Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System [1 ed.] 9781617619595, 9781607412595

Noise-induced hearing loss is one of the most common occupational diseases. Approximately 30 million workers in the USA

184 113 25MB

English Pages 411 Year 2010

Report DMCA / Copyright

DOWNLOAD FILE

Polecaj historie

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System [1 ed.]
 9781617619595, 9781607412595

Citation preview

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved. Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved. Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

OTOLARYNGOLOGY RESEARCH ADVANCES

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

DEAFNESS, HEARING LOSS AND THE AUDITORY SYSTEM

No part of this digital document may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or by any means. The publisher has taken reasonable care in the preparation of this digital document, but makes no expressed or implied warranty of any kind and assumes no responsibility for any errors or omissions. No liability is assumed for incidental or consequential damages in connection with or arising out of information contained herein. This digital document is sold with the clear understanding that the publisher is not engaged in rendering legal, medical or any other professional services.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

OTOLARYNGOLOGY RESEARCH ADVANCES Handbook of Pulmonary Diseases: Etiology, Diagnosis and Treatment Krisztián Fodor and Antal Tóth (Editors) 2009. ISBN: 978-1-60741-898-6 Snoring: Causes, Diagnosis and Treatment Eugene Lefebvre and Renaud Moreau (Editors) 2010. ISBN: 978-1-60876-215-6 Snoring: Causes, Diagnosis and Treatment Eugene Lefebvre and Renaud Moreau (Editors) 2010. ISBN: 978-1-61668-854-7 (Online Book) Handbook of Pharyngeal Diseases: Etiology, Diagnosis and Treatment Aaron P. Nazario and Julien K. Vermeulen (Editors) 2010. ISBN: 978-1-60876-430-3 Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System Derick Fiedler and Rowland Krause (Editors) 2010. ISBN: 978-1-60741-259-5

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Laryngeal Diseases: Symptoms, Diagnosis and Treatments Oldrich Nemecek and Viktor Mares (Editors) 2010. ISBN: 978-1-60876-107-4

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

OTOLARYNGOLOGY RESEARCH ADVANCES SERIES

DEAFNESS, HEARING LOSS AND THE AUDITORY SYSTEM

DERICK FIEDLER AND Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

ROWLAND KRAUSE EDITORS

Nova Science Publishers, Inc. New York Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010 by Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or by any means: electronic, electrostatic, magnetic, tape, mechanical photocopying, recording or otherwise without the written permission of the Publisher. For permission to use material from this book please contact us: Telephone 631-231-7269; Fax 631-231-8175 Web Site: http://www.novapublishers.com NOTICE TO THE READER The Publisher has taken reasonable care in the preparation of this book, but makes no expressed or implied warranty of any kind and assumes no responsibility for any errors or omissions. No liability is assumed for incidental or consequential damages in connection with or arising out of information contained in this book. The Publisher shall not be liable for any special, consequential, or exemplary damages resulting, in whole or in part, from the readers‘ use of, or reliance upon, this material. Any parts of this book based on government reports are so indicated and copyright is claimed for those parts to the extent applicable to compilations of such works.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Independent verification should be sought for any data, advice or recommendations contained in this book. In addition, no responsibility is assumed by the publisher for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from any methods, products, instructions, ideas or otherwise contained in this publication. This publication is designed to provide accurate and authoritative information with regard to the subject matter covered herein. It is sold with the clear understanding that the Publisher is not engaged in rendering legal or any other professional services. If legal or any other expert assistance is required, the services of a competent person should be sought. FROM A DECLARATION OF PARTICIPANTS JOINTLY ADOPTED BY A COMMITTEE OF THE AMERICAN BAR ASSOCIATION AND A COMMITTEE OF PUBLISHERS.

LIBRARY OF CONGRESS CATALOGING-IN-PUBLICATION DATA Deafness, hearing loss, and the auditory system / editors, Derick Fiedler and Rowland Krause. p. ; cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN:  (eBook) 1. Hearing disorders. 2. Deafness. I. Fiedler, Derick. II. Krause, Rowland. [DNLM: 1. Hearing Loss--etiology. 2. Hearing Loss--pathology. 3. Hearing Loss--therapy. WV 270 D2773 2009] RF290.D427 2009 617.8--dc22 2009041960

Published by Nova Science Publishers, Inc. New York Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

CONTENTS Preface Chapter 1

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

1

Chapter 2

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

81

Chapter 3

Current Concepts in Radiation-Induced Sensori-Neural Hearing Loss Wong-Kein Low

111

Target Antigens in Immune-Mediated Hearing Loss: From Recognition to Diagnostic Tool Boulassel Mohamed-Rachid

137

Damage of Chronic Sublethal Hypoxia to the Immature Auditory Brainstem Ze Dong Jiang

159

Chapter 4

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

vii

Chapter 5

Chapter 6

Treatment of Tinnitus with Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Tobias Kleinjung and Berthold Langguth

Chapter 7

An Informatics Approach to Studying the Role of Myosins in the Inner Ear Christopher M. Frenz

197

Tight Junction Proteins and the Etiology of Hereditary Hearing Loss in Humans and Animal Models Tamar Ben-Yosef

211

A Systematic Review of Hearing Protective Devises: Types, Uses and Safety Regina P. El Dib

227

Chapter 8

Chapter 9

Chapter 10

DPOAE Abnormalities after Perinatal Asphyxia Ze Dong Jiang

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

181

249

vi Chapter 11

Role of Otoacoustic Emissions in Hearing Loss Evaluation Arturo Moleti, Renata Sisto, Angelo Tirabasso and Stefano Di Girolamo

Chapter 12

Sodium Enoxiparin in the Treatment of Idiopathic Sudden Sensorineural Hearing Loss Angelo Salami, Renzo Mora, Francesco Mora, Barbara Crippa, Luca Guastini and Massimo Dellepiane

Chapter 13

Chapter 14

Chapter 15

Chapter 16

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Contents 263

279

Molecular Screening of Deafness in Populations and Patients with Nonsyndromic Congenital Deafness from the Volga-Ural Region of Russia Lilya U. Dzhemileva, Irina M. Khidiyatova and Elza K. Khusnutdinova

291

Signs of Binaural Processing with Bilateral Cochlear Implants in the Case of Someone with more than 50 Years of Unilateral Deafness Celene McNeill, William Noble and Anna O’Brien

301

Cisplatin Ototoxicity: A Key for the Understanding of the Inner Ear Pathology José Ramón García-Berrocal, Rafael Ramírez-Camacho, José Ángel González-García and Rodrigo Martínez-Monedero Modeling the Perceptual Masking Properties of the Human Auditory System Using Neural Networks Hossein L. Najafi

313

323

Chapter 17

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition Serajul Haque, Roberto Togneri and Anthony Zaknich

333

Commentary

Acupuncture in the Treatment of Ear Disorders Wong-Kein Low

373

Index

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

375

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

PREFACE Noise-induced hearing loss is one of the most common occupational diseases. Approximately 30 million workers in the USA alone are exposed to hazardous noise at work. There is no effective treatment for permanent hearing loss resulting from excessive noise exposure. Furthermore, the condition can be easily prevented using preventative measures such as personal hearing protection devices or hearing protectors. This book summarizes the evidence for the effectiveness and safety of different types of hearing protective devises among workers exposed to noise in the workplace. Furthermore, sensori-neural hearing loss is a frequent complication of radiotherapy of head and neck tumours, when the auditory pathways have been included in the radiation field. This book focuses on reviewing three aspects of radiation-induced SNHL which have significant impact on modern-day medicine. Otoacoustic emissions (OAEs) are a promising technique for the early detection of mild hearing loss. The authors of this book re-evaluate recently published OAE data and its role in hearing loss evaluation. Other chapters provide new insights into our understanding of the influence of chronic sublethal hypoxia on functional integrity of the auditory system, interventions in hearing impairment due to perinatal asphyxia and the effects of clinical studies using rTMS in tinnitus patients .Chapter 1 - The ventral complex of the lateral lemniscus (VCLL) is a group of neurons thought to play an important role in coding the temporal aspects of sound. This review chapter will examine the current literature available describing the anatomy, physiology, cellular morphology and spatial organisation of the VCLL and its constituent neurons. Studies included in this chapter are distinguished by species and include both in vivo and in vitro data. Intracellular and extracellular responses to noise and tone bursts from VCLL neurons, as well as 3-dimensional reconstructions of the VCLL illustrating the functional distribution of various auditory response properties are highlighted. An examination of the ongoing and controversial search for a tonotopic distribution within the VCLL is presented along with arguments on both sides. Finally, a powerful, fast inhibitory circuit within the VCLL is described that may co-ordinate the timing of higher brain centre responses. This and other possible functional roles are proposed in an attempt to uncover the significance of these enigmatic nuclei in mammalian auditory processing. Chapter 2 - Excessive noise exposure can lead to metabolic and/or mechanical effects resulting in alterations of the structural elements of the organ of Corti. Besides occupational noise exposure, there are several sources of leisure noise exposure attributing to noiseinduced hearing loss (NIHL). These leisure noise exposures include exposure to loud music

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

viii

Derick Fiedler and Rowland Krause

or even participation in non-musical activities. The major sources of the former activities are attendance at discotheques, nightclubs and live concerts, and usage of personal music players. Recently, it was estimated that the prevalence of leisure noise in the UK has tripled since the early 1980s. Therefore, an increase in prevalence of NIHL in adolescents and young adults is assumed, especially in the mainstream media. Epidemiologic literature is, however, more equivocal. The lack of hearing deterioration is most likely explained by the fact that leisure noise exposure is insufficient to cause important hearing loss. Nevertheless, the effects of leisure noise exposure on the auditory system of young people is a cause for concern. The purpose of this chapter is to review the sources of leisure noise in relation to their contribution in life-time noise exposure of individuals and to present epidemiologic data regarding NIHL in young people caused by leisure noise. Differences in attitude of young people regarding leisure noise exposure and usage of hearing protection are discussed, as well as the importance of prevention and sensitization campaigns. Moreover, short- and long-term effects of several leisure noise sources are reviewed. Shortcomings in the current literature are stressed, and proposals for additional research regarding the usefulness of several audiological tests to detect minimal cochlear damage after exposure to loud leisure noise are pointed out. Chapter 3 - Sensori-neural hearing loss is a frequent complication of radiotherapy of head and neck tumours, when the auditory pathways have been included in the radiation field. This chapter focuses on the review of three aspects of radiation-induced SNHL that have a significant impact on modern day medicine: 1) effect of radiation on retro-cochlear nervous pathways, 2) cellular and molecular processes involved in radiation-induced ototoxicity, 3) combined ototoxic effects of radiation and cisplatin. A model of radiation-induced damage of the sensor-neural auditory system has been proposed, which is based on the concept of dosedependent, reactive oxygen species related cochlear cell apoptosis without significant damage to the retro-cochlear pathway. This model supports the feasibility of cochlear implantation, should one be clinically indicated in such patients. It can explain clinical observations such as radiation-induced sensori-neural hearing loss being dose-dependent and affects the high frequencies more than the lower frequencies. It also opens up the possibility of preventive strategies targeted at different stages of the apoptotic process. The use of anti-oxidants which target upstream pathways, appear promising. In particular, it may have a major role in chemoradiation where combined ototoxic effects lead to significant sensori-neural hearing loss. Chapter 4 - As early as 1931, immune dysfunction was hypothesized as the impairment of the inner ear‘s function. Since then, a variety of antigens have been described and implicated in immune-mediated hearing loss. Among these are collagen, heat shock protein70, myelin protein P0, glycolipids, β-tubulin protein, calcium binding protein S-100β, Raf-1 protein and more recently β-actin, cochlin, β-tectorin and choline transporter-like protein 2. Of these target autoantigens, cochlin and β-tectorin seem to be specifically expressed in the inner ear. The validity of these proteins as autoantigens and their utility as clinical marker - or more importantly as predictors of hearing loss - is yet to be determined. Central to their utility is their ability to distinguish between individuals with or without immune-mediated hearing loss. Identifying one or more proteins as autoantigens predictive of hearing loss, would provide an opportunity to diagnose, treat and possibly monitor immune-mediated inner ear hearing loss. Chapter 5 - Acute severe or lethal hypoxia is well known to damage the immature auditory system. However, limited information is available about whether chronic sublethal

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Preface

ix

hypoxia also damages this system. Previous studies in animal experiments showed that chronic sublethal hypoxia adversely affects the immature cerebral cortex. Recent studies have revealed that chronic sublethal hypoxia damage functional integrity of the immature auditory system in both human infants and animal models of chronic sublethal hypoxia. In human infants, a typical clinical problem that is associated with chronic sublethal hypoxia is bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD), a major perinatal problem that often leads to neurodevelopmental deficits. In recent years, the influence of this problem on the immature brain has attracted considerable attention. The understanding of the influence of chronic sublethal hypoxia associated with BPD on the immature auditory system remains very limited. Brainstem auditory function has recently been studied in very preterm infants who suffered chronic sublethal hypoxia due to BPD. In the maximum length sequence brainstem auditory responses the components that reflect central auditory function were significantly abnormal, while the components that reflect peripheral neural function of the auditory system did not show any major abnormalities. The results suggest that chronic sublethal hypoxia damage neural conduction, reflecting impaired myelination and synaptic dysfunction, in the immature central auditory system, with no major effect on neural function of the peripheral auditory system. Newborn rats reared in chronic sublethal hypoxia showed a significant reduction in myelination in major white tracts and a patchy distribution of the residual myelination in the auditory brainstem. There was also a reduction in myelin basic protein expression. It appears that chronic sublethal hypoxia inhibits myelination and the expression of myelin basic protein in the auditory brainstem. These recent novel findings provide new insights into our understanding of the influence of chronic sublethal hypoxia on functional integrity of the very immature auditory system. Chapter 6 - The pathophysiology of tinnitus remains incompletely understood and treatment is elusive. Recent neurophysiological and neuroimaging data suggest that some forms of tinnitus are associated with synchronized hyperactivity in the auditory cortex. Therefore targeted modulation of tinnitus-related cortical hyperactivity has been considered as a new promising treatment strategy. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) is a non-invasive method for modifying neural activity in the stimulated area and at a distance along functional anatomical connections. This chapter will summarize the effects of clinical studies using rTMS in tinnitus patients. This technique can be applied in two different ways in diagnosing and treating tinnitus patients. One approach uses single sessions of high-frequency rTMS applied to the temporal cortex. This method has shown success in suppressing tinnitus transiently during the time of stimulation and has been suggested as a predictor for treatment outcome of direct electrical epidural stimulation with implanted electrodes. Low-frequency rTMS is an efficient method to selectively reduce the abnormally increased activity in cortical areas. Several small controlled studies demonstrated beneficial effects in tinnitus patients after repeated sessions of low-frequency rTMS. In some patients, treatment effects outlasted the stimulation period by twelve months and more. However, available studies are characterized by the high interindividual variability of treatment effects and only moderate effect sizes. Basic research should focus on a better understanding of the neurobiological effects of this technique. Clinical studies with larger sample sizes can provide more information on the impact of patient-related (e.g. hearing loss, age, tinnitus-duration) and stimulation-related (e.g. frequency, target control) factors on treatment outcome.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

x

Derick Fiedler and Rowland Krause

Chapter 7 - The ear is a complex organ system that contains a diversity of cell and tissue types and as such the elucidation of the various biological factors that can contribute to deafness can be quite challenging. The starting point for any such investigation is generally a comprehensive search of the biomedical literature to see what factors have already been determined to play a role and in what ways. The overwhelming volume of biomedical literature available, however, can often make uncovering such information difficult as standard keyword based search techniques often fail to narrow down search results into readily manageable quantities. This study will demonstrate the use of the informatics technique of text mining via regular expression based pattern matching as a means of uncovering the various genetic causes of deafness described in the biomedical literature. Several genetic causes of deafness can be linked to genes encoding proteins of the myosin family. As such, a text mining technique is illustrated that will match the expression of myosin variants to various parts of the ear in an effort to better elucidate the roles of myosins in the normal and aberrant function of the ear. Chapter 8 - Multicellular organisms have separate compartments of different compositions. In the cochlea of the inner ear separation between endolymph and perilimph, two compartments with very different sodium and potassium ion concentrations, is necessary for normal hearing. This compartmentalization is achieved by tight junctions (TJs), which form the major selective barrier of the paracellular pathway between epithelial cells. TJ strands contain at least five types of membrane-spanning proteins. To date, mutations in five different TJ membrane integral proteins have been associated with hearing loss. Mutations in claudin 14 and in tricellulin are associated with hereditary hearing loss in humans. Claudin 9-, claudin 11- and claudin 14-mutant mice are deaf. In zebrafish, a mutation of the cldnj gene leads to abnormal auditory and vestibular functions. There are different etiologies for hearing loss associated with each of these proteins, including hair cell loss, reduced endocochlear potentials, and abnormal embryonic development of the inner ear. Nevertheless, in all cases the underlying cause is dysfunction of the paracellular barrier. These findings elucidate the crucial role played by TJs in the auditory apparatus, and enhance our understanding of the hearing process. This knowledge will be further enhanced by identification of hearing-related phenotypes associated with additional TJ proteins and generation of additional animal models in years to come. Chapter 9 - Introduction: Noise-induced hearing loss is one of the most common occupational diseases. Approximately 30 million workers in the USA alone are exposed to hazardous noise at work (WHO 2002). There is no effective treatment for permanent hearing loss resulting from excessive noise exposure. The condition can be easily prevented using preventative measures such as personal hearing protection devices or hearing protector (i.e., earplugs, earmuffs). Objective: To summarize the evidence for the effectiveness and safety of different types of hearing protective devices among workers exposed to noise in the workplace. Methods: A systematic review of the literature was conducted. The authors searched the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL); Pubmed; EMBASE; LILACS and; Current Controlled Trials for ongoing trials. The date of the last search was 16th February 2009. Studies were included if they had a randomized or quasi-randomized design, if they were among noise exposed (> 80 dB(A)) workers and, if there was at least two hearing protectors to be compared. The reviewer selected relevant trials, assessed methodological quality and extracted data.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Preface

xi

Results: Two studies were included with a total of 46 participants. It was not possible to combine the included studies in a meta-analysis. Both included studies evaluated earplugs as hearing protectors. The representations of meta-analysis with only one study showed that participants that worn HL SmartFit had fewer number of difficulties per number of conversations compared to E-A-R Push-Ins earplugs. In addition, participants reported higher rate of satisfaction wearing HL SmartFit when compared to E-A-R Push-Ins or Sonomax SonoCustoms. Conclusion: The evidence found in this review showed that the HL SmartFit earplug is more effective compared to E-A-R Push-Ins foam earplug and Sonomax SonoCustoms regarding the number of difficulties per number of conversations and satisfaction. Future trials should have standardized outcomes measures such as attenuation, speech intelligibility and audibility (hear communication) in the noise environment, worker acceptance of HPDs and the likelihood that workers will wear them consistently, hearing loss thresholds by audiometric test, costs and others. Chapter 10 - Perinatal asphyxia is an important risk for acquired hearing impairment in infants and children. Selection and implementation of a proper plan to intervene in hearing impairment requires accurate information about all frequencies important for speech and language development. Thus, it is crucial to obtain detailed information about cochlear function in infants with hearing impairment. Distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) have been widely used to examine cochlear function in infants. Recent studies found that infants after perinatal asphyxia showed a decrease in DPOAE pass rates at most frequencies in the first few days after birth. The decrease occurred mainly between 1 and 5 kHz, particularly 1 and 2 kHz. Overall DPOAE pass rate was also decreased. These results suggest that perinatal asphyxia damages the neonatal cochlea, which occurs mainly at the frequencies 1-5 kHz, particular at 1 and 2 kHz. At 1 month the decreased DPOAE pass rates did show any improvement. It seems that the impairment detected in the first few days after birth is unlikely to improve in later neonatal period. Follow-up studies revealed that at 6 months after birth DPOAE pass rates were increased slightly at most frequencies, but were decreased slightly at some other frequencies. At 12 months infants after perinatal asphyxia still demonstrated a decrease in DPOAE pass rates at most frequencies, particularly 1 and 2 kHz. Similarly, overall DPOAE pass rates were also decreased. The same was true for the comparison of the pass rates at 12 months with those at 6 months. These longitudinal prospective studies of DPOAEs indicate that hypoxia-ischemia that is associated with perinatal asphyxia adversely affects cochlear function. The major affected frequencies are 1 and 2 kHz. At 1 year, cochlear function remains relatively poor. Therefore, cochlear function, mainly at 1 and 2 kHz, is impaired after perinatal asphyxia, which is persistent during the postnatal development, with only slightly improvement. These findings provide useful information for selecting and implementing early interventions in hearing impairment due to perinatal asphyxia. Chapter 11 - Otoacoustic Emissions (OAEs) are a promising technique for the early detection of mild hearing loss. The correlation between OAE levels and the audiometric threshold level has been well-established for transient-evoked OAEs (TEOAEs), Distortion products (DPOAEs) and stimulus-frequency OAEs (SFOAEs). Spontaneous OAEs are also correlated with minima of the audiometric threshold microstructure. The use of TEOAEs in neonatal hearing screening tests is widely accepted. The diagnostic potential of OAEs has not been fully exploited yet, partly due to intrinsic limitations (OAE responses cannot be

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

xii

Derick Fiedler and Rowland Krause

measured at all in the profoundly hearing-impaired subjects) that permit their application only to subjects with mild or medium hearing loss levels (HL < 40-50 dB), partly because the OAE generation mechanisms are quite complex and not yet fully understood. The constant improvement of innovative acquisition and analysis techniques and the better understanding of cochlear mechanics are improving the effectiveness of OAE techniques in at least three main diagnostic fields: (a) objective evaluation of the hearing threshold level (b) objective evaluation of the frequency resolution of hearing (cochlear tuning) (c) monitoring small modifications of the hearing functionality in subjects with normalhearing or mild hearing loss. As regards the first two applications, at the present stage, the OAE techniques can surely be considered as an important objective complement to the correspondent psychoacoustical techniques, i.e. Tonal Audiometry (TA) and the measure of the Critical Bandwidth (CB). OAE measurements can also complement the information coming from other objective techniques, such as the Auditory Brainstem Response (ABR). The third use of OAEs is very promising to monitor the hearing function of subjects exposed to noise, particularly in the case of professional exposure. Some recent studies suggest indeed that OAEs could be more sensitive than TA for this specific diagnostic task. In this study the authors report a re-analysis of recently published OAE data of a population of subjects with long-term exposure to industrial noise and of another group of subjects exposed to intense noise in a discotheque, before and after exposure. The results show the potential of OAE-based tests in all the three above-mentioned tasks: (a) dichotomous OAE-based tests can be effective for detecting hearing loss down to sub-clinical levels (10 dB HL); (b) cochlear tuning can be objectively estimated from the OAE latency; (c) OAE levels are sensitive indicators of noise exposure in subjects with normal hearing or mild hearing loss. Chapter 12 - The audiovestibular system can be affected by an immunological etiology; the presence of immune mediated sensorineural hearing loss (IMSNHL) as part of or in combination with other autoimmune diseases is well documented in the literature. Hearing loss can be caused by autoimmune disorders localized to the inner ear or secondary to systemic immune diseases (Cogan‘s syndrome, juvenile chronic arthritis, ulcerative colitis, Wegener‘s granulomatosis, scleroderma, pulseless disease, and SLE). A systemic autoimmune disorder can be present in fewer than one-third of cases The clinical presentation of immune inner-ear disease is extremely variable and depends on the type of immune reaction and on the site of injury within the inner ear. IMSNHL typically presents with an idiopathic, progressive unilateral and successive bilateral rapidly progressive sensorineural hearing loss; the course of the hearing loss occurs over weeks to months and is most common in middle-aged women; it may be accompanied by tinnitus and vertigo and is almost always unilateral. IMSNHL is still a diagnostic and therapeutic dilemma, and predicting recovery from it is very difficult. Different factors may influence a prognosis: e.g., severity of hearing loss, duration of symptoms before treatment, presence of vertigo, type of audiogram, and age of patients. The therapeutic approaches normally used for this pathological condition include the systemic and local administration of cortisone, vasoactive agents, anticoagulants, vitamin

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Preface

xiii

complexes, a cytotoxic agent and plasmapheresis. These drugs can be effective in reversing such hearing loss, although at the cost of occasionally severe side effects. Currently, evaluating the importance of an autoimmune phenomenon in the genesis of inner-ear disease is difficult because the clinical and biological criteria of autoimmune deafness have not yet been well defined. A positive response to treatment is a criterion for the diagnosis of immune inner-ear disease. This chapter aims to assess the effect of sodium enoxaparin on the recovery of hearing in patients affected by ISSNHL. Sodium enoxaparin was administered subcutaneously at a dose of 4,000 IU once a day for 10 days. Sodium enoxaparin is a particular kind of heparin with a low molecular weight (LMWH) and is endowed with a high antithrombotic activity. The literature does not report any therapeutic protocols for autoimmune IMSNHL treatment with sodium enoxaparin or other kinds of unfractionated heparin. Our decision to use enoxaparin was based both on the pathogenesis of this condition and on evaluation of the other classes of drugs currently used. Chapter 13 - The authors studied the molecular basis of NSHL in the Volga-Ural region. The Volga–Ural region of Russia is of particular interest, because its ethnic populations mostly belong to the Turkic, Finno-Ugric, and Slavonic linguistic groups and have complex ethnogenesis and combine the Caucasian and Mongoloid components in various proportions. A total number of 100 patients of Tatars, Russian or mixed ethnicity and 768 population samples were analyzed by PCR-SSCP followed by direct sequencing of the GJB2 gene. The GJB6 gene deletion and the common non-syndromic deafness-causing mitochondrial mutations were also tested when appropriate. The 35delG mutation was predominant among patients from Volga-Ural region. Mutation 312del14 in GJB2 gene is the second most frequent cause of non-syndromic hearing impairment in the Volgo-Ural region. The authors‘ data testify to the founder effect and suggest an eastward distribution of 35delG, since its frequency in Finno-Ugric populations gradually decreases from Estonia to Komi. The question whether the Volga-Ural region could be one of the founder sources for the 235delC and 167delT mutations, widespread in Asia and Israel community, is open. Also, the 312del14 mutation in GJB2 is the second most frequent cause of non-syndromic hearing impairment in the Volga-Ural region. Chapter 14 - A case is presented of a 70-year-old man with a profound sensorineural hearing loss in the right ear since childhood and who developed sudden severe hearing loss in the left ear at age 63. Eventually, after he received cochlear implants in both ears, he started to present behavioural auditory processing skills associated with binaural hearing, such as improved ability understanding speech in the presence of background noise, and sound localization. Responsiveness and outcomes were measured using cortical auditory evoked potentials, speech perception in noise, sound localization performance , and a self-rating questionnaire. The results suggest that even after more than 50 years of unilateral deafness it is possible to develop binaural interaction and sound localization. Chapter 15 - Ototoxicity is defined as the tendency of certain therapeutic agents to cause functional impairment and cellular degeneration of the inner ear and of the eighth cranial nerve. Cisplatin (cis-diamminedichloroplatinum II; CDDP) is the first generation platinumcontaining antitumoral drug known to be effective against a variety of solid tumors. Ototoxicity has been observed in up to 36% of patients receiving cisplatin. Risk factors for cisplatin ototoxicity include renal insufficiency, co-administration with aminoglycosides

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

xiv

Derick Fiedler and Rowland Krause

and/or radiation therapy, and increased cumulative doses. Monitoring for ototoxicity should be individualized: an audiogram (high frequencies and ultrahigh frequencies) should be obtained at the onset of therapy, before each successive dose, and with the onset of symptoms. Second generation platinum derived drugs have been developed in order to minimize the toxic effect on the inner ear. Only 1% of intracellular platinum (Pt) is bound to nuclear DNA with the great majority of the drug available to interact with other cellular targets. The quantification of Pt inside the inner ear by quadrupole inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometry (ICP-MS) has shown the presence of Pt-biomolecules in nuclear, cytosolic and mitochondrial fractions. The Ptbiomolecules binding could play a role in ototoxicity since the complexes were different depending on the drug and represents a future outlook in the management of cisplatin ototoxiciy. Although classically the most prominent change seen in the cochlea after cisplatin administration consists of loss of outer hair cells (OHCs), new directions in the research allowed us to provide a main role to the supporting cells (Deiter‘s cells) since they appeared more sensitive than outer hair cells. In vitro and in vivo experiments have shown that apoptotic cell death is the primary mechanism of cisplatin antitumoral action. A novel investigation has shown that cisplatin induces apoptosis in hair cells, supporting cells, spiral ganglion cells, stria vascularis cells and spiral ligament fibrocytes by the activation of caspases, evoking an intrinsic pathway of proapoptotic signalling. This innovative idea has facilitated the development of several strategies to prevent oxidative stress-induced apoptosis of inner ear cells that have been exposed to cisplatin. However, the loss of the population of some type of inner ear cells could be irreversible, and then it would be necessary to replace these cells. The conversion of inner ear stem cells to sensory neurons and the search of a possible common pathway of inner ear damage need to be explored and they will lead the future trends in inner ear research. Chapter 16 - Modeling of the perceptual masking properties of the Human Auditory System is investigated. An artificial neural network is trained to model the perceptual masking map of the human auditory system. Successful application of the model to data hiding is demonstrated. Chapter 17 – Automatic speech recognition (ASR) broadly encompasses the recognition of human speech by a machine or by some artificial intelligence. The recognition process should be robust, that is, it should accurately recognise the spoken word in the presence of speaker variabilities, word perplexities, and speech corrupted by noise which are introduced during transmission and in the communication channels itself. Research in the past several decades has produced speech processing techniques like the short-time Fourier transform (STFT), the linear prediction (LP) and autoregressive (AR) methods, and the mel-frequency cepstral coefficients (MFCC), which have contributed significantly to robust speech recognition. The ability of the human auditory system to recognize speech in adverse and noisy conditions has motivated speech researchers to include features of human perception in speech recognition systems. Particularly in the early 1980s, several computational models of the auditory periphery based on physiological measurements of the response on individual fibres of the auditory nerve were proposed. These ―cochlear models‖ only provided marginal improvements at higher computational costs when applied to speech recognition. As a result, a decline in the interest in auditory models was observed until computing resources were able to meet the intensive computational requirements of such models. In recent years, there has

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Preface

xv

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

been a resurgence in perceptual speech processing after research provided evidence that it may lead to improved recognition performances. This chapter describes several psychoacoustic properties of the peripheral auditory system applied to a speech recognition front-end. Dynamic behaviour of the auditory nerves are incorporated in speech parametrization utilizing temporal processing, so that time domain information as appropriate time constants are incorporated in speech parameterization. A simplified method of synaptic adaptation as determined by psychoacoustic observations in an auditory nerve is described. It utilizes a high pass infinite impulse response (IIR) temporal filter to enhance the signal onsets and the subsequent dynamic and the steady-state characteristics. Speech features are extracted in the temporal mode utilizing a zero-crossing algorithm. The two-tone suppression as observed in the non-linear response of the basilar membrane is described in a zero-crossing auditory front-end using a temporal companding strategy. This may introduce asymmetric gain control without degrading the spectral contrast. The word recognitions are evaluated by continuous density hidden Markov models and are shown to provide improvements over conventional parameterizations in clean and noise conditions. Some of these perceptual algorithms may also benefit people with sensorineural hearing loss and may be implemented in hearing aids and cochlear implants for the hearing impaired through VLSI implementations. Commentary - It remains to be seen if the use of acupuncture in the treatment of ear disorders can be supported by solid well-designed research. Although it is unrealistic to expect it to work in established hearing defects due to cochlear hair-cell loss, it will be interesting to find out if it is of value in treating potentially reversible conditions such as early sudden hearing loss and Meniere‘s disease.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved. Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

In: Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System Editors: D. Fiedler and R. Krause, pp.1-79

ISBN: 978-1-60741-259-5 ©2010 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

Chapter 1

THE VENTRAL COMPLEX OF THE LATERAL LEMNISCUS: A REVIEW David A.X. Nayagam1*, Janine C. Clarey1 and Antonio G. Paolini2 1

The Bionic Ear Institute, 384-388 Albert St., East Melbourne, Victoria, Australia 2 School of Psychological Science, La Trobe University, Australia

ABSTRACT

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The ventral complex of the lateral lemniscus (VCLL) is a group of neurons thought to play an important role in coding the temporal aspects of sound. This review chapter will examine the current literature available describing the anatomy, physiology, cellular morphology and spatial organisation of the VCLL and its constituent neurons. Studies included in this chapter are distinguished by species and include both in vivo and in vitro data. Intracellular and extracellular responses to noise and tone bursts from VCLL neurons, as well as 3-dimensional reconstructions of the VCLL illustrating the functional distribution of various auditory response properties are highlighted. An examination of the ongoing and controversial search for a tonotopic distribution within the VCLL is presented along with arguments on both sides. Finally, a powerful, fast inhibitory circuit within the VCLL is described that may co-ordinate the timing of higher brain centre responses. This and other possible functional roles are proposed in an attempt to uncover the significance of these enigmatic nuclei in mammalian auditory processing.

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS 2-DG AM AMPA AP AVCN BDA CF *

2-Deoxyglucose Amplitude Modulation -amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid Action Potential Anteroventral Cochlear Nucleus Biotinylated Dextran Amine Characteristic Frequency

Corresponding Author

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

2

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini CN CNIC CV DAB dB DCN DNLL DPO EPSP FM FSL GABA HRP IC ILD INLL IPSP ISI(H) ITD LNTB LSO LVPO MGN MNTB MSO NBQX NLL OCA PST(H) PVCN RMP RPO SOC SD SPL TC VCL VCN VNLL VNLLa VNLLc VNLLd VNLLm VNLLv VNTB

Cochlear Nucleus Central Nucleus of the Inferior Colliculus Co-efficient of Variance 3,3‘-diaminobenzidine Decibel Dorsal Cochlear Nucleus Dorsal Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus Dorsal Periolivary Nucleus Excitatory Post-Synaptic Potential Frequency Modulation First Spike Latency γ-aminobutyric acid Horseradish Peroxidase Inferior Colliculus Interaural Level Disparity Intermediate Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus Inhibitory Post-synaptic Potential Interspike Interval (Histogram) Interaural Temporal Disparity Lateral Nucleus of the Trapezoid Body Lateral Superior Olive Lateroventral Periolivary Nucleus Medial Geniculate Nucleus Medial Nucleus of the Trapezoid Body Medial Superior Olive 2,3-dihydroxy-6-nitro-7-sulfamoyl-benzo[f]quinoxaline-2,3-dione Nuclei of the Lateral Lemniscus Octopus Cell Area Post-stimulus Time (Histogram) Posteroventral Cochlear Nucleus Resting Membrane Potential Rostral Periolivary Nucleus Superior Olivary Complex Standard Deviation Sound Pressure Level Tuning Curve Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus Ventral Cochlear Nucleus Ventral Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus Ventral Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus (anterior region) Ventral Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus (columnar region, in echolocating species) Ventral Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus (dorsal region) Ventral Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus (multipolar region, in echolocating species) Ventral Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus (ventral region) Ventral Nucleus of the Trapezoid Body

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

3

INTRODUCTION The ventral complex of the lateral lemniscus (VCLL) is a group of neurons that is embedded within the lateral lemniscal tract. Connecting the brainstem with the midbrain, this pathway is a major component of the mammalian auditory system. Despite the large number of studies of this region, the function of this group of cells and their role in auditory processing is unknown. The aim of this chapter is to collate what is known about the VCLL, as there has not been a review of this body of literature for over half a decade. The current chapter will examine the results of anatomical and electrophysiological studies, both in vivo and in vitro, from a wide range of species. Ultimately, this compendium of our current understanding of the structure and function of the VCLL will provide a basis for future studies to pose more insightful questions and perform pertinent experiments to uncover the significance of this part of the auditory brain.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

GENERAL OVERVIEW OF THE ASCENDING AUDITORY PATHWAY The cochlea‘s efferent fibres form the auditory nerve (the cochlear division of the vestibulocochlear nerve or cranial nerve VIII) and synapse on the most rostral aspect of the medulla (ponto-medullary junction) on neurons of the dorsal and ventral cochlear nuclei (DCN and VCN, respectively) (Berne et al., 1998). These nuclei are the source of axons that form the basis of the entire ascending central auditory system. They provide a wide range of modes of neural integration. The different types of cells in the cochlear nuclei (CN) form the basis of at least four parallel systems in the auditory brainstem (Young, 1998). The axons from the cochlear nuclei course out along three primary pathways: the dorsal acoustic stria, the intermediate acoustic stria and the trapezoid body. The trapezoid body contains fibres bound for both of the superior olivary complexes (SOC; on each side of the brain stem). These binaural SOC nuclei are concerned with sound localisation based on interaural temporal and level disparities (ITD and ILD, respectively). The medial superior olive (MSO) discriminates auditory signals on the basis of phase differences, the lateral superior olive (LSO) on the basis of intensity differences (Kandel et al., 1991). Axons stemming from these superior olivary nuclei connect with the crossed and uncrossed axons travelling up bilaterally from the cochlear nucleus, via the dorsal and intermediate stria, to form the lateral lemnisci, which terminate in the inferior colliculi (IC). Figure 1 shows a stylised overview of the ascending auditory pathway of mammals. The lateral lemnisci are important neuronal fibre tracts in the ascending auditory pathway. Each lateral lemniscus streams dorsally in humans (rostrally in four legged animals) through the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus (NLL). At this stage, some fibres synapse and there is an extensive crossing between the two sides via the commissure of Probst.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

4

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

- modified from Berne et al. (1998): with kind permission from Elsevier Limited.

Figure 1. General overview of the ascending auditory pathway. The central auditory pathways extend from the cochlear nucleus (CN) to the primary auditory cortex. The lateral lemniscus is one of these three main pathways. Formed from crossed and uncrossed axons from the CN and superior olivary complex (SOC), it courses dorsally (rostrally in four-legged animals) to the inferior colliculus (IC) passing through the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus (NLL) along the way, where some fibres terminate. The dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus (DNLL) as well as the ventral complex of the lateral lemniscus (VCLL) are illustrated (not to scale). Also illustrated is the octopus cell area (OCA; not to scale) of the CN, which contains cells that project to the VCLL.

Ultimately, the lateral lemniscal fibres all synapse in the IC. Here the neurons are arranged tonotopically and are predominantly binaural. These IC cells mainly project to the ipsilateral medial geniculate nucleus (MGN) of the thalamus, via the brachium of the inferior colliculus. The MGN gives rise to the auditory radiation, which ends in the ipsilateral auditory cortex, found within the transverse temporal gyrus of the temporal lobe in humans (Kandel et al., 1991). It seems clear that, at least from above the level of the CN, there are extensive bilateral interactions in the ascending pathway, in contrast to some other sensory systems. Decussation of pathways occurs at many stages throughout the ascending auditory system. The second order neurons of the CN, project bilaterally to higher areas (Nolte and Angevine, 1995) making it possible for sound localisation and other binaural processes to be performed within the brainstem itself.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

5

As well as ascending projections and commissural connections, there is also a significant amount of feedback in the auditory system. Primarily, projections from the auditory cortex descend to the MGN and IC. There are also projections from the IC to the SOC, CN and NLL. Furthermore, many of the auditory nuclei including the lemniscal nuclei contain intrinsic projections. These will be discussed in more detail later. The present review concentrates on the rat auditory system as a model for the human brainstem. The general organisation of the rat auditory system is essentially equivalent to that of other terrestrial mammals, including humans. Here, specific differences between the NLL of the rat and other mammalian species have been identified and will be discussed below. The species under investigation will be routinely identified where appropriate for ease of comprehension.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

ANATOMY OF THE NUCLEI OF THE LATERAL LEMNISCUS The NLL are anatomically and functionally distinct cell groups situated in the pons between the lower brainstem and the auditory midbrain intercalated with fibres of the lateral lemniscus. The lemniscal nuclei have been extensively studied in echolocating bats (Zook and Casseday, 1982a, b; Covey and Casseday, 1986; Metzner and Radtke-Schuller, 1987; Zook and Casseday, 1987; Ross et al., 1988; Covey and Casseday, 1991; Covey, 1993a; Huffman and Covey, 1995; Vater et al., 1997) and in these animals are thought to play a vital role in echolocation. The current review will concentrate on findings from non-echolocating species, which lack the specialisations necessary for echolocation. In non-echolocating species, there is contention as to the parcellation of the NLL. Studies on cat (Adams, 1979; Glendenning et al., 1981; Malmierca et al., 1998), rat (Caicedo and Herbert, 1993; Merchán and Berbel, 1996), rabbit (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999), opossum (Willard and Martin, 1983) and guinea pig (Schofield and Cant, 1997) have identified different nuclei within the NLL; however, the delineations are not uniform across species. Some studies have identified two (rat: Merchán and Berbel, 1996; opossum: Willard and Martin, 1983; cat: Adams, 1979; Whitley and Henkel, 1984; Adams, 1997), whilst others have identified three nuclei (guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Glendenning et al., 1981): the dorsal, ventral and intermediate nuclei of the lateral lemniscus (DNLL, VNLL and INLL respectively). Whilst the DNLL is widely considered a separate structure both anatomically and physiologically (cat: Aitkin et al., 1970a; Glendenning et al., 1981), the distinction between the INLL and the VNLL is not so well established. Furthermore, different studies have divided the VNLL into one (rat: Merchán and Berbel, 1996; guinea pig: Malmierca et al., 1996; gerbil: Nordeen, 1983), two (cat: Roth et al., 1978; bat: Zook and Casseday, 1982b; Covey and Casseday, 1986), or sometimes three (rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997; guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Adams, 1979) subdivisions. A consensus is yet to be reached on whether the INLL should be classified as part of the VNLL or as a separate entity and whether the VNLL is a single nucleus or is made up of subdivisions. The differences across species no doubt play a large role in this debate; analogies may not be as straightforward as a one-to-one correlation between species. Therefore, for the purposes of this chapter the term, ventral complex of the lateral lemniscus (VCLL; after Merchán et al., 1997), has been adopted to encompass both the VNLL including its various subdivisions, as well as the INLL (see below). This nomenclature

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

6

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

(VCLL) is equivalent to the term ―ventral nuclei of the lateral lemniscus‖ popularised by Oertel and Wickesberg (2002) who also felt it best to represent the various ventral clusters of the NLL as a single nuclei with subdivisions. However, the term ventral complex of the lateral lemniscus (VCLL) avoids ambiguity when using the abbreviation ―VNLL‖ to describe only the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus (without including the intermediate nucleus of the lateral lemniscus) Figure 2 shows the parcellation of the rabbit VCLL as well as the inputs, outputs and cell types within the various parts of the structure. These topics will be discussed below.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

VCLL Parcellation Many researchers have conducted studies over the years, using different species, to gain a better understanding of the spatial organisation of this particular part of the auditory system, from the early classical anatomy studies (Held, 1893; van Gehuchten, 1906; Cajal, 1909) to the abundance of contemporary studies in morphology and functional projections (rat: Bajo et al., 1993; Caicedo and Herbert, 1993; Merchán et al., 1994; Merchán and Berbel, 1996; Kelly et al., 1998a; Kelly et al., 1998b; Zhao and Wu, 2001; Nayagam et al. 2006; Kelly et al. 2009; rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997; opossum: Willard and Martin, 1983; mouse: Willard and Ryugo, 1983; mole: Kudo et al., 1990; guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; ferret: Moore, 1988; cat: Adams, 1979; Glendenning et al., 1981; Whitley and Henkel, 1984; Spangler et al., 1985; Smith et al., 1993; Adams, 1997; Saint Marie et al., 1997; bat: Schweizer, 1981; Zook and Casseday, 1982a, b; Covey and Casseday, 1986; Vater and Feng, 1990; Huffman and Covey, 1995). Based predominantly on cytoarchitecture and projection patterns, different studies have parcellated the VCLL to different degrees. The differences between studies are due in part to the different species used and in part to the criteria for classification used. The arguments involved in the delineation of the internal subdivisions of the VCLL are in many regards closely linked with the arguments for and against the classification of the INLL as a separate nucleus. A difficult aspect of distilling these findings is the different nomenclature used by different authors, which may refer to equivalent regions. The present review, has adopted the terminology proposed by Schofield and Cant (1997) in guinea pig and subsequently adopted by Oertel and Wickesberg (2002) in their review chapter, who discussed ventral, dorsal and anterior divisions of the VNLL (VNLLv, VNLLd, VNLLa respectively) and a separate INLL. The major findings across non-echolocating species are correlated, where possible, to these divisions of the VCLL. Table 1 summarises key literature from non-echolocating species with regard to the various putative parcellations of the VCLL. Note that whilst some authors omit the ‗N‘ in their abbreviations, e.g. ILL, VLL etc. it has been re-inserted throughout this chapter for continuity.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

7

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

- Modified from Oertel and Wickesberg (2002): with kind permission from Springer Science and Business Media.

Figure 2. Diagrammatic representation of the parcellation of the ventral complex of the lateral lemniscus. Schematic shows the various putative sub-divisions of the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus (VNLL), including the ventral, dorsal, and anterior regions (VNLLv, VNLLd, VNLLa respectively). Also shown is the INLL, which lies dorsally to the VNLL in some species. Collectively these structures are termed the VCLL. The different regions are characterised by the presence of different neuron types or distributions in certain species. The parcellation and morphological distribution presented in this figure is derived from a study in guinea pig (Schofield and Cant, 1997). The VNLLv contains predominantly spherical bushy cells (circles), which are innervated by specialised calyx-like synapses (arcs) from octopus cells in the contralateral posteroventral cochlear nucleus (PVCN). Multipolar cells (stars) and giant cells (large stars) have also been identified in the VNLLv of some species. The VNLLd and VNLLa are distinct from the VNLLv due to the characteristic lack of spherical bushy cells. Both these regions contain mainly multipolar cells. VNLLa and VNLLd are separated from one another based on the directionality of the dendrites, in the VNLLa the dendrites are aligned with the lemniscal fibres, whereas in the VNLLd they lie perpendicular to the ascending fibres. The major excitatory input to the VCLL stems from the various cells of the contralateral cochlear nucleus. The VCLL also receives minor inputs from the ipsilateral CN and the ipsilateral medial nucleus of the trapezoid body (MNTB). The major VCLL projection is to the ipsilateral IC but there are also minor projections to the ipsilateral DNLL, and from the INLL to the medial geniculate nucleus (MGN) on both sides. Major projections are denoted with heavy lines and minor projections are denoted with light lines.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Table 1. Summary of major papers investigating VCLL parcellation.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Author(s)/Year Species Caicedo and Herbert (1993) Rat Merchán and Berbel (1996) Rat

Parcellation INLL exists VNLL

Kelly et al. (1998a) Kelly et al. (1998b) Malmierca et al. (1996) Schofield and Cant (1997)

Rat Rat Guinea Pig Guinea Pig

Batra and Fitzpatrick (1997)

Rabbit

VNLL, INLL VNLL, INLL VNLL VNLLa VNLLd VNLLv INLL VNLLd, VNLLm, VNLLl

Willard and Martin (1983) Glendenning et al. (1981)

Opossum Cat

VNLL INLL, VNLL

Techniques Nissl stain (Thionin) Nissl stain (Cresyl Violet), Retrograde tracing (BDA, HRP/Biocytin) from IC Nissl stain (Cresyl Violet) Retrograde tracing (Fluorogold) from IC Nissl Stain (Cresyl Violet) Nissl stain (Cresyl Violet), Antero/Retrograde tracing to/from VCN

Extracellular electrophysiology + Nissl stain (Thionin)

Notes Didn‘t examine VNLL Continuous helicoidal laminae throughout the ventro-dorsal extent of the structure Uses terms VNLL and INLL but no evidence for separate nuclei Used Caicedo and Herbert‘s (1993) study as a basis Divisions based on differences in cell types and their packing density..

VNLLd = INLL VNLLm = VNLLa VNLLl = VNLLv

Nissl / Golgi / Fibre and HRP stain Antero/retrograde tracing (autoradiographic and INLL had similarities and differences with both VNLL and degeneration) to/from VCN, Nissl staining and 2-DG DNLL. Lateral tegmentum may correspond with VNLLa Whitley and Henkel (1984) Cat VNLL with 3 zones Autoradiographic tracing from IC Based on projections, some variations along dorso-ventral axis identified following Adams (1979). Dorsal zone = INLL Adams (1979) Cat, Human VNLL with 3 zones (cat) Nissl / Protargol stain, retrograde tracing (HRP) Dorsal, middle and ventral zones identified based on cytological or 2 zones (human) from IC differences in cell type. Dorsal zone = INLL and is absent in humans Malmierca et al. (1998) Cat VCLL Retrograde tracing (BDA) from IC Arbitrarily defined dorsal, middle and ventral regions. Dorsal region = INLL and had different clustering pattern Nayagam et al. (2006) Rat VCLL, 3D reconstructions of intracellular Dorsal region contains cells with different response properties, (VNLL + INLL) electrophysiological recording sites; Nissl stain providing evidence for INLL (Thionin) Kelly et al. (2009) Rat VNLL, INLL Single and double labelled retrograde tracing Distinct projections to each subdivision of the VCLL and IC (Flurogold, Fluoro Ruby, Fast Blue) from IC, suggesting anatomical and functional seperation f the INLL DNLL, INLL and VNLL; Nissl stain (Cresyl Violet) Species, divisions of VCLL described, and techniques used to gather evidence are shown. Note that whilst some authors omit the ‗N‘ in their abbreviations, e.g. ILL, VLL etc., it has been reinserted throughout this review for continuity. Abbreviations: 2-DG, 2-Deoxyglucose; BDA, biotinylated dextran amine; HRP, horseradish peroxidase.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

9

Ventral VNLL The ventral part of the VNLL (VNLLv) is characterised primarily by a population of compact and distinctive cells similar in shape and afferent synapses to the spherical bushy cells of the CN (sometimes described in the literature as 'globular' cells, Schofield and Cant, 1997). These cells appear dark with Nissl stain and are distributed in alternating dense and loosely packed bands (Schofield and Cant, 1997). They have also been described as oval shaped and resembling the principal cells of the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body (MNTB) because of their appearance and also because they are covered in endbulbs (otherwise known as calyces) (Adams, 1979). The VNLLv has been identified in several species including humans (Adams, 1997), cats (Adams, 1979, 1997), rabbits (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999 - termed VNLLl), guinea pigs (Schofield and Cant, 1997), mice (Willard and Ryugo, 1983) and moles (Kudo et al., 1990). It is represented to varying degrees in different species, and is prominent in humans (38% of VNLL) compared with cats (4%, Adams, 1997). In guinea pigs, the VNLLv is the largest and most complex subdivision of the VNLL and its borders have been delineated from the adjacent structures based on cytoarchitecture and inputs from the CN (Schofield and Cant, 1997). In this and most non-echolocating species, the VNLLv is not a homogeneous cell group, containing giant neurons, multipolar cells (cat: Henkel, 1983), elongated cells (cat: Adams, 1979), as well as the characteristic bushy cells (guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997). This is not to say that segregation between cell types does not exist within the VNLLv. In guinea pigs, the globular cells are preferentially located rostrally, whereas the giant cells are most numerous in the posterior and medial portion of the subnucleus (Schofield and Cant, 1997). Similarly in cats, giant cells of the VNLLv are more common posteriorly and medially (Henkel, 1983) but globular cells are found ventrally (Adams, 1979). In mice (as well as some echolocating bats) the segregation is almost complete with globular cells isolated to a single, almost homogeneous subdivision of the VNLLv (mouse: Willard and Ryugo, 1983; bat: Zook and Casseday, 1985; Covey and Casseday, 1986; Vater and Feng, 1990). In many species, the borders of the VNLLv are not distinct and there is considerable overlap with neighbouring regions (Adams, 1979). However, in echolocating bats, the VNLLv is thought to correspond with a distinct and highly segregated group of bushy cells known as the columnar region (see below). In rats, opossums and some cat studies, the VNLLv has not been specifically identified (rat: Merchán and Berbel, 1996; Nayagam et al., 2006; opossum: Willard and Martin, 1983; cat: Glendenning et al., 1981). However, these studies have concurred that small, round, darkly staining bushy cells, akin to those that characterise the VNLLv are present within the VNLL. Glendenning et al‘s study (cat: 1981) found small darkly staining cells preferentially distributed ventrally, in agreement with previous cat studies (Adams, 1979). However, in rats these cells have been identified interspersed throughout the VNLL (M.S. Malmierca, personal communication in: Oertel and Wickesberg, 2002). The VNLLv receives a variety of thick and thin axonal inputs (see below). Bushy, T stellate and octopus cells in the contralateral CN are the main sources of these inputs (various species: Cant and Benson, 2003; guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Glendenning et al., 1981; Adams, 1997). The thick axon projection from the octopus cell area (OCA) of the contralateral PCVN is considered to have important functional ramifications. Studies have identified large secure calyx-like terminal endings on bushy cells in the VNLLv that receive input from the OCA (guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Adams, 1997). In guinea pigs

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

10

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

the thick axons from the OCA only innervated cells in the VNLLv (Schofield and Cant, 1997), whilst thin axons arising from multipolar and spherical bushy cells terminate throughout the VCLL (Schofield and Cant, 1997). A minor projection from the ipsilateral VCN, as well as the ipsilateral MNTB, also targets cells of the VNLLv. The importance of the VNLLv, and more specifically the bushy (globular) cells that characterise this region and their connection with octopus cells of the contralateral posteroventral cochlear nucleus (PVCN) will be discussed below.

Anterior VNLL The anterior division of the VNLL (VNLLa) in guinea pigs contains only multipolar cells with dendrites orientated parallel with lemniscal fibres (Schofield and Cant, 1997). This region appears to be an interstitial nucleus, in that it is a cluster of cells located within the fibres of the lemniscal tract. In guinea pig, it has been classified as a separate part of the VNLL based on its ascending projection to the contralateral IC (Schofield and Cant, 1997). The VNLLa has been identified in guinea pig (Schofield and Cant, 1997) and rabbit (termed VNLLm, Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997), although not in the same relative anatomical position. In the rabbit a lightly staining interstitial cluster of multipolar cells termed ‗VNLLm‘ was defined on physiological grounds in a similar location to VNLLd in guinea pig (see below), however, the cell densities of these two regions do not correlate. Hence, Batra and Fitzpatrick (2002b) surmised that the region termed VNLLa in guinea pigs correlated with their VNLLm in rabbit, as the cells in these two regions appeared to have similar responses. The VNLLa has not been identified per se in the cat (Aitkin et al., 1970a; Guinan et al., 1972b; Adams, 1979; Guinan et al., 1972a). A cluster of cells medial and adjacent to the VNLL classified by Glendenning et al. (1981) as the lateral tegmentum may correspond with Schofield and Cant‘s (1997) VNLLa, however, this has yet to be shown conclusively. It is unlikely that the paralemniscal zone defined in cats (Henkel and Edwards, 1978; Henkel, 1981; May et al., 1990), a region involved in acoustically driven motor responses, corresponds with the VNLLa in guinea pigs or VNLLm in rabbits (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 2002b) as it lies outside the boundaries of the VNLL. Schofield and Cant (guinea pig; 1997) suggested that the reason the VNLLa is undefined in most studies, including rat, could be that it has been overlooked due to its light staining and the scattered distribution of cells amongst the lemniscal fibres. It may be that the cells are incorporated into or relocated to a different area of the VNLL, an idea supported by Batra and Fitzpatrick (rabbit; 2002b). Schofield and Cant (1997) also acknowledge the possibility that in other species the VNLLa simply does not exist. Dorsal VNLL There is some confusion in the literature and thus it is important to make the distinction between the dorsal division of the VNLL (VNLLd) and the INLL. In some studies the region defined as VNLLd (cat: Adams, 1979; Whitley and Henkel, 1984; rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999) correlates with the region that has come to be known in other studies as INLL (Glendenning et al., 1981). In the present review, VNLLd refers to the dorsal region of the VNLL as well as cell areas surrounding the VNLLv, which are distinguished from VNLLv by their lack of spherical bushy cells. The INLL is considered separately and will be discussed below.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

11

The VNLLd in guinea pigs is populated primarily with multipolar cells, elongated to give the appearance of horizontal striations (orthogonal to the direction of lemniscal fibres) and arranged into loose vertical columns. These arrangements are not as distinct as in the adjacent INLL; this difference allows these two regions to be set apart. The guinea pig VNLLd also contains a small number of giant cells that have an elongated soma and stain more darkly than the multipolar cells, however not as many as in the neighbouring reticular formation, which allows the border between these regions to be distinguished (Schofield and Cant, 1997). In rat and some cat studies the VNLLd has not been specifically identified, however differences in the fibro-dendritic structure and cell types of the VNLL along its dorsoventral axis have been reported (various species: Merchán et al., 1997; rat: Merchán and Berbel, 1996; cat: Malmierca et al., 1998). Rat-specific differences will be discussed further below.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Arguments for and Against the INLL Subdivision All studies have agreed that there are at least two nuclei of the lateral lemniscus (DNLL, VNLL). However, not all studies have concurred with the three-subdivision nomenclature (DNLL, INLL, and VNLL) with some researchers finding the INLL was a dorsal part of the VNLL (rat: Merchán and Berbel, 1996; cat: Adams, 1979; Whitley and Henkel, 1984; Adams, 1997). The early classical studies recognised the two subdivisions (DNLL and VNLL) on the basis of delimiting fibre bands and cytoarchitecture (Held, 1893; van Gehuchten, 1906). However, later studies, especially on the bat (Zook and Casseday, 1979; Covey and Casseday, 1991), began to distinguish the INLL as a separate nucleus (rat: Kelly et al., 1998b; guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: van Noort, 1969; Brunso-Bechtold and Thompson, 1978; Roth et al., 1978; Glendenning et al., 1981; Saint Marie et al., 1997). The pivotal argument in favour of three nuclei came from a comprehensive study by Glendenning et al. (1981) in the cat. This study was in part addressing the work of Zook and Casseday (1979), who reported a heavy projection from the lateral nucleus of the trapezoid body (LNTB) of the bat to the region equivalent to the INLL in the cat. Addressing this discovery, Glendenning et al. (1981) performed a series of tracing studies, both retrograde and anterograde, in an attempt to elucidate the ascending projections to the various nuclei of the lateral lemniscus. They concluded that the region known as the INLL shared similarities as well as differences with the VNLL and DNLL. Like the VNLL it received a major projection from the contralateral VCN, a projection not received by the DNLL. However, it also received a large projection from the SOC, a projection pattern similar to the DNLL but unlike the VNLL. In at least two respects it was also found to be different to both the VNLL and DNLL: it received a large projection from the MNTB and a small projection from the LNTB. On these ‗connectivity‘ grounds, Glendenning et al. (1981) argued in favour of defining the INLL as a separate nucleus within the lateral lemniscus of the cat. Glendenning et al.‘s (1981) findings were supported by other researchers and across other species (rat: Moore and Moore, 1987; Caicedo and Herbert, 1993; cat: Spangler et al., 1985). Some researchers were able to distinguish the INLL from the VNLL based on cellular cytoarchitecture. Schofield and Cant (1997) reported that the most dorsal region of the VNLL could be distinguished from the dorsally bordering INLL by the orientation or arrangement of the constituent neurons (see above). A recent study (Kelly et al., 2009) provides strong support to the arguments for

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

12

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

parcellating of the NLL into 3 separate divisions (DNLL, INLL, and VNLL) based on the distinct projections patterns revealed by tract-tracing experiments in a large sample of rats. The opposing view is one that suggests that the INLL is not a separate nucleus, but a dorsal division of the VNLL. Merchán and Berbel (rat; 1996) are the foremost advocates of the non-existence of the INLL as a separate nucleus and, furthermore, that the VNLL is a single nucleus, with no subdivisions (see above). Their argument on the basis of connectivity and cytoarchitecture is that there is no fundamental distinction between INLL and VNLL in the rat. A subsequent study in the rat by Kelly et al. (1998b) supported this view. An alternate approach by some researchers is to distinguish between monaural and binaural nuclei. In bats, Covey and Casseday (1991) used the terms ―monaural nuclei‖ or ―monaural complex‖ to describe the INLL, and the two monaural subdivisions of the VNLL that are present in the bat. Nonetheless, they still acknowledge the INLL as a separate nucleus. Malmierca et al. (cat; 1998) used the term VCLL to include the ventral and intermediate nuclei and the gamut of subdivisions identified by other researchers in a variety of species using various criteria (rat: Caicedo and Herbert, 1993; guinea pig: Malmierca et al., 1996; Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: van Noort, 1969; Adams, 1979; Glendenning et al., 1981; bat: Covey and Casseday, 1991). However, Malmierca et al. (1998) outlined four fundamental features of the dorsal VCLL region (that corresponds with the INLL of other studies): 1) Different clustering pattern of fibrodendritic laminae (cat: Malmierca et al., 1998); 2) Different extent of labelling in the projection to the IC (cat: Whitley and Henkel, 1984); 3) Contains cells that are apparently excitatory rather than inhibitory (rat: Riquelme et al., 1998; Riquelme et al., 2001; cat: Saint Marie et al., 1997; bat: Winer et al., 1995; Vater et al., 1997); and 4) Receives glycinergic input from the MNTB (cat: Glendenning et al., 1981; Spangler et al., 1985). An intracellular and extracellular electrophysiological approach to identifying dorsoventral differences within the rat VCLL based on neural responses was performed by Nayagam et al. (2006). This study revealed that the dorsal part of the VCLL contained a population of cells that responded differently to acoustic stimulation compared to cells in the remainder of the structure. These differences (discussed in more detail below) provided physiological evidence for considering the dorsal part of the VCLL as a separate structure. Despite some fundamental similarities, anatomical differences do exist between species. Bats and cats especially seem to differ significantly from rats, mice and guinea pigs (see below). In humans there doesn‘t appear to be a dorsal division of the VCLL that corresponds with the INLL of other species, which receives input from the MNTB. This is understandable given the dearth of cells in humans in the region where the MNTB would be (Adams, 1997). The INLL certainly is the most controversial of the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus, if indeed it is a separate nucleus at all. One thing is certain, the region known as the INLL is different in some ways to the rest of the VCLL, be that a difference on cytoarchitectonic (cat: Stotler, 1953; van Noort, 1969), hodological (cat: Brunso-Bechtold and Thompson, 1978; Roth et al., 1978; Adams, 1979; bat: Zook and Casseday, 1979) or on electrophysiological grounds.

VCLL Parcellation in Echolocating Species In studies performed on echolocating bats, it has become widely accepted that the monaural NLL are significantly hypertrophied and unusually ordered in structure (Zook and Casseday, 1982b; Zook and DiCaprio, 1988). The VCLL in these echolocating animals seems

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

13

to contain three distinct nuclei, with morphologically distinct cell populations; the INLL, the VNLL columnar region (VNLLc) and the VNLL multipolar region (VNLLm)1 (Covey and Casseday, 1986, 1991; Huffman and Covey, 1995; Vater et al., 1997). The bat INLL is a large region, possibly larger than DNLL and VNLL combined, but lacking in any distinct form or shape (Zook and Casseday, 1979). The three regions are separated based on their constituent neuronal types. The INLL contains mainly elongate neurons with several long dendrites that extend from either side of the soma orthogonally to the ascending fibres of the lateral lemniscus, but parallel to the fibres that enter the nucleus (Covey and Casseday, 1991). The VNLLc is made up of densely packed neurons, distributed in columns (hence the name) between bundles of lateral lemniscal fibres. Almost all neurons in the VNLLc are of the same type and are very similar to spherical bushy cells of the anteroventral cochlear nucleus (AVCN). They are round or oval shaped, with one main dendrite that branches profusely from the soma. Some of the branches appear to extend parallel to the ascending fibres of the lateral lemniscus (Zook and Casseday, 1982a; Covey and Casseday, 1986, 1991). The VNLLm on the other hand contains mainly multipolar type cells (hence the name). These neurons‘ somas and dendrites do not appear to have any consistent orientation with respect to the fibres of the lateral lemniscus. The cells are larger than those in the VNLLc and have several sparsely branching thick dendrites with no apparent directional preference (Covey and Casseday, 1986, 1991). In non-echolocating mammals such as the cat, guinea pig, mouse and rat, the same principal cell types are found in the VCLL as in the bat‘s VCLL (rat: Nayagam et al., 2006; mouse: Willard and Ryugo, 1983; guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Adams, 1979). However, as previously discussed, the different neuron types are not as clearly segregated as they are in the echolocating bat (Covey and Casseday, 1991). These characteristic differences in the neurons of each subdivision, coupled with enhanced segregation of cell types, allows the VCLL of the echolocating bat to be parcellated without the controversy of its nonecholocating counterpart. In the bat, there is a projection from the MNTB to the INLL as well as the VNLLc (Covey, 1993b). This is different to the cat where the projection from the MNTB to the INLL is what sets it apart from the VNLL (Glendenning et al., 1981).

VCLL Anatomy and Parcellation in the Rat Different rat atlases have published plates showing the stereotaxic size, shape and position of the VNLL and INLL. These plates are not consistent with each other over different editions of the atlases (Paxinos and Watson, 1998; Swanson, 1998; Paxinos and Watson, 2005). However, in general the VCLL extends dorsoventrally ~2.5 mm, lateromedially ~1.2 mm and rostrocaudally ~1.2 mm at its largest extents. It is centred ~2.6 mm lateral of the midline, ~7 mm ventral of the cortical surface and ~8.2 mm caudal to Bregma. The borders of the VCLL in the rat have been studied by Merchán and Berbel (1996) and Caicedo and Herbert (1993), as well as defined in various rat brain atlases (Paxinos and 1

The multipolar region of echolocating species VNLL (VNLLm) should not be confused with the VNLLm defined by Batra and Fitzpatrick (1997) in the rabbit, which refers to the middle zone of the VNLL.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

14

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Watson, 1998; Swanson, 1998; Paxinos and Watson, 2005). Different rat atlases have published plates showing the stereotaxic size, shape and position of the VNLL and INLL. These plates are not consistent with each other over different editions of the atlases (Paxinos and Watson, 1998; Swanson, 1998; Paxinos and Watson, 2005). However, in general the VCLL extends dorsoventrally ~2.5 mm, lateromedially ~1.2 mm and rostrocaudally ~1.2 mm at its largest extents. It is centred ~2.6 mm lateral of the midline, ~7 mm ventral of the cortical surface and ~8.2 mm caudal to Bregma. Merchán and Berbel (1996) reported on the anatomic location, cytoarchitecture and morphology of the rat VCLL and described the dorsal border of the VCLL as sharp and easily defined. They claim that the horizontal cell group, a narrow band of tightly packed flat neurons, forms the border between the VNLL and the DNLL. This supported the work of Bajo et al. (1993), as well as Caicedo and Herbert (1993). However, it is contrary to studies in the cat which reported that although the VNLL is separated from the INLL by fibres, the border is not well defined and regions of overlap occur (Glendenning et al., 1981). Laterodorsally the border with the nucleus sagulum is clear, as the latter contains smaller and more densely packed neurons. Similarly, the lateroventral border is distinct within the lateral limb of the lemniscus. The lemniscal fibres, which surround the VCLL, define the rostral and caudal limits of the nucleus. In the medial and ventral zones, the borders are more blurred due to the absence of clear anatomical landmarks. On the ventral aspect, the proximity to the ventral nucleus of the trapezoid body (VNTB) makes the border more difficult to establish. However, the different orientations of the fibre fascicles that traverse the two nuclei allow the border to be distinguished. The VCLL is crossed ventrodorsally by lemniscal fibres, where the fibres of the trapezoid body cross the VNTB lateromedially. Thus, neurons in the VCLL are oriented vertically, as opposed to neurons in the VNTB which are oriented horizontally. Although more ambiguous than the other borders, the medial border may also be defined as cells of the neighbouring reticular formation are smaller and more loosely arranged than in the VCLL (Merchán and Berbel, 1996). The most prominent anatomical study of the rat VCLL favoured a classification scheme that treated the VCLL as a single nucleus containing a homogeneous cellular morphology based on cytology (Merchán and Berbel, 1996). However, the authors of this study acknowledge similarities to earlier findings in cats (Adams, 1979) and bats (Covey and Casseday, 1991) which proposed three divisions of the VCLL. Although they admit to observing cytoarchitectural differences between the dorsal, middle and ventral zones of the rat VNLL, they are careful to avoid the word ‗division‘. Differences, they claim, may be explained by assuming the fibro-dendritic laminae within the VCLL forms a helicoidal shape. The different patterns of the helicoid‘s coils may underpin the disparities in the VCLL‘s appearance. The wide dorsal zone seems to show coils that are more open; the middle zone‘s coils are horizontal and closely bunched giving the neurons a denser appearance; and the ventral zone has coils that are more open and obliquely oriented. Merchán and Berbel (1996) proposed that perhaps these coiling differences match the subdivisions proposed in bats and cats. They emphasise that their 3D reconstructions revealed a homogeneous pattern within the VCLL and therefore argue against parcellating the nucleus into sub-nuclei. A comprehensive physiological study which examined the VCLL based on a large sample of neural response properties to dichotically presented acoustic stimuli was performed by Nayagam et al. (2006). They constructed a 3-dimensional model of the rat VCLL from Nissl stained sections and reconstructed the location of their electrophysiological recording

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

15

sites within this model. By examining the distribution of cells with common neural response properties the authors were able to propose a parcellation for the VCLL. They found the dorsal part of the VCLL, the region corresponding with the INLL in other species, contained a different functional population of cells than the ventral portion of the nuclei. This description of dorso-ventral distinctions within the rat VCLL is in line with descriptions from other species (rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997; guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat and human: Adams 1979), the rat atlases and a subsequent tracing study (Kelly et al., 2009), but contrasts with descriptions from some anatomical tracing studies of the rat VCLL (Merchán and Berbel, 1996). Morphological and tracing studies have not agreed on structural sub-divisions within the rat VCLL. However, the most recent data from electrophysiological and tract-tracing studies (Nayagam et al., 2006; Kelly et al., 2009) suggest that the INLL is a separate subdivision of the VCLL. It is important to consider the various putative parcellations in other species when studying functional responses from the rat VCLL. A lot of the debate is a matter of semantics: how exactly does one define a ‗nucleus‘? Various definitions (be they based on cytoarchitecture, projection patterns, response types or something else) may well lead to different interpretations.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Cytoarchitecture and Morphology of VCLL Neurons There are several types of neurons found in the INLL as well as the VNLL and its various subdivisions. The VCLL contains cells that resemble bushy and stellate cells of the CN (rat: Zhao and Wu, 2001; guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Adams, 1979; Glendenning et al., 1981). There is little information on VCLL neuronal morphology in non-echolocating species that includes the structure of the dendrites. A comprehensive study (Zhao and Wu, 2001), performed on rats, identified four main classes of cell: stellate (I, II, and elongate) and bushy. They separated morphological classes based on the number, length and orientation of dendrites, size and shape of soma, and extent of dendritic branching.

Cytoarchitecture and Morphology of INLL Neurons In the cat, the INLL seems to contain predominantly multipolar cells and, to a lesser extent, horizontal cells that have finely dispersed Nissl substance and often lack a cap of Nissl substance around their nuclei. These cells resemble the multipolar cells found in the DNLL (Adams, 1979). Adams (1979) refers to the INLL as a dorsal division of the VNLL (VNLLd). In contrast, the cells of the bat INLL were very different to those of the DNLL and VNLL. Most of the INLL cells in bat are elongate or globular, and their dendrites are orientated orthogonally to the ascending lateral lemniscal fibres (Covey and Casseday, 1986). There is also a scattering of multipolar cells in this division, albeit less densely packed than in the ventral divisions. The multipolar cells here lack the columnar arrangement observed in the ventral divisions (Covey and Casseday, 1986). The Covey and Casseday (1986) study on bats also distinguished a transitional zone anteriorly, which contained medium sized globular, multipolar, and elongate cells that had a columnar organisation.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

16

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Cytoarchitecture and Morphology of VNLL Neurons In the echolocating bat, the multipolar (VNLLm) and the columnar (VNLLc) regions of the VNLL and their cell types are distinctive and well defined. The columnar region in particular has very distinct cytoarchitectural features; it is populated almost exclusively with small darkly staining cells (~10m in diameter), with round or oval somas, little cytoplasm and eccentric nuclei. These cells are described as reminiscent of spherical bushy cells in the AVCN2 in their oval to round shape and their single thick dendrite which extends some distance from the soma before branching profusely (Covey and Casseday, 1991). At least some of these dendritic branches extend parallel to the ascending fibres of the lateral lemniscus. As mentioned previously, the cells in this area are tightly packed and arranged in columns parallel to the fibres of the lateral lemniscus (Zook and Casseday, 1979; Covey and Casseday, 1986). Ventral to the columnar region is the multipolar region, or VNLLm. Most of the cells in this area have many large dendrites radiating from an irregularly shaped soma. There is also a sparse distribution of globular and elongate cells in this area. Although the cells in this multipolar region do have some preponderance to be distributed in columns, it certainly lacks the distinctive columnar organisation found in the adjacent (dorsal) VNLLc (Covey and Casseday, 1986). Furthermore, the somas and processes of cells in the VNLLm have no consistent orientation relative to the lateral lemniscal tract (Covey and Casseday, 1991). The columnar region in the moustache bat is in a different location; in this species it is the most ventral of the lateral lemniscal structures (Zook and Casseday, 1982b). In non-echolocating species, the cell types are not so well defined. The cat VNLL contains medium and large multipolar cells in the dorsal VNLL and globular cells in the ventral zone of the VNLL (Adams, 1979, 1981). Much of the cytoarchitecture revealed in cats and guinea pigs has already been discussed. The predominant cell types discovered in these species were globular cells, giant cells, multipolar cells and vertical cells. In several species, the globular cells are segregated from other cell types (at least partially); in cats they are concentrated in the ventral region of the VNLL (Adams, 1979) and in guinea pigs their population is more numerous in the rostral zone of the ventral VNLL (VNLLv rostral) (Schofield and Cant, 1997). The segregation peaks in mice (and some bats), where they are almost completely isolated from other cell types (mouse: Willard and Ryugo, 1983; bat: Zook and Casseday, 1985; Covey and Casseday, 1986; Vater and Feng, 1990). The neuron types described by Schofield and Cant (guinea pig; 1997) correlate to some degree with the more recent study by Zhao and Wu (2001) in rat. Humans have an increased proportion of globular cells in the VNLL, although these data come from one study only (Adams, 1997). Cytoarchitecture and Morphology of VCLL Neurons in the Rat One of the most cited anatomical studies in the rat VCLL found, using a combination of Nissl stained sections and injections of the tracer biotinylated dextran amine (BDA), that the VCLL seems to contain only flat stellate neurons (Merchán and Berbel, 1996). They found neurons and fibres were oriented in parallel and form anisotropic fibrodendritic laminae in the VCLL. However, in light of studies that found multiple neuronal types in the VCLL of other species (opossum: Willard and Martin, 1983; cat: Adams, 1979; bat: Zook and Casseday, 1982b), the existence of other morphological classes of neurons in the rat VCLL would seem 2

This differs with the same authors‘ claim in their 1986 study that the cells of the VNLLc are almost exclusively ―small darkly staining round to oval multipolar cells.‖ Covey and Casseday, 1986, p. 2927. [italics added]

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

17

likely. Merchán and Berbel (1996) acknowledged these previous studies, particularly the Covey and Casseday (1991) bat study, and claimed that findings of elongate neurons in the INLL, round and oval neurons in the VNLLc, and multipolar neurons in the bat VNLLm may not be incompatible with the results in the rat. 3D reconstructions of the rat VCLL reveal a helicoidal lamina formed by the cells, dendrites and fibres. Merchán and Berbel (1996) argued that differences in the pattern of coiling of this lamina underlies differences in neuronal shape and appearance of each division, and may explain why other studies have identified different cell classes when it is possible that there may be only one type of cell orientated in different ways. A more recent rat study by Zhao and Wu (2001) filled individual cells with Neurobiotin to allow direct observation of their dendritic structures (Figure 3). In this study, the authors were able to distinguish four types of neurons in two categories – bushy and stellate. The stellate neurons were of three types: stellate I, stellate II and elongate. The cell bodies of the bushy cells were round or oval, with short to medium length dendrites that bifurcated profusely into many fine endings (Zhao and Wu, 2001). These cells were found mainly in the ventral area of the VNLL. The stellate cells had fewer dendritic branches than the bushy cells. The dendritic arbors of stellate I cells had no directional preference. Elongate cells had a similar number of dendrites to stellate I, however, their cell bodies were elongated and possessed dendritic trees that ran along the longitudinal axis of the cell body. The stellate II cells had longer but fewer dendrites than the other two types. The various cell types were distributed throughout the VNLL with distributions similar to other mammals such as guinea pigs and cats. Different types of neurons were preferentially located in different regions of the rat VNLL (Zhao and Wu, 2001). Bushy and stellate I cells from this study were localised mostly in the ventral area of the VNLL, whereas stellate II and elongate cells were localised mostly in the dorsal area. Similarly, cat and guinea pig oval and globular cells (guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Adams, 1979), which Zhao and Wu (2001) correlate with their own stellate II cells, were located mainly in the dorsal and middle zones. Zhao and Wu (2001) state that the dendritic morphology of neurons is an important feature for distinguishing neuronal types. Acknowledging the work of Merchán and Berbel (1996), they conceded that because all the cells found in the VNLL had three to seven primary dendrites, they could all be seen as multipolar cells of the same neuronal type. However, they go on to point out that upon closer examination of the dendritic morphologies, they were able to identify differences between cell types, and thus consider the VNLL to be a heterogeneous neuronal group. The dendritic pattern of the VCLL neurons may give a clue as to their functional role: large dendrites that extend over a range of lemniscal fibres may integrate inputs from a range of frequencies, whereas small dendritic fields may be more narrowly tuned. The dendritic trees of bushy cells in the rat VCLL are smaller and more compact with a greater degree of bifurcation but a more restricted coverage than the dendritic trees of stellate cells which radiated away from the cell body and covered a relatively wide area (Zhao and Wu, 2001). Stellate II neurons (with their longer dendrites) receive input from a wider range of fibres than stellate I neurons. Elongate neurons (with their dendrites orientated in parallel with the fibres) may be involved in integrating information from a narrow band of frequencies, however, due to a small pool of results, this is merely conjecture and requires further study. All four neuron types identified by Zhao and Wu (2001) had axonal collaterals that terminated near the cell‘s dendritic area in a feedback mechanism.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

18

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

- Modified from Zhao and Wu (2001): with kind permission from John Wiley and Sons, Inc.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 3. Morphology of Neurobiotin labelled neurons of the VCLL. Camera lucida drawings of four Neurobiotin-labelled cells of the rat VCLL. The cells shown are typical examples of the four morphological types described by Zhao and Wu (2001). In all cases dorsal is at the top of the page.

In comparison with other species, it was found that the dendritic morphologies of bushy cells in the rat VNLL resembled the equivalent short compact dendritic morphologies of cells in the bat VNLLc, and cat and mouse CN (mouse: Wu and Oertel, 1984; cat: Osen, 1969; Brawer et al., 1974; bat: Covey and Casseday, 1991). The only difference being that the bushy cells in the rat VNLL appear to possess three to six primary dendrites, as opposed to one or two thick primary dendrites in the VNLLc and CN (Zhao and Wu, 2001). Parallels were also found between the dendritic features of the stellate II neurons (characterised by a small number of long dendritic branches) and multipolar neurons found in the medial section of the mouse VNLL (Willard and Ryugo, 1983) and the bat VNLLm (Covey and Casseday, 1991). Nayagam et al (2006) filled several cells in the VCLL with Neurobiotin in the course of gathering their electrophysiological recordings in vivo. These cells were reconstructed digitally and their position was located within a 3D model of the VCLL. The morphologies of the cells correlated with those described previously by Zhao and Wu (2001), supporting the description of the VCLL as containing heterogeneous cell types.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

19

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

TOPOGRAPHY AND TONOTOPICITY WITHIN THE VCLL Tonotopy refers to the spatial arrangement of frequency specific regions where sound is received, perceived or transmitted. Here, it refers to topologically neighbouring neurons in the brain that represent similarly adjacent tonal frequencies. Tonotopicity is well established as the basic principle of organisation within the mammal auditory system, from the cochlea to the cortex (for review see: Irvine, 1992). Studies have shown that the CN (various species: Masterton and Imig, 1984; cat: Bourk et al., 1981; rat: Saldaña, 1993), the LSO and MSO (cat: Elverland, 1978; Casseday and Covey, 1983; Henkel and Spangler, 1983), the DNLL (rat: Merchán et al., 1994; Kelly et al., 1995), and the central nucleus of the inferior colliculus (CNIC) (rat: Clopton and Winfield, 1973; Ryan et al., 1988; Kelly et al., 1991; Saldaña and Merchán, 1992; guinea pig: Malmierca et al., 1995; cat: Rose et al., 1963; Merzenich and Reid, 1974; Semple and Aitkin, 1979) contain neurons and fibres that are spatially oriented in isofrequency planes. Uncovering the tonotopic arrangement, if one exists, of a given nucleus is an important step towards understanding the function of that nucleus. For example, it has become well established that in the CNIC single neurons are tuned to sound frequencies and are organised along isofrequency contours that preserve the tonotopic gradient established in the cochlea. High frequencies are represented by neurons located ventromedially and low frequencies by dorsolaterally located neurons within the CNIC (rat: Clopton and Winfield, 1973; Flammino and Clopton, 1975; Kelly et al., 1991; Kelly et al., 1998a; rabbit: Syka et al., 1981). These studies in the IC have been confirmed with both c-fos and 2-deoxyglucose (2-DG) mapping (rat: Coleman et al., 1982; Huang and Fex, 1986; Friauf, 1992). The reason that the tonotopicity of the IC has been singled out by way of example, is because there is a significant projection from the VCLL to the ipsilateral IC (rat: Beyerl, 1978; Ito et al., 1996; Merchán and Berbel, 1996). Hence, the nuclei that are the source (CN) and target (IC) of the VCLL are tonotopically organised; does it follow that, assuming a point-to-point connection, the VCLL too will be tonotopically arranged? As it turns out, this is not necessarily true. The issue of a tonotopic arrangement in the VCLL is a highly contentious one (for review see Glendenning and Hutson, 1998). As yet, no clear layout has been agreed upon in the VCLL of rats and other mammals. Some researcher‘s data favour a progressive dorsal to ventral, high to low frequency, quasi-linear model (rat: Friauf, 1992; cat: Aitkin et al., 1970a), whilst others‘ findings suggest a more haphazard distribution, often involving multiple frequency gradients within the nuclei (bat: Covey and Casseday, 1995; Covey and Casseday, 1986; Metzner and Radtke-Schuller, 1987; Covey and Casseday, 1991). More recently, some studies have suggested a concentric lamina model with a helicoid (rat: Merchán and Berbel, 1996) or mosaic (cat: Malmierca et al., 1998) arrangement; and still others maintain that there is no tonotopicity evident in the VCLL based on various lines of evidence (cat: Adams, 1979; Glendenning et al., 1990; Glendenning and Hutson, 1998; Guinan et al., 1972a). The best way to establish a tonotopic arrangement within a given nucleus is by way of reconstructing electrophysiological recording sites. Tracing studies can only demonstrate topography of inputs and outputs. In many cases, topography of an area‘s connections correlates with or implies tonotopicity, as pathways through the auditory system tend to conserve the point-topoint connections of the original cochleotopicity. In the case of the VCLL, there are

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

20

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

conflicting results and the electrophysiological data has so far not validated the findings of anatomical tracing studies that have shown a tonotopic arrangement.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Tonotopic Organisation in the VCLL One of the prime difficulties in describing the organisation of the VCLL is that electrophysiological studies in different mammals have yielded a diverse and complex set of results that are not easily generalised. Aitkin et al. (1970a) proposed a weak dorsal to ventral, high to low frequency representation in the cat VCLL based on a subset of their electrophysiological penetrations. However, a later electrophysiological study by Guinan et al. (cat: 1972b; 1972a) failed to confirm this finding, and found no evidence for any tonotopic trends. Similarly, 2-DG methods showed no evidence for a tonotopic map in the VCLL of the cat (Glendenning and Hutson, 1998). A medial to lateral, high to low frequency spread was found within the INLL of the horseshoe bat by Metzner and Radtke-Schuller (1987), however, no single tonotopic gradient could be discerned in the VNLL. Covey and Casseday (1991) reported multiple tonotopic maps with the VNLLc, VNLLm and INLL of the echolocating bat based on a series of electrophysiological recordings. There has only been a limited amount of electrophysiological work performed in the rat that has addressed the issue of tonotopicity (Kelly et al., 1998a; Zhang and Kelly, 2006a; Nayagam et al. 2006). An early study by Aitkin et al. (1970a) which investigated the tonotopic organisation and discharge characteristics of cells in the cat VCLL and DNLL to monaural stimulation, mapped out the characteristic frequencies (frequency at which the cell responds with the lowest threshold; CF) obtained from a small sample of extracellular recordings. They concurred with earlier cytoarchitectonic studies (see above) which considered the DNLL and VCLL as separate midbrain structures in the cat. They found that in each of these regions neuron CFs were roughly distributed with low frequencies to high respectively dorsal to ventral (Aitkin et al., 1970a; Aitkin et al., 1970b). In the DNLL, the distribution of neurons with low and high CFs suggested that the tonotopic organisation is not strictly low to high, dorsal to ventral, but rather a concentric onion-like distribution of low to high, from inside to outside (Saint Marie et al., 1999). The VCLL progression was even less clear and this may have been caused by a non-optimal orientation of the electrode penetration with respect to the gradient of tonotopicity (Aitkin et al., 1970a). This study became accepted as the benchmark for many years despite contrary evidence from Guinan (1972b; 1972a) who did not find a tonotopic gradient within the cat VCLL using similar electrophysiological recordings. Adams (1979) also did not find evidence for tonotopicity in cat VCLL using tracing techniques. More recently, challenges to Aitkin et al.‘s (1970a) proposal have surfaced. Covey and Casseday (1986) originally performed a detailed anatomical tracing study combined with electrophysiology of the tracer injection sites to attempt to elucidate tonotopicity within the VCLL of echolocating bats (see below). In a subsequent study, Covey and Casseday (1991) attempted to find separate tonotopic representations in each cytoarchitectural and connectional subdivision using standard electrophysiological techniques. They found that the individual monaural nuclei of the bat lateral lemniscus (INLL, VNLLc and VNLLm) each have a complete tonotopic representation. Furthermore, their results suggest a concentric ‗onion like‘ organisation within the INLL and the VNLLm, with high frequencies represented more centrally and low frequencies more peripherally. The

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

21

VNLLc was found to contain more broadly tuned units that, where possible to identify a CF, were arranged with low frequencies represented dorsally and high frequencies ventrally, similar to the original Aitkin (cat; 1970a) findings. How do these findings in the bat relate to the dispute over the frequency representation within the cat VCLL (Aitkin et al., 1970a; Adams, 1979; Guinan et al., 1972a) and whether or not it is tonotopically organised? Assuming the connections in the cat and the bat are similar, which some studies have suggested is likely (cat: Warr, 1966; van Noort, 1969; Warr, 1969; Roth et al., 1978; Glendenning et al., 1981), Covey and Casseday (bat; 1986) the cat VCLL would be expected to have multiple rather than single tonotopic representations. Covey and Casseday (bat; 1991) point out that although other studies on cat found either a single dorsal to ventral tonotopic representation (Aitkin et al., 1970a) or no tonotopicity (Guinan et al., 1972b; Guinan et al., 1972a), it is possible that multiple tonotopic organisations were missed because of the small sample sizes. They suggest that there is a homology between connections in the echolocating bat and other mammals, with the exception of a small binaural area in the ventral nucleus of the cat. In the rat, Kelly et al. (1998a) made electrophysiological recordings from single neurons of the DNLL, but in the process recorded from cells of the INLL and other surrounding structures as well. They reported that reconstructions of electrode penetrations along several angles oblique to the dorsoventral axis did not reveal any patterns of tonotopicity within the DNLL or the INLL, both from individual animals and across the sample. However, the authors do concede a number of limiting factors that may impede their ability to detect tonotopic organisation. These factors are: a relatively high margin of error in localising extracellular recordings due to the small size of the nucleus, difficulty in sampling enough neurons to cover the full range of frequencies in a single electrode penetration, and non-linear maps are difficult to detect with straight electrodes (Kelly et al., 1998a). Furthermore, as this study used extracellular recordings, despite efforts to ensure that recordings were of neurons and not fibres of passage, a possibility of contamination exists. A more recent and more detailed extracellular electrophysiological study of the rat VCLL has also reported a lack of apparent tonotopicity along the dorsoventral axis (Zhang and Kelly, 2006a), however they did not examine possible 3D tonotopic distributions along the rostrocaudal or lateromedial axes. Nayagam et al. (2006) recently used a combination of intracellular electrophysiology and 3D computer-assisted reconstructions of VCLL recording sites to create a more accurate map of the VCLL than previously attempted. This technique reduces the constraints previously faced by the extracellular electrophysiological studies without 3D reconstructions and allows a greater degree of insight into potential tonotopicity. The data from their study did not reveal any clear evidence of tonotopicity, even when rotated and examined from all directions (Figure 4). They did not report any frequency organisation within single tracks or when reconstructed across many animals. However, this technique would not be able to show a helical or complex mosaic tonotopic distribution if the position of the helix/mosaic was different, with respect to the VCLL, from one animal to another. The overall picture from electrophysiological studies is that there is no obvious tonotopicity within the VCLL. Certainly in non-echolocating species, with the exception of the indistinct single gradient proposed by Aitkin (cat; 1970a), no evidence for tonotopicity has been demonstrated. However, the possibility of tonotopicity cannot be completely ruled out. This lack of obvious tonotopicity is unusual amongst brainstem nuclei and some have described the VCLL as unique based on this characteristic (cat: Glendenning and Hutson,

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

22

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

1998). Table 2 summarises results of studies investigating potential tonotopicity in the VCLL of non-echolocating species using electrophysiological techniques. Table 2. Tonotopic organisation in the VCLL determined by electrophysiological studies in non-echolocating mammalian species. Author(s)/Year

Species

Tonotopicity

Techniques

Aitkin et al. (1970a)

Cat

Guinan et al. (1972b; 1972a) Kelly et al. (1998a)

Cat

Dorsal – Ventral; Low – High frequency No evidence found

Rat

No evidence found

Zhang and Kelly (2006a)

Rat

No evidence found

Nayagam et al. (2006)

Rat

No-evidence found

Extracellular recordings in vivo Extracellular recordings in vivo Extracellular recordings in vivo Extracellular recordings in vivo Intracellular recordings in vivo with 3D reconstructions

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Topographic Organisation of the VCLL with Respect to Inputs and Outputs Anatomical tracing studies, using restricted injections of retrograde tracer into the IC, as well as injections of anterograde tracer into the VCLL itself, have attempted to describe the topography of the VCLL with respect to the established tonotopicity of the target IC nuclei. The major projection from the VNLL to the IC is inhibitory, and it is not necessarily the case that projection neurons in the VNLL have the same CF as their IC target (various species: Oertel and Wickesberg, 2002). This is an important point to bear in mind when reviewing results from anatomical tracing studies. Tracing studies can highlight the organisation of neurons‘ connections as well as visualise the organisation of the underlying fibrodendritic laminae of a given nucleus. The arrangement of the extracellular matrix and the fibrodendritic laminae of the VCLL is uncertain. Different visualisation techniques have yielded different results across species. Many descriptions of the VCLL topography have been proposed, including horizontal bands of spindle neurons (rat: Moore and Moore, 1987; cat: Roth et al., 1978; Adams, 1979; Brunso-Bechtold et al., 1981), fibres in a ladder rung pattern known as cross-bridges (cat: Glendenning et al., 1981; Spangler et al., 1985) and multiple laminae representations in bats (Covey and Casseday, 1991). Table 3 summarises the results of studies investigating potential topographic organisations of the VCLL in non-echolocating species using tracing and activation techniques. Merchán and Berbel (1996) interpret the various aforementioned descriptions of topography (horizontal bands, cross-bridges, and multiple laminae) in the VCLL as consistent with a helicoidal organisation. Their rat study lends support to the spiral organisation first proposed by Willard and Martin (1983) in the North American opossum. Willard and Martin (1983) also considered the VCLL a single nucleus and described the dendritic arrangement within the VCLL as a tightly woven helical spiral that encircles columns of lemniscal fibres.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

23

- Nayagam et al. (2006)

Figure 4. Spatial distribution of intracellular unit‘s CF within the VCLL. A: Standard coronal sections of the rat‘s VCLL collapsed from 3 rostrocaudal ranges (given in Bregma coordinates, under each section), from most rostral (left) to most caudal (right). Outlines of major structures are shown, and there is some overlap between nuclei delineations due to the collapsing process. Unit CFs are given in ranges and are colour coded from low frequency (red) to high frequency (violet). B: Computer generated semi-opaque surface model of the VCLL showing the same distribution of units as in (A) but visualised in three dimensions. The viewing aspect of the reconstruction is taken from rostral (left), dorsal (middle) and lateral (right). Orthogonal orientation arrows represent 500 m and are colour coded (dorsal – green, lateral – red and rostral – blue).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

24

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Merchán and Berbel‘s (1996) findings using tract-tracing following injections of BDA, as well as combination injections of horseradish peroxidase (HRP) and biocytin combined with 3D computer reconstructions, supported a dorsoventral helicoidal distribution of neurons and processes. In addition, they found a concentric topography with respect to the tonotopic map of the IC. The helicoids were arranged in isofrequency laminae, whereby neurons from the central helicoidal laminae send projections to ventromedial high frequency regions of the IC and peripheral helicoidal laminae neurons project to dorsolateral low-frequency portions of the IC. When viewing sections of the VCLL, the individual coils of the helicoids appear as horizontal bands. When reconstructed in 3D, the true nature of the helicoidal structure becomes apparent. These horizontal bands are reminiscent of c-Fos immunoreactive horizontal bands that have been observed after pure tone stimulation (rat: Friauf, 1992). Merchán and Berbel hypothesised that their horizontal bands could therefore correspond to the isofrequency planes indicated by the Friauf (1992) study. In addition, their helicoidal model may account for the multiple tonotopic representations found in Covey and Casseday‘s (1991) bat study. They also noted a similar concentric pattern of fibrodendritic laminae, which seems to be present in the VNLL of cats, which might indicate that their findings for the projection of VNLL to CNIC in the rat are common to other mammals. Malmierca et al. (cat; 1998) proposed a new ‗complex mosaic‘ arrangement with interdigitating clusters of neurons throughout the VCLL, each representing projections to different frequency-specific regions of the IC. These data were based on computer-assisted 3D reconstructions of retrograde-labelled cells and terminal fibres, following focal injections of BDA in different frequency band regions of the IC (Figure 5). These authors challenged the idea of a single frequency gradient and the recently published helicoidal arrangement in the VNLL (rat: Merchán and Berbel, 1996). They also found an overall medial to lateral, high to low frequency gradient. The authors claimed that their findings were compatible with the electrophysiological studies in cat (Aitkin et al., 1970a) and rabbit (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997) even though Aitkin et al.‘s (1970a) study suggested a dorsoventral tonotopic gradient not a mediolateral one. Malmierca et al. (1998) suggested that given the oblique angle of Aitkin et al.‘s electrode penetrations with respect to the orientation of the VCLL, they were in fact traversing the nucleus from dorsolateral to ventromedial and thus the recorded high to low sequence of CFs may reflect a mediolateral rather than dorsoventral distribution. Similarly, Malmierca et al.‘s (1998) reasoning for drawing comparisons with the rabbit was based on Batra and Fitzpatrick (1997) recordings of single units with CFs < 2kHz in the medial region of the VCLL. Malmierca et al. (1998) also describe their findings as compatible with previous anatomical studies, in particular those of Whitley and Henkel‘s (1984). They found that their smallest, medial injection on the edge of the VNLL, which produced labelling in the lateral part of the CNIC, could be interpreted as evidence for mediolateral tonotopicity (cat: Whitley and Henkel, 1984). Kelly et al. (1998b) performed two studies to investigate the topography of the rat‘s lateral lemniscal nuclei. Although the studies tended to concentrate on the DNLL and the emerging theory of a concentric onion-like frequency representation, the VNLL was also examined. They found distinct bands in the INLL and VNLL when retrograde tracer was injected into the low frequency region of the ipsilateral IC. They were considering the INLL and VNLL as a single nucleus, much like Merchán and Berbel (1996) (see above). However, Kelly et al. (1998b) retained the terms INLL and VNLL to distinguish between different positions along the lemniscal pathway. A similar injection into middle and high frequency

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

25

areas of the IC resulted in a more even distributions throughout the INLL and VNLL with less evidence of banding. Injections into IC regions representing frequencies above ~20kHz showed no evidence of banding in the VNLL or INLL. The difference was not due to inconsistent spread of tracer as other auditory brainstem nuclei showed highly localised patterns of high and low frequency labelled areas following the same injections. The findings of Kelly et al. (1998b) therefore support, in part, the findings by Merchán and Berbel (1996) of a banded pattern of labelling, but only for frequencies below 20 kHz.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Table 3. Topographic organisation in the VCLL determined by tracing and 2-DG activation studies in non-echolocating species. Author(s)/Year Whitley and Henkel (1984)

Species Topography Cat Widespread divergent projection to IC with specific variations

Merchán and Berbel (1996) Malmierca et al. (1998)

Rat

Glendenning and Hutson (1998)

Cat

Langner et al. (2006)

Gerbil

Benson and Cant (2008)

Gerbil

Cat

Concentric Helicoid. Peripheral – Central; Low – High frequency Complex but orderly mosaic of frequency representations. Medial – Lateral; High – Low frequency No Evidence Found. Randomly distributed clusters after trracing. Widespread activation of 2-DG

Low – High; Dorsal – Ventral. Helical periodicity map Topographic organisation from: CN to VCLL- high frequencies central, low frequencies lateral and caudal; VCLL to IC - relationship between frequencies axid of IC and mediallateral position in VCLL

Techniques Anterograde / autoradiographic tracing from VCLL to IC Retrograde tracing from IC to VCLL; 3D reconstructions Retrograde tracing from IC to VCLL; 3D reconstructions Triple injections (into high, middle and low frequency regions of IC) and retrograde tracing from IC to VCLL. 2DG with tonal stimulation in high middle or low frequency ranges 2-DG activation. Retrograde tracing from IC to VCLL Anterograde (from CN) and retrograde from (IC) tracing of focal injections of BDA.

The bands of labelling in the VNLL found in the Kelly et al. (1998) study and the Merchán and Berbel (1996) study are reminiscent of the findings of other researchers that used c-fos activation following pure tone stimulation (rat: Friauf, 1992) or retrograde tracing from the IC (cat: Roth et al., 1978; Glendenning et al., 1981). The laddering of afferent projections from the contralateral CN to the VNLL, similar to those found by Merchán and Berbel (1996) and Kelly et al. (1998) in the rat (from efferent projections to the IC), has also been reported in the rat (Friauf and Ostwald, 1988; Kandler and Friauf, 1993). It has been suggested that the banding may be caused by periodic ladder-rung like collaterals being given off by ascending fibres from the CN to the IC and passing through the VCLL (rat: Merchán and Berbel, 1996). This implies that the topography indicated by the banding of CN inputs is reflected in bands of VNLL cells that project to a specific frequency region in the IC. In an attempt to uncover topography, Glendenning and Hutson (cat; 1998) used a combination of 2-DG (followed by tonal stimulation at high, middle or low frequencies) or retrograde triple labelling (injected into high, middle and low frequency regions of the IC) techniques to visualise any potential spatial maps of frequency. Both methods yielded well-

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

26

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

organised tonotopically arranged cell labelling in brainstem nuclei with the exception of the VCLL. Retrograde labelling revealed clusters of neurons randomly distributed throughout the VCLL. 2-DG was activated throughout the VCLL, in support of the retrograde transport. This finding is consistent with the anterograde tracing studies of Whitley and Henkel (cat: 1984) which found widespread divergent projections from the VCLL to the IC. Glendenning and Hutson (1998) concluded that there is no apparent orderliness of the cat VCLL afferents or efferents with respect to frequency. Recent studies in the gerbil (Langner et al., 2006; Benson and Cant, 2008) have examined the connections from the CN through the VCLL to the CNIC. Both studies have demonstrated a topography in these afferent and efferent connections. Langner et al. (2006) suggested a helical organisation best fitted their 2-DG activation and retrograde tracing data. Benson and Cant (2008) concluded that a lateral to medial distribution in the VCLL is topographically related to the established frequency axis of the IC. They recommend future electrophysiological studies would be wise to consider a horizontal rather than the more traditional dorso-ventral approach to uncover potential tonotopic maps arising from this pattern of connectivity. They also described a concentric pattern of connections from frequency specific regions of the CN to the centre (high frequency) or periphery (low frequency) of the VCLL. Their results are compatible with previous descriptions of VCLL topography in other species (rat: Merchán and Berbel, 1996; opossum: Willard and Martin, 1983; mouse: Willard and Ryugo, 1983; cat: Warr, 1982) and lends strong support to the case for a clear topography in the afferents and efferents to and from the VCLL.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Tonotopic and Topographic Organisation of the Echolocating Bat VCLL In the bat, the situation is somewhat different and both topography and tonotopicty of the VCLL has been described after anatomical tracing studies, but with limited electrophysiological confirmation. Covey and Casseday (Covey and Casseday, 1995; 1986; 1991) found frequency gradients within the VCLL (INLL and VNLL) of the big brown bat. The columnar area of the VNLL (VNLLc) was organised with projections from low frequency neurons located dorsally and projections from high frequency cells ventrally, but other areas of VNLL and INLL were not as well organised. Covey and Casseday (1986) studied tonotopicity within the VCLL of echolocating bats, using a combination of retrograde labelling, anterograde labelling and electrophysiology. They recorded the CFs of neurons at the tracer injection sites in the AVCN and the IC to provide an indication of the frequency of the anterogradely or retrogradely labelled cells in the columnar region of the VNLL. The labelled cells were not entirely located within the columnar region; however, this is the area that they concentrated on since its connections seemed more precisely organised than other nuclei of the lateral lemniscus. They found an orderly progression of dorsal to ventral, low to high frequencies in the columnar area, VNLLc. Their findings appeared to agree with the Aitkin et al. (1970a) study in the cat. However, the results in the bat are more complicated than a simple dorsoventral distribution of CFs. Anterograde and retrogradely labelled cells in the VNLLc were arranged in ―sheets‖ that were aligned orthogonally to the lemniscal fibres. The thickness, extent and orientation of these sheets were determined by the tonotopic range of the injection site as opposed to the amount of tracer injected. In other words, a large

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

27

- Modified from Malmierca et al. (1998): with kind permission from The Society for Neuroscience

Figure 5. Stereo-images of 3D distribution of retrograde label within the VCLL of the rat following injections into high and low frequency regions of the IC. Computer generated stereo pairs showing the VCLL viewed from the caudal aspect. Retrogradely labelled clusters are shown as DARK shaded fragments within the solid VCLL surface. A: Shows labelled clusters which are mainly located adjacent to the rostrolateral surface following focal injections of BDA into high frequency regions of the IC. B: Shows labelled clusters, which are more widely distributed following focal injections of BDA into low frequency regions of the IC. To visualise the 3D image for each pair the viewer must cross their eyes such that the stereo pairs merge into a single image with the illusion of depth. Scale bar is 500 m. D: dorsal, M: medial.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

28

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

injection and a small injection with different orientations, if given in the same frequency range, will produce sheets of similar characteristics. The precise relationship between tonotopicity and the projection sheets was explained by Covey and Casseday (1986) in terms of divergent projections from a point within an isofrequency contour within the AVCN forming a sheet in the columnar area, that in turn projected (converged) to another sheet within the IC, thus maintaining the same frequency representation as the original AVCN source. The authors also referred to the compression that must be occurring in the system to allow the entire frequency range to be represented within the very small space of the VNLLc. Two possible scenarios arise from this compression: first, each sheet represents a relatively broad range of frequencies, and second, only selected frequencies are represented by individual sheets (Covey and Casseday, 1986). There was also a relative ‗over-representation‘ of the 25 – 50 kHz range within the IC which corresponded to the range of frequency modulated (FM) echolocation in the bats studied. Whether this over-representation is occurring at the VNLLc or at a lower level remains unknown. In tracing studies of the VCLL, the ascending projections to the ipsilateral IC imply a haphazard tonotopic arrangement; they are uneven, widespread and patchy (Oertel and Wickesberg, 2002). This apparent mismatch in frequency representations between the VCLL projection neuron and the IC targets allows for the possibility of cross-frequency interactions. In the bat, broadly tuned VNLLc inhibitory units impinge on narrowly tuned IC targets. This pattern of wideband inhibition acting on narrowband excitation has been observed in other species and is of potential functional importance (see below).

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Evaluation of Tonotopic and Topographic Models Merchán and Berbel (rat: 1996) suggest two possibilities given that a clear tonotopicity has yet to be demonstrated in non-echolocating species, particularly the rat. First, that VNLL tonotopicity is a specialisation that has evolved in bats to support echolocation in some way or second, that non-echolocating mammals do have some kind of tonotopic arrangement that has yet to be recognised. They believe that tonotopicity in rats should not be ruled out. However, there are a third and/or fourth possibility worth stating here: that tonotopicity is not necessary for the function of the VCLL or, more specifically, that broadly tuned elements (that do not support a tonotopic arrangement) are necessary for the VCLL‘s function. Several topographical organisation schemes have been proposed and evidence given to support each of them. Typically, when evaluating conflicting scientific arguments, one looks for the simplest explanation that can account for all the variables without introducing unnecessary complications (Occam‘s razor). In this case, the helicoidal models are certainly more complex than single gradients, multiple gradients or lack of tonotopicity. However, the various, seemingly incongruent observations across species (isofrequency banding, multiple gradients, mediolateral as well as dorsoventral frequency arrangements) may be explained by a helicoidal configuration. Ultimately, whether the tonotopic or topographic model is one of a continuous distribution, a multiple representation, a helicoid, a 3D cluster of interdigitating laminae, a combination of these, a disparate ensemble of these models in different species, or simply none at all, is a matter for future studies to attempt to determine.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

29

CONNECTIONS OF THE VENTRAL NUCLEUS OF THE LATERAL LEMNISCUS

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Ascending Auditory Afferents to the VCLL In response to sound, early studies considered the VCLL to be a monaural structure (cat: Aitkin et al., 1970a; Brugge et al., 1970b; Guinan et al., 1972b) and believed that the major projections to the VCLL were from the contralateral VCN and ipsilateral SOC. However, binaural units within the VCLL and bilateral projections to the VCLL from several brainstem nuclei, including the VCN and SOC, were subsequently identified (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 2002b; guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Glendenning et al., 1981; rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999). The most comprehensive attempt to identify all the ascending afferents to the NLL was performed by Glendenning et al. (cat: 1981) and became the benchmark for subsequent studies. The researchers used anterograde degeneration, retrograde HRP techniques and autoradiographic analysis to trace the ascending afferents to the various nuclei of the lateral lemniscus. The present review will concentrate on the projections to the ‗monaural‘ nuclei (VCLL), omitting the DNLL. The aim of their study was not to specify which cell classes gave rise to specific inputs, but rather to provide an indication of which nuclei project to the VCLL. Recall the subdivisions proposed by Glendenning et al. (1981) included the INLL as a separate nucleus, and for now the INLL and VNLL will be discussed separately. At the time of the study it was already well established that the main route of fibres to the VNLL was from the contralateral VCN via the trapezoid body and then rostral via the lateral band of the contralateral lateral lemniscus (rat: Browner and Webster, 1975; monkey: Strominger and Strominger, 1971; cat: Warr, 1966; van Noort, 1969; Warr, 1969, 1972; Brunso-Bechtold and Thompson, 1978). In addition, a few fibres had been traced from the CN via the dorsal acoustic stria to the contralateral VNLL (cat: van Noort, 1969; Warr, 1969; Osen, 1972; Warr, 1972; Adams and Warr, 1976). HRP bolus injections restricted to the VNLL showed retrogradely labelled cells almost exclusively in the VCN (almost 90% of labelled cells) with three times as many cells in the AVCN compared to the PVCN. Only five out of nine hundred and ninety-four cells were labelled in the DCN. These findings confirmed the earlier work in other animals using a variety of techniques (rat: Browner and Webster, 1975; monkey: Strominger and Strominger, 1971; cat: Stotler, 1953; Harrison and Warr, 1962; Warr, 1966; van Noort, 1969; Warr, 1969, 1972). This domination of direct projections from the CN to neurons of the VNLL is what set it apart from the DNLL. In cats, a small, unique dorsomedial region of the VNLL also seemed to receive substantial projections from the ipsilateral VCN via the trapezoid body (cat: Glendenning, 1981). This supported reports of ipsilateral input from the VCN in other species, although the segregation of ipsilateral and contralateral projections may not be as clear as in cats (rat: Browner and Webster, 1975; monkey: Strominger and Strominger, 1971; guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Warr, 1969). There have been previous reports of minor projections from the SOC to the VNLL, but far less than was present in the DNLL (cat: Stotler, 1953; Elverland, 1978). The earlier studies generally agreed that the LSO alone projects bilaterally to the NLL (various species: Niemer and Cheng, 1949; rat: Browner and Webster, 1975; cat: Stotler, 1953; van Noort, 1969; Elverland, 1978; bat: Zook and Casseday, 1979). The MSO was found to be the source of

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

30

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

strictly ipsilateral ascending projections, some of which terminated in the VNLL (cat: Elverland, 1978). In addition, the LSO was found not to project to the VNLL, although ascending bilaterally. Glendenning et al. (1981) agreed with these previous findings and noted that the majority of nuclei of the SOC contribute to the projections to the VNLL, including the MNTB, MSO, LSO and the periolivary nuclei. The primary contributors are the MNTB, VNTB and other periolivary nuclei, which make up 90% of the afferents from the SOC (Glendenning et al., 1981). Tracing studies have also provided evidence of cross-bridges, which are collaterals of ascending fibres that traverse the nuclei laterally (ladder-like rungs), from SOC projections that turn at right angles to the primary ascending axons travelling to the IC (cat: van Noort, 1969; Elverland, 1978). Glendenning et al. (1981) stated that further investigation was required to address the issue of cross-bridges, however, after ablations of the CN or SOC the degenerating anterograde material showed thin bridges between the medial and lateral bands of the lateral lemniscus, passing through the NLL. These degenerating bridges were larger and more obvious after SOC ablations than CN ablations (Glendenning et al., 1981). ‗Crossbridged cells‘ have previously been noted by Roth et al. (cat; 1978) after HRP injections in the IC. Glendenning et al. (1981) postulated that the horizontal cells described by Adams (cat; 1979) may provide a substrate for the cross-bridges. They go on to suggest that there may be a connection between their own findings of cross-bridged degenerating fibres, Adams‘ horizontal cells and Roth et al.‘s HRP labelled horizontal bridges (cat: Roth et al., 1978; Adams, 1979; Glendenning et al., 1981). Cross bridges have implications in sound processing as they provide a means for convergence of input from the SOC and CN onto cells at many levels of the NLL. HRP injections into the INLL result in labelled cells throughout the AVCN (large and multipolar cells), as well as most of the PVCN (excluding the OCA). These injections also labelled many cells in the LNTB, the ventral part of the trapezoid body and the MNTB, but not the MSO or LSO (bat: Zook and Casseday, 1987). In contrast, similar injections into the VNLL of bats do label cells in the octopus cell region of the PVCN (Zook and Casseday, 1979). Cat studies (Glendenning et al., 1981) support the findings in bat, in that, for the most part, the projections to the INLL are very similar to those of the VNLL, and arise mainly from the AVCN. However, there are nonetheless differences in the projection patterns to INLL and to VNLL. In particular, the projection from the SOC to the INLL is different to the projection from the SOC to the VNLL or DNLL; the INLL receives major projections from the MNTB, and, in the bat, from the LNTB as well (bat Zook and Casseday, 1987; cat: Glendenning et al., 1981). Although previous studies did not find a projection to the INLL from the contralateral NLL, Glendenning et al. (1981) did find a very sparse projection (1%-2%) from the contralateral DNLL. However, the INLL may yet be distinguished from the DNLL by the absence of a larger (13%) commissural projection between DNLLs (cat: Glendenning, 1981).

Sources of Excitatory and Inhibitory inputs to the VCLL All principal cell groups of the VCN project to the VNLL: small and large spherical bushy cells, globular bushy cells, octopus cells and type I multipolar (T stellate) cells (for review see: Cant and Benson, 2003). In a study on rat, the synaptic physiology of the VNLL was investigated by examining synaptic potentials of VNLL neurons in response to electrical stimulation of the lateral lemniscus ventral to the VNLL (Wu, 1999). The author suggested that, although inhibitory post-synaptic potentials (IPSPs) are present and somewhat obscure

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

31

the excitatory post-synaptic potentials (EPSPs), some of the short latency, large amplitude and short duration EPSPs observed in cells with an onset response to stimulation (see below) may be from octopus cells in the PVCN (Wu, 1999). This study also found medium-sized bead-like boutons in the VNLL that probably originated from bushy or stellate (multipolar) cells in the AVCN and PVCN which send thin axons through the VNLL en route to the IC (see also; various species: Schwartz, 1992; rat: Friauf and Ostwald, 1988; mouse: Iwahori, 1986; guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; bat: Covey, 1993a). Presumably these smaller synaptic connections would result in the lower amplitude and longer latency EPSPs recorded in the Wu (1999) study. Since the AVCN projection to the IC is excitatory (cat: Oliver, 1987), and the VNLL receives collaterals from this projection, it is likely that the AVCN provides excitatory inputs to cells in the VNLL as well, probably using glutamate or aspartate as a neurotransmitter (rat: Zhao and Wu, 2001; guinea pig: Suneja et al., 1995). Zhao and Wu (2001) suggested that the source of EPSPs in the rat VNLL was probably from octopus, bushy and stellate (multipolar) cells in the VCN. EPSPs recorded from stellate I cells in the VCLL generated only one suprathreshold action potential (AP) and were short in duration. In comparison, EPSPs recorded from stellate II and elongate cells were much longer and generated many APs. The short EPSPs may be due to low input resistance or may be due to the influence of IPSPs truncating the EPSPs. Further study involving pharmacological manipulation was recommended to clarify this issue (Zhao and Wu, 2001). Both bushy and stellate cells in the VNLL receive inhibitory projections which lead to IPSPs that were recorded by Zhao and Wu (2001). IPSPs in the VCLL may be originating from cells in the ipsilateral MNTB and periolivary nuclei (cat: Warr and Beck, 1996; Elverland, 1978; Glendenning et al., 1981; Spangler et al., 1985; bat: Vater and Feng, 1990; Huffman and Covey, 1995). Collaterals of glycinergic (guinea pig: Helfert et al., 1989) projections from the MNTB principal neurons bound for the LSO and GABAergic neurons from VNTB and LNTB (rat: Moore and Moore, 1987; guinea pig: Helfert et al., 1989; González-Hernández et al., 1996; gerbil: Roberts and Ribak, 1987; cat: Adams and Mugnaini, 1990; bat: Vater et al., 1992; Winer et al., 1995) are other possible sources of IPSPs in the VCLL (Zhao and Wu, 2001). Another possible source of IPSPs within the VCLL is from internal intrinsic projections (further discussion below). The presence of synaptic potentials mediated by many different neurotransmitters, both excitatory and inhibitory (AMPA, NMDA, GABAA, Glycine) in individual cells of the VCLL further supports an integrative and processing role rather than simple relay of signals within the auditory system (rat: Irfan et al., 2005).

Major Ascending Efferents from the VCLL The VNLL is a Major Source of Inhibition to the IC The DNLL projects bilaterally to the CNIC, whilst the VNLL primarily projects ipsilaterally to the CNIC in many mammalian species (rat: Beyerl, 1978; Druga and Syka, 1984; Tokunaga et al., 1984; Tanaka et al., 1985; Coleman and Clerici, 1987; opossum: Willard and Martin, 1983; mouse: Willard and Ryugo, 1983; mole: Kudo et al., 1990; guinea pig: Shneiderman et al., 1993; Nordeen, 1983; ferret: Moore, 1988; cat: van Noort, 1969; Roth et al., 1978; Adams, 1979; Brunso-Bechtold et al., 1981; Glendenning et al., 1981; Glendenning and Masterton, 1983; Saint Marie and Baker, 1990; Hutson et al., 1991; bat:

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

32

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Schweizer, 1981; Zook and Casseday, 1982a, b; Frisina et al., 1989). Retrograde tracing studies indicate that 25-50% of the projections to the IC originate from the VNLL (mole: Kudo et al., 1990; ferret: Moore, 1988; cat: Roth et al., 1978; Brunso-Bechtold et al., 1981; bat: Ross et al., 1988; Ross and Pollak, 1989). Nearly 68% of the glycinergic projections to the IC (identified with retrograde labelling of tritiated glycine) are from the VNLL (cat: Saint Marie and Baker, 1990) and, coupled with the high proportion of glycinergic and GABAergic cells identified within the VNLL (rat: Moore and Moore, 1987; Aoki et al., 1988; Campos et al., 2001; Riquelme et al., 2001; gerbil: Roberts and Ribak, 1987; Saint Marie and Baker, 1990; bat: Vater et al., 1992; Winer et al., 1995), provides strong evidence for the VNLL being the major source of inhibitory input to the IC. The extent of the terminations within the IC also suggest that the VNLL is well placed to widely influence IC responses (guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997). In addition to the projection to the CNIC, sparse projections from the dorsal VNLL to the external cortex of the IC, medial geniculate body, DNLL, dorsomedial periolivary region and the VNTB have been described (chinchilla: Morest et al., 1997; cat: Kudo, 1981; Whitley and Henkel, 1983, 1984; Kudo and Nakamura, 1988). A projection from giant multipolar cells of the VNLLv to the auditory thalamus is interesting as it appears to bypass the IC, and this subcollicular projection has been described in cats (Henkel, 1983) and some species of bats (Casseday et al., 1989). There are also some minor projections to the superior colliculus (rat: Tanaka et al., 1985; cat: Kudo, 1981; bat: Casseday and Covey, 1983). Some recent studies have identified small numbers of cholinergic neurons within the VCLL (guinea pig: Motts et al. 2008; goldfish: Giraldez-Perez et al., 2009) and have proposed that acetylcholine may play a role in auditory processing. However, the source of these cholinergic projections and their precise circuitry within the auditory brainstem remains unknown. Few studies have explicitly investigated the INLL‘s projections separately from the VNLL. However, it has been suggested that the INLL projects to both the contralateral and ipsilateral CNIC in the cat (Brunso-Bechtold et al., 1981). Unlike the VNLL, the INLL has been identified as an excitatory structure (cat: Saint Marie et al., 1997).

Intrinsic Projections of the VCLL Adams (1979) described a population of oval cells in the ventral VNLL of the cat that were not stained by retrograde HRP label following widespread injections in the IC. He believed that these oval cells projected internally within the VCLL. The oval cells were described as having large endbulbs and resembled the principle cells of the MNTB (Adams, 1979). They received a thick axon input which originated from octopus cells of the PVCN (guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Adams, 1997). Other studies have also described both bushy and stellate cells in the middle and ventral zones of the VNLL that project to other cells within the VNLL (rat: Zhao and Wu, 2001; cat: Whitley and Henkel, 1984), typically as collaterals or descending branches of an ascending projection to the IC (Zhao and Wu, 2001). The local projections typically terminate in bouton terminals within the ventral part of the VCLL and most likely form inhibitory circuits (Zhao and Wu, 2001). The nature and function of these intrinsic projections within the VCLL have been described as ―the missing key piece in this puzzle‖ (Merchán et al., 1997 p. 214). Nayagam et al. (2005, 2006) recently proposed a function for these inhibitory VNLL interneurons. They suggested that this intrinsic circuit

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

33

within the VCLL was activated by octopus cells of the contralateral PVCN and provided fast inhibitory control of action potential timing in the VCLL, and by extension, the IC as well. This circuit will be described in more detail below.

Input from the Octopus Cell Area and Calyceal Synapses in the VCLL

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Characteristics of Input to the VCLL from Octopus Cells VNLL neurons receive convergent excitatory input from a variety of cell types within the contralateral CN (rat: Zhao and Wu, 2001; cat: Glendenning et al., 1981), including a predominantly exclusive projection (with the occasional collateral to the SOC) from the OCA of the contralateral CN (guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Adams, 1997, Smith et al., 2005). Octopus cells respond to the onsets of sounds with exquisitely timed responses (cat: Godfrey et al., 1975; Rhode and Smith, 1986), termed onset-ideal (OI). This response pattern is a result of the detection of synchrony in auditory nerve fibre inputs representing a wide range of CFs (Oertel et al., 2000; mouse: Golding et al., 1995). Consequently, octopus cells are very broadly tuned to sound stimulus frequency and often it is not possible to distinguish a CF for these cells. The function of these cells is unknown, although they seem well suited to encode onsets, transients, and temporal features of complex, periodic stimuli (Oertel and Wickesberg, 2002). Characteristics of Calyceal Synapses in the VCLL Of Mammals Some afferent projections to the VNLL terminate in specialised calyceal synapses. Similar large synaptic connections known as ‗endbulbs of Held‘, are described in the AVCN (cat: Ibata and Pappas, 1976; Ryugo and Sento, 1991), others known as ‗calyces of Held‘ have been found in the MNTB (cat: Morest, 1968; rat: Lenn and Reese, 1966; various species: Morest and Jean-Baptiste, 1975). First described by Held (1893), this specialisation in some subcollicular neurons is essentially a secure synapse that typically leads to faithful one-to-one transmission of input to output (cat: Adams, 1997). This synapse allows the precise timing information contained in nerve fibre discharges to be preserved within the AVCN, thereby allowing a faithful representation of the original auditory stimulus. Studies on the AVCN and the MNTB calyces have shown that one AP in the presynaptic axon generates one AP in the postsynaptic cell (cat: Guinan and Li, 1990). However, this does not necessarily lead to the conclusion that the postsynaptic cell faithfully reflects the primary input, as it is possible that other sources may modify the output (gerbil: Kopp-Scheinpflug et al., 2002; bat: Vater et al., 1997).The calyx consists of a large and complex terminal which envelops a high proportion of the post-synaptic cell and may incorporate multiple synapses (Adams, 1997). Stotler (cat; 1953) first described large calyces in the VNLL and Pfeiffer (cat; 1966) first reported their physiological properties, discovering by means of extracellular recordings that the post-synaptic APs would faithfully follow the pre-synaptic potentials with a fixed delay of approximately 0.5 msec. This discovery of VCLL cells that receive a calyx-like synapse is important as it allows insight into the properties of the cell from which the projection originated (by examining the pre-synaptic potentials in single-cell recordings). Furthermore,

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

34

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

the fact that the VCLL contains calyces suggests that it plays an important role in coding auditory timing information. Subsequent to the discovery in cats, specialised calyceal synaptic endings have been described now in the VCLL of many mammalian species. Typically these calyceal endings are not distributed throughout the VCLL, but in subregions of the VNLL (guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997) and are known to provide a substrate for fast, robust synaptic signal transmission to cells that receive them (bat: Vater et al., 1997). In echolocating bats there have been several studies that have indicated that VNLLc neurons receive large putatively excitatory synaptic endings that resemble endbulbs of Held (Zook and Casseday, 1985; Covey and Casseday, 1986; Vater and Feng, 1990; Covey, 1993a; Huffman and Covey, 1995; Vater et al., 1997). The study by Vater et al. (1997) in bat attempted to determine, firstly, if the large synaptic terminals in the VNLLc were indeed typical of calyces and, secondly, what other types of inputs did VNLLc cells receive and how were the synapses on these cells organised? In this study, the authors used similar criteria for distinguishing calyx-like endings as have been used in the MNTB (for calyces of Held) and in the CN (for endbulbs of Held). The criteria are three fold: 1) very large boutons containing sparsely distributed round type vesicles with multiple active zones; 2) an asymmetric synapse whereby the postsynaptic membrane density is more prominent than the presynaptic membrane density within the active zones; 3) an enlarged extracellular space in the elongated zone of apposition between the calyx and the postsynaptic cell (Vater et al., 1997). Vater et al. (1997) found that calyx-like endings can contact both the soma of a particular cell and the surrounding dendrites of other cells, and this was a robust organisational feature of the VNLLc of bats. Small round vesicle boutons may represent extensions of a calyx-like ending, and may be found in locations around the soma and on dendritic profiles (Vater et al., 1997). The more symmetrically organised flattened and pleomorphic vesicles have also been observed in perisomatic locations and on dendritic profiles. In this way, VNLLc neurons may receive input from a wide range of frequencies, albeit with some amount of delay introduced for the non-somatic transmission. It has been hypothesised that the VNLLc neurons of bats do not simply reflect the response properties of their inputs but are able to extract even greater timing resolution over a wide frequency range by modifying the inputs (Vater et al., 1997).

Source of VCLL Calyces VCLL calyces are the terminals of thick myelinated axons, which, are likely to contribute to rapid neural propagation (guinea pig: Schofield, 1995; Schofield and Cant, 1997). These fibres can innervate multiple targets, and equally a single recipient cell may receive input from two afferent thick fibres (Vater et al., 1997). In non-echolocating species, the source of the thick axon projection that ends in these calyces has been identified by several studies as the octopus cells of the PVCN. Using anterograde and retrograde transport, Schofield and Cant (1997) correlated the distal segments of thick axons with octopus cells of the PVCN. The electrophysiological signatures of cells that gave rise to the calyces in the VNLL matched those of octopus cells (various species: Rhode and Greenberg, 1992; cat: Godfrey et al., 1975) in the CN. The fact that both octopus cells and endbulbs are calretinin positive (octopus cells are the only CN cell type that are calretinin positive), strengthens the case. A similar circuit appears to be present in the human VNLL as well (Adams, 1997) and provides further support for the suggestion that octopus cells are the origin of the calyces in the VNLL

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

35

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

(rat: Wu, 1999; Zhao and Wu, 2001; guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997; cat: Warr, 1966; van Noort, 1969; Warr, 1969). There have been other possible sources suggested for the thick axon projection to the VCLL. In big brown bats the calyces in the VNLL arise from both the AVCN and PVCN (Huffman and Covey, 1995). Covey and Casseday (bat: 1986), as well as Vater and Feng (bat: 1990), proposed that the endbulbs originated from large multipolar cells. However, in cat (Osen, 1972; Adams and Warr, 1976; Adams, 1993) and horseshoe bat (Vater and Feng, 1990), the calyceal endings originated purely from the PVCN and were probably from octopus cells or large multipolar cells that are known to precisely encode the onset of sound. In contrast, results from Huffman and Covey (1995), as well as Vater and Braun (1994) (also in bat), did not show that the calyces originated from octopus cells.

Neurons Receiving Calyces in the VCLL A study by Schofield and Cant (guinea pig; 1997) identified the VNLL cells with calyces as globular cells (mouse: Willard and Ryugo, 1983; cat: Adams, 1979; bat: Zook and Casseday, 1985; Vater and Feng, 1990). Adams (cat and human; 1997) also described the projection of octopus cells to globular bushy cells of the contralateral VNLL by using anterograde filling of axons, immunostaining for calretinin, and making single unit electrophysiological recordings. He found converging lines of evidence that, coupled with the results from other studies, supported the claim that the terminations of the axons of octopus cells were calyces of Held and that these endbulbs terminate upon globular bushy cells of the VNLL. In a study in rat brain slices (Zhao and Wu, 2001), it was suggested that synapses similar to calyces of Held are found on VNLL bushy cells. By providing a large synaptic current when activated by APs originating from the VCN, these terminals can provide secure and faithful transmission of signals through the VNLL even when the receiving cell‘s input resistance is low. Zhao and Wu (2001) believe that this configuration might be significant for preserving the fine temporal structure of auditory stimuli. In mice and bats, globular cells are segregated from other cell types (mouse: Willard and Ryugo, 1983; bat: Zook and Casseday, 1985; Covey and Casseday, 1986; Vater and Feng, 1990). In some species, globular/bushy cells tend to be preferentially distributed ventrally within the VCLL (Schofield and Cant, 1997). There have been no reports of calyceal synapses within, or thick axon projections to, the INLL. Furthermore, octopus cells are not known to innervate the ipsilateral VNLL of any species. Therefore, VNLL cells receiving octopus cell input respond exclusively to stimulation of the contralateral ear. In summary, at least in non-echolocating species, octopus cells give rise to thick myelinated axons that terminate in large calyx-like synapses on globular cells of the VNLL, akin to endbulbs of Held in other auditory nuclei (Schofield and Cant, 1997; Adams, 1997). Given that nerve propagation velocity in myelinated axons (saltatory conduction) is known to be a function of axon diameter, and given the secure nature of calyceal connections, these characteristics suggest that this pathway provides fast and faithful transmission of neural signals (Zhao and Wu, 2001; Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999). The presence of a specialised circuit of this nature suggests that the VNLL may be important in conveying and processing timing information.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

36

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

RESPONSE PROPERTIES OF VCLL NEURONS There are relatively few electrophysiological studies of the VCLL in non-echolocating animals. Of the studies that exist, mainly extracellular responses have been recorded in response to acoustic stimulation in the rat (Zhang and Kelly, 2006a, b), rabbit (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997, 2002a, b; Batra, 2006; Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999), and cat (Aitkin et al., 1970a; Aitkin et al., 1970b; Guinan et al., 1972b; Guinan et al., 1972a). Intracellular responses to current injection have been recorded in slice preparation (rat: Wu, 1999; Zhao and Wu, 2001). Responses to complex stimulation such as frequency or amplitude-modulated tones have been studied to a limited extent (Zhang and Kelly, 2006b; Batra, 2006; Huffman et al., 1998a, b) but are beyond the scope of this chapter. To date, there have been only two studies that have investigated the intracellular neural responses to acoustic stimulation in vivo (Nayagam et al. 2005, 2006).

Frequency Tuning Properties of VCLL Neurons

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

There is a great deal of convergence that occurs within the VNLL; many cells within the structure have been described as coincidence detectors (various species: Oertel and Wickesberg, 2002), and although this endows the cells with greater temporal accuracy it comes at the expense of frequency acuity (bat: Covey and Casseday, 1991). Therefore, it would be expected that many VNLL cells would be broadly tuned.

Tuning Properties of VCLL Neurons in Echolocating Bats Covey and Casseday (1991) recorded frequency tuning curves (TCs) from neurons in the monaural nuclei (INLL, VNLLc and VNLLm) of echolocating big brown bats. Their results showed that the TCs of units located in the VNLLc were much broader than units found in the other monaural NLL. The TCs of VNLLc cells were asymmetrical and resembled broad-band filters with steep upper cut-offs and shallow lower cut-offs. The TCs of neurons in the INLL and the VNLLm were typically more symmetrical V-shaped responses with similar upper and lower slopes. However, on average, all the monaural NLL in bat showed broader TCs than other neurons at the midbrain level. In particular, the neurons of the VNLLc were not well suited to frequency encoding (Covey and Casseday, 1991). However, another study in the mustache bat found that the INLL contained neurons that detected and responded optimally to combinations of sound (Portfors and Wenstrup, 2001). Recently, Xie et al. reporting on the response properties of neurons in the INLL of the Mexican free-tailed bat described mainly broadly tuned neurons with no surround inhibition or inhibition dependent properties (bat: Xie et al., 2005). Clearly, even within the bat genus, there are many species-specific differences in the response properties and specialisations of VNLL and INLL cells. Tuning Properties of VCLL Cells in Non-Echolocating Species Studies performed on rabbits to determine the frequency TCs of neurons in the lateral lemniscus have provided contradictory findings. Metzner and Radtke-Schuller (1987) found no increased frequency selectivity in the VCLL compared to neurons at lower auditory levels, while Batra and Fitzpatrick (1999) observed a range of tuning selectivity when the

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

37

isofrequency contours of responses recorded at a range of intensities and frequencies were mapped in unanaesthetised preparations (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999). Neurons that responded to acoustic stimuli with an onset type response typically had broad frequency tuning, a finding compatible with inputs from broadly tuned, onset cells of the CN. On the other hand, neurons that responded with sustained and transient responses tended to be more narrowly tuned. Tuning of short latency, sustained neurons tended to be narrower for higher frequency units. In the rat, Zhang and Kelly (2006a) recently reported on the tuning properties of VCLL units. They found a majority (55%) of extracellularly recorded units had V- or Ushaped frequency TCs with discernable CFs. They also found many units with more complex multipeaked (37%) or other (8%) tuning curve (TC) patterns. However, it must be remembered that in extracellular electrophysiological studies, it is difficult to differentiate neural responses from fibres of passage, and it is possible that some of these data may in fact reflect responses from ascending lemniscal fibres. In a subset of their sample (17/96 units) Zhang and Kelly (2006a) used the AMPA (-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl4-isoxazolepropionic acid) antagonist NBQX (2,3-dihydroxy-6-nitro-7-sulfamoylbenzo[f]quinoxaline-2,3-dione) to differentiate neural responses from fibres. The authors do not explicitly state whether or not this sample of 'definite' units differed in any way from the larger sample of 'assumed' cells. However, the synaptic antagonist, NBQX, did substantially reduce both monaural and binaural responses under all stimulus conditions. Figure 6 shows intracellular threshold tuning curves recorded by Nayagam et al. (2006) from 9 VCLL neurons in response to suprathreshold best frequency tones. These neurons responded with predominantly single peaked V-shaped tuning curves as well as some multipeaked curves similar to the descriptions from Zhang and Kelly (2006a).

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Classification of Response Types of VCLL Neurons For the most part, it is possible to classify the neuronal response types found in the NLL using the same categories established in the CN (cat: Pfeiffer, 1966; Blackburn and Sachs, 1989); for review of VCN response types, see Rhode and Greenberg (1992). As the VCN is a principal input to the VCLL, it is interesting to consider to what extent the response types in the VCN are reflected in the VCLL. Table 4 provides a list of common synonymous terms used in various studies to describe responses of cells in the VCLL to pure tone and noise bursts. Table 4. Table of synonymous descriptors (grouped horizontally) used to label VCLL neural responses to tone and noise bursts.

Onset Regular On-Pause Adapting

Synonymous Terms Phasic Chopper Pauser Primary-Like

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

On Multimodal Notch Sustained

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

38

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Response Types in Echolocating Species In echolocating bats, the most comprehensive electrophysiological study that addressed the response properties of monaural lateral lemniscal neurons was performed by Covey and Casseday (1991). In this study, the authors reported on the firing patterns and the classification they assigned based on responses to pure tones at the unit‘s CF and 20 dB above threshold. In summary, Covey and Casseday (1991) categorised neurons into ‗phasic‘, ‗choppers‘, ‗tonic‘, ‗primary-like‘ and ‗pausers‘; the phasic neurons could be further divided into two types, constant-latency and variable-latency. Phasic constant-latency neurons usually fired one action potential (AP) per stimulus, tightly locked to the stimulus onset, whereas the phasic variable-latency neurons fired between one and three APs per stimulus at the onset, but with as much as several milliseconds variance in first spike latency. The constant-latency and the variable-latency types were clearly segregated in the monaural NLL of echolocating bats. The constant-latency neurons were almost exclusive to the columnar region (making up 95% of the population in this area), whereas the variable latency units were found in the INLL and the VNLLm, where they represented about 20-30% of the population. The choppers that were classified in the Covey and Casseday (1991) study responded throughout the duration of the stimulus with regularly timed APs (regular interspike intervals; ISIs) and with tight locking of the first AP to the time of stimulus onset. They were mostly ‗fast‘ choppers with ISIs of less than 2.5 ms (Tsuchitani, 1988a, b). The choppers were located in the VNLLm and INLL but not in the VNLLc. The tonic neurons showed a constant high rate of discharge throughout the duration of the stimulus, with practically no adaptation even with long duration stimuli (of over 50 msec). The first AP of the tonic neurons was not locked to the onset of the sound, and the APs sometimes had regular ISIs (as regular as a chopper response) and sometimes did not. A few of the tonic neurons became more chopper-like and the first AP became more synchronised to the stimulus onset as the stimulus sound pressure level (SPL) was increased. These tonic neurons were only found in the INLL and VNLLm. Another response type identified in the bat was a primary-like response that resembled the responses found in the auditory nerve (cat: Kiang et al., 1965a, b). These responded throughout the stimulus duration, but with a diminishing firing rate after the initial transient ‗on‘ response. These units were rare, with less than 5% of the population in each of the three monaural nuclei. The last response type identified by Covey and Casseday (1991) was the pauser response, which discharged initially then underwent a period of inactivity before resuming a lower level of sporadic discharge for the remainder of the stimulus. Again, these neurons were rare, making up less than 4% of the population of the VNLLm and the INLL. They were not found at all in the VNLLc. Therefore, in the big brown bat at least, there are several different response types in the monaural NLL. Most of the neurons (59%) in these nuclei had monotonic rate-level functions (firing rate increased or remained level over increasing SPL), and the majority of the nonmonotonic cells were choppers. The response types in the INLL and the VNLLm were mixed. However, between these two structures was a region, the VNLLc, that was almost entirely populated with one type of neuron (distinct both physiologically and cytoarchitecturally), and these neurons showed a phasic constant-latency response.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

39

- Nayagam et al. (2006)

Figure 6. Intracellular threshold tuning curves from nine VCLL neurons. Response type to suprathreshold characteristic frequency (CF) tones is indicated in the bottom left of each panel or by arrows. Numbers next to each plot provide the cell‘s ID.

In the INLL of the Mexican free-tailed bat, four response types were described by Xie et al. (2005) in response to CF tone bursts, sustained chopper, primary like, onset chopper and onset. All these responses were monotonic, unselective for species-specific calls, and unresponsive to blocking inhibition with the exception on one unit which was highly influenced by inhibition. Given that VNLL and INLL in particular is highly developed in bats and dolphins it is likely that it plays a prominent role in echolocation, such as the extraction of spectral and

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

40

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

temporal features of a stimuli, or delay processing. The spatial segregation of response types is an interesting feature of the echolocating bat, however, has not been described to the same extent in other non-echolocating species.

Response Types in Non-Echolocating Species Other species may share, at least in part, the neuron types and segregation that is most evident in echolocating bats (cat: Glendenning et al., 1981; bat: Covey and Casseday, 1991). Table 5 summarises the results of prominent studies investigating neural responses to acoustic stimulation in the VCLL of various non-echolocating species (excluding rats). Aitkin et al. (1970a) found that the response patterns in the VCLL of cats were similar to the responses from the DNLL. They recorded sustained activity (sometimes with pauses in firing) as well as single APs at stimulus onset, in response to pure tones. In many cases, the firing patterns were both frequency and SPL dependent. Of the 17 units they studied for monotonicity, all but two were monotonic. These findings fit with those of Covey and Casseday (1991) in echolocating bats. Aitkin et al. (1970a) concluded that the timing and the extent of APs in a discharge train was the net result of excitatory and inhibitory inputs acting on the neuron. Further, these influences were frequency and SPL dependant and the balancing of excitation and inhibition determined the number of APs fired for a given stimulus. This conclusion was similar to that drawn from previous studies in the CN (cat: Greenwood and Maruyama, 1965) and the IC (cat: Rose et al., 1963), and was subsequently echoed in several VCLL electrophysiological studies in other species(rat: Wu, 1999; Zhao and Wu, 2001; rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999). Response types found in the rabbit VNLL in more recent studies have, for the most part, been in agreement with the types of responses found in other species; however, there has been some debate over the proportions of neurons displaying different patterns (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999). Some authors attribute these disparities to the use of anaesthesia, which influences discharge patterns (rabbit: Kuwada et al., 1989; cat: Brownell et al., 1979; Ritz and Brownell, 1982; rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999). The types of responses found in the rabbit are: sustained, which were similar to sustained responses observed in the VCLL in other species such as cat (Guinan et al., 1972b; Guinan et al., 1972a) and bat (Covey and Casseday, 1991); onset responses; primary-like; and chopper types. The neurons in Batra and Fitzpatrick‘s (rabbit: 1999) study were classified as either phasic or sustained based on their responses to tone bursts at CF at 60–80 dB SPL. Phasic neurons were separated into onset or transient sub-types based on the shape of the poststimulus time histogram (PSTH). Phasic-onset neurons were characterised by ongoing firing rates (between 35–75 ms after stimulus onset) less than 25 APs per second, whereas phasictransient responses gave an initial (between 0–35 ms) discharge rate greater than twice the driven sustained response (cat: Aitkin et al., 1970a). The sustained neurons were separated into three sub-types based on PSTH shape: long-latency, short-latency, and strongly adapting. Long-latency status was given if the latency was more than 7 ms greater than the median of the other neurons of similar CFs in the sample. Most of the neurons were located in the VNLLm and VNLLl, with VNLLm containing mainly onset neurons and VNLLl mainly short latency sustained neurons. Table 6 summarises the results of studies that have investigated the neural responses in the rat VCLL. The intracellular brain slice study of Wu (1999) categorised rat VCLL neurons into two types, I and II, that displayed different intrinsic membrane properties. They found

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

41

that type I cells had linear current-voltage relationships; they fired several APs in response to depolarising current injections. Type I cells were sub-classified, based on their discharge patterns, into three distinct classes: regular, adaptation and onset-pause. Type II cells have non-linear current-voltage relationships and only fired one AP at the onset of depolarising injections; they were referred to as onset cells (1999). Extending Wu‘s (1999) findings, Zhao and Wu (2001) used whole cell patch-clamp recordings from rat VCLL neurons in brain slice preparations to examine the membrane properties and synaptic responses of single cells. The physiological results were correlated with cytoarchitectural and morphological findings derived from iontophoretically-injected Neurobiotin-filled neurons (see below). Following depolarising current injection, they found that VCLL neurons could be classified into one of five distinct groups: onset, regular, adapting, bursting, and onset-pause. Non-linear type II neurons showed onset responses of one to three small spikes at the onset of depolarisation elicited by positive current injection (up to 1 nA). Regular responses were observed in 19% of neurons and showed a continuous train of APs with almost regular ISIs. Adapting units (20%) were similar to the regular cells, in that they fired continuous trains of spikes. However, in response to low intensity current, there was a delay of 65-75 ms between the onset of current injection and the generation of the first AP; as the current level was increased, this delay was reduced. At lower current levels adapting neurons showed variable ISIs but at higher current levels the ISIs were regular.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Table 5. Electrophysiological studies of neuron response type in the VCLL of nonecholocating species (excluding rats). Author(s)/Year

Species Technique/Stimulus

Response Type

Aitkin et al. (1970a)

Cat

n = 17

Guinan et al. (1972b; 1972a)

Cat

Batra and Rabbit Fitzpatrick (1999)

Extracellular recordings in vivo / pure tones at CF

Extracellular recordings in vivo / pure tones at CF

Extracellular recordings in vivo (unanaesthetised) / pure tones at CF

Onset Sustained Pauser n = 25 Onset/Phasic (n = 6) Sustained (n = 17) Primary-Like Pauser Chopper - Transient Chopper - Sustained Long Latency (n = 1) Inhibition (n = 1) n = 160 Phasic / Onset (n = 86) Onset Transient Offset Sustained (n = 74) Short-Latency Long Latency Strongly Adapting Inhibited (n = 10 / 175)

% in VCLL

Location

-

-

24

-

8 4 24 32 4 4

VNLLm 49 5 16 VNLL1 38 16 2 6

Percentages and sample do not necessarily sum to 100%, as some responses form intersecting sets and different samples are represented.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

42

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Table 6. Electrophysiological studies of neuron response type in rat VCLL. Author(s)/ Year

Species

Technique/Stimulus

Response Type

Wu (1999)

Rat

Intracellular recordings in vitro / current injection

n = 54 Onset (type II) Regular (type I) Adapting (type I) Onset-pause (type I) n = 59 Onset (non-linear) Regular (linear) Adapting (linear) Bursting (linear) Onset-pause (linear) n = 99

Zhao and Wu (2001)

Rat

Zhang and Kelly (2006a)

Rat

Nayagam et Rat al. (2005, 2006)

Whole cell patch clamp in vitro / current injection

Extracellular recordings in vivo / pure tones at CF

Intracellular recordings in vivo / pure tones at CF

Onset (n = 33) Phasic Phasic Burst Phasic Burst with High Spon Double Burst Sustained (n = 55) Primary Like Pauser Late Response Irregular Sustained Other (n = 6) Fast Adapting n = 24 Pauser (n = 3)

% in Location VCLL

Morphology

23 32 20 29

-

Bushy -

27 19 20 27 7

ventral ventral mainly dorsal Through-out ventral

Bushy Stellate I Stellate II Elongate Stellate I

Binaural Type

n = 96; EI (60.4%), EI (13.5%), EE/EI (13.5%), E0 (12.5%)† 13 11 12

-

-

2

-

-

33 8 7 7

-

-

6

-

-

13

Through-out

EI (66%), E0 (33%) Onset Ideal (n=2) 8 VNLL only E0 Onset Chopper (n = 21 INLL only EI (40%), E0 5) (40%), EI (20%) Transient Chopper 25 Through-out Stellate II E0 (33%), (n = 6) EE (33%), EI (33%) Onset-A (n = 3) 13 Through-out Bushy EE (50%) E0 (50%) * Onset-B (n =5) 21 Through-out Stellate I EO (80%) ** Percentages and sample sizes do not necessarily sum to 100%, as some responses form intersecting samples and different samples are represented. Values are rounded to the nearest integer. † 96 E0, EI, EE and combined EE/EI responses were recorded but not correlated with response type. * Only two of the three Onset-A cells in this study were presented with binaural stimuli. ** Only four out of the five Onset-B cells were presented with binaural stimuli.

Bursting trains of discharges were seen in 27% of the neurons arising from a hump of depolarisation that lasted longer than 100 ms at low current levels. Following the burst was a period of regular sustained firing at higher current levels. Finally, the onset-pause type of response was seen in 7% of neurons. A small amount of current produced a single spike at the onset of depolarisation, which became sustained firing as the current was increased. However, a period of inactivity after the first spike remained. Higher currents elicited an initial short train of spikes, followed by a pause, before settling into sustained firing. In all types of

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

43

responses, except the onset class, the firing frequency and the strength of the current injection were proportional (Zhao and Wu, 2001). A recently published study by Zhang and Kelly (2006a) comprehensively investigated extracellular VCLL responses to monaural and binaural tone bursts in the rat (Figure 7). They used micro-iontophoretic injection of an AMPA receptor antagonist NBQX to deactivate local synapses at the recording site to help establish whether the recording was from a cell or fibre. However, this technique was only used in 17 out of 96 neurons and it is unknown whether these 'proven' cells differed in any significant way to the larger sample of 'assumed' cells. However, the fact that all units showed a decreased firing rate when presented with the drug suggests that this study was recoding from cells and not fibres of passage. In response to 100 ms contralateral tone bursts 20 dB SPL above threshold at CF they sorted 99 neurons into 9 types based on temporal firing patterns. They divided their 9 types into 3 main categories: onset, sustained and other. Within the onset category, they described phasic (n = 13), phasic burst (n = 11), phasic burst with high spontaneous rate (SR) (n = 12), and double bursts (n = 2). The sustained category contained primary-like (n = 33), pauser (n = 8), late response (n = 7), and irregular sustained (n=7). The ‗other‘ category contained fast adapting (n = 6) responses that, although they could have been classified as onset, were considered as a special group and excluded from further onset versus sustained analysis. They concluded that onset and sustained cells were not segregated within the VCLL, although they only examined the distribution of neurons in the dorsoventral plane and did not distinguish between various classes of onset neurons. They found that onset units, when compared with sustained units, had shorter average first spike latencies (onset mean FSL: 8.3 ms; sustained mean FSL: 14.8 ms) and a higher degree of temporal acuity (onset mean FSL standard deviation; SD: 0.59 ms; sustained mean FSL SD: 4.2 ms). Sustained neurons became less tightly locked to the onset of the stimulus with increasing stimulus level; however, onset neurons maintained their low jitter at various stimulus levels. Intracellular recordings by Nayagam et al. (2006) have distinguished several classes of neurons based on their responses to noise and tone burst. Six response types were determined based on the single and average responses to CF tones. Intracellular recordings allow classification to be made based not only on AP times and pattern, but also on the synaptic events, membrane profile and, if filled with a neuronal dye, the cellular morphology as well. The 24 contralaterally excited responses determined in this study were termed onset ideal (n = 2), onset chopper (n = 5), onset-A (n = 3), onset-B (n = 5), transient chopper (n = 6) and pauser (n = 3). Within these six intracellular types, the first four were varieties of onset response separated by subtle yet distinct differences in their membrane profiles. In response to broadband noise stimulation a complex range of responses was observed intracellularly. These noise responses did not consistently correlate with the responses to tones; however, it was observed that onset neurons with ongoing depolarised membranes in response to tones would typically respond with sustained firing when presented with noise. Onset ideal units responded at a shorter latency compared to all other types and were only responsive to contralateral stimulation. Following their precisely-timed onset AP, a period of hyperpolarisation was observed, which may serve to prevent later arriving EPSPs from evoking APs (Figure 8). These onset ideal responses may be a result of a thick axon projection (ending with a calyceal synapse) from the OCA of the contralateral CN (Nayagam et al., 2005). A future study could be designed to visualise synapses on the somas of physiologically characterised and dye filled neurons, particularly the onset ideal cells. This

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

44

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

would prove that the onset ideal responses are indeed from bushy cells with calyx-like synapses. Onset chopper neurons showed a characteristic second AP discharged at a regular interspike interval, in response to CF tones. These cells also had a short and narrow distribution of FSLs. Onset-A cells were distinguished by a negatively graded membrane potential throughout the stimulus and a bushy cell morphology, whereas onset-B neurons showed stable depolarisation and a stellate cell morphology. Onset-A neurons had a longer and wider distribution of FSL compared with onset-B cells. Presumably, onset ideal, onset-A and onsetB type responses would be difficult to differentiate based only on extracellular recordings and response pattern; they may have been considered as one class of cell with a wide distribution of FSLs.

- Zhang and Kelly (2006a): with kind permission from The American Physiological Society.

Figure 7. Extracellularly recorded temporal response patterns of rat VCLL neurons. Post-stimulus time histograms (PSTHs) representing the number of spikes occurring in 5 ms time bins before, during, and after presentation of a 100 ms CF tone burst 20 dB SPL above cell threshold. Figure shows nine extracellular response subtypes of three major response groups. A: Responses of onset units. B: Responses of sustained units. C: Response of other unit. Spike counts are based on 20 repetitions of the stimulus. Dashed vertical lines and black bars below the x-axes of the lower histograms of section A, B, and C indicate the limits of the tone presentation.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

45

- Nayagam et al. (2006)

Figure 8. Intracellular responses to noise and tones of onset ideal neurons. A: Response from a onset ideal neuron showing the similarity in intracellular profile between noise and tone stimuli. Top panel shows 10 trace overlay (grey) and average response (black) to contralaterally and ipsilaterally presented 80 dB RMS noise (contralateral and ipsilateral stimuli were offset by 100 ms). Lower panels show single trace responses to contralaterally presented CF (middle) and off-CF (bottom) tones presented at 80 dB SPL. B: 10 trace overlay and average responses (top, middle) and single trace responses (bottom) from three onset ideal neurons with similar intracellular profiles to noise and to the cell shown in (A). Horizontal dashed lines show resting membrane potential (RMP) and thick horizontal bars show stimulus duration.

Nayagam et al.'s (2006) intracellular responses to CF tones are comparable to extracellular in vivo responses from the VNLL of other non echolocating species (Aitkin et al., 1970a; Guinan et al., 1972b; Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999). These studies have described onset, regular, sustained, and chopping response. Batra and Fitzpatrick (1999) described neurons that were suppressed below spontaneous rate after the initial onset peak. This may correspond with onset ideal units from the Nayagam et al. (2006) study. They also found at least one other class of onset cell, preferentially distributed within the medial limb of the VNLL that did not appear to be innervated by octopus cells. They concluded that the VNLL

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

46

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

contains multiple populations of onset neurons, consistent with the Nayagam et al. (2006) study. Transient chopper neurons from the Nayagam et al. (2006) study may also correspond with the chopper neurons described in previous extracellular studies, although transient chopper neurons in the present study had a wider distribution of FSLs. The Nayagam et al. (2006) study did not record any neurons that chopped throughout the stimulus within the VCLL, however, this response type was observed in the DNLL (data not shown). The dynamic range of onset responses in the Nayagam et al., (2006) study was narrower than that of pauser and transient chopper responses, consistent with the extracellular findings of Batra and Fitzpatrick (1999). The neurons presented in this study all showed latencies that decreased with increasing stimulus level or proximity to CF. This result is also in agreement with in vivo extracellular studies of the VCLL (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999). Although phasic and sustained offset (including offset chopper) responses to noise were observed, these types were not classified in response to CF tones. Nayagam et al.'s (2006) intracellular responses to acoustic stimuli in vivo are similar in many ways to intracellular responses to current injection in vitro within the VCLL (Wu, 1999; Zhao and Wu, 2001). Both techniques found different response classes, some with onset, some with sustained and some with pauser profiles, but in response to current injection there was more sustained activity (regular, adapting). Additional circuitry (particularly the inhibitory projections to the VNLL) is most likely the reason for the reduction in sustained spike rate in vivo. The illustrated contour plots (see below), showing broadly tuned onset components and narrowly tuned sustained components, as well as a mixture of excitation and inhibition off-CF, supports the notion of convergent input streams. Additionally, in most cells, different responses to noise and tones were observed, suggesting that specific mechanisms are activated for broad and narrow band stimulation. Powerful, short-duration inhibition before the first AP was recorded from units with both onset and long duration firing patterns and is thought to be activated by octopus cells in the contralateral CN (Nayagam et al., 2005) (see below). Long duration inhibition was also seen in response to both contralateral and ipsilateral stimuli, sometimes in conjunction with excitation but sometimes without any excitation present (I0 and 0I response types). These long duration IPSPs may be the result of ascending projections from the MNTB, other nuclei within the SOC (Glendenning et al., 1981) or even intrinsic circuitry (Zhao and Wu, 2001; Whitley and Henkel, 1984). Of the 6 response classes to CF tones described by Nayagam et al. (2006), evidence was found for a preferential spatial distribution of two of these types; onset ideal units were only located within the VNLL and onset chopper units were localised to the INLL. The dorsal part of the VCLL (INLL) contained a greater proportion of cells that responded with excitation to stimulation of the contralateral ear and inhibition from ipsilateral stimulation. The proportion of neurons from Nayagam et al.'s (2006) study for which the rate of AP discharge was a non-monotonic function of SPL (~9%) was comparable with the proportion reported by Aitkin (1970a) (~12%) and suggests a level-dependant inhibitory input, despite the lack of visible IPSPs in the intracellular traces. Aitkin (1970a) suggested that the mechanism causing non-monotonicity within the VNLL is activated before or along with the excitatory process. Given that the two neuronal types in Nayagam et al.'s (2006) sample that showed non-monotonic responses (transient chopper and onset-A) had long FSLs with a wide distribution, it is conceivable that they received projections from local inhibitory neurons with

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

47

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

a shorter FSL. Intrinsic connections within the VNLL have been described previously as local collaterals of ascending axons and are believed to be inhibitory (Zhao and Wu, 2001).

- Nayagam et al. (2006)

Figure 9. Morphology and intracellular responses of a transient chopper neuron. A: Single trace in response to a contralaterally presented CF tone at 80 dB SPL. B: Single traces showing responses to CF tones at 3 SPLs. C: Single traces in response to a range of off-CF tones at 80 dB SPL. Arrow indicates membrane hyperpolarisation to a high frequency tone. D: Overlay of 10 traces (grey) and average response (black) to contralaterally presented 80 dB SPL CF tone (top) and contralaterally and ipsilaterally presented 80 dB RMS noise (bottom; contralateral and ipsilateral stimuli offset by 100 ms). Horizontal dashed lines show RMP and thick horizontal bars show stimulus duration. E: Morphology of recorded transient chopper neuron revealed by Neurobiotin labelling. The presumed axon is indicated by ‗a‘. Inset shows neuron‘s location (black dot) within the VCLL.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

48

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

- Nayagam et al. (2006)

Figure 10.Intracellular responses to tones of a transient chopper neuron Same cell as shown in Figure 9. A: Rate-level function (black) and average FSL (grey) at CF in steps of 10 dB SPL. B: ILD function showing the firing rate in response to a range of ipsilateral CF tone SPLs presented concurrently with an 60 dB SPL contralateral CF tone (black), and average FSL of the ILD function (grey). C: Iso-level frequency function (black) and average FSL (grey) at 70 dB SPL (data collected in steps of 1 kHz). Breaks in the frequency function were due to corrupted results at 15 and 19 kHz. D: Frequency contour plots showing changes in MP, as a colour gradient, with respect to frequency (incremented in 1 kHz steps) and time from the contralateral (top) and ipsilateral (bottom) stimulus (50 ms, 70 dB SPL) onset. The change in MP is indicated by the colour spectrum in the key; RMP is shown as green, whilst red indicates more positive MPs and blue indicates more negative MPs. The range of depolarisation shown does not encompass the entire AP height (range from RMP to maximum depolarisation / hyperpolarisation shown = +/- 4 mV). Grey area in bottom panel represents data not collected. E: 10 trace overlay (grey) and average response (black) to various ipsilaterally presented off-CF tones at 80 dB SPL. Horizontal dashed line is RMP and dark horizontal bar indicates stimulus duration.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

49

Figure 9 and Figure 10 give examples of the sort of intracellular electrophysiological data recorded by Nayagam et al. (2006). The examples given are from a transient chopper neuron in response to CF (Figure 9A,B) and off-CF (Figure 9C) tones as well as averaged noise and tones (Figure 9D). This particular transient chopper showed an increased firing rate and decreased FSL with increasing SPL; however was sightly non-monotonic at high SPLs (Figure 9A). Off-CF, the onset component of this cell's response was dominant and broadband, whilst the ongoing depolarisation was limited to around CF (Figure 9C, Figure 10D). In response to 80dB noise, this cell showed an EI response (Figure 9D, bottom traces). E0 (n = 2) and EE (n = 2) responses to noise were also seen in transient chopper units (binaural responses in the VCLL are discussed below). Ipsilaterally, strong inhibition was seen at stimulus onset that gradually decayed throughout the 80 dB noise or tone stimulus. There was no evidence of mixed excitation with inhibition during the ipsilateral response (Figure 9D, bottom traces, Figure 10E). The bandwidth of the ipsilaterally-evoked inhibition was greater than the contralaterally-evoked and sustained excitation, and the latency of the inhibition was longer with that of the excitation (6.3 ms ipsilateral inhibition vs. 5.6 ms contralateral excitation). Supra-threshold contour plots show the average ipsilateral response plotted across the same frequency range as the contralateral excitation (Figure 10D). In the contralateral response plot, more negative contours at 15 kHz and 19 kHz are caused by an artefact and are not indicative of membrane hyperpolarisation, unlike the true hyperpolarisation seen in Figure 10D, top panel at 32 – 34 kHz. When contralateral and ipsilateral tones were combined, the resultant firing rate was suppressed. Total suppression of firing occurred when ipsilateral CF tones were ~20 dB higher than contralateral CF tones (Figure 10B). This cell was iontophoretically filled with Neurobiotin and a digital reconstruction of the cell showing its dendritic morphology and location in relation to the VCLL and the lateral lemniscus tract is presented in Figure 9E. This cell had a small number of large dendrites that travel ~0.2 – 1 mm away from the soma before branching sparsely, consistent with the stellate morphology described by Zhao and Wu (2001). There was a tendency for the dendrites to be aligned with the lateral lemniscus fibre tract. Reconstructions of recorded cells may be used to correlate the physiological responses with the cell's morphology (discussed in more detail below). Data from other the 5 intracellular response types were presented in a similar fashion, for more details refer to Nayagam et al. (2006). Mean FSL to 80 dB noise (Figure 11, white box plots) and CF tones (Figure 11, filled box plots) was calculated for each of Nayagam et al.'s (2006) 6 intracellular CF response types .All neurons had FSLs between 3.3 and 31 ms and in all cases the mean CF tone latency was less than the mean noise latency (Figure 11). Onset ideal neurons showed the shortest latency responses and the distribution of their FSLs was narrow. Onset ideal neurons had similar mean FSL for noise and tones (noise: 4.4 ms, tones: 3.9 ms). Onset chopper neurons also had similar latencies for noise and tones (noise: 6 ms, tones: 5.9 ms). Onset-A cells showed the longest mean latency and had a large spread of FSLs (9.6 ms) particularly in response to noise. Transient chopper neurons were the only type with a very wide distribution (27.3 ms). All electrophysiological studies have described a heterogeneous neural population that responds to noise and tones with a variety of response patterns. The major classes of response are similar to those described in other auditory structures, such as onset, sustained and chopping. In studies that investigated binaural responses, the VCLL was consistently found to contain a range of cells that responded with a complex range of binaural interactions. More

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

50

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

recent investigations, have also found evidence of inhibitory influence on spike timing within the VCLL, and this will be discuss further below.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Powerful, Fast Inhibition before First Action Potential in the VCLL Nayagam et al. (2005) proposed a novel mechanism to explain their observation of powerful and fast inhibition acting on some of the cells in the ventral VCLL (VNLL). In order to understand this proposed mechanism, it is important to recall three key findings. Firstly, the specialised calyx-like endings in the ventral VCLL (VNLL) are thought to play an important role in understanding the function of this nucleus. The neurons receiving calyx-like endings receive more secure input (various species: Morest, 1968) and respond to acoustic transients across a broader frequency bandwidth (various species: Oertel et al., 2000; Oertel and Wickesberg, 2002) with shorter latency than the other neurons of the VNLL (cat: Pfeiffer, 1966; bat: Covey and Casseday, 1986). Secondly, the ventral VCLL (VNLL) is known to contain cells that are immunoreactive for glycine (Saint Marie and Baker, 1990; Saint Marie et al., 1997, Irfan et al., 2005), and is thus considered to be a major source of inhibition to the IC. VCLL cells receive input from all major cell types of the CN via ladder-rung like collaterals from the ascending lemniscal pathway to the IC. Particularly important is a projection from the OCA of the PVCN to bushy cells of the ventral VCLL. This thick-axon projection terminates in large, secure, calyx-like synapses capable of conveying the precisely timed and broadly tuned excitatory onset ideal responses of octopus cells to the inhibitory VCLL target cells. Thirdly, within the VCLL are intrinsic connections, which have been described as collaterals of ascending axons from VCLL cells to the IC (Wu, 1997; Zhao and Wu, 2001). The intracellular observation of broadly tuned, powerful, pre-spike onset inhibition from onset and sustained cells of the VNLL (Figure 12 A,B,C) suggested the involvement of the OCA and the thick axon projection to the VCLL (Nayagam et al. 2005). However, the octopus cell projection to the VNLL is excitatory, not inhibitory, so it cannot be the source of this fast inhibition. Nayagam et al. (2005) proposed that the fast inhibition was from another VNLL neuron that was itself activated by octopus cell input. This possibility was consistent with previous descriptions of local circuits within the VCLL. Examination of response latencies (Figure 12, D,E) verified that a cell group existed within the VNLL that met the requirements of the proposed local circuit: a cell group that responded to noise and tones with an ideal onset response (Figure 8). These onset ideal cells responded only to contralateral stimulation and were found only in the ventral part of the VCLL (VNLL). These characteristics were consistent with octopus cell input (Figure 13 a,a1). It was found that fast onset inhibition delayed the first AP of the cell from which it was recorded (Figure 13 b1). A downstream effect of a delayed population of inhibitory VCLL onset and sustained responses (Figure 13 b2,b3) would be expected in the IC. IC cells receiving sustained excitation from the CN, combined with delayed onset inhibition or sustained inhibition from the VCLL would be expected to show pauser and onset responses respectively (Figure 13 c1,c2), both of which have been observed (Nayagam et al. 2005).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

51

- Nayagam et al. (2006)

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 11. Intracellular first spike latency distribution of VCLL response types Box plots showing mean (vertical dotted lines), median (vertical solid line), 25th percentile (left box border) and 75th percentile (right box border) FSL for the 6 VCLL response types. FSL to 80 dB RMS noise (unshaded boxes) and 80 dB SPL CF tones (shaded boxes) are shown. Crosses indicate FSLs for two responses; due to the small sample size box plot not shown. Asterisk indicates the onset ideal sample included 8 units that were classified according to their noise response. Whiskers shown for onset ideal noise sample indicate 10th and 90th percentile.

Furthermore, as the local fast inhibitory circuit in the VCLL is likely to be a collateral of an ascending projection to the IC, fast pre-spike inhibition would also be expected in the IC target of this projection (Figure 13 d1); this has indeed been observed (or predicted) in several studies (Adams, 1997; Carney and Yin, 1989; Kuwada et al., 1997; Nayagam et al., 2005; Smith et al., 1993)

Correlation between Physiology and Morphology of Neurons in the VCLL The most detailed attempt to correlate firing patterns with morphology in nonecholocating species was the rat study by Zhao and Wu (2001). As detailed above, they found two distinct morphological types, bushy and stellate. Stellate cells were subdivided into three types (I, II or elongate) based on their somatic and dendritic morphologies. Zhao and Wu (2001) found that bushy cells tended to have onset firing patterns and non-linear currentvoltage relationships. Stellate I cells showed regular or onset-pause responses, whereas stellate II had adapting responses and elongate cells displayed burst patterns. shows the in vitro intracellular responses to current injection recorded by whole cell patch clamp in rat VCLL neurons correlated, where possible, with the cell‘s morphological type (refer Figure 3).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

52

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

- Modified from Nayagam et al. (2005): with kind permission from The American Physiological Society.

Figure 12. Onset inhibition in VNLL neurons. A: In vivo intracellular responses recorded from a VNLL neuron to contralaterally presented noise bursts (80 dB RMS; 20 trace overlay, grey) and a CF tone burst (80 dB SPL; single trace, red). Horizontal dashed line indicates the mean RMP (-47 ± 4 mV). The arrow indicates membrane hyperpolarisation and the horizontal bar indicates stimulus duration. B: Single traces to off-CF tone bursts. Horizontal bar indicates time scale from stimulus onset. C: Spontaneous IPSPs were also observed in this cell (black trace) and are compared to the onset component of a single noise response (grey trace). Asterisks indicate truncated APs. D: Distribution of latencies of first AP (black bars) and maximum hyperpolarisation (white bars) for cells with onset inhibition. E: FSL distribution of VNLL OI neurons. Vertical dashed lines indicate the average of each sample. Bin widths are 0.25 ms.

Bushy cells in the VNLL with their onset firing patterns and non-linear current-voltage relationship have been compared to VCN bushy cells (rat: Zhao and Wu, 2001; mouse: Wu and Oertel, 1984) and to principal cells in the MNTB (rat: Banks and Smith, 1992; Zhao and Wu, 2001; Wu and Kelly, 1991). These cells all share lower membrane input resistance when the cell is excited and non-linear current-voltage relationships. It has been proposed that these properties are useful for maintaining and conveying the precise timing of synaptic inputs. Lower membrane resistance makes for faster repolarisation, shortening the excitation and preventing temporal summations, hence allowing the cells to reflect precisely the firing patterns of their afferents. These properties are invaluable for effective encoding of temporal information (Oertel, 1997; Zhao and Wu, 2001; Oertel, 1983, 1991). Bushy cells in the VNLL also resemble bushy cells in the VCN and principal cells in the MNTB morphologically with

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

53

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

round to oval somas and profuse dendritic trees in spatially restricted areas (Zhao and Wu, 2001). Bushy cells in the rat VNLL closely resemble bushy cells found in the big brown bat (Covey and Casseday, 1991). The onset pattern produced by these cells also matches the onset responses found in VNLL cells of cat (Aitkin et al., 1970a; Guinan et al., 1972b; Adams, 1997), rabbit (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999) and rat (Wu, 1999). The small, oval (putative bushy) cells located ventrally in the VCLL are thought to be the recipients of a calyx-like synapse and the targets of a thick axon projection from the OCA of the contralateral PVCN (guinea pig: Schofield and Cant, 1997).

- Modified from Nayagam et al. (2005): with kind permission from The American Physiological Society.

Figure 13. Pathways mediating fast inhibition within the VNLL and IC. a: VNLL neuron receiving input from the OCA via a thick axon projection ending in a calyx. a1: Intracellular response of a VNLL OI cell. b: VNLL neuron receiving onset inhibition via a collateral (red) of an ascending axon. b1: VNLL cell with intermittent onset inhibition. Red trace is a response when inhibition was present and the black trace is a response when it was absent. Inset: shows the average response (first 15 ms from stimulus onset) with (n=3) and without (n=7) inhibition. b2, b3: Overlayed traces (n=20) of onset inhibition in two VNLL cells showing a sustained and onset response, respectively. c: An IC neuron receiving delayed inhibition from a VNLL cell with onset inhibition. c1: An IC neuron that receives delayed and sustained inhibition from a VNLL cell (as in b2) would be expected to exhibit a phasic response. c2: An IC neuron that receives delayed and onset inhibition from a VNLL cell (as in b3) would be expected to exhibit a pauser response. Inset shows the average response (30 stimulus presentations; first 50 ms from stimulus onset). Pause in AP firing is between 11.7 and 19.4 ms. d: An IC neuron receiving input from a VNLL OI cell (as in a1) would be expected to show onset inhibition (d1) similar to that observed within the VNLL. Horizontal dashed lines indicate RMPs that were -29, -52, -47, -43, -56, -52 and -26 mV for traces shown in a1, b1-b3, c1, c2 and d1, respectively. Traces shown in a1, b1, c1, c2, and d1 (excluding insets) are responses to a single stimulus presentation. In all cases, stimuli were 80 dB noise bursts. Inhibitory projections are shaded and excitatory projections are unshaded. Arrows indicate membrane hyperpolarisation. All traces correspond with neurons in the circuit diagram with the equivalent letter.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

54

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

- Modified from Zhao and Wu (2001): with kind permission from John Wiley and Sons, Inc.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 14. Correlation of in vitro intracellular responses to current injection with cellular morphology. Responses to intracellular injections of depolarising current of cells in the VCLL of the rat. Three current levels are presented for the adapting neuron. Correlation of response type with neuronal morphology is indicated where possible. The proportion of the VCLL sample made up by each cell type is shown as a percentage. The resting membrane potentials (RMPs) of onset, regular, adapting, bursting, and onset-pause were -57.5 ± 1.3 mV, -56.5 ± 1.9 mV, -58.9 ± 2.1 mV, -59,6 ± 1.3 mV, and -55.0 ± 1.3 mV, respectively.

Regular and onset-pause firing patterns were observed in stellate I cells by Zhao and Wu (2001). In other nuclei, regular cells and the onset-pause cells have different ion conductances on the cell membrane (rat: Kanold and Manis, 1999). However, these regular and onset-pause cells do share some similarities, including the timing of the first spike, as well as the frequency of responses to positive current injection (Zhao and Wu, 2001). These findings of Zhao and Wu (2001) are supported by in vivo studies in other species. Most VNLL neurons in bat (Metzner and Radtke-Schuller, 1987; Covey and Casseday, 1991), cat (Guinan et al., 1972b; Guinan et al., 1972a) and rabbit (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999) responded to tone bursts with sustained, regular (chopper) discharge patterns. This corresponds nicely with the regular stellate I cells in the rat. A pause in the response was also reported in bat (Metzner and Radtke-Schuller, 1987; Covey and Casseday, 1991), however, whether it is a direct result of the membrane properties identified in the rat, or as a result of other in vivo effects, needs further study (Zhao and Wu, 2001). Stellate II cells were characterised by linear current-voltage relationships and adapting firing patterns (Wu, 1999; Zhao and Wu, 2001). Although they showed sustained firing throughout the duration of the stimulus (like stellate I cells) they did have slight, but definite, differences when compared with the stellate I cells. In response to lower intensity current, stellate I cells fired APs with obvious adaptation. The first spike was often delayed from the onset of stimulus. However, the authors speculate that stellate II neurons encode intensity rather than timing, because the discharge rate was proportional to the strength of the stimulus.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

55

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

These cells have wide dendritic trees (see above) and it was speculated that these arbours may be used to integrate information across a broader range of ascending lemniscal fibres than stellate I cells (Zhao and Wu, 2001).

- Modified from Nayagam et al. (2006)

Figure 15. Morphology and intracellular responses of onset-A and onset-B neurons. A,C: Overlay of 10 traces (grey) and average response (black) to contralaterally presented 80 dB SPL CF tones (top) and contralaterally and ipsilaterally presented 80 dB RMS noise (bottom; contralateral and ipsilateral stimuli offset by 100 ms). Horizontal dashed lines show RMP and thick horizontal bars show stimulus duration. B,D: Morphology of recorded onset-A and onset-B (respectively) neurons revealed by Neurobiotin labelling. The presumed axons are indicated by ‗a‘. Discontinuities within a given process have been indicated by dashed lines. Insets show neurons' location (black dot) within the VCLL.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

56

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The elongate cells in the VNLL of rats were not found in the in vitro intracellular study by Wu (1999) but were identified by Zhao and Wu (2001). Zhao and Wu (2001) postulated that a clue to their function may lie with the cartwheel cells of the DCN first encountered by Hirsch and Oertel (mouse; 1988) and subsequently identified by Zhang and Oertel (mouse; 1993), as well as by Manis et al. (guinea pig: 1994). Cartwheel cells generate a slow wave of Ca2+ dependant depolarisation, on which bursts of Na+ APs ride (Zhao and Wu, 2001). These Na+ and Ca2+ dependant burst responses are also found in the auditory thalamus (various species: Steriade et al., 1993; rat: Tennigkeit et al., 1996; Tennigkeit et al., 1998; guinea pig: Jahnsen and Llinas, 1984). Zhao and Wu (2001) suggested that similar dual ion channels may mediate the burst response in the elongate cells of the VNLL. Whether these channels are indeed sodium and calcium is yet to be determined. This type of response is consistent with the transient phasic neurons reported in rabbit (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999), which responded with bursts of four or more non-chopping APs. The dendritic arbours of this type of unit were orthogonal to the lemniscal pathway implying a possible basis for integration across a wide range of inputs (Zhao and Wu, 2001). In the rat, Nayagam et al. (2006) correlated three physiological response types with cellular morphology by combining intracellular in vivo recordings with Neurobiotin injections. They classified Neurobioton-filled cells using the cytoarchitectural criteria established by Zhao and Wu (2001). Two of the three cells filled, a stellate II / transient chopper and an onset-A / bushy cell, correlated with the findings from Zhao and Wu (2001). However, the third cell (an onset-B / stellate I) did not correlate with the regular response found by Zhao and Wu (2001; Figure 9, Figure 15). It is possible that the regular sustained firing to current injection in vitro is mitigated by additional neural inhibition in vivo. The bushy cell described by Nayagam et al. (2006) did not appear to receive calyx-like synaptic connections based on its intracellular response profile, therefore it appears that only a subset of bushy cells within the VCLL are the recipients of the thick-axon, calyceal terminating projection from the OCA.

Binaural Responses of VCLL Neurons In vivo electrophysiological studies of the DNLL have been conducted in the cat (Aitkin et al., 1970a; Brugge et al., 1970a, b) and echolocating bat (Covey and Casseday, 1995; Metzner and Radtke-Schuller, 1987; Covey, 1993a; Markovitz and Pollak, 1993, 1994; Yang and Pollak, 1994a, b; Yang et al., 1996) and these studies agree that the DNLL is a binaural nucleus. In contrast, the VCLL has been considered to be a monaural structure because its inputs originate primarily from the contralateral CN (cat: Aitkin et al., 1970a; Glendenning et al., 1981; bat: Covey and Casseday, 1991). However, there have been relatively few studies that have recorded VCLL neural responses to bilaterally presented sounds to confirm this assertion. Although all studies tend to agree that most VCLL cells are excited by stimulation of the contralateral ear, some studies have shown that a proportion of VCLL cells are responsive to input from the ipsilateral ear (rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997; cat: Aitkin et al., 1970a; Guinan et al., 1972b; rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999; rat: Nayagam et al., 2006). Binaural responsiveness is assessed by whether there are responses to stimulation of both or either ear and sensitivity to interaural disparities are typically assayed by introducing ILDs or ITDs. In the big brown bat, Covey and Casseday (1991) found no evidence for

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

57

binaural responses in the VNLLc and VNLLm nuclei. However, they found a small number of binaural cells in the INLL but not enough to exclude the INLL from being included in the ‗monaural‘ complex. There have been reports of both forms (ITD and ILD) of binaural sensitivity in the VCLL of non-echolocating mammals. However, there are significant differences between mammalian species with respect to the existence and the extent of binaural units within the VCLL (Zhang and Kelly, 2006a). It is possible that there is a distinct population of neurons in the medial VNLL (VNLLm) of rabbits that is sensitive to ITDs (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997), supporting the anatomical evidence that the medial part of the VCLL is specialised for binaural processing (Glendenning et al., 1981; Henkel and Spangler, 1983)3. Zhang and Kelly (2006a) have recently reported widely distributed cells responsive to ILDs throughout the VCLL of the rat. There are a handful of in vivo electrophysiological studies in the VNLL of nonecholocating animals that have identified binaural responses (rat: Zhang and Kelly, 2006a; Nayagam et al., 2006; rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997; Batra and Fitzpatrick, 2002a, b; cat: Aitkin et al., 1970a; Guinan et al., 1972b; Guinan et al., 1972a; rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999). Batra and Fitzpatrick (1997) found 21 neurons that were sensitive to ITDs of low frequency tones (less than 2kHz), although some had CFs of up to 5.5kHz. The locations of the neurons were reconstructed and found to lie in the medial portion of the VCLL of rabbits or in the adjacent reticular formation. Five of the neurons were visualised with tracer dye and found to be within the low cell density, medial part of the VNLL. Most of the neurons responded to the onset of binaural stimulation at the most favourable ITD. The responses would vary cyclically with a period equal to that of the tone when the ipsilateral stimuli was progressively delayed or advanced with respect to the contralateral stimuli. Most of the neurons studied did not produce onset responses to ipsilateral stimulation and did not follow dynamic changes in ITD produced by a binaural-beat stimulus. However, there were some (6/14) neurons that did respond weakly to an ipsilateral stimulus and about a third of the neurons responded (although sometimes poorly) to dynamic changes in ITD (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997).The difference between the binaural responses from their study and those found in the SOC suggested that the ITD sensitivity recorded in the VCLL originated from a unique population of the SOC or there was a substantial transformation in the VCLL. It is unlikely that the VCLL ITD sensitivity is a direct result of converging phase-locked inputs (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997). Batra and Fitzpatrick (1997) found that many neurons were influenced by ipsilateral, as well as contralateral stimulation. The two major types of binaural interactions were ILD of the type E0/I, and sensitivity to ITDs. E0/I type responses were exhibited by both onset and sustained type neurons, whereas ITD sensitivity was mostly limited to onset cells. Binaural cells were located throughout the VCLL, but there appeared to be a preference for cells receiving inhibition, and ITD sensitive neurons, to be located medially and around the boundary of the nucleus. Monaural and transient neurons were more likely found in ventral VNLL (referred to as VNLL1). Batra and Fitzpatrick (1999) described many VCLL E0/I neurons that were unresponsive to ITDs. At least some of these cells were located in the marginal ventral VNLL (VNLL1). This supports findings of Aitkin et al. (1970a) who found 3

Glendenning et al. (1981) refer to this region as the lateral tegmentum, whilst Schofield and Cant (1997) refer to it as the VNLLa. It is not the same as the multipolar region of the VNLL in bats or the middle VNLL used by Adams (1979) (see above).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

58

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

that binaural neurons appeared to be present only at the margins of the nucleus. The idea that the binaural and monaural neurons may be at least partially segregated provides some reconciliation between different studies. Aitkin et al. (cat: 1970a) found few (11%) binaural neurons in the cat VCLL, whereas Guinan et al. (cat: 1972b) found over 60%. It is possible that the two studies sampled different populations, and therefore arrived at different estimates of the number of binaural neurons (Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1999). In the rat, there has been two published studies that have investigated responsiveness to binaural stimulation (Zhang and Kelly, 2006a; Nayagam et al., 2006). In the Zhang and Kelly study, a substantial proportion (87%) of tested units responded with some form of binaural ILD sensitivity. The majority of interactions (59%) were excited by contralateral stimulation and inhibited by ipsilateral stimulation. Binaural cells were found distributed throughout the dorsoventral extent of the VCLL. However, Zhang and Kelly (2006a) did not examine the spatial distribution of binaural types within the structure, but rather the relationship between the dorsoventral position and the strength of the binaural suppression. Nayagam et al (2006) found that the VCLL exhibited a diverse and complex suite of binaural and inhibitory inputs. Figure 16A,B,C presents example intracellular responses showing E0, EI and EE types. Their intracellular data show that a number of cells that would appear as E0 extracellularly actually received some concurrent contralateral inhibition, either notch, short or long duration. This inhibition, observed as IPSPs, is only visible in intracellular recordings and had not been reported previously. EE units tested in their study showed evidence of complex EE/EI interactions at different ILDs. This finding is consistent with results of previous extracellular studies (Zhang and Kelly, 2006a). However, due to the complex nature of binaural excitatory and inhibitory interaction, Nayagam et al‘s (2006) classifications were simplified. They reported binaural type for 137 intracellularly recorded VCLL neurons in response to 80 dB SPL bilateral noise bursts (separated by 100 ms). All binaural types (E0, EI, EE, I0, II, IE, 0E) were found within the VCLL. The proportion of each type is shown in Figure 16D. Almost all units (97%) responded to contralateral stimulation, mostly with excitation (83%). Contralaterally inhibited neurons, both monaural and binaural were located throughout the structure. However, of the contralaterally excited cells, the EI units were preferentially distributed within the INLL, while E0 and EE units were common throughout the VNLL and INLL. Roughly half of VCLL units tested (55%) received monaural inputs and the other half (45%) received binaural inputs. Within the VNLL, two thirds of units (67%) were monaural, while in the INLL the proportions were reversed with one third (35%) monaural (Figure 16E). The spatial distribution of the three forms of contralaterally excited responses (E0, EI and EE) is shown in 3D (Figure 17B) and collapsed into three coronal sections (Figure 17A). The various binaural interactions observed in VCLL neurons, as well as the variable response types to noise and tones, suggest convergence of excitatory and inhibitory inputs across a wide range of frequencies onto VCLL cells.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

59

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

FUNCTIONAL ROLE OF THE VCLL IN AUDITORY PROCESSING Many possible functional roles have been suggested for the VCLL, however, the exact function of the VCLL remains poorly understood. Contemporary studies have suggested that the VCLL is a crucial integrative nucleus in the auditory brainstem, rather than a simple relay station (rat: Wu, 1999), and is a major source of inhibition to the IC (rat: Zhang et al., 1998; cat: Saint Marie and Baker, 1990). Several roles in auditory processing (various species: Oertel and Wickesberg, 2002; bat: Ross et al., 1988) have been proposed for the VCLL in various species, including a role in pitch perception (cat: Malmierca et al., 1998), temporal processing (bat: Covey and Casseday, 1991), pattern recognition (various species: Oertel and Wickesberg, 2002), a filter of time-variant signals (bat: Covey and Casseday, 1986) or multiple functions (cat: Glendenning and Hutson, 1998). The observed responses in the VNLL of echolocating (Covey and Casseday, 1991) and non-echolocating species (Aitkin et al., 1970a; Adams, 1997; Guinan et al., 1972a) have been mainly onset, although the recent Zhang and Kelly (rat; 2006a) and Nayagam et al. (rat; 2006) studies found an increased population of sustained neurons. Batra and Fitzpatrick‘s (rabbit; 1997) study also described neurons with onset responses in the VNLL that may be sensitive to ITDs. These findings led to the conclusion that the VNLL may be involved in the localisation of acoustic transients (rabbit: Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997). This certainly seems likely in the echolocating bat, where the specialised needs of echolocation have led to an anatomically and physiologically specialised auditory pathway. The lemniscal nuclei of bats are significantly different to the VCLL of non-echolocating species. The lemniscal nuclei of bats are divided into monaural and binaural regions and contain highly segregated cell types that perform different roles in sound processing. Covey and Casseday (bat; 1986) suggested a possible frequency-to-time converter role for the NLL. They viewed the entire network from the AVCN, through the various VCLL nuclei to the IC, as a means to provide the IC with a set of delays of the AVCN output (Covey and Casseday, 1986). In 1991, Covey and Casseday described a monaural subdivision of the VCLL, termed VNLLc, which contained cells capable of responding very accurately to the onset of contralateral stimulation with a constant latency over a wide range of frequencies and intensities. These cells were not found in the rest of the complex, leading to the conclusion that the VNLLc was suited to precisely encode timing, particularly with respect to onsets and acoustic transients. The rest of the structure contained cells with sustained or chopper responses and thus it was concluded that these other areas were responsible for coding the ongoing properties of a sound. Three transformations occur within the brainstem of echolocating species that have a bearing on the processing of time-distributed information. These are: 1) a conversion of excitatory to inhibitory signals, 2) changes in the temporal response pattern, often from sustained to onset, and 3) a large increase in the range of response latencies. These transformations occur multiple times within segregated processing streams of the bat brainstem and create increasingly complex and diverse response properties at each successive level before ultimately converging at the IC (Covey and Casseday, 1999; Covey, 2003).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

60

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

- Nayagam et al. (2006)

Figure 16. Intracellular binaural responses types to noise in the VCLL. A-C: In vivo intracellular recordings from VCLL neurons to contralateral and ipsilateral 50 ms, 80 dB RMS noise bursts (10 trace overlay shown in grey; 10 trace average shown in black). All traces shown are from VCLL neurons that were excited by contralateral stimulation. Horizontal dashed lines indicate RMP and dark horizontal bars indicate duration of contralateral (left) and ipsilateral (right) stimuli that were offset by 100 ms. A: Examples of responses from cells that were not influenced by ipsilateral noise stimulation (E0), showing sustained (top) and onset (bottom) firing patterns. B: Examples of responses that were inhibited by ipsilateral stimulation (EI) with inhibitory post-synaptic potentials (IPSPs) of varying duration and temporal summation properties (top traces, medium duration; middle traces, long duration; bottom traces, short duration). Different contralateral responses were observed for EI units, including onset (top), sustained (middle) and chopper (bottom) patterns. C: Examples of various responses that were excited by ipsilateral stimulation (EE). Ipsilateral excitation was transient (top traces), continuous (middle traces) or delayed (bottom trace). EE units showed a variety of contralateral response patterns including onset (top two traces), delayed (third trace) and chopper (bottom trace). Asterisk shows excitatory post-synaptic potentials (EPSPs) in the absence of action potentials (APs). Arrows show APs generated during underlying long-duration inhibition. D: Distribution of VCLL units across 8 binaural categories showing differences in binaural type distribution between the ventral and intermediate nuclei of the lateral lemniscus (VNLL and INLL respectively). E: Proportion of monaural (E0, I0, 0I and 0E) and binaural (EI, EE, II, IE) units in the VCLL, VNLL and INLL.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

61

- Modified from Nayagam et al. (2006)

Figure 17. Spatial distribution of intracellular unit‘s binaural type within the VCLL Only units that were excited by contralateral stimulation (E0, EI and EE; key provided bottom of panel A) are shown. Format the same as Figure 4. The viewing aspects of the 3D reconstructions in (B) are rostral (left), oblique between rostral and lateral (middle) and lateral (right). Each orthogonal orientation arrow is 500 m, although the central panel‘s rostral and lateral orientation axes are 2D representations of 3D objects that are projecting ―out of the page‖ at 45 degrees to the page surface and therefore appear shorter than the other axes.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

62

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Bat studies (Covey and Casseday, 1999; Covey, 2003) provide neural models which show that convergence of these three separate processing streams creates the potential for IC neurons to filter temporal features of sound including tuning for duration, direction of frequency sweeps, modulation rate, and interstimulus intervals (gap detection). The ability to discriminate these temporal features of sound is a critical part of echolocation. However, these features are not exclusive to the realm of navigating caves or hunting insects. Processing animal vocalisations in non-echolocating species, or comprehending human speech requires the ability to discriminate the same temporal features mentioned above. Findings from studies in non-echolocating species, suggest that despite the differences in the setup of the lemniscal nuclei, the basic ingredients for performing the three transformations in the brainstem are preserved. Furthermore, the three transformations may all occur within the VCLL: 1) The VNLL receives excitatory input from the CN and sends inhibitory projections to the IC. 2) Response patterns are altered in the VCLL, via either membrane properties of VCLL neurons or convergence of inhibition and excitation. Indeed, several classes of onset responses within the VCLL were described by Nayagam et al. (2006) that, with the exception onset ideal cells, showed differing responses to noise versus tones. 3) Fast, pre-spike inhibition has been shown to actively delay the output of a subset of VCLL and IC responses which will contribute to the increase in the range of response latencies of IC neurons. The VCLL contains a heterogenous neural population and it is possible that these three transformations occur in segregated processing streams within the structure that converge at the IC. These functions proposed in echolocating species do not require a tonotopic arrangement of the VCLL, in line with the results from Nayagam et al. (rat; 2006). However, they also do not require binaurally responsive neurons. Several recent studies in non-echolocating species (Nayagam et al., 2006; Zhang and Kelly, 2006a; Batra and Fitzpatrick, 1997; Guinan et al., 1972b) have reported binaurally responsive neurons within the VCLL and thus it is likely that, at least in these species, the VCLL performs additional functional processing. A recent study by Langner et al. (gerbil; 2006) found that the transfer functions of neurons in the IC changed from comb filters at sound onset into band-pass filters after the initial phase. They postulated that the inhibition (precisely synchronised to the signal periodicity) that is responsible for this mechanism was from the VCLL. Furthermore, these IC neurons were tuned to modulation frequencies and arranged in periodicity maps. There is significant feedback from the IC, through the VCLL (rat: Caicedo and Herbert, 1993; guinea pig: Malmierca et al., 1996), to the SOC and CN (cat: Whitley and Henkel, 1984). Thus, it was expected that the VCLL would show some evidence for the periodotopy found in the IC. Using 2-DG activation as well as retrograde tracing of BDA from the IC, they found evidence for a helical periodicity map with 7 to 8 turns (compatible with the findings of Merchán and Berbel, 1996) of a low to high, dorsal to ventral pitch representation within the VCLL. However, this finding has not been validated by electrophysiological studies. Based predominantly on tracing studies performed by Merchán and Berbel (1996) in the rat as well as new tracing studies in the cat, Merchán et al. (1997) proposed a processing rationale for the VCLL that is in line with the general organisational principal of the auditory system: i.e. an orderly spatial representation of tonal frequency. They suggested that the VCLL processes information on a frequency-frame basis, preserving the cochlea‘s spectral decomposition of the input signal. However, Nayagam et al. (2006), using 3D reconstructions of intracellular response locations, found no evidence to support an orderly tonotopic

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

63

distribution within the VCLL. The apparent lack of tonotopicity within the VCLL described by the Nayagam et al. (2006) and other studies (Glendenning, 1981; Guinan et al., 1972a,b; Kelly et al., 1998a; Zhang and Kelly, 2006a) suggests a role that is removed from direct processing of frequency. If these neurons are involved in frequency processing, then it is possibly through an indirect means such as influencing sideband inhibition in IC neurons. There is a crucial difference, which has been largely overlooked between tonotopicity and topography. A nucleus may send or receive topographically ordered projections with respect to the tonotopicity of the target or source, but this does not lead inexorably to a tonotopic arrangement within that nucleus. Batra and Fitzpatrick (1997) described a population of neurons in the medial VCLL of the rabbit that was sensitive to ITDs. These neurons responded with onset responses and obviously had low CFs. They were located in a region that had previously been described in the cat as the lateral tegmentum (Glendenning et al., 1981), however Batra and Fitzpatrick believed that the ITD sensitive cells in their study lay within the boundaries of the lateral lemniscal tract in the rabbit. The authors claimed that the ITD sensitivity of these neurons was a feature that was not created by bilateral convergence of phase-locked inputs but rather transmitted from the SOC. The VCLL has already been shown to contain a large proportion of onset responses (Aitkin et al., 1970a; Guinan et al., 1972b; Adams, 1997; Covey and Casseday, 1991; Guinan et al., 1972a) and has been implicated to play a role in processing acoustic transients (Covey and Casseday, 1991). Batra and Fitzpatrick concluded that these features combined with ITD sensitivity, which provides insight into the location of a stimulus along the azimuth, suggests that the nucleus plays a role in localising acoustic transients. The recent electrophysiological investigation of the VCLL by Nayagam et al. (2006) did not test low CF neurons for ITD sensitivity. Typically, if a neuron were creating ITDs through bilateral convergence of phase-locked inputs then it would be affected by monaural contralateral as well as monaural ipsilateral stimulation. Even if not driven to discharge, synaptic inputs following monaural contralateral and ipsilateral stimulation would alter the membrane potential and this would be visible in intracellular traces. However, in the Batra and Fitzpatrick (1997) study, ITD sensitivity was not generated within the VCLL but rather transmitted from the SOC. In this situation, ITD sensitive neurons cannot be identified based on their synaptic inputs following monaural contralateral and ipsilateral stimulation. However, reconstructions of the location of neuron‘s CFs within the VCLL, by Nayagam et al. (2006), provided no evidence for an increased distribution of low CF cells in the medial part of the structure. This finding suggests that in the rat VCLL, ITD sensitive neurons, if they exist, are not localised to a medial portion of the nucleus. This does not rule out the possibility of ITD sensitive cells in the rat VCLL performing a role in localising acoustic transients as proposed in the rabbit. It is likely that species specific differences in hearing ranges and parcellation may underlie the different functional observation between studies. Oertel and Wickesberg (2002), by a process of elimination, speculated that the VCLL is involved in processing the transient and temporal features of auditory stimulus. In reaching this working hypothesis, the authors considered the ubiquitous presence of the VNLL across all mammalian species as well as reptiles and birds. This suggests that this nucleus plays a fundamental role in localising or recognising sound. Given that the VCLL had mainly been described as a monaural structure, the authors focused on the recognition and interpretation of the temporal patterns within natural and complex sounds (Oertel and Wickesberg, 2002), as this is a function that depends on monaural rather than binaural cues. One of the temporal

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

64

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

patterns that the VCLL may be well suited to encode, is the detection of the interval between acoustic transients. This is useful for encoding periodic sounds, such as low-frequency tones, harmonic complexes, and click trains, which are modelled by repeated acoustic transients. These signals produce coincident firing of a wide frequency range of auditory nerve fibres. Octopus cells have biophysical specialisations which allow them to detect and sum this coincident firing, and transmit the periodic signal to the VCLL (Golding et al., 1995; Oertel et al., 1990; Golding et al., 1999). This interpretation is in line with the current descriptions of the VCLL and the prominent projection it receives from octopus cells that are well suited to convey the accurate timing of onset and pattern information. The intrinsic circuitry of the VCLL proposed by Nayagam et al. (2005) suggests that the cells receiving octopus cell input send fast onset inhibition, via local collaterals, to other cells in the VCLL in order to delay the VCLL‘s inhibitory output to the IC. How this fits with the role that the octopus cell projection may play in the detection of acoustic transients is unclear. The fact that the pathway terminates on the bushy cells of the VCLL suggests that the signals encoding acoustic transients are converted to play another role. It is possible that the fast inhibitory local circuit is one of many parallel processing channels stemming from the octopus cell projection to the VCLL. However, the proportion of VCLL cells described by Nayagam et al. (2005) that received fast onset inhibition (14.3%) was roughly equivalent to the proportion of cells with OI responses (proposed octopus cell targets; 17.9%), suggesting a one to one relationship. If each octopus cell activates a fast inhibitory circuit, then it is unlikely that parallel processing streams performing other functions stem from the cells receiving octopus cell input in the ventral VCLL. Oertel and Wickesberg (2002) ruled out a role for the VCLL in localising sound in the horizontal plane based on the fact that many older studies had shown the lemniscal nuclei to be predominantly monaural structures. However, given data from Nayagam et al. (2006) and other recent studies (Zhang and Kelly, 2006a; Batra and Fitzpatrick, 2002b) showing that the VCLL contains a high proportion of binaural units responsive to interaural level and time cues, a role in azimuth localisation may be worth reconsidering. Given Nayagam et al.'s (2006) finding of an increased proportion of ILD sensitive neurons in the excitatory INLL, APs signalling azimuth may be separated from APs signalling onset, which stem from inhibitory VNLL cells. From an anatomical perspective, functionality would depend, in part, on which tonotopic model one assumes to be correct. Malmierca et al.‘s (cat; 1998) 3D mosaic arrangement of interdigitating clusters of neurons suggests a situation whereby disparate frequency specific regions of the CNIC receive inputs from compartments within the VCLL that share large interfaces, creating a diversity of neuronal properties. This is similar to the relationship between the cerebral cortex and the pontine nuclei (Malmierca et al., 1998). The functional implications of this would seem to depend on the spatial relationship of the dendritic arbours to the cluster borders. Given the mosaic organisation of the VCLL revealed in Malmierca et al.‘s (cat; 1998) study, these authors proposed a role for the VCLL in pitch perception based on temporal features of the stimulus. This is in line with evidence suggesting that pitch perception requires not only spectral (Goldstein, 1973) but also temporal (Moore and Rosen, 1979; Shofner et al., 2005) information. Their proposal was founded on a model initially described by (Licklider, 1951) and refined by (Meddis and Hewitt, 1991). In this model, the summation of autocorrelated neural responses across frequency-selective channels was described as a

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

65

mechanism for pitch perception based on extraction of both temporal and spectral information from an auditory signal. This model had three requirements, which are: 1) delayed versions of the input signal, 2) convergence of the delayed signal onto single cells, and 3) convergence between frequency channels. Malmierca et al. (1998) suggest that the VCLL is capable of satisfying these three requirements via: 1) Multiple ladder-rung like collaterals of the long lemniscal fibres creating delay lines along the dorsoventral axis of the VCLL. 2) Local collaterals with ventral neurons supplying convergent input on successively more dorsal neurons (cat: Whitley and Henkel, 1984). 3) The spatially specific, but still interdigitating clusters of frequency representation creating cross-frequency interactions. However, this proposal suffers from at least two setbacks. Firstly, the output of the ventral VCLL is inhibitory; this is not accounted for by the model. Secondly, recent studies have shown that response latency is unaffected by position along the dorsoventral axis of the VCLL (Zhang and Kelly, 2006a) and thus the requirement for delayed versions of the input signal is not satisfied in this way. Results from Nayagam et al. (2006) suggest that a modified version of Malmierca et al.‘s (1998) proposal may still satisfy the requirements of the model. This modification would involve altering the site of integration, from the VCLL to the IC, with the VCLL acting as a pre-processor setting up the appropriate timings and inputs. In this way, the three model requirements may still be met via: 1) Delays of the original input signal created by the fast onset inhibition described in Nayagam et al. (2005) and observed in the IC of this and previous studies (see above). 2) Convergence of the delayed signal on single cells at the level of the IC via inputs and intrinsic circuitry. 3) The lack of tonotopicity within the VCLL together with the organised topography of its inputs and outputs, described by some tracing studies, combining to create cross-frequency interaction in the VCLL. This proposal is supported by data from Nayagam et al. (2006); it accounts for the inhibitory nature of the structure and it brings together seemingly disparate results from anatomical and electrophysiological studies. Once again, this proposal only utilises a subset of the response types and properties observed in the VCLL, and therefore, it is likely that if the VCLL is involved in timing based pitch perception, it is only one of many functional roles performed by the nucleus. The lack of a clearly demonstrable tonotopicity within the VCLL contrasts with the basic organising principle of the auditory brainstem, and may give a clue as to its function in temporal processing (Oertel and Wickesberg, 2002; Glendenning and Hutson, 1998). Onset ideal cells within the VCLL are broadly tuned and are a crucial part of the local neural circuitry activated by octopus cell input. Components of this circuit have been described in many species and it is particularly prominent in humans. The onset of a natural sound is an important acoustic event resulting in broadband activation of coincidence detecting neurons such as octopus cells. Onset responses may provide a reference point for the ongoing frequency component processing of a stimulus. Powerful broadband onset inhibition within the VCLL is well placed to control the timing of subsequent APs, whilst remaining independent of APs signalling other frequency-dependant parameters of the stimulus. Nayagam et al. (2005) have proposed that the onset ideal cells are recipients of octopus cell input and provide a source of powerful, fast, pre-spike onset inhibition in VCLL cells, and probably IC cells as well. Additionally, they suggested that this onset inhibition is capable of mediating the timing of the inhibitory output of the VNLL to the IC (Nayagam et al., 2006). When viewed across a neural population, the onset inhibition within the VCLL could

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

66

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

coordinate the VCLL‘s inhibitory output to the IC. This presumably creates a population of IC neurons that respond to a spectrally complex stimuli with onsets that occur within a narrower time window than they would without the VCLL circuitry (Nayagam et al., 2006).

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

CONCLUSION At a perceptual level, the binding of a distributed signal within the sensory system into a cohesive conscious thought is a crucial but poorly understood aspect of neural processing. The activity of various auditory neurons, coding different properties of many distinct but concurrent sounds, is combined to create a unified perception of a listeners‘ auditory environment. Identifying the neural correlates of this conscious experience is one of the great challenges that face neuroscientists, and is well beyond the scope of the current discussion. However, the neural circuitry proposed in Nayagam et al. (2005) may provide the basis for grouping or binding responses to different frequency components of an auditory signal at the level of the IC. A complex stimulus (such as human speech) with two or more spectral peaks excites neural populations in the brainstem tuned to those spectral peaks. The population response to the higher amplitude peak occurs with a shorter latency that the population response to the lower amplitude peaks. In a situation without the VCLL, these brainstem responses would result in an IC population with onsets that occur within a wide time-window. However, the addition of the inhibitory VCLL, in a parallel processing stream to the excitatory projections from the brainstem to the IC, dramatically alters the timing of IC responses to the spectrally complex stimulus. The onset of the complex stimulus, a broadband event, stimulates octopus cells of the PVCN, which in turn activate the inhibitory VNLL onset ideal cells. These onset ideal cells project to the IC, and send collaterals of this powerful, fast, and well-timed inhibitory projection to other cells of the VCLL. The resulting inhibition arrives at the IC target neurons before the excitation from the brainstem population responding to the higher amplitude spectral peak, and actively delays the IC‘s response to this component of the signal. The IC‘s response to the onset of later arriving lower amplitude peaks is unaffected. Similarly, delayed and later arriving inhibition from the other VCLL cells may refine the IC‘s onset responses by attenuating post-onset excitatory inputs. The overall result is a population of IC neurons which respond to a complex stimulus incorporating multiple spectral peaks, with onsets that are grouped into a narrower time-window and bookended by inhibition from the VCLL (Nayagam et al., 2006). This mechanism may be viewed as time-domain binding of the various neural responses to different features of a complex frequency domain signal. The advantage of this setup is that it allows parallel processing streams to de-compose and examine different temporal and spectral components of an input signal and yet retain the ability to re-integrate this information into a perceptually unified entity. This feature-binding role of the VCLL does not require tonotopic organisation and is consistent with previous suggestions that the VCLL is involved in processing timing. The fine-tuning of first spike timing by onset-evoked inhibition provides an explanation of how the IC might re-integrate its multitude of inputs, following stimulation with complex sounds such as human speech. Humans have an increased proportion of globular cells in ventral VNLL compared to other terrestrial mammals. We have a greater projection from the OCA to the ventral VCLL

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

67

and more calyces on bushy cells of the VCLL (Adams, 1997). Therefore, the powerful onset inhibition described by Nayagam et al. (2005), and the entire neural circuit responsible for adjusting spike timing in the VCLL and IC must play an important and enhanced function in humans, once again suggesting a role in interpreting temporal patterns within natural sounds like speech. In humans, the fact that there does not seem to be an equivalent of an INLL suggests that this nucleus may perform a role that is redundant in humans. However, the lack of a clearly distinguishable INLL does not exclude the possibility that the circuits that make up the INLL in other species are incorporated into the VNLL of humans. Additionally, the high proportion of globular cells with large calyx-like inputs in human VNLL (Adams, 1997) implies the kind of processing that requires a specialised and fast neural circuit is more important in humans than in other species. The various roles proposed for the VCLL are not mutually exclusive and it is possible, if not likely, that the VCLL plays multiple roles in auditory processing.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors would like to thank the Bionic Ear Institute, the Department of Otolaryngology, The University of Melbourne, Dame Elisabeth Murdoch and the International Brain Research Organisation for financial support of this work. We would also like to thank the scientists and publishers who have kindly allowed reproduction of their original figures.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

REFERENCES Adams JC (1979) Ascending projections to the inferior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 183:519538. Adams JC (1981) Cytology of the Nuclei of the Lateral Lemniscus. Anatomical Record 199:A6-A6. Adams JC (1993) Non-Primary Inputs to the Cochlear Nucleus Visualized Using Immunocytochemistry. In: The Mammalian Cochlear Nuclei: Organization and Function (Merchán MA, ed), pp 133-141. New York: Plenum Press. Adams JC (1997) Projections from octopus cells of the posteroventral cochlear nucleus to the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus in cat and human. Aud Neurosci 3:335-350. Adams JC, Warr WB (1976) Origins of axons in the cat's acoustic striae determined by injection of horseradish peroxidase into severed tracts. J Comp Neurol 170:107-121. Adams JC, Mugnaini E (1990) Immunocytochemical evidence for inhibitory and disinhibitory circuits in the superior olive. Hear Res 49:281-298. Aitkin LM, Anderson DJ, Brugge JF (1970a) Tonotopic organization and discharge characteristics of single neurons in nuclei of lateral lemniscus of the cat. J Neurophysiol 33:421-440. Aitkin LM, Anderson DJ, Brugge JF (1970b) Frequency Organization and Response Characteristics of Single Neurons in Nuclei of Lateral Lemniscus of Cat. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 47:59.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

68

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Aoki E, Semba R, Keino H, Kato K, Kashiwamata S (1988) Glycine-like immunoreactivity in the rat auditory pathway. Brain Res 442:63-71. Bajo VM, Merchán MA, Lopez DE, Rouiller EM (1993) Neuronal Morphology and Efferent Projections of the Dorsal Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus in the Rat. J Comp Neurol 334:241-262. Banks MI, Smith PH (1992) Intracellular recordings from neurobiotin-labeled cells in brain slices of the rat medial nucleus of the trapezoid body. J Neurosci 12:2819-2837. Batra R (2006) Responses of neurons in the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus to sinusoidally amplitude modulated tones. J Neurophysiol 96:2388-2398. Batra R, Fitzpatrick DC (1997) Neurons sensitive to interaural temporal disparities in the medial part of the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus. J Neurophysiol 78:511-515. Batra R, Fitzpatrick DC (1999) Discharge patterns of neurons in the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus of the unanesthetized rabbit. Journal of Neurophysiology 82:1097-1113. Batra R, Fitzpatrick DC (2002a) Processing of interaural temporal disparities in the medial division of the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus. J Neurophysiol 88:666-675. Batra R, Fitzpatrick DC (2002b) Monaural and binaural processing in the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus: a major source of inhibition to the inferior colliculus. Hearing Res 168:90-97. Benson CG, Cant NB (2008) The ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus of the gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus): organization of connections with the cochlear nucleus and the inferior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 510:673-690. Berne RM, Levy MN, Stanton BA, Koeppen BM (1998) Physiology, 4 Edition: Mosby-Year Book, Inc. Beyerl BD (1978) Afferent projections to the central nucleus of the inferior colliculus in the rat. Brain Res 145:209-223. Blackburn CC, Sachs MB (1989) Classification of Unit Types in the Anteroventral Cochlear Nucleus - Pst Histograms and Regularity Analysis. J Neurophysiol 62:1303-1329. Bourk TR, Mielcarz JP, Norris BE (1981) Tonotopic organization of the anteroventral cochlear nucleus of the cat. Hear Res 4:215-241. Brawer JR, Morest DK, Kane EC (1974) Neuronal Architecture of Cochlear Nucleus of Cat. J Comp Neurol 157:251-300. Brownell WE, Manis PB, Ritz LA (1979) Ipsilateral Inhibitory Responses in the Cat Lateral Superior Olive. Brain Res 177:189-193. Browner RH, Webster DB (1975) Projections of the trapezoid body and the superior olivary complex of the Kangaroo rat (Dipodomys merriami). Brain, Behavior And Evolution 11:322-354. Brugge JF, Anderson DJ, Aitkin LM (1970a) Sensitivity of Single Neurons in Dorsal Nucleus of Lateral Lemniscus of Cat to Binaural Tonal Stimulation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 47:77-&. Brugge JF, Anderson DJ, Aitkin LM (1970b) Responses of Neurons in Dorsal Nucleus of Lateral Lemniscus of Cat to Binaural Tonal Stimulation. J Neurophysiol 33:441-&. Brunso-Bechtold JK, Thompson GC (1978) Ascending Auditory Projections to Nuclei of Lateral Lemniscus as Demonstrated by Horseradish-Peroxidase. Anatomical Record 190:350-350.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

69

Brunso-Bechtold JK, Thompson GC, Masterton RB (1981) HRP study of the organization of auditory afferents ascending to central nucleus of inferior colliculus in cat. J Comp Neurol 197:705-722. Caicedo A, Herbert H (1993) Topography of Descending Projections from the Inferior Colliculus to Auditory Brain-Stem Nuclei in the Rat. J Comp Neurol 328:377-392. Cajal RyS (1909) Histologie du Systeme Nerveux de l'Homme et des Vertébrés. In: Tome I: Instituto Ramon y Cajal, pp 774-838. Madrid. Campos ML, De Cabo C, Wisden W, Juiz JM, Merlo D (2001) Expression of GABA(A) receptor subunits in rat brainstem auditory pathways: Cochlear nuclei, superior olivary complex and nucleus of the lateral lemniscus. Neuroscience 102:625-638. Cant NB, Benson CG (2003) Parallel auditory pathways: projection patterns of the different neuronal populations in the dorsal and ventral cochlear nuclei. Brain Research Bulletin 60:457-474. Casseday JH, Covey E (1983) Laminar projections to the inferior colliculus as seen from injections of wheat germ agglutinin-horseradish peroxidase in the superioir ollivary complex of the cat. Soc Neurosci Abstr 9:766. Casseday JH, Kobler JB, Isbey SF, Covey E (1989) Central acoustic tract in an echolocating bat: an extralemniscal auditory pathway to the thalamus. J Comp Neurol 287:247-259. Clopton BM, Winfield JA (1973) Tonotopic organization in the inferior colliculus of the rat. Brain Res 56:355-358. Coleman JR, Clerici WJ (1987) Sources of projections to subdivisions of the inferior colliculus in the rat. J Comp Neurol 262:215-226. Coleman JR, Campbell M, Clerici WJ (1982) Observations on tonotopic organization within the rat inferior colliculus using 2-deoxy-D[1-3H] glucose. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 90:795-800. Covey E (1993a) Response Properties of Single Units in the Dorsal Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus and Paralemniscal Zone of an Echolocating Bat. J Neurophysiol 69:842-859. Covey E (1993b) The monaural nuclei of the lateral lemniscus: parallel pathways from cochlear nucleus to midbrain. In: The Mammalian Cochlear Nuclei: Organization and Function (Merchan MA, Juiz JM, Godfrey DA, Mugnaini E, eds), pp 321-334. New York: Plenum Publishing Co. Covey E (2003) Brainstem mechanisms for analyzing temporal patterns of echolocation sounds: a model for understanding early stages of speech processing? Speech Communication 41:151-163. Covey E, Casseday JH (1986) Connectional Basis for Frequency Representation in the Nuclei of the Lateral Lemniscus of the Bat Eptesicus-Fuscus. J Neurosci 6:2926-2940. Covey E, Casseday JH (1991) The Monaural Nuclei of the Lateral Lemniscus in an Echolocating Bat - Parallel Pathways for Analyzing Temporal Features of Sound. J Neurosci 11:3456-3470. Covey E, Casseday JH (1995) The lower brainstem auditory pathway. In: Hearing by Bats (Popper AN, Fay RR, eds), pp 235-295. New York: Springer-Verlag. Covey E, Casseday JH (1999) Timing in the auditory system of the bat. Annu Rev Physiol 61:457-476. Druga R, Syka J (1984) Ascending and descending projections to the inferior colliculus in the rat. Physiol Bohemoslov 33:31-42.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

70

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Elverland HH (1978) Ascending and intrinsic projections of the superior olivary complex in the cat. Exp Brain Res 32:117-134. Faingold CL, Anderson CAB, Randall ME (1993) Stimulation or Blockade of the Dorsal Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus Alters Binaural and Tonic Inhibition in Contralateral Inferior Colliculus Neurons. Hearing Res 69:98-106. Flammino F, Clopton BM (1975) Neural responses in the inferior colliculus of albino rat to binaural stimuli. J Acoust Soc Am 57:692-695. Friauf E (1992) Tonotopic Order in the Adult and Developing Auditory-System of the Rat as Shown by C-Fos Immunocytochemistry. European Journal of Neuroscience 4:798-812. Friauf E, Ostwald J (1988) Divergent Projections of Physiologically Characterized Rat Ventral Cochlear Nucleus Neurons as Shown by Intra-Axonal Injection of HorseradishPeroxidase. Experimental Brain Research 73:263-284. Frisina RD, O'Neill WE, Zettel ML (1989) Functional organization of mustached bat inferior colliculus: II. Connections of the FM2 region. J Comp Neurol 284:85-107. Fu XW, Brezden BL, Wu SH (1997a) Hyperpolarization-activated inward current in neurons of the rat's dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus in vitro. J Neurophysiol 78:2235-2245. Fu XW, Wu SH, Brezden BL, Kelly JB (1996) Potassium currents and membrane excitability of neurons in the rat's dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus. J Neurophysiol 76:11211132. Fu XW, Brezden BL, Kelly JB, Wu SH (1997b) Synaptic excitation in the dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus: Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings from rat brain slice. Neuroscience 78:815-827. Giraldez-Perez RM, Gaytan SP, Torres B, Pasaro R (2009) Co-localization of nitric oxide synthase and choline acetyltransferase in the brain of the goldfish (Carassius auratus). J Chem Neuroanat 37:1-17. Glendenning KK, Masterton RB (1983) Acoustic Chiasm: Efferent projections of the lateral superior olive. J Neurosci 3:1521-1537. Glendenning KK, Hutson KA (1998) Lack of topography in the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus. Microscopy Research and Technique 41:298-312. Glendenning KK, Brunso-Bechtold JK, Thompson GC, Masterton RB (1981) Ascending Auditory Afferents to the Nuclei of the Lateral Lemniscus. J Comp Neurol 197:673-703. Glendenning KK, Masterton RB, Hutson KA, Nudo RJ (1990) Ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus: Nontonotopic organization. Anat Rec Abstract 226:37A. Godfrey DA, Kiang NY, Norris BE (1975) Single unit activity in the posteroventral cochlear nucleus of the cat. J Comp Neurol 162:247-268. Golding NL, Robertson D, Oertel D (1995) Recordings from slices indicate that octopus cells of the cochlear nucleus detect coincident firing of auditory nerve fibers with temporal precision. J Neurosci 15:3138-3153. Golding NL, Ferragamo MJ, Oertel D (1999) Role of intrinsic conductances underlying responses to transients in octopus cells of the cochlear nucleus. J Neurosci 19:2897-2905. Goldstein JL (1973) An optimum processor theory for the central formation of the pitch of complex tones. J Acoust Soc Am 54:1496-1516. González-Hernández T, Mantolán-Sarmiento B, González-González B, Pérez-González H (1996) Sources of GABAergic input to the inferior colliculus of the rat. J Comp Neurol 372:309-326.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

71

Greenwood DD, Maruyama N (1965) Excitatory and inhibitory response areas of auditory neurons in the cochlear nucleus. J Neurophysiol 28:863-892. Guinan J, John J., Li RY-S (1990) Signal processing in brainstem auditory neurons which receive giant endings (calyces of Held) in the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body of the cat. Hearing Res 49:321-334. Guinan J, John J., Norris BE, Guinan SS (1972a) Single auditory units in the superior olivary complex: II. Locations of unit categories and tonotopic organization. Int J Neurosci 4:147-166. Guinan J, John J., Guinan SS, Norris BE (1972b) Single auditory units in the superior olivary complex. I. Responses to sound and classification based on physiological properties. Int J Neurosci 4:101-120. Harrison JM, Warr WB (1962) A study of the cochlear nuclei and ascending auditory pathways of the medulla. J Comp Neurol 119:341-379. Held H (1893) Die Zentrale Gehörleitung. Arch Anat Physiol, Anat Abt 17:201-248. Helfert RH, Bonneau JM, Wenthold RJ, Altschuler RA (1989) GABA and glycine immunoreactivity in the guinea pig superior olivary complex. Brain Res 501:269-286. Henkel CK (1981) Afferent sources of a lateral midbrain tegmental zone associated with the pinnae in the cat as mapped by retrograde transport of horseradish peroxidase. J Comp Neurol 203:213-226. Henkel CK (1983) Evidence of sub-collicular auditory projections to the medial geniculate nucleus in the cat: an autoradiographic and horseradish peroxidase study. Brain Res 259:21-30. Henkel CK, Edwards SB (1978) The superior colliculus control of pinna movements in the cat: possible anatomical connections. J Comp Neurol 182:763-776. Henkel CK, Spangler KM (1983) Organization of the efferent projections of the medial superior olivary nucleus in the cat as revealed by HRP and autoradiographic tracing methods. J Comp Neurol 221:416-428. Hirsch JA, Oertel D (1988) Intrinsic properties of neurones in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of mice, in vitro. J Physiol 396:535-548. Huang CM, Fex J (1986) Tonotopic organization in the inferior colliculus of the rat demonstrated with the 2-deoxyglucose method. Exp Brain Res 61:506-512. Huffman RF, Covey E (1995) Origin of Ascending Projections to the Nuclei of the Lateral Lemniscus in the Big Brown Bat, Eptesicus-Fuscus. J Comp Neurol 357:532-545. Huffman RF, Argeles PC, Covey E (1998a) Processing of sinusoidally frequency modulated signals in the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus of the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. Hearing Res 126:161-180. Huffman RF, Argeles PC, Covey E (1998b) Processing of sinusoidally amplitude modulated signals in the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus of the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. Hearing Res 126:181-200. Hutson KA, Glendenning KK, Masterton RB (1991) Acoustic Chiasm .4. 8 Midbrain Decussations of the Auditory-System in the Cat. J Comp Neurol 312:105-131. Ibata Y, Pappas GD (1976) The fine structure of synapses in relation to the large spherical neurons in the anterior ventral cochlear of the cat. J Neurocytol 5:395-406. Irfan N, Zhang H, Wu SH (2005) Synaptic transmission mediated by ionotropic glutamate, glycine and GABA receptors in the rat's ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus. Hear Res 203:159-171.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

72

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Irvine DRF (1992) Physiology of the auditory brainstem. In: The Mammalian Auditory Pathway: Neurophysiology (Popper AN, Fay RR, eds), pp 153-231. New York: SpringerVerlag. Ito M, vanAdel B, Kelly JB (1996) Sound localization after transection of the commissure of Probst in the albino rat. J Neurophysiol 76:3493-3502. Iwahori N (1986) A Golgi study on the dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus in the mouse. Neurosci Res 3:196-212. Jahnsen H, Llinas R (1984) Voltage-dependent burst-to-tonic switching of thalamic cell activity: an in vitro study. Arch Ital Biol 122:73-82. Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM (1991) Principals of Neural Science, 3 Edition: Appleton and Lange. Kandler K, Friauf E (1993) Prenatal and Postnatal-Development of Efferent Connections of the Cochlear Nucleus in the Rat. J Comp Neurol 328:161-184. Kanold PO, Manis PB (1999) Transient potassium currents regulate the discharge patterns of dorsal cochlear nucleus pyramidal cells. J Neurosci 19:2195-2208. Kelly JB, Glenn SL, Beaver CJ (1991) Sound frequency and binaural response properties of single neurons in rat inferior colliculus. Hear Res 56:273-280. Kelly JB, Buckthought AD, Kidd SA (1998a) Monaural and binaural response properties of single neurons in the rat's dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus. Hearing Res 122:25-40. Kelly JB, Liscum A, van Adel B, Ito M (1995) Retrograde labelling in the rat's dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus from frequency specific regions of the central nucleus of the inferior colliculus. Assoc Res Otolaryngol Abstr 18:40. Kelly JB, Liscum A, van Adel B, Ito M (1998b) Projections from the superior olive and lateral lemniscus to tonotopic regions of the rat's inferior colliculus. Hearing Res 116:4354. Kelly JB, van Adel BA, Ito M (2009) Anatomical projections of the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus in the albino rat (rattus norvegicus). J Comp Neurol 512:573-593. Kiang NY, Pfeiffer RR, Warr WB, Backus AS (1965a) Stimulus coding in the cochlear nucleus. Trans Am Otol Soc 53:35-58. Kiang NY, Pfeiffer RR, Warr WB, Backus AS (1965b) Stimulus Coding in the Cochlear Nucleus. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 74:463-485. Kidd SA, Kelly JB (1996) Contribution of the dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus to binaural responses in the inferior colliculus of the rat: Interaural time delays. J Neurosci 16:7390-7397. Kopp-Scheinpflug C, Dehmel S, Dorrscheidt GJ, Rubsamen R (2002) Interaction of excitation and inhibition in anteroventral cochlear nucleus neurons that receive large endbulb synaptic endings. J Neurosci 22:11004-11018. Kudo M (1981) Projections of the Nuclei of the Lateral Lemniscus in the Cat - an Autoradiographic Study. Brain Res 221:57-69. Kudo M, Nakamura Y (1988) Organization of the lateral lemniscal fibers converging onto the inferior colliculus in the cat: An anatomical review. In: Auditory Pathway, Structure and Function (Syka J, Masterton RB, eds), pp 171-183. New York: Plenum Press. Kudo M, Nakamura Y, Tokuno H, Kitao Y (1990) Auditory brainstem in the mole (Mogera): nuclear configurations and the projections to the inferior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 298:400-412.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

73

Kuwada S, Batra R, Stanford TR (1989) Monaural and Binaural Response Properties of Neurons in the Inferior Colliculus of the Rabbit - Effects of Sodium Pentobarbital. J Neurophysiol 61:269-282. Langner G, Braun S, Simonis C, Benson CG, Cant NB (2006) New evidence for a pitch helix in the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus in the gerbil. Assoc Res Otolaryngol Abstr 29:771. Lenn NJ, Reese TS (1966) The fine structure of nerve endings in the nucleus of the trapezoid body and the ventral cochlear nucleus. Am J Anat 118:375-389. Li L, Kelly JB (1992) Binaural Responses in Rat Inferior Colliculus Following Kainic Acid Lesions of the Superior Olive - Interaural Intensity Difference Functions. Hearing Res 61:73-85. Licklider JC (1951) A duplex theory of pitch perception. Experientia 7:128-134. Malmierca MS, Le Beau FEN, Rees A (1996) The topographical organization of descending projections from the central nucleus of the inferior colliculus in guinea pig. Hearing Res 93:167-180. Malmierca MS, Rees A, Le Beau FEN, Bjaalie JG (1995) Laminar Organization of Frequency-Defined Local Axons within and between the Inferior Colliculi of the GuineaPig. J Comp Neurol 357:124-144. Malmierca MS, Leergaard TB, Bajo VM, Bjaalie JG, Merchán MA (1998) Anatomic evidence of a three-dimensional mosaic pattern of tonotopic organization in the ventral complex of the lateral lemniscus in cat. J Neurosci 18:10603-10618. Manis PB, Spirou GA, Wright DD, Paydar S, Ryugo DK (1994) Physiology and morphology of complex spiking neurons in the guinea pig dorsal cochlear nucleus. J Comp Neurol 348:261-276. Markovitz NS, Pollak GD (1993) The Dorsal Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus in the Moustache Bat - Monaural Properties. Hearing Res 71:51-63. Markovitz NS, Pollak GD (1994) Binaural Processing in the Dorsal Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus. Hearing Res 73:121-140. Masterton RB, Imig TJ (1984) Neural mechanisms for sound localization. Annu Rev Physiol 46:275-287. May PJ, Vidal PP, Baker R (1990) Synaptic organization of tectal-facial pathways in cat. II. Synaptic potentials following midbrain tegmentum stimulation. J Neurophysiol 64:381402. Meddis R, Hewitt MJ (1991) Virtual pitch and phase sensitivity of a computer model of the auditory periphery I: pitch identification. J Am Soc Audiol 89:2866-2882. Merchán MA, Berbel P (1996) Anatomy of the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus in rats: A nucleus with a concentric laminar organization. J Comp Neurol 372:245-263. Merchán MA, Saldaña E, Plaza I (1994) Dorsal Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus in the Rat Concentric Organization and Tonotopic Projection to the Inferior Colliculus. J Comp Neurol 342:259-278. Merchán MA, Malmierca MS, Bajo VM, Bjaalie JG (1997) The Nuclei of the Lateral Lemniscus Old Views and New Perspectives. In: Acoustic Signal Processing in the Central Auditory System (Syka J, ed), pp 211-226. New York: Plenum Press. Merzenich MM, Reid MD (1974) Representation of the cochlea within the inferior colliculus of the cat. Brain Res 77:397-415.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

74

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Metzner W, Radtke-Schuller S (1987) The Nuclei of the Lateral Lemniscus in the Rufous Horseshoe Bat, Rhinolophus-Rouxi - a Neurophysiological Approach. Journal of Comparative Physiology a-Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 160:395-411. Moore BC, Rosen SM (1979) Tune recognition with reduced pitch and interval information. Q J Exp Psychol 31:229-240. Moore DR (1988) Auditory brainstem of the ferret: sources of projections to the inferior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 269:342-354. Moore JK, Moore RY (1987) Glutamic acid decarboxylase-like immunoreactivity in brainstem auditory nuclei of the rat. J Comp Neurol 260:157-174. Morest DK (1968) The growth of synaptic endings in the mammalian brain: a study of the calyces of the trapezoid body. Z Anat Entwicklungsgesch 127:201-220. Morest DK, Jean-Baptiste M (1975) Degeneration and phagocytosis of synaptic endings and axons in the medial trapezoid nucleus of the cat. J Comp Neurol 162:135-156. Morest DK, Kim JN, Bohne BA (1997) Neuronal and transneuronal degeneration of auditory axons in the brainstem after cochlear lesions in the chinchilla: Cochleotopic and noncochleotopic patterns. Hearing Res 103:151-168. Motts SD, Slusarczyk AS, Sowick CS, Schofield BR (2008) Distribution of cholinergic cells in guinea pig brainstem. Neuroscience 154:186-195. Nayagam DAX, Clarey JC, Paolini AG (2005) Powerful, onset inhibition in the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus. J Neurophysiol 94:1651-1654. Nayagam DAX, Clarey JC, Paolini AG (2006) Intracellular responses and morphology of rat ventral complex of the lateral lemniscus neurons in vivo. J Comp Neurol 498:295-315. Niemer WR, Cheng SK (1949) The ascending auditory system: A study of retrograde degeneration. Anat Rec 103:490. Nolte J, Angevine JB (1995) Human Brain: In Photographs and Diagrams, 1 Edition: Mosby-Year Book, Inc. Nordeen KW, Killackey, J. P., and Kitzes, L. M. (1983) Ascending auditory projections to the inferior colliculus in the adult gerbil, Meriones unguiculatus. J, Comp Neurol 214:131143. Oertel D (1983) Synaptic responses and electrical properties of cells in brain slices of the mouse anteroventral cochlear nucleus. J Neurosci 3:2043-2053. Oertel D (1991) The role of intrinsic neuronal properties in the encoding of auditory information in the cochlear nuclei. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1:221-228. Oertel D (1997) Encoding of timing in the brain stem auditory nuclei of vertebrates. Neuron 19:959-962. Oertel D, Wickesberg RE (2002) Ascending Pathways Through Ventral Nuclei of the Lateral Lemniscus and Their Possible Role in Pattern Recognition in Natural Sounds. In: Integrative Functions in the Mammalian Auditory Pathway (Oertel D, Popper AN, Fay RR, eds), pp 207-237. New York; London: Springer. Oertel D, Wu SH, Garb MW, Dizack C (1990) Morphology and physiology of cells in slice preparations of the posteroventral cochlear nucleus of mice. J Comp Neurol 295:136-154. Oertel D, Bal R, Gardner SM, Smith PH, Joris PX (2000) Detection of synchrony in the activity of auditory nerve fibers by octopus cells of the mammalian cochlear nucleus. P Natl Acad Sci USA 97:11773-11779. Oliver DL (1987) Projections to the inferior colliculus from the anteroventral cochlear nucleus in the cat: possible substrates for binaural interaction. J Comp Neurol 264:24-46.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

75

Osen KK (1969) Cytoarchitecture of the cochlear nuclei in the cat. J Comp Neurol 136:453484. Osen KK (1972) Projection of the cochlear nuclei on the inferior colliculus in the cat. J Comp Neurol 144:355-372. Paxinos G, Watson C (1998) The Rat Brain In Stereotaxic Co-Ordinates, 4th Edition, 4th Edition Edition. San Diego: Academic Press. Paxinos G, Watson C (2005) The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, 5th Edition. The new coronal set. Burlington: Elsevier Academic Press. Pfeiffer RR (1966) Classification of response patterns of spike discharges for units in the cochlear nucleus: tone-burst stimulation. Exp Brain Res 1:220-235. Portfors CV, Wenstrup JJ (2001) Responses to combinations of tones in the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 2:104-117. Rhode WS, Smith PH (1986) Encoding timing and intensity in the ventral cochlear nucleus of the cat. J Neurophysiol 56:261-286. Rhode WS, Greenberg S (1992) Physiology of the Cochlear Nuclei. In: Auditory Research, Volume 2: The Physiology of the Mammalian Auditory Central Nervous System (Fay RR, Popper AN, eds). Riquelme R, Merchán MA, Ottersen OP (1998) GABA and glycine in the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus: an immunocytochemical and in situ hybridization study in rat. Assoc Res Otolaryngol Abstr 21:93. Riquelme R, Saldaña E, Osen KK, Ottersen OP, Merchán MA (2001) Colocalization of GABA and glycine in the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus in rat: An in situ hybridization and semiquantitative immunocytochemical study. J Comp Neurol 432:409424. Ritz LA, Brownell WE (1982) Single Unit Analysis of the Posteroventral Cochlear Nucleus of the Decerebrate Cat. Neuroscience 7:1995-2010. Roberts RC, Ribak CE (1987) GABAergic neurons and axon terminals in the brainstem auditory nuclei of the gerbil. J Comp Neurol 258:267-280. Rose JE, Greenwood DD, Goldberg JM, Hind JE (1963) Some Discharge characteristics of single neurons in the inferior colliculus of the cat: I. Tonotopic organization, relation of spike counts to intensity, and firing pattern of single elements. J Neuophysiol 26:294-320. Ross LS, Pollak GD (1989) Differential ascending projections to aural regions in the 60 kHz contour of the mustache bat's inferior colliculus. J Neurosci 9:2819-2834. Ross LS, Pollak GD, Zook JM (1988) Origin of ascending projections to an isofrequency region of the mustache bat's inferior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 270:488-505. Roth GL, Aitkin LM, Andersen RA, Merzenich MM (1978) Some features of the spatial organization of the central nucleus of the inferior colliculus of the cat. J Comp Neurol 182:661-680. Ryan AF, Furlow Z, Woolf NK, Keithley EM (1988) The spatial representation of frequency in the rat dorsal cochlear nucleus and inferior colliculus. Hear Res 36:181-189. Ryugo DK, Sento S (1991) Synaptic connections of the auditory nerve in cats: relationship between endbulbs of held and spherical bushy cells. J Comp Neurol 305:35-48. Saint Marie RL, Baker RA (1990) Neurotransmitter-Specific Uptake and Retrograde Transport of [H-3] Glycine from the Inferior Colliculus by Ipsilateral Projections of the Superior Olivary Complex and Nuclei of the Lateral Lemniscus. Brain Res 524:244-253.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

76

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Saint Marie RL, Shneiderman A, Stanforth DA (1997) Patterns of gamma-aminobutyric acid and glycine immunoreactivities reflect structural and functional differences of the cat lateral lemniscal nuclei. J Comp Neurol 389:264-276. Saint Marie RL, Luo L, Ryan AF (1999) Spatial representation of frequency in the rat dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus as revealed by acoustically induced c-fos mRNA expression. Hearing Res 128:70-74. Saldaña E (1993) Descending projections from the inferior colliculus to the cochlear nuclei in mammals. In: The Mammalian Cochlear Nuclei: Organization and Function (Merchan MA, Juiz JM, Godfrey DA, Mugnaini E, eds), pp 153-165. Ney York: Plenum. Saldaña E, Merchán MA (1992) Intrinsic and Commissural Connections of the Rat Inferior Colliculus. J Comp Neurol 319:417-437. Schofield BR (1995) Projections from the cochlear nucleus to the superior paraolivary nucleus in guinea pigs. J Comp Neurol 360:135-149. Schofield BR, Cant NB (1997) Ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus in guinea pigs: Cytoarchitecture and inputs from the cochlear nucleus. J Comp Neurol 379:363-385. Schwartz IR (1992) The superior olivary complex and lateral lemniscal nuclei. In: The mammalian auditory pathway: neuroanatomy (Webster DB, Popper AN, Fay RR, eds), pp 117-167. New York: Springer-Verlag. Schweizer H (1981) The connections of the inferior colliculus and the organization of the brainstem auditory system in the greater horseshoe bat (Rhinolophus ferrumequinum). J Comp Neurol 201:25-49. Semple MN, Aitkin LM (1979) Representation of Sound Frequency and Laterality by Units in Central Nucleus of Cat Inferior Colliculus. J Neurophysiol 42:1626-1639. Shneiderman A, Chase MB, Rockwood JM, Benson CG, Potashner SJ (1993) Evidence for a Gabaergic Projection from the Dorsal Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus to the Inferior Colliculus. Journal of Neurochemistry 60:72-82. Shofner WP, Whitmer WM, Yost WA (2005) Listening experience with iterated rippled noise alters the perception of 'pitch' strength of complex sounds in the chinchilla. J Acoust Soc Am 118:3187-3197. Smith PH, Joris PX, Yin TCT (1993) Projections of Physiologically Characterized Spherical Bushy Cell Axons from the Cochlear Nucleus of the Cat - Evidence for Delay-Lines to the Medial Superior Olive. J Comp Neurol 331:245-260. Smith PH, Massie A, Joris PX (2005) Acoustic stria: anatomy of physiologically characterized cells and their axonal projection patterns. J Comp Neurol 482:349-371. Spangler KM, Warr WB, Henkel CK (1985) The projections of principal cells of the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body in the cat. J Comp Neurol 238:249-262. Steriade M, McCormick DA, Sejnowski TJ (1993) Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain. Science 262:679-685. Stotler WA (1953) An experimental study of the cells and connections of the superior olivary complex of the cat. J Comp Neurol 98:401-431. Strominger NL, Strominger AI (1971) Ascending brain stem projections of the anteroventral cochlear nucleus in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 143:217-242. Suneja SK, Benson CG, Gross J, Potashner SJ (1995) Evidence for Glutamatergic Projections from the Cochlear Nucleus to the Superior Olive and the Ventral Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus. J Neurochem 64:161-171.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

77

Swanson LW (1998) Brain Maps: Structure of the Rat Brain, 2nd edition, Second revised Edition Edition. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science B.V. Syka J, Radionova EA, Popelar J (1981) Discharge characteristics of neuronal pairs in the rabbit inferior colliculus. Exp Brain Res 44:11-18. Tanaka K, Otani K, Tokunaga A, Sugita S (1985) The Organization of Neurons in the Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus Projecting to the Superior and Inferior Colliculi in the Rat. Brain Res 341:252-260. Tennigkeit F, Schwarz DW, Puil E (1996) Mechanisms for signal transformation in lemniscal auditory thalamus. J Neurophysiol 76:3597-3608. Tennigkeit F, Schwarz DW, Puil E (1998) Modulation of bursts and high-threshold calcium spikes in neurons of rat auditory thalamus. Neuroscience 83:1063-1073. Tokunaga A, Sugita S, Otani K (1984) Auditory and non-auditory subcortical afferents to the inferior colliculus in the rat. J Hirnforsch 25:461-472. Tsuchitani C (1988a) The inhibition of cat lateral superior olive unit excitatory responses to binaural tone bursts. II. The sustained discharges. J Neurophysiol 59:184-211. Tsuchitani C (1988b) The inhibition of cat lateral superior olive unit excitatory responses to binaural tone bursts. I. The transient chopper response. J Neurophysiol 59:164-183. van Gehuchten A (1906) Le System Nerveux de l'Homme. Fourth Edition., 4 Edition. van Noort J (1969) The anatomical basis for frequency analysis in the cochlear nuclear complex. Psychiatr Neurol Neurochir 72:109-114. Vater M, Feng AS (1990) Functional organization of ascending and descending connections of the cochlear nucleus of horseshoe bats. J Comp Neurol 292:373-395. Vater M, Braun K (1994) Parvalbumin, Calbindin D-28k, and Calretinin Immunoreactivity in the Ascending Auditory Pathway of Horseshoe Bats. J Comp Neurol 341:534-558. Vater M, Covey E, Casseday JH (1997) The columnar region of the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus in the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus): Synaptic arrangements and structural correlates of feedforward inhibitory function. Cell and Tissue Research 289:223-233. Vater M, Habbicht H, Kossl M, Grothe B (1992) The Functional-Role of Gaba and Glycine in Monaural and Binaural Processing in the Inferior Colliculus of Horseshoe Bats. Journal of Comparative Physiology a-Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 171:541-553. Warr WB (1966) Fiber degeneration following lesions in the anterior ventral cochlear nucleus of the cat. Exp Neurol 14:453-474. Warr WB (1969) Fiber degeneration following lesions in the posteroventral cochlear nucleus of the cat. Exp Neurol 23:140-155. Warr WB (1972) Fiber degeneration following lesions in the multipolar and globular cell areas in the ventral cochlear nucleus of the cat. Brain Res 40:247-270. Warr WB, Beck JE (1996) Multiple projections from the ventral nucleus of the trapezoid body in the rat. Hearing Res 93:83-101. Whitley JM, Henkel CK (1983) A Subcollicular Projection to the Medial Geniculate-Nucleus from the Ventral Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus - a Hrp and Fluorescent Dye Study. Anatomical Record 205:A214-A215. Whitley JM, Henkel CK (1984) Topographical organization of the inferior collicular projection and other connections of the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus in the cat. J Comp Neurol 229:257-270.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

78

David A.X. Nayagam, Janine C. Clarey and Antonio G. Paolini

Willard FH, Ryugo DK (1983) Anatomy of the central auditory system. In: The Auditory Psychobiology of the Mouse (Thomas C, ed), pp 201-304. Springfield, IL. Willard FH, Martin GF (1983) The auditory brainstem nuclei and some of their projections to the inferior colliculus in the North American opossum. Neuroscience 10:1203-1232. Winer JA, Larue DT, Pollak GD (1995) Gaba and Glycine in the Central Auditory-System of the Moustache Bat - Structural Substrates for Inhibitory Neuronal Organization. J Comp Neurol 355:317-353. Wu SH (1999) Physiological properties of neurons in the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus of the rat: Intrinsic membrane properties and synaptic responses. J Neurophysiol 81:2862-2874. Wu SH, Oertel D (1984) Intracellular injection with horseradish peroxidase of physiologically characterized stellate and bushy cells in slices of mouse anteroventral cochlear nucleus. J Neurosci 4:1577-1588. Wu SH, Kelly JB (1991) Physiological properties of neurons in the mouse superior olive: membrane characteristics and postsynaptic responses studied in vitro. J Neurophysiol 65:230-246. Wu SH, Kelly JB (1995a) In vitro brain slice studies of the rat's dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus .2. Physiological properties of biocytin-labeled neurons (vol 73, pg 794, 1995). J Neurophysiol 74:U11-U11. Wu SH, Kelly JB (1995b) In-Vitro Brain Slice Studies of the Rats Dorsal Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus .1. Membrane and Synaptic Response Properties. J Neurophysiol 73:780-793. Wu SH, Kelly JB (1996) In vitro brain slice studies of the rat's dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus .3. Synaptic pharmacology. J Neurophysiol 75:1271-1282. Xie R, Meitzen J, Pollak GD (2005) Differing roles of inhibition in hierarchical processing of species-specific calls in auditory brainstem nuclei. J Neurophysiol 94:4019-4037. Yang LC, Pollak GD (1994a) The Roles of Gabaergic and Glycinergic Inhibition on Binaural Processing in the Dorsal Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus of the Moustache Bat. J Neurophysiol 71:1999-2013. Yang LC, Pollak GD (1994b) Gaba and Glycine Have Different Effects on Monaural Response Properties in the Dorsal Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus of the Moustache Bat. J Neurophysiol 71:2014-2024. Yang LC, Liu Q, Pollak GD (1996) Afferent connections to the dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus of the mustache bat: Evidence for two functional subdivisions. J Comp Neurol 373:575-592. Young ED (1998) Cochlear Nucleus. In: The Synaptic Organization of the Brain, 4 Edition (Shepherd GM, ed). New York: Oxford University Press. Zhang DX, Li L, Kelly JB, Wu SH (1998) GABAergic projections from the lateral lemniscus to the inferior colliculus of the rat. Hearing Res 117:1-12. Zhang H, Kelly JB (2006a) Responses of neurons in the rat's ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus to monaural and binaural tone bursts. J Neurophysiol 95:2501-2512. Zhang H, Kelly JB (2006b) Responses of Neurons in the Rat's Ventral Nucleus of the Lateral Lemniscus to Amplitude-Modulated Tones. J Neuophysiol 96:2905-2914. Zhang S, Oertel D (1993) Cartwheel and superficial stellate cells of the dorsal cochlear nucleus of mice: intracellular recordings in slices. J Neurophysiol 69:1384-1397.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

The Ventral Complex of the Lateral Lemniscus: A Review

79

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Zhao M, Wu SH (2001) Morphology and physiology of neurons in the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus in rat brain slices. J Comp Neurol 433:255-271. Zook JM, Casseday JH (1979) Connections of the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus in the mustache bat, Pteronotus Parnellii. Soc Neurosci Abstr 5:34. Zook JM, Casseday JH (1982a) Origin of ascending projections to inferior colliculus in the mustache bat, Pteronotus parnellii. J Comp Neurol 207:14-28. Zook JM, Casseday JH (1982b) Cytoarchitecture of auditory system in lower brainstem of the mustache bat, Pteronotus parnellii. J Comp Neurol 207:1-13. Zook JM, Casseday JH (1985) Projections from the cochlear nuclei in the mustache bat, Pteronotus parnellii. J Comp Neurol 237:307-324. Zook JM, Casseday JH (1987) Convergence of ascending pathways at the inferior colliculus of the mustache bat, Pteronotus parnellii. J Comp Neurol 261:347-361. Zook JM, DiCaprio RA (1988) Intracellular labeling of afferents to the lateral superior olive in the bat, Eptesicus fuscus. Hear Res 34:141-147.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved. Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

In: Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System Editors: D. Fiedler and R. Krause, pp.81-110

ISBN: 978-1-60741-259-5 ©2010 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

Chapter 2

NOISE-INDUCED HEARING LOSS IN YOUTH CAUSED BY LEISURE NOISE H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

ABSTRACT Excessive noise exposure can lead to metabolic and/or mechanical effects resulting in alterations of the structural elements of the organ of Corti. Besides occupational noise exposure, there are several sources of leisure noise exposure attributing to noise-induced hearing loss (NIHL). These leisure noise exposures include exposure to loud music or even participation in non-musical activities. The major sources of the former activities are attendance at discotheques, nightclubs and live concerts, and usage of personal music players. Recently, it was estimated that the prevalence of leisure noise in the UK has tripled since the early 1980s. Therefore, an increase in prevalence of NIHL in adolescents and young adults is assumed, especially in the mainstream media. Epidemiologic literature is, however, more equivocal. The lack of hearing deterioration is most likely explained by the fact that leisure noise exposure is insufficient to cause important hearing loss. Nevertheless, the effects of leisure noise exposure on the auditory system of young people is a cause for concern. The purpose of this chapter is to review the sources of leisure noise in relation to their contribution in life-time noise exposure of individuals and to present epidemiologic data regarding NIHL in young people caused by leisure noise. Differences in attitude of young people regarding leisure noise exposure and usage of hearing protection are discussed, as well as the importance of prevention and sensitization campaigns. Moreover, short- and long-term effects of several leisure noise sources are reviewed. Shortcomings in the current literature are stressed, and proposals for additional research regarding the usefulness of several audiological tests to detect minimal cochlear damage after exposure to loud leisure noise are pointed out.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

82

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

INTRODUCTION The fact that excessive occupational noise exposure can lead to noise-induced hearing loss (NIHL) is well known. Besides occupational noise exposure, non-occupational noise exposure is also a cause for concern, especially in young people. The purpose of this chapter is to give insight in the prevalence of NIHL caused by leisure noise exposure, and the sources of leisure noise. Research regarding temporary and permanent auditory effects caused by listening to personal music players and attending discotheques, nightclubs or live concerts is discussed. Further, preventive strategies are suggested, and finally, the current legislation regarding leisure noise exposure is reviewed.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

PATHOPHYSIOLOGY AND DIAGNOSIS OF NOISE-INDUCED HEARING LOSS Sound perception is based on mechanical sound energy reaching the middle ear, inducing movement of the stapes footplate into the oval window, and producing pressure changes in the perilymph of the cochlea. The traveling wave of the basilar membrane (BM) deflects the stereocilia of the hair cells which stretches the tip links and forces them to open ionic channels on the stereocilia. The resulting potassium influx depolarizes the hair cells of the organ of Corti, and consequently releases neurotransmitters generating action potentials in the cochlear nerve by which neural afferent information is transferred to the brainstem. The motile activity of the outer hair cells (OHCs) [Brownell, 1990] amplifies the sound-induced micro-mechanical movement of the BM, especially at low-input levels. Thereby the input for the inner hair cells (IHCs) is enhanced. This non-linear active process, i.e. the cochlear amplifier, is responsible for the high sensitivity, sharp frequency selectivity and wide dynamic range of the human auditory system [Norton, 1992]. Excessive noise exposure can lead to metabolic and/or mechanical changes resulting in alterations of the structural elements of the organ of Corti [Sliwinska-Kowalska & Jedlinska, 1998; Céranic, 2007; Talaska & Schacht, 2007]. Metabolic disturbances induce cell death through toxic reactions, while mechanical damage results in direct physical loss of the integrity of hair cells and surroundings cells [Talaska & Schacht, 2007]. Morphological changes may include buckling of the pillar bodies, decreasing the height of the organ of Corti and thereby uncoupling the OHC stereocilia from the tectorial membrane [Nordmann et al, 2000]. This temporary uncoupling could affect hair cell stimulation, and is sufficient to explain temporary threshold shift (TTS). It is suggested that TTS and permanent threshold shift (PTS) arise from a fundamentally different mechanism [Nordmann et al, 2000], and that a PTS cannot be predicted form the initial TTS [Melnick, 1991] as it might depend on the recovery or repair processes within the cochlea [Nordmann et al, 2000]. Morphological correlates of PTS are focal losses of hair cells and adjacent afferent nerve fiber degeneration [Nordmann et al, 2000]. There are biochemical events that accompany morphological changes in the cochlea leading to degeneration of the sensory cells and eventually to degeneration of the spiral ganglion [Talaska & Schacht, 2007]. Prolonged noise exposure results in an accumulation of calcium, saturating the calcium-binding capacities of the OHCs [Heinrich & Feltens, 2006].

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

83

The disturbance of calcium homeostasis can result in an overload of calcium in the mitochondria, excitotoxic neural swelling, and reduction of cochlear blood flow. Thereby, reactive oxygen species (ROS) enzymes are activated and free radicals are generated [Henderson et al, 2006]. Although ROS formation is not limited to hair cells, but also occur in other structures such as supporting cells and the stria vascularis, the primary damage is concentrated on the OHCs [Sliwinska-Kowalska & Jedlinska, 1998; Lucertini et al, 2002; Sliwinska-Kowalska & Kotylo, 2008]. This might suggest that OHCs have a lower antioxidant capacity, and might therefore be more susceptible to ROS [Talaska & Schacht, 2007]. The co-existence of apoptosis and necrosis involved in OHC damage causes eventually cell death [Hu et al, 2002] and this induces disturbed physiological pathways in other cochlear cell types [Heinrich & Feltens, 2006]. Apoptosis is an active process that eliminates injured cells which could spread lesion to neighboring cells. The morphological correlate is nuclear condensation and fragmentation [Hu et al, 2002]. Necrosis, however, is a passive process associated with cell swelling resulting in cell rupture. Eventually, it results in damage to surrounding cells or inflammatory responses [Henderson et al, 2006]. However, necrosis is found to account for only a small portion of this damage, and apoptosis persists even several days after noise exposure [Hu et al, 2002]. Therefore, the clinical utility of delayed interventions must be further explored, as well as the role of antioxidant prevention of NIHL in humans [Le Prell et al, 2007]. Long periods of continuous or intermittent noise exposure can lead to a slowly developing chronic hearing loss, or NIHL. By analogy with NIHL, hearing loss caused by music is sometimes referred as music-induced hearing loss. Typically, noise is an unwanted, unpleasant acoustic signal [Hausler, 2004]. However, instead of the term music-induced hearing loss, the term NIHL is mainly used because noise is referred as an excessive acoustic signal possibly causing hearing damage, whether unwanted or not. According to the Position Statement of the American College of Occupational and Environmental Medicine (ACOEM) on NIHL in 2002 (ACOEM, 2002), NIHL can be defined as a sensorineural, mostly bilateral symmetrical hearing loss initially presented in the frequency range 3 to 6 kHz. NIHL almost never progresses to a profound hearing loss, but the rate of hearing loss is greatest during the first 10 to 15 years of exposure and does not progress after cessation of noise exposure. However, it was postulated that hearing loss is only detected as soon as a considerable amount of hair cells is damaged [Daniel, 2007]. This suggests that there might be a period of latent damage making it impossible to distinguish audiometrically between subjects whether exposed to noise or not [LePage, 1998]. Moreover, the risk of hearing loss is influenced by the noise exposure level, duration, number of exposures and individual susceptibility [Mills & Going, 1982]. Henderson et al (1993) states that there is a large variability in susceptibility to NIHL in demographic studies, as well as laboratory studies, which seemed to be caused by several factors. First, some of the variability could be explained by the interaction between noise exposure and other ototoxic agents exacerbating NIHL. Second, the role of auditory characteristics such as middle ear muscle reflex, efferent system and subject‘s history of noise exposure could attribute to the variability of NIHL, and to a smaller extent, non-auditory characteristics such as gender, eye color and smoking [Henderson et al, 1993]. The diagnosis of NIHL is based on audiometric evaluation in combination with a history of noise exposure [Alberti, 1998]. Besides the loss of hearing sensitivity, concomitant tinnitus and impaired speech discrimination can be present. The sensitivity of pure-tone audiometry

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

84

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

for the early detection of NIHL however has been questioned. The ACOEM has recommended additional research on NIHL, including in the area of early indicators of hearing loss such as otoacoustic emissions (OAEs) (ACOEM, 2002). OAEs are low-level sounds reflecting the non-linear active processes of the cochlea [Norton, 1992], which were discovered by Kemp in 1978 [Kemp, 1978]. It is an non-invasive, objective technique that does not require active participation of the subject. OAEs represent an indication of the integrity of the OHCs, but their presence does not exclude hearing impairment (e.g. retrocochlear hearing loss). However, OAEs depend highly on normal middle ear function [Probst et al 1991]. Nevertheless, OAE amplitude reduction may reflect OHC damage due to noise exposure [Sliwinska-Kowalska & Kotylo, 2008]. So, OAEs are a promising tool in the detection of sub-clinical OHC damage and pre-clinical hearing loss, overlooked by pure-tone audiometry. This has mainly been established in industrial or military settings [Hotz et al, 1993; Kowalska & Sulkowski, 1997; Hall & Lutman, 1999; Desai et al, 1999; Konopka et al, 2001; Attias et al, 2001; Lucertini et al, 2002; Céranic, 2007; Sliwinska-Kowalska & Kotylo, 2008]. Two types of OAEs, transient evoked OAEs (TEOAEs) and distortion product OAEs (DPOAEs), are commonly used. In contrast to TEOAEs, DPOAEs have a more highfrequency response which makes them an ideal tool for detecting relatively high frequencyspecific cochlear damage such as NIHL [Probst et al 1993]. However, some studies indicate that TEOAEs are more sensitive than DPOAEs in the detection of minimal cochlear alteration caused by excessive noise exposure [Plinkert et al 1995; Lapsley-Miller et al 2004].

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

PREVALENCE OF NOISE-INDUCED HEARING LOSS CAUSED BY LEISURE NOISE In the mainstream media, an increase in prevalence of NIHL in adolescents and young adults attributed to leisure noise exposure is postulated. General statements regarding hearing loss in the youngest generation are not uncommon (‗more young people lose hearing‘, ‗the deaf MP3 generation‘, etc.), but are usually based on possibilities or estimations. For example, in the United States, it was estimated that non-occupational noise exposure probably has a similar impact on the burden of adult hearing loss as occupational noise exposure which represents 5 to 10% of the burden [Dobie, 2008]. Two types of epidemiologic research can provide firm evidence; either cross-sectional studies or longitudinal data. However, there are methodological difficulties in the accurate estimation of the number of subjects exposed, and in obtaining a sample of young individuals with representative sound levels, patterns and duration of exposure [MRC Institute of Hearing Research, 1986]. Furthermore, the criteria used to define hearing impairment, and the used audiological techniques to measure slight deterioration in hearing, as well as numerous confounding factors in lifestyle regarding noise exposure (e.g. environmental noise) make the design of such research complicated. Finally, longitudinal studies require an adequate longterm planning, as well as identical methodological conditions. Research regarding the prevalence of NIHL caused by leisure noise has revealed inconsistent results. There are some adequately performed studies that do not report an increase in prevalence of hearing loss caused by leisure noise.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

85

First, the prevalence of hearing loss in 500 18-year-old males was 14%, which was comparable with the prevalence of hearing loss in the same age group in previous studies [Axelsson et al, 1994]. In 1998, the prevalence of hearing loss in 951 Swedish 18-year-old conscripts was estimated at 19.6% [Augustsson & Engstrand, 2006]. However, taking different techniques into account, the authors concluded that there is no increase in prevalence as compared with 30 years earlier [Persson et al, 1993]. Second, the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANE III) [Niskar et al, 1998; Niskar et al, 2001], performed between 1988 and 1994, estimated the prevalence of a noise-induced threshold shift in children from 6 to 19 year old (n=5249). A noise-induced hearing threshold shift was defined as: (1) pure-tone thresholds at 0.5 and 1.0 kHz equal or better than 15 dB HL, (2) a difference of at least 15 dB between the poorest pure-tone threshold at 3.0, 4.0 or 6.0 kHz on the one hand and at 0.5 and 1.0 kHz on the other hand, and finally, (3) pure tone threshold at 8.0 kHz at least 10 dB better than the poorest threshold at 3.0, 4.0 and 6.0 kHz. It was stated that the prevalence of a noise-induced hearing threshold shift in at least one ear amounted to 12.5%, representing approximately 5.2 million children. In those children, the prevalence of noise-induced threshold shift was significantly higher in boys (14.8%) than in girls (10.1%). Further, it was observed that 12- to 19-year old children had a significantly higher prevalence of noise-induced threshold shift (15.5%) than 6- to 11-year-olds (8.5%). However, the median hearing thresholds of this oldest group of children can be compared with those of children aged 12 to 17 years from the Health Examination Survey of 1966 to 1970 [Holmes et al, 2004]. All median thresholds except at 1 kHz were better in children from the NHANE III, indicating better hearing conservation over the past 20 years. More importantly, the largest improvement was at frequencies 4 and 6 kHz, suggesting a decrease in hearing loss suggestive of noise exposure. More recently, the prevalence of hearing loss was examined in US adults from 17 to 25 years entering an industrial workforce [Rabinowitz et al, 2006]. The criteria for an audiometric notch were defined identically as the noise-induced hearing threshold shifts by Niskar et al (2001). The prevalence of audiometric notches was 23.2% for the new-hires between 1985 and 1989, 17.2% between 1990 and 1994, 19.3% between 1995 and 1999, and 20.4% between 2000 and 2004. So, no increasing prevalence of audiometric notches, nor high frequency hearing loss (average hearing loss at 3, 4 and 6 kHz greater than 15 dB) was reported for hearing tests performed between 1985 and 2004. There are, however, other studies reporting an increase in prevalence of hearing loss caused by leisure noise. Montgomery & Fujikawa (1992) evaluated hearing thresholds in almost 1500 students from second, eight and twelfth grade. The prevalence of hearing loss of second graders and eighth graders had increased compared to the prevalence of hearing loss ten years ago [Montgomery & Fujikawa, 1992]. However, the history of noise exposure was not investigated, and could therefore not been correlated with hearing loss. Similarly, a higher prevalence of impaired hearing, typically at high frequencies, was observed in children who started school in 1987 and 1997 than those who started in 1977 [Gissel et al, 2002]. Further, in an interdisciplinary long-term study the effects of leisure noise exposure on hearing were yearly evaluated during a four-year period in adolescents (aged 14 to 17 years) [Biassoni et al, 2005]. During the third year, adolescents were divided into two subgroups based on hearing threshold shifts: small versus larger shifts. The ears of adolescents in these subgroups were labeled as ‗tough ears‘ or ‗tender ears‘, respectively. The authors concluded

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

86

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

that leisure activities associated with both subgroups could result in permanent hearing loss in adolescents with tender ears, but does not always cause hearing damage in tough ears. Thus, literature is so far not conclusive concerning the possible increase in prevalence of NIHL caused by leisure noise. An extensive amount of literature investigated the relation between hearing loss and leisure noise exposure. Some found a high-frequency deterioration of hearing which was attributed to noise during leisure time [Litke, 1971; Lipscomb, 1972; Lees et al, 1985; Spaeth et al, 1993]. However, others found no or only slight correlation between hearing loss and leisure time noise exposure [Hanson & Fearn, 1975; Strauss et al, 1977; Axelsson et al, 1981a; Axelsson et al, 1981b; Carter et al, 1982; Carter et al, 1984; Bradley et al, 1987; Lindeman et al, 1987; Mercier & Hohmann, 2002]. A review by Zenner et al (1999), translated in Maassen et al (2001), indicated that the reason no correlation between music exposure and hearing loss was found, is based on methodological shortcomings such as averaging music exposure without dividing in subgroups with extreme exposure, testing non-suitable subgroups or no extreme groups with substantial (life time) exposure, and failing to obtain a control group without any music exposure. Furthermore, these authors stated: ―We can conclude from the cited studies that the noise induced PTS, which has to be expected according to ISO 1999, has been sufficiently demonstrated empirically, and that there is a considerable risk for ear damage resulting from electronically amplified music.‖,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

and they further estimated that: ―If the reported music listening habits of the 15 year olds are constant for 10 years, it can be expected that this will result in 10% of young Germans having an average hearing loss of 10 dB or more at 3 kHz. To this music-induce hearing loss, a further 10 dB hearing loss has to be added in 25 year olds due to age and therefore it can be expected that 10% of the 25 year olds have hearing thresholds above 20 dB at 3 kHz.‖ [Zenner et al, 1999; Maassen et al, 2001].

Thus, it seems that the majority of properly designed research was not able to demonstrate a clear impact of leisure noise on auditory function. The most likely explanation for this lack of hearing deterioration could be that leisure noise exposure is insufficient to cause widespread hearing loss. It is plausible that the pattern of exposure to leisure noise is less frequent compared to occupational noise exposure, and attributes only for a small period in life probably between five and ten years [Hetu & Fortin, 1995; Axelsson & Prasher, 1999; Smith et al, 2000]. Moreover, the individual behavior regarding use of hearing protectors might have changed [Rabinowitz et al, 2006] and/or there might be an alteration in noise exposure habits [Mostafapour et al, 1998] due to greater public awareness of the potential harmful effects of leisure noise exposure. However, it is also suggested that it is too soon to detect permanent effects of recent advances in technology [Morata, 2007], such as MP3 players. Finally, pure-tone audiometry is possibly not the most sensitive technique to detect subtle cochlear changes, and other measures – high-frequency audiometry, high-definition audiometry and OAEs – might reveal subclinical cochlear damage. For example, significant weakening of TEOAEs was found in adolescents more exposed to amplified music than the less exposed adolescents [Mansfield et al, 1999].

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

87

Besides hearing loss, there is a high prevalence of tinnitus reported in individuals with occupational NIHL [Axelsson & Prasher, 2000]. While there seems to be an increased awareness of noise exposure in occupational settings, it is plausible that the prevalence of tinnitus due to leisure noise exposure is on the rise. In subjects reporting tinnitus, the level of social noise exposure determines the audiological characteristics as measured with pure-tone audiometry, speech-in-noise test, and otoacoustic emissions [Davis et al, 1998]. Also, it was found that 8.7% of 1285 Swedish participants between 13 and 19 years reported tinnitus, and 17.1% reported noise sensitivity [Widen & Erlandsson, 2004]. Both symptoms were more common in older than younger adolescents which could be explained by the longer period of leisure noise exposure. Thus, subjective audiological symptoms including tinnitus and hypersensitivity to sound should be considered in the evaluation of temporary or permanent thresholds shifts because it might indicate subtle cochlear changes [Schmuziger et al, 2006]. Tinnitus is also found to be a more suffering symptom than hearing loss [Metternich & Brusis, 1999; Axelsson & Prasher, 1999]. Therefore, more research is needed to estimate the prevalence of non-occupational noise-induced permanent tinnitus. In summary, there is a large amount of research concerning the impact of leisure noise exposure on hearing, especially during music-related activities. However, the majority of these studies are characterized with poor methodology which leads to inconsistent results regarding a correlation between these aforementioned activities and hearing damage, as well as an assumed increase in prevalence of hearing loss caused by these activities. Nevertheless, in our opinion, as long as firm evidence based on adequately executed research is lacking, caution is necessary. As such, more research is urgently needed to gain more insight.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

LEISURE NOISE ACTIVITIES Generally, leisure noise exposure can be categorized into exposure to loud music or participation in non-musical activities. The major sources of music-related leisure noise are: (1) using personal music players (PMPs), (2) attendance at nightclubs or discotheques, (3) attendance at live concerts, (4) listening to home stereo/ radio‘s, and (5) playing a musical instrument, playing in a band or an orchestra [MRC Institute of Hearing Research, 1986; Clark, 1991; Meyer-Bisch, 1996; Jokitulppo et al, 1997; Mansfield et al, 1999; Smith et al, 2000; Jokitulppo & Bjork, 2002; Jokitulppo et al, 2006]. Loud music listening is possible in almost every surroundings; associated with activities at home, while driving or traveling by bike, bus, train or underground [Rice et al, 1987a; Wong et al, 1990]. Smith et al (2000) studied the prevalence and type of social noise in England in 18- to 25year-olds. It was found that the prevalence of leisure noise since the early 1980s has tripled from 6.7% to 18.8%, whereas occupational noise has decreased from 8.9% to 3.5%. About 11% received significant noise exposure from nightclubs, 3% from Hi-fis, 2% from PMPs, and 0.6% from attendance at live concerts. Significant noise exposure was defined as a situation in which raised voices between two normal hearing people four feet apart holding an conversation are needed [Smith et al, 2000]. Jokitulppo & Björk (2002) investigated the noisy leisure-time of Finnish adults (25-58 years) and found that 58% listened to a home stereo/radio, 34% went to nightclubs/pubs, 9% used a portable stereo player, 7% attended at live concerts, and only a small amount played a musical instrument (7%), in a band (1%) or

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

88

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

orchestra (1%) [Jokitulppo & Bjork, 2002]. In Argentina, participation of adolescents in musical leisure activities was found highest for attendance at discotheques, followed by attendance at live concerts, usage of PMPs and playing musical instruments [Serra et al, 2005]. Thus, the contribution of PMPs to the total social noise exposure seems to be less than the attendance of discotheques or nightclubs. Recently however, it was found that more than 90% of 1016 American students listened to some type of personal music system through earphones [Torre, III, 2008]. It seems plausible that the technical evolution since the introduction of the Sony Walkman to the MP3 players has contributed to the current popularity of PMPs. There has been a miniaturization of the devices, an improvement of the storage and battery capacity, and online availability of music and podcasts. Thus, it is not known if findings from other PMPs – personal cassette players (PCPs) or Compact Disc players (CD players) - apply directly to MP3 player use [Vogel et al, 2007]. For example, there was a 5 dB increase in averaged levels between CD players and MP3 players using the same measurement technique [Keith et al, 2008]. Therefore, the effects of PMPs and especially MP3 players on the auditory system of young people is a cause for concern. This concern is based on the close coupling of headphones to the tympanic membrane, the ability of such devices to generate high maximum output levels, and uncertainties regarding listening habits. Besides these music-related leisure noise sources, there are other sources of leisure noise such as attending or participating in (motor) sport events, watching movies or going to the theatre, using home tools, shooting firearms, use of fireworks, and other noisy toys. Participation in non-musical activities is, however, less common than in musical activities [Biassoni et al, 2005], and the highest participation in non-musical activities is watching movies or going to the theatre (26%), and using home tools indoors (19%) [Jokitulppo & Bjork, 2002].

1. Personal Music Players 1.1. Listening habits There is a large variation in listening habits of adolescents using PMPs, between studies, as well as within studies. This variation is seen in duration of use (years), listening time (hours per week) and listening levels. It can largely be explained by the studied population; either no representative population was studied [e.g. Williams, 2005], or there were differences in sociodemographic characteristics in subjects between studies. Moreover, differences in questions regarding using PMPs explain the inconsistencies between studies. For example, the listening time is sometimes referred to as listening during one single session [Zogby, 2006], or for a full day [Torre, III, 2008]. Thereby, adequate comparisons in listening habits between studies are difficult. With consideration of these shortcomings, there are, however, some tendencies regarding listening habits with respect of gender and age. First, females spend less time listening to PMPs [Catalano & Levin, 1985; Rice et al, 1987a; Meyer-Bisch, 1996; Torre, III, 2008], and prefer to listen at less intensive output levels [Catalano & Levin, 1985; Smith et al, 2000; Williams, 2005; Torre, III, 2008]. Catalano & Levin (1985) found that females listened on average 8.04 hours per week, whereas the average for males was 13.97 hours per week. Rice et al (1987a) reported that

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

89

males listened on average about one hour per week more to their personal cassette players than females. Moreover, there was a higher percentage of females only occasionally listening to PCPs (17.3%) than males (10.9%) [Meyer-Bisch, 1996]. Further, it was found that 25.2% of the females used a PCP for at least two hours per week, whereas in males this was 37.6%. Recently, Torre (2008) reported that 11.2% more females listened to PMPs less than one hour per day. In contrast, 5.4% more males listened between one and three hours, 4.6% more males between three and five hours, and finally, 1.2% more males listened longer than five hours per day as compared to females. On average, males set their personal cassette player 7.5 dB higher than females [Smith et al, 2000]. The mean 8 hour, A-weighted equivalent continuous noise level was 75.3 dB for the females, and 80.6 dB for the males [Williams, 2005]. Also, medium volume setting of PMPs was preferred for most women, whereas men were more likely to listen at a very loud listening level [Torre, III, 2008]. Second, listening habits tend to changes during lifetime [Fearn & Hanson, 1984; West & Evans, 1990; Smith et al, 2000; Meyer-Bisch, 1996; Zogby, 2006; Zenner et al, 1999; Maassen et al, 2001]. The percentage of young people listening through headphones increased from 10%, 12% and 35% for the age groups 9 to 12 years, 13 to 16 years and 18 to 25 years, respectively [Fearn & Hanson, 1984]. In contrast, the use of PMPs decreased from 67% in younger (15-16 years) to 39% in older (19-23 years) exposed adolescents [West & Evans, 1990]. Moreover, the most frequent use of personal cassette player was reported to range between 13 and 19 years [Smith et al, 2000], and Meyer-Bisch (1996) mentioned less often usage of personal cassette players after the age of 28. The percentage of high school (14-18 years) students using an Apple iPod or another brand of MP3 player was three times higher than for adults (18-+70 years), indicating that usage declines with increasing age [Zogby, 2006]. Median PMP listening duration increased from 16 years, with a median of approximately one hour 20 minutes at the age of 19 years [Zenner et al, 1999; Maassen et al, 2001]. In 2006, the length of a typical session of PMP usage was reported to range between one and four hours for a larger amount of adults, and between 30 minutes and one hour for students [Zogby, 2006]. Median listening levels were above 90 dBA between the ages of 13 and 16 years, reaching a median level of almost 100 dBA at 14 years [Zenner et al, 1999; Maassen et al, 2001]. Moreover, the typical volume setting for a student listening to a PMP was more often loud, somewhat loud or very loud, whereas adults listen to PMPs more at medium, somewhat low, low or very low volume settings [Zogby, 2006]. Besides gender and age differences in the output level of PMPs, the preferred listening levels of these devices depend on several other factors, such as the presence of background noise, the type of music, the earphone style, and the type of personal music device. First, it was found that the presence of background noise increases the preferred listening levels. The results depend on whether the measurements were performed in laboratory or field conditions. For example, Rice et al (1987b) found a signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of approximately 5 dB in laboratory conditions, whereas in the field study no correlation was seen between listening levels and background traffic noise. In contrast, the preferred listening levels in laboratory conditions had on average a signal-to-noise ratio of 12 dB, and in the field condition 17 dB [Airo et al, 1996]. Recently, the mean SNR in 55 subjects ranging in age from 15 to 48 years was about 13 dB measured at public areas [Williams, 2005]. Finally, the

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

90

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

increase in preferred listening levels of an MP3 player in 38 subjects from 20 to 36 years was on average 9.4 dB higher for street noise and 7.7 dB higher for multi-talker babble, as compared to the quiet condition [Hodgetts et al, 2007]. Thus, it is important to evaluate the preferred listening levels of PMPs in the presence of background levels as these potentially result in a higher risk to hearing-related symptoms. Furthermore, increasing the listening levels might pose users at risk by decreasing their ability to hear warning signals. As such, adolescents should be advised to adjust the volume control of PMPs in quiet conditions. Second, the preferred listening levels might be dependent on the type of music. One study evaluated the preferred listening levels for four music samples and found a difference of 10 dB between the quietest and loudest average listening levels in 14 subjects (16-26 years) [Airo et al, 1996]. Maximum output levels for different types of music were investigated by Turunen-Rise et al (1991) and more extensively by Fligor & Cox (2004). In the former study, sound pressure levels ranged from 100 to 108 dBA for pop music, 103 dBA for light classical music, and 98 dBA for classical music [Turunen-Rise et al, 1991]. Fligor & Cox (2004) measured maximum output levels of CD players for eight different styles of music relative to white noise. Country, rock and adult contemporary music samples were similar to the output levels measured with white noise. However, the output levels of the other music samples were overestimated by the white noise output levels. These overestimations were 2.5 dB, 3.9 dB, 4.9 dB, 5.6 dB and 12 dB for rap/ R&B, pop music, classical music, dance music and jazz, respectively. Moreover, the temporal pattern of these genres were evaluated. It was noticed that the highest peak SPLs were seen during the 10-sec rock and pop music sample, whereas the highest continuous 10-sec average SPL was during the rock sample. Further, dance music revealed the highest number of peaks in the 10-sec sample within 3 dB of the highest peak, and the temporal pattern of the jazz music sample was least pronounced. The number of transients during the 10-sec dance music sample ranged from 28 to 60 depending on the combination CD player and headphone [Fligor & Cox, 2004]. Third, the earphone style determines the preferred listening levels of PMPs. Preferred listening levels were on average 1.3 dB higher for the supra-aural earphones than with semiaural earphones [Airo et al, 1996]. Further, significant lower preferred listening levels were chosen for the over-the-ear headphone as compared to earbuds [Hodgetts et al, 2007]. This difference was 2.6 dB in quiet, 4.3 dB in presence of street noise and 3.8 dB with multi-talker babble. Also, it was reported that earbuds produce significant higher maximum output levels than supra-aural headphones, up to 7 dB [Fligor and Cox 2004]. MacLean et al (1992) found a 6.2 dB higher output level for the earbuds than for the supra-aural headphones. The differences might be explained by the physical coupling of the headphones, as well as their differences in operating characteristics [MacLean et al, 1992]. Finally, outputs level of a tight fit of earbuds and supra-aural headphones was on average 16 dB higher than those of a loose fitting [Keith et al, 2008]. Thus, the coupling of the ear- and headphone, as well as the geometry of the ear defines the noise exposure using PMPs.

1.2. Short- and long term auditory effects Generally, temporary reduction in hearing sensitivity may be the result of listening to high output levels of PMPs during a short period (hours), while permanent hearing deterioration may be caused by listening to PMPs during a much longer period (years), even at lower listening levels. As mentioned previously, the exact relationship between TTS and

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

91

PTS is uncertain. However, it is assumed that in the long term temporary hearing shifts may result in permanent hearing reduction. Short-term effects of listening to PMPs were investigated measuring hearing thresholds, and recently, also by OAEs before and after a listening session. Lee et al (1985) found significant TTSs of 10 dB in 6 of the 16 subjects, as well as a TTS of approximately 30 dB in one subject after listening to three-hour during music with output levels ranging from 80 to 104 dB SPL [Lee et al, 1985]. Turunen-Rise et al (1991) described median TTSs of less than 12 dB after one hour listening to two types of pop music at a preset listening level between 85 and 95 dBA in six subjects [Turunen-Rise et al, 1991]. Hellström (1991) evaluated TTSs after exposure to one hour modern music via PMPs at loud, but comfortable volume. The mean listening levels ranged from 91 to 97 dBA with mean TTS from 4.01 to 5.74 dB [Hellstrom, 1991]. Loth et al (1992) found an average TTS of 5 dB in 12 subjects after one-hour at preferred equivalent levels between 89 and 94 dBA [Loth et al, 1992]. Pugsley et al (1993) reported no significant larger deterioration in hearing after listening to a PMP during one hour in 30 subjects than in the control group of 15 subjects [Pugsley et al, 1993]. Hellström et al (1998) exposed 21 subjects to music via PCP at a loud, but comfortable level. Subjects were divided in three groups depending on their listening pattern. The average equivalent listening levels were 91.4 dBA, 91.9 dBA and 97.1 dBA with mean TTSs 15.7 dB, 4.5 dB and 4.0 dB, respectively [Hellstrom et al, 1998]. Recently, Bhagat & Davis (2008) measured hearing thresholds, DPOAEs and synchronized SOAEs (SSOAEs) in 20 subjects after listening to music during 30 minutes at a preset listening level. There were no significant differences in hearing thresholds before and after music exposure. There were, however, significant reductions in DPOAE half-octave band levels centered from 1.4 to 6.0 kHz, but variable shifts in frequency and level of SSOAEs [Bhagat & Davis, 2008]. From these TTS studies, we may be conclude that there are only slight, if any, reductions in hearing thresholds after listening to PMPs, but changes in OAEs might be early warning signs of the harmful effects of high levels of music exposure. There are however some shortcomings, as well as considerable variability in literature regarding temporary hearing damage after listening to PMPs. First, there is only one study [Pugsley et al, 1993] which included a control group to evaluate the threshold changes in a experimental group based on the test-retest reliability of hearing thresholds in a control group. Second, Lee et al (1985) considered thresholds shifts of at least 10 dB to be significant while others only reported the mean or median TTS [Hellstrom, 1991; Turunen-Rise et al, 1991; Hellstrom et al, 1998]. Third, there are methodological differences in music exposure, as well as in the determination of output levels. The music exposure ranged from one hour to three hours with different genres of music: with rock/fusion music [Lee et al, 1985], pop music [Turunen-Rise et al, 1991], contemporary music [Pugsley et al, 1993] or the favorite music cassette of the subjects [Hellstrom, 1991; Hellstrom et al, 1998]. Mainly, user-preferred listening levels were used with either standard headphones [Hellstrom, 1991; Turunen-Rise et al, 1991; Hellstrom et al, 1998] or user-preferred headphones [Lee et al, 1985]. However, one study [Pugsley et al, 1993] did not perform output measures to correlate with the results from the hearing test. The output levels were measured on an artificial ear with coupler [Lee et al, 1985], on KEMAR [Turunen-Rise et al, 1991], or via a miniature microphone in the external ear canal [Hellstrom, 1991; Hellstrom et al, 1998]. There are also several studies investigating the permanent auditory effects caused by long-term listening to PMPs. Wong et al (1990) found no differences in mean hearing

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

92

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

thresholds between 78 young PCP users and 25 non-users [Wong et al, 1990]. However, the half-octave frequencies 3.0 and 6.0 kHz were not tested. Meyer-Bisch (1996) compared high definition audiometry in three groups. There were significant poorer hearing thresholds in the most exposed PCP group (n=54, > 7hrs/wk) compared to the controls (n=358). No significant differences were seen between the less exposed PCP group (n=195, 2-7 hrs/wk) and the control group. There was also twice more subjective auditory suffering - presence of tinnitus and/or hearing fatigue - in the total PCP group than in the controls [Meyer-Bisch, 1996]. LePage & Murray (1998) noticed a decline in click-evoked OAEs significantly proportional with increasing PCP listening time from less than one hour to more than six hours per week [LePage & Murray, 1998]. Mostafapour et al (1998) evaluated hearing thresholds, speech reception threshold and speech discrimination in 50 subjects with estimated lifetime exposure of PMPs low, medium or high. No significant differences between the three groups were found [Mostafapour et al, 1998]. Peng et al (2007) found significant differences in hearing thresholds at conventional and extended high-frequencies between 120 PMP users and 30 controls, but no significant differences between the three subgroups of PMP use (one to three years, three to five years, longer than five years) were seen. Moreover, the hearing thresholds at extended high-frequencies of normal-hearing subjects at conventional frequencies were significantly higher than those of the control group. Therefore, the authors concluded that long-term use of PMPs could induce NIHL, and extended high-frequency audiometry is more sensitive than audiometry at conventional frequencies [Peng et al, 2007]. Montoya et al (2008) found a reduction of TEOAE and DPOAE incidence and amplitudes, as well as an increase in DPOAE thresholds with longer duration of listening to PMPs in years, and for more hours per week [Montoya et al, 2008]. The results of the aforementioned studies might indicate that with more extensive use of PMPs, hearing deteriorates. However, several shortcomings in these studies can be pointed out. First, in some studies [LePage & Murray, 1998; Peng et al, 2007], there was no consideration of other sources of leisure noise. So, it is impossible to distinguish hearing damage caused by PMPs from deterioration caused by several confounding sources of leisure noise. Second, mostly either the duration of use in years [Peng et al, 2007], or the use per week [Meyer-Bisch, 1996; LePage & Murray, 1998] of PMPs was investigated. In our opinion, the lifetime noise exposure from PMPs must be evaluated encompassing both parameters. Third, subjects were not always representative for the population [Wong et al, 1990; Mostafapour et al, 1998]. It is possible that with a more representative group, more extreme listening habits appear. Finally, the mentioned listening habits are based on a retrospective estimation by the subjects which could lead to errors in the quantification of PMP usage. Previously, risk assessments were established, and ranged from 0.065 to 30% [Catalano & Levin, 1985; MRC Institute of Hearing Research, 1986; Rice et al, 1987a; Clark, 1991; Ising et al, 1994; Ising et al, 1995; Passchier-Vermeer, 1999; Williams, 2005]. Moreover, PMPs are supplementary sound sources and might accumulate with other noise exposure [Meyer-Bisch, 1996]. The variability in risk assessment could be explained by the definition for NIHL and the damage-risk criteria for hearing loss, which are directly adopted from occupational settings. According to some authors, extrapolation of occupational risk criteria to leisure noise exposures is not justified because of the differences in spectral and temporal pattern between industrial noise and music [Turunen-Rise et al 1991; Mostafapour et al 1998; Metternich & Brusis, 1999; Smith et al 2000; Fligor and Cox 2004]. Amplified music

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

93

emphasizes low-frequencies, and is characterized with transients superimposed on a relatively steady-state signal [Peng et al, 2007]. As such, it can be assumed that A-weighted sound levels underestimate the low-frequency contribution, and C-weighted sound levels could be proposed. However, industrial criteria are usually expressed in dBA. Moreover, it was also found that aversive sounds – noise or disliked music - induced larger TTSs than enjoyable sounds [Lindgren & Axelsson, 1983; Swanson et al, 1987]. Nevertheless, the regulation for occupational noise exposure is based on the exposure level and duration of noise exposure which also determines leisure noise exposure and there are currently no criteria available for this latter source. Thus, the criteria adopted from occupational noise exposure can be applied to evaluate the impact of leisure noise, although this must be done with caution. Therefore, users should be warned for the gradual and insidious development of hearing loss associated with long-term listening to PMPs at high intensity levels [Smith et al, 2000].

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

2. Attendance of Discotheques, Nightclubs or Live Concerts 2.1. Attendance habits Generally, it is assumed that discotheques or nightclubs have a larger impact on young people‘s hearing than concerts. The noise exposure in the latter can be louder, but are attended less frequently and lead to less overall noise immission [Smith et al, 2000; Serra et al, 2005]. In 700 adolescents ranging in age from 16 to 25 years, attending discotheques regularly was found to be 79%, whereas attendance at pop and rock concerts and techno parties was 53% and 36% respectively [Mercier & Hohmann, 2002]. The average hours per week in 1323 adolescents (age range 28-58 years) was 4.4 for nightclubs/ pubs and 2.4 for concerts [Jokitulppo et al, 2006]. An average of 6.2 hours per week was determined for discotheque attendance [Zenner et al, 1999; Maassen et al, 2001]. Some tendencies regarding gender and age differences can be found in discotheque, nightclubs or concert attendance. First, more males (91%) attended nightclubs than females (84%) which was investigated in 494 university students [Meecham & Hume, 2001]. MeyerBisch (1996) also found more occasionally discotheque attendance in females, and more frequent attendance (at least twice a month) by males. Further, a higher proportion of males regularly (once a month) or intensively (twice a month) attended concerts. Second, attending discotheques and concerts was reported more often in older exposed (19-23 years) than younger exposed (15-16 years) adolescents [West & Evans, 1990]. Moreover, the highest proportion of attendance was at 21-22 years, and rare after 28 years [Meyer-Bisch, 1996]. It was reported that the highest median visits per month was at the age of 17-18 years, with a decreasing trend from 22 years [Zenner et al, 1999; Maassen et al, 2001]. However, the more extreme noise exposure habits indicate that visits of more than twice a month was exceeded at the age of 13 years, with the highest 10% value of approximately seven visits a month at 19 years. The mean of sound levels, measured in discotheques and rock concerts was 103.4 dBA [Clark, 1991]. Smith et al (2000) reported sound levels in nightclubs and discotheques ranging from 85 to 105 dBA with a mean of 101 dBA. Another study found sound levels in nightclubs in the range of 97 to 106 dBA [Meecham & Hume, 2001]. Factors influencing these sound levels are: measurement position, time in the evening, variations between clubs, as well as between evenings in the same club [Ising, 1994; Smith et al, 2000; Meecham &

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

94

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Hume, 2001]. Recently, average sound levels for concerts ranged from 95 to 102 dBA for pop concerts, 97 to 103 dBA for heavy metal concerts, and 96 to 107 dBA for rock concerts with maximum sound levels 126, 113 and 118 dBA, respectively [Opperman et al, 2006]. So, it is clearly a misconception that some music genres, especially rock and heavy metal concerts, are louder than pop concerts. As in listening habits of PMPs, it is difficult to establish general attendance habits in a representative sample of adolescents. Moreover, there are only few studies addressing the sound levels with respect of the influencing parameters.

2.2. Short- and long term effects Clark (1991) reported that research in the seventies and eighties regarding temporary auditory effects indicate moderate TTS up to 30 dB recovering within hours to days after noise exposure [Clark, 1991]. For example, Axelsson & Lindgren (1978) found mean TTSs at 4 kHz of approximately 12 dB in listeners, and 10 dB in pop musicians. TTSs amounted up to 45 dB HL [Axelsson & Lindgren, 1978]. More recently, in a small population (n=22) TTSs after rock concerts were found to be 10.9, 9.8 and 20.9 dB depending on the position in the arena with respective LAeq of 89.29, 99.6 and 101.3 dBA [Yassi et al, 1993]. Peak levels were almost 140 dBA which is assumed to cause irreversible acoustic trauma due to impulse noise. Sixty percent of the subjects assessed the sound levels as too loud or intolerable. However, only slightly more than half the subjects were aware of their TTSs which is a cause for concern and emphasizes the insidious nature of NIHL. The average TTS after attending discotheques increased from 6.2 dB to 10.01 dB with increasing attendance from one to two hours [Liebel et al, 1996]. Significant effects on TEOAE reproducibility and amplitudes were also found, but not on DPOAEs. Therefore, OAEs were regarded as not ideal measures of hearing shifts after noise exposure. Opperman et al (2006) found that largest mean hearing shifts were located at 3 and 4 kHz. Significant hearing shifts, according to OSHA and ASHA criteria, were present in 64% of the subjects wearing no earplugs, whereas in 27% wearing earplugs. This might be explained by the fact that the real-world attenuation of hearing protection devices is less than theoretically established mainly by the fitting of the earplugs into ear canal. Therefore, it is important to instruct adolescents correctly wearing earplugs. The permanent auditory effects of concert and discotheque attendance in adolescents can be considered more dangerous than the use of PMPs [Meyer-Bisch, 1996; Mercier & Hohmann, 2002; Biassoni et al, 2005]. However, only few studies evaluated the long-term auditory effects of attending discotheques, nightclubs or concerts. Meyer-Bisch (1996) noticed a significant reduction in hearing thresholds, especially at 4 kHz, in the concert group as compared to the control group which never or only occasionally used a PMP and went only rarely to concerts or discotheques. No significant differences in hearing thresholds between the discotheque attendees and control group could be established. However, there was three times more auditory suffering in the discotheque group than the controls, whereas in the concert group this was four times higher than in the control group. Meecham and Hume (2001) reported no significant association between attendance of nightclubs and occurrence of post exposure tinnitus, but they indicate that attendance increased the risk of and reduced recovery from tinnitus after noise exposure.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

95

3. Conclusion It can be concluded that there is a large variation in listening and attendance habits of the most popular music-related leisure noise activities. Gender and age differences are two major sociodemographic factors partially explaining the variation in habits. More research is needed to extensively evaluate the type of leisure noise activities, as well as listening or attendance habits in a representative sample of adolescents. TTS studies indicate that there are only slight reductions in hearing thresholds after listening to PMPs, but more damage seem to be present after attending discotheques, nightclubs or live concerts. However, more sensitive methods to evaluate minimal cochlear changes should be incorporated in future TTS studies with consideration of the mentioned shortcomings of the existing studies. Furthermore, PTS studies indicate that visiting discotheques, nightclubs or concerts poses more risk to hearing than listening to PMPs. The measured sound pressures levels indicate theoretically that frequently attending these activities might cause hearing loss. As previously mentioned however, a clear association between hearing loss and attendance of these activities has not been proven yet, and more research is needed addressing hearing loss in young people with more extensive listening habits for multiple sources of leisure noise.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

PREVENTION OF NOISE-INDUCED HEARING LOSS CAUSED BY LEISURE NOISE It is currently inconceivable that several parties would take place without music exposure. However, it is remarkable that young people tend to listen to music in almost every environment, individually using their PMPs, as in groups in discotheques, nightclubs, concerts festivals etc. Plath (1998) describes this phenomenon as the fear of silence, as well as the reduction of unwanted background noise by listening to music [Plath, 1998a; Plath, 1998b]. However, adolescents extent this behavior to excessive music-listening in which music has to be felt, consistent with exuberant behavior [Clark, 1991]. Zenner et al (1999) and Maassen et al (2001) mentioned that such behavior compensates for the frustration and problems experienced by adolescents. Florentine et al (1998) stated that music-listening behavior is sometimes associated with dependency-like disorders [Florentine et al, 1998], and recently, it was found that there was a correlation between general risk behavior such as smoking, drinking and leaving school on the one hand, and risk behavior regarding loud music on the other hand [Bohlin & Erlandsson, 2007]. Hetu & Fortin (1995) analyzed the temporal and spectral features of discotheque music, especially house music. It was postulated that the pulsating character of house music influences cardiac activity, and thus the general level of arousal. Moreover, low-frequency emphasizing maximizes beat perception, mid-frequency reduction prevents annoyance by permitting communication, and high-frequency enhancement allows perception of highpitched sounds to stress the beat. Furthermore, these authors described the phenomenology of music-listening explaining the possessing of music. First, penetration of the music environment is limited due to the high sound pressure levels, as well as uninterrupted nature of discotheque or nightclub music (confinement). Second, listeners are enclosed in a musical

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

96

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

field shared by other listeners (immersion). Third, music is heard without any focus of attention needed; communication is hardly impossible (passive hearing). Finally, the music and the light show are responsible for auditory, vestibular and proprioceptive sensations (excitement) [Hetu & Fortin, 1995]. The opposite of such excessive social lifestyle is presented when adolescents experience a hearing loss which affects their quality of life [Arlinger, 2003]. The handicap of NIHL in adolescents has an impact on the individual in interactions, but also in their broader communications with family and friends, as well as on the society. Their educational achievement is hampered, and eventually also their employability. In Argentina, preemployment medical examination is failed in a high percentage of young people because of hearing loss [Serra et al, 2005]. Therefore, it is important to prevent NIHL, in occupational settings, but also during leisure activities. Preventive strategies should incorporate a two-layered approach. First, by educating the adolescents and second, by protective measures such as legislation. It was reported that not only adolescents, but several other parties are involved in the prevention of NIHL. To prevent NIHL caused by discotheque attendance, the authorities, discotheque owners and decorators, as well as disc-jockeys are possible responsible [Vogel et al, 2009a]. Preventing NIHL due to PMP-listening, manufacturers of earphones and MP3 players, authorities, music industry, parents, media and community centers are potentially involved [Vogel et al, 2009b]. Improving the awareness and knowledge regarding the problems associated with excessive loud music-listening, consequences and preventive measures should be aimed for. Chung et al (2005) conducted a web-based survey, and found that only 8% of the 3310 respondents indicate hearing loss as a ‗very big problem‘ [Chung et al, 2005]. Among the seven health concerns – hearing loss, sexually transmitted diseases, alcohol or drug abuse, depression, smoking, nutrition and weight issues and acne – hearing loss was perceived as the least ‗very big problem‘. Further, others reported that knowledge regarding the irreversibility of NIHL was limited [Weichbold & Zorowka, 2002; Shah et al, 2009]. Others have found a greater awareness of the risks in their population [Crandell et al, 2004; Rawool & ColligonWayne, 2008]. However, since there is still a great deal of unawareness, or al lot of misconceptions regarding to NIHL caused by leisure noise, information is necessary. Hearing education campaigns improve the knowledge and awareness of NIHL, and improve motivation to protect the hearing [Becher et al, 1996]. However, it was found that the behavior of adolescents did not change: the frequency of discotheque attendance after a hearing education campaign even increased, the average duration of discotheque attendance, the use of hearing protection and their listening behavior using headphones was not influenced by the campaign [Weichbold & Zorowka, 2002; Weichbold & Zorowka, 2003; Weichbold & Zorowka, 2007]. Only the use of regeneration breaks at discotheques during which the ears can rest in a silent environment was increased. However, after the campaign, sound levels were more often judged as too loud, and the authors hypothesize that there might be less resistance for political action at that point. It is possible that behavioural changes in adolescents are not achieved because the consequences of their behavior, hearing loss, is not immediately perceived or not experienced as serious enough. Widen & Erlandsson (2004) found that subjects reporting tinnitus and noise sensitivity are more worried regarding hearing loss than those without symptoms, and were more likely to report the use of hearing protectors [Widen et al, 2006]. Moreover, subjects with permanent tinnitus assessed loud music-listening as more risky, and subjects

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

97

with only occasional tinnitus listened more often to loud music [Bohlin & Erlandsson, 2007]. It was found that, even after perceiving hearing loss and/or tinnitus, only 14% was willing to avoid noisy leisure activities, 86% was prepared to reduce their attendance at such activities, and 12% did not have any intention to change their behavior regarding music exposure. So, previous hearing-related symptoms do not necessarily implicate a more responsible behavior in the future. However, they sometimes trigger behavioral changes [Bogoch et al, 2005]. The high sound levels in discotheques, concerts and other parties are quite easily ascribed to the demands of the attendees by nightclub or discotheque owners and concert organizers. However, Mercier & Hohmann (2002) report that the sound levels are judged too high by 43%, 47% and 52% of the visitors in discotheques, concerts and techno parties, respectively. It was reported that sound levels in discotheques was at least sometimes perceived as too loud by over 60% of adolescents [Weichbold & Zorowka, 2002], and it should be more quiet by more than 40% [Weichbold & Zorowka, 2005]. Also, at festivals, the sound levels were too loud for 25% of the attendees [Mercier et al, 2003]. It was even found that 85% of adolescents would not change their attendance behavior even if sound levels in discotheques were lowered, and almost 10% would visit discotheques more often when the sound levels were turned down [Weichbold & Zorowka, 2005]. Therefore, besides hearing conservation campaigns, more effort should be undertaken from politicians to regulate the sound levels in various establishments, as well as supervise the compliance of the legislation. Vogel et al (2008) also states that MP3 players should be equipped with a clear indicator of the volume, as well as with a warning signal when a hazardous doses is reached [Vogel et al, 2008].

CASE STUDIES

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

1. Short-term Auditory Effects 1.1. Case one Case one represents a 26-year old male which participated at our current study on leisure noise exposure. He mentioned a five-hour attendance in a nightclub about nine hours before his visit. There was bilateral a normal otoscopy, type A tympanogram and present middle ear muscle reflexes at 1.0 kHz. Pure-tone audiometry and DPOAEs were performed, and are reflected in Figure 1 and 2, respectively. Pure-tone audiometry revealed normal hearing at the right ear, and a audiometric notch of 30 dBHL at 4 kHz at the left ear. DPOAEs evoked with L1/L2=65/55 dBSPL showed reduced emission amplitudes at the right ear and significantly deteriorated amplitudes at the left ear. 1.2. Case two Case two is a 24-year old male participating at the study regarding the short-term auditory effects of listening to an MP3 player during one hour. He listened to poprock music at a loud, but comfortable listening level. Pure-tone audiometry and DPOAEs (L1/L2=65/55 dBSPL) were measured before and after exposure. Normal otoscopy, type A tympanogram and normal middle ear muscle reflexes at 1.0 kHz were obtained before music exposure. Figure 3 and 4 show the results of the right ear for audiometry and DPOAEs, respectively, before and after the listening session. Temporary thresholds shifts varied between 5 and 10

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

98

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

dB, which can be regarded as the test-retest reliability of hearing thresholds. The emission amplitudes decreased considerably; the largest temporary emissions shifts were 4.4, 6.1 and 4.0 dB at frequency band with centre frequencies 4.0, 6.0 and 8.0 kHz.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 1. Hearing thresholds at octave frequencies from 250 to 8000 Hz, and half-octave frequencies 3000 and 6000 Hz for the right ear (circles) and left ear (crosses).

Figure 2. DPOAE amplitudes (grey lines) and noise amplitudes (shaded areas) from 841 to 8000 Hz for the right ear (right) and left ear (left).

Figure 3. Pure-tone audiometry of case number two. The solid line represents the pre-exposure measurement; the dashed line the post-exposure measurement of listening to a PMP for one hour. Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

99

Figure 4. DPOAE amplitudes (grey lines) and noise amplitudes (shaded areas) of case number two: preexposure measurement (right) and post-exposure measurement (left) of listening to a PMP for one hour.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

2. Long-term Auditory Effects 2.1. Case three Case three is a 22-year old male student participating at our current study regarding leisure noise exposure. There was a bilateral normal otoscopy, type A tympanogram and normal middle ear muscle reflexes at 1.0 kHz. Pure-tone audiometry was measured (Figure 5) and shows bilateral (quasi-)normal hearing thresholds at conventional frequencies. Extended high-frequency audiometry was performed at 10.0, 12.5 and 16.0 kHz. Hearing thresholds were lowered, with the largest hearing loss of 60 dBHL at 16.0 kHz for the right ear. Figure 6 represents the DPOAEs (L1/L2=65/55 dBSPL) and reduced emission amplitudes are observed at both ears, with the left ear more deteriorated than the right ear. His noise exposure history can be described as substantially, as reflected in Table 1. He listens to his PMPs at maximum volume setting, mostly while using public transportation. After attending a discotheque, he usually experiences a post-exposure tinnitus.

Figure 5. Reflected are the hearing thresholds at frequencies from 0.250 to 16.0 kHz of both ears (right ear: circles; left ear: crosses) of case number three. Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

100

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

Figure 6. DPOAE amplitudes (grey lines) and noise amplitudes (shaded areas) for both ears (right ear: right figure; lef ear: left figure) of case number three.

Table 1. Summary of the noise history of case number three. Leisure activity

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Listening to a PMP Listening to a stereo Attendance of discotheques or parties Attendance of live concerts Visiting a cinema or theater Attendance of sport events

Weekly or monthly exposure (hours) 9 hrs/wk 14 hrs/wk 15 hrs/wk 6 hrs/month 2 hrs/month 5 hrs/wk

Duration of exposure (years) 7 7 6 4 6 3

Figure 7. Pure-tone audiometry for case number four for the right ear (circles) and lef ear (crosses).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

101

2.2. Case four Case four is a male of 22 years old. He also voluntarily participated at a current study regarding leisure noise exposure. Since six years, he experiences a hearing loss and tinnitus at both ears. A high-frequency hearing loss, especially at 4 and 6 kHz, was confirmed by a ENT-specialist. He wears hearing protector devices rigorously, and a hearing test is performed yearly. At the time of testing, normal otoscopy, type A tympanogram and normal middle ear muscle reflexes at 1.0 kHz were obtained. Pure-tone audiometry (Figure 7) reveals bilateral high-frequency sensorineural hearing loss from 2.0 kHz at the left ear and 3.0 kHz at the right ear. DPOAEs (L1/L2=65/55 dBSPL) were measured and revealed absent emission amplitudes in the high-frequencies, and slightly more preserved emission amplitudes at the right ear (Figure 8).

3. Conclusion These case studies illustrate the short- and long-term auditory effects of leisure noise exposure. In cases with hearing loss as established with pure-tone audiometry, the emission amplitudes are also reduced. Moreover, in cases with audiometric normal hearing, emission amplitudes indicate cochlear damage. Therefore, it can be concluded that OAEs complement pure-tone audiometry in the detection of cochlear hearing loss caused by noise exposure and more specifically leisure noise exposure. The clinical utility of OAEs in the diagnosis of NIHL must be further explored.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

REGULATION There is increasing excessive noise exposure everywhere. Long-term exposure to high noise levels have caused moderate to severe hearing problems in millions of industrial workers [Dobie, 2008]. Moreover, recent studies of environmental noise have demonstrated that children compared to workers may receive more decibels from an 8-hour working day in a factory and that persons regularly attending sport events are exposed to intensity levels exceeding all federal guidelines (WHO, 1997).

Figure 8. DPOAE amplitudes (grey lines) and noise amplitudes (shaded areas) measured in case number four for the right ear (right figure) and left ear (left figure). Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

102

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge Table 2. Permissible Noise Exposure (OSHA, 1992).

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Exposure duration per day (hours) 8 6 4 3 2 1 ½ ¼

Exposure level (dBA) 90 92 95 97 100 105 110 115

Most of the guidelines, criteria and legislation are setup for occupational purposes. Estimations of the risk for developing a non-occupational hearing loss is nowadays compared to hearing loss caused by occupational exposure. The problem of noise and hearing conservation is regulated in specific standards, regulations and legislations for recordkeeping and the general industry. A regulation is a rule prescribed by the government and is more formal than a standard. A standard is a set of rules or guidelines, but can be developed also by consensus groups, such as the American National Standards Institute (ANSI). Legislation consists of laws prescribed by the American Congress or by local governing bodies [Sutter, 1996]. In Europe, these standards and legislations are expressed as European Community Directives. The most recent directive is Directive 2003-10-EC of the European Parliament and Council on the minimum health and safety requirements regarding the exposure of workers to the risks arising from physical agents (Directive 2003-10-EC, 2003). Other nations use a code of practice, which has less legal force than regulations and legislations. An example of this is the Australian national standard in which a 35 page code of practice provides the guidelines on how the standard, described in two short paragraphs, should be implemented. Finally, recommendations are sometimes used, which act more as guidelines rather than enforceable rules [Sutter, 1996]. Most of these standards are based on little valid, reliable, and scientific data. Political, social and economical factors have often strong influence on the final rules and regulations. As a consequence permissible safety exposure limits (PEL), rules and guidelines differ very much according to country. An overview of all standards, rules and regulations of the different countries and states is not within the scope of this chapter. As an example the different parameters of the 1910.95 OSHA Noise Standard are provided here (OSHA, 1992). In the 1910.95 OSHA Noise Standard, PELs for noise are described as the maximum duration of exposure per working day, permitted for various noise intensity levels (in dBA). The exposure-duration table of the OSHA standard is described in Table 2. The PELs depend on two parameters, a criterion level and an exchange rate. The criterion level is the steady noise intensity level, which is allowed for a full eight-hour working day. This is 90 dBA in most jurisdictions, but in Europe and other nations these values may be much lower. Applying the equal-energy rule of sound, increasing the noise level above the criterion level, must result in a decrease of the allowed exposure time. This level-duration relation

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

103

depends on the exchange rate. The exchange rate is the amount by which the permissible exposure level can be increased when the exposure duration is divided by two. There are worldwide two types of exchange rates in use: 3 dBA and 5 dBA exchange rates. These two exchange rates produce different guidelines worldwide. The 3 dBA exchange rate is more stringent. The maximum permitted duration for a 105 dBA noise exposure using the 3 dBA exchange rate is 15 minutes, whereas it is one hour with the 5 dBA exchange rate. Most international experts evaluate the 3 dBA rule as more logical for occupational noise exposure. As can be seen in Table 2, the OSHA advices a PEL of 90 dBA and an exchange rate of 5 dBA. The US Army and Air Force now use 3 dB exchange rates. Since most of the regulations and standards across the world are made for occupational purposes, one should be cautious to extrapolate these criteria and permissible exposure levels to the situations of leisure noise exposure. There is an increasing need for an international debate on legislation and regulation of noise exposure outside occupational activities. Legislation on output levels of personal music players and sound levels in discotheques, nightclubs, live concerts or at festivals is urgently needed and should not be based on occupational regulations and legislations.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

CONCLUSION NIHL is the result of long-term noise exposure in occupational setting, as well as during non-occupational, leisure activities. These leisure activities include participation in nonmusical activities, as well as music-related exposure. The most common sources of music exposure during leisure activities are listening to PMPs, and attending discotheques, nightclubs, or live concerts. An increased prevalence of NIHL in youth caused by these leisure noise exposures is assumed in the mainstream media. However, scientific results concerning this issue are equivocal. There is a definite amount of high-frequency hearing loss in young people, but there are only small associations observed between leisure noise exposure and hearing loss as determined by pure-tone audiometry and clear, firm evidence of an increased prevalence of NIHL is not yet noticed. Several explanations are possible. First, it could be that leisure noise exposure is insufficient to cause widespread hearing loss. Second, recent technological improvements in PMPs, and subsequently improved listening comfort and habits do not cause hearing loss yet. Finally, it is suggested that pure-tone audiometry is not sensitive enough to detect minimal cochlear damage. Nevertheless, the aforementioned sources of leisure noise can be hazardous for hearing in some highly susceptible, or even in many young people with extreme listening or attendance habits. Therefore, further research on several domains is essentially to prevent NIHL in young people. First, a careful inventory of leisure activities and listening habits in a representative sample of adolescents is needed, as well as a well-grounded understanding of music-listening habits. As mentioned above, there are gender differences in listening habits, and these habits also tend to change during life. Therefore, factors determining risk behavior regarding noise exposure must be further explored. We prefer a multidisciplinary context with at least a

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

104

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

hearing specialist and psychologist to gain insight in risk behavior excessive music listening by adolescents. Second, the long-term risks of cumulative noise exposures of adolescents must be evaluated. We recommend using OAEs and/or extended high-frequency audiometry complementary with pure-tone audiometry. The usefulness of OAEs in the diagnosis of NIHL must be further explored, but are promising as indicated by our case studies. Finally, hearing education campaigns should educate adolescents, and other parties involved in the prevention of NIHL. As education is not enough to induce behavioral changes in adolescents, we insist in a strict legislation to limit sound levels in several establishments with high intensity levels, as well as a limitation of the output levels of PMPs.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

REFERENCES Airo, E., Pekkarinen, J. & Olkinuora, P. S. (1996). Listening to music with earphones: an assessment of noise exposure. Acta Acustica, 82, 885-94. Alberti, P. (1998). Traumatic sensorineural hearing loss. In H. Ludman & A. Wright (Eds.), Diseases of the ear (6th, pp. 483-494). London: Arnold/Hodder Headline. American College of Occupational and Environmental Medicine. Position Statement on noise-induced hearing loss. 2002. Available at: http://www.acoem.org/guidelines. aspx?id=846. Accessed on June 16, 2009. Arlinger, S. (2003). Negative consequences of uncorrected hearing loss--a review. Int J Audiol, 42 Suppl 2, 2S17-20. Attias, J., Horovitz, G., El-Hatib, N. & Nageris, B. (2001). Detection and Clinical Diagnosis of Noise-Induced Hearing Loss by Otoacoustic Emissions. Noise Health, 3, 19-31. Augustsson, I. & Engstrand, I. (2006). Hearing ability according to screening at conscription; comparison with earlier reports and with previous screening results for individuals without known ear disease. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 70, 909-13. Axelsson, A. & Lindgren, F. (1978). Temporary threshold shift after exposure to pop music. Scand Audiol, 7, 127-35. Axelsson, A. & Prasher, D. (1999). Tinnitus: A warning signal to teenagers attending discotheques? Noise Health, 1, 1-2. Axelsson, A. & Prasher, D. (2000). Tinnitus induced by occupational and leisure noise. Noise Health, 2, 47-54. Axelsson, A., Jerson, T. & Lindgren, F. (1981b). Noisy leisure time activities in teenage boys. Am Ind Hyg Assoc J, 42, 229-33. Axelsson, A., Jerson, T., Lindberg, U. & Lindgren, F. (1981a). Early noise-induced hearing loss in teenage boys. Scand Audiol, 10, 91-6. Axelsson, A., Rosenhall, U. & Zachau, G. (1994). Hearing in 18-year-old Swedish males. Scand Audiol, 23, 129-34. Becher, S., Struwe, F., Schwenzer, C. & Weber, K. (1996). [Risk of hearing loss caused by high volume music--presenting an educational concept for preventing hearing loss in adolescents]. Gesundheitswesen, 58, 91-5. Bhagat, S. P. & Davis, A. M. (2008). Modification of otoacoustic emissions following earlevel exposure to MP3 player music. Int J Audiol, 47, 751-60.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

105

Biassoni, E. C. et al (2005). Recreational noise exposure and its effects on the hearing of adolescents. Part II: development of hearing disorders. Int J Audiol, 44, 74-85. Bogoch, I. I., House, R. A. & Kudla, I. (2005). Perceptions about hearing protection and noise-induced hearing loss of attendees of rock concerts. Can J Public Health, 96, 69-72. Bohlin, M. C. & Erlandsson, S. I. (2007). Risk behaviour and noise exposure among adolescents. Noise Health, 9, 55-63. Bonfils, P., Bertrand, Y. & Uziel, A. (1988). Evoked otoacoustic emissions: normative data and presbycusis. Audiology, 27, 27-35. Bradley, R., Fortnum, H. & Coles, R. (1987). Patterns of exposure of schoolchildren to amplified music. Br J Audiol, 21, 119-25. Brownell, W. E. (1990). Outer hair cell electromotility and otoacoustic emissions. Ear Hear, 11, 82-92. Carter, N. L., Murray, N., Khan, A. & Waugh, D. (1984). A longitudinal study of recreational noise and young people's hearing. Australian Journal of Audiology, 6, 45-53. Carter, N. L., Waugh, R. L., Keen, K., Murray, N. & Bulteau, V. G. (1982). Amplified music and young people's hearing. Review and report of Australian findings. Med J Aust, 2, 125-8. Catalano, P. J. & Levin, S. M. (1985). Noise-induced hearing loss and portable radios with headphones. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 9, 59-67. Céranic, B. (2007). The value of otoacoustic emissions in the investigation of noise damage. Audiological Medicine, 5, 10-24. Chung, J. H., Des Roches, C. M., Meunier, J. & Eavey, R. D. (2005). Evaluation of noiseinduced hearing loss in young people using a web-based survey technique. Pediatrics, 115, 861-7. Clark, W. W. (1991). Noise exposure from leisure activities: a review. J Acoust Soc Am, 90, 175-81. Crandell, C., Mills, T. L. & Gauthier, R. (2004). Knowledge, behaviors, and attitudes about hearing loss and hearing protection among racial/ethnically diverse young adults. J Natl Med Assoc, 96, 176-86. Daniel, E. (2007). Noise and hearing loss: a review. J Sch Health, 77, 225-31. Davis, A. C., Lovell, E. A., Smith, P. A. & Ferguson, M. A. (1998). The contribution of social noise to tinnitus in young people - a preliminary report. Noise Health, 1, 40-6. Desai, A., Reed, D., Cheyne, A., Richards, S. & Prasher, D. (1999). Absence of otoacoustic emissions in subjects with normal audiometric thresholds implies exposure to noise. Noise Health, 1, 58-65. Dobie, R. A. (2008). The burdens of age-related and occupational noise-induced hearing loss in the United States. Ear Hear, 29, 565-77. European Parliament and Council. (2003). Directive 2003-10-EC on the minimum health and safety requirements regarding the exposure of workers to the risks arising from physical agents (noise), 2003-10-EC. Fearn, R. W. & Hanson, D. R. (1984). Hearing damage in young people using headphone to listen to pop music. Journal of sound and vibration, 96, 147-9. Fligor, B. J. & Cox, L. C. (2004). Output levels of commercially available portable compact disc players and the potential risk to hearing. Ear Hear, 25, 513-27. Florentine, M., Hunter, W., Robinson, M., Ballou, M. & Buus, S. (1998). On the behavioral characteristics of loud-music listening. Ear Hear, 19, 420-8.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

106

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

Gissel, S., Mortensen, J. T. & Juul, S. (2002). Evaluation of hearing ability in Danish children at the time of school start and at the end of school. Int J Adolesc Med Health, 14, 43-9. Gorga, M. P., et al. (1997). From laboratory to clinic: a large scale study of distortion product otoacoustic emissions in ears with normal hearing and ears with hearing loss. Ear Hear, 18, 440-55. Hall, A. J. & Lutman, M. E. (1999). Methods for early identification of noise-induced hearing loss. Audiology, 38, 277-80. Hanson, D. R. & Fearn, R. W. (1975). Hearing acuity in young people exposed to pop music and other noise. Lancet, 2, 203-5. Hausler, R. (2004). [The effects of acoustic overstimulation]. Ther Umsch, 61, 21-9. Heinrich, U. & Feltens, R. (2006). Mechanisms underlying noise-induced hearing loss. Drug Discovery Today: Disease Mechanisms, 3, 131-5. Hellstrom, P. A. (1991). The effects on hearing from portable cassette players: a follow-up study. Journal of sound and vibration, 151, 461-9. Hellstrom, P. A., Axelsson, A. & Costa, O. (1998). Temporary threshold shift induced by music. Scand Audiol Suppl, 48, 87-94. Henderson, D., Bielefeld, E. C., Harris, K. C. & Hu, B. H. (2006). The role of oxidative stress in noise-induced hearing loss. Ear Hear, 27, 1-19. Henderson, D., Subramaniam, M. & Boettcher, F. A. (1993). Individual susceptibility to noise-induced hearing loss: an old topic revisited. Ear Hear, 14, 152-68. Hetu, R. & Fortin, M. (1995). Potential risk of hearing damage associated with exposure to highly amplified music. J Am Acad Audiol, 6, 378-86. Hodgetts, W. E., Rieger, J. M. & Szarko, R. A. (2007). The effects of listening environment and earphone style on preferred listening levels of normal hearing adults using an MP3 player. Ear Hear, 28, 290-7. Holmes, A. E., Niskar, A. S., Kieszak, S. M., Rubin, C. & Brody, D. J. (2004). Mean and median hearing thresholds among children 6 to 19 years of age: the Third National Health And Nutrition Examination Survey, 1988 to 1994, United States. Ear Hear, 25, 397-402. Hotz, M. A., Probst, R., Harris, F. P. & Hauser, R. (1993). Monitoring the effects of noise exposure using transiently evoked otoacoustic emissions. Acta Otolaryngol, 113, 478-82. Hu, B. H., Henderson, D. & Nicotera, T. M. (2002). Involvement of apoptosis in progression of cochlear lesion following exposure to intense noise. Hear Res, 166, 62-71. Ising, H. (1994). [Potential hearing loss caused by loud music. Current status of knowledge and need for management]. HNO, 42, 465-6. Ising, H., Babisch, W., Hanel, J., Kruppa, B. & Pilgramm, M. (1995). [Empirical studies of music listening habits of adolescents. Optimizing sound threshold limits for cassette players and discoteques]. HNO, 43, 244-9. Ising, H., Hanel, J., Pilgramm, M., Babisch, W. & Lindthammer, A. (1994). [Risk of hearing loss caused by listening to music with head phones]. HNO, 42, 764-8. Jokitulppo, J. & Bjork, E. (2002). Estimated leisure-time noise exposure and hearing symptoms in a finnish urban adult population. Noise Health, 5, 53-62. Jokitulppo, J. S., Bjork, E. A. & kaan-Penttila, E. (1997). Estimated leisure noise exposure and hearing symptoms in Finnish teenagers. Scand Audiol, 26, 257-62. Jokitulppo, J., Toivonen, M. & Bjork, E. (2006). Estimated leisure-time noise exposure, hearing thresholds, and hearing symptoms of Finnish conscripts. Mil Med, 171, 112-6.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

107

Keith, S. E., Michaud, D. S. & Chiu, V. (2008). Evaluating the maximum playback sound levels from portable digital audio players. J Acoust Soc Am, 123, 4227-37. Kemp, D. T. (1978). Stimulated acoustic emissions from within the human auditory system. J Acoust Soc Am, 64, 1386-91. Konopka, W., Zalewski, P. & Pietkiewicz, P. (2001). Evaluation of Transient and Distortion Product Otoacoustic Emissions before and after shooting practice. Noise Health, 3, 29-37. Kowalska, S. & Sulkowski, W. (1997). Measurements of click-evoked otoacoustic emission in industrial workers with noise-induced hearing loss. Int J Occup Med Environ Health, 10, 441-59. Lapsley-Miller, J.A., Marshall, L. & Heller, L.M. (2004). A longitudinal study of changes in evoked otoacoustic emissions and pure-tone thresholds as measured in a hearing conservation program. Int J Audiol, 43, 307-22. Le Prell, C. G., Yamashita, D., Minami, S. B., Yamasoba, T. & Miller, J. M. (2007). Mechanisms of noise-induced hearing loss indicate multiple methods of prevention. Hear Res, 226, 22-43. Lee, P. C., Senders, C. W., Gantz, B. J. & Otto, S. R. (1985). Transient sensorineural hearing loss after overuse of portable headphone cassette radios. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg, 93, 622-5. Lees, R. E., Roberts, J. H. & Wald, Z. (1985). Noise induced hearing loss and leisure activities of young people: a pilot study. Can J Public Health, 76, 171-3. LePage, E. L. & Murray, N. M. (1998). Latent cochlear damage in personal stereo users: a study based on click-evoked otoacoustic emissions. Med J Aust, 169, 588-92. LePage, E. L. (1998). Occupational noise-induced hearing loss: origin, characterisation and prevention. Acoustics Australia, 26, 57-61. Liebel, J., Delb, W., Andes, C. & Koch, A. (1996). [Detection of hearing loss in patrons of a discoteque using TEOAE and DPOAE]. Laryngorhinootologie, 75, 259-64. Lindeman, H. E., van der Klaauw, M. M. & Platenburg-Gits, F. A. (1987). Hearing acuity in male adolescents (young adults) at the age of 17 to 23 years. Audiology, 26, 65-78. Lindgren, F. & Axelsson, A. (1983). Temporary threshold shift after exposure to noise and music of equal energy. Ear Hear, 4, 197-201. Lipscomb, D. M. (1972). The increase in prevalence of high frequency hearing impairment among college students. Audiology, 11, 231-7. Litke, R. E. (1971). Elevated high-frequency hearing in school children. Arch Otolaryngol, 94, 255-7. Loth, D., Avan, P., Menguy, C. & Teyssou, M. (1992). [Secondary auditory risks from listening to portable digital compact disc players]. Bull Acad Natl Med, 176, 1245-52. Lucertini, M., Moleti, A. & Sisto, R. (2002). On the detection of early cochlear damage by otoacoustic emission analysis. J Acoust Soc Am, 111, 972-8. Maassen, M., et al. (2001). Ear damage caused by leisure noise. Noise Health, 4, 1-16. MacLean, G. L., Stuart, A. & Stenstrom, R. (1992). Real ears sound pressure levels developed by three portable stereo system earphones. Am J Audiol 52-5. Mansfield, J. D., Baghurst, P. A. & Newton, V. E. (1999). Otoacoustic emissions in 28 young adults exposed to amplified music. Br J Audiol, 33, 211-22. Meecham, E. A. & Hume, K. I. (2001). Tinnitus, attendance at night-clubs and social drug taking in students. Noise Health, 3, 53-62.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

108

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

Melnick, W. (1991). Human temporary threshold shift (TTS) and damage risk. J Acoust Soc Am, 90, 147-54. Mercier, V. & Hohmann, B. W. (2002). Is Electronically Amplified Music too Loud? What do Young People Think? Noise Health, 4, 47-55. Mercier, V., Luy, D. & Hohmann, B. W. (2003). The sound exposure of the audience at a music festival. Noise Health, 5, 51-8. Metternich, F. U. & Brusis, T. (1999). [Acute hearing loss and tinnitus caused by amplified recreational music]. Laryngorhinootologie, 78, 614-9. Meyer-Bisch, C. (1996). Epidemiological evaluation of hearing damage related to strongly amplified music (personal cassette players, discotheques, rock concerts)--high-definition audiometric survey on 1364 subjects. Audiology, 35, 121-42. Mills, J. H. & Going, J. A. (1982). Review of environmental factors affecting hearing. Environ Health Perspect, 44, 119-27. Montgomery, J. K. & Fujikawa, S. (1992). Hearing thresholds of students in the second, eighth, and twelfth grades. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in School, 23, 61-3. Montoya, F. S., Ibarguen, A. M., Vences, A. R., del Rey, A. S. & Fernandez, J. M. (2008). Evaluation of cochlear function in normal-hearing young adults exposed to MP3 player noise by analyzing transient evoked otoacoustic emissions and distortion products. J Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg, 37, 718-24. Morata, T. C. (2007). Young people: their noise and music exposures and the risk of hearing loss. Int J Audiol, 46, 111-2. Mostafapour, S. P., Lahargoue, K. & Gates, G. A. (1998). Noise-induced hearing loss in young adults: the role of personal listening devices and other sources of leisure noise. Laryngoscope, 108, 1832-9. MRC Institute of Hearing Research (1986). Damage to hearing arising from leisure noise. Br J Audiol, 20, 157-64. Niskar, A. S., et al. (1998). Prevalence of hearing loss among children 6 to 19 years of age: the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. JAMA, 279, 1071-5. Niskar, A. S., et al. (2001). Estimated prevalence of noise-induced hearing threshold shifts among children 6 to 19 years of age: the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1988-1994, United States. Pediatrics, 108, 40-3. Nordmann, A. S., Bohne, B. A. & Harding, G. W. (2000). Histopathological differences between temporary and permanent threshold shift. Hear Res, 139, 13-30. Norton, S. J. (1992). Cochlear function and otoacoustic emissions. Seminars in Hearing, 13, 1-14. Occupational Safety and Health Administration. 1992. Occupational Noise Exposure, 191095. Opperman, D. A., Reifman, W., Schlauch, R. & Levine, S. (2006). Incidence of spontaneous hearing threshold shifts during modern concert performances. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg, 134, 667-73. Passchier-Vermeer, W. (1999). Pop music trough headphones and hearing loss. Noise Control Eng J, 47, 182-6. Peng, J. H., Tao, Z. Z. & Huang, Z. W. (2007). Risk of damage to hearing from personal listening devices in young adults. J Otolaryngol, 36, 181-5. Persson, B. O., Svedberg, A. & Gothe, C. J. (1993). Longitudinal changes in hearing ability among Swedish conscripts. Scand Audiol, 22, 141-3.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Noise-Induced Hearing Loss in Youth Caused by Leisure Noise

109

Plath, P. (1998a). [Socio-acousis. Non-occupationally-induced hearing loss due to noise, 1]. HNO, 46, 887-92. Plath, P. (1998b). [Sociacusis. Non-occupationally induced hearing damage by noise, 2]. HNO, 46, 947-52. Plinkert, P. K., Hemmert, W. & Zenner, H. P. (1995). [Comparison of methods for early detection of noise vulnerability of the inner ear. Amplitude reduction of otoacoustic emissions are most sensitive at submaximal noise impulse exposure]. HNO, 43, 89-97. Probst, R., Harris, F. P. & Hauser, R. (1993). Clinical monitoring using otoacoustic emissions. Br J Audiol, 27, 85-90. Probst, R., Lonsbury-Martin, B. L. & Martin, G. K. (1991). A review of otoacoustic emissions. J Acoust Soc Am, 89, 2027-67. Pugsley, S., Stuart, A., Kalinowski, J. & Armson, J. (1993). Changes in hearing sensitivity following portable stereo system use. Am J Audiol, 2, 64-7. Rabinowitz, P. M., Slade, M. D., Galusha, D., xon-Ernst, C. & Cullen, M. R. (2006). Trends in the prevalence of hearing loss among young adults entering an industrial workforce 1985 to 2004. Ear Hear, 27, 369-75. Rawool, V. W. & Colligon-Wayne, L. A. (2008). Auditory lifestyles and beliefs related to hearing loss among college students in the USA. Noise Health, 10, 1-10. Rice, C. G., Rossi, G. & Olina, M. (1987a). Damage risk from personal cassette players. Br J Audiol, 21, 279-88. Rice, C.G., Breslin, M. & Roper, R.G. (1987b). Sound levels from personal cassette players. Br J Audiol, 21, 273-278. Schmuziger, N., Fostiropoulos, K. & Probst, R. (2006). Long-term assessment of auditory changes resulting from a single noise exposure associated with non-occupational activities. Int J Audiol, 45, 46-54. Serra, M. R. et al (2005). Recreational noise exposure and its effects on the hearing of adolescents. Part I: an interdisciplinary long-term study. Int J Audiol, 44, 65-73. Shah, S., Gopal, B., Reis, J. & Novak, M. (2009). Hear today, gone tomorrow: an assessment of portable entertainment player use and hearing acuity in a community sample. J Am Board Fam Med, 22, 17-23. Sliwinska-Kowalska, M. & Jedlinska, U. (1998). Prolonged exposure to industrial noise: cochlear pathology does not correlate with the degree of permanent threshold shift, but is related to duration of exposure. J Occup Health, 40, 123-31. Sliwinska-Kowalska, M. & Kotylo, P. (2008). Evaluation of individuals with known or suspected noise damage to hearing. Audiological Medicine, 5, 54. Smith, P. A., Davis, A., Ferguson, M. & Lutman, M. E. (2000). The prevalence and type of social noise exposure in young adults in England. Noise Health, 2, 41-56. Spaeth, J., Klimek, L., Doring, W. H., Rosendahl, A. & Mosges, R. (1993). [How badly does the "normal-hearing" young man of 1992 hear in the high frequency range?]. HNO, 41, 385-8. Strauss, P., Quante, M., Strahl, M., Averhage, H. & Bitzer, M. (1977). [Is hearing of the students damaged by environmental noise in their leisure time?]. Laryngol Rhinol Otol (Stuttg), 56, 868-71. Sutter, A. H. (1996). Current Standards for Occupational Exposure to Noise. In A. Axelsson et al. (Eds.), Scientific Basis of Noise-Induced Hearing LossNew York: Thieme Medical Publishers.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

110

H. Keppler, B. Vinck and I. Dhooge

Swanson, S. J., Dengerink, H. A., Kondrick, P. & Miller, C. L. (1987). The influence of subjective factors on temporary threshold shifts after exposure to music and noise of equal energy. Ear Hear, 8, 288-91. Talaska, A. E. & Schacht, J. (2007). Mechanisms of noise damage to the cochlea. Audiological Medicine, 5, 3-9. Torre, P., III (2008). Young adults' use and output level settings of personal music systems. Ear Hear, 29, 791-9. Turunen-Rise, I., Flottorp, G. & Tvete, O. (1991). Personal cassette players ('Walkman'). Do they cause noise-induced hearing loss? Scand Audiol, 20, 239-44. Vogel, I., Brug, J., Hosli, E. J., van der Ploeg, C. P. & Raat, H. (2008). MP3 players and hearing loss: adolescents' perceptions of loud music and hearing conservation. J Pediatr, 152, 400-4. Vogel, I., Brug, J., van der Ploeg, C. P. & Raat, H. (2007). Young people's exposure to loud music: a summary of the literature. Am J Prev Med, 33, 124-33. Vogel, I., Brug, J., van der Ploeg, C. P. & Raat, H. (2009a). Prevention of adolescents' musicinduced hearing loss due to discotheque attendance: a Delphi study. Health Educ Res. Vogel, I., Brug, J., van der Ploeg, C. P. & Raat, H. (2009b). Strategies for the prevention of MP3-induced hearing loss among adolescents: expert opinions from a Delphi study. Pediatrics, 123, 1257-62. Weichbold, V. & Zorowka, P. (2002). [Effect of information about hearing damage caused by loud music. For adolescents the music in discoteques is too loud despite loudness limits]. HNO, 50, 560-4. Weichbold, V. & Zorowka, P. (2003). Effects of a hearing protection campaign on the discotheque attendance habits of high-school students. Int J Audiol, 42, 489-93. Weichbold, V. & Zorowka, P. (2005). [Will adolescents visit discotheque less often if sound levels of music are decreased?]. HNO, 53, 845-1. Weichbold, V. & Zorowka, P. (2007). Can a hearing education campaign for adolescents change their music listening behavior? Int J Audiol, 46, 128-33. West, P. D. & Evans, E. F. (1990). Early detection of hearing damage in young listeners resulting from exposure to amplified music. Br J Audiol, 24, 89-103. Widen, S. E. & Erlandsson, S. I. (2004). Self-reported tinnitus and noise sensitivity among adolescents in Sweden. Noise Health, 7, 29-40. Widen, S. E., Holmes, A. E. & Erlandsson, S. I. (2006). Reported hearing protection use in young adults from Sweden and the USA: effects of attitude and gender. Int J Audiol, 45, 273-80. Williams, W. (2005). Noise exposure levels from personal stereo use. Int J Audiol, 44, 231-6. Wong, T. W., Van Hasselt, C. A., Tang, L. S. & Yiu, P. C. (1990). The use of personal cassette players among youths and its effects on hearing. Public Health, 104, 327-30. World Health Organization. 1997. Prevention of noise-induced hearing loss: report of an informal consultation held at the World Health Organization, Geneva, on 28-30 October 1997. Yassi, A., Pollock, N., Tran, N. & Cheang, M. (1993). Risks to hearing from a rock concert. Can Fam Physician, 39, 1045-50. Zenner, H. P., et al. (1999). [Hearing loss caused by leisure noise]. HNO, 47, 236-48. Zogby, J. (2006). Survey of teens and adults about the use of personal elctronic devices and head phones. Am Speech Lang Hear Assoc.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

In: Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System Editors: D. Fiedler and R. Krause, pp.111-135

ISBN: 978-1-60741-259-5 ©2010 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

Chapter 3

CURRENT CONCEPTS IN RADIATION-INDUCED SENSORI-NEURAL HEARING LOSS Wong-Kein Low Department of Otolaryngology, Singapore General Hospital, Singapore 169608

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

ABSTRACT Sensori-neural hearing loss is a frequent complication of radiotherapy of head and neck tumours, when the auditory pathways have been included in the radiation field. This chapter focuses on the review of three aspects of radiation-induced SNHL that have a significant impact on modern day medicine: 1) effect of radiation on retro-cochlear nervous pathways, 2) cellular and molecular processes involved in radiation-induced ototoxicity, 3) combined ototoxic effects of radiation and cisplatin. A model of radiationinduced damage of the sensor-neural auditory system has been proposed, which is based on the concept of dose-dependent, reactive oxygen species related cochlear cell apoptosis without significant damage to the retro-cochlear pathway. This model supports the feasibility of cochlear implantation, should one be clinically indicated in such patients. It can explain clinical observations such as radiation-induced sensori-neural hearing loss being dose-dependent and affects the high frequencies more than the lower frequencies. It also opens up the possibility of preventive strategies targeted at different stages of the apoptotic process. The use of anti-oxidants which target upstream pathways, appear promising. In particular, it may have a major role in chemo-radiation where combined ototoxic effects lead to significant sensori-neural hearing loss.

I. INTRODUCTION Radiation-induced sensori-neural hearing loss (SNHL) has long been recognized as a complication of radiotherapy (RT) for head and neck tumours, when the auditory pathways had been included in the radiation fields. It is believed that radiation therapy has been a much larger etiologic factor of hearing loss than suspected and should clearly be recognized as a major factor in the etiology of adult hearing disorders [1,2,3]. In recent years, improved radiotherapy techniques can potentially reduce unnecessary radiation exposure to ear

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

112

Wong-Kein Low

structures. Nevertheless, radiation-induced SNHL remains an important cause of hearing loss clinically, especially in chemo-radiation [4]. In the past, SNHL resulting from radiotherapy which involved inner ear structures was deemed inevitable and conventional hearing aids were prescribed to improve hearing when necesary. With recent advancements in medical science and technology, modern hearing devices and preventive measures based on molecular and cellular processes have become available. Nowadays, there is a need to better understand the effects of radiation on the auditory system in order to effectively manage radiation-induced SNHL. Seemingly irrelevant questions in the past have become important today. This chapter focuses on the review of three aspects of radiation-induced SNHL which have a significant impact on modern day medicine:

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

1. Effect of radiation on retro-cochlear nervous pathways. Cochlear implants are widely used nowadays and are indicated for patients with severe to profound hearing loss due to cochlear damage. In cochlear implantation, a pre-requisite for a successful outcome is intactness of the retro-cochlear pathways. It is, therefore, important to ascertain if radiotherapy damages the retro-cochlear pathways. 2. The cellular and molecular processes involved in radiation-induced ototoxicity. It becomes necessary to understand these processes as they can potentially be targeted by medical intervention strategies that are available today. 3. The combined ototoxic effects of radiation and cisplatin (CDDP). Chemoradiotherapy using CDDP is increasingly being used to treat advanced head and neck cancers, and the extent of inner ear damage by combined therapy should be appreciated [5].

II. CHARACTERISTICS OF RADIATION-INDUCED SNHL Existence of Radiation-Induced SNHL Animal studies The effects of radiation on the inner ear was documented as early as 1905, when Ewald placed radium beads in the middle ear of pigeons and noted labyrinthine symptoms [6]. In 1933, Girden & Culler were first to report the effects of radiation on hearing [7]. Experimenting on dogs subjected to various X-ray doses, a hearing impairment averaging 5.5 dB was recorded. Novotny (1951) studied the ionizing effects of radiation in guinea pigs and found a hearing impairment of about 8.4dB at 4000 Hz [8]. Kozlov (1959) also noted a hearing imparment of 3.9–9.1dB over 500–8000 Hz in guinea pigs [9]. Gamble et al. (1968) subjected the ears of 50 guinea pigs to 500–6000 rads of radiation and found decreased sensitivity of cochlear microphonics of 20–40dB, particularly in the high frequencies [10]. This was consistent with microscopic findings which demonstrated damage to the Organ of Corti and cochlear duct in rats, after doses of 100–3000 rads [11]. Tokimoto & Kanagawa (1985) in their study on guinea pigs concluded that even in the absence of gross anatomical cochlear changes, post-irradiation functional hair cell defects could exist [12].

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Current Concepts in Radiation-Induced Sensori-Neural Hearing Loss

113

Histologically, Keleman (1963) studied temporal bones in rats which had received 100– 3000 rads of radiation [11]. Haemorrhage was noted to be the most prominent finding with destruction of the cochlear duct, organ of corti and its surrounding elements. Early changes of the stria vascularis accompanied by significant inflammatory responses, were found in the inner ears of guinea pigs that had received 6000 rads of radiation [10].

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Human studies Several clinical studies have recorded SNHL in patients who have had RT for head and neck malignancies, where inner ear structures were included in the radiation fields. Leach (1965) observed SNHL in some of the 56 patients who had received 3000–12000 rads of RT for different head and neck cancers [13]. Morretti (1976) retrospectively studied 137 postirradiated patients with nasopharyngeal carcinoma (NPC), and found 7 to have SNHL of at least 10dB [14]. There had also been suggestions that radiation did not result in significant SNHL [15]. After a systematic review of the literature, it was concluded that the conflicting results from the various studies were attributable to variations in patient groups, size, study design, followup period, radiotherapy techniques and presentation of audiometric results [16]. From the pooled data generated from this systematic review, it was found that about one-third of patients who had received 70 Gy in 2 Gy per fraction near the inner ear, developed hearing loss of at least 10dB at 4 kHz. Schnecht & Karmody (1966) reported the histological features of a deafened man who had received 5,220 rads of radiation to the region of the ears several years ago [17]. Degeneration of the Organ of Corti was noted, with atrophy of the basilar membrane, spiral ligament and stria vascularis. Progressive hearing loss across the various frequencies had been attributed to obliterating endarteritis and eventual fibrosis, leading to vascular compromise [14].

Prevalence and Epidemiology It had been difficult to establish the exact prevalence of radiation-induced hearing loss because statistics relating to ototoxicity were often not well kept or easily interpreted [2]. Not only was hearing not often considered important in the face of life-threatening diseases, hearing losses were frequently not recorded in those who did not survive. Clinical studies had reported varying prevalence, depending on factors such as dose, period of follow-up and definition/criteria used for hearing loss [16]. In NPC where the radiation dose received by the ear is relatively high, the incidence of post-RT SNHL had been reported to be as high as 24% [18]. In SNHL after RT in NPC patients, sex and age were found to be independent prognostic factors [18]. Males were noted to be more susceptible than females in developing SNHL after radiation. Older patients were at greater risk, as pre-existing degenerative changes could have made them more vulnerable to radiation injury [19]. Post-RT middle ear effusion was identified to be another predicting factor, as toxic materials from chronic inflammation could affect the inner ear [20]. However, it is argued that the development of post-RT middle ear effusion could have been just another manifestation of radiation damage related to individual variation in susceptibility to radiation [18].

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

114

Wong-Kein Low

Effect of Radiation Dose Gamble et al. (1968) found in guinea pigs that the extent of inner ear injury correlated with the radiation dose applied [10]. Bohne et al. (1985) confirmed in chinchillas that the higher the radiation dose received, the greater the damage to the inner ear [21]. A systematic review on human studies reported increasing loss with increasing dose, starting at about 40 Gy applied in 2 Gy per fraction [16]. Guidelines for tolerance doses in normal tissues are being used in clinical practice [22]. It is noteworthy that early human studies had reported the effects of very high doses of radiation to the ears, doses that are no longer used in clinical practice today [23,24]. Although these could have potentially provided rare opportunities to study the effect of excessively high doses of radiation on the inner ear in humans, documentation of the audiometric data was so poor that it was impossible to draw conclusions on the relationship between high radiation dose and SNHL.

High Frequency Hearing Loss

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

High frequency hearing levels were affected more than those in the lower frequencies after irradiation [16,25]. This corresponded to histological observations made in animals that the basal part of the cochlea (which respond to higher frequency sounds) was usually more damaged by radiation than the apical part. Keleman (1963) demonstrated in rats that the apical turn of the cochlea was least affected by radiation doses of 100-3000 rads [11]. Winther (1970) similarly reported in guinea pigs that post-irradiated apical hair cells remained intact while the outer hair cells of the 2 basal turns were affected [26]. Early threshold shifts at high stimulus frequencies are indicators of probable subsequent shifts to low frequencies [27].

Early vs. Late Onset Traditionally, radiation-induced SNHL is regarded to have either early or late onsets [25]. In early-onset SNHL, hair cell damage in guinea pigs occurred as early as 6 hrs. following radiation of 4,000–7,000 rads [26]. Tokimoto & Kanagawa (1985) demonstrated in guinea pigs that sensori-neural loss appeared 3–10 hrs depending on dose administered, and outer hair cells in the basal turn of the cochlea were destroyed about 6 hrs after radiation [12]. In humans, SNHL often occurred near the end or shortly after the completion of fractionated RT [28]. Early-onset SNHL due to inflammatory causes or transient functional disturbances in the stria vascularis, may recover with time (18,28]. In late-onset hearing loss, Schuknecht & Karmordy (1966) observed marked atrophy of the cochlear stria vascularis in a patient who had developed hearing loss 8 years after radiotherapy (5200 rads) for carcinoma of the ear [17]. Grau et al. (1991) studied 22 NPC patients with well-documented pre- and post-RT hearing levels over 7-84 months, and found SNHL (especially the higher frequencies) developing 12 months post-RT [29]. Merchant et al. (2004) were of the opinion that radiation-induced SNHL could only occur 4 years after irradiation [30]. Delayed-onset hearing loss was found to correlate with age, pre-existing

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Current Concepts in Radiation-Induced Sensori-Neural Hearing Loss

115

SNHL and radiation dosage [31]. Late-onset radiation-induced SNHL had generally been attributed to progressive vascular compromise from radiation-induced vasculitis obliterans [14]. However, after a review of the relevant literature, Sataloff et al. (1994) were not convinced that late-onset hearing loss existed at all [32]. They argued that although significant hearing losses over the whole spectrum of the speech range were often seen several years after radiation, there was little or no convincing evidence to support the notion that hearing loss developing several years following radiation was causally related to radiation therapy itself. Nevertheless, the existence of early-onset radiation-induced SNHL and its progressive course had been well documented [19,33]. SNHL observed in patients years after radiotherapy may well be a result of the progressive nature of radiation-induced inner ear damage, rather than from an event which only begins after a long post-irradiation period.

III. EFFECT OF RADIATION ON RETRO-COCHLEAR PATHWAYS

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Modern RT for head and neck tumours can potentially induce SNHL, as the cochlea and parts of the auditory neural pathways are often included in the radiation fields. Kwong et al. (1996) reported sensori-neural hearing loss in NPC patients after RT, but did not differentiate between sensory or neural deafness [18]. Radiation-induced SNHL is generally believed to be a result of cochlear damage [32]. However, it cannot be assumed that retro-cochlear deafness does not occur, whether alone or in combination with cochlear deafness. The intactness of retro-cochlear auditory pathways is essential, if cochlear implantation were to be considered for restoration and rehabilitation of hearing loss in such patients.

The Sensori-Neural Auditory Pathways According to Hackney (1987), the hair cells of the Organ of Corti transduce vibrations within the cochlea into neural signals [34]. Outer hair cells are contractile and may contribute to mechanical feedback processes, whilst the inner hair cells are apparently the primary sensory cells being innervated by the majority of the afferent fibres. These run in the cochlear nerve to the brain stem where they bifurcate, projecting cochleotopically to the dorsal and ventral cochlear nuclei. A divergence continued in the main routes taken by the ascending pathways; one runs bilaterally from the ventral cochlear nucleus to the superior olivary complex and then to the inferior colliculi, the other runs from the dorsal cochlear nucleus to the contralateral inferior colliculus. Fibres from the brainstem nuclei travelling to each inferior colliculus form a tract, the lateral lemniscus, and may make contact with one of the nuclei within it. The pathway continues to the medial geniculate bodies and on to the auditory cortex, preserving its cochleotopicity at all levels. A descending system parallels the ascending system throughout. The presence of commissural and decussating connections from the level of the brainstem onwards, provides the anatomical basis for the analysis of binaural information. The division of the pathway forms the anatomical substrate for the parallel processing of different features of the auditory environment.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

116

Wong-Kein Low

Mechanisms of Damage to the Nervous System Awwad (1990) gave an account on radiation damage to the 3 main cell categories found in the central nervous system namely neurons, vascular endothelial cells and glial cells [35]. Glial cells have a slow turn-over rate with a small precursor (stem-cell) compartment (1%). Endothelial cells also have a slow turn-over rate but proliferate rapidly after injury. Neurons are non-proliferating end-cells in the adult organism. Myelination of the nerve axons is accomplished by the oligodendrocytes in the central nervous system and by Schwann cells in the peripheral nerves. The following 4 types of damage can be demonstrated in the rat spinal cord: (a) Transient demyelination is mediated by damage to the oligodendrocytes. In man, transient myelopathy has a relatively short latent period of 3 months and is reversible within 3 months. The main manifestations are paraesthesia and an electric shock-like pain, which are common after irradiation to cervical spine (b) White matter necrosis has a latency period of about 3-6 months and requires doses >20Gy. It is mediated by oligodendrocyte damage interacting with vascular injury. This probably is responsible for early myelopathy found in humans (c) Vascular damage has a latency period of >7 months and requires relatively lower doses (0.05). Similarly, a subset of 16 ears which recorded post-RT hearing deterioration, also did not have statistically significant difference (p=0.366) between the pre- and post-RT wave 1-5 latencies. This suggested that although the radiation delivered to the auditory system had damaged the cochlea, the retro-cochlear pathway was spared. The study suggested that in patients who have had radiotherapy for nasopharyngeal carcinoma, the retro-cochlear auditory pathways are functionally intact even in the longer term. In the same study, the mean radiation doses delivered to the cochlea and internal auditory meatus ranged from 24.1-62.2 Gy and 14.4 – 43.4 Gy respectively. Based on the actual doses applied to the auditory pathways, more meaningful correlations between radiation dose and retro-cochlear damage in humans could be derived. Most clinical studies on radiation-induced SNHL reported only the total dosage received by the patients for tumours in the head and neck region, and assumed that the ear structures received the full dosage prescribed. This is not necessarily the case because specific ear structures may not have received the full radiation dose but a gradation in dosage.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Feasiblity of Cochlear Implantation in Irradiated Ears The finding that retro-cochlear pathways remained functionally intact after irradiation suggested that cochlear implantation could be a viable option for hearing restoration should one be clinically indicated in these patients. To further substantiate that the retro-cochlear auditory pathways remained functionally intact after RT, a case-control study of cochlear implant recipients who had prior irradiation for NPC was conducted in our clinic [52]. They received their RT 11-28 years prior to cochlear implantation and the post-implant follow-up period ranged from 9 to 46 months. The implanted ear of each patient had favourable pre-operative promontory stimulation results. Post-implant, all patients were satisfied with their hearing outcomes and the improvement in speech discrimination scores was comparable to the controls.

IV. CELLULAR AND MOLECULAR BASIS OF OTOTOXICITY The above review therefore suggests that radiation-induced SNHL in doses used clinically, is mainly an intra-cochlear event. The cellular and molecular processes involved in ototoxicity from non-radiation causes such as from CDDP, aminoglycoside and acoustic

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

120

Wong-Kein Low

trauma had been well described. Protecting hair cells from irreversible degradation has been a primary objective because of the finite number of hair cells in the inner ear. Hair cells stop differentiating during development and are post-mitotic in nature so that the number of cells one is born with (about 16,000) is a life-time supply [53].

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Non-Radiation Causes of Ototoxicity Given the similarities in audiometric changes and cochlear pathology from different causes of hearing loss like noise, ototoxic drugs and aging, there may be a common factor underlying these seemingly different etiologies [54]. The cellular and molecular mechanisms leading to hearing loss appear to be similar and a final common pathway may hinge upon apoptosis [53,55]. According to Miller & Marx (1998), apoptosis is a highly regulated active form of programmed cell death which allows a cell to self-degrade in order for the body to eliminate unwanted or dysfunctional cells [56]. Apoptosis can be triggered in a cell through either the extrinsic pathway or the intrinsic pathways [57]. The extrinsic pathway is initiated through the stimulation of the transmembrane death receptors, such as the Fas receptors, located on the cell membrane. In contrast, the intrinsic pathway is initiated through the release of signal factors by mitochondria within the cell. The Bcl-2 family of proteins and the caspase cascade had been found to be important in effecting downstream pathwauys. The apoptotic processes are modulated by the p53 tumor suppressing gene. Over the past decade, a growing body of evidence suggests the importance of ROS as a possible mechanism leading to sensori-neural hearing loss [55]. There is compelling evidence implicating reactive oxygen species (ROS) in damage associated with cochlear ischemia, noise trauma, presbycusis, meningitis-associated hearing loss and aminoglycoide and CDDP ototoxicity [58]. It is well established that ROS are generated in hair cells exposed to CDDP, aminoglycosides and noise [55]. Studies have reported that enhancing antioxidant levels through drug application or genetic manipulation, promotes hair cell survival and preserve function [59]. Hair cell death is potentiated when knockout mice lacking the enzymes responsible for maintaining antioxidant homeostasis are exposed to loud noise [60]. Although the relationship between ROS and other cell death events is not fully understood, recent studies have shown that mitochondria-associated oxidants are involved in processes which regulate cytochrome c translocation and caspase activation in the central nervous system [61].

Radiation-Induced Ototoxicity Although CDDP and gentamicin have been shown to induce cochlear cell apoptosis the cellular and molecular processes resulting from radiation-induced ototoxicity was still unclear. We therefore, embarked on a study aimed at investigating the biophysical changes of dose-related gamma radiation-induced cochlear cell apoptosis in the OC-k3 cochlear cell line [62]. Using flow cytometry and TUNEL assay, radiation-induced apoptosis was found to be dose-dependant with a greater degree of apoptosis resulting after 20 Gy than 5 Gy. Apoptosis occurred predominantly at 72 hrs post-irradiation and microarray analysis showed associated

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Current Concepts in Radiation-Induced Sensori-Neural Hearing Loss

121

dose-dependant apoptotic gene regulation changes (Figure 1). Western blotting revealed phosphorylation occurring at 3, 24, 48 and 72 hrs post-irradiation and p53 up-regulation at 72 hrs. Although early activation of c-jun occurred at 3 hrs, it was not sustained with time. Associated dose-dependant intracellular generation of ROS was also demonstrated (Figure 2). The study demonstrated for the first time, a dose-dependant radiation-induced cochlear cell apoptosis which was associated with ROS generation and with p53 possibly playing an important role. A model of radiation-induced SNHL based on ROS and p53 dependant cochlear hair cell apoptosis, was therefore proposed. 0 Gy

5 Gy

20 Gy

DAPI

TUNEL

Figure 1. TUNEL assay confirms irradiation-induced apoptosis in OC-k3 cells at 72 hrs and is dose related. The cells were counterstained with DAPI, a nuclear-specific dye. More TUNEL positive nuclei were demonstrated at 72 hrs after 20 than 5 Gy of irradiation. The bar scale represents 50 micron. The results are representative of 3 separate experiments. Source: Low et al. [62], reproduced with kind permission of Springer and Business Media. Relative DCF fluorescence intensity

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

450

300

150

0 0Gy

5Gy

20Gy

100Gy

Figure 2. Radiation-induced generation of ROS is dose related. Intracellular generation of ROS in OC-k3 cells was detected 1 hr after treatment with different doses of radiation. The value for each dose was calculated as a relative intensity of DCF fluorescence compared to 0Gy, which was set at 100. Each value represents the mean  SD of 3 separate experiments. Significantly more intracellular ROS were generated by higher doses of radiation (p 80 dB(A)) workers and, if there was at least two hearing protectors to be compared. The reviewer selected relevant trials, assessed methodological quality and extracted data. Results: Two studies were included with a total of 46 participants. It was not possible to combine the included studies in a meta-analysis. Both included studies evaluated earplugs as hearing protectors. The representations of meta-analysis with only one study showed that participants that worn HL SmartFit had fewer number of difficulties per number of conversations compared to E-A-R Push-Ins earplugs. In addition, participants

*

Corresponding author: Post-doctoral Fellow, McMaster University, St. Joseph´s Healthcare Hamilton, 50 Charlton Ave East, Hamilton, ON, Canada L8N 4A. Emails: [email protected] Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

228

Regina P El Dib reported higher rate of satisfaction wearing HL SmartFit when compared to E-A-R PushIns or Sonomax SonoCustoms. Conclusion: The evidence found in this review showed that the HL SmartFit earplug is more effective compared to E-A-R Push-Ins foam earplug and Sonomax SonoCustoms regarding the number of difficulties per number of conversations and satisfaction. Future trials should have standardized outcomes measures such as attenuation, speech intelligibility and audibility (hear communication) in the noise environment, worker acceptance of HPDs and the likelihood that workers will wear them consistently, hearing loss thresholds by audiometric test, costs and others.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

1. INTRODUCTION Noise-induced hearing loss is one of the most common occupational diseases. Approximately 30 million workers in the USA alone are exposed to hazardous noise at work (WHO 2002) and an annual worldwide incidence of noise-induced hearing loss of 1,628,000 cases (Leigh 1999). With a worldwide population of 6.525 billion this is equal to 25 per 100,000 per year (El Dib 2009). The condition is permanent and irreversible. Early damage is typically sustained in the basal turn of the cochlea and affects hearing in the frequency range from 3000 to 6000 hertz (Hz) (the frequency of speech) (El Dib 2009). Long-term exposure to noise levels beyond 80 dB(A) carries an increased risk of hearing loss which increases exponentially with the noise level. The risk of hearing impairment (average hearing loss > 35 dB(A) at 1, 2 and 3 kHz) at age 60 due to 40 years of exposure to noise levels of 100 dB(A) has been estimated at 55% (Malchaire 1997). There is no effective treatment for permanent hearing loss resulting from excessive noise exposure. The ideal situation for preventing hearing loss would be the reduction of noise level at the source and, in most cases this is hard to obtain. Therefore, the condition can be easily prevented using preventative measures such as personal hearing protection devices or hearing protector (i.e., earplugs, earmuffs) and hearing conservation programmes or hearing loss prevention programme. The effectiveness of personal protective measures at preventing hearing loss from accumulated noise exposure depends mainly on how regularly they are used by workers. Studies have shown that if workers do not wear hearing protection 100% of the time its effectiveness will quickly diminish. For example, wearing hearing protection for only 90% of the time will decrease effectiveness to less than one third (Arezes 2002). Educational or behavioural interventions to promote its use are therefore important preventative measures. A systematic review of the literature evaluated the effectiveness of interventions to influence workers to wear hearing protection to decrease their exposure to noise. The evidence found in this review showed that tailored interventions (the use of communication or other types of treatments that are specific for an individual or a group to change behavior) improve the mean use of hearing protective device versus non-intervention. Tailored education was more effective in the use of HPDs compared with target education programmes. However, the authors concluded that future trials should be performed with standard outcomes and interventions to allow the combining of results in a meta-analysis (El Dib 2009). Intention to use the devices, perceived benefits and barriers to the use of hearing protection, proper training, fit the hearing protection devices (HPDs) properly to achieve a

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

A Systematic Review of Hearing Protective Devises: Types, Uses and Safety

229

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

optimum attenuation are important variables to help workers on the permanent use of personal protective measures in the noise working environment. The HPDs should be in the ear far enough to satisfy a cursory visual compliance check (Brad 2007). The difference between a good insertion and a poor insertion is enough to cause a 30-40 dB improvement in attenuation at various frequencies when the personal protective measures is deeply inserted and achieves a good seal in the ear (Brad 2007). There are different fit test methods available to measure the attenuation of hearing protectors and, thereby the effectiveness of HPDs through audiograms with and without their HPDs both before and after HPD insertion, before and after HPD removal during the work shifts. How much attenuation a hearing protector provides depends on its characteristics and how the worker wears it. The noise attenuation of hearing protectors as they are worn in the occupational environment is usually different from that realized in the laboratory. Hearing protectors often make communication difficult by reducing and distorting sounds. The main types of hearing protectors: (i) formable earplugs (such as silicon or spun mineral fiber) made of expandable foam; (ii) pre-molded and custom-molded earplugs made from flexible plastics; (iii) semi-aural devices, or canal caps (semi-inserts) , consisting of flexible tips on a lightweight headband; and (iv) earmuffs having rigid cups with soft plastic cushions that seal around the ears. Figures 1, 2, 3 and 4 show the different types of earmuffs and earplugs.

Figure 1. Over-the-Head earmuff.

Figure 2. Earmuff helmet mounted.

Figure 3. Foam earplugs.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

230

Regina P El Dib

Figure 4. Pre-formed earplugs.

Figure 5. Semi-inserted (canal caps).

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 6. Earplug dB blocker vented.

Figure 7. Custom-devices.

Usually the foam earplug is the most comfortable hearing protector, however requires more time to fit and requires good hygiene. The pre-former earplugs have an easier insertion, but can be uncomfortable to some for long wear. The semi-insert device is convenient and lightweight, but it is usually indicated for intermittent exposure. The earmuffs have different electronic models available for enhanced communication and special applications, but also are indicated for intermittent exposure; and the custom-devices are customized with easy insertion; however have a high initial cost (Alaska Occupational Audiology and Health Services). Despite the fact that earplugs and earmuffs have been widely used, its effectiveness and safety with regards a higher attenuation; better communication between workers while on the use of HPDs, comfort, convenience and good seal was never determined, mainly, which hearing protector presents better performance for workers exposure to noise levels use it.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

A Systematic Review of Hearing Protective Devises: Types, Uses and Safety

231

2. OBJECTIVES To summarize the evidence for the effectiveness and safety of different types of hearing protective devices among workers exposed to noise in the workplace.

3. CRITERIA FOR CONSIDERING STUDIES FOR THIS REVIEW Types of Studies All randomized and quasi-randomized controlled trials and cluster randomized controlled trials which fulfilled the criteria outlined below were included.

Types of Participants All workers exposed to noise levels above 80 dB(A) and who had the opportunity to wear ear protection because of occupational exposure to noise.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Types of Interventions This review considered all types of personal hearing protection devices or hearing protector (earmuffs, earplugs and canal caps). We also considered different types of earmuffs such as helmet mounted and over the head as well as different types of earplugs (custom-molded silicone, expandable foam earplug, etc). We also considered the combination of different types of hearing protection devices (e.g. earmuffs in conjunction with earplugs). Interventions were to be compared against each other. The workers should be preliminary trained on how to correctly wear the hearing protective device and its adjustments, as would it be in a laboratory study, to allow a better definition of its attenuation and consequently, a better evaluation of the hearing protection performance.

Types of Outcome Measures Primary outcome measure  Attenuation, using the real ear attenuation at threshold – REAT – test, which is based on the difference between the open and occluded hearing level of a subject;  Speech intelligibility and audibility (hear communication) in the noise environment, analyzed by videotape during the worker´s activities through various signs of speech communication difficulties such as removal of earplugs, distance between communicating people at less than one arm´s length, cupping ear with the hand,

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

232

Regina P El Dib



place ear next to the mouth of the speaker, increased use of hand gestures, writing, requests for clarification, repeated phrases in conversation, etc. Elimination of threshold shift (caused by continuous noise of high intensity).

Secondary outcome measures  Worker acceptance of HPDs and the likelihood that workers will wear them consistently (intention to use the devices): comfort and convenience, awareness of risk, ease of fit/good seal (self-reported use of hearing protection);  Perceived benefits and barriers to use of hearing protection (self-reported use of hearing protection);  Hearing loss thresholds by audiometric test;  Costs (as a narrative analysis). There were no specific exclusion criteria.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Search Strategy for Identification of Studies There was no language restriction. Trials were obtained from the following sources: We searched the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL, The Cochrane Library, Issue 1 2009), PUBMED (1966 to February 2009), EMBASE (1980 to February 2009), LILACS (1982 to February 2009), and Current Controlled Trials to identify ongoing trials. The date of the last search was 16th February 2009. The search strategy was composed only of terms for hearing protection in order to maximize sensitivity. As we searched with both subject headings and free text words, it was expected that all studies of hearing protection would be identified. The following exhaustive list of synonyms for hearing protective devices was identified: ((Ear Protective Device) OR (Ear Protective Devices) OR Earplugs OR Earplug OR (Hearing Protective Devices) OR (Hearing Protective Device) OR Earmuffs OR Earmuff OR (hearing protector) OR (hearing protection) OR (ear muff) OR (ear muffs) OR (ear plug) OR (ear plugs) OR (ear defender) OR (ear defenders)) AND ((Occupational Noise) OR (Occupational Noises))

We modeled subject strategies for databases on the search strategy designed for CENTRAL. Where appropriate, we combined subject strategies with adaptations of the highly sensitive search strategy for identifying randomized controlled trials and controlled clinical trials. Search strategies for the type of study (randomized clinical trials) for each databases including PubMed are shown in Appendix 1. Reference lists of the identified relevant studies were scrutinized for additional citations. Specialists in the field and authors of the included trials were contacted for unpublished data.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

A Systematic Review of Hearing Protective Devises: Types, Uses and Safety

233

4. METHODS OF THE REVIEW The author screened the trials identified by the literature search, extracted the data, assessed trial quality and analyzed the results.

4.1. Quality Assessment The methodological quality of the included trials in this review was measured using the criteria described in the Cochrane Handbook (Higgins 2008), since scales and checklists are not a reliable method to assess the validity of a primary study (Jüni 1999): Selection bias – Was allocation concealment adequate? MET: adequate concealment of allocation; UNCLEAR: not described, not reported; NOT MET: inadequate; Not used.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Selection bias - Was the generation of allocation sequence adequate? MET: Adequate generation (random); UNCLEAR: Unclear, not described in the paper or by contacting authors; NOT MET: Inadequate generation of allocation; Not used. Detection bias – Was there a blinded assessment of outcomes? MET: assessor unaware of the assigned treatment when collecting outcome measures; UNCLEAR, not reported: blinding of assessor not reported and cannot be verified by contacting investigators; NOT MET: assessor aware of the assigned treatment when collecting outcome measures. Attrition bias – Were any withdrawals described? MET: lesser than 20% and equally for both comparison groups; UNCLEAR: not reported in paper or by authors; NOT MET: greater than 20% or/and not equally for both comparison groups.

4.2. Data Extraction A standard form was used to extract the following information: characteristics of the study (design, methods of randomisation); participants; interventions; outcomes (types of outcome measures, continuous or dichotomous) (Appendix 2).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

234

Regina P El Dib

4.3. Data Analysis For dichotomous data, risk relative (RR) was used as the effect measure. For continuous data the weighted mean difference (WMD), in which the effect estimates of individual studies are weighted by dispersion measures, was used.

4.3.1. Heterogeneity It was not necessary to measure heterogeneity in this version of the review. Inconsistency among the pooled estimates should be quantified using the i-squared statistic. This illustrates the percentage of the variability in effect estimates resulting from heterogeneity rather than sampling error (Higgins 2003; Higgins 2005). I2 = [(Q - df)/Q] x 100% test, where Q is the chi-squared statistic and df its degrees of freedom. 4.3.2. Sensitivity analysis Sensitivity analysis to assess the effect of including or not including studies based on their methodological quality was not carried out in this review due to the low number of included studies. 4.3.3. Addressing publication bias Again, due to the small number of studies included in this review we were not able to assess publication bias by preparing a funnel plot (trial effect versus trial size).

5. RESULTS

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

5.1. Description of Studies 5.1.1. Study selection The search strategy identified approximately 585 titles (see Figure 8). Following assessment of 47 full text articles only 21 publications were considered for inclusion in this review. 19 studies were subsequently excluded (Nakao 2008; Abel 2006; Neitzel 2006; Lin 2005; Pääkkönen 2005; Ong 2004; Horie 2002; Wagstaff 2001; Pääkkönen 1998; Bhattacharya 1993; Dancer 1992; Sataloff 1986; Royster 1984; Chung 1983; Erlandsson 1980; Ivarsson 1980; Royster 1980; Smith 1980; Taniewski 1978). Two studies which met the minimal methodological requirements were included in the review (Wagoner 2007 and Lempert 1983). 5.1.2. Included studies Two studies were included in this review (Wagoner 2007 and Lempert 1983) with a total of 46 participants. 5.1.3. Design of the studies Wagoner 2007 study was a cross-over multicenter randomized controlled trial which subjects were recruited from the maintenance area of two airports (American Trans Air and Purdue University).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

A Systematic Review of Hearing Protective Devises: Types, Uses and Safety

Other Sources

Database Search Pubmed Embase Central Lilacs Current Controlled Trials

0

235

585 529 26 16 06 08

Current Control

Full paper review

Excluded

538

Irrelevant/narrative review

538

47

Excluded after full reading text 26 Others outcomes/others questions 26

Excluded

19

Case series Controlled clinical trial Retrospective studies Cross-sectional comparative study RCT; however one of group was not exposure

03 11 03 01 01

to occupational noise

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Included

02 studies

1.

Wagoner L, McGlothlin J, Chung K, Strickland E, Zimmerman N, Carlson G. Evaluation of noise attenuation and verbal communication capabilities using three ear insert hearing protection systems among airport maintenance personnel. J Occup Environ Hyg 2007; 4(2):114-22.

2.

Lempert BL, Edwards RG. Field investigations of noise reduction afforded by inserttype hearing protectors. Am Ind Hyg Assoc J 1983; 44(12):894-902.

Figure 8. Chart of the systematic review.

Lempert 1983 study was a multicenter cluster randomized controlled trial at fifteen different industrial plants. There were two field investigations, one conducted in 1977 and the second in 1981.

5.1.4. Participants and duration of trials In Wagoner 2007 study a total of 28 subjects participated in the study. All subjects were male and had an average of 41.3 years. The follow-up period was not described. In Lempert 1983 study seven workers were selected randomly from each of four groups (high-active, high-passive, low-active and low-passive; please see description under types of intervention session), for a total of 28 workers at each plant. The authors did not report the follow-up period.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

236

Regina P El Dib

5.1.5. Types of intervention In Wagoner 2007 each subject wore all three HPDs types: Sonomax SonoCustoms (Sonomax, Montreal, Canada) (custom-device), E-A-R Push-Ins foam earplugs (E-A-R, Indianapolis, Ind.) (user-molded), and Howard Leight SmartFits (HLS, Smithfield, R.I.) (adaptable shape). The subjects wore each of the three earplugs at different times and in different orders. All subjects received manufactures‘ directions for the insertion of the earplugs. The first field investigation conducted in 1977 in Lempert 1983 study included an evaluation of three types of earplugs (one performed twin-flanged type, one performed singleflaged type known as V-51R, and one user-formed acoustic type. The second field investigation included also acoustic wool, custom-molded, and user-molded expandable foam. All workerd were categorized by the following variables: physical activity (either active e.g. production-line worker or passive e.g. supervisor) and by noise exposure (either high e.g. greater than 90dB or low e.g. less than 90dB. For the purpose of this review we focused just on the high classification with regards the noise exposure category. At each plant, all participants used the same earplug type and this characterized this study as a cluster design.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

5.1.6. Types of outcome measures In Wagoner 2007 study the outcomes of interest were: speech intelligibility (whether a difficulty was present during a conversation), and average attenuation results for each HPDs. The authors also analyzed the length of conversation and average noise level to help on the assessment of speech intelligibility. The videotape of the worker´s activities was analyzed for various signs of speech communication difficulties. The only outcome evaluated in Lempert 1983 study was the real-ear noise attenuation. 5.1.7. Notes Wagoner 2007 study: 1. The study performed the trial as a laboratory and field studies and in both situation there was the randomization process; 2. As the subjects were recruited from the maintenance area of airports we hypothesized that all participants were exposed (> 80 dB(A)) as well as the study described the occurrence of communication abilities in noisy environments; 3. The authors did not report the study period or the justification of the sample size. Lempert 1983 study: 1. One of the plants included in the first field investigation in 1977 was repeated in 1981, but the authors of the study just included the 1981 data from this plant in the paper to avoid repeat the same participants in their analysis; 2. The authors did not report the justification of the sample size, the setting of the study, neither the sex nor age of the participants.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

A Systematic Review of Hearing Protective Devises: Types, Uses and Safety

237

5.1.8. Excluded studies 19 studies are described in the table 1 (Nakao 2008; Abel 2006; Neitzel 2006; Lin 2005; Pääkkönen 2005; Ong 2004; Horie 2002; Wagstaff 2001; Pääkkönen 1998; Bhattacharya 1993; Dancer 1992; Sataloff 1986; Royster 1984; Chung 1983; Erlandsson 1980; Ivarsson 1980; Royster 1980; Smith 1980; Taniewski 1978). The main reason for exclusion was that the studies were not randomized or quasi-randomized controlled trials. 5.1.9. Awaiting assessment No study is awaiting assessment.

5.2. Methodological Quality of Included Studies 5.2.1. Allocation (sequence generation and allocation concealment) Wagoner 2007 study described the method of allocation as ‗table of random digits‘, therefore it was considered ‗adequate‘. However, allocation concealment was not reported (‗unclear‘). Lempert 1983 study did not report the generation of allocation neither the allocation concealment making this study classified as ‗unclear‘. Table 1. Characteristics of excluded studies

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Study ID Nakao 2008 Abel 2006 Neitzel 2006

Lin 2005 Pääkkönen 2005 Ong 2004 Horie 2002 Wagstaff 2001 Pääkkönen 1998 Bhattacharya 1993 Dancer 1992 Sataloff 1986 Royster 1984 Chung 1983 Erlandsson 1980 Ivarsson 1980 Royster 1980 Smith 1980 Taniewski 1978

Reason for exclusion Case series Controlled clinical trial Controlled clinical trial (the subjects were only randomized to four test groups of five workers to facilitate testing; they were not randomized to the interventions) Case series Controlled clinical trial Cross-sectional comparative study Case series Controlled clinical trial Controlled clinical trial Randomized controlled trial; however one of the group was not exposure to occupational noise Controlled clinical trial Controlled clinical trial Controlled clinical trial Controlled clinical trial Retrospective study Retrospective study Controlled clinical trial Retrospective study Controlled clinical trial

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

238

Regina P El Dib

5.2.2. Blinding Both studies (Wagoner 2007 and Lempert 1983) did not report whether investigators and patients were blinded to the treatment allocation (‗unclear‘). 5.2.3. Description of drop-outs/withdrawals Wagoner 2007 study had withdrawals of less than 20% and therefore the assessment of attrition bias was recorded as 'met' (just two subjects dropped out). The intention-to-treat analysis was not used. In Lempert 1983 study there was replacement of participants for those who chose not to participate after the randomization process, therefore, we considered it as withdrawal, however the authors did not described the number of participants who were replaced. This study was classified as ‗unclear‘.

5.3. Effect of Interventions It was not possible to combine the included studies (Wagoner 2007 and Lempert 1983) in a meta-analysis. Furthermore, it was not possible to perform representation of meta-analysis from Lempert 1983 study due to the fact that the authors did not present the data from high versus low noise exposure separately.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

5.3.1. Attenuations and speech intelligibility The mean attenuations at each octave band were not statistically different among the HPDs testes in Wagoner 2007 laboratory study. In addition, the type of HPDs and the sequence in each the plug was testes were not significantly different from each other regarding the speech intelligibility (Wagoner 2007 laboratory study).

5.3.2. Elimination of threshold shift The included studies did not report this outcome. 5.3.3. Hearing loss thresholds by audiometric test The included studies did not report this outcome. 5.3.4. Costs (as a narrative analysis) The included studies did not report this outcome.

6. DISCUSSION The ideally way to prevent noise-induced hearing loss for workers is the removal of hazardous noise from the workplace. However, when engineering controls and work practices are not feasible for reducing noise exposures to safe levels, the best solution is the wearing of hearing protectors consistently.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

(1) Sonomax SonoCustoms versus E-A-R Push-Ins foam earplugs (field study)

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 9. Representation of meta-analysis from Wagoner 2007 field study that compared Sonomax SonoCustoms versus E-A-R Push-Ins foam earplugs. There was no statistically significant difference between the earplugs regarding the number of difficulties per number of conversations (RR 0.78 [95% Confidential Interval (CI) 0.47 to 1.30]). Note: the type of Sonomax used in this outcome was the full block.

Figure 10. Representation of meta-analysis from Wagoner 2007 field study that compared Sonomax SonoCustoms versus E-A-R Push-Ins foam earplugs. There was a statistically significant difference favouring participants wearing the Sonomax regarding the satisfaction of earplugs (WMD 8.00 [95% CI 6.06 to 9.94]).

(2) Sonomax SonoCustoms versus HL SmartFit (field study)

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 11. Representation of meta-analysis from Wagoner 2007 field study that compared Sonomax SonoCustoms versus HL SmartFit. There was no statistically significant difference between the earplugs regarding the number of difficulties per number of conversations (RR 1.68 [95% CI 0.95 to 2.96]). Note: the type of Sonomax used in this outcome was the full block.

Figure 12. Representation of meta-analysis from Wagoner 2007 field study that compared Sonomax SonoCustoms versus HL SmartFit. There was a statistically significant difference favouring participants wearing HL SmartFit regarding the satisfaction of earplugs (WMD -3.00 [95% CI -5.07 to -0.93]).

(3) E-A-R Push-Ins versus HL SmartFit (field study)

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 13. Representation of meta-analysis from Wagoner 2007 field study that compared E-A-R Push-Ins versus HL SmartFit. There was a statistically significant difference favouring the HL SmartFit regarding the number of difficulties per number of conversations (RR 2.14 [95% CI 1.29 to 3.55]).

Figure 14. Representation of meta-analysis from Wagoner 2007 field study that compared E-A-R Push-Ins versus HL SmartFit. There was a statistically significant difference favouring participants wearing HL SmartFit regarding the satisfaction of earplugs (WMD -11.00 [95% CI -13.00 to -9.00]).

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

242

Regina P El Dib

Various hearing protectors are available at the market including different materials and styles designed for specific applications and worker preferences to facilitate communication, quickly insertion and to provide a good attenuation values. It is also well know that different HPDs provide a wide range of protection levels. To determine which hearing protector is the effectiveness device to provide workers exposure to noise levels above 80 dB(A) highest attenuation values, best communications properties, elimination of threshold shift caused by continuous noise of high intensity as well as comfort and convenience, we had decided to summarize the findings in the literature in a systematic review. The systematic reviews (a summary of research that uses explicit methods to perform a thorough literature search and critical appraisal of individual studies to identify the valid and applicable evidence) aim to reduce uncertainty in order to help to make uniform clinical decisions. This systematic review offers up to date but limited evidence supported by two randomized controlled trials on the effectiveness of just one type of hearing protective device: earplugs. The methodological quality of the studies was very poor, even though there was a substantial risk of selection bias in both included studies due to the lack of description in the allocation concealment procedure. However, Lempert 1983 study used a valuable method to avoid contamination bias and it is often used in occupational health settings: the cluster randomization. The two studies that we found (Wagoner 2007 and Lempert 1983) compared different types of earplugs to determine its attenuation values and speech intelligibility. It was possible to present some of the data from Wagoner 2007 study graphically, but not possible to combine the results with the other included study due to the diversity of the outcomes reported, furthermore in Lempert 1983 study the authors did not separate the data from high versus low noise exposure categories. With regards to the number of difficulties per number of conversations outcome reported in Wagoner study, there was only a statistically significant difference favouring the participants that worn HL SmartFit compared to the other earplug E-A-R Push-Ins (Figure 13). The same applies for the outcome satisfaction self-reported by the participants which there were a statistically significant difference favouring the HL SmartFit earplug when compared to Sonomax SonoCustoms and the E-A-R Push-Ins devices (Figure 12 and 14). Although audiometric test are the ideal way for managing a hearing loss induced by noise exposure and evaluate the worker, this is considered a long-term outcome to be studied and support the effectiveness of HPDs. Thereby, there are others methods that provide us an immediate assessment of HPD effectiveness such as perceived benefits and barriers to use of hearing protection. None included studies reported about the perceived benefits and barriers of each hearing protector. This would be important information by characterizing this population and even the information to be used for the design of further types of earplugs and earmuffs that best fit the workers exposed to noise levels above 80 dB(A). A cross-sectional study of systematic reviews has found that the majority of Cochrane reviews highlight the absence or poor evidence around the questions on health care that has been covered by them. Around half of the reviews analysed in this study did not offer enough evidence for clinical practice, and the authors asked for further research (El Dib 2007). The same was observed in this systematic review of hearing protectors that, although it is considered the highest level of evidence in the research field, unfortunately, it does not provide us enough evidence to determine which HPD is more effective with regards

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

A Systematic Review of Hearing Protective Devises: Types, Uses and Safety

243

attenuation values, hear communication, comfort, etc. Future researchers should focus on the first part of this chapter to allow a better design of their clinical trials and answer which HPD has highest attenuation values, best communications properties, most comfort and convenience for the workers exposed at noisy activities.

7. REVIEWERS' CONCLUSIONS 7.1. Implications for Practice The evidence found in this review showed that the HL SmartFit earplug is more effective compared to E-A-R Push-Ins foam earplug regarding the number of difficulties per number of conversations. Furthermore, the HL SmartFit earplug is also more effective related to the satisfaction in wear the hearing protector self-reported by the participants when compared to Sonomax SonoCustoms and the E-A-R Push-Ins devices.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

7.2. Implications for Research More randomized controlled trials are needed with standard outcomes and interventions to make the combination of the results into a meta-analysis. To avoid the risk of contamination cluster randomised trials are preferred, proper adjustments should be made for the cluster effect and intra cluster correlation coefficients should be reported. Future trials should have standardised outcomes measures such as attenuation, speech intelligibility and audibility (hear communication) in the noise environment, worker acceptance of HPDs and the likelihood that workers will wear them consistently, hearing loss thresholds by audiometric test, costs and others. Drop-outs and loss to follow up should be reported. Future researchers should guide their clinical trials with reliable and realistic protocols to assess the real attenuation values and consider workers‘ beliefs, comfort and convenience.

8. POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST None known.

REFERENCES Abel SM, Odell P. Sound attenuation from earmuffs and earplugs in combination: maximum benefits vs. missed information. Aviat Space Environ Med, 2006, 77(9), 899-904. Alaska Occupational Audiology and Health Services. Hearing Protection Devices (HPD's). Available from http://www.alaskaoccupationalaudiology.com/hearing-protection-devices. htm Arezes, PM; Miguel, AS. Hearing protectors acceptability in noisy environments. Annals of Occupational Hygiene, 2002, 46(6), 531-536.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

244

Regina P El Dib

Bhattacharya, SK; Tripathi, SR; Kashyap, SK. Assessment of comfort of various hearing protection devices (HPD). J Hum Ergol (Tokyo), 1993, 22(2), 163-72. Chung, DY; Hardie, R; Gannon, RP. The performance of circumaural hearing protectors by dosimetry. J Occup Med, 1983, 25(9), 679-82. Dancer, A; Grateau, P; Cabanis, A; Barnabé, G; Cagnin, G; Vaillant, T; Lafont, D. Effectiveness of earplugs in high-intensity impulse noise. J Acoust Soc Am, 1992, 91(3), 1677-89. El Dib, R; Mathew, JL. Interventions to promote the wearing of hearing protection. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2009, Issue 4. Art. No.: CD005234. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD005234.pub3. El Dib, RP; Atallah, NA; Andriolo, RB. Mapping the Cochrane evidence for decision-making in health care. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice, 2007, 13(4), 689-92. Erlandsson, B; Håkanson, H; Ivarsson, A; Nilsson, P. The difference in protection efficiency between earplugs and earmuffs. An investigation performed at a workplace. Scand Audiol, 1980, 9(4), 215-21. Higgins, JPT; Green, S. (Editors). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions 5.0.0 [updated February 2008]. The Cochrane Collaboration, 2008. Available from www.cochrane-handbook.org. Higgins, JPT; Green, S. (Editors). Cochrane Reviewers' Handbook 4.2.5. Assessment of study quality. Section 4 [updated May 2005]. In: The Cochrane Library, Issue 3, 2005. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, 2005. Higgins, JPT; Thompson, SG; Deeks, JJ; Altman, DG. Measuring inconsistency in metaanalysis. British Medical Journal, 2003, 327, 555-7. Horie, S. Improvement of occupational noise-induced temporary threshold shift by active noise control earmuff and bone conduction microphone. Journal of Occupational Health, 2002, 44(6), 414-420. Ivarsson, A; Erlandsson, B; Håkanson, H; Nilsson, P. Differences in efficiency of earplugs and earmuffs measured as hearing impairments from two workshops. Scand Audiol Suppl, 1980, (Suppl 12), 194-9. Jüni, P; Witschi, A; Bloch, R; Egger, M. The hazards of scoring the quality of clinical trials for meta-analysis. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 1999, 282(11), 1054-60. Leigh, J; Macaskill, P; Kuosma, E; Mandryk, J. Global burden of disease and injury due to occupational factors. Epidemiology, 1999 Sep, 10(5), 626-31. Lempert, BL; Edwards, RG. Field investigations of noise reduction afforded by insert-type hearing protectors. Am Ind Hyg Assoc J, 1983, 44(12), 894-902. Lin, JH; Li, PC; Tang, ST; Liu, PT; Young, ST. Industrial wideband noise reduction for hearing aids using a headset with adaptive-feedback active noise cancellation. Med Biol Eng Comput, 2005, 43(6), 739-45. Malchaire, J; Piette, A. A comprehensive strategy for the assessment of noise exposure and risk of hearing impairment. Annals of Occupational Hygiene, 1997, 41(4), 467-84. Nakao, T; Horie, S; Tsutsui, T; Kawanami, S; Sasaki, N; Inoue, J. Earplug-type earphone with built-in microphone improves monosyllable intelligibility in noisy environments. J Occup Health, 2008, 50(2), 194-6. Neitzel, R; Somers, S; Seixas, N. Variability of real-world hearing protector attenuation measurements. Ann Occup Hyg, 2006, 50(7), 679-91.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

A Systematic Review of Hearing Protective Devises: Types, Uses and Safety

245

Ong, M; Choo, JT; Low, E. A self-controlled trial to evaluate the use of active hearing defenders in the engine rooms of operational naval vessels. Singapore Med J, 2004, 45(2), 75-8. Pääkkönen, R; Lehtomäki, K; Savolainen, S. Noise attenuation of communication hearing protectors against impulses from assault rifle. Mil Med, 1998, 163(1), 40-3. Pääkkönen, R; Lehtomäki, K. Protection efficiency of hearing protectors against military noise from handheld weapons and vehicles. Noise Health, 2005, 7(26), 11-20. Royster, LH; Royster, JD; Cecich, TF. An evaluation of the effectiveness of three hearing protection devices at an industrial facility with a TWA of 107 dB. J Acoust Soc Am, 1984, 76(2), 485-97. Royster, LH. An evaluation of the effectiveness of two different insert types of ear protection in preventing TTS in an industrial environment. Am Ind Hyg Assoc J, 1980, 41(3), 161-9. Sataloff, R; Sataloff, RT. Documenting hearing conservation tests equipment's effectiveness. Occup Health Saf, 198, 55(2), 28-36. Smith, CR; Wilmoth, JN; Borton, TE. Custom-molded earplug performance: a retrospective study. Scand Audiol, 1980, 9(2), 113-7. Taniewski, M; Zaborski, L; Szczepański, C; Nitka, J. [Effectiveness of ear protective devices]. Med Pr, 1978, 29(4), 307-16. Wagoner, L; McGlothlin, J; Chung, K; Strickland, E; Zimmerman, N; Carlson, G. Evaluation of noise attenuation and verbal communication capabilities using three ear insert hearing protection systems among airport maintenance personnel. J Occup Environ Hyg, 2007, 4(2), 114-22. Wagstaff, AS; Woxen, OJ. Double hearing protection and speech intelligibility-room for improvement. Aviat Space Environ Med, 2001, 72(4), 400-4. WHO - World Health Organization. The World Health Report (Chapter 4) Selected occupational risks. http://www.who.int/whr/2002/chapter4/en/index8.html 2002. Witt B. Putting the personal back into PPE: hearing protector effectiveness. Occupational Health & Safety, 2007, 76(6), 90, 92, 94.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

246

Regina P El Dib

APPENDIX 1 Database PUBMED

EMBASE

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

LILACS

Search strategy for randomized clinical trials (randomized controlled trial [Publication Type] OR controlled clinical trial [Publication Type] OR randomized controlled trials [MeSH Terms] OR random allocation [MeSH Terms] OR double blind method [MeSH Terms] OR single blind method [MeSH Terms] OR clinical trial [Publication Type] OR clinical trials [MeSH Terms] OR (clinical* [Text Word] AND trial* [Text Word]) OR single* [Text Word] OR double* [Text Word] OR treble* [Text Word] OR triple* [Text Word] OR placebos [MeSH Terms] OR placebo* [Text Word] OR random* [Text Word] OR research design [MeSH Terms] OR comparative study [MeSH Terms] OR evaluation studies [MeSH Terms] OR follow-up studies [MeSH Terms] OR prospective studies [MeSH Terms] OR control* [Text Word] OR prospectiv* [Text Word] OR volunteer* [Text Word]) ((Randomized controlled trial) or (Controlled study) or Randomization (Double blind procedure) or (Single blind procedure) or (Clinical trial) or (clinical adj5 trial*) or (doubl* or singl* or tripl* or trebl*) or (blind* or mask*) or Placebo* or Random* or (Methodology latin square) or crossover or cross-over or (Crossover Procedure) or (Drug comparison) or (Comparative study) (comparative trial*) or (control$ or prospectiv$ or volunteer$) or (Evaluation and Follow Up) or (Prospective study)) (Pt randomized controlled trial) OR (Pt controlled clinical trial) OR (Mh randomized controlled trials) OR (Mh random allocation) OR (Mh double blind method) OR (Mh single blind method) AND NOT (Ct animal) AND NOT (Ct human and Ct animal) OR (Pt clinical trial) OR (Ex E05.318.760.535$) OR (Tw clin$) AND (Tw trial$) OR (Tw ensa$) OR (Tw estud$) OR (Tw experim$) OR (Tw investiga$) OR (Tw singl$) OR (Tw simple$) OR (Tw doubl$) OR (Tw doble$) OR (Tw duplo$) OR (Tw trebl$) OR (Tw trip$) AND (Tw blind$) OR (Tw cego$) OR (Tw ciego$) OR (Tw mask$) OR (Tw mascar$) OR (Mh placebos) OR (Tw placebo$) OR (Tw random$) OR (Tw randon$) OR (Tw casual$) OR (Tw acaso$) OR (Tw azar) OR (Tw aleator$) OR (Mh research design) AND NOT (Ct animal) AND NOT (Ct human and Ct animal) OR (Ct comparative study) OR (Ex E05.337$) OR (Mh follow-up studies) OR (Mh prospective studies) OR (Tw control$) OR (Tw prospectiv$) OR (Tw volunt$) OR (Tw volunteer$) AND NOT ((Ct animal) AND NOT (Ct human and Ct animal))

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

A Systematic Review of Hearing Protective Devises: Types, Uses and Safety

APPENDIX 2 EXTRACTION SHEET ID – author, year of publication:

Action What will be asked to the author:

Methods

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10.

Design: Multicentre or Single-centre: Period: Sample size: Generation of Allocation: Allocation concealment: Blinded assessment of treatment allocation: Withdrawals and drop outs: Intention-to-treat analysis: Follow-up:

Participants 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6.

N: Sex: Age (mean): Setting: Inclusion criteria: Exclusion criteria:

Intervention 1. Experimental group: 2. Control Group:

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

247

248

Regina P El Dib

Outcomes 1. Primary Outcome: 2. Secondary Outcome: 3. Continuous or Dichotomous:

Notes

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

1. Conflict of interest: 2. Comments and notes:

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

In: Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System Editors: D. Fiedler and R. Krause, pp.249-262

ISBN: 978-1-60741-259-5 ©2010 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

Chapter 10

DPOAE ABNORMALITIES AFTER PERINATAL ASPHYXIA Ze Dong Jiang* Department of Paediatrics, University of Oxford, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, United Kingdom

ABSTRACT

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Perinatal asphyxia is an important risk for acquired hearing impairment in infants and children. Selection and implementation of a proper plan to intervene in hearing impairment requires accurate information about all frequencies important for speech and language development. Thus, it is crucial to obtain detailed information about cochlear function in infants with hearing impairment. Distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) have been widely used to examine cochlear function in infants. Recent studies found that infants after perinatal asphyxia showed a decrease in DPOAE pass rates at most frequencies in the first few days after birth. The decrease occurred mainly between 1 and 5 kHz, particularly 1 and 2 kHz. Overall DPOAE pass rate was also decreased. These results suggest that perinatal asphyxia damages the neonatal cochlea, which occurs mainly at the frequencies 1-5 kHz, particular at 1 and 2 kHz. At 1 month the decreased DPOAE pass rates did show any improvement. It seems that the impairment detected in the first few days after birth is unlikely to improve in later neonatal period. Follow-up studies revealed that at 6 months after birth DPOAE pass rates were increased slightly at most frequencies, but were decreased slightly at some other frequencies. At 12 months infants after perinatal asphyxia still demonstrated a decrease in DPOAE pass rates at most frequencies, particularly 1 and 2 kHz. Similarly, overall DPOAE pass rates were also decreased. The same was true for the comparison of the pass rates at 12 months with those at 6 months. These longitudinal prospective studies of DPOAEs indicate that hypoxia-ischemia that is associated with perinatal asphyxia adversely affects cochlear function. The major affected frequencies are 1 and 2 kHz. At 1 year, cochlear function remains relatively poor. Therefore, cochlear function, mainly at 1 and 2 kHz, is impaired after perinatal asphyxia, which is persistent during the postnatal development, with only *

Corresponding author: Telephone: ++44 1865 221364; Telefax: ++44 1865 221366. E-mail address: [email protected]

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

250

Ze Dong Jiang slightly improvement. These findings provide useful information for selecting and implementing early interventions in hearing impairment due to perinatal asphyxia.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

INTRODUCTION Experiments in animal models showed that hypoxemia have a direct effect on the cochlea and an indirect effect by way of cardiovascular collapse and cerebral ischaemia (Sohmer et al., 1986). The effects may lead to sensory or sensorineural hearing impairment. In human infants there is a critical period extending from before birth to about possibly 3 months at which time the auditory system is particularly susceptible to the effects of asphyxia (Jiang, 1995). Infants who suffer hypoxemia or hypoxia-ischemia during the perinatal period, i.e. perinatal asphyxia, has been recognized to be at risk of acquired hearing impairment (Borg, 1997; Fahnenstich et al., 1999; Jiang, 1995,1998; Jiang et al., 2004a, Mencher and Mencher, 1999; Newton, 2001; Sano et al., 2005; Wilkinson and Jiang, 2006). Recent studies using the brainstem auditory evoked responses (BAERs) have provided further evidence that hearing impairment occurs in infants after perinatal asphyxia, although such impairment is largely temporary (Jiang et al., 2004a; Wilkinson and Jiang, 2006). In a previous study of change in BAER threshold during the neonatal period in 92 term infants after perinatal asphyxia, we found that the threshold was elevated significantly on day 1 in these infants (Jiang et al., 2004a). The elevated threshold was decreased progressively on days 3 and 5, but was still significantly higher than in normal controls. The elevation was continuously decreased more slowly on day 10 and 15, and to a near normal level on day 30. Threshold elevation was seen in 31.7% of the babies on day 1, and 34.5% during the first 3 days. The rate of elevation was decreased progressively thereafter. On day 30, 10.6% of the subjects still had threshold elevation. It seems that hearing impairment after perinatal asphyxia is mostly temporary rather than permanent. However, permanent sensorineural hearing impairment does happen in some of the infants after perinatal asphyxia (Jiang, 1995). Selection and implementation of a proper plan to intervene in hearing impairment, e.g. fitting a hearing aid or performing a cochlear implant, requires accurate information about the hearing impairment at all frequencies important for speech and language development. Thus, it is crucial to obtain detailed information about cochlear function in the infant or child who has hearing impairment. The BAER, which is elicited mostly by click stimuli, lacks frequencyspecificity, and cannot identify which frequencies of the cochlear audiogram are affected in the cases with hearing impairment. In the last decade, otoacoustic emissions (OAEs), particularly distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs), have been widely used to examine cochlear function (American Academy of Pediatrics, 1999; Joint Committee on Infant Hearing, 1995,2000; Salata et al., 1998). The emissions are the result of non-linear properties of the cochlear basilar membrane, i.e., when two closely related pure tones at stimuli f1 and f2 are presented simultaneously, distortion products are generated with the largest occurring in the human ear at 2f1–f2. The level of the retrogradely conducted sound is recorded by a probe microphone in the external auditory canal. The DP audiogram has proven useful in estimating audiometric configuration (Gaskill and Brown, 1993; Kimberley et al., 1994; Lonsbury-Martin and Martin, 1990). The frequency pattern of DPOAE amplitude reduction or absence often follows the configuration of hearing loss in the audiogram. DPOAE, which can be obtained quickly, have now become

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

DPOAE Abnormalities after Perinatal Asphyxia

251

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

a widely used objective audiometric tool to assess infant‘s cochlear function and identify hearing impairment (Berg et al., 2005; Cone-Wesson et al., 2000; Gorga et al., 2000; Joint Committee on Infant Hearing, 1995,2000; Kemp and Ryan, 1993; Kemp et al., 1990; Mencher and Mencher, 1999; Norton and Stover, 1994; Noton et al., 2000a,b; Probst et al., 1991; Sininger, 1993; Sininger and Abdala, 1998; Smurzynski et al., 1993;Uziel and Piron, 1991; Vatovec et al., 2001). The emissions are absent when hearing loss due to cochlear pathology is 45 to 50 dB or greater (Sininger and Abdala, 1998). Hypoxia for a few minutes can severely inhibit OAE (Frolenkov et al., 1998; Kemp and Brown, 1984; Whitehead et al., 1992). Severe or lethal hypoxia damages cochlear function, leading to the DPOAEs disappearing at low-level stimulation (45–60 dB sound pressure level - SPL) (Rebillard et al., 1993). Understanding of what frequencies on the cochlear audiogram are affected by perinatal asphyxia is important for early intervention of hearing impairment aimed at improving auditory-learning and later speech, language, and cognitive development. In recent years, we have studied DPOAEs across the frequencies between 0.50 and 10 kHz, shortly after birth (at term and 1 month after birth) in infants who were born with perinatal asphyxia to elucidate what frequencies in the cochlear audiogram are susceptible to perinatal asphyxia (Jiang et al., 2005). These infants were selected in an intensive care unit. All has clinical signs of hypoxiaischemia (Levene, 2001; Levene and Evans, 2005; Volpe 2001), depressed Apgar score ( 6 at 5 min), and umbilical cord blood pH C) of the 12S rRNA gene was performed in the GJB2 negative patients who were compatible with a maternal inheritance of hearing impairment. Screening of the (GJB6-D13S1830) and 12S rRNA gene mitochondrial mutations 1555A>G, 7445A>G, 7472insC, 7510T>C, 7511T>C was negative in patients who had no GJB2 mutations. Specific GJB2 mutations spectrum and different GJB2 contributions for deafness were observed for seven main Volga-Ural ethnic groups studied (Table 3). The genetic origin of deafness is suspected in more than half of the congenital deafness cases [6]. Despite being a heterogeneous disorder with many genes contributing to its pathology, a high proportion of autosomal recessive nonsyndromic sensorineural hearing loss (ARNSHL) has been shown to be linked to DFNB1, which codes GJB2, a gap junction protein, connexin 26. DFNB1 causes 20% of all childhood deafness and may have a carrier rate ranging from 0 % to 8 % in different populations [3]. A high number of sequence variations have been described in the GJB2 gene and the associated pathogenic effects are not always clearly established. The prevalence of a number of mutations is known to be population specific, and therefore population specific testing should be a prerequisite step when molecular diagnosis is offered. Moreover, population studies are needed to determine the contribution of GJB2 variants to deafness. 35delG GJB2 gene mutation is particularly common, representing two-thirds of all Cx26 mutations in DFNB1 patients originating from various ethnic backgrounds [7]. In our early reports we studied the 35delG GJB2 gene mutation frequency in patients with nonsyndromic recessive deafness and in ethnic groups of the Volga–Ural region. So, we present our findings from the molecular diagnostic screening of the GJB2 and GJB6 genes over a four year period, together with a population-based study of GJB2 variants. Mutations were found in 59 of 100 (59%) NSHL patients and more frequently in familial (33.1%) than in sporadic cases (25.9%). Assortative mating can accelerate the genetic response to relaxed selection and it may have doubled the frequency of connexin deafness in developing countries during the past two centuries. In combination with positive selection, linguistic homogamy may also have accounted for the rapid fixation of the mutations required for the acquisition of speech 100,000 – 150,000 years ago [22]. This high frequency of 35delG mutation of the GJB2 gene could be attributed to the gene drift/founder effects in a certain population. The high level of Cx26 mutations and the finding that separate populations carry other common mutations suggest that heterozygote advantage for 35delG exists or existed under certain circumstances. Given the widespread expression of the gene in many tissues, reduced expression may confer a selective advantage via a multitude of pathways, making it difficult to predict a selection mechanism [2]. Nance et al. proposed a hypothesis for the high frequency of DFNB1 in many large populations of the world, on the basis of an analysis of the proportion of noncomplementary marriages among the deaf during the 19th century, which suggested that the frequency of DFNB1 may have doubled in the United States during the past 200 years. These so-called

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

296

Lilya U. Dzhemileva, Irina M. Khidiyatova and Elza K. Khusnutdinova

noncomplementary marriages between individuals with the same type of recessive deafness are incapable of producing hearing offspring, and the square root of their frequency among deaf marriages provides an upper limit for the prevalence of the most common form of recessive deafness at that time. To explain the increase, they suggested that the combination of intense assortative mating and relaxed selection increased both the gene and the phenotype frequencies for DFNB1. The proposed model assumed that in previous millennia the genetic fitness of individuals with profound congenital deafness was very low and that genes for deafness were then in a mutational equilibrium. The introduction of sign language in Europe in the 17th - 18th centuries was a key event that dramatically improved the social and economic circumstances of the deaf, along with their genetic fitness. In many countries, schools for the deaf were established, contributing to the onset of intense linguistic homogamy, i.e., mate selection based on the ability to communicate in sign language [22]. Our findings correlate with earlier reported data concerning Russian subjects with hearing loss [23] and confirm the main contribution of the 35delG mutation in deafness among Caucasian populations [24]. Other mutations, identified in this study, 312del14, del167T and 235delC have been reported previously [3]. Sequence analysis of further 200 control chromosomes does not detect any deletion, suggesting that the 312del14 mutation is not variant of alleles in the general population and that it is the basis of the disease in the deaf families. The 312del14 mutation is believed to be pathological: first, because of it location and conservation and, second, because no change has been observed in a series of normal control. The molecular mechanism underlies pathogenesis of the GJB2 gene 312del14 mutation, affects the cytoplasmic loop of connexin 26 and results in a premature termination of protein biosynthesis. So, 312del14 GJB2 mutation in the intracellular loop also causes loss of intracellular transport: connexons are formed but ionic transport is deficient because of incorrect alignment of connexons or inability to form a functional gap junction between two connexons [10]. Also the 312del14 mutation in GJB2 gene is the second most frequent cause of non-syndromic hearing impairment in the Volga-Ural region. In parallel, we have performed a molecular epidemiology study on 768 blood samples and established the frequency of the GJB2 variants in the Volga-Ural populations (Table 3). This study has revealed prevalence of the GJB2 sequence variations among Volga-Ural region populations. GJB2 gene contribution in deafness among deaf patients from the VolgaUral region (46% all patient‘s alleles) is mainly defined by recessive mutation 35dellG. The 35delG frequency in Mordvinians and Russians were similar to those established for the populations of Southern Europe, including Spain (3%) [7], Greece (3.5%) [1], and Italy, and for the Estonian population, which has the highest known frequency of 35delG mutation [24]. Other ethnic groups of the Volga–Ural region were similar in 35delG frequency (0.02) to the North and Central European populations and to American Caucasians [11]. This mutation is specific for some European populations (German, French, Estonian) with an allelic frequency up to 30% among deaf patients and carrier frequency from 0% to 6% in normal populations and represents the most prevalent GJB2 pathogenic mutation as it accounts for up to 80% of the pathogenic alleles in patients [24]. The Volga-Ural region shows a significantly higher carrier rate of the 35delG (1.4%), compared to the North-East part of Europe (0.9 % – 1/110 among 1,212 controls) and a lower carrier rate compared to other south European areas such as Spain (2.31 % – 1/43), Italy (3.45 % – 1/32) and Greece (3.54 % – 1/28) [24]. This situation has direct implications for genetic counseling as well as for the development of potential diagnostic kits.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Molecular Screening of Deafness in Populations and Patients with Nonsyndromic… 297

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

We identified 167delT GJB2 gene mutation in Chuvashes (2/100, 2%) and Komis (2/80, 2.5 %). Morell et al. (1998) found homozygosity for 167delT and compound heterozygosity for this mutation of GJB2 and the 35delG mutation in Ashkenazi Jewish families with nonsyndromic recessive deafness [14]. In the Ashkenazi-Jewish population, the prevalence of heterozygosity for 167delT, which is rare in the general population, was 4.03%. The frequency of the 167delT mutation carriers (total 4.76%) predicted a prevalence of 1 deaf person among 1,765 persons, which may account for most cases of nonsyndromic recessive deafness in the Ashkenazi Jewish population. Conservation of the haplotype flanking the 167delT mutation suggested that this allele had a single origin, whereas multiple haplotypes with the 35delG mutation suggested that this site is a hotspot for recurrent mutations. So, the 167delT mutation is the second most common connexin 26 mutation described and has only been reported previously among the Ashkenazi Jews [13]. The ancestry of GJB2 167delT is more difficult to determine, because the shared haplotype carrying 167delT is common in the general population. Recent studies have established that the 235delC mutation among East Asian populations was derived from a common ancestral founder. Yan (2003) stated that high frequency of the 235delC mutation in multiple East Asian populations suggested that it resulted from recurrent deletion at a mutation hotspot or was derived from a common ancestral founder [25]. Among East Asians, they observed significant linkage disequilibrium between 235delC and 5 linked polymorphic markers, suggesting that 235delC had derived from a common founder. The detection of this mutation only in East Asians, but not in Caucasians, and the small chromosomal interval of the shared haplotype suggested that it was an ancient mutation that had arisen after the divergence of Mongoloids and Caucasians. The finding that this mutation appears with a single haplotype argues against the possibility of recurrent mutation as an explanation for the high frequency of the allele. The 235delC mutation was screened in a total of 768 populations samples and detected in Mordvinians (carrier frequency is 0,012).

CONCLUSION Although a high heterogeneity of sequence variation was observed in patients, the 35delG mutation remains the most common pathogenic mutation in our region. Our data testify to the founder effect and suggest an eastward distribution of 35delG, since its frequency in FinnoUgric populations gradually decreases from Estonia to Komi. The question whether the Volga-Ural region of Russia could be one of the founder sources for the 235delC and 167delT mutations, widespread in Asia and Israel community remains open. The 312del14 mutation in GJB2 is the second most frequent cause of non-syndromic hearing impairment in the VolgaUral region. The high frequency of GJB2 mutations among deaf patients, testing of GJB2 among deaf children in the Volga-Ural region is worthwhile to provide special services at an early age. Furthermore, unnecessary invasive follow-up could be avoided, since GJB2 mutations are not associated with syndromic hearing loss or with inner ear malformations [1]. Mutations in GJB2 account for a large percentage of hearing loss in the Volga-Ural populations, making GJB2 of primary importance in diagnostic and research program for understanding genetics of deafness in our region.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Table 3. Frequency of the GJB2 sequence variations among population of Volga-Ural region5 Population of Volga-Ural region Bashkirs

N

240

480

Tatars

96

192

Russians

92

184

Chuvash

100

200

Mordvinians

80

160

Komi

80

160

Udmurts

80

160

5

Number of chromosomes

carrier frequency

del35G alleles frequency

carrier frequency

del167T alleles frequency

carrier frequency

del235C alleles frequency

0 0 (0-0.015) 1 .010 (0.000264 – 0.05667)

0 0 (0-0.007) 1 0.0052 (0.000132 – 0.02867)

0 0 (0-0.015) 0 0 (0-0.030)

0 0 (0-0.007) 0 0 (0-0.019)

0 0 (0-0.015) 0 0 (0-0.030)

0 0 (0-0.007) 0 0 (0-0.019)

2 0.021 (0.002615 -0.075533) 0 0 (0-0.036) 5 0.062 (0.020603 -0.139857) 0 0 (0 – 0.040) 3 .037 (0.007801 - 0.10570)

2 0.0104 (0.001264 – 0.03712) 0 0 (0-0.018) 5 0.0313 (0.010223 – 0.07141) 0 0 (0 – 0.022) 3 0.0188 (0.003884 – 0.05381)

0 0 (0-0.039) 2 .020 (0.002431 - 0.07038) 0 0 (0 – 0.04) 2 .025 (0,003042 - 0,08740) 0 0 (0 – 0.04)

0 0 (0-0.019) 2 0.0100 (0.001213 – 0.03565) 0 0 (0 – 0.022) 2 0,0125 (0,001517 - 0,04442) 0 0 (0 – 0.022)

0 0 (0-0.039) 0 0 (0-0.036) 1 .012 (0.000316-0.06768) 0 0 (0 – 0.04) 0 0 (0 – 0.04)

0 0 (0-0.019) 0 0 (0-0.018) 1 0.0063 (0.00015 – 0.03432) 0 0 (0 – 0.022) 0 0 (0 – 0.022)

Number of unrelated chromosomes tested frequency (95% confidence interval)

Molecular Screening of Deafness in Populations and Patients with Nonsyndromic… 299

REFERENCES [1] [2] [3] [4] [5] [6] [7] [8] [9] [10]

[11]

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

[12] [13]

[14]

[15]

[16] [17] [18] [19]

Petersen, M; Willems, P. Non-syndromic, autosomal-recessive deafness. Clin Genet, 2006, 69, 371-392. Walter, EN. The genetic of deafness. Ment Retard, 2003, 9, 109-119. Rothrock, CR; Murgia, A; Sartorato, EL. et al. Connexin 26 35delG does not represent a mutational hotspot. Hum. Genet, 2003, 113, 18-23. del Castillo, I; Villamar, M; Moreno-Pelayo, M. et al. A Deletion involving the connexin 30 gene in nonsyndromic hearing impairment. NEJM 2002, 346, 243-249. Lucotte, G; Mercier, G. Meta-analysis of GJB2 mutation 35delG frequencies in Europe. Genet Test, 2001, 5, 2, 149-152. Morton, NE. Genetic epidemiology of hearing impairment. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 2000, 630, 16-31. Estivill, X; Fortina, P; Surrey, S; Rabionet, R. et al. Connexin – 26 mutations in sporadic and inherited sensorineural deafness. Lancet, 1998, 351, 394-398. Goodenough, DA; Goliger, JA; Paul, DL. Connexins, connexons and intracellular communication. Ann Rev Biochem, 1996, 65, 475-502. Van Camp, G; Smith, RH. [Hereditary hearing loss homepage] 2004: http://dnalabwww.uia.ac.be/dnalabhhhh/. Bruzzone, R; White, TV; Scherer, SS; Fishbeck, KH; Paul, DL. Null mutation of connexin 32 in patients X-linked dominated Charcot-Mari-Tooth disease. Neuron, 1994, 135, 1253-1260. Gasparini, P; Rabionet, R; Barbujani, G. et al. High carrier frequency of the 35delG deafness mutation in European populations. Eur J Hum Genet, 2000, 8, 19-23. Green, GE; Scott, DA; McDonald, JM. et al. Carrier rates in the midwestern United States for GJB2 mutations causing inherited deafness. JAMA, 1999, 281, 2211-2216. Sobe, T; Vreugde, S; Shahin, H. et al. The prevalence and expression of inherited connexin 26 mutations associated with nonsyndromic hearing loss in the Israeli population. Hum Genet, 2000, 106, 50-57. Morell, RJ; Kim, HJ; Hood, LJ. et al. Mutations in the connexin 26 gene (GJB2) among Ashkenazi Jews with nonsyndromic recessive deafness. N Engl J Med, 1998, 339, 1500-1505. Kudo, T; Ikeda, K; Kure, S. Novel mutations in the connexin 26 gene (GJB2) responsible for childhood deafness in the Japanese population. Am J Med Genet, 2000, 90, 141-145. Park, HJ; Hahn, SH; Chun, YM. et al. Connexin26 mutations associated with nonsyndromic hearing loss. Laryngoscope, 2000, 110, 1535 -1538. Dai, P ; Yu, F ; Han, B. et al. GJB2 mutation spectrum in 2,063 Chinese patients with nonsyndromic hearing impairment. J Transl Med, 2009, 7, 26 - 29. Richard, S; Kelsell, DP; Sirimana, T. et al. Recurrent mutations in the deafness gene GJB2 (connexin 26) in British Asian Families. J Med Genet, 2001, 38, 530-533. del Castillo, I; Villamar, M; Moreno-Pelayo, MA. et al.. A deletion involving the connexin 30 gene in nonsyndromic hearing impairment. New Engl J Med, 2002, 346, 243-249.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

300

Lilya U. Dzhemileva, Irina M. Khidiyatova and Elza K. Khusnutdinova

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

[20] Orita, M; Iwahana, H; Kanazawa, H. et al. Detection of polymorphism of human DNA by gel electrophoresis as single cell conformation polymorphism. Proc Natl Acad Sci, 1989, 86, 2766-2770. [21] Kelsell, DP; Dunlop, J; Stevens, HP. et al. Connexin 26 gene mutations in hereditary non-syndromic sensorineural deafness. Nature, 1997, 387, 80-83. [22] Nance, WE; Kearsey, M J. Relevance of connexin deafness (DFNB1) to human evolution. Am J Hum Genet, 2004, 74, 1081-1087. [23] Anichkina, A; Kulenich, T; Zinchenko, S. et al. On the origin and frequency of the 35delG allele in GJB2-linked deafness in Europe. Eur J Hum Genet, 2001, 9(2),151. [24] Gasparini, P; Rabionet, R; Barbujani, G. et al. High carrier frequency of the 35delG deafness mutation in European populations. Genetic Analysis Consortium of GJB2 35delG. Eur J Hum Genet, 2000, 8(1), 19-23. [25] Yan, D; Park, HJ ; Ouyang, XM ; et al. Evidence of a founder effect for the 235delC mutation of GJB2 (connexin 26) in east Asians. Hum Genet, 2003, 114(1), 44-50. [26] Morell, RJ; Kim, HJ; Hood, LJ. et al. Mutations in the connexin 26 gene (GJB2) among Ashkenazi Jews with nonsyndromic recessive deafness. New Eng J Med, 1998, 339, 1500-1505.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

In: Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System Editors: D. Fiedler and R. Krause, pp.301-311

ISBN: 978-1-60741-259-5 ©2010 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

Chapter 14

SIGNS OF BINAURAL PROCESSING WITH BILATERAL COCHLEAR IMPLANTS IN THE CASE OF SOMEONE WITH MORE THAN 50 YEARS OF UNILATERAL DEAFNESS Celene McNeill1,2*, William Noble3,4 and Anna O’Brien5 1

Healthy Hearing & Balance Care Macquarie University, Australia 3 University of New England, Australia 4 University of Iowa 5 National Acoustic Laboratories, Australia

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

2

ABSTRACT A case is presented of a 70-year-old man with a profound sensorineural hearing loss in the right ear since childhood and who developed sudden severe hearing loss in the left ear at age 63. Eventually, after he received cochlear implants in both ears, he started to present behavioural auditory processing skills associated with binaural hearing, such as improved ability understanding speech in the presence of background noise, and sound localization. Responsiveness and outcomes were measured using cortical auditory evoked potentials, speech perception in noise, sound localization performance , and a self-rating questionnaire. The results suggest that even after more than 50 years of unilateral deafness it is possible to develop binaural interaction and sound localization.

INTRODUCTION Plasticity of the auditory brain has been of increasing interest, especially since the advent of cochlear implants. Pre-lingually deaf children have responded well to electric auditory stimulation and the earlier the intervention the more their auditory processing skills are *

Corresponding author: 1204/1 Newland St, Bondi Junction NSW, Australia 2026, E-mail: [email protected]

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

302

Celene McNeill, William Noble and Anna O‘Brien

similar to those of normal hearing children. Adults with acquired profound hearing loss also present very good outcomes but these have been reported to correlate negatively with duration of deafness and absence of previous auditory stimulation (Tyler and Summerfield, 1996). Adults with long-term deafness have been considered less likely to develop good auditory outcomes with cochlear implantation. However, outcomes of electric stimulation in long-term deafness have not been widely explored. The present study is of a 70 year-old male, profoundly deaf in the right ear since childhood. He had experienced the common problems associated with a unilateral hearing loss such as head shadow effects, difficulty understanding speech in noise and inability to localize sounds. At the age of 63 he developed a sudden severe sensorineural hearing loss in his left ear and a hearing aid was fitted, with limited success. Six months later he received a cochlear implant in the right ear and retained a hearing aid in the left ear. After three years he stopped wearing the hearing aid for lack of perceived benefit and received an implant in that ear. Two years following constant bilateral electric stimulation he began showing signs of binaural function. Unilateral hearing loss is known to alter neuronal activation and binaural interactions in the auditory pathways. Khosla et al. (2003) found reduction in ipsilateral-contralateral amplitude differences for N1-P2 by measuring cortical auditory evoked potential (CAEP) in patients with profound left ear deafness. This finding indicates reorganisation in the auditory cortex in unilateral left deafness, with cortical activation increasing in the left hemisphere. In contrast, patients with unilateral right deafness have not shown evidence of reduced ipsilateral-contralateral amplitude differences. This suggests there is less compensatory plasticity increase in activation of the left hemisphere with deafness in the right ear alone. This case study is of a 70 year-old male (P.M.) with a profound sensorineural hearing loss in the right ear since childhood and a later onset fluctuating moderate-severe sensorineural hearing loss in the left ear. The duration of profound hearing loss in the right ear was longer than 50 years and was attributed to mumps in early childhood. P.M. first became aware of his profound hearing loss in the right ear at school when he was 8 years old. In 2001 he had a sudden hearing loss in the left ear, which was diagnosed as secondary endolymphatic hydrops. Computerized tomography scans of the temporal bones revealed an asymmetry in size of the cochlear aqueducts, the left being larger than the right.

TEST METHODS, MEASURES AND RESULTS Speech Perception and Auditory Evoked Potentials P.M. was referred to the audiologist for hearing aid assessment after the episode of sudden left hearing loss, and a hearing aid was fitted in the left ear. Hearing levels in the left ear continued to fluctuate, making it difficult to programme the hearing aid. P.M. was not considered a suitable candidate for a cochlear implant, as audiological assessment showed aided speech recognition scores of 95% with his left hearing aid in free-field using CID sentences. This result was above CI candidacy guidelines at the time, which recommend aided speech scores in quiet worse than 70% as a criterion for implantation (Dowell et al., 2003). Furthermore, the right ear had not had auditory stimulation for over 50 years, which

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Signs of Binaural Processing with Bilateral Cochlear Implants…

303

was considered a contra-indication for implantation. In spite of this it was agreed that an implant in the right ear would be attempted, as there was ―nothing to lose‖. P.M‘s CAEP with the CI in the right ear and hearing aid in the left ear were recorded using a high frequency stimulus 6 months and 9 months after implantation to follow his cortical responses. The stimulus was selected based on previous evidence for robust cortical responses to 4 kHz tone bursts in adult CI users (Kelly et al., 2005). Changes were also expected for cortical responses in this frequency region based on previous evidence for high frequency cortical reorganisation in humans with acquired hearing loss (e.g. Dietrich et al., 2001; Thai-Van et al., 2003). The results 6 months following cochlear implantation showed auditory responses elicited via a CI even after more than 50 years of unilateral auditory deprivation. Changes in speech scores over time and differences in performance comparing left hearing aid, right implant and bimodal stimulation also reflected the CAEP results. Speech test performance became very poor in the left ear after implantation of the right ear, but the hearing loss was fluctuating at this time and more difficult speech material was used for post-CI testing. Despite this, at nine months post-CI the CUNY speech scores indicated that bimodal listening was superior to the CI alone. Eighteen months after implantation P.M. reported great satisfaction with the implant. He was using bimodal stimulation (CI in the right ear and hearing aid in the left ear) but reported that he was relying mostly on the CI. He was back at work and reporting significant improvement in hearing ability. The difference in bimodal listening compared to CI or hearing aid alone was evident in the cortical responses at six and nine months post-CI (Figure 1 and 2). These show a binaural interaction effect, with different cortical responses in the bimodal condition than with either device alone. Hearing in the left ear continued to deteriorate. Speech scores with the hearing aid alone deteriorated to 20% using BKB/A in quiet, in spite of hearing aid optimisation. P.M. started to rely more and more on the right CI alone for hearing and communication. After three years of attempted bimodal hearing, and the left ear having deteriorated so greatly, P.M. had his left ear implanted in 2005. 4.0

µV

-5.0 -100

0.0

100

200

300 ms

400

500

Thicker line: hearing aid alone, Dashed line: CI alone Thinner line: bimodal Figure 1. CAEP 6 months post-CI.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

600

Celene McNeill, William Noble and Anna O‘Brien

304

1.5

µV

-3.8 -100

0.0

100

200

300 ms

400

500

600

Thicker line: hearing aid alone Dashed line: CI alone Thinner line: bimodal

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 2. CAEP at 9 months post-CI.

Twelve months after receiving the second implant P.M. scored 90% with bilateral implants for BKB/A sentences presented at 65 dB SPL in babble noise at +10 dB signal-tonoise ratio. CAEP with bilateral implants was attempted but waveforms resulted in a large artefact so that it was not possible to objectively determine whether a cortical response was present. This illustrates one of the problems of CAEP recordings in bilateral CI users (McNeill et al, 2009). The artefact usually occurs when the speech processor is activated and lasts at least as long as the duration of the stimulus (Gilley et al, 2005). Distribution of the artefact on the scalp varies according to type of CI and mode of stimulation, and although it can occur with unilateral CIs it is more prominent with bilateral stimulation. CAEP has the potential to be a fast and reliable tool for CI assessment but there are still some limitations that need to be overcome in order to make it more clinically useful. While further research is undertaken regarding the use of CAEP with bilateral CI, performance and subjective measures are relied on to assess responsiveness and outcomes.

Auditory Localization Background A primitive function of the binaural auditory system is enabling listeners to tell the whereabouts of audible events in the environment — basic to safe and effective orientation. This hearing function relies on detection and discrimination of interaural differences that vary with the location of audible events relative to the listener‘s position; referred to as spatial hearing (Blauert, 1983) or auditory localization (Mills, 1972). For human listeners, sounds occurring at points away from the body‘s midline, and containing energy up to about 1200 Hz, can be spatially distinguished on the basis of differences in phase relations between the two ears. As energy in a signal extends higher in frequency, becoming more complex, the

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Signs of Binaural Processing with Bilateral Cochlear Implants…

305

head casts an increasingly marked acoustic shadow, which in turn yields detectable interaural differences in the overall level of the signal at any position away from the body‘s midline. Cochlear implants do not allow reliable detection of low-frequency phase differences, but the shadowing effect of the head is a biophysical given, hence people with bilateral implants should be able to detect the whereabouts of complex sounds on the basis of interaural level differences. Studies confirm that bilateral CI users are indeed able to localize such sounds (Dunn et al., 2008; Litovsky et al., 2004; van Hoesel & Tyler, 2003).

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Test procedure Localization was tested in a medium-sized anechoic chamber using a circular array (1.7meter radius) of 20 loudspeakers at 18° intervals in the horizontal plane. P.M. sat in the centre of the array, the seat adjusted to align his interaural axis with the loudspeakers at 90° and 270° azimuth. Loudspeakers were masked with a curtain of optically opaque acoustically transparent material printed with progressively numbered marks at 10° intervals. The listener‘s task was to identify the number judged closest to the source, on each trial, by reference to the numbers on the curtain or a map of the loudspeaker layout. On an initial set of trials, the listener kept his head stationary during each trial, while fixating a point at 0°. In a repeat test session the same procedure was used, followed by groups of trials in which he was free to move his head. Various signals were used in groups of 40 trials, with random presentation (each loudspeaker activated twice), and sound level at 65 dB, but jittered at random through 3 dB. Tests were conducted under conditions of both bilateral and unilateral CI listening. Results Initial test (head stationary): Various narrowband and broadband signals were employed in different groups of trials, as well as a speech signal (BKB/A sentence spoken by a male). Figures 3a-c show scatterplots of source-response relations listening to the speech signal with both CIs, under right CI only, and left only. When listening with only the right CI activated, as indicated in Figure 3a, all the sounds were heard as located around the rightward (90°) loudspeaker; and all were heard as coming from around the leftward (270°) loudspeaker when only the left CI was activated (Figure 3b). In the bilateral condition (Figure 3c) all signals were correctly lateralized — there are no errors across the midline. The data also show that P.M. discriminated to a certain extent among sources within the left and right hemifields, but that he attributed some rearward signals to somewhat ―mirror-image‖ locations in front. His performance with the noise signals (only tested bilaterally) was similar to that shown in Figure 3c. Repeat test (head stationary then mobile): On a second visit, testing was repeated under bilateral CI conditions using broadband noise, with the further condition added of allowing P.M. to move his head/torso while the sound was activated. A longer (2-sec.) as well as shorter (0.9-sec.) duration signal was also employed. Under stationary listening for both durations, and under mobile listening for the shorter signal the outcome was essentially the same as that shown in Figure 3c. Under mobile listening with a 2-sec. signal, there were signs that the front-rear signals were resolved to their correct regions (Figure 4).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

306

Figure 3. Localization response patterns for male speech with a) right CI only, b) Celene McNeill, William Noble and Anna O‘Brien left CI only and c) bilateral CIs.

a

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

b

c

Figure 3. Localization response patterns for male speech with a) right CI only, b) left CI only and c) bilateral CIs.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Signs of Binaural Processing with Bilateral Cochlear Implants…

307

Figure 4. Localisation response pattern for 2 second broadband stimulus, head mobile.

Table 1. P.M.’s self-ratings on ten SSQ subscales compared with the averages of 36 bilateral CI patients; right-hand column shows SSQ benefit (from second implant) scores [In square brackets next to P.M.’s SSQ scores are scores of a case of unilateral deafness]

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

SSQ Subscales Speech Speech in quiet

P.M. (n = 1) 9.5 [7.5]

Speech in noise

6.5 [6.0]

Speech in speech contexts

8.3 [8.3]

Multiple speech-stream processing and switching

6.0 [4.2]

Spatial Localization

7.2 [0]

Distance and Movement

6.8 [0.5]

Quality Sound quality and naturalness

5.8 [9.4]

Identification of sound and objects

5.6 [9.4]

Segregation of sounds

8.7 [2.7]

Listening effort*

5.0 [2.0]

CI+CI (n = 36) 8.1 (1.3) 5.7 (1.9) 5.3 (2.2) 4.1 (2.2)

P.M. benefit scores

5.8 (2.3) 5.7 (1.9)

+4.2

6.9 (2.0) 6.6 (2.1)

+4.0

+5.0 +4.0 +3.8 +2.0

+3.6

+3.6 +4.3

6.0 (2.2) 6.1 (1.8)

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

+4.0

308

Celene McNeill, William Noble and Anna O‘Brien

Self-rating

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Background Various measures have been devised to assess people‘s self-ratings of their abilities in the domain of hearing disabilities; a recent one is the Speech, Spatial and Qualities of Hearing scale (SSQ: Gatehouse & Noble, 2004). This scale was developed to cover as broad as possible a range of hearing functions and experiences, with particular attention to capacities that implicate the binaural system, including spatial hearing. The SSQ has been applied in the case of people with one versus two CIs (Noble et al., 2008; Summerfield et al., 2006), and revealed evident self-rated advantage for spatial hearing in the case of the bilateral CI profile. Thus, it is appropriate to apply the SSQ in the present case. Test procedure The SSQ comprises 49 items in three main sections, addressing speech hearing, spatial hearing and other qualities of hearing. Most of the items comprising these sections can be aggregated as 10 subscales that have been labelled (Gatehouse & Akeroyd, 2006) Speech in Quiet, Speech in Noise, Speech in Speech Contexts, Multiple Speech-Stream Processing and Switching, Localization, Distance and Movement, Sound Quality and Naturalness, Identification of Sound and Objects, Segregation of Sounds, and Listening Effort. Each item is accompanied by a 0-10 scoring ruler such that zero represents complete inability with respect to the item in question, and 10 represents perfect ability. A paper-pencil version of the SSQ was mailed to P.M, which he completed and returned. Shortly thereafter, a new version of the SSQ was sent to him with the request he complete that. This new version — SSQ(B) — is currently under development for use as a benefit measure. Respondents are asked to rate each item on a –5-to-+5 scoring ruler in terms of whether their abilities and experiences are much worse (-5), unchanged (0) or much better (+5) as a consequence of whatever intervention has been undertaken. In the case of P.M. the intervention of interest was the acquisition of a second CI. It was fully recognized (and explained to P.M.) that applying the SSQ(B) in his case was purely exploratory, given that he had been using two implants for three years, and thus may not be able to rate his abilities and experiences now against those he remembered from three years previously, when he had a CI in the right ear and a hearing aid in the left. P.M. nonetheless felt able to respond to the SSQ(B).

RESULTS In Table 1 are the self-ratings of P.M. on the ten subscales of the SSQ, and, for comparison, the averages (SD‘s in brackets) of 36 bilateral CI cases from the University of Iowa Hospital (Noble et al., 2008). P.M. rates his abilities in the speech and spatial domains higher than the Iowa sample, although it can be noted from the standard deviation values that his ratings are within the Iowa range. There is also a measure of similarity between this case and the Iowa sample in the relative ranking of the subscales (P.M.‘s Speech in Noise rating might be seen as aberrant in this respect, and we return to that in the Discussion). By contrast, the ratings on three of the four qualities subscales are lower than the Iowa sample. In square

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Signs of Binaural Processing with Bilateral Cochlear Implants…

309

brackets alongside P.M.‘s SSQ subscale scores are the scores of a case of right ear unilateral deafness. This case can be seen as an approximation to how P.M. might have rated his abilities and experiences while his left ear was functioning normally. We return to the comparisons with the Iowa sample and this other individual case in the Discussion section. On the SSQ(B) P.M. rates his abilities/experience as consistently improved under bilateral compared with unilateral listening, but not uniformly across subscales. This outcome is in agreement with the Iowa data, when unilateral and bilateral patients‘ scores are compared (Noble et al., 2008). That said, we reiterate that the findings using the SSQ(B) can only be regarded as indicative given the length of time since P.M. listened with only one CI.

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Performance Data The speech test data demonstrate that P.M. is functioning almost at a normal hearing level when listening to a purely auditory signal in background noise. This highly proficient performance is echoed in his self-ratings. It can be assumed, from the CAEP observations in his earlier (bimodal) profile, that the bilateral CI condition is enabling binaural processing of speech. The localization performance data from the initial test session, with immobile listening, demonstrate the evident contrast between unilateral and bilateral listening. There are no reliable cues to direction with only one CI activated and the sensation experienced would lead to attribution to a source more or less in line with the activated side. By contrast, interaural level differences become available with two devices active, leading to accurate lateralization and a degree of azimuthal discrimination. Because of the geometry of interaural differences it is not straightforward to distinguish sources at ―mirror-image‖ positions behind and in front of the interaural axis. The repeat session confirmed the latter outcome for stationary listening. The condition in which the listener was free to move during signal presentation showed some resolution of front/back reversals, especially when the signal is 2-sec. rather than 1-sec. Such resolution is feasible because the change of interaural differences under head movement is in one direction when a sound is in the front hemifield, and in the opposite direction when the sound is in a ―mirror-image‖ location to the rear.

Self-rating Data The particular history of the present case, namely, most of his life with normal hearing in one ear, makes comparison with typical CI patients difficult to predict. At interview, P.M. reported a lifelong involvement in stage acting and singing, which could explain his particularly high self-ratings for speech understanding. We also note a strong influence of visual input in speech understanding: On an item of the SSQ asking how he gets on when not all conversation members are in sight P.M. gave a very low rating (which pulls down his speech-in-noise average score). He also observed that he was quite conscious of the reduction in quality and identifiability of sounds since becoming reliant solely on electric stimulation

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

310

Celene McNeill, William Noble and Anna O‘Brien

(and was aware of the livelier quality of what he hears on the side that was more recently normal). These features may account for the lower Qualities ratings than found in the Iowa sample. It is instructive to note the sharp contrasts in Spatial and certain Qualities ratings between P.M. and the case of unilateral deafness. In the latter case Spatial hearing is rated as nonexistent, but naturalness and identifiability are rated very highly. These contrasts are telling as regards the substantial benefit for spatial hearing provided by bilateral implantation, whilst also indicating the loss of quality that flows from the limited patterning available by this means of connection to the audible world. Nonetheless it confirms that the provision of bilateral auditory information, despite degraded signal quality and the age of the brain, can enable cortical plasticity to take place and auditory processing skills to develop.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

REFERENCES Blauert, J. (1983). Spatial hearing. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Dietrich, V., Nieschalk, M., Stoll, W., Rajan, R. & Pantev, C. (2001). Cortical reorganization in patients with high frequency cochlear hearing loss. Hearing Research, 158, 95-101. Dowell, R., Hollow, R. & Winton, L. (2003). Changing selection criteria for cochlear implants – the Melbourne experience. Cochlear N95506 ISSI. Dunn, C. C., Tyler, R. S., Oakley, S., Gantz, B. J. & Noble, W. (2008). Comparison of speech recognition and localization performance in bilateral and unilateral cochlear implant users matched on duration of deafness and age at implantation. Ear & Hearing, 29(3), 352-359. Gatehouse, S. & Akeroyd, M. (2006). Two-eared listening in dynamic situations. International Journal of Audiology, 45(Supplement 1), S120-S124. Gatehouse, S. & Noble, W. (2004). The Speech, Spatial and Qualities of Hearing Scale (SSQ). International Journal of Audiology, 43(1), 85-99. Gilley, P. M., Sharma, A., Finley, C. C., Panch, A. S., Martin, K. & Dorman, M. (2005). Minimization of cochlear implant stimulus artefact in cortical auditory evoked potentials. Clinical Neurophysiology, 116, 648-657. Kelly, A. S., Purdy, S. C. & Thorne, P. R. (2005). Electrophysiological and speech perception measures of auditory processing in experienced adult cochlear implant users. Clinical Neurophysiology, 116, 1235-1246. Khosla, D., Ponton, C. W., Eggermont, J. J., Kwong, B., Dort, M. & Vasama, J. P. (2003). Differential ear effects of profound unilateral deafness on the adult human central auditory system. Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, 4, 235-249. Litovsky, R. Y., Parkinson, A., Arcaroli, J., Peters, R., Lake, J., Johnstone, P., et al. (2004). Bilateral cochlear implants in adults and children. Archives of Otolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, 130, 648-655. McNeill, C., Sharma, M. & Purdy, S. C. (2009). Are cortical auditory evoked potentials useful in the clinical assessment of adults with cochlear implants? Cochlear Implants International, 10, 78-84. Mills, A. W. (1972). Auditory localization. In J. V. Tobias (Ed.), Foundations of modern auditory theory (Vol. II, 303-348). New York: Academic Press.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Signs of Binaural Processing with Bilateral Cochlear Implants…

311

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Noble, W., Tyler, R. S., Dunn, C. & Bhullar, N. (2008). Unilateral and bilateral cochlear implants and the implant-plus-hearing aid profile: Comparing self-assessed and measured abilities. International Journal of Audiology, 47, 505-514. Summerfield, A. Q., Barton, G. R., Toner, J., McAnallen, C., Proops, D., Harries, C., et al. (2006). Self-reported benefits from successive bilateral cochlear implantation in postlingually deafened adults: randomised controlled trial. International Journal of Audiology, 45, S99-S107. Thai-Van, H., Micheyl, C., Moore, B. C. J. & Collet, L. (2003). Enhanced frequency discrimination near the hearing loss cut-off: a consequence of central auditory plasticity induced by cochlear damage? Brain, 126, 2235-2245. Tyler, R. S. & Summerfield, A. Q. (1996). Cochlear implantation: Relationships with research on auditory deprivation and acclimatization. Ear & Hearing, 17(3), 38S-50S. van Hoesel, R. J. & Tyler, R. S. (2003). Speech perception, localization, and lateralization with bilateral cochlear implants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113(3), 1617-1630.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved. Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

In: Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System Editors: D. Fiedler and R. Krause, pp.313-322

ISBN: 978-1-60741-259-5 ©2010 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

Chapter 15

CISPLATIN OTOTOXICITY: A KEY FOR THE UNDERSTANDING OF THE INNER EAR PATHOLOGY José Ramón García-Berrocal*, Rafael Ramírez-Camacho, José Ángel González-García and Rodrigo Martínez-Monedero Servicio de Otorrinolaringología, Hospital Universitario Puerta de Hierro, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Spain

ABSTRACT

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Ototoxicity is defined as the tendency of certain therapeutic agents to cause functional impairment and cellular degeneration of the inner ear and of the eighth cranial nerve. Cisplatin (cis-diamminedichloroplatinum II; CDDP) is the first generation platinum-containing antitumoral drug known to be effective against a variety of solid tumors. Ototoxicity has been observed in up to 36% of patients receiving cisplatin. Risk factors for cisplatin ototoxicity include renal insufficiency, co-administration with aminoglycosides and/or radiation therapy, and increased cumulative doses. Monitoring for ototoxicity should be individualized: an audiogram (high frequencies and ultrahigh frequencies) should be obtained at the onset of therapy, before each successive dose, and with the onset of symptoms. Second generation platinum derived drugs have been developed in order to minimize the toxic effect on the inner ear. Only 1% of intracellular platinum (Pt) is bound to nuclear DNA with the great majority of the drug available to interact with other cellular targets. The quantification of Pt inside the inner ear by quadrupole inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometry (ICP-MS) has shown the presence of Pt-biomolecules in nuclear, cytosolic and mitochondrial fractions. The Pt-biomolecules binding could play a role in ototoxicity since the complexes were different depending on the drug and represents a future outlook in the management of cisplatin ototoxiciy. Although classically the most prominent change seen in the cochlea after cisplatin administration consists of loss of outer hair cells (OHCs), new directions in the research allowed us to provide a main role to the supporting cells (Deiter‘s cells) since they appeared more sensitive than outer hair cells.

*

Corresponding author: E-mail: [email protected]

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

314

José Ramón García-Berrocal, Rafael Ramírez-Camacho et al. In vitro and in vivo experiments have shown that apoptotic cell death is the primary mechanism of cisplatin antitumoral action. A novel investigation has shown that cisplatin induces apoptosis in hair cells, supporting cells, spiral ganglion cells, stria vascularis cells and spiral ligament fibrocytes by the activation of caspases, evoking an intrinsic pathway of pro-apoptotic signalling. This innovative idea has facilitated the development of several strategies to prevent oxidative stress-induced apoptosis of inner ear cells that have been exposed to cisplatin. However, the loss of the population of some type of inner ear cells could be irreversible, and then it would be necessary to replace these cells. The conversion of inner ear stem cells to sensory neurons and the search of a possible common pathway of inner ear damage need to be explored and they will lead the future trends in inner ear research.

Keywords: Cisplatin, ototoxicity, hearing loss, apoptosis, caspases, inner ear, outer hair cells, supporting cells, ICP-MS, stem cells.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

INTRODUCTION Ototoxicity is defined as the tendency of certain therapeutic agents to cause functional impairment and cellular degeneration of the inner ear and of the eighth cranial nerve. Cisplatin (cis-diamminedichloroplatinum II; CDDP) is still one of the cornerstone in the treatment of epithelial malignancies. Unfortunately, cisplatin damages, indiscriminately, tumour and normal cells. Thus, severe side effects arise from the lesion of various cell types in peripheral nerves, renal tubules, bone marrow and gastrointestinal tract. Ototoxicity has been observed in up to 36% of patients receiving cisplatin [1]. Cisplatin-induced ototoxicity is characterized by a cumulative, dose-related sensorineural hearing loss and tinnitus and less frequently by vestibular impairment [2,3]. Hearing loss is progressive, irreversible, bilateral and it usually affects high frequencies first and may gradually spread to lower frequencies [4]. Risk factors for cisplatin ototoxicity include renal insufficiency, co-administration with aminoglycosides and/or radiation therapy, and increased cumulative doses. Monitoring for ototoxicity should be individualized: an audiogram (high frequencies and ultrahigh frequencies) should be obtained at the onset of therapy, before each successive dose, and with the onset of symptoms. Second generation platinum derived drugs have comparable antitumor spectrum to cisplatin, they present several side effects such as myelosuppression but they induces less nephrotoxicity and ototoxicity than CDDP. Although the ototoxic effect of cisplatin has been extensively studied, the cellular mechanisms by which cisplatin induce degeneration of the organ of Corti remains elusive. Furthermore, the cellular sites of cisplatin uptake and accumulation in the cochlea have not yet been properly identified. It is commonly accepted that binding of cisplatin to DNA results in the formation of cisplatin-DNA adducts, which produce severe distortions in the DNA double helix. This antineoplastic effect is mediated by the platination of nucleophilic centers in DNA bases, leading to intra- or inter-strand cross-linking of the bases, to abnormal base pairing or to DNA strand breakage. Cisplatin-DNA adducts may damage outer hair cells, stria vascularis, spiral ligament and spiral ganglion cells leading to ototoxicity.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Cisplatin Ototoxicity: A Key for the Understanding of the Inner Ear Pathology

315

CISPLATIN OTOTOXICITY: A REAPPRAISAL OF THE CLASSIC THEORY Although classically the most prominent change seen in the cochlea after cisplatin administration consists of loss of outer hair cells (OHCs), new directions in the research allowed us to provide a main role to the supporting cells (Deiter‘s cells) since they appeared more sensitive than outer hair cells. Supporting cells (Deiter‘s cells) appeared more sensitive than outer hair cells and the alteration of the supporting cell ultrastructure preceded detectable changes in outer hair cells [5] (Figure 1). Recent studies suggest that the toxic drug initially affects the supporting cells (Deiters and Hensen cells) that help to maintain the metabolic homeostasis of outer and inner hair cells, which experience structural and functional lesions when there is an impairment of their metabolism-regulating cells [5,6]. This hypothesis may justify the increase in hearing loss in animals with a long-term survival after cisplatin injection. This alteration correlates with the degeneration of the cytoplasm of the Hensen cells that was observed prior to the destruction of inner hair cells. This finding could support the theory that the exhaustion of hair cells is influenced by damage to supporting cells and suggest an active role of supporting cells in the maintenance of hair cell function and structure. Supporting cells may provide the necessary metabolic and electrolytic conditions (K+ active exchange) for hair cells mechanical and bioelectrical function.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

CELLULAR ACTION: CISPLATIN INDUCES AN INTRINSIC APOPTOTIC PATHWAY INSIDE THE INNER EAR In vitro and in vivo experiments have shown that apoptotic cell death is the primary mechanism of cisplatin toxicity [7]. In the cochlea, cisplatin has been shown to induce apoptosis in hair cells, supporting cells, spiral ganglion cells and stria vascularis cells [8] by means of activation of caspases evoking an intrinsic pathway of pro-apoptotic signalling [9] (Figure 2), up-regulation of the pro-apoptotic tumor suppressor gene p53, a significant increase in bax-positive cells, a decrease in bcl-2-positive cells and the release of cytochrome c [10-12]. A high accumulation of Pt-DNA adducts has been observed in the nuclei of marginal cells of the stria vascularis but not in OHCs which have been discussed as the main target of cisplatin-induced cell damage [13]. The excessive DNA platination in the marginal cells represents the earliest event in short-term cisplatin ototoxicity triggering their functional impairment and apoptotic destruction. Marginal cell damage may lead to an impaired uptake of K+ from the intra-strial space as well as an impairment K+ secretion into the endolymph with subsequent dysfunction and loss of hair cells. The impairment of K+ recycling process that is essential to the endocochlear potential is a known toxic effect of cisplatin induced by apoptosis of type I spiral ligament fibrocytes [14] and by the direct apoptotic effect of cisplatin on the stria vascularis, responsible for the decreased cellular Na+, K+ ATPase and Ca2+ ATPase activities [15]. Morphologically, edema formation in the stria vascularis was followed by severe atrophy after CDDP treatment. The majority of marginal cells affected by CDDP exhibited expression of cleaved caspase-3 indicating that caspases are involved in the process of apoptotic cell death after CDDP treatment. However, they showed expression of

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

316

José Ramón García-Berrocal, Rafael Ramírez-Camacho et al.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

cleaved caspase-9 indicating that apoptosis was started by permeabilization of mitochondrial membranes. Studies on morphologic cell changes after cisplatin injection have shown the presence of blebs in diverse regions of the apical region such as kinocilium and other marginal areas in IHC and OHC [16] (Figure 3). Blebs were first recognized as a hallmark of apoptosis. Thus, blebs form within 30 minutes after exposure to cisplatin administration, coinciding with channel-mediated K+ and Cl- loss during ―apoptotic volume decrease‖ (AVD) preceding mitochondrial cytochrome C release and caspase-3 activation [17]. During apoptosis caspase3 cleaves the Rho-kinase I (ROCK I), leading to a deregulated activity of this kinase and consequently to bleb formation [18]. These morphological findings, together with the severe hearing loss found in these animals, suggest that these blebs are a manifestation of apoptosis.

Figure 1. Organ of Corti of a guinea pig after CDDP injection showing degeneration of Deiters cells (black arrow) with outer hair cells preservation (scanning microscopy; x1500).

Figure 2. Immunostaining of active caspase-3 in the nuclei of inner ear cells from rats 7 days after a single dose of cisplatin (5mg/kg). MC, marginal cells of stria vascularis; RM Reissner´s membrane; OHC, outer hair cells; IC, interdental cells of the spiral limbus. Magnification 400x.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Cisplatin Ototoxicity: A Key for the Understanding of the Inner Ear Pathology

317

Figure 3. Scanning micrograph of the organ of Corti of a Hartley guinea pig after cisplatin treatment (5mg/kg) showing blebs emerging from the basal body of the cuticular plate in inner hair cells (x5500).

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

ROLE OF PLATINUM-BIOMOLECULES AND INTRACELLULAR PLATINUM CONCENTRATION IN OTOTOXICITY Only 1% of intracellular platinum (Pt) is bound to nuclear DNA with the great majority of the drug available to interact with other cellular targets. The quantification of Pt inside the inner ear by quadrupole inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometry (ICP-MS) has shown the presence of Pt-biomolecules in nuclear, cytosolic and mitochondrial fractions. By means of this technique, we have found a 10-fold higher deposition of Pt in inner ear than in brain, and the Pt clearance rate is faster in brain [19]. The inner ear and brain are in anatomical proximity with a common embryological origin (ectoderm) and a common vascularization. These different concentrations detected in Pt in inner ear and in brain suggest that Pt has a higher histological affinity for the structures of the organ of Corti. A revision of the proteins that are present in the inner ear but not in the brain was reported by Thalman et al [20, 21], who describes the organ of Corti proteins (OCPs), OCP1, OCP2 and oncomodulin, among others. OCP1 and OCP2 are restricted to the supporting cells of the organ of Corti and adjacent epithelia. This distribution coincides with the boundaries of the epithelial gap junction system of the cochlea, as defined by staining for connexin 26. OCP2 is diffusely distributed within the cytoplasm of the supporting cells, but is apparently absent from the nucleus [20]. Cochlear gap junction system may assist with removal of the excess K+ from the hair cells and with its recirculation-via the spiral ligament-by the stria vascularis. The endolymphatic potential causes a massive K+ current through the hair cells and it needs an acidification of the endolymph. The presence of an acidification system in cells bordering the scala media with a distribution similar to OCP2 suggests the role of OCP2 in pH homeostasis mechanisms [6]. Recently, the binding of Pt to biomolecules with molecular weights of 12 kDa and 25-65 kDa for inner ear samples has been demonstrated in rats after cisplatin injection [22, 23] (Figure 4).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

318

José Ramón García-Berrocal, Rafael Ramírez-Camacho et al.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 4. Chromatograms of inner ear cytosol from rats treated with cisplatin, oxaliplatin or carboplatin performed by size exclusion inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometry (SEC-ICP-MS). Molecular weight calibration markers: 1-70 kDa, 2- 66 kDa, 3- 29 kDa, 4- 12.4 kDa, 5- 6.5 kDa and 6- 3 kDa.

Figure 5. In vitro culture of an organ of Corti explant from a newborn mouse. The hair cells (shown in red) and the innervation of the auditory neurons (shown in green) are damaged usually in the hearing loss in humans. The regeneration of these cells and their innervation is a major goal in the search of a new therapy for the hearing loss.

The Pt-biomolecules binding could play a role in ototoxicity since the complexes were different depending on the drug and represents a future outlook in the management of cisplatin ototoxiciy.

THERAPY STRATEGIES Cisplatin may directly lead to the generation of reactive oxygen species (ROS) or may induce the release of reactive oxygen molecules normally sequestered within mitochondria

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Cisplatin Ototoxicity: A Key for the Understanding of the Inner Ear Pathology

319

that may trigger several mechanisms of apoptosis. ROS-mediated damage could occur as a consequence of antioxidant depletion and increased lipid peroxidation in the cochlea of rats [24]. Thus, several strategies have been suggested to prevent oxidative stress-induced apoptosis of OHCs that have been exposed to cisplatin: prevention of the formation of ROS either by binding the toxin or reversing the toxin´s binding, inhibition of the lipid peroxidation, addition of exogenous free-radical scavengers and antioxidant enzymes, inhibitors of caspases and gene therapy (to upregulate antiapoptotic gene products such as Bcl-2) [12, 25]. Likewise, the administration of antioxidants could stimulate endogenous mechanisms (heat shock proteins, glutathione, adenosine A1 receptors, heme-oxygenase-1, kidney injury molecule) to prevent oxidative stress caused by cisplatin. However, protective therapy should preserve the antineoplastic efficacy of cisplatin. Unfortunately, once loss of hair or ganglion cells has been established, an irreversible hearing loss can be expected. In mammals the sensorineural hearing loss is irreversible; underlying this loss of auditory function is the inability to replace inner ear cells by cell division and by differentiation from endogenous cells in the inner ear epithelia. The advances in stem cell research are a new approach to inner ear regeneration in mammals. A major advance in the prospects for the use of stem cells for the replacement of inner ear cells came with the recent discovery that hair cells could be generated in vivo from embryonic stem cells, from adult inner ear stem cells and from neural stem cells [26-28}. Loss of stem cells postnatally in the cochlea may correlate with the loss of regenerative capacity and may limit our ability to stimulate regeneration [29-31]. It may be possible to replace inner ear cells that have degenerated with the transplantation of cells that restore auditory function. In this way, the spiral ganglion neurons obtained from the cochlea of a newborn mouse reinnervated hair cells in a toxin-treated organ of Corti and expressed synaptic vesicle markers at points of contact with hair cells [32}. These findings suggest that it may be possible to replace degenerated neurons by grafting new cells into the organ of Corti. If we use in the cell therapy stem cells as a source of transplanted cells, we can control the cell fate decisions that determine the phenotype. We can differentiate these stem cells to sensory neurons that can be grafted to the inner ear [33]. A distant future therapy for the hearing loss might be a combination of stem cell, gene therapy and drug treatment in conjunction with technical devices.

CONCLUSION Cisplatin is still the first-line treatment for some neoplastic entities. Its main adverse effects are ototoxicity, nephrotoxicity, bone marrow toxicity, gastrointestinal toxicity, liver toxicity and peripheral nervous system toxicity. Most of these adverse effects can be prevented and treated but inner ear damage is one of the most common reasons to discontinue chemotherapy with Pt-based compounds. Cisplatin induces apoptosis in hair cells, supporting cells, spiral ganglion cells, stria vascularis cells and spiral ligament fibrocytes by the activation of caspases, evoking an intrinsic pathway of pro-apoptotic signalling. Several strategies have been suggested to prevent oxidative stress-induced apoptosis of OHCs that have been exposed to cisplatin. The

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

320

José Ramón García-Berrocal, Rafael Ramírez-Camacho et al.

stimulation of endogenous mechanisms to prevent oxidative stress could support the administration of antioxidants. However, loss of hair and/or ganglion cells caused by cisplatin will induce an irreversible hearing loss. The differentiation of inner ear stem cells and the search of a possible common pathway of inner ear damage need to be explored and they will lead the future trends in inner ear research.

REFERENCES

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

[1]

Nagy, JL; Adelstein, DJ; Newman, CW; Rybicki, LA; Rice, TW; Lavertu, P. Cisplatin ototoxicity: the importance of baseline audiometry. Am J Clin Oncol, 1999, 28, 305308. [2] Schaefer, SD; Wright, CG; Post, JD; Frenkel, EP. Cis-platinum vestibular toxicity. Cancer, 1981, 47, 857-859. [3] Anniko, M; Sobin, A. Cisplatin: evaluation of its ototoxic potential. Am J Otolaryngol, 1986, 7, 276-293. [4] Waters, GS; Ahmad, M; Katsarkas, A; Stanimir, G; McKay, J. Ototoxicity due to cisdiamminedichloroplatinum in the treatment of ovarian cancer: influence of dosage and schedule of administration. Ear Hear, 1991, 12, 91-102. [5] Ramírez-Camacho, R; García Berrocal, JR; Buján, J; Martin-Marero, A; Trinidad, A. Supporting cells as a target of cisplatin-induced inner ear damage: therapeutic implications. Laryngoscope, 2004, 114, 533-537. [6] Ramírez-Camacho, R; García-Berrocal, JR; Trinidad, A; González-García, JA; Verdaguer, JM; Ibáñez, A; Rodríguez, A; Sanz, R. Central role of supporting cells in cochlear homeostasis and pathology. Med Hypotheses, 2006, 67, 550-555. [7] Boulikas, T; Vougiouka, M. Cisplatin and platinum drugs at the molecular level. Oncol Rep, 2003, 10, 1663-1682. [8] Alam, SA ; Ikeda, K ; Oshima, T ; Suzuki, M ; Kawase, T ; Kikuchi, T ; Takasaka, T. Cisplatin-induced apoptotic cell death in Mongolian gerbil cochlea. Hear Res, 2000, 141, 28-38. [9] García-Berrocal, JR; Nevado, J; Ramírez-Camacho, R; Sanz, R; González-García, JA; Sánchez-Rodríguez, C; Cantos, B; España, P; Verdaguer, JM; Trinidad Cabezas, A. Antitumoral drug cisplatin induces an intrinsic apoptotic pathway inside the inner ear. British J Pharmacol, 2007, 116, 779-784. [10] Devarajan, P; Savoca, M; Castaneda, MP; Parks, MS; Esteban-Cruciani, N; Kalinec, G; Kalinec, F. Cisplatin-induced apoptosis in auditory cells: role of death receptor and mitochondrial pathways. Hear Res, 2002, 174, 45-54. [11] Zhang, M; Liu, W; Ding, D; Salvi, R. Pifithrin-alpha suppressers p53 and protects cochlear and vestibular hair cells from cisplatin-induced apoptosis. Neuroscience, 2003, 120, 191-205. [12] Rybak, LP; Whitworth, CA; Mukherjea, D; Ramkumar, V. Mechanism of cisplatininduced ototoxicity and prevention. Hear Res, 2007, 226, 157-167.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Cisplatin Ototoxicity: A Key for the Understanding of the Inner Ear Pathology

321

[13] Thomas, JP; Lautermann, J; Liedert, B; Seller, F; Thomale, J. High accumulation of platinum-DNA adducts in strial marginal cells of the cochlea is an early event in cisplatin but not in carboplatin ototoxicity. Mol Pharmacol, 2006, 70, 23-29. [14] Liang, F; Schulte, BA; Qu, C; Hu, W; Shen, Z. Inhibition of the calcium and voltagedependent big conductance potassium channel ameliorates cisplatin-induced apoptosis in spiral ligament fibrocytes of the cochlea. Neuroscience, 2005, 135, 263-271. [15] Cheng, PW; Liu, SH; Hsu, CJ; Lin-Shiau, SY. Correlation of increased activities of Na+, K+ ATPase and Ca2+ ATPase with the reversal of cisplatin ototoxicity induced by D-methionine in guinea pigs. Hear Res, 2005, 205, 102-109. [16] Ramírez-Camacho, R; García-Berrocal, JR; Trinidad, A; Verdaguer, JM; Nevado, J. Blebs in inner and outer hair cells: a pathophysiological hypothesis. J Laryngol Otol, 2008, 10, 1-5. [17] Okada, Y; Maeno, E; Shimizu, T; Dezaki, K; Wang, J; Morishima, S. Receptormediated control of regulatory volume decrease (RVD) and apoptotic volume decrease (AVD). J Physiol, 2001, 532, 3-16. [18] Coleman, ML; Sahai, EA; Yeo, M; Bosch, M; Dewar, A; Olson, MF. Membrane blebbing during apoptosis results from caspase-mediated activation of ROCK I. Nat Cell Biol, 2001, 3, 339-346. [19] Ramírez-Camacho, R; Esteban-Fernández, D; Verdaguer, JM; Gómez Gómez, MM; Trinidad, A; García-Berrocal, JR; Palacios Corvillo, MA. Cisplatin-induced hearing loss does not correlate with intracellular platinum concentration. Acta Otolaryngol, 2008, 128, 505-509. [20] Thalmann, R; Henzl, MT; Thalmann, I. Specific proteins of the organ of Corti. Acta Otolaryngol, 1997, 117, 265-268. [21] Thalmann, R; Henzl, MT; Killick, R; Ignatova, EG; Thalmann, I. Toward an understanding of cochlear homeostasis: the impact of location and the role of OCP1 and OCP2. Acta Otolaryngol, 2003, 123, 203-208. [22] Esteban-Fernandez, D; Gómez-Gómez, MM; Cañas, B; Verdaguer, JM; Ramírez, R; Palacios, MA. Speciation analysis of platinum antitumoral drugs in impacted tissues. Talanta, 2007, 72, 768-773. [23] Esteban Fernández, D; Verdaguer, JM; Ramírez-Camacho, R; Palacios, MA; GómezGómez, MM. Accumulation, fractionation and analysis of platinum in toxicologically affected tissues after cisplatin, oxaliplatin and carboplatin administration. J Anal Toxicol, 2008, 32, 1-7. [24] Rybak, LP; Husain, K; Whitworth, C; Somani, SM. Dose dependent protection by lipoic acid against cisplatin-induced ototoxicity in rats: antioxidant defense system. Toxicol Sciences, 1999, 47, 195-202. [25] Lopez-Gonzalez, MA; Guerrero, JM; Rojas, F; Delgado, F. Ototoxicity caused by cisplatin is ameliorated by melatonin and other antioxidants. J Pineal Res, 2000, 28, 73-80. [26] Li, H; Liu, H; Heller, S. Pluripotent stem cells from the adult mouse inner ear. Nat Med., 2003, 9, 1293-9. [27] Li, H; Roblin, G; Liu, H; Heller, S. Generation of hair cells by stepwise differentiation of embryonic stem cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci, USA, 100, 13495-500. [28] Tateya, I. Fate of neural stem cells grafted into injured inner ears of mice. Neuroreport, 14, 1677-81.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

322

José Ramón García-Berrocal, Rafael Ramírez-Camacho et al.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

[29] Oshima, K; Grimm, CM; Corrales, CE; Senn, P; Martinez Monedero, R; Geleoc, GS; et al. Differential distribution of stem cells in the auditory and vestibular organs of the inner ear. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol., 2007, 8, 18-31. [30] Martinez-Monedero, R; Edge, AS. Stem cells for the replacement of inner ear neurons and hair cells. Int J Dev Biol., 2007, 51, 655-61. [31] Martinez-Monedero, R; Oshima, K; Heller, S; Edge, AS. The potential role of endogenous stem cells in regeneration of the inner ear. Hear Res., 2007 May, 227, 48-52. [32] Martinez-Monedero, R; Corrales, CE; Cuajungco, MP; Heller, S; Edge, AS. Reinnervation of hair cells by auditory neurons after selective removal of spiral ganglion neurons. J Neurobiol., 2006, 66, 319-31. [33] Martinez-Monedero, R; Yi, E; Oshima, K; Glowatzki, E; Edge, AS. Differentiation of inner ear stem cells to functional sensory neurons. Dev Neurobiol., 2008 Apr, 68, 669-84.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

In: Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System Editors: D. Fiedler and R. Krause, pp.323-332

ISBN: 978-1-60741-259-5 ©2010 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

Chapter 16

MODELING THE PERCEPTUAL MASKING PROPERTIES OF THE HUMAN AUDITORY SYSTEM USING NEURAL NETWORKS Hossein L. Najafi* Department of Computer Science, University of Wisconsin, River Falls, River Falls, Wisconsin, USA

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

ABSTRACT Modeling of the perceptual masking properties of the Human Auditory System is investigated. An artificial neural network is trained to model the perceptual masking map of the human auditory system. Successful application of the model to data hiding is demonstrated.

Keywords: Neural Networks, Perceptual Map, Perceptual Coding, Back Propagation, Data Hiding.

I. INTRODUCTION The human auditory system is known to have physiological mechanisms to differentiate and mask noise-like data when presented relative to tone-like signals such as music and speech [1] [2]. This masking mechanism is known to be caused by the action of the cochlea of the ear and is a phenomenon in which one sound interferes with our perception of another sound [3]. For this masking phenomenon to take place, the two signals must have certain spectral and temporal properties relative to each other. Figure 1 (borrowed from John G. BEERENDS [4]) demonstrates a schematic representation of the frequency-domain, the timedomain and the time-frequency-domain of the masking pattern of a sinusoidal tone.

*

Corresponding author: email: [email protected].

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

324

Hossein L. Najafi

Intensity

Short Sinusoidal Tone

Masked Threshold for a Second Tone

Time

Intensity

Short Sinusoidal Tone

Masked Threshold for a Second Tone

Frequency Masked Threshold Short Sinusoidal Tone

Intensity

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Time

Frequency

Figure 1. Time-Domain, Frequency Domain and Time-Frequency Domain Masking.

Note that the cochlea is capable of performing both frequency and temporal masking. Frequency masking occurs when two signals that are close in frequency are played at the same time. In this case, one sound simply vanishes in the presence of the other sound. Temporal masking occurs when a one signal is played immediately before or after another one. In both cases one signal has the potential to mask the other. Post-masking refers to the effect of masking after a strong sound, and can be in effect up to about 200 ms. When a sound is masked by something that appears after it, it is called pre-masking. Pre-masking is relatively shorter and may last up to about 20 ms [5]. Many examples of modeling perceptual masking properties [6] [7] and perceptual measuring techniques [8] [9] [10] [11] [12] [13] [14] [15] [16] [17] are presented in the literature. The majority of these works focus on hand-crafting mathematical models of the cochlea and its signal processing capabilities. Such models are often too general and do not focus on the complete Auditory System‘s masking ability. Other models can only predict

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Modeling the Perceptual Masking Properties of the Human Auditory System…

325

certain aspects of auditory sensation and do not offer a comprehensive masking model of the cochlea. In addition, to compute the masking thresholds using these models, a series of transformations (Fourier Transforms, Scale Conversions, matrix multiplications, etc.) must be performed on the signal. Such signal transformations are computationally expensive and hence not attractive for real-time applications. Mourjopoulos et al. [18] trained a neural network using the mathematical masking models to improve the computation complexity of the process. In this way, the network was trained to be an efficient representation of the corresponding hand-crafted mathematical model. However, the trained network, at best, can perform as well as the corresponding mathematical model. In this paper we present a unique approach to modeling the perceptual masking map of the human auditory system using a back-propagation neural network [19] [20] [21]. Quality of the model is verified by its ability to inject imperceptible data into an audio stream.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

II. PROPOSED SOLUTION The main objective of the work presented here is to build a computational model of the perceptual masking map of the human auditory system. To accomplish this, an artificial neural network was trained using the back-propagation algorithm. The first step in training a neural network is to create a feature vector set from which training and test sets can be produced. To accomplish this, real audio signals off of a reference tape were used. To improve the generalization capability of the network, the reference tape was composed of a variety of audio signals such as news clips, clips from movies, music clips, etc. The data sets were generated through interactive experiments, as described next, and were generated in such a way to represent the masking ability of the human auditory system. As depicted in Figure 2, the audio signals from the tape are first decomposed into a set of k corresponding audio bands Ab = {Ab1, Ab2, …, Abk}, using a bank of band-pass filters. The resulting band activities are then presented to an Activity Generator subsystem. The Activity Generator subsystem produces activity levels, AbL={Ab1L, Ab2L, …, AbkL}, and activity durations, AbD={Ab1D, Ab2D, …, AbkD} of the audio bands. The Minimum Activity Detector Subsystem monitors the bands for an absolute minimum signal activity level and duration before any kind of masking is possible. When minimum signal activity is detected, the Random Data Generator subsystem is flagged to generate a data vector, D(Pi). The data signal, D(Pi), is then generated using one of the predefined parameter vectors Pi = [PLi, PDi, PTi, PBi], randomly selected from the parameter set P = {P1, P2, … , Pn}. Here, PL is the Level Parameter and identifies initial magnitude of D, PD is the Duration Parameter and identifies length of D, PT is the Decay Rate Parameter and identifies the rate at which PL is decayed during data transmission, and PB identifies a pair of neighboring bands to be used for data transmission. The data injection subsystem then injects the randomly generated data signal, with the specified magnitude, duration and decay rate into a pair of neighboring bands, identified by PB, using (FSK) across the bands1. The resulting audio stream is then produced to a user who is given two seconds to respond if the data was heard by pressing a button on the keyboard. This user response is recorded as perceptual masking flag, f. 1

Although the data infusion can be performed via different mechanisms, for the purpose of this study Frequency Shift Keying (FSK) across a pair of neighboring bands was used.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

326

Hossein L. Najafi

AbL AbD

Ab2

D(Pi)

A + D(P)

Ab1

A

Random Data Generator

Y

Data Injection Subsystem

Filter Bank

Input Audio

Minimum Activity Present?

Minimum Activity Detector

Ab

Activity Generator

User Response

f: Audible/ Not Audible

D(Pi)

Abk

AbL AbD

Feature Vector Set Training Set Sampling Test Set Neural Network

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 2. Training and Test Data Generation System

Data generated in this manner is then used to produce the feature vectors. Each feature vector is represented by V = AbL + AbD + Pi + f , where AbL is the activity level vector, AbD is the activity duration vector, Pi is the data parameter vector used, and f is the perceptual masking flag produced by the user. The feature vector set, V, is then randomly sampled into a training set and a test set. These sets are then used to train and test a neural network using the standard backpropagation algorithm with momentum and adaptive learning rate. The number of hidden units is experimentally selected to produce best generalization. The trained neural network produced in this manner is then considered to be the computation model of the perceptual masking map of the human auditory system, and will be referred to as the Perceptual Masking Model (PMM) for the rest of this paper. Note that, without affecting the merit of the proposed approach (i.e., Transmitting data at the Perceptual Entropy Envelope limit), other classification methods such as Projection Pursuit Regression [22], Radial Basis Functions [23], or Support Vector Machine [24] [25], can be used in place of the back-propagation neural network.

III. APPLICATION OF THE MODEL TO DATA HIDING Quality of the Perceptual Masking Model described in the previous section is examined by its ability to hide data in an audio stream. The model is used to monitor an audio stream for ―opportunities‖ to infuse data signals into the stream such that the infused signals are masked according to the Perceptual Entropy Envelope of the audio signal. The model is also

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Modeling the Perceptual Masking Properties of the Human Auditory System…

327

used to help identify and recover data signals that were injected into the channel from the corresponding audio stream. This section provides an overview of the data infusion and recovery subsystems.

The data infusion subsystem uses a bank of PMMs, each representing a specific set of data parameters, for monitoring the audio channel for data transmission opportunities. To accomplish this, the input audio signal is first processed through the same Activity Detector used for generating the training and test sets. The outputs of the activity detector, AbL and AbD are then fed into the PMMs in the bank (See Figure 3). In addition to the band activity, each PMM also receives one of the predefined set of data parameters, Pi. Each PMM will generate its output activation value () between zero and one. This value is a measure of the probability of hiding the data if transmitted with the corresponding parameters. Perceptual Masking Models with output activation values greater than threshold () are considered active. The threshold value was experimentally selected to provide maximum data capacity while keeping the perceptual quality of the audio signal intact. Active PMMs compete for data transmission. Data parameters associated with the active PMM that has the greatest output activation will be used to transmit the data. If no PMM is active, then no transmission will take place. Otherwise, the data infusion subsystem will use the data parameter Pi, associated with the selected active Perceptual Masking Model, pmmi, to insert the data into the audio stream. Data will be inserted into the bands identified by PBi at initial level specified by PLi, with a duration specified by PDi, and its magnitude is decayed according to PTi. In this manner, the PMM bank controls the transmission frequency of the data signal, the level at which the data signal is transmitted and the timing of transmission of the data signal. Under the control of the this system, the data infusion subsystem will then combine a narrowband FSK data signal with the audio signal at the frequency, level and duration determined by the PMM bank such that the data signal has a high probability of being masked by the audio signal. PMM Bank P1

Data

Filter Bank Ab1

Input Audio

A

Ab2

P2 Ab

Activity Generator

AbL AbD

. . .

Abk

Parameter Selector Subsystem

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

A. Data Infusion Subsystem

Pn

Ab

Figure 3. Data Infusion Subsystem

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

D Ps f

Data Injection Subsystem

Output Audio A + D(Ps)

328

Hossein L. Najafi

This process is summarized in Figure 3 with the Parameter Selector Subsystem implementing the following algorithm: Given a set of Perceptual Masking Models, PMM = {pmm1, pmm2, … pmmn}, where pmmi is associated with the data parameters Pi, and where each Perceptual Masking Model, pmmi has an output activation i that represents the probability of successful transmission, if the data vector is transmitted with the data parameters Pi. 1. Identify a subset of Active Perceptual Masking Models, APMM = {apmm1, apmm2, …, apmmk} such that i > , where  is a predefined threshold and selected experimentally to maximize data hiding capacity, while keeping the perceptual quality of audio signal intact. 2. If APMM is empty (i.e., there is no active PMM), set Inject Data Flag f to false. No data will be transmitted. 3. Otherwise, set Inject Data Flag f to true and from the set APMM, identify the apmms such that apmms = argmax( i ) . apmmi

4. Output the Inject Data Flag f and the data parameters, Ps associated with the selected Active Perceptual Masking Model apmms.



Note that the proposed algorithm, has the same order of computational complexity as the method proposed by Mourjopoulos et al. [18].

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

B. Data Extraction Subsystem The data extraction subsystem (see Figure 4) utilizes the same PMM bank used by the data infusion subsystem. The received audio signal is decomposed and processed through the PMM bank in the same manner as at the transmitting end. The output of the parameter selector, f and Ps will be used to identify the location in the audio stream where a data signal with parameters Ps is hiding. If the Inject Data Flag f is true, then the corresponding parameters Ps identify the FSK bands and the level, duration and decay rate of the data signal. Given this information, an FSK decoder will then retrieve the data signal from the audio stream.

IV. SIMULATION RESULTS Feasibility of the proposed solution was investigated by using a small number of frequency bands. The transmission bands were fixed, and the network was trained to only identify the masking level, data duration and data decay rate. Specifically, four MPEG bands centered at approximately 2.2K, 2.55K, 2.95K and 3.5K were used. The two center bands (2.55K and 2.95K) were used as the FSK transmission bands with the center at about 2.77K. Forty sets of data parameters representing combination of five data levels, four decay rates and two data durations were used. Data levels, were set at 32 dB from full scale and worked

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Modeling the Perceptual Masking Properties of the Human Auditory System…

329

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

down by 3 dB. Decay rates were at 0.74 dB, 0.58 dB, 0.46 dB, and 0.36 dB per sample. Data durations where set at 24 and 32 data bits. The Data Generation System depicted in Figure 2 was used by a group of listeners to generate the training and test data. Listeners were asked to play an audio file composed of a variety of audio signals such as news clips, clips from movies, music clips, etc. The audio stream also contained data with one of the forty random parameters. The listeners were asked to press a button on the keyboard if and when they heard an audio anomaly. If the button was pushed within two seconds of a data transmission, that data transmission was marked as audible. Activities in each sub-band (i.e., level and duration of activities), the parameters used for the data signals and the corresponding audible flag produced by the user were then stored in a data file. The data produced by the Data Generation System was then randomly sampled into a training set and a test set. These sets were used to train and test a 3-layer feed-forward neural network using the back-propagation learning algorithm, with momentum and adaptive learning rate. To help identify a system with best generalization capability, a number of networks with different hidden layer size were trained and tested, and the network with the best performance was selected for data insertion and extraction. In specific, the number of hidden units was increased from 2 to 10. Each network was then trained and tested using 40 different training and test sets. The performance of each network was measured in terms of its True Positive (TP), False Positive (FP), True Negative (TN), and False Negative (FN) Classification Rates. The following observations were considered in selecting the best performing network:    

Networks with larger TP will have higher transmission rates Networks with larger FP will have higher audible distortions Networks with larger FN will have higher missed opportunities Networks with larger TN will have higher perceptual quality.

Using these observations, the best performing network was identified to have the maximum True Positive classification accuracy (i.e. highest insertion rate) and minimum ratio of False Positive to True Positive (i.e., relative lowest audible distortion) as formalized by the following function, g:

TP /(TP  FP  FN  TN ) TP 2 g  FP / TP FP * (TP  FP  FN  TN ) Table 1 shows the average values of g as a function of the number of hidden units. Note that the network architecture that employed 3 hidden units give the best average performance (i.e., g is maximum). Among the 40 networks trained with 3 hidden units, the network that gave the best performance was then selected and used for data insertion and extraction. This network had a g value of 4.41 and its Classification Rates are listed in Table 2. Note that FP is only 2%, indicating that data inserted by best performing network will be very inaudible. Also, note that TP+TN is over 60%, indicating that the network is transmitting

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

330

Hossein L. Najafi

at close to the limits of the Perceptual Entropy Envelope. However, the FN of 36% indicates that there are missed opportunities and offers room for improving the rate of insertion. This best performing network was then used as part of the data infusion and data extraction subsystems. The network bank was composed of forty versions of this network with each having their data parameter inputs set equal to one of the forty parameters used in this experiment. The data insertion threshold, was varied from 0.5 to 0.9 and it was identified that a value of 0.7 would lead to maximum data insertion capacity, while preserving perceptual quality of the audio signal. The ultimate quality of the system was tested using a new audio file. Users were asked to listen to two audio files, one with and one without the infused data. They were then asked to identify which one contained the data. Results showed that users could not clearly identify the tape with the data. Approximately 50% of the time they identified the tape with the data correctly, and the other 50% of the time they incorrectly identified the tape with no data on it. The quality of the data retrieval was tested in terms of the number of infused data vectors that could successfully be retrieved. This test was conducted in a lossless environment. Results indicated that all data transmissions could be retrieved successfully. PMM Bank P1

Data

Filter Bank

Input Audio

A

Ab2

P2 Ab

Activity Generator

AbL AbD

. . .

Abk

Parameter Selector Subsystem

Ab1

D Ps f

Data Injection Subsystem

Output Audio A + D(Ps)

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Pn

Ab

Figure 4. Data Extraction Subsystem

Table 1. Values of g as a Function of Number of Hidden Units

g

Number of Hidden Units 2 3 4 5 1.51 2.02 1.74 1.97

6 1.81

7 1.61

8 1.84

9 1.78

10 1.56

Table 2. Best network’s performance on an independent test set Best Network Performance TNR FNR FPR TPR 34% 36% 2% 28%

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Modeling the Perceptual Masking Properties of the Human Auditory System…

331

CONCLUSIONS A system that employed Artificial Neural Networks to model the masking properties of the Human Auditory System was presented. Quality of the model was examined by its application to the audio data hiding problem. Experimental results demonstrated good capacity, transparency, and robustness of the model.

REFERENCES [1] [2]

[3] [4]

[5] [6]

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

[7]

[8] [9]

[10] [11] [12]

[13]

[14]

Hellman, R. Asymmetry of Maksing Between Noise and Tone, Perception and Psychophysics, Vol. 11 (3), 241-246, 1972. Schroeder, MR; Atal, BS; Hall, JL. Optimizing Digital Speech Coder by Exploiting Masking Properties of the Human Ear, Journal of Acoustic Society, Am. 66 (6), 16471652, 1979. Moore, BCJ. An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing., London, England: Academic Press, 3rd Edn., 1989. Beerends, JG; Stemerdink, JA. A Perceptual Audio Quality Measure Based on a Psychoacoustic Sound Representation., Journal of Audio Engineering, Vol. 40, No. 12, 1992. Lincoln, B. ―An experimental high fidelity perceptual audio coder,‖ March 1998, Project in MUS420 Win97 Bauer, FL. Decrypted Secrets – Methods and Maxims of Cryptology., Berlin, Heidelberg, Germany: Springer-Verlag, 1997, ISBN 3-540-60418-9. Nicchiotti, G; Ottaviano. E. Non-Invertible Statistical Wavelet Watermarking, EUSIPCO'98,- Ninth European Signal Processing Conference, September 8-11. 1998. pp. 2289-2292. Cox, IJ; Miller, ML. A Review Of Watermarking And The Importance Of Perceptual Modeling. Proceedings of Electronic Imaging, 1997. Nikolaidis, N; Pitas, I. Robust Image Watermarking In The Spatial Domain. Signal Processing, vol. 66, no. 3, pp. 385-403, May 1998, ISSN 0165-1684, European Association for Signal Processing (EURASIP). Karjalainen, M. A New Auditory Model For The Evaluation Of Sound Quality Of Audio Systems, in Proc. ICASSP, 1985, 608-611. Fielder, LD. Evaluation Of The Audible Distortion And Noise Produced By Digital Audio Converters, J. Audio Eng. Soc., vol. 35, July-August 1987, 517-535. Seitzer, D; Brandenburg, KH; Kappust, R; Eberlein, E; Gerhäuser, H; Krägerloh, S; Schott, H. Dsp Based Real Time Implementation Of An Advanced Analysis Tool For Audio Channels, In Proc. ICASSP, 89, 2057-2060. Herre, J; Eberlein, E. Schott, H. Brandenburg, K. Advanced Audio Measurement System Using Psychoacoustic Properties, presented at the 92nd Convention of Audio Engineering Society, J. Audio Eng. Soc. (Abstracts), vol. 40, May 1992, p. 447. Paillard, B; Mabilleau, P; Morisette, S; Soumagne, J. PERCEVAL: Perceptual Evaluation Of The Quality Audio Signals, J. Audio Eng. Soc., vol. 40, JanuaryFebruary, 1992, 21-31.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

332

Hossein L. Najafi

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

[15] Hamza Özer, Bülent Sankur, Nasir Memon, An SVD-Based Audio Watermarking Technique, MM-SEC‘05, New York, New York, 2005. [16] Al-Khassaweneh, M; Aviyente, S. Robust Watermarking On The Joint Spatial-Spectral Domain, in Proceedings of IEEE DSP Workshop, August, 2004. [17] Maria Calagna, Luigi V. Mancini, Huiping Guo, Sus hi1 Jajodia, A Robust Watermarking System based on SVD Compression, ACM SAC‘06, April 23-27 2006, Dijon, France [18] Mourjopoulos, J; Tsoukalas, D. Neural Network Mapping To Subjective Spectra Of Music Sounds, , J. Audio Eng. Soc., vol. 40, no. 4, April, 1992, 253-259. [19] Rumelhart, DE; Hinton, GE; Williams, RJ. Learning Internal Representation by Error Propagation, Parallel Distributed Processing, Chapter 8, MIT Press, 1986. [20] Werbos, PJ. Beyond Regression: New Tools for Prediction and Analysis in the Behavioral Sciences, Ph.D. Thesis. Harward University, Cambridge, MA, 1974. [21] Reed, RD; Marks II, RJ. Neural Smithing – Supervised Learning In Feedforward Artificial Neural Networks. MIT press, 1999. [22] Friedman, JH; Stuetzle, W. Projection Pursuit Regression, Journal of the American Statistical Association, Vol 76, 817-823., 1981. [23] Maruyama, M; Girosi, F; Poggio, T. A Connection Between GRBF and MLP, Artificial Intelligence Memo 1291, Massachusetts Institute of Technology., 1991. [24] Vapnik, VN. The Nature of Statistical Learning Theory, Springer, 1995. [25] Schölkopf, B; Burges, C; Smola, A. (ed.), Advances in Kernel Methods - Support Vector Learning, MIT Press, 1999.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

In: Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System ISBN: 978-1-60741-259-5 c 2010 Nova Science Publishers, Inc. Editor: D. Fiedler and R. Krause, pp. 333-372

Chapter 17

P ERCEPTUAL F EATURES FOR R OBUST S PEECH R ECOGNITION Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri School of Electrical, Electronic and Computer Engineering University of Western Australia

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Abstract Automatic speech recognition (ASR) broadly encompasses the recognition of human speech by a machine or by some artificial intelligence. The recognition process should be robust, that is, it should accurately recognise the spoken word in the presence of speaker variabilities, word perplexities, and speech corrupted by noise which are introduced during transmission and in the communication channels itself. Research in the past several decades has produced speech processing techniques like the short-time Fourier transform (STFT), the linear prediction (LP) and autoregressive (AR) methods, and the mel-frequency cepstral coefficients (MFCC), which have contributed significantly to robust speech recognition. The ability of the human auditory system to recognize speech in adverse and noisy conditions has motivated speech researchers to include features of human perception in speech recognition systems. Particularly in the early 1980s, several computational models of the auditory periphery based on physiological measurements of the response on individual fibres of the auditory nerve were proposed [1],[2],[3]. These “cochlear models” only provided marginal improvements at higher computational costs when applied to speech recognition. As a result, a decline in the interest in auditory models was observed until computing resources were able to meet the intensive computational requirements of such models. In recent years, there has been a resurgence in perceptual speech processing after research provided evidence that it may lead to improved recognition performances [12],[13],[14]. This chapter describes several psychoacoustic properties of the peripheral auditory system applied to a speech recognition front-end. Dynamic behaviour of the auditory nerves are incorporated in speech parametrization utilizing temporal processing, so that time domain information as appropriate time constants are incorporated in speech parameterization. A simplified method of synaptic adaptation as determined by psychoacoustic observations in an auditory nerve is described. It utilizes a high pass infinite impulse response (IIR)

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

334

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

temporal filter to enhance the signal onsets and the subsequent dynamic and the steadystate characteristics [15],[16]. Speech features are extracted in the temporal mode utilizing a zero-crossing algorithm [5]. The two-tone suppression as observed in the non-linear response of the basilar membrane is described in a zero-crossing auditory front-end using a temporal companding strategy [18],[17]. This may introduce asymmetric gain control without degrading the spectral contrast. The word recognitions are evaluated by continuous density hidden Markov models and are shown to provide improvements over conventional parameterizations in clean and noise conditions. Some of these perceptual algorithms may also benefit people with sensorineural hearing loss and may be implemented in hearing aids and cochlear implants for the hearing impaired through VLSI implementations [18].

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

1

Introduction

Speech processing may be categorized into two main branches: speech production/synthesis and speech perception/recognition. Automatic speech recognition (ASR) has emerged in recent years as one of the most important research areas in the field of speech science and technology. ASR broadly encompasses the recognition of human speech by a machine or by some artificial intelligence. It may form the basis of a man-machine interface for human-computer interactions. One of the main functions of an ASR system is to transform speech produced by a speech production mechanism such as human voice captured by a microphone, a telephone, or some other transducer to a text sequence, usually in terms of a sequence of words. Other important applications of ASR are in document preparations and dictations, automated dialog systems over media such as telephones and radio links, device controls through voice activation, voice “finger-printing” for identification and forensic applications, database access and web enabling via voice, and many more. The acoustic speech signals are transmitted to the receiver through some communication medium. The ASR system should accurately and robustly recognise the spoken word after it has passed through the communication channel. However, in spite of focused research in this field for the past several decades, robust and reliable speech recognition has not been achieved [13],[20],[21]. Several factors contribute to this degradation. The speech may be corrupted by external noise during the transmission process. The recognition process may fail due to speaker variabilities, that is, due to the variable speed, style, accent of the spoken word. The channel itself may introduce distortions by the convolutive noise added by the channel impulse response. A word may be misrecognised due to the context, perplexity and multiple meanings such as in the English language. Research in speech technology in the past several decades has produced significant advances in ASR. Speech signals are pseudo-periodic with substantial random components, the statistical properties of which vary significantly over time. The short-time Fourier transform (STFT) method of speech processing [25] produces a series of feature vectors at a rate sufficient to capture the rapid instantaneous transitions in the spoken word within a short duration over which the statistical properties are reasonably stationary. This has contributed significantly to robust speech recognition. The linear prediction (LP) and autoregressive (AR) method of speech analysis utilizes the speech production process to determine the parameters of the vocal tract [22]. AR modeling yields the allpole spectrum of the speech waveform, from which the frequencies and bandwidths of the individual resonances corre-

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

335

sponding to the formant positions of the vocal tract may be obtained. Another significant improvement was the cepstral method of speech processing introduced by Bogert et al. [23] in 1963, which provided reduction of feature dimensionality and decorrelation of the vocal tract excitation and the glottal waveform. Traditionally, speech enhancement methods using spectral subtraction have been employed to improve performance in noisy conditions. The recognition problem is one of determining the most probable word spoken, and usually done by template matching with a predetermined database of sound representation. Since speech has numerous variabilities which are usually unpredictable, it is possible to model speech as a context dependent random process and then use statistical tools to analyze it. As a result, speech recognition has primarily focussed on statistical methods. The most popular and effective statistical template matching is based on a set of hidden Markov models (HMM). It utilizes statistical modeling and a grammar rule to select the highest probability of some hidden or unobservable parameters of the speech from a sequence of observation feature vectors. The Markov chain rule is one of the most widely used models describing class dependence which has contributed substantially to robust speech recognition. In recent years, the superb ability of the human auditory system to recognise spoken words under extreme conditions has motivated researchers to include processing features of human auditory system and perceptual features in the automatic speech recognition process. In the early 1980s, several computational models of the auditory periphery, based on physiological measurements of the response on individual fibres of the auditory nerve, were proposed [1],[2],[3],[4]. These were generally referred to as “cochlear models”. The primary objective of such models was speech processing for auditory research [4], hearing aids [5], cochlear implants [6], and to a lesser extent, for automatic speech recognition. While comparatively little is known about the neural mechanisms of the central auditory processing stages, much more is known about the peripheral auditory system, which encompasses first stages of auditory processing. These models of the peripheral auditory system usually include a perceptual filterbank corresponding to the critical bands of human hearing, a non-linear rectification process, automatic gain control to compress the dynamic range that can be coded into the auditory nerve, synapse processing consisting of short-term synaptic adaptation, and lateral suppression. The initial process in a peripheral auditory system enhances some perceptual information in the speech signal which assists the higher auditory system in the proper identification of the speech segments [1]. Some of these perceptual features enhance the speech segments, others suppress it to increase spectral contrasts or to suppress sources of stationary noise. It was shown that such approaches tend to provide robustness and recognition accuracy with a degree of improvement over conventional speech parameterizations either in clean conditions or in environment mismatch and noisy conditions [7], [8]. However, one disadvantage with cochlear models was the higher computation cost involved with such processing. Moreover, the use of perceptual features resulted only in marginal improvements in many cases when applied to speech recognition [2],[9],[10] and in specific cases even degraded ASR performance [11]. As a result, a decline in the interest in auditory models was observed until computing resources were able to meet the intensive computational requirements of such models. In recent years there has been a resurgence in perceptual processing of speech for speech recognition after research provided evidence

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

336

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

that such processing may lead to improved performance [2],[13],[14],[40],[47]. Based on the progress in understanding auditory processing mechanisms, several aspects of sound processing in the auditory periphery are modeled and simulated in common front ends for ASR systems. For example, the transformation of speech vectors to spectral magnitudes emphasizes the role of the basilar membrane (BM) as a spectral analyzer. The filterbank decomposition techniques and the use of mel or Bark scales (critical bands) simulates the tonotopic processing in the BM [27]. Static compression with log or cube-root functions, and spectral smoothing are consistent with the perceptual features of auditory compression [28]. The spectrum is pre-emphasized to implement the functionalities of the outer and the middle ear, that is, the unequal sensitivity of human hearing at different frequencies corresponding to the equal loudness curves of perception [1]. In the filterbank analysis, the progressively higher bandwidths at higher frequencies give poorer frequency resolution which simulates the loss of synchrony and the ability of the auditory system to phase lock at higher frequencies. RASTA processing has some relation to the models of forward temporal masking [29]. Auditory perception is a psychoacoustic process in which the mapping between the acoustic speech and the human perception is non-linear. Because of this, an analytic treatment of auditory processing in the inner ear is often intractable, and relies substantially on experimental methods [30]. A distinction should be made between the two processes of psychoacoustic perception, which is a dynamic process, and speech recognition, which is primarily a static process. A comparison between a conventional and the perceptual speech recognition process is shown in Fig. 1. Auditory models for ASR integrate some of the dynamic features of psychoacoustic phenomena related to speech perception with the static speech features. On the other hand, conventional speech processing for ASR usually ignores the detailed effects of psychoacoustic perception. The primary objective of this project is to identify the perceptual features which are relevant and useful for the improvement of ASR performance in clean and noisy conditions. Speech

Preprocessing

Speech parameterization

Speech recognition

Conventional speech recognition

Speech

Preprocessing

Perceptual processing

Speech parameterization

Speech recognition

Perceptual speech recognition

Figure 1: Comparison between the conventional and the perceptual speech recognition process. In this chapter, the performance of two perceptual properties of the peripheral audi-

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

337

tory system, synaptic adaptation and two-tone suppression are investigated for their roles in automatic speech recognition in clean conditions and in additive noise environment. The chapter is arranged as follows. Section 2 gives an overview of some methodologies of auditory processing related to automatic speech speech recognition. In section 3, a simplified synaptic adaptation model is developed in a zero-crossing auditory model used as a speech recognition front-end. It is shown that rapid adaptation can be implemented in temporal processing of speech, not otherwise implementable in the spectral domain. In section 4, the two-tone suppression is investigated and a method of introducing the two-tone suppression in a zero-crossing auditory front-end using temporal companding is described. This is followed in section 5 by an overall discussion and conclusion drawn from the experiments.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

2

Some Methodologies of Perceptual Speech Recognition

In our study, we have particularly emphasized on three methodologies. Firstly, we have adopted temporal processing of speech, utilizing time-domain filters, rather than processing in the spectral domain. The temporal-place representation of auditory processing is much less affected by background noise than the rate-place representation [31]. There are several other advantages of processing in the time-domain. Auditory functions are usually dynamic in nature with substantial transient properties, which, unlike steady-state emissions, can not be fully characterized by the spectrum, although short-time Fourier transform processing may alleviate this to some extent. This is primarily because, by their very nature, transients are time-varying or non-stationary signals [32]. In many cases, the fine temporal structures and time variations are removed by the spectral feature extraction methods in the log spectral domain [14]. Recursive filtering which can simulate many of the psychophysical behaviors of human perception, is better implemented in the time domain than in the spectral domain because of the aliasing problem and the requirement of inverting an infinite dimensional Fourier transform. Time-varying cues are also useful in identifying CV (consonant followed by a vowel sound) transitions [25]. Secondly, we have chosen a temporal auditory model using a zero-crossing algorithm for implementations. The conventional features used for speech recognition, e.g., the MFCC and the PLP, mainly extract the spectral properties of the signal, and lacks in temporal information. it is well known that this deficiency in the MFCCs, and almost all other features conventionally used for speech recognition, are somewhat inadequate for the recognition of rapidly changing events such as the bursts in plosives. Moreover, formant transitions, a key aspect in the perception of speech, are also implemented inadequately in the MFCC. This is, however, compensated by the delta-delta coefficients in the MFCC, which again increase the dimensionality of the feature vectors. Using spectro-temporal features, similar to those found in the primary auditory cortex, should overcome the above mentioned problems. One method of spectro-temporal speech parameterization is the zero-crossing algorithm for speech processing, such as in the the zero-crossing peak amplitudes (ZCPA) auditory model proposed by Kim et al. [13]. It is based on the principle that any stimulus periodicity in the filter subband can be extracted from the zero-crossing intervals, the inverse of which shows up as a dominant frequency corresponding to the formant peaks within that subband. This is known as the dominant frequency principle [33]. The dominant frequency in a subband has more power than others, which makes the zero-crossing algorithm more

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

338

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

robust in noisy environments. Moreover, the zero-crossing analysis is amenable to simple transformations instead of complex transformations between the time and the frequency domains such as in the spectral processing. Thirdly, for the feature extraction process, larger time windows and filters with appropriate time constants consistent with human perception are utilized. Since time domain processing is based on extracting frequency information from periodicity, it generally utilizes larger window lengths than the short-time FFT methods. Auditory models do not make strong assumptions of quasi-stationarity. This is consistent with human auditory functions which have longer integration times with higher time constants. For example, forward masking may last up to 200 ms [14]. However, STFT processing of speech using a filterbank approach (e.g. MFCC) is based on smaller stationary window lengths of 20-25 ms. Too short a window length produces a poor spectral representation of the speech, which usually degrades in noise condition, and is also sensitive to temporal effects of voicing. Performances at higher SNRs decrease with decreasing window lengths [25]. This has motivated several researchers to emphasize time interval processing with longer time constants for integration into the speech recognition systems [34],[35].

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

3

Speech Recognition with Synaptic Adaptation

Synaptic adaptation is a dynamic mechanism of the human auditory system. In response to tone bursts, a single auditory-nerve fibre exhibits an increased neural discharge in the initial 15 ms at the signal onset. This decays monotonically in time, reaching a steady-level within about 50 ms. This decrease in response rate, referred to as “synaptic adaptation”, has been determined by physiological experiments and from responses to pure tones [37],[38]. In this section, we demonstrate a method of introducing synaptic adaptation as observed in the peripheral auditory system for automatic speech recognition. The adaptation process is implemented in a ZCPA auditory model in the temporal domain and evaluated by a HMM recognizer. We first analyse the process of synaptic adaptation in the auditory system, followed by an analysis of the zero-crossing algorithm. This is followed by an implementation method of a simplified adaptation scheme in the ZCPA auditory model using an infinite impulse response (IIR) filter. At the end, the results are presented demonstrating the benefit of the scheme both analytically and quantitatively as percentage improvements in recognition performance.

3.1

The Synaptic Aadaptation Process

The synaptic adaptation process is shown in Fig. 2 as a post-stimulus time histogram (PSTH) for an afferent nerve fibre. Initially, there is a surge in the response at the beginning of the stimulus which gradually decays to a steady-state response. The decay consists of an initial rapid phase with a time constant of 3 ms (rapid synaptic adaptation). It is followed by a slower exponential decay with a time constant of about 40 ms (short-term synaptic adaptation). It is observed from the figure that in the absence of the stimulus the auditory nerve fibre produces spikes at a small but discernable rate. This is called the spontaneous rate. When the tone is removed, the spike rate decreases to slightly lower than the spontaneous rate for a short time before resuming its normal rate. The synaptic adaptation process

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

339

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 2: Poststimulus time histogram (PSTH) of an auditory nerve fibre to a pure tone burst showing the rapid and short-term synaptic adaptation (adopted from [36]). indicates that the auditory nerve is more responsive to changes than to steady inputs. The rate of fall from the onset transient down to the synaptic adaptation level is independent of the intensity of the tone signal level [37]. Synaptic adaptation in the AN results primarily from the release of transmitter substances (neurotransmitters) from the hair cells into the synaptic cleft at the beginning of a signal onset, and the subsequent transmitter depletion of Ca 2+ in the voltage-sensitive Ca 2+ channels [39],[14]. After the release of neurotransmitters by the hair cells, the voltagesensitive Ca 2+ channels located close to the synapses at the basal part of these cells open upon depolarization of the cell membrane. This process fills up the “readily releasable pool” (RRP). The cleft is filled up at a rate that is proportional to the concentration gradient of the transmitter across the membrane. Some transmitter within the cleft is lost which gives rise to the slow depletion of the neurotransmitters in the cleft. Some flow back through the cell membrane for recycling (reuptake) and are reprocessed to fill up the RRP. At the beginning of an acoustic stimulus, plenty of vesicles are available to fuse, causing a strong initial AN response. Subsequently, as the RRP is refilled at a lower rate than the initial vesicle fusion rate due to the loss in the cleft, the RRP depletes with time. Auditory nerve activity is, thus, depressed shortly after the stimulus onset and during sustained stimuli, producing the synaptic adaptation effect. This process is shown in Fig. 3. Synaptic adaptation accentuates signal onsets by following a high initial discharge rate. A rapid synaptic adaptation component enhances voicing, and short-term synaptic adaptation is found to improve the immunity of the system to stationary noise [20]. This principle is used in RASTA processing of speech and shown to improve the robustness of the system [29]. However, the main difference is that RASTA processing is a bandpass modulation filtering operating on the logarithmic spectrum, which completely suppresses dc modulations, whereas the auditory nerve shows a sustained discharge rate to steady-state stimuli.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

340

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

Figure 3: Synaptic adaptation process in the synapses between the inner hair cells and an auditory

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

nerve.

The perceptual feature of synaptic adaptation has been employed in ASR by several researchers. Cohen [8] implemented an ASR front-end utilizing a Bark-scaled filterbank in which a compressive power law transformation was applied to the output of each filter approximating loudness scaling. A reservoir-type synaptic adaptation was used to model the neural discharge rate. It was shown that the error rates and decoding times in a word recognition task were substantially lower with the auditory model than with a mel filterbank implementation (3.9% vs. 6.3%). The generalized synchrony detector (GSD) model proposed by Seneff [1] incorporated an IHC-synaptic processing stage which consisted of a saturating half-wave rectification, short-term synaptic adaptation, synchrony suppression at higher frequencies and rapid synaptic adaptation, as observed from psychophysical measurements. Jankowski [40] designed a speech recognizer using Seneff’s GSD auditory model and compared the word error rates with a conventional mel filterbank using isolated words and a HMM recognizer. The GSD front-end produced better recognition performance results than the mel filterbank. Strope et al. [12] described a dynamic model with a logarithmic synaptic adaptation stage based on forward masking data. The adaptation mechanism implemented was a modified form of the automatic gain control (AGC), which added an exponentially adapting linear offset to the logarithmic intensity. It showed improvements in robustness to background noise when used as a front-end for DTW and HMM based recognitions. Holmberg et al. [14] incorporated a simplified model of synaptic adaptation for ASR into the MFCC feature parametrization as a competitive strategy to RASTA and the cepstrum mean subtraction (CMS). A first-order infinite impulse response (IIR) high-pass filter was used to represent the decaying exponential effects of the rapid and the short-term synaptic adaptation. Evaluated with Aurora 2 database and a HMM recognizer, it showed improved ASR performance compared to the baseline MFCC, and MFCC in combination with RASTA and CMS processing. In contrast, we have proposed a time-domain approach to the adaptation and demonstrated a method of implementation in a auditory model of the human peripheral auditory system used as an ASR front-end. To demonstrate the implementation, we first introduce the zero-crossing algorithm for speech processing.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

3.2

341

The Zero-crossing Analysis

For a real-valued zero mean stationary Gaussian random process Zt , t ∈ T for all t1 , . . . , tN ∈ T , the number of zero-crossings D, can be defined in terms of a clipped process Mt for discrete time t as D=

N X

(Mt − Mt−1 )2 ,

0≤D≤N−1

(1)

t=1

where Mt =

(

1 0

if Zt ≥ 0 if Zt < 0

for t=1,. . . ,N. If we define µ=E{Zt} as the mean vector (E{.} denotes the expected value), and γk as the covariance matrix as a function of the lag k such that γk = Cov(Zt (k)) = E{(Zt − µ)(Zt+k − µ)}.

(2)

To make the covariance invariant to scaling, it is usually useful to normalize γk with respect to γ0, and define the normalized parameter ρk such that we can express

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

ρk =

γk γ0

k = 0, ±1, . . ..

(3)

Using the Wiener-Khintchin theorem, a relationship between the covariance matrix γk and its spectral representation may be obtained. Koopmans [41] has shown that using the Wiener-Khintchin theorem, it is possible to define a monotone increasing function, F (ω), (ω being the angular frequency with a maximum value of π), such that γk =



cos(kω)dF (ω)

(4)

−π

where F (ω) is the spectral distribution function (cumulative) of Zt . The function Zt must satisfy the convergence property, that is, it must be square integrable, Z∞

dt | Z(t) |2< ∞

(5)

−∞

If we substitute for k=1 in Eqn. (3), then we may write

ρ1 =

γ1 γ0 Rπ

=

cos(ω)dF (ω)

−π



. dF (ω)

−π Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

(6)

342

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

The expected value of the zero-crossings count, D, of the Gaussian Zt , t ∈ T , with zero mean is related to its spectral representation [33] by

cos



πE{D} (N − 1)





=

cos(ω)dF (ω)

0



(7) dF (ω)

0

where F (ξ) =

Z π

f (ξ)dξ

(8)

−π

is a spectral distribution function (cumulative) and f (ξ), π ≤ ξ ≤ π, is the spectral density of Zt . Eqn. (7) shows that πE{D}/(N −1) which is the normalized expected zero-crossing rate of the random process, Zt , may be represented in terms of the angular (weighted) spectral content of Zt . Therefore, within a subband, if a certain frequency, ω0 , becomes dominant, that is, has more power (weights) than in other bands, then it is seen from Eqn. (7)

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

πE{D} ≈ ω0 (N − 1)

(9)

which states that this frequency may be represented by a corresponding average zerocrossing rate. This is referred to as the dominant frequency principle [33]. A dominant frequency in a subband has more power than others, which makes the zero-crossing algorithms more robust in noisy conditions [13]. The resolution of the subband dominant frequency estimates decreases rapidly at higher frequencies. One method to circumvent this is by up-sampling the high frequency subband signals using frequency-dependent interpolation factors [21].

3.3

The ZCPA Auditory Model

An auditory model based on the zero-crossing algorithm is the zero-crossing with peak amplitudes (ZCPA) proposed by Kim et al. [13]. It is based on the dominant frequency principle and duplicates the functions of the peripheral auditory system in some details. It is an enhancement of the ensemble interval histogram (EIH) model proposed by Ghitza [2], where it replaces the multiple levels of the EIH with a single zero level to extract the speech features from the zero-crossings. Fig. 4 shows the schematic of the ZCPA temporal auditory model used as an ASR front-end. Digitized speech is input to a bank of bandpass filters with frequency characteristics closely resembling the human frequency response along the length of the basilar membrane. It utilizes a zero-crossing detector in each channel to estimate the dominant frequencies within a filterbank channel. Each speech frame is represented by a frequency histogram obtained from the inverse of the time interval between two successive upward going zero-crossings in the filter channel outputs. The intensity is measured by a peak detector from the peak amplitudes between each zero-crossing interval. In this experiment, a computationally efficient version of the ZCPA with fewer number of filters to reduce the computational cost and suitable for hardware realization was implemented.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition xi(n;m)

s(n)

Cochlear Filter Bank C

Zero-crossings detector, Di

343

Interval histogram b

Peak detector /compressive nonlinearity, Pik

ZCPA ya(m,b) Σ

• •

Figure 4: The ZCPA auditory model used as an ASR front-end utilizing a zero-crossing detector and a peak detector for feature extraction. An ensemble interval histogram is constructed by summing the histograms of all the frames. An expression of the ZCPA histogram output may be obtained as follows. If we denote ms (n) the clean message speech signal which is corrupted with bandlimited white Gaussian noise, ϕ(n), with a rectangular power spectrum of bandwidth, W , then the noisy speech s(n) = ms (n) + ϕ(n) is given by s(n) =

M −1 X

Al cos(ωl n + θl ) + Aϕ ϕ(n),

l = 0, 1, . . ., M − 1

(10)

l=0

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

P

−1 where the message speech ms (n)= M l=0 Al cos(ωl n + θl ), l ∈ M contains M frequency components each with a phase θl within the signal, and Al and Aϕ are the signal and noise amplitudes, respectively. If the input noisy signal, s(n), is filtered by a bank of bandpass cochlear filters with bandwidth, Bi , where i = 1, . . . , C corresponds to the number of channels in the filterbank, then the output at the i-th bandpass filter may be expressed in discrete form as

xi (n) = Ai cos(ωi n + θi ) + Aϕ ϕ(n).

(11)

If w(n) is a window of finite length, then the value of xi (n) at the frame index m is given by xi (n; m)=xi(n)wi (m−n), i = 1, . . ., C, where C is the number of filter channels. Let Di be the number of upward-going zero-crossings at the i-th filter in a windowed frame xi (n; m) and Pik is the peak amplitude between the k-th and the (k + 1)-th zerocrossing in xi (n; m). Then the output y(m, b) at time m is given in [13] as y(m, b) =

C DX i −1 X

log(Pik + 1)δbjk ,

1≤b 0, Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

346

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

Figure 5: Rapid and short-term synaptic adaptation effects to a 2 kHz tone bursts of 150 and 625 ms durations with a time constant τ =40 ms using the transfer function Ha(z) (from [17])

Var{θˆN } (16) 2 Therefore, if the variance is sufficiently small, then the probability that the difference between the true value and the estimated value is greater than  will also be smaller. Based on the above analysis, a simplified adaption scheme suitable for ASR applications is presented. We define the first order high-pass infinite impulse response (IIR) filter function as Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Pr{|θˆN − θ| ≥ } ≤

Ha(z) =

10τ fr (1 − z −1 ) (10τ fr + 0.05) + (10τ fr − 0.05)z −1

(17)

where τ is the synaptic adaptation time constant in seconds and fr is the frame rate equal to 100 Hz [17]. Fig. 5 shows the transient effects of the rapid and the short-term synaptic adaptation implemented using Eqn. (17) with a time constant τ =40 ms to a 2 kHz tone of 625 ms duration preceded by a tone of 150 ms may be observed. Particularly, the initial onset at the beginning of the tone and the gradual decay due to the adaptation process depending on the filter time constant can be observed in the figure. Further in Fig. 5, effects similar to forward masking are also observed, e.g., the first tone burst is reduced in magnitude due to the presence of the second tone burst following it when the time duration separating the two tones is less than the time constant of the filter. It has been reported that forward (post-masking) and simultaneous masking can last up to 200 ms, whereas backward masking (pre-masking) are of much shorter duration, typically 10-30 ms [14]. Longer time constants are important for speech processing which may give better recognition performance. The best time constant for ASR lies between 200 and 300 ms [14]. A time constant of 250 ms was used in all our experiments utilizing temporal synaptic adaptation with a cutoff frequency of 0.636 Hz which is well below the modulation spectrum of 1-16 Hz. The modulation frequencies are regarded important for speech intelligibility [29] and the cutoff of 0.636 Hz of the high-pass filter would not interfere with

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

347

the modulation frequencies. The synaptic adaptation filtering was implemented by summing the high-pass synaptic adaptation filter output with the original FIR filter output for each channel of the filterbank. One particular issue that needs to be considered is that whether the frame sizes of 40120 ms and the frame rates 10 ms used in auditory models, being much smaller than the adaptation time constant of 250 ms, are able to capture the effects of adaptation and the forward masking in the speech signal. The conventional frame sizes are even smaller, typically in the range of 20-25 ms. In the case of forward masking which may last upto 200 ms, the masking effect in a particular frame may depend on the statistics of the previous frames [44]. However, for the temporal synaptic adaptation used in ASR, we did not consider the effects of the previous frames, since the main objective of the adaptation process is to enhance the speech onsets, which occur within the initial 15 ms of the input stimulus. This effect may be extracted from a reasonable frame size of 80 ms which have been employed in our experiments. Moreover, the effect of synaptic adaptation on onsets is more prominent than the forward masking effect across the frames, since speech perception is more sensitive to rapid variations and dynamic changes than to the steady-state characteristics. In the HMM a state corresponds to an acoustically stable region or a number of continuous number of frames with stationary observation vectors. Onset region detection techniques based on a hidden Markov model (HMM) utilize localization of onset regions as temporal sequences within a frame which are then combined together for speech recognition process using the HMM. Frames containing onsets regions in the speech can be detected directly from the state transition sequence in the HMM. Moreover, by using a frame advance of 10 ms a frame-based method can still capture some of the rapid synaptic adaptation effects, depending on the time alignment of the frame with respect to the onset time. In conventional feature extraction by spectral methods, detection of CVC transitions in speech which are typically dynamic in nature similar to synaptic adaptation, follow a similar detection procedure by the frame-based method. However, in the MFCC, these dynamic effects are captured more efficiently by incorporating delta-delta features. Fig. 6 shows the post-stimulus time histogram (PSTH) obtained from the ZCPA auditory model with an IIR adaptation filter given by Eqn. (17) but with a time constant τ =40 ms. The input was a 1 kHz tone of 50 ms duration. The PSTH was taken with 70 presentations of the input and collected in 400 bins with 0.2 ms bin-width. It is observed that at the signal onset, there is a rapid increase in the histogram count, followed by a rapid decrease (due to the rapid synaptic adaptation) within about 3 ms (pointer ‘A’). This is followed by a more gradual and slower decay giving rise to short-term synaptic adaptation (pointer ‘B’) and a nearly constant steady-state response. It is observed in Fig. 6 that effects similar to the rapid synaptic adaptation may be implemented in temporal processing of speech such as in the ZCPA, not otherwise possible in spectral implementation. The Figs. 5 and 6 have been implemented with pure tone bursts to demonstrate the effects of synaptic adaptation using Eqn. (17). Speech signals are, however, more complex and the time constant of 40 ms may not be appropriate for speech recognition purposes. Westerman et al. [38] measured the time constants in the Mongolian Gerbil to be a few milliseconds for rapid synaptic adaptation and roughly 40-60 ms for short-term synaptic adaptation. Spoor et al. [45] found that synaptic adaptation time constants in humans might differ considerably from values measured in animals. He estimated that human time

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

348

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri 700

600

A B

Histogram count

500

400

300

200

100

0 0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

Time (ms)

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 6: Post Stimulus Time Histogram (PSTH) obtained from the ZCPA showing the effects of rapid synaptic adaptation (pointer ‘A’) and short-term synaptic adaptation (pointer ‘B’). The PSTH was obtained with a synaptic adaptation filter with a time constant τ =40 ms and was taken with 70 presentations of a 1 kHz tone input of 50 ms duration in 400 bins with a bin width of 0.2 ms.

constants for recovery from synaptic adaptation are about a factor of four longer compared to gerbil data. It has been reported that forward (post-masking) and simultaneous masking can last up to 200 ms whereas backward masking (pre-masking) are of much shorter duration, typically 10-30 ms [46]. This was verified by Holmberg et al. by a series of experiments using Aurora 2 and 3 speech corpus and evaluating word error rates for a range of synaptic adaptation time constants. It was found that for the case of clean training with Wiener filtering, the best time constant is 80 ms, but for the case with adaptation and multicondition training, it was found that the best time constant lies between 200 ms and 300 ms. Therefore, a synaptic adaptation time constant of 240 ms was chosen by Holmberg et al. in the experimentations. This time constant is also motivated by the notion that higher levels in the auditory pathway, with presumably longer adaptation time constants than the auditory nerve, contribute to the temporal processing of speech. The 240-ms time constant is consistent with other auditory models that have been used as ASR front ends [47] and with the high-pass corner frequency employed in RASTA [29] processing. Motivated by these experimental results, we have chosen a time constant of 250 ms as the adaptation time constant in all our experiments for temporal synaptic adaptation. This time constant gives a cutoff frequency of 0.636 Hz which is well below the modulation spectrum of 1-16 Hz. The modulation frequencies are regarded important for speech intelligibility [29] and the cutoff of 0.636 Hz of the high-pass filter would not interfere with the modulation frequencies. The synaptic adaptation filtering was implemented by summing the high-pass synaptic adaptation filter output with the original FIR filter output for each channel of the filterbank. Fig. 7 shows the ZCPA with the simplified synaptic adaptation used as a preprocessing front-end. The synaptic adaptation as implemented in Eqn. (17) was introduced in each filter channel with a time constant τ =250 ms. The input speech was first normalized between

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition Speech s(n)

Hamming windowed frames

FIR filter bank

Preemphasis

Zerocrossing detector

Rapid and shortterm adaptation

349

Interval histogram

Peak detector

ZCPA_ADP ya(m,b)

ZCPA

Figure 7: The ZCPA auditory model with the synaptic adaptation (ZCPA ADP) processed on a frame data of 80 ms. ±1 to reduce the effects of loudness. Normalization also improves noise performance by enhancing spectral contrast. Speech frames were pre-emphasized to model the outer and the middle ear functionalities that approximate the unequal sensitivity of human hearing at different frequencies. A pre-emphasis coefficient of -0.97 was used. It was then processed by a bank of 16 finite impulse response (FIR) filters of order 70 with the CFs uniformly spaced on the equivalent rectangular bandwidth (ERB) scale [48] between 10-3500 Hz. 10 5 0

Gain (dB)

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

−5 −10 −15 −20 −25 −30 −35 −40 0

500

1000

1500

2000

2500

3000

3500

Frequency, (Hz)

Figure 8: Frequency response of the perceptual FIR filterbank consisting of 16 FIR filters each of order 70 with the centre frequencies of each channel spaced at the ERB scale.

The frequency response of the perceptual FIR filterbank is shown in Fig. 8. Our choice of FIR filters for the filterbank was motivated by the fact that FIR filters consistently performed better than carefully designed cochlear filters when applied in ASR applications [13]. In most applications, real-time operations through hardware implementation is often the main goal. All digital signal processors (DSPs) available have architectures in powers of two, 2n , where n is the number of available bits. This is particularly suited to FIR filter-

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

350

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

ing in hardware. Moreover, FIR filters are always stable, have exactly linear phase, and are simple to implement. However, FIR filters of particularly higher orders may also introduce substantial lag or time delay. Since the outputs are extracted as frequency histogram counts, the time delay effects on the output are not considered [13]. To reduce computational time, we implemented the simplified model with fewer number of filters with some optimization of the parameters and feature extraction algorithm. However, fewer number of filters result in greater frequency overlap of adjacent frequency channels, particularly at higher frequencies due to wider bandwidths. This would result in a lower frequency resolution and an additional effect of loss of synchrony at higher frequencies. However, both of these effects are consistent with perceptual properties of the inner ear and the cochlea. This is also supported by the observation that decreasing the number of electrodes in cochlear implant patients have little or no effects on speech intelligibility [24]. Gajic et al. [21] made a study of the ASR performance obtained by several different choices of filter bandwidths and number of histogram bins and has quantified the results. It was observed that the choice of filter bandwidths had a significant influence on the ASR performance, while it was not very sensitive to the particular choice of the number of histogram bins. However, the influence of increased number of filters was tested by evaluating features based on 16 and 20 filters, and no significant performance difference was observed [21]. In the ZCPA, the width of the derivative windows in units of time over which the features are extracted are functions of the centre frequencies of the filterbank. In temporal processing it is usually required to use large window sizes especially for lower frequency channels to capture about 10 periods of the signal for the accumulation of temporal information [21]. Although longer time windows give better parameter estimates, the window size should not be too large to violate the stationarity assumption in the short-term processing method. Considering these, the largest window size was limited to 80 ms corresponding to the lowest frequency channel. In each filter output, inverse of zero-crossing intervals were collected in 26 frequency bins uniformly spaced between 10-4000 Hz on the ERB scale. The interval histogram was weighted by the logarithm of the peak value within two successive zero-crossings. Frame data was processed at a frame rate of 10 ms. The histogram was normalized between 0 and +1 with respect to the maximum value. Normalising the histogram produces two effects. Firstly, it limits the dynamic range of the histogram and secondly, the compression due to normalising reduces the effects of any biasing in the histogram. Thirteen cepstra were generated and retained from each speech frame. Figs. 9 (a) and (b) show the spectrogram of the 35-th frame for the male CV utterance /ba/ without adaption and with temporal synaptic adaptation with τ =250 ms, respectively [17]. The processing started at the 35th frame of the whole utterance /ba/ and ended after the processing of that frame. A single frame was taken to study the time-frequency characteristics within this frame for greater resolution and for comparison for the two cases with synaptic adaptation and without synaptic adaptation. It is assumed that the steady-state vowel /a/ and the corresponding formants are captured in this frame in addition to the high frequency speech segments in that frame. It is observed in (b) that the high frequency segments are enhanced by the application of the temporal synaptic adaptation strategy. However, the low frequency formants are not affected. Fig. 10 shows a spectrogram-like (narrowband) time-frequency plots of the point pro-

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

351

Frequency (kHz)

6.4 4.8

3.2 1.6

0 20

40

60

80

Time (frame segment)

(a) Without synaptic adaptation /ba/

Frequency (kHz)

6.4 4.8 3.2 1.6 0 20

40

60

80

Time (frame segment)

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

(b) With synaptic adaptation /ba/ Figure 9: Spectrogram of the ZCPA for the 35-th frame of the male utterance /ba/ in clean condition (a) without synaptic adaptation, and (b) with synaptic adaptation with a time constant τ =250 ms. It is seen that the high frequency segments are enhanced by the application of the adaptation filtering.

cess obtained from the simulated firing pattern for the voiced plosive /ba/ in clean conditions. The plot was obtained using 120 FIR filters from the upward zero-crossing events of the waveform and was collected over a fixed derivative window of 80 ms for each frame. It has been implemented for greater detailed modelling of the the peripheral auditory system and is not suitable for speech recognition applications because of computational costs and complexities. The effect of fixed derivative windows, in contrast to channel dependent derivative windows in other implementations in this project, is a histogram bias at high frequencies which is equivalent to a pre-emphasis operation. The utterance ‘ba’ was chosen to emphasize the CV transitions for the plosive /b/ followed by the formants associated with the vowel /a/. The formant structures are visible as a narrowband spectrogram. In Fig. 10, the upper panel shows the base ZCPA without synaptic adaptation and the lower panel shows the same plot with temporal synaptic adaptation. It is seen in the upper panel that the burst after the closure is emphasized with temporal synaptic adaptation in the high

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

352

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

Figure 10: Spectrogram-like time-frequency plot (narrowband) of the point process obtained from

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

the simulated firing pattern of the ZCPA for the CV utterance /ba/ in clean condition without synaptic adaptation (upper panel) and with synaptic adaptation (time constant τ =250 ms)(lower panel). The lower panel shows enhanced onsets with adaptation at higher frequencies (corresponding to lower channels).

frequency regions (lower numbered channels), indicating increased discharge rate at higher frequencies.

3.5

CV Discriminatory Characteristics of Temporal Synaptic Adaptation

Measuring the discrimination effectiveness of feature vectors is an important method of determining robustness which aids in the selection of optimal features for a given dimensionality. Compared to conventional speech features such as MFCC and the PLP, temporal auditory models usually employ larger window lengths which is particularly sensitive to non-stationary segments of speech. These non-stationary segments such as the consonantvowel (CV) transitions are poorly represented in such models. To test the effectiveness of the adaptation model in CV discrimination, we implemented three experiments - frequency discrimination, vowel clustering by grouped scatter plots and LDA separability measures using within class and between class scatter matrices, which are explained in the following three subsections. The speech corpus used for the CV discrimination tasks was the UCLA-SPAPL CV corpus which is an extensive database of 1728 isolated CV utterances by four speakers

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

353

(2 males and 2 females). Each CV was spoken in 8 different annotations by each of the four speakers in 18 consonants, each consonant in three vowel contexts, thus constituting a total of 1728 utterances. For example, corresponding to the plosive /b/, the utterances are /ba/,/bee/ and /boo/ in three vowel contexts /a/,/ee/ and /oo/. The CV combinations were digitally recorded at a sampling rate of 16 kHz, and are shown labeled in ASCII in Table 1. 3.5.1

Frequency Discrimination by Synaptic Adaptation

The IIR adaptation filtering primarily enhances the high frequency components associated with frication and voice onsets. Since most speech energies are concentrated in the lower frequency regions, the contributions to the high frequency histograms were mainly due to the increased zero-crossing counts instead of the contributions of the interval peak values. Hence, a quantitative measure of the effects of the high pass synaptic adaptation filtering on speech perception could be obtained by analyzing the histogram counts at the output of the higher frequency channels compared to the lower frequency channels. For this discrimination, we chose a threshold frequency of 1340 Hz. The choice of this frequency is rather heuristic, and is partly based on consideration that it should be higher than the first formant, which is the dominant formant of the common vowels. Again a too high threshold may eliminate the high frequency artefacts of speech such as consonants (voiced and unvoiced plosives and the fricatives), which may compromise the experimental results. Table 1: The UCLA-SPAPL CV corpus with the consonant types and the corresponding vowel

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

tokens. Articulation Voiced plosives Aspirated plosives Nasals Fricatives Affricates

Consonants /b/,/d/,/g/ /p/,/t/,/k /m/,/n/ /s/,/Z/,/sh/,/SH/ /f/,/V/,/th/,/TH/ /j/,/ch/

Vowel contexts /a/ as in “bought” /ee/ as in “beat” /oo/ as in “boot”

The vowel discrimination with synaptic adaptation was measured as the percentage increase in the histogram counts with synaptic adaptation (ZC adp ) over the histogram counts without synaptic adaptation (ZC), as obtained using the formula P

f

P

ZCadp (f ) − f ZC(f ) P × 100%, f ZC(f )

f ≥ 1340 Hz.

(18)

The consonant types /b/,/d/,/g/,/p/,/t/,/k/,/m/,/n/,/s/,/SH/,/j/,/ch/ in the three vowel contexts /a/,/ee/and /oo/ were used in the experiment. The results are summarized in Fig. 11. It is observed that the largest increase in the histogram counts is achieved for the fricatives, which contain substantial high frequency components. As for the vowels, the largest percentage increase are recorded for the vowel /ee/ in all consonant types (14.16 % for /SHee/), followed by the vowel /oo/ in fricatives (11.49 %) and affricates (9.19 %). This may be due to the higher second formant for the vowel /ee/ (1730 Hz) compared to vowel /a/ (1090 Hz) and vowel /oo/ (870 Hz).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

354 3.5.2

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri Vowel Clustering with Synaptic Adaptation

ASR relies to a larger extent on the correct classification of vowels than consonants [25]. The mixture likelihood approach to clustering, which is a class of unsupervised learning, is based on maximization of the likelihood estimation of finite mixture models utilizing the EM algorithm. A more simple measure of unsupervised clustering may be obtained by the linear discriminant analysis. In our experiment, a qualitative measure of clustering of the three vowel classes with synaptic adaptation was obtained by grouped scatter plots of the CV utterances in the UCLA-SPAPL database, which was represented by an utterance matrix, each row corresponding to an utterance represented by a m-cepstrum vector in the m-dimensional space. In two dimensional linear discrimination, any two fixed dimensions of the utterance matrix may be plotted as the x and y coordinates, with the means associated with each class as the centroid. The discrimination among the classes is essentially reflected in the extent of the correlation between these two components of the feature vector set, as exemplified by scattering from the individual class centroids. For our experiments, we selected the voiced plosives /b/,/d/,/g/ and the aspirated plosives /p/,/t/,/k/ in three vowel contexts /a/,/ee/ and /oo/, for a total of 18 utterances. The 18 CVs were spoken by 2 male and 2 female speakers, each speaker uttering the same CV in five different annotations, thus constituting a total of 360 CVs. Features were extracted from each utterance from an interval histogram at the rate of 13 cepstra per frame. These were collapsed into a single vector by taking the mean of all the frames, thus converting the 360 CVs into a single 360x13 utterance matrix.

% increase in histogram

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

16 14 12 10

Vowel /a/

8

Vowel /ee/

6

Vowel /oo/

4 2 0 Plosives

Aspirated

Nasal

Fricatives Affricates

Articulation

Figure 11: Graph showing effects of temporal synaptic adaptation on speech articulation as percentage increase in the high frequency histogram counts in the ZCPA with temporal synaptic adaptation (ZCadp) over the base ZCPA. The two dimensional grouped scatter plots of the utterance matrix are shown in Fig. 12 without synaptic adaptation in (a) and with synaptic adaptation in (b). The increased clustering of the vowel /ee/ having a higher second formant (1730 Hz) is observed in (b). However, the other vowels /aa/ and /oo/ having lower second formants (1090 Hz for /a/ and 870 Hz for /oo/) appears to be just as scattered. This further emphasizes the role of synaptic

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

355

adaptation on high frequency articulation and dynamic behaviours of speech. Improved speech recognition performance may result from this effect.

3.6

Linear Discriminant Analysis by Class Separability Measure

Class separability measures may be used to determine the discrimination effectiveness of a particular feature vector. In Sec. 3.5.2, a qualitative representation of vowel clustering with synaptic adaptation was presented as grouped scatter plots. In this section, we present a quantitative measure of separability among the vowel classes. A quantitative measure of the separability, or divergence among the three vowel classes may be determined by one of several methods such as Kulback-Liebler distance measure between density functions, and the Brattacharayya distance. However, determination of these separability measures are feasible only when a Gaussian assumption of the distribution of the class members is valid. A simpler and more general quantitative criterion may be obtained from the scatter characteristics of the feature vector samples in the m-dimensional space using the linear discriminant analysis approach based on the within-class and between-class scatter matrices [49]. It utilizes a linear transformation which maps the feature space into a lower dimensional space such that a class separability criterion is optimized. For our experiment, we obtained the two quantitative measures of separability, J1 and J2 , among the three vowel classes utilizing the three voiced plosives /b/,/d,/g/, each in three vowel contexts, /a/,/ee/,/oo/. The 9 CVs were uttered by 4 speakers in 5 different styles, for a total of 180 CV utterances (with 60 utterances in each of the three vowel classes). The two quantitative measures (unnormalized) of separability, J1 and J2 are obtained as

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

J1 = tr(Sw−1Sb )

(19)

det(Sb + Sw ) . det(Sw )

(20)

L X X 1 (x − µl )(x − µl )T (N − 1) l=1 x∈w

(21)

L 1 X Nl(µl − µ)(µl − µ)T . N l=1

(22)

J2 = where Sw =

l

Sb =

In the above equations, {xn }K n=1 is a set of feature vectors each of dimension K = 13, N =180 is the total number of observations in the three vowel classes, Nl is the observations in each class l, l ∈ L, where L=3 is the number of classes, µl is the class mean vector and µ is the total mean. It is seen that Sw is the covariance matrix of individual classes summed over all classes and its traces are sum of variances. J1 takes large values when samples in the l-dimensional space are well clustered around their mean, within each class, and the clusters of the different classes are well separated. It has the advantage of being invariant under linear transformations [49]. J2 gives an alternate criterion with determinants used in place of traces. This is justified for scatter matrices that are symmetric positive definite and thus

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

356

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri 0.5

0.5 1 2 3

0.4

0.3

0.3

0.2

0.2

0.1

0.1

0

0

−0.1

−0.1

−0.2

−0.2

−0.3

−0.3

−0.4

−0.5 −0.5

1 2 3

0.4

−0.4

−0.4

−0.3

−0.2

−0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

(a) Without adaptation

0.4

0.5

−0.5 −0.5

−0.4

−0.3

−0.2

−0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

(b) With adaptation (τ =250 ms)

Figure 12: Scatter plots of voiced and unvoiced plosives /b/,/d/,/g/ and /p/,/t/,/k/, respectively, in three vowel contexts /a/,/ee/,/oo/ for (a) Base ZCPA and (b) ZCPA with synaptic adaptation ( τ =250 ms) in clean condition. Increased clustering is observed for vowel /ee/ in (b).

their eigenvalues are positive and the trace is equivalent to the sum of the eigenvalues [49]. Hence, these measures are useful benchmarks of separability of feature vectors for speech recognition. The computed measures of separability, J1 and J2 (unnormalized), are shown in Table 2. It is observed that with synaptic adaptation (ZCPA ADP) demonstrates higher between class separability than the base ZCPA for the three vowel classes /a/,/ee/,/oo/.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

4

Speech Recognition using Two-tone Suppression

Neural two-tone suppression is a phenomenon of the human auditory system in which there is a reduction to one tone due to the presence of another tone at a nearby frequency. Twotone suppression is a non-linear property of the cochlea and originates in the mechanical phenomena at the basilar membrane (BM) arising from an interaction between the outer hair cells and the BM [50]. Lyon [3] has proposed that although the main source of two-tone suppression is cochlear non-linearity, the nature of that non-linearity is not “saturation”. It is more like an automatic gain control in which the mechanical gain gradually reduces as a function of the response level over a very wide dynamic range of input power levels. The gain control or multichannel compression by itself improves audibility. For a probe tone located at a particular CF, and a suppressor tone with frequencies both higher and lower than the CF, the magnitude of suppression increases monotonically with suppressor intensity [50]. Moreover, the rate of growth of suppression magnitude with suppressor intensity is higher for suppressors in the region below CF than for those in the region above CF. Suppression has also been observed for a probe embedded in a band of noise [51]. The gain control or multichannel compression in the auditory system may improve audibility. However, the asymmetric amplification due to compression may result in the degradation of the spectral contrast. This degradation may be prevented by introducing two-tone suppression as observed in the peripheral auditory system. The method may be applied to the speech processors of hearing aid and cochlear implant patients to improve audibility and speech intelligently through low-power analog VLSI implementations [18]. This property

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

357

of spectral enhancement associated with two-tone suppression indicates that it may also be usefully utilized for performance enhancement in automatic speech recognition.

4.1

The Companding Architecture

A procedure to reproduce two-tone suppression in a cochlear model as observed in the biological cochlea was proposed by Kates [52] utilizing a traveling wave amplification with a cascade of adaptive-Q filter sections. However, the disadvantage of such a system is that it is not only complex, but may cause interactions between the feedback and the feedforward parameters in a traveling wave architecture. As a result, Turicchia et al. [18] proposed an alternate methodology, the companding strategy, which performed multichannel syllabic compression and spectral contrast enhancement via two-tone suppression simultaneously. Hence, the companding strategy may be effectively used in an ASR front-end for the enhancement of recognition performance. The principle of companding is based on the non-linear interactions between a compression block and an expansion block. It utilizes non-coupled filterbanks with the compressionexpansion blocks to reproduce the compressibility effects of the two-tone suppression in the biological cochlea parametrically by a set of independent and programmable parameters. The concept of companding is not new and has been applied in other fields of speech processing, particularly for improving the signal-to-noise ratio in cases where the dominant noise occurs after the compression block. It has also been applied in novel analog filtering circuits [53],[54]. Table 2: Quantitative measures of separability (unnormalized), J1 and J2, among the three vowel

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

classes /a/,/ee/ and /oo/ based on LDA of the within- and between-class scatter matrices of base ZCPA and ZCPA with adaptation.

Base ZCPA ZCPA ADP

J1 21.98 24.49

J2 112.50 120.49

The companding architecture is shown in Fig. 13, the detailed implementation of which is given in [18]. Every channel has a pre-filter, a compression block, a post-filter, and an expansion block. The two-tone suppression effect is produced due to the presence of the second filterbank between the compression and the expansion blocks, and the non-linear interaction between signals in the first filterbank, the compressor, and the second filterbank. The pre-filterbank and the post-filterbank have the same perceptual frequency scale, but the bandwidth are different for the two cases - the first filterbank is broadly tuned, while the second filterbank is sharply tuned. Both the compression block and the expansion block consist of a feed-forward path made up of an envelope detector (ED), a non-linear block and a multiplier. The envelope detector consists of a half-wave rectifier followed by a lowpass filter. The dynamics of the compression and the expansion is controlled by the time constant of the ED (τED ), which is scaled with the resonant frequency, fi , for the channel i such that τED =wτi, where w=10 and τi = 1/(2πfi). The compression and expansion characteristics are non-linear and are determined by

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

358

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

the parameters n1 and n2 , respectively as shown in Fig. 13. Particularly, the exponent n1 in the compression block determines the compression for values n1 < 1. However, the parameter n2 determines the overall compression characteristics of the system. For values of 0 < n2 < 1, the effect of the channel is to implement syllabic compression with an overall compression index of n2 . Expansion takes place in the expansion block and depends on the ratio n2 /n1 when n2 > n1 . If we represent the input by x0 given by x0 = a1 sin(ω1 t) + a2 sin(ω2t + ϕ0 )

(23)

where ω1 and ω2 are the frequencies of two tones in x0 , then the output, y0 , is given in [18] as y0 = =

h

n /n1

"



a1 2 a1

i

(a1 + f2 a2 )n2 (n1 −1)/n1 sin(ω1 t + ϕ1 + ϑ1)

a1 + f2 a2 a1

(n1 −1)/n1 #n2

sin(ω1 t + ϕ1 + ϑ1).

(24)

It is seen from Eqn. (24) that the presence of a second tone (suppressor) with amplitude a2 at a frequency ω2 suppresses the tone with amplitude a1 at frequency ω1 . If only a single tone is present such that a2 =0, then for n2 =1, the output has the amplitude of a1 only.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

4.2

Temporal Companding in the ZCPA Auditory Model

In this section, the biologically inspired companding strategy which performs simultaneous multichannel syllabic compression and spectral contrast enhancement via two-tone suppression is implemented in a ZCPA auditory model used as an ASR front-end. There are several significant differences between this implementation and the implementation by [18]. Firstly, in [18], it is implemented in the spectral domain, whereas we implemented the companding in the time domain using a zero-crossing algorithm, which we term as temporal companding. The advantages of temporal processing of speech have been stated in Sec. 1. Secondly, we propose several enhancements to the original companding model in our temporal implementation. These are (1) The suppression model proposed in [18] utilized cascaded second order biquad filter sections with 50 channels. This is not only complex, but also computationally expensive. The temporal companding was implemented using simple linear phase FIR bandpass filters with only 16 channels for computational efficiency and hardware implementation. The FIR filterbank has been explained in detail in Sec. 3.4 for the case of synaptic adaptation. (2) In the companding scheme in [18], a final summation across all the channels were taken to obtain a summary output. This might cause interference due to phase differences across channels due to adjacent channel interferences in the filterbank outputs. The companding scheme alleviated this by applying the first order lowpass filter twice for obtaining zero phase, once in the forward time direction and once in the reverse time direction. In the ZCPA, instead of taking a final summation at the output across all channels, the usual ensemble interval histogram is constructed from the zero-crossing intervals. Since the phase information is not considered in a frequency histogram construction [13], this eliminated the additional filtering required to minimize the phase effects. (3)

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition F

G

x0

ED

x1e

()

n1 1

y0

x3

x2

x1

ED

x3e

359

( ) (n2

n1 ) / n1

Expansion block

Compression block

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Figure 13: The companding architecture for a single channel for implementing the two-tone suppression effect. The parameters n1 and n2 determine the compression/expansion, that is, compression takes place for n1 < 1, and expansion for n2 > n1 (from Turicchia et al. [18]) The method also extends the capabilities of the ZCPA auditory model with the perceptual feature of two-tone suppression. Fig. 14 shows the ZCPA auditory model with the two-tone suppression using the temporal companding method. It consists of a companding stage followed by a ZCPA feature extraction stage. In the companding stage, two filterbanks are used - a compression filterbank, F , and a expansion filterbank, G, each consisting of 16 FIR filters spaced between 10-3500 Hz at the ERB frequency scale. However, the compression filterbank has bandwidths which are 20% higher than the second filterbank. In each channel, the compression block and the expansion block consisted of an envelope detector (ED) (a half-wave rectifier followed by a first order low-pass filter) and a non-linear unit depending on values of n1 and n2 , as shown in Fig. 13. The filter time constant was made inversely proportional to the centre frequency of the respective channel filter (τi = 1/(2πfi)), where i is the channel index. The half-wave rectifier, Eqn. (25), is given in [1] which combines the rectification process with the compressive non-linear saturation effects as observed in the biological cochlea. This being a tangent function has the characteristics of being exponential for negative inputs, linear for small input values and compressive for larger signals, y = 1 + A tan−1 Bx, = e

ABx

,

x>0 x≤0

(25)

where x and y are the input and output, respectively, and A=10 and B=65 are constants equivalent to gain. The half-wave rectifier removes the negative values from an input speech signal and normally can not be used for feature extraction using the zero-crossing algorithm since all zero-crossings are removed. However, when used with companding, feature extraction is still possible primarily due to the presence of the forward path after the FIR filtering, and due to the presence of the multiplier in Fig. 13. The values for the compression and the expansion parameter were n1 =0.5 and n2 =1.1, respectively. For these set of parameters, the compression applied is (n1 -1)= -0.5, as seen from the compression block in Fig. 13. An interval histogram, yt , was constructed (Eqn. (26)) from the zero-crossing intervals which were collected in 26 frequency bins spaced at the ERB frequency scale between 10-4000 Hz,

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

360

Speech

s(n)

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

80 ms Hamming window at 10 ms frame rate

F

x0

Zerocrossing detector

G

FIR filterbank

Compression block

FIR filterbank

Expansion block

y0

Zerocrossing interval histogram

ZCPA _2TONE

yt(m,b)

Peak detector Companding stage

Figure 14: The ZCPA with companding for the implementation of two-tone suppression. The features are extracted into an interval histogram by a zero-crossing detector and a peak detector (from [17])

yt (m, b) = Z{y0 } =

C DX i −1 X

log(Pik + 1)δbjk ,

1≤b1340Hz ZC(f )

FD = P

(28)

f ≤1340Hz ZC(f )

where ZC is the zero-crossing count as obtained from the interval histogram. A higher value of FD would indicate a high frequency enhancement or a low frequency suppression of a CV utterance, and vice versa. The high and low frequency discrimination was based on a frequency of 1340 Hz. The choice of this frequency was rather heuristic and was partly based on consideration that it should be higher than the first formant, which is the dominant formant of the common vowels. The comparison of CV discrimination by the two algorithms of synaptic adaptation and the two-tone suppression in clean conditions is given in Table 4.

0.5

Histogram count

Histogram count

0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 30 80

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

20

60 40

10

Histogram bin

0 0

20

Time (frame segment)

(a) Base ZCPA

0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 30 80

20

60 40

10

Histogram bin

0 0

20

Time (frame segment)

(b) ZCPA with 2-tone suppression

Figure 17: The 3-D time-frequency plots of the ZCPA histogram for (a) ZCPA and (b) ZCPA with two-tone suppressed output for the digit utterance ‘one’ in clean condition. The magnitude inhibition may be observed along the z-axis (vertical) at lower frequencies (higher histogram bins).

It is observed from Table 4 that both synaptic adaptation and two-tone suppression have a higher FD than the base ZCPA for all articulation types. Moreover, in all cases, the vowel /ee/ has a higher FD than the vowels /a/ and /oo/. This result is expected since /ee/ has higher frequency formants than /a/ and /oo/. However, a significant observation made in Table 4 is that for the vowel /ee/, the increase in FD was higher with synaptic adaptation than with two-tone suppression (3.33 vs. 2.96 for /bee/), while for vowels /a/ and /oo/, the increase in FD is higher with two-tone suppression than with synaptic adaptation (0.23 vs. 0.40 for /ba/ and 0.51 vs. 0.78 for /boo/). This result emphasizes the frequency dependence of the perceptual features in speech, with the synaptic adaptation having a greater effect on high frequency articulation, and the two-tone suppression having greater effect on lowfrequency articulation. One exception to this rule is observed for /jee/ where two-tone

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

364

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

suppression performed better than synaptic adaptation. This may be due to the fact that this CV contains substantial frication in addition to having higher formants, and the increase in FD value is mainly due to the reduction in the low frequency counts.

4.4

HMM Recognition Results

In this section, we make a comparison of the recognition performances of synaptic adaptation and two-tone suppression as word error rates using continuous density HMM and isolated digits TIdigits/NOISEX 92 speech corpus. The isolated digits were chosen over continuous digits to make a better comparison of the two algorithms of adaptation and twotone suppression for speech recognition. There were 55 male speakers in the training set, each speaker with two utterances of the digits 1-9, ‘oh’ and ‘zero’ (total 1210 utterances in the training set) and a separate set of 31 male speakers in the test set, each speaker with 2 utterances of the digits 1-9, ‘oh’ and ‘zero’ (total 682 utterances in the test set). Continuous Gaussian density HMM with 15 states per digit, 5 mixture components per state with diagonal covariances were used to define each model. A 3-state silence model was inserted at the beginning of each utterance. The Baum-Welch re-estimation using a flat-start scheme and 12 estimation iterations was used for training under clean conditions. Test speech was corrupted with 4 different types of additive noise from the NOISEX 92 database - Gaussian white noise, factory noise, babble noise and Volvo noise. In the testing phase, a left-to-right Viterbi decoder using word-level lattice network, a dictionary and a token passing algorithm with dynamic programming was used for decoding.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

4.4.1

Comparison of Recognition Performances of the Base Models - ZCPA, MFCC and PLP

We first make a comparison of the effectiveness of the base ZCPA (without the perceptual features added) auditory front-end in a speech recognition task with the standard MFCC (with the zeroth coefficient) and the more perceptual PLP. Table 5 shows the word error rates as percentage recognition of these three features in clean condition and two noise types - stationary Gaussian white noise, and non-stationary real-world factory noise. For each type of parameter, 13 cepstra were generated per frame at a frame rate of 10 ms and a sampling frequency of 20 kHz. The dynamic (delta-delta) features were not used for the following reason. It was reported that the ZCPA algorithm do not perform well with deltadelta coefficients and the contribution of dynamic features of the EIH’s to performance improvements is much smaller than that of MFCC [19]. This may be due to the fact that the length of the time-window is channel dependent in the EIH’s i.e., it varies inversely with the characteristic frequency of the channel. For example, the length of the time-window at the channel with the lowest characteristic frequency spans upto 80 ms, which is much longer length when compared with the frame rate of about 10 ms. Thus appropriate dynamic features cannot be obtained in the ZCPA with the derivative window of 80 ms duration [13]. Therefore, for a fair comparison with all parameters, these are compared without the dynamic coefficients. Table 5 shows that the MFCC and PLP performs better than ZCPA in clean conditions, but ZCPA performs better than the MFCC or the PLP in white noise conditions as the noise

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

365

Table 4: Comparison of high and low frequency discrimination (FD) of CV utterances with synaptic adaptation and two-tone suppression in the ZCPA auditory model.

/ba/ /bee/ /boo/ /pa/ /pee/ /poo/ /ma/ /mee/ /moo/

Base ZCPA 0.22 2.82 0.46 0.28 2.31 0.45 0.33 1.92 0.43

FD ZCPA ADP 0.23 3.33 0.51 0.30 2.79 0.50 0.36 2.32 0.47

ZCPA 2TONE 0.40 2.96 0.78 0.46 2.53 0.74 0.63 2.10 0.73

/SHa/ /SHee/ /SHoo/ /ja/ /jee/ /joo/

Base ZCPA 0.58 2.89 1.97 0.56 2.51 1.45

FD ZCPA ADP 0.65 3.43 2.28 0.62 2.98 1.64

ZCPA 2TONE 0.95 3.41 2.56 0.86 3.19 1.96

increases, which is in agreement with the results of [13]. However, in factory noise, the ZCPA performs better than the MFCC only at very low SNR (0 dB). 4.4.2

Comparison of Recognition Performances of ZCPA with Synaptic Adaptation and with Two-Tone Suppression

Table 6 shows the word recognition results of the base ZCPA, ZCPA with adaptation (ZCPA ADP) and ZCPA with two-tone suppression (ZCPA 2TONE). The feature extraction with adaptation were described in detail in Sec. 3.4 and in Sec. 4.2 for two-tone suppression with companding.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Table 5: Comparisons of HMM recognition rates (%) of the base ZCPA, MFCC and PLP for isolated digits (TIdigits corpus) with male speakers.

SNR (dB) Clean 30 15 10 5 0

ZCPA

White MFCC 0

PLP

ZCPA

Factory MFC 0

PLP

95.4 81.8 77.3 68.2 50.0 40.9

100.0 81.8 21.0 18.2 11.1 9.1

100.0 81.8 27.3 21.2 13.6 9.1

86.3 81.8 68.2 50.0 45.4

100.0 95.4 86.3 54.5 27.3

100.0 95.4 86.3 81.5 59.1

It is observed from Table 6 that the performance in clean conditions is improved with synaptic adaptation and with two-tone suppression over the base ZCPA. In noise conditions, it follows the same trend as given by [13]. That is, the best performance is achieved in Volvo noise, followed by factory noise, white noise and babble noise. Thus the response depends on the type of noise, with better performance in real-world noise environments. Real-world non-Gaussian noise contain significant low frequency components and are characterized by the absence of high frequency noise perturbations which usually corrupt the lower intensity articulatory cues. Moreover due to the dominant frequency principle, the formants are

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

366

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

Table 6: Percentage word recognition rates with continuous density HMM with isolated digits

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

(TIdigits) of the base ZCPA, ZCPA with synaptic adaptation and ZCPA with two-tone suppression. SNR (dB)

ZCPA

White ZCPA ADP

ZCPA 2TONE

Clean 40 30 15 10 5

95.4 90.9 81.8 77.3 68.2 50.0

95.4 95.4 90.9 72.7 59.1 31.8

95.4 95.4 54.5 50.0 45.4 40.9

ZCPA

Factory ZCPA ADP

ZCPA 2TONE

95.4 86.3 81.8 68.8 50.0

90.9 81.8 77.3 72.7 68.8

95.4 95.4 90.9 86.3 72.7

preserved in presence of low frequency noise perturbations. A similar trend is observed with synaptic adaptation and two-tone suppression, with the best performance observed with Volvo noise and the worst performance with babble noise. However, with synaptic adaptation, improvements are observed over the base ZCPA only in white Gaussian noise at high SNRs, with degradations observed at low SNRs. With Factory noise, a reciprocal effect, that is, a degradation at high SNRs with improvement at low SNRs, is observed. In both babble and Volvo noise, there is a degradation particularly at low SNRs. In general, synaptic adaptation performed better over the base ZCPA in stationary Gaussian white noise (except at very low SNRs), but performed poorly in non-Gaussian noise. With two-tone suppression, the recognition results are improved over both the base ZCPA and with synaptic adaptation under all noise types and at all SNRs, except for white Gaussian noise at low SNRs. A significant observation made from the Table 6 is that synaptic adaptation performs better than two-tone suppression in stationary white Gaussian noise and worse in non-stationary non-Gaussian noise (factory and babble), whereas an opposite effect is observed with two-tone suppression. That is, two-tone suppression performs better in non-Gaussian real-world noise (factory, babble, Volvo) and worse in Gaussian white noise. However, at very low SNR Gaussian noise, there is a degradation in recognition rates with both synaptic adaptation and the two-tone suppression. This is due to three reasons. Firstly, if the unit sample response, h(n), of the FIR filter is finite in length and zero outside the interval [0,N -1], then the variance (power) of the output uncorrelated white noise, ϕ=[ϕ(0), ϕ(1), . . . , ϕ(N − 1)], with zero mean at the output of the i-th filter can be expressed as n

o

σϕ2 i = E |ϕi (n)|2 = hH Rϕ h = hH Cϕ h = Cϕ h2

(29)

where h is the vector of filter coefficients, Rϕ is noise autocorrelation matrix and Cϕ is autocovariance matrix whose diagonals are simply the variances of the input noise process. Thus, it is observed that the variance of the noise ϕ(n) at the output of the FIR filter is obtained by multiplying the input noise variance by the square of the FIR filter coefficient vector, h. Thus, for a large order FIR filter, output noise variances may be significantly large. Secondly, high-pass filtering of white noise is equivalent to differentiating it, by which higher frequencies get more power. Hence, the high pass adaptation filtering further

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

367

increases the zero-crossing perturbation and decreases low frequency formant contrasts. Thirdly, it is observed from Eqn. (14), that the zero-crossing variance is directly proportional to the square of the noise amplitudes,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

σl2

A2ϕ . A2l

(30)

With synaptic adaptation in non-stationary factory noise, the improvements were observed to be opposite to that with white noise with a degradation at high SNRs, which is consistent with the results in [13]. This may be due to the fact that larger window lengths would give poor estimates in time domain processing in the presence of non-stationary noise. For babble noise, the results were similar to the non-stationary factory noise, while for the pseudo-stationary Volvo (car) noise, the results were similar to white noise, but with improved response at lower SNRs due to the presence of significant low frequency components. The fact that the performance with two-tone suppression in white noise was worse than with synaptic adaptation was due to the increased variance of the noise ϕ(n) at the output of the FIR filter obtained by multiplying the noise variance by the square of the FIR filter coefficient vector, h, as explained above. Since the companding utilizes two FIR filterbanks, a pre-filter and a post-filter, the noise variance is substantially increased over the base ZCPA and ZCPA ADP. It is usually important in speech recognition tests to perform a statistical significance test to establish that the differences in error-rates between two algorithms tested on the same data set are statistically significant or not. The McNemar’s test [55] can be used for this purpose, which requires the errors made by an algorithm to be independent events. This is particularly applicable for isolated word recognition, that is, when the uttered words are not context dependent or dependent on some language model. The recognition results in Table 5 and Table 6 were further tested using the McNemar’s two-tail test relating to the 682 test utterances and it was found that the differences in recognition results due to the three algorithms (base ZCPA, ZCPA with synaptic adaptation and ZCPA with two-tone suppression) were statistically significant 90% in white noise, 60% in factory noise, 80% in babble noise and 70% in Volvo noise for a significance level of α=0.03.

5

Conclusion

It has been reported in [13], that although the base ZCPA performs better than the MFCC in noisy conditions, the relative performance improvement for the ZCPA over the MFCC is greater in white Gaussian noise than in real-world noise, such as the factory noise. It is stated therein that the reason for this is not clear. It is shown in this paper that one reason for this may be that in the ZCPA, the zero-crossing variance is reduced more for the high frequency white noise than the lower frequency real-world (factory, car, babble) noise. Therefore, although overall the ZCPA performs better in real-world noise than in white noise, the relative improvement in performance compared to the MFCC is higher in white noise than in non-Gaussian real-world noise.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

368

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

In Sec. 4.3, it is observed that synaptic adaptation has a greater effect on high frequency articulation, whereas two-tone suppression has a greater effect on low-frequency articulation. Moreover, in Table 6, it is observed that synaptic adaptation performs better in white Gaussian noise while two-tone suppression performs better in non-Gaussian realworld noise. This effect may be observed from Eqn. (13) and Eqn. (14), in which it is seen that the zero-crossing variance is reduced for the high frequency noise components and the corresponding higher zero-crossing rates, respectively. The high pass synaptic adaptation filter further enhances the high frequency components which further reduces the variance. For the case of two-tone suppression, the performance improvement is mainly due to improvement of spectral contrasts due to companding utilizing low-pass filters. Hence, twotone suppression is a low frequency enhancement technique, whereas adaptation scheme is a high frequency enhancement technique. Consequently, with a zero-crossing algorithm, the performance with two-tone suppression is expected to be reciprocal to that obtained for the synaptic adaptation, with improved response in low-frequency real-world noise due to reduced zero-crossing variance. Another reason of improved performance is that the higher energy low frequency peak values are emphasized in the two-tone suppression. These observations are consistent with the results in Table 6. In summary, these results demonstrate the frequency dependence of the perceptual features in speech recognition utilizing temporal methods and using a zero-crossing algorithm. The synaptic adaptation has a greater effect on high frequency articulation and the two-tone suppression has a greater effect on lowfrequency articulation. In noise conditions, it is observed that synaptic adaptation is more immune to white Gaussian noise than two-tone suppression while two-tone suppression is more immune to the non-Gaussian, real-world noise than synaptic adaptation. Additionally, these results may be applied to speech processing, particularly in cochlear implants and hearing aids for improved intelligibility. In this research, a classical frame based method embedded in a hidden Markov model (HMM) technique was used to evaluate the effects of perceptual features on speech recognition performance. The classical frame-based framework (typically 25 ms) is based on the assumption that the features are stationary (or quasi stationary) within the frame window duration. However, time constants associated with perceptual features are several order higher than this duration. There may be some model-feature mismatch between the frame-based approach and auditory events like rapid adaptation, and also due to the application of wider window lengths (30 ms to 120 ms) in perceptual processing with some loss of assumption of stationarity. As a result, a frame-based approach for auditory processing using HMMs has always been a compromise. Nevertheless, frame-based HMM technique for ASR in auditory models have been shown to provide improved performances as demonstrated by several researchers [8],[12],[13],[14], and additionally in this research. The following issues may be addressed in any future research. It was observed that in the ZCPA the application of perceptual features, such as synaptic adaptation and two-tone suppression, degraded the performance over the baseline performance in white noise at very low SNRs. The reason for this degradation was explained in Sec 4.4.2. One way the performance in white noise may be improved is by adjusting the FIR filter order for a desired filter response. For example, in the minimum variance method of spectrum estimation of a stochastic process, the noise power spectrum estimate at the filter output decreases as the filter order increases [43]. Further research may be undertaken in this direction. Proper in-

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

369

tegration of dynamic features (delta-delta) features with channel dependent variable derivative window lengths may also be explored. Effects of frame synchronous processing on forward and backward masking related to the adaptation and other perceptual features of the human auditory system may be further investigated.

References [1] Seneff, S. (1988). A joint synchrony/mean-rate model of auditory processing. J. Phonetics, 85(1), 55-76. [2] Ghitza, O. (1988). Auditory models and human performance in tasks related to speech coding and speech recognition. IEEE Trans. Speech Audio Process., 2(1), 115-132. [3] Lyon, R.F. & Mead, C. (1988). An analog electronic cochlea. IEEE Trans. Acoust. Speech Signal Process., 36(7), 1119-1134. [4] Zhang, X., Heinz, M. G. & Carney, L. H. (1988). Nonlinear compression in an auditorynerve model. Proc. of The First Joint BMES/EMBS Conf. Serving Humanity, Advancing Technology, Atlanta, Oct. 1999, 13-16.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

[5] Baer, T., Moore, B. C. J. & Gatehouse, S. (1993). Spectral contrast enhancement of speech in noise for listeners with sensorineural hearing impairment: Effects on intelligibility, quality, and response times. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev., 3(1), 49-72. [6] Grayden, D. B., Burkitt, A. N., Kenny, O. P., Clarey, Paolini, J. N. A. G. & Clark, G. M. (2004). A cochlear implant speech processing strategy based on an auditory model. Int. Conf. on Intelligent Sensors, Sensor Networks and Information Processing (ISSNIP 2004), Melbourne, 491-496. [7] Hunt, M. J. & Lefebvre, C. (1986). Speech recognition using a cochlear model. IEEE Int. Conf. Acoust. Speech, Signal Processing (ICASSP 1986) , Tokyo. [8] Cohen, J. R. (1989). Application of an auditory model to speech recognition. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 85(6), 2623-2629. [9] Zwicker, E., Terhardt, E. & Poulus, E. (1979). Automatic speech recognition using psychoacoustic models. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 65(2). [10] Searle, C. L., Jacobson, J. & Rayment, S. G. (1979). Stop consonant discrimination based on human audition. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 65, 799-809. [11] Bloomberg, M., Carlson, R., Elenius, K. & Granstrom, B. (1984). Auditory models and isolated word recognition. Q Prog. Stat. Rep., Speech Transmiss. Lab. (Royal Institute of Technology, Stockholm), 1-15. [12] Strope, B. & Alwan, A. (1997). A model of dynamic auditory perception and its application to robust word recognition. IEEE Trans. Speech Audio Process., 95(5), 451-464. Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

370

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

[13] Kim, D. S., Lee, S. Y. & Kil, R. M. (1999). Auditory processing of speech signals for robust speech recognition in real world noisy environments. IEEE Trans. Speech and Audio Process., 7(1), 55-69. [14] Holmberg, M., Gelbart, D. & Hemmert, W. (2006). Automatic Speech Recognition with an adaptation model motivated by auditory processing. IEEE Trans. Audio, Speech, Lang. Process., 14(1), 44-49. [15] Haque, S., Togneri, R. & Zaknich, A. (2007). A temporal auditory model with adaptation for automatic speech recognition. Proc. IEEE Int. Conf. on Acoust. Speech, and Signal Processing (ICASSP 2007) . [16] Haque, S. Togneri, R. & Zaknich, A. (2006). Zero-Crossings with adaptation for automatic speech recognition. Proc. The Eleventh Australasian International Conference on Speech Science and Technology (SST 2006), 199-204, Dec. 6-8, 2006, Auckland. [17] Haque, S., Togneri, R. & Zaknich, A. (2009). Perceptual features for automatic speech recognition in noisy environments. Speech Communication., 51(1), 58-75. [18] Turicchia, L. & Sarpeshkar R. (2005). A bio-inspired companding strategy for spectral enhancement. IEEE Trans. Speech Audio Process., 13(2), 243-253.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

[19] Sandhu, S. & Ghitza, O. (1995). A comparative study of mel cepstra and EIH for phone classification under adverse conditions. Proc. IEEE Int. Conf. on Acoust. Speech, and Signal Processing (ICASSP 1995). [20] Abdelatty, A. M., Spiegel, J. V., Mueller, P., Haentjens, G. & Berman, J. (1999). An acoustic-phonetic feature-based system for automatic phoneme recognition in continuous speech. Proc. IEEE Int. Conf. on Acoust. Speech, and Signal Processing (ICASSP 1999). [21] Gaji´c, B. & Paliwal, K. K. (2003). Robust speech recognition using features based on zero-crossings with peak amplitudes. Proc. IEEE Int. Conf. on Acoust. Speech, and Signal Processing (ICASSP 2003). [22] Markel, J. D. Gray, A. H. Linear Prediction of Speech. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 1976. [23] Bogert, B. P., Healy, M. J. & Tukey, J. W. Title: The quefrency analysis of time series for echoes: cepstrum, pseudo-autocovariance, cross-cepstrum, and shape tracking. In: Rosenblatt, M. editor. Title: Time Series Analysis. New York: John Wiley; 1963; 209243. [24] Loizou, P. C. (2006). Coclear and Brainstem Implants: Speech processing in vocodercentric cochlear implants. In: A. Moeller editor. Adv. Otorhinolaryngol. vol 64; 2006; 109-143. [25] Pitton, J. W., Wang K. & Juang, B. (1996). Time-Frequency analysis and auditory modeling for automatic recognition of speech. Proc. IEEE, 84(9), 1199-1215. Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Perceptual Features for Robust Speech Recognition

371

[26] Ghulam, M., Fukuda, T., Horikawa, J. & Nitta, T. (2005). Pitch-synchronous ZCPA (PS-ZCPA)-based feature extraction with auditory masking. Proc. IEEE Int. Conf. on Acoust. Speech, and Signal Processing (ICASSP 2005). [27] Zwicker, E., Flottorp, G. & Stevens, S. S. (1957). Critical bandwidth in loudness summation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 29, 548-557. [28] Hermansky, H. (1990). Perceptual linear Prediction (PLP) analysis of speech. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., vol. 87, 1738-1752. [29] Hermansky H. & Morgan, N. (1994). RASTA processing of speech. IEEE Trans. Speech Audio Process., 2, 587-589. [30] Deng, L. & O’Shaughnessy, D. Speech Processing - A Dynamic and OptimizationOriented Approach. New York: Marcel Dekker; 2003. [31] Sachs, M. B., Voigt, H. F. & Young, E. D. (1983). Auditory nerve representation of vowels in background noise. J. Neurophysiology. 50(1). [32] Loughlin, P., Groutage, D. & Rohrbaugh, R. (1997). Time-frequency analysis of acoustic transients. Proc. IEEE Int. Conf. on Acoust. Speech, and Signal Processing (ICASSP 1997). [33] Kedem, B. (1986). Spectral analysis and discrimination by zero-crossings. Proc. IEEE, 74(11), 1477-1493.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

[34] Hermansky, H. (1994). Speech Beyond 10 ms (Temporal Filtering in Feature Domain). Proc. Int. Workshop on Human Interface Technology, Aizu, Japan. [35] Patterson, R. D. (1999). Time-interval information in the auditory representation of speech sounds. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 105(2), 1305. [36] Pickles, J. O. An Introduction to the Physiology of Hearing, London: Academic; 1988. [37] Smith, R. & Zwislocki, J. J. (1975). Short-term adaptation and incremental responses of single auditory-nerve fibres. Biological Cybernetics, 17, 169-182. [38] Westerman, L. & Smith, R. L. (1984). Rapid and short-term adaptation in auditory nerve responses. Hearing Research, 15, 249-260. [39] Meddis, R. (1988). Simulation of mechanical to neural transduction in the auditory receptor. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 79(3), 702-711. [40] Jankowski Jr., C. R., Vo, H. H. & Lippman, R. P. (1995). A comparison of signal processing front ends for automatic word recognition. IEEE Trans. Speech Audio Process., 3, 286-293. [41] Koopmans, L. H. The Spectral Analysis of Time Series. New York: Academic Press; 1995. Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

372

Serajul Haque and Roberto Togneri

[42] Oxenham, A. J. (2001). Forward masking: adaptation or integration? J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 109, 732-741. [43] Hayes, M. H. Statistical Digital Signal Processing and Modeling. New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.; 1996. [44] Gunawan, T. S. & Ambikairajah, E. (2006). On the use of simultaneous and temporal masking in noise suppression applications. Proc. of 11th Int. Conf. Speech Science and Technology (SST 2006), Auckland. [45] Spoor, A., Eggermont, J. J. & Odenthal, D. W. Title: Comparison of human and animal data concerning adaptation and masking of eighth nerve compound action potential. In: Ruber, J. Elberling, C. & Solomon, G., editors, Title: Electrocochleography. Baltimore, University Park, MD; 1976; 183-198. [46] Zwicker, E. & Fastl, H. Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models. 2nd Edition. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1990. [47] Tchorz, J. & Kollmeier, B. (1999). A model of auditory perception as front-end for automatic speech recognition. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 106(4). [48] Moore, B. C. J. & Glasberg, B. R. (1983). Suggested formulae for calculating auditory-filter bandwidths and excitation patterns. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 74(3), 750-753. [49] Theodoridis, S. & Koutroumbas, K. Pattern recognition. San Diego: Elsevier; 1999.

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

[50] Ruggero, M. A., Narayan, S. S., Temchin, A. N. & Recio, A. (2000). Mechanical bases of frequency tuning and neural excitation at the base of the cochlea. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci., USA, vol., 97, 11744-11750. [51] Rhode, W. S. (1978). Some observations on cochlear mechanics. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 64, no. 1, 158-176. [52] Kates, J. M. (1995). Two-tone suppression in a cochlear model. IEEE Trans. Speech Audio Process., 3(5), 396-406. [53] Dolby, R. (1967). An audio noise reduction system. J. Audio Eng. Soc., 15(4). [54] Frey D., Tsividis, Y., Efthivoulidis, G. & Krishnapura, N. (2001). Syllabic- Companding log domain filters. IEEE Trans. Circuits Syst. II., 48(4), 329-339. [55] Gillick, L. & Cox, S.J. (1989). Some statistical issues in the comparison of speech recognition algorithms. Proc. IEEE Int. Conf. on Acoust. Speech, and Signal Processing (ICASSP 1989).

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

In: Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System Editors: D. Fiedler and R. Krause, pp.373

ISBN: 978-1-60741-259-5 ©2010 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

COMMENTARY: ACUPUNCTURE IN THE TREATMENT OF EAR DISORDERS Wong-Kein Low

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Department of Otolaryngology, Singapore General Hospital, Singapore

As part of Traditional Chinese Medicine (TCM), acupuncture is one of the oldest healing practices in the world that can be traced back at least 2,500 years. The general theory of acupuncture is based on the premise that there are patterns of energy flow (qi) through the body that are essential for health. Health is achieved through balancing the forces of yin and yang, and disease is caused by an imbalance leading to a blockage in the flow of qi along specific pathways known as meridians. Acupuncture applied at specific acupoints along the meridians, has the potential to correct imbalances of the flow of qi. There have been many studies which had attempted to find out if acupuncture is effective in treating a wide range of conditions. The 1997 NIH Consensus Statement on Acupuncture concluded that overall, the results were difficult to interpret because of problems with the size and design of the studies. By targeting the relevant acupoints on the meridians associated with the ear, acupuncture has been used to treat ear-related symptoms such as hearing loss, vertigo and tinnitus. A meridian which is of particular interest, is that of the kidney. According to TCM, the kidney opens into the ear, and kidney disorders can manifest in the ear. It can be argued that this is not logical, given that the ear and the kidney are located at almost opposite ends of the torso! Let us pause and ponder. The kidney and ear lobe share a similar shape, but probably by chance. It is, however, no coincidence that the ear and kidney have certain associations in Western Medicine. Medications such as gentamicin have toxic side effects, which specifically target the inner ear and kidney. Some congenital malformations of the ear, for example, the branchio-oto-renal syndrome, also affect the kidney. These observations may suggest an embryological relationship between the two organ systems. The TCM concept of using acupuncture on the kidney meridian as a means to treat certain ear conditions, may not be that far-fetched after all. It remains to be seen if the use of acupuncture in the treatment of ear disorders can be supported by solid well-designed research. Although it is unrealistic to expect it to work in established hearing defects due to cochlear hair-cell loss, it will be interesting to find out if it is of value in treating potentially reversible conditions such as early sudden hearing loss and Meniere‘s disease.

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved. Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

INDEX

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

A ablations, 30 abnormalities, ix, 139, 159, 160, 164, 165, 167, 169, 170, 172, 173, 178, 182, 183, 187, 189, 219, 292 acclimatization, 311 acetylcholine, 32, 146 acid, 1, 2, 37, 74, 76, 153, 198, 199, 216, 218, 321 acidic, 179 acidification, 317 ACM, 332 acoustic, 3, 12, 14, 29, 36, 37, 40, 46, 50, 59, 63, 64, 65, 67, 69, 83, 94, 106, 107, 118, 119, 132, 163, 167, 182, 236, 251, 254, 260, 264, 277, 292, 305, 334, 336, 339, 370, 371 acoustic emission, 107 acoustic neuroma, 118, 132, 182 acoustical, 264, 265 actin, viii, 137, 140, 144, 154, 155, 209, 210 action potential, 31, 33, 38, 60, 82, 372 action potential (AP), 31, 38 activation, xiv, 22, 25, 26, 62, 65, 120, 121, 123, 124, 125, 126, 127, 133, 134, 138, 184, 188, 192, 284, 286, 302, 314, 315, 316, 319, 321, 327, 328, 334 activity level, 325, 326 acupuncture, xv, 373 acute, 126, 130, 131, 152, 154, 162, 183, 280, 283 acute infection, 126 adducts, 314, 315, 321 adenosine, 319 adenovirus, 213, 222, 224 adherens junction, 212, 217, 221, 224 adhesion, 142, 156, 213, 223, 224, 284, 288

adhesions, 146 adipose tissue, 143 adolescents, viii, 81, 84, 85, 86, 87, 88, 89, 90, 93, 94, 95, 96, 97, 103, 104, 105, 106, 107, 109, 110, 127 ADP, 349, 356, 357, 365, 366, 367 adult population, 106, 182 adults, viii, 81, 84, 85, 87, 89, 105, 106, 107, 108, 109, 110, 127, 129, 131, 177, 190, 257, 261, 281, 310, 311 AEP, 302 afferent nerve, 82, 338 afferent nerve fiber, 82 Ag, 284 AGC, 340 aging, 120 aid, 250, 302, 303, 304, 308, 311, 357 air, 120, 134, 146, 264 airports, 234, 236 albumin, 127 alcohol, 96 algorithm, xv, 269, 325, 326, 328, 329, 334, 337, 338, 340, 342, 345, 350, 354, 358, 359, 360, 361, 364, 367, 368 allele, 209, 219, 292, 294, 297, 300 alleles, 216, 220, 292, 294, 295, 296, 298 allergens, 289 allergy, 283, 286, 289 alters, 66, 76, 161, 207 ambiguity, 6 American Academy of Pediatrics, 250, 253, 259, 260 amine, 8, 16 amino, 1, 37, 144, 153, 198, 199, 213, 214, 215, 218 amino acid, 153, 198, 199, 214, 215, 218 aminoglycosides, xiii, 120, 313, 314

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

376 amniotic fluid, 251 AMPA, 1, 31, 37, 43 amplitude, 31, 36, 66, 68, 71, 84, 169, 170, 171, 178, 185, 250, 251, 265, 302, 343, 345, 358, 360, 361 ampulla, 143, 203 amygdala, 184 anaesthesia, 40 anatomy, vii, 1, 6, 76, 142, 191 animal models, ix, x, 124, 144, 147, 159, 163, 164, 211, 213, 215, 217, 221, 250, 262 animal studies, 122 animals, 3, 4, 5, 12, 21, 29, 36, 57, 114, 117, 122, 139, 140, 141, 142, 161, 163, 172, 173, 188, 217, 218, 315, 316, 347 ANOVA, 121 antagonist, 37, 43, 161, 174 anti-apoptotic, 124, 319 antibiotics, 292 antibodies, 151, 152, 153, 154 antibody, 139, 141, 144, 145, 146, 148, 152, 153, 156, 172, 220, 282, 288 anticoagulant, 282 anticoagulants, xii, 279, 281 anticoagulation, 287 antigen, 138, 139, 140, 141, 142, 143, 144, 145, 146, 147, 150, 151, 152, 155, 156, 206, 284 antihistamines, 138 anti-inflammatory drugs, 283 antineoplastic, 134, 314, 319 antioxidant, 83, 120, 319, 321 antiphospholipid, 285 antiphospholipid antibodies, 288 antiphospholipid syndrome, 286 antitumor, 314 APA, 288 apoptosis, viii, xiv, 83, 106, 111, 120, 121, 122, 123, 124, 125, 127, 129, 133, 134, 161, 314, 315, 316, 319, 320, 321 apoptotic, viii, xiv, 111, 120, 121, 122, 123, 124, 125, 126, 129, 314, 315, 316, 319, 320, 321 apoptotic effect, 315 application, xii, xiv, 120, 138, 153, 185, 186, 188, 199, 208, 263, 269, 270, 272, 275, 323, 331, 350, 351, 368 AR modeling, 334 arginine, 216 arthritis, xii, 151, 279, 284 articulation, 354, 355, 363, 368 artificial intelligence, xiv, 333, 334 Ashkenazi Jews, 292, 297, 299, 300 aspartate, 31

Index asphyxia, vii, xi, 162, 164, 169, 170, 174, 175, 176, 177, 178, 249, 250, 251, 252, 253, 254, 255, 256, 257, 258, 259, 260, 261, 262 aspirin, 126 assessment, 92, 104, 109, 163, 187, 190, 222, 233, 234, 236, 237, 238, 242, 244, 247, 262, 280, 287, 302, 304 assessment tools, 287 assumptions, 338 asthma, 285 astrocyte, 154 asymmetry, 222, 302 atherosclerosis, 141, 152 ATPase, 214, 223, 315, 321 atrophy, 113, 114, 117, 139, 315 attribution, 309 audiology, 261 audition, 118, 369 auditory cortex, ix, 4, 5, 115, 116, 181, 183, 184, 186, 191, 192, 193, 194, 302, 337 auditory evoked potential, xiii, 174, 177, 178, 301, 302, 310 auditory evoked potentials, xiii, 174, 178, 301, 310 auditory hallucinations, 182, 186, 187, 189, 193, 194 auditory nerve, xiv, xv, 3, 33, 38, 64, 70, 74, 75, 117, 179, 183, 262, 333, 335, 339, 344, 348 auditory stimuli, 35 Aurora, 340, 348 autoantibodies, 144, 147, 149, 151, 152, 154, 156, 157, 284, 287, 289 autoantigens, viii, 137 autocorrelation, 366 autoimmune, xii, xiii, 138, 139, 141, 143, 149, 151, 152, 153, 154, 155, 156, 157, 279, 280, 281, 283, 284, 285, 286, 288, 289 autoimmune disease, xii, 139, 149, 152, 154, 279, 281, 283, 284, 288 autoimmune diseases, xii, 139, 149, 279, 283, 284 autoimmune disorders, xii, 279, 283, 284, 285 autoimmune hepatitis, 141 autoimmunity, 150, 151, 280, 281, 283, 284 autoreactive T cells, 147, 285 autosomal dominant, 145, 146, 155, 156, 200, 206, 207, 208, 210, 292, 293 autosomal recessive, 156, 201, 206, 208, 210, 215, 216, 220, 224, 292, 293, 295 axid, 25 axon terminals, 75 axonal, 9, 17, 76, 161, 163, 165, 172, 174, 178, 180

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Index axons, 3, 4, 9, 10, 30, 31, 32, 34, 35, 47, 50, 53, 55, 56, 67, 74, 75, 116, 161, 165

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

B B cell, 42, 44, 285 babies, 175, 176, 250, 260, 262 background noise, xiii, 89, 95, 301, 309, 337, 371 bacterial, 253, 284 bandwidth, 49, 50, 343, 344, 349, 357, 360, 371 Bangladesh, 292 barrier, x, 127, 211, 212, 213, 214, 217, 218, 219, 221, 222, 223, 224 barriers, 128, 217, 218, 228, 232, 242 base pair, 314 basement membrane, 139, 143, 150, 151 basilar membrane, xv, 82, 113, 139, 144, 146, 150, 205, 250, 264, 277, 334, 336, 342, 356 batteries, 287 battery, 88, 149 Bax, 123, 124, 161 bcl-2, 315 Bcl-2, 120, 123, 161, 319 Bcl-2 proteins, 123 behavior, 86, 95, 96, 97, 103, 110, 228 behavioral change, 97, 104 benchmark, 20, 29, 356 beneficial effect, ix, 181 benefits, 138, 189, 228, 232, 242, 243, 311 benign, 287 bias, 233, 234, 238, 242, 351 bifurcation, 17 bilirubin, 176 binding, viii, xiv, 66, 82, 137, 140, 142, 148, 156, 206, 209, 220, 225, 285, 286, 288, 313, 314, 317, 318, 319 bioinformatics, 199 biomolecules, xiv, 313, 317, 318 biopsy, 284 biosynthesis, 146, 296 birth, xi, 172, 174, 176, 179, 198, 217, 249, 250, 251, 252, 253, 254, 255, 256, 257, 258, 259, 260, 292 birth weight, 179, 260 birthweight, 178, 179 blocks, 357 blood, 83, 122, 127, 139, 148, 163, 182, 184, 190, 251, 281, 282, 283, 285, 286, 294, 296 blood flow, 83, 286 blood transfusion, 127 blood vessels, 122, 139, 182 blot, 152, 161 body weight, 143

377 BOLD, 184, 190 bolus, 29 bone marrow, 314, 319 boutons, 31, 34 bovine, 139, 141, 148, 155 brain activity, 184 brain damage, 160, 162, 178, 179, 262 brain development, 175 brain injury, 154, 177, 179, 261 brain stem, 3, 74, 76, 115, 131, 145, 177, 178, 261 brain structure, 179 brain tumor, 133 brainstem nuclei, 21, 25, 26, 29, 78, 115 branching, 13, 15, 16, 49, 212 broadband, 43, 49, 65, 66, 305, 307 bronchopulmonary dysplasia, ix, 159, 160, 162, 174, 177, 178, 179, 180

C Ca2+, 56, 315, 321 calcium, viii, 56, 77, 82, 137, 142, 219, 321 calcium carbonate, 219 calibration, 318 calmodulin, 206 calyx, 7, 9, 33, 34, 35, 44, 50, 53, 56, 67 campaigns, viii, 81, 96, 97, 104 cancer, 154 candidates, 186 capillary, 139, 169, 281, 286 caps, 229, 230, 231 CAR, 213, 214, 221 carcinoma, 113, 114, 119, 128, 129, 130, 131, 132, 143 cardiac activity, 95 carrier, 146, 292, 295, 296, 297, 298, 299, 300 case study, 302 caspase, xiv, 120, 123, 124, 125, 126, 134, 208, 314, 315, 316, 319, 321 caspase-dependent, 125, 134 cataract, 131 cation, 217, 369 cats, 9, 10, 12, 14, 16, 17, 24, 29, 32, 34, 40, 75 Caucasian population, 206, 292, 296, 296, 297 cauda equina, 116 causation, 280 CD8+, 145 cDNA, 140, 142, 216 cell adhesion, 156, 223 cell body, 17 cell cycle, 123, 127

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

378 cell death, xiv, 82, 83, 120, 122, 123, 124, 125, 126, 132, 133, 134, 172, 314, 315, 320 cell division, 142, 319 cell fate, 123, 319 cell line, 120, 124, 134, 135, 142, 154, 200, 202, 218 cell membranes, 142 cell surface, 222, 284 cellular immunity, 284 central nervous system, 116, 120, 128, 131, 135, 143, 172, 174, 176, 218, 251 cerebellum, 144 cerebral blood flow, 184, 190 cerebral cortex, ix, 64, 159, 162, 174, 179, 183, 184 cerebral hypoxia, 179 cerebral metabolism, 188 cerebral palsy, 174, 179, 262 cerebrospinal fluid, 214 cerebrovascular, 283 cerebrum, 144 c-fos, 19, 24, 25, 76 channel interference, 358 channels, xiv, 56, 64, 82, 208, 214, 333, 339, 343, 350, 352, 353, 358 chemotherapy, 128, 129, 130, 134, 135, 319 childhood, xiii, 295, 299, 301, 302 children, xi, 85, 101, 106, 107, 108, 127, 131, 135, 174, 175, 176, 249, 261, 297, 301, 310 Chi-square, 251 cholinergic, 32, 74, 146 cholinergic neurons, 32 chopping, 45, 49, 56 chromosome, 142, 145, 206, 215, 220, 225 chromosomes, 216, 296, 298 chronic pain, 191 cis, xiii, 313, 314, 320 cisplatin, viii, xiii, xiv, 111, 112, 127, 128, 130, 131, 133, 134, 135, 313, 314, 315, 316, 317, 318, 319, 320, 321 c-jun, 121 classes, xiii, 15, 16, 29, 41, 43, 46, 49, 62, 199, 280, 287, 354, 355, 356, 357 classical, 6, 11, 90, 116, 183, 368 classification, 6, 14, 38, 43, 71, 236, 326, 329, 354, 370 clinical assessment, 182, 310 clinical presentation, xii, 279, 281, 285 clinical trial, 232, 237, 243, 244, 246 clinical trials, 232, 243, 244, 246 cloning, 155, 218 clustering, 8, 12, 352, 354, 355, 356 clusters, 6, 24, 25, 26, 27, 64, 65, 355

Index CNS, 161, 177, 218, 219, 224, 225 cochlear implant, viii, xiii, xv, 111, 112, 115, 119, 129, 250, 301, 302, 303, 310, 311, 334, 335, 350, 357, 368, 369, 370 Cochrane, x, 227, 232, 233, 242, 244 Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 242, 244 codes, 270, 295 coding, 1, 34, 59, 66, 72, 195, 215, 294, 344, 369 codon, 215 co-existence, 83 cognitive development, 251 cognitive dysfunction, 116 cognitive function, 174 cognitive performance, 178 coil, 124, 185, 186, 187, 189, 194 collagen, viii, 137, 139, 141, 144, 150, 151, 284 collateral, 33, 51, 53, 161 college students, 107, 109 combined effect, 277 commissure, 3, 72 communication, xi, xiv, 9, 95, 222, 228, 229, 230, 231, 236, 242, 243, 245, 284, 299, 303, 333, 334 communication abilities, 236 community, xiii, 96, 109, 291, 297 comorbidity, 182 compaction, 141 complement, xii, 101, 263, 282, 283, 286 complexity, 161, 266, 325, 328 compliance, 97, 229, 254 complications, 28, 116, 169, 175 components, ix, xiii, 46, 66, 142, 159, 160, 161, 163, 164, 165, 167, 169, 170, 173, 185, 205, 213, 214, 216, 223, 266, 276, 291, 292, 334, 343, 345, 353, 354, 364, 365, 367, 368 composition, 207, 212, 214, 217, 219, 222, 224 compounds, 319 comprehension, 5 compressibility, 357 computation, 325, 326, 335 computer systems, 197 concealment, 233, 237, 242, 247 condensation, 83 conductance, 321 conduction, ix, 35, 128, 159, 161, 165, 169, 170, 172, 173, 176, 178, 182, 219, 244 conductive, 200 cone photoreceptors, 207 confidence interval, 298 configuration, 28, 35, 250 confounding variables, 127 confusion, 10

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Index conjecture, 17 connectivity, 11, 12, 26, 160, 188 consanguineous, 206, 215, 216, 220 consensus, 5, 102 conservation, 85, 97, 102, 107, 110, 228, 245, 296 constraints, 21 construction, 199, 202 consumption, 283 consumption coagulopathy, 283 contact dermatitis, 285 contamination, 21, 242, 243 contemporary music, 90, 91 continuity, 6, 8, 179 continuous data, 234 contractions, 182 control, 33, 65, 71, 86, 90, 91, 92, 94, 119, 124, 139, 141, 144, 145, 148, 154, 182, 188, 215, 216, 217, 244, 246, 252, 293, 296, 319, 321, 327, 334, 335, 340, 356 control group, 86, 91, 92, 94, 139, 141, 144 controlled studies, ix, 181, 187 controlled trials, 231, 232, 237, 242, 243, 246 convergence, 30, 36, 58, 62, 63, 65, 341 conversion, xiv, 59, 138, 314 corpus callosum, 161 correlation, xi, 5, 86, 87, 89, 95, 116, 122, 140, 171, 180, 182, 243, 263, 265, 266, 267, 270, 354 correlation coefficient, 243 correlations, 119 cortex, ix, 19, 32, 131, 161, 177, 178, 181, 183, 184, 185, 186, 188, 192, 193, 195 cortical neurons, 179 cortical silent period, 188 corticospinal, 161 corticosteroids, 138, 147, 148, 156, 280, 281 costs, xi, xiv, 228, 243, 333, 351 coupling, 88, 90 coxsackievirus, 213, 222 CPA, 345, 349, 356, 365, 366, 371 cranial nerve, xiii, 3, 218, 313, 314 C-reactive protein, 282, 283, 286 critical period, 163, 250 cross-linking, 314 cross-sectional study, 84, 242 cryopreserved, 148 C-terminal, 144, 220, 225 cues, 63, 64, 309, 337, 365 Cybernetics, 371 cytoarchitecture, 6, 9, 11, 12, 14, 15, 16 cytochrome, 120, 123, 124, 315, 316 cytokine, 142, 148, 284, 285

379 cytokines, 142, 285, 289 cytology, 14 cytomegalovirus, 289 cytometry, 120, 124, 145, 147 cytoplasm, 16, 127, 315, 317 cytoskeleton, 144, 161, 224 cytosol, 318 cytosolic, xiv, 220, 313, 317 cytotoxic, xiii, 279, 281

D data analysis, 267 data set, 325, 367 database, 334, 335, 340, 353, 354, 364 death, 120, 122, 124, 125, 126, 133, 134, 148, 170, 174, 217, 315, 320 deaths, 125, 126 decay, 264, 277, 325, 328, 338, 346, 347 decay times, 264 decisions, 242, 319 decoding, 340, 364 decomposition, 62, 336 defects, 112, 151, 155, 218, 219, 373 defense, 321 deficiency, 146, 219, 337 deficit, 162 deficits, ix, 156, 159, 162, 178 definition, 86, 92, 108, 113, 231, 283 deformability, 281 degradation, 120, 143, 148, 334, 356, 366, 367, 368 degrading, xv, 334 Delphi study, 110 demyelination, 116 dendrites, 7, 10, 13, 15, 16, 17, 18, 34, 49, 170 dendritic structures, 17 density, xv, 8, 34, 57, 147, 164, 334, 342, 355, 364, 366 dependent variable, 369 depolarization, 214, 339 deposition, 284, 317 deposits, 155, 209, 219 depressed, 189, 195, 251, 339 depression, 96, 176, 179, 182, 187, 188, 190, 191, 194, 195, 262 deprivation, 160, 303, 311 destruction, 113, 123, 129, 285, 315 detection, vii, xi, 33, 62, 64, 84, 101, 107, 109, 110, 141, 143, 145, 163, 164, 259, 260, 263, 265, 271, 276, 287, 297, 304, 305, 345, 347 developing brain, 161, 163, 172, 176 developing countries, 295

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

380 developmental deficits, 162 developmental process, 217 dexamethasone, 178 diabetes, 141, 285 diabetes mellitus, 141, 285 diagnostic criteria, 281, 285, 287 diagnostic kits, 296 differentiation, 143, 319, 320, 321 diffusion, 175, 213, 222 dimensionality, 335, 337 direct observation, 17 directionality, 7 disabilities, 308 discharges, 33, 42, 75, 77, 344 discriminant analysis, 354, 355 discrimination, 128, 269, 271, 304, 309, 311, 352, 353, 354, 355, 363, 365, 369, 371 discrimination tasks, 352 disease activity, 148, 152 diseases, xii, 139, 141, 142, 279, 284, 286, 287 disequilibrium, 297 disinhibition, 187 disorder, xii, 145, 155, 279, 281, 282, 284, 292, 295 dispersion, 234 displacement, 265 distortions, 314, 329, 334 distress, 182, 183, 188 distribution, vii, ix, xiii, 1, 7, 10, 15, 16, 19, 20, 21, 23, 24, 26, 27, 28, 43, 44, 46, 49, 51, 52, 58, 60, 61, 63, 127, 150, 159, 172, 179, 214, 269, 291, 292, 297, 317, 322, 341, 342, 355 distribution function, 341, 342 divergence, 115, 297, 355 diversity, x, 64, 197, 198, 204, 242 division, 3, 10, 12, 14, 15, 17, 68, 115, 142, 319 DNA, xiv, 123, 127, 133, 282, 292, 293, 294, 300, 313, 314, 315, 317, 321 DNA damage, 123, 127, 133 DNA repair, 123, 127 donors, 148, 152 dopamine, 188 dopaminergic, 188 dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, 187 dosage, 115, 119, 320 dosimetry, 244 double helix, 314 DPO, 2 Drosophila, 134 drug abuse, 96 drug toxicity, 122 drug treatment, 319 drug-induced, 126

Index drugs, xiii, xiv, 120, 134, 188, 279, 280, 281, 287, 292, 313, 314, 320, 321 dysplasia, ix, 159, 160, 162, 174, 177, 178, 179, 180

E early warning, 91 ears, 85, 96, 99, 100, 101, 106, 107, 112, 113, 114, 117, 119, 132, 177, 182, 210, 229, 251, 254, 255, 260, 261, 265, 266, 270, 276, 277, 288, 301, 304 ectoderm, 317 edema, 315 EEG, 186, 190 effusion, 113 elaboration, 172 electric charge, 185 electric current, 185 electrical properties, 74 electrodes, ix, 21, 181, 186, 350 electroencephalogram, 189, 195 electromagnetic, 185 electron, 131, 172 electrophoresis, 300 electrophysiological study, 20, 21, 38, 172 electrophysiology, 8, 20, 21, 26, 175, 176 ELISA, 140, 142, 147, 157 ELL, 220 embryonic development, x, 211, 221, 224, 225 embryonic stem, 223, 319, 321 embryonic stem cells, 223, 319, 321 emission, 97, 98, 99, 101, 107, 184, 190, 192, 194, 259, 260, 261, 262, 275, 276, 277, 280 emotional, 183, 184, 187, 189 emotional distress, 184, 189 emotional state, 183 emphysema, 169 employability, 96 employment, 96 encephalomyelitis, 143, 154 encephalopathy, 174, 177, 179, 262 encoding, x, 36, 52, 64, 74, 197, 206, 210, 215, 218, 220, 223, 224 endocarditis, 283 endogenous mechanisms, 319, 320 endolymphatic hydrops, 139, 151, 302 endothelial cell, 116, 147, 212, 213, 223, 284, 286 energy, 82, 102, 107, 110, 221, 304, 368, 373 environment, xi, 66, 95, 96, 106, 115, 161, 172, 217, 228, 229, 231, 243, 245, 304, 330, 335, 337

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Index environmental factors, 108, 220, 292 enzyme immunoassay, 154 enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay, 142 enzymes, 83, 120, 150, 319 eosinophils, 285 epidemiology, 296, 299 epilepsy, 116 epileptic seizures, 189 epithelia, 155, 210, 212, 213, 221, 222, 317, 319 epithelial cells, x, 139, 211, 212, 214, 218, 220, 221, 222, 223 epithelial transport, 222 epithelium, 143, 210, 213, 215, 220, 223 epitope, 145, 151, 153 equilibrium, 296 ERA, 118 erythrocyte, 281, 282, 283, 286 erythrocyte sedimentation rate, 282, 283, 286 ethnic background, 295 ethnic groups, 292, 295, 296 ethnicity, xiii, 291, 293, 294 etiologic factor, 111 etiology, iv, xii, 111, 138, 147, 221, 279, 284 eukaryotes, 202 eukaryotic cell, 140 evoked potential, 176, 177, 179, 302 evolution, 88, 300 exchange rate, 102, 103 exchange transfusion, 253 excitability, 70, 186, 188, 192, 195 excitation, 28, 40, 46, 49, 50, 52, 58, 60, 62, 66, 70, 72, 183, 335, 372 excitotoxic, 83 exclusion, 232, 237, 267, 318 experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis, 143, 154 experimental condition, 169 extracellular matrix, 22, 144, 155 extraction, 39, 65, 153, 199, 208, 328, 329, 330, 337, 338, 343, 345, 347, 350, 359, 371 extraction process, 338, 345 extrapolation, 92, 283

F false negative, 269 false positive, 199, 202, 205 family, x, 96, 120, 124, 143, 154, 156, 197, 205, 206, 213, 214, 216, 217, 222, 223, 293 FDG, 184, 186, 190 females, 88, 89, 93, 113, 353 fetal, 172, 174, 178, 253, 262 fetal brain, 178

381 fetus, 172, 253, 262 FFT, 338 fibers, 70, 72, 74, 82, 139, 161, 172, 229 fibrillar, 150 fibrinogen, 282 fibrocytes, xiv, 314, 315, 319, 321 fibrosis, 113, 284 filament, 146 filters, 36, 62, 200, 202, 325, 337, 338, 342, 343, 345, 349, 350, 351, 358, 359, 360, 368, 372 financial support, 67 finite impulse response (FIR) filters, 349 FIR filters, 345, 349, 350, 351, 359 Fisher exact test, 294 flow, 83, 120, 145, 147, 184, 185, 190, 286, 339, 373 flow cytometry analysis, 145 fluctuations, 266 fluid, 154, 214, 217, 219, 251, 264 fluorescence, 121, 125 fMRI, 184, 186, 192 forensic, 334 founder effect, xiii, 291, 294, 295, 297, 300 Fourier, xiv, 325, 333, 334, 337 fractionation, 321 fracture, 212, 222, 223 fragmentation, 83 frameshift mutation, 215 free radicals, 83, 134, 319 frequency distribution, 277 frequency resolution, xii, 257, 259, 263, 275, 344, 350 functional imaging, 184, 186 functional magnetic resonance imaging, 192 fungal, 284 fusion, 91, 220, 339

G G protein, 199, 208 GABA, 2, 69, 71, 75 GABAergic, 31, 32, 70, 75, 78 gadolinium, 280 Gamma, 118 gamma radiation, 117, 120 gamma-aminobutyric acid, 76 ganglia, 116, 117 ganglion, xiv, 82, 117, 127, 139, 143, 144, 203, 314, 315, 319, 320, 322 gangliosides, 142 gastrointestinal, 126, 314, 319 gastrointestinal tract, 314

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

382 Gaussian, 341, 342, 343, 344, 355, 364, 365, 366, 367, 368 gels, 210, 300 gender, 83, 88, 89, 93, 103, 110 gender differences, 103 gene, x, xiii, 120, 121, 125, 133, 145, 146, 155, 156, 200, 201, 206, 207, 208, 209, 210, 211, 215, 217, 218, 219, 224, 225, 291, 292, 293, 294, 295, 296, 297, 299, 300, 315, 319 gene pool, 292 gene therapy, 319 gene transfer, 133, 224 generalization, 325, 326, 329 generation, x, xii, xiv, 41, 84, 121, 123, 124, 125, 126, 178, 184, 187, 188, 211, 221, 225, 233, 237, 263, 265, 266, 276, 313, 314, 318 genes, x, 123, 133, 156, 161, 197, 202, 205, 207, 208, 215, 219, 220, 292, 294, 295, 296 genetic linkage, 220 genetics, 297 genome, 202 genotype, 294 gentamicin, 120, 126, 134, 224, 373 gestation, 165, 170, 172, 176 gestures, 232 GFP, 216 gland, 143, 286 glass, 146 glial, 116, 161, 179 glial cells, 116 glial fibrillary acidic protein, 179 glioblastoma, 143 glioma, 126, 134 gliosis, 162 glucose, 69, 192 glutamate, 31, 71 glutathione, 124, 127, 133, 134, 319 glutathione peroxidase, 133 glycine, 32, 50, 71, 75, 76, 213, 216 glycolipids, viii, 137, 141, 153 glycoprotein, 146, 156, 161 grades, 108 grafting, 319 granzymes, 126 gray matter, 178 groups, xiii, 5, 30, 41, 44, 86, 89, 91, 92, 95, 102, 113, 127, 128, 139, 170, 233, 235, 237, 251, 291, 292, 293, 294, 295, 296, 305 growth, 74, 142, 161, 170, 174, 178, 205, 210, 356 growth factor, 142, 205 growth spurt, 170 GST, 143, 220

Index guidelines, 101, 102, 103, 104, 189, 195, 260, 281, 302 Guillain-Barré syndrome, 141, 142 gyrus, 4

H haemoglobin, 127 haemostasis, 283 hallucinations, 182, 186, 187, 189, 193, 194 haplotype, 297 harmful effects, 86, 91 harmonics, 345 head and neck cancer, 112, 113, 126, 129 headache, 189 healing, 373 health, 96, 102, 105, 127, 152, 242, 244, 373 health care, 242, 244 hearing impairment, vii, xi, xiii, 84, 107, 112, 130, 142, 146, 156, 163, 187, 190, 200, 201, 206, 207, 209, 210, 219, 228, 244, 249, 250, 251, 253, 254, 259, 260, 261, 266, 267, 270, 274, 291, 295, 296, 297, 299, 369 heat, 137, 140, 152, 155, 175, 319 heat shock protein, viii, 137, 140, 152, 155, 175, 319 heavy metal, 94 height, 48, 82 helix, 21, 73, 138, 314 hemangioma, 118, 131 hematocrit, 127 heme, 319 hemisphere, 302 Heparin, 289 hepatitis, 141 heterogeneity, 183, 187, 234, 297 heterogeneous, 17, 18, 49, 223, 295 heterozygosity, 297 heterozygote, 295 heuristic, 353, 363 high risk, 172 high-frequency, ix, 84, 86, 92, 95, 99, 101, 103, 104, 107, 146, 181, 183, 186, 189 high-risk, 163, 177, 262 hippocampus, 184 histogram, 40, 44, 269, 338, 339, 342, 343, 344, 347, 349, 350, 351, 353, 354, 358, 359, 360, 361, 362, 363 histological, 113, 114, 317 histopathology, 284 homeostasis, 83, 120, 147, 224, 315, 317, 320, 321 homogenous, 148

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Index homology, 21, 201 homozygosity, 297 hormone, 146, 156, 208 hormones, 142, 286 host tissue, 285 House, 105, 191 HRP, 2, 8, 24, 29, 30, 32, 69, 71 HSP, 140, 141, 146, 149, 152 Hsp70, 152 human brain, 5, 172, 177, 178 human cerebral cortex, 175 human genome, 202 human subjects, 163 humans, x, 3, 4, 5, 8, 9, 12, 65, 67, 83, 114, 116, 119, 126, 139, 144, 146, 150, 211, 220, 221, 276, 285, 303, 318, 347 humoral immunity, 285 hunting, 62 hybrid, 217 hybridization, 75, 215 hybridoma, 146 hydrolysis, 127 hydrophobicity, 216 hygiene, 230 hyperactivity, ix, 181, 183 hyperbilirubinemia, 262 hyperlipoproteinemia, 286 hyperreactivity, 151 hypersensitivity, 87 hypertension, 152 hypertensive, 154 hypothesis, 24, 34, 63, 138, 140, 143, 172, 188, 198, 215, 295, 315, 321 hypothyroidism, 286 hypoxemia, 169, 179, 250, 262 hypoxia, vii, viii, xi, 159, 160, 161, 162, 163, 164, 169, 170, 172, 173, 175, 176, 178, 179, 180, 249, 250, 251, 253, 259, 260, 262 hypoxia-ischemia, xi, 162, 164, 170, 176, 179, 249, 250, 251, 259 hypoxic, 161, 162, 172, 174, 176, 178 hypoxic-ischemic, 176

I ICAM, 284 idiopathic, xii, 137, 140, 144, 152, 155, 279, 280, 288 IgE, 285 IgG, 282, 283, 285, 286 IL-1, 142, 285 IL-10, 285 IL-13, 142, 285

383 IL-2, 285 IL-4, 142, 285 imaging, 175, 184, 186, 187, 188, 190, 192, 280 imaging techniques, 186, 187 imbalances, 373 immersion, 96 immune cells, 143 immune reaction, xii, 279, 285 immune response, 138, 142, 284, 285 immune system, 137, 284, 285, 286 immunity, 144, 280, 284, 339 immunization, 138, 139, 142, 145, 284 immunoassays, 149 immunocompetent cells, 284 immunocytochemistry, 215 immunoglobulin, 148, 213, 282, 285 immunoglobulin superfamily, 213 immunohistochemistry, 178 immunological, xii, 139, 149, 279, 281, 282, 285, 287 immunopathogenesis, 147, 284 immunoreactivity, 68, 71, 74, 148 immunosuppressive, 138, 149, 283, 287 immunosuppressive agent, 138, 287 immunosuppressive drugs, 283 immunotherapy, 138, 148 impairments, 146 implants, xiii, xv, 112, 189, 301, 304, 305, 308, 310, 311, 334, 335, 368, 370 implementation, xi, xv, 249, 250, 334, 337, 338, 340, 344, 345, 347, 349, 351, 357, 358, 360 in situ hybridization, 75, 215 in utero, 253, 262 in vitro, vii, 1, 3, 42, 46, 51, 54, 56, 70, 71, 72, 78, 207, 218, 220, 224, 288 in vivo, vii, xiv, 1, 3, 18, 22, 36, 41, 42, 45, 46, 54, 56, 57, 74, 122, 133, 134, 218, 314, 315, 319 incidence, 92, 113, 116, 117, 127, 128, 130, 162, 205, 228, 260, 261, 281, 286, 292 inclusion, 234 indication, 26, 29, 84, 186, 303 indicators, xii, 84, 114, 128, 264, 265, 275, 286 indirect effect, 250 industrial, xii, 84, 85, 92, 101, 107, 109, 235, 245, 264, 267 infants, ix, xi, 159, 160, 162, 163, 164, 165, 166, 167, 168, 169, 170, 171, 172, 173, 175, 176, 178, 179, 180, 249, 250, 251, 252, 253, 254, 255, 256, 257, 258, 259, 260, 261, 262 infection, 117, 126, 140, 152, 169, 253, 283, 284, 285, 292 infectious, 152, 154, 283

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

384 infectious diseases, 152 infinite, 333, 338, 340, 346 inflammation, 113, 145, 169, 284, 288 inflammatory, 83, 113, 114, 139, 141, 142, 145, 149, 153, 282, 283, 284, 285, 286, 287 inflammatory bowel disease, 285 inflammatory cells, 284 inflammatory disease, 141 inflammatory mediators, 284 inflammatory response, 83, 113, 284 inflammatory responses, 83, 113 information age, 197 information processing, 198 inheritance, 292, 293 inherited, 209, 216, 217, 293, 299 inhibition, 28, 36, 39, 40, 46, 49, 50, 51, 52, 53, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 72, 74, 77, 78, 126, 161, 183, 188, 195, 285, 319, 362, 363 inhibitor, 123, 126, 134, 161 inhibitors, 161, 319 inhibitory, vii, 1, 12, 22, 28, 30, 31, 32, 40, 46, 50, 51, 58, 59, 60, 62, 64, 65, 66, 67, 71, 77 injection, 20, 24, 26, 28, 36, 41, 42, 43, 46, 51, 54, 56, 67, 78, 141, 280, 286, 315, 316, 317, 325 injections, 16, 22, 24, 25, 27, 29, 30, 32, 41, 54, 56, 69, 139 injury, xii, 113, 114, 116, 122, 123, 124, 154, 161, 173, 175, 176, 177, 179, 244, 261, 279, 280, 319 innervation, 116, 318 innovation, 149 insects, 62 insertion, 219, 229, 230, 236, 242, 329, 330 insight, 21, 33, 63, 82, 87, 104, 198, 217 insomnia, 182, 191 instruments, 88 insulin, 141 integration, 3, 56, 65, 338, 372 integrins, 224 integrity, vii, ix, 82, 84, 123, 144, 159, 160, 163, 164, 165, 167, 169, 173, 217, 219 intensive care unit, 251 interaction, 58, 65, 74, 83, 127, 141, 156, 223, 286, 301, 303, 356, 357 interaction effect, 303 interactions, 4, 28, 49, 57, 58, 65, 96, 147, 148, 302, 334, 357 intercellular adhesion molecule, 284, 288 interferon (IFN), 142, 145, 284, 285 interleukin, 142, 284 interneurons, 32

Index interstitial, 10, 126, 141, 169, 288 interstitial nephritis, 126, 141 interval, 44, 64, 74, 164, 165, 166, 167, 168, 169, 215, 220, 267, 297, 338, 342, 343, 344, 350, 353, 354, 358, 359, 360, 361, 362, 363, 366, 371 intervention, 112, 124, 126, 195, 228, 235, 236, 251, 259, 260, 301, 308 intracranial, 128, 189 intravenous, 148, 184 intrinsic, xi, xiv, 5, 31, 32, 40, 46, 50, 64, 65, 70, 74, 120, 133, 134, 263, 314, 315, 319, 320 invasive, ix, 84, 163, 181, 185, 192, 265, 297 ion channels, 56 ionic, 82, 122, 123, 212, 214, 217, 296 ionizing radiation, 130 ions, 212, 219 ipsilateral, 4, 7, 10, 19, 24, 28, 29, 31, 32, 35, 45, 46, 47, 48, 49, 55, 56, 57, 58, 60, 63, 287, 302 IR, 76 irradiation, 112, 114, 115, 116, 118, 119, 120, 121, 122, 123, 124, 125, 128, 129, 130, 131, 132, 133, 135 ischaemia, 162, 163, 164, 175, 176, 178, 250, 253 ischemia, xi, 120, 124, 160, 162, 164, 170, 175, 176, 179, 249, 250, 251, 259 ischemic, 154, 160, 176, 177, 178, 179, 262, 283 ISO, 86 isoforms, 156, 220 isolation, 219

K K+, 208, 214, 217, 218, 223, 315, 316, 317, 321 kinase, 143, 154, 207, 316

L L1, 97, 99, 101, 267 L2, 97, 99, 101, 267 laboratory studies, 83, 281, 285 labyrinthitis, 142, 150 lamina, 17, 19, 73, 217, 220, 223, 224 Laminar, 69, 73 laminin, 147, 150 language, xi, 232, 249, 250, 251, 254, 296, 334, 367 language development, xi, 249, 250, 254 large-scale, 225 laser, 125, 209

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Index latency, xii, 31, 37, 38, 40, 43, 49, 50, 51, 59, 65, 66, 116, 118, 164, 165, 166, 167, 174, 264, 266, 267, 269, 274, 276, 277 late-onset, 114, 115, 122 laterality, 184 left hemisphere, 302 legislation, 82, 96, 97, 102, 103, 104 leisure time, 86, 104, 109 lesions, 74, 77, 137, 139, 141, 143, 151, 160, 162, 164, 167, 174, 315 leukemia, 131, 134 leukocyte, 285 lifestyles, 84, 109 ligament, xiv, 113, 139, 143, 145, 147, 314, 315, 317, 319, 321 limbic system, 191 limbus, 139, 144, 147, 203, 214, 217, 316 limitations, xi, 104, 148, 187, 189, 254, 263, 304 linear, xiv, xv, 19, 21, 41, 42, 51, 52, 54, 82, 84, 132, 250, 265, 266, 267, 270, 333, 334, 335, 336, 340, 350, 354, 355, 356, 357, 358, 359, 371 linear model, 19, 270 linear regression, 270 lingual, 156 linguistic, xiii, 291, 292, 295, 296 linkage, 297 links, 82, 191, 334 lipid, 125, 127, 146, 222, 319 lipid metabolism, 146 lipid peroxidation, 125, 319 lipoprotein, 286 listening, 82, 86, 87, 88, 89, 90, 91, 92, 93, 94, 95, 96, 97, 98, 99, 103, 105, 106, 107, 108, 110, 303, 305, 309, 310 liver, 218, 319 localised, 17, 25, 46, 63 localization, xiii, 70, 72, 73, 148, 150, 153, 154, 155, 216, 218, 265, 301, 304, 309, 310, 311, 347 location, 10, 14, 16, 47, 49, 55, 63, 182, 203, 205, 296, 304, 309, 321, 328 locus, 206, 208, 209, 210, 215, 220, 225 longitudinal study, 84, 105, 107, 132, 175, 179, 259, 260, 262, 266 losses, 82, 113, 115, 122, 138, 145, 285 low birthweight, 176, 253 low molecular weight, xiii, 280, 281, 288 low-level, 84, 251, 253 LTD, 188, 190, 193 LTP, 193 lung, 126, 162, 164, 169, 174, 176, 178, 179 lung disease, 162, 174, 176, 178, 179

385 lupus erythematosus, 141 lymphadenopathy, 143 lymphocytes, 138, 147, 284

M mAb, 146 macrophages, 285 magnetic, ix, 175, 181, 185, 187, 190, 192, 193, 194, 195 magnetic field, 185, 187 magnetic resonance imaging, 175, 190, 192 magnetoencephalography, 183, 191 maladaptive, 183, 188 males, 85, 88, 89, 93, 104, 353 mammalian, 67, 69, 72, 74, 75, 76 mammalian brain, 74 mammals, 3, 5, 13, 17, 19, 20, 21, 24, 28, 57, 66, 76, 142, 319 manipulation, 31, 120, 126, 197 man-machine interface, 334 mapping, 19, 145, 206, 207, 208, 336 margin of error, 21 Markov chain, 335 Markov model, xv, 334, 347, 368 Marx, 120, 132 mask, 246, 323, 324, 348 masking, xiv, 277, 323, 324, 325, 326, 328, 331, 336, 338, 340, 345, 346, 347, 348, 369, 371, 372 mass spectrometry, xiv, 313, 317, 318 mast cell, 285 maternal, 216, 262, 295 maternal inheritance, 295 matrix, 22, 144, 150, 155, 156, 264, 325, 341, 354, 355, 366 maturation, 161, 165, 172, 173, 176, 178, 180, 208, 210, 223 Mb, 215, 220 MB, 68, 76, 133 MBP, 172, 173 MCI, 137 measurement, 88, 93, 98, 99, 182, 187, 190, 254 measures, x, xi, 86, 91, 94, 96, 112, 124, 148, 173, 179, 190, 227, 228, 229, 232, 233, 234, 236, 243, 254, 257, 304, 308, 310, 352, 355, 356, 357 meconium, 251 media, viii, 81, 84, 96, 103, 147, 212, 214, 261, 317, 334 median, 40, 51, 85, 89, 91, 93, 106, 116, 288 medication, 193, 287 medications, 123, 124, 253

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

386 medicine, vii, viii, 111, 112 medulla, 3, 71, 118, 144, 173 MEG, 183, 190 MEK, 154 melatonin, 321 membranes, 43, 139, 185, 217, 316 meningitis, 120, 124, 175, 253 mental development, 176 meridian, 373 mesoderm, 218 MET, 233 meta-analysis, xi, 227, 228, 238, 239, 240, 241, 243, 244 metabolic, vii, 81, 82, 162, 167, 177, 184, 192, 281, 315 metabolic rate, 162, 177 metabolism, 146, 161, 188, 262, 315 methionine, 321 MFC, 365 microarray, 120 microelectrode, 224 microenvironment, 214 microscopy, 125, 316 microstructure, xi, 263, 266 microtubule, 177 microtubules, 142 midbrain, 3, 5, 20, 36, 69, 71, 73, 173 middle-aged, xii, 279, 281 migration, 160, 285 military, 84, 245 mining, x, 197, 199, 202, 204, 205, 207 mirror, 305, 309 misconception, 94, 96 mitochondria, 83, 120, 123, 127, 208, 318 mitochondrial, xiii, xiv, 124, 133, 291, 292, 295, 313, 316, 317, 320 mitochondrial membrane, 316 mitogen, 143, 154 mitogen-activated protein kinase, 154 mitosis, 123 mitotic, 120, 123 mobility, 254 modality, 118 modeling, 324, 325, 334, 335, 370 modifier gene, 208 modulation, ix, 62, 127, 181, 186, 339, 346, 347, 348 molecular mechanisms, 120 molecular weight, xiii, 143, 144, 147, 148, 280, 281, 288, 317 molecules, 127, 142, 143, 146, 156, 222, 223, 224, 318 Møller, 190, 191, 193

Index momentum, 326, 329 monoclonal, 146, 153, 156, 173, 286 monoclonal antibodies, 146, 173 monoclonal antibody, 153, 156 monogenic, 292 mononuclear cell, 148, 285 monotone, 341 morphological, 7, 15, 16, 18, 41, 51, 82, 126, 139, 172, 210, 213, 219, 289, 316 morphological abnormalities, 139 morphology, vii, 1, 6, 14, 15, 17, 43, 44, 49, 51, 54, 56, 73, 74, 151, 217 mosaic, 19, 21, 24, 25, 64, 73 motivation, 96 motorcortex, 192 MRI, 186, 189, 190, 192 mRNA, 76, 155, 215, 220 MSS, 281 multicellular organisms, 212 multidisciplinary, 103, 190 multiple sclerosis, 141, 143, 285 multiplier, 357, 359 multivariate, 133, 265, 267, 270, 271, 272, 275 mumps, 302 muscle, 83, 97, 99, 101, 116, 143, 144, 182 muscle atrophy, 116 muscle cells, 144 muscle contraction, 182 music, 81, 82, 83, 86, 87, 88, 89, 90, 91, 92, 94, 95, 96, 97, 103, 104, 105, 106, 107, 108, 110, 182, 323, 325, 329 musicians, 94 mutagenesis, 217, 219, 225 mutant, x, 198, 208, 211, 213, 216, 217, 218, 219, 220, 224 mutant proteins, 220 mutants, 221 mutation, x, xiii, 133, 145, 146, 150, 155, 156, 198, 199, 200, 201, 202, 206, 207, 208, 209, 210, 211, 213, 214, 215, 216, 217, 218, 219, 220, 221, 291, 292, 293, 294, 295, 296, 297, 299, 300 myelin, viii, ix, 137, 140, 141, 153, 159, 161, 172, 173, 218, 219, 224, 225 myelin basic protein, ix, 159, 172, 173 myelination, ix, 159, 160, 163, 165, 167, 169, 170, 172, 173, 174, 178, 179 myosin, x, 197, 200, 201, 202, 203, 205, 206, 207, 208, 210

N Na+, 56, 214, 218, 223, 315, 321

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Index N-acety, 124 NAM, 135 nasopharyngeal carcinoma, 113, 119, 128, 129, 130, 131, 132 nasopharynx, 130 natural, 63, 65, 67, 183, 285 neck, vii, viii, 111, 112, 113, 115, 119, 123, 126, 129, 133, 182, 187 neck cancer, 133 necrosis, 83, 116, 126, 134, 284, 286 necrotic cell death, 125 neonatal, xi, 161, 162, 164, 169, 170, 173, 175, 176, 177, 178, 179, 180, 249, 250, 253, 254, 260, 261, 262, 263 neonates, 175, 176, 177, 179, 257, 260, 261 neoplastic, 126, 319 nephritis, 126, 141, 207 nephrotoxicity, 134, 314, 319 nerve, xv, 33, 35, 73, 82, 115, 116, 117, 118, 132, 139, 141, 146, 161, 165, 219, 257, 280, 338, 339, 340, 344, 369, 371, 372 nerve cells, 139 nerve conduction velocity, 165 nerves, xv, 116, 314, 333 nervous system, 116, 182, 183 neural function, ix, 159, 164, 169, 173 neural mechanisms, 335 neural network, xiv, 323, 325, 326, 329 neural stem cell, 319, 321 neuritis, 280 neuroanatomy, 76, 191 neurobehavioral, 175 neurobiological, ix, 181, 187, 188, 189, 190, 195 neurobiology, 187 neuroblastoma, 143, 154 neurofilament, 178 neuroimaging, ix, 160, 181, 189 neurological disease, 142 neuroma, 118 neuron response, 41, 42 neuronal cells, 116, 161 neuronavigation, 186 neuropathological, 178 neuropathology, 163, 179 neuropathy, 201, 206, 209 neuroplasticity, 195 neuroprotection, 179 neuropsychiatry, 192 neuropsychology, 192 neuroscience, 191 neuroscientists, 66 neurosurgery, 177, 261 neurotransmission, 161

387 neurotransmitters, 31, 82, 146, 339 Newton, 107, 250, 253, 261 NHL, 111, 114, 115, 122, 137, 281 NIC, 32 NICU, 259 NIH, 373 nitric oxide, 70, 261 nitric oxide synthase, 70 NMDA, 31 non-autoimmune disease, 149 non-Gaussian, 365, 366, 367, 368 non-invasive, ix, 84, 163, 181, 185, 265 nonsense mutation, 220 normal conditions, 217 NSAIDs, 283 N-terminal, 208 nuclear, xiv, 72, 77, 83, 121, 123, 127, 134, 144, 208, 282, 313, 317 nuclei, 1, 3, 4, 5, 8, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 26, 29, 30, 31, 35, 36, 38, 46, 54, 57, 59, 60, 62, 64, 67, 69, 71, 72, 74, 75, 76, 78, 79, 115, 121, 125, 162, 172, 315, 316 nucleotides, 215 nucleus, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 19, 21, 22, 24, 29, 50, 56, 57, 59, 63, 65, 67, 68, 69, 70, 71, 72, 73, 74, 75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 115, 123, 127, 143, 161, 162, 172, 317 nutrition, 96

O observations, viii, xv, 28, 111, 114, 129, 140, 309, 329, 333, 345, 355, 372, 373 obsessive-compulsive, 193 obsessive-compulsive disorder, 193 occluding, 221 occupational, vii, x, 81, 82, 84, 86, 87, 92, 96, 102, 103, 104, 105, 109, 227, 228, 229, 231, 237, 242, 244, 245 occupational health, 242 occupational risks, 245 OCD, 193 oligodendrocytes, 116, 161, 172, 173 oligonucleotides, 293 olive, 3, 67, 70, 72, 77, 78, 79, 162 oncogene, 143, 154 oncology, 131, 133, 134 operator, 187, 360 optic glioma, 118 orbitofrontal cortex, 184 organ, x, 81, 82, 113, 123, 135, 139, 141, 143, 145, 146, 147, 150, 197, 198, 205, 212, 214,

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

388

Index

215, 217, 218, 220, 223, 224, 280, 314, 317, 318, 319, 321, 373 organelle, 126, 142 oscillations, 76, 183 otitis media, 261 otoacoustic emissions, xi, 84, 87, 104, 105, 106, 107, 108, 109, 249, 250, 260, 261, 262, 267, 275, 276, 277, 282, 293 otosclerosis, 139, 150 ototoxic drugs, 120, 292 ototoxicity, viii, xiii, xiv, 111, 112, 113, 119, 120, 123, 124, 125, 126, 127, 128, 129, 133, 134, 135, 313, 314, 315, 318, 319, 320, 321 oxidants, viii, 111, 120, 124, 125, 126, 129 oxidative, xiv, 106, 132, 133, 314, 319 oxidative stress, xiv, 106, 132, 133, 314, 319 oxide, 70, 261 oxygen, viii, 83, 111, 120, 127, 160, 163, 164, 169, 184, 190, 253, 262, 318

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

P p53, 120, 121, 123, 124, 125, 133, 134, 315, 320 pain, 116, 183, 185, 189, 191 parallel processing, 64, 66, 115 paralysis, 116, 138 parameter, 193, 265, 282, 325, 326, 327, 328, 330, 341, 345, 350, 358, 359, 363, 364 parameter estimates, 350 parameter vectors, 325 parotid, 117, 130 parotid gland, 130 passive, 83, 96, 125, 222, 235, 236 patent ductus arteriosus, 169 pathogenesis, xiii, 138, 140, 141, 144, 145, 149, 152, 179, 207, 217, 280, 283, 285, 286, 287, 296 pathogenic, 138, 143, 149, 216, 220, 292, 295, 296, 297 pathology, 109, 117, 120, 178, 251, 254, 284, 288, 295, 320 pathophysiological, 288, 321 pathophysiology, ix, 175, 181, 182, 188, 216, 284 pathways, vii, viii, 3, 4, 19, 69, 71, 73, 79, 83, 111, 112, 115, 117, 118, 119, 120, 124, 125, 126, 127, 129, 132, 133, 134, 161, 170, 178, 183, 295, 302, 320, 373 pattern recognition, 59 patterning, 310 PBMC, 148 PCP, 89, 91, 92 PCR, xiii, 291, 294 pediatric, 130, 174

peptides, 140, 145, 147, 155 perception, xiii, xiv, 59, 64, 65, 66, 73, 76, 82, 95, 110, 182, 183, 184, 185, 186, 189, 191, 192, 265, 276, 277, 301, 310, 311, 323, 333, 334, 336, 337, 338, 347, 353, 369, 372 perceptual processing, 335, 368 perinatal, ix, xi, 159, 160, 161, 162, 163, 164, 169, 170, 173, 174, 175, 176, 177, 178, 179, 249, 250, 251, 252, 253, 254, 255, 256, 257, 258, 259, 260, 261, 262, 292 periodicity, 25, 62, 337, 338 peripheral blood, 148, 285, 294 peripheral blood mononuclear cell, 148, 285 peripheral nerve, 116, 314 peripheral nervous system, 141, 143, 319 peripheral neuropathy, 126 periventricular, 164 permeability, 124, 128, 212, 213, 214, 217, 218, 223, 224 permeabilization, 316 permeation, 222 peroxidation, 319 persistent pulmonary hypertension of the newborn, 261 personal communication, 9 personality, 191 personality traits, 191 perturbation, 344, 345, 360, 367 PET, 184, 186, 188, 190, 192 PET scan, 184 pH, 223, 251, 317 phagocytosis, 74 phantom limb pain, 183 pharmacokinetics, 134 pharmacological, 31 pharmacology, 78 phenomenology, 95 phenotype, 145, 208, 220, 294, 296, 319 phenotypes, x, 211, 213, 218, 221 phenotypic, 208, 285 phosphatidylcholine, 146 phospholipids, 285 phosphorylation, 121, 133, 143, 154 photon, 117 photoreceptors, 207 physical activity, 236 physiological, xiv, 10, 12, 14, 33, 41, 49, 56, 71, 83, 140, 163, 167, 174, 182, 212, 213, 214, 262, 289, 323, 333, 335, 338 physiology, vii, 1, 30, 74, 79 pilot study, 107, 194, 195 pitch, 59, 62, 64, 65, 70, 73, 74, 76, 182, 345 placebo, 126, 187, 194, 246

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Index plasma, xiv, 127, 208, 212, 216, 218, 222, 313, 317, 318 plasma membrane, 208, 212, 216, 218, 222 plasmapheresis, xiii, 148, 279, 281 plasminogen, 288 plastic, 188, 229 plasticity, 183, 184, 191, 195, 302, 310, 311 platelets, 283, 286 platinum, xiii, xiv, 130, 134, 135, 313, 314, 317, 320, 321 PLP, 337, 352, 364, 365, 371 point mutation, 199, 200, 202 polarity, 142, 213, 222, 223 polarization, 285 polymerase, 294 polymerase chain reaction, 294 polymorphisms, 216, 300 polypeptides, 142 pons, 5, 118, 173 positron emission tomography, 184, 190, 192, 194 postsynaptic, 33, 34, 78, 161 post-translational, 147 post-translational modifications, 147 potassium, x, 72, 82, 200, 211, 321 power, 264, 266, 269, 270, 271, 274, 337, 340, 342, 343, 344, 356, 357, 360, 366, 368 pre-clinical, 84, 187 prediction, xiv, 270, 271, 333, 334 predictors, viii, 137, 187, 189 pre-existing, 113, 114, 127 prefrontal cortex, 187 pregnancy, 154, 282, 288 premature infant, 177 prematurity, 174, 179, 180 preprocessing, 348 presbycusis, 105, 120, 124 pressure, 38, 82, 90, 95, 107, 251, 254, 261, 264, 265 presynaptic, 33, 34, 161 preterm infants, ix, 159, 160, 162, 164, 166, 169, 170, 172, 173, 174, 175, 176, 177, 178, 180 prevention, viii, 81, 83, 96, 104, 107, 110, 228, 319, 320 preventive, viii, 82, 96, 111, 112, 129 primary biliary cirrhosis, 141 primary brain tumors, 131 priming, 187, 194 PRISM, 294 probe, 250, 254, 264, 356, 360, 361, 362 production, 142, 145, 161, 172, 197, 236, 284, 285, 286, 334 prognosis, xii, 279, 281

389 prognostic factors, 113 program, 107, 125, 199, 200, 202, 293, 297 pro-inflammatory, 145, 285 prokaryotic, 140 proliferation, 140, 143, 148, 160, 284 promoter, 133, 183, 215 propagation, 34, 35, 277, 325, 326, 329 proteases, 126 protection, vii, viii, x, 81, 94, 96, 105, 110, 126, 132, 152, 227, 228, 231, 232, 242, 243, 244, 245, 321 protein family, 146, 154, 218 proteins, viii, x, 120, 123, 127, 137, 144, 146, 147, 150, 152, 154, 155, 156, 161, 197, 200, 202, 205, 211, 213, 214, 215, 216, 217, 220, 221, 222, 223, 224, 317, 319, 321 Proteins, v, 211, 213 proteoglycans, 150 protocols, xiii, 187, 189, 243, 280, 286 proto-oncogene, 143 pseudo, 334, 367, 370 PST, 2 psychiatric disorders, 182 psychoacoustic properties, xv, 333 pulmonary hypertension, 261 pulses, 185, 186, 338 PUMA, 123, 134 pyramidal cells, 72

Q quadrupole, xiv, 313, 317 quality of life, 96, 128 quasi-linear, 19 quinine, 191

R Radial Basis Function, 326 radiation, vii, viii, xiv, 4, 111, 112, 113, 114, 115, 116, 117, 118, 119, 120, 121, 122, 123, 124, 125, 126, 127, 128, 129, 130, 131, 132, 133, 134, 313, 314 radiation damage, 113, 116, 131 radiation therapy, xiv, 111, 115, 122, 128, 129, 130, 131, 133, 134, 313, 314 radiculopathy, 116 radio, 87, 334 radiotherapy, vii, viii, 111, 112, 113, 114, 115, 116, 117, 118, 119, 123, 126, 128, 129, 130, 131, 132, 133, 135 radium, 112

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

390 random, 233, 237, 246, 305, 329, 334, 335, 341, 342, 344, 345 rating scale, 182 ratings, 307, 308, 309, 310 RCD, 151 reactive oxygen species (ROS), 83, 111, 120, 121, 123, 124, 125, 126, 129, 318 reactivity, 143, 146, 151, 152, 155, 284 reception, 92 receptors, 71, 120, 142, 188, 199, 208, 319 recognition, xiv, xv, 59, 63, 74, 302, 310, 333, 334, 335, 336, 337, 338, 340, 345, 346, 347, 351, 355, 356, 357, 364, 365, 366, 367, 368, 369, 370, 371, 372 recognition algorithms, 372 recognition test, 367 reconciliation, 58 reconstruction, 23, 49 recovery, xii, xiii, 82, 94, 161, 253, 279, 280, 281, 283, 327, 345, 348 recreational, 105, 108 recruiting, 283 rectification, 335, 345, 361 recycling, 146, 315, 339 refining, 198 reflection, 123, 265, 266 reflectivity, 265 reflexes, 97, 99, 101 regeneration, 96, 174, 177, 318, 319, 322 regenerative capacity, 319 regional, 179, 180, 184 regression, 267, 270 regression analysis, 270 regular, x, 38, 41, 42, 44, 45, 46, 51, 54, 56, 197, 198, 199, 202, 203, 205, 207, 269 regulation, 93, 102, 103, 121, 123, 143, 214, 315 regulations, 102, 103 rehabilitation, 115 relationship, 25, 28, 52, 58, 64, 75, 90, 114, 120, 124, 141, 341, 360, 373 reliability, 91, 98 renal, xiii, 126, 139, 141, 152, 218, 283, 313, 314, 373 renal failure, 283 repair, 82, 123, 127 repetitions, 44 replication, 123, 127 reproduction, 67 research design, 246 reservoir, 340 resistance, 31, 35, 52, 96 resolution, xii, 34, 257, 259, 263, 267, 275, 309, 336, 342, 344, 350

Index resources, xiv, 209, 210, 333, 335 respiration, 262 respiratory, 164, 169, 177 respiratory distress syndrome, 169, 177 response time, 369 responsiveness, 56, 58, 140, 149, 304 resting potential, 214 retina, 143, 144, 207, 209 retrovirus, 219 rheumatoid arthritis, 139, 151 rheumatoid factor, 282, 283, 286 rhinitis, 285 Rho-kinase, 316 risk assessment, 92 risk factors, 162, 178, 179, 260, 261, 286 risks, 96, 102, 104, 105, 107, 245 RNA, 135 robustness, 331, 335, 339, 340, 352 rodent, 161, 180 rodents, 148 roentgen, 130, 221 roughness, 277 RRP, 339 R-squared, 270

S sampling, 21, 204, 234, 342, 353, 364 sampling error, 234 satisfaction, xi, 228, 239, 240, 241, 242, 243, 303 saturation, 356, 359 scaffolding, 220 scaling, 340, 341 scalp, 177, 304 scatter plot, 352, 354, 355 scattering, 15, 354 Schwann cells, 116, 141 scleroderma, xii, 279, 284 scores, 119, 176, 302, 303, 307, 309 screening programs, 260 secretion, 221, 315 segregation, 9, 13, 16, 29, 40 seizures, 189 selecting, xi, 250, 259, 280, 329 selectivity, 36, 82 self-report, 182, 232, 242, 243 semicircular canals, 218 senescence, 123 sensation, 189, 195, 309, 325 sensations, 96 sensitivity, 56, 57, 58, 63, 73, 82, 83, 87, 90, 96, 109, 110, 112, 128, 138, 148, 206, 214, 232, 265, 266, 267, 336, 345, 349

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

Index sensitization, viii, 81 sensorineural hearing loss, xii, xiii, xv, 101, 104, 107, 130, 131, 137, 139, 150, 151, 152, 153, 155, 157, 206, 213, 215, 220, 279, 280, 281, 283, 285, 286, 287, 288, 289, 293, 295, 301, 302, 314, 319, 334 sensors, 265 sensory systems, 4 sequelae, 122, 178, 284 sequencing, xiii, 140, 142, 146, 291, 293 serine, 123, 143, 207 serological marker, 141, 149 Sertoli cells, 219, 223, 225 serum, 139, 142, 145, 149, 151, 152, 153, 154, 157, 176, 253, 287 sexually transmitted diseases, 96 shock, viii, 116, 137, 140, 152, 155, 175, 284, 319 short period, 90, 118 short-term, 91, 97, 315 side effects, xiii, 124, 125, 279, 281, 286, 314, 373 sign, 189, 296 signal quality, 310 signal transduction, 127, 143 signaling, 124, 126 signalling, xiv, 64, 65, 314, 315, 319 signals, 3, 31, 35, 59, 64, 71, 90, 115, 143, 144, 264, 305, 323, 324, 325, 326, 329, 334, 337, 342, 345, 347, 357, 359, 370 signal-to-noise ratio, 89, 267, 304 significance level, 367 signs, 91, 116, 118, 164, 183, 215, 218, 231, 236, 251, 284, 302, 305 skeletal muscle, 143 skills, xiii, 301, 310 SLE, xii, 141, 143, 144, 279 smoking, 83, 95, 96 SNR, 89, 266, 267, 365, 366 SOC, 2, 3, 4, 5, 11, 29, 30, 33, 46, 57, 62, 63 social anxiety, 194 social life, 96 sodium, x, xiii, 56, 207, 211, 280, 282, 283, 286, 287 solid tumors, xiii, 313 somatosensory, 177 sounds, 33, 56, 63, 66, 67, 69, 76, 84, 93, 95, 114, 124, 129, 182, 229, 302, 304, 305, 307, 309, 371 spatial, vii, 1, 6, 19, 25, 40, 46, 58, 62, 64, 75, 304, 308, 310 specialisation, 28, 33

391 specificity, 138, 139, 149, 153, 203, 250, 265, 284 spectral analysis, 269 spectral component, 66 spectroscopy, 146 spectrum, 48, 115, 122, 208, 209, 293, 295, 299, 314, 334, 336, 337, 339, 343, 344, 346, 348, 360, 368 speculation, 280 speech discrimination, 83, 92, 118, 119, 128 speech discrimination score, 119 speech perception, xiii, 301, 310, 334, 336, 347, 353 speech sounds, 371 speed, 200, 202, 334 spinal cord, 116 spindle, 22 spine, 116 spleen, 144 splenomegaly, 143 sporadic, 38, 295, 299 sprouting, 161, 163, 180 stages, viii, 4, 69, 111, 123, 126, 129, 172, 198, 335 standard deviation, 43, 282, 308 standard error, 166, 167, 168, 171, 270, 272 standards, 102, 103, 109, 275 stapes, 82, 264 statistics, 113, 347 stellate cells, 15, 17, 31, 32, 51, 78 stem cells, xiv, 223, 314, 319, 320, 321, 322 STH, 40, 339, 347 strain, 155, 161, 218 strength, 43, 54, 58, 76, 117 stress, xiv, 95, 106, 132, 133, 140, 163, 164, 314, 319, 320 striae, 67 stroke, 133 structural changes, 122 sub-cellular, 142 subdomains, 217, 218 subgroups, 85, 86, 92, 269, 270, 272 subject strategies, 232 subjective, 87, 92, 110, 148, 182, 187, 304 substrates, 74 subtraction, 335 suffering, 87, 92, 94, 192 sulphate, 142 supplemental, 127 supply, 120, 253 suppression, xv, 49, 58, 161, 186, 193, 257, 334, 335, 337, 340, 356, 357, 358, 359, 360, 361, 362, 363, 364, 365, 366, 367, 368, 372

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

392

Index

suppressor, 156, 315, 356, 358, 360, 361, 362 surgery, 132, 138, 310 survival, 120, 122, 124, 133, 162, 208, 315 survival rate, 162 survivors, 162, 261 susceptibility, 83, 106, 113, 127, 134, 276 swelling, 83 symmetry, 275 symptoms, xii, xiv, 87, 90, 96, 106, 112, 155, 184, 215, 279, 281, 285, 288, 313, 314, 373 synapses, 7, 9, 33, 34, 35, 43, 50, 71, 161, 339, 340 synaptic transmission, 167, 169, 173, 178 synaptogenesis, 160, 161, 170 synchronization, 165 synchronous, 344, 345, 369, 371 syndrome, xii, 139, 141, 142, 153, 156, 191, 193, 201, 207, 279, 282, 284, 285, 373 synergistic, 126, 128 syntax, 198 synthesis, 334 systemic lupus erythematosus, 141 systems, xiv, 3, 110, 122, 138, 170, 186, 205, 245, 333, 334, 336, 338, 373

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

T T cells, 143, 145, 147, 154, 155, 285 targets, xiv, 10, 28, 34, 53, 64, 127, 152, 313, 317 taxonomy, 277 technology, 86, 112, 334 teenagers, 104, 106, 110 template matching, 335 temporal lobe, 4 terminals, 32, 34, 35, 146 Tesla, 185 testes, 223, 238 testicular cancer, 126 testis, 218, 219, 225 test-retest reliability, 91, 98 text mining, x, 197, 199, 202, 204, 205 TGF, 142 Th cells, 284 thalamus, 4, 32, 56, 69, 77, 184 therapeutic agents, xiii, 152, 313, 314 therapeutic approaches, xii, 279, 281 therapeutic interventions, 149 therapeutic targets, 152 therapy, xiv, 112, 126, 128, 129, 132, 149, 169, 182, 183, 188, 280, 286, 287, 288, 313, 314, 318, 319 three-dimensional, 73, 131 threonine, 143

threshold level, xi, xii, 263, 265, 266, 267, 268, 269, 270, 271, 272 thresholds, xi, 85, 86, 87, 91, 92, 94, 95, 97, 98, 99, 105, 106, 107, 108, 117, 118, 128, 132, 141, 189, 216, 217, 219, 228, 232, 238, 243, 261, 271, 325 thrombocyte, 281 thrombocytopenia, 283, 286, 289 thrombosis, 285 thromboxane, 286 thymus, 144 thyroid, 143, 146, 286 tight junction, x, 210, 211, 212, 222, 223, 224, 225 timing, vii, 1, 33, 34, 35, 40, 50, 52, 54, 59, 64, 65, 66, 67, 74, 75, 156, 327 tinnitus, vii, ix, xii, 83, 87, 92, 94, 96, 99, 101, 105, 108, 110, 181, 182, 183, 184, 185, 186, 187, 188, 189, 190, 191, 192, 193, 194, 195, 279, 280, 314, 373 TIP, 344 tissue, x, 118, 126, 129, 131, 140, 142, 143, 148, 150, 154, 197, 198, 205, 209, 212, 214, 287, 288 TNF, 284, 285 toddlers, 259, 261 tolerance, 114, 129, 131, 132, 142, 182 tonic, 38, 72 topographic, 22, 28 topology, 214 toxic, xiv, 82, 113, 217, 313, 315, 373 toxic effect, xiv, 313, 315 toxic side effect, 373 toxicity, 122, 126, 127, 128, 130, 169, 315, 319, 320 toxin, 141, 319 training, 228, 270, 325, 326, 327, 329, 348, 364 transcranial direct current stimulation, 193, 195 transcranial magnetic stimulation, ix, 181, 185, 190, 192, 193, 194, 195 transcription, 127, 199, 205 transcription factor, 199, 205 transducer, 334 transduction, 206, 214, 371 transection, 72, 183 transfer, 62, 133, 140, 145, 147, 224, 346 transformation, 57, 77, 336, 340, 355, 360 transformations, 59, 62, 325, 338, 356 transgene, 209 transitions, 123, 216, 334, 337, 347, 351, 352 translational, 147, 149 translocation, 120

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,

Index transmembrane, 120, 146, 200, 201, 207, 214, 215, 216, 218, 222, 223 transmembrane region, 214 transmission, xiv, 33, 34, 35, 71, 167, 169, 173, 178, 254, 264, 266, 325, 327, 328, 329, 333, 334 transplantation, 319 transport, 26, 34, 71, 127, 142, 146, 156, 212, 222, 223, 296 transportation, 99 trauma, 94, 120, 124, 182, 217, 283, 292 triggers, 134 tubular, 126 tumor, 120, 133, 142, 315 tumor growth, 142 tumors, xiii, 130, 131, 133, 135, 143, 313 tumour, 117, 118, 284, 314 tumours, vii, viii, 111, 115, 117, 119, 123, 126, 127, 135 tympanic membrane, 88, 254, 264 tympanometry, 254, 257, 259, 282, 293 tyrosine, 147, 213

Copyright © 2010. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated. All rights reserved.

U ulcerative colitis, xii, 279, 284, 288 ultrasound, 178 ultrastructure, 315 umbilical cord, 251 umbilical cord blood, 251 underlying mechanisms, 183 utricle, 218

V valine, 216 values, 102, 216, 242, 243, 254, 271, 272, 286, 294, 308, 327, 329, 345, 347, 353, 355, 358, 359, 360, 361, 368 variability, ix, 83, 91, 92, 127, 169, 181, 187, 220, 234, 257, 265, 266, 271 variables, 28, 163, 169, 173, 229, 236 variance, 38, 166, 270, 344, 345, 346, 360, 366, 367, 368

393 variation, 88, 95, 113, 127, 178, 297 vascular disease, 141, 152 vascular occlusion, 122 vascularization, 317 vasculitis, 115, 122 vasculogenesis, 161 vasoconstriction, 286 vector, 325, 326, 328, 341, 354, 355, 366, 367 velocity, 35, 165, 264 vertebrates, 74, 139 vertigo, xii, 138, 279, 280, 281, 287, 373 very low birth weight, 179, 260 vesicle, 34, 219, 319, 339 vesicles, 34, 339 vessels, 147, 245 vestibular schwannoma, 132 vestibular system, 215, 218 vestibulocochlear nerve, 3 vibration, 105, 106, 264 videotape, 231, 236 viscosity, 281 visible, 46, 58, 63, 272, 351 visualization, 172 vocalisations, 62 voice, 334, 353 voicing, 338, 339 vulnerability, 109, 134, 161, 174, 262

W water, 127, 212, 213 wavelet, 266, 269, 274 wear, xi, 228, 230, 231, 232, 243 white matter, 160, 161, 173, 175 WMD, 234, 239, 240, 241 word recognition, xv, 334, 365, 366, 367, 369, 371 World Health Organization (WHO), x, 101, 110, 227, 228, 245 X-linked, 207, 292, 299

Z ZO-1, 220, 225

Deafness, Hearing Loss and the Auditory System, Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2010. ProQuest Ebook Central,