores, interpreting, 194 language and literacy promotion, 255–256 language learning, promotion of, 261 learning objectives, 621 learning space, designing of, 571–572 media literacy development, 459–460 motivation, sociocultural foundations of, 551 observational learning, use of, 476 organizing learning, 360 penalties, imposing of, 584–585 physical differences in classroom, 135–136 portfolios, creating, 669–670 positive behaviors, encouraging, 307 poverty, teaching students who live in, 55 praise, appropriate use of, 304–305 preoperational children, caring for, 108 preparing for testing, 691–692 problem solving skill development, 398–399 punishment, use of, 313–314 remote learning, routines and rules for, 567–568 rubrics, development of, 671 self-determination and autonomy, supporting, 516 self-efficacy, supporting, 481 self-management, student, 319–320 standardized test results, explaining, 687–688 student engagement, 575 teacher expectations, 614–615 transfer of learning, 411–412 universal design for learning (UDL), 642–643 Vygotsky’s theory, applying in teaching, 126 welcoming all families, 280 writing objective test items, 661–662 Guskey, Thomas, 674, 668 Haidt, Jonathan, 175–176 Hall, G. Stanley, 18 Handicap as opposed to a disability, 187

Hawking, Stephen, 187 Headline writing, 277 Health, student achievement and, 51–52 Hearing impairments, 233 Heredity, intelligence and, 198 Heritage language, 258–259 Heuristics, 395–396 Hierarchy of needs, 513–514 High functioning autism spectrum disorder and cautions in the use of the term, 233–234 High-Leverage Teaching Practices, TeachingWorks, 14 High school-aged children. See also Adolescents classroom rules for, 565–566 effective managers of, 573 first weeks of class, 573 formal operational stage (Piaget), 111–112 in-person teaching, rules for, 565–566 peers, influence of, 148–152 physical development, 136 psychosocial development (Erikson), 159–162 relationships with teachers, 153 self-concept, development of, 166 special challenges with, 593 transition to high school, caring communities, 579–580 High-stakes testing, 688–689 Higher-order thinking, 189 Highlighting, as learning strategy, 381–382 Highly mobile students, 54 Highly structured, review, and skill-building tasks, 443–445 Hippocampus, 91 Hispanic students. See Latina/o Americans Home environment. See Environment, influence of Homelessness, 54 Homework, 625, 628–631 Homosexual students, 65 Hostile aggression, 150 Housekeeping procedures in classroom management, 563, 570 Human agency, 477 Hybrid instruction, 463 Hybrid learning, 4 Hyperactive child, 219 Hyperactivity disorders, 149, 217, 219–221 Hypothalamus, 91

Hypothesis/hypotheses, definition, 26 Hypothetico-deductive reasoning, 111 “I” message, 596 Identity, 109, 160 defined, 162 psychosocial development (Erikson), 158–165 technology and, 163 Identity achievement, 162 Identity-development models, 72 Identity diffusion, 162 Identity-first language, 188–189 Identity-first reference, 189 Identity foreclosure, 162 IEP (Individualized Education Program), 210, 230 IES Practice Guides, 22, 352, 364, 384, 427–429, 633 Ignoring behaviors, 300, 303, 323 Illegal alien, 43 Illusion of understanding, 208 Imagery, meaningful connections, 360–361 Images, 350 Imaginary audience, 112 Imaging techniques, brain, 91–92 Imitative learning, 124 Immersion/emersion stage of nigrescence, 58 Immersion strategies structured English immersion (SEI), 270 two-way immersion, 272–274 Immersive virtual learning environment (IVLE), 454 Immigrant students, teaching of, 266–269 Immigrants, 266 Impairment, 187 Implicit memory, 349, 355–356 Importance value, 526 Impulse control adolescents, 96 executive functioning, development of, 115 psychosocial development (Erikson), 159 Tools of the Mind under Impulse control, 125 In-group prejudices, 44–45 Incentives, 511–512, 525, 548 Inclusion, 210 Inclusive classrooms, differentiated instruction, 641–642 Inclusive opportunities, 550–551 Inclusive playgrounds, 139

Independent work, 627 Indicators in learning standards, 616 Indigenous communities, 49, 57–58, 77, 78, 252, 436 Individual interests, 535–536 Individualistic goal framing, 521 Individualized Education Program (IEP), 210, 230 Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA), 209–212, 216 assistive technology, 641–642 autism spectrum, 233–234 components and provisions, 209–211 emotional and behavioral disorders, 223–224 emotional disturbance, defined, 223 positive behavior supports, 317–318 response to intervention (RTI), 213–214 Inductive reasoning, 111 Industry (Erikson), 159–162 Infants brain development, 91–96 language development, 250–251 psychosocial development (Erikson), 160 sensorimotor stage (Piaget), 106 theory of cognitive development (Piaget), 106 Inferiority, psychosocial development (Erikson), 159–162 Inferring relationships, 111 Informal assessments, 665–666 Information, motivation and, 516 Information processing, 334–334 Information retrieval, memory, 356–357 Initiative (Erikson), 159 Inquiry learning, 431–436 Insight, 193, 376 Instructed learning, 124 Instruction. See Teaching Instructional manner in teacher metacognitive support, 379 Instructional objectives, 616 Instructional reasoning, 610 Instructional support, 13–15 Instructional time, 561 Instrumental aggression, 150 Instrumental value tasks, 547–548 Integration, 210, 540 Integrity (Erikson), 164

Subject Index Intellectual disabilities, 229. See also Learning disabilities behavioral views of learning, 317 learner differences and learning needs, 229–230 Intelligence cultural influences and, 38 defining, 189–190 gifted and talented students, 236–240 measures of, 194–197 neuroscience and, 194 as process, 193 sex differences, 197–198 student beliefs about, 530 Intelligence quotient (IQ), 194–197 academic achievement and, 196–197 Interactional differentiation, 639 Interest areas/learning spaces, classroom planning, 570–572 Interest value, 526, 548 Interests, motivation and, 535–536 Interference in forgetting, 345–346 Interim (growth or benchmark) assessment, 654, 657, 658, 689 Interleaved practice, 362 Intermittent reinforcement schedule, 299 Internal comparisons, selfconcept development, 166 Internalization and internalizationcommitment stages of nigrescence, 58 Internalize (moral rules and principles), 176 International Mind, Brain, and Education Society, 98 Interpersonal intelligence, 190–192 Intersectionality, 42 Interval schedule, 299 Interventions, autism spectrum disorders, 234 Intimacy (Erikson), 163–164 Intrapersonal intelligence, 198–199 Intrinsic cognitive load, 343 Intrinsic motivation, 511–512 Intrinsic task motivation, 203 Intrinsic value, 526, 548 Investigative listener, 445 IRF (initiation, response, follow-up), 631 Irving, John, 201, 203 James, William, 18, 39

Jason Flatt Act, 227 Jigsaw classroom, 446, 450 Joint attention speech, 250 Journal of Educational Psychology, 18, 99 Journals, 277, 665–666 Juvenile diabetes, 232 Keyword method, 363–364 Khan, Salman, 460 Kinesthetic learning styles, 207 Knowledge cognition and, 331–332 construction of, internal and external processes, 423 construction of, teaching implications, 123–124 constructivist views of learning, 421–423 executive functioning, development of, 115 expert knowledge and problem solving, 399–402 expert teachers, 610 making meaningful connections, 359–362 metacognition and, 375 pedagogical content knowledge (PCK), 610 self-regulation and, 585–586 situated and general knowledge, 423–424 Knowledge acquisition components, intelligence, 193 Knowledge construction process, 75 Kohlberg, Lawrence, 171–173 KWL (know, want, learn) strategy, 376–377, 385 Labeling for students, 187–189 possible bias in application of, 189 Laboratory School, University of Chicago, 18 Language development. See also Bilingualism; English learners (ELs) academic language, 260–269 autism spectrum disorders, 233–234 brain plasticity and, 97 brain regions for, 95 communication and academic language, 260–261 cultural differences, 249–283 developmental milestones, 249–251 diversity in, 254–262. See also Language diversity dual-language development, 256–259 emergent literacy, 253–254

encouraging, 223 preoperational stage of development, 107–108 second-language learning, 257 sex differences, 197–198 signed language, 259 sociocultural theory (Vygotsky) on, 119–120 theory of multiple intelligences, 190–192 Language disabilities communication impairments, 222–223 language disorders, 223 neuroscience and, 215–216 Language diversity. See also Bilingualism; English learners (ELs) dialects, 263–264 emergent literacy and, 254–255 immigrant students and English learners, teaching of, 266–269 native-language maintenance instruction, 271 second-language learning, 257 semilingual, 271 Language loss, 258–259 Langue des Signes Quebecoise (LSQ), 259 Lateralization, 95 Latina/o Americans cultural groups, 43 culturally responsive classroom management, 597–601 discrimination, legacy of, 62–64 diversity in learning, 77–79 ethnic-racial identity, 57–58 graduation rates, 61 poverty rates, 49 Learned helplessness, 218, 528 Learner differences. See also Learning disabilities defining intelligence, 189–190 differentiated instruction, 639–643 gifted and talented students, 236–240 Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA), 209–212 intelligence as process, 193 language and labeling, 187–189 learning and thinking styles, 207–208 measuring intelligence, 194–197

S-9

referrals, process for, 214 sex differences and, 197–198 types of learners, 630 Learning. See also Behavioral learning theories; Cognitive view of learning; Learning challenges; Learning disabilities; Theory of cognitive development behavioral views of learning, 291 brain regions and, 91 classroom time for, 560–561 cognitive development and, 122 constructivist views of, 421–423 diversity in, 77–79 gender and sexual orientation in, 64–74 instructed learning, 124 motivation to learn, strategies to, 546–550 neuroscience and, 99–101 performance and, 320–321 self-efficacy and, 480–481 social cognitive views, 500–501 universal design, differentiation with, 642–643 Learning approaches for different cultures, 77–79 Learning challenges, 215–234. See also Learning disabilities neuroscience and, 215–216 Learning disabilities See also Specific learning disabilities (SLD) autism spectrum disorders, 6, 213, 233–236 communication impairments, 222–223 cooperative learning, use of, 449–450 differentiated instruction, 639–643 emotional and behavioral disorders, 223–229 health and sensory impairments, 230–233 high-stakes tests and, 690 hyperactivity and attention disorders, 219–221 intellectual disabilities, 229–230 labeling and language, 187–189 note taking strategies, 382 overview of, 217–219 production deficiencies, 386 referrals, process for, 214 response to intervention (RTI), 213–214

S-10 Subject Index Learning goals, 522, 616–618 Learning management system (LMS), 453 Learning preferences, 77–78, 207–208 Learning sciences cognitive apprenticeships, 436–437 collaboration and cooperation, 438–450 constructivist perspectives, application of, 418–462 constructivist views of learning, 421–423 defined, 427 digital media, interactive learning and, 451–461 inquiry-based learning, 431–436 learner-centered teaching, 424–427 Learning Skills Research Centre, 207 Learning spaces/interest areas, classroom planning, 570, 571–572 Learning strategies, 379–387 application of, 386–387 attention, 379, 381, 385 drawing, 384–385 note taking, 382–383 organizing, visual tools for, 383–385 reading strategies, 385–386 students with learning disabilities, 217, 218–219 summaries, 381 transfer of learning, 408–411 underlining and highlighting, 381–382 Learning styles, 207–208 problems with, 19, 208 Learning targets, 619–621, 659–679 Least restrictive environment (LRE), 210 Left brain learning, 98 Legal issues Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA), 213 child abuse, 155–156 Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA), 209–212 Vocational Rehabilitation Act (1973), 212 Legally blind, 232 Lengua de señas nicaragüense, 259 Lesbians, 54, 72–73, 589 Lesson study, 616 Levels of processing theory, 345 LGBTQ students, 72–74, 589

Limited bilingual students, 283. See also English learners (ELs) Limited capacity of working memory, 345 Limited English proficient (LEP), 270 Linguistic intelligence, 190–192 Lip reading, 233 Listening empathetic listening, 595–596 phonological loop, 340–342 Literacy emergent, 253–254 language diversity and emergent, 254–256 media and digital, 451–461 Literature response groups, teaching English language learners, 267 Loci method, 363 Locus of causality, 511, 528, 548 Logical consequences, 566, 583 Logical-mathematical intelligence, 191–192 LOGO programming, 456 Long-term memory, 349 capacity and duration of, 349 explicit (declarative) memories, 349–355 implicit memories, 349, 355–356 individual differences, 357–358 information processing system, 334 retrieving information, 356–357 Longitudinal studies, 23 Low expectations for students in poverty, 53 teacher expectations, 612–613 Low vision, defined, 232 Lower class, defined, 48 Magic middle, 125–126 Mainstreaming, 210 Maintenance rehearsal, 344 Males ADHD diagnoses, 219–220 gender identity and sexual orientation, 63–64, 70–72 gender roles and bias, 64–70 genderlects, 263 sex and intelligence, 197–198 stereotype threat, 45–46 substance use disorder, 227–228 Mandated reporting, 155 Manipulatives, 123–124 Mapping, Cmaps, 383–384 Marcellino, Rosa, 229 Maslow, Abraham, 513–514 Massed practice, 364

Massive multi-player online games (MMOs), 455 Mastery experiences, 478, 479 Mastery goals, 522–523 Matching exercises, 659–660 Mathematics and mathematic skills, 475. See also STEM (science, technology, engineering and mathematics) fields comprehension and problem solving, 390–391, 397 ethnic and racial achievement data, 59–61 neuroscience and, 215, 216 self-concept and, 165–167 sex differences, 197–198 specific learning disabilities, 215–217 stereotype threat, 45–46 technology, use of, 119 Maturation, 89, 104, 136–137 Mean, test scores, 681–682 Meaning, knowledge construction and, 345 Meaning, language development, 250–251 Means-ends analysis, 395 Measurement, 653–654 data-based decision making, 653 intelligence, measures of, 194–197 standard error of, 655 Measures of Effective Teaching (MET) Project, 694 Media aggressive behavior and, 151 identity and technology, 163 impact of, 6–7 literacy and use, 459 prejudice and, 45–46 Median, test scores, 681 Mediation of mental processes, 119 Medulla oblongata, 91 Melting pot, 267 Memory cognitive load and retaining information in working, 343–346 emotions and body, 540 executive control processes, 375 executive functioning, development of, 115 explicit memory, 349–355 implicit memory, 349, 355–356 individual differences, 346–347, 357–358 information processing system, 333–334 learning strategies, 382

long-term memory, 349–358 mnemonics, 363–364 neuroscience of, 216 problem solving schemas, 392–392, 397, 399 retrieval practice, 377, 385 robust knowledge, 412 rote memorization, 364 sensory memory, 333–339 short-term memory, 333, 340–341 working memory, 190, 340–347 Menarche, 136 Mental age, 194 Mental health issues, stigma of, 228–229 Mental retardation, 229. See also Intellectual disabilities Mentorship, for developing critical thinking skills, 403 Merryn, Erin, 155 Meta-analysis, 9 Metacognition, 375–379 development of, 376–379 executive control processes, 375 individual differences, 375–376 KWL (know, want, learn) strategy, 376, 385 self-regulation and, 484, 485 teachers supporting student metacognition, 378 Metacognitive knowledge, 378, 407 Metacognitive skills, 407, 408 Metalinguistic awareness, 252–253 Metamotivation, 486 #MeToo movement, 68 Middle class, defined, 48 Middle school aged children concrete-operational stage (Piaget), 108–111 peers, influence of, 148–152 psychosocial development (Erikson), 159–162 relationships with teachers, 152–154 self-concept, development of, 166 sociocultural theory (Vygotsky), 120 Mindful abstraction, 409 Mindfulness training, 495 Mindset about ability, 530–533 about failure, 531–532 Minecraft, 455 Minority group, 56 Mirror neuron systems, 331 Mixed methods, research, 23–24 Mixon, Justin, 276

Subject Index Mnemonics, 363–364 CAPS, 386 as learning strategy, 380, 385 PARS, 385 Mobile learning, 452–453 Mode, test scores, 681 Model-evidence link, 429 Modeling, 470, 478–479 aggressive behavior, 151–152 cognitive behavior modification, 498–499 instruction manner, 379 observational learning, 472–477 triadic reciprocal causality, 470 Moderately difficult goals, 530 Monitoring skills, 375, 378, 400–402 Monolingual children, 256, 268 Moral action, 178 Moral awareness, 178 Moral commitment, 178 Moral development, 171–178 Moral dilemmas, 171 Moral judgments, 174, 178 Moral realism, 174 Moral reasoning, 171 Morality of cooperation, 174 Moratorium (Erikson), 162 Motherese, 250 Motivation, 380, 387, 411, 472, 509–510. See also Behavioral learning theories anxiety, 542–543 attribution theories, 527–529 autonomy, support of, 548 building blocks of students’ motivation, 547 classroom strategies, summary of, 548 curiosity and, 536–537 emotions, 540–542 expectancy x value theories, 525–527 flow, 539–540 general approaches to, 509–513 goals and goal orientations, 519–525 grades and, 677 interests, tapping of, 535–536 intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, 511–512 intrinsic task motivation, 203 learning, strategies for, 546–551 Maslow’s hierarchy of needs, 513–514 mindsets and, 530–533 needs and, 513–518 observational learning and, 474

self-determination and, 513–518 self-efficacy and, 480 self-regulation and, 486 and transfer, 408, 411 triadic reciprocal causality, 470–472 Motivation to learn, 546–551 Motivational components, 509 Motor skills, development and, 97, 106–107, 115, 135–137 Movement, theory of multiple intelligence, 191, 192 Movement management, 576 Moving residences, impact on students, 54 Mozart effect, 97 Multicultural education, 75 creation of culturally welcoming classrooms, 75–82 diversity in learning, 77–79 lessons for teachers, 79–82 overview of, 75–82 Multimedia learning activities, 451–462 Multiple-choice tests, 659–660 Multiple representations of content, 425–426 Multiple texts, integrating, 413 Multitasking, 337 Musical intelligence, 191, 193 Myelination, 94 Nancekivell, Sharlene, 208 National Assessment of Educational Progress, 60 National Association for Gifted Children (NAGC), 236 National Association for the Education of Young Children, 123 National Center for Children in Poverty, 54 National Center on Response to Intervention, 214 National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), 253 National Institute on Drug Abuse, 228 National Rifle Association, 44 National School Climate Survey, 73 Native Americans, 64, 137 Native American languages, 252, 258 Native-language maintenance instruction, 271 Natural (logical) consequences, 566, 583 Naturalist intelligence, 191 Nature vs. nurture, 89, 237

Near-infrared optical tomography (NIR-OT), 91, 92 Needs, 513–618 for autonomy, 515, 516 for competence, 515, 516 deficiency and being, 513–514 growth vs. safety, 513–514 Maslow’s hierarchy of, 513–514 need-supportive vs. needthwarting classroom, 516 for relatedness, 515, 516, 518 Negative behavior functional behavioral assessments, 316–317 ignoring of, 300, 303, 323 response to, 310, 311 Negative correlation, 21 Negative emotions, 543–545 Negative reinforcement, 297–298, 311 Neglect, child, 155, 156 Neglected children indicators of child abuse, 156 in peer popularity, 149 Neglecting parents, 145 Negotiations, 597 Neo-Piagetian theories, 115 Nervous system, brain development, 91–102 Neurodevelopmental disorder, 233 Neurogenesis, 93 Neuromyths, 98 Neurons, 91–94, 97 Neuroscience behavioral learning and, 298–299 cognitive learning and the brain, 330–331 forgetting, memory and, 345–346 learning and teaching implications, 99–101 of learning challenges, 215–216 Neutral stimulus, 294 Next Generation Science Standards (NGSS), 407 Nigrescence, 58 NIR-OT (near-infrared optical tomography), 91, 92 No Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act (2002), 8, 16, 70 Nonacademic self-concept, 165, 167 Nondiscriminatory evaluations, 209 Norm groups, 658 Norm-referenced grading, 673–674 Norm-referenced testing, 658 Normal distribution, 682–684

S-11

Note taking skills, 382–383 Obama, Barack, 229, 469 Obesity, 139 Object permanence, 106 Observational learning, 320, 470, 472–477 Obsessive-compulsive disorders, 39 Occipital lobe, 91, 95 Offensiveness in assessment bias, 656 OK2BME website, 137 Online sources, 405–406 Operant conditioning, 296–301 challenges and criticisms, 320 encouraging positive behaviors, 303–304, 307 praise, 304 punishment, 291, 297–298, 310–314 Operants, defined, 296 Operations (Piaget), 107 Opportunity gaps, 61 Oral fluency, 217 Organization, 360, 609, 610, 620 Organization of thoughts, Piaget on, 104 Organizational skills, 467, 505 Organizing, learning strategies, 381, 383–385 Originality and creativity, 203 Out-group prejudices, 44–45 Outcome expectations, 472 Overlapping in classroom management, 576 Overlearning, 409 Overregularize in language development, 252 Overt aggression, 150 Overweight children and obesity, 139 Pacific Islanders, 61 Paraphrase rule, 595 PARCC tests, 689 Parenting styles, 144–145 Parietal lobe, brain, 91, 95 PARS, 385 Part learning, 364 Participant observation, 23 Participants/subjects, research, 21 Participation structures, 78, 560, 565 Participatory appropriation, 436 Partnership for Assessment of Readiness for College and Careers (PARCC), 685 Passive construction, language development, 252 PATHS (Promoting Alternative THinking Strategies), 9–10 PATHS curriculum, 9–10 Pavlov, Ivan, 294

S-12 Subject Index Pedagogical content knowledge (PCK), 610 Peer cultures, 148 bioecological model of development, 148–152 poverty, effect of, 52 Peer Helpers Program, 154 Penalties. See Discipline Perceived cost, 527 Percentile rank scores, 683 Perception, 335, 336 Perceptual narrowing, 250 Perfectionism, dangers of, 169 Perfectionistic concerns, 169 Perfectionistic striving, 169 Performance assessments, 666–671 Performance components, triarchic theory of successful intelligence, 192 Performance goals, 523–524 Performances, 666–668 Permissive parents, 144 Person-first language, 188–189 Person-first reference, 188 Personal caring, 153 Personal choice in moral development, 173, 174 Personal development, 89 Personal interest, 511, 545 Personal knowledge, 378 Personal learning environment (PLE), 453 Personal learning network (PLN), 454 Personal territories, classroom planning, 570 Personality, learning and thinking styles, 207–208 Personalized education approach, 536 Perspective-taking ability, 171 PET (positron emission tomography) scans, 91, 92 Pew Research Center, 57 Phonemic awareness, 217, 285 Phonetic speech sound, 250 Phonological loop, 340–342 Physical abuse of children, 155, 156 Physical activity, 137–139 Physical development, 89 body image and disordered eating, 140–141 obesity, 139 overview, 134–137 play, recess, and physical activity, 137–139 Physical disabilities Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA), 213 assistive technology, 641–342 cooperative learning, use of, 448–450 English learners, 281–282 high-stakes tests and, 691

Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA), 209–212 language and labeling, 187–189 physical activity and, 138–139 positive behavior supports (PBS), 315–318 trends in education, 6 Vocational Rehabilitation Act (1973), 212 Physical neglect of children, 155, 156 Physiological arousal, 479 Physiological needs, 513–514 Piaget, Jean, 102, 103–115, 138, 422, 423, 440, 447, 501. See also Theory of cognitive development (Piaget) implications for teaching, 122–124 limitations, 113–114 theory of cognitive development, 103–115 Pianta, Robert, 11–13 Pituitary gland, 91 Planning, of instructional content constructivist approaches, 620–622 design process, 638 flexible and creative plans, 618–620 learning goals and targets, 616–618 research on, 616 Understanding by Design (UbD), 637–639 Planning skill, 378 Planning tasks, students Framework for Teaching, 12–13 learning strategies, 381 metacognition and, 375 problem solving and, 400–402 sociocultural theory (Vygotsky), 119 Plasticity, brain, 93, 97 culture and, 97 synaptic plasticity, 93 Play cultural differences and, 138 physical development and, 137–139 plan, 125–126 value of, 138 Playgrounds for all. See Inclusive playgrounds Podcasts academic optimism, 611 brain-based education, 100 classroom management, 560 cooperative learning, 440 efficacy judgments, 482 learning theories, differences in, 330 positive and negative reinforcements, 297

procrastination, 531 self-efficacy, 477, 531 student sense of self, 165 teacher involvement, importance of, 11 as teaching tool, 208, 278 test taking skills, 657 Point/Counterpoint ADHD, pills or skills, 220–221 brain-based education, 98–99 building self-esteem, schools role, 168–169 English learners, teaching strategies, 270–271 gender differences and teaching, 70–71 grade retention, 676–677 grit, 488–489 homework valuable, 629–630 inquiry- and problem-based learning approaches, 434–435 making learning fun, 537 multitasking, 337–338 research as guide to education, 25–26 rewards for learning, 322–323 teaching of critical thinking and problem solving skills, 405 zero tolerance policies, 585–586 Pons (brain), 91 Popular children, 149 Popularity, 149 Portfolios, 666–671 Positive behavior supports (PBS), 315–318, 599 Positive correlation, 21 Positive practice overcorrection, 311 Positive reinforcement, 297 Positive teacher–student relationships, 11, 578–579, 591 Positron emission tomography (PET) scans, 91, 92 Poverty academic outcomes and, 50–54 achievement gap and, 61 affects children’s educational outcomes, 51 assessment bias, 656–657 deep, 54 gifted and talented students, identifying, 240 rates of, 49 and social class, 49–54 and social inequality, 49 trends in education, 6 Practical intelligence, 193, 197, 198 Practice. See also Deliberate practice; IES Practice Guides; Retrieval practice

brain, retrieval practice/ testing effect, 385 deliberate practice, 365 developmentally appropriate practice, 123 distributed learning/practice, 364, 286 interleaved practice, 362 as learning strategy, 381, 412 massed practice, 364 memory, retrieval practice, 375, 385 positive practice overcorrection, 311 retrieval practice, 361–362, 375, 385 Pragmatics, 78 of classroom, 78 language skills, 252 Praise-and-ignore approach, 303 PRAXIS II ADHD, 217 advance organizers, 429 alternatives to standardized testing, 689 antecedents, 301 applied behavior analysis, 302 attention, 338 attribution theory, 527 authentic tests, 666 bilingual issues, 256 characteristics of cooperative learning, 438 classroom space, 570 concept mapping, 384 concepts of standardized testing, 681 criterion-/norm-referenced tests, 658 cultural and gender differences in the classroom, 43 developing basic skills, 365 discovery learning, 432 distinctions between Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s theories, 127 economic conditions/ socioeconomic status (SES), 47 encouraging/discouraging behaviors, 306 Erikson’s psychosocial theory of development, 158 families, 146 forms of cooperative learning, 447 gender bias, 68 human development, 135 implications of Piaget’s theory, 123 implications of Vygotsky’s theory, 124 individual education programs (IEP), 210

Subject Index inquiry learning, 431 instructional objectives, 616 intelligence testing, 195 interpreting achievement tests, 685 the larger community, 37 learning by association, 295 learning/cognitive styles, 207 learning strategies, 379 Maslow’s hierarchy of needs, 513–514 memory and instruction, 340 memory and recall, 366 memory strategies, 364 modeling, 470 moral development, 175 multicultural education, 75 multiple intelligences, 190 observational learning, 474 operant conditioning, basics of, 296 planning thematic units, 620 portfolio assessment, 668 prior knowledge, 353 problem solving, 388 procedures and routines to minimize confusion and misbehavior, 563 promoting intrinsic motivation to learn, 511 promoting student engagement, 574 questioning, 633 racial bias, 63 reasoning, 111 rules, 565 scoring rubrics, 670 self-determination, 515 self-esteem, 168 self-regulation, 499 standardized testing: major issues, 689 student-centered learning, 424 student misbehavior, 582 taxonomies of educational objectives, 618 teacher-centered instruction, 625 teacher professionalism, 12, 15 teacher–student communication, 595 teacher’s role in studentcentered instruction, 639 teaching and management, 313 teaching concepts, 351 test anxiety, 543 thinking skills, 403 traditional assessment, 657 transfer of learning, 408 types of assessment, 654 Pre-encounter nigrescence, 58 Precorrection, 317

Preferences, learning, 207 Prejudice, 44, 82 reduction efforts, 75 stereotypes and discrimination, 43–46 Premack principle, 304–305 Preoperational stage of development, 105–108 Preparation tasks (Danielson model), 12 Preschool-aged children physical development, 135–137 psychosocial development (Erikson), 159 self-concept, development of, 166 sociocultural theory (Vygotsky), 120 Presentation punishment, defined, 298 Pretest, 654 Priming, 355 Principle, defined, 26 Prior knowledge. See also Scaffolding cognitive views of learning, 362, 366 complex cognitive processes, 380, 382 constructivism, 423, 427, 431, 434–435, 455 social cognitive views of learning and motivation, 501 teaching every student, 611, 624, 624, 640 Private speech, 119–121 Probing questions. See Deep questions Problem, defined, 389 Problem-based learning, 432–433 Problem general frame, 379 Problem solving, 25, 388–402 attention, 390, 399 cognitive development, 111 cognitive processes, 388–402 cognitive views of learning, 356, 362, 365, 366 complex cognitive processes, 386, 388–402, 405, 409–412 creativity, 201–207 crystallized intelligence, 194 culture and diversity, 55, 76 defining goals and representing the problem, 390–395 examples and, 394 expert knowledge and, 399–402 learning differences and needs, 216, 217 mathematics comprehension and problem solving, 391, 396

memory, problem solving schemas, 392, 393, 400 planning tasks and, 400–402 Point/Counterpoint, 405 problem identification, 389 procedural knowledge and, 399–400 reconstruction, 357 representations and, 392, 392, 395 schema-driven, 392–393 self-concept and, 165 skill development, 389 sociocultural theory (Vygotsky), 118–119. See also Scaffolding specific learning disabilities, 216, 217 successful intelligence, 193 teaching of critical thinking and problem solving skills, 405 translation and, 392–393 triarchic theory of successful intelligence, 193 well structured vs. ill structured problems, 389 Procedural fairness, 653 Procedural knowledge, 332 development of, 365–366 metacognition and, 375 problem solving and, 399–402 Procedural memory, 355 Procedures, classroom, 563–569 for in-person learning, 563–567 for remote learning, 567–569 Process portfolio, 668, 669 Production, observational learning and, 473 Production deficiencies, 386 Productions, memory, 356 Professional organizations, 15 Professional responsibilities, 12 Framework for Teaching, 12–13 Project Look Sharp, 459 Promotive interaction, 440–442 Prompting to support metacognition, 379 Pronunciations, dialects and, 263 Propositional networks, 349–350 Prototype, 351 Proximal development, zone of (ZPD), 121–122 Proximal goals, 520 Pruning, synapses, 93–94 Psychological tools, 119 Psychomotor domain, 618 Psychosocial, defined, 158 Psychosocial development (Erikson), 158–169 Puberty, 136–137

S-13

Punishment, 291, 297–298, 310–314 PurpleState virtual internship, 454 Pygmalion effect, 611 Qualitative change in development, 90 Qualitative research, 23, 26 Quality talk (QT), 634–635 Quantitative change in development, 90 Quantitative research, 23, 26 Quasi-experimental studies, 29 Quebec Sign Language, 259 Questioning. See also Deep questions as teaching method, 631–635 Race, 42–46, 56–63. See also Diversity assessment bias, 656–657, 671 caring relationships and school connections, 579 culturally relevant pedagogy, 76–77 culturally responsive classroom management, 597–601 culturally welcoming classrooms, creation of, 75–82 defining, 56 discrimination, legacy of, 62–64 diversity in learning, 77–79 identity, 57–59 immigrant students, teaching of, 266–269 intelligence, differences in, 197–198 intersectionality, 42 labels and terminology, 189 moral reasoning, 175–176 motivation and, 529 parenting styles, 144 poverty rates, 49 racial identity, 55–57 racism, 63–64 school achievement and, 59–61 self-concept, ideas of, 165–167 stereotypes, prejudice, discrimination, 43–46 stereotype threat, 45–46 trends in education, 6–7 White privilege, 59 Racial and ethnic pride, 165 Racial identity, 57–59 Racial inequality, legacy of, 62–64 Racism, 63–64 Radical constructivism, 422 Random, 21 Range, test scores, 681 Ratio schedule of reinforcement, 299

S-14 Subject Index Reading brain research and, 100 cultural differences, 97 emergent literacy, 253–254 language diversity and emergent literacy, 254–255 neuroscience and, 215, 216 poverty, student achievement and, 50, 53 specific learning disabilities, overview, 216–219 summer reading programs, 53 Receptive language, growth in, 255 Receptive vocabulary, 250–251 Recess, importance of, 138–139 Reciprocal questioning, 446 Recitation, 631–635 Reconstruction in remembering, 357 Reflective teachers, 6, 610 Refugees, 267 Rehearsal, executive control process, 375 Reinforcement, 296, 353 classical conditioning, 294–295 encouraging positive behaviors, 303–304, 307 observational learning and, 474 schedules, 299–300 Reinforcer, 296 Rejected children, 149–150 Rejecting parents, 144 Rejection, peer groups, 150 Relatedness, need for, 515–518 Relational aggression, 150–151 Relationship skills, SEL dimensions, 154 Relationships, inferring using hypothetico-deductive reasoning, 111 Reliability, assessments, 654–657, 670–671 Remembering. See Memory Remembering questions, 619, 633 Removal punishment, 298, 312 Representations cognitive development theories and, 115 problem solving and, 381, 382, 384 Representativeness heuristics, 397 Reprimands, 311 Research descriptive studies, types of, 20–23 as a guide to education, debate about, 25–26 insights from, 98

methods, summary chart, 23 role of time in, 23 in support of student learning, summary chart, 27–29 teachers as researchers, 24 theories for teaching, 26–27 Resistance culture, 52–53 Respondents, 294 Response, contiguity and, 293 Response cost, 311, 312 Response set, 397 Response to intervention (RtI), 213–214 Responsible decision making, SEL dimensions, 154 Restorative justice, 586–587 Retention, observational learning and, 473 Retention in grade, 674–675 Retrieval, 356–357 Retrieval practice cognitive views of learning, 361–362 complex cognitive processes, 377, 382, 384, 385, 410, 412 Reversibility, Reversible thinking (Piaget), 107–109 Rewards, 511–512, 525, 548 Right brain learning, 98 Ripple effect, 475 Risk taking behavior, adolescents, 96 Roam and review, 277 Robust knowledge, 412–413 Role-playing games (RPGs), 455 Roles, cooperative learning groups, 445 Rosa’s Law, 229 Rosenshine’s six teaching functions, 625 Rote memorization, 364 Routines, classroom, 563, 564–570 in-person learning, 563 remote learning, 567–569 Rubrics, scoring, 668–671 RULER (Recognizing, Understanding, Labeling, Expressing, and Regulating emotions), 9, 494 Rules, classroom, 563–567 in-person learning, 565–567 remote learning, 567–569 Safety needs (Maslow), 513–514 SBAC tests, 689 Scaffolding advance organizers as, 429 constructivism and, 425, 427–429 defined, 124 development and, 121, 124, 125

Schema-driven problem solving, 395 Schemas, 63, 352–353 cognitive load theory, 392 schema-driven problem solving, 392–395 self-explanation, 392–393 worked examples, 392–393 Schemes (Piaget), 104–107, 115 School connections and belonging, 578–580 School disruptions, academic outcomes and, 53–54 School-wide positive behavioral interventions and supports (SWPBIS), 317 Science, 475. See also STEM (science, technology, engineering and mathematics) fields ScienceDaily, 208 Scientifically based research, 22 Scoring rubrics, 668–670 Scripted cooperation, 626 Scripts, in implicit memory, 355 Seating arrangements, 570 Second-language learning, 257. See also Bilingualism common errors and accomplishments, 261 Secondary school students. See High school-aged children Section 509, 212 Seizure disorders, 231 Selected-response tests, 659–660 Self-actualization, 513–514 Self-awareness, 154, 426–427 Self-concept, 165–167, 477 defined, 165 development of, 166 gender differences, 167 poverty, effect of, 52 psychosocial development (Erikson), 158–169 structure of, 165–166 Self-control, 487. See also Selfregulation Self-determination in classroom, 515 how to support, 517 motivation and, 513–518 Self-efficacy, 526, 528–529 agency and, 477–483 social cognitive theory and, 477–483 teachers’ sense of, 482–483 teaching toward, 496–500 triadic reciprocal causality, 470, 483 Self-esteem, 168–170, 477 global self-esteem, 165, 168 need for, 513–514 social goals, 524–525

Self-evaluation, self-regulated learning and, 499–500 Self-explanation, 393, 413 Self-fulfilling prophecy, 611–612 Self-handicapping strategies, 531 Self-influence, 492 Self-instruction, 498 Self-knowledge, theory of multiple intelligence, 196–200 Self-management, 318–320, 498–499. See also Selfregulation SEL dimensions, 154 Self-motivation. See Motivation Self-regulated learning, 484–485 cognitive behavior modification, 498–499 models of, 480–482 social cognitive model of, 480–482 teaching toward, 496–500 Zimmerman’s three-phase model of, 491–492 Self-regulation, 484, 562 achievement gap and, 61 development of, 485–490 developmental changes in, 490 vs. discipline, 587, 596 of emotions, 493–496 failure of, 466 organizational skills and, 467, 504–505 self-reinforcement and, 474 sociocultural theory (Vygotsky), 120 steps toward preventing management problems, 576–577 teacher stress, 494–496 teaching toward, 496 technology and, 492–493 Self-regulatory knowledge, 332, 371, 405 Self-reinforcement, 319, 474 Self-starter, 484 Semantic memory, 349 Semilingual, 271 Semiotic function, 107 Sensitive periods in development, 90 second language learning, 257 Sensorimotor stage of development (Piaget), 106 Sensory memory, 333–339 Serial-position effect, 364 Seriation, 110 Sex in curriculum and media, 67–68

Subject Index intelligence, differences in, 197–198 Sexual abuse, 155–156 Sexual orientation, 64–74 creating safe spaces, 74 defined, 65 discrimination based on, 73–74 reaching out to help students, 73–74 sexual identity development, 74 Sexual minority youth, 73 Sexual orientation and gender identity issues (SOGI), 137 Shaping, 305–306 Shared regulation, 490, 496 Sheltered instruction, 274–278 Sheltered Instruction Observational Protocol (SIOP), 274–276 Short-term memory, 342 information processing system, 334 Sickle cell disease, 232 Sign language, 233 Signed English, 259 Signed languages, 259 Simon, Theodore, 194 Single-subject experimental studies, 21, 24 Situated learning, 409, 424 Situational interests, 535–536 Skill-building tasks, for cooperative learning, 442–443 Skills for argumentation, 407 Skinner, B. F., 296–297, 299, 500 Sleep, adolescents and, 96 Smarter Balanced Assessment Consortium (SBAC) tests, 689 Social aggression, 150, 151 Social and emotional learning (SEL), 8, 151, 199 exercise and recess, 138 five dimensions of, 154 PATHS, 9–10 principles, 228 research on, 9 social-emotional trauma, 10 supporting, 153–154 and teaching whole child, 8–10 Social awareness, SEL dimensions, 154 Social class, 47–54 Social cognitive theory, 469–472 modeling, 472–477 motivation and, 512, 524 overview of, 467, 501 self-efficacy and agency, 477–476 self-management, 498–499

self-regulated learning, 490–492 self-regulated learning, teaching toward, 496–500 Social considerations, English language learners, 274 Social constructivism, 420–427 Social context. See Bioecological model of development (Bronfenbrenner) Social conventions and moral reasoning, 179, 180 Social development, 89 early and late maturation, 136–137 Piaget on, 104 psychosocial development (Erikson), 158–169 Vygotsky’s theory, 116–122 Social goals, 524–525 Social inequality, poverty and, 49 Social intuitionist model (Haidt), 175–176 Social isolation (time out), 312 Social learning, 493–494 Social learning theory, 320–321, 470 Social media body image and disordered eating, 140–141 bullying. See Cyber aggression (bullying) identity and technology, 163 Social negotiation (constructivism), 425 Social organization, diversity in learning and, 77 Social persuasion (sources of self-efficacy), 478, 479 Social skills autism spectrum disorders, 233–234 gifted students, 238 practical intelligence and, 197 preventing problems, classroom management, 575–577 theory of multiple intelligence, 196–200 Social transmission (Piaget), 104 Socialized speech, 120 Sociocultural theory (Vygotsky), 116 assisted learning, 124 cooperative learning and, 439, 442 implications for teaching, 122, 124–125 limitations of, 122 overview of, 116–121 Socioeconomic status (SES), 48 achievement gap and, 59–60 assessment bias, 656–657

cultural differences, 47–54 Generation 1.5, 269–270 gifted and talented students, identifying, 240 poverty rates, 49–51 trends in education, 6 Sociohistoric theory. See Sociocultural theory (Vygotsky) Sociolinguistics, 78–79 Software appropriate for young children, checklist, 458 Sounds, language development, 250, 253 Source monitoring, 354 Southern Poverty Law Center, 45 Spaced practice. See Distributed learning/practice Spasticity, 230 Spatial information, brain regions for, 95 Spatial intelligence, 196–291 Special education. See Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA) Specific learning disability (SLD), 216–219. See also Learning disabilities autism spectrum disorders, 233–234 focusing attention, 380 gifted and talented students, identifying, 240 response to intervention (RTI), 213 Speech impairments, 222–223 Speech reading, 233 Spermarche, 136 Spiral curriculum, 426 Spock, Benjamin, 158 Spreading activation, 356–357 Stability, 528 Standard deviation, 681–682 Standard error of measurement, 655 Standard scores, 683–684 Standardized testing, 653–654, 660. See also Assessment accountability and highstakes testing, 688–689 No Child Left Behind Act and, 22 test reports, interpreting, 688–689 types of scores, 681–684 Standards, learning, 616–618 Stanford Achievement Test, 10th Edition, 685 Stanford-Binet test, 681–168 Stanine scores, 684 Statistically significant, 21

S-15

Stem, multiple choice questions, 660 STEM (science, technology, engineering and mathematics) fields, 475 and gender bias, 67 Stereotype threat, 45–46 Stereotypes, 44, 62. See also Bias Sternberg, Robert, 193, 203 Stimulated recall, 609 Stimulus, 394 Stimulus control, 300 Stimulus-driven attention, 335 Storage and retrieval, executive control processes, 375 Story grammar, 353 Strategies. See also Learning strategies executive functioning, development of, 115 Strategy instruction, specific learning disabilities and, 218 Strategy knowledge, 378 Stress brain research and, 100–101 English learners and, 277, 284 student achievement and, 51 teacher burnout, 482, 496 Structured controversies, 447–448 Structured English immersion (SEI), 270 Student teachers, advice for, 16 Student teams achievement divisions (STAD), 443 Students. See also Learning agency and control with, 497 assessment, involvement in, 665–666 current teaching trends, 4–11 grading, effects of, 674 learning, research summary chart, 27 learning gap, 61 learning styles, 19 penalties for, 584 sharing control with in classroom management, 566–567, 587–588 specific learning disabilities, 216–219, 239 student-led conferences, 280–281 student ownership of learning, 427 teacher expectations and, 610–613 teacher relationships and, 11 trauma effects, 10 Stuttering, 222, 223 Subjects, research, 21 Substance use disorder, 227–228 Subtractive bilingualism, 258

S-16 Subject Index Successful intelligence, 193 Successive approximation, 306 Suicide. See Death by suicide Summaries, learning strategies and, 380–381 Summarizing, paraphrase rule, 626 Summative assessment, 654, 668 Summer, student achievement and, 53–54 Survival needs (Maslow), 513–514 Sustaining expectation effect, 611 Symbols, preoperational stage of development, 107 Synapses, 91 Synaptic plasticity, 93 Syntax, language development, 252–253, 260 T score, 684 Talented students. See Gifted and talented students Task analysis, 306 Task goals, 522 Task value, 526 Tasks authentic, 547 multitasking, 337–338 Taxonomy, 618–619 Taylor, Breonna, 63 Teacher attention, as reinforcement, 303–304 Teacher–student interactions, domains of, 14–15 Teachers. See also Teaching abolitionist, 77 advice for beginners, 15 agency and control, 497–498 attributions, 529 burnout, 482, 496 caring relationships, 578–580 characteristics of effective teachers, 609 child abuse, recognizing, 155–156 culturally welcoming classrooms, creation of, 75–82 diversity of, 7 evaluation, 695 expectations of, 610–613 expert teachers, 609 multicultural classroom, tips for, 79–82 relationships with students, 152–153 as researchers, 23–24 response to cheating, 177 sense of efficacy, 7, 482–483 value of, 10–11 Teachers’ Casebook becoming a great teacher, 2, 30–32

bullies and victims, 556, 603–605 communicating and creating community in classroom, 246–247, 285–287 conversation about race, 34, 83–85 failure to self-regulate, 466–467, 504–505 giving meaningful grades, 650–651, 697–699 including all students, 184, 242–245 mean girls, 132, 181–183 motivating students, 506, 553–555 necessity is the mother of inventive teaching, 418, 463–465 reaching and teaching every student, 606, 646–649 remembering the basics, 328, 370–371 sick of class, 288, 325–327 symbols and cymbals, 86, 129–131 uncritical thinking, 372–373, 416–417 Teaching. See also Classroom organization and management; Guidelines anxiety, helping students cope, 542–543 attention and, 338–339 constructivist, learnercentered, 425–427 current trends in, 4–11 dialects and, 263–264 differentiated instruction, 639–643, 645 direct instruction, 624–626, 637 of English learners, 266–268 fitting to goals, 635–637 Framework for Teaching, 12–13 gender and sexual orientation in, 64–74 gender bias in, 67–71 goals, motivation and, 519–525 good teaching, defined, 11–15 homework and independent work, valuable, 627–631 of immigrant students, 267–269 learner-centered, 424–427 making meaningful connections, 359–362 meaningful lessons, 362–364 memorization, 364–365 metacognitive development skills, 376–377 moral climate of classroom, 174–175 motivation, supporting, 546–551

neuroscience and, 99–101 observational learning and, 474–477 poor, 54 questioning, discussion, and dialogue, 631–635 research on, 608–615, 624 Rosenshine’s six functions of, 625 self-efficacy in learning and, 480–481 sociocultural theory (Vygotsky), implications of, 122, 124–125 student learning research, summary chart, 27–28 students who live in poverty, 54 TeachingWorks, 13–14 theories for, 26–27 theory of cognitive development, implications of, 122–127 toward self-efficacy and selfregulated learning, 496–500 transfer of learning, 408–411 Understanding by Design (UbD), 637–639 whole child and SEL, 8–10 TeachingWorks, 13–14 Teasing, 565, 588. See also Bullying Technical tools, cognitive development and (Vygotsky), 119 Technology, 475. See also STEM (science, technology, engineering and mathematics) fields assistive, 641–642 digital media, learning and, 451–461 identity and, 163 and learning, 451 powered pedagogy, 460–461 rich environments, 452–456 STEM fields, and gender bias, 67 Technology-rich environments (TREs), 452–456 Television, aggressive behavior and, 151 Temporal lobe, brain, 91, 94, 95 Terminology, cultural intersections and, 42–43 Testing. See Assessment; Standardized testing Testing effect, 361–362, 385 Thalamus, 91 Theory defined, 24 development, questions for, 88–90 teaching theories, 24–25 Theory-based concepts, 351–352

Theory Into Practice, 24, 511, 547 Theory of cognitive development, Piaget, 90, 103–114 cooperative learning and, 439, 442 four stages of, 105–113 implications for teachers, 122–124 limitations of, 113–114 neo-Piagetian views, 115 private speech, 119–121 underestimation of children’s abilities, 114 Theory of cognitive development, Vygotsky 116–122 cultural tools, 118–119 implications for teaching, 124–126 language and private speech, 119–121 limitations, 122 zone of proximal development, 121–122 Theory of fluid intelligence, 196 Theory of mind, 171 Theory of moral development Haidt, 175–176 Kohlberg, 171–173 Nucci, 173–175 Theory of multiple intelligences (MI), 198–200 Think-Pair-Share, 277 Thinking styles, 207 Thorndike, E. L., 18 Three-tiered system, autism spectrum disorder interventions, 214 Tier 1, 2, and 3 prevention, 317–318 Time for learning, 560–561, 575 Time on task, 561, 602 Time out, 312 Title IX of the Education Amendments of 1972, 73 Token reinforcement systems, 308–309 Tonic-clonic seizure, 231 Tools, cognitive development and (Vygotsky), 118–119 Tools for Getting Along, 499 Tools of the Mind, 125 Top-down processing, 335 Tracking, 54 Transfer of learning, 408–411 Transfer responsibility in scaffolding, 428 Transgender people, 65–65, 72 gender bias and discrimination, 67–74 Transition programming, 230 Translation, problem solving and, 392–395

Subject Index Trauma, 224–227 dealing for immigrant students, 276 effects on students, 10 symptoms, 312 Trauma-informed educators, 225 Tree diagrams, 384 Trends in education, 4–11 Triadic reciprocal causality, 470–472 Triarchic theory of successful intelligence, 193 Tripod Student Perception Survey, 694 True/false testing, 659 True score, 655 Trust, importance of, 160 Tryptophan, 52 TV Talk Show, 277 Type 1 diabetes, 232 Type 2 diabetes, 232 Unconditioned response (UR), 294 Unconditioned stimulus (US), 294 Underlining, as learning strategy, 381–382 Understanding by Design (UbD), 637–639 Understanding questions, 619, 632 Undesirable behaviors, response to, 310–314 Unfair penalization in assessment bias, 656 Uninvolved parents, 144 United Spinal Association, 188 United States, education trends, 4–11 Universal design, 642 Universal design for learning (UDL), 313, 642–643 University of Chicago, Laboratory School, 18 Unknown onset seizure, 231 Upper class, defined, 48 U.S. Institute of Education Sciences (IES), 22 Utility value, 526 Vagueness, 609 Validity, 655–656, 670–671 Value, 526 Value-added modeling (VAM), 693–694 Variability, test scores, 682 Variable-interval reinforcement schedule, 299 Variable-ratio reinforcement schedule, 299 Venn diagrams, 384 Verbal skills. See Language development; Vocabulary development Verbalization in problem solving, 396

Vicarious experiences (as sources of self-efficacy), 478–479, 482 Vicarious learning, 472–477 Vicarious reinforcement, 474 Video examples ABC student, 24 academic optimism, 611 academic vocabulary, 261 adaptation, 104 ADHD students, strategies for, 243 adolescents, 162 anxiety, 541 assessing knowledge, 666 assisted learning, 124–125 assistive device to communicate, 232 authentic questions, 631 autism spectrum disorders, teaching of, 233–234 behavioral learning theory, 291 best-works portfolio, 668 Black racial identity, 58 bullying, 588 care for students, 10 caring relationships, 578 characteristics of teacher, 12 classroom management, 560 classroom rules, 565 clear communicate goals, 624 code switching, 264 cognitive approach, 331 community of learners, 79 conferences, 679 conservation, 107 constructivist view of learning, 421 contiguity, 293 creativity vs. innovation, 201 critical thinking skills, 404 culturally relevant pedagogy, 76 curiosity, motivation and, 535 deep learning, 429 desegregation and discrimination, 46 dialect and accents, 263 differentiated instruction, 641 digital media, learning and, 451 disabilities, 209 diversity among students, 6 domain-specific knowledge, 332 early maturing, 136 eating disorder, 139 effective teachers, qualities of, 610 elaboration, 359 emergent literacy skills, 254 emotional and behavioral disorders, 224

English learners, teaching of, 269 formative classroom assessments, 654 friendships, 148 gender roles and bias, 66 goal-focused planning, 616 goal setting, 520 good teaching models, 12 guided reading, grouping for, 612 IEP (Individualized Education Program), 210 immigrant students, 276–277 independent work, 627 informal assessment, 665 inquiry-based learning, 431 intrinsic and extrinsic motivations, 511 knowledgeable and inventive, teacher as, 13 Kohlberg’s stages of moral development, 172 least restrictive environment (LRE), 210 literacy skill development, 253 long-term storage, 350 memory strategies, 331, 344 metacognition, 376 modeling, 474 moral development, 171–173 motivation, 513–514, 547 multicultural teaching principles, 81 Native Americans, discrimination of, 57 peer teaching, 424 physical activity, 138 physical development goals, 139 planning of instruction, 615 portfolios, 666, 668 positive behavior support system, 31 problem based learning, 432 problem solving skill development, 389 psychosocial development, 159 receptive language, growth in, 255 reinforcement methods, 303–304, 308–309 routine, classroom, 563 RtI program, 213 rubric formation, 669 scaffolding, use of, 428 Section 504 services, 212 self-efficacy, source of, 479–480 self-evaluation, 499 self-regulation, 485, 491, 587 sheltered instruction, 274 SIOP system, 274, 274

S-17

social and emotional learning (SEL), 153 state test data, use of, 691 student engagement, 574, 609 student’s culture, 43 students’ interests, 535–536 students learning through online, 451 taking notes, 382 tapping into cultural funds of knowledge, 279 teacher as researcher, 23 teacher expectations, 610 teachers support a growth mindset, 532 theories of development, 363 theory of multiple intelligences, 197 token reinforcement systems, 308 transfer of learning, 408 vocabulary, importance of, 260 Vygotsky’s theory, 119 working memory, 342 zone of proximal development, 121 Videogames, aggressive behavior and, 151 Violence, effect of media, 151 Virtual learning environments (VLEs), 453 Visual cortex, brain, 95 Visual impairments, 232 Visual information, brain regions for, 95 Visual-spatial information, working memory and, 216 Visual strategies for teaching English language learners, 272 Visuo-spatial sketchpad, 340–342 Vocabulary development. See also Language development developmental milestones and, 250–251 preoperational stage of development, 107 Vocational Rehabilitation Act (1973), 212 Voicing problems, 223 Volition, 486–489 Vygotsky, Lev, 116–121, 422– 423, 425, 427, 440, 501 implications for teaching, 124–126 limitations, 122 theory of cognitive development, 116–121 zone of proximal development, 119, 423 Warlpiri Sign Language, 259 Warm demanders, 599

S-18 Subject Index Warmth, teacher, 609 Webb, Norman, 621 Webb’s Depth of Knowledge (DOK), 620 Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-V), 195 Welcome centers, 278 What Works Clearinghouse, 22, 25 White coat syndrome, 295 White matter (brain), 94 White privilege, 59 The Whiteness Project, 59 Wikis, 451, 453, 462 Williams, Marco, 59

Willpower, 486–489, see also Volition Within-class ability grouping, 640 Withitness in classroom management, 576 Wolf Reintroduction and Management Learning Unit, 442 Work-avoidant learners, 524 Work ethic, 487 Worked examples cognitive views of learning, 343 complex cognitive processes, 392–394, 413, 436

constructivism and designing learning environments, 436 Working-backward strategy, 396 Working class, defined, 48 Working memory, 333, 340–343 capacity of, 342 cognitive load and retaining information, 343–344 fluid intelligence and, 216 individual differences, 346–347 information processing system, 334

limited capacity, 345 model of, 340 neuroscience of, 215 Writing headlines, 277 Writing skills, 216, 218 Yale Center for Emotional Intelligence, 200 z score, 684 Zero reject, 209 Zero tolerance policies, 585 Zimmerman’s three-phase model of self-regulated learning, 491, 492 Zone of proximal development (ZPD), 121–122, 423, 427

This page intentionally left blank

This page intentionally left blank